Early Brain Development For Social Work Practice: Integrating Neuroscience With Piaget's Theory of Cognitive Development

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 23:640647, 2013

Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC


ISSN: 1091-1359 print/1540-3556 online
DOI: 10.1080/10911359.2013.775936

Early Brain Development for Social Work Practice:


Integrating Neuroscience with Piagets Theory of
Cognitive Development
Tess Lefmann and Terri Combs-Orme
College of Social Work, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, and The Urban
Child Institute, Memphis, Tennessee, USA

Despite social works claim of a bio-psychosocial approach to human behavior and development,
the profession fails to incorporate important physiological knowledge into practice, research, and
education. This article provides an example of the usefulness of understanding the ontology of early
brain development by demonstrating how such knowledge can be integrated into Piagets theory of
cognitive development. We suggest that social works focus on evidence-based practice demands the
incorporation of neuroscience into the professions body of knowledge.
Keywords: Early brain development, Piaget, neuroscience

Social work lacks an ontogenetic view of development, encompassing development from a single
cell through every stage to death. Despite growing knowledge in neuroscience and genetics to
inform such an ontogenetic view, a strong biological component has yet to be incorporated into
social work practice, research, or education. This is especially troubling given the current emphasis
on evidence-based practice (Gilgun, 2005). Other fields, such as psychology (Ray, 2004) have
recognized that evidence-based practice must be based on the most up-to-date science about
human biology, whereas social work has yet to heed Saleebys (1992) call of 20 years ago to
incorporate biology into the professional knowledge base.
Instead, social work has emphasized classic stage models of development in human behavior
in the social environment (HBSE) coursework content (see any textbook on HBSE). Such models
describe (but do not explain) development as a sequence of stages wherein specific developmental
tasks (behaviors) are mastered in sequence; individuals who fail to satisfactorily complete one
stage are expected to experience difficulties successfully achieving subsequent stages. HBSE
courses in social work typically feature the models of Erikson, Vygotsky, Piaget, and others (e.g.,
see Zaparanick & Wodarski, 2004), though little attention has been paid to linking such theories
directly to social work practice, and those links are far from obvious (see discussion in Thyer,
2001).
We suggest that as a transition to a more scientific approach that heeds Saleebys (1992) call,
stage models may easily be integrated with important knowledge about brain development to
enrich the content and make it more useful to social work. As an example of how this integration
This work was supported by funding from The Urban Child Institute in Memphis, TN.
Address correspondence to Tess Lefmann, College of Social Work, University of Tennessee, 204 Henson Hall,
Knoxville, TN 37996, USA. E-mail: tlefmann@utk.edu

640

EARLY BRAIN DEVELOPMENT FOR SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE

641

might be accomplished, this paper overlays the early biological development of the brain with
Piagets sensorimotor stage of development.

PIAGETS COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY


Piaget came to the study of psychology through his search for how knowledge developed in
the human child (Mayer, 2005). With a degree in the natural sciences and zoology, he entered
the field primed with a developmental and genetic perspective (Duveen, 2000). After conducting
many studies using traditional methods of data collection, Piaget sought to elicit more genuine
responses in children, thus changing his methods to a less guided form of research that combined
naturalistic observation, psychometrics, and the psychiatric clinical examination. The synthesis of
these methods was used to observe how children responded to and articulated certain situations
with their own reasoning as a way of examining their thought processes (Mayer, 2005). The
results formed the stages of development within Piagets theory. Like Freud, Piaget believed that
development of the structures of the brain is key to the developmental tasks he described, but too
little was known at the time, and technology was inadequate to explore and demonstrate those
connections (Kolb & Fantie, 2009). Every psychological explanation comes sooner or later to
biology or logic : : : Piaget said in 1950 (p. 3).
Piagets four major stages are the sensorimotor period (birth to age 2); the preoperational
thought period (about age 2 to age 7); the concrete-operations period (about age 7 to age 11);
and the formal operations period (about age 11 to age 15). Piaget (1973) stated that there is
a constant order of succession : : : that is, in order to reach a certain stage, previous steps must
be taken : : : thus we reach a hierarchy of mental structures which are built in a certain order of
integration (pp. 1011). In this explanation, we see foreshadowing of scientific descriptions of
the development of the brain: from the bottom up, and from the inside out, from simple automatic
functions such as breathing, to the more complex, up to higher-order reasoning and problem
solving.
Neuroscience research in the last two decades calls attention to the enormous impact of
experience and the environment on infant development and the foundational nature of this stage
for the entire life course (Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). For this reason, this paper focuses on
Piagets earliest (sensorimotor) period of development and the associated maturational changes in
the brain.

Piagets Sensorimotor Period


The study of infancy proved to be challenging for Piaget, as he was of course unable to rely
on language as a tool for understanding infants inner processes (Duveen, 2000). However, his
belief in development as a universal process (a belief that is tempered now by an understanding
of how culture interacts with physiology to influence development [Keller, 2008]) allowed him
the freedom to observe small sample sizes, mainly his three children. Although Piaget usually did
not describe his methods, we know that he differentiated the sub-stages within the sensorimotor
period based on interventions shaped by Piaget and his wife, such as rearranging objects within
a room and assessing the childrens reactions, (Mayer, 2005).
The period from birth to age 2 may be the most dynamic and important phase of brain
development in humans (Knickmeyer et al., 2008), and it is certainly fascinating. Key maturational
events put elementary cognitive processes in place that then allow for the development of abstract
thought, planning, and cognitive flexibility (Levin, Culhane, Hartmann, Evankovich, & Mattson,
1991) as observed by Piaget. Piagets observations of children between birth and 2 two led to his

642

T. LEFMANN AND T. COMBS-ORME

differentiation of six sub-stages within the sensorimotor period, during which childrens learning
is based on movement and the five senses.
Stage 1: Birth to 1 Month
At birth, the brain weighs only about 25% of the adult brain, but in the first 3 years of
life, it will grow to 90% of adult weight (DiPietro, 2000), chiefly through the development
of wiring and connections. Three key processes during this time drive the growth: dendritic
arborization, synaptogenesis, and myelination. Dendritic arborization is the branching out of
dendrites, connections that reach the axons of other neurons and begin transmitting messages
from neuron to neuron, putting into motion the communication between neurons that is the basis
of brain functioning (Webb, Monk, & Nelson, 2001). Functioning of the brain occurs in the
connections among neurons, as chemical substances or neurotransmitters excite or inhibit the
firing of the neurons.
Piagets first stage emphasizes action through reflexive actions (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969),
including sucking objects, following moving or interesting objects with the eyes, and closing the
hand when objects make contact with the palm (Piaget, 1963). The child is learning about his or
her world through the senses. Piagets focus on the sensorimotor is appropriate to what is occurring
in the brain in these early days. During the first 5 weeks of rapid development, glucose uptake, the
brains energy metabolism (Chugani, Phelps, & Mazziotta, 1987), is highest in the sensorimotor
cortex, thalamus, brainstem, and cerebellar vermis (Johnson, 2001). These areas, together, form
a section of the basic motor pathway of the primary motor cortex (Terumitsu, Ikeda, Kwee, &
Nakada, 2009), which is responsible for generating the neural impulses controlling the execution
of movement.
The process of building connections among neurons, called synaptogenesis, is the basis of
learning. In this first month of life, the infant is learning to track objects and then to reach for
them, for example, by repeatedly practicing and building synapses in the parts of the brain that
process and connect visual and motor behavior. The phrase Neurons that fire together, wire together
is an expression of this fact: Repeated signaling builds and strengthens connections among the
neurons; this is the essence of learning. The infants early movements and exploration build as
many as 40,000 synapses per second through the first year of life (Tau & Peterson, 2010).
The behavioral systems that function early in development described by Piaget show the earliest
pattern of myelination (Webb et al., 2001). Myelination is the process through which the myelin
sheath, formed of lipids (fats) and proteins, is developed and wrapped around axons to increase
the speed at which impulses are transmitted between neurons. It can be compared to the insulation
on electrical wires; without that insulation, messages between the neurons would be inefficient
and slow. Early in the first year, movements are jerky and automatic but, over the four sub-stages
in the first year, become more coordinated and intentional. Improved myelination is critical to this
refinement of movement (Deoni et al., 2011).
Benes, Turtle, Khan, and Farol (1994) found that the pathway that carries information about
the visual tracking of moving objects and the ability to coordinate movements of the eyes, head,
neck, and trunk (the rhombencephalic portion of the medial longitudinal fasciculus) is one of the
earliest central nervous system tracts to myelinate, allowing the infants early sucking, grasping,
and following objects described by Piaget, as myelination improves signaling and the infants
accuracy in tracking and grasping.
Stage 2: 1 to 4 Months
Piagets second sub-stage of the sensorimotor period describes primary circular reactions,
which are basically new habits (repetitive actions) and accommodations based on learning (1963).
For example, when a child systematically sucks his or her thumb, no longer due to chance contact

EARLY BRAIN DEVELOPMENT FOR SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE

643

but by coordinating his or her hand and mouth, Piaget referred to this as acquired accommodation
(Piaget). We also may call this learning. Repeated action improves the infants ability to act
intentionally.
Vision is not well developed at birth (a fact almost universally acknowledged as new parents
hold their newborns close to their faces to establish eye contact), but the visual cortex and
synaptogenesis in that area develop rapidly between the second and fourth postnatal months
(Huttenlocher & de Courten, 1987). This rapid development in vision corresponds with Piagets
observations of the infants ability to coordinate actions such as sucking his or her thumb, thus
allowing for new levels of learning. As his or her vision improves, for example, he or she becomes
more competent at reaching for and grasping specific objects he or she wants.
Using magnetic resonance imaging, Barkovich, Kjos, Jackson, and Norman (1988) found
increases in signal intensity of white matter in infants by 4 months. White matter, which consists
of myelinated axons, facilitates communication among different parts of the brain. The posterior
portion of the corpus callosum (called the splenium) communicates somatosensory information
between the two halves of the parietal lobe and the visual center of the occipital lobe during
this time (Luna et al., 2001). The greater the communication among brain regions, the more
competent the infant is at integrating motor, sensory, and cognitive information, the very skills
Piaget described.
Stage 3: 4 to 8 Months
Piagets third stage focuses on secondary circular reactions, which mark the beginning of
the infants ability to distinguish between iself and the outside environment (Thomas, 1996).
Piaget described the difference between primary and secondary circular reactions as being about
intention. Primary circular reactions in sub-stage 2 are simple organic movements just for the
sake of movement, while secondary reactions are centered on creating a result in the external
environment. Again, we see how learning occurs through repetition; the infant is learning that he
or she can take specific actions that cause specific effects.
This learning corresponds to completion of dendritic arborization (laying down of the connections) in the hippocampus, the brain region most associated with learning and memory (Paldino
& Purpura, 1979). Also about this time, the cerebellum, which is involved in motor coordination
and balance, has more than doubled in size (Knickmeyer et al., 2008). This remarkable growth
supports the rapid motor development during infancy and the intentional movement that the infant
is beginning to use to influence his or her environment.
Stage 4: 8 to 12 Months
Piagets fourth sub-stage focuses on emergence of practical intelligence: envisioning goals or
desired ends and then employing existing schemes to achieve the ends (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969).
Such activities are evidence of the brains executive functions (planning and carrying out plans),
and are supported by the frontal cortex, which plays a crucial role in higher-order cognition (Luna
et al., 2001). Not coincidentally, the frontal cortex is dramatically increasing its synaptic density
at this time (Huttenlocher, 1990).
Gains in neuromuscular functioning are occurring rapidly during this time, too, supported by
completion of dendritic arborization in the previous sub-stage, as myelination of the corticospinal
tract reaches mature levels in the brain stem by about 1 year and in the spinal cord by about 28
months (Brody, Kinney, Kloman, & Gilles, 1987). As neuromuscular functioning improves, the
infant becomes ever better at carrying out his or her goal of banging on his or her high chair with
a spoon to make noise or throwing his or her bottle to get Mom and Dad to react.
And last, during this fourth stage (between 8 and 12 months), the frontal, parietal, and occipital
lobes continue myelination, furthering the development of sensory systems and conscious thought

644

T. LEFMANN AND T. COMBS-ORME

(Paterson et al., 2006). Soon the infant will be able to plan and carry out a series of actions to
get what he or she wants more effectively.
Stage 5: 12 to 18 Months
Piagets (1963) fifth sub-stage focuses on tertiary circular reactions, in which the 1-year-old
experiments to find out how things are new. For example, the infant has discovered that banging
a spoon on his or her high chair makes a satisfying noise; will banging the spoon on the wall do
that, too? How about filling the spoon with beets and banging the spoon on the wall? This action
also creates a satisfying visual display!
Key to this stage is the development of object relations, meaning the way children conceive of
themselves in relation to the objects of the world as well as the way they see objects relationships
to one another (Thomas, 1996). The idea of object permanence, the understanding that objects
continue to exist even when they cannot be seen, heard, or touched, is a key component of object
relations (Thomas, 1996) and believed by Piaget to be a big cognitive breakthrough for the child.
This is the beginning of conceptual thinking; the infant can think about his or her ball, even when
his or her ball cannot be seen (Piaget, 1954).
Diamond (1990) demonstrated that the maturation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)
is associated with successful performance in the object retrieval task, a test demonstrating object
permanence. In this task, the DLPFC is the highest cortical area responsible for motor planning,
organization, and regulation (Johnson, 2001), and according to Diamond, plays an important role
in integrating both the inhibitory skills necessary to prevent the infant reaching to the wrong
hiding place and for remembering where the object was hidden.
The caudate nucleus of the basal ganglia also increases in volume during this time (Knickmeyer
et al., 2008). The caudate nucleus is crucial for the control of movement, suggesting that its
growth relates to an increase in motor development (Herrero, Barcia, & Navarro, 2002). It is not
coincidental that most infants begin to walk during this time, as they now have an understanding
that they can move from one place to another as well as control their muscles and legs to do so.
Stage 6: 18 to 24 Months
The final stage of Piagets sensorimotor period is marked by the beginning of insight and
creativity, ushering the way into Piagets preoperational thought period (Thomas, 1996), when
children become increasingly adept at using symbols, as seen in the increase in playing and
pretending, for example riding a stick and pretending it is a horse. Piaget emphasized that
by repeating this inventiveness across a wide range of objects and actions, children construct
new ways of dealing with objects and new knowledge about objects themselves, thus forming
reflective intelligence (Piaget, 1963).
The infant also shows an increasing ability to voluntarily initiate and suppress behavior,
demonstrating a key component of this operational stage. In humans, development of this skill is
promoted by maturation of integrated functioning (connections) among higher parts of the brain:
the neocortex, striatum, thalamus, and cerebellum (Luna et al., 2001).
By the time the infant is 2 years of age, his or her total brain volume (number, size, and density
of neurons and glia as well as dendritic and axonal number and density) is about 83% of adult
size (Knickmeyer, 2008). Most of that growth is in the first year, as the brain increases about
101%, compared to 15% in the second year (Webb et al., 2001).
However, brain size is only part of maturation. Synaptic pruning, the loss of synapses, is
also important for enhancing widely distributed brain functions by refining synaptic connections
and enhancing rapid transfer of information throughout the brain (Luna et al., 2001). Increasing
cognitive capacity during childhood, thus, corresponds with a gradual loss, rather than constant
formation, of new synapses and presumably a strengthening of remaining synaptic connections

EARLY BRAIN DEVELOPMENT FOR SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE

645

(Casey, Gieed, & Thomas, 2000). This fact harkens back to Neurons that fire together, wire
together. Synapses that are not reinforced by experience are pruned back.
Importance to Social Work Practice, Research, and Education
How does understanding how the brain develops better our understanding of behavior and, in
particular, development? Casey et al. (2000) proposed that understanding where in the brain a
particular function resides helps constrain our theories and models of behavior.
Understanding the function of the brain, thus, has applications not just to theory but to practice,
research, and education in social work. Understanding how the brain develops in infancy and
connecting ontogeny with functioning helps social workers understand the tremendous importance
of infancy for brain development and future functioning and achievement.
Application to Practice
Infancy is not a period that is well represented in the social work knowledge base, despite
the fact that many (if not most) of the problems in which social workers intervene arguably
have roots in this period of life (Combs-Orme, Wilson, Cain, Page, & Kirby, 2003). Enhanced
understanding of the importance of infancy through integration of neuroscience content should
provide social workers with knowledge to influence social policy and service design and delivery
(Egan, Neely-Barnes, & Combs-Orme, 2011).
Based on Shonkoff and Phillipss (2000) compilation of research about early brain development,
we have within our reach a number of avenues for enhancing early brain development and, in so
doing, enriching the lives of individuals and improving our society overall (Dawson, Ashman, &
Carver, 2000).
Knickmeyer et al. (2008) state that although the first year of life is a period of developmental
vulnerability, it may also be a period in which therapeutic interventions could have the greatest
positive effect. Understanding of the ontogeny of brain development during this time provides a
better basis for identifying children at risk for developmental disorders than simple observations of
infant skills. Such understanding is also more useful for the initiation of appropriate interventions
(Knickmeyer et al.).
The maturation of certain key regions of the brain can also be used to establish milestones
for normal development (Barkovich et al., 1988), which can be useful for better understanding
of parenting best practices. Infants rely on their caregivers for the provision and regulation of
interactive and other stimuli (Combs-Orme et al., 2003; Shapiro & Applegate, 2000). As a result,
the quality of a childs caregiving experiences influences the neuronal wiring and development
of the brain (Schore, 2001). Indeed, research on attachment theory and neuroscience has begun
to merge, as science comes to understand how early parenting and relationships contribute to
the development of the brain (Schore 2001). Social work practitioners should be well versed in
this environmental interaction so that knowledge can be passed on to parents and communities
appropriately and effectively.
For example, the Urban Child Institute (UCI) in Memphis, Tennessee, is a large nonprofit organization whose goal is to raise awareness about and improve early brain development in Memphis.
The UCI promotes social policy (e.g., universal pre-kindergarten), sponsors research, and maintains
a constant program of public education and training of professionals in the needs of young children.
Partnerships with service providers integrate knowledge of early brain development into various
types of interventions located in diverse locations. (See www.theurbanchildinstitute.org)
With some exceptions (e.g., Egan et al., 2011; Farmer, 2009; Shapiro & Applegate, 2000), social
work has yet to integrate neuroscience knowledge into social work practice, but this application
is crucial to the further development of evidence-based practice.

646

T. LEFMANN AND T. COMBS-ORME

Application to Research
Recent advances in cognitive neuroscience have allowed researchers to investigate the structure
and function of the infant brain (Paterson et al., 2006). However, few studies or articles have
examined the intersection between brain and behavior over the first few years of life or attempted
to synthesize the research on human neural development (Webb et al., 2001). Indeed, DiPietro
(2000) has emphasized the failure to link brain development and behavior more closely. This
burgeoning knowledge base should be incorporated into and furthered by the social work research
agenda so that we can establish a fully comprehensive bio-psychosocial assessment of human
development (a perspective long claimed by the profession).
Increasingly, it is clear that biology must be an important component of understanding human
behavior as well as behavior change (Combs-Orme, 2013). For example, evidence suggests that
effective cognitive behavior therapy results in part from altering neural pathways (e.g., Clark &
Beck, 2010). Social work is at risk of falling behind other professions if we do not incorporate
biology into intervention research.
Application to Education
Biological components of bio-psychosocial development addressed in courses in HBSE are
inadequate and in some cases misleading (Combs-Orme, 2012; Farmer, 1999). With advances into
the ontogeny of human behavior, it is necessary to incorporate this knowledge into the social work
curriculum. It is time to recognize that courses in HBSE cannot review and evaluate every theory
and framework of human behavior that has ever been advanced in order to focus on scientifically
valid and heuristic knowledge that can more fully inform complex social work practice.

REFERENCES
Barkovich, J. A., Kjos, B. O., Jackson, D. E., & Norman, D. (1988). Normal maturation of the neonatal and infant brain:
MR imaging at 1.5 T1 . Neuroradiology, 166, 173180.
Bastian, A., & Thach, W. T. (2002). Structure and function of the cerebellum. In M. Manto & M. Pandolfo (Eds.), The
cerebellum and its disorders (pp. 4966). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Benes, F. M., Turtle, M., Khan, Y., & Farol, P. (1994). Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation
occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51(6), 477.
Brody, B., Kinney, H., Kloman, A., & Gilles, F. (1987). Sequence of central nervous system myelination in human infancy:
I. An autopsy study of myelination. Journal of Neuropathology & Experimental Neurology, 46, 283301.
Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to
cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54, 241257.
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional
development. Annals of Neurology, 22, 487497.
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression: Convergence with neurobiological findings. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(9), 418424.
Combs-Orme, T. (1988). Infant mortality: Legacy of success for social work. Social Service Review, 62, 83102.
Combs-Orme, T. (1990). Social work practice in maternal and child health. New York, NT: Springer Publishing Company.
Combs-Orme, T. (2013). Epigenesis and the social work imperative. Social Work, 58(1), 2330.
Combs-Orme, T., Wilson, E., Cain, D. S., Page, T. & Kirby, L. D. (2003). Context-based parenting in infancy: Background
and conceptual issues. Child and Adolescent Social Work, 20(6), 437472.
Dawson, G., Ashman, S. B., & Carver, L. J. (2000). The role of early experience in shaping behavioral and brain
development and its implications for social policy. Development and Psychopathology, 12(4), 695712.
Deoni, S. C., Mercure, E., Blasi, A., Gasston, D., Thomson, A., Johnson, M., : : : Murphy, D. G. (2011). Mapping
infant brain myelination with magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(2), 784791. doi: 10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.2106-10.2011
Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the AB and delayed response
tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 60, 231267.

EARLY BRAIN DEVELOPMENT FOR SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE

647

DiPietro, J. A. (2000). Baby and the brain: Advances in child development. Annual Review of Public Health, 21, 455471.
Duveen, G. (2000). Piaget ethnographer. Social Science Information, 39(1), 7997.
Egan, M., Neely-Barnes, S., & Combs-Orme, T. (2011). Integrating neuroscience into the lifespan. Journal of Social Work
Education, 47(2), 269282.
Farmer, R. L. (1999). Clinical HBSE concentration: A transactional model. Journal of Social Work Education, 35(2), 289.
Farmer, R. L. (2009). Neuroscience and social work practice: The missing link. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gilgun, J. F. (2005). The four cornerstones of evidence-based practice in social work. Research on Social Work Practice,
15(1), 5261.
Herrero, M. T., Barcia, C., & Navarro, J. M. (2002). Functional anatomy of thalamus and basal ganglia. Childs Nervous
System, 18, 386404.
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517527.
Huttenlocher, P. R., & de Courten, C. (1987). The development of synapses in striate cortex of man. Human Neurobiology,
6, 19.
Johnson, M. H. (2000). Functional brain development in infants: Elements of an interactive specialization framework.
Child Development, 71(1), 7581.
Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in humans. Nature, 2, 475483.
Keller, H. (2008). Culture and biology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 668681.
Knickmeyer, R. C., Gouttard, S., Kang, C., Evans, D., Wilber, K., Smith, J. K., : : : Gilmore, J. H. (2008). A structural
MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(47), 1217612182.
Kolb, B., & Fantie, B. D. (2009). Development of the childs brain and behavior. In C. R. Reynolds & E. Fletcher-Janzen
(Eds.), Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology (3rd ed., pp. 1946). New York, NY: Springer,
Levin, H. S., Culhane, K. A., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., & Mattson, A. J. (1991). Developmental changes in
performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 377395.
Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P., Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J., : : : Sweeney, J. A. (2001).
Maturation of widely distributed brain function subserves cognitive development. Neuroimage, 13, 786793.
Mayer, S. J. (2005). The early evolution of Jean Piagets clinical method. History of Psychology, 8(4), 362382.
Paldino, A., & Purpura, D. (1979). Quantitative analysis of the spatial distribution of axonal and dendritic terminals of
hippocampal pyramidal neurons in immature human brain. Experimental Neurology, 64, 604619.
Paterson, S. J., Heim, S., Friedman, J. T., Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. A. (2006). Development of structure and function
in the infant brain: Implications for cognition, language and social behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
30, 10871105.
Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J., & Cook, M. (Trans.) (1954). The development of object construct. In The construction of reality in the child
(pp. 396). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company, Inc.
Piaget, J. (1973). The child and reality: Problems of genetic psychology. New York, NY: Penguin Books.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Ray, O. (2004). How the mind hurts and heals the body. American Psychologist, 59, 2940.
Saleebey, D. (1992). Biologys challenge to social work: Embodying the person-in-environment perspective. Social Work,
37(2), 112118.
Schore, A. N. (2001). Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant
mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22(12), 766.
Shapiro, J. R., & Applegate, J. S. (2000). Cognitive neuroscience, neurobiology, and affect regulation: Implications for
clinical social work. Clinical Social Work Journal, 28(1), 921.
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Tau, G. Z., & Peterson, G. S. (2010). Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews, 35,
147168.
Terumitsu, M., Ikeda, K., Kwee, I. L., & Nakada, T. (2009). Participation of primary motor cortex area 4a in complex
sensory processing: 3.0-T fMRI study. Neuroreport, 20(7), 679683.
Thomas, R. M. (1996). Comparing theories of child development. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.
Thyer, B. (2001). What is the role of theory in research on social work practice? Journal of Social Work Education, 37(1),
925.
Webb, S. J., Monk, C. S., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: Implications
for human development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(2), 147171.
Zaparanick, T. L., & Wodarski, J. S. (2004). A curriculum for human behavior in the social environment. Journal of
Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 10(3), 124.

Copyright of Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment is the property of Taylor
& Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like