Panic Disorder

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Psychopharmacology

http://jop.sagepub.com/ Acute and chronic treatment with serotonin reuptake inhibitors exert opposite effects on respiration in rats: possible implications for panic disorder
Kristina Annerbrink, Marie Olsson, Jan Hedner and Elias Eriksson J Psychopharmacol 2010 24: 1793 originally published online 13 October 2009 DOI: 10.1177/0269881109106908 The online version of this article can be found at: http://jop.sagepub.com/content/24/12/1793

Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

British Association for Psychopharmacology

Additional services and information for Journal of Psychopharmacology can be found at: Email Alerts: http://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://jop.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Citations: http://jop.sagepub.com/content/24/12/1793.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Dec 7, 2010 OnlineFirst Version of Record - Oct 13, 2009 What is This?

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

Original Paper

Acute and chronic treatment with serotonin reuptake inhibitors exert opposite effects on respiration in rats: possible implications for panic disorder
Kristina Annerbrink1, Marie Olsson2, Jan Hedner3 and Elias Eriksson1

Journal of Psychopharmacology 24(12) 17931801 ! The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0269881109106908 jop.sagepub.com

Abstract
Prompted by the suggested importance of respiration for the pathophysiology of panic disorder, we studied the influence of serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs) as well as other serotonin-modulating compounds on respiration in freely moving rats. The effect on respiration after acute administration of compounds enhancing synaptic levels of serotonin, that is, the serotonin reuptake inhibitors paroxetine and fluoxetine, the serotonin-releasing agents m-chlorophenylpiperazine and d-fenfluramine, and the selective 5-HT1A antagonist WAY-100635, were investigated. All serotonin-releasing substances decreased respiratory rate in unrestrained, awake animals, suggesting the influence of serotonin on respiratory rate under these conditions to be mainly inhibitory. In line with a previous study, rats administered fluoxetine for 23 days or more, on the other hand, displayed an enhanced respiratory rate. The results reinforce the assumption that the effect of subchronic administration of a serotonin reuptake inhibitor on certain serotonin-regulated parameters may be opposite to that obtained after acute administration. We suggest that our observations may be of relevance for the fact that acute administration of SRIs, d-fenfluramine, or m-chlorophenylpiperazine often is anxiogenic in panic disorder patients, and that weeks of administration of an SRI leads to a very effective prevention of panic.

Keywords
serotonin, panic disorder, respiration, fluoxetine, paroxetine, m-chlorophenylpiperazine, WAY-100635, d-fenfluramine

Introduction
The theory that panic disorder is due to an aberration in the control of respiration (Klein, 1993, Nardi et al., 2009) is supported by the following observations: i) panic attacks in contrast to normal fear are often characterized by respiratory symptoms, such as hyperventilation and shortness of breath; ii) panic attacks can be elicited by respiratory stimulants such as carbon dioxide, sodium lactate, caeine and doxapram (Abelson et al., 1996; Gorman et al., 1984; Kent et al., 2001; Lee et al., 1993; Liebowitz et al., 1984; Nardi et al., 2008; Papp et al., 1997; Pitts and McClure, 1967); iii) patients with panic disorder display enhanced respiratory variability between panic attacks both while awake and during sleep (Abelson et al., 2001; Gorman et al., 1988; Martinez et al., 2001; Papp et al., 1993; Perna et al., 1994; Stein et al., 1995; Wilhelm et al., 2001); and iv) twin studies suggesting that the same genetic determinants cause both CO2 hyper-reactivity and increased risk for panic disorder (Battaglia et al., 2008, 2009). Whether the respiratory abnormalities characterizing panic disorder patients are best explained in terms of chemoreceptor hyper-responsiveness (Klein, 1993) or an irregularity of respiration unrelated to chemoreceptor function (Sinha et al., 2000; Struzik et al., 2002) is, however, a matter of controversy. Chronic treatment with serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs) is an eective way of preventing panic attacks, suggesting that brain serotonergic neurons are parts of or inuence those neuronal circuits that generate panic anxiety

(Den Boer and Westenberg, 1988; Modigh et al., 1992). The inuence of brain serotonergic activity on anxiety in panic disorder patients is, however, complex, as illustrated by the fact that acute administration often elicit a paradoxical increase in anxiety, and that serotonin releasing drugs, such as d-fenuramine (Garattini et al., 1987) and m-chlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP) (Eriksson et al., 1999), may trigger panic attacks (Charney et al., 1987; Mortimore and Anderson, 2000). Why acute facilitation of serotonergic activity elicits anxiety in responsive subjects, whereas long-term treatment with SRIs prevents panic attacks, remains unknown. The idea that serotonergic neurons participate in the regulation of respiration gains support from the presence of serotonergic nerve terminals and serotonergic receptors in respiratory nuclei such as the nucleus tractus solitarii,

1 Department of Pharmacology, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden. 2 Department of Pharmacy, Apoteket AB, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden. 3 Sleep Laboratory Services, Pulmonary Medicine, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden.

Corresponding author: Kristina Annerbrink, Department of Pharmacology, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, POB 431, SE 405 30 Gothenburg, Sweden Email: kristina.annerbrink@pharm.gu.se

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

1794
the hypoglossal nucleus, and the preBotzinger complex (Feldman et al., 2003; Richerson et al., 2005; Richter et al., 2003). How these neurons inuence baseline respiration, and/ or the response to hypercapnea and hypoxia, however, remains a matter of controversy, dierent studies yielding conicting results. Partly the divergences in outcome of studies seems related to whether the studied animals have been i) awake and under restraint stress; ii) awake and freely moving; iii) asleep; or iv) anaesthetized or otherwise pretreated during the experiment. Moreover, the possibility that serotonin may exert dierent eects on the dierent components of the breathing cycle, that is respiratory rate and tidal volume, respectively, and that dierent serotonergic receptors may exert dierent and even opposite eects on the regulation of ventilation, may also contribute to the lack of consensus on this issue. According to one theory, serotonergic neurons serve as central chemoreceptors, responding to acidosis or enhanced CO2 levels (Johnson et al., 2005; Richerson, 2004; Severson et al., 2003) and exert mainly a stimulatory inuence on respiration. In line with this view, which is, however, not undisputed (Mulkey et al., 2004), electric activation of raphe cell bodies (where serotonergic neurons originate) or local exposure of raphe neurons to acidosis or enhanced CO2 levels have been reported to stimulate respiration, as has administration of certain serotonin receptor agonists (Manzke et al., 2003). Moreover, baseline respiration, as well as the respiratory response to CO2, is reported to be reduced in rats in which serotonergic transmission has been impaired by administration of a serotonergic neurotoxin (Martin-Body and Grundy, 1985). When interpreting these results, it should however be considered that many of these studies have been performed on anaesthetized or otherwise pretreated animals, rather than in animals that are intact, awake, and freely moving. Supporting the alternative view that serotonin may serve mainly to inhibit respiration, rather than to stimulate it, serotonin synthesis inhibitors have been shown to induce hyperventilation without aecting the response to CO2 (Annerbrink et al., 2003; Bach et al., 1993, Mitchell et al., 1983; Olson, 1987). In line with this, tryptophan depletion in humans, which leads to impaired serotonergic transmission, enhance ventilation without inuencing CO2 sensitivity (Struzik et al., 2002). Given that serotonergic neurons inuence the regulation of respiration, and that panic disorder is characterized by dysregulated respiration, it is tempting to speculate that an inuence of SRIs on ventilation may contribute to the marked eects of these drugs when administered to panic disorder patients, including both the anxiety-provoking eect observed at acute treatment and the impressive anti-panic eect obtained by subchronic administration. In a previous study, we showed that administration of the SRI paroxetine for 5 or 15 weeks increased respiratory rate, but not ventilation, in awake, unrestrained, freely moving rats (Olsson et al., 2004). However, this study did not address the possible eect of acute administration. The aim of the present study was to compare the respiratory eect of acute SRI administration with that of chronic treatment, the a priori hypothesis being that acute administration would exert the opposite eect of

Journal of Psychopharmacology 24(12)


chronic treatment, that is, that it would reduce respiratory rate, just as acute SRI administration exerts the opposite eect to that of chronic treatment on anxiety in panic disorder patients. In addition, we assessed the inuence of acute administration of the serotonin-releasing, panic-provoking agents mCPP and d-fenuramine, the a priori hypothesis being that these drugs too would exert an eect on respiration opposite to that of subchronic SRI administration, that is that they would reduce respiratory rate. Finally, the eects of acute administration of 5-HT1A receptor agonists and antagonists on respiration were assessed.

Materials and methods


Ethics
The study has been carried out in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, as adopted and promulgated by the NIH, and was approved by the Ethics Committee for Animal Experiments, University of Gothenburg, Sweden.

Animals
Male Wistar rats were obtained from B&K (Sollentuna, Sweden), and housed in cages with 34 rats per cage under controlled conditions: temperature 212 C, humidity 5565%, and reversed 12/12 h light/dark cycle. Food and water were available ad libitum at all times except during the experiments.

Respiratory measurement
All experiments took place in a silent room with lights on and an observer placed approximately 1 m from the rat. Two different pieces of equipment were used to measure respiratory rate and tidal volume in freely moving rats. For experiments 1, 2, 4, 5, and 7, a plethysmograph, allowing the animals to move freely, that was built within the department and described in detail previously (Annerbrink et al., 2003) was used. For experiments 3 and 6, the Unrestrained Whole Body Plethysmograph from Buxco Research Systems (Wilmington, NC, USA) was used. Before the day of the acute experiment, the animals were habituated to the plethysmograph on three dierent days (15 min + 15 min + 15 min). Before recording started on the day of the experiment, the animals were rated by means of gross observation with respect to motor activity. Animals that were not still after having spent 1015 minutes in the plethysmograph were disqualied, as were those moving during the 2 minutes of registration of respiratory rate (breaths per min BPM), tidal volume (ml/breath), and minute ventilation (ml/min). The rater was blind as to whether the rat was given saline or active substance.

Drugs
All drugs were acquired from Sigma-Aldrich, Stockholm, Sweden, and administered via intraperitoneal injections.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

Annerbrink et al.

1795

Serum concentrations
Serum concentrations of uoxetine and noruoxetine were assessed using high-performance liquid chromatography followed by liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry at the Division of Clinical Chemistry and Pharmacology, Lund University Hospital.

Statistics
Dierences between groups in experiments 1, 3, 4, and 6 were statistically evaluated using ANOVA followed by Fishers PLSD test. In experiments 2 and 5, statistical dierences were evaluated using paired and unpaired t-tests, respectively. Since rats not being still before the registration were disqualied, recordings were not obtained from all animals at all time points in experiment 7, the missing values precluding the use of two way ANOVA when analysing the eect of treatment. Instead, the two groups were compared for each time of assessment using unpaired t-test. All values are expressed as mean (SD). While p-values from ANOVA are presented in the results section, p-values from Fishers PLSD test and t-tests are shown in the tables. p 0.05 was considered statistically signicant.

Experiments
Experiment 1: 40 male Wistar rats were randomized into 4 groups and received saline or uoxetine (1, 10, or 30 mg/kg). Measurements took place 1 h after the injection. After the recordings, the rats were decapitated so that trunk blood for analysis of uoxetine and noruoxetine levels in serum could be obtained. Experiment 2: 15 male Wistar rats received saline and paroxetine (10 mg/kg) in a randomized cross-over design with a minimum wash-out period of 10 days. Measurements took place 1 h after the injection. Experiment 3: 40 Wistar rats were randomized into 4 groups and received saline or 8-OH-DPAT (0.01, 0.1, or 0.15 mg/kg). Measurements took place 1 h after the injection. Experiment 4: 15 male Wistar rats received saline or mCPP (5, 10, or 20 mg/kg). Measurements took place 1 h after the injection. To reduce the number of rats used for this experiment, the same animal could receive more than one treatment, but no animal received more than two treatments, and there was always a wash-out period of 10 days between treatments. In total, 15 rats were used. Experiment 5: 20 male Wistar rats were randomized into 2 groups and received either saline or d-fenuramine (5 mg/kg). Experiment 6: 40 male Wistar rats were randomized into 4 groups: saline + saline, saline + uoxetine (10 mg/kg), WAY 100635 (1 mg/kg) + saline, and WAY 100635 (1 mg/kg) + uoxetine (10 mg/kg). All rats received 2 injections with a 20 min interval. Measurements took place 1 h after the second injection. Experiment 7: 30 male Wistar rats received daily injections of saline or uoxetine (10 mg/kg) for 30 days. Respiration was measured on days 2, 4, 9, 16, 23, and 30, and took place 24 h after the latest injection. On day 30, the rats were decapitated so that trunk blood for analysis of uoxetine and noruoxetine in serum could be obtained. Eleven additional rats (that were not assessed with respect to respiration) were treated with uoxetine (10 mg/kg) daily and decapitated on days 2 (n 4), 8 (n 4), and 14 (n 3) for determination of uoxetine and noruoxetine levels in serum.

Results
Experiment 1: administration of uoxetine (1, 10, and 30 mg/ kg) 1 h before recording reduced respiratory rate (ANOVA: F 5.4, p 0.005) and minute ventilation (ANOVA: F 3.9, p 0.02) but not tidal volume (ANOVA: F 0.9, p 0.5). Post hoc comparisons (Fishers PLSD) revealed a signicant reduction of respiratory rate after uoxetine administration in doses of 10 and 30 mg/kg but not in the dose of 1 mg/kg, and of minute ventilation in the dose of 30 mg/kg but not in the doses of 1 or 10 mg/kg (Table 1). Ten rats were disqualied (see Materials and methods). Mean serum uoxetine and noruoxetine concentrations (nmol/L) 1 h after uoxetine administration were as follows: 1 mg/kg (n 10): uoxetine 50.1 35.7, noruoxetine 88.2 15.2; 10 mg/kg (n 9): uoxetine 1092.8 315.9, noruoxetine 1203.2 308.2; 30 mg/kg (n 7): uoxetine 3598.5 425.4, noruoxetine 1371.8 245.9. Experiment 2: administration of paroxetine (10 mg/kg) 1 h before recording signicantly reduced respiratory rate. Neither tidal volume nor minute ventilation was signicantly altered (Table 2). Seven rats were disqualied. Experiment 3: administration of 8-OH-DPAT (0.01, 0.1, and 0.15 mg/kg) 1 h before registration did not inuence respiratory rate (ANOVA: F 0.4, p 0.8), tidal volume (ANOVA: F 1.4, n 0.3) or minute ventilation (ANOVA: F 1.1, p 0.4) (Table 3). Four rats were disqualied. Experiment 4: administration of mCPP (5, 10 and 20 mg/kg) 1 h before registration reduced respiratory rate (ANOVA: F 44.6, p < 0.0001) and tidal volume (ANOVA: F 4.9, p 0.006), but not minute ventilation (ANOVA: F 0.4, p 0.7). Post hoc comparisons (Fishers PLSD)

Table 1. Respiratory response to fluoxetine Treatment Saline Fluoxetine 1 mg/kg Fluoxetine 10 mg/kg Fluoxetine 30 mg/kg n 9 6 8 7 Respiratory rate (mean SD) 121.9 9.6 114.8 1 7.0 107.0 15.41 96.7 9.32 Tidal volume (mean SD) 1.4 0.4 1.5 0.4 1.3 0.3 1.2 0.3 Minute volume (mean SD) 171.7 47.7 165.3 37.7 134.7 32.73 114.4 22.64

Fishers PLSD: 1p 0.03, 2p 0.0007, 3p 0.05, 4p 0.005 as compared with saline.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

1796
Table 2. Respiratory response to paroxetine Treatment Saline Paroxetine 10 mg/kg n 8 8 Respiratory rate (mean SD) 112.6 14.3 95.6 17.51

Journal of Psychopharmacology 24(12)

Tidal volume (mean SD) 2.1 0.4 2.4 0.6

Minute volume (mean SD) 231.0 30.6 225.0 33.0

Paired t-test: 1p 0.04 as compared with saline.

Table 3. Respiratory response to 8-OH-DPAT Treatment Saline 8-OH-DPAT 0.01 mg/kg 8-OH-DPAT 0.1 mg/kg 8-OH-DPAT 0.15 mg/kg n 8 9 9 10 Respiratory rate (mean SD) 131.5 14.7 138.1 9.7 132.3 15.1 132.4 18.7 Tidal volume (mean SD) 3.5 0.7 3.1 0.7 3.0 0.4 3.0 0.5 Minute volume (mean SD) 453.1 110.0 416.6 93.4 396.5 82.0 381.5 59.2

Table 4. Respiratory response to mCPP Treatment Saline mCPP 5 mg/kg mCPP 10 mg/kg mCPP 30 mg/kg n 13 8 8 8 Respiratory rate (mean SD) 109.2 17.2 66.2 10.51 57.4 6.91 53.8 9.91 Tidal volume (mean SD) 1.7 0.6 2.7 0.8 2.6 0.9 3.0 1.3 Minute volume (mean SD) 178.0 49.6 173.2 44.12 153.0 55.73 163.5 62.24

Fishers PLSD: 1p < 0.0001, 2p 0.02, 3p 0.02, 4p 0.001 as compared with saline.

Table 5. Respiratory response to d-fenfluramine Treatment Saline d-fenfluramine 5 mg/kg mg/kg n 9 9 Respiratory rate (mean SD) 95.3 9.5 72.7 17.11 Tidal volume (mean SD) 1.8 0.3 2.7 1.12 Minute volume (mean SD) 172.0 32.8 179.5 43.5

Unpaired t-test: 1p 0.003, 2p 0.04 as compared with saline.

Table 6. Respiratory response to WAY 100635 and fluoxetine Treatment Saline + saline Saline + fluoxetine (10 mg/kg) WAY 100635 (1 mg/kg) + saline WAY 100635 (1 mg/kg) + fluoxetine (10 mg/kg) n 10 10 10 10 Respiratory rate (mean SD) 154.2 15.3 124.4 9.61 123.3 23.31 115.6 12.42 Tidal volume (mean SD) 2.9 0.5 3.2 0.4 3.5 0.43 3.2 0.4 Minute volume (mean SD) 433.1 43.1 384.9 39.7 418.0 92.2 362.2 56.8

Fishers PLSD: 1p 0.001, 2p < 0.0001, 3p 0.01 as compared with saline.

revealed signicant eects of mCPP on respiratory rate (reduction) and tidal volume (increase) in the doses of 5, 10, and 20 mg/kg (Table 4). Ten rats were disqualied. Experiment 5: administration of d-fenuramine (5 mg/kg) decreased respiratory rate and increased tidal volume, but did not aect minute ventilation (Table 5). Two rats were disqualied. Experiment 6: administration of uoxetine (10 mg/kg), WAY 100635 (1 mg/kg), or WAY 100635 (1 mg/kg) + uoxetine (10 mg/kg) 1 h before registration inuenced respiratory

rate (ANOVA: F 11.3, p < 0.0001) and tidal volume (ANOVA: F 5.6, p 0.02), but not minute ventilation (ANOVA: F 2.7, p 0.06). Post hoc comparisons (Fishers PLSD) revealed a signicant reduction of respiratory rate after uoxetine, WAY 100635, and WAY 100635 + uoxetine, and an increase in tidal volume after WAY 100635 (p 0.01) (Table 6). No rats were disqualied. Experiment 7: rats treated for 23 or 30 days with uoxetine displayed a signicant reduction in respiratory rate and increase in tidal volume as assessed 24 h after drug

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

Annerbrink et al.
Table 7. Respiratory response to chronic administration of fluoxetine A Day Day Day Day Day Day Day B Day Day Day Day Day Day Day C Day Day Day Day Day Day Day Respiratory rate Fluoxetine (10 mg/kg) (mean SD) 103.0 18.0 112.5 27.4 115.4 23.5 119.3 20.8 135.6 13.5 122.0 16.0 Tidal volume Fluoxetine (10 mg/kg) (mean SD) 1.4 0.4 1.3 0.3 1.3 0.3 1.2 0.3 1.3 0.3 1.7 0.3 Minute volume Fluoxetine (10 mg/kg) (mean SD) 142.0 27.9 144.7 32.7 148.4 29.2 146.3 36.6 170.4 38.3 200.9 26.7

1797

of experiment 2 4 9 16 23 30

n 14 14 13 10 14 12

n 12 13 15 10 13 9

Saline (mean SD) 93.5 17.2 104.7 24.9 98.6 23.4 112.3 14.1 104.4 17.2 95.3 19.6

p-value (unpaired t-test) 0.2 0.4 0.07 0.4 < 0.0001 0.003

of experiment 2 4 9 16 23 30

n 14 14 13 10 14 12

n 12 13 15 10 13 9

Saline (mean SD) 1.8 0.7 1.3 0.3 1.4 0.3 1.4 0.5 1.6 0.5 2.3 0.8

p-value (unpaired t-test) 0.09 0.7 0.4 0.4 0.04 0.02

of experiment 2 4 9 16 23 30

n 14 14 13 10 14 12

n 12 13 15 10 13 9

Saline (mean SD) 159.1 42.4 133.7 37.1 138.6 31.0 152.8 41.6 162.5 43.7 207.0 39.0

p-value (unpaired t-test) 0.2 0.4 0.4 0.7 0.6 0.7

Table 8. Serum concentrations of fluoxetine and norfluoxetine after chronic fluoxetine administration Serum concentrations Group Day Day Day Day 2 8 14 30 n 4 4 3 10 Fluoxetine (nmol/L, mean SD) <20 <20 <20 52.7 35.3 Norfluoxetine (nmol/L, mean SD) 234.7 98.8 871.1 278.5 1597.4 411.2 1986.5 453.4

administration; in contrast, no signicant eects of the treatment was observed after 2, 4, 9, or 16 days (Table 7). Minute ventilation was never inuenced by subchronic administration of uoxetine. Mean serum uoxetine and noruoxetine concentrations in rats administered uoxetine for dierent periods are shown in Table 8.

Discussion
In line with our hypothesis, uoxetine and paroxetine exerted an eect on respiration the opposite of that observed after chronic treatment, that is a reduction in respiratory rate. Rats receiving the high doses of uoxetine also displayed a reduction in minute ventilation, but otherwise no such eect was observed, since the reduction in respiratory rate was usually accompanied by an increase in tidal volume. Our previous observation that subchronic administration of an SRI inuences respiratory rate in the opposite direction, that is enhances it, was conrmed; a signicant increase in

respiratory rate was thus observed after 23 days of administration of uoxetine and onwards. Acute administration of SRIs increases synaptic levels of serotonin by blocking the reuptake inhibitor (Stahl, 1998). At the same time, however, these compounds induce an eective silencing of the serotonergic nerve cell activity, elicited by enhanced activation of autoreceptors of the 5HT1A subtype (Aghajanian et al., 1970; Blier and de Montigny, 1985). If serotonergic neurons serve as chemoreceptors, exerting a CO2-driven stimulation of respiration, such an inhibition of serotonergic cell ring may, tentatively, lead to a reduction in respiratory rate. The observed eect of acute administration of SRIs on respiratory rate could hence be due either to enhanced extracellular concentrations of serotonin, which would support the theory that serotonin mainly inhibits respiration, or to a feed-back inhibition of these neurons, impairing their capability to respond to CO2 uctuations in a dynamic manner, which would support the opposite view, that is that serotonin is a respiratory stimulant.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

1798
To address the latter of these alternative possibilities, we administered the 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT, which eectively reduces the ring rate in serotonergic neurons (Blier and de Montigny, 1990). No support was, however, obtained for the theory that the eect of SRIs is secondary to 5-HT1Amediated inhibition of serotonergic neurons, since there was no eect of 8-OH-DPAT, administered at doses activating 5-HT1A autoreceptors without inuencing postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (Forster et al., 1995; Sharp et al., 1989). Notably, previously a dose-dependent increase in respiratory rate has been observed after administration of 8-OH-DPAT to awake, freely moving guinea pigs (Stone et al., 1993). Although partly dierent from our observation, probably due to the use of higher doses of the drug, this nding also argues against the notion that inhibition of serotonergic cell ring reduces respiratory rate. Also in rat, a stimulatory inuence of 5-HT1A agonists on respiration has been reported, but these studies have been undertaken using animals that have been asleep or the subject of various pretreatments (Choi et al., 2005; Edwards et al., 1990; Mendelson et al., 1990; Valic et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2007). To assess the alternative explanation of the eect of acute administration of SRIs on respiratory rate, that is that it is secondary to enhanced synaptic output, two compounds known to eectively increase extracellular serotonin concentrations by reversing the direction of the serotonin transporters, that is mCPP and d-fenuramine (Eriksson et al., 1999; Laferrere and Wurtman, 1989), were administered. The observation that both these compounds caused a robust decrease in respiratory rate, that is the same eect as that observed after uoxetine and paroxetine, supports the notion that the eects of acute SRI administration on respiratory rate are indeed secondary to an increase in extracellular concentrations of serotonin. WAY-100635 is a 5HT1A antagonist that enhances the ring of some but not all serotonergic neurons in awake animals (Fornal et al., 1996; Kasamo et al., 2001; Mlinar et al., 2005). If it is presumed that respiration-regulated serotonergic neurons are among those that are under tonic inhibitory inuence of serotonin, and hence the subject of enhanced ring when exposed to WAY-100635, the observation that WAY100635 elicited the same eect as uoxetine, paroxetine, mCPP, and d-fenuramine, that is a reduction in respiratory rate, lends further support for the notion that serotonin exerts an inhibitory inuence on this parameter. The outcome of the WAY-100635 experiment may, however, seem contradictory to the lack of eect of the 5HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT on this parameter, the simplest explanation being that respiratory rate-modulating serotonergic neurons are equipped with inhibitory 5HT1A receptors, but that these are saturated by the endogenous transmitter in awake rats, and hence not further activated by the administration of an agonist. When WAY-100635 is co-administered with an SRI, the former drug may counteract the inhibitory inuence of the latter on serotonergic cell ring, and hence potentiate the stimulatory inuence on extracellular levels of serotonin (Arborelius et al., 1996; Hjorth, 1993; Invernizzi et al., 1996). The nding that WAY-100635 plus uoxetine did not reduce respiratory rate signicantly more than did either drug alone (although the eect of the combination was numerically

Journal of Psychopharmacology 24(12)


larger) suggests that respiratory rate-modulating serotonergic neurons are not inhibited by SRIs and hence is in line with the nding with 8-OH-DPAT. Notably, there was, however, a non-signicant tendency for a reduction in minute ventilation in the group given both compounds (ANOVA: F 2.7, p 0.06; Fisher s PLSD: p 0.07), but not in the groups receiving only one of the two drugs. The fact that WAY100635 did not counteract the eect of uoxetine on respiratory rate suggests that SRI-induced reduction in respiratory rate is not mediated by postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. Acute administration of various compounds having in common that they enhance extracellular serotonin concentrations thus reduces respiratory rate in freely moving, unrestrained, quiet but awake male rats. Since this eect is usually accompanied by an increase in tidal volume, it seldom leads to a signicant reduction in minute ventilation; the results hence suggest that serotonin modulates the respiratory pattern rather than inhibits breathing. The eects on respiratory rate were, however, more clear-cut than those on tidal volume, and did sometimes lead to a reduction in minute ventilation, hence justifying the assumption that the treatment primarily inuences respiratory rate, and that the eects on tidal volume are secondary to this inuence, rather than vice versa. The present results thus challenge the notion that the inuence of serotonin on respiration is predominantly stimulatory (see Introduction), but rather suggest that serotonin, in the awake, non-stressed rat, reduces respiratory rate but increases tidal volume, the net eect on minute ventilation being none or inhibitory. This notion also gains support from the previous observation that the serotonin synthesis inhibitor PCPA causes an increase rather than a decrease in minute ventilation (Annerbrink et al., 2003). Notably, this eect was, however, due to an increase in tidal volume rather than respiratory rate. One possible explanation would be that serotonin at normal synaptic concentrations mainly serves to keep tidal volume low, but at higher levels acts as an inhibitor of respiratory rate. We have previously reported that administration of paroxetine for ve weeks or more enhances respiratory rate in freely moving awake animals, that is that the drug given subchronically exerts an eect opposite to that observed when it was given acutely in the present study (Olsson et al., 2004). This observation to some extent contrasts with a report by Henderson and co-workers suggesting that repeated administration of a low dose of uoxetine do not inuence baseline respiratory rate in goats (Henderson et al., 1999). Our present observation that also long-term administration of uoxetine enhances respiratory rate in rat, however, conrms our previous nding with paroxetine. The eect of uoxetine was signicant when treatment had been ongoing for 23 days, but not seen after 16 days, and is hence consistent with a previous study failing to observe an eect on respiration in rats of 15 days of systemic uoxetine administration (Taylor et al., 2004). As shown in this study, subchronic administration of uoxetine led to a gradual and marked increase in serum levels of both the substance itself and its active metabolite noruoxetine as measured 24 h after the latest injection (that is at the time of respiratory recording). The high concentrations obtained after subchronic treatment could not,

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

Annerbrink et al.
however, solely explain the increase in respiratory rate, since the uoxetine and noruoxetine levels observed 60 min after acute administration (1 h) were even higher, and did not result in enhanced respiratory rate, but had the opposite eect. An issue that has been debated for a long time is why the symptom-reducing eect of SRIs in depression and in a number of anxiety disorders requires weeks of treatment to be observed. Some have argued that SRIs exert only weak eects on extra-cellular serotonin levels at acute administration due to the activation of an inhibitory feed-back inuence on nerve cell activity and transmitter release, and that this feed-back is abated during long-term administration, leading to a gradual augmentation of the inuence of the drug on serotonergic output (Stahl, 1998). An alternative theory, however, suggests that the stimulatory eect of acute administration of an SRI on serotonergic output after a few weeks of treatment is reversed to its opposite in synapses regulating, for example, anxiety, as the result of various adaptive mechanisms (such as down-regulation of postsynaptic receptors) (Stahl, 1998). The observation that SRIs in patients with panic disorder are not only devoid of anti-panic eects during the rst days of treatment, but initially often aggravate anxiety (Nutt and Glue, 1989; Pohl et al., 1988), may be taken as support for the latter view, as may the observation that drugs causing a marked and immediate increase in synaptic serotonin concentrations, such as fenuramine and mCPP, do not exert acute anti-anxiety eects, but, on the contrary, may elicit anxiety in patients with panic disorder (Charney et al., 1987; Mortimore and Anderson, 2000). The eect of subchronic administration of an SRI in anxiety-modulating synapses being opposite in direction to that of acute administration is also strengthened by the recent ndings that acute SRI administration to man enhances amygdala reactivity (Bigos et al., 2008) whilst subchronic treatment reduces it (Harmer et al., 2006). The present observation, that the eect of acute administration of an SRI on respiratory rate in rat is opposite to that of subchronic administration, thus adds to an already existing body of data suggesting that the eect of subchronic treatment with SRIs on serotonergic output, at least in some neuronal circuits, is opposite in direction to that observed after acute administration. Of interest in this context is a recent nding suggesting that tryptophan depletion does not increase proneness to panic in SRI-treated panic disorder patients (Toru et al., 2006); however, the literature on this issue is not unanimous (Bell et al., 2002). As discussed above, numerous observations support the notion that panic attacks may be caused by dysregulated respiration (see Introduction). To what extent the eects of serotonin-modulating drugs on respiration revealed by our study are of importance for the inuence of these drugs on anxiety in panic disorder patients remains an open question, but it is notable that the acute eect of SRIs on respiratory rate in rat, just as in the eect on anxiety in panic disorder patients, was found to be opposite to that of subchronic treatment, and that panic-provoking serotonin-releasing agents exerted an eect similar to that of acute SRI exposure. In this context, the lack of eect of low doses of 8-OH-DPAT on respiratory rate is well in line with the observation that 5HT1A agonists neither reduce panic attacks nor elicit them (Sheehan et al., 1993; Van Vliet et al., 1996). Clarifying the

1799
possible role of respiration for the anxiogenic and anti-panic eects of serotonin modulators will, however, probably require further insight into the possible role of respiratory control for the generation of panic attacks. Notably, not only serotonin, but also endogenous opioids (Preter and Klein, 2008) and acetylcholine (Battaglia, 2002) have recently been attributed importance in this context. In conclusion, acute administration of the SRIs uoxetine and paroxetine, the 5HT1A-antagonist WAY-100635, and the serotonin releasing agents d-fenuramine and mCPP all caused a signicant reduction in respiratory rate when given to freely moving rats. Our observation that subchronic administration of an SRI, on the other hand, enhanced respiratory rate, lends further support to the notion that the eect of long-term administration of SRIs on the serotonergic output in certain brain regions is opposite to that observed after acute administration.

Acknowledgements
This study was sponsored by the Swedish Research Council (grant no 8668), Torsten and Ragnar Soderbergs Foundation, the Swedish Brain Power Initiative, and the Swedish Lundbeck Foundation. Ms Inger Oscarsson and Ms Gunilla Bourghardt are gratefully acknowledged for excellent technical assistance.

Statement of interest
None.

References
Abelson JL, Nesse RM, Weg JG, Curtis GC (1996) Respiratory psychophysiology and anxiety: cognitive intervention in the doxapram model of panic. Psychosom Med 58: 302313. Abelson JL, Weg JG, Nesse RM, Curtis GC (2001) Persistent respiratory irregularity in patients with panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry 49: 588595. Aghajanian GK, Graham AW, Sheard MH (1970) Serotonincontaining neurons in brain: depression of firing by monoamine oxidase inhibitors. Science 169: 11001102. Annerbrink K, Olsson M, Melchior LK, Hedner J, Eriksson E (2003) Serotonin depletion increases respiratory variability in freely moving rats: implications for panic disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 6: 5156. Arborelius L, Nomikos GG, Hertel P, et al. (1996) The 5-HT1A receptor antagonist (S)-UH-301 augments the increase in extracellular concentrations of 5-HT in the frontal cortex produced by both acute and chronic treatment with citalopram. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 353: 630640. Bach KB, Lutcavage ME, Mitchell GS (1993) Serotonin is necessary for short-term modulation of the exercise ventilatory response. Respir Physiol 91: 5770. Battaglia M (2002) Beyond the usual suspects: a cholinergic route for panic attacks. Mol Psychiatry 7: 239246. Battaglia M, Pesenti-Gritti P, Medland SE, Ogliari A, Tambs K, Spatola CA (2009) A genetically informed study of the association between childhood separation anxiety, sensitivity to CO(2), panic disorder, and the effect of childhood parental loss. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 6471. Battaglia M, Pesenti-Gritti P, Spatola CA, Ogliari A, Tambs K (2008) A twin study of the common vulnerability between heightened sensitivity to hypercapnia and panic disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B: 586593.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

1800
Bell C, Forshall S, Adrover M, et al. (2002) Does 5-HT restrain panic? A tryptophan depletion study in panic disorder patients recovered on paroxetine. J Psychopharmacol 16: 514. Bigos KL, Pollock BG, Aizenstein HJ, Fisher PM, Bies RR, Hariri AR (2008) Acute 5-HT reuptake blockade potentiates human amygdala reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 32213225. Blier P, De Montigny C (1985) Serotoninergic but not noradrenergic neurons in rat central nervous system adapt to long-term treatment with monoamine oxidase inhibitors. Neuroscience 16: 949955. Blier P, De Montigny C (1990) Electrophysiological investigation of the adaptive response of the 5-HT system to the administration of 5-HT1A receptor agonists. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 15(Suppl 7): S4248. Charney DS, Woods SW, Goodman WK, Heninger GR (1987) Serotonin function in anxiety. II. Effects of the serotonin agonist MCPP in panic disorder patients and healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 92: 1424. Choi H, Liao WL, Newton KM, et al. (2005) Respiratory abnormalities resulting from midcervical spinal cord injury and their reversal by serotonin 1A agonists in conscious rats. J Neurosci 25: 45504559. Den Boer JA, Westenberg HG (1988) Effect of a serotonin and noradrenaline uptake inhibitor in panic disorder; a double-blind comparative study with fluvoxamine and maprotiline. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 3: 5974. Edwards E, Whitaker-Azmitia PM, Harkins K (1990) 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B agonists play a differential role on the respiratory frequency in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 3: 129136. Eriksson E, Engberg G, Bing O, Nissbrandt H (1999) Effects of mCPP on the extracellular concentrations of serotonin and dopamine in rat brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 20: 287296. Feldman JL, Mitchell GS, Nattie EE (2003) Breathing: rhythmicity, plasticity, chemosensitivity. Annu Rev Neurosci 26: 239266. Fornal CA, Metzler CW, Gallegos RA, Veasey SC, Mccreary AC, Jacobs BL (1996) WAY-100635, a potent and selective 5-hydroxytryptamine1A antagonist, increases serotonergic neuronal activity in behaving cats: comparison with (S)-WAY-100135. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 278: 752762. Forster EA, Cliffe IA, Bill DJ, et al. (1995) A pharmacological profile of the selective silent 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, WAY-100635. Eur J Pharmacol 281: 8188. Garattini S, Mennini T, Samanin R (1987) From fenfluramine racemate to d-fenfluramine. Specificity and potency of the effects on the serotoninergic system and food intake. Ann N Y Acad Sci 499: 156166. Gorman JM, Askanazi J, Liebowitz MR, et al. (1984) Response to hyperventilation in a group of patients with panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 141: 857861. Gorman JM, Fyer MR, Goetz R, et al. (1988) Ventilatory physiology of patients with panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45: 319. Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM (2006) Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biol Psychiatry 59: 816820. Henderson DR, Konkle DM, Mitchell GS (1999) Effects of serotonin re-uptake inhibition on ventilatory control in goats. Respir Physiol 115: 110. Hjorth S (1993) Serotonin 5-HT1A autoreceptor blockade potentiates the ability of the 5-HT reuptake inhibitor citalopram to increase nerve terminal output of 5-HT in vivo: a microdialysis study. J Neurochem 60: 776779. Invernizzi R, Bramante M, Samanin R (1996) Role of 5-HT1A receptors in the effects of acute chronic fluoxetine on extracellular serotonin in the frontal cortex. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 54: 143147.

Journal of Psychopharmacology 24(12)


Johnson PL, Hollis JH, Moratalla R, Lightman SL, Lowry CA (2005) Acute hypercarbic gas exposure reveals functionally distinct subpopulations of serotonergic neurons in rats. J Psychopharmacol 19: 327341. Kasamo K, Suzuki T, Tada K, et al. (2001) Endogenous 5-HT tonically inhibits spontaneous firing activity of dorsal hippocampus CA1 pyramidal neurons through stimulation of 5-HT(1A) receptors in quiet awake rats: in vivo electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychopharmacology 24: 141151. Kent JM, Papp LA, Martinez JM, et al. (2001) Specificity of panic response to CO(2) inhalation in panic disorder: a comparison with major depression and premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Am J Psychiatry 158: 5867. Klein DF (1993) False suffocation alarms, spontaneous panics, and related conditions. An integrative hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50: 306317. Laferrere B, Wurtman RJ (1989) Effect of D-fenfluramine on serotonin release in brains of anaesthetized rats. Brain Res 504: 258263. Lee YJ, Curtis GC, Weg JG, Abelson JL, Modell JG, Campbell KM (1993) Panic attacks induced by doxapram. Biol Psychiatry 33: 295297. Liebowitz MR, Fyer AJ, Gorman JM, et al. (1984) Lactate provocation of panic attacks. I. Clinical and behavioral findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41: 764770. Manzke T, Guenther U, Ponimaskin EG, et al. (2003) 5-HT4(a) receptors avert opioid-induced breathing depression without loss of analgesia. Science 301: 226229. Martin-Body RL, Grundy HR (1985) Effects of neurotoxin-induced brainstem lesions on the respiratory responses of conscious rats. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 12: 427437. Martinez JM, Kent JM, Coplan JD, et al. (2001) Respiratory variability in panic disorder. Depress Anxiety 14: 232237. Mendelson WB, Martin JV, Rapoport DM (1990) Effects of buspirone on sleep and respiration. Am Rev Respir Dis 141: 15271530. Mitchell GS, Smith CA, Vidruk H, Jameson LC, Dempsey JA (1983) Effects of p-chlorophenylalanine on ventilatory control in goats. J Appl Physiol 54: 277283. Mlinar B, Tatini F, Ballini C, Nencioni S, Della Corte L, Corradetti R (2005) Differential autoinhibition of 5-hydroxytryptamine neurons by 5-hydroxytryptamine in the dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroreport 16: 13511355. Modigh K, Westber P, Eriksson E (1992) Superiority of clomipramine over imipramine in the treatment of panic disorder: a placebo-controlled trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol 12: 251261. Mortimore C, Anderson IM (2000) d-Fenfluramine in panic disorder: a dual role for 5-hydroxytryptamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 149: 251258. Mulkey DK, Stornetta RL, Weston MC, et al. (2004) Respiratory control by ventral surface chemoreceptor neurons in rats. Nat Neurosci 7: 13601369. Nardi AE, Freire RC, Zin WA (2008a) Panic disorder and control of breathing. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. Nardi AE, Freire RC, Zin WA (2009) Panic disorder and control of breathing. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 167(1): 133143. Nardi AE, Valenca AM, Nascimento I, et al. (2008b) A caffeine challenge test in panic disorder patients, their healthy firstdegree relatives, and healthy controls. Depress Anxiety 25: 847853. Nutt DJ, Glue P (1989) Clinical pharmacology of anxiolytics and antidepressants: a psychopharmacological perspective. Pharmacol Ther 44: 309334. Olson EB, Jr (1987) Ventilatory adaptation to hypoxia occurs in serotonin-depleted rats. Respir Physiol 69: 227235. Olsson M, Annerbrink K, Bengtsson F, Hedner J, Eriksson E (2004) Paroxetine influences respiration in rats: implications for the

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

Annerbrink et al.
treatment of panic disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 14: 2937. Papp LA, Klein DF, Gorman JM (1993) Carbon dioxide hypersensitivity, hyperventilation, and panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 150: 11491157. Papp LA, Martinez JM, Klein DF, et al. (1997) Respiratory psychophysiology of panic disorder: three respiratory challenges in 98 subjects. Am J Psychiatry 154: 15571565. Perna G, Battaglia M, Garberi A, Arancio C, Bertani A, Bellodi L (1994) Carbon dioxide/oxygen challenge test in panic disorder. Psychiatry Res 52: 159171. Pitts FN, Jr, McClure JN, Jr (1967) Lactate metabolism in anxiety neurosis. N Engl J Med 277: 13291336. Pohl R, Yeragani VK, Balon R, Lycaki H (1988) The jitteriness syndrome in panic disorder patients treated with antidepressants. J Clin Psychiatry 49: 100104. Preter M, Klein DF (2008) Panic, suffocation false alarms, separation anxiety and endogenous opioids. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 603612. Richerson GB (2004) Serotonergic neurons as carbon dioxide sensors that maintain pH homeostasis. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 449461. Richerson GB, Wang W, Hodges MR, Dohle CI, Diez-Sampedro A (2005) Homing in on the specific phenotype(s) of central respiratory chemoreceptors. Exp Physiol 90: 259266; discussion 266269. Richter DW, Manzke T, Wilken B, Ponimaskin E (2003) Serotonin receptors: guardians of stable breathing. Trends Mol Med 9: 542548. Severson CA, Wang W, Pieribone VA, Dohle CI, Richerson GB (2003) Midbrain serotonergic neurons are central pH chemoreceptors. Nat Neurosci 6: 11391140. Sharp T, Bramwell SR, Grahame-Smith DG (1989) 5-HT1 agonists reduce 5-hydroxytryptamine release in rat hippocampus in vivo as determined by brain microdialysis. Br J Pharmacol 96: 283290. Sheehan DV, Raj AB, Harnett-Sheehan K, Soto S, Knapp E (1993) The relative efficacy of high-dose buspirone and alprazolam in the treatment of panic disorder: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 88: 111.

1801
Sinha S, Papp LA, Gorman JM (2000) How study of respiratory physiology aided our understanding of abnormal brain function in panic disorder. J Affect Disord 61: 191200. Stahl SM (1998) Mechanism of action of serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors. Serotonin receptors and pathways mediate therapeutic effects and side effects. J Affect Disord 51: 215235. Stein MB, Millar TW, Larsen DK, Kryger H (1995) Irregular breathing during sleep in patients with panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 152: 11681173. Stone RA, Worsdell YM, Fuller RW, Barnes PJ (1993) Effects of 5-hydroxytryptamine and 5-hydroxytryptophan infusion on the human cough reflex. J Appl Physiol 74: 396401. Struzik L, Duffin J, Vermani M, Hegadoren K, Katzman MA (2002) Effects of tryptophan depletion on central and peripheral chemoreflexes in man. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 133: 183195. Taylor NC, Li A, Green A, Kinney HC, Nattie EE (2004) Chronic fluoxetine microdialysis into the medullary raphe nuclei of the rat, but not systemic administration, increases the ventilatory response to CO2. J Appl Physiol 97: 17631773. Toru I, Shlik J, Maron E, Vasar V, Nutt DJ (2006) Tryptophan depletion does not modify response to CCK-4 challenge in patients with panic disorder after treatment with citalopram. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 186: 107112. Valic M, Pecotic R, Dogas Z (2008) Phrenic nerve activity is enhanced by 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT in spontaneously breathing anesthetized rats. J Physiol Pharmacol 59: 1725. Van Vliet IM, Westenberg HG, Den Boer JA (1996) Effects of the 5-HT1A receptor agonist flesinoxan in panic disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 127: 174180. Wang X, Dergacheva O, Kamendi H, Gorini C, Mendelowitz D (2007) 5-Hydroxytryptamine 1A/7 and 4alpha receptors differentially prevent opioid-induced inhibition of brain stem cardiorespiratory function. Hypertension 50: 368376. Wilhelm FH, Trabert W, Roth WT (2001) Physiologic instability in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Biol Psychiatry 49: 596605.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com by guest on November 24, 2012

You might also like