Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Trustworthiness in B2C E-Commerce: An Examination of Alternative Models1

Mark A. Serva The University of Delaware John Skip Benamati Miami University Mark A. Fuller Washington State University

Abstract
Advancing research on trust requires clarifying the different conceptualizations of trust and trust-related constructs. The purpose of this study is to advance the theoretical conceptualization of trustworthiness by synthesizing previous research and testing three alternative conceptualizations within the ecommerce context. Data collected from multiple studies involving over 700 participants were used to examine the relative merits of trustworthiness as a one-dimensional construct, a grouping of three firstorder constructs, and a second-order construct. Our results indicate that a one-dimensional view may be too simplistic, given the variety of factors that online consumers must weigh. Instead, the study suggests that trustworthiness is multidimensional and that both first- and second-order conceptualizations have a place in e-commerce trust research. Trust researchers should be guided by the research question, hypotheses, and research design in deciding which conceptualization to use. ACM Categories: H.1.2, K.4.4 Keywords: Trustworthiness, Trust, World Wide Web, Electronic Commerce, Second Order Factors, Ability, Integrity, Benevolence

Introduction
Research into trust has increased dramatically in the last decade, and numerous studies have investigated differing models of trust (Lander et al., 2004; Lewicki & Bunker, 1995; Lewis & Weigert, 1985; Tyler & Degoey, 1996). Little research, however, has explored the specific nature of trustworthiness, often modeled as a precursor to trust (Mayer et al., 1995; McKnight et al., 2002a). The distinction between trust and trustworthiness is an important one. Trust is often described as a general willingness to depend on another in situations of risk (Mayer et al., 1995; McKnight et al., 2002a). Trustworthinessreferred to as trusting beliefs within the information systems disciplineis the set of beliefs about the other party that precedes that willingness (Mayer et al., 1995). Research that seeks to understand the factors that influence the formation of trust must be clear in how it conceptualizes and analyzes the antecedents of trust. In information systems research, prior trust studies have proven useful in understanding user interactions in the context of e-commerce (Gefen, 2003b; Pavlou & Gefen, 2004). Consumers trust i.e., their willingness to transact with online

Acknowledgement
The authors appreciate and gratefully acknowledge the cooperation of David Gefen of Drexel University, who provided data that increased the quality of our study.

David Gefen served as the Senior Editor for this paper.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

89

businessesis believed to be driven by their assessment of the business trustworthiness (McKnight et al., 2002b; Reichheld & Schefter, 2000). Unfortunately, information systems has not consistently conceptualized trust and its antecedents and consequences (Gefen, 2002b; McKnight et al., 2002a). As an example, some recent business-toconsumer (B2C) e-commerce research has blended the conceptualizations of trustworthiness and trust, rather than defining one as a precursor to the other (Bhattacherjee, 2002; Gefen et al., 2003; Pavlou, 2003; Suh & Han, 2002). This inconsistency raises questions about the proper conceptualization of these two important constructs, and also hinders theoretical advances on trust and related constructs. The purpose of this study is to clarify and advance the theoretical conceptualization of trustworthiness within a B2C context by synthesizing previous research and testing alternative models. We first test a more parsimonious model of trustworthiness to determine if trust theory can be simplified by modeling trustworthiness as a one-dimensional factor. We also extend McKnight et al. (2002a) by providing a theoretical rationale and the theoretical implications for a second-order model of trustworthiness. Finally, we extend both McKnight et al. (2002a) and Gefen (2002b) by testing their respective multidimensional conceptualizations against alternative conceptualizations.

In contrast, trust is defined as a willingness to depend on the trustee (McKnight et al., 2002a). This definition of trust distinguishes it from the beliefs that precede ite.g., beliefs about a trustees ability, benevolence, and integrity. This study examines the distinction between trustworthiness and trust by examining a context consistent with past IS researchi.e. that of trustworthiness and initial trust within a B2C context (McKnight et al., 2002a). Initial trust describes trust in the context of an unfamiliar trustee, in our case consumer trust with unfamiliar online retailers (McKnight et al., 2002a). The distinction between trust and trustworthiness is supported by the connection between trust research with the theory of reasoned action (TRA) (Fishbein & Azjen, 1975; McKnight et al., 2002a). TRA posits that an individuals beliefs affect ones attitudes, which affect ones intention to perform the behavior. Applying TRA to models of trust, trusting beliefs (trustworthiness) would precede and affect trusting attitudes (trust). TRA therefore differentiates the individuals beliefs about the trustee (trustworthiness) from the individuals resulting willingness to take an action (trust). Trustworthiness is therefore not the same as trust, but rather it forms the basis for trust and downstream trust-related actions. While some studies involving trust and trustworthiness clearly differentiate these two concepts (Mayer et al., 1995; McKnight et al., 2002a), other IS studies have not (Bhattacherjee, 2002; Pavlou, 2003; Suh & Han, 2002). As an example, recent IS studies utilize trustworthiness indicators (i.e., ability, benevolence, and integrity), but refer to the construct as trust. Bhattacherjee defines trust as a willingness to be vulnerable, but includes indicators that measure Amazon.coms ability, benevolence, and integrity as part of trust, rather than trustworthiness (Bhattacherjee, 2002). In addition, this same research uses the trust indicator Overall, Amazon.com is trustworthy [emphasis added], which has a clear overlap with the conceptually distinct construct of trustworthiness. These survey items, therefore, assess the trustors perception that Amazon.com has beneficial characteristics (i.e., is trustworthy), not whether consumer is willing to depend on Amazon.com (i.e., trusts Amazon.com). This approach also diverges from TRA by equating beliefs with attitudes.

Literature Review
Trustworthiness versus Trust While past research has posited that a variety of beliefs make up trustworthiness (see McKnight et al., 2002a for a detailed review), recent research has primarily focused on three specific factors which may parsimoniously capture the concept of trustworthinessi.e., ability, benevolence, and integrity (Mayer et al., 1995). We refer to these constructs throughout the paper as the dimensions of trustworthiness. Ability is the perceived skills, competencies, and characteristics that enable a party to have influence within a specific domain (Mayer et al., 1995). Benevolence is the trustors belief that the trustee wants to do good toward the trustor. Integrity is the belief that the trustee adheres to a set of principles that the trustor finds acceptable (Mayer et al., 1995). We define trustworthiness as a confident trustor perception that the trusteehas attributes that are beneficial to 2 the trustor (McKnight et al., 2002a, p.337).
2

We use the term beneficial in a broad sense, and the term benevolence to denote a specific trustworthiness dimension.

The term beneficial in our trustworthiness definition applies across the three dimensions and reflects the common belief that a trustee will produce a result desirable to the trustor. The benevolence dimension reflects the trustors beliefs that the trustee has good intentions toward the trustor.

90

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

In a B2C context, blending trust with trustworthiness may overestimate formed trust, since the practice assumes that if a party (e.g., Amazon.com) is perceived to have beneficial characteristics, it will be trusted. This assumption effectively omits the role of trust within the nomological network, since it is possible that a consumer may perceive that Amazon.com has beneficial characteristics (is trustworthy), but still may not be willing to depend on 3 it for books. Trustworthiness may instill trust, which indicates that trustworthiness and trust are distinct but related constructs. Hence, trusts role within the nomological network within these studies may be misrepresented, since it may actually be trustworthiness that was studied. This misrepresentation requires clarification, since perceptions that a party is trustworthy is not the same as the trust that results from those perceptions. Although prior studies have helped shed some light on the effects of trust (Gefen, 2002b; Kim & Prabhakar, 2004; Suh & Han, 2002; Van Slyke et al., 2004), the different conceptualizations of trust and trustworthiness across studies may also be limiting the IS discipline's ability to advance knowledge in this domain. This study seeks to clarify the theoretical nature of trustworthiness, and as a result more clearly differentiate it from trust. Toward this end, this study explores and compares trustworthiness as a one-dimensional construct, a collection of first-order constructs, and as a secondorder construct. Information systems and other disciplines have relied on each of these, but the implications of each of the conceptualizations in a B2C context have not been previously explored. In the next sections, therefore, we discuss each of these views of trustworthiness, along with the implications of each. Trustworthiness as a One-Dimensional Construct Model 1A in Figure 1 assumes that trustworthiness is a one-dimensional construct that has ability, benevolence, and integrity indicators that all covary. Trustworthiness is viewed as a composite belief that results in a party being more or less trusted (Mayer et al., 1995). Researchers have viewed such onedimensional models of trustworthiness differently,
3

including belief in a persons composite competence and integrity (Lieberman, 1981), in a person's predictability (Good, 1988), or as the extent that someone is motivated (or not motivated) to lie (Hovland et al., 1953).

Figure 1. Hypothesized Models of Trustworthiness A one-dimensional model of trustworthiness favors parsimony over completeness. Existing theory could be simplified, however, if trustworthiness is a simple trait that is or is not possessed. This is similar to the calculative model of trust that posits a trustor weighs the pros and cons of trusting another in a business relationship (Doney & Cannon, 1997). This view of trustworthiness may be sufficient, therefore, in contexts where the trustor can focus on one aspect of the trustee (Gefen, 2002a) in deciding whether or not to take a risk. Within an online shopping context, however, a onedimensional view may be too simplistic. Consumers may expect that a vendor will be successful across a number of dimensions before they are willing to make a purchase. An online retailer may be reliable

Possible reasons include distrust of Internet-based transactions, fear of releasing personal information (including credit card information), and low disposition to trust. While these have been commonly included as additional determinants of trust, these constructs also reinforce that the consumer must perceive that different factors (including trustworthiness), are present before trust can occur.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

91

in shipping its products, but fails to keep personal information private. The decision to buy a product from the company, therefore, becomes complex. Is the online retailer trustworthy within this context? Measuring a trustors assessments of trustees trustworthiness may be problematic under a onedimensional view of trustworthiness, since it focuses on a limited view of the concept (e.g., the companys shipping efficiency) that may ignore other important aspects of the trustee (e.g., protecting customer privacy). Moreover, a one-dimensional view assumes that the trustor can make aggregate assessments of the trustees tendency to act in a beneficial manner across differentand possibly conflictingdimensions. Trustworthiness as a Multidimensional Construct Recent research has found that multidimensional constructs are helpful in explaining higher-order concepts that span their component dimensions. Examples have included concern for information privacy (Stewart & Segars, 2002), general mental aptitude (Law & Wong, 1999), and employee job satisfaction (Netemeyer et al., 1990). To explore the possibility that trustworthiness in an online context is multidimensional, we examine two alternative multidimensional models of trustworthiness. The first models trustworthiness as a grouping of firstorder constructs. The second models trustworthiness as a second-order construct. Trustworthiness As a Grouping of First-Order Constructs A three-dimensional, first-order model of trustworthiness has arguably been the predominant conceptualization of trustworthiness (Model 1B in Figure 1) (Mayer et al., 1995). Trustworthiness is proposed to exist as a logical grouping of ability, benevolence, and integrity (Mayer et al., 1995), with each factor independently and distinctly affecting trust. This model may provide a more complete conceptualization than the one-dimensional model of trustworthiness. Viewing trustworthiness as a first order model allows the three latent variables to capture the variance that is specific to ability, benevolence, and integrity (Law & Wong, 1999). A first-order model, therefore, allows researchers to draw conclusions about the three constructs relative effects on trust across different contexts (Gefen, 2002b; Lee & Turban, 2001). In contrast, the previously discussed one-dimensional model captures the variance common only to all measurement indicators, but treats the variance specific to each dimension as error (Law & Wong, 1999).

Modeling trustworthiness as three first-order constructs suggests a formative view of trustworthiness. A formative model assumes that the three indicators cause an additive trustworthiness perception that then influences the 4 formation of trust. A change in any of the first-order dimensions is assumed to result in a consistent change in trust. The magnitude of the change is represented by the weight of the regression path between the dimension and trust. Within a formative conceptualization, however, trustworthiness' indicators do not necessarily covary (Chin, 1998). In fact, formative dimensions are often designed to be somewhat mutually exclusive so that they capture different aspects of the latent variable. As an example, Chin (1998) presents the amount of beer, wine, and hard liquor consumed as formative indicators of mental inebriation. All feed mental inebriation, and need not be correlated. In another example, changes in income and education are presented as formative indicators of socio-economic status (SES) (Chin, 1998). Because a change in income does not necessarily reflect a change in education (two SES indicators), each is assumed to contribute independently to SES. If a researcher chooses to adopt a formative model of trustworthiness, then the research question, hypotheses, and design must be consistent with an additive or cumulative process of trustworthiness and trust formation. The researcher using a firstorder conceptualization assumes that ability, benevolence, and integrity combine in an independent mannerthat is, that a change in one 5 dimension does not necessarily affect the other two. This conceptualization assumes that when faced with a change in one aspect of trustworthiness, the trustor is a dispassionate thinker who can compartmentalize perceptions of the trustee. Consistent with this view, one of the advantages of a first-order model is that it allows researchers to draw conclusions about each of the first-order constructs relative effects on trust, possibly across different contextse.g., when levels of risk are different.

Within a first-order conceptualization, trustworthiness is typically not included as a latent variable. Instead, ability, benevolence, and integrity are modeled as directly causing changes in trust perceptions. 5 Most first-order conceptualizations, including the one presented in this paper, include interdependencies between ability, benevolence, and integrity in the form of covariance arrows. These covariances are not meant to model causality or directionality, nor do they attempt to give meaning to these interdependencies. Instead, they control for the significant correlation that is usually present among the factors.

92

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

This model also has limitations, however. When ability, benevolence, and integrity data are collected, significant intercorrelations between the constructs are typically found (Bhattacherjee, 2002; McKnight et al., 2002a; Schoorman et al., 1996). These intercorrelations may result in model multicollinearity, which can mask the true effects of the three constructs on trust (Bollen, 1989). In addition, presenting trustworthiness as three distinct beliefs implicitly posits that trustworthiness exists not as a theoretical construct, but as a semantic convenience. Research questions and hypotheses may address only the first-order constructs, not the concept represented by the grouping (Law et al., 1998). The implications of this limitation can be seen in IS studies that utilize this approach. None contains any research questions, hypotheses, or inferences about the effects of trustworthiness itself. Rather, these studies focus on the effects of the individual trustworthiness beliefse.g. ability, benevolence, or integrityon trust (Gefen, 2002b; Lee & Turban, 2001). A considerable limitation of this approach, therefore, is that it cannot forward insights into the role of trustworthiness per se within B2C electronic commerce or other research domains (Law et al., 1998). Trustworthiness as a Second-Order Construct While the theoretical justification for the first-order model has been explored extensively (Mayer et al., 1995; Schoorman et al., 1996), little theoretical development has been proposed for a second-order model of trustworthiness. In this section, we explore the justification for and the relative merits of a second-order conceptualization. Model 1C in Figure 1 models trustworthiness as a higher-order factor that explains the common variance across ability, benevolence, and integrity. Ability reflects the belief that an online retailer has the capacity to act in a beneficial manner; benevolence reflects the belief that the online retailer intends to act in a beneficial manner; and integrity reflects the belief that the online retailer will act predictably and in agreement with the customers views. A second-order conceptualization posits that a commonality exists across the three dimensions representing the trustors composite belief that the trustee will act in a beneficial manner (McKnight et al., 2002a). While McKnight et al., posited such a second-order conceptualization, they did not discuss the underlying theoretical rational, nor the relative advantages and disadvantages versus other conceptualizations. Modeling construct trustworthiness suggests a as a second-order reflective view of

trustworthiness. Each first-order latent construct in a reflective model represents a different manifestation of the second-order factor. A change in any of the first-order factors is assumed to result in a change in the other underlying indicators. Hence, measures (or first-order constructs, in the case of second order factors) must be strongly correlated to support a reflective conceptualization. Studies including trustworthiness commonly find a high degree of intercorrelations among the three trustworthiness dimensions (Bhattacherjee, 2002; Lee & Turban, 2001; McKnight et al., 2002a; Schoorman et al., 1996), which supports the presence of a higherorder factor (Bollen, 1989). Unlike a first-order conceptualization, a secondorder model assumes that individuals maintain consistency in their perceptions of different aspects of the trustee (e.g., online retailers). A variety of cognitive consistency theories (Abelson et al., 1968)e.g., cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957) and balance theory (Heider, 1946)supports such a viewpoint. Such theories posit that individuals strive to resolve conflicting perceptions. In essence, the mental elements that make up perceptions As a specific example, cognitive dissonance theory predicts that when there is inconsistency between attitudes or behaviors, changes need to occur to eliminate that dissonance. One mechanism for reducing dissonance is through changing the dissonant beliefs so they are no longer inconsistent (Festinger, 1957). Individuals try to confirm existing perceptions about others, even if it means accepting beliefs that lack foundation (Kahnemann et al., 1982; Mynatt et al., 1977), or rejecting conflicting information in favor of confirming evidence. Using second-order factors to model consistency in individuals cognitive processes has been used in other recent IS studies. Stewart and Segars (2002) modeled concern for information privacy (CFIP) as a second order factor representing an individuals cognitive state or perception about corporate control over personal information. The underlying dimensions (collection of information, unauthorized access to information, errors in information, and secondary use of information) represent the different ways CFIP is perceived. This cognitive process is similar to a second order conceptualization of trustworthiness, which represents an individuals cognitive state or perception that the trustee will act in a beneficial manner. From a modeling perspective, second-order models have a number of advantages. Second-order constructs can result in more parsimonious nomological networks by reducing the number of

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

93

causal linkages. While multicollinearity can be problematic in first-order models, second-order factors provide a mechanism to explain the common variance across first-order dimensions (Erez & Judge, 2001; Gerbing & Anderson, 1984). In our model, we interpret the second-order construct as the composite belief that another will act in a beneficial manner (McKnight et al., 2002a). Ability, benevolence, and integrity are also represented, allowing a second-order model to capture the variance specific to each first-order dimension. Hence, a second-order model gives meaning to both sources of variance, resulting in a more complete conceptualization. For this reason, second-order factor models are preferred over a first-order model, ceterus paribus (Erez & Judge, 2001; Gerbing & Anderson, 1984). Many of the advantages of the first-order model reflect the disadvantages of the second-order model of trustworthiness. The differential effects of ability, benevolence, and integrity on trust cannot be modeled under a second-order conceptualization. Changes in the second-order factor are modeled as affecting the first-order dimensions, not vice versa. In addition, the paths from trustworthiness to the underlying dimensions represent factor loadings, and cannot be interpreted as causal paths.

commonly used in MIS studies (Chin & Todd, 1995). A 0.05 standard is considered acceptable for RMR (Gefen et al., 2000). To facilitate comparison across studies, we report only the standardized regression paths and factor loadings. We followed accepted procedures for assessing unidimensionality and discriminant validity (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Gefen, 2003a). To assess the fit for a one-dimensional model of trustworthiness, we tested a CFA model which allowed the one-dimensional view to covary with the latent trust variable. To test the fit for the two multidimensional models of trustworthiness, we included separate constructs for ability, benevolence, integrity, and trust that were also allowed to covary. For models that passed initial levels of fit, the procedure recommends examining modification indices that exceed 5.0. This level indicates the model chi-square level would drop significantly if the path were added. Residual covariance levels exceeding 2.58 were also examined, which reflects a significant degree of shared variance, and a potential threat to unidimensionality. For discriminant validity, we conducted chi-square different tests and examined the modification indices between latent variables and measurement indicators. Study 1 Data were collected from 289 students attending an introduction to MIS class at a university in the midwestern United States. Students answered questions about Books-a-million (or BAMM.com), an online bookseller. The researchers provided an exercise for the participants to complete online, which was designed to familiarize students with BAMM.coms interface and to establish initial trustworthiness and trust perceptions. The exercise included such activities as using the BAMM.com search feature to find a book written by a specific author, a review of the websites return policy, and an examination of BAMM.coms Online Gift Delivery Service. Finally, students simulated the process of buying a book, although the actual purchase was optional. We reviewed the completed exercise forms to ensure that students did actually complete the exercise. Data were collected immediately after the website exercise, which students completed in less than twenty minutes. We asked if students had previously heard of BAMM.com, visited the BAMM.com website, or ever purchased anything from BAMM.com. To focus our results on trustworthiness effects on initial trust, any student

Research Methodology
We used data from three studies to test the efficacy of the three models of trustworthiness. For Studies 1 and 2, we collected data from over 700 students at universities in the midwestern and northwestern United States. For Study 3, we reanalyzed data from a previously published study (Gefen, 2002b). We used the following procedures across all models. For all first-order constructs, we fixed the latent variance to 1 to set the scale (Kline, 1998). For the second-order constructs, we fixed one factor loading to 1 to establish model identifiability (Kline, 1998). To assess model fit, we relied on the GFI and AGFI fit indices, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio, the root mean square residual (RMR). Acceptable levels for GFI and AGFI exceed 0.90 and 0.80, respectively (Segars & Grover, 1993), although 0.90 is sometimes cited for AGFI (Chin & Todd, 1995). For RMSEA, 0.08 has been cited as a reasonable error of approximation (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). Other researchers have recommended the lower 0.06 value as an acceptable level (Hu & Bentler, 1999). A 2:1 ratio has been cited for the chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio (Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Hair et al., 1998), although a more accepting 3:1 is

94

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

who responded yes or did not respond to any of the above questions was removed from the dataset. Twenty-one students (7.2%) indicated they had previous experiences with BAMM.com, and forty-two (14.5%) did not answer one of the questions. In total, sixty-three students were removed, resulting in a final sample of 226. Of the remaining students, 1 (<1%) was a freshman, 169 (74%) were sophomores, 42 (19%) were juniors, and 10 (4%) were seniors. One student (<1%) listed his or her status as Other, and three (2%) did not answer the question. Student ages ranged from 19 to 23, with the average age being 20. Males comprised 51% (n=115) of the sample. Study 1 Measures and Instrument Validation We adapted established measures of trustworthiness and trust to our specific context (McKnight et al., 2002a). The measures were originally developed using a second-order conceptualization of trustworthiness. A purpose of

Study 1, therefore, was to determine if onedimensional and first-order views of trustworthiness represented valid alternative models. Table 1 lists the correlations among the study indicators (see Appendix), along with the means and standard deviations. Composite reliabilities are also listed in Table 1, and indicate that the reliability levels for ability (0.93), benevolence (0.89), integrity (0.93), and trust (0.89) all exceed the 0.80 standard (Gefen, 2003a; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). We next assessed model unidimensionality. Testing a one-dimensional model of trustworthiness (Figure 2A), the fit statistics failed to meet accepted standards (GFI=0.81; AGFI=0.74; RMSEA=0.11; chi-square=321.55; df=89). We rejected this model based on the unacceptable levels of fit. We next tested a CFA model that allowed ability, benevolence, integrity, and trust to covary (Figure 2B). All fit indices met or exceeded accepted standards (GFI=0.92; AGFI=0.89; RMSEA=0.060; chi-square/df=1.82; RMR=0.023).

Mean Ability Benevolence Integrity Trust 3.57 3.29 3.34 3.25

Std. Dev. 0.60 0.62 0.56 0.74

Ability (0.93) 0.73 0.72 0.78

Benev. (0.89) 0.87 0.76

Integrity

Trust

(0.93) 0.68 (0.89)

Cronbachs alpha levels are listed in the diagonal. All correlations (n=226) are significant (p<0.001) Correlations at the Indicator Level
Mean A1 A2 A3 A4 B1 B2 B3 I1 I2 I3 I4 T1 T2 T3 T4 3.63 3.48 3.56 3.63 3.39 3.33 3.16 3.35 3.36 3.41 3.24 3.17 2.92 3.40 3.50 Std Dev 0.70 0.70 0.73 0.73 0.75 0.73 0.73 0.66 0.66 0.68 0.67 0.95 0.85 0.88 0.85 A1 0.63 0.60 0.48 0.43 0.36 0.32 0.45 0.43 0.43 0.35 0.39 0.42 0.48 0.50 A2 A3 A4 B1 B2 B3 I1 I2 I3 I4 T1 T2 T3

0.71 0.64 0.53 0.45 0.36 0.53 0.47 0.45 0.42 0.46 0.51 0.53 0.51

0.67 0.59 0.45 0.37 0.49 0.46 0.52 0.47 0.51 0.49 0.53 0.51

0.53 0.42 0.32 0.43 0.41 0.45 0.36 0.42 0.51 0.56 0.57

0.62 0.61 0.60 0.54 0.56 0.59 0.48 0.54 0.57 0.54

0.47 0.52 0.51 0.57 0.46 0.36 0.41 0.49 0.39

0.59 0.51 0.43 0.60 0.32 0.43 0.37 0.32

0.70 0.58 0.61 0.40 0.50 0.48 0.40

0.57 0.59 0.31 0.47 0.46 0.37

0.54 0.42 0.50 0.45 0.42

0.34 0.47 0.40 0.39

0.59 0.66 0.55

0.63 0.55

0.66

Table 1. Study 1 Construct Correlations and Reliabilities

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

95

eA1

eA2

eA3

eA4

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1

A2

A3

A4

B1

B2

B3

I1

I2

I3

I4

.72 .60 .74 .68 .79 .71 .63 .77

.72 .71 .68

TW

.81

GFI=.810 AGFI=.744 RMSEA = .108 Chi-Sq = 321.551 df = 89 Chi-sq / df = 3.613 RMR=.034

Trust .73 .76 T2 .85 T3 .76

T1

T4

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 2A: Study 1 One-Dimensional Measurement Model All paths are significant p<0.001.

eA1

eA2

eA3

eA4

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1 .68

A2 .82 .83

A3 .77

A4

B1 .86

B2 .74 .68

B3

I1 .83

I2 .79 .73

I3 .74

I4

Ability

Benevolence

Integrity

.73

.87

.72 .76 .78 .68

Trust .73 .75 T2 .85 T3 .77 .59 T1 T4

GFI=.920 AGFI=.886 RMSEA = .060 Chi-Sq = 152.598 df = 84 Chi-sq / df = 1.817 RMR=.023

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 2B: Study 1 Multidimensional Measurement Model All paths are significant p<0.001

96

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

Eight of the modification indices between residual variances exceeded 5.0the highest being 8.78. None of the standardized residual covariances, however, exceeded 1.77, which is well below the recommended 2.58 cutoff. Discriminant validity was assessed using an accepted procedure (Gefen et al., 2003a). We first examined the modification indices to determine if any of the undeclared paths between latent constructs and measurement items exceeded 5.0. Such a level would indicate a threat to unidimensionality, since the indicator would crossload with another factor. None of the modification indices exceeded 5.0. We then ran multiple models that constrained each pair of the construct covariances to 1. Of the six constrained models, all exhibited significant increases in chi-square (the 2 lowest differential was (1) =23.23; p<0.001), indicating that each covariance path was significantly different from 1. Discriminant validity is confirmed. Study 1 Results Having rejected a one-dimensional model of trustworthiness, we then compared the first-order causal model of trustworthiness with a second-order
eA1 eA2 eA3 eA4 eB1 eB2

causal model. Because these scales were originally developed using a second-order conceptualization, we were especially interested in examining the fit levels for a first-order conceptualization. Both models fit the data well. The first-order model (Figure 3A) exhibits acceptable fit (GFI=0.92; AGFI=0.89; RMSEA=0.060; chi-square/df=1.82; RMR=0.023). Among the first-order factors, ability (=0.50; p<0.001) and benevolence (=0.48; p<0.001) are similar in their predictive power. The integrity to trust path is not significant, however (=0.10; p=0.53). The three constructs explain 69% of the trust variance. The covariance paths among the three first-order dimensions suggest that significant levels of interdependencies among the three trustworthiness dimensions are also expected. This assumption is apparently valid, given the high correlation levels (0.72; 0.73; 0.87) between the firstorder constructs. The second-order model also exhibits acceptable fit (GFI=0.91; AGFI=0.87; chisquare/df=1.987; RMR=0.028). Only the RMSEA (0.066) somewhat exceeds the more stringent 0.06 standard (Hu & Bentler, 1999), but is well below the 0.08 standard. Consistent with the first-order model, trustworthiness explains 69% of the trust variance.

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1 .68

A2 .82 .83

A3 .77

A4

B1 .86

B2 .74 .68

B3

I1 .83

I2 .79 .73

I3 .74

I4

Ability

Benevolence

Integrity

.73

.87

.72

.50

.48

-.10

eTrust .69 .73 .75 T2 Trust .85 T3 .77

T1

T4

GFI=.920 AGFI=.886 RMSEA = .060 Chi-Sq = 152.598 df = 84 Chi-sq / df = 1.817 RMR=.023

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 3A: Study 1 First-Order Causal Model


Note: For the Benevolence Trust Path, p=0.01 For the Integrity Trust Path, p=0.53 All other paths are significant p<0.001.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

97

eA1

eA2

eA3

eA4 .58

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1 .68

A2 .82 .83

A3 .76

A4

B1 .87

B2 .74 .67

B3

I1 .83

I2 .80 .73

I3 .73

I4

Ability .70 eAbility eBenev .84

Benevolence eIntegrity .85 .92 .88

Integrity .78

TW

GFI=.910 AGFI=.874 RMSEA = .066 Chi-Sq = 170.846 df = 86 Chi-sq / df = 1.987 RMR=.028


T1

.83 .69 Trust .73 .76 T2 .85 T3 .76 eTrust

T4

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 3B: Study 1 Second-Order Causal Model All paths are significant p<0.001. Study 1 indicates that while both models of trustworthiness are valid, results for each model, must be interpreted differently. The first-order model for trustworthiness assumes that ability, benevolence, and integrity each contribute independently to the formation of trust. The strong relationships between ability to trust and benevolence to trust indicate that improving perceptions of each may result in increased trust by online consumers. Integrity apparently has no effect within this context. Because trustworthiness is modeled as a collection of the three constructs, no conclusions can be drawn about trustworthiness' role in the model. In contrast, the second-order model results indicate that ability, benevolence, and integrity are each strong manifestations of trustworthiness. This result supports the assumption that changes in trustworthiness will result in changes to the three underlying factors. Finally, the results indicate that perceptions that the online vendor will act in a beneficial manner are significantly related to trust. No conclusions can be drawn, however, on the direct effects of ability, benevolence, and integrity on trust. Study 2 To confirm our results, we collected a second sample. Similar to the data collected in Study 1, students attended an introductory class in management information systems. Students used the same survey instrument, and the data were collected approximately four months after the initial data collection. For Study 2, 430 students completed surveys. Nineteen students had previous experience with BAMM.com, and five did not answer the question. To again focus on the effects of initial trust, these twenty-four observations were removed, resulting in a final sample size of 406. Of these, 21 were freshman (5%), 172 were sophomores (42%), 167 were juniors (41%), and 43 were seniors (11%). Three students (<1%) listed their status as Other. Males comprised 62% (n=253) of the sample. The students ages ranged from 18 to 46, with the average age being 21. The indicator and construct correlations, means, and standard deviations are listed in Table 2. Composite reliability levels for ability (0.84), benevolence, (0.81), integrity (0.84), and trust (0.87) were again acceptable.

98

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

Mean Std. Dev. Ability Benev. Integrity Trust Ability 3.66 0.60 (0.84) Benevolence 3.42 0.63 0.70 (0.81) Integrity 3.45 0.56 0.68 0.73 (0.84) Trust 3.39 0.77 0.66 0.71 0.70 (0.87) Cronbachs alpha levels are listed in the diagonal. All correlations (n=407) are significant (p<0.001) Correlations at the Indicator Level
Mean A1 A2 A3 A4 B1 B2 B3 I1 I2 I3 I4 T1 T2 T3 T4 3.63 3.64 3.66 3.72 3.45 3.49 3.32 3.46 3.51 3.45 3.37 3.35 3.19 3.51 3.50 Std Dev 0.69 0.73 0.72 0.68 0.74 0.76 0.74 0.68 0.69 0.69 0.67 0.99 0.88 0.87 0.89 A1 0.68 0.63 0.56 0.42 0.33 0.36 0.38 0.35 0.43 0.35 0.39 0.42 0.46 0.41 A2 A3 A4 B1 B2 B3 I1 I2 I3 I4 T1 T2 T3

0.68 0.60 0.42 0.37 0.34 0.38 0.35 0.41 0.39 0.44 0.49 0.56 0.46

0.64 0.39 0.41 0.39 0.38 0.43 0.49 0.37 0.42 0.45 0.51 0.43

0.41 0.37 0.40 0.42 0.42 0.45 0.37 0.43 0.50 0.52 0.50

0.56 0.54 0.50 0.49 0.42 0.38 0.38 0.43 0.49 0.42

0.64 0.47 0.45 0.40 0.44 0.34 0.45 0.47 0.37

0.54 0.54 0.48 0.52 0.38 0.40 0.46 0.41

0.64 0.49 0.52 0.29 0.41 0.47 0.40

0.63 0.54 0.31 0.36 0.44 0.39

0.58 0.37 0.41 0.46 0.36 0.32 0.41 0.42 0.31 0.67 0.62 0.55

0.69 0.56

0.66

All correlations (n=430) are significant (p<0.001).


Table 2. Study 2 Construct Correlations and Reliabilities We used the same procedure as Study 1 to assess unidimensionality (Gefen, 2003a). The onedimensional model covarying with trust was again rejected for unacceptable fit (GFI=0.79; AGFI=0.72; RMSEA=0.12; chi-square/df=6.835; RMR=0.037). The first-order measurement model (Figure 4) exhibited acceptable fit (GFI=0.94; AGFI=0.91; RMSEA=0.059; chi-square/df=2.413; RMR=0.021). An examination of the modification indices indicated 6 that thirteen modification indices exceeded 5.0. None of listed the residual covariances was significant, however. All were less than 1.75. Discriminant validity was evaluated using the same procedure as Study 1. No paths between latent variables and measurement items exceeded 5.0. Constraining covariance paths individually again increased the chi-square levels significantly (minimum increase was 69.89, p<0.001). Discriminant validity is confirmed. Study 2 Results
6

While eleven of the MI were less than 7.4, two of the MIs were quite high (19.21and 16.09). We added in these covariances to assess the effect on the first- and second-order causal models. For the first-order model, the maximum change was a 0.02 increase in the standardized path between integrity and trust. It remained insignificant, however. For the second-order model, the maximum change was a 0.02 decline in the factor loading between trustworthiness and integrity. It remained highly significant (p<0.001). Because the magnitude of a modification index is a function of sample size (Gefen, 2003a), we also used a random sample of 226 (the same size as Study 1) to judge the magnitude of the MIs in Study 2 versus the MIs in Study 1. The

The results for the first-order and second-order causal models are shown in Models 5A and 5B of Figure 5. Fit levels for the first-order model are acceptable (GFI=0.94; AGFI=0.91; RMSEA=0.059; chi-square/df=2.413; RMR=0.021).

modification indices dropped to 6.62 and to 7.48, respectively. These levels are comparable to the levels from Study 1.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

99

eA1

eA2

eA3

eA4

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1 .77

A2 .83 .82

A3 .75

A4

B1 .72

B2 .76 .79

B3

I1 .75

I2 .81 .74

I3 .71

I4

Ability

Benevolence

Integrity

.62 .65

.81

.69

.64

.72

Trust .74 .80 T2 .87 T3 .74

T1

T4

GFI=.939 AGFI=.912 RMSEA = .059 Chi-Sq = 202.700 df = 84 Chi-sq / df = 2.413 RMR=.021

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 4: Study 2 Measurement Model All paths are significant p<0.001. Ability and benevolence are again significantly related to trust (p<0.001), but integrity is not (=0.03, p=0.74). The three first-order constructs explain 62% of the trust variance. Fit levels for the secondorder model are again somewhat lower, but still acceptable (GFI=0.93; AGFI=0.90; RMSEA=0.065; chi-square/df=2.711; RMR=0.026). All three factor loadings are highly significant (p<0.001). Trustworthiness explains 66% of the trust variance. Results for Study 2 confirm results from Study 1. For the first-order model, ability and benevolence are again significant predictors of trust, while integrity is not. For the second-order model, all paths from trustworthiness to the underlying dimensions are highly significant (p<0.001). The ordering of the magnitude of the three factor loadings is the same. The causal path between trustworthiness and trust is also highly significant (=0.81, p<0.001 versus =0.83, p<0.001). External Validation of Results To further assess the external validity of our findings, we tested the first- and second-order models with data from a previously published study (Gefen, 2002b). The original studies relied on the first-order conceptualization of trustworthiness. Our intent was to determine if a second-order conceptualization was applicable within Gefens alternative context. Similar to Studies 1 and 2, the Gefen study tested a model of the dimensions of trust and trustworthiness within an Internet purchasing environment. Unidimensionality and reliability results all meet acceptable standards, and are available in Gefen (2002b). Composite reliability levels for ability (0.88), benevolence (0.86), integrity (0.91), and trust (0.91) also exceed accepted standards.

100

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

eA1

eA2

eA3

eA4

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1 .77

A2 .83 .82

A3 .75

A4

B1 .72

B2 .76 .79

B3

I1 .75

I2 .81 .74

I3 .71

I4

Ability

Benevolence

Integrity

.62

.81

.65 .47 .37

.03

.62 Trust .74 .80 T2 .87 T3 .74 eTrust

T1

T4

GFI=.939 AGFI=.912 RMSEA = .059 Chi-Sq = 202.700 df = 84 Chi-sq / df = 2.413 RMR=.021

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 5A: Study 2 First-Order Causal Model Note: For the Integrity Trust Path, p=0.74 All other paths are significant p<0.001.

eA1

eA2

eA3

eA4

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

eI4

A1 .77

A2 .83 .83

A3 .75

A4

B1 .73

B2 .76 .78

B3

I1 .75

I2 .81 .75

I3 .71

I4

.62

Ability eBenev

Benevolence

.76 eIntegrity

Integrity

.73

eAbility

.79

.87

.85

TW

GFI=.929 AGFI=.900 RMSEA = .065 Chi-Sq = 233.121 df = 86 Chi-sq / df = 2.711 NFI = .932 CFI = .956 RMR=.026
T1

.81 .66 Trust .74 .80 T2 .87 T3 .73 eTrust

T4

eT1

eT2

eT3

eT4

Figure 5B: Study 2 Second-Order Causal Model Note: All paths are significant p<0.001.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

101

Some important differences exist between the Gefen study and our two studies. In Study 1 and 2, students had no previous contact with Books-amillion.com. In the Gefen study, participants had probable knowledge of and experience with the online vendor, Amazon.com (see Appendix). The questions used in the Gefen study differed from the ones included in this study, but the definitions of the three trustworthiness constructs closely paralleled our definitions. Ability was defined as the belief that Amazon.com has appropriate skills and competence. Benevolence was defined as the belief that Amazon.com wants to do good to the customer. Integrity was defined as the belief that Amazon.com adheres to accepted rules of conduct. Trust in Amazon.com reflected a respondents willingness to purchase an item. Given the similarity in definitions and theoretical foundations, we believe the Gefen study represents a fair comparison with our results. Moreover, the use of different (but theoretically consistent) questions may represent a stronger test of external validity, since the context differs from the one examined in our two studies. For consistency with the earlier studies, we tested both one-dimensional and a multidimensional CFA measurement model. The one-dimensional model was again rejected based on unacceptable fit results (e.g., RMSEA=0.17). The multidimensional model results were again acceptable (GFI=0.95; AGFI=0.91; RMSEA=0.073; chi-square/df=2.52; RMR=0.029). Causal model results are listed in Figure 6. The fit levels for the first-order model (Figure 6A) indicate acceptable fit (GFI=0.95; AGFI=0.91; RMSEA=0.073; chi-square/df=2.52; RMR=0.029). Unlike Study 1 and 2, integrity is a significant predictor of trust (=0.52, p<0.001). The benevolence to trust path is again significant (=0.27, p=0.03). Ability, which was the strongest predictor of trust in Study 1 and Study 2 is not significant (=-0.03, p=0.64). The three first-order constructs explain 53% of the trust variance. For the second-order model (Figure 6B), the fit levels are also acceptable (GFI=0.94; AGFI=0.89; RMSEA=0.079; chi-square/df=2.79; RMR=0.036). The standardized regression path between trustworthiness and trust is again highly significant (=0.74; p<0.001). Trustworthiness explains 55% of the trust variance. The results for this study contrast somewhat with the earlier two studies. For the first-order model, the integrity to trust path is highly significant. This path was not significant in Studies 1 and 2. We explore possible explanations for this difference in the

discussion section, including the additional level of familiarity that the users had with the online vendor. For the second-order model, the ordering of the factor loadings is the same as the earlier studies. Benevolence reflects the strongest factor loading, followed by integrity and ability. The strength of the regression path between trustworthiness and trust is consistent with the prior two studies.

Discussion
The results of this study provide theoretical and practical insights into alternative conceptualizations of trustworthiness by synthesizing previous research and testing three alternative conceptualizations of trustworthiness within the e-commerce context. Data collected using scales originally developed for first- and second-order models of trustworthiness within an e-commerce context were both tested for validity. Our results indicate that viewing trustworthiness as a one-dimensional construct may be somewhat limited, in that this conceptualizations parsimony may not fully represent the underlying data. Instead, our results suggest trustworthiness is better represented as a multidimensional construct, and that both firstand second-order conceptualizations have a place in e-commerce trust research, depending on the purpose of the study. Contributions to Theory This studys theoretical findings are important, because a constructs conceptualization has implications for its definition, operationalization, and practical interpretation (Stewart & Segars, 2002). An assumption of the first-order model is that trustworthiness is an aggregation of its dimensions. In contrast, the second-order assumes that consumers will strive to maintain cognitive consistency when considering online vendors. Ecommerce researchers must consider carefully the research questions, hypotheses, and design before selecting the appropriate conceptualization for trustworthiness. While both multidimensional models appear to be valid, each also has relative advantages and disadvantages. Although the results of previous studies into trustworthiness have consistently found high correlations between the three trustworthiness dimensions, our results confirm that they are conceptually different and do represent different characteristics of the trustee (Mayer et al., 1995). Research questions that propose different consequences of ability, benevolence, and integrity

102

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

eA1

eA2

eA3

eB1

eB2

eB3

eI1

eI2

eI3

A1 .82

A2 .84 .88

A3

B1 .85

B2 .79 .80

B3

I1 .87

I2 .88 .90

I3

Ability .80

Benevolence .80 .68 -.03 .27 .52

Integrity

.53 .87 T1

Trust .95 T2

eTrust

GFI=.945 AGFI=.905 RMSEA = .073 Chi-Sq = 95.575 df = 38 Chi-sq / df = 2.515 RMR=.029

eT1

eT2

Figure 6A: Confirmatory Study First-Order Model Note: For the Ability Trust Path, p=0.74. For the Benevolence Trust Path, p=0.03. All other paths are significant p<0.001.
eA1 eA2 eA3 eB1 eB2 eB3 eI1 eI2 eI3

A1 .82

A2 .84 .87

A3

B1 .85

B2 .79 .80

B3

I1 .87

I2 .87 .90

I3

.65

Ability

.87 Benevolence

.76

Integrity

eBenev eAbility .81 .93 .87 eIntegrity

GFI=.936 AGFI=.894 RMSEA = .079 Chi-Sq = 111.424 df = 40 Chi-sq / df = 2.786 RMR=.036

TW .74

.55 .89 T1

Trust .93 T2

eTrus t

eT1

eT2

Figure 6B: Confirmatory Study Second-Order Causal Model Note: All paths are significant p<0.001. Adapted with permission from Gefen (2002b)

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

103

may, therefore, wish to consider a first-order conceptualization (Law et al., 1998). As an example, studies that utilize experimental designs to control or manipulate the trustworthiness factors should consider treating trustworthiness as three disparate constructs. In contrast, the common theme in the second-order model is a consumers composite belief that an online vendor will act in a beneficial manner, where changes in trustworthiness are reflected in all three subdimensions. This conceptualization allows for the study of trustworthiness itself on the formation of trust, in situations where finer grained views of trustworthiness are not necessary. We also find that the selection of a conceptualization has implications for interpretations of a study's findings. In Figure 6, for example, the first-order model indicates that Amazon.coms ability does not play a significant role in trust formation. In contrast, the second-order model indicates that ability, benevolence, and integrity all significantly reflect trustworthiness beliefs and that trustworthiness strongly influences trust formation. In this instance, interpreting trustworthiness as a second-order model suggests a slightly different result, indicating that ability does play an important role in trust formation. In addition, and as discussed earlier, applying the first-order model limits research questions and hypotheses for these studies to only addressing the dimensions of trustworthiness, not the trustworthiness itself (Law et al., 1998). The insights in this study therefore provide a foundation for future studies of trustworthiness, as well as for the potential reinterpretation of the findings from past studies. This research also contributes theoretical support for the McKnight et al. (2002a) study that presented trustworthiness as a second order factor. For a second-order model to be a valid conceptualization of trustworthiness, researchers must assume that changes in any of the first-order factors results in a consistent change in the other underlying factors. Although McKnight et al. (2002a) positioned trustworthiness as a second-order factor, they were not explicit as to the theoretical rationale. We posit that the commonality across trustworthiness dimensions can be explained by the cognitive consistency literature (e.g., cognitive dissonance). The ability to capture both dimensional differences as well as the commonality across dimensions reflects the richness that is possible with secondorder factors. Another finding of this research relates to the relative importance of ability, benevolence, and

integrity in second-order models (Figures 3B, 5B, 6B), which was consistent across all three studies. This result suggests that benevolence is the most significant factor, followed by integrity and then ability in the B2C electronic commerce domain. Interestingly, the same order of significance was found in McKnight et al. (2002a), which this study extends. While this could be dismissed as an artifact of the survey, it should be noted that the Gefen (2002b) data, gathered with a different instrument, also demonstrated the same relative importance. We note, however, that while the ordering was consistent, the differences in the magnitudes were small. This consistency was not found in the causal paths in the first-order models (Figures 3A, 5A, 6A). Paths between the trustworthiness dimensions and trust were consistent in our Study 1 and Study 2, but differed in the external validation analysis. This variability may stem from the different contexts in which the data were gathered. In the present research, the e-vender was an unknown bookseller, Booksamillion.com. In the Gefen (2002b) study, it was Amazon.com. This illustrates the advantage of using a first-order model for studying the relative effects of the dimensions of trustworthiness on the formation of trust across different contexts. Finally, this paper clarifies the distinct role of trustworthiness and trust in the nomological network, constructs often not clearly distinguished from one another in the information systems literature. The theory of reasoned action provides a useful framework for understanding the role of trustworthiness in establishing trusting attitudes. Trustworthiness represents the composite belief that an online retailer has beneficial characteristics. In contrast, trust represents a general willingness to rely on the online retailer. The inconsistency in conceptualization of these two constructs in the literature may be partially due to the various interpretations of trustworthiness, and in many instances the lack of an explicit trustworthiness construct. The second-order model establishes this construct, which should facilitate future studies into this important predictor of trust. Contributions to Practice Although this paper is focused on the theoretical nature of trustworthiness, our findings have implications for e-commerce practitioners. This research examined the effect of the three dimensions of trustworthiness on consumer trust in two contexts, a new online relationship (BAMM.com) and a more established context (Amazon.com).

104

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

Applying a first-order model, the relative significance of the three trustworthiness dimensions effect on trust was demonstrated to differ across these two contexts. In new online relationships, our results showed that while ability and benevolence were significant factors in trust formation, integrity was not. In Gefens more established context, ability became insignificant while integrity and benevolence demonstrated strong effects on trust. This result may indicate that to attract new customers, online vendors should provide more cues on their websites regarding their integrity. The increased focus on privacy policies may be a demonstration of this practice. With repeat customers, however, the insignificance of ability may be due to a consumer comfort level with the online vendors ability based on past experiences. This same comfort level does not appear to be reached with integrity and benevolence, since both significantly influence trust in this context. Applying a second order view of trustworthiness to the same data highlights the need for retailers to signal consumers that they are trustworthy across the three dimensions. Violating one trustworthiness dimension may serve to deflate beliefs on other dimensions. If a product is not shipped in a timely manner, this failure along the ability dimension may also lower a consumer's perceptions of the vendor's benevolence and integrity. Similarly, negative information about a vendors integrityperhaps from a third part reputation servicemay also shed doubts on the vendors ability and benevolence in the eyes of the potential customer. While further research should be directed at this topic, the current message to practitioners is that the three dimensions are closely intertwined and all should be carefully nurtured by online vendors. Limitations and Future Research This study is not without limitations. Although two different scales were applied, both were originally developed for multidimensional conceptualizations of trustworthiness. It is possible that a onedimensional model would have performed better with a scale more focused on one aspect of trustworthiness (e.g., predictability). We note that this approach will likely reduce the explained variance in trust, since only one aspect of trustworthiness would be included. This possibility should be explored in future studies, however, perhaps using other one-dimensional trustworthiness scales (Jarvenpaa et al., 2000). The generalizability of the results may be limited since we used undergraduate students. In an effort to increase external validity, however, we collected

data from two different universities and further validated our results using data from an outside study, which used MBA students. We do encourage other researchers to extend the external validity of our results by replicating our study in other contexts using other samples. The purpose of this study was to extend the understanding of the role that ability, benevolence, and integrity play in the formation of trustworthiness beliefs and trusting attitudes. We were not focused on how to best demonstrate all three dimensions to consumers. Further research is needed to discover and recommend how online retailers can best convey ability, benevolence, and integrity perceptions to online consumers.

References
Abelson, R., Aronson, E., McGuire, W., Newcombe, T., Rosenberg, T. and Tannenbaum, P. (1968). Theories of Cognitive Consistency: A Source Book, Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. Anderson, J.C. and Gerbing, D.W. (1988). "Structural Equation Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step Approach," Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 103, pp. 411-423. Bentler, P.M. and Bonett, D.G. (1980). "Significance Tests and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance Structures," Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 88, pp. 588-606. Bhattacherjee, A. (2002). "Individual Trust in Online Firms: Scale Development and Initial Test," Journal of Management Information Systems, Vol. 19, No. 1, pp. 211-241. Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural Equations with Latent Variables, New York: John Wiley & Sons. Browne, M.W. and Cudeck, R. (1993). "Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit," in Bollen, K.A. and Long, J.S. (Eds.), Testing Structural Equation Models, Newbury Park, CA: Sage, pp. 136-162. Chin, W.W. (1998). "Issues and Opinions on Structural Equation Modeling," MIS Quarterly, Vol. 21, No. 1, pp. vii-xvi. Chin, W.W. and Todd, P.A. (1995). "On the Use, Usefulness, and Ease of Use of Structural Equation Modeling in MIS Research: A Note of Caution," MIS Quarterly, Vol. 19, No. 2, pp. 237246. Doney, P.M. and Cannon, J.P. (1997). "An Examination of the Nature of Trust in BuyerSeller Relationships," Journal of Marketing, Vol. 61, pp. 35-51.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

105

Eagley, A. and Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes, Orlando, Florida: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Erez, A. and Judge, T. (2001). "Relationship of Core Self-Evaluations to Goal-Setting, Motivation, and Performance," Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 86, No. 6, pp. 1270-1279. Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Fishbein, M. and Azjen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research, Reading, MA: AddisonWesley. Gefen, D. (2002a). "Nurturing Clients' Trust to Encourage Engagement Success During the Customization of ERP Systems," OMEGA, Vol. 30, pp. 287-299. Gefen, D. (2002b). "Reflections on the Dimensions of Trust and Trustworthiness among Online Consumers," DATA BASE for Advances in Information Systems, Vol. 33, No. 3, pp. 38-53. Gefen, D. (2003a). "Assessing Unidimensionality Through Lisrel: An Explanation and Example," CAIS, Vol. 12, pp. 23-47. Gefen, D. (2003b). "Managing User Trust in B2C EServices," E-Service Journal, Vol. 2, No. 2, pp. 7-24. Gefen, D., Karahanna, E. and Straub, D. (2003). "Trust and Tam in Online Shopping: An Integrated Model," MIS Quarterly, Vol. 27, No. 1, pp. 51-90. Gefen, D., Straub, D.W. and Boudreau, M.-C. (2000). "Structural Equation Modeling and Regression: Guidelines for Research and Practice," CAIS, Vol. 4, No. 7, pp. 1-70. Gerbing, D.W. and Anderson, J.C. (1984). "On the Meaning of Within-Factor Correlated Measurement Errors," Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 11, pp. 572-580. Good, D. (1988). "Individuals, Interpersonal Relations, and Trust," in Gambetta, D.G. (Ed.), Trust, New York: Basil Blackwell, pp. 131-185. Hair, J.F., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L. and Black, W.C. (1998). Multivariate Data Analysis with Readings, 5th Edition, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Heider, F. (1946). "Attitudes and Cognitive Organization," Journal of Psychology, Vol. 21, pp. 107-112. Hovland, C.I., Janis, I.L. and Kelley, H.H. (1953). Communication and Persuasion, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Hu, L.T. and Bentler, P.M. (1999). "Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives,"

Structural Equation Modeling, Vol. 6, No. 1, pp. 1-55. Jarvenpaa, S., Tractinsky, N. and Vitale, M. (2000). "Consumer Trust in an Internet Store," Information Technology and Management, Vol. 1, No. 1-2, pp. 45-72. Kahnemann, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kim, K.K. and Prabhakar, B. (2004). "Initial Trust and the Adoption of B2C E-Commerce: The Case of Internet Banking," Database for Advances in Information Systems, Vol. 35, No.3, pp. 50-64. Kline, R. (1998). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, New York: Guilford. Lander, M.C., Purvis, R.L., McCray, G.E. and Leigh, W. (2004). "Trust-Building Mechanisms Utilized in Outsourced Is Development Projects: A Case Study," Information & Management, Vol. 41, No. 4. Law, K. and Wong, C.-S. (1999). "Multidimensional Constructs in Structural Equation Analysis: An Illustration Using the Job Perception and Job Satisfaction Constructs," Journal of Management, Vol. 25, No. 2, pp. 143-160. Law, K., Wong, C.-S. and Mobley, W.H. (1998). "Toward a Taxonomy of Multidimensional Constructs," Academy of Management Review, Vol. 23, No. 4, pp. 741-755. Lee, M. and Turban, E. (2001). "A Trust Model for Consumer Internet Shopping," International Journal of Electronic Commerce, Vol. 6, No. 1, pp. 75-91. Lewicki, R. and Bunker, B. (1995). "Trust in Relationships: A Model of Trust Development and Decline," in Bunker B. and Rubin J.Z. (Eds.), Conflict, Cooperation, and Justice, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Lewis, J.D. and Weigert, A. (1985). "Trust as a Social Reality," Social Forces, Vol. 63, pp. 967985. Lieberman, J.K. (1981). The Litigious Society, New York: Basic Books. Mayer, R.C., Davis, J.H. and Schoorman, F.D. (1995). "An Integration Model of Organizational Trust," Academy of Management Review, Vol. 20, No. 3, pp. 709-734. McKnight, H., Choudhury, V. and Kacmar, C. (2002a). "Developing and Validating Trust Measures for E-Commerce: An Integrative Typology," Information Systems Research, Vol. 13, No. 3, pp. 334-359. McKnight, H., Choudhury, V. and Kacmar, C. (2002b). "The Impact of Initial Consumer Trust on Intentions to Transact with a Web Site: A

106

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

Trust Building Model," Journal of Strategic Information Systems, Vol. 11, pp. 297-323. Morwitz, V. and Pluzinski, C. (1996). "Do Polls Reflect Opinions or Do Opinions Reflect Polls? The Impact of Political Polling on Voters' Expectations, Preferences, and Behavior," Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 23, No. 1, pp. 53-67. Mynatt, C., Doherty, M. and Tweeny, R. (1977). "Confirmation Bias in a Simulated Research Environment: An Experimental Study of Scientific Inference," Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 29. Netemeyer, R., Johnston, M. and Burton, S. (1990). "Analysis of Role Conflict and Role Ambiguity in a Structural Equations Framework," Journal of Applied Psychology, No. 75, pp. 148-157. Nunnally, J.C. and Bernstein, I.H. (1994). Psychometric theory, 3rd Edition, New York: McGraw-Hill. Pavlou, P. and Gefen, D. (2004). "Building Effective Online Marketplaces with Institution-Based Trust," Information Systems Research, Vol. 15, No. 1, pp. 37-59. Pavlou, P.A. (2003). "Consumer Acceptance of Electronic Commerce: Integrating Trust and Risk with the Technology Acceptance Model," International Journal of Electronic Commerce, Vol. 7 No. 3, pp. 101-134. Reichheld, F.F. and Schefter, P. (2000). "E-Loyalty: Your Secret Weapon on the Web," Harvard Business Review, Vol. 78, No. 4, pp. 105-113. Schoorman, F.D., Mayer, R.C. and Davis, J.H. (1996). "Empowerment in Veterinary Clinics: The Role of Trust in Delegation," 11th Annual Meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Segars, A.H. and Grover, V. (1993). "Re-Examining Perceived Ease of Use and Usefulness: A Confirmatory Factor Analysis," MIS Quarterly, Vol. 17, No. 4, pp. 517-525. Stewart, K.A. and Segars, A.H. (2002). "An Empirical Examination of the Concern for Privacy Instrument," Information Systems Research, Vol. 13, No. 1, pp. 36-49. Suh, B. and Han, I. (2002). "Effect of Trust on Customer Acceptance of Internet Banking," Electronic Commerce: Research and Applications, Vol. 1, pp. 247-263. Tyler, T. and Degoey, P. (1996). "Trust in Organizational Authorities: The Influence of Motive Attributions and Willingness to Accept Decisions," in Kramer R.M. and Tyler T. (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Van Slyke, C., Belanger, F. and Comunale, C.L. (2004). "Factors Influencing the Adoption of Web-Based Shopping: The Impact of Trust," New york: Spring 2004. Vol. 35, iss. 2. Database for Advances in Information Systems, Vol. 35, No. 2.

About the Authors


Mark A. Serva is an Assistant Professor of MIS at the University of Delaware. Dr. Servas research interests include the measurement of trust and trustworthiness, trust in an e-commerce environment, and virtual communities. He has previously published in the Information Resource Management Journal, Journal of Management Education, and Information Systems Frontiers. John "Skip" Benamati is an Associate Professor of MIS in the Richard T. Farmer School of Business at Miami University, Oxford, OH. Dr. Benamatis research interests are changing IT, IT management/strategy, and electronic commerce. His work has appeared in the Journal of Management Information Systems, Communications of the ACM, Information and Management, Journal of Information Technology and Management, and elsewhere. Mark A. Fuller is an Associate Professor and Chair of the Department of Information Systems at Washington State University. He received his Ph.D. in Management Information Systems from the University of Arizona in 1993. Dr. Fuller's research interests focus on virtual teamwork, technology mediated learning, and e-commerce. Past research has appeared in journals such as Information Systems Research, Group Decision and Negotiation, Decision Support Systems, the Journal of Information Systems Education, and the Journal of Management Education.

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

107

Appendix: Study Questions


Ability 1. Books-a-million.com is competent and effective in selling books online. 2. Books-a-million.com performs its role of selling books online very well. 3. Overall, Books-a-million.com is a capable and proficient Internet bookseller. 4. In general, Books-a-million.com is very knowledgeable about selling books. Benevolence 1. I believe that Books-a-million.com would act in my best interests. 2. If I required help, Books-a-million.com would do its best to help me. 3. Books-a-million.com is interested in my well-being, not just its own. Integrity 1. Books-a-million.com is truthful in its dealings with me. 2. I would characterize Books-a-million.com as honest. 3. Books-a-million.com would keep its commitments. 4. Books-a-million.com is sincere and genuine. Trust 1. If I needed a book in a hurry, I would feel comfortable depending on Books-a-million.com. 2. I can always rely on Books-a-million.com whenever I need to buy a book. 3. I feel that I could count on Books-a-million.com to help me purchase the books I need. 4. If I needed the best book on a specific topic, I would be willing to rely on the information provided by Books-amillion.com. Gefen (2002) Scales Ability 1. 2. 3. 4. Amazon.com are competent Amazon.com understands the market they work in. Amazon.com knows about books Amazon.com knows how to provide excellent service.

Benevolence 1. I expect that Amazon.com is ready and willing to assist and support me. 2. I expect that Amazon.com have good intentions toward me. 3. I expect that Amazon.com intentions are benevolent. 4. I expect that Amazon.com puts customers interests before their own. 5. I expect that Amazon.com are well meaning. Integrity 1. Promises made by Amazon.com are likely to be reliable. 2. I do not doubt the honesty of Amazon.com. 3. I expect that Amazon.com will keep promises they make. 4. I expect that the advice given by Amazon.com is their best judgment. 5. I can count on Amazon.com to be sincere. Trust 1. Even if not monitored, Id trust Amazon.com to do the job right. 2. I trust Amazon.com.

108

The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems - Summer 2005 (Vol. 36, No. 3)

You might also like