Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Parenting Behaviors, Perceptions, and Psychosocial Risk: Impacts on Young Children's Development Frances Page Glascoe and Shirley

Leew Pediatrics 2010;125;313; originally published online January 25, 2010; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2008-3129

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/125/2/313.full.html

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2010 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

ARTICLES

Parenting Behaviors, Perceptions, and Psychosocial Risk: Impacts on Young Childrens Development
AUTHORS: Frances Page Glascoe, PhDa and Shirley Leew, PhDb,c
aDepartment of Pediatrics, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee; bAlberta Health Services, Calgary, Alberta, Canada; and cDepartment of Rehabilitation Medicine, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada

WHATS KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Delayed language skills lead to difculties in mastering basic reading skills and reading comprehension and to school failure. Parenting skills associated with optimal language development are relatively undened, especially with respect to approaches with implications for primary care. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: This study identies parenting behaviors and psychosocial risk factors associated with positive versus problematic developmental trajectories, and it suggests critical avenues for primary care interventions regarding parenting behaviors.

KEY WORDS child development, developmental-behavioral screening, parentchild interactions, psychosocial risk, language development ABBREVIATIONS BPCISBrigance Parent-Child Interactions Scale OR odds ratio CI condence interval www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2008-3129 doi:10.1542/peds.2008-3129 Accepted for publication Aug 6, 2009 Address correspondence to Frances Page Glascoe, PhD, Vanderbilt University, Department of Pediatrics, 284 Forrest Rd, Mertztown, PA 19539. E-mail: frances.p.glascoe@vanderbilt.edu PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). Copyright 2010 by the American Academy of Pediatrics FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

abstract
OBJECTIVE: The goal of this study was to assess which parenting behaviors, perceptions, and risk factors were associated with optimal versus delayed development. METHODS: A total of 382 families from the national Brigance Infant and Toddler Screens standardization and validation study participated. Data sources included parent questionnaires, child testing, and examiner observations of parent-child interactions. Parenting styles research was operationalized with the Brigance Parent-Child Interactions Scale, a brief measure of parenting behaviors and perceptions. RESULTS: Six positive parenting behaviors and perceptions predicted average to above-average development on the Brigance screens. Conversely, 2 positive parenting behaviors and negative perceptions of children indicated child performance nearly 2 SDs below the mean on Brigance screens. Psychosocial risk factors associated with fewer positive parenting behaviors and with negative perceptions included 3 children in the home, multiple moves, limited English, and parental depression. CONCLUSIONS: A dearth of positive parenting behaviors plus negative perceptions of children, with or without psychosocial risk factors, negatively affect child development, which is apparent as early as 6 months of age. The older the child is, the greater the performance gaps are. Language development is particularly at risk when parenting is problematic. Findings underscore the importance of early development promotion with parents, focusing on their talking, playing, and reading with children, and the need for interventions regarding psychosocial risk factors. Pediatrics 2010;125:313319

PEDIATRICS Volume 125, Number 2, February 2010

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

313

Promoting early language development for young children is important to pediatric primary care. Childrens early acquisition of language is strongly predictive of overall development, including later success in school and lifelong health.14 Optimally, young children develop crucial language and cognitive skills required for academic literacy well before they enter formal schooling.5 Children who are not ready for school by age 6 are likely to experience academic failure, low selfesteem, behavior problems, depression, social isolation, and health problems.68 People who do not experience optimal early development often report poorer health as adults; indeed, adults who were not read to as children and were not well adjusted were 5 times more likely to report poor health than were those who were.5 Effective parenting practices are important factors in the promotion of optimal early childhood development. Stimulation behaviors used by parents, such as frequent talking and playing, are considered critical for the development of language and cognitive skills.9,10 Results from early childhood support programs for disadvantaged children suggest that investments in early childhood affect outcomes that endure for a lifetime.11 Head Start is a program that has a long history of success. Notably, in 1994, because the majority of 4-year-old children entering Head Start were found to be behind national averages, Early Head Start was initiated, with enrichment and stimulation programs for parents to encourage language and literacy development.11 Why is there a focus on parents interaction skills? Hart and Risely,9 in a landmark study, found 5 parenting behaviors that predicted future achievement in children, namely, (1) using rich vocabulary when speaking with children, (2) using words to respond to
314 GLASCOE and LEEW

childrens behavior, (3) using verbal guidance to encourage development, (4) putting emphasis on language for communicating, and (5) being responsive to a toddlers emerging attempts to communicate. Subsequent research highlighted the importance to language development of communication balance between adults and toddlers and adult responses focusing on the childs interests, initiations, and communication.11,12 The transactional model of development emphasizes the dependence of optimal child development on sensitive, skillful caregiver responses.13 Parents verbal behaviors contribute to childrens cognitive performance up to 3 years of age, even when family resources and individual differences are controlled.14 Optimal language development occurs within interactions that are stimulating and supportive, in which young children experience predictable, developmentally appropriate responses from adults.5 Environmental risk factors such as family size, maternal mental health, and socioeconomic inuences have direct effects on parents interaction styles, which in turn affect childrens cognitive (eg, language) development.5,15,16 Individual differences in child development are therefore sensitive to a complex interplay of socioeconomic, psychosocial, and childinherent risk factors. Why is this important to primary care? Pediatricians have long promoted early book-sharing and reading aloud, as exemplied by the Reach Out and Read program. Reach Out and Read is effective in improving language outcomes among older toddlers and preschool-aged children, particularly those in disadvantaged environments.1621 Perhaps book reading by parents easily provides necessary language stimulation such as rich vocabulary, a focus of interest shared with the child, and age-appropriate commu-

nication. Such brief effective interventions for parents within pediatric primary care merit attention for a number of important reasons. First, pediatric primary care providers have frequent contact with almost all children in the rst years of life. Moreover, most parents seek primary care for their children,22,23 including parents with risk factors for suboptimal child development, such as socioeconomic disadvantage or maternal depression.2426 Furthermore, most parents take seriously the recommendations of primary care providers27 and often change their behaviors in response to recommendations.19 The 2006 Bright Futures/medical home policy statement from the American Academy of Pediatrics28 emphasizes surveillance, that is, identifying and promoting resilience factors and intervening promptly regarding risks, including the problematic parenting behaviors associated with adverse developmental outcomes. The policy statement clearly emphasizes the causal impact of parenting practices during the early years and the effectiveness of brief interventions in primary care.29,30 In this study, we sought to identify parenting behaviors, perceptions, and psychosocial risk factors that are associated with child language development outcomes. We hypothesized that positive parenting requires a range of skills that includes reading aloud but also importantly includes a focus on caregivers frequent use of language to comment on childrens interests at their developmental level, to point out and to label new things, and to respond to emerging skills. Primary care providers could help focus developmental promotion activities by increasing parents knowledge about effective behaviors to use in their interactions with their toddlers. Information about the risks associated with parents lack of positive perceptions

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

ARTICLES

and behaviors also could help physicians make decisions about referrals to parent-training interventions.

METHODS
Subjects and Sites The families enrolled in this study participated in the national standardization and validation study of the Brigance Infant and Toddler Screens.31 Participants were 382 families that reected US population parameters in September 2008.32 Two hundred three children were 2 weeks through 11 months of age, and 179 were 12 through 24 months of age. Forty-eight percent of children were female. Parents had completed, on average, 13.2 grades of school; 22.5% had not completed high school and 31% had college or postgraduate training. Seventy percent were white, 14% black, 12% Hispanic, and 4% other minorities. Children with known disabilities (as well as children thought to perform well above average, although they rarely were identied by 0 2 years) were not excluded because they ensured a nationally representative sample. They were included at levels proportional to their prevalence in the overall population (9%).23 Families were recruited from 22 sites in 17 US states, including pediatric practices, public health departments, day care centers, and early intervention programs. All study protocols were translated into Spanish, for use with families requiring Spanishlanguage materials. The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the institutional review board of Vanderbilt University, and informed consent was obtained from parents accompanying each child. Measurements Child Development Testing The Brigance Infant and Toddler Screens were administered to each
PEDIATRICS Volume 125, Number 2, February 2010

child. The Brigance screens have 90 to 110 items for each year of age. Six subtests (ne motor, gross motor, self-help, social-emotional, receptive language, and expressive language) are grouped into 2 factors, that is, nonverbal (ne motor, gross motor, selfhelp, and social-emotional) and communication (receptive language and expressive language). Cutoff scores (generally 16th percentile), ageequivalent scores, quotients, and percentiles are provided. The Brigance Infant and Toddler Screens were validated against diagnostic measures of development, including the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, the Alberta Infant Motor Scale, the Rosetti Infant Toddler Language Scale, and the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale. Diagnostic measures and subtests with similar content correlate well with Brigance screen subsets (correlation coefcients of 0.70 0.91 and reliability indices of 0.98 0.99). The Brigance screen was 76% to 77% sensitive to a range of developmental problems and 85% to 86% specic in correctly detecting normal development. Chronologic ages of children born 4 weeks early were corrected for prematurity. Parent-Completed, Previously Validated, Questionnaires Parents completed a demographic questionnaire eliciting information about psychosocial risk factors, in-

cluding screenings for depression (Family Psychosocial Screen)33 and anxiety.34 Experimental Measures of Parenting Parents responded to the Brigance Parent-Child Interactions Scale (BPCIS), an experimental 18-item measure of parenting behaviors and perceptions about their children.31 BPCIS items were drawn from extensive relevant literature.9,13,29,35 Table 1 provides examples of and sources for the parenting interaction styles associated with BPCIS statements that inuence language development. BPCIS items are listed in Table 2. Three-point response options (eg, rarely, sometimes, or almost always) were used in construction of the BPCIS because of enhanced veracity when choices are more than dichotomous.36 Professional Examiner Observations An observation version of the BPCIS was used by examiners, who also were asked to report on the frequency and length of contact with families (eg, number of previous ofce visits, months of enrollment, and/or hours of service contact). Procedures Examiners at each site were pediatric nurse practitioners, early childhood educators, pediatricians, or psychologists. All were recruited through vari-

TABLE 1 BPCIS Statements Associated With Parent Interaction Styles and Language Development
BPCIS Item I help my child learn by talking and showing him or her new things. I talk to my child in a special way. My child is fun to be with. I can make my child feel better when he or she is upset. I talk with my child when feeding or eating with him or her. My child is not very much fun to be with. My child does not seem to calm down or seem very interested when I talk to him or her. Engenders Responsivity Efcacy Guidance style Reciprocity Endearments/guidance style Emotional attachment Efcacy Feedback Social responsiveness Reciprocity Efcacy Source Hart and Risely9 Yoder et al12 Dunst et al13 Hart and Risely9 MacDonald35 Dunst et al13 Hart and Risely9 Hart and Risely9 Yoder et al12 Dunst et al13 Dunst et al13

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

315

TABLE 2 BPCIS Items (Parent Report Version)45


Item 1. I play with my child and show him or her things about toys. 2. I hug and kiss my child. 3. I mostly talk to my child when he is crying. 4. I help my child learn by talking and showing him or her new things. 5. I look at or read childrens books to my child.a 6. My child does not calm down or seem very interested when I talk to him. 7. I make up games or songs for my child. 8. When my child looks at or touches a toy, I talk to him about the toy. 9. When my child is looking at me, I talk or make sounds with him. 10. My child doesnt seem to like me. 11. I enjoy feeding my child or eating with him. 12. I talk to my child in a special way. 13. My child is not very much fun to be with. 14. I can make my child feel better when he or she is upset. 15. When my child looks at or touches something, the rst thing I say is no. 16. Most of the time I like my child. 17. My child does not need my help learning new things. 18. I talk with my child when feeding or eating with him or her. Not very often Not very often Not very true Not very true Not very often Mostly true Not very often Not very often Not very often Mostly true Not very often Not very often Mostly true Not very often Mostly true Not very true Mostly true Not very often Response Options Sometimes Sometimes Sometimes true Sometimes true Sometimes Sometimes true Sometimes Sometimes Sometimes Sometimes true Sometimes Sometimes Sometimes true Sometimes Sometimes true Sometimes true Sometimes true Sometimes Often Often Mostly true Mostly true Often Not very true Often Most of the time Often Not very true Often Often Not very true Often Not very true Mostly true Not very true Most of the time

dance of 8 months (range: 120 months). Among families involved through child-nd and assessment services (with many also receiving home-based services), contact length was recorded for 91.9%, with a median of 19.0 hours (range: 1998 hours). The amount of contact explains the relatively high concordance between examiners and parents. Seventy-one percent of parents and examiners had identical ratings about frequency of parental book-sharing.

RESULTS
Prevalence of Developmental Delay Of the 382 participating children, 16.8% performed below the ageappropriate cutoff score on the communication factor of the Brigance screens. This value is consistent with recent studies on the prevalence of signicant developmental delay in young children.39 Prediction of Developmental Status From Parenting Behaviors and Perceptions Cutoff scores for the communications factor of the Brigance screens served as the dependent variable in a logistic regression analysis. Predictors were parenting behaviors (with the exclusion of reading aloud, which was analyzed separately) and perceptions, drawn from the BPCIS. The following 4 positive parenting behaviors predicted optimal developmental outcomes signicantly: I help my child learn by talking and showing him or her new things (odds ratio [OR]: 2.0 [95% condence interval [CI]: 1.38 2.78]), I talk to my child in a special way (OR: 4.2 [95% CI: 3.48 4.88]), I can make my child feel better when he or she is upset (OR: 1.7 [95% CI: 1.50 1.82]), and I talk with my child when feeding or eating with him or her (OR: 1.9 [95% CI: 1.39 2.72]). The absence of the following 2 negative perceptions

The publisher of the scale gives permission for these items to be adapted or reproduced. a Combined with examiner report to create an indicator of book-sharing.

ous professional societies. Participating professionals recruited families of young children who were enrolled in their programs or practices. Examiners administered the Brigance Infant and Toddler Screens directly to children and completed the BPCIS observation version. Parents completed the parent-report version of the BPCIS and reported measures of psychosocial risk. Analyses Data were analyzed by using SPSS (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL). Variables were tested for outliers, heteroscedasticity, and multicolinearity. Stepwise logistic regression analyses were performed with forward inclusion, to identify plausible predictors and to build a model in the absence of fully established theory.37 Questions to parents about reading aloud to their children might be af316 GLASCOE and LEEW

fected by social desirability,38 and this phenomenon was minimized as follows. If parents and examiners agreed on reports of frequent reading, then the family was designated as book readers (46%); if either parents or examiners indicated that parents were reading not very often or sometimes with their children, then the family was designated as limited book readers (54%). Justication for combining parent and examiner observations was based on the frequency of contact between examiners and parents. Among families participating within medical settings, the number of previous ofce visits was recorded for 79.7%, for a median of 5 visits (range: 119 visits). Among families participating through day care and early intervention programs, the duration of attendance was recorded for 64.1%, with a median atten-

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

ARTICLES

creased with age, as shown in Fig 2, and the magnitude of the discrepancy between groups increased steadily with the age of the child. Prediction of Developmental Status From Parenting Behaviors, Perceptions, and Psychosocial Risk Factors
FIGURE 1
Communication factor scores according to parenting practices and perceptions. Squares indicate 2 positive parenting behaviors/perceptions, that is, I talk to my child in a special way, I help my child learn by showing him or her new things, I can make my child feel better when he or she is upset, and I talk with my child when feeding or eating with him or her, and no negative perceptions. Diamonds indicate 2 positive parenting behaviors/perceptions and 1 negative perception, that is, My child is not very much fun to be with and My child does not seem to calm down or seem very interested when I talk to him or her.

also contributed: My child is not very much fun to be with (OR: 4.5 [95% CI: 3.455.61]) and My child does not seem to calm down or seem very interested when I talk to him or her (OR: 1.8 [95% CI: 1.28 2.36]). As shown in Fig 1, parents who endorsed 2 of the positive behaviors or held 1 negative perception had children with 3 times the risk of delay, compared with children whose parents reported 2 positive behaviors and no negative perceptions of their children (OR: 3.0 [95% CI: 2.24 3.94]). The impact of parenting behaviors and perceptions on childrens developmental status increased with childrens ages. No meaningful differences in communication scores were seen among children 0 to 5 months of age, but signicant differences were found in all older age groups. The older the children were, the greater the differences in developmental outcomes. Prediction of Developmental Status From Frequency of Book-Sharing To examine relationships between book-sharing and language skills for infants and toddlers, children were grouped according to cutoff scores
PEDIATRICS Volume 125, Number 2, February 2010

on the communication factor of the Brigance screens. Of the families who seemed to be nonreaders or limited book readers, 21% had children at risk for developmental problems, compared with 12% of the families who were book readers. When data were analyzed through logistic regression, families for whom book reading was uncommon were almost twice as likely to have a child with delays (OR: 1.9 [95% CI: 1.36 2.60]). By 6 months of age, children whose parents read aloud frequently outperformed children whose parents read rarely; this pattern continued through 24 months of age. Differences in communication skills in-

Logistic regression analysis was conducted with cutoff values on the communication factor of the Brigance screens as the dependent variable and with psychosocial risk factors and all 18 parenting behaviors and perceptions as predictors. Six variables were predictive of results below cutoff scores on the Brigance screens, that is, 3 siblings in the home (OR: 2.73 [95% CI: 1.873.99]), 2 household moves in the past year (OR: 1.9 [95% CI: 1.372.72]), elevated scores on the depression screen (OR: 3.5 [95% CI: 2.255.42]), limited English facility (OR: 25.0 [95% CI: 11.3255.42]), and negative (ie, not very often/rarely) endorsements on the parenting behavior items of I talk to my child in a special way (OR: 2.8 [95% CI: 1.61 4.96]) and I help my child learn by showing him or her new things (OR: 1.9 [95% CI: 1.272.96]). Nonsignicant psychosocial variables were employment, ethnicity, parent education, anxiety, and all remaining BPCIS behaviors and perceptions.

FIGURE 2
Communication factor scores on the Brigance screens according to frequency of book-sharing.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

317

DISCUSSION
Although book-sharing remained an expected contributor (ie, there is an abundance of literature demonstrating its positive inuence on oral language development and school success), this study revealed other parenting behaviors and perceptions that contributed to language developmental status. Specically, parents who reported activities such as, I help my child learn by talking and showing him or her new things, I talk to my child in a special way, I can make my child feel better when he or she is upset, and I talk with my child when feeding or eating with him or her and/or perceived their children as fun to be with and interested when talked to had children who, by 6 months of age, were more likely to have language skills in the average range, compared with parents with fewer positive behaviors and perceptions. Notably, the important language achievement gaps widened with increasing age. The psychosocial risk factors that seemed to affect positive parenting practices and positive perceptions included 3 children in the family, 2 household moves in the past year, elevated scores on depression screening, and limited English facility. It is hardly surprising that parents with multiple burdens and fewer resources are less likely to enjoy interacting with their REFERENCES
1. Foorman BR, Anthony J, Seals L, Mouzaki A. Language development and emergent literacy in preschool. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2002;9(3):173184 2. Lonigan CJ, Burgess SR, Anthony JL. Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. 2000;36(5):596 613 3. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Dev. 1998;69(3):848 872

children, to remain interested in talking to them, or to engage in positive parenting practices. Therefore, interventions for psychosocial risk factors are also crucial for optimal child development. Future research in parenting behaviors that affect child development is crucial. Because this study was crosssectional, it was not possible to establish a cumulative impact on child development of parenting behaviors, perceptions, and psychosocial risk, although previous research indicated a causal relationship between parents beliefs and actions and child outcomes.4044 Therefore, we recommend cross-validation through a longitudinal study. Another critical focus for future studies is to operationalize parenting behaviors/perceptions into developmental promotion interventions that are brief and workable in busy primary care settings, following the example of effectiveness research established by the Reach Out and Read program.38 In the interim, clinicians should advise parents routinely on the value of talking frequently with their children, modeling and expanding childrens utterances, actively teaching new words, sitting down to meals and talking together, and describing to children what they are seeing and doing. A second clinical implication of this study is the need to monitor the effectiveness of such advice. Especially challenged

families may not respond well to brief advice and may require moreintensive assistance. Such families are likely to have psychosocial risk factors, and a third implication of this study reinforces the value of American Academy of Pediatrics policy28 on identifying and intervening with issues such as stress, depression, housing needs, and English language training. Complex family problems often are beyond the scope of primary care services and are less than amenable to the brief counseling that is effective with less-challenged families. Psychosocial interventions usually require social work support and/or early interventions for delayed or troubled toddlers. Therefore, a fourth clinical implication is the recommendation to develop treatment collaborations among primary care, social services, and/or community early intervention providers. In delivering and monitoring the effectiveness of brief advice on parenting skills and psychosocial risk factors, clinicians clearly can have a greater positive inuence on the developmental and life trajectories of children and families in their care.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We are grateful to the families and early childhood professionals who contributed to this project and to Drs Pamela High, Melissa Wake, and Tracy King for their comments on early drafts of this manuscript.

4. Sameroff AJ, Seifer R, Barocas R, Zax M, Greenspan S. Intelligence quotient scores of 4-year-old children: social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics. 1987;79(3):343350 5. Hertzman C. The Case for an Early Childhood Development Strategy. Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia, Centre for Health Services and Policy Research; 2000. Report 2000:13r 6. Barnett WS. Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school outcomes. Future Child. 1995;5(3):2550

7. Hertzman C. Making Early Childhood Development a Priority: Lessons From Vancouver. Vancouver, Canada: Canadian Centre for Policy Alternatives; 2004 8. Yoshikawa H. Long-term effects of early childhood programs on social outcomes and delinquency. Future Child. 1995;5(3): 5175 9. Hart B, Riesly TR. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1995 10. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Shannon JD, Cabrera

318

GLASCOE and LEEW

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

ARTICLES

11.

12.

13.

14.

15. 16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

NJ, Lamb ME. Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004;75(6):1806 1820 Fenichel E, Mann TL. Early Head Start for low-income families with infants and toddlers. Future Child. 2001;11(1):134 141 Yoder PJ, Warren SF, McCathren R, Leew S. Does social responsivity facilitate communication development in very young children with developmental disabilities? In: Wetherby AM, Warren SF, Reichle J. Transitions in Prelinguistic Communication. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1998:39 58 Dunst C, Lowe L, Bartholomew P. Contingent social responsiveness, family ecology, and infant communicative competence. NSSLHA J. 1990;17:39 49 Lugo-Gil J, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Family resources and parenting quality: links to childrens cognitive development across the rst 3 years. Child Dev. 2008;79(4): 10651085 Belsky J. The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev. 1984;55(1):8396 Needlman R, Toker KH, Dreyer BP, Klass P, Mendelsohn AL. Effectiveness of a primary care intervention to support reading aloud: a multicenter evaluation. Ambul Pediatr. 2005;5(4):209 215 Mendelsohn AL, Mogilner LN, Dreyer BP, et al. The impact of a clinic-based literacy intervention on language development in inner-city preschool children. Pediatrics. 2001;107(1):130 134 Needlman R, Fried LE, Morley DS, Taylor S, Zuckerman B. Clinic-based intervention to promote literacy: a pilot study. Am J Dis Child. 1991;145(8):881 884 Nelson CS, Wissow LS, Cheng TL. Effectiveness of anticipatory guidance: recent developments. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2003;15(6): 630 635 Sharif I, Rieber S, Ozuah PO. Exposure to Reach Out and Read and vocabulary outcomes in inner city preschoolers. J Natl Med Assoc. 2002;94(3):171177 Theriot JA, Franco SM, Sisson BA, Metcalf SC, Kennedy MA, Bada HS. The impact of early literacy guidance on language skills of 3-year-olds. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2003;42(2): 165172

22. Klass P. Pediatrics by the book: pediatricians and literacy promotion. Pediatrics. 2002;110(5):989 995 23. Newacheck PW, Strickland B, Shonkoff JP, et al. An epidemiologic prole of children with special health care needs. Pediatrics. 1998; 102(1):117123 24. La Paro KM, Justice L, Skibbe LE, Pianta RC. Relations among maternal, child, and demographic factors and the persistence of preschool language impairment. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2004;13(4):291303 25. Parker S, Greer S, Zuckerman B. Double jeopardy: the impact of poverty on early child development. Pediatr Clin North Am. 1988;35(6):12271240 26. Petterson SM, Albers AB. Effects of poverty and maternal depression on early child development. Child Dev. 2001;72(6):1794 1813 27. Young KT, Davis K, Schoen C, Parker S. Listening to parents: a national survey of parents with young children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1998;152(3):255262 28. American Academy of Pediatrics, Council on Children With Disabilities, Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, Bright Futures Steering Committee, Medical Home Initiatives for Children With Special Needs Project Advisory Committee. Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics. 2006;118(1):405 420 29. Shonkoff JP. From neurons to neighbors: old new challenges for developmental and behavioral pediatrics. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003;24(1):70 76 30. Blair M, Hall D. From health surveillance to health promotion: the changing focus in preventive childrens services. Arch Dis Child. 2006;91(9):730 735 31. Glascoe FP. The Brigance Infant and Toddler Screen: standardization and validation. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23(3):145150 32. US Census Bureau. Race and Ethnicity, 2006 2008. Available at: http:// factnder.census.gov/. Accessed July 1, 2009 33. Kemper KJ, Kelleher KJ. Family psychosocial screening: instruments and techniques. Ambul Child Health. 1996;1:325339 34. Anxiety Disorders Association of America.

Anxiety disorders in children: a test for parents. Available at: www.adaa.org/ GettingHelp/SelfHelpTests/selftest parents.asp. Accessed July 1, 2001 35. Macdonald J. Becoming Partners With Children: From Play to Conversation: A Developmental Guide for Professionals and Parents. Chicago, IL: Riverside; 1989 36. Babbie E. The Practice of Social Research. 10th ed. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth; 2004 37. Menard SW. Applied Logistic Regression Analysis. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2002 38. Weitzman CC, Roy L, Walls T, Tomlin R. More evidence for Reach Out and Read: a homebased study. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5): 1248 1253 39. Rosenberg SA, Zhang D, Robinson CC. Prevalence of developmental delays and participation in early intervention services for young children. Pediatrics. 2008;121(6). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/ content/full/121/6/e1503 40. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, MillerLoncar CL. Early maternal and child inuences on childrens later independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Dev. 2000;71(2):358 375 41. Landry SH, Miller-Loncar CL, Smith KE, Swank PR. The role of early parenting in childrens development of executive processes. Dev Neuropsychol. 2002;21(1):15 41 42. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR. Environmental effects on language development in normal and high-risk child populations. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2002;9(3):192200 43. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR. The importance of parenting during early childhood for school-age development. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;24(23):559 591 44. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR. Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Dev Psychol. 2006;42(4):627 642 45. Glascoe FP, Brigance A. Brigance Infant and Toddler Screen: Parent-Child Interactions Form . North Billerica, MA: Curriculum Associates; 2002

PEDIATRICS Volume 125, Number 2, February 2010

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

319

Parenting Behaviors, Perceptions, and Psychosocial Risk: Impacts on Young Children's Development Frances Page Glascoe and Shirley Leew Pediatrics 2010;125;313; originally published online January 25, 2010; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2008-3129
Updated Information & Services References including high resolution figures, can be found at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/125/2/313.full.ht ml This article cites 34 articles, 11 of which can be accessed free at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/125/2/313.full.ht ml#ref-list-1 Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/misc/about.xhtml#permis sions Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/misc/addir.xhtml#reprint sus

Permissions & Licensing

Reprints

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2010 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 23, 2011

You might also like