Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Stereotype and Perception Change in Intercultural Negotiation

Zhaleh Semnani-Azad Katia Sycara Michael Lewis Wendi L. Adair


University of Waterloo,
Department of
Psychology
Carnegie Mellon University,
Robotics Institute
University of Pittsburgh,
School of Information
Science
University of Waterloo,
Department of
Psychology
zsemnani@uwaterloo.ca katia@cs.cmu.edu ml@sis.pitt.edu wladair@uwaterloo.ca


Abstract
Stereotypes are cognitive schemas that influence
our perception, beliefs and behavior toward members
of a social group [12]. While culture is a salient social
group characteristic and an important contextual cue
for schema activation [27], there is limited research on
cultural stereotypes and perception change in
international negotiations. Thus, we examined
perception formation and perception change across
stages of negotiation. North American observers
viewed a negotiation (videos) between North American
and Middle Eastern business men, with different stages
and one of three negotiation outcomes: (a) negotiators
did not reach agreement, or (b) reached an agreement
by compromising, or (c) by employing an expanding
the pie problem solving approach. After viewing the
videos, participants rated negotiators on positive and
negative attributes as a measure of perception. We
found in-group bias across all observers, change in
perceptions across different stages, and variation of
initial stereotypes as a function of negotiation
outcome.

1. Introduction
The increasing global and regional economic
integration through trade, foreign investments, capital
flows, migration, and spread of technology, has vastly
increased interactions between agents, managers and
employees across cultures [15, 4]. From multinational
mergers and acquisitions, to the buying and selling of
goods and services or the management of a multi-
cultural workforce, cross-cultural negotiation has
become a fundamental component of business today.
In cross-cultural negotiation though, cultural
differences often give rise to complex and challenging
processes, making it difficult for parties to reach an
agreement. Prior research has demonstrated that
intercultural negotiations tend to be significantly less
successful than intra-cultural negotiations [2, 16]. The
poor negotiating outcomes of international negotiations
have been attributed to cultural differences in
communication styles and cognitive schemas [2, 6].
One form of cognitive schema that interferes with
successful intercultural negotiation is a stereotype.
Stereotypes are cognitive structures that encompass
knowledge, beliefs, and expectations about a particular
social group [17]. Stereotypes affect perception,
restrict individuation, direct behavior, and elicit
positive or negative emotions toward a social group
[13]. For example, research illustrates that negotiators
from U.S. and Japan have distinct expectations and
strategic repertoires when negotiating domestically or
with someone from the other culture, and their
intercultural schemas are based on the stereotypes they
hold of negotiators from the other culture [1]. A
fundamental component of stereotype is social
categorization, a natural process that occurs
spontaneously in our everyday perception [35, 37].
We tend to categorize people whom we know little
about and we come to understand who we are by the
groups in which we belong to [34]. We are more likely
to categorize people using perceptually salient cues
such as sex, race, age, and physical attractiveness [8].
Hence, culture is a salient category, and automatic
social categorization is often based on prominent,
culturally relevant feature such as race [9, 11].
Yet, there is limited research on the influence of
cultural stereotypes in cross-cultural negotiation.
Given that culture is a very prominent social category,
and that stereotypes are part of our everyday life and
influence our judgments and behavior toward others
[40], cultural stereotypes that negotiators hold can
greatly influence their decisions and behavior, and
ultimately negotiation process and outcomes.
Stereotypes can inform us of a persons perception and
a persons perception can inform us of the stereotypes
a person holds [35]. Prior studies have examined
gender stereotypes in negotiation and illustrate that
stereotypes can influence negotiation process and
outcomes [20]. Consequently, cultural stereotypes
may also significantly influence intercultural
negotiations. Furthermore, stereotype beliefs are not
rigid and they change across social contexts [29].
2012 45th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences
978-0-7695-4525-7/12 $26.00 2012 IEEE
DOI 10.1109/HICSS.2012.547
899
Thus in our study we investigate the role of stereotypes
in perception formation and perception change across
stages of an intercultural negotiation between Middle
Eastern and American businessmen. We examined
Middle Eastern sample since research on Middle
Eastern negotiation is minimal. Due to globalization,
Middle Eastern and North American markets have
come to rely on each other more heavily for various
resources [10]. Moreover, given that countries of these
regions have suffered from recent conflicts, there may
be higher employment of negative bias and stereotypes
in the negotiation context.
To examine negotiator perception and stereotypes,
observer ratings were measured as opposed to self-
report ratings of negotiators since observer ratings have
been shown to have higher validity and be better
predictors of behavioral outcomes [28, 30].
Additionally, negotiation is a complex process where
parties interdependently make decisions about how to
distribute resources and/or resolve conflicts [38]. It
involves a combination of different stages, such as
deciding on how to distribute and divide resources,
activities pertaining to relationships and climate
between parties, use of various influence tactics, and
power balance between parties [25]. Accordingly,
negotiation is cognitively taxing, and requesting
negotiators to constantly rate their opponent on various
attributes would be problematic. So, in order to capture
a more accurate understanding of stereotype bias in
intercultural negotiation, we gathered perception
ratings from neutral observers. We captured
observer perception through their attribute (trait)
ratings, where North American observers rated North
American and Middle Eastern negotiators on a series
of positive and negative attributes. Therefore, in this
study participants act as observers, watch intercultural
negotiation between the businessmen, and rate North
American and Middle Eastern businessmen on various
traits. We examine changes in observer ratings over
various stages of the negotiation process, and when
faced with different negotiation outcomes.
2. Role of Stereotypes in Cross-cultural
Negotiation
Culture can be defined as the unique nature of a
social group with regard to values, norms, practices,
and institutions [24]. Culture is a visible social
categorization and is an important contextual cue for
schema activation [14, 27]. It influences peoples
cognitive schemas and the behaviors they engage in
[19]. Aside from influencing intercultural negotiators
assumptions about their counterparts character and
negotiation tactics [1], we propose that stereotypes also
elicit a negativity bias towards negotiators from a
dissimilar culture and an in-group preference for
negotiators from a similar culture.
Although stereotypes can elicit positive or
negative emotions toward a social group, negative
attitudes towards an out-group are most common [13].
According to prior research, negative attitudes or
stereotypes toward an out-group occur because of
illusionary correlation [18]. People tend to pay more
attention to things (people or events) that are novel and
unique, or deviant. Since interactions with out-group
members are generally rare and that undesirable
behaviors occur infrequently, people tend to
overestimate the association between out-group
membership and negative behaviors. This results in an
illusionary correlation between the out-group and the
undesirable behaviors [18], increasing dislike and
negative perception toward a member of the other
group. Thus, due to in-group bias and illusory
correlation, intercultural negotiators are more likely to
hold a negative perception of each other than intra-
cultural negotiators, and this negative stereotype
should consist of attributes including self-interested,
competitive, and not trustworthy. Moreover, a stronger
negativity bias toward the out-group (negotiator of a
different culture) may be evident when negotiators
engage in a more distributive and competitive
approach, in which information sharing, realization of
trade-offs, and value-creation opportunities are
diminished. We capture the negativity bias of
intercultural negotiators via observer ratings, since
these ratings are thought to have higher validity than
self-report ratings of negotiators [28, 30]. More
specifically, we predict that a North American observer
will hold negative perception of the out-group, Middle
Eastern negotiator, and this negative bias will be
evident in higher ratings of the Middle Eastern
negotiator on negative attributes, and lower rating on
the positive attributes.

Hypothesis 1a: North American observers will
illustrate negativity bias toward the out-group by
rating the Middle Eastern negotiator lower on
positive traits, and higher on negative traits
compared to the negotiator from the in-group
(North American negotiator).

A common product of stereotypes and social
categorization is ethnocentrism, placing ones social
group (or cultural group) at the center of the universe
[22]. In this case, people are inclined to suppose that
their own groups values, beliefs, behaviors, and
organizing principles are superior to other groups
[33]. In fact, prior research shows that people tend to
judge members of their in-group more favorably such
as considering them to be smarter, more attractive,
900
more cooperative, fairer, more trustworthy, and more
hard working than members of out-groups [7, 36]. So,
intracultural negotiators (parties are of the same
culture) are more likely to trust, share information, and
hold positive perception of their counterpart, in
comparison to intercultural negotiators (parties are of
different cultures). Consequently, we predict that a
North American observer will illustrate an in-group
preference by ratings the North American negotiator
higher on positive attributes, and lower on negative
attributes.

Hypothesis 1b: North American observers will
illustrate in-group preference by rating the
negotiator from their own culture higher on
positive traits compared to the negotiator from the
out-group (Middle Eastern negotiator).

Stereotype beliefs however, are not rigid and they
are subject to change across different situations and
with the increase of inter-group (inter-cultural)
interactions [29]. Thus, culturally held stereotypes can
change with positive intergroup contact (lowering
negativity bias); however a negative intergroup contact
can escalate the negativity bias toward the out-group
[32, 35]. Transactional negotiation, pertaining to
trading or exchanging ideas and positions, is a mixed
motive, complex and dynamic interplay of cooperation
and competition [3, 21]. From the cooperative
perspective, negotiators tend to take the problem
solving approach where they share information, realize
common goals, and work interdependently to reach a
mutual agreement. In this case, negotiators can
expand the pie through realization of trade-offs, and
value-creation opportunities. From the competitive
perspective, negotiators instead perceive the
negotiation as a distributive "win-lose" bargaining
process, view each other as distinct entities (with no
common ground) and tend to focus on individual gain
[39]. Given that negotiation is a dynamic social
interaction with multiple phases, it is likely that
negotiators perceptions toward each other may change
from one stage to another depending on level of
cooperativeness (corresponding to positive intergroup
contact) and competitiveness (corresponding to
negative intergroup contact) employed by their
counterpart.
Adair & Brett (2005) report four negotiation
phases observed in international negotiations. The first
phase is characterized by affective posturing, where
negotiators exhibit affective persuasion and influence
tactics based on status, relationships, and normative or
other contextual factors, to establish their position
(posturing). The second phase is characterized by
reciprocal priority information in which negotiators
engage in a detailed discussion of the issues and
exchange relevant information. The third phase
consists of both priority information and rational
persuasion. At this point, negotiators have a general
understanding of their counterparts preferences,
however, negotiators are not necessarily cooperative
since they engage in value claiming (competitive) and
value creating (cooperative) behaviors [31].
Reciprocal sequence of offers occurs in the fourth
phase, where parties reduce alternatives and move to a
final decision [26]. Our research uses a stage approach
to examine whether in-group bias and negative
stereotypes change during the course of a negotiation.
We included the first and second phase of the
negotiation, and a third phase that incorporated the
final outcome. We expect that negativity bias will
increase in the first stage (which is more competitive)
and decrease in the second stage (which is more
cooperative) [3].

Hypothesis 2a: North American observers will
illustrate high levels of negativity bias toward the
Middle Eastern negotiator in the first stage of the
negotiation, and low negativity bias in the second
stage.

Furthermore, we examine the extent to which
positive or negative intercultural interactions (through
final negotiation outcomes) influence perceptions of
observer participants. We manipulate negotiation
outcomes into three different possible scenarios: 1)
Blow-up, in which negotiators employ a distributive
stance, are competitive, and do not reach an agreement.
2) Compromise, where negotiators are still distributive
but are more cooperative and end the negotiation by
meeting each others demands halfway. 3) Pie-
expansion, in which negotiators are cooperative and
employ a problem solving approach by focusing on
common interests. We expect the negotiators negative
perceptions toward the out-group will be exaggerated
in the Blow-up condition, which is highly competitive
and distributive. We predict lower negativity bias in
the other conditions, since they are more cooperative;
however negotiators positive perceptions toward the
in-group will be exaggerated, especially the Pie-
expansion condition, which employs a cooperative,
problem solving approach.

Hypothesis 2b: The negativity bias toward the
Middle Eastern negotiator will be exaggerated in
the blow-up outcome, compared to the other
outcomes.

Hypothesis 2c: The positivity bias toward the
North American negotiators will be higher in the
901
more cooperative conditions, especially in the pie-
expansion condition.
3. Method
Participants and Procedure. One hundred and
twenty North American students (N: Blow-up = 36;
Compromise= 41; Pie-expansion= 43) from University
of Waterloo and Carnegie Mellon University
participated in the current study in exchange for $15
(CAD or USD). All North American participants were
Canadian or American-Caucasians, and primarily
associated with the North American culture (Canadian
or American). 56% of participants were men, and the
average age of participants was 22.9. The study took
place online and all participants watched video-clips of
the negotiation and rated the negotiators online.
Upon reading and granting consent to
participation, participants responded to demographic
questions about their nationality, culture, and religion.
Then participants viewed a photograph of two
negotiators and watched three videos of an intercultural
negotiation between the negotiators from the
photograph (see Fig.1). After viewing the photograph
and each of the video clips, participants responded to
the perception questionnaire where they rated the
negotiators on various attributes. Hence, participants
acted as observers and we examined how they
perceived the negotiators before negotiation and during
the course of the negotiation interaction.


Fig. 1. Static photograph of the negotiators, Nabil (left) and
Paul (right).

Negotiation Simulation. Before viewing the
photograph of the negotiators, participants were
informed that the Middle Eastern merchant, Nabil,
owns a textile mill and produces rugs for both domestic
and foreign markets. The Western merchant, Paul,
buys rugs from different parts of the world to sell to
department stores. During Nabil and Pauls first
transaction Paul claims that the initial product shipped
was of inferior quality. After viewing the photograph
and responding to the first set of questions, participants
were presented with three video clips of the
negotiation. The photograph and the first two video
clips were standard across all conditions. In the first
video, the conflict is introduced where the North
American merchant claims that the initial rugs shipped
were of inferior quality, contrary to his initial
agreement with the Middle Eastern merchant. The
Middle Eastern merchant on the other hand, claims that
high quality rugs were shipped. In the second video,
both negotiators share more information about the
situation, and explain their position. We had three
versions/conditions for the third (final) video, each
capturing a different negotiation outcome. For the
blow-up condition, in the final video, the conflict
escalates and negotiators engage in distributive and
competitive tactics via threats and arguments. In the
compromise condition, the negotiators are more
cooperative and reach an agreement by involving a
third party to propose a fair market price on the rugs.
In the pie-expansion condition, the merchants are more
cooperative and engage in the problem solving
approach, by adding issues to the table. In this video,
the Middle Eastern merchant agrees to ship free
samples of Egyptian towels, to help cover losses the
North American merchant incurred from the rugs.
Perception Questionnaire. Participants
responded to the perception questionnaire on a 7-point
scale (1= Strongly Disagree; 7= Strongly Agree) four
times; one after the photograph, and one after each
video clip. We carried out an exploratory factor
analysis to group the items into different categories,
and included items with a factor loading of .7 or
higher. Based on the analysis, we categorized the items
pertaining to the photograph into five traits: Respect,
Cooperative, Trust, Competitive, and Dishonest. A
sixth trait of Fairness emerged from the questionnaire
pertaining to the video clips (see Appendix A). The
summary of intercorrelations of the trait components
for both Paul and Nabil are included below.
Table 1. Summary of intercorrelations of traits Paul
Attributes 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Respect __ .596** .619** .755** -.44** -0.172
2. Cooperative .596** __ .639** .700** -.55** -.188*
3. Fair .619** .639** __ .667** -.34** -0.068
4. Trust .755** .700** .667** __ -.47** -0.083
5. Competitive -.435** -.55** -.34** -.47** __ .268**
6. Dishonest -0.172 -.188* -0.068 -0.083 .268** __
Attributes 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Respect
__ .359** .657** .809** -.52** -.215*
2. Cooperative
.359** __ .242** .293** -0.14 -0.174
3. Fair
.657** .242** __ .678** -.43** -0.146
4. Trust
.809** .293** .678** __ -.53** -.195*
5. Competitive
-.52** -0.14 -.43** -.53** __ .184*
6. Dishonest
-.215* -0.174 -0.146 -.195* .184* __
902
Table 2. Summary of intercorrelations of traits Nabil

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level
* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level
4. Results
Before examining participants perception ratings,
we averaged observer scores in each trait category that
emerged from the factor analysis. Then, we carried out
a series of repeated measures ANOVA to examine
overall perception and perception change within stages
of the negotiation, and between the different
negotiation outcomes. We conducted two sets of
analyses: 1) examining changes from initial
perception/stereotype (static picture) and first video
clip, 2) examining perception change across all three
videos.
Hypothesis 1. We postulated that we would
observe negativity bias toward out-group such that
North American observers would rate the Middle
Eastern negotiator lower on positive traits (Respect,
Cooperative, Trust, Fair), and higher on negative traits
(Competitive, Dishonest). We also predicted a
positivity bias toward in-group where North American
observers would rate the North American merchant
higher on positive traits and lower on negative traits.
Overall, there was no significant difference between
the three conditions (F (2, 118) = 1.43, p> .05). The
results illustrate a significant difference between the
initial ratings for the static picture and the first video
(F (1, 118) = 3.69, p = .057), where overall negotiators
had higher ratings in the static picture (M =4.22,
SE=0.04) than the first video clip (M =4.15, SE=0.04).
A marginally significant interaction emerge for the
Time x Negotiator interaction (F (1, 118) = 3, p =
.086), where the North American negotiator received
higher ratings than the Middle Eastern merchant
especially in the static picture. When examining ratings
on the different traits for each negotiator, we observed
a significant difference (F (4, 472) = 14.79, p < .01).
Contrary to our predictions, North American observers
perceived the North American negotiator as more
competitive (M =4.65, SE=0.08) and less cooperative
(M =4.02, SE=0.07) than his Middle Eastern
counterpart (M =4.36, SE=0.09), (M =4.57, SE=0.09).
Hence, the initial perception toward the out-group was
more positive.
When examining perception change across the
three videos (different stages of negotiation), we
observed significant differences in ratings between the
three conditions (blow-up, compromise, pie-expansion)
(F (2, 118) = 3.7, p = .028), across the three stages of
the negotiation (F (2, 236) = 26.52, p < .01), and
ratings between the two negotiators (F (1, 118) =
46.14, p < .01). Overall, the North American negotiator
received higher ratings on the positive traits such as
Respect (M =4.52, SE=0.08), Cooperative (M =4.32,
SE=0.08), Fairness (M =4.55, SE=0.07), and Trust (M
=4.32, SE=0.09), while receiving lower ratings on the
negative traits: Competitive (M =4.24, SE=0.09), and
Dishonest (M =3.62, SE=0.09). Thus, we observed
positivity bias toward the in-group. We also observed
negativity bias toward the out-group, where the Middle
Eastern merchant received lower ratings on the positive
attributes; Respect (M =3.87, SE=0.08), Cooperative
(M =3.9, SE=0.07), Fairness (M =3.83, SE=0.08), and
Trust (M =3.4, SE=0.08), and received higher ratings
on the negative traits: Competitive (M =4.6, SE=0.09),
and Dishonest (M =3.93, SE=0.09). Thus, we find
partial support of hypothesis 1.
Hypothesis 2. We postulated that North American
observers will illustrate high levels of negativity bias
toward the Middle Eastern negotiator in the first stage
of the negotiation (more competitive), and low
negativity bias in the second stage (more cooperative).
Across the first two stages of the negotiation (video 1
and video 2 ratings), we did not find significant
differences across the three conditions (F (2, 118) =
1.01, p> .05), however we found significant differences
in the ratings of the two stages of the negotiation (F (1,
118) = 24, p < .01), and ratings given to the two
negotiators (F (1, 118) = 37.4, p < .01). Significant
interactions of Time x Trait x Negotiator (F (5, 590) =
36.83, p < .01) and Time x Trait x Negotiator x
Condition (F (10, 590) = 3.46, p < .01) also emerged.
Although in the initial stage of the negotiation, the
North American merchant was viewed as more
Respectable (M =4.3, SE=0.16), Fair (M =4.13,
SE=0.18), Trustworthy (M =4.1, SE=0.18), and less
Dishonest (M =3.8, SE=0.19), he was also perceived as
less Cooperative (M =3.93, SE=0.17), and more
Competitive (M =4.53, SE=0.17). In contrast, the
Middle Eastern opponent was perceived as more
Cooperative (M =4.57, SE=0.15), and less Competitive
(M =4.3, SE=0.18). Yet, the Middle Eastern merchant
was judged to be less Respectable (M =4.19, SE=0.17),
Fair (M =3.95, SE=0.19), and Trustworthy (M =3.86,
SE=0.16), and more Dishonest (M =3.97, SE=0.19).
Hence, overall we observed negativity bias toward the
Middle Eastern merchant in the first phase of the
negotiation, where the problem and conflict is
introduced.
Interestingly and contrary to our predictions, this
negativity bias escalated in the second phase, where the
problem was further discussed and the negotiators
engaged in information sharing. In this stage,
observers rated the Middle Eastern negotiator lower on
Respect (M =3.37, SE=0.18), Cooperativeness (M
903
=3.05, SE=0.2), Fairness (M =3.26, SE=0.19),
Trustworthiness (M =2.95, SE=0.17); they rated the
Middle Eastern negotiator higher on Competitiveness
(M =4.97, SE=0.17), and Dishonesty (M =4.18,
SE=0.19). Positivity bias toward the in-group also
increased, as per observers higher ratings of the North
American merchant on Respect (M =4.61, SE=0.16),
Cooperativeness (M =4.27, SE=0.18), Fairness (M
=4.55, SE=0.16), Trustworthiness (M =4.42, SE=0.2),
and lower ratings on Competitiveness (M =4.164,
SE=0.19), and Dishonesty (M =3.63, SE=0.18). Thus,
we found an opposite pattern to our predictions for
H2a.
We also predicted that the negativity bias toward
the Middle Eastern negotiator will be exaggerated in
the blow-up condition, while the positivity bias toward
the North American negotiators will be exaggerated in
the pie-expansion condition. Although we did not find
differences between the conditions for the first, and
second stages of the negotiation, we did find
significant differences in attribution ratings for the
third stage (F (2, 118) = 11.16, p < .01), where the
negotiation outcomes (third video) varied across the
conditions. We observed a significant Trait x
Negotiator x Condition interaction (F (10, 590) = 5.44,
p < .01). We averaged ratings on the positive traits
(respect, cooperative, fair, trust) and on the negative
traits (competitive, dishonest) to examine differences
in negativity bias across the conditions (see Fig. 2).
The findings illustrate negativity bias toward the
Middle Eastern merchant where his ratings on the
positive traits were lower in all conditions
(Compromise: M =4.26, SE=0.19) (Pie-expansion: M
=4.57, SE=0.19), especially the blow-up condition (M
=3.12, SE=0.2). The ratings on positive traits for the
North American merchant in contrast, were higher
(Blow-up: M =3.8, SE=0.2) (Pie-expansion: M =4.9,
SE=0.18), especially in the compromise condition (M
=5.4, SE=0.19). Also, the Middle Eastern merchants
ratings on negative traits for the compromise (M =4,
SE=0.22), and pie-expansion (M =3.8, SE=0.21)
conditions were higher than the North American
negotiator (Compromise: M =3.2, SE=0.22) (Pie-
expansion: M =3.53, SE=0.22). Interestingly, the
ratings on negative attributes for the Middle Eastern
negotiator (M =4.31, SE=0.22) did not vary from the
North American negotiator (M =4.4, SE=0.23) for the
blow-up condition.


Fig. 2. Ratings of negotiators on positive and negative traits
across all conditions.

Overall, compared to the North American
merchant, the Middle Eastern negotiator received a
higher rating on competitiveness, and lower ratings on
positive attributes (respectable, cooperativeness,
fairness, trustworthiness), especially for the blow-up
condition (see Table 1). Interestingly, the Middle
Eastern negotiator received lower rating on dishonestly
compared to the North American negotiator, especially
in the blow-up condition. On the other hand, the North
American negotiator received higher ratings on the
positive attributes compared to the Middle Eastern
negotiator, and was viewed as less competitive
compared to the Middle Eastern merchant. Moreover,
this positivity bias toward the in-group was
exaggerated in the compromise condition.
Table 2. Observer ratings on 6 traits across all conditions.
North American

Traits Blow-up Compromise
Pie-expansion
M (S.E.) M (S.E.) M (S.E.)
Respect 3.7 (.19) 5.3 (.18) 4.8 (.18)
Cooperative 3.7 (.18) 5.6 (.18) 4.9 (.18)
Fair 3.9 (.18) 5.8 (.17) 5.2 (.17)
Trust 3.7 (.21) 4.9 (.21) 4.6 (.2)
Competitive 4.7 (.24) 3.5 (.23) 3.9 (.23)
Dishonest 4 (.21) 3 (.21) 3.2 (.2)
904

Middle Eastern

Traits Blow-up

Compromise

Pie-
expansion
M (S.E.) M (S.E.) M (S.E.)
Respect 3.4 (.21) 4.3 (.2) 4.4 (.19)
Cooperative 2.8 (.18) 4.6 (.17) 4.9 (.17)
Fair 3.3 (.19) 4.6 (.18) 4.9 (.18)
Trust 2.9 (.21) 3.6 (.2) 3.9 (.2)
Competitive 5.3 (.23) 4.2 (.22) 3.9 (.21)
Dishonest 3.4 (.22) 3.9 (.21) 3.7 (.21)
5. Discussion
We investigated how initial perceptions and
stereotypes toward a negotiating partner of another
culture are formed, how these perceptions change
across the different stages of the negotiation, and how
perception change occurs when faced with different
negotiation outcomes. North American observer
ratings on positive and negative traits of a North
American and Middle Eastern business man were
gathered, after viewing three phases of an intercultural
negotiation. Also, three possible negotiation outcomes
were presented to the observers, where negotiators
were competitive and did not reach an agreement,
negotiators were cooperative and reached a mutual
agreement, negotiators were cooperative and employed
a problem solving approach, and expanded the pie of
resources to be distributed.
Overall as predicted, our results indicate in-group
bias (i.e. positive in-group stereotype) across all
observers, where they rated the negotiator of their own
culture higher on positive attributes than the negotiator
of the other culture. Negativity bias (i.e. negative out-
group stereotype) was also found, where observers
rated the negotiator from the other culture higher on
competitiveness. Participants initial ratings were used
to capture their initial stereotypes. Interestingly,
observers initial perceptions were positive toward the
negotiators of the other culture, where the Middle
Eastern merchant was viewed as more cooperative and
less competitive compared to the North American
negotiator. This may be due to higher exposure to
diversity and multiculturalism in the North American
society. Domestic workforces are becoming more
diverse, ethnically and culturally, particularly in
countries such as Canada and United States [23]. This
increase in diversity and opportunity of interacting
with members of various cultures may have lowered
negativity bias toward out-groups [32].
The initial positive perception toward the Middle
Eastern negotiator however, quickly changed where as
soon as the negotiation began (initial stage), the
observers showed negativity bias. Contrary to what
we expected, this negativity bias escalated in the
second stage of the negotiation, where more
information sharing took place. According to past
research [3]; although the second stage is still
competitive, the mere act of information exchange
creates a more cooperative setting. Thus, we should
expect a more positive perception of the counterpart to
be generated in this phase. Yet, the findings illustrated
the opposite pattern. One reason for this maybe
because these are observer ratings and perceptions as
opposed to the perception of the actual negotiator
involved in the interaction. Although observer ratings
are claimed to be better predictors of behavioral
outcomes [29], observer perceptions (positive or
negative) may be exacerbated or diminished,
depending on the context. For example, prior research
on procedural justice illustrate that under some
conditions the treatment of others (third-party,
observer) relate to justice judgments as is ones own
treatment [5]. Hence in some situations, observer
judgment may vary from ones own judgment and
perception.
The findings also showed perceptions change, for
the better or for the worse, when presented with
different negotiation outcomes. Although negativity
bias toward the negotiator of another culture still
persists, this bias declines when both negotiators are
more cooperative and reach an agreement through
compromise. On the other hand, when negotiators are
competitive and employ distributive tactics, negativity
bias toward the out-group escalates. For future
research, we plan to examine rating of the two
negotiators by Middle Eastern observers, as well as
examine and compare observer ratings with the ratings
of the actual actor or agent involved in an intercultural
negotiation. We also plan to investigate to what extent
observer perceptions are predictive of negotiator
perception and judgment.
Our study contributes to the cross-cultural
negotiation literature by defining stereotypes that
North Americans have of negotiators from the other
culture. In addition, we show that exposure to different
negotiation strategies and outcomes can shift
stereotypes in both a positive or negative direction.

Acknowledgment
This research was funded by a MURI award through
ARO grant W911NF-08-1-0301.






905
10. References


[1] W.L. Adair, M.S. Taylor and C.H. Tinsley, Starting
Out on the Right Foot: Negotiation Schemas When
Cultures Collide, Negotiation and Conflict
Management Research, 2(2), 2009, pp.138-163.
[2] W.L. Adair, T. Okumura, and J.M. Brett, Negotiation
behaviour when cultures collide: The United States and
Japan, Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 2001, pp.
371-385.
[3] W.L. Adair, and J.M. Brett, The negotiation dance:
Time, culture, and behavioral sequences in negotiation.
Organization Science, 16(1), 2005, pp. 3351.
[4] J. Bhagwati, In defense of globalization, Oxford, New
York: Oxford University Press, 2004.
[5] K. Bos, and E.A. Lind, The psychology of own versus
others treatment: Self-oriented and other-oriented
effects on perceptions of procedural justice, Society of
Personality and Social Psychology, 27 (10), 2001, pp.
13241333.
[6] J.M. Brett, and T. Okumura, Inter- and intracultural
negotiation: U.S. and Japanese negotiators, Academy
of Management Journal, 41, 1998, pp. 495510.
[7] M.B. Brewer, In-group bias in the minimal group
situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis
Psychological Bulletin, 1979, pp. 307-324.
[8] M.B. Brewer, A dual process model of impression
formation, In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.),
Advances in social cognition (vol. 1, pp. 136).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1988.
[9] M.B. Brewer, and A. Feinstein, Dual processes in the
cognitive representation of persons and social
categories, In S. Chaiken, & Y. Trope, Dual Processes
in Social Psychology (pp. 255-270). New York:
Guilford Press, 1999.
[10] A. Dastmalchian, M. Javidan, & K. Alam, Effective
leadership and culture in iran: An empirical study.
Applied Psychology: An International Review. Special
Issue: Leadership and Culure in the Middle East:
Norms, Practices, and Effective Leadership Attributes in
Iran, Kuwait, Turkey, and Qatar, 50(4), 2001, pp. 532-
558.
[11] S. T. Fiske, Stereotyping, Prejudice and
Discrimination, In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, and G.
Lindzey, The Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 357-
411). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1998.
[12] S. T. Fiske, and S.E., Taylor, Social cognition. New
York: McGraw-Hill, Inc, 1991.
[13] S. T. Fiske, A. Cuddy, P. Glick, and J. Xu, A Model
of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and
Warmth Respectively Follow From Status and Power,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2002, pp.
878-902.
[14] M.J., Gelfand, and A. Realo, Individualism-
collectivism and accountability in intergroup
negotiations, Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 1999,
pp.721736.
[15] D. Goldblatt, D. Held, A. McGrew, and J. Perraton
Economic globalization and the nation state: shifting
balances of power, Soundings issue, 7, 1997, pp. 61-
77.
[16] R., Gulbro, and P. Herbig, Negotiating successfully
in cross-cultural situations, Industrial Marketing
Management, 25, 1996, pp. 235-241.
[17] D. Hamilton and J. Sherman, Stereotypes, In J.
Wyer, & T. Srull, Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 1-
68). Hilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994.
[18] D.L. Hamilton and R.K. Gifford, Illusory correlation
in inter- personal perception: A cognitive bias of
stereotypic judgments, Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 12, 1976, pp. 392-407.
[19] G. Hofstede, Cultures consequences: International
differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage, 1980.
[20] L.J. Kray, L. Thompson, and A. Galinsky, Battle of
the Sexes: Gender Stereotype Confirmation and
Reactance in Negotiations, Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 2001, pp. 942-958.
[21] D.A. Lax, and J.K. Sebenius, The manager as
negotiator: Bargaining for cooperation and competitive
gain. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster Inc, 1986.
[22] R.A. Le Vine, and D.T. Campbell, Ethnocentrism:
theories of conflict, ethnic attitudes, and group behavior.
New York, USA: Wiley, 1972.
[23] M.R. Limaye, and D. Victor, Cross-Cultural Business
Communication Research: State of the Art and
Hypotheses for the 1990s, Journal of Business
Communication, 28(3), 1991, pp. 277-299.
[24] A.L. Lytle, J.M. Brett, Z.I. Barness, C.H. Tinsley, and
M. Janssens, A paradigm for confirmatory cross-
cultural research in organizational behavior, in L.L
Cummings and B.M. Staw, editor, Research in
Organizational Behavior. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press,
1995.
[25] W. Mastenbroek, Negotiating: A conceptual model,
Group and Organization Studies, 1980, pp. 324-339.
[26] I. E. Morley, J. M. Stephensen, The Social
Psychology of Bargaining, Allen & Unwin, London,
U.K.
[27] M.W. Morris, and M.J. Gelfand, Cultural differences
and cognitive dynamics: Expanding the cognitive
perspective on negotiation. In M. J., Gelfand & J.M.,
Brett (Ed.), Handbook of Negotiation and Culture.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 2004.
[28] M.K. Mount, M.R. Barrick, and J.P. Strauss, Validity
of observer ratings of the big five personality factors,
Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(2), 1994, pp. 272-
280.
[29] P.J. Oakes, S.A. Haslam, and J.C. Turner,
Stereotyping and social reality. Oxford, UK: Blackwell,
1994.
[30] I.S. Oh, G. Wang, and M.K. Mount, Validity of
observer ratings of the five-factor model of personality
traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology,
2010.
[31] M. Olekalns, P.L., Smith, & T. Walsh, The process of
negotiating: Strategy and timing as predictors of
outcomes Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 68(1), 1996, pp. 6877.

906
[32] T.F. Pettigrew, and L.R. Tropp, A meta-analytic test of
intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 90, 2006, pp. 751-783.
[33] M.H. Segal, P.R. Dasen, J.W. Berry, and Y.H.
Poortinga, Human Behavior in Global Perspective. New
York: Pergamon, 1990.
[34] K. Smith, and D. Berg, Paradoxes of group life:
Understanding conflict, paralysis and movement in
group life. New York: Jossey-Bass, 1987.
[35] C. Stangor, The study of stereotyping, prejudice, and
discrimination within social psychology: A quick
history of theory and research. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.),
(pp. 1-22). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press, 2009.
[36] H. Tajfel, Social identity and intergroup relations.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1982.
[37] S.E. Taylor, S.T. Fiske, N.L. Etcoff, and A.J.
Ruderman, Categorical and contextual bases of person
memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 36(7), 1978, pp. 778-793.
[38] L. Thompson, and R. Hastie, Social perception in
negotiation, Organizational Behaviour and Human
Decision Process 47, 1990, pp. 98-123.
[39] RE. Walton, R.B. McKersie, A Behavioral Theory of
Labor Negotiations: An Analysis of a Social Interaction
System. McGraw-Hill, New York, 1965.
[40] S.C. Wheeler, and R.E. Petty, The effects of
stereotype activation on behavior: A review of possible
mechanism, Psychological Bulletin, 127(6), 2001, pp.
797-826.
907
Appendix A:
1. Perception Questionnaire: Trait Categories

Respect (2 items)
Paul/Nabil is honourable
I respect Paul/Nabil

Cooperative (3 items)
Paul's/Nabils goal is to meet the other merchant half
way on issues
Paul/Nabil is interested to work out a solution that
serves both of merchants' interests as much as
Paul/Nabil would do what he can to help the other
merchant

Fair (2 items)
Paul/Nabil is giving the other merchant's perspective
fair consideration.-Paul
Paul/Nabil is a fair negotiator

Trust (2 items)
Paul/Nabil is trustworthy.
I would consider doing business with Paul/Nabil.

Competitive (2 items)
Paul/Nabil is trying to maximize his own gain
regardless of the outcome for the other merchant
Paul/Nabil would do what he can to increase his own
outcome, especially at the expense of the other...

Dishonest (2 items)
I believe Nabil would ship a low quality product if he
knew that Paul would not find out; OR I believe Paul
would falsely complain about the quality of the
product if he knew that it would result
Paul/Nabil cheats

908

You might also like