Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Perceived mate fidelity and paternal resemblance predict

mens investment in children


Coren L. Apicella
*
, Frank W. Marlowe
Peabody Museum, Department of Anthropology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
Initial receipt 1 August 2003; final revision received 14 June 2004
Abstract
Here, we investigate whether variation in male parental investment can be explained in terms of (1)
mens perception of the degree of resemblance between themselves and their offspring and (2) mens
perception of their mates fidelity. In a sample of men from Londons Heathrow airport, both variables
were found to predict reported investment. We also examined whether the predictors of investment
varied when men were no longer in a relationship with the mother of their children and are therefore no
longer investing in mating effort with them. Among men no longer in a relationship with the mother of
their children, resemblance became a stronger predictor of investment, while fidelity was no longer a
significant predictor. Overall, men provided less investment to their children if they were no longer in a
relationship with the mother of their children.
D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Paternity confidence; Paternal investment; Cues to paternity
1. Introduction
Optimal allocation of reproductive effort to either parenting or mating is contingent upon
variables that affect the fitness returns for either type of effort (Hurtado & Hill, 1992;
Maynard Smith, 1977). The genetic relatedness between parent and offspring most obviously
1090-5138/04/$ see front matter D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.06.003
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 617 495 1870; fax: +1 617 496 8041.
E-mail address: apicella@fas.harvard.edu (C.L. Apicella).
Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378
influences the fitness returns for parenting. Indeed, a number of studies have reported that
men care for and invest less in their stepchildren than for their genetic children (Anderson,
Kaplan, Lam, & Lancaster, 1999; Anderson, Kaplan, & Lancaster, 1999; Flinn, 1988;
Marlowe, 1999). This parental discrimination is also reflected in crime statistics against
children. Children living in stepfamilies are much more likely to be both physically abused
and killed, as compared with children living with their biological families (Daly & Wilson,
1988a, 1988b). These are circumstances in which it is obvious that the child is not genetically
related to the male.
Because men are never fully certain of their paternity, they may need to rely on cues to assess
it. Wilson and Daly (1992) proposed that a males estimation of the degree of phenotypic
resemblance between himself and his offspring may be one possible cue to his paternity.
Studies have found that Canadian, Mexican, and U.S. mothers that have just given birth allege
paternal resemblance much more than maternal resemblance (Daly & Wilson, 1982; Regalski
& Gaulin, 1993). Even more, the bias in paternal resemblance is exaggerated while mothers
are in the presence of the father (McLain, Setters, Moulton, & Pratt, 2000), suggesting an
evolved strategy on the part of females to increase mens paternity confidence and, presumably,
thereby increasing paternal investment (Daly & Wilson, 1982; McLain et al., 2000).
There is some evidence that resemblance may be related to investment. When asked to
make hypothetical investment decisions, some studies found that males react more favorably
towards children whose faces are morphed to resemble their own faces (Platek, Burch,
Panyavin, Wasserman, & Gallup, 2002; Platek et al., 2003). This study will examine whether
mens perceived resemblance to their offspring predicts their reported parental investment.
Another possible cue to paternity confidence may be a males perception of his mates
fidelity. Significant sex differences in the valuation of chastity have been found across
cultures, with males valuing it more than females do (Buss, 1989) and desiring characteristics,
such as faithfulness and sexual loyalty, as extremely important in a long-term mate (Buss &
Schmitt, 1993; but see Buckmaster, Nell, & Jankowiak, 2002, on female mate guarding). This
suggests that men look for cues that predict future sexual conduct when choosing a long-term
mate and prefer characteristics such as faithfulness and chastity to ensure future paternity
(Buss & Schmitt, 1993). Accordingly, a males estimation of his mates fidelity may also be a
cue to his paternity. This study also examines whether mens perception of the fidelity of their
childrens mother predicts their reported parental investment.
Some parenting effort may actually be a form of mating effort (Schaik & Paul, 1996; Smuts
& Gubernick, 1992). This study therefore also examines the effects of paternal resemblance
and mate fidelity on reported parental investment in men who are in a relationship with the
mother of their children and those who are no longer in a relationship with the mother of their
children because it is unlikely that men engage in mating effort with former mates.
2. Methods
The participants in this study were 170 adult males recruited by one of the authors
(Apicella). About 75% of the men were recruited from Londons Heathrow Airport and a
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 372
major train/subway station in London. The researcher attempted to approach all men who
were alone and appeared old enough to be a father. Men who were first noticed by the
researcher were approached first. Men who appeared overly busy or on a cell phone were not
approached. However, because the researcher generally stayed in the same areas while
recruiting, individuals who appeared less busy at a later time were subsequently approached.
Only men who reported that they had biological children were asked to complete the
anonymous survey. Both travellers and workers at the airport/train station were recruited for
participation. The rest of the men were recruited from the general public around
Northampton, England. Four individuals refused to complete the questionnaire.
Fathers were asked to respond to a number of statements using five-point Likert scales
ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Points for each item were summed
together to obtain a single score for each independent and dependent measure. The amount
of perceived resemblance between a father and his offspring was measured by mens
responses to the following statements: bI believe that my child looks more like me than its
mother,Q bI think my child shares similar personality traits with me,Q and bMost people think
my child looks like me.Q A coefficient of reliability of .79 (Cronbachs a) was obtained for
these three measures of resemblance. Mens perception of the fidelity of the mother of their
children was measured by their responses to the following statements: bThe mother of my
child is/was trustworthy,Q bI believe the mother of my child is/was faithful to me,Q and bThe
mother of my child dresses in revealing clothing.Q The last statement was reverse scored. A
Cronbachs a of .61 was obtained for these three measures. Finally, the following statements
were designed to measure mens investment: bI believe I give my child a lot of attention,Q bI
spend a lot of time with my child,Q and bI am/was involved with my childs schoolwork.Q A
Cronbachs a of .83 was obtained for these three measures of investment.
Multiple regression analyses were employed to examine the combined effects of perceived
mate fidelity and perceived resemblance on mens reported investment. In all regressions, we
controlled for the childs age and sex and the fathers relationship status (whether he is still
with the childs mother), ethnicity, total number of children, and number of hours worked per
week (Table 1). We do not have coresidence data but assume that it is highly correlated with
the age of the child and whether men are in a relationship with the mother of their children.
Income was not included in the regression analyses because only 77.6% of men reported their
annual income and we wanted to use our whole sample in the analyses. However, we ran
separate analyses and found that income did not influence investment nor did it affect the
significance of our predictors.
2.1. Participants
The participants ranged in age from 21 to 62 years old (M=42.33, S.D. =8.15). According
to self-identification, 93% of the participants were White, 3% were Black, and 4% were
Asian. The majority (67.1%) of participants reported residing in the UK, but 17 different
countries outside the United Kingdom were also reported. Median annual income for those
men who reported it was o47,838. The average annual income in London at this time was
o30,888 (Office of National Statistics, 2003). Thus, the airport sample appears to be
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 373
somewhat biased toward white-collar workers. However, the 22.4% of individuals who did
not report their income may have lower incomes.
Most (77.6%) of the participants reported that they were currently in a relationship with the
mother of their children, while 22.4% of the participants reported they were no longer in a
relationship with the mother of their children. The mean age of men in a relationship was
42.17, while the mean age of separated men was 42.89. All men surveyed reported that they
had been in a relationship with the mother of their children for at least 2 years. Only the oldest
child was used in the analyses because it eliminated any possible practice effects (men were
asked to begin with their oldest child when answering investment questions) and any other
erroneous effects resulting from fathers not wanting to report that they differentially invest in
their children. Childrens ages ranged from 3 weeks of age to 26 years old (M=12.32,
S.D. =7.31), of which 51.5% were male and 48.5% were female.
3. Results
Overall, men who are in a relationship with the mother of their children report significantly
higher levels of investment compared with men who are no longer in a relationship with the
Table 1
Summary statistics of all variables used in the analyses, by relationship status
Entire sample
In a relationship
(77.6%)
Not in a relationship
(22.4%)
Age of child* M = 12.32 M = 11.53 M =15.05
S.D. = 7.31 S.D. = 7.24 S.D. = 6.96
n = 170 n =132 n =38
Sex of child Male (51.5%) Male (49.6%) Male (57.9%)
Female (48.5%) Female (50.4%) Female (42.1%)
Ethnicity: White (W),
Black (B), and Asian (A)
W (92.9%) B (2.9%)
A (4.5%)
W (92.4%) B (3%)
A (4.5%)
W (94.7%) B (2.6%)
A (2.6%)
Number of children M = 2.07 M = 2.10 M =1.97
S.D. = 1.07 S.D. = 1.10 S.D. = 0.97
n = 170 n =132 n =38
Hours worked per week Median = 47.58 Median = 48.07 Median = 45.94
Resemblance* M = 9.98 M = 10.27 M =8.97
S.D. = 2.64 S.D. = 2.59 S.D. =2.58
n = 167 n =129 n =38
Fidelity** M = 12.43 M = 13.10 M =10.11
S.D. = 2.17 S.D. = 1.44 S.D. = 2.64
n = 170 n =132 n =38
Investment** M = 10.09 M = 10.56 M =8.67
S.D. = 2.75 S.D. = 2.56 S.D. = 2.81
n = 144 n =108 n =36
* Significant difference between separated men and men in a relationship, p b.05.
** Significant difference between separated men and men in a relationship, p b.005.
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 374
mother of their children (Table 1; z =3.56; p b.0005, equal variances not assumed).
Furthermore, men no longer in a relationship with the mother of their children reported lower
fidelity in the mother as compared with men still in a relationship with the mother of their
children (z =6.41; p b.0005, equal variances not assumed).
For the sample as a whole, the various independent variables together explained nearly
25% of the variance in paternal investment (Table 2, Column 1). Men reported greater
investment in children if they perceived them as having greater resemblance to themselves
(b =.285, p b.0005). Men also reported greater investment in children when they perceived
their childrens mother as having greater fidelity (b =.278, p =.004).
For men still in a relationship with the mother of their children, we were able to explain
just over 19% of the variance in paternal investment (Table 2, Column 2). Greater paternal
investment was reported if greater resemblance (b =.201, p =.034) and greater fidelity were
reported (b =.322, p =.001). For men who reported being separated or divorced from the
mother of their children, the independent variables explained just over 40% of the variance in
paternal investment (Table 2, Column 3). For this group, greater resemblance predicted
paternal investment (b =.709, p b.0005), while greater mate fidelity did not (b =.158,
p =.266).
4. Discussion
This study examined whether perceived paternal resemblance and mate fidelity, two
possible cues to paternity, predict parental investment in men. We have found that as mens
paternal resemblance and mate fidelity increases, so does their reported parental investment.
However, among men who were no longer in a relationship with the mother of their children
Table 2
Multiple regression model with investment as the independent variable
Variable
Entire sample In a relationship Not in a relationship
b p b p b p
Age of child .076 .315 .097 .292 .002 .989
Sex of child (h=1 ; U = 2) .206 .007 .223 .017 .096 .545
Relationship status
(separated =1; in a
relationship =2)
.061 .513
Black .055 .468 .005 .960 .143 .353
Asian .108 .157 .106 .255 .232 .105
Number of children .015 .846 .029 .744 .166 .288
Number of hours worked .080 .292 .181 .051 .394 .018
Resemblance .285 .000 .201 .034 .709 .000
Fidelity .278 .004 .322 .001 .158 .266
Adjusted R
2
.248 .191 .405
F 6.135 4.07 3.98
p .000 .000 .003
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 375
and thus less likely to be investing in mating effort with them, we found that paternal
resemblance became a stronger predictor of investment, while mate fidelity no longer
predicted investment. When we examined the differences in perception of mate fidelity
between men in a relationship with the mother of their children, compared with those who are
separated from her, we found that it was significantly lower for the latter. This finding
suggests that a males perception of the mothers fidelity could be redefined during or after
the breakup of a relationship. It is also possible that lower mate fidelity could have caused the
breakup. Either way, it seems that when fidelity is in doubt, men should rely more heavily on
other cues, such as resemblance, to estimate their paternity for each child independently.
Men in a relationship with the mother of their children reported higher levels of parental
investment as compared with men who were no longer in a relationship. This is consistent
with other findings that men invest more in their genetic children from their current mates as
opposed to their genetic children from their former mates (Amato, 1987; Anderson, Kaplan,
& Lancaster, 1999; Weiss & Willis, 1985, 1993).
This study used mens current perceptions of their mates fidelity as a possible cue to
paternity. It is important to note that this may not be as useful a measure for paternity as mens
perception of their mates fidelity at the time the child was conceived. Despite this, we do
believe that this is still a useful measure. Not only are mens perceptions of their mates
fidelity influenced by their mates past behaviors, but men may also be able to reassess their
paternity by using cues long after the conception of the child. For instance, a man may believe
that his mate was faithful at the time of conception, but her later infidelities could cause the
male to reconsider his earlier assessment.
Perceived paternal resemblance predicted parental investment in men, but from an
evolutionary perspective, we must ask how men in the past could gauge their resemblance to
their offspring. Modern men are continuously exposed to images of themselves through
mirrors, pictures, and even videos and, therefore, are likely to have a well-developed visual
template of bself-imageQ for estimating paternity. It is quite possible that ancestral males had a
similar template of self-image, albeit not as detailed and extensive as modern males do.
Although a mans own face was largely inaccessible to ancestral males, except for occasional
glimpses of reflections in water, the rest of the body was accessible. Nonvisual cues of self,
such as personality, behavioral traits, and smell, were also available. In addition, a male could
also use his mothers and siblings faces as templates because mothers share 50% of their
genes with their offspring, and siblings share at least 25% of their genes, on average, with one
another. Using a brother as a visual template for assessing paternity would be useful so long
as the brother is not actually the father of the child in question.
This study provides preliminary evidence that men may be responding to changes in
paternity confidence by varying their levels of parental investment. Our findings are
consistent with the findings reported by Fox and Bruce (2001) that paternity confidence,
measured by how often fathers wonder if their children are really their genetic offspring and
whether they looked for certain things about their children to verify this, predicts the degree
to which a child is loved and wanted by his father in a sample of men from Tennessee. Other
factors in addition to paternity confidence, such as father effect on offspring quality and
survivability (Blurton Jones, Marlowe, Hawkes, & OConnell, 2000; Maynard Smith, 1977)
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 376
and mens chances of gaining additional mates, also influence mens investment and,
therefore, should be examined in relation to paternity confidence. Finally, further progress is
needed in the development of more suitable measures of paternity confidence.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank John Lycett, Robin Dunbar, Janet Brailey, Paul
Vandeman, and Steven Gaulin for helpful comments and editorial suggestions. This paper is
based on a Master of Science thesis carried out at the University of Liverpool. The data were
previously reported at the annual meeting of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society:
Lincoln, NE, 2003.
References
Amato, P. R. (1987). Family processes in one-parent, step parent, and intact families: the childs point of view.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 327337.
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., Lam, D., & Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers: II.
Reports by Xhosa high school students. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 433451.
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers: II. Reports
from Albuquerque men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 405431.
Blurton Jones, N. G., Marlowe, F. W., Hawkes, K., & OConnell, J. F. (2000). Paternal investment and hunter
gatherer divorce rates. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons (Eds.), Adaptation and human behavior: an
anthropological perspective 2000 (pp. 6990). New York7 Aldine de Gruyter.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 149.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating.
Psychological Review, 100, 204232.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1982). Whom are newborn babies said to resemble? Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 6978.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988a). Homicide. New York7 Aldine de Gruyter.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988b). Evolutionary psychology and family homicide. Science, 242, 519524.
Flinn, M. V. (1988). Step- and genetic parent/offspring relationships in a Caribbean village. Ethology and
Sociobiology, 9, 335369.
Fox, G. L., & Bruce, C. (2001). Conditional fatherhood: identity theory and parental investment theory as
alternative sources of explanation of fathering. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 394403.
Hurtado, A. M., & Hill, K. (1992). Paternal effect on offspring survivorship among ache and Hiwi hunter
gatherers: implications for modeling pair-bond stability. In B. Hewlett (Ed.), Fatherchild relations: cultural
and biosocial contexts (pp. 3155). Chicago7 Aldine.
Jankowiak, W., Nell, D., & Buckmaster, A. (2002). Extra-marital affairs: a reconsideration of the meaning an
universality of the bdouble standardQ. Journal of Comparative and Cross-Cultural Research, 13, 221.
Marlowe, F. (1999). Male care and mating effort among Hazda foragers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,
46, 5764.
Maynard Smith, J. (1977). Parental investment: a prospective analysis. Animal Behaviour, 25, 19.
McLain, D. K., Setters, D., Moulton, M. P., & Pratt, A. E. (2000). Ascription of resemblance of newborns by
parents and nonrelatives. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 1123.
Office of National Statistics. (2003). UK 2003: the official yearbook of the United Kingdom of Great Britain and
Northern Ireland. Norwich, UK7 Crown Publishing Group.
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 377
Platek, S. M., Burch, R. L., Panyavin, I. S., Wasserman, B. H., & Gallup Jr., G. G. (2002). Reactions to childrens
faces: resemblance affects males more than females. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 159166.
Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Burch, R. L., Frederick, D. A., Myers, T. E., & Gallup Jr., G. G. (2003). How much
resemblance is enough? Sex difference in reactions to resemblance, but not the ability to detect resemblance.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 8187.
Regalski, J. M., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (1993). Whom are Mexican infants said to resemble? Monitoring and fostering
paternal confidence in the Yucatan. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 97113.
Smuts, B., & Gubernick, D. (1992). Maleinfant relationships in non-human primates: paternal investment or
mating effort? In B. Hewlett (Ed.), Fatherchild relations: cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 130).
Chicago7 Aldine.
van Schaik, C. P., & Paul, A. (1996). Male care in primates: does it ever reflect paternity? Evolutionary Anthro-
pology, 5, 152156.
Weiss, Y., & Willis, R. (1985). Children as collective goods and divorce settlements. Journal of Labor Economics,
3, 268292.
Weiss, Y., & Willis, R. (1993). Divorce settlements: evidence and interpretation. Journal of Labor Economics, 11,
607629.
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1992). The man who mistook his wife for a chattel. In J. Tooby, & L. Cosmides (Eds.),
The adapted mind (pp. 289322). Oxford7 Oxford University Press.
C.L. Apicella, F.W. Marlowe / Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2004) 371378 378

You might also like