Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Olfaction

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Olfaction, also known as olfactics,[1] is the sense of smell. This


sense is mediated by specialized sensory cells of the nasal cavity of
vertebrates, which can be considered analogous to sensory cells of
the antennae of invertebrates. In humans, olfaction occurs when
odorant molecules bind to specific sites on the olfactory receptors.
These receptors are used to detect the presence of smell. They come
together at the glomerulus, a structure which transmits signals to the
olfactory bulb (a brain structure directly above the nasal cavity and

Human olfactory system. 1: Olfactory


bulb 2: Mitral cells 3: Bone 4: Nasal
epithelium 5: Glomerulus 6: Olfactory
receptor neurons

below the frontal lobe).[2] Many vertebrates, including most


mammals and reptiles, have two distinct olfactory systemsthe
main olfactory system, and the accessory olfactory system (used
mainly to detect pheromones). For air-breathing animals, the main
olfactory system detects volatile chemicals, and the accessory olfactory system detects fluid-phase

chemicals.[3] Olfaction, along with taste, is a form of chemoreception. The chemicals themselves that
activate the olfactory system, in general at very low concentrations, are called odorants. Although taste and
smell are separate sensory systems in land animals, water-dwelling organisms often have one chemical
sense.[4]
Volatile small molecule odorants, non-volatile proteins, and non-volatile hydrocarbons may all produce
olfactory sensations. Some animal species are able to smell carbon dioxide in minute concentrations.[5]

Contents
1 Study of olfaction
2 Main olfactory system
2.1 Receptor neuron
2.2 Olfactory bulb projections
3 Accessory olfactory system
4 Human olfactory system
5 Olfactory coding and perception
6 Genetics of olfaction
7 Interactions of olfaction with other senses
7.1 Olfaction and taste
7.2 Olfaction and audition
8 Disorders of olfaction
9 Quantifying olfaction in industry
10 Olfaction in plants and animals
10.1 Insect olfactory system
11 See also
12 References
13 Further reading
14 External links

Study of olfaction
Early scientific study of olfaction includes the extensive doctoral
dissertation of Eleanor Gamble, published in 1898, which compared
olfactory to other stimulus modalities, and implied that smell had a
lower intensity discrimination.[6] As the Epicurean and atomistic
Roman philosopher Lucretius (1st Century BCE) speculated,
different odors are attributed to different shapes and sizes of "atoms"
(odor molecules in the modern understanding) that stimulate the
olfactory organ [1]

The Lady and the Unicorn a Flemish


tapestry depicting the five senses

(http://www.academia.edu/321004/Lucretius_the_Biochemistry_of_Olfaction_and_Scientific_Discovery). A
modern demonstration of that theory was the cloning of olfactory receptor proteins by Linda B. Buck and
Richard Axel (who were awarded the Nobel Prize in 2004), and subsequent pairing of odor molecules to
specific receptor proteins. Each odor receptor molecule recognizes only a particular molecular feature or
class of odor molecules. Mammals have about a thousand genes that code for odor reception.[7] Of the genes
that code for odor receptors, only a portion are functional. Humans have far fewer active odor receptor genes
than other primates and other mammals.[8] In mammals, each olfactory receptor neuron expresses only one
functional odor receptor.[9] Odor receptor nerve cells function like a key-lock system: If the airborne
molecules of a certain chemical can fit into the lock, the nerve cell will respond. There are, at present, a
number of competing theories regarding the mechanism of odor coding and perception. According to the
shape theory, each receptor detects a feature of the odor molecule. Weak-shape theory, known as odotope
theory, suggests that different receptors detect only small pieces of molecules, and these minimal inputs are
combined to form a larger olfactory perception (similar to the way visual perception is built up of smaller,
information-poor sensations, combined and refined to create a detailed overall perception). An alternative
theory, the vibration theory proposed by Luca Turin,[10][11] posits that odor receptors detect the frequencies
of vibrations of odor molecules in the infrared range by quantum tunnelling. However, the behavioral
predictions of this theory have been called into question.[12] There is no theory yet that explains olfactory
perception completely.

Main olfactory system


In vertebrates smells are sensed by olfactory sensory neurons in the olfactory epithelium. The olfactory
epithelium is made up of at least six morphologically and biochemically different cell types.[13] The
proportion of olfactory epithelium compared to respiratory epithelium (not innervated) gives an indication of
the animal's olfactory sensitivity. Humans have about 10 cm2 (1.6 sq in) of olfactory epithelium, whereas
some dogs have 170 cm2 (26 sq in). A dog's olfactory epithelium is also considerably more densely
innervated, with a hundred times more receptors per square centimeter.[14]

Molecules of odorants passing through the superior nasal concha of the nasal passages dissolve in the mucus
lining the superior portion of the cavity and are detected by olfactory receptors on the dendrites of the
olfactory sensory neurons. This may occur by diffusion or by the binding of the odorant to odorant binding
proteins. The mucus overlying the epithelium contains mucopolysaccharides, salts, enzymes, and antibodies
(these are highly important, as the olfactory neurons provide a direct passage for infection to pass to the
brain). This mucus acts as a solvent for odor molecules, flows constantly and is replaced approximately
every 10 minutes.
In insects smells are sensed by olfactory sensory neurons in the chemosensory sensilla, which are present in
insect antenna, palps and tarsa, but also on other parts of the insect body. Odorants penetrate into the cuticle
pores of chemosensory sensilla and get in contact with insect odorant binding proteins (OBPs) or
Chemosensory proteins (CSPs), before activating the sensory neurons.

Receptor neuron
The binding of the ligand (odor molecule or odorant) to the receptor leads to an action potential in the
receptor neuron, via a second messenger pathway, depending on the organism. In mammals, the odorants
stimulate adenylate cyclase to synthesize cAMP via a G protein called Golf. cAMP, which is the second
messenger here, opens a cyclic nucleotide-gated ion channel (CNG), producing an influx of cations (largely
Ca2+ with some Na+) into the cell, slightly depolarising it. The Ca2+ in turn opens a Ca2+-activated chloride
channel, leading to efflux of Cl, further depolarizing the cell and triggering an action potential. Ca2+ is then
extruded through a sodium-calcium exchanger. A calcium-calmodulin complex also acts to inhibit the
binding of cAMP to the cAMP-dependent channel, thus contributing to olfactory adaptation. This
mechanism of transduction is somewhat unique, in that cAMP works by directly binding to the ion channel
rather than through activation of protein kinase A. It is similar to the transduction mechanism for
photoreceptors, in which the second messenger cGMP works by directly binding to ion channels, suggesting
that maybe one of these receptors was evolutionarily adapted into the other. There are also considerable
similarities in the immediate processing of stimuli by lateral inhibition. Averaged activity of the receptor
neurons can be measured in several ways. In vertebrates, responses to an odor can be measured by an
electro-olfactogram or through calcium imaging of receptor neuron terminals in the olfactory bulb. In
insects, one can perform electroantenogram or also calcium imaging within the olfactory bulb.

Olfactory bulb projections


Olfactory sensory neurons project axons to the brain within the olfactory nerve, (cranial nerve I). These
nerve fibers, lacking myelin sheaths, pass to the olfactory bulb of the brain through perforations in the
cribriform plate, which in turn projects olfactory information to the olfactory cortex and other areas.[15] The
axons from the olfactory receptors converge in the outer layer of the olfactory bulb within small (~50
micrometers in diameter) structures called glomeruli. Mitral cells, located in the inner layer of the olfactory
bulb, form synapses with the axons of the sensory neurons within glomeruli and send the information about
the odor to other parts of the olfactory system, where multiple signals may be processed to form a
synthesized olfactory perception. A large degree of convergence occurs, with twenty-five thousand axons
synapsing on twenty-five or so mitral cells, and with each of these mitral cells projecting to multiple
glomeruli. Mitral cells also project to periglomerular cells and granular cells that inhibit the mitral cells
surrounding it (lateral inhibition). Granular cells also mediate inhibition and excitation of mitral cells
through pathways from centrifugal fibers and the anterior olfactory nuclei.
The mitral cells leave the olfactory bulb in the lateral olfactory tract, which synapses on five major regions
of the cerebrum: the anterior olfactory nucleus, the olfactory tubercle, the amygdala, the piriform cortex, and
the entorhinal cortex. The anterior olfactory nucleus

projects, via the anterior commissure, to the contralateral


olfactory bulb, inhibiting it. The piriform cortex has two
major divisions with anatomically distinct organizations
and functions. The anterior piriform cortex (APC) is better
associated with determining the chemical structure of the
odorant molecules and whereas the posterior piriform
cortex (PPC) is best known for its strong role in
categorizing odors and assessing similarities between
odors (e.g. minty, woody, citrus are odors which can be
distinguished via the PPC despite being highly-variant
chemicals and in a concentration-independent
manner).[16] The piriform cortex projects to the medial
dorsal nucleus of the thalamus, which then projects to the
orbitofrontal cortex. The orbitofrontal cortex mediates
conscious perception of the odor. The 3-layered piriform
cortex projects to a number of thalamic and hypothalamic
nuclei, the hippocampus and amygdala and the
orbitofrontal cortex but its function is largely unknown.
The entorhinal cortex projects to the amygdala and is
involved in emotional and autonomic responses to odor. It
also projects to the hippocampus and is involved in
motivation and memory. Odor information is stored in
long-term memory and has strong connections to
emotional memory. This is possibly due to the olfactory
system's close anatomical ties to the limbic system and hippocampus, areas of the brain that have long been
known to be involved in emotion and place memory, respectively.
Since any one receptor is responsive to various odorants, and there is a great deal of convergence at the level
of the olfactory bulb, it seems strange that human beings are able to distinguish so many different odors. It
seems that there must be a highly-complex form of processing occurring; however, as it can be shown that,
while many neurons in the olfactory bulb (and even the pyriform cortex and amygdala) are responsive to
many different odors, half the neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex are responsive to only one odor, and the
rest to only a few. It has been shown through microelectrode studies that each individual odor gives a
particular specific spatial map of excitation in the olfactory bulb. It is possible that, through spatial encoding,
the brain is able to distinguish specific odors. However, temporal coding must be taken into account. Over
time, the spatial maps change, even for one particular odor, and the brain must be able to process these
details as well.
Inputs from the two nostrils have separate inputs to the brain with the result that it is possible for humans to
experience perceptual rivalry in the olfactory sense akin to that of binocular rivalry when there are two
different inputs into the two nostrils.[17]
In insects smells are sensed by sensilla located on the antenna and maxillary palp and first processed by the
antennal lobe (analogous to the olfactory bulb), and next by the mushroom bodies and lateral horn.

Accessory olfactory system


Many animals, including most mammals and reptiles, but not humans, have two distinct and segregated
olfactory systems: a main olfactory system, which detects volatile stimuli, and an accessory olfactory
system, which detects fluid-phase stimuli. Behavioral evidence suggests that these fluid-phase stimuli often

function as pheromones, although pheromones can also be detected by the main olfactory system. In the
accessory olfactory system, stimuli are detected by the vomeronasal organ, located in the vomer, between
the nose and the mouth. Snakes use it to smell prey, sticking their tongue out and touching it to the organ.
Some mammals make a facial expression called flehmen to direct stimuli to this organ.
The sensory receptors of the accessory olfactory system are located in the vomeronasal organ. As in the
main olfactory system, the axons of these sensory neurons project from the vomeronasal organ to the
accessory olfactory bulb, which in the mouse is located on the dorsal-posterior portion of the main olfactory
bulb. Unlike in the main olfactory system, the axons that leave the accessory olfactory bulb do not project to
the brain's cortex but rather to targets in the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and from there
to the hypothalamus, where they may influence aggressive and mating behavior.

Human olfactory system


In female humans, the sense of olfaction is strongest around the time of ovulation, significantly stronger than
during other phases of the menstrual cycle and stronger than the sense in males.[18]
The MHC genes (known as HLA in humans) are a group of genes present in many animals and important
for the immune system; in general, offspring from parents with differing MHC genes have a stronger
immune system. Fish, mice and female humans are able to smell some aspect of the MHC genes of potential
sex partners and prefer partners with MHC genes different from their own.[19][20]
Humans can detect individuals that are blood-related kin (mothers/fathers and children but not husbands and
wives) from olfaction.[21] Mothers can identify by body odor their biological children but not their
stepchildren. Preadolescent children can olfactorily detect their full siblings but not half-siblings or step
siblings and this might explain incest avoidance and the Westermarck effect.[22] Functional imaging shows
that this olfactory kinship detection process involves the frontal-temporal junction, the insula, and the
dorsomedial prefrontal cortex but not the primary or secondary olfactory cortices, or the related piriform
cortex or orbitofrontal cortex.[23]

Olfactory coding and perception


How olfactory information is coded in the brain to allow for proper perception is still being researched and
the process is not completely understood. When an odorant is detected by receptors, the receptors in a sense
break the odorant down and then the brain puts the odorant back together for identification and
perception.[24] The odorant binds to receptors which recognize only a specific functional group, or feature,
of the odorant, which is why the chemical nature of the odorant is important.[25]
After binding the odorant, the receptor is activated and will send a signal to the glomeruli.[25] Each
glomerulus receives signals from multiple receptors that detect similar odorant features. Because multiple
receptor types are activated due to the different chemical features of the odorant, multiple glomeruli will be
activated as well. All of the signals from the glomeruli will then be sent to the brain, where the combination
of glomeruli activation will encode the different chemical features of the odorant. The brain will then
essentially put the pieces of the activation pattern back together in order to identify and perceive the
odorant.[25]

It is a general idea that the layout of brain structures corresponds to physical features of stimuli (called
topographic coding), and similar analogies have been made in olfaction with concepts such as a layout
corresponding to chemical features (called chemotopy) or perceptual features.[26] While chemotopy is a
highly controversial concept,[27] there is more evidence for perceptual information implemented in the
spatial dimensions of olfactory networks.[26]
Although conventional wisdom and lay literature, based on impressionistic findings in the 1920s, have long
presented human oflaction as capable of distinguishing between roughly 10,000 unique odors, recent
research has suggested that the average individual is capable of distinguishing between over one trillion
unique odors.[28] Researchers in the most recent study noted that this estimate is "conservative" and that
some subjects of their researchtests involved testing the psychophysical responses to combinations of over
128 unique odor molecules with combinations composed of up to 30 different component moleculesmight
be capable of deciphering between a thousand trillion odorants while they estimated their worst performer
could still distinguish between 80 million scents.[29] Authors of the study concluded, "This is far more than
previous estimates of distinguishable olfactory stimuli. It demonstrates that the human olfactory system,
with its hundreds of different olfactory receptors, far out performs the other senses in the number of
physically different stimuli it can discriminate."[30] However, it has also been noted by the authors that the
ability to distinguish between smells is not analogous to being able to consistently identify them and that
subjects were not typically capable of identifying individual odor stimulants from within the odors the
researchers had composed from multiple odor molecules. In November 2014, the study was strongly
criticized by Caltech scientist Markus Meister who wrote that the study's "extravagant claims are based on
errors of mathematical logic".[31]

Genetics of olfaction
Different people smell different odors and most of these differences are caused by genetic differences.[32]
Although odorant receptor genes make up one of the largest genes families in the human genome, only a
handful of genes have been linked conclusively to particular smells. For instance, the odorant receptor
OR5A1 and its genetic variants (alleles) are responsible for our ability (or failure) to smell -ionone, a key
aroma in foods and beverages.[33] Similarly, the odorant receptor OR2J3 is associated with the ability to
detect the "grassy" smelling odor, cis-3-hexen-1-ol.[34] The preference (or dislike) of cilantro (coriander) has
been linked to the olfactory receptor OR6A2.[35]

Interactions of olfaction with other senses


Olfaction and taste
Olfaction, taste and trigeminal receptors (also called chemesthesis) together contribute to flavor. The human
tongue can distinguish only among five distinct qualities of taste, while the nose can distinguish among
hundreds of substances, even in minute quantities. It is during exhalation that the olfaction contribution to
flavor occurs, in contrast to that of proper smell, which occurs during the inhalation phase.[36] The olfactory
system is the only human sense that bypasses the thalamus and connects directly to the forebrain.[37]

Olfaction and audition

Olfaction and sound information has been shown to converge in the olfactory tubercles of rodents.[38] This
neural convergence is proposed to give rise to a percept termed smound.[39] Whereas a flavor results from
interactions between smell and taste, a smound may result from interactions between smell and sound.

Disorders of olfaction
The following are disorders of olfaction:[40]
Anosmia inability to smell
Dysosmia things smell different from memory or expectation
Hyperosmia an abnormally acute sense of smell.
Hyposmia decreased ability to smell
Olfactory Reference Syndrome psychological disorder which causes the patient to imagine he or she
has strong body odor
Parosmia things smell worse than they should[41]
Phantosmia "hallucinated smell," often unpleasant in nature

Quantifying olfaction in industry


Scientists have devised methods for quantifying the intensity of
odors, in particular for the purpose of analyzing unpleasant or
objectionable odors released by an industrial source into a
community. Since the 1800s, industrial countries have encountered
incidents where proximity of an industrial source or landfill produced
adverse reactions to nearby residents regarding airborne odor. The
basic theory of odor analysis is to measure what extent of dilution
with "pure" air is required before the sample in question is rendered
indistinguishable from the "pure" or reference standard. Since each
person perceives odor differently, an "odor panel" composed of
several different people is assembled, each sniffing the same sample
of diluted specimen air. A field olfactometer can be utilized to
determine the magnitude of an odor.

Nasal Ranger, an olfactometer, in use.

Many air management districts in the US have numerical standards of acceptability for the intensity of odor
that is allowed to cross into a residential property. For example, the Bay Area Air Quality Management
District has applied its standard in regulating numerous industries, landfills, and sewage treatment plants.
Example applications this district has engaged are the San Mateo, California wastewater treatment plant; the
Shoreline Amphitheatre in Mountain View, California; and the IT Corporation waste ponds, Martinez,
California.

Olfaction in plants and animals


The tendrils of plants are especially sensitive to airborne volatile organic compounds. Parasites such as
dodder make use of this in locating their preferred hosts and locking on to them.[42] The emission of volatile
compounds is detected when foliage is browsed by animals. Threatened plants are then able to take
defensive chemical measures, such as moving tannin compounds to their foliage. (see Plant perception).

The importance and sensitivity of smell varies among different organisms; most mammals have a good sense
of smell, whereas most birds do not, except the tubenoses (e.g., petrels and albatrosses), certain species of
vultures and the kiwis. Among mammals, it is well-developed in the carnivores and ungulates, which must
always be aware of each other, and in those that smell for their food, like moles. Having a strong sense of
smell is referred to as macrosmatic.
Figures suggesting greater or lesser sensitivity in various species reflect experimental findings from the
reactions of animals exposed to aromas in known extreme dilutions. These are, therefore, based on
perceptions by these animals, rather than mere nasal function. That is, the brain's smell-recognizing centers
must react to the stimulus detected, for the animal to show a response to the smell in question. It is estimated
that dogs in general have an olfactory sense approximately a hundred thousand to a million times more acute
than a human's. This does not mean they are overwhelmed by smells our noses can detect; rather, it means
they can discern a molecular presence when it is in much greater dilution in the carrier, air.
Scenthounds as a group can smell one- to ten-million times more acutely than a human, and Bloodhounds,
which have the keenest sense of smell of any dogs, have noses ten- to one-hundred-million times more
sensitive than a human's. They were bred for the specific purpose of tracking humans, and can detect a scent
trail a few days old. The second-most-sensitive nose is possessed by the Basset Hound, which was bred to
track and hunt rabbits and other small animals.
Bears, such as the Silvertip Grizzly found in parts of North America, have a sense of smell seven times
stronger than that of the bloodhound, essential for locating food underground. Using their elongated claws,
bears dig deep trenches in search of burrowing animals and nests as well as roots, bulbs, and insects. Bears
can detect the scent of food from up to 18 miles away; because of their immense size, they often scavenge
new kills, driving away the predators (including packs of wolves and human hunters) in the process.
The sense of smell is less-developed in the catarrhine primates, and nonexistent in cetaceans, which
compensate with a well-developed sense of taste. In some strepsirrhines, such as the red-bellied lemur, scent
glands occur atop the head. In many species, olfaction is highly tuned to pheromones; a male silkworm
moth, for example, can sense a single molecule of bombykol.
Fish too have a well-developed sense of smell, even though they inhabit an aquatic environment. Salmon
utilize their sense of smell to identify and return to their home stream waters. Catfish use their sense of smell
to identify other individual catfish and to maintain a social hierarchy. Many fishes use the sense of smell to
identify mating partners or to alert to the presence of food.

Insect olfactory system


Insects have been used as a model system to study olfaction. Insects use primarily their antennae for
detecting odors. Sensory neurons in the antenna generate odor-specific electrical signals called spikes in
response to binding of odors. The sensory neurons send this information via their axons to the antennal lobe,
where they synapse with other neurons in semidelineated (with membrane boundaries) structures called
glomeruli. The antennal lobes have two kinds of neurons, projection neurons (mostly excitatory) and local
neurons (inhibitory, and some excitatory). The projection neurons send their axon terminals to mushroom
bodies and the lateral horn (both of which are part of the protocerebrum of the insects). Recordings from
projection neurons show in some insects strong specialization and discrimination for the odors presented
(especially for the projection neurons of the macroglomeruli, a specialized complex of glomeruli responsible
for the pheromones detection). Olfaction is essential for hunting in many species of wasps, including
Polybia sericea.

See also

Chemesthesis
Electronic nose
Machine olfaction
Nasal administration olfactory transfer
Odor
Olfactometer
Olfactory ensheathing glia
Olfactory fatigue
Phantosmia
Scent transfer unit
Vibration Theory of Olfaction
Evolution of olfaction

References
1. Wiktionary: http://en.wiktionary.org/wiki/olfactics
2. Daniel Schacter, Daniel Gilbert, Daniel Wegner (2011). "Sensation and Perception". Psychology. Worth Publishers.
pp. 166171. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
3. Hussain A, Saraiva LR, Korsching SI (2009). "Positive Darwinian selection and the birth of an olfactory receptor
clade in teleosts" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2657432). PNAS 106 (11): 43138.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0803229106 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0803229106). PMC 2657432
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2657432). PMID 19237578
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19237578).
4. Boroditsky, Lera (1999). "Taste, Smell, and Touch: Lecture Notes" (http://www.saylor.org/site/wpcontent/uploads/2011/08/Psych306-Reading-6.1.pdf) (PDF). p. 1.
5. "Better Smelling Through Genetics: Mammalian Odor Perception"
(http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2590501/). Ncbi.nlm.nih.gov. 2012-10-19. Retrieved 2012-12-30.
6. G. A. Kimble; K. Schlesinger (1985). Topics in the History of Psychology, Volume 1. L. Erlbaum Associates.
7. Buck L, Axel R (April 1991). "A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor
recognition". Cell 65 (1): 17587. doi:10.1016/0092-8674(91)90418-X (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2F00928674%2891%2990418-X). PMID 1840504 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1840504).
8. Gilad Y, Man O, Pbo S, Lancet D (March 2003). "Human specific loss of olfactory receptor genes"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC152291). PNAS 100 (6): 33247. doi:10.1073/pnas.0535697100
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0535697100). PMC 152291
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC152291). PMID 12612342
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12612342).
9. Pinel, John P.J. (2006) Biopsychology. Pearson Education Inc. ISBN 0-205-42651-4 (page 178)
10. Turin L (December 1996). "A spectroscopic mechanism for primary olfactory reception"
(http://chemse.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=8985605). Chemical senses 21 (6): 77391.
doi:10.1093/chemse/21.6.773 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1093%2Fchemse%2F21.6.773). PMID 8985605
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8985605).
11. Turin L (June 2002). "A method for the calculation of odor character from molecular structure". Journal of
Theoretical Biology 216 (3): 36785. doi:10.1006/jtbi.2001.2504 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1006%2Fjtbi.2001.2504).
PMID 12183125 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12183125).
12. Keller A, Vosshall LB (April 2004). "A psychophysical test of the vibration theory of olfaction". Nature
Neuroscience 7 (4): 3378. doi:10.1038/nn1215 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fnn1215). PMID 15034588
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15034588). See also the editorial on p. 315.
13. Doty, R. L. (2001). Olfaction. 425. Retrieved from
http://journals2.scholarsportal.info.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/tmp/9139085232678633444.pdf
14. Bear, Connors and Paradiso, Mark, Barry and Michael (2007). Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain. USA: Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins. pp. 265275.
15. Morris, H., & Schaeffer, J. P. (1953). The Nervous system-The Brain or Encephalon. Human anatomy; a complete
systematic treatise. (11th ed., pp.1218-1219). New York: Blakiston.
16. Margot C (July 2009). "A noseful of objects". Nat. Neurosci. 12 (7): 8134. doi:10.1038/nn0709-813
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fnn0709-813). PMID 19554043 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19554043).

17. Zhou W, Chen D (2009). "Binaral rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2901510). Curr Biol 19 (18): 15615.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cub.2009.07.052). PMC 2901510
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2901510). PMID 19699095
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19699095).
18. Navarrete-Palacios E, Hudson R, Reyes-Guerrero G, Guevara-Guzmn R (July 2003). "Lower olfactory threshold
during the ovulatory phase of the menstrual cycle". Biological Psychology 63 (3): 26979. doi:10.1016/S03010511(03)00076-0 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2FS0301-0511%2803%2900076-0). PMID 12853171
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12853171).
19. Boehm T, Zufall F (February 2006). "MHC peptides and the sensory evaluation of genotype". Trends in
neurosciences 29 (2): 1007. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.11.006 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.tins.2005.11.006).
PMID 16337283 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16337283).
20. Santos PS, Schinemann JA, Gabardo J, Bicalho Mda G (April 2005). "New evidence that the MHC influences odor
perception in humans: a study with 58 Southern Brazilian students". Hormones and behavior 47 (4): 3848.
doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.005 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.yhbeh.2004.11.005). PMID 15777804
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15777804).
21. Porter RH, Cernoch JM, Balogh RD (1985). "Odor signatures and kin recognition". Physiol Behav 34 (3): 4458.
doi:10.1016/0031-9384(85)90210-0 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2F0031-9384%2885%2990210-0). PMID 4011726
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4011726).
22. Weisfeld GE, Czilli T, Phillips KA, Gall JA, Lichtman CM (July 2003). "Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in
human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance.". Journal of experimental child psychology 85 (3): 27995.
doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00061-4 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2FS0022-0965%2803%2900061-4).
PMID 12810039 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12810039).
23. Lundstrm JN, Boyle JA, Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M (August 2009). "The neuronal substrates of human olfactory
based kin recognition". Human Brain Mapping 30 (8): 257180. doi:10.1002/hbm.20686
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1002%2Fhbm.20686). PMID 19067327 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19067327).
24. Wilson DA (June 2001). "Receptive fields in the rat piriform cortex.". Chemical senses 26 (5): 57784.
doi:10.1093/chemse/26.5.577 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1093%2Fchemse%2F26.5.577). PMID 11418503
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11418503).
25. Leon M, Johnson BA (2003). "Olfactory coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb". Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42
(1): 2332. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00142-5 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2FS0165-0173%2803%2900142-5).
PMID 12668289 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12668289).
26. Auffarth, B. (2013). "Understanding smell -- the olfactory stimulus problem."
(http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0149763413001644). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 37:
16671679. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.009 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.neubiorev.2013.06.009).
27. Soucy, Edward R. et al. (2009). "Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb.". Nature Neuroscience
12.2: 210220. doi:10.1038/nn.2262 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fnn.2262).
28. Helen Briggs (20 March 2014). "Nose can detect one trillion odours" (http://www.bbc.com/news/health-26638085).
BBC News. pp. 23 March 2014.
29. Sarah C.P. Williams (20 March 2014). "Human Nose Can Detect a Trillion Smells"
(http://news.sciencemag.org/biology/2014/03/human-nose-can-detect-trillion-smells). Science Now/AAAS News.
Retrieved 23 March 2014.
30. Bushdid, C.; Magnasco, M.O.; Vosshall, L.B.; Keller, A. (21 March 2014). "Humans Can Discriminate More than 1
million Olfactory Stimuli" (http://www.sciencemag.org/content/343/6177/1370). Science 343 (6177): 13701372.
doi:10.1126/science.1249168 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1126%2Fscience.1249168). PMID 24653035
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24653035).
31. Meister, M. (2014). "Can Humans Really Discriminate 1 Trillion Odors?" (http://arxiv.org/abs/1411.0165). arXiv.
arXiv:1411.0165.
32. Howgego, J. (2013) Sense for scents traced down to genes (http://www.nature.com/news/sense-for-scents-traceddown-to-genes-1.13493). Nature News, 1 Aug 2013
33. Jaeger SR, McRae JF, Bava CM, Beresford MK, Hunter D, Jia Y, Chheang SL, Jin D, Peng M, Gamble JC, Atkinson
KR, Axten LG, Paisley AG, Tooman L, Pineau B, Rouse SA, Newcomb RD (2013). "A Mendelian Trait for
Olfactory Sensitivity Affects Odor Experience and Food Selection". Current Biology 23 (16): 16011605.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.030 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cub.2013.07.030). PMID 23910657
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23910657).

34. McRae JF, Mainland JD, Jaeger SR, Adipietro KA, Matsunami H, Newcomb RD (2012). "Genetic Variation in the
Odorant Receptor OR2J3 is Associated with the Ability to Detect the "Grassy" Smelling Odor, cis-3-hexen-1-ol"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3408771). Chemical Senses 37 (7): 585593.
doi:10.1093/chemse/bjs049 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1093%2Fchemse%2Fbjs049). PMC 3408771
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3408771). PMID 22714804
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22714804).
35. Callaway, E. (2012) taste of coriander linked to genetic variants (http://www.nature.com/news/soapy-taste-ofcoriander-linked-to-genetic-variants-1.11398Soapy) Nature News, 12 Sep 2012
36. Masaoka Y, Satoh H, Akai L, Homma I (2010). "Expiration: The moment we experience retronasal olfaction in
flavor". Neurosci Lett. 473 (2): 9296. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.02.024
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.neulet.2010.02.024). PMID 20171264
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20171264).
37. Annual Review of Psychology (February 2001), 52 (1), pg. 423-452,Richard L. Doty
38. Wesson DW, Wilson DA (2010). "Smelling Sounds: Olfactory-auditory convergence in the olfactory tubercle"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2846283). J Neurosci 30 (8): 30131021.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6003-09.2010 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.6003-09.2010).
PMC 2846283 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2846283). PMID 20181598
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20181598).
39. Peeples, Lynne. "Scientific American, ''Making scents of sounds'' Lynne Peeples, 23 February 2010, accessed 25
February 2010" (http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=making-scents-of-sounds-n).
Scientificamerican.com. Retrieved 2012-12-30.
40. Hirsch, Alan R. (2003) Life's a Smelling Success
41. Gilbert, Avery (2008). "Freaks, Geeks, and Prodigies". What the Nose Knows. Crown Publishers. p. 52. ISBN 978-14000-8234-6.
42. Fountain, Henry (3 October 2006). "This Plant Has the Sense of Smell (Loves Tomatoes, Hates Wheat)"
(http://www.nytimes.com/2006/10/03/science/03find.html). The New York Times.

Further reading
Gordon M.s Shepherd Neurogastronomy: How the Brain Creates Flavor and Why It Matters New
York : Columbia University Press, 2012 ISBN 978-0-231-15910-4

External links
Mammalian Odor Perception through Genetics
Wikimedia Commons has
(http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2590501/)
media related to Olfactory
Research on Interesting Questions About Smells
system.
(http://andreaskeller.squarespace.com/olfaction-smell-odor/)
Insect Olfaction of Plant Odour (http://www.olfacts.nl)
Smells and Odours - How Smell Works at thenakedscientists.com
(http://www.thenakedscientists.com/HTML/Columnists/peterbrennancolumn.htm)
Olfaction at cf.ac.uk (http://www.cf.ac.uk/biosi/staff/jacob/teaching/sensory/olfact1.html)
Structure-odor relations: a modern perspective at flexitral.com
(http://www.flexitral.com/research/review_final.pdf) (PDF)
Chirality & Odour Perception at leffingwell.com (http://www.leffingwell.com/chirality/chirality.htm)
ScienceDaily Artille 08/03/2006, Quick -- What's That Smell? Time Needed To Identify Odors
Reveals Much About Olfaction at sciencedaily.com
(http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/08/060803091235.htm)
Scents and Emotions Linked by Learning, Brown Study Shows at brown.edu.com
(http://www.brown.edu/Administration/News_Bureau/2004-05/04-069.html)
Sense of Smell Institute at senseofsmell.org (http://www.senseofsmell.org/feature/odor/index.php).
Research arm of international fragrance industry's The Fragrance Foundation
Olfactory Systems Laboratory at Boston University (http://people.bu.edu/dmattw/)

Smells Database (http://chemconnections.org/Smells/)


Olfaction and Gustation (http://nba.uth.tmc.edu/neuroscience/s2/chapter09.html), Neuroscience
Online (electronic neuroscience textbook by UT Houston Medical School)
Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Olfaction&oldid=668160934"
Categories: Olfaction Limbic system
This page was last modified on 22 June 2015, at 18:54.
Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may
apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia is a registered
trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.

You might also like