Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 4

VAN DE VIJVER, F.J.R., VAN HEMERT, D.A.

(2008) CROSS-CULTURAL
PERSOANLITY ASSESSMENT IN Boyle, G.J., Matthews, G., Saklofske, D.H., The SAGE
Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment: Personality Measurement and Testing, VoL.2,
SAGE, pp.55-73

RESPONSE STYLES

People often show tendencies to respond according to certain systematic patterns


(response styles) lowering the validity of instruments. Three main types of response styles are
extreme responding (preferring the end-points of a scale), acquiescent responding (agreeing with
items irrespective of item content), and social desirability (portraying oneself in a favorable
manner). The latter two response styles are different in that acquiescence pertains to endorsing
items independent of their contents, whereas social desirability pertains to endorsing items
dependent of their contents. This distinction is only apparent when scales use a combination of
positively and negatively worded items; when all items are scaled in the same direction it is
impossible to disentangle the two response styles.
In addition to individuals, cultures also differ with respect to the frequency with which
they show different types of responding behavior (see Johnson and Van de Vijver, 2003, for an
overview of the literature on socially desirable responding; Johnson et al., 2005). Many authors
provided evidence for consistent cross-cultural differences in response styles. In a meta-analysis
across 41 countries, Fischer et al. (submitted) calculated acquiescence scores for various scales
in the personality, social-psychological, and organizational domains. They showed that 3.1% of
the overall variance was shared among all scales, pointing to a systematic influence of response
styles in cross-cultural comparisons.
Similarly, in a large-scale study involving 26 countries, Harzing (2006) found consistent
cross-cultural differences in acquiescence and extremity responding. Moreover, she found that
cross-cultural differences in response styles are systemati-cally related to various country
characteristics. All studies conducted thus far in which response styles are compared across
several countries report the same findings: There are systematic differences across countries and
country differences in response styles are related to other country variables. It is counterproductive to dismiss response styles as confounding factors in cross-cultural research that
should be eliminated or, at least, statistically controlled. As shown below, cross-cultural research
shows that response styles can be viewed as communication styles, as ways in which participants
present themselves. Therefore, the relevant question is how to interpret the common response
styles in terms of their correlates with other cultural characteristics.
Culture-level correlates of response styles
Not many large-scale cross-cultural studies have been done to shed light on the crosscultural meaning of response styles; most studies involve two-culture comparisons (e.g.
Bachman and O'Malley, 1984; Grimm and Church, 1999; Heine and Renshaw, 2002; Hui and
Triandis, 1989) or provide only post-hoc explanations of differences. In a compar ison of
European countries, Van Herk et al. (2004) found that Mediterranean countries, particularly

Greece, showed higher acquies-cent and extreme responding than north-western European
countries in surveys on consumer research.
They explained these differences in terms of the individualism versus collectivism
dimension, at the same time acknowledging that the causal relationship might well be reversed,
so that response styles are responsible for country scores on individualism and collectivism (see
also Berry et al., 2002). Two classes of country variables have been shown to be associated with
response styles in cross-cultural studies, namely socioeconomic context and cultural values.
Socioeconomic and political indicators such as gross national product (GNP), politi-cal rights,
and level of democracy seem to be relevant to socially desirable and acquiescent responding.
Van Hemert et al. (2002) reported significant correlations between these indicators and
social desirability as measured by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Lie scale across 31
countries; lower scores were found in wealthier and more democratic countries. Similarly,
Fischer et al. (submitted) correlated ARS scores based on 17 different instruments or scales to a
number of socioeconomic indica-tors and found the level of acquiescence to be negatively
correlated with survey quality, wealth-related indices, democratic rights, and positive
correlations with prevalence rates for psychiatric disorders.
They suggest that survey responses are systematic indica-tors of culturally appropriate
expressiveness that are also reflected in the larger sociopolit-ical climate of the societies. An
explanation of these findings might be found in the con-cept of resources. Ross and Mirowsky
(1984) found that external resources influence cross-cultural differences in social desirability;
less powerful (i.e. less resourceful) social groups often give more socially desirable responses.
Multicountry research on the importance of cultural values for response styles started with Bond
and Smith's (1996) meta-analysis of studies on the Asch conformity paradigm. Conformity can
be expected to tap something similar to social desirability.
They found several measures of cultural values, such as indi-vidualism (several
measures), autonomy and conservatism (Schwartz, 1994), to be strong predictors of conformity.
The authors argue that these measures reflect the individualism versus collectivism dimension in
such a way that more individualistic countries showed less conformity. Another concept close to
social desirability was studied by Triandis et al. (2001) in a 12-country study of deception (both
self-reported and other-reported) in scenarios of negotiation situations. High vertical
(hierarchical) collectivism was posi-tively related to the tendency to deceive. Yet, the pattern was
reversed at the individual level. Van Hemert et al. (2002) also found higher collectivism to be
associated with higher social desirability.
Harzing (2006) found in a study involving 26 countries that acquiescence and extreme
responding are more prevalent in countries with higher scores on Hofstede's collec-tivism and
power distance, and GLOBE's uncertainty avoidance. She also reported that extraversion (at
country level) is a positive predictor of acquiescence and extremity scor-ing. Finally, she found
that English-language questionnaires tend to evoke less extremity scoring and that answering
items in one's native language is associated with more extremity scoring. A convincing case for a
substantive cultural interpretation of response styles was made by Smith (2004). He calculated
acqui-escence scores for variables from five multi-country surveys on values, beliefs, reports of
own behavior, and reports of others' behavior (Hofstede, 1980; House et al., 2004; Leung et al.,

2002; Schwartz, 1994; Smith et al., 2002), and added social desirability scores based on the EPQ
(Van Hemert et al., 2002). The GLOBE study (House et al., 2004) provided indices on society 'as
it should be' and society 'as it is'. All six acquiescent response bias indices were positively
interrelated, and correlations with GLOBE 'as should be' were much stronger than correlations
with GLOBE 'as is'.
This can be taken as evidence for a common factor. Congruent with Harzing's (2006)
findings, acquiescent bias scores were positively related to Hofstede's collectivism and power
distance and GLOBE uncertainty avoidance. In addition, depend-ing on whether 'as is' or 'as
should be' scores were used to predict acquiescent bias, 'as is' family collectivism or 'as should
be' institu-tional collectivism and uncertainty avoidance explained cross-cultural variance. Smith
con eluded that response bias at a country level can be meaningfully interpreted in terms of
cultural values and reflects cultural commu-nication styles and patterns of intergroup ralatinne
This idea features in many recent cross-national studies, but specific correlations sometimes
differ. For example, Johnson et al. (2005) studied country-level correlates of individual-level
extreme responding across respondents from 19 countries and acquies-cent responding across
respondents from 10 countries using hierarchical linear model-ing. Extreme responding was
positively related to power distance and masculinity, and acquiescence was negatively related to
power distance, uncertainty avoidance, indi-vidualism, masculinity, and GNP.
These findings were only partly in agreement with other research. The correlations found
for extreme responding fitted the hypotheses and the negative correlations of acquiescence with
individualism and GNP replicated previous findings (Smith, 2004; Van Hemert et al., 2002).
Diverging results included the role of power distance in predicting acquiescence, which was
reversed in Smith's (2004) and Harzing's (2006) studies. This difference might be explained by
the limited number of countries for the acquiescence analyses and the multilevel design of the
study in which individual-level response styles were used rather than country-level scores, and
individual-level control variables such as gender, age, and employment were used. Another
multilevel study on extreme responding was performed by De Jong et al. (2007). They used a
method based on item response theory for measuring extreme responding, allowing for
differential useful-ness of items in the calculation of extreme responding. A multilevel analysis
(26 coun-tries, 12,500 individuals) with individual-level demographic variables (i.e. age, gender,
and education) and Hofstede's measures revealed significant effects of gender and age and
positive correlations with individual-ism, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity. These results
differ from findings by Johnson et al. (2005), who did report a relationship with power distance
and not with individual-ism and uncertainty avoidance, and findings by Harzing (2006), who
also observed a positive correlation with power distance.
New developments
In our view, the study of response styles slowly gets the attention it deserves in crosscultural research. We see several promising directions in which research on cross-cultural
response styles can advance our insight in cross-cultural differences on self-report measures.
First, Lalwani et al. (2006) distin-guish two distinct ways of socially desirable responding in
individualistic and collectivis-tic cultures rather than focus on the frequency only. Social
desirability has been viewed as both other-deception (i.e. conscious lying or position supposemy
retatea to narcissism. in contrast, persons in more horizontally collec-tivist societies, which focus

on sociability and maintaining good relationships, show more impression management and put
forward a favorable self-image.
Second, work has been done on the effects of questionnaire characteristics in relation to
cross-cultural differences. For example, Van Dijk et al. (submitted) tested whether the domain
(i.e. subject) of a questionnaire has an impact on the extent of acquiescence or extremity
responding in a multicountry International Social Survey Program dataset. They found some
evidence for higher acqui-escence and extreme responding scores in domains with high personal
relevance, such as family, than in domains with low personal relevance, such as government.
Furthermore, culture-level correlations with socioeco-nomic indices and values indicated that
wealthier countries have lower acquiescence scores, and individualist, happy countries and
countries with low uncertainty avoidance have lower acquiescence scores for personally relevant
domains.
Extremity responding turned out to be higher in uncertainty avoidant countries (in
concordance with De Jong et al., 2007, and Harzing, 2006), and countries with low subjective
wellbeing, independent of the domain of the questions. A third development can be found in multilevel studies focusing on the interaction of individual-level and culture-level variables. Smith
and Fischer (2008) performed a multi-level analysis of acquiescence, extreme responding, and
culture. They demonstrated interactions in response styles between indi-viduals' predispositions
and cultural context in such a way that interdependent persons show more acouiescence in more
collectivis-tic cultures and independent persons respond more extremely in more individualistic
cul-tures. They argue that cultural context acts as a 'press' towards different communication
styles, but these effects can be reinforced or mitigated by personal characteristics. Another
example of a multilevel approach that includes interaction effects was pro-vided by De Jong
(2006). He included individual-level sociodemographic and per-sonality variables as well as
country-level value dimensions in a study on social desir-able responding across 25 countries and
more than 12,000 respondents. Main effects were found for individualism (negatively related to
social desirability) and uncertainty avoidance, power distance, and masculinity (positive
correlations). In addition, the effect of individual-level conscientiousness was stronger in
countries high on uncertainty avoidance, the effect of agreeableness was mitigated by countrylevel individualism, and the effect of extraversion was stronger in more masculine countries and
countries high on power distance.
Conclusions
Most research on response styles, across cultures, finds systematic and not just random
variation, indicating a substantive basis of response styles at the country level. Differences can
be interpreted in terms of socioeconomic context as well as values.

You might also like