Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Magic: A Problem in Semantics

Author(s): Dorothy Hammond


Source: American Anthropologist, New Series, Vol. 72, No. 6 (Dec., 1970), pp. 1349-1356
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the American Anthropological Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/672852
Accessed: 10-11-2016 14:51 UTC
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/672852?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

American Anthropological Association, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to American Anthropologist

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Magic: A Problem in Semantics


DOROTHY HAMMOND

Brooklyn College, C.U.N.Y

Anthropologists, from Tylor to the present, have so defined magic that, alt

shade into or overlap with religion, it is a separate phenomenon distinct


Theorists have made different features the chief means of differentiatio

how defined the distinction cannot be easily or consistently maintai

suggests that the concept of magic as a distinct entity is the factitious


centric classification, and that magic should be included within religion
the practices of which religious ritual is composed. [magic; religion; the
HE PUBLICATION OF a considerable

pears to be an inherent contradiction benumber of articles, compilations,


tween the
and
ideas of "secular" and "supernatu-

monographs during the last decade


ral."
testifies
Moreover, the ethnographic data comto movement of the anthropological
of that a sharp distinction bepel study
recognition
religion out of its long stay in thetween
doldrums.
magic and religion can hardly be sus-

Reawakened theoretical interest has been

tained. Spiro, in the course of making a


accompanied by renewal attacks onpoint
the about religion in his narrow sense of
the term, illustrates his argument with a
perdurable problem of definition. Current
definitions are not sufficiently incisive. To
de- belief (1966:110). This may be the
magical
fine religion in Tillich's terms as "the ultimate
result of momentary carelessness, but Spiro's
concern" leads to the possible inclusion
of demonstrate the difficulty of mainslip does
anything from politics to sport and thus
can- consistency in the differentiation.
taining

Evans-Pritchard, who also advocates a


not single out a specific cultural institution.
The even more prevalent definition by clear
referdistinction between magic and religion
(1965:33),
attributes the confusion conence to a dichotomy between secular and
sacred, the idea of the "set apart," is a mere
cerning their relationship to semantic difficircularity, religion being the attribution
of (1965:110). By this he means the
culties
religious significance. Recent attempts
at of translating the diverse terminolproblems
definition aim less at universal essences ogies
than,from various cultures into anthropomore modestly, at a clear designation of
the categories. But the semantic problem
logical
topic. The need to provide a ready index
for
is in
reality twofold: it consists not only of
the difficulties of translation, but also the
identification has led a number of anthropologists to resume Tylor's position that more
reli- elementary issue of what anthropologion entails a belief in spiritual beings
gists mean by their own concepts.
(Goody 1961; Horton 1960; Spiro 1966).
As a general rule, twentieth-century anConsistency in the application of Tylor's
thropologists do not dichotomize magic and

definition commits these writers to another

religion. They tend rather to hyphenate

of his concepts, that magic is to be consid-them and refer explicitly or implicitly to


some sort of continuum. However, the conered as separate from religion. Spiro thus
includes magic with politics and art as func-ception of magic and religion as overlapping
tional alternatives to religion (1966:116).
or intergrading phenomena still conveys the
Horton describes magic as "secular" (1960:
idea that the terms denote at least partially
218). This logical consequence of the defini-distinct entities. Despite the overlap, they
tion is disconcerting, for it contains what apcontrast sufficiently to indicate that the
terms have the same hierarchical status in a

Accepted for publication 20 November 1969.

classification of belief systems.


1349

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

1350 American Anthropologist [72, 1970


The consensus on some distinction becentrism assigned magic to an earlier stage
tween magic and religion is limited to
in the
the evolution of belief systems. Because
accepted Tylor's definition of religion,
idea that there is a distinction. Beyond he
this,
a review of the literature discloses irreconpersonification served to differentiate becilable disagreements among theorists, tween
as
the earlier and later stages. Unlike rewell as inconsistencies within discussions
ligion, magical belief lacks personification,

and its practices exploit supernatural mechaand discrepancies between conceptual


nisms (Frazer 1900:48-60). These two
schemes and ethnographic data. The source
characteristics-impersonal forces and maof the difficulty may well lie in the distinction itself. If the idea that magic and nipulative
relitechniques-remain as current
critical diagnostic features.
gion have equal categorical status is rejected
and Malinowski each added
and magic subordinated within the more Durkheim
inanother
dimension
to the definition of
clusive category, it follows that the distinc-

magic.
tion makes a part into an equivalent
and Durkheim based his distinction o
theof
organization of personnel: religion in
separate whole. This mixing of levels
classification would account for the continuvolves the community and establishes a

ing confusion of which Evans-Pritchard sochurch, but magic concerns individuals and
justly complains. Anthropologists have been
forms only a clientele (1915:43-45). Malistruggling with the spurious, and therefore
nowski focused on the purposes of ritual. H
insoluble, problem of the extent and content
attributed intangible, long-term goals to reli
gious rituals in contrast to the employmen
of the contrast between religion and magic.

of magic to supplement inadequacies in


The real problem is what is the place of

magic in religion; how does the part fit into


technology for the achievement of concrete
immediate ends (1948:67-70).

the whole?

Like most ethnological terminology, reli- Because most supernatural belief systems
gion and magic are words in common usage.
contain both personified and nonpersonified
conceptions of supernatural power, the criteTheir employment to classify institutions of
other cultures has necessitated radical redefirion of personification only rarely permits
nition to free the terms from their cultural

definite assignment. Frazer was entirely

matrix and to eliminate inherent value judg- aware of this fact, which he accounted for

ments. To what extent all ethnocentrism has

as part of the evolutionary sequence. Such

been overcome is open to question. Ward H.beliefs represented transitions from magic to
Goodenough holds that anthropological us- religion or the conservative retention of
age still expresses ethnocentric rather thanolder elements into the later stage. But with-

objective judgment (1963:480). The fact


out the explanation provided by an evolu-

that current usage continues to reflect theotionary doctrine, the intermediate is merely
indeterminate, and much of the ethnories of nineteenth-century evolutionists supports his charge. The delineation of magic as
graphic data demonstrate the inadequacy of
a distinct anthropological category is morethe criterion.
the work of Frazer than of any other single The differentiation made by Durkheim received one of its most direct attacks from
theorist. By redefining religion as belief in
spiritual beings, Tylor had made it possibleWarner on the basis of his fieldwork in
to include a wide variety of belief systems in
Australia (1937:229-243). Among the
Murngin, the same totemic rituals are used
the same category as Christianity. Magic,
however, he discussed merely as a survival;for individual therapy as for communal welthus relegated, the term retained its pejora-fare; the community is directly or indirectly
tive connotation (Tylor 1889: Vol. 1, Ch. 4).participant in transactions between the maFrazer, however, related magic more closely
gician and client, and thus magic has a
to religion, although an underlying ethno-church no different from religion. Other de-

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

HAMMOND] Magic: A Problem in Semantics 1351


on the conception
scriptive materials bear out Warner's
point of supernatural power.
described
that the line between public and Benedict
private
is religion as entailing two
polar attitudes, one of which might predomiextremely tenuous.
a given instance, but both might be
Malinowski's contrast between nate
thein practiexpressed
simultaneously.
These attitudes
cal goals of magical ritual and the
broad
soBenedict termed
cial values fostered by religious ritual
seems"animism," Tylor's concept, and
adapted from Marto hinge more on terminology than
on"animatism,"
subett.aMarett
had coined "animatism" to desigstance. The difference seems to be
stylistic
a primitive diffuse supernaturalism
choice of concrete or abstractnate
phrasing.

from which
animism and mana had both
Trobriand garden magic for a plentiful
harevolved (1914:14).
In Benedict's usage, anivest contributes not only to subsistence
but
also to those values to Trobriand ethos exmism reflects "man's experience with persons" and animatism reflects their "experipressed in the production of excessive
amounts of food and its lavish display.ence
The with things" (1938:647). Practice folanalysis of what social values mean in lows
spe- concept: where the supernatural is perritual strives for rapport; where the
cific terms often reveals that the "goodsonified,
life"
is impersonal, ritual strives for
of a people entails, or is symbolizedsupernatural
by,

control.
Mana and its analogues-wakan,
good crops or increase in cattle herds
or

manitu, and orenda--conceptualize the imsome equally material good. And it is more
supernatural. Magic falls neatly
the symbolic significance of these itemspersonal
than
the technological uncertainty that tends
into
toplace as the body of formalized procesurround them with ritual (W. H. Gooddures by which such power is controlled and

enough 1963:478; Nadel 1957:193).

utilized.

Goode, like Benedict, dismisses dichotWhen applied to ethnographic data, none


of these theories completely differentiates omy in favor of a continuum. Unlike Benemagic from religion even in regard to the se-dict, he does not bypass the different criteria
lected critical trait. Nor do they correlatesuggested by various theorists but boldly atwith one another; the increase ceremonies tempts to overcome the contradictions

of the Arunta are classed as religion by within and among the separate schemes by
Durkheim but magic by Frazer, and Tro-combining them all into a single structure.
briand garden rituals, which MalinowskiHis suggested bipolar continuum opposes

terms magic, would be religion according toideal types of magic and religion in which
Durkheim. If more features are taken into
magic contrasts with religion in the concrete
account, the line blurs all the more.
specificity of its goals, its manipulative attiMarett is probably the best-known early
tude, instrumental character, belief in impertheorist to advance the concept of a magisonal power, professional-client relationcoreligious continuum (1914:28). He conships, individualism, voluntarism, and un-

curred in Frazer's concept of magic asscheduled


an
performance (1951:52-55).
earlier stage of religion, but his own judg-Goode's model rests on the expectation

ment that belief in mana was as much reli-

that on analysis the descriptive data will

gion as animism led him to reject personifiform significant clusters toward the two
cation as a main criterion and to reject
poles.
a If the traits do not exhibit substantial
clear-cut distinction between magic and relicorrelation, the ideal types are merely hypothetical constructs. There seems to have
gion.
been little testing of the actual extent of corBenedict, far more than Marett, cogently
relation on which the relevance of Goode's
formulated the idea that magic and religion

form a continuum. She disregarded evolutionary sequence and put aside most of the
discrepant subsidiary criteria to concentrate

model depends. Notable exceptions, how-

ever, come readily to mind. Australian in-

crease ceremonies are manipulative but

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

1352 American Anthropologist [72, 1970

of Trobriand
primitive religion
magic clearly has cog
communal and obligatory.
garden
tivespecific
value. Benedict,
like Marett, relied o
ritual is instrumental with
concrete
mana or mana-like
concepts to explicate
goals, but it is also obligatory,
communal,
implicit
magic.
and calendrical. Among world
the view
Zuni,
theingods
critics
of this interpretation ha
may be manipulated ratherThe
than
supplicated.
pointed
out its major flaw.
Malinowsk
Bunyoro magic horns used
instrumentally
in
behalf of a client become
"personalized"
surely
goes too far in his dismissal of m
(Beattie 1967:229).
concepts as part of religion at all (194
Of the two continuum models, Benedict's58). Norbeck, however, justly contends t

has had wider influence. Her comprehensivemany ethnographic accounts of magic


and economical theory, Norbeck (1961:35)scribe no such concept nor indicate that t
notes, provides the conceptual basis implicitpractitioners of magic entertain any idea
in the analyses of many ethnologists. A sam-this kind (1963:510). Apparent logical n
pling of the literature confirms Norbeck's cessity or desire for a neat theory are insuf
comment. There is some shying away fromcient grounds for imputing its covert pr
the term "supernatural"; "nonempirical" is ence.
substituted as though "empirical" were any Norbeck (1961:49), therefore, su
less a category of the observer than "natu- that religion includes three power c
ral." An occasional obeisance is made to
personified power, impersonal power
other variables, but discussions largelymana,
tend
and "a conception of supernat
to follow Benedict's emphasis on concepficiency or power inherent in certai
tions of supernatural power and the modes
chanical cause and effect sequence
of access to it.
short, although some magical practic
In much of the writing the terms most
be related to mana, a large residue a
frequently associated with magic are "imperas unexplained automatic efficacy. Ma
sonal," "mechanical," "automatic," "comploits a seeming cause-and-effect s

pulsive," "coercive," "efficient," "learned without any accounting for either th


techniques," and "acquired skills." Such as-of the cause or the nexus between them.
sociations indicate that usage gives more Some anthropologists take this quality of beweight to the employment of power than to ing inexplicable to be the very essence of
the nature of the power. This emphasis what constitutes magic. It is a "mysterious
seems a natural corollary of the shifts in an- art" (Bidney 1963:505); magic lacks theory
thropological interest. Nineteenth-century (Howells 1948:49); magic has no cosmotheorists such as Tylor and Frazer were pri-logical content (Middleton 1967:ix); magic
marily concerned with the cognitive func- specifically refers to that which cannot be

tion of religion. For a considerable timemade orderly and comprehensible (Galtier

thereafter, attention turned to sociological 1963:506).


and affective functions. Discussions of these
If magical systems are without rationale
subjects could refer to the proceduresorof
meaning, it is their pervasiveness and permagic without much concern for the consistence that are truly inexplicable. Magic is
ception of its power.
presumed to have only expressive functions
More recently, however, reflecting general
and an attributed instrumental utility. But it
intellectual interest in problems of meaning,
is most unlikely that magic can have these
anthropologists are again turning to values
the for human beings unless they somestudy of how men create a meaningful order
how perceive its relevance to their concepout of their experiences in a chancy andtion
in- of reality. Otherwise, magic is utterly
different universe: "attempts to provide anomalous-a
orisymbol without a referent, a
entation for an organism which cannot live
projection without a source.
in a world it is unable to understand"

There are substantial data to show that

(Geertz 1968:314). In Benedict's analysis


such a position is hardly tenable. The fairly

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

HAMMOND] Magic: A Problem in Semantics 1353


when men
magic . . . that results are
frequent attribution of all deaths
andmake
major
be expected"
(Beattie 1964:206). Magic
misfortunes to witchcraft andtosorcery,
for
"iscognition
never lookedin
upon as one of the forces
example, patently demonstrates
nature which
can be utilized by man, but
magic. Witchcraft and sorceryofreflect
social
regarded as and
a cherished cultural possesconflict and obviously expressishostility

resentment of loss. Rituals are used as in-

sion which derives its powers from man's

abstinence
and from his knowledge of tradistruments of prevention and vengeance,
but

tion" (Evans-Pritchard 1967:4). The power


they also express a cosmology. The cosmos
is held to be benign or neutral; evil derives
that makes magic effective is a projection of
from the character of human beings.
Nocapacity to act effectively by means of
man's
matter how elliptically phrased, it is ahis
seriknowledge and skill.
ous statement about the nature and locus of
Analysis of myths for the meaning of
evil.
magic also seems to have encountered some
Primitive societies rarely produce their
anthropological resistance. It is true, as Naown theologians. Usually the task ofdel
ab-remarks, that there are magical systems
stracting conceptual content from symbolic
without associated myths (1957:192); but
this
expression in myth and ritual devolves
onsometimes can also be said of other

ethnologists who have tended to concentrate


kinds of rituals. According to Evans-Pritc

on myth. Kluckhohn (1965:147) suggests


ard, the relatively few Zande myths abou
that the preference reflects the importance
magic do not account for its power (19
of statements of belief in Western religions.
10-12). Although Trobriand mythology

Perhaps it has seemed easier to move from


much richer, Malinowski emphasized that
one set of words-the myth-to anothertoo
setlacks such explanation (1948:117).
of words-a statement of its meaning-than
There are, however, myths elsewhere that do
to translate rituals into statements. That
have bearing on what magic signifies. In
which is communicated by a gesture isWinnebago
often
mythology Hare achieves a
sense of identity with his human kinsmen;
With difficulty or not, a reasonably
close
with
the acquisition of arrows, a bow, and
reading of the anthropological literature
dis- to use them he acquires the symthe skill
bolic essence of human culture. And thus, as
closes that magic ritual does indeed commuhard to put into words.

nicate its meaning. The gesture is underthe exemplar of what is truly human, he

stood, but the commenters seem to resist

proceeds to make curing rituals (Radin

recognizing their own insight. Those who1948).

deny cognition in magic seem not to be lis- It is hard to see how a myth could more
tening to what they themselves say. Howells,plainly state its meaning than the Nupe myth
for example, in one passage finds no ade-that tells of a man who once attempted to
quate explanation for the potency of magic seek out god. His long arduous quest was
(1948:49-50); elsewhere he writes that
fruitless, and he returned to report that god
was both distant and indifferent. Men, theremagic gives man the "conviction that he can

take care of himself. . . . Man can put fore,


his must help themselves by learning how
best foot forward with magic; he can act to
on make "medicine" (Nadel 1954:17-18).
his own behalf. . ." (1948:63).
The reluctance to find the rituals and myths
Statement after statement can be found
of magic meaningful might hint that anthrothat point to the central concept in magic.
pologists too share the attitude Malinowski
"In magic man depends on his own strength
(1948:51) describes: ".'Magic' seems to stir
." (Frazer 1900:711). "Magic is the one
up in everyone some hidden mental forces,
and only specific power, a force unique of
some lingering hopes in the miraculous,
its kind, residing exclusively in man..."
some dormant beliefs in man's mysterious
(Malinowski 1948:56). "Magic is not
possibilities."
thought to take place by itself .... It is only These human potentialities are not so

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

1354 American Anthropologist [72, 1970


Erwin R. Goodenough,
discussing the
mysterious. Norbeck (1961:50)
suggests
persistence
of magical practices
in Judaism,
that magic has its mundane
analogue
in
writes that
religious
technology. To a certain extent
this
is symbols
obvi-"originally of
direct
inherent
. . . have gradously true, and yet it seems
to and
miss
the power
main
point. Horton (1968:66) argues
ually been that
made to a
refer
techin addition to more
spiritual
(1953:160). The
nological prototype tends remote
to give
riseforces"
to exof the adjective
"remote" is highly sugplanatory models that are use
secular
or sciengestive. this
In small-scale
societies, effective actific in character. Although
sequence
tion isit
performed
by the the
self and by familiar
need not be universally true,
supports
others. In large-scale
societies,
idea that magic and technology
per se
are the exercise
of power by
authorities or even unnot closely linked. Magic seems
todistant
concern

may provide
techniques far less thanknown
the agents
user's
skill,the source for

the projection
of "remote
spiritual forces."
knowledge, or talents. Leach's
comment
that
the immediacies
are never wholly supermark. But "libido" in theBut
strictly
psycho-

seded,closer
and even those
religions that place
tency" (1967:98) is perhaps
to the

moststrictly
weight on a psychotranscendent omnipotent
the mark. But "libido" in the
expressing
the projecanalytic sense-and judgingdeity
byretain
thepractices
tenor
of

tion of the means


immediate.
his article that is how Leach
it-Spiro,
who
defines religion
scarcely refers directly to man's capacity
for narrowly as

effective action on the basis


belief
ofinculturally
superhuman beings,
ac-argues that the
dependency
on adults "develops imquired skills. Levi-Strauss child's
plainly
considers

powerful action,
figures" and that these
magic to be a projection ages
of of
human
"deep-seated perceptive
sets, acquired in the
but he does not make entirely
clear the
very early
experience of the
child, comprise
grounds for his interpretation.
Moreover,
his

essential
basis for the actor's conviction
analysis seems to involvethe
an
unnecessarily
that the beliefs
comprise the religious
complex sequence of processes
ofwhich
projectradition of his society
are true" (1964:
tion, introjection, and reprojection
(1966:
220-221).
113). On the same basis it can be argued

On the whole, it seems both economical that the validity of magical belief is equally

rooted in the child's experience. The child


and reasonable to reinterpret Norbeck's
triad of power concepts on the basic asquite early discovers his own power to sumsumption that these are best understood asmon and dismiss, to grasp and release, to
projective symbols. The power of spiritualbuild and destroy. Maturation and enculturation further his sense of mastery, and this
beings reflects the authority of other people.
(Whether this is the authority of the clan experience
as
will be repeated every day of his
Durkheim had it or that of the father in the
effectively functioning life. Magic is no mere
Freudian view is here immaterial.) Magical
compensatory fantasy to palliate the inadepower reflects the capabilities of the self,
quacies of primitive technologies. The simand mana, the dynamic forces of the physi-pler hand technologies would more likely in-

cal universe.

duce a greater sense of mastery than maMan's ability to acquire skills and use chine technology, no matter how productive

them to shape his world is thus projected asand efficient. Witness the satisfaction in
one of the universal superordinate powers. building a fire, even with the help of a bo
The traditional hostility between deistic be-of matches and the Sunday paper, compare
lief and magical belief is understandable. An to resetting the thermostat. Moreover, it
ideology that holds man to be wholly depen-not the incompetents of a society who are
dent on powerful gods must view a concep- granted magical power.
tion of man as one of the ruling forces in
The awareness of the power of the self i
the cosmos as hubris of a most literal and
by no means necessarily limited to master
damnable kind.

over technological processes and materia

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

HAMMOND] Magic: A Problem in Semantics 1355


sion of magic
into religion. The composite
objects. Although it is the dependency
of the
ritual statesfigthat the gods do not rule alone;
child that "develops images of powerful
ures," this need not develop an accompanytheir will and power are accompanied by the
ing feeling of utter helplessness. will
The
andbeloved
power of men.
Examination
child in almost any society may well
have of
a the concept indicates that
distinction between magic and religion,
keen sense of his own ability to the
manipulate
adults. In the give-and-take of intimate
assowhether phrased
as dichotomy or polarity, is
unwarranted.
Magic is not an entity distinct
ciation those in subordinate positions
are not
religion
but a form of ritual behavior
without means to gain compliancefrom
with
their
desires from their superiors. And
andthose
thus an in
element of religion. That the
positions of authority are obviously
ablehas
toled only to confusion supdistinction
control the actions of other people.
ports the judgment that the abstraction is
One must also recognize that based
beliefs
in
on misinterpretation.
It has not only
falsified
the relation
impersonal power, such as mana,
have
an between religion and
equally early and significant base
magic, and
in led
the
to obscurantism concerning
child's experience with pressure,
motion,
magic,
but it has also given rise to a trunheat, light, and all the other manifestations
cated concept of religion as a whole. The
of a dynamic universe. There has
beenconcepts
no
cosmological
inherent in religions
attempt in this paper to analyze are
mana
con- than belief in spiritual bemore complex
cepts. It has, however, been assumed
thatthe index of "belief in suings only. Because
the existence of impersonal power
is one
of recognizes only one asperhuman
beings"
the fundamental postulates of pect
many
of religion,
reliit seems preferable to subgions.
stitute the definition that religion entails beThe position that religious cosmology is
lief in superordinate agencies.
comprised of three equally legitimate conREFERENCES CITED
cepts of superordinate power makes the realBEATTIE, JOHN
ity of magic as a separate entity altogether
1964 Other cultures. New York: Free Press.
doubtful. The potency symbolized in magi1967 Divination in Bunyoro, Uganda. In
Magic, witchcraft, and curing. John Midcal practices is interwoven with the other
dleton, ed. Garden City: Natural History
powers. It can be used to exploit mana or
constrain the gods. Conversely, such power
may be the gift of a supernatural being. It
seems appropriate then to limit the range of
meaning of the term-to restrict "magic" to
the designation of mechanical religious practices. This is, in effect, the way many anthropologists actually use the term, although

they do so without any explicit rationale.


Magic would serve as a denotation for one
type of ritual behavior, and thus belong to

the set of terms in that category, such as


prayer or sacrifice. With these propitiatory
rites it is contrastive, not with religion.
Just as prayer and sacrifice express the belief that the universe is governed by personi-

fied beings whom one can reach by words


and gifts, so magical practices express the
belief in human powers as effective forces.
The use of such practices in rituals addressed to the gods is not a discordant intru-

Press.

BENEDICT, RUTH

1938 Religion. In General anthropology.

Franz Boas, ed. Boston: Heath.


BIDNEY, DAVID

1963 Comment on Murray and Rosalie

Wax, the notion of magic. Current Anthro-

pology 4:495-518.

DURKHEIM, ]MILE

1915 The elementary forms of the religious

life. Joseph Ward Swain, London: Allen


and Unwin. [original ed. 1912.]

EVANS-PRITCHARD, E. E.

1965 Theories of primitive religion. London: Oxford University Press.


1967 The morphology and function of
magic: a comparative study of Trobriand
and Zande ritual and spells. In Magic,
witchcraft, and curing. John Middleton,

ed. Garden City: The Natural History

Press.

FRAZER, JAMES G.

1900 The golden bough. 2nd ed. London:


Macmillan Co. [References are to 1922
abridgment.]

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

1356 American Anthropologist [72, 1970]


GALTIER, CHARLES

versity of Chicago Press.

MALINOWSKI,
BRONISLAW
1963 Comment on Murray
and
Rosalie

Magic, science,
and religion. Glencoe:
Wax, the notion of magic. 1948
Current
Anthro-

pology 4:495-518.

GEERTZ, CLIFFORD

The Free Press.

MARETT, R. R.

1914
The analysis
threshold ofof
religion. London:
1968 Ethos, world-view and
the
sacred symbols. In Every Methuen.
man his way.
MIDDLETON,
JOHN PrenAlan Dundes, ed. Englewood
Cliffs:
tice-Hall.
1967 Introduction. In Magic, witchcraft, and
GOODE, WILLIAM J.
curing. John Middleton, ed. Garden City:
1951 Religion among the primitives. Glen- The Natural History Press.
coe: The Free Press.

NADEL, S. F.

1954 Nupe religion. London: Routledge and


Kegan Paul.
1953 Jewish symbols in the Greco-Roman
1957 Malinowski on magic and religion. In
period. Vol. 2. Bollinger Series 37. New

GOODENOUGH, ERWIN R.
York: Pantheon Books.

GOODENOUGH, WARD HUNT

Man and culture. Raymond Firth, ed.

New York: Humanities Press.

NORBECK, EDWARD
1963 Cooperation in change. New York:

Russell Sage Foundation.

GOODY, JACK

1961 Religion in primitive society. New


York: Harper.

1963 Comment on Murray and Rosalie


1961 Religion and ritual: the definition

problem. British Journal of Sociology 12:Wax, the notion of magic. Current An142-164.
thropology 4:495-518.

HORTON, ROBIN

RADIN, PAUL

1948 Winnebago hero cycles. Indiana Pub1960 A definition of religion and its uses.
lications in Anthropology and Linguistics,
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Insti:ute 90:201-220.
Memoir 1. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
1968 Ritual man in Africa. In Readings in
SPIRO, MELFORD
anthropology. Morton Fried, ed. New
York: Crowell.
1964 Religion and the irrational. In SymHOWELLS, WILLIAM
posium on new approaches to the study of
religion. Proceedings of the American Eth1948 The heathens. Garden City: Doubleday.
nological Society. Seattle: University of

KLUCKHOHN, CLYDE

1965 Myths and rituals: a general theory.


In Reader in comparative religion. William A. Lessa and Evon Z. Vogt, eds. New
York: Harper and Row.

LEACH, EDMUND R.

1967 Magical hair. In Myth and cosmos.


John Middleton, ed. Garden City: The
Natural History Press.

LIVI-STRAUSS, CLAUDE

1966 The savage mind. Chicago: The Uni-

Washington Press.

1966 Religion: problems of definition and


explanation. In Anthropological approaches
to the study of religion. A.S.A. Monographs 3:85-126. London: Tavistock.

TYLOR, EDWARD B.

1889 Primitive culture. 2nd ed. New York:

Holt. [First ed. 1871.]


WARNER, W. LLOYD

1937 A black civilization. New York: Harper.

This content downloaded from 83.137.211.198 on Thu, 10 Nov 2016 14:51:18 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like