Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/17402378

Social learning and Piagets


cognitive theory of moral
development

Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology April 1969


DOI: 10.1037/h0027000 Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

64 465

4 authors, including:

Philip A Cowan
University of California, Berkeley
121 PUBLICATIONS 4,830 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Philip A Cowan on 14 November 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal ol Personality and Social Psychology
1969, Vol. 11, No. 3, 261-274

SOCIAL LEARNING AND PIAGET'S COGNITIVE


THEORY OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT
PHILIP A. COWAN,1 JONAS LANGER, JUDITH HEAVENRICH, AND MARJORIE NATHANSON
University of California, Berkeley

The present study provides a replication, with extended analyses, of a previous


study of moral judgment by Bandura and McDonald. In a pretest -using
Piaget-type pairs of moral judgment stories, 77 5- to 12-year-olds indicated
which of two children in the stories was naughtier (names) and then justified
their judgments (explanations). Thirty-two of these subjects, half with a ma-
jority of high level judgments and half with a majority of low, were exposed
to an adult model responding at the opposite level. A posttest with six repeated
items, six new items, and six somewhat unusual items was given either im-
mediately or 2 weeks after the modeling ("conditioning") session. Bandura and
McDonald's results were replicated with additional support, but it was found
that the extent of the modeling effects depended upon a number of variables
either singly or in interaction: subjects' pretest scores, the particular measure
(names versus explanations), time between conditioning and posttest, type of
item, and direction of conditioning (up or down). A theoretical analysis showed
that neither the present study nor that of Bandura and McDonald could be used
directly to affirm or deny Piaget's hypotheses. Most of the present results serve
as a basis for more differentiated statements concerning the models' effects, but
some of the findings raise questions which cannot yet be answered within the
social learning framework.

According to social learning theory (cf. empirically. In the article itself the authors
Bandura & Walters, 1963), the child's acquisi- were somewhat cautious about the extent to
tion of adult moral standards is, to a consider- which their findings posed serious problems
able extent, a gradual process of imitating the for Piaget's theory. However, a discussion of
observable values and behavior of others. the study in a more general presentation of
Piaget (1932), on the other hand, has main- social learning theory (Bandura & Walters
tained that the development of the child's 1963) indicates that they considered the re-
moral judgments goes through a necessary sults to have important negative implications:
sequence of stages in which the child must re- "The so-called developmental stages were
solve a moral dilemma by progressively con- readily altered by the provision of adult
structing his own ethical position. These con- models [p. 209, italics added]." "Experi-
trasting views were discussed by Bandura mental manipulation based on a social learn-
and McDonald (1963) in a report of a study ing paradigm, revealed that the develop-
which had two aims. Their primary goal was mental sequence proposed by Piaget is by no
to demonstrate "that children's moral orienta- means predetermined or invariant [p. 207]."
tions can be altered and even reversed by the Since Piaget's theory rests fundamentally on
manipulation of response-reinforcement con- the notion of invariant sequence, Bandura
tingencies and 'by the provision of appropriate and Walter's claim almost implies a refutation
social models [p. 275]." Their second im- of Piaget's theory, at least in the area of moral
plicit aim was to question the adequacy of development,
Piaget's theory by showing that "moral judg- The present paper deals with both theoreti-
ment responses are less age specific than im- cal and empirical issues raised by the Bandura
plied by Piaget [p. 27S]" and that Piaget's and McDonald experiment and by the way it
theory of demarcated sequential stages of was discussed. On the theoretical side we will
moral development could not be substantiated argue that Bandura and McDonald's interpre-
1 tation of Piaget is open to question, and that
Requests for reprints should be sent to Philip A.
Cowan, Department of Psychology, University of their data have limited relevance to Piaget's
California, Berkeley, California 94720. position. On the empirical side we will treat
261
262 P. A. COWAN, J. LANGER, J. HEAVKNRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

their study within the framework of social respond at a level of moral judgment (name
learning theory, replicating and extending the and explanation) opposite to that of the
experiment in order to clarify their results child's, so that high subjects were to be "con-
and interpretations. After a brief description ditioned" down while low subjects were to be
of Bandura and McDonald's experiment, some conditioned up. The other third of the high
specific theoretical and empirical issues will and low subjects were verbally reinforced by
be set forth in detail. the experimenter for responses opposite to
their general moral orientation but no model
Bandura and McDonald's Experiment was provided. After the experimental phase
A group of 12 pairs of stories was read to each child was taken to another room where
a large number of 5- to 11-year-old boys and still a third experimenter read 12 more pairs
girls. Each story was about a child whose ac- of stories, 6 of which were new and 6 of which
tions'result in undesirable consequences. In had been given in the pretest.
each pair of stories, one child was intention- In all trials, the subject was asked to name
ally "doing something naughty," but the con- the child in the story who was naughtier
sequence was small (e.g., he was stealing a (names) and to state the reason for his choice
cookie and broke 1 cup), while the other child (explanations). Only the names data were
was either trying to be helpful or was in- reported on the grounds that there was a
volved in an accident but a large negative very high correlation between the level of
consequence resulted (in opening a door he names and explanations responses during the
knocked over a tray and 15 cups were pretest phase.
broken). Half of the pairs of stories were The pretest results showed a gradual in-
taken with slight modification from Piaget crease in the average number of high level
(1948) and the others were new. On the basis moral responses as a function of age. Both
of pretest performance, subjects were chosen high and low subjects exposed to models
who consistently responded with apparently showed a substantial (and approximately
lower level moral judgments (low subjects,2 equal) change in responses in the direction
judging on the basis of larger negative con- opposed to their initial moral judgment level.
sequences) or who consistently responded with This change held up during the immediately
apparently high level moral judgments (high subsequent posttest when the model was not
subjects, judging on the basis of good in- present. Subjects without models showed no
tent). We refer to "apparent" levels of moral significant changes from pretest to treatment
judgment because there is some question to posttest.
whether Piaget would agree with Bandura Bandura and McDonald (1963) concluded
and McDonald's criteria. that the results "fail to substantiate Piaget's
Two weeks after the pretest, with a differ- theory of demarcated sequential stages of
ent experimenter, two-thirds of the high and moral development" and that the "utility of
low subjects were introduced to an adult who Piaget's stage theory of morality is further
was also going to be tested on the stories. limited by the finding that children's judg-
This time 24 pairs of stories were read al- mental responses are readily modifiable, par-
ternately, 12 to the adult who acted as model ticularly through the utilization of adult
and 12 to the child. The model's task was to modeling cues [p. 280]." The authors pointed
to the powerful influence of social models, but
2
Bandura and McDonald use Piaget's terms "ob- did not clearly indicate whether they assumed
jective moral orientation" and "subjective moral that social modeling is either a necessary or
orientation" to describe subjects at lower and higher a sufficient condition for the acquisition and
developmental stages, respectively. We have found
that most readers expect an "objective" stage to modification of moral judgment responses.
come after a subjective one. Since we will be dis-
cussing both ascending and descending changes in Theoretical Issues
stage, it seems that clarity would be facilitated by The theoretical issues raised by the Ban-
relabeling the orientations low and high although
this implies more of an absolute dichotomy than dura and McDonald study center around the
either Piaget or Bandura and McDonald intended. definition and measurement of stages. Ban-
SOCIAL LEARNING AND PLACET'S THEORY OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT 263

dura and McDonald interpreted Piaget's stage tions. Also, Piaget was concerned with a num-
theory as implying that moral judgment ber of aspects of moral judgments, not only
stages are (1) age specific, and (2) clear-cut, the intention-consequences dimension.
and that they follow a (3) predetermined, (4) 3. Predeterminism, It is not clear what
necessary sequence. Bandura and McDonald meant when they
1. Age specific. Piaget has never argued said that Piaget's theory refers to a "prede-
that his stages are age specific. The ages he termined" sequence of stages. In one state-
ment they apparently minimized Piaget's em-
mentions are those at which 75% of the sub-
jects pass a given criterion. Data, then, are phasis on social interaction. "Typically the
discussed in terms of numbers of subjects emergence of these presumably age specific
modes of behavior is attributed to ontogenetic
passing, not number of items passed. Ob-
viously, some subjects acquire high level factors rather than to specific social stimulus
moral judgments before the given age and events [p. 274]." In another paraphrase of
Piaget's position, they stated, "As the child
even more may do so afterwards. Bandura
and McDonald's provision of a cross-sectional matureshe gains increasing autonomy, his
growth curve of average number of responses, relations become based upon mutual rec-
iprocity and cooperation giving rise to the
in which the longitudinal discontinuities may
cancel each other out, is simply not relevant emergence of subjective morality [p. 275]."
either to Piaget's discussion of the ages at This latter statement correctly implies that
which stages emerge, or to his discussion of Piaget considers social interaction to be an
continuity and discontinuity in development important factor in the change from lower to
higher levels. He is not, then, positing a pre-
(cf. Piaget, 1950).
2. Clear-cut stages. Bandura and Mc- determined developmental force in the sense
Donald's stories were pretested to remove any of a maturation theory like that of Gesell.
irrelevant cues which might lead subjects to However, for Piaget it is not the opportunity
to copy models which is the important part
judge the depicted actions of children in
terms other than intentions or consequences. of social interaction. Rather, it is the fact
that interaction forces the adoption of more
The interviews were constructed to pull
than one point of view. Thus, social interac-
strongly for a choice of either alternative both
in names and explanations. Bandura and Mc- tion provides an impetus for disturbing pres-
Donald, then, have constructed a situation to ent cognitive organization, leading to a state
elicit clear-cut responses, but Piaget specif- of disequilibration, and ultimately resulting in
ically disclaimed the idea that any given child a new level of cognitive organization. This
new level follows both logically and psy-
will consistently perform only at one clear-cut
chologically from the old and only in that
stage. Using the orally presented stories with
children between 8 and 10, Piaget (1948) sense is it predetermined.
One of the most cogent arguments against
concluded,
social imitation as the prime variable in the
Though we could not point to any stages properly learning of moral judgments is the fact that
so called which followed one another in necessary lower-level judgments predominate at earlier
order, we were able to define processes whose final
terms were quite distinct from one another. These ages. This predominance occurs in spite of
processes might mingle and overlap more or less in the fact that adults presumably do not pro-
the life of each child, but they marked nevertheless vide pervasive models of, or reinforcement for,
the broad divisions of moral development [p. 175]. lower level responses. If it is to be argued
Piaget then went on to search for more clear- that adults do in fact model and reinforce
cut distinctions by observing the behavior of low level judgments then it is difficult to
children much younger than the 8- to 10-year- explain the observed change in level of moral
olds he had interviewed. He found that in judgment with age; that is, social learning
natural settings the younger children more cannot have it both ways, at least not without
consistently acted on the basis of the con- a much more complex explanation of modeling
sequences of their own and others actions and effects.
disregarded the intention behind these ac- 4. Necessary sequence of stages. The crucial
264 P. A. COWAN, J. LANCER, J. HEAVENRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

question raised by Bandura and McDonald's countersuggestions is part of the technique


experiment is whether they provide an ade- used to assess the child's level; for example,
quate demonstration of a reversal in the "nec- after a child gives an answer, Piaget may
essary sequence" of stages of moral develop- say "Yes, but the child who was here before
ment. We must also keep in mind that the gave quite a different answer," and then go
possibility of reversing a sequence in a labora- on to elaborate it. We are not presenting the
tory does not necessarily disprove the uni- circular argument that if modeling had an
versality of the sequence under natural con- effect, the children were not really "at" a
ditions of life. Bandura and McDonald's pre- given level, and if it did not have an effect,
test data did support the idea that high level then social learning theory is wrong. Rather,
moral judgments are more often found in we wish to indicate that Piaget and social
older children. learning theory may sometimes be dealing
Piaget's general theory of development with different phenomena, measured in quite
claims to describe changes in stages of cogni- different ways.
tive organization. Social learning theory tends The above considerations indicate that
to be skeptical about the notion of stages, Bandura and McDonald have not always
preferring to discuss or measure general classes stated Piaget's position accurately and have
of stimulus-related behavior. For Bandura not provided definitive procedures to assess
and McDonald, the classification of a child stages or changes in stages of moral develop-
as having a particular moral orientation is ment.8 In the present study, we have accepted
based upon the relative number of name re- most of Bandura and McDonald's procedures
sponses which he gives at a low or high level. in order to replicate and clarify their findings
In their experiment the average number of within their own framework whenever pos-
high responses for the low subjects in the pre- sible. Therefore, our experiment will also
test was approximately 2-3 out of 12 (and have limited applications to the moral stage
vice versa for the high subjects). This means phenomena discussed by Piaget.
that most of their subjects were not pure In addition to the methodological issues,
cases; they tended to be in transition. Con- one aspect of Bandura and McDonald's em-
sequently, Piaget, as well as social learning pirical findings raises further questions con-
theory, would expect these children to change cerning their claim to have altered the sub-
in either direction as a result of environmental ject's developmental stage. The authors failed
pressure. to comment on the finding that subjects
Piaget makes a distinction between re- changed from a 20% level of response in
sponses to tests and the general organization the pretest to approximately 50% in the con-
or structure of reasoning underlying the re- ditioning and posttest periods (see our Figure
sponses. The child's explanation of his moral 2 for the relevant data from their study).
judgment concerning who is naughtier is just While this clearly represents a change in
as important as the choice itself. In Bandura moral judgment responses, a SO-SO split does
and McDonald's study the data concerning not clearly indicate that a new stage rather
explanations were obtained in all phases of than confusion or conformity has been
the study but were only reported for the pre- achieved.
test. Despite the fact that a high correlation
between names and explanations scores existed Empirical Issues
during this phase, it is possible that condi- Although Bandura and McDonald's experi-
tioning might lower this relationship in sub- ment has little direct bearing on Piaget's
sequent phases. A demonstration of change in theory, it begins to study an interesting phe-
necessary sequence of development in Piaget's nomenon. It goes beyond most social learning
terms, then, requires the use of explanations studies of the effects of models on the copying
data throughout. of simple behaviors, to an examination of so-
The manner of probing for explanations is 3
Admittedly, this is difficult to do. For the con-
also an issue in the diagnosis of stage. In troversy in another area of Piaget's work see Braine
Piaget's clinical method, resistance to adult (1959, 1964) and Smedslund (1963, 1965).
SOCIAL LEARNING AND PIAGET'S THEORY OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT 265

cial influence on the complex kinds of verbal planation would be affected by adult models in
judgments which children make. Unlike the the same way.
usual experiment in which the subject is 8. Generality of change, (a) Time between
placed in an environment exactly like that of conditioning and posttest: Since all develop-
the model, the children had to make choices mental theories accept the fact of short-term
in stories with similar form but different con- changes in response, it is incumbent upon
tent than that presented to the model. As the Bandura and McDonald to demonstrate that
first study in this field, of course, it examined their effects were long term. The posttest for
changes in a limited type of response, over a generalization was given by different experi-
limited time period, under a restricted num- menter's than those used in the conditioning
ber of experimental conditions. The present phase, but it was given in the same school im-
study raises a number of questions concern- mediately after the conditioning trials. It is
ing the effects of certain methodological vari- important to ascertain whether the effects are
ables, subject variables, and experimental maintained over time.
conditions on the adult model's influence. (b) New versus repeat items: The question
5. Ejects of objects' pretest levels. Ban- of whether the changes in moral judgments
dura and McDonald's subjects averaged about were generalized was only partly answered by
20% of their responses in the to-be-condi- Bandura and McDonald's study. We know
tioned direction during the operant (pretest) that both new and repeat items were given in
phase. Since Piaget suggested that children in that posttest but no separate analyses were
transition would be more susceptible to social presented. It is possible that children altered
influence, it is important to compare subjects their responses to repeated items but not to
close to extreme high or low responding with new ones and thus arrived at the 50% post-
subjects who are closer to the middle of the test level.
continuum. (c) Stimulus cues: On what basis did sub-
6. Conditioning phase. No trial-by-trial jects discriminate and then generalize the de-
data were presented for the conditioning phase sired response? Bandura and McDonald as-
of Bandura and McDonald's study. The num- sumed that subjects shifted from judgments
ber of responses in the conditioned direction based upon intent to judgments based upon
during the conditioning phase averaged 50%. consequences, or vice versa. However, as
The acquisition curves should be examined in Turiel (1966) has pointed out, all Bandura-
detail in order to ascertain whether the curves McDonald-Piaget items contrast good inten-
are typical learning curves. If they rise grad- tions and large negative consequences with
ually from 20% to about 80%, then the 50% bad intentions and small negative conse-
posttest scores, obtained immediately after quences. Even though the model gave an ex-
conditioning, actually represent a substantial planation along with each name choice, sub-
decline in responses in the conditioned direc- jects could have been learning a discrimination
tion. This would not indicate that models are based on amount of consequences. This pos-
ineffective, but would serve to qualify state- sibility is investigated in the present study by
ments concerning the extent to which the adding some items in which good intentions
were paired with small negative consequences
modeling effects were generalized.
and bad intentions with large ones. In this
7. Names versus explanations scores. In the case, it is obvious which child in the stories
theoretical section it was noted that only is naughtier, but the subject is given a choice
changes in "names" scores were examined. Ex- of explanations.
planations scores, in response to the question 9. Effects oj direction oj conditioning. In
"Why was naughtier?" do not fully Bandura and McDonald's study, the models'
correspond to Piaget's assessment of moral influence in conditioning subjects from low
reasoning, but they do provide another index to high was approximately equal to the effect
of level or moral judgment. Despite the high of conditioning from high to low. This result
correlation of names and explanations scores is consistent with the social learning position
in the pretest, we need to know whether ex- that moral judgment responses are habits
266 P. A. COWAN, J. LANGER, J. HEAVENRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

which can be modified in any developmental study, and one female experimenter served as a
direction. Organismic-developmental theories model during the experimental or conditioning phase. 5
such as Piaget's imply that it is possible to Stimulus Items
change responses, and possible but very dif-
The present experiment employed 48 different pairs
ficult to alter the child's stage. If changes of stories. Before the pretest the experimenter clari-
occur, they should take place more readily in fied with the subject the meaning of "naughty." After
the direction of spontaneous development than each pair of stories was read, the experimenter asked
in the reverse direction.4 In some studies using 'Which child do you think was naughtier?" (names)
and then asked, "Why?" (explanations). Six of the
Kohlberg's (1964) technique for diagnosing items (i.e., pairs of stories) were Piaget's original
stages of moral development, Turiel (1966, set as modified slightly by Bandura and McDonald.
1969) hypothesized and found that subjects They were used in the pretest and repeated in the
exposed to moral judgments in a stage di- posttest. Twenty-five items were taken directly from
the Bandura and McDonald study, 0 with eight modi-
rectly above their dominant stage changed in fled so that the material consequences in both stories
that direction more than did subjects exposed involved the same kind of objects. Eleven new but
to examples two stages above or one stage similar items were created for reasons not pertinent
below. The posttests were conducted 1 week to the present report.
after conditioning in contrast to Bandura and It has been pointed out that all Bandura and Mc-
Donald items contrasted good intentions and large
McDonald's immediate posttest. negative consequences with bad intentions and small
negative consequences. Once having made the choice
The Present Study of which child was naughtier, one is almost com-
mitted to a certain level of explanation, that is,
The experiment to be described below repre- having said that John (who broke IS cups) is
sents an attempt to replicate Bandura and naughtier, one must justify the choice on the basis
McDonald's study as closely as possible, while of quantity of damage. In order to provide some
at the same time adding some controls and alternative to this choice, six items were created in
which good intentions were paired with small nega-
some extended analyses in order to answer tive consequences and bad intentions with large ones.
the empirical questions just raised. The study For example, the child helping his father accidentally
is not a hypothesis-testing experiment in the spills a small amount of paint, while the child caus-
usual sense. It seeks to provide a more dif- ing mischief dumps a whole bucket on purpose.
These items were called "switch" items to distinguish
ferentiated statement concerning the condi- them from both the new and repeat items described
tions under which, and the extent to which, above. In these items all the children agree on which
adult models affect the responses of children child was naughtier (names), but they have a choice
to Piaget-type moral judgment stories. in the explanation of their response. They can em-
phasize either the amount or the intent. These items
were only used in the posttest in order to keep the
METHOD first part of the experiment parallel with that of
Subjects Bandura and McDonald.
Each item was separately scored for high or low
Eighty subjects, 38 girls and 42 boys, were chosen level of moral judgment on the basis of names and
randomly from the population of all children in one explanations given by the subject. In the names
Berkeley elementary school situated in a low-income scoring a high level of moral judgment was indicated
residential area. Their ages ranged from 5 years 6 by the subject's choice of the naughtier child as the
months to 12 years 6 months. Since children were one who showed "bad" intentions despite minimal
chosen in numbers proportional to the distribution consequences. This is a somewhat arbitrary pro-
of ages in the school, unequal numbers of subjects cedure both because it ignores the child's possible
were tested at each age level. The data from 3 sub- confusion after having heard 9 or 10 pairs of stories
jects were not complete; 11 subjects constituted the and because, by adult standards, the designation of
pool from which four matched subsamples were either child's intent as "bad" is often open to ques-
later drawn. tion. For example, it is not clear that a child who
drops one of his sister's books in a puddle because
Experimenters she had been mean to him is a naughty child. How-
ever, if one is forced to choose the "naughtier" child,
The same two experimenters, one female and one
5
male, served as testers through all phases of the We wish to thank Ed De Avila for serving as
one of the experimenters and Carolyn Cowan for her
4
Spontaneous development is in the direction from participation as the model.
6
low to high as both Piaget and Bandura and Mc- Bandura graciously made available to us his
Donald's Figure 1 suggest. items and his script for the model's responses.
SOCIAL LEARNING AND PIAGET'S THEORY OF MOKAL DEVELOPMENT 267

the alternative is generally clear. This raises Turiel's names scoresthe number of times they had chosen
question (1966) of the extent to which some of these the child with bad intent as naughtier rather than
items actually represent moral judgments; at times the child who caused the largest negative conse-
the moral relevance is assumed by the experimenter quences. In order to make a clearer separation be-
rather than perceived by the child, tween subjects in transition and subjects at a low
In contrast to Bandura and McDonald's experi- or high level, four groups of 8 subjects were chosen
ment where the experimenter questioned the child's instead of two groups of 16. The groups were called
explanations until a clear emphasis either on intent definitely low (0-2 high responses) tentatively low
or consequences emerged, the experimenters in the (3-5), tentatively high (7-9), and definitely high
present study tended to accept the child's explana- (10-12). It should be noted that there were only 8
tion as.long as it could be understood at all. This "pure cases," 5 subjects who responded to all 12
was decided during preliminary investigation in items at a high level and 3 who responded to them
order to avoid forcing the child's answer into our all at a low level. Because of our inability to com-
preconceived categories and to avoid the social in- plete the testing of one subject, there were only 7
fluence process during the pretest. Explanations subjects in the tentatively low subgroup. The groups
ranged from clear emphasis on consequences (he was were matched for age and were approximately equal
naughtier because he broke more cups) to justifica- in their distribution of boys and girls.
tions for the child's behavior (he did it because he Two weeks after their pretest, subjects were each
was angryan answer to why the child did it, not brought back to the experimental room. The ex-
why it was naughty), to somewhat contradictory perimenter explained that she wanted to find out
emphasis on both intent and consequences (she was what children and adults thought of the stories she
naughtier because she helped her mother and the was going to read, and so an adult (the model)
dishes broke), to clear-cut concentration on intent. would be working along with him. Twenty-four dif-
Unfortunately the greater freedom allowed for sub- ferent item pairs were read, one to the model, and
jects resulted in greater vagueness for scoring. After then one to the subject. The model responded accord-
a number of unsuccessful attempts to arrive at inter- ing to the set of answers developed by Bandura and
scorer consistency within a more refined scoring sys- McDonald. 7 If the subject was in either of the low
tem, we had to resort to the original two-category groups all the name choices and explanations of the
high-low decision. A low response was one that model were at a high moral judgment level; the re-
clearly referred to the quantity of consequences as a verse was true for the high subjects. Both model and
subject's justification for his choice, while a high subject were given verbal approval for answers in
response was any other scorable response that focused the desired direction. Since Bandura and McDonald
on the motivation or underlying explanation for the found that verbal approval accompanying modeling
behavior of the child in the story. The interrater produced no greater effects than modeling alone, it
agreement on explanation scores between two of the was used here simply to make the experience more
present authors was 89%. The interrater agreement pleasant for the child.
between two new scorers was 88%. In neither case In each group of subjects, half were given an
did the scorers have access to the name score when immediate posttest after the model left the room. In
they were examining the explanations. Despite the order to examine the maintenance of effects over time
fact that high-level explanations in this scoring sys- the other four subjects were posttested after a 2-week
tem represented a more heterogeneous collection of interval. The posttest consisted of 18 items, the 6
responses than low-level explanations, the division Piaget items from the pretest (repeat), 6 "new"
between high and low appeared to be a meaningful items, and 6 "switch" items (in which "bad" intent
one in Bandura and McDonald's framework. The and large consequences were paired).
correlation between level of names and explanations In summary, there were three phases of the study:
in pretest subjects chosen for further testing was .87 pretest, experimental ("conditioning") and posttest.
W = 29). In each phase the child responded to 12 Bandura-
We have here an illustration of good interrater McDonald-Piaget items. On the basis of their pre-
agreement and high names-explanations correlation test scores, four groups of eight subjects were chosen,
over all phases based on conceptually gross explana- tentative and definite high and low. The low sub-
tions data. The need to replicate Bandura and Mc- jects were exposed to a model giving high moral
Donald's procedure blocked the possibility that more judgment responses while the high subjects were ex-
detailed but open-ended questioning would have led posed to a model giving low responses. Half of the
to a more differentiated explanations scoring on subjects were posttested immediately after the ex-
which raters could agree. The possibility that the perimental phase and half were retested 2 weeks
more differentiated explanation would show a lower later. In the posttest, six switch items were added to
correlation with names scores in conditioning and the six repeat items and the six new ones. The loss
posttest phase will have to be examined in another
7
study. If the subject were to be conditioned up, the
model would explain her answer by contrasting the
Procedure intent of the children in each story; if subjects were
All 77 children responded to the 12 pretest items. to be conditioned down, the model contrasted the
As in the Bandura-McDonald experiment, groups for amount of negative consequences without referring
further testing were selected on the basis of their to the intent.
268 P. A. COWAN, J. LANGER, J. HEAVENRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

100 ditioning, and posttest with separate curves


for subjects conditioned up and down; Figure
2a also includes the relevant points from
so . Bandura and Me Donald
Bandura and McDonald's Figures 2 and 3
(pp. 278, 280). In, order to keep clear the
a
in
(estimated) between-study comparisons, data from the
<u switch items in the present posttest were ex-
60
cluded.
It is evident from Figure 2 a that the name
score trends from pretest to the immediate
I 40 posttest are remarkably similar to those re-
ported by Bandura and McDonald. Starting
"c
<u Present Study from a pretest low of 20% of the responses in
I 20 - the conditioned direction, subjects condition-
ing scores moved to about 60% (about 10%
higher than Bandura & McDonald) and re-
mained close to that level in the immediate
5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 posttest. The results from Figures 1 and 2a
Age indicate that we have satisfactorily replicated
the findings of Bandura and McDonald. Given
FIG. 1. Percentage of high moral judgment responses the present state of our field this represents a
as a function of age.
significant finding in itself. We can now pro-
of complete data from one up tentative subject left
ceed with a number of analyses enabling us
31 experimental subjects in all. to be more precise concerning the observed
changes in subjects' behavior.
RESULTS 8 Effect of subjects' pretest level. A four-way
Replication. It is necessary to establish analysis of variance performed on the name
that we have in fact replicated the Bandura scores (up versus down; tentative versus
and McDonald study. The percentage of high definite; immediate versus 2 weeks; pretest
moral judgment responses (names) given by versus conditioning versus posttest) indicates
all 77 subjects is shown as a function of age
in Figure 1. Also shown in this figure is an Up Immediate
-_- Up 2-week
estimated average of male and female names o Down Immediate
scores derived from Bandura and McDonald's '
90 X UpSandura & McDonald o Down 2-week
a Down Bandura 6 McDonald
Figure 1 (p. 278). Within the age range com- g 80
mon to the two studies (ages 5-10), the shape | 70
of the curves and the asymptotes were quite
I 60
similar. The pretest data indicate that the de-
velopmental characteristics of the children in 50
the two samples are comparable, although our 8 40
I
sample shows a comparative developmental V>
e 30
lag of 1-3 years at the lower age levels. It s
o1 20
should be noted that Bandura and McDonald's S
subjects were largely upper-middle-class while -c 10

the present subjects were lower-middle to _L -L. J_ _L


Pre Cond Imm 2 w k Pre Cond Imm 2wk
lower-class. Post Post Post Post
Figures 2a and 2b present the average Phase of experiment
names and explanations scores in pretest, con-
FIG. 2. Effects of a model on moral judgments as
8 a function of phase of experiment, direction of con-
Preliminary analysis revealed that there were no
significant experimenter effects on pretest scores and ditioning, and time between conditioning and posttest.
so this possible source of variance was disregarded in a. (left) Names scores.
any of the analyses described below. b. (right) Explanations scores.
SOCIAL LEARNING AND PIAGKT'S THEORY OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT 269

that there were only two statistically sig- in the to-be-conditioned direction) changed
nificant effects, both main effects: less than subjects with a pretest score of 1 or
2. An interesting curvilinear relationship is
1. Conditioning and posttest name scores present in both names and explanations
were significantly different from pretest scores scores; subjects with pretest scores closer to
(F= 54.97, dj= 2/46, p < .01). 5 out of 12 also changed less than subjects in
2. The difference between tentatives and the middle range. The greatest change in
definites, built in by selection in the choice of names scores occurred for subjects with a pre-
pretest groups, was maintained as a main test score of 3, while the greatest change in
effect during conditioning and posttest phases explanations scores occurred for subjects with
(F=U.72, df=l/23, p<.0l). Tenta- a pretest score of 1. Though it cannot be
tive subjects, already farther in the condi- determined definitely from these data, it is
tioned direction than definite subjects during . possible that pure cases were more resistant
the pretest, finished the experiment closer to to change, and that a ceiling effect reversed
the experimenter's goal. The absence of inter- the change scores at the upper end. Whatever
actions indicates that while the final level of the interpretation, the equal amount of change
responding was a function of the starting shown by tentatives and definites is an artifact
level, the amount of change shown by tenta- of the curvilinear relation between change
tive and definite subjects was approximately
and pretest level.
equal and was not affected by the direction
Conditioning phase. Before examining the
of conditioning. It will be seen below that the
same results were present in the explanations explanations scores in Figure 2b.we will take
a closer look at subjects' performance during
scores.
the conditioning phase. The learning curves
The assumption had been made that a pre- are presented in Figures 3 a (names) and 3b
test score of 0-2 out of 12 (definites) in- (explanations). The circles on the ordinate
dicated a stable low or high level, and that represent the average percentage of pretest
scores of 3-5 (tentatives) would designate responses in the to-be-conditioned moral judg-
subjects in transition. It is possible, however, ment direction. The circles to the right of the
that the specification of subjects definitely at learning curves represent percentage posttest
a low or high level should have been more judgments in the conditioned direction. In
precise. In order to examine this possibility each graph the learning data are presented in
the average pretest to posttest change in blocks of two trials, and are separated for sub-
names and explanations scores is shown in jects conditioned up or down. Subjects con-
Table 1 as a function of pretest scores. For ditioned up showed most of their learning of
subjects classified as definites it is evident that names responses within the first two trials,
the "pure cases" (subjects with no responses but gradually learned to match their explana-

TABLE 1
AVERAGE PRETEST TO POSTTEST CHANGE AS A FUNCTION OF PRETEST SCORE

Pretest score
9 A

0 i 2 3 4 s
Names
8 4 4 2 8 5
M change 4.75 6.50 6.50 8.50 3.75 4.00
Explanations
* 4 4 6 7 3 2
M change 3.50 9.75 5.50 4.86 4.33 4.00

Note.The amount of possible change ranges from 12, for pretest 0, to 7 for pretest 5.
a
The n for explanations does not total 31 because 5 subjects began with more than 5 explanations scores in the to-be-condi-
tioned direction. Their change scores are consistent with the above trends.
270 P. A. COWAN, J. LANGER, J. HEAVENRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

fjoo Up
o Down

75

50

25

J L J I I J_ J I J I
Pre 2 6 8 10 12 Post Pre 2 6 8 10 12 Post
Trials Trials

FIG. 3. Changes in moral judgment during conditioning. (The points to the


left of the curves represent average pretest response and those to the right
of the curves represent average posttest responses.)
a. (left) Name scores.
b. (right) Explanations scores.

tions responses to those of the model. By con- The Pearson product-moment correlation
trast, subjects conditioned down showed most between name scores and explanations was .86
of their learning of explanations responses in the pretest, .88 during conditioning, and .92
within the first two trials, while gradually for the posttest phase. Despite the high cor-
matching their name responses to those of the relation, the explanations data in Figure 3a
model. Responses in the posttest were equal and 3b revealed different trends than the
to or above those at the end of the condi- names scores. Also, in Figure 2b, it can be
tioning phase. seen that differences between groups seem
Figures 3a and 3b indicate that posttest re- more exaggerated than in Figure 2a. Subjects
sponses did not represent a decline from con- in each of the four major subgroups had been
ditioning performance. They also indicate matched on the basis of names scores, but the
either that the rate of learning names and ex- pretest explanations scores for each subgroup
planations is different for subjects conditioned need not have been equal. In fact (Figure 2b)
up and down or that different processes may the low subjects exposed to the up condition
be involved. had begun by making more responses counter
Names versus explanations. It should be to their usual orientation than the high sub-
recalled that explanations were scored as high : jects exposed to conditioning down. This dif-
or low on the basis of a rather arbitrary two- ference continued through the conditioning
category classification. In the original at- phase, but the posttest revealed a possible in-
tempts to develop a more differentiated set teraction between up-down and immediate-
of categories, the disagreement among scorers 2-week variables in the explanations scores.
increased as we moved from pretest to condi- Up and down subjects showed similar levels
tioning phase responses. However, when the of response during the immediate posttest,
two-choice category was used for explana- both groups being farther in the conditioned
tions, scorer agreement did not vary across direction than during the average of the con-
the phases. In increasing the scoring precision, ditioning phase. However, examining the sub-
it is evident that we may have lost important jects tested after a 2-week interval, we found
information concerning the precise nature of that subjects conditioned up increased their
the changes in moral reasoning which could high level while subjects conditioned down
have occurred in this or in Bandura and Mc- tended to remain at the conditioning phase
Donald's experiment. level. This interaction in the explanations
SOCIAL LEARNING AND PIAGET'S THEORY OP MORAL DEVELOPMENT 271

scores between up-down and immediate-2- TABLE 2


week variables occurred only in the posttest PERCENTAGE OF EXPLANATIONS IN CONDITIONED
and created a statistically significant triple DIRECTION AS A FUNCTION OF DIRECTION OF
interaction among direction of conditioning, CONDITIONING, TIME OF POSTTEST, TYPE
OF ITEM, AND PHASE OF EXPERIMENT
time of posttest, and phase of experiment
variables (F = 3.87, df = 2/46, p = .05). Direction of conditioning
This effect is not present in the names scores.
However, as in the previous four-way analysis Up Down
Phase of experiment
of variance of names scores, the analysis of
explanations shows that the tentatives moved Im- Im-
mediate 2 w k . mediate 2wk.
significantly closer to the desired end of the
scale than did definites, but there were no Pretest 22 41 23 20
differences in amount of change. Again, Table Conditioning phase (last
1 indicates this result to be an artifact of the 4 trials) 61 75 56 47
Posttest
curvilinear relation between pretest level and Repeat 70 80 66 58
amount of change. No other main effects or New 83 83 73 33
interactions were statistically significant. Switch 78 83 50 42
Generalization effects. The increased time
between conditioning and posttest seemed to
facilitate generalization of conditioning ef- items. After 2 weeks, high subjects condi-
fects to posttest items, or at least did not in- tioned down increased their modeling of re-
terfere with the modeling effects (except for peat items over the last four trials of the
the down-2-week group's explanations scores). conditioning phase. However, given a choice
In order to determine whether this conclusion of explanation in the switch items, these sub-
holds for all types of items, the differential jects responded in the model's direction pro-
effects of the main variables (up-down; tenta- portionately less often than they responded to
tive-definite; immediate-2 weeks) on repeat, regular items at the end of conditioning. In
new, and switch items in the posttest were the new items they seemed to be returning
investigated in a four-way analysis of vari- toward their original high-level moral judg-
ance. The analysis was performed on explana- ments.
tion scores because they alone provided a Since there were no switch items in the pre-
reasonable possibility of choosing either level test, it is not possible to make conclusive in-
of moral judgment in answering the switch ferences concerning changes in level of switch
items; that is, subjects could reasonably item responding. However, the lowered level
justify their name choice either in terms of of modeling effects in the down-2-week group
intent or consequences in the stories. The seems to indicate a definite decline in model-
statistically significant triple interaction (F = ing effects for these subjects. In light of the
4.31, df = 2/46) of direction of conditioning, fact that responses to new items in the other
time of posttest, and repeat, new, and switch groups stayed at the same level or at an in-
items is shown in Table 2 by presenting the creased level from the conditioning-phase, the
percentage of responses in the conditioned score midway between conditioning and pre-
direction for each relevant cell. Also shown is test level seems to represent a return to the
the percentage of responses in the conditioned original level of responding for the down-2-
direction during the pretest and during the week group. This trend could be more firmly
last four trials of the conditioning phase. established with a longer conditioning-posttest
Subjects conditioned up kept going up in the interval.
posttest on all three types of items despite the
DISCUSSION
absence of the model. Subjects conditioned
down and posttested immediately continued in The two experiments under discussion were
the conditioned direction for repeat and new conducted by experimenters with different
items, but showed less tendency to give the theoretical biases. The subject populations
model's level of explanation in the switch were different, some of the items varied
272 P. A. COWAN, J. LANGER, J. HEAVENRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

slightly, and in our experiment, unlike Ban- which are based upon Piaget's orientation
dura and McDonald's, the same experimenter and which are well worked out both theoreti-
was present in all experimental phases. De- cally and operationally.
spite these differences the basic results of The present study indicates that adult
the Bandura-McDonald experiment were rep- models have different effects under different
licated. Not only did models influence sub- methodological and experimental conditions.
jects' choices of which child was naughtier The pretest scores of the subjects, the measure
(names), but they also influenced the ex- of moral response (names versus explana-
planations or justifications given by subjects tions), the direction of conditioning, the time
for their choices. Our study has established between conditioning and posttest, and the
that children were not simply changing from type of item (repeat, new, switch), singly or
one moral orientation to a state of random in combination, serve to moderate the model's
responding (the 50% conditioning and post- influence. Under no combination of variables
test level), and that they were not simply was the model completely ineffective, but the
changing their responses on the basis of a amount of conditioning and the maintenance
discrimination of the amount of consequences and the generalization of changes were not
in the stories (switch items). In general, then, uniform across all conditions.
the present results provide a great deal of There are three findings which necessitate
support for Bandura and McDonald's con- further discussion: the complexity of condi-
tention that moral responses of children can tioning phase trends, the fact that most groups
be modified in either developmental direction increased their responses in the conditioned
by exposure to adult models. However, the direction during the posttest, and the differ-
central questions to be raised concern the ences between subjects conditioned up and
precise specifications under which this modi- down. In the conditioning phase it is note-
fication occurs and the issue of what the worthy that subjects conditioned up showed
modification may mean. The theoretical points most of their learning of names in the first
raised in the introduction lead us to conclude two trials, and that subjects conditioned
that the findings do not necessarily have nega- down showed most of their explanations learn-
tive implications for Piaget's theory primarily ing in the same period. Despite the fact that
because the diagnosis of stage and the op- social learning theory acknowledges the pos-
portunities for diagnosis of moral reasoning sibility of one- or two-trial learning, it is still
in both studies were limited to operations con- difficult to understand how a child's moral
sistent with the social learning point of view. orientation could be changed by only two
The present study essentially had to remain exposures to an adult model. We certainly
with Bandura and McDonald's procedures in need more information concerning what is
order to demonstrate that their results had happening during the time when the child is
been replicated. Although the explanations alternating responses with adult models. Social
data do provide results concerning an aspect learning theorists tend to use the mechanisms
of moral reasoning, Piaget's procedures would of imitation as a prime explanatory concept
differ in the flexibility and scope of the inter- in understanding changes in behavior. In the
view and in the number of dimensions of present situation it is still not clear what the
moral judgment in addition to intention- subjects were imitating. They certainly could
consequences which would be assessed. We no not copy the model directly, since they and
longer have to assume that this "unstandard- the model always responded to items different
ized" approach works against the discovery of in content. The switch items in the present
lawful relationships, since Piaget's method study showed that subjects were not simply
has led to some of the most replicable results imitating the model's discriminations of
in the field of psychology. To focus more amount of consequences. In order to substitute
clearly on the issue of changes in stage, it social learning principles of modification for
would be helpful to replicate the format of Piaget's hypothesis of change through re-
the Bandura and McDonald study using structuring one's own activities and opera-
Kohlberg's (1964) moral judgment stages, tions, it is as incumbent upon social learning
SOCIAL LEARNING AND PIAGET'S THEORY OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT 273

theorists to clarify their use of basic terms as test. It is possible that even though the model
it is for Piagetians to clarify theirs. was no longer present, the fact that the same
There is a good possibility that the results experimenter gave the posttest was enough to
reflect a social coercion or Asch effect rather reinstate the model's effects. This hypothesis
than a change in moral reasoning, Piaget's does not, of course, explain why the responses
theory of moral judgment suggests that the of the down-2-week group declined.
low child often tends to accept adult rules and The differences in performance of subjects
statements as absolute, while the high child conditioned up and down both in the con-
tends to make his own decisions. Thus, to ditioning and posttest phases is apparently
the extent that social coercion can change a not explainable within the point of view of
child's level of moral judgment, or at least social learning theory. If moral responses are
his responses, the effect should be more long simply habits, then there is no a priori reason
lasting in the low children than in the high why the model would produce somewhat dif-
(as we found). The interpretation that the ferent effects on low and high subjects. In
child is temporarily responding to a form of the 2-week posttest, the modeling effects were
social coercion is supported by the fact that much more pronounced on subjects condi-
high subjects maintained their new level of tioned up than on those conditioned down.
responding only on repeat items. Given new Perhaps this is some indication that change is
items (new or switch) after 2 weeks, these more likely to be maintained in the natural
subjects either stayed at the conditioning direction of development, as Turiel (1966,
level or, in the case of new items, began to 1969) and Werner (1948) suggested. Perhaps,
revert to their pretest level of response. The also, the experimental situation is perceived
details of conditioning effects could be ex- differently by subjects at different levels of
amined in three ways. Another child em- development. Some of the further tests sug-
ployed as a confederate could ask the subject gested above may help to clarify these in-
what "really went on" after the experiment. terpretations. At any rate, it would be well
Second, there could be an experimental condi- for social learning theory to pay more atten-
tion in which the subject is told to imitate tion to the developmental level of subjects as
the model in order to ascertain whether imita- a moderator variable which may affect the in-
tion could lead to even greater effects than fluence of adult models on children's responses.
the experimenters have obtained in the two It is apparent that the results of Bandura
studies under discussion. An unpublished and McDonald's study, which have been de-
study (Kuhn & Langer, 1968) indicates that scribed as "unmistakably clear" (Zajonc,
the usual modeling experiment presents im- 1966, p. 49) are in fact exceedingly complex.
plicit instructions to the child to do what the
While Piaget's point of view has been used in
model does. Finally, the tests for generaliza-
tion could include moral judgments covering the present study to draw attention to some
other moral dimensions and could take place of this complexity, the theoretical differences
outside the experimental setting in a real life between social learning and Piaget's cogni-
context in order to obtain a better idea of tive theory of moral development remain un-
the extent to which the model's influence can resolved.
be generalized. If these tests should indicate REFERENCES
that the present experimental paradigm elicits
short-term social coercion effects, these effects BANDUEA, A., & MCDONALD, F. J. Influence of social
reinforcement and the behavior of models in shap-
would not be simple ones. The fact that up ing children's moral judgments. Journal of Ab-
and down subjects showed different learning normal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 274-281.
curves for names and explanations measures BANDUEA, A., & WALTERS, R. H. Social learning
would also have to be explained. theory and personality development. New York:
In our experiment the conditioning effects Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963.
BEAINE, M. D. S. The ontogeny of certain logical
were greater than those for Bandura and Mc- operations; Piaget's formulation examined by non-
Donald's subjects; some groups showed in- verbal methods. Psychological Monographs, 1959,
creased conditioning effects during the post- 73(S, Whole No. 47S).
274 P. A. COWAN, J. LANGEE, J. HEAVENRICH, AND M. NATHANSON

BKAINE, M. D. S. The development of a grasp of SMEDSLUND, J. The development of transitivity of


transitivity of length: A reply to Smedslund. Child length: A comment on Braine's reply. Child De-
Development, 1964, 35, 799-810. velopment, 1965, 36, 577-580.
KOHLBERG, L. Development of moral character and TURIEL, E. An experimental test of the sequentiality
moral ideology. In M. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman of developmental stages in the child's moral judg-
(Eds.), Review of research in child development. ments. Journal oj Personality and Social Psy-
Vol. 1. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1964. chology, 1966, 3, 611-618.
KUHN, D., & LANCER, J. Cognitive developmental TURIEL, E. Developmental processes in the child's
determinants of imitation. Unpublished manuscript, moral thinking. In P. H. Mussen, J. Langer, & M.
University of California, Berkeley, 1968. Covington (Eds.), New directions in developmental
PIAGET, J. The moral judgment oj the child. (Orig. psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston,
publ. 1932) Glencoe, III.: Free Press, 1948. 1969.
PIAGET, J. Psychology of intelligence. New York: WERNER, H. Comparative psychology of mental de-
velopment. Chicago: Follet, 1948.
Harcourt, Brace & World, 1950.
ZAJONC, R. B. Social psychology: An experimental
SMEDSLUND, J. Development of concrete transitivity approach. Belmont, Calif.: Wadsworth, 1966.
of length in children. Child Development, 1963, 34,
389-405. (Received February 2, 1968)

View publication stats

You might also like