Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Educational Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association

2010, Vol. 102, No. 4, 804 816 0022-0663/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021075

Ability Self-Concepts and Subjective Value in Literacy:


Joint Trajectories From Grades 1 Through 12

Isabelle Archambault Jacquelynne S. Eccles and Mina N. Vida


Universite de Montreal University of Michigan

Because literacy skills are critical for most academic subject matters, researchers have become increas-
ingly interested in understanding childrens motivation in this domain as a way to increase academic
success. In this study, we extend previous work by looking at the heterogeneity of childrens motivational
changes in literacy across Grades 112. We used a cross-sequential design based on 3 different cohorts
of children (N 655) coming from 10 public elementary schools. Data were collected over an 8-year
period (1988 1996) starting when children were in 1st, 2nd, and 4th grades. Using multiple-process
growth mixture modeling, we identified 7 groups of children showing distinct trajectories of change in
literacy subjective task value and ability self-concept across school years. For all children, ability
self-concept and subjective task value decreased between Grade 1 and Grade 12; however, this decline
was more apparent for some youths than for others. Our findings suggest the importance of school-based
interventions that aim to prevent decreases in student motivation for literacy over time and that focus on
the early detection of children at risk for dramatic declines.

Keywords: ability self-concepts, task value, literacy, motivation, trajectories

In most industrialized societies, literacy skills are essential for study, we investigated how different groups of children change
daily functioning. Good reading and writing abilities are associated from Grade 1 through Grade 12 along two important components
with important life consequences, such as better employment rates of motivation: subjective task value and ability self-concepts for
and higher socioeconomic status (SES; Guthrie, Schafer, & school-based literacy activities. Moreover, we examined whether
Hutchinson, 1991; Madden, Slavin, Karweit, Dolan, & Wasik, specific individual and family characteristics could predict these
1993; Raudenbush & Kasim, 1998). Because these skills are distinct motivational pathways.
central to most academic subject matters (Wilson & Trainin,
2007), many youths who do not acquire basic reading and writing
abilities become alienated from school, leading to disengagement and Ability Self-Concept and Subjective Task Value
eventually to dropout (Janosz, Archambault, Morizot, & Pagani, in Literacy
2008). In light of such consequences, researchers have become
increasingly interested in understanding childrens motivation in Subjective task value and ability self-concepts represent two widely
literacy as a way to increase their reading comprehension and studied components of literacy motivation (Baker & Wigfield, 1999;
writing skills (Guthrie, McRae, & Klauda, 2007). In the present Chapman & Tunmer, 1995; Durik, Vida, & Eccles, 2006; Eccles,
1984; Guthrie, Wigfield, & VonSecker, 2000; Jacobs, Lanza,
Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002; Wigfield & Eccles, 1994).
Ability self-concept refers to childrens own assessment of their
This article was published Online First October 4, 2010. skills and capability to perform well in a specific domain, whereas
Isabelle Archambault, Ecole de Psychoeducation, Universite de Mon- subjective task value is generally defined in terms of child interest,
treal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; Jacquelynne S. Eccles and Mina N. Vida,
perceived utility, cost, and intrinsic motivation (Chapman & Tun-
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.
mer, 1995; Durik et al., 2006; Eccles et al., 1983; Schunk &
The preparation of this article was made possible by a postdoctoral
fellowship awarded to Isabelle Archambault by the Programme Persever- Zimmerman, 1997; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). Childrens self-
ance et Reussite Scolaire du Fond Quebecois pour la Recherche sur la perceptions and beliefs have been shown to predict their selection
Societe et la Culture (Quebec, Canada). This research was supported by of activities and level of commitment to them (Eccles, 1984;
National Institute for Child Health and Human Development Grant Eccles et al., 1983). Children tend to select classes, participate
HD17553 to Jacquelynne S. Eccles, A. Wigfield, P. Blumenfeld, and R. actively, and achieve well in domains that they enjoy, find impor-
Harold. We thank the principals, teachers, students, and parents of the tant, and believe they can succeed at. Children who enjoy reading
cooperating school districts for their participation in this project. We also and writing generally believe it is important in their everyday life
thank the following people for the work they did on this project: Janis
and future and report positive beliefs about their own reading and
Jacobs, Jennifer Fredricks, Corinne Alfeld, Carol Freedman-Doan, and
Lori Rudy.
writing abilities. They also spend more time reading for leisure,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Isabelle choose language arts as elective courses in high school more often,
Archambault, Ecole de Psychoeducation, Universite de Montreal, C.P. and are more likely to engage in language arts activities (Durik et
6128, succ. Centre-Ville, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. E-mail: isabelle al., 2006; Guthrie, Wigfield, et al., 2006). In addition, these chil-
.archambault@umontreal.ca dren show higher involvement in deep texts processing (Schiefele,
804
TRAJECTORIES OF SELF-CONCEPT AND VALUE IN LITERACY 805

1999), show better written text comprehension (Guthrie, Wigfield, ues for English, math, and sports before and after controlling for
et al., 2006), report higher grades in literacy-related activities ability beliefs. Globally, they found that in every domain, both
(Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Pin- dimensions of achievement motivation decreased across Grades
trich & Schunk, 1996; Sweet, Guthrie, & Ng, 1998), and aspire for 112. Further, they demonstrated that perception of ability ex-
careers that require strong reading comprehension skills (Baker & plained around 40% of the decrease in childrens subjective task
Wigfield, 1999; Durik et al., 2006; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). value in each of these domains. Although task value can have
several other sources within individuals or contexts (e.g., percep-
Joint Development of Ability Self-Concept and tions of individual progress, autonomy in the classroom; Assor,
Subjective Task Value Kaplan, & Roth, 2002; Spinath & Steinmayr, 2008), the findings
from this study suggest that the changes children experience in
Although children who both feel competent and perceive greater terms of their self-concept and beliefs are related and, therefore,
value in reading and writing appear to be at an advantage, longi- ought to be integrated together in studying child development.
tudinal studies indicate that these beliefs globally diminish as
children get older (Durik et al., 2006; Eccles, Wigfield, & Schief- Individual Differences in Trajectories of Change
ele, 1998; Jacobs et al., 2002; Stipek & Mac Iver, 1989; Wigfield
& Eccles, 1994; Wigfield et al., 1997). As suggested by many Although there certainly is a group of children who follow a
authors (Higgins & Parsons, 1983; Nicholls & Miller, 1984; Par- trajectory of change characterized by the same general decline of
sons & Ruble, 1977; Stipek, 1984; Wigfield, Eccles, Mac Iver, literacy ability self-concept and subjective task value that is found
Reuman, & Midgley, 1991), the nature of this decrease is likely in the average population, this general pattern is unlikely to rep-
explained, at least in part, by changes children encounter as they resent the reality of every child. Some might maintain more
grow up. Because children in the early elementary years have a positive and stable subjective task value and ability self-concept
limited experience in age-segregated schools, they tend to be over the years, whereas others might report low motivation very
overly optimist. As such, they may have positive beliefs about early in their academic experience. Unfortunately, we know little
their ability to achieve or unrealistic expectations of how interest- about individual and group differences in the development of these
ing school subjects will be. As child cognitive skills develop, two components. In fact, most research on childrens motivation
school environments change and become more focused on perfor- fails to consider simultaneously the multidimensionality of chil-
mance and social comparisons (Ames, 1992; Eccles, Lord, & drens motivation and the nature and course of its development for
Midgley, 1991; Feldlaufer, Midgley, & Eccles, 1988; Ruble, Bog- different groups.
giano, Feldman, & Lobel, 1980; Stipek & Mac Iver, 1989). As a Some research has addressed differences in the way motiva-
result, children who are experiencing academic difficulties are tional constructs cluster together. A study by Roeser, Eccles, and
likely to develop more nuanced impressions of their own compe- Sameroff (1998) identified four subgroups of adolescents on the
tencies, especially when they compare themselves with peers. basis of their pattern of academic school and social-emotional
Globally, these changes in child perceptions of themselves and functioning in the seventh grade: a positive adjustment group, a
others could lead them to feel less competent in a given domain. poor school value group, a poor mental health group, and a
To protect their more general sense of self-worth, children may multiple problem group. In the literacy domain, another study by
then begin to reduce the value that they place on tasks associated Baker and Wigfield (1999) identified different clusters of fifth-
with failure (Covington, 1992, 1998; Eccles et al., 1998; Harter, and sixth-grade children on the basis of their reading motivation.
1982). They distinguished seven groups characterized by distinct motiva-
The changes operating over time on childrens task value and tional profiles. One third of youths were characterized by high
ability beliefs are neither similar nor independent. Ability self- scores on different dimensions of reading motivation (e.g., self-
concept is more likely to remain stable across school years than efficacy, importance beliefs, involvement, curiosity), 15% re-
subjective task value (Durik et al., 2006). Further, both compo- ported low scores, and the remaining half presented mixed profiles
nents are part of a whole and need to be integrated to understand (low or high scores on only some dimensions).
childrens motivational experience over time. According to Eccles Other studies modeled longitudinal patterns of student engage-
et al.s (1983) expectancy-value model, childrens ability self- ment and motivation in school. For example, a study by Ratelle,
concepts in a specific domain directly influence their expectations Guay, Larose, and Senecal (2004) described the trajectories of
of success and task value in this domain. Therefore, children who different types of school motivation at the time of transition from
feel less competent in literacy should come to value activities in high school to college. These authors demonstrated that some
this domain less. They are also likely to spend less time and effort motivational styles were more stable over the years than others.
developing their reading and writing skills (Deci & Ryan, 1985; They also showed that a decrease in motivation was observed after
Eccles et al., 1998; Jacobs et al., 2002). Although some studies the school transition but only for a small group of students.
(Nurmi & Aunola, 2005; Spinath & Spinath, 2005; Spinath & Similarly, Midgley, Eccles, and their colleagues have shown that
Steinmayr, 2008) have failed to find evidence for these predic- the trajectories of change in both ability self-concept and subjec-
tions, others have highlighted a relationship between ability self- tive task value for mathematics vary substantially across the junior
concepts and subjective task value (Durik et al., 2006; Eccles et al., high school transition depending on student classroom experiences
1983; Eccles & Wigfield, 1995; Jacobs & Eccles, 2000; Jacobs et and initial levels of competence (see Eccles, Midgley, et al., 1993,
al., 2002; Marsh, Trautwein, Ludtke, Koller, & Baumert, 2005; for summary). Finally, Archambault, Janosz, Morizot, and Pagani
Wigfield & Eccles, 1994). In their research, Jacobs et al. (2002) (2009) identified six groups of students presenting distinct trajec-
studied the longitudinal changes of childrens subjective task val- tories of behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in school
806 ARCHAMBAULT, ECCLES, AND VIDA

from 12 to 16 years of age. They showed that although the majority petence in a specific domain, children tend to develop high ability
of students presented stable and positive educational pathways self-concepts in this domain and to achieve better over time.
across high school, one third of adolescents reported important Otherwise, when parents hold negative estimates of their chil-
changes and disconnectedness on multiple facets (behavioral, af- drens reading/English competencies, children tend to feel less
fective, cognitive) of their school experience over time. competent in this domain and report lower achievement. Further,
Taken together, these studies highlight the relevance of consid- combined with the fact that they often are exposed to low quality child
ering the multidimensionality of childrens motivation when care and attend schools with both large concentrations of children
studying development across the whole academic experience. Fur- with special needs and less resources (e.g., extracurricular activities,
ther, they also point out the importance of distinguishing motiva- professional services), children from low SES families often de-
tion both for individuals and groups to obtain a more integrated velop their reading abilities more slowly (Aikens & Barbarin,
portrait of childrens development (Willett & Sayer, 1996). Build- 2008; Chapman & Tunmer, 1995; Chapman et al., 2000; Evans,
ing upon this work, in the present study we modeled the joint 2004; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998; Yeung, Linver, & Brooks-
trajectories of literacy ability self-concept and subjective task Gunn, 2002). Over the long run, many experience increased dif-
value that children follow across Grades 112. Unlike prior re- ficulties with academic material because of their limited literacy
search that has focused on the average decline characterizing the and may respond to this increased difficulty by disengaging from
majority of children, we identified distinct groups following het- the academic activities at school (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997;
erogeneous pathways on these two dimensions. Moreover, we Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997).
examined whether different individual and family characteristics Despite the substantial literature on individual and family pre-
can contribute to predict these trajectories of change over time. dictors of childrens motivational changes over time, we know
little about the early role of these predictors on childrens distinct
Predictors of Subjective Task Value and Ability trajectories of change. Hence, on the basis of the natural principle
Self-Concept Trajectories of multifinality, which stipulates that the same characteristic may
influence different outcomes over time (Cicchetti & Rogosch,
Researchers have identified several individual, familial, social, 1996), it is important to consider early factors that predict the
and academic factors that have contributed to childrens ability distinct motivational pathways that children follow across their
self-concepts and subjective task value in reading and writing whole school experience. As suggested by Spinath and Steinmayr
activities (Deci, Vallerand, Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991; Eccles et al., (2008), understanding such factors represents an important step for
1983; Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, & Blumenfeld, 1993; Guthrie, researchers and practitioners in understanding characteristics that
Hoa, Wigfield, Tonks, & Perencevich, 2006; Wigfield & Eccles, have the potential to buffer childrens motivational decrease. This
2002). In the present study, we only considered early characteris- knowledge will support early prevention programs for children
tics related to children or families. Among these characteristics, placed at risk of experiencing such declines.
past history of achievement represents a good predictor of chil-
drens motivational changes (Corpus, McClintic-Gilbert, & Hay- Goals of the Study
enga, 2009; Harter & Connell, 1984; Jacobs et al., 2002; Marsh,
Byrnes, & Yeung, 1999; Marsh et al., 2005; Skaalvik & Hagtvet, Overall, research highlights that for the majority of children,
1990). Children who achieve well and invest efforts in school their ability self-concept and subjective task value tend to decrease
would be more likely to receive positive feedback on their abilities over time. Yet, some students present more stable and positive
and, accordingly, would report more positive perceptions of com- motivation pathways, whereas others report low motivation over
petencies and greater value of academic tasks over time (Harter & time (Archambault et al., 2009; Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Ratelle et
Connell, 1984). al., 2004; Roeser et al., 1998). Further, such declines appear to be
Gender is also an important predictor of childrens motivation influenced by the transaction between individual and environment
changes in literacy-related activities. Although gender differences factors (Sameroff & Fiese, 1990). However, there is a lack of
do not appear in every study (Anderman et al., 2001), over time knowledge on how these two components of motivation develop
girls tend to place higher value on literacy activities and, as a for different groups of children across the whole school experi-
result, report higher confidence in their abilities in this domain ence. Further, despite the substantial literature on individual and
than boys (Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Durik et al., 2006; Eccles, family predictors of child motivational changes over time, we
1984; Eccles et al., 1989; Jacobs et al., 2002; Marsh, 1989; know little about the role of early predictors that affect distinct
Wigfield et al., 1991, 1997). Moreover, girls advantage on boys trajectories of change. In this research, we overcome these limi-
regarding their self-concept should increase over time (Durik et al., tations by studying the heterogeneity of motivational changes in
2006; Jacobs et al., 2002). literacy across Grades 112. Instead of focusing on the general
Finally, parents perceptions of their childrens abilities and positive, negative, or declining trajectories found in previous re-
family socioeconomic background have also received considerable search, we expected to identify more groups of children following
attention in the literature (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002; Brooks-Gunn distinct trajectories of subjective task value and ability self-
& Duncan, 1997; Chapman & Tunmer, 1995; Chapman, Tunmer, concepts. Furthermore, we assess the contribution of early specific
& Prochnow, 2000; Eccles et al., 1983; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; individual and family characteristics to explain interindividual
Jacobs & Eccles, 1992; Parsons, Adler, & Kaczala, 1982; Rimm- differences in the development of these trajectories. By studying
Kaufman, Pianta, Cox, & Bradley, 2003; Wilson & Trainin, 2007). the joint development of these motivational dimensions and by
According to Eccles et al.s (1983) expectancy-value model, when identifying factors that predict these trajectories of change, we
parents report high positive believes about their childrens com- hope to enrich our understanding of individual development. We
TRAJECTORIES OF SELF-CONCEPT AND VALUE IN LITERACY 807

also believe that the results of the present study can help guide the This study used a cross-sequential design based on three differ-
elaboration of specific interventions directed to particular groups ent cohorts of children. As showed in Table 1, during the first year
of children. of the study, children of the first, second, and third cohorts were in
Because there are few comparable studies, it is difficult to first, second, and fourth grades, respectively. Using such a design,
hypothesize the number or shape of the trajectories that might be we were able to get information on child subjective literacy value
expected. Nevertheless, consistent with past research that studied and ability self-concept from Grade 1 to Grade 12. Data for the
motivation at various ages or for the population average (Archam- 11th grade were missing in every cohort. However, these data
bault et al., 2009; Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Jacobs et al., 2002; were not problematic given that our analytic method allowed for
Ratelle et al., 2004), we were able to formulate the following the presence of missing data on the repeated measures that were
hypotheses. First, we predict the existence of at least three trajec- used to build the trajectories.
tories: a high trajectory where children report elevated and stable
literacy subjective task value and ability self-concepts over time, a Measures and Procedures
low trajectory where children report low subjective task value in
reading and writing activities and negative perception of their Data were collected between 1988 and 1996. The assessment
abilities from school entry to the end of high school, and a took place in the participants classrooms during the spring period.
declining trajectory where children report a constant decline on During the first three years of data collection, questions were read
subjective task value and ability self-concepts. In line with the aloud to the children to foster their understanding. During these
typical normative declines in motivation during adolescence waves of data collection, mothers were asked to complete ques-
(Jacobs et al., 2002), we predict that this last group will charac- tions about family characteristics and their own perception of her
terize the most children. Second, for theoretical reasons (Deci & childs competency in literacy. Finally, teachers were also asked to
Ryan, 1985; Eccles et al., 1983), we also predict that for most answer questions regarding their perception of individual childs
children, a decline in their ability self-concept would precede a abilities and efforts in reading. A 3-year interruption in data
decline in subjective task value. Finally, we predict that compared collection occurred because of a break in funding. During the next
with groups of children reporting stable and high competence three waves, which took place in junior and senior high school,
beliefs and value over time, those following low or decreasing participants read the questionnaires themselves (for more details,
trajectories of ability self-concept and subjective task value will see Jacobs et al., 2002).
differ in specific ways on early individual and family characteris- Task beliefs. We assessed child literacy beliefs during the five
tics that place them at risk. These include being a boy, having a waves of data collection. Comparable wording was used over
past history of disengagement and underachievement, coming years; however, to be relevant to what children learned in school,
from a low income family, and having parents who hold negative we asked the children in elementary school about their self-beliefs
perceptions of their competencies (Eccles et al., 1996). about reading and the children in secondary school about their
self-beliefs about English. We assessed Reading/English Ability
Method Self-Concept using five items that measured childrens perception
of their abilities in reading (e.g., Compared to other subjects how
good are you at reading?). We measured Reading/English Sub-
Sample
jective Task Value using four items that asked children how
This study is part of the Childhood and Beyond Longitudinal important and useful reading/English was to them (i.e., How
Project (Eccles et al., 1983). The sample was composed of 655 useful is what you learn in reading). Both scales were answered
children (49.2% boys) who participated in the first year of the using 17 Likert response scales (e.g., 1 not useful, 7 very
study and who presented valid answers on at least three of the five useful) and showed good internal reliability in all waves (for more
waves of data collection. Children originally came from 10 ele- details, see Jacobs et al., 2002). To sharpen the comparison of
mentary public schools located in four middle-class communities individual changes over time and to ensure that changes in child
of a large city in the Midwest. Every child at the appropriate grade ability self-concept and value over time are not due to changes in
level in each school was asked to participate. Of the children, childrens understanding of the questions, we centered scores on
75% 80% agreed to take part in the study and received parental these two scales, across cohorts, on the grand mean (the mean of
consent to do so. These children were followed over their elemen- the whole sample for all time points; Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992).
tary, middle, and high school years (for more information about At each grade, the grand mean of 5.2230 was subtracted from
this sample, see Jacobs et al., 2002). student scores on ability self-concept, and the grand mean of

Table 1
Number of Children by Cohort and Grade at Each Year of Data Collection

Time point (year of assessment)

Cohort Time 1 (1988) Time 2 (1989) Time 3 (1990) Time 4 (1994) Time 5 (1996)

1 (n 215) Grade 1 Grade 2 Grade 3 Grade 7 Grade 9


2 (n 232) Grade 2 Grade 3 Grade 4 Grade 8 Grade 10
3 (n 208) Grade 4 Grade 5 Grade 6 Grade 10 Grade 12
808 ARCHAMBAULT, ECCLES, AND VIDA

5.0508 was subtracted from student scores on subjective task Schafer, 1999). To limit the danger of identifying a local solution
value. This strategy enabled us to the interpret students scores on that is not the true maximum likelihood, we used the random starts
ability self-concept and value in relation to the sample mean. features available in Mplus and tested 500 random starting value
sets for all of our models (McLachlan & Peel, 2000).
Covariates at the First Year of Assessment Finally, we selected the best model on the basis of the conver-
gence of different statistical indices and on substantive arguments
Childrens gender. Information on gender was obtained from (see B. O. Muthen, 2003). We used the log-likelihood ratio (LLR),
official records. Female participants were coded as 0, and male the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987), the Bayes-
participants were coded as 1. ian information criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), and the sample-
Family income. Family income was self-reported by the size adjusted Bayesian information criterion (ABIC; Sclove,
mother on a 9-point scale (1 under $10,000 a year, 9 over 1987). The smallest absolute values of these indices generally
$80,000). indicate a better fit model. However, it is preferable to rely on the
Reading/learning efforts and grades. Childrens grades, BIC and ABIC rather than the AIC (particularly the ABIC), as
reading, and learning efforts were reported by teachers on the first these are stricter and usually favor the selection of a more parsi-
year of assessment. Both scales were measured on a 15-point scale monious model with fewer trajectories (see Kass & Wasserman,
(E minus to A plus). 1995). To further secure our decision in selecting the best model,
Parental perception of their childs reading ability. Paren- we used the adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT; Lo, Men-
tal perception of their childs reading ability was self-reported dell, & Rubin, 2001) and the bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT;
by the mother at Time 1. This scale comprises five items ( .94) McLachlan & Peel, 2000). For a given model, a nonsignificant
that assessed maternal perception of their childs reading compe- LMR-LRT and BLRT test suggests that a model with one fewer
tencies (e.g., How good is your child at reading). Parents class would provide a better fit. Finally, we also relied on the
responses were evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 not at all Entropy Index that summarizes the quality of the classification
good, 7 very good). (Ramaswamy, DeSarbo, Reibstein, & Robinson, 1993) on the
basis of pertinent theory.
Analytic Strategy
Results
Multiple-process growth mixture modeling (GMM; B. O. Muthen,
2001, 2004) was implemented in Mplus to identify subgroups of We estimated the means, correlations, and distribution statistics
children following distinct trajectories of literacy subjective task for the total sample. The standard deviations and distribution
value and ability self-concepts. GMM does not assume that the statistics (i.e., skewness and kurtosis) were not large for ability
population of children is homogeneous and can be represented by self-concept and subjective task value scales, suggesting that the
a single multivariate normal distribution. Rather, GMM assumes identification of mixture distributions would not be a direct result
that individuals follow heterogeneous, quantitatively and qualita- of severe nonnormal distributions. All correlations were in the
tively distinct trajectories of change (B. O. Muthen, 2001; Nagin, expected directions. Further, correlations between the covariates
1999). On the basis of repeated measures, Mplus permits one to were all small (around .10), suggesting no multicollinearity prob-
estimate simultaneously the change parameters (i.e., intercept and lem.
slope) on two different but related outcomes. This kind of joint We tested up to 10 different models. In all models, an intercept
development model (see L. K. Muthen & Muthen, 2006) allowed and both linear and quadratic slopes were specified. Time-
us to examine simultaneously the developmental course of chil- invariant covariates were also included in the models and allowed
drens subjective task value and ability self-concept from Grade 1 to influence latent growth parameters (i.e., intercept and slope) as
through Grade 12. To identify childrens membership in these joint well as the categorical latent variable (i.e., class membership). The
trajectories, GMM classifies each individual in a probabilistic means of these growth factors were the only parameters allowed to
manner. vary across classes. Correlations between temporally adjacent re-
Covariates were included in the different models to predict child siduals measurement variances were also estimated.
membership in each trajectory (see B. O. Muthen, 2001, 2004). On the basis of the different fit statistics (see Table 2), we opted
The relationships identified with the covariates are model-based for a seven-class model. Although the information criteria (LLR,
rather than post hoc estimations. Because Mplus allows missing AIC, BIC, ABIC) consistently decreased as the number of classes
data in all parts of the GMM except for covariates, missing values increased, this decrease stabilized at the five-class solution. While
on literacy subjective task value and ability self-concept were not testing the BLRT for models from three to 10 classes, results were
problematic. Yet, we used the PATTERN option in Mplus to inconclusive ( p .05 in every model). We thus decided not to rely
consider the type of missing data associated with the multiple on this test. Otherwise, the LMR-LRT favored a seven-class
cohort design. To make the distinction between longitudinal ef- model. The Entropy Index of .77 for this model indicated adequate
fects and cohort effects, we tested the assumption of invariance of quality of classification (i.e., not too many classification errors).
growth parameters across cohorts and found no variations. For the The classification of the final model is presented in Table 3. This
covariates, we imputed missing data (less than 10%) using the classification table is based on the posterior probabilities of being
NORM multiple imputations program (Schafer, 1999). NORM classified in each latent class and suggests that the seven-class
uses an iterative method based on EM algorithm to impute missing model provides an acceptable classification, although two latent
data by drawing values from the conditional distribution of the classes showed average probabilities slightly lower than .80 diag-
variables given their observed data (for technical details, see onally. On the basis of these indicators, but also because this
TRAJECTORIES OF SELF-CONCEPT AND VALUE IN LITERACY 809

Table 2 Students from the three declining trajectoriesEarly Decline


Summary of Fit Statistics for Different Growth Mixture Models (7.8%), Constant Decline (28.1%), and Late Decline (13.3%)
trajectoriesall evidenced substantial motivational declines over
No. of class LLR AIC BIC ABIC years. The main difference between these three groups lies in the
1 11,904 23,929 24,198 24,008 proportion of boys and girls at the moment when their motivational
2 11,445 23,035 23,363 23,131 declines started. Children in the Early Decline and Late Decline
3 11,343 22,859 23,244 22,971 trajectories were mostly boys (70.6%, ASR 3.2; 69.0%, ASR
4 11,262 22,723 23,167 22,852 3.6, respectively); in contrast, there was a slight majority of girls
5 9,565 19,343 19,818 19,481
in the Constant Decline group (57.5%, ASR 2.2). For the
6 9,536 19,309 19,838 19,464
7 9,513 19,286 19,869 19,456 Constant Decline and the Early Decline children, the decrease in
8 9,488 19,259 19,896 19,445 ability self-concept and subjective task value began in the second
9 9,456 19,221 19,911 19,423 grade. Yet, by the sixth grade, the Early Decliners reported a
10 9,303 19,193 19,874 19,397 motivation-level far below the average, whereas the decrease
Note. LLR log-likelihood ratio; AIC Akaike information criterion; showed by the Constant Decliners was slower. Finally, the Late
BIC Bayesian information criterion; ABIC sample-size adjusted Decliners followed a similar trend but in later grades. During the
Bayesian information criterion. transition to secondary school, these children reported high sub-
jective task value and ability self-concept. In the ninth grade, their
motivation was still around the average; however, by the 12th
seven-class solution was theoretically more interesting because it grade, their motivation especially their ability self-concept be-
included the Late Decline Trajectory, we opted for this model. liefswas quite low.
In the last group, the Low Trajectory, children are characterized
The Course of Childrens Literacy Subjective Task by weak perception of their reading abilities and low valuing of
literacy from the very beginning of school entry. They presented
Value and Ability Self-Concept
the lowest scores until ninth grade, when their motivation shifted
Figure 1 illustrates the seven trajectories of literacy subjective task and increased slightly. This Low group represented 2.2% of our
value and ability self-concept. This figure presents child mean pre- sample, mostly boys (63.6%, ASR 2.6).
dicted scores on the two dimensions across Grades 112. Overall,
contrary to our first hypothesis, none of the groupsneither children Predictors of the Different Trajectories
in the highest trajectory nor children in the lowest trajectory
reported stable patterns of ability self-concept and subjective task Membership in each school motivation trajectory was regressed
value. Further, this figure illustrates three declining trajectories instead on a series of theoretically meaningful covariates measured in
of one, and, in contrast to our second hypothesis, children of all early schooling. Because the dependent variable consists of an
groups except the Moderate trajectory reported similar changes on unordered categorical latent variable, the log-odds of motivation
their ability self-concept and subjective task value over time. trajectory membership must be calculated relative to a reference
The first group illustrated in Figure 1, the High Trajectory, is group. Because children in the High Trajectory maintained the
composed of children presenting the most positive profile. Of all highest subjective task value and ability self-concept over all
groups, these children maintained the highest level of literacy years, we used this group as the reference.
subjective task value and ability self-concepts over years. None- The predictive effects of the covariates are reported as odds
theless, they showed a small motivational decrease from Grade 1 ratios (with 95% confidence intervals) in Table 4. These odds
to Grade 7. This group represents only 10% of the sample and, ratios represent the change in odds of being in one trajectory
unsurprisingly, is composed of a majority of girls (66.7%, adjusted compared with the High Trajectory when the value of the predictor
standardized residual [ASR]1 2.7). increases by one unit. Compared with children from the High
In the next group, the Moderate Trajectory (20.3%), students group, the odds of being a boy was 2.16 3.31 times higher for
motivation fluctuated around the mean. Of all trajectories, it is the children in the Transitory, Early, Late Decline, and Low Trajec-
only one in which child ability self-concept and subjective task tory. Compared with children from the High group, the Early,
value do not follow similar patterns of changes over time. In the Late, and Constant Decliners were also more likely to live in a
first years of schooling, members of the Moderate Trajectory lower income family. Further, except for the Late Decliners,
reported higher literacy subjective task value than ability self- youths of all other trajectories earned lower grades in their first
beliefs. Yet, up to Grade 9, their literacy subjective task value years of schooling than the High group. Finally, the odds of having
decreased consistently, whereas their ability perception remained a parent reporting a low perception of his/her childs reading
stable. By the end of 12th grade, both boys and girls (girls ability was higher in the Early Decline group.
52.6%, ASR 0.05) from this group reported mean-level mo-
tivation on the two dimensions. Discussion
Children in the Transitory Decline group (18.3%) reported the In this study, we assessed the joint development of child ability
same mean level of ability self-concept and subjective task value in self-concept and subjective task value in the domain of literacy
Grade 1. Their motivation decreased until the transition to junior high
school when their scores on both scales began to increase. Over all the
years, these children, mostly boys (73.3%, ASR 5.8), maintained 1
ASRs are interpreted as Z scores (e.g., ASR values 1.96 are signif-
higher ability self-concepts than subjective task value. icant at the two-tailed .05 level).
810 ARCHAMBAULT, ECCLES, AND VIDA

Table 3
Classification Table Based on Estimated Posterior Probabilities for the Final Model

Class High Moderate Transitory decline Constant decline Early decline Late decline Low

High .892 .020 .018 .052 .000 .018 .000


Moderate .031 .779 .076 .0816 .013 .012 .009
Transitory decline .018 .063 .808 .049 .049 .000 .013
Constant decline .072 .049 .046 .774 .045 .014 .000
Early decline .000 .011 .060 .054 .861 .000 .015
Late decline .091 .007 .000 .059 .000 .843 .000
Low .000 .019 .045 .000 .038 .000 .899

from the beginning of elementary school through the end of high Trajectories of Ability Self-Concept and Subjective
school. We identified seven groups of children with distinct tra- Task Value
jectories of change over the school years. We further demonstrated
that a confluence of risk factors predicted these different pathways, On the basis of research that has examined child and adolescent
suggesting that school-based interventions aimed at promoting motivation and engagement patterns over time (Baker & Wigfield,
child motivation beliefs should be concerned with individual dif- 1999; Jacobs et al., 2002; Janosz et al., 2008), we expected to
ferences over development. identify at least three distinct trajectories of literacy beliefs: stable

High Trajectory Moderate Trajectory Transitory Declining Trajectory


2
2 (10.0%) 2
(20.3%) (18.3%)

1,5 1,5
1,5

1 1
1

0,5 0,5
0,5

0 0 0

-0,5 -0,5 -0,5

-1 -1 -1

-1,5 -1,5 -1,5

-2 -2 -2
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

2 2 Low Trajectory
2 Early Decline Trajectory 2 Constant Decline Trajectory Late Decline Trajectory
(2.2%)
(7.8%) (28.1%) (13.3%)
1,5 1,5
1,5 1,5

1 1
1 1

0,5 0,5
0,5 0,5

0 0 0
0

-0,5 -0,5 -0,5 -0,5

-1 -1 -1 -1

-1,5 -1,5 -1,5 -1,5

-2 -2 -2 -2
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

Legend Self-Concept of Ability Subjective Task Value

Figure 1. Mean predicted value and ability self-concept scores by profile.


TRAJECTORIES OF SELF-CONCEPT AND VALUE IN LITERACY 811

and high, stable and low, and decreasing. We further assumed that

[1.26, 8.68]
[0.56, 1.13]

[0.50, 1.26]
[0.38, 0.96]

[0.35, 1.24]
95% CI
for most children, a decline in competency beliefs would precede
a decline in subjective task value. Overall, these hypotheses were

Low
partially supported. However, contrary to expectations, we found
much more between-child variation than expected in both compe-
tence beliefs and value of literacy. Moreover, of all the groups

3.31

0.62
OR

0.79

0.79

0.66
identified in this study, we found three groups in which child
motivation declined from Grade 1 to Grade 12 and none in which
[1.06, 3.44] children maintained stable beliefs across grades.
[0.48, 0.83]

[0.50, 1.13]
[0.85, 1.62]

[0.46, 1.95]
Most research on group differences in the development of
95% CI

engagement and motivation have identified youths presenting pos-


Late decline

itive and stable attitudes over time. Consistent with those findings

Note. All covariates are significant predictors based on the chi-square test from the total model. The reference group is the high trajectory. OR odds ratio.
(Archambault et al., 2009; Janosz et al., 2008; Ratelle et al., 2004),
we identified by the end of 12th grade a High Trajectory group in
0.64

which students reported high competency beliefs about reading


OR

2.25

0.75
1.17

0.95

and English and positive task value about the usefulness and
importance of this domain for their future. Unlike those in the
general population (Jacobs et al., 2002), these childrens scores
[1.30, 5.14]
[0.32, 0.94]

[0.71, 1.48]
[0.43, 0.87]

[1.05, 6.81]
95% CI

were relatively stable over years.


Results of the Multinomial Logistic Regression: Predictors of Value and Ability Self-Concept Trajectories Membership

Early decline

Researchers have suggested two different but related explana-


tions for this observed small motivational decline in highly moti-
vated children. For one, classroom changes in terms of increased
control by teachers, social comparisons between peers, and focus
2.58

0.61
OR

0.55

2.59

on individual differences in performance become more salient in


1.02
High versus

later elementary school (Deci, Ryan, & Williams, 1996; Eccles,


Wigfield, Midgley, et al., 1993). Second, changes in child cogni-
tive ability makes social comparison more common. As a result,
[0.37, 1.66]
[0.53, 0.85]

[0.61, 1.18]
[0.57, 0.97]

[0.86, 2.34]
95% CI

children are likely to become less motivated in domains in which


Constant decline

they feel less competent (Harter, 1992; Ruble et al., 1980; Stipek
& Mac Iver, 1989). Both of these age-related changes can result in
motivated children becoming more aware of themselves and oth-
ers, which in turn can lead them to process and evaluate the
0.69
OR

0.77
0.79

0.85

1.42

feedback they receive differently. At school entry, most children


like school, participate in activities, and feel competent in the
academic tasks they initiate. During this time, the focus in the
[1.20, 3.88]
[0.65, 1.03]

[0.77, 1.28]
[0.44, 0.88]

[0.77, 1.78]

classroom is more on task mastery than on relative performance


95% CI
Transitory decline

(Freedman-Doan et al., 2000; Patrick, Mantzicopoulos, Samara-


pungavan, & French, 2008). However, as they get older and are
confronted with an increasing number of academic activities, a
greater focus on relative performance levels, and a sharp increase
0.63

in testing, children begin to develop a better sense of which


2.16
OR

0.82

1.00

1.17

activities they do or do not enjoy. Hence, they become better able


to provide sensitive and objective evaluations of themselves, their
interests, and their abilities in different domains. For children from
[0.68, 7.47]
[0.66, 1.12]

[0.79, 1.21]
[0.44, 0.85]

[0.71, 2.28]

the High Trajectory, these new insights only lead to a relative


95% CI

decline in their competency beliefs and the value they attach to


Moderate

school subjects. However, this is not the case for most of the
children: 90% of our children experienced substantial declines in
p .10. p .05. p .01.

their literacy motivation across their primary and secondary school


0.61

years.
OR

2.25
0.86

0.98

1.28

Children in declining trajectories evidenced the sharpest drop in


motivation. As expected, such pathways represented half the chil-
dren in our sample. In the Early, Constant, and Late Decline
of reading ability
Parental perception

trajectories, children evidenced motivation decreases, but the rate


Time 1 predictor

learning efforts

of this decline varied across the school years. All of these children
Reading grades
Family income
Gender (boys)

Reading and

started their first grade year with high motivational beliefs. Most
reported that reading was useful for them and that believed they
Table 4

were good at it. Yet, over time, their beliefs about reading became
more negative. This decline is particularly worrisome for Early

812 ARCHAMBAULT, ECCLES, AND VIDA

Decliners and Constant Decliners because the changes that they Children in one group did evidence noticeable differences be-
experienced began in early schooling. This premature loss of tween their ratings for subjective task value and ability self-
confidence and decrease in their valuing of literacy are detrimental concept in literacy. In their first years of schooling, children from
for sustaining engagement in learning. For this group, in the this Moderate Trajectory reported lower ability self-concept than
domain of reading and writing there is a risk of this trend becom- subjective task value. Over time, the importance and usefulness
ing firmly embedded and thus difficult to change. As they disen- that they assigned to reading and writing decreased steadily,
gage from learning, the likelihood that they will acquire the basic whereas their competency beliefs ratings were stable. Around
reading and writing requirements necessary to succeed in other eighth grade, the two dimensions converged. The early gap that
school subjects is also likely to decline, which in turn may lead to existed between the two dimensions in this group suggests that
an increase in both their rate of disengagement from school and the some young children acknowledge the value of reading even
likelihood that they will eventually drop out of school before though they do not have great confidence in the reading ability.
graduating (Janosz et al., 2008). Although we did not investigate Such children may evaluate their abilities with reference to the
the consequences associated with being on these declining trajec- ability of peers over time, or they may fail to achieve as well as
tories in this article, we hope to do so in the future. they hoped in reading. As a result, these students may begin to feel
We also identified groups of children who reported both a less competent and gradually reduce the importance that they
decrease and an increase of ability self-concept and subjective task attach to literacy. According to Wigfield and Eccles (1994), such
value at different time periods of their academic experience. Con- disengagement could act as a protective mechanism used by chil-
sistent with our expectations, we identified a Low Trajectory group dren to preserve their self-esteem. To prevent such disaffection, it
in which children reported low motivational beliefs from their is important that teachers and practitioners adapt learning activities
early grades. Yet, unexpectedly, the motivational profile of these to challenges and support student valuing of reading and compe-
children was not stable. It decreased up to the end of elementary tency beliefs.
school and then went back to its initial level. A similar pattern was
observed in the Transitory Decline group. The perceptions of
children from this group changed dramatically in eighth grade: Predictors Associated With Trajectories of Change
From that point, they felt increasingly better about their ability in
Our findings also highlight important individual and family
reading and writing and assigned growing importance to these
predictors associated with child motivational trajectories. Unsur-
tasks. This change likely reflects either individual changes or
prisingly, we demonstrated that student gender and early academic
contextual changes in their experiences in English classrooms. In
difficulties remained strong predictors of motivational pathways.
American schooling, students experience transition to high school
Consistent with previous studies (Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Durik
at this time, and curriculum shifts from a focus on reading and
et al., 2006), we found a higher proportion of girls in the High
writing to a focus on literature. Perhaps these changes helped spark
Trajectory, in which children maintained the highest and most
the interest of a subset of our participants. These two trajectories
stable literacy value and ability self-concepts over time. Con-
highlight the importance of investigating the circumstances under
versely, the proportion of boys was significantly higher in groups
which adolescents can experience motivational increases as they
evidencing a motivational decline, especially during the elemen-
move into secondary school. A better understanding of these
tary school years. Moreover, children who reported a decline in
circumstances could enhance our ability to design effective inter-
their competencies beliefs over years demonstrated lower levels of
vention strategies for children with early signs of low literacy
academic achievement than children who maintained high beliefs
motivation. Such work would also contribute to our understanding
about literacy from early grades on. However, such findings need
of the interpersonal and contextual processes underlying develop-
to be interpreted while keeping in mind that the relationship
mental shifts in child motivation.
between achievement and motivation is reciprocal (Corpus et al.,
2009; Marsh et al., 1999, 2005; Nurmi & Aunola, 2005). There-
Relations Between Ability Self-Concept and Subjective fore, the greater motivational decrease observed among low
Task Value Over Time achievers might be explained by their motivation prior to the study
instead of by their early achievement.
In most trajectories, children reported parallel changes in their Children evidencing declines in their literacy motivation were
feeling of competence and value of reading and writing activities. also more likely to live in low SES families than children who
Such results were quite surprising. However, they are consistent maintained high levels of ability self-concepts and subjective task
with theories proposing that child ability self-concept in a specific values for reading (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997). Even though
domain directly influences the value given to tasks related to this these children start school with reasonably high ability self-
domain (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Eccles et al., 1983). Actually, the fact concepts and attach considerable importance to reading, these
that both dimensions vary conjointly in most trajectories does not children are less likely to feel competent about their ability to
allow us to disentangle the specific effect of one component on the master reading. One explanation being that disadvantaged families
other. The changes found on subjective task value could be influ- may be less well equipped to intervene and help children catch up
enced by changes on ability self-concept (and vice versa) or by a in their reading competence. Consequently, children from lower
third variable such as perception of individual progress (Spinath & income families are likely to reduce the importance that they attach
Steinmayr, 2008). More research is needed to sort out the effect to these skills to preserve their self-esteem and to continue having
that each component has on the other in the development of greater academic difficulties than children from more wealthy
specific trajectory of changes. families.
TRAJECTORIES OF SELF-CONCEPT AND VALUE IN LITERACY 813

Finally, our finding that parental beliefs were more positive for need to be adapted to better stimulate their interest. An important
children showing the earliest motivational decline and presenting challenge is to bring about changes in curriculum and the daily
the greatest number of family risk factors than parental beliefs for practices of teachers that are likely to help them respond to the
children evidencing the highest motivational level was quite un- differential needs of students.
expected. According to Eccles et al. (1983), parental perception of
child ability should influence the development of their child be- Limitations
liefs children whose parents hold high estimates of their reading
ability should develop high reading ability self-concepts. Evi- This study has several limitations that are worth mentioning.
dence, in general, supports this prediction at the population level. First, the sample was composed primarily of children coming from
One potential explanation for our results might be that parents of middle-class European American families. Thus, our findings can-
the high risk group of children are less educated and acquainted not be generalized to other populations. Future work should ad-
with the demands of the educational system. Therefore, they might dress this limit and study the development of childrens literacy
have an unrealistic perception of their childs actual difficulties ability self-concept and subjective task value with more diverse
and abilities. If so, this would further exacerbate the dynamics populations of children. A second limitation concerns the factors
noted previously: If these parents are not aware that their child that were included in our model to predict trajectory membership.
needs help in learning to read, they will not be motivated to seek Because GMM models only allow the inclusion of a limited
out the supplementary assistance that their child might need. Our number of factors, we selected only a restricted subset of individ-
results do not allow us to go such in depth in our conclusions, but ual and family-related characteristics as covariates. Although those
more research could help clarify this idea and improve our under- characteristics have been shown to influence child motivation, we
standing of the family role on childrens motivation over time. omitted some individual (e.g., classroom attention and behaviors),
family (e.g., number of books available at home, parental invest-
Implications ment in school), teacher (e.g., teacherstudent relationships,
teacher practices), and school (e.g., school neighborhood, children
This study has several implications for research and practice. classroom ratio) characteristics that could have been related to
First, although some practices used by teachers in many upper child motivation trajectories. Finally, the characteristics that we
elementary and secondary schools have been criticized for their selected were only assessed once at school entry. Therefore, we
harmful consequences on student motivation (Brophy & Evertson, were not able to determine whether these characteristics were
1978; Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1988), the practices used stable and whether changes could have influenced child motivation
with early elementary school children have received less attention. variations over time.
Considering that 10% of the children in our study reported rela-
tively low reading ability and task value by the third grade, and Conclusions
that nearly one third reported similarly low self- and task beliefs by
the end of elementary school situation, it is important to identify By assessing different trajectories of ability self-concept and
how the practices used by elementary school teachers can influ- subjective task value in the literacy domain, this study demon-
ence these patterns. This knowledge could help researchers and strates that children do not all evidence the same trajectories of
professionals in the development of effective strategies promoting change over time in their motivational beliefs. Because literacy
childrens self-concept of ability and value of literacy. skills represent the performance standard for most academic sub-
This study also brings new light on the heterogeneity that exists ject matters, findings from this research highlight the necessity of
in the development of literacy-related beliefs. Although the vast investing financial resources for screening children who present
majority of children evidence motivational declines as they pass early risks of following negative literacy motivational pathways
through primary and secondary school, the extent and timing of over time (namely boys), who come from low SES families, and
such changes vary between individuals. It remains unclear whether who present reading difficulties and important motivational de-
these differences reflect specific factors that operate at different clines in the first two years of schooling. These children should be
points in time for different children and what these factors might the targets for interventions that promote intrinsic motivation and
be. Moreover, there is certainly a need to understand how children competence for reading while they still perceive themselves as
in the most problematic declining groups compare with each other having good abilities and believe reading and writing is important.
and whether characteristics measured later in the academic expe-
rience could influence the nature and course of their motivation. References
However, what is clear is that prevention and intervention efforts
need to be developed with these individual differences in mind. A Aikens, N. L., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Socioeconomic differences in
strategy designed for children who already evidence low motiva- reading trajectories: The contribution of family, neighborhood, and
tional believes in the first grade might not be appropriate for school contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 235251.
adolescents whose motivational beliefs decline as they move into doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.235
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.
secondary school. Further, different types of interventions might
doi:10.1007/BF02294359
be needed depending on whether parents view their childs ability Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation.
as unrealistically high or realistically low. Finally, different inter- Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261271. doi:10.1037/0022-
ventions may be needed for boys and girls. For example, as boys 0663.84.3.261
spent less time reading for pleasure (Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Anderman, E. M., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Roeser, R., Wigfield, A., &
Coles & Christine, 2002), the kind of books teachers propose may Blumenfeld, P. (2001). Learning to value mathematics and reading:
814 ARCHAMBAULT, ECCLES, AND VIDA

Relations to mastery and performance-oriented instructional practices. analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 382393. doi:10.1037/
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 76 95. doi:10.1006/ 0022-0663.98.2.382
ceps.1999.1043 Eccles, J. (1984). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In T. Sondereg-
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. (2009). Adolescent ger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Psychology and gender
behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in school: Relationship (Vol. 32, pp. 97132). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
to dropout. Journal of School Health, 79, 408 415. doi:10.1111/j.1746- Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M.,
1561.2009.00428.x Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic
Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, but relevance is behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motiva-
excellent: Autonomy-enhancing and suppressing teacher behaviours pre- tion (pp. 75146). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
dicting students engagement in schoolwork. British Journal of Educa- Eccles, J. S., Flanagan, C., Lord, S., Midgley, C., Roeser, R., & Yee, D.
tional Psychology, 72, 261278. doi:10.1348/000709902158883 (1996). Schools, families, and early adolescents: What are we doing
Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of childrens motivation for wrong and what can we do instead? Journal of Developmental &
reading and their relations to reading activity and reading achievement. Behavioral Pediatrics, 17, 267276.
Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 452 477. doi:10.1598/RRQ.34.4.4 Eccles, J. S., Lord, S., & Midgley, C. (1991). What are we doing to early
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child adolescents? The impact of educational contexts on early adolescents.
development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371399. doi:10.1146/ American Journal of Education, 99, 521542. doi:10.1086/443996
annurev.psych.53.100901.135233 Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D.,
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on Flanagan, C., & Mac Iver, D. (1993). Development during adolescence:
children. The Future of Children, 7, 5571. doi:10.2307/1602387 The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents experiences
Brophy, J. E., & Evertson, C. M. (1978). Context variables in teaching. in schools and in families. American Psychologist, 48, 90 101. doi:
Educational Psychologist, 12, 310 316. doi:10.1080/00461527809529185 10.1037/0003-066X.48.2.90
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. of adolescents achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs.
Chapman, J. W., & Tunmer, W. E. (1995). Development of young chil- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 215225. doi:10.1177/
drens reading self-concepts: An examination of emerging subcompo- 0146167295213003
nents and their relationship with reading achievement. Journal of Edu- Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Flanagan, C. A., Miller, C., Reuman, D. A., &
cational Psychology, 87, 154 167. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.154 Yee, D. (1989). Self-concepts, domain values, and self-esteem: Rela-
Chapman, J. W., Tunmer, W. E., & Prochnow, J. E. (2000). Early reading- tions and changes at early adolescence. Journal of Personality, 57,
related skills and performance, reading self-concept, and the develop- 283310. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00484.x
ment of academic self-concept: A longitudinal study. Journal of Edu- Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and
cational Psychology, 92, 703708. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.703 gender differences in childrens self- and task perceptions during ele-
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in mentary school. Child Development, 64, 830 847. doi:10.2307/
developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 1131221
597 600. doi:10.1017/S0954579400007318 Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Midgley, C., Reuman, D., Mac Iver, D., &
Coles, M., & Christine, H. (2002). Gendered readings: Learning from Feldlaufer, H. (1993). Negative effects of traditional middle schools on
childrens reading choices. Journal of Research in Reading, 25, 96 108. students motivation. The Elementary School Journal, 93, 553574.
doi:10.1111/1467-9817.00161 doi:10.1086/461740
Corpus, J. H., McClintic-Gilbert, M. S., & Hayenga, A. O. (2009). Within-year Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed.
changes in childrens intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations: Con- Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
textual predictors and academic outcomes. Contemporary Educational Psy- Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. American
chology, 34, 154 166. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.01.001 Psychologist, 59, 7792. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77
Covington, M. V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on Feldlaufer, H., Midgley, C., & Eccles, J. S. (1988). Student, teacher, and
motivation and school reform. New York, NY: Cambridge University observer perceptions of the classroom environment before and after the
Press. transition to junior high school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 8, 133
Covington, M. V. (1998). The will to learn. New York, NY: Cambridge 156. doi:10.1177/0272431688082003
University Press. Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Childrens competence and value
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth trajectories in two
and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Devel- male-sex-typed domains. Developmental Psychology, 38, 519 533. doi:
opmental Psychology, 33, 934 945. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934 10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.519
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, A. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self- Freedman-Doan, C., Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Blumenfeld, P., Arbreton,
determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press. A., & Harold, R. D. (2000). What am I best at? Grade and gender
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., & Williams, G. C. (1996). Need satisfaction and differences in childrens beliefs about ability improvement. Journal of
the self-regulation of learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 379 402. doi:10.1016/S0193-
165183. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(96)90013-8 3973(00)00046-0
Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Guthrie, J. T., Hoa, L. W., Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., & Perencevich,
Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective. Educa- K. C. (2006). From spark to fire: Can situational reading interest lead to
tional Psychologist, 26, 325346. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2603&4_6 long-term reading motivation? Reading Research and Instruction, 45,
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). Income effects across the 91117.
life-span: Integration and interpretation. In G. J. Duncan & J. Brooks- Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A., & Klauda, S. L. (2007). Contributions of
Gunn (Eds.), Consequences of growing up poor (pp. 596 610). New concept-oriented reading instruction to knowledge about interventions
York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. for motivations in reading. Educational Psychologist, 42, 237250.
Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and ability Guthrie, J. T., Schafer, W. D., & Hutchinson, S. R. (1991). Relations of
beliefs as predictors of high school literacy choices: A developmental document literacy and prose literacy to occupational and societal char-
TRAJECTORIES OF SELF-CONCEPT AND VALUE IN LITERACY 815

acteristics of young Black and White adults. Reading Research Quar- high school: Beliefs of pre- and posttransition teachers. Journal of Youth
terly, 26, 30 48. doi:10.2307/747730 and Adolescence, 17, 543562. doi:10.1007/BF01537831
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Humenick, N. M., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, Muthen, B. O. (2001). Second-generation structural equation modeling
A., & Barbosa, P. (2006). Influences of stimulating tasks on reading moti- with a combination of categorical and continuous latent variables: New
vation and comprehension. Journal of Educational Research, 99, 232246. opportunities for latent class/latent growth modeling. In L. M. Collins &
doi:10.3200/JOER.99.4.232-246 A. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 291322).
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., & VonSecker, C. (2000). Effects of integrated Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/
instruction on motivation and strategy use in reading. Journal of Edu- 10409-010
cational Psychology, 92, 331341. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.331 Muthen, B. O. (2003). Statistical and substantive checking in growth
Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child mixture modeling: Comment on Bauer and Curran (2003). Psychologi-
Development, 53, 8797. doi:10.2307/1129640 cal Methods, 8, 369 377. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.369
Harter, S. (1992). The relationship between perceived competence, affect, Muthen, B. O. (2004). Latent variables analysis: Growth mixture modeling
and motivational orientation within the classroom: Processes and pat- and related techniques for longitudinal data. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), Hand-
terns of change. In R. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Achievement book of quantitative methodology for the social sciences (pp. 345368).
and motivation (pp. 77114). New York, NY: Cambridge University Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Press. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2006). Mplus users guide (4th ed.). Los
Harter, S., & Connell, J. P. (1984). A model of the relationship among Angeles, CA: Author.
childrens academic achievement and their self-perceptions of compe- Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semipara-
tence, control, and motivational orientations. In J. Nicholls (Ed.), The metric, group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139 157.
development of achievement motivation (pp. 219 250). Greenwich, CT: doi:10.1037/1082-989X.4.2.139
JAI. Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (1984). Reasoning about the ability of self
Higgins, E. T., & Parsons, J. E. (1983). Social cognition and the social life and others: A developmental study. Child Development, 55, 1990 1999.
of the child: Stages as sub-cultures. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & doi:10.2307/1129774
W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development (pp. Nurmi, J.-E., & Aunola, K. (2005). Task-motivation during the first school
years: A person-oriented approach to longitudinal data. Learning and
15 62). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Instruction, 15, 103122. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.04.009
Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The impact of mothers gender-role
Parsons, J. E., Adler, T. F., & Kaczala, C. M. (1982). Socialization of
stereotypic beliefs on mothers and childrens ability perceptions. Jour-
achievement attitudes and beliefs: Parental influences. Child Develop-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 932944. doi:10.1037/
ment, 53, 310 321. doi:10.2307/1128973
0022-3514.63.6.932
Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. N. (1977). The development of achievement-
Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Parents, task values, and real-life
related expectancies. Child Development, 48, 10751079. doi:10.2307/
achievement-related choices. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
1128364
Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002).
Patrick, H., Mantzicopoulos, P., Samarapungavan, A., & French, B. F.
Changes in childrens self-competence and values: Gender and domain
(2008). Patterns of young childrens motivation for science and teacher
differences across grades one though twelve. Child Development, 73,
child relationships. Journal of Experimental Education, 76, 121144.
509 527. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00421
doi:10.3200/JEXE.76.2.121-144
Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. (2008). School
Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H. (1996). Motivation in education: Theory,
engagement trajectories and their differential predictive relations to
research, and applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
dropout. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 21 40. doi:10.1111/j.1540-
Ramaswamy, V., DeSarbo, W., Reibstein, D., & Robinson, W. (1993). An
4560.2008.00546.x empirical pooling approach for estimating marketing mix elasticities
Kass, R. E., & Wasserman, L. (1995). A reference Bayesian test for nested with PIMS data. Marketing Science, 12, 103124. doi:10.1287/
hypotheses and its relationship to the Schwarz criterion. Journal of the mksc.12.1.103
American Statistical Association, 90, 928 934. doi:10.2307/2291327 Ratelle, C. F., Guay, F., Larose, S., & Senecal, C. (2004). Family correlates
Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of of trajectories of academic motivation during a school transition: A
components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767778. doi:10.1093/ semiparametric group-based approach. Journal of Educational Psychol-
biomet/88.3.767 ogy, 96, 743754. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.743
Madden, N. A., Slavin, R. E., Karweit, N. L., Dolan, L. J., & Wasik, B. A. Raudenbush, S. W., & Kasim, R. M. (1998). Cognitive skill and economic
(1993). Success for all: Longitudinal effects of a restructuring program inequality: Findings from the National Adult Literacy Survey. Harvard
for inner-city elementary schools. American Educational Research Jour- Educational Review, 68, 3379.
nal, 30, 123148. Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Pianta, R. C., Cox, M. J., & Bradley, R. H. (2003).
Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of Teacher-rated family involvement and childrens social and academic
self-concept: Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Educational outcomes in kindergarten. Early Education and Development, 14, 179
Psychology, 81, 417 430. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.417 198. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1402_3
Marsh, H. W., Byrnes, B. M., & Yeung, A. S. (1999). Causal ordering of Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Academic and
academic self-concept and achievement: Reanalysis of a pioneering emotional functioning in adolescence: Longitudinal relations, patterns,
study and revised recommendations. Educational Psychologist, 34, 155 and prediction by experiences in middle school. Development and Psy-
167. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3403_2 chopathology, 10, 321352. doi:10.1017/S0954579498001631
Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Ludtke, O., Koller, O., & Baumert, J. (2005). Ruble, D., Boggiano, A., Feldman, N., & Lobel, J. (1980). A developmen-
Academic self-concept, interest, grades, and standardized test scores: tal analysis of the role of social comparison in self-evaluation. Devel-
Reciprocal effects models of causal ordering. Child Development, 76, opmental Psychology, 16, 105115. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.16.2.105
397 416. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00853.x Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (1990). Transactional regulation and early
McLachlan, G. J., & Peel, D. (2000). Finite mixture models. New York, intervention. In S. J. Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff (Eds.), Early intervention:
NY: Wiley. A handbook of theory, practice, and analysis (pp. 119 149). New York,
Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1988). The transition to junior NY: Cambridge University Press.
816 ARCHAMBAULT, ECCLES, AND VIDA

Schafer, J. L. (1999). NORM: Multiple imputation of incomplete multi- Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1994). Childrens competence beliefs,
variate data under a normal model (Version 2) [Computer software for achievement values, and general self-esteem: Change across elementary
Windows 95/98/NT]. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State Univer- and middle school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 107138. doi:
sity, Department of Statistics. 10.1177/027243169401400203
Schiefele, U. (1999). Interest and learning from text. Scientific Studies of Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Students motivation during the
Reading, 3, 257279. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0303_4 middle school years. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1997). Social origins of self- Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Mac Iver, D., Reuman, D. A., & Midgley, C.
regulatory competence. Educational Psychologist, 32, 195208. doi: (1991). Transitions during early adolescence: Changes in childrens
10.1207/s15326985ep3204_1 domain-specific self-perceptions and general self-esteem across the tran-
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of sition to junior high school. Developmental Psychology, 27, 552565.
Statistics, 6, 461 464. doi:10.1214/aos/1176344136 doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.4.552
Sclove, L. S. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J.,
problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333343. doi: Freedman-Doan, C., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in childrens
10.1007/BF02294360 competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary
Skaalvik, E. M., & Hagtvet, K. A. (1990). Academic achievement and school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89,
self-concept: An analysis of causal predominance in a developmental
451 469. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.451
perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 292307.
Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of childrens motivation for
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.292
reading to the amount and breadth or their reading. Journal of Educational
Spinath, B., & Spinath, F. M. (2005). Longitudinal analysis of the link
Psychology, 89, 420 432. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.420
between learning motivation and competence beliefs among elementary
Willett, J. B., & Sayer, A. S. (1996). Cross-domain analyses of change over
school children. Learning and Instruction, 15, 87102. doi:10.1016/
time: Combining growth modeling and covariance structure analysis. In
j.learninstruc.2005.04.008
R. Schumacher & G. Marcoulides (Eds.), Advanced structural equation
Spinath, B., & Steinmayr, R. (2008). Longitudinal analysis of intrinsic
motivation and competence beliefs: Is there a relation over time? Child modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 125157). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Development, 79, 15551569. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01205.x Wilson, K. M., & Trainin, G. (2007). First-grade students motivation and
Stipek, D. J. (1984). Sex differences in childrens attributions for success achievement for reading, writing, and spelling. Reading Psychology, 28,
and failure on math and spelling tests. Sex Roles, 11, 969 981. doi: 257282. doi:10.1080/02702710601186464
10.1007/BF00288127 Yeung, W. J., Linver, M. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). How money
Stipek, D., & Mac Iver, D. (1989). Developmental change in childrens matters for young childrens development: Parental investment and
assessment of intellectual competence. Child Development, 60, 521 family processes. Child Development, 73, 18611879. doi:10.1111/
538. doi:10.2307/1130719 1467-8624.t01-1-00511
Sweet, A. P., Guthrie, J. T., & Ng, M. M. (1998). Teacher perceptions and
student reading motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90,
210 223. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.210 Received June 1, 2009
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emer- Revision received July 26, 2010
gent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848 872. Accepted August 4, 2010

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!


Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available
online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and you will be
notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

You might also like