Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

Joumal of Personality and Social Psychology 2013 American Psychological Association

2013. Vol. 105. No. 4, 667-687 0022-3514/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.l037/a0033485

Subcomponents of Psychopathy Have Opposing Correlations With


Punishment Judgments

Jana Schaich Borg Rachel E. Kahn


Stanford University and Duke University University of New Orleans

Walter Sinnott-Aimstrong Robert Kurzban


Duke University University of Pennsylvania

Paul H. Robinson Kent A. Kiehl


Utversity of Pennsylvania Law School University of New Mexico and Lovelace Biomdical and
Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, New Mexico

Psychopathy research is plagued by an enigma: Psychopaths reliably act immorally, but they also
accurately report whether an action is morally wrong. The current study revealed that cooperative
suppressor effects and conflicting subsets of personality traits within the construct of psychopathy might
help explain this conundrum. Among a sample of adult male offenders (N = 100) who ranked deserved
punishment of crimes. Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) total scores were not linearly correlated
with deserved punishment task performance. However, these null results masked significant opposing
associations between task performance and factors of psychopathy: the PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective
(i.e., manipulative and callous) factor was positively associated with task performance, while the PCL-R
Social Deviance (i.e., impulsive and antisocial) factor was simultaneously negatively associated with task
performance. These relationships were qualified by a significant interaction where the Interpersonal/
Affective traits were positively associated with task performance when Social Deviance traits were high,
but Social Deviance traits were negatively associated with task performance when Interpersonal/
Affective traits were low. This interaction helped reveal a significant nonlinear relationship between
PCL-R total scores and task performance such that individuals with very low or very high PCL-R total
scores performed better than those with middle-range PCL-R total scores. These results may explain the
enigma of why individuals with very high psychopathic traits, but not other groups of antisocial
individuals, usually have normal moral judgment in laboratory settings, but still behave immorally,
especially in contexts where Social Deviance traits have strong infiuence.

Keywords: psychopathy, moral judgment. Deserved Punishment Test

Psychopaths are infamous for their immoral behavior. Still should be considered criminally (or morally) responsible for their
unclear, however, is whether psychopaths' behavior is due to crimes (McMillan & Malatesti, 2010).
personality traits that interfere with them knowing what is morally
wrong or, instead, personality traits that interfere with them from
Psychopathy
caring about what is morally wrong. This unresolved issue has
long been at the center of scientific debates about the neural Psychopathy was originally described as a constellation of in-
mechanisms of moral judgment and how to treat psychopaths terpersonal, affective, and behavioral personality traits that in-
(Glenn, 2010), as well as legal debates about whether psychopaths cluded a superficial and manipulative interpersonal style, a pro-

This article was published Online First July 8. 2013. This research was supported in part by National Institute on Drug Abuse
Jana Schaich Borg, Institute for Neuro-Innovation and Transiational Neurosci- Grant 1R01DA026505-01A1 (Principal investigator: Kent A. Kiehl). We
ences, Stanford University, and Department of Psychiatry, Duke University; Ra- are grateful to the staff and inmates of the New Mexico Corrections
chel E. Kahn, Department of Psychology, University of New Orleans; Walter Department for their support and assistance in making this research pos-
Sinnott-Armstrong, Department of Philosophy and Kenan Institute for Ethics, sible.
Duke University; Robert Kunban, Department of Psychology, University of Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jana
Pennsylvania; Paul H. Robinson, University of Pennsylvania Law School; Kent A. Schaich Borg, who is now at the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Kiehl, Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Bryan Research Building
and the Mind Research Network (MRN), an affiliate of Lovelace Biomdical and Room 361, 311 Research Drive, Box 3209, Durham, NC 27710. E-mail:
Environmental Research Institute (LBERI), Albuquerque, New Mexico. borg@neuro.duke.edu

667
668 SCHAICH BORG ET AL,

found lack of empathy or remorse, frequent impulsivity and commitment to one moral conclusion (Schaich Borg, Sinnott-
irresponsibility, and socially deviant behavior or "antisociality" Armstrong, Calhoun, & Kiehl, 2011), Most of the well-established
(Cleckley, 1941, 1976), While some psychopathic traits are similar psychological research relevant to moral judgment assesses either
to the criteria for antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) contained the quality of reasoning that leads up to a moral verdict (e,g,,
in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th Colby & Kohlberg, 1987) or the development of the ability to
ed,; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), psychop- differentiate moral rules from conventional mies (Turiel, 1983),
athy and ASPD differ in at least two important ways. First, adults More recently, attempts have been made to study moral verdicts as
with psychopathic traits represent a minority or distinct subgroup well (e,g,, Cushman, Young, & Huser, 2006), but these tests
of individuals with ASPD (Hare, Hart, & Harpur, 1991; Hart & cannot be validated in the same way as more traditional psycho-
Hare, 1997) who have a particularly severe and chronic pattem of logical tests because there is no universally accepted criteria for
antisocial behavior (Hart, Knopp, & Hare, 1988; Hemphill, Hare, identifying what is morally wrong across individuals and cultures
& Wong, 1998; Kosson, Smith, & Newman, 1990), Second, and and, thus, no universally accepted criteria for assessing the quality
perhaps more important, ASPD criteria do not include the constel- of moral verdicts. Therefore, at present, moral verdict tests are
lation of interpersonal or affective traits that characterize psychop- usually evaluated by their ability to elicit consistent moral conclu-
athy (except lack of remorse or guilt), and instead focus primarily sions across people and populations.
on the antisocial behavioral traits. For example, criteria for ASPD To date, there is little evidence that psychopaths as defined by
include traits of impulsivity, irresponsibility, and a lifelong pattem total scores on the PCL-R have deficits in either the processes
of violating the rights and norms of others (indicated by failure to leading up to a moral verdict or the ability to produce normal
conform to laws and repeated fighting or assaults) but do not moral verdicts. The first moral judgment assessments were based
include traits such as having a superficial and manipulative inter- on Lawrence Kohlberg's moral development theory. To Kohlberg,
personal style (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), As a the most important aspect of moral development was the quality of
consequence of these differences, approximately 75% of male one's moral justifications, not the outcome of one's moral verdicts,
prison inmates would qualify for a diagnosis of ASPD, but only Kohlberg observed that humans progress sequentially through six
30% of those would also qualify for a diagnosis of psychopathy stages of moral reasoning in which concepts such as consequences
according to the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; to oneself, maintaining social order, or maintaining universal
Hare, 1991; 2003; Hart & Hare, 1996; Meloy, 1988), rights for humanity are preferentially used to justify one's moral
verdicts. The Moral Judgment Interview (Colby & Kohlberg,
1987) and the Defining Issues Test (Rest, Cooper, Coder, Masanz,
Assessment of Psychopathy
& Anderson, 1974) were developed to assess the sophistication of
Psychopathy is often clinically assessed in incarcerated adults participants' moral justifications according to this developmental
with the PCL-R (Hare, 1991, 2003), The PCL-R is an expert-rated scheme. Psychopaths' moral reasoning as measured by both of
scale informed by extensive collateral reports and a semistructured these moral development tests is normal (or potentially even above
interview. Using comprehensive descriptions provided in the man- average; Link, Scherer, & Byme, 1977; Lose, 1997; O'Kane,
ual, individuals are rated on 20 specific trait items such as super- Fawcett, & Blackbum, 1996),
ficial charm, pathological lying, lack of remorse, impulsivity, A second class of moral judgment assessments were later de-
irresponsibility, and criminal versatility. These item scores are veloped based on the observation that children as young as age 2
summed to create PCL-R total scores that range from 0 to 40, In years 6 months can differentiate between conventional wrongs
the PCL-R manual, the reported mean PCL-R total scores in large (acts judged to be wrong because societal rules say they are wrong,
samples of participants from North American prison and forensic such as wearing one's pants inside out) and moral wrongs (acts
psychiatric samples are 23,6 (SD = 7,9) and 20,6 (SD = 7,8), judged to be wrong regardless of societal mies, such as hitting
respectively (Hare, 1991), Empirical research suggests PCL-R someone; Smetana, 1981), Variations of moral versus conven-
total scores are dimensional (Edens, Marcus, Lilienfeld, & Poyth- tional tests were designed to assess whether children could distin-
ress, 2006; Guay, Ruscio, Knight, & Hare, 2007; Walters, Duncan, guish between these types of transgressions, and using one of these
& Mitchell-Perez, 2007; Walters, Gray, et al,, 2007), but categor- variations, Blair and colleagues (Blair, 1995; Blair, Jones, Clark, &
ical cutoffs are used in practice to infer recidivism risk levels (Hare Smith, 1995) reported that individuals with psychopathy do not
et al,, 1991; see also Harris, Rice, & Quinsey, 1994), When distinguish between moral versus conventional transgressions.
categorical cutoffs are used, a psychopath is traditionally defined However, in both reports, psychopaths' performance was charac-
in North American participants as someone who receives a total terized by inflation of their ratings of conventional wrongs, not
PCL-R score of 30 or above (Hare, 1991, 2003), The PCL-R was impairment in their ratings of moral wrongs. Further, it was
originally developed for use with male offenders and forensic subsequently shown that psychopaths can differentiate between
patients but has become widely used in populations of female moral and conventional norms as well as incarcerated nonpsycho-
offenders, sex offenders, and substance abusers as well (for a pathic controls if doing so is clearly congment with the experi-
review see Hare, Clark, Grann, & Thomton, 2000), menter's expectations (Aharoni, Sinnott-Armstrong, & Kiehl,
2011), This finding suggests that psychopaths' abnormal perfor-
mance in earlier studies was a result of their efforts to look "more
Psychopathy and Moral Judgment
moral" to the experimenter, not an inability to distinguish between
In this article, moral judgment will be used to refer to the event moral and conventional transgressions.
of judging some act, institution, or person as morally wrong or Most recentiy, tests have been developed to examine people's
right, A moral verdict is the outcome of a moral judgment or the moral verdicts. The most common way to assess moral verdicts is to
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 669

ask people how they would resolve philosophical moral thought the DPT was chosen as the moral judgment assessment for the present
scenarios (Cushman et al., 2006). Thus far, most reports have indi- study. We hypothesized that if psychopaths have impaired moral
cated that psychopaths' verdicts of how to resolve philosophical punishment judgments, PCL-R scores should be negatively correlated
moral thought scenarios are not different from those of nonpsycho- with measures indexing the similarity between individuals' DPT
paths (Cima, Tonnaer, & Huser, 2010; Glenn, Raine, Schug, 2009; scenario rank orderings and previously agreed-upon DPT scenario
Glenn, Raine, Schug, Young, & Huser, 2009). This suggests that rank orderings.
psychopaths can successfully report intuitions consistent with com-
mon moral principles. In addition, in a simpler method of assessing
Subcomponents of Psychopathy May Correlate With
moral verdicts, psychopaths' ratings of the severity of moral viola-
tions depicted in pictures are equally severe as ratings provided by
External Variables in Opposing Directions
nonpsycbopaths (Harenski, Harenski, Shane, & Kiehl, 2010). Psy- When examining the association between psychopathy and
chopathy has been shown to be negatively associated with valuing moral judgment, it is important to consider whether the constella-
certain types of moral virtues (Abaroni, Antonenko, & Kiehl, 2011), tion of traits that form the constmct of psychopathy have the
but it has not yet been demonstrated that this negative correlation potential to be differentially associated with moral judgment. Psy-
translates into different pattems of moral verdicts. Therefore, overall chopathic personality traits measured by the PCL-R can be mean-
there is little evidence supporting tbe hypothesis that psychopaths ingfully divided into an Interpersonal/Affective factor (Factor 1)
have deficits in moral judgment during laboratory tasks in controlled and a Social Deviance factor (Factor 2; Hare, 2003; Harpur, Hare,
environments. This lack of correlation between psychopathy and & Hakstian, 1989). The Interpersonal/Affective factor includes
moral judgment impairment has proven puzzling for researchers in traits corresponding with psychopaths' social doininance, manip-
medical, psychological, or legal fields trying to understand why ulation, and efficacy (as well as separate traits associated with lack
psychopaths still consistently perform immoral acts outside experi- of general affect, including empathy). The Social Deviance factor
mental contexts. includes traits corresponding to psychopaths' impulsivity, irre-
sponsibility, and antisocial behavior. The Interpersonal/Affective
and Social Deviance factors of the PCL-R are modestly correlated
Deserved Punishment Test
(Hare, 2003), so when their shared variance correlates with per-
One kind of moral judgment that has not been assessed in psycho- sonality traits or behavior in the same way (either in the same
paths is judgment about the amount of punishment deserved by those direction or not at all), the associations between the unique aspects
committing differing types of wrongdoing. Robinson and Kurzban's of the PCL-R factors and variables of interest are often obscured
(2007) Deserved Punishment Test (DPT) was designed to assess or "suppressed" unless their shared variance is removed through
whether society has shared intuitions of justice that lead people to regression or partial correlation. This phenomenon, called coop-
agree on an ordinal ranking of offenders according to their blame- erative suppression, is well documented (Paulhus, Robins,
worthiness. The DPT asks participants to read 24 short scenarios Trzesniewski, & Tracy, 2004) and has previously been shown to
describing a situation where "John" performs an act that may or may conceal relationships between PCL-R factor scores and otber
not be considered a criminal offense (such as accidentally taking personality measures (Blonigen et al., 2010; Hicks & Patrick,
somebody else's umbrella, stealing a tnicrowave from somebody's 2006). Technically, cooperative suppression in PCL-R factors can
house, or intentionally shooting someone) and rank order them ac- be identified as cases when the sum of partial regression coeffi-
cording to how much punishment, if any, John deserved for each act. cients between PCL-R factors and an extemal variable are greater
This task was intentionally designed to be a ranking task because than the sum of the simple, or zero-order, regression coefficients
ratings of the relative seriousness of crimes are more consistent across between each PCL-R factor in isolation and the same extemal
cultures than the ratings of the absolute seriousness of crimes (Rob- variable (Cohen & Cohen, 1975).
inson & Kurzban, 2007). Robinson & Kurzban (2007) demonsti-ated When cooperative suppression effects are taken into account,
that participants from community samples have "almost perfect" PCL-R factor scores have been shown to correlate with other types of
agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977) about what the "normal" ranking personahty traits or behaviors in opposite directions. For example, the
of punishment deserved by each scenario should be in the DPT. This Interpersonal/Affective and Social Deviance factors of the PCL-R
agreement is so strong that the results of the DPT have been used to have been shown to have mutually repulsive effects in the prediction
argue that shared intuitions of justice could be useful for guiding of self-reports of emotional distress and fearfulness (Hicks & Patrick,
policy and legal theory relevant to legal sentencing (Carlsmith & 2006; Patrick, 1994), anger and hostility (Hicks & Patrick, 2006;
Darley, 2008; Robinson & Darley, 2007). Reidy, Shelley-Tremblay, & Lilienfeld, 2011), positive affect (Pat-
The DPT has three critical characteristics that make it well suited to rick, 1994), achievement (Verona, Patrick, & Joiner, 2001), stress
study the relationship between psychopathy and moral judgment: (a) reactivity (Verona et al., 2001), interpersonal aggression (Kennealy,
It asks for judgments that have no clear correct or incorrect answers, Hicks, & Patrick, 2007), and attempted suicide history (Verona,
minimizing the potentially dominating influence of impression man- Hicks, & Patrick, 2005). Thus, the Interpersonal/Affective and Social
agement, (b) it allows for a wide range of performance, and (c) Deviance factors of the PCD-R often have contrasting associations
performance can be indexed by a continuous variable representing with extemal criteria, despite their shared ability to represent parts of
how similar a participant's ranking order is to the modal ranking the overall constmct of psychopathy, but these effects are not detect-
order, rather than a constrained interval or ordinal variable. These able unless statistical models take their shared variance into account.
characteristics make the test both more resilient to intentional manip- These cooperative suppression effects could potentially explain why
ulation and more sensitive to subtle differences than moral judgment previous studies have reported difficulty in identifying relationships
tests previously used in incarcerated populations. For these reasons. between psychopathy and moral judgment. The unique aspects of the
670 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

two PCL-R factors may have opposing associations with moral level of education, 22.3% had no high school degree, 40.4%
judgment that cancel each other out when PCL-R total scores are used completed high school or obtained a General Educational Devel-
to assess psychopathy. opment (GED) degree, 25.5% had some college education, 8.5%
had a 2-year college degree, 1.1% had a 4-year college degree, and
Current Study 2.2% had a graduate degree.
Participants were excluded if they were older than 60 years; had
In the current study, we assessed whether the Interpersonal/ a history of seizures, epilepsy, or psychosis as assessed by the
Affective factor (Factor 1) and the Social Deviance factor (Factor Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon,
2) of psychopathy have common or distinct correlations with & Williams, 1997); had experienced a clinical loss of conscious-
moral judgment as measured by DPT performance. To do this, we ness exceeding 30 min; or had other major medical problems.
implemented regression models that allowed us to investigate These exclusions were made as part of SWANC, in accordance
whether the PCL-R factors have contrasting associations with with standard clinical neuroimaging protocols. In addition, all
moral judgment that are masked when their shared variance is not participants were required to show fluency in English at or above
taken into account. If true, the opposing relationships between a fourth-grade reading level and have an estimated IQ (see de-
PCL-R factors and moral judgment could cancel each other out scription of IQ measure in the Materials section) of at least 70
when both sets of traits are present in high-scoring psychopaths, (M = 98.3, SD = 14.4).
explaining why their moral judgment appears normal. To maxi- Consistent with the average wage in the correctional system,
mize our chances of being able to detect conflicting correlations participants were paid $1.00/hr for their time. The study was
between subsets of personality traits, we administered the DPT to approved by the Human Research Review Committee at the Uni-
a large sample of incarcerated individuals with a wide range of versity of New Mexico, and all participants provided written
PCL-R scores, including many who scored above the clinical informed consent.
criteria for psychopathy. In addition, previous research has shown
that anxiety can interact with psychopathy (Lilienfeld & Penna,
Materials
2001) and that individuals with high levels of psychopathic traits
may manipulate their answers to impress experimenters (Blair, Moral judgment. The deserved punishment test (DPT; Rob-
1995; Book, Holden, Starzyk, Wasylkiw, & Edwards, 2006; Wil- inson & Kurzban, 2007) consists of 24 short scenarios describing
son & McCarthy, 2011). Therefore, we also assessed whether trait a situation in which "John" performs an act that may or may not be
anxiety or impression management influenced the association be- considered a criminal offense (see Appendix A for transcription of
tween PCL-R and DPT perfonnance. each scenario). Robinson & Kurzban (2007) collected two separate
Overall we predicted that (a) PCL-R total scores would not data sets in community population samples to help illustrate how
correlate with DPT scenario ratings, consistent with previous stud- consistent relative assessments of the punishment deserved for
ies reporting a lack of relationship between psychopathy and moral crimes can be. One sample of 64 participants was given the test in
judgment; (b) the variance in DPT performance accounted for by person and another sample of 246 participants was given the test
the addition of both PCL-R factor scores simultaneously in a online. Both samples gave highly consistent rankings of the pun-
regression model would be more than expected if the variance ishments deserved in the 24 DPT scenarios (Robinson & Kurzban,
accounted for by each factor in isolation was simply added (indi- 2007, summarized in Table 1 of the present study). Participants
cating a cooperative suppression effect); and (c) when both factor only reversed the modal pairwise rankings of the DPT scenarios
scores were entered simultaneously in a regression model, PCL-R 4% and 8.2% of the time, respectively (Robinson & Kurzban,
factor scores would correlate with DPT scenario ratings in oppos- 2007). Further, the Kendall's coefficient of concordancea sta-
ing directions. If these predictions were confirmed, the results of tistic that measures agreement among rank orderings of the same
the present study could help explain the lack of relationship set of stimuli (across the entire ranking, not just pairwise compar-
between psychopathy and moral judgment reported in the past and isons)was 0.95 for the cohort of 64 participants and 0.88 for the
could help reconcile intuitions that psychopaths' immoral behavior cohort of 246 participants, where a coefficient of 1.0 indicates
should somehow be related to impairments in moral judgment. perfect agreement and 0.0 indicates no agreement (Robinson &
Kurzban, 2007). This level of agreement was similar across the
different demographic groups represented in the larger sample of
Method 246 participants.'

Participants
' The demographics of Robinson and Kurzban's (2007) nonincarcerated
One hundred adult male itimates were recruited from the West- populations were as follows: (a) University population (A^ = 64): gender: 36%
em New Mexico Correctional Facility, a minimum-medium secu- male, 64% female; race: 91% White, 9% non-White; age: 2% age 20 years or
younger, 11% ages 21-30, 5% ages 31-40, 11% ages 41-50, 22% ages
rity institution in Grants, New Mexico, as part of the larger
51-60,47% ages 61 or older; highest level of education: 5% some college, 2%
Southwest Advanced Neuroimaging Cohort-Adult sample 2-year college degree, 38% 4-year college degree, 39% master's degree, 17%
(SWANC-A) from 2007 to 2010. Participants' ages ranged from doctorate or professional degree; and (b) Internet population (N = 246):
18 to 56 years (M = 33.7; SD = 9.4). Ethnic/racial composition gender: male 50%, female 50%; race: 6% Hispanic, 70% White, 1% Native
was 52% Hispanic or Latino, 36% White, 7% Black or African American, 5% mixed, and 4% other; age: 6% age 20 years or younger, 15%
ages 21-30, 21% ages 31-40, 18% ages 41-50, 17% ages 51-60, 23% ages
American, 3% American Indian or Alaska Native, and 2% of other 61 or older; highest level of education: 4% no high school degree, 18% high
or mixed ethnicity. Participants were classified based on their school graduate, 47% some college, 7% 2-year college degree, 14% 4-year
self-classification into these categories. When asked their highest college degree, and 10% graduate degree.
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 671
Table 1
Comparison of Punishment Ratings Provided by Present Incarcerated Population With Those Previously Provided by
Nonincarcerated Populations

University nonincarcerated population Intemet nonincarcerated population (n =


(n = 64, Robinson and Kurzban, 2007) 246, Robinson and Kurzban, 2007) Present incarcerated population (n = 100)
Mean Modal % Assigning Mean Modal % Assigning Mean Modal % Assigning
Scenario rank ranking "no punishment" rank ranking "no punishment" rank ranking "no punishment"
SI (defending) 0.6 0 91 1.2 0 82 2.03 0 78
S2 (coercion) 0.3 0 92 2.3 0 75 1.03 0 82
S3 (umbrella) L4 0 92 0.3 0 87 0.53 0 87
S4 (hallucination) 0.3 0 92 L7 0 71 1.44 0 79
S5 (pies) 3.9 5 27 5.4 5 8 3.51 0 41
S6 (T-shirt) 6.3 6* 8 7.4 1
S7 (short change) 6.8 9 7.1
r 6
6.31
6.98
7*
6*
18*

S8 (radio) 7.6 r8 5 8.4


e8 0 7.28 8
14*
12*
S9 (drill) 8.8 9 3 9.2 9 0 9.48 10 7
SIO (microwave) 9.9 10 2 10.2 10 0 9.48 10 6
Sll (TV) 10.4 11 2 10.6 11 0 9.63 11 8
S12 (slap) 12.1 12 0 11.6 12 0 10.52 12 12*
S13 (head-butt) 13 13 2 12.2 13 2 10.96 13 14*
S14 (stitches) 14.1 14 0 13.7 14 0 11.73 14 13*
S15 (necklace) 14.8 15 13.6 15 0 14.74 16 2
S16 (robbery) 15.6 16 0 15.3 16 0 14.46 16 6
S17 (clubbing) 16.8 17 0 16.7 17 1 15.94 17 1
S18 (pit bulls) 18.2 18 0 17.8 18 0 16.53 18 4
S19 (infant) 18.6 19 3 19 19 1 21J6* 22* 0
S20 (stabbing) 19.7 20 0 20.1 20 0 19.87 19 0
S21 (ambush) 20.9 21 0 20.9 21 0 20.47 21 0
S22 (abduction) 21.7 22 0 21.4 22 0 20.6 21 1
S23 (burning) 23 23 0 22.2 23 0 22.5 23 0
S24 (ransom) 23.9 24 0 23.5 24 0 23.64 24 0
Note. Bold entries marked with an asterisk indicate notable differences. S = scenario.

Replicating the procedures implemented with Robinson and PCL-R consists of 20 items, each of which is scored on a 3-point
Kurzban's population sample given the test in person, we pasted scale; 0 {doesn't apply), 1 {applies somewhat), and 2 {definitely
each scenario onto one index card for a total of 24 cards. Partic- applies) to the individual. Total scores range from 0 to 40 (Hare,
ipants were asked to read each scenario and rank order the cards to 1991). In the present population, PCL-R total scores ranged from
refiect the amount of punishment, if any, John deserved for each 3.2-36.8 {SD = 7.8), with 25 participants scoring a 30 or above.
act. Once completed, participants were asked to review and com- Double ratings were conducted on approximately 17% of the
pare each scenario to the one next to it and confirm their answers. sample, and reliability was high; interclass correlation coefficient
The approved rank orderings were then recorded. Community (ICC) = .96 for total scores, .92 for the Interpersonal/Affective
participants have "almost perfect" agreement (Landis & Koch, factor, and .97 for the Social Deviance factor.
1977) about what the "normal" ranking of punishment deserved by General intelligence. A two-subtest form of the Wechsler
each scenario should be (Robinson & Kurzban, 2007; provided for Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997) was used to
comparison in Table 1). The rankings of the present incarcerated collect a full-scale IQ estimate. The two subtests. Vocabulary and
population were compared with this previously established "nor- Matrix Reasoning, have been shown to provide a reliable estimate
mal" ranking using the statistical tests described in detail later. of full-scale IQ (Ryan, Lopez, & Werth, 1999). One individual did
(For further details about the theory behind the deserved punish- not complete the IQ assessment.
ment test, see Robinson & Kurzban, 2007.) In the present study, Anxiety. Trait anxiety was assessed using the Spielberger
one inmate scored over three standard deviations below the mean State-Trait Anxiety Scale (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lush-
on the DPT task and was suspected of not paying attention to the ene, 1970). The STAI is a 40-item self-report measure containing
task and was therefore excluded from all analyses. a 20-item section that assesses trait anxiety (how much anxiety a
Psychopathy. All participants were evaluated for psychopa- person generally feels). Participants rate each statement on a
thy using the PCL-R (Hare, 1991, 2003). This assessment includes 4-point scale ranging from almost never to almost always (e.g., "I
a semistmctured interview that provides in-depth information feel nervous and restless" or "I am 'cool, calm, and collected'"
about participants' school, family, work, and criminal history, as [reverse-scored]). Within our sample, reliability for the trait anx-
well as their interpersonal and emotional skills. All participants iety scale of the STAI was Cronbach's alpha = .93. Five individ-
signed a release form allowing the researcher to review their uals did not complete the STAI, and an additional four had missing
institutional files (criminal history, social worker reports, family or incomplete data (total missing, n = 9).
history, education, employment, and so forth), which was used as Social desirability and impression management. The Bal-
sources of collateral information for scoring the PCL-R. The anced Inventory of Desired Responding (BIDR-6; Paulhus, 1991)
672 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

is a 40-item self-report scale designed to assess whether individ- tistic used to assess agreement among raters, was used to assess
uals are likely to distort their answers on tests or questionnaires in agreement in DPT scenario rankings across participants. A Kend-
an effort to portray themselves positively. The BIDR is composed all's tau statistic was calculated for each participant to measure the
of two scales: a Self-Deceptive Enhancement (SDE) scale and an similarity (or distance) between each participant's rank ordering
Impression Management (IM) scale, each composed of 20 items and the "normal" or modal rank order agreed upon by the nonin-
(e.g., "I never swear" or "I always obey laws, even if I'm unlikely carcerated university population from Robinson and Kurzban,
to get caught"). For the purposes of the present study, only the IM 2007 (provided in Table 1). The nonincarcerated university pop-
scale was used. The IM scale assesses the extent to which an ulation was chosen as the reference population because, like the
individual deliberately tries to present himself or herself in a present population, they completed the task with physical cards in
favorable light to others. All items on the BIDR are rated on a person, whereas Robinson and Kurzban's second community pop-
7-point scale (1 = not true, 4 = somewhat true, and 7 = very true). ulation completed the task over the Intemet.^ The Kendall's tau
Higher scores on the BIDR suggest an individual has greater statistic is a metric that counts the number of pairwise disagree-
socially desirable responding, or is attempting to present himself ments between two lists and gives a value from 1 to 1, where
or herself in a more positive light. In past research, the BIDR has values of 1 represent a perfect negative relationship between the
been shown to successfully measure socially desirable responding two lists (or complete disagreement), values of 1 represent a
in offender populations similar to the one used in the present study perfect positive relationship between the two lists (or complete
(Kroner & Weekes, 1996). In addition, intemal consistency for the agreement), and values of 0 represent no relationship at all
two scales has been adequate (.80) in both general and forensic (Lapata, 2006). Therefore, if a participant ranked the DPT scenar-
samples (Kroner & Weekes, 1996; Paulhus, 1991). In our sample, ios in the exact same order as Robinson and Kurzban's nonincar-
this scale demonstrated adequate intemal consistency as well cerated university population, that participant would have a Ken-
(Cronbach's ct = .78). Two individuals did not complete the dall's tau value of I. If a participant ranked the DPT scenarios in
BIDR, and IM scores were not calculated for an additional three the exact opposite order as Robinson and Kurzban's nonincarcer-
participants due to missing or incomplete data (total missing, ated university population, that participant would have a Kendall's
n = 5). tau value of 1. If there was no relationship between a partici-
Demographic variables. All participants completed a demo- pant's DPT scenario rankings and the rankings provided by Rob-
graphic questionnaire that included questions regarding ethnicity, inson and Kurzban's university nonincarcerated population, that
age, education level, income level prior to incarceration, number of participant would have a Kendall's tau value of 0.
years incarcerated, number of times incarcerated, and prison se- We conducted hierarchical regression analyses to test the effects
curity level. Education level was coded using a 7-point scale of psychopathy on DPT performance using Kendall's tau as the
ranging from 1 (some high school) to 3 (some college) to 7 dependent variable. In all ofthese analyses, Kendall's tau was logit
(doctoral/professional degree). Similarly, income level was coded transformed (log[x/l - x]) to improve heteroscedasticity.^ Finally,
on a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (less than $20,000/year) to 6 significant interactions between PCL-R factors were analyzed
(more than $100,000/year). Age, number of years incarcerated, using the regression equation from the full sample to calculate the
and number of times incarcerated were coded as continuous vari- predicted tau values if Factors 1 and 2 were high ( 1 SD above the
ables. Prison security level ranged from Level 2 (minimum secu- mean) or low (1 SD below the mean; Holmbeck, 2002). The slopes
rity) to Level 4 (medium security). of the simple effects from these post hoc analyses were then used
to assess statistical significance.
Procedure
Results
This study was conducted as part of a larger neuroimaging
research project that began in 2007. In total, the participants
completed 8-15 hr of research over the course of five to six visits. Contribution of Demographic Variables
The assessments pertinent to the present research took between
3-4 hr, and participants were paid for their time as they progressed To test the contribution of demographic and relevant cognitive
through the research study. As recommended by the institutional variables to PCL-R total and factor scores and determine whether
review board, participants were told they could skip any questions they needed to be considered in analyses investigating the rela-
that they did not feel comfortable answering.
^ The modal ranking of the university population reversed the order of
Data Analysis one set of scenarios compared with the Internet population, indicated by an
asterisk and bold font in Table 1, but the basic results of the present study
We analyzed data with SPSS Version 19. Pearson correlations do not change if the modal rank provided by Robinson and Kurzban's
Internet population is used as the reference rank order instead of the modal
and analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to assess the rank order provided by Robinson and Kurzban's university population.
association between PCL-R scores and other potentially con- ' The logit transform was chosen because it is the standard transforma-
founding variables. The DPT was intentionally designed to assess tion for proportional variables bounded by 0 and 1 (Lesaffre, Rizopoulos,
relative (as opposed to absolute) judgments of moral punishment, & Tsonaka, 2007). The transformation did reduce heteroscedasticity in the
and performance on the DPT is defined by the order in which Kendall's tau distribution and was therefore used for analyses reported in
the main text, but as a cautionary measure, all the analyses were repeated
participants rank the 24 DPT scenarios. Therefore, DPT perfor- on nontransformed values of Kendall's tau. The results of these models
mance was assessed using statistical tests comparing ordinal rank- were comparable to the results discussed in the main text and are provided
ings. Kendall's coefficient of concordance, a nonparametric sta- in Appendix B.
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 673
tionship between psychopathy and moral judgment, we computed consistent with previous reports that all three variables correlate
two-tailed Pearson correlations between all PCL-R scores (total. positively with moral reasoning as defined by Kohlberg's moral
Interpersonal/Affective factor. Social Deviance factor, and all four development stages (Freeman & Giebink, 1979; Gibbs, Basinger,
PCL-R facet scores), all demographic variables (age, IQ, educa- Grime, & Snarey, 2007), In addition, Kendall's tau was positively
tion level, income level, number of years incarcerated, number of correlated with years of incarceration (r = ,35, p < ,05), even when
times incarcerated, and prison security level), and task perfor- age was taken into account (r= .16,p< ,05), but not number of times
mance (see Table 2), Age was negatively correlated with PCL-R in prison (p = .11). An ANOVA showed that White participants had
total (r = ,22, p = ,03) and Social Deviance factor scores (r = significantiy higher Kendall's tau values on the DPT than Hispanic
,30, p < ,01), but not significantiy associated with Interpersonal/ participants, F(\, 89) = 8,95, p < ,01, Because age was the only
Affective factor scores. This is consistent with previous literature variable significantly correlated with both psychopathy scores and
showing age-related changes in psychopathic traits are usually Kendall's tau, age was included as a covariate in all subsequent
explained by a decline in Social Deviance traits (such as impul- regression models using either PCL-R total or Social Deviance factor
sivity and antisocial behavior) rather than changes in Interpersonal/ scores as independent variables and Kendall's tau as the dependent
Affective traits (such as callousness or superficial charm), which variable by entering age in the first block of each hierarchical regres-
tend to remain stable across age spans (Hare, 2003; Harpur &
sion. Regression models including IQ and education levels as cova-
Hare, 1994),
riates were also tested, but adding these independent variables to the
To assess potential effects of ethnicity, we compared PCL-R models did not affect the statistical relationship between psychopathy
scores for the two largest ethnic groups in our sample (White, n = variables and task performance (Appendix C), so they were not used
36; Hispanic, n = 52) using ANOVAs, Sample sizes for the other
as primary analyses.
ethnic groups were too small for statistical comparison. No sig-
nificant differences in PCL-R total (F(l, 86) = 0,27, p = ,61) or
factor scores (Interpersonal/Affective: F(l, 85) = 0.01, p = ,93; Validation of the Deserved Punishment Test in an
Social Deviance: F(l, 86) = 1,10, p = ,30) were found between Incarcerated Sample
ethnic groups.
Associations between all demographic variables and Kendall's tau, We validated the use of the DPT in our incarcerated population
the measurement of how similar a participant's rankings are to the by comparing three characteristics of DPT performance in our
rankings provided by nonincarcerated populations (see Methods), are population with that of nonincarcerated populations: (a) the modal
also reported in Table 2, Kendall's tau was positively correlated with rankings of each DPT scenario, (b) the amount of agreement across
age, IQ, and education level (aUp < ,05), In previous studies of moral participants about how the DPT scenarios should be ranked (indi-
verdicts, the relationship of age, IQ, and education level to task cated by Kendall's coefficient of concordance), and (c) the distri-
performance has not been examined, but the present results are bution of Kendall's tau values from individual participants.

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Between Study Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 M(SD)
1, PCL-R Total 22,6 (7,8)
PCL-R factors
2, Interpersonal/Affective .88 7,0 (3,8)
3. Social Deviance .87 .60 13,3 (3,9)
PCL-R facets
4, Interpersonal .74 .84 .47 2.9 (2,2)
5, Affective .76 .87 .56 .46 4,1 (2,3)
6, Lifestyle .76 .55 .84 .49 .46 6,1 (2,1)
7, Antisocial .73 .49 .84 .31 .49 .42 7,2 (2,4)
Control variables
8, Age - . 2 2 - , 1 3 - . 3 0 -,03 -,17 -.33 -,19+ 33,7 (9,4)
9, IQ ,01 ,06 -,07 ,14 - , 0 2 -,09 -,03 ,18+ 98,3 (14,4)
10. Education level ,09 ,13 -,04 .29 - , 0 2 ,01 -,09 .27 .46 2,3(1,1)
11, Income level ,05 .08 -,02 ,15 -,04 -,09 ,05 ,03 ,12 .22 2,6(1,6)
12, Years incarcerated ,07 ,06 .04 ,18 - , 0 6 -,02 ,06 .39 .46 .26 -,02 4,8 (6,2)
13, No, of times incarcerated ,08 ,02 ,11 ,01 ,05 ,06 ,12 .43 -.25 -,07 -,10 -,18 2,1 (1,7)
14, Prison security level ,16 ,14 ,18+ ,05 ,19+ ,02 .29 -,12 .19 ,10 -,01 .29 .19+ 2,5 (0,7)
15, BIDR-IM Scale -,20+ -,10 - . 2 8 -,08 -,08 -.21 -.27 ,14 ,10 ,12 ,13 ,09 -.10 -,03 63,9(17,1)
16, STAI Trait Anxiety -,06 -,11 ,01 -,07 - , 1 2 .09 -,06 ,18 ,10 ,19 -,11 -.05 .09 -,07 ,08 37,0(11,3)
Dependent variable
17, Kendall's tau -,05 ,09 -.20 .21 -,04 -.14 - . 2 2 .30 .55 .37 ,13 .35 -,17 ,01 ,11 -,06 0,74(0,36)
Note. N = 100, Kendall's tau was inverse logit transformed before the reported correlations were calculated. The mean and standard deviation of
Kendall's tau in the table reflects the logit-transformed variable; as reported in the text; the mean and standard deviation of the nontransformed variable
was 0,81 (SD = 0,11), with a range from 0,47-0,99, PCL-R = Psychopathy Checklist-Revised; BIDR-IM = Balanced Inventory of Desired
Responding-Impression Management; STAI = Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Correlations in bold signiflcant at p < 05
+ p < ,08,
674 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

The modal rankings provided for how much punishment each anxiety was not considered to be a confounding variable and was
scenario deserved were almost identical between the present in- not included as a covariate in subsequent regression analyses.
carcerated population and the previously described nonincarcer-
ated populations (Table 1). Minor differences in rankings are
Ruling Out Potentially Confounding Influences of
indicated by bold type and an asterisk in Table 1. The consistency
of modal rankings across the three populations indicates that they
Impression Management
are a reasonable reference with which to compare individuals' To confirm that DPT performance was not sensitive to impres-
moral punishment judgments. sion management, we calculated the two-tailed Pearson correlation
Kendall's coefficient of concordance was used to statistically between participants' Kendall's tau values and their scores on the
assess how much participants agreed in their rankings of how BIDR IM subscale. There was no significant correlation between
much punishment each of the 24 items in the DPT deserved. Our BIDR IM subscale scores (p = .27) and Kendall's tau. Therefore,
incarcerated sample presented very high agreement in their rank- impression management did not correlate with moral judgment and
ings of deserved punishment, with a Kendall's W of .86 overall was not included as a covariate in subsequent regression analyses.
(p < .001). Similarly, Robinson and Kurzban's university popu-
lation of 64 participants provided rankings with a Kendall's W of
.95 and Robinson and Kurzban's Intemet-based population of 246 PCL-R Total Scores and DPT Performance
participants provided rankings with a Kendall's W of .88 (Robin- To test the effects of psychopathy on DPT performance, we
son & Kurzban, 2007). This means that like nonincarcerated implemented a hierarchical regression with Kendall's tau as the
participants, incarcerated participants have "almost perfect" agree- dependent variable, age in the first block (because it was correlated
ment (Landis & Koch, 1977) about what the "normal" ranking of with both PCL-R total scores and Kendall's tau as described
punishment deserved in each scenario should be in the DPT. In previously in the Contribution of Demographic Variables sec-
other words, all three populations of incarcerated or nonincarcer- tion),'* and PCL-R total scores in the second block. As summa-
ated participants have shared intuitions of how offenders should be rized in Table 3, PCL-R total scores did not account for a signif-
ranked according to their blameworthiness. icant amount of variance in DPT performance (p = .84). The R^
We also calculated a Kendall's tau statistic for each participant change due to the addition of PCL-R total scores was 0. Thus, as
to measure how similar each participant's rank ordering was to the predicted, when PCL-R total scores are used to index psychopa-
modal ranking provided by nonincarcerated populations. The mean thy, there was no significant linear association between psychop-
Kendall's tau value from the present incarcerated population was athy and moral judgment.
0.81 (SD = 0.11), with a range from 0.47-0.99 and a distribution
that was positively skewed. The mean Kendall's tau value from the
previously published nonincarcerated university population was
Isolated PCL-R Factor Scores and DPT Performance
0.93 (SD = 0.06) with a range from 0.69-1.00, and the Kendall's To test the effects of each factor score in isolation on DPT
tau value from the previously published nonincarcerated Intemet performance, we implemented a hierarchical regression with Ken-
population was 0.85 (SD = 0.11) with a range from 0.42-0.99. dall's tau as the dependent variable, age in the first block, and
Both of these samples were also positively skewed (Appendix D). either the Interpersonal/Affective PCL-R factor (Factor 1) or the
These comparable Kendall's tau distributions indicate that the Social Deviance PCL-R factor (Factor 2) scores in the second
current sample of incarcerated adults provided a distribution of block. Consistent with the zero-order correlations provided in
punishment rankings similar to those provided by nonincarcerated Table 2, neither the Interpersonal/Affective PCL-R factor (p =
adults. Overall, the results of these three metrics from our incar- .19) nor the Social Deviance PCL-R factor (p = .25) scores
cerated population support the use of the DPT as an assessment of individually accounted for a significant amount of variance. The
robust shared intuitions of justice that can be used to probe the R^ change due to the addition of Interpersonal/Affective scores or
relationship between psychopathy and moral punishment judg- the Social Deviance scores was 0.02 or 0.01, respectively. Thus,
ments. compatible with our predictions, when PCL-R factor scores are
used in isolation to index psychopathy, psychopathy is not asso-
Ruling Out the Confounding Influences of Anxiety ciated with moral judgment.

To ensure that anxiety could not explain or alter correlations


between PCL-R total scores and DPT performance, we examined
Combined PCL-R Factor Scores and DPT
the interaction among trait anxiety using the STAI (Spielberger et Performance
al., 1970), psychopathy, and DPT performance. We performed a To test whether cooperative suppression could reveal opposing
hierarchical regression with each participant's Kendall's tau value relationships between PCL-R factors and DPT performance that
as the dependent variable, age entered in Step 1 (because it was would not be observable when either PCL-R factor score was
correlated with both PCL-R total scores and Kendall's tau as assessed in isolation, we implemented a hierarchical regression
described previously in the Contribution of Demographic Vari- with Kendall's tau as the dependent variable, age in the first block,
ables section),'* total PCL-R scores and STAI scores entered in and both the PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective factor (Factor 1) and
Step 2, and a Psychopathy X anxiety interaction term (PCL-R
scores X STAI scores) entered in Step 3. There were no significant "* As a cautionary measure, all the analyses reported here were repeated
interactions between psychopathy and trait anxiety for DPT per- without age included in the model. The results of the models without age
formance, ^R^ = .01, f (1, 86) = 0.57, p = .45, b = -0.08. Thus, were comparable to the results discussed in the main text.
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 675
Table 3
Regression Analyses With Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Total and Factor Scores as Predictors
of Deserved Punishment Test Performance (as Measured by Logit-Transformed Kendall's Tau)

Variable B SE
Psychopathy total scores
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .31"
Step 2 <.O1
PCL-R Total <0.01 0.01 .02
Psychopathy factor scores (entered independentiy)
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .32"
Step 2 .02
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.01 0.01 .13
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .27**
Step 2 .01
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor -0.01 0.01 -.12
Psychopathy factor scores (entered simultaneously)
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .25*
Step 2 .07*
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.03 0.01 .30**
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor -0.03 0.01 -.30*
Psychopathy factor interaction
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .28"
Step 2 .07*
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.03 0.01 .30**
PCI^R Social Deviance Factor -0.02 0.01 -.22+
Step 3 .06**
Interpersonal/Affective X Social Deviance <0.01 <0.01 .26**
Psychopathy total scores with quadratic term
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .31"
Step 2 .08*
PCL-R Total <0.01 0.01 .08
PCL-R Total^ (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .29**
Psychopathy factor scores with quadratic term
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .27"
Step 2 .11"
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.01 0.01 .10
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective^ (quadratic) 0.01 <0.01 .32"
Step 1 .09**
Age 0.01 <0.01 .28**
Step 2 .02
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor -0.01 0.01 -.06
PCL-R Social Deviance^ (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .11
Note. SE = standard error; PCL-R = Psychopathy Checklist-Revised.
V = 076. * p < . 0 5 . "p<.Q\.

the PCL-R Social Deviance Factor (Factor 2) scores in the secotid all's tau values, meaning they predicted participants would provide
block. As predicted, the R^ change due to the addition of both punishment rankings that were less similar to the modal punish-
factor scores simultaneously was 0.07, more than expected if the ment rankings ( = -0.30, p = .02). Given that neither factor
variance accounted for by each variable in isolation was simply significantly correlated with tau on their own, these results suggest
added. This indicates that cooperative suppression was present in that the PCL-R factors only predict DPT performance when their
the relationship between PCL-R factor scores and DPT perfor- shared variance is removed through regression analysis. In other
mance. Also predicted, PCL-R Factor 1 and Factor 2 scores words, the characteristics of the Interpersonal/Affective factor that
correlated with DPT perfonnance in opposite directions (Table 3). predict "normal" DPT perfonnance are the characteristics that are
Interpersonal/Affective factor (Factor 1) scores predicted larger unique and that do not overlap with the Social Deviance factor.
Kendall's tau values, meaning they predicted participants would Likewise, the characteristics of the Social Deviance factor that
provide punishment rankings that were more similar to the modal predict deviant DPT performance are the characteristics that are
punishment rankings ( = 0.7,0, p = .01). PCL-R Social Deviance unique and that do not overlap with the Interpersonal/Affective
factor (Factor 2) scores simultaneously predicted smaller Kend- factor.
676 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

Regression Analysis: Interaction Between PCL-R scores were high (-1-1 SD in Figure 1), obscuring any effects of the
Factor Scores and DPT Performance Social Deviance factor but permitting the observation of the effects
of the Interpersonal/Affective factor.
To test whether an interaction between the Interpersonal/Affec-
tive factor (Factor 1) and the Social Deviance factor (Factor 2)
could account for variance in DPT performance, we repeated the Implications of the Interaction Between PCL-R Factor
previously described regression with the two centered factor scores Scores and DPT Performance for PCL-R Total Scores
in the second block, and a multiplicative interaction term between
the two factors in a third block. The interaction was significant PCL-R total scores are used more often than PCL-R factor
( = 0.26, p < .01). Collectively, the interaction term, the Inter- scores to assess psychopathy in both research and clinical settings.
personal/Affective factor, and the Social Deviance factor ac- Thus, in order to make our results more directiy comparable to past
counted for 13.1% ofthe variance {p < .01) in Kendall's tau, with research and applicable to clinical settings, we translated the
the individual factors accounting for 7.0% of the variance and the results of the significant PCL-R factor interaction into correspond-
interaction term accounting for an additional 6.1% ofthe variance. ing PCL-R total scores. This was done by calculating the mean
To plot this interaction, we calculated the simple slopes of the PCL-R total scores for participants with low (1 SD below the
effects of the Interpersonal/Affective factor for high (1 SD above mean) Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) scores or high (1 SD
the mean) and low ( 1 SD below the mean) Social Deviance factor above the mean) Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) scores. The
scores according to the procedures described by Holmbeck (2002; mean PCL-R total score for participants with low Interpersonal/
see Figure 1). The effect ofthe Interpersonal/Affective factor was Affective (Factor 1) scores was 17.5, with a range of 3.2-29. The
significant when Social Deviance factor scores were high ( = mean PCL-R total score for participants with high Interpersonal/
0.54, p < .001), but not when Social Deviance factor scores were Affective (Factor 1) scores was 28.9, with a range of 16-36.8.
low {p = .71). In contrast, the effect of the Social Deviance factor Given that the Social Deviance Factor only predicted worse DPT
was significant when Interpersonal/Affective factor scores were performance when Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) scores were
low ( = 0.46, p = .001), but not when Interpersonal/Affective low, this curiously suggests that participants with mean PCL-R
factor scores were high {p = .90). total scores of 17.5 may have worse DPT performance than those
with mean PCL-R total scores of 28.9. In other words, participants
To help interpret the results of the interaction, we plotted Inter-
with PCL-R total scores near the clinical cutoff for psychopathy
personal/Affective factor scores as a function of Social Deviance
(PCL-R total scores of 30 or above) may perform better than those
factor scores (Figure 2). This plot illustrated that the factor scores
with high levels of social deviance traits, but who have PCL-R
correlated positively in this study population (r = .60) as reported
total scores far below the clinical cutoff for psychopathy.
in Table 2, but in addition clarified that Social Deviance factor
scores started to plateau near their maximum possible value of 20 To test these nonlinear predictions explicitly, we performed a
when Interpersonal/Affective factor scores were only slightly past hierarchical regression with Kendall's tau as the dependent vari-
their mean of 7.01 (out of a possible score of 20). Thus, part of the able, age in the first block (recall that age was correlated with both
interaction effects identified in the regression analyses could be PCL-R total scores and Kendall's tau), and both a centered linear
due to the fact that Social Deviance factor scores had already and a centered quadratic PCL-R total predictor in the second
plateaued near their ceiling when Interpersonal/Affective factor block. The relationship between the quadratic PCL-R Total pre-
dictor and tau was significant ( = 1.46, p < .01), and the linear
and quadratic PCL-R total terms collectively accounted for 7.9%
of the variance in DPT performance. This quadratic relationship
between PCL-R total scores and Kendall's tau is illustrated in the
0.85 -Low Social scatter plot provided in Figure 3. Thus, PCL-R total scores do
Deviance correlate with DPT performance, but they do so in a nonlinear
0.8 - (-1 SD)
~ .055, p .71
fashion. Participants with very low PCL-R total scores (who have
0.75 - simultaneously low Interpersonal/Affective and Social Deviance
-High Social factor scores) have normal DPT performance. Participants with
0.7
Deviance very high PCL-R total scores (who have simultaneously high
(+1 SD)
0.65 - Interpersonal/Affective and Social Deviance factor scores) have
= .536./)< .01 normal DPT performance as well. In contrast, participants with
0.6 -
midlevel PCL-R total scores (who have higher Social Deviance
0.55 - scores than Interpersonal/Affective scores in the present popula-
tion) have impaired DPT performance. Interestingly, when hierar-
05
chical regressions were performed with centered quadratic PCL-R
Low High
Interpersonal/Affective Intetpersonal/Atective Interpersonal/Affective or Social Deviance factor terms, only the
(-1 SD) (+1.SD) Interpersonal/Affective quadratic term was significant (Table 3;
Figure 4). Thus, the quadratic relationship between PCL-R total
Figure 1. Interaction between Deserved Punishment Test (DPT) perfor- scores and DPT performance is likely driven by a combination of
mance (as measured by logit-transformed Kendall's tau) and Psychopathy the nonlinear relationship between Interpersonal/Affective factor
Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) scores at
scores and DPT performance and the nonlinear relationship be-
high and low levels of PCL-R Social Deviance (Factor 2) scores. SD =
standard deviation. tween Interpersonal/Affective and Social Deviance factor scores.
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 677

only approached significance (p = .09). Therefore, the two-factor


model of psychopathy described performance in the current study
better than the four-facet model of psychopathy.

Discussion
i Thus far, it has proven difficult to demonstrate deficits in moral
judgment in individuals with psychopathy, despite their clearly
Q
immoral behavior. This study provides evidence that one reason
Is for this historical difficulty may be that Interpersonal/Affective
'3
o and Social Deviance psychopathic traits can predict moral judg-
1/3
ment performance in opposing directions, ultimately canceling
each other out and obscuring the relationship when PCL-R total
scores are used as the only index of psychopathy. Consistent with
previous studies showing no relationship between psychopathy
2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 and moral judgment, the present study also showed PCL-R total
scores did not linearly predict DPT performance. However, by
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor having a large sample size with a full range of PCL-R scores,
choosing a task that minimized impression management and al-
Figure 2. Scatter plot of Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) Inter- lowed for a wide range of performance, and testing for cooperative
personal/Affective (Factor 1) scores relative to PCL-R Social Deviance suppression effects, we demonstrated that PCL-R factor scores do
(Factor 2) scores. predict moral judgment. When entered into a regression together.
Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) PCL-R scores predict that in-
carcerated participants will rate the relative amount of punishment
Four Factor Model of Psychopathy and crimes deserve more like other participants, while the Social
DPT Performance Deviance (Factor 2) PCL-R scores predict that incarcerated par-
Some models of psychopathy describe PCL-R scores as being ticipants will rate the relative amount of punishment crimes de-
composed of four facets (Hare, 2003). The PCL-R Interpersonal/ serve less like other participants. The factors' opposing effects are
Affective factor can be divided into two facets: Facet 1 (Interper- driven by each factor's unique qualities, because the zero-order
sonal, e.g., lying, manipulation, and superficial charm) and Facet 2 correlations between each factor and DPT performance were not
(Affective, e.g., shallow affect and lack of empathy). The PCL-R significant, likely because of a lack of correlation between their
Social Deviance factor can be divided into Facet 3 (Lifestyle, e.g., strong shared variance and Kendall's tau.
impulsivity, irresponsibility, parasitic lifestyle, and lack of goals) The associations between the Interpersonal/Affective factor and
and Facet 4 (Antisocial, e.g., juvenile delinquency, adult antisocial the Social Deviance factor were qualified by a significant multi-
behavior, criminal versatility, and lack of behavioral control; Hare, plicative interaction. Specifically, Interpersonal/Affective traits
2003). To compare the four-facet model to the two-factor model were positively associated with task performance when Social
and determine whether specific facets could account for the coop- Deviance traits were high, but Social Deviance traits were nega-
erative suppression between the Interpersonal/Affective factor and
the Social Deviance factor, we implemented a hierarchical regres-
sion with Kendall's tau as the dependent variable, age in the first
block, and all four facets in the second block. The R^ change due
to the addition of the facet scores simultaneously was 0.10 (p =
.02). The Interpersonal facet (Facet 1) predicted normal DPT
performance ( = 0.31, p = .01) while the Antisocial facet (Facet
4) predicted more deviant DPT performance ( = -0.23, p = fi
.04). Neither the Affective facet (Facet 2; p = .60) nor the
Lifestyle facet (Facet 3; p = .29) significantly predicted DPT
c
performance. Therefore, the positive relationship between the In-
terpersonal/Affective factor and DPT performance is likely due
more to the interpersonal traits, rather than the affective traits, of
this factor, and the negative relationship between the Social De-
viance factor and DPT performance is likely due more to the
antisocial traits, rather than the lifestyle traits, of this particular
factor. A model including age in the first block, centered Interper- 10 20 30 40
sonal facet, centered Antisocial facet, and a multiplicative inter-
action between the centered Interpersonal and Antisocial facets did PCL-R Total Score
not account for as much variance in DPT performance as the Figure 3. Scatter plot of Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) total
interaction model using factor scores (Ai?^ = .11). Further, the scores relative to deserved punishment test performance (as measured by
interaction between the Interpersonal facet and the Antisocial facet logit-transformed Kendall's tau).
678 SCHAICH BORG ET AL,

2.0

1,5

1,0 \ .
Ken

,5 J

0,0
0 5 10 15 0 5 10 15 20
Interpersonal/Affective Factor Social Deviance Factor
Figure 4. Scatter plots of Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) factor scores (Interpersonal/Affective and
Social Deviance) relative to deserved punishment test performance (as measured by logit-transformed Kendall's
tau).

tively associated with task performance when Interpersonal/Affec- scores when Interpersonal/Affective scores were low, but not when
tive traits were low. The consequences of this interaction in the Interpersonal/Affective scores were high (because Social Deviance
present population were that participants who had either very low scores had already reached their ceiling). These population char-
PCL-R total scores or very high PCL-R total scores had more acteristics may have made it difficult to statistically detect the
normal DPT performance than participants who had middle range effects of the Interpersonal/Affective scores when Interpersonal/
PCL-R total scores. Put differently. Social Deviance psychopathic Affective scores were low or detect the effects of the Social
traits predicted deviant moral punishment judgment performance, Deviance factor when Interpersonal/Affective scores were high.
but Interpersonal/Affective psychopathic traits counteracted these Thus, it would be informative in future studies to determine
negative associations when they were sufficiently present. whether the interaction between PCL-R factors and moral judg-
Although the nonlinear properties of the present incarcerated ment performance reported here persists in samples with many
population needs to be replicated in other incarcerated populations, participants who have low Social Deviance and high Interpersonal/
this interaction between opposing infiuences provides one mech- Affective scores. That said, the characteristics of the present sam-
anism by which the construct of psychopathy might manifest as a ple may be similar to the characteristics of other incarcerated
taxon, despite its composition of dimensional traits (Walters, Dun- populations because the antisocial behavioral characteristics of the
can, & Mitchell-Perez, 2007; Walters, Gray, et al 2007), The Social Deviance factor are likely to be much more strongly rep-
mean PCL-R total scores for participants who had an Interperson- resented in incarcerated samples than nonincarcerated samples.
al/Affective (Factor 1) score above the population mean (where the Most studies of psychopathy do not provide extensive details of
positive relationship between Interpersonal/Affective scores and their reported correlations between PCL-R factor scores. How-
DPT performance is significant) in our sample was 28,9, This ever, one study of male sex offenders providing adequate detail
average is very close to the clinical PCL-R cutoff of 30 used to reported a relationship between Interpersonal/Affective and Social
diagnose psychopathy, and thus suggests that people who have Deviance scores similar to the relationship reported here, including
PCL-R total scores of above 30 may indeed perform differently on a lack of participants scoring high in Interpersonal/Affective traits
the DPT than those who have high psychopathic traits but score but low in Social Deviance traits (Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 1993;
below 30, However, our data suggest their performance would be see Figure 2), Also interesting, in another study of primarily
a result of qualified opposing linear interactions between specific noninstitutionalized and noncriminal participants, researchers re-
subsets of psychopathic traits and moral judgment, not a result of ported a lack of correlation between Interpersonal/Affective and
simply having "enough" general psychopathic traits. It would be Social Deviance scores in higher scoring participants who had
useful in future studies to assess whether cooperative suppression PCL-R total scores of 15-27, despite significant correlations be-
and PCL-R factor distributions can lead to similar taxonomic tween Interpersonal/Affective and Social Deviance scores across
effects on other behaviors, perhaps explaining why individuals their entire sample (DeMatteo, Heilbrun, & Marczyk, 2006),
with very high psychopathy scores can have such distinct psychi- Therefore, it is possible that the distribution of factor scores
atric profiles despite their diagnosis being grounded in the identi- reported here represents fundamental characteristics of the rela-
fication of dimensional, continuous traits. tionship between the PCL-R factors and thus may be present in
When interpreting the interaction effects in the present study, it other study populations as well.
is important to note that the present population sample did not One important implication of the present study's results is that
include many participants with low Social Deviance (Factor 2) the different subsets of personality traits in psychopathy may lead
scores, especially low Social Deviance scores with simultaneous to different constellations of behaviors depending on which traits
high Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) scores (Figure 2), Further, dominate in a given individual or in a particular life situation. The
Social Deviance scores correlated with Interpersonal/Affective results of the present study suggest that psychopaths have impaired
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 679
moral judgment and thus may have more antisocial behavior in reinforces the notion that impairments in moral judgment are not
contexts where the Social Deviance (Factor 2) traits would be likely to be the strongest explanation for psychopaths' antisocial
more infiuential (such as when behavioral controls are required, behavior. Nonetheless, the results of this study demonstrate that
i.e., when anger is invoked, when opportunities for strong and features of psychopathy do influence moral judgment, which pro-
exciting sensations are presented, or when strong immediate re- vides evidence to bridge our intuitive notions of the relationship
wards are offered). Conversely, individuals with psychopathy may between moral judgment and moral behavior with the dramatic
have fully intact moral judgment and restrained antisocial behavior clinical features of psychopathy.
in contexts where tbe Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) traits
would be more influential (such as when no powerfully provoking
References
stimulus is in the immediate environment or when manipulation of
other people is the ideal strategy). Further research is needed to test Aharoni, E., Antonenko, O., & Kiehl, K. A. (2011). Disparities in the moral
and develop this hypothesis. If true, the results may have impli- intuitions of criminal offenders: The role of psychopathy. Joumal of
cations for treatment programs aimed at modifying psychopathic Research in Personality, 45, 322-327. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.005
traits, which historically have had debatable and inconsistent suc- Aharoni, E., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Can psycho-
pathic offenders discern moral wrongs? A new look at the moral/
cess in adult populations (Salekin, Woriey, & Grimes, 2010).
conventional distinction. Joumal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 484-
Speciflcally, our results may suggest that different strategies 497. doi:10.I037/a0024796
should be taught for situations in which Interpersonal/Affective American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-
traits are likely to dominate (including strategies that exploit moral ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
reasoning) compared with situations in which Social Deviance Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality:
traits are likely to dominate (which might require strategies that Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1-29. doi:10.1016/0010-
manage behavior rather than try to alter judgment). 0277(95)00676-P
The deserved punishment test (DPT) was chosen for this study Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1995). Is the psychopath
because it is a validated moral judgment test that would maximize "morally insane"? Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 741-752.
doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00087-M
statistical ability to detect associations with psychopathy. Addi-
Blonigen, D. M., Patrick, C. J., Douglas, K. S., Poythress, N. G., Skeem,
tionally, the content of the task was very relevant to an incarcer-
J. L., Lilienfeld, S. O., . . . Kmeger, R. F. (2010). Multimethod assess-
ated population, and the brain regions involved in punishment ment of psychopathy in relation to factors of internalizing and external-
judgments (Buckholtz et al, 2008) overlap with the brain regions izing from the Personality Assessment Inventory: The impact of method
believed to be involved in psychopathy (Kiehl, 2006). However, it variance and suppressor effects. Psychological Assessment, 22, 96-107.
should be recognized that deciding how much punishment a moral doi:10.1037/a0017240
wrong deserves is only one kind of moral judgment among others. Book, A. S., Holden, R. R., Starzyk, K. B., Wasylkiw, L., & Edwards, M. J.
In addition, how much punishment a moral wrong is seen to (2006). Psychopathic traits and experimentally induced deception in
deserve can potentially be influenced by nonmoral factors such as self-report assessment. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 601-
arbitrary legal rules or evolved motives to cooperate (Cushman, 608. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.011
2008; Dariey & Pittman, 2003; DeScioli & Kurzban, 2009, 2012; Buckholtz, J. W., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., Zald, D. H., Gore, J. C ,
Jones, O. D., & Marois, R. (2008). The neural correlates of third-party
Kurzban, DeScioli, & O'Brien, 2007), and these nonmoral factors
punishment. Neuron, 60, 930-940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.016
can result in situations where punishment is deemed necessary Carlsmith, K. M., & Darley, J. M. (2008). Psychological aspects of retrib-
even though no moral wrong was committed. Thus, it is important utive justice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
for future studies to develop other appropriate moral judgment psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 193-236). San Diego, CA: Elsevier, doi:
tasks that allow a wide range of performance to replicate our 10.1016/S0065-2601 (07)00004-4
PCL-R factor results and assess whether PCL-R factor scores Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Psychopaths know right
have opposing effects on other types of moral decision making as from wrong but don't care. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
well. 5, 59-67. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp05I
The data from this study help begin to unravel the enigma of Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity: An attempt to reinterpret the
so-called psychopathic personality (1st ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
psychopaths' simultaneous reports of normal moral judgment and
Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.) St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
immoral behavior. First, in a population of individuals scoring
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation
above the recommended clinical diagnostic threshold for psychop- analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
athy, there is now evidence supporting the powerful intuition that Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment:
psychopathic traits influence moral judgment, and equally impor- Theoretical foundations and research validations and standard issues
tant, this evidence provides plausible explanations for why these scoring manual (Vols. 1 and 2). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University
influences have been so difficult to uncover in the past. However, Press.
in other ways, these data underscore the enigma of apparently Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of
intact moral judgment in psychopathy. Most important, this study causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108,
adds to the mounting evidence that PCL-R total scores do not 353-380. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006
correlate linearly with moral judgment. Further, even the nonlinear Cushman, F., Young, L., & Huser, M. (2006). The role of conscious
reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of
PCL-R total results suggest that many variables other than psy-
harm. Psychological Science, 17, 1082-1089. doi:10.1 lll/j.1467-9280
chopathy affect how people make moral judgments or behave. .2006.01834.x
Supporting this idea, it is important to acknowledge that PCL-R Darley, J. M., & Pittman, T. S. (2003). The psychology of compensatory
factor scores accounted for only 13.1% of the variance in DPT and retributive justice. Personality and Social Psycholotogy Review, 7,
performance (when age is placed in the first block). This finding 324-336. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704 05
680 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

DeMatteo, D., Heilbrun, K., & Marczyk. G. (2006). An empirical inves- ation with criminal conduct. In D. M. Stoff, J. Breiling, & J. D. Maser
tigation of psychopathy in a noninstitutionalized and noncriminal sam- (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 22-35). New York, NY:
ple. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 24, 133-146. doi:10.1002^sl.667 Wiley.
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112, Hart, S. D., Knopp, P. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Performance of male
281-299. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.008 psychopaths following conditional release from prison. Joumal of Con-
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2012). A solution to the mysteries of morality. sulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 227-232. doi:10.1037/0022-006X
Psychological Bulletin. Advance online publication, doi: 10.1037/ .56.2.227
a0029065 Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and
Edens, J. F., Marcus, D. K., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Poythress, N. G. (2006). recidivism: A review. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 139-
Psychopathic, not psychopath: Taxometric evidence for the dimensional 170. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8333.1998.tb00355.x
structure of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 131- Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2006). Psychopathy and negative emotion-
144. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.l 15.1.131 ality: Analyses of suppressor effects reveal distinct relations with emo-
tional distress, fearfulness, and anger-hostility. Joumal of Abnormal
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997).
Psychology, 115, 276-287. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.276
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-TV Axis I Disorders-Clinical
Holmbeck, G. N. (2002). Post hoc probing of significant moderational and
Version (SCID-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
mediational effects in studies of pdiatrie populations. Joumal of Pdi-
Freeman, S. J. M., & Giebink, J. W. (1979). Moral judgment as a function
atrie Psychology, 27, 87-96. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.87
of age, sex, and stimulus. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary
Kennealy, P. J., Hicks, B. M., & Pati-ick, C. J. (2007). Validity of factors
and Applied, 102, 43-47. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1979.9915093
ofthe Psychopathy Checklist-Revised in female prisoners: Discriminant
Gibbs, J. C , Basinger, K. S., Grime, R. L., & Snarey, J. R. (2007). Moral relations with antisocial behavior, substance abuse, and personality.
judgment development across cultures: Revisiting Kohlberg's universal- Assessment, 14, 323-340. doi:10.1177/1073191107305882
ity claims. Developmental Review, 27, 443-500. doi:10.10l6/j.dr.2007 Kiehl, K. A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy:
.04.001 Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry Research, 142,
Glenn, A. L. (2010). How can studying psychopaths help us understand the 107-128. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.013
neural mechanisms of moral judgment. Cell Science Reviews, 6, 30-35. Kosson, D. S., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Evaluating the
Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., & Schug, R. A. (2009). The neural correlates of construct validity of psychopathy in Black and White inmates: Three
moral decision making in psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 5-6. preliminary studies. Joumal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 250-259.
doi:10.1038/mp.2008.I04 doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.99.3.250
Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., Schug, R., Young, L., & Huser, M. (2009). Kroner, D. G., & Weekes, J. R. (1996). Balanced inventory of desirable
Increased DLPFC activity during moral decision making in psychopa- responding: Factor structure, reliability, and validity with an offender
thy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 909-911. doi:10.1038/mp.2009.76 sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 323-333. doi:
Guay, J. P., Ruscio, J., Knight, R. A., & Hare, R. D. (2007). A taxometric 10.1016/0191 -8869(96)00079-7
analysis of the latent structure of psychopathy: Evidence for dimension- Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P., & O'Brien, E. (2007). Audience effects on
ality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 701-716. doi:10.1037/ moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 75-84.
0021-843X. 116.4.701 doi: 10.1016/j .evolhumbehav.2006.06.001
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R). Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer
Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159-174. doi:10.2307/
Hare, R. D. (2003). Manual for the Revised Hare Psychopathy Checklist 2529310
(2nd ed.). Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Lapata, M. (2006). Automatic evaluation of information Ordering: Kend-
Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., & Thornton, D. (2000). Psychopathy and all's tau. Computational Linguistics, 32, 471-484. doi:10.1162/coli
the predictive validity of the PCL-R: An international perspective. .2006.32.4.471
Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 18, 623-645. doi:]0.1002/1099- Lesaffre, E., Rizopoulos, D., & Tsonaka, R. (2007). The logistic transform
0798(200010)18:5<623::AID-BSL409>3.0.CO;2-W for bounded outcome scores. Biostatistics, 8, 72-85.
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Penna, S. (2001). Anxiety sensitivity: Relations to
Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the
psychopathy, DSM-IV personality disorder features, and personality
D5A/-/V criteria for antisocial personality disorder. Journal ofAbnormai
traits. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 15, 367-393. doi:10.1016/S0887-
Psychology, 100, 391-398. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.391
6185(01)00070-6
Harenski, C. L., Harenski, K. A., Shane, M. S., & Kiehl, K. A. (2010).
Link, N. F., Scherer, S. E., & Byrne, P. N. (1977). Moral judgement and
Aberrant neural processing of moral violations in criminal psychopaths.
moral conduct in the psychopath. Canadian Psychiatrie Association
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 863-874. doi:10.1037/a0020979
Joumai/Revue de l'Association des psychiatres du Canada, 22, 341-
Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1994). Assessment of psychopathy as a 346.
function of age. Joumal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 604-609. doi: Lose, C. A. (1997). Level of moral reasoning and psychopathy within a
10.1037/0021-843X.103.4.604 group of federal inmates. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section
Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstian, A. R. (1989). Two-factor concep- B: The Sciences and Engineering, 57(7-B), 4716.
tualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and assessment implica- McMillan, J., & Malatesti, L. (2010). Responsibility and psychopathy. In
tions. Psychological Assessment, 1, 6-17. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.6 L. Malatesti & J. McMillan (Eds.), Responsibility and psychopathy:
Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1994). Psychopathy as a Interfacing law, psychiatry, and philosophy (pp. 185-198). New York,
taxon: Evidence that psychopaths are a discrete class. Joumal of Con- NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/med/9780199551637.003
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 387-397. doi:10.1037/0022-006X .0010
.62.2.387 Meloy, J. R. (1988). Psychopathic mind: Origin, dynamics, and treatment.
Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy and antisocial personality Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
disorder. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 9, 129-132. doi:10.1097/ O'Kane, A., Fawcett, D., & Blackburn, R. (1996). Psychopathy and moral
00001504-199603000-00007 reasoning: Comparison of two classifications. Personality and Individ-
Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1997). Psychopathy: Assessment and associ- ual Differences, 20, 505-514. doi:!0.1016/0191-8869(95)00203-0
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 681
Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: Startling new insights. approach. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 28, 235-266. doi: 10.1002/
Psychophysiology, 31, 319-330. doi:10.111 l/j.1469-8986.1994 bsl.928
.tb02440.x Schaich Borg, J., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A.
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the (2011). Neural basis of moral verdict and moral deliberation. Social
criminal psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Jourrtal of Abnormal Neuroscience, 6, 398-413. doi:10.1080/17470919.2011.559363
Psychology, 102, 82-92. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.82 Smetana, J. G. (1981). Preschool children's conceptions of moral and
Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In J. P. social rules. Child Development, 52, 1333-1336. doi: 10.2307/1129527
Robinson, P. R. Shaver & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of per- Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for
sonality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 17-59). New York, NY: the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychol-
Academic Press. ogists Press.
Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Tracy, J. L. (2004). Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and
Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivari- convention. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
ate Behavioral Research, 39, 303-328. doi:10.1207/ Verona, E., Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2005). Psychopathy and
sl5327906mbr3902_7 suicidality in female offenders: Mediating influences of personality and
Reidy, D. E., Shelley-Tremblay, J. F., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychop- abuse. Joumal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 1065-1073.
athy, reactive aggression, and precarious proclamations: A review of doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.6.1065
behavioral, cognitive, and biological research. Aggression and Violent Verona, E., Patrick, C. J., & Joiner, T. E. (2001). Psychopathy, antisocial
Behavior, 16, 512-524. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.06.002 personality, and suicide risk. Joumal of Abnormal Psychology, 110,
Rest, J., Cooper, D., Coder, R., Masanz, J., & Anderson, D. (1974). 462-470. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.l 10.3.462
Judging the important issues in moral dilemmas: An objective test of Walters, G. D., Duncan, S. A., & Mitchell-Perez, K. (2007). The latent
development. Developmental Psychology, 10, 491-501. doi:10.1037/ structure of psychopathy: A taxometric investigation ofthe Psychopathy
hOO36598 Checklist-Revised in a heterogeneous sample of male prison inmates.
Robinson, P. H., & Darley, J. M. (2007). Intuitions of justice: Implications Assessment, 14, 270-278. doi:10.1177/1073191107299594
for criminal law and justice policy [Scholarship at Penn Law Paper 150]. Walters, G. D., Gray, N. S., Jackson, R. L., Sewell, K. W., Rogers, R.,
Retrieved from http://lsr.nellco.org/upenn_wps/150 Taylor, J., & Snowden, R. J. (2007). A taxometric analysis of the
Robinson, P. H., & Kurzban, R. (2007). Concordance and conflict in Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL:SV): Further evidence
intuitions of justice. Minnesota Law Review, 91, 1829-1907. of dimensionality. Psychological Assessment, 19, 330-339. doi:
Ryan, J. J., Lopez, S. J., & Werth, T. R. (1999). Development and 10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.330
preliminary validation of a Satz-Mogel short form of the WAIS-III in a Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San
sample of persons with substance abuse disorders. International Joumal Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.
of Neuroscience, 98, 131-140. doi: 10.3109/00207459908994796 Wilson, M. S., & McCarthy, K. (2011). Greed is good? Student disciplin-
Salekin, R. T., Worley, C. B., & Grimes, R. D. (2010). Treatment of ary choice and self-reported psychopathy. Personality and Individual
psychopathy: A review and brief introduction to the mental models Differences, 51, 873-876. doi:10.1016/j.paid.201I.07.028

{Appendices follow)
682 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

Appendix A
Deserved Punishment Test (DPT) Scenarios (S; Originally Published in Robinson & Kurzban, 2007)

51 Defending Attack 58 Clock Radio From Car


John is knocked down from behind by a man with a knife who As he is walking to a party in a friend's neighborhood, John sees
moves to stab him. As the man lunges for him, John stabs him a clock radio on the backseat of a car parked on the street. Later
with a piece of glass he finds on the ground, which is the only that night, on his retum from the party, he checks the car and
thing he can do to save himself from being killed. The man later finds it unlocked, so he takes the clock radio from the back seat.
dies of his injuries.
59 Electric Drill From Garage
52 Coercive Threat to Child John does not have all the tools he needs for his workshop but
knows of a family two streets over who sometimes leave unlocked
A man grabs John's child and puts a sharp knife to her
the door to the detached garage next to their house. When he
throat. He tells John that he will kill the child if John does not
next sees his chance, he enters the detached garage through the
steal an expensive digital camera from a nearby shop or he
unlocked door and takes a medium-size electric drill, intending
attempts to contact police. Because the man can see everything he
to keep it forever.
does, John does as he is told in order to save his child. (What
punishment for John?)
510 Microwave From House
53 Umbrella Mistake While a family is on vacation, John jimmies the back door to
their house and steps into their kitchen. On the counter, he sees
John takes another person's umhrella assuming it to be his their microwave, which he carries away.
own because it is has the same unusual color pattem as his own, a
fact that the police confirm. 511 Smashing TV
While a family is away for the day, John breaks in through a
54 Wolf Hallucination
bedroom window and mmmages through the house looking for
Another person slips a dmg into John's food, which causes him valuables. He can only find an 18-inch television, which angers
to hallucinate that he is being attacked by a wolf. When John him. When he gets it outside, he realizes that it is an older model
strikes out in defense, he does not realize that he is in fact than he wants, so he smashes it onto the driveway, breaking it
striking a person, a fact confirmed by all of the psychiatrists into pieces.
appointed by the state, who confirm that John had no ability to
prevent the hallucination. 512 Slap and Bruising at Record Store
A record store patron is wearing a cap that mocks John's
55 Whole Pies From Buffet favorite band. John follows him from the store, confronts him, then
slaps him in the face hard, causing him to stumble. The man's
The owner has posted mies at his all-you-can-eat buffet that
face develops a harsh black and yellow bruise that does not go
expressly prohibit taking food away; patrons can only take what
away for some time.
they eat at the buffet. The owner has set the price of the buffet
accordingly. John purchases dinner at the buffet, but when he
leaves he takes with him two whole pies to give to a friend. 513 Head-Butt at Stadium
While attending a football game, John becomes angry as he
56 Logo T-Shirt From Store overhears an opposing fan's disparaging remarks about John's
team. At the end ofthe game, John sticks his face in the man's face
John notices in a small family owned music store a T-shirt with and head-butts him, causing a black eye and a gash that requires
the logo of his favorite band. While the store clerk is preoccupied two stitches to close.
with inventory, John places the $15 T-shirt in his coat and walks
out, with no intention of paying for it. 514 Stitches After Soccer Game
Angry after overhearing another parent's remarks during a soc-
57 Short Change Cheat cer match in which John's son is playing, John approaches the man
John is a cab driver who picks up a high school student. Because after the game, grabs his coffee mug, knocks him down, and then
the customer seems confused about the money transaction, John kicks him several times while he is on the ground, knocking him
decides he can trick her and gives her $20 less change than he out for several minutes and causing cuts that require five
knows she is owed. stitches.

(Appendices continue)
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 683

515 Necklace Snatch at Mall S20 Stabbing


As a woman searches her purse for car keys in a mall parking John is offended by a woman's mocking remark and decides to
lot, John runs up and grahs her gold necklace, but it does not hurt her hadly. At work the next day, when no one else is around,
break. He yanks the woman to the ground by her necklace, he picks up a letter opener from his desk and stahs her. She
where she gashes her head, requiring stitches, John runs off later dies from the wound.
without the necklace,

S21 Ambush Shooting


516 Attempted Robhery at Gas Station
John knows the address of a woman who has highly offended
John demands money from a man buying gas at a gas station.
him. As he had planned the day before, he waits there for the
When the man refuses, John punches the man several times in
woman to retum from work, and when she appears, John shoots
the face, hreaking his jaw and causing several cuts that each
her to death.
require stitches. He then runs off without getting any money,

517 Clubbing During Robbery S22 Abduction Shooting


To force a man to give up his wallet during a rohhery attempt, A woman at work reveals John's tnisdeeds to his employer,
John heats the man with a cluh until he relinquishes his vfallet, thereby getting him fired, John devises a plan to get even with her.
which contains $350, The man must be hospitalized for 2 days, The next week he forces the woman into his car at knife point
and drives her to a secluded area where he shoots her to death.
518 Mauling by Pit Bulls
Two vicious pit hulls that John keeps for illegal dog flghting S23 Burning Mother for Inheritance
have just leamed to escape and have attacked a person who came
John works out a plan to kill his 60-year-old invalid mother for
to John's house. The police tell John he must destroy the dogs,
the inheritance. He drags her to her bed, puts her in, and lights her
which he agrees to do hut does not intend to do. The next day,
the dogs escape again and maul to death a man delivering a oxygen mask vdth a cigarette, hoping to make it look like an
package, accident. The elderly woman screams as her clothes catch fire,
and she hums to death, John just watches her bum.
519 Infant Death in Car
S24 Ransom, Rape, Torture and Strangling
John is driving to see a man about buying an illegal gun but
must hahysit his friend's toddler son. It occurs to him that it is John kidnaps an 8-year-old girl for ransom, rapes her, and
too hot to safely leave the toddler in the car, but he decides to then records the child's screams as he hums her with a ciga-
leave him anyway and to retum soon. He gets talking with the rette lighter, sending the recording to her parents to induce them
seller, however, and forgets ahout the toddler, who passes out to pay his ransom demand. Even though they pay as directed, John
and dies. strangles the child to death to avoid leaving a witness.

(Appendices continue)
684 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

Appendix B
Regression Analyses With Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Total and Factor Scores as Predictors of Deserved
Punishment Test Performance (Measured hy Raw, Nontransformed Kendall's Tau Values)

Variable B SE ^R^

Psychopathy total scores


Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .23*
Step 2 <.O1
PCL-R Total <0.01 <0.01 .03
Psychopathy factor scores (entered independently)
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .24*
Step 2 .02
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor <0.01 <0.01 .15
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .19t
Step 2 .01
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor >-0.01 <0.01 -.12
Psychopathy factor scores (entered simultaneously)
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .16
Step 2 .09*'
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor <0.01 <0.01 .35**
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor >-0.01 <0.01 -.34**
Psychopathy factor interaction
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .20*
Step 2 .09**
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor .01 <0.01 .34**
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor >-0.01 <0.01 -.26*
Step 3 .05*
Interpersonal/Affective X Social Deviance <0.01 <0.01 .23*
Psychopathy total scores with quadratic term
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .23*
Step 2 .06*
PCL-R Total <0.01 <0.01 .09
PCL-R Total^ (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .25**
Psychopathy factor scores with quadratic term
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .20*
Step 2 .09**
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor <0.01 <0.01 .13
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective^ (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .26**
Step 1 .05*
Age <0.01 <0.01 .20*
Step 2 .02
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor >-0.01 <0.01 -.08
PCL-R Social Deviance'^ (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .09
Note. SE = standard error; PCL-R = Psychopathy Checklist-Revised.
V < .07. *p = 06. * p < . 0 5 . * * p < . 0 1 .

(Appendices continue)
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 685

Appendix C
Regression Analyses With IQ, Education Level, Age, and Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Total and Factor Scores
as Predictors of Deserved Punishment Test Performance (Measured by Logit-Transformed Kendall's Tau)

Variable B SE
Psychopathy total scores
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .47**
Education level 0.03 0.03 .11
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .17
Step 3 <.O1
PCL-R Total <0.01 <0.01 .01
Psychopathy factor scores (entered independently)
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01. <0.01 .47**
Education level 0.02 0.03 .08
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .19*
Step 3 .01
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.01 0.01 .11
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .47**
Education level 0.03 0.03 .11
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .13
Step 3 .01
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor -0.01 0.01 -.11
Psychopathy factor scores (entered simultaneously)
Step 1 .31*'
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .46**
Education level 0.02 0.03 .07
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .13
Step 3 .05*
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.02 0.01 .27*
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor -0.02 0.01 -.27*
Psychopathy factor interaction
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .49"
Education level -0.01 0.03 -.03
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .21*
Step 3 .05*
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.03 0.01 .28"
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor -0.01 0.01 -.17
Step 4 .09**
Interpersonal/Affective x Social Deviance 0.01 <0.01 .32**
Psychopathy total scores with quadratic term
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .48**
Education level 0.01 0.03 .02
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .20*

(Appendices continu)
686 SCHAICH BORG ET AL.

Appendix C {continued)

Variable B SE AR^

Step 3 .09"
PCL-R Total <0.01 <0.01 .10
PCL-R TotaP (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .32"
Psychopathy factor scores with quadratic term
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .44"
Education level >-0.01 0.03 -.01
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .17
Step 3 .07"
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective Factor 0.01 0.01 .11
PCL-R Interpersonal/Affective^ (quadratic) 0.01 <0.01 .26"
Step 1 .31"
IQ 0.01 <0.01 .47"
Education level 0.03 0.03 .10
Step 2 .02+
Age 0.01 <0.01 .15
Step 3 .03
PCL-R Social Deviance Factor >0.01 0.01 -.03
PCL-R Social Deviance^ (quadratic) <0.01 <0.01 .16
Note. SE = standard error; PCL-R = Psychopathy Checklist-Revised.
V = -08. V =-07. ' p =-06. > < .05. " p < . 0 1 .

{Appendices continue)
PSYCHOPATHY AND PUNISHMENT JUDGMENTS 687

Appendix D
Histograms Illustrating the Distribution of Kendall's Tau Values From the Present Incarcerated Population and
Previously Published Nonincarcerated Populations

University Non-incarcerated Population (N = 64)

g 20 -
g* 10
O
o
Kendall's Tau

Intemet Non-incarcerated Population (N = 246)


60 1

Kendall's Tau

Incarcerated Population (N = 100)


20 -I
15 -
10 -
5 -
O
o

Kendall's Tau

Received June 12, 2012


Revision received March 18, 2013
Accepted May 23, 2013
Copyright of Journal of Personality & Social Psychology is the property of American
Psychological Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or
posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like