Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 49

Prevention in

C o u n s e l i n g p s yc h o l o g y:
Theory, Research, Practice and Training
A Publication of Prevention Section in Division 17 of American Psychological Association
Volume 2 Issue 1 December, 2008

Burn in Glen Sannox by Barry Jackson, licensed under the Creative Commons
Editor: Debra L. Ainbinder, Lynn University

Associate Editor: Max Nisenbaum, University of Houston

Editorial Board:
Michael Waldo, Chair Eve Adams, Rachel Navarro,
University of New Mexico University of New Mexico University of New Mexico
Jonathan Schwartz, Nicole Coleman,
University of Houston University of Houston

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice and Training is a publication of the
Prevention Section of the Society for Counseling Psychology. The publication is dedicated to the
dissemination of information on prevention theory, research, practice and training in counseling
psychology, stimulating prevention scholarship, promoting collaboration between counseling psychologists
engaged in prevention, and encourages student scholars. The publication focuses on prevention in specific
domains (e.g., college campuses) employing specific modalities (e.g., group work), and reports summaries
of epidemiological and preventive intervention research. All submissions to the publication undergo blind
review by an editorial board jury, and those selected for publication are distributed nationally through
electronic and hard copies.

SUBMISSION GUIDELINES

The Prevention Section of the Society of Counseling Psychology publishes Prevention in Counseling
Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice and Training. This is a blind peer reviewed publication
presenting scholarly work in the field of prevention that is distributed nationally. Contributions can focus
on prevention theory, research, practice or training, or a combination of these topics. We welcome student
submissions.

As a publication of the Prevention Section of Division 17, presentations and awards sponsored by the
section will be highlighted in these issues. We will also publish condensed reviews of research or
theoretical work pertaining to the field of prevention. All submissions need to clearly articulate the
prevention nature of the work.

Submissions to this publication need to conform to APA style.

All submissions must be electronically submitted.

Please send your documents prepared for blind review with a cover letter including all identifying
information for our records. Submissions should be emailed to the Editor, Debra L. Ainbinder at
DAinbinder@lynn.edu.
Volume 2, December,
Issue 1 Prevention in 2008
C o u n s e l i n g p s yc h o lo g y

Section News
1 Message from the chairprevention is a paradigm change
Jonathan Schwartz
3 Prevention perspective
Michael Waldo, Jonathan Schwartz
6 Editors note
Debra L. Ainbinder

Theory, Research, Practice & Training


7 The strong kids curricula for building social and emotional resiliency in children and adolescents
Oanh K. Tran
11 Asian Americans coping with frequent racial discrimination: Role of acculturation and enculturation behaviors
Brandon Yoo, Suzie Jeon
17 Preventing school violence through establishing school connectedness
Adam M. Volungis
22 Preventing microaggressions: Recommendations for promoting positive mental health
Kevin L. Nadal
28 The relationship between resilience and anger in university students
Rachel B. Kirkpatrick, Tierra M. Freeman
39 Examination of perceived drinking norms and binge drinking attitudes in college
Jessica M. Bendjebar, Robert G. Riedel, Debra L. Ainbinder

Prevention Section at the 2008 APA National Conference


45 2008 Awards and acknowledgements
Message from the ChairPrevention is a Paradigm Change

Jonathan Schwartz, University of Houston

I am really excited to be the current chair of psychology, and provide leadership for other profes-
this active and accomplished section. This year, sions in the years ahead.
based largely on the work of the Prevention Sec-
tion, the book Realizing social justice: The chal- In light of these accomplishments it is impor-
lenge of preventive interventions edited by M. tant to note the connection to our sections mission.
Kenny, A. Horne, P. Orpinas & R. Reese; will be The purpose of the section is to further the theory,
published by the American Psychological Associa- research, teaching, and practice of prevention, advo-
tion. Every year, the Prevention Section presents a cacy, and public interest initiatives among counsel-
symposium at the American Psychological Associa- ing psychologists. The Section incorporates an array
tion Conference, The theory and research presented of preventive, developmental, policy, advocacy, and
are often disseminated in professional journals and community-focused activities. Section members
books. For example, the section presented a sym- recognize that science is the key to accomplishing
posium at the 2005 APA Conference about Preven- this purpose. So what does it really mean to pro-
tion and Social Justice. This symposium inspired a mote prevention in counseling psychology? I be-
series of articles on the topic that will be published lieve it means taking a view from a different per-
in the Journal of Primary Prevention in January spective. Preventionists often talk about travelling
2009. Another example of this is the article by upstream rather then saving those drowning down-
Hage, S. M., Romano, J., Conyne, R., Kenny, M., stream, providing prevention services to people
Mathews, C., Schwartz, J. P., & Waldo, M. (2007) upstream before they fall in and remediation ser-
Guidelines on prevention practice, research, train- vices are warranted (Rappaport, 1972). I would ar-
ing, and social advocacy for psychologists. The gue that true prevention requires both a holistic view
Counseling Psychologist, 35, 493-536. This article (a view of the whole river and surrounding area),
originated as a symposium presentation at the APA which requires distance, and a connection which re-
conference and recently was awarded 2007 Out- quires true collaboration and participation. Distance
standing Contribution Award to The Counseling is required to have the perspective necessary to view
Psychologist. At the August 2008 APA Council a problem beyond the individual struggling with it.
meeting, Division 17 Council representatives pre- Involvement in prevention forces the interventionist
sented a new business item at Council to adopt the to see beyond the individual issues to the multiple
Practice Guidelines on Prevention Practice, Re- ecological systems in which he/she interacts. This
search, Training, and Social Advocacy for Psy- requires a careful consideration of cultural relevance
chologists as APA policy. In making the motion, and social justice, which necessitates a connection
Council representatives articulated the importance with the target population. We know that prevention
of gaining the support and co-sponsorship of many will not be effective when it is conducted by an out-
APA divisions and finalizing the best practice pre- sider without a holistic understanding of the commu-
vention guidelines with the goal of gaining eventual nity in which they interact. Prevention does not lend
approval from APA. With APA approval, preven- itself to a single manaulized theoretical approach
tion best practices guidelines will inform prevention built to fit all populations; rather, prevention may be
research, training, practice, and social advocacy in shaped by approaches based on the problem being

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 1
Message from the chair Schwartz

addressed and the population being targeted. True


exposure to the participants, open mindedness to
overcome cultural encapsulation and true egalitar-
ian connectedness with the target community is
needed.
So to return to the upstream parable, an
example of a prevention program that changes the
environment would be to build a fence so people
dont fall in the water. However, traveling up-
stream to build a fence without understanding why
people are going in the water will not work. By the
same token, we also dont want to ignore the people
who are already in the water. First, a systems per-
spective would note that the people in the water
will be affected by a prevention program in the
community even if it is aimed at preventing people
from falling in the water. Finally, from a preven-
tion perspective, an effective program might be cre-
ated to prevent further damage. A well conducted
prevention program would understand why people
are going in the water and if a lifeboat would be
effective in getting them out.
References
Hage, S. M., Romano, J., Conyne, R., Kenny, M.,
Mathews, C., Schwartz, J. P., & Waldo, M.
(2007). Guidelines on prevention practice,
research, training, and social advocacy for
psychologists. The Counseling Psychologist,
35, 493-536.
Rappaport, J. (1972). From an address delivered for
Psy Chi. University of Illinois.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 2
Prevention Perspective

Michael Waldo, New Mexico State University


Jonathan Schwartz, University of Houston

There has been considerable effort to cate- more effective to walk upstream, find the place
gorize preventive interventions (Conyne, 2004; where people are falling in the river, and build a
Romano & Hage, 2000). Three categories that are fence there to prevent that from happening.
typically suggested are: Primary Prevention- pre-
venting a problem from occurring; Secondary Pre- The river metaphor offers a compelling illus-
vention - early detection and remediation of a prob- tration of the wisdom of prevention, but the linear
lem, preventing it from continuing and getting nature of the river may be misleading. There is a
worse; and Tertiary Prevention - preventing a prob- river named The Little Rio Grande in a theme park
lem that cant be re-mediated from causing other at the New Mexico/ Texas border which might be a
problems (Caplan, 1964). There has even been de- more accurate model for prevention. One of the iro-
bate about which kind of prevention is true pre- nies of the Little Rio Grande (in addition to its
vention, with some authors arguing that primary name) is that even though it is little (about ten feet
prevention is the only real form of prevention wide), many times during the year it is larger than
(Felner, Felner, & Silverman, 2000). We think the the real Rio Grande for which it is named, because
lines between these categories are fuzzy, at best, by the time the real Rio Grande reaches Texas it has
and not very useful. In fact, we think that what been depleted through irrigation to a series of small
form of prevention any particular intervention takes pools. A second irony that is central to ideas about
is more a matter of perspective than inherent char- prevention is that instead of flowing from the moun-
acteristic. This way of looking at prevention is tains to the sea, the Little Rio Grande flows in a cir-
handy because it allows recognizing the primary, cle. There are pumps that keep the water moving,
secondary and tertiary implications of all preventive allowing patrons of the park to float lazily on the
interventions. Recognizing how any intervention river all day, repeatedly passing the spot from which
has primary, secondary and tertiary effects allows they embarked, and never leaving the park.
planning to maximize the benefits of those effects. The metaphor of a circular river seems apt
It also allows marketing the intervention to con- for prevention. Efforts to prevent intimate partner
stituencies that are invested in the different levels of violence offer a good example. Psychologists work-
prevention, and gathering resources from those con- ing in the area of domestic violence have intervened
stituencies. to stop the violence (secondary prevention) and help
One of the classic ways of explaining pre- victims cope (tertiary prevention) (Schwartz &
vention is through a metaphor describing a danger- Waldo, 2003; Walker, 1984). Frustration over the
ous river (Rappaport, 1972). In this metaphor, a damage the violence has already caused prior to in-
scenario is described in which people on the shore tervention has led some psychologists to focus up-
of a swift river see others in the river drowning. stream on helping people learn to avoid violence
The people on shore jump in the river to save those before they are married, including interventions to
who are drowning, but in time become exhausted prevent dating violence (Avery-Leaf & Cascardi,
with this effort because there seems to be an end- 2002; Foubert, 2000; Wolfe & Jaffe, 1999). While
less flow of victims. They decide it would be much this approach has shown promise, it appears that

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 3
Prevention perspective Waldo & Schwartz

many gender role attitudes have been established by dary prevention. Also, communication skill training
the time people reach young adulthood, leading to limits the impact of other problems. For example,
efforts to go further up-stream and intervene with communication skill training can help people who
teens (Foshee et al., 1996). One potential obstacle tend to be introverted reduce their isolation, and help
in changing teens attitudes and behaviors is the people who tend to be harshly critical of others ex-
extraordinary influence of peer relations, resulting press their views in ways that are less alienating.
in some psychologists developing violence preven- Communication skills may not change peoples un-
tion interventions to establish nonviolent norms at derlying tendencies, but it does limit the impact
the head waters in elementary schools (Gamache & problems associated with those tendencies have on
Snapp, 1995). And, of course, psychologists work- their lives. In this way, communication skills train-
ing in elementary schools are confronted with the ing can be seen as tertiary prevention.
difficulty of having any impact on children inde-
pendent of the powerful influence of the childrens In our view, rather than categorizing preven-
families (Schwartz, Hage, Bush & Keys, 2006). tive interventions singularly as either primary, sec-
When those families are prone to violence, the most ondary or tertiary, it is more accurate and complete
effective primary prevention for children may actu- to describe the potential impact the interventions
ally take the form of secondary and tertiary preven- may have in each category. It is also useful. Psy-
tion with their parents. Like the Little Rio Grande, chologists who see the logic and benefits of primary
effort to engage in primary prevention by focusing prevention may have difficulty finding funding to
up stream has led full circle. And, from this per- support it (Duncan, 1994). Third party payers, insti-
spective, secondary and tertiary prevention are also, tutions and individuals often more readily compen-
always, primary prevention. sate remedial interventions than primary prevention
(Leviss, 2001). Recognizing the primary prevention
Similarly, primary prevention always con- implications of remedial interventions can allow
tains secondary and tertiary prevention components. psychologists to maximize the primary prevention
The examples offered above suggest that all forms impact of remediation. For example, communica-
of remedial (secondary prevention) and rehabilita- tion skills training for men who have been arrested
tive (tertiary prevention) interventions also prevent for spouse abuse can include helping these men
problems, and can be viewed as primary preven- communicate with their children about avoiding vio-
tion. It is also possible to see remedial and rehabili- lence in their intimate relationships (Cahn & Lloyd,
tative aspects in all primary prevention interven- 1996). Also, recognizing the remedial impact of pri-
tions. Staying with prevention of domestic violence mary prevention interventions can allow targeting
as an example, communication skills training is one these interventions to overcome deficits without
of most commonly employed procedures for treat- stigmatizing participants or insisting that they pre-
ing (secondary and tertiary prevention) both perpe- sent themselves as troubled in order to receive help.
trators and victims of battering (Pence & Paymer, For example, workshops on communication in inti-
1993; Schwartz & Waldo, 2003). Communication mate relationships can be offered to members of col-
skill training is also a common form of primary pre- lege fraternities (Schwartz, Griffin, & Russell,
vention, done with a wide variety of populations 2006). These interventions could serve as primary
who do not present with any psychological prob- prevention for violence in fraternity members dat-
lems (e.g., Waldo, 1989). However, even when ing relationships and subsequent marriages. They
conducted with people who have not come for help could also help more withdrawn fraternity members
with psychological problems, communications establish relationships with other members.
skills training can be seen as having characteristics
that are common to secondary and tertiary preven- We have found that shifting between pri-
tion. Communication skill training is intended to mary, secondary and tertiary perspectives enriches
overcome a deficit, lack of skills. In this way, com- our understanding of preventive interventions and
munication skills training can be seen as a secon- research. We hope other psychologists who are in-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 4
Prevention perspective Waldo & Schwartz

vested in prevention will try applying alternative In L. F. Novick & G. P. Mays (Eds.), Public
perspectives to their work, and share their views health administration: principles for popula-
through this publication. tion-based management (pp. 413-431).
Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.
References
Rappaport, J. (1972). From an address delivered for
Cahn, D. D., & Lloyd, S. A. (1996). Family vio- Psy Chi. University of Illinois.
lence from a communication perspective.
Romano, J. & Hage, S. M. (2000a). Prevention and
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. counseling psychology: Revitalizing commit
Caplan, G. (1964). Principles of Preventive Psy- ments for the 21st century. The Counseling
chiatry. New York: Basic Books. Psychologist, 28, 733-763.

Conyne, R. K. (2004). Preventive Counseling: Schwartz, J. P., Griffin, M., Russell, M., & Fron-
Helping People to Become Empowered in taura-Duck (2006). Journal of College Coun-
Systems and Settings, New York: Brunner- seling, 9, 90-96.
Routledge. Schwartz, J. P., & Hage, S. M., Bush, I., & Key, L.
Duncan, D. F. (1994). The prevention of primary S. (2006). Family of origin dynamics as con-
prevention, 1960-1994: Notes toward a case tributing factors to the intergenerational
study. The Journal of Primary Prevention, transmission of intimate violence: A review
15(1), 73-79. of the literature. Trauma, Violence, and
Abuse, 7, 206-221.
Felner, R., Felner, T., & Silverman, M. (2000). Pre-
vention in mental health and social interven- Waldo, M. (1989). Primary prevention in university
tion. IN J. Rappaport & E. Seidman (Eds.), residence halls: Paraprofessional led Rela-
Handbook of community psychology (pp. 9- tionship Enhancement groups for college
42). New York: Kluwer. roommates. Journal of Counseling and De-
velopment, 67, 465-471.
Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Greene, W. F.,
Koch, G. G., Linder, G. F., & MacDougall, Walker, L. E. (1984). The battered woman syn-
J. E. (2000). The safe dates program: 1-year drome. New York: Springer-Verlag.
follow-up results. American Journal of Pub-
lic Health, 90, 1619-1622.
Foubert, J. D. (2000). The longitudinal effects of a
rape-prevention program on fraternity men's
attitudes, behavioral intent, and behavior.
Journal of American College Health, 48,
158-163.
Gamache, D., & Snapp, S. (1995). Teach your chil-
dren well: Elementary schools and violence
prevention. In E. Peled, P. J. Jaffe, & J. Ed-
leson (Eds.). Ending the cycle of violence:
Community responses to children of bat-
tered women (pp. 209-231). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Leviss, P. S. (2001). Financing the publics health.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 5
Editors Note

Debra L. Ainbinder
Lynn University

I am truly honored to take on the role of As you read this edition of Prevention in
Editor for Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice
Theory, Research, Practice and Training. This is and Training, you will note the quality of our contri-
truly an exciting time to be working in the field of butions and the diversity of prevention issues cov-
prevention. Thanks to the efforts of the Prevention ered within. This is the beginning of a global focus
Section leadership, prevention work is becoming for the publication which will spotlight the theoreti-
more valued and acknowledged in APA and in the cal and empirical work of counseling psychologists
field of counseling psychology in general. This and prevention scientists. Look for our revised sub-
publication is an additional opportunity for the Sci- mission guidelines and publication schedule for
entist-Practitioners in our field to publish their work 2009 available in early February. Heres to a New
focusing on prevention and to add to the body of Year filled with Peace and a focus on prevention.
knowledge in counseling psychology and in the
specialization of prevention.

Our editorial board has lofty goals for this


publication, all of which I look forward to imple-
menting. As our first goal, we published this issue
at the end of 2008 in our hopes to rejuvenate the
publication and expand its horizons. To this end, I
want to personally thank the Editorial Review
Board and especially its Chair, Michael Waldo for
their efforts in reviewing this issues submissions
on a very tight deadline. The past Editor and now
Section Chair, Jonathan Schwartz has been an im-
measurable help throughout this process and for
that I am grateful. Our new Associate Editor, Max
Nisenbaum, is to be commended for stepping up
and completing his work in the shortest of time
given to an Editor. To all our authors in this issue, I
am aware of the time pressures put upon each of
you and thank you for rising to the challenge with
excellent pieces that are included in this publica-
tion. I commend and thank all of you.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 6
The Strong Kids Curricula for Building Social and Emotional
Resiliency in Children and Adolescents

Oanh K. Tran
California State University, East Bay

Childrens mental health has received con- Given the mental health needs of children
siderable concern and attention over the past sev- and youths, proactive interventions are required to
eral decades. Children and youths are at increased prevent and intervene before social or emotional dif-
risk for social and emotional problems and aca- ficulties develop or become severe and interfere with
demic failure due to difficulties in managing their development and academic success. Social and emo-
emotions (Greenberg, Domotrovich, & Bumbarger, tional learning (SEL), a framework developed to ad-
2001; Zins, Bloodworth, Weissberg, & Walberg, dress the mental health challenges confronted in
2004), developing positive social relationships schools, promotes the acquisition of social and emo-
(Merrell, Gueldner, & Tran, 2007), and unreadiness tional competency skills through educative, evidence
for the schooling process (Sprague, 2006). Several -based practices which can be infused through
estimates are indicated in the research literature on school-based curricula or school/classroom environ-
the number of children (below the age of 17) re- ment (Greenberg et al., 2003; Zins et al., 2004). SEL
quiring mental health services (e.g., 12-22%, is defined as the process through which children
Greenberg et al., 2003; 3-5%, Hoagwood & Erwin, and adults develop the skills necessary to recognize
1997; 1-2 children in a classroom of 30, Merrell, and manage emotions, develop care and concern for
2003). The conflicting estimates in the literature others, make responsible decisions, form positive
suggest that it is difficult to ascertain the prevalence relationships, and successfully handle the demands
rate of mental health problems in children as mental of growing up in todays complex society (CASEL,
health problems can often co-occur or go unnoticed, 2002, p. 1). SEL includes a broad range of tools and
which makes it difficult to assess and intervene ef- systematic techniques used to promote mental
fectively. Despite the inconsistency in prevalence health, teach social, emotional, and life skills; and
rates, many of these children who need mental prevent negative life outcomes, through effective
health services do not receive appropriate or any curricular programming as an integral part of a
services at all (Greenberg et al., 2003). school program (Ragozzino, Resnik, OBrien, &
Weissberg, 2003; Zins et al., 2004). Studies on SEL
The suffering of emotional and behavioral have found an increase in attendance and decrease in
problems not only affects the individual child or drop out rates (Ragozzino et al., 2003), while en-
family, but the school system and society as a hancing students connections to school through
whole. Individuals with mental health problems are supportive environments (Greenberg et al., 2003).
less productive in educational and occupational set- Figure 1 depicts the association of social and emo-
tings. Specifically, failure to acquire adequate so- tional learning and paths to success in school and
cial and emotional competency skills are associated life (Adapted from Zins et al., 2003).
with many negative outcomes, including higher
than average rates of mental illness, incarceration, An example of an innovative, evidence-
family strife, and unemployment or underemploy- based social and emotional learning program is the
ment (Asher & Coie, 1990; Rudolph & Asher, Strong Kids Social and Emotional Learning Curric-
2000). ula for promoting childrens mental health and resil-
iency through the development of healthy compe-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 7
The strong kids curricula Tran

tency skills (Merrell, Carrizales, Feuerborn, Gueld- Lesson 1: About Strong Kids (purpose of
ner, & Tran, 2007a, 2007b, 2007c). Strong Kids Strong Kids, overview of curricula, pre-
initially developed in 2001, with field trials since test, rules and expectations, introductory
2003 have examined the effectiveness and utility of activities)
the curriculum in addressing childrens mental Lesson 2: Understanding Your Emotions,
health problems (see Castro-Olivo, 2006; Feuer- Part 1 (increasing awareness of emotions
born, 2004; Gueldner, 2006; Harlacher, 2008; Is- and emotional variability)
ava, 2006; Levitt, 2008; Merrell, Juskelis, Tran, & Lesson 3: Understanding Your Emotions,
Buchanan, 2006; Tran, 2007). These studies pro- Part 2 (increasing awareness of ways of
vide strong evidence for the efficacy and utility of expressing emotion, comfort and discom-
Strong Kids as a prevention and early intervention fort with emotions, connections between
tool for social and emotional development. Strong feelings and events)
Kids is based on the premise that social and emo- Lesson 4: Dealing With Anger (learning to
tional skills can be taught to deal with adversity express anger in appropriate rather
(stressors, negative experiences, risk factors) and than maladaptive ways)
help build resiliency and coping skills to deal with Lesson 5: Understanding Other's Feelings
stressors. Strong Kids was designed to target the (empathy training)
five pathways to wellness advocated by Cowen Lesson 6: Clear Thinking, Part 1
(1994). These pathways include: (a) early attach- (introduction to cognitive processes)
ments that are wholesome, (b) becoming compe- Lesson 7: Clear Thinking, Part 2 (cognitive
tent with developmentally appropriate skills, (c) restructuring)
being exposed to settings that encourage wellness, Lesson 8: Power of Positive Thinking
(d) feeling a sense of empowerment or being in (learned optimism and attribution
control of ones future, (e) possessing coping skills retraining)
to deal with stress effectively. Strong Kids incorpo- Lesson 9: Solving People Problems (conflict
rates components suggested by researchers in the resolution training)
field of education and mental health to be critical Lesson 10: Letting Go of Stress (relaxation
programming characteristics that make a program training)
effective, such as the focus on multiple domains, Lesson 11: Achieving Your Goals (goal set-
behavioral approach, modeling, practicing, testing, ting and increasing positive
and effective teaching principles (see Greenberg et activities)
al., 2003). The 12 semi-scripted, 35-45 minute les- Lesson 12: Finishing UP! (review, post-test,
sons of the Strong Kids include: how to get help if needed)

Evidence- Provide
Provide
Evidence- Greater
Greater
BasedSEL
SEL Opportunities&&
Opportunities
Based Attachment,
Attachment,
Programming Rewards for
Rewards for
Programming PositiveBehavior
Behavior Engagement, &&
Engagement,
Positive Commitment
Commitment
toSchool
to School

Better
Better
Academic
Academic
TeachSEL
SEL Performance
Performance
Teach
Competencies LessRisky
Risky andSuccess
and Success
Competencies Less
Self-awareness Behavior,More
More inSchool
in School
Safe,Caring,
Safe, Caring, Self-awareness Behavior,
Socialawareness
awareness andLife
Life
Cooperative,
Cooperative, Social Assets, &&
Assets, and
Well-Managed Self-management
Self-management Positive
Positive
Well-Managed Relationship skills Development
Learning
Learning Relationship skills Development
Environments Responsible
Responsible
Environments decision making
decision making

Figure 1. Model of social and emotional learning and paths to success.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 8
The strong kids curricula Tran

Children and adolescents are societys fu- disorders in school-aged children: Current
ture, thus immediate care, concern, and attention state of the field. Prevention & Treatment,
should be placed on them to support their social and 4, 163.
emotional development. The demands on students
today, place them at greater risk for mental illness Greenberg, M. T., Weissberg, R. P., OBrien, M.
than ever before. Social and emotional learning, a T., Zins, J. E., Fredericks, L., Resnik, H., et
proactive, educative and preventative approach to al. (2003). Enhancing school-based preven
address the increase challenges and needs in tion and youth development through
schools and society is an ideal option if we are to coordinated social, emotional, and academic
effectively address the mental health concerns. The learning. American Psychologist, 58, 466
Strong Kids curricula continues to be field tested 474.
and empirically supported with promising results Gueldner, B. A. (2007). The effectiveness of a social
for building social and emotional resiliency in chil- -emotional learning program with
dren and adolescents. middleschool students in a general education
setting and the impact of consultation
References support using performance feedback.
Asher, S. R., & Coie. J. D. (Eds.). (1990). Peer Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
rejection in childhood. Cambridge, UK: of Oregon, Eugene.
Cambridge University Press. Harlacher, J. E. (2008). Social and emotional
Castro-Olivo, S. M. (2006). The effects of a cultur- learning as a universal level of support:
ally-adapted social-emotional learning cur- Evaluating the follow-up effect of Strong
riculum on social-emotional and academic Kids. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
outcomes of Latino immigrant high school University of Oregon, Eugene.
students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Hoagwood, K., & Erwin, H. D. (1997).
University of Oregon, Eugene. Effectiveness of school-based mental health
Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional services for children: A 10-year research re
Learning. (CASEL, 2002). Guidelines for view. Journal of Child and Family Studies,
social and emotional learning: High quality 6, 435-451.
programs for school and life success. Isava, D. M. (2006). An investigation of the impact
Retrieved March 18, 2006, from, http:// of a social-emotional learning curriculum on
www.casel.org/downloads/ problem symptoms and knowledge gains
GuidelinesAug02.pdf among adolescents in a residential treatment
Cowen, E. L. (1994). The enhancement of center. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
psychological wellness: Challenges and op University of Oregon, Eugene.
portunities. American Journal of Levitt, V. H. (2008). Promoting social-emotional
Community Psychology, 22, 149179. competency through quality teaching
Feuerborn, L. L. (2004). Promoting emotional practices: The impact of consultation on a
resiliency through instruction: The effects of multidimensional treatment integrity model
a classroom-based prevention program. of the Strong Kids program. Unpublished
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon,
University of Oregon, Eugene. Eugene.

Greenberg, M. T., Domitrovich, C., & Bumbarger, Merrell, K. W. (2003). Behavioral, social, and
B. (2001). The prevention of mental emotional assessment of children and adoles
cents (2nd ed.). Maywah, NJ: Lawrence

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 9
The strong kids curricula Tran

Erlbaum. Tran, O. K. (2007). Promoting social and emotional


learning in schools: An investigation of
Merrell, K. W., Carrizales, D. C., Feuerborn, L., massed versus distributed practice schedules
Gueldner, B. A., & Tran, O. K. (2007a). and social validity of the Strong Kids curricu
Strong KidsGrades 3-5: A social- lum in late elementary aged students.
emotional learning curriculum. Baltimore: Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
Paul H. Brookes Publishing. of Oregon, Eugene.
Merrell, K. W., Carrizales, D. C., Feuerborn, L., Zins, J. E., Bloodworth, M. R., Weissberg, R. P., &
Gueldner, B. A., & Tran, O. K. (2007b). Walberg, H. J. (2004). The scientific base
Strong KidsGrades 6-8: A social-emo linking social and emotional learning to
tional learning curriculum. Baltimore: school success. Building academic success
Paul H. Brookes Publishing. on social and emotional learning: What does
Merrell, K. W., Carrizales, D. C., Feuerborn, L., the research say? New York: Teachers Co
Gueldner, B. A., & Tran, O. K. (2007c). lege.
Strong TeensGrades 9-12: A social- Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., & OBrien, M. U.
emotional learning curriculum. Baltimore: (2003, October). Links Between Safe School
Paul H. Brookes Publishing. Environments and Academic Achievement.
Merrell, K. M., Gueldner, B. A., & Tran, O. K. Retrieved Feb 19, 2007, from http://
(2007). Social and emotional learning: A www.casel.org/projects_products/
school-wide approach to intervention for schoolsuccess.php#pwrpt
socialization, friendship problems, and
more. In B. J. Doll & J. A. Cummings
(Eds.), Population-based Services of School
Psychologists. Bethesda, MD: National As
sociation of School Psychologists.
Merrell, K. W., Juskelis, M. P., Tran, O. K., & Bu
chanan, R. (2008). Social and emotional learn
ing in the classroom: Evaluation of Strong Kids
and Strong Teens on students' social-emotional
knowledge and symptoms. Journal of Applied
School Psychology, 24, 209224.
Ragozzino, K., Resnik, H., O'Brien, M.U., &
Weissberg, R. (2003). Promoting Academic
Achievement through Social and Emotional
Learning. Educational Horizons, 81, 169
171.
Rudolph, K. D., & Asher, S. R. (2000). Adaptation
and maladaptation in the peer system.
Developmental processes and outcomes. In
A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. Z. Miller
(Eds.), Handbook of developmental
psychopathology (pp. 157-175). New York:
Kluwer.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 10
Asian Americans Coping with Frequent Racial Discrimination:
Role of Acculturation and Enculturation Behaviors

Brandon Yoo & Suzie Jeon


Arizona State University

Asian Americans are portrayed as the model with racial discrimination, individuals may problem
minority who are problem free and more successful solve by making a formal complaint, or seek social
than other racial minority groups (Wu, 2002). As a support to get advice, assistance, or information. In
result, there is an extant research examining the contrast, internal coping strategies reflect attempts
psychological consequences of racism among Asian by individuals to change oneself accommodating the
Americans. However, there has been a small but self to the demands of the environment. For in-
growing body of research that suggests racism has a stance, in dealing with racial discrimination, indi-
negative effect on the health and well-being of viduals may cognitively restructure by attributing
Asian Americans, similar to other racial minorities. the stress associated with minority status to racism,
For instance, Asian Americans who perceive fre- or accept the racist event as fate and part of life as a
quent racial discrimination are at increased risk of racial minority in the United States.
physical illnesses (Gee, 2002), drug use (Gee,
Delva, & Takeuchi, 2006), depression (Gee, Studies on culture differences in coping have
Spencer, Yip, & Takeuchi, 2007; Lee, 2003), prob- shown that individuals from collectivistic cultures
lems with career development, interpersonal prob- (such as in Asian cultures) are less likely to cope by
lems, and self-esteem (Liang & Fassinger, 2008), directly interacting and actively influencing their
and lower psychological well-being (Yoo & Lee, source of stress (i.e., external coping); but rather,
2005, 2008). In response to such threats, it is they are more likely to internally focus on accom-
speculated that a wide range of coping strategies modating and reframing their source of stress (i.e.,
may be used to manage the stress related to racial internal coping) (Heppner, Heppner, Lee, Wang,
discrimination from minimizing or denying racial Park, & Wang, 2006; Yeh & Wang, 2000). The pref-
discrimination to directly dealing with racial dis- erence of internal coping over external coping by
crimination (see Miller & Kaiser, 2001, and Miller those endorsing collectivistic values is due to the
& Major, 2000, for review). However, cultural strong value placed on relationship and harmony
characteristics are important factors that may deter- with others (Inman & Yeh, 2006; Yeh, Arora, &
mine the choice and efficacy of coping strategies Wu, 2006). For instance, in 3 studies, Taylor and
used by Asian Americans (Harrell, 2000). Identify- colleagues (2004) found Asian Americans were less
ing these strategies is critical to the prevention re- likely to use social support (i.e., external coping) to
search and practice in reducing deleterious conse- cope with general stress as compared to European
quences of racial discrimination (Grossman, 2005). Americans because they were concerned about the
relational ramifications of seeking support, including
Cross-cultural studies on coping often dif- creating tension , losing face, receiving criticism,
ferentiate between external and internal coping and making the situation worse.
strategies (Chang, 2001; Rothbaum, Weisz, & Sny-
der, 1982; Tweed, White, & Lehman, 2004). Exter- However, empirical evidence is mixed in
nal coping strategies reflect attempts by individuals whether Asian Americans are more likely to use in-
to engage in an active and ongoing negotiation with ternal coping over external coping strategies when
the stressful environment. For instance, in dealing dealing with frequent racial discrimination. Noh and

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 11
Acculturation, discrimination, and coping Yoo & Jeon

colleagues (1998), for example, found that a large and instrumental social support coping) and less in-
proportion of Chinese, Laotian and Vietnamese ternal coping strategies (measured by cognitive re-
refugees who encountered racial discrimination structure and acceptance coping) when dealing with
chose to regard it as a part of life, or simply ignore increased instances of racial discrimination. Further-
or avoid it (i.e., internal coping). Alternatively, Yoo more, Asian Americans who are more enculturated
and Lee (2005) found Asian American college stu- (measured by language proficiency associated with
dents were more likely to use problem solve coping their ethnic group and social affiliation with people
(i.e., external coping) and not cognitive restructure from their ethnic group) will be more likely to use
coping (i.e., internal coping) when perceiving in- internal coping strategies and less external coping
creased racial discrimination. A possible explana- strategies when dealing with increased instances of
tion for the discrepancy in these results may be due racial discrimination.
to the individuals level of adherence to Asian cul-
tural norms and behaviors. Therefore, Asian Ameri- Method
cans who are more acculturated into the mainstream The sample consisted of 132 self-identified
White culture in the U.S. may be more likely to use Asian American college students from a large, pub-
external coping strategies when dealing with fre- lic Midwestern university. Participants were 52
quent encounters of racial discrimination, while males and 80 females with a mean age of 20.6 years
Asian Americans who are more enculturated in (SD=2.5). Asian ethnic groups were diverse with the
maintaining their Asian ethnic cultures are more largest ethnic group being Korean Americans (24%
likely to use internal coping strategies when dealing of total sample). Approximately half were U.S.-
with frequent encounters of racial discrimination. born. Students were distributed across classes (1st
The present study examines the role of ac- year = 35; 2nd year = 38; 3rd year = 25; 4th year =
culturation and enculturation behaviors in the rela- 18; other = 13).
tionship between racial discrimination and external/ Racial discrimination was measured using
internal coping strategies among Asian American vignettes from the Yoo and Lee (2008) study. Par-
college students using a quasi-experimental design. ticipants were asked to read 1 of 2 vignettes imagin-
Consistent with the cultural theory of coping ing themselves in a situation where they were likely
(Inman & Yeh, 2006), we hypothesize that Asian to be rejected because of their race 1 out of 5 times
Americans who are more acculturated (measured by (single incident) or 5 out of 5 times (multiple inci-
language proficiency in English and social affilia- dents). Acculturation and enculturation behaviors
tion with Whites) will be more likely to use exter- (i.e., English language proficiency, ethnic language
nal coping strategies (measured by problem solve proficiency, White social affiliation, and ethnic so-

Figure 1. Interaction effect between racial discrimination and language proficiency in English on the use of acceptance coping.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 12
Acculturation, discrimination, and coping Yoo & Jeon

cial affiliation) were measured based on Tsai, Ying, ciency, White Social Affiliation, and Ethnic Social
and Lee (2000) model of acculturation. External Affiliation. In Step 2, all hypothesized two-way in-
coping (i.e., problem solve and instrumental social teraction terms were entered, including Racial Dis-
support) and internal coping (i.e., cognitive restruc- crimination X English Language Proficiency, Racial
ture and acceptance) strategies were measured us- Discrimination X Ethnic Language Proficiency, Ra-
ing subscales from Coping Strategies Inventory cial Discrimination X White Social Affiliation, and
(Tobin, Holroyd, Reynolds, 1984) and COPE In- Racial Discrimination X Ethnic Social Affiliation.
ventory (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989).
Three significant interaction effects were
Results found on acceptance and cognitive restructure cop-
ing. As hypothesized, individuals who were more
Given the diversity among Asian Ameri- proficient in English (i.e., more acculturated) were
cans, we tested for possible within-group differ- less likely to use acceptance coping strategy and
ences on the coping measures used in this study. cognitive restructure coping (i.e., internal coping)
We found significant differences between gender when dealing with increased instances of racial dis-
and nativity status on problem solve coping. In par- crimination (see Figure 1 and 2, respectively). In
ticular, men (M=3.25) and U.S.-born (M=3.23) re- addition, individuals who were more likely to so-
ported greater use of problem solve coping than cially affiliate with their ethnic group members (i.e.,
women (M=2.97) and foreign-born (M=2.92), re- more enculturated) were more likely to use cognitive
spectively. Consequently, we elected to control for restructure coping (i.e., internal coping) when deal-
gender and nativity status. Four hierarchical multi- ing with increased instances of racial discrimination
ple regression analyses (Aiken & West, 1991) were (see Figure 3).
used to examine the hypothesized two-way interac-
tions between racial discrimination and accultura- Conclusion
tion/enculturation on external/internal coping strate-
gies (i.e., problem solve, instrumental social sup- In dealing with the stress associated with fre-
port, cognitive restructure, and acceptance coping). quent encounters of racial discrimination, Asian
All dichotomous variables were dummy coded and American may engage in a wide range of coping
continuous variables centered to reduce multicollin- strategies (Noh, et al., 1998; Yoo & Lee, 2005).
earity. In Step 1, covariates and predictors were en- However, the choice of coping strategies utilized
tered, including Gender (0=male; 1=female), Nativ- may depend on the individuals cultural characteris-
ity Status (0=foreign-born; 1=U.S.-born), Racial tics. When dealing with increased instances of racial
Discrimination (0=Single; 1=Multiple), English discrimination, our results suggest that Asian Ameri-
Language Proficiency, Ethnic Language Profi- cans who are more acculturated (i.e., higher profi-

Figure 2. Interaction effect between racial discrimination and language proficiency in


English on the use of cognitive restructure coping.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 13
Acculturation, discrimination, and coping Yoo & Jeon

ciency in speaking English) are less likely to use coping strategies, such as internal coping. The ex-
internal coping strategies (i.e., cognitive restructure perience, coping, and health consequences of racism
and acceptance coping), although more enculturated will specifically depend on the individuals ecology.
individuals (i.e., higher comfort and interactions Second, counselors must appreciate the diversity and
with peers of same ethnic background) are more dynamics of how Asian Americans experience and
likely to use internal coping strategy (i.e., cognitive cope with racism. Therapists need to appropriately
restructure coping). These findings are consistent make assessments (such as acculturation and encul-
with the theory that Asian Americans who adhere to turation levels) and treatment plans (such as integra-
the collectivistic values are more likely to utilize tion of assignments that involve more internal cop-
coping strategies that are consistent with their be- ing strategies for more enculturated individuals) that
liefs emphasizing balance and harmony (Inman & correspond to cultural needs of their clients. Third,
Yeh, 2006). Although, more importantly, our study educators must help normalize the experience of ra-
highlight the diversity among Asian Americans cism faced by Asian American. Often, Asian Ameri-
(i.e., their different adherence levels to Asian val- cans also internalize the model minority myth
ues) and the complexity of different coping strate- (Oyserman & Sakamoto, 1997) believing that they
gies utilized by them when dealing with frequent should be problem-free and not experience racism.
experiences of racism. This may create an unfair burden and pressure on
these students. Curriculum and workshops highlight-
The results of this study have significant ing the unique experiences of racism faced by Asian
implications for researchers, counselors, and educa- Americans, why the model minority label is incor-
tors focused on preventing harmful effects from rect, and promoting different ways to cope depend-
racism among Asian Americans. First, it is impor- ing on ones acculturation levels can be greatly
tant to recognize that racism do play a significant beneficial to students well-being.
role in the lives of Asian Americans, despite the
model minority myth. It is only with this acknowl-
edgment that efforts can then be made to prevent References
and reduce deleterious consequences of racism.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regres
Second, researchers need to continually and sys-
sion: Testing and interpreting interactions.
tematically identify ecological protective and exac-
erbative factors of racism that affects the social, Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
health, and academic well-being of Asian American Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J.
students. As our study highlights, not all Asian K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A
Americans are collectivistic or utilize collectivistic theoretically based approach. Journal of

Figure 3. Interaction effect between racial discrimination and ethnic social affiliation on the use of cognitive restructure coping.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 14
Acculturation, discrimination, and coping Yoo & Jeon

Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267- illicit drug use, and alcohol dependence
283. among Filipino Americans. American
Journal of Public Health, 96, 1-8.
Chang, E. C. (2001). A look at the coping strategies
and styles of Asian Americans: similar and Grossman, J. M. (2005). Discrimination among
different? In C.R. Snyder (Ed.), Coping and Asian American Youth. Prevention in
copers: Adaptive processes and people. Counseling Psychology, 1, 15-16.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Harrell, S. P. (2000). A multidimensional
Clark, R., Anderson, N.B., Clark, V.R., & conceptualization of racism-related stress:
Williams, D.R. (1999). Racism as a stressor Implications for the well-being of people of
for African Americans: A biopsychosocial color. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
model. American Psychologist, 54, 805-816. 70, 42-57.
Cross, S. E. (1995). Self-construals, coping, and Heppner, P. P., Heppner, M. J., Lee, D. G., Wang,
stress in cross-cultural adaptation. Journal Y.W., Park, H.J., & Wang, L.F. (2006). De-
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 673-679. velopment and validation of a collectivistic
coping styles inventory. Journal of Counsel-
Gee, G. C. (2002). A multilevel analysis of the rela ing Psychology, 53, 107-125.
tionship between institutional and individual
racial discrimination and health status. Inman, A. G., & Yeh, C. (2006). Stress and coping.
American Journal of Public Health, 92, 615 In F. Leong, A. G. Inman, A. Ebreo, L. Lang,
- 623. L., & Kinoshita, M. Fu (Eds.), Handbook of
Asian American psychology (pp. 323-340).
Gee, G. C., Ro, A., Gavin, A., & Takeuchi, D. T. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
(2008). Disentangling the effects of racial
and weight discrimination on body mass in Kaspar, V., and S. Noh. (2000). Adolescent Coping
dex and obesity among Asian Americans. with Discrimination-Related Stress: An
American Journal of Public Health, 98, 493 Analysis of the Stress-Buffering Effect on De-
- 500. pression in Youth. Paper presented at the
Fourth National Metropolis Conference. To-
Gee, G. C., Ryan, A., Laflamme, D.J., & Holt, J. ronto, Ontario, Canada.
(2006). Self-reported discrimination and
mental health status among African descen Lee, R. M. (2003). Do ethnic identity and other-
dants, Mexican Americans, and other group orientation protect against
Latinos in the New Hampshire REACH discrimination for Asian Americans?
2010 Initiative: The added dimension of Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 133-
immigration. American Journal of Public 141.
Health, 96, 1821-1828.
Liang, C. T. H., & Fassinger, R. E. (2008). The role
Gee, G. C., Spencer, M., Chen, J., Yip, T., & Takeu of collective self-esteem for Asian
chi, D. T. (2007). The association between Americans experiencing racism-related
self-reported discrimination and 12-month stress: A test of moderator and mediator
DSM-IV mental disorders among Asian hypotheses. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Americans nationwide. Social Science and Minority Psychology, 14, 19-28.
Medicine, 64, 1984-1996.
Miller, C. T., & Kaiser, C. R. (2001). A theoretical
Gee, G. C., Delva, J., Takeuchi, D. T. (2005). Rela perspective on coping with stigma. Journal
tionship between self-reported unfair of Social Issues, 57, 73-92.
treatment and prescription medication use,

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 15
Acculturation, discrimination, and coping Yoo & Jeon

Miller, C. T., & Major, B. (2000). Coping with Black and White. New York: Basic Books.
stigma and prejudice. In T. F. Heatherton,
R. E. Kleck, M. R. Hebl, & J. G. Hull Yeh, C. J., Arora, A. K., & Wu, K. (2006). A new
(Eds.), The social psychology of stigma (pp. theoretical model of collectivistic coping. In
243-272). New York, NY: The Guilford P. T. P. Wong & C. J. Wong (Eds.), Hand-
Press. book of multicultural perspectives on stress
and coping (pp. 55-72). New York: Springer.
Noh, S., Morton, B., Feng, H., & Violet, K. (1998).
Experiences of racism among Southeast Yeh, C. & Wang, Y. (2000). Asian American coping
Asian refugees in Canada. In R. Roth (Ed.), attitudes, sources, and practices: Implications
Psychologists facing the challenge of a for indigenous counseling strategies. Journal
global culture with human rights and mental of College Student Development, 41, 94-103.
health. Germany: Pabst Science Publishers. Yoo, H. C., & Lee, R. M. (2005). Ethnic identity
Oyserman, D. & Sakamoto, I. (1997). Identity, and approach-type coping as moderators of
cultural constructs, and stereotype percep the racial discrimination/well-being relation
tion. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, in Asian Americans. Journal of Counseling
33, 435-453. Psychology, 52, 497-506.

Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Yoo, H. C. & Lee, R. M. (2008). Does ethnic
Changing the world and changing the self: identity buffer or exacerbate the effects of
A two-process model of perceived control. frequent racial discrimination on situational
Journal of Personality and Social well-being of Asian Americans? Journal of
Psychology, 42, 537. Counseling Psychology, 55, 63-74.

Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Kim, H. S., Jarcho,


J., Takagi, K., & Dunagan, M. S. (2004). Author contact information:
Culture and social support: Who seeks it Brandon Yoo
and why? Journal of Personality & Social Suzie Jeon
Psychology, 87(3), 354-362. Arizona State University
Tobins, D.L., Holroyd, K.A., & Reynolds, R.V. Asian Pacific American Studies
(1984). Users manual for the Coping PO Box 875503 Tempe, AZ 85287-5503
Strategies Inventory. Unpublished yoo@asu.edu or Suzie.Jeon@asu.edu
manuscript, Ohio State University.
Tsai, J.L., Ying, Y., & Lee, P.A. (2000). The
meaning of Being Chinese and Being
American: Variation among Chinese
American young adults. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 31 (3), 302-322.
Tweed, R. G., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2004).
Culture, stress, and coping: Internally- and
externally-targeted control strategies of
European Canadians, East Asian Canadians,
and Japanese. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 35, 652-667.
Wu, F. H. (2002). Yellow: Race in America beyond

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 16
Preventing School Violence through Establishing
School Connectedness

Adam M. Volungis
Indiana University Bloomington

School violence has always been existent in decade the problem has been studied from multiple
our high schools, but the highly publicized shoot- perspectives in an effort to understand the causes
ings in the 1990s in Littleton, Colorado, Jonesboro, and how to prevent traumatic and sometimes deadly
Arkansas, and Paducah, Kentucky received exten- school violence. There are now some signs of prom-
sive media attention resulting in a dramatic increase ise with research beginning to show trends that seem
in public awareness and concern that such violent to be efficacious in preventing school violence. One
acts could occur in their community. In 2001 the area that has received a great deal of attention, and
Surgeon General put out a call to recognize and appears very robust, is research demonstrating that
confront the concern of youth violence in the the more students feel connected to their school, the
United States (U. S. Department of Health and Hu- less likely they are to commit acts of school violence
man Services, 2001). Research has shown increas- (Derzon, 2006; Karcher, 2002).
ingly high rates of adolescents are engaging in vio-
lent behavior (Farrington, 2004). Adolescents com- School Violence & School Connectedness Link
mit more violent crimes than any other age group The quality of relationships between students
(Pastore & Maguire, 2002). In fact, 27% of serious and the faculty, staff, and administration of the
violent crimes and victimizations that occur in the school is often referred to as school connectedness.
United States are committed by individuals under This approach focuses on positive individual traits,
the age of 18 (Federal Bureau of Investigation, thoughts, emotions, and strengths that can be fos-
1999). Thus, it is no surprise that many of these tered by key groups of individuals within schools,
violent behaviors take place in school considering and even the community. Rather than being simply
adolescents spend a significant portion of their problem-focused, fostering school connectedness
waking hours in this very social, and often diverse, utilizes a positive approach to school violence
environment. through preventative, systemic effects. Research has
School violence in this context refers to tak- shown that students who feel connected with school
ing part in a physical fight and/or using or threaten- personnel through established trusting relationships
ing another person with a weapon. Hence, although appears to not only positively affect academic
seemingly obvious, school violence is considered a achievement, socio-emotional well-being (ranging
problem because it has negative impacts ranging from effective interpersonal skills to feeling less
from psychological (e.g., posttraumatic stress) to alienated), and resiliency (Karcher, 2004; Ryan,
physical (e.g., death) at the individual and institu- Gheen, & Midgley, 1998; Shochet et al., 2006;
tional levels (McNeely & Falci, 2004; Shochet, Smith & Sandhu, 2004; Townsend & McWhirter,
Dadds, Ham, & Montague, 2006; Smith & Sandhu, 2005), but also appears to have a vital role in pre-
2004). Furthermore, the prevalence and concern venting school violence (Brookmeyer, Fanti, & Hen-
about school violence is not a secret or unknown rich, 2006; Henrich, Brookmeyer, & Shahar, 2005;
fact (see Jimerson, Morrison, Pletcher, Furlong, Karcher, 2002). Figure 1 highlights some of more
2006). However, what to do, or how to prevent salient and significant relationships often associated
violence is a little more ambiguous. Within the past with school connectedness. Overall, purposefully

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 17
Preventing school violence Volungis

enhancing school connectedness is significantly from recent literature indicating that relationships
more effective than other approaches that empha- with school personnel is vital in increasing the likeli-
size harsh discipline for misbehaving students (e.g., hood of preventing violent acts by students (Daniels,
zero tolerance) or training to notice warning signs Bradley, Cramer, Winkler, Kinebrew, & Crockett,
by way of profiling students, which can actually 2007a, 2007b). For example, the Safe School Initia-
decrease school connectedness (McNeely, Nonne- tive, a joint project between the U. S. Secret Service
maker, & Blum, 2002; Smith & Sandhu, 2004). and the U. S. Department of Education, suggest that
incidents of targeted school violence rarely are sud-
The need to belong, or feel accepted, is very den or impulsive non-targeted acts (Vossekuil, Fein,
powerful and can significantly affect emotional ad- Reddy, Borum, & Modzeleski, 2002). Often times
justment and related cognitive processes perpetrators, or would-be perpetrators, share their
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Stated differently, plans of a school violent act with other students be-
interpersonal attachments can be a significant moti- fore it takes place (OToole, 2000). However, stu-
vational factor in making decisions and correspond- dents who would normally feel uncomfortable to
ing behavioral responses. Subsequently, the more rat out a peer, and keep this information to them-
connected students feel within their school the less selves, are more prone to inform an adult of a poten-
likely they are to engage in unhealthy behaviors. tial plan for school violence if they trust and feel
For example, in a sample of high school students, connected with this individual. In other words, stu-
Resnick, Harris, and Blum (1993) compared stu- dents are more likely to break the code of silence
dents with high connectedness scores to those with with another adult role model. This is a vital, almost
low connectedness scores. Those students report- necessary, behavior of the students considering the
ing high levels of school connectedness were found majority of shooting incidents are stopped by meth-
to have significantly lower rates of emotional dis- ods other than law enforcement intervention
tress, suicidal behavior, and violence. Resnick et (Vossekuil et al., 2002).
al. (1997) found similar findings with regard to
lower levels of distress, risk behavior, and aggres- Fostering strengths and providing hope and
sion based upon cross-sectional analysis of inter- personal insight through connectedness has also
view data from over 12,000 adolescents (grades 7- been shown to improve overall psychological well-
12) that participated in the National Longitudinal being (or at least minimize the chances of experienc-
Study on Adolescent Health. In fact, students with ing mental health distress in the future) based upon
high levels of school connectedness have been longitudinal studies (e.g., Ozer, 2005; Shochet et al.,
found to be less likely to be perpetrators of violence 2006). Thus, not only are students able to identify
(or victims) in both positive and negative school specific individuals that they feel comfortable with
climates (Wilson, 2004). Furthermore, Herrenkohl, to break this code of silence, they also have the
Hill, Chung, Guo, Abbott, and Hawkins (2003) mental stamina to take responsibility for speaking
found in their prospective study that even youths up and being able to accept/cope with any social re-
who experienced aggression at a young age had a percussions from other students for not being quiet.
lower probability of violence at age 18 if they were
later exposed to such protective factors as school Conclusion
connectedness. These high levels of school con- This research linking the connection between
nectedness seem to largely come from students establishing student school connectedness and
perceptions of being supported, cared for, and re- school violence should be viewed as a call to school
spected by school personnel (McNeely et al., 2002). personnel (e.g., counselors, teachers, and administra-
The premise here is that school personnel, tion) to recognize that they can have a significant
especially counselors and teachers, play a vital role impact on preventing school violence simply by es-
in this process. Targeting these individuals as a tablishing trusting relationships. In other words, the
main component of enhancing this construct comes concept of preventing school violence can often be

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 18
Preventing school violence Volungis

perceived as a daunting macro-level problem to be dents. Such skills counselors should be aware of in
handled by formal protocols e.g., what can I, one their own interactions and in training school person-
counselor/teacher, really do to prevent school vio- nel include: providing dignity and respect, trust and
lence? Although such formal protocols are impor- acceptance of each student, separating negative be-
tant, the message here is that the consistent applica- havior from the student, providing positive-
tion of fostering trusting relationships and meaning- reinforcing feedback of desired behaviors, genuine
ful interactions with students can be done at the mi- empathy, reflecting feelings, not being afraid to ask
cro-level. Of course, if school personnel are indi- questions, and observation of non-baseline student
vidually effective at enhancing their students level behaviors, which can then be followed up on
of school connectedness then this could ultimately (Daniels et al., 2008).
result in macro-level effects.
In sum, it is vital for counselors to explicitly
There is no denying that there are significant convey to school personnel and individuals within
acts of school violence occurring in the schools of the community the importance of school connected-
the U.S. and that these acts can cause significant ness. Due to the seemingly simple nature of es-
emotional and physical trauma. What has been pre- tablishing school connectedness with students, this
sented here is a review of the robust literature dem- concept may be perceived as superficial and over-
onstrating a significant micro-level relationship be- looked. However, establishing school connected-
tween school connectedness and school violence. ness is not an easy task to achieve with every student
Counselors can play a significant role in contributing and the possible positive consequences (e.g., im-
to minimizing school violence. A practical approach proved psychological well-being, seeking alterna-
for counselors is training and psychoeducation of tives from violent behavior) are too significant to
school personnel that have frequent contact with stu- ignore. It is not about coming down hard on stu-

--
Alienated

-- Non-violent Disruptive Behaviors

+ Pro-social Behaviors
School Con-
nectedness
-- School
Violence

+
Academic Achievement
+
Interpersonal Relationships
+

Help Seeking Behavior

Figure 1. Common School Connectedness Relationships

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 19
Preventing school violence Volungis

dents for their mistakes or looking to identify prob- Daniels, J. A., Volungis, A. M., Pshenishny, E.,
lem students. It is about establishing healthy and Ghandi, P., Winkler, A., Cramer, D. P., et
trusting relationships with school personnel in the al. (2008). A Qualitative investigation of
first place, so it is not even necessary to contem- averted school rampages. Manuscript
plate punitive punishment or weeding out high risk submitted for publication.
students. In the end, it appears that it comes down
to a fundamentally important, but often neglected, Derzon, J. (2006). How effective are school-based
relational approach to students that can have one of violence prevention programs in preventing
the greatest impacts in preventing school violence. and reducing violence and other antisocial
behaviors? A meta-analysis. In S. R.
Jimerson & M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook
Author Note: Correspondence concerning this arti- of school violence and school safety: From
cle should be addressed to Adam M. Volungis, MA, research to practice (pp. 429-441). Mahwah,
LMHC, Doctoral Student, Indiana University NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bloomington, Department of Counseling and Edu- Farrington, D. (2004). Conduct disorder, aggression,
cational Psychology, W. W. Wright Education and delinquency. In R. Lerner & S. Steinberg
Building, 201 N. Rose Ave., Bloomington, IN (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology
47405. E-mail: avolungi@indiana.edu. Phone: 812- (pp. 627-664). New York: Wiley. Federal
856-8302. Fax: 812-856-8317. Bureau of Investigation. (1999). Uni form
crime reports for the United States.
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
References Office.
Henrich, C. C., Brookmeyer, K. A., & Shahar, G.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need (2005). Weapon violence in adolescence:
to belong: Desire for interpersonal Parent and school connectedness as
attachments as a fundamental human protective factors. Journal of Adolescent
motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, Health, 37, 306-312.
497-529
Herrenkohl, T. I., Hill, K. G., Chung, I-J., Guo, J.,
Brookmeyer, K. A., Fanti, K. A., & Henrich, C. C. Abbott, R. D., & Hawkins, J. D. (2003).
(2006). Schools, parents, and youth Protective factors against serious violent
violence: A multilevel, ecological analysis. behavior in adolescence: A prospective study
Journal of Child and Adolescent of aggressive children. Social Work
Psychology, 35, 504-514. Research, 27, 179-191.
Daniels, J. A., Bradley, M. C., Cramer, D. P., Jimerson, S. R., Morrison, G. M., Pletcher, S. W.,
Winkler, A., Kinebrew, K., & Crockett, D. & Furlong, M. J. (2006). Youth engaged in
(2007a). In the aftermath of a school antisocial and aggressive behaviors: Who
hostage event: A case study of one school are they? In S. R. Jimerson & M. J. Furlong
counselors response. Professional School (Eds.), Handbook of school violence and
Counseling, 10, 482-489. school safety: From research to practice
Daniels, J. A., Bradley, M. C., Cramer, D. P., (pp. 3-19). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Winkler, A., Kinebrew, K., & Crockett, D. Erlbaum.
(2007b). The successful resolution of armed Karcher, M. J. (2002). The cycle of violence and
hostage/barricade events in schools: A disconnection among rural middle school
qualitative analysis. Psychology in the students: Teacher disconnection as a
schools, 44, 601-613.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 20
Preventing school violence Volungis

consequence of violence. Journal of School Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M. H., & Midgley, C. (1998).
Violence, 1, 35-51 Why do some students avoid asking for
help? An examination of the interplay
Karcher, M. J. (2004). Connectedness and school among students academic efficacy,
violence: A framework for developmental teachers social-emotional role, and the
interventions. In E. R. Gerler (Ed.), classroom goal structure. Journal of
Handbook of school violence (pp. 7-39). Educational Psychology, 90, 528-535.
New York: Haworth.
Shochet, I. M., Dadds, M. R., Ham, D., &
McNeely, C. A., & Falci, C. (2004). School Montague, R. (2006). School connectedness
connectedness and the transition into and is an underemphasized parameter in
out of health-risk behavior among adolescent mental health: Results of a
adolescents: A comparison of social community prediction study. Journal of
belonging and teacher support. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
School Health, 74, 284-292. 35, 170-179.
McNeely, C. A., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Blum, R. Smith, D. C., & Sandhu, D. S. (2004). Toward a
W. (2002). Promoting school positive perspective on violence prevention
connectedness: Evidence from the national in schools: Building connections. Journal of
longitudinal study of adolescent health. Counseling & Development, 82, 287-293.
Journal of School Health, 72, 138-146.
Townsend, K. C., & McWhirter, B. T. (2005). Con
OToole, M. (2000). The school shooter: A threat nectedness: A review of the literature with
assessment perspective. Quantico, VA: implications for counseling, assessment, and
Federal Bureau of Investigation. research. Journal of Counseling &
Ozer, E. J. (2005). The impact of violence on urban Development, 83, 191-201.
adolescents: Longitudinal effects of U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
perceived school connection and family (2001). Youth violence: A report of the
support. Journal of Adolescent Research, Surgeon General. Retrieved March 29, 2008,
20, 167-192. from http://www.surgeongeneral.gov/library/
Pastore, A. L., & Maguire, K. (Eds.). (2002). youthviolence
Sourcebook of criminal justice statistics Vossekuil, B., Fein, R., Reddy, M., Borum, R., &
2001. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Modzeleski, W. (2002). The final report and
Justice. findings of the safe school initiative:
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Implications for the prevention of school
Bauman, K. E., Harris, K. M., Jones, J., et attacks in the United States. Washington,
al. (1997). Protecting adolescent from harm: DC: U.S. Secret Service, National Threat
Findings from the national longitudinal Assessment Center.
study on adolescent health. Journal of the Wilson, D. (2004). The interface of school climate
American Medical Association, 278, 823- and school connectedness and relationships
832. with aggression and victimization. Journal
Resnick, M. D., Harris, L., & Blum, R. (1993). The of School Health, 74, 293-299.
impact of caring and connectedness on
adolescent health and well-being. Journal
of Pediatrics & Child Health, 28, S3-S9.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 21
Preventing racial, ethnic, gender, sexual minority, disability,
and religious microaggressions: Recommendations for pro-
moting positive mental health

Kevin L. Nadal
John Jay College of Criminal Justice- City University of New York

In the past two years, several counseling fashioned forms of racism, in which individuals
psychologists have written about a phenomenon speak and behave in blatantly racist ways (e.g., when
known as racial microaggressions (Sue et al., someone uses a racial slur to verbally attack or de-
2007a; Sue, Bucerri, Lin, Nadal, & Torino, 2007b; scribe another person). Microinsults include state-
Sue, et al., 2008). Racial microaggressions are de- ments or actions that indirectly belittle a person of
fined as brief and commonplace daily verbal, be- color and are often unconcsious and unintentional.
havioral, or environmental indignities, whether in- For example, when someone tells an African Ameri-
tentional or unintentional, that communicate hos- can that she/he is articulate or when someone tells
tile, derogatory, or negative racial slights and in- an Asian American that she/he speaks good Eng-
sults toward people of color (Sue et al., 2007a). lish, the individual fails to recognize that she/he
These authors purport that because the US has be- may be indirectly insulting the person (Sue et al.,
come more politically correct, that overt racism no 2007a; Sue, et al., 2007b; Sue, et al., 2008). Why
longer exists. Instead, the presence of blatant ra- wouldnt the African American person be articulate?
cism and hate crimes towards racial/ethnic minori- Why wouldnt the Asian American person speak
ties are likely to be viewed as offensive, unpleasant, good English? These are two statements reflect sub-
and appalling; views held by overtly racist groups tle racial biases that the person may hold- that Afri-
like the Ku Klux Klan are likely to be perceived as can Americans are unintelligent or that Asian
unacceptable by all members of US society. In fact, Americans are foreigners.
most White Americans in the US may perceive
themselves as good people that they treat people Microinsults can also take behavioral forms,
of all racial groups the same (Sue, 2005). However with examples including an individual who follows a
while overtly racist acts may have decreased in the person of color around a store or an individual who
past two decades, covert racist acts (or racial micro- holds her/his purse or wallet when they walk by a
aggressions) may have increased. Several studies person of color (Sue et al., 2007a; Sue, et al., 2008).
have supported that racial microaggressions (which In both of these cases, the individual is sending the
are often unconscious and unintentional by the per- message that the person of color is a criminal and/or
petrator) may result in an array of emotions for peo- dangerous. However, if confronted, the perpetrator
ple of color including frustration, anger, sadness, of the microinsult may deny her/his behavior and
belittlement, and alienation and that the cumulative reject that she/he was intending to be racist. Finally,
nature of these microaggressions therefore may po- microinvalidations are statements and behaviors that
tentially lead to mental health problems including negate or nullify a person of colors experiences or
depression, anxiety, and trauma (Sue et al., 2007b; realities. This can be exemplified by someone who
Sue et al., 2008). tells a person of color that she/he is being too sensi-
tive about race or makes statements like, I dont
There are three forms of microaggressions see race. I only see people. In asserting such decla-
that are hypothesized to exist: microassaults, micro- rations, the individual is ignoring the persons racial
insults, and microinvalidations (Sue et al., 2007a). reality and is denying that she/he is capable of ra-
Microassaults are acts that resemble the old- cism (Sue et al., 2007a; Sue, et al., 2007b). These

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 22
Preventing microaggressions Nadal

statements can be invalidating to the person of (whether intentional or conscious) may impart hurt-
color, in that racism may be a real and regular part ful and derogatory messages towards the member of
of her/his life and that many Whites can be subtly the oppressed group. Initial research on racial micro-
racist towards them. As a result, the person of color aggressions has supported that these messages may
may feel belittled and put down, while the perpetra- lead to a variety of emotional and psychological
tor of the microinvalidation may be completely un- stressors and may have lasting impacts on the mental
aware of the impact of her/his statement. health of the recipients of these microaggressions
(Sue, et al., 2007b; Sue, et al., 2008). Accordingly, it
Based on the empirical support of the exis- would be hypothesized that these same mental health
tence of racial microaggressions, it is likely that outcomes may occur for recipients of gender, sexual
similar experiences can occur for different minority orientation, ethnic, religious, and ability microag-
groups, including lesbian, gay, bisexual, or trans- gressions. The purpose of this paper is to describe
gender (LGBT) persons, women, disabled persons, various types of microaggressions that may occur
or ethnic and religious minority groups. In fact, towards other different minority groups, while pro-
many bodies of literature have focused on subtle viding recommendations for preventing microag-
sexism, subtle heterosexism, and other forms of gressions in psychology and in everyday life.
subtle oppression (e.g., Morrison & Morrison,
2002; Sue & Capodilupo, 2007; Swim, Hyers, Microaggressions towards various oppressed groups
Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001; Wall, 2008) that fit in
line with the classification of microaggressions. There are many types of gender microaggres-
Given this, the definition of microaggressions can sions that may occur towards women, which is evi-
be expanded to the following: microaggressions are denced by the literature on everyday sexism (see
brief and commonplace daily verbal, behavioral, or Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001, for a re-
environmental indignities, whether intentional or view), sexism in school systems (See Leaper &
unintentional, that communicate hostile, deroga- Brown, 2008, for a review) and sexism the work-
tory, or negative racial slights and insults toward place (See Fitzgerald, Hulin, & Drasgow, 1994, for a
members of oppressed groups. review). Examples of gender microaggressions may
include: sexual objectification, invisibility/second-
For example, a gender microassault may class citizenship, denial of individual sexism, and
include a man referring to a woman as a bitch or assumptions of traditional gender roles (Nadal, in
a whore (Nadal, in press; Sue & Capodilupo, press; Sue & Capodilupo, 2007). Sexual objectifica-
2007) The message that is conveyed is that a tion occurs when a woman is treated like an object
woman is inferior and that men have the right to (e.g., a group of men who catcall a woman as she
dehumanize them. A sexual minority microinsult walks by on the street). Second-class citizenship in-
might include a heterosexual person asking a les- cludes times when women are ignored and when
bian or gay person if she/he chose to be lesbian or men are given preferential treatment (e.g., a woman
gay (Nadal, Rivera, & Corpus; in progress). The is ignored by a bartender or electronic store salesper-
message that is transmitted is that heterosexuality is son). Denial of individual sexism occurs when a man
the norm and that being LGBT is unnatural or ab- tells a woman that he couldnt be sexist (e.g., an em-
normal. Disability microinvalidations might include ployer says I treat men and women the same, al-
an able-bodied person telling a disabled person that though he doesnt know the names of his female em-
she/he is too sensitive about being discriminated ployees). Assumptions of traditional gender roles
against because of her/his disability. The message may occur when women are encouraged to be or act
that is communicated is that there is something a certain way because of their gender (e.g., a woman
wrong with the disabled person and not with an in her thirties is told that she should be married and
ableist society. have children instead of pursuing her career).
These types of statements and behaviors In terms of LGBT discrimination, there are

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 23
Preventing microaggressions Nadal

several studies in sexual prejudice (see Herek, 2000 ample, Asian Americans reported being insulted or
for a review), antigay harassment (see Burn, penalized on their ways of communicating in class-
Kadlec, & Rexer 2005 for a review), sexual stigma room settings and/or for their ways of eating. Afri-
(see Herek, 2007 for a review), modern heterosex- can Americans reported similar experiences of feel-
ism (see Morrison & Morrison, 2002 for a review), ing the need to communicate in White American
and modern homonegativity (see Wall, 2008 for a ways in the workplace, while also learning messages
review) that suggest that sexual minority microag- about maintaining White American standards of
gressions exist. There are several types of sexual beauty. While there are clearly racial implications
minority microaggressions (or microaggressions on for both of these themes, the focus is on cultural val-
the basis of sexual orientation and/or transgender ues which indicate that microaggressions can be eth-
identity) that may manifest in everyday life. These nic-based. Given this, ethnic microaggressions may
include: use of heterosexist terminology, endorse- include ways that non-Americans are subtly dis-
ment of heteronormative culture/ behaviors, as- criminated (e.g., when individuals do not conform to
sumption of universal LGBT experience, and as- American cultural values), but may also include
sumption of sexual pathology/abnormality (Nadal, ways that specific cultural groups are treated based
Rivera, & Corpus, in progress; Sue & Capodilupo, on their particular ethnicity or heritage (e.g., subtle
2007). Use of heterosexist terminology may include discrimination towards Arab Americans, Caribbean
instances where individuals use the term gay to Americans, Mexican Americans, or Filipino Ameri-
connote something bad, or use derogatory terms cans).
like faggot or dyke. Endorsement of heteronor-
mative culture/behaviors include times when het- There are no known studies that focus spe-
erosexuals assume that LGBT persons should cifically on religious microaggressions, which may
speak, act, or behave more like heterosexuals (e.g., reflect the dearth of literature on religious discrimi-
someone telling a gay man to act straight at a so- nation. In fact, previous authors have noted that
cial function). Assumption of universal experience there is a significantly smaller number of published
may be exemplified by someone who stereotypes works on religious discrimination, in comparison to
all LGBT persons to be the same and to have identi- literature describing racial and ethnic discrimination
cal experiences (e.g., assuming that it is necessary (Hodge, 2007; Weaver et al., 1998). Nonetheless,
for all LGBT to come out during late adoles- there is some literature that is written on Islamopho-
cence/early adulthood). Assumption of sexual pa- bia or prejudice towards Muslims (Gottschalk &
thology/abnormality occurs when heterosexual indi- Greenberg, 2008; Hassouneh & Kulwicki, 2007;
viduals behave in ways in which they presume that Rippy & Newman, 2006), as well as anti-Semitism
all LGBT are sexually promiscuous (e.g., asking a or prejudice towards Jews (Marcus, 2007; Simon &
LGBT person if she/he has HIV/AIDS and/or if Schaler, 2007). Given this literature, as well as the
she/he has numerous sexual partners). types of microaggressions that may occur towards
other minority groups, some examples of religious
Ethnic microaggressions would be defined microaggressions may include: 1) pathology of non-
as subtle discrimination that is based on ones eth- Christianity, 2) assumptions of Christianity, and 3)
nic or cultural group. While there arent any studies assumptions of religious stereotypes. Examples of
that focus specifically on these types of microag- pathology of non-Christianity may include someone
gressions, the studies on racial microaggressions who assumes that non-Christians are evil or
give examples of ethnic microaggressions. For ex- immoral. This could be demonstrated through the
ample, in a study of Asian Americans one theme 2008 presidential campaign, in which Barack Obama
included pathologizing cultural values/ was often referred to as a Muslim as a way of deter-
communication styles (Sue et al., 2007b), while in ring Christian voters from trusting him. Another ex-
a study of African Americans, a theme included ample may include when someone tells a non-
assumed superiority of White cultural values/ religious/ atheist/ agnostic person that she/he is
communication styles (Sue et al., 2008). For ex- going to hell, is immoral, or is pathological. As-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 24
Preventing microaggressions Nadal

sumptions of Christianity may occur when others sions from the perspectives of religious minorities
presume that strangers and acquaintances are Chris- and disabled persons; similarly, microaggressions
tian and/or celebrate Christian traditions (e.g., when based on social class, age, and size may also be of
people automatically tell a stranger Merry Christ- research interest in the future.
mas when they do not know her/his religious be-
liefs). Assumptions of religious stereotypes may Recommendations for preventing microaggressions
include instances when individuals act in ways that Because previous authors have supported
express that they believe religious stereotypes are that microaggressions may lead to emotional and
true (e.g., an individual stares in fear at a Muslim psychological stressors (Sue, et al., 2007b; Sue, et
person on an airplane or an individual makes a re- al., 2008) it is important for psychologists and other
mark about how Jewish people are cheap). practitioners to attempt to prevent the continuation
Similar to religious microaggressions, there of microaggressions in both professional and per-
are no known studies on ability microaggressions. sonal settings. In understanding microaggressions
However, many authors have written on ableism and working to prevent them, psychologists and
which is defined as the devaluation of disability, in other practitioners can promote positive mental
which individuals assume that it is better for indi- health for clients, students, and all members of soci-
viduals to walk than roll, speak than sign, read ety. In order to prevent further injuries to the mental
print than read Braille, spell independently than use health of oppressed groups, psychologists must
a spell-check, and hang out with nondisabled kids abide by APA Ethical Guidelines and aim to become
as opposed to other disabled kids (Hehir, 2002, p. multiculturally competent, by increasing ones (a)
1). As a result of disability prejudice and discrimi- knowledge, (b) awareness, and (c) skills in working
nation, many disabled persons may develop inter- with culturally diverse populations (American Psy-
nalized ableism, which in turn may have an impact chological Association, 2002; Sue & Sue, 2007).
on their mental health and self-esteem (Campbell, First, knowledge of microaggressions can be learned
2008). Given these experiences, some major exam- by studying and inquiring about the existences and
ples of disability microaggressions might include: processes of microaggressions for members of vari-
1) assumptions of inferiority, 2) second-class citi- ous oppressed groups. This can occur by talking
zenship, and 3) denial of ones individual ableism. with others about their experiences with microag-
Assumptions of inferiority might consist of in- gressions and learning how these experiences are
stances when able-bodied individuals assume that real, injurious, and likely based on institutional and
disabled persons are intellectually or physically in- societal discrimination. Individuals must gain
ferior. This can be exemplified by an individual knowledge of the impacts of microaggressions on
who speaks slowly to a disabled person in a wheel- both privileged/dominant and oppressed/target
chair, making the assumption that the disabled per- groups and keep up with current research on micro-
son is not intelligent, does not speak English, or has aggressions and their impacts on self-esteem, emo-
hearing disabilities. Second-class citizenship may tional distress, and other psychological problems for
comprise of encounters when individuals give pref- both groups.
erential treatment towards able-bodied persons or Awareness of microaggressions can increase
who persons convey annoyance or frustration with by learning how these subtle forms of discrimination
having to accommodate disabled persons. Denial of may occur in ones own interpersonal relationships
individual ableism may include experiences when and how they as individuals might play roles as per-
able-bodied persons deny that they have the capac- petrators or recipients of microaggressions. For ex-
ity to hold biases and prejudices towards disabled ample, individuals must become aware of how their
persons. These are merely examples of the types of membership in privileged/dominant groups may in-
microaggression that may occur for both religious fluence their interactions with members of op-
and disabled persons. Further research would be pressed/target groups and how they may be uncon-
helpful in exploring different types of microaggres-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 25
Preventing microaggressions Nadal

scious of ways that subtle discrimination manifests chology, 445 W. 59th St, New York, NY 10019, via
in their own behaviors (Sue & Sue, 2007). This re- email at knadal@jjay.cuny.edu, via telephone at
quires individuals to be fully aware of the various (212) 237-8795 (office), or via fax at (212) 237-
messages they have learned from their various cul- 8930.
tural groups, and the values, beliefs, and biases that
they have attained from their various identities. Ac-
cordingly, Whites must become aware of the biases Special acknowledgements: I wish to acknowledge
and stereotypes they have learned about people of my mentor Derald Wing Sue, Ph.D., Sidney Smith,
color; men must be alert to ways that they may be Silvia Mazzula, David Rivera, and my former re-
unintentionally sexist; heterosexual persons must be search team for inspiring this paper.
attentive to ways that they assume that heterosexu-
ality is the norm and that homosexuality and bi-
sexuality is abnormal or deviant. This concept References
would also apply based on ability, religion, age,
social class, and other social identities/ reference American Psychological Association. (2002).
groups. Individuals must be aware of the spectrum Ethical principles of psychologists and code
of messages and values that they have internalized of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060
and how these may influence their interactions with -1073.
others in unconscious ways. Burn, S.M., Kadlec, K., and Rexer, R. (2005).
Finally, upon gaining awareness of microag- Effects of Subtle Heterosexism on Gays,
gressions, individuals can acquire the skills to deal Lesbians, and Bisexuals. Journal of
with various forms of microaggressions in profes- Homosexuality, 49(2), 23-38.
sional and personal relationships (Sue & Sue, Campbell, F.A.K. (2008). Exploring internalized
2007). For example, individuals can learn how to ableism using critical race theory. Disability
confront microaggressions when they transpire, by & Society, 23(2), 151-162.
practicing ways of initiating uncomfortable conver-
sations and dialogues. Additionally, psychologists Fitzgerald, L.F., Gelfand, M.J., & Drasgow, F.
must remain non-defensive and open-minded to the (1995). Measuring sexual harassment: Theo-
perspectives of others when others may confront retical and psychometric advances. Basic and
them on their subtly discriminatory behavior. Indi- Applied Social Psychology, 17(4), 425-445.
viduals can gain skills in preventing microaggres-
sions and promoting positive mental health through Gottschalk, P. & Greenberg, G. (2008).
a number of methods, including: a) gathering infor- Islamophobia: Making Muslims the enemy:
mation about personal experiences of microaggres- Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
sions; b) teaching others about the presence of mi- Hassouneh, D.M. & Kulwicki, A. (2007).
croaggressions; c) training others how to cope with Mental health, discrimination, and trauma in
microaggressions; and d) researching the various Arab Muslim women living in the US: A
impacts that microaggressions may have on the pilot study. Religion & Culture, 10(3), 257-
mental health of various oppressed groups. In en- 262.
gaging in these various types of practices, psy- Hehir, T. (2002). Eliminating ableism in education.
chologists can do their part to prevent microaggres- Harvard Educational Review, 72(1), 1-32.
sions from further damaging the mental health of
many individuals. Herek, G. M. (2000). The psychology of sexual
prejudice. Current Directions in Psychologi
Author Note: All correspondence to be addressed cal Science, 9, 19-22.
to Kevin L. Nadal, Ph.D. via US mail at John Jay
Herek, G.M. (2007). Confronting sexual stigma and
College of Criminal Justice, Department of Psy-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 26
Preventing microaggressions Nadal

prejudice: Theory and practice. Journal of 114.


Social Issues, 63(4), 905925.
Sue, D.W., Bucerri, J.M., Lin, A.I., Nadal, K.L., &
Hodge, D.R. (2007).Social justice and people of Torino, G.C. (2007). Racial microaggres-
faith: A transnational perspective. Social sions and the Asian American experience.
Work, 52(2), 139-148. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psy-
chology, 13(1), 72-81.
Leaper, C. & Brown, C.S. (2008). Perceived experi-
ences with sexism among adolescent girls. Sue, D.W. & Capodilupo, C.M. (2007). Racial, gen-
Child Development, 79(3), 685-704. der, and sexual orientation microaggressions:
Implications for counseling and psychothera-
Marcus, K.L. (2007). The resurgence of py. In D.W. Sue & D. Sue (Authors). Coun-
anti-Semitism on American college seling the Culturally Diverse, 5th Edition.
campuses. Current Psychology, 26(3-4), New York: John Wiley & Sons.
206-212.
Sue, D.W., Capodilupo, C.M., Torino, G.C.,
Morrison, M.A. & Morrison, T.G. (2002). Bucceri, J.M., Holder, A.M., Nadal, K.L., &
Development and validation of a scale Esquilin, M.E. (2007a). Racial
measuring modern prejudice toward gay microaggressions in everyday life:
men and lesbian women. Journal of Implications for counseling. The American
Homosexuality, 43(2), 15-37. Psychologist, 62(4), 271-286.
Rippy, A.E. & Newman, E. (2006). Perceived Sue, D.W., Nadal, K.L., Capodilupo, C.M., Lin,
religious discrimination and its relationship A.I., Rivera, D.P., & Torino, G.C (2008).
to anxiety and paranoia among Muslim Racial Microaggressions against Black
Americans. Journal of Muslim Mental Americans: Implications for counseling.
Health, 1(1), 5-20. Journal of Counseling and Development.
Simon, R.J. & Schaler, J.A. (2007). Anti-Semitism Sue, D.W. & Sue, D. (2007). Counseling the Cultu-
the world over in the twenty-first century. rally Diverse, 5th Edition: Theory and
Current Psychology, 26(3-4), 152-182. Practice. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Nadal, K.L. (in press). Gender
microaggressions and women: Implications Swim, J.K, Hyers, L.L., Cohen, L.L., & Ferguson,
for mental health. In M. Paludi (Ed.), M.J. (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for
Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide. its incidence, nature, and psychological
Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. impact from three daily diary studies.
Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 31-53.
Nadal, K.L., Rivera, D.P. & Corpus, M.J.H. (in
progress). Sexual orientation and Walls, N.E. (2008). Toward a multidimensional
transgender microaggressions in everyday understanding of heterosexism: The
life: Experiences of lesbians, gays, Changing nature of prejudice. Journal of
bisexuals, and transgender individuals. In Homosexuality, 55(1), 1-51.
D.W. Sue (Ed.), Microaggressions and
Marginalized Groups in Society: Race, Weaver, A.J., Kline. A.E., Samford, J.A., Lucas,
Gender, Sexual Orientation, Class and L.A., Larson. D.B., & Gorsuch. K.L. (1998).
Religious Manifestations. New York: Is religion taboo in psychology? A
Wiley & Sons. systematic analysis of research on religion in
seven major American psychology
Sue, D.W. (2005). Racism and the conspiracy of association journals: 1991 -1994. Journal of
silence. Counseling Psychologist, 33, 100- Psychology and Christianity, 17, 220-232.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 27
The Relationship between Resilience and Anger in University
Students

Rachel B. Kirkpatrick, University of Missouri-Kansas City


Tierra M. Freeman, Kentucky State University

Coping with painful events and unpleasant situations discussed previously (Blum, 1997;
emotions is a struggle for every human being. The Christiansen & Evans, 2005; Waller, Okamoto,
ability to cope effectively with these events and Miles, & Hurdle, 2003). These factors are vital in
emotions can be termed resilience (Blum, 1998). helping adolescents to become more successful. The
Those who are less resilient may turn to unhealthy current investigation explored a number of charac-
actions or beliefs about themselves to confront the teristics that are associated with resilience in a uni-
difficulties encountered in their lives. For example, versity population. In addition, the more informa-
in 2005, over 1.8 million Americans turned to vio- tion researchers and practitioners have on such pro-
lent crime (Bureau of Justice, 2007) and in the same tective factors and their relationships with adaptive
year, 22.7% of Americans reported binge drinking functioning, the more effective mental health inter-
(Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Ad- ventions can become. Furthermore, beginning to
ministration, 2007). Mental health statistics are understand the motivation behind lashing out in an-
staggering as well with 14.8 million Americans suf- ger or turning to drugs and alcohol can also assist
fering from depression and 40 million Americans mental health practitioners in helping struggling in-
suffering from anxiety disorders (National Institute dividuals to adapt more effectively.
of Mental Health, 2007).
Thus, the literature surrounding the concept
The goal of this study was to examine fac- of resilience and the protective factors related to re-
tors that may help reduce some of these ineffective silience will be discussed. Research on emotional
coping strategies. Both resilience and anger expres- and anger expression will also be examined with
sion may have a significant effect on how a person particular focus on the relationships of those con-
copes with difficult situations. Resilience research structs to promoting resiliency. It was expected that
has focused on specific outcomes in adolescence resilience would be inversely related to levels of an-
such as academic achievement (Belgrave, Chase- ger and that those who were resilient would exhibit a
Vaughn, Gray, Addison, & Cherry, 2000; Connell, greater ability to control their emotions in an adap-
Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Ripple & Luthar, 2000; tive manner.
Rodgers & Rose, 2001), resistance to drugs and al-
cohol (Waller, Okamoto, Miles, & Hurdle, 2003), Resilience
and overcoming the effects of bullying (Christensen Resilience is a widely studied construct that
& Evans, 2005). The previously mentioned studies can be applied to children, adolescents, adults, and
provided valuable information on how to help ado- the elderly across different ethnic and socio-
lescents cope with these situations, as well as, gave economic backgrounds (Belgrave et al., 2000; Con-
insight into why particular adolescents may struggle nell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Ripple & Luthar, 1999;
more with stressful circumstances. Smith & Carlson, 1997; Smokowski, Reynolds, &
Many protective factors such as family sup- Bezruczko, 1999; Waller et al., 2003). Generally
port, teacher influence, internal optimism, and intel- speaking, resilience can be defined as positively cop-
ligence have been found to influence success in the ing with stressful

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 28
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

events (Smokowski et al., 1999) or adaptive coping varies from person to person according to cognitive
in the face of multiple risk factors (Waller et al., ability, maturity, and emotional resources. Stress
2003). The concept of resilience has played a large can also be exacerbated if a particular event was ex-
role in influencing the field of psychology to move perienced alone instead of as a part of a group
away from studying the negative outcomes that (Blum, 1998). Determining how resilient an individ-
some individuals experience (Blum, 1998). In- ual is also has to do with certain internal characteris-
stead, the field of resilience seeks to focus on those tics as well as elements in their environment. These
individuals who have overcome difficult life cir- environmental characteristics are referred to as pro-
cumstances and have become successful, as well as, tective factors.
what factors promote such adaptive functioning.
Protective Factors
The bulk of the literature in resilience re-
search focuses on the healthy development and suc- Protective factors can include individual
cess of adolescents. Exposure to risk and experi- traits such as self-esteem, intelligence, internal locus
encing certain protective factors influence how re- of control, insight, and temperament (Smith & Carl-
silient an individual is (Blum, 1998; Smith & Carl- son, 1997; Smokowski et al., 1999). External fac-
son, 1997; Smokowski et al., 2000). Risk factors tors are also important such as family support, guid-
can be characteristics of an individual, family, or ance, and the outside influence of a teacher or reli-
the environment. It has been found that cumulative gious figure (Blum, 1998; Smith & Carlson, 1997).
risk factors can have a more negative outcome on a Smokowski et al. (1999), however, also found some
child than any one single risk factor (Smith & Carl- unique protective factors that were cited much less
frequently in the literature. For example, through
son, 1997).
qualitative research, future optimism and goals, per-
Some examples of risk factors include pres- severance, determination, learning from the behavior
sure to abuse substances (Waller et al., 2003), pre- of others, and having a past mastery experience all
vious exposure to violence (Christiansen & Evans, played a role in the resilience of inner city, at-risk
2005), and lack of resources or parental support adolescents. Interventions that focus specifically on
(Smokowski et al., 2000). Risk factors also occur in an adolescents gender and/or ethnic group can also
academic settings. Being overage for grade, fresh- lead to increased resilience (Belgrave et al., 2000).
men-year grades, teacher ratings, and absences
were all strongly related to dropout with absences Another external factor that was found to be
found to be the most powerful predictor (Ripple & important involved previous exposure to stressors.
Luthar, 1999). Even though stress and risk are When addressing adolescent victimization, it was
similar, risk factors can lead to more negative out- found that those adolescents who had witnessed
comes while the various perceptions of a stressful some violence or family conflict were less vulner-
event can influence how it is viewed (Smith & able to victimization (Christiansen & Evans, 2005).
This suggests that previous exposure to stressors
Carlson, 1997).
may increase ones resilience. Christiansen and Ev-
Such differences have been noted in the ex- ans also state, however, that overexposure to stress-
tant literature. For example, Smith and Carlson ors increases the likelihood of victimization. There-
(1997) focused on how certain stressful events may fore, a balance must be found between no exposure
be viewed differently by different age groups. In to stress and an overload of stressful experience.
adolescence, even minor disagreements with par-
ents can be perceived as incredibly stressful; how- As mentioned above, family involvement
ever, in a different age group, this event may not be and support are vital components in helping to de-
perceived as stressful at all. Blum (1998) goes on velop resilience. Specifically addressing adolescent
to explain this difference in perceptions even fur- substance abuse, it was found that family members
ther stating that the perception of stressful events may play a role as either a protective or risk factor.
A study by Waller et al. (2003) found that adoles-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 29
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

cents may find it harder to resist peer pressure to hardiness was also found to be positively related to
use a substance from a family member than from coping and social support (Blum, 1998).
another peer who is not related. Additionally, the
researchers found that these family members can Resilience and its relationship to the regula-
strongly influence adolescent choices not to experi- tion of emotions has been reported in the literature
ment with substances. The generalizability of the (Eisenberg, Guthrie, Fabes, Reiser, Murphy, Hol-
Waller et al findings should be taken with caution gren, Maszk, & Losoya, 1997; Eisenberg, Spinrad,
as their sample included only Native Americans. Fabes, Reiser, Cumberland, Shepard, Valiente,
Moreover, Rodgers and Rose (2001) found that if Losoya, Guthrie, & Thompson, 2004). Specifically,
students are not feeling supported by their parents, resiliency was found to mediate the level of socially
a strong relationship with a teacher can provide that appropriate behavior and emotional control. In a
support and continue to influence the students re- study by Eisenberg et al. (1997), the relationship be-
silience. tween resilience, emotional regulation, and socially
appropriate behavior were examined. Socially ap-
To summarize, multiple factors are involved propriate behavior was identified in this study as
in helping an individual to develop resilience. Pre- both prosocial and socially constructive. Partici-
vious exposure to stressors can be helpful in devel- pants were found to be more socially appropriate if
oping resilience. The support of family members is they were more able to regulate their emotions and
also important when it comes to decision making also had higher levels of resiliency. One specific
and self-efficacy (Connell et al., 1994; Ripple & analysis in this study examined levels of emotional
Luthar, 2000; Rodgers & Rose, 2001). Other inter- intensity. Emotional intensity was defined by higher
nal characteristics such as optimism, insight, and levels of negative emotions such as anxiety and an-
internal locus of control are also factors that have ger. Those participants with higher levels of emo-
been related to more resilient individuals (Smith & tional intensity were less socially appropriate, which
Carlson, 1997; Smokowski et al., 1999). An area also indicated lower levels of resilience and emo-
that has been less well studied is an individuals tional regulation.
emotional expression.
In a similar study, it was found that effortful
Resilience and Emotional Expression control of emotions, as well as impulsivity, were re-
lated to resiliency and problem behaviors (Eisenberg
The relationship of resilience to the expres- et al., 2004). Specifically, resiliency was negatively
sion of emotion is a relatively new area of research correlated with parent and teacher reports of prob-
and is not often found in educational or psychologi- lem behaviors, both internalizing and externalizing.
cal literature. In one study, Connor, Davidson, and In addition, impulsivity was found to be positively
Lee (2003) found that resilience helps adults cope correlated with externalizing and internalizing prob-
with trauma more effectively. More specifically, lems. Interestingly, Eisenberg et al. (2004) also
higher levels of anger in relationship to a traumatic found that resiliency mediated the relationship be-
experience led to greater emotional distress and tween impulsivity and effortful control. Children in
lower overall health status (Connor et al., 2003). this study who had low effortful control were also
The specific relationship between resilience and found to have low levels of resilience. In light of the
anger was not examined; however, lower levels of proposed investigation, it can be inferred that lower
anger were associated with less emotional distress, levels of anger control would also be associated with
which could indicate higher levels of resilience. lower levels of resilience.
Such a relationship is supported by findings that
hardiness (a construct related to resilience) had an Anger Expression
inverse relationship with stress, depression, and an-
ger (Maddi, Brow, Khoshaba, & Vaitkus, 2006). In Specifically looking at anger expression,
addition, there is some information in the literature that helps
us to link this concept to resilience. First, it would

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 30
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

be helpful to discuss the role of anger in humans. insight into varying anger levels may be linked to
Anger has long been regarded as an emotion in all resilience in that insight into ones own behavior is a
humans that helps us to adapt and survive key component of resilience (Smokowski et al.,
(Spielberger, Reheiser, & Sydeman, 1995; Spielber- 1999).
ger, Ritterband, Sydeman, Reheiser, & Unger,
1995). Even though anger is adaptive, too much of In summary, multiple links exist between
it can have adverse effects such as depression, anxi- resilience and different forms of emotional expres-
ety, schizophrenia, and even health problems sion. Insight is a valuable factor in levels of resil-
(Spielberger et al., 1995). ience, as well as anger and violent behavior
(Lundeberg et al., 2004; Smokowski et al., 1999).
Early in psychologys history, anger was Many who lack an internal locus of control may use
seen as an instinctive response (Spielberger et al., aggressive behavior in an attempt to gain control.
1995). Currently, it is believed that anger is pro- Aggressive behaviors or even just having an aggres-
voked by frustration or another intense stimulus sive attitude can have an inverse effect on resilience
(Spielberger et al., 1995). According to the work of (Hall, 2006). Perception of various situations as
Spielberger et al. (1995), anger is a bipolar, unidi- more stressful has also been found to be strongly
mensional emotion. It ranges from extreme sup- related to increased anger and decreased resilience
pression to overtly aggressive behavior. How much (Blum, 1998; Spielberger et al., 1995). Finally, indi-
anger is experienced may be due to an individuals viduals who have lower levels of emotional regula-
perception of how stressful the situation is tion and resilience were found to engage in more
(Spielberger et al., 1995). Along with this finding, socially inappropriate behavior and exhibit higher
Blum (1998) also found that resilient individuals levels of negative emotionality (Eisenberg et al.,
tend to view situations as less stressful than those 1997). Taken all together the following research
who are less resilient. Therefore, it may be possible questions were put forth.
that those who are more resilient perceive less
stress in their daily lives and these lower levels of Goals of the Study
stress lead to lower levels of anger. The question of how resilience is related to
Aggressive expression of anger has also the expression of emotions has only been partially
been linked to locus of control (Hall, 2006), which, addressed in the research discussed above. In addi-
as mentioned previously, is an important facet of tion, little research to date has examined how anger
resilience (Smith & Carlson, 1997). Those with an relates to resilience. In contrast to resilience litera-
internal locus of control have been found to have ture, which focuses mainly on children and adoles-
higher levels of anger control while those with an cents, the literature surrounding anger and anger ex-
external locus of control use aggression to exert pression has largely focused on university students
some control over their environment (Hall, 2006). and adults. The current study sought to address the
Also found in the study by Hall, aggressive attitude missing link between resilience and anger in a sam-
and physical aggression were both inversely related ple of undergraduate students.
to resilience in a sample of undergraduates. The first hypothesis of this study was that
Focusing more specifically on undergradu- resilience would be inversely related to levels of
ate males in dating relationships, Lundeberg, Stith, overall anger. The second hypothesis of this study
Penn, and Ward (2004) found differences in levels was that resilient individuals would have signifi-
of insight between men who were physically abu- cantly higher levels of control over their anger ex-
sive to their partners and those who were not. The pression. Finally, anger expression outward has
major difference that was found was that men who been more attributed to aggressive behavior
are more aware of their escalating anger were more (Spielberger et al., 1995; Spielberger et al., 1995);
likely to be the non-abusive men in the study. This therefore, the final hypothesis of this study was that
resilience would be inversely related to anger ex-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 31
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

pression directed outward. ment includes items such as I am able to adapt


when changes occur and I believe I can achieve
Method my goals, even if there are obstacles. In this study,
reliability was very good, as evidenced by Cron-
Participants
bachs at .86
Data were collected from undergraduate In the sample used to standardize this assess-
and graduate students (n=143) in the School of ment, a total of 827 participants were used that in-
Education at a Mid-western urban university during cluded both clinical and non-clinical populations.
the fall and spring terms. Students completed a The mean age was 43.8 years; however, age was not
confidential, self-report survey regarding their abil- found to be significantly correlated with resilience
ity to cope with anger, express anger, and feelings scores (Connor & Davidson, 2003). Cronbachs
of resiliency. The participants consisted of 39 was used to determine internal consistency for the
males and 104 females. Participants included 106 CD-RISC and for the non-clinical population was
Caucasians, 20 African Americans, 7 Asian Ameri- 0.89. Test-retest reliability was measured with a
cans, 4 Latino/a, and 6 students endorsing them- portion of the clinical population and r was found to
selves as Other. The average age of participants be 0.87. Finally, convergent and divergent validity
was 25.3 with an age range of 18 to 57. were measured using both clinical and non-clinical
participants. For the combined group, the CD-RISC
Procedures
was shown to be negatively correlated with the Per-
Permission was gained from various univer- ceived Stress Scale (Pearson r = -0.76, P< .0001)
sity professors to recruit participants from their un- (Connor & Davidson, 2003). This scale measures
dergraduate and/or graduate classes. These stu- how stressful a test-taker views current events in
dents were approached in the classroom in order to their life.
elicit their participation. They were given the op-
Negative correlations were also found in
tion to participate or leave the classroom without
comparison to the Sheehan Stress Vulnerability
penalty or knowledge of the instructor. Participants
Scale (Connor & Davidson, 2003) (Spearman r = -
were read an information script that explained the
0.32, P< .001). This assessment gives an estimate of
potential risks and rewards for participating in the
how vulnerable a test-taker is to the adverse effects
study. Completion of the surveys indicated each
of stress. Using the clinical sample, Connor and
participants informed consent. Participants com-
Davidson also found that the CD-RISC was posi-
pleted a demographic questionnaire in order to help
tively correlated with the Kobasa hardiness measure
identify the sample population. Participants were
(Pearson r = .083, P< .0001) as well. This assess-
then administered the CD-RISC and the STAXI-2
ment examines levels of internal hardiness, which is
consecutively. No identifying information was on
a construct similar to that of resilience. Evidence
any of the forms filled out by participants in order
was also found supporting the idea that as clinical
to ensure anonymity of responses.
improvements occur, scores on the CD-RISC in-
Measures crease (F = 17.36; df 1,47; P< .0001) (Connor &
Davidson, 2003).
Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-
RISC; Connor & Davidson, 2003). This 25-item State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2
assessment measures level of resilience on a Likert (STAXI-2; Spielberger, 1999). This 57-item scale
scale. Responses to each question can range on a 5- includes measures of the types of anger one feels
point response system from not true at all (0) to and how that anger is expressed. The standardiza-
true nearly all of the time (4). Test-takers can tion of this assessment involved both normal adults
receive a score ranging from 0-100 with higher and hospitalized psychiatric patients. Responses are
scores indicating greater resilience. This assess- based on a 4-point Likert scale ranging

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 32
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

from not at all (1) to very much so (4) on the have shown how the Anger Control subscales relate
first section and almost never (1) to almost al- to the Anger Expression subscales. Specifically, the
ways (4) on the remaining sections. Each of the Anger Control correlated negatively with Anger Out
subscales have reported internal consistency, meas- (r = -.59 for males; r = -.58 for females), in addition,
ured by Cronbachs , ranging from 0.73-0.95 correlations of the Anger In with the Anger Out and
(Spielberger & Reheiser, 2003). For this particular Anger Control were found to be basically zero for
study, the researcher only used the 32-item Anger both genders (Spielberger et al., 1995). Jacobs,
Expression and Control subscale. This subscale has Latham, and Brown (1988) reported test-retest reli-
four separate components that are measured: Anger abilities ranging from .64 to .86 over a two-week
Out, Anger In, Anger-Control/Out, and Anger- period on the Anger Expression subscales.
Control/In.
Results
This subscale specifically examines how Analysis of the data collected was completed
individuals express and attempt to control feelings in SPSS version 14.0. In order to test the previously
of anger. It includes items such as I control my mentioned hypotheses, multiple regression analyses
urge to express my angry feelings and I endeavor were performed. Regression analysis was used to
to become calm again to assess anger control. To assess potentially predictive relationships that may
assess anger expression inward and outward, items exist between resilience and each separate dependent
such as I say nasty things and I pout or sulk are variable including anger expression in/out and anger
implemented. An overall anger expression index is control in/out. According to Tabachnick and Fidell
calculated as well as individual scores for the four (2007), the regression is significant at .05 or less.
components mentioned previously. Scores on this The assumptions of normality, linearity, multivariate
subscale can range from 0-96 with higher scores outliers, and multicollinearity were tested prior to
indicating a lesser ability to control anger expres- regression analysis and were not violated. One uni-
sion. In this study, reliability ranged from fair to variate outlier was found but was not significant
very good with Cronbachs on the Anger Expres- enough to be deleted from the dataset.
sion Index at .68, Anger Control Out was .80, An-
ger Expression Out was .70, Anger Control In Descriptive statistics and bi-variate correla-
was .83, and Anger Expression In was found to tions are presented in Table 1. Resilience was found
be .79. to correlate with anger control out (r = .38, p < .01)
and anger control in (r = .39, p < .01). The overall
Studies have been completed to validate the anger expression index (r = -.35, p < .01) was found
empirical independence of the Anger In and Anger to be inversely related to resilience. Additionally,
Out subscales (Spielberger, Reheiser, & Sydeman, resilience was found to be negatively correlated with
1995) and have shown little to no correlation be- anger expression out (r = -.26, p < .01). Thus, it can
tween the two. In addition, correlational studies

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 33
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

be seen that as level of resilience increases, ones healthy way with stressful circumstances and the
ability to control anger, both inward and outward, ability to adapt in positive ways despite experiencing
also increases. Additionally, overall anger expres- multiple risk factors (Smokowski et al., 1999;
sion having an inverse relationship with resilience Waller et al., 2003). Some of the characteristics
indicates that those who possess higher levels of found in resilient individuals include positive self-
resilience also are less likely to express anger in esteem, intelligence, internal locus of control, in-
general. More specifically, they are less likely to sight, and temperament (Smith & Carlson, 1997;
express anger outwardly, as indicated by the nega- Smokowski et al., 1999). The concept of resilience
tive correlation between resilience and anger ex- can also be partially contributed to external factors
pression out. such as family support, guidance, and the outside
The regression analysis showed that resil- influence of a teacher or religious figure (Blum,
ience was associated with anger control out scores, 1998; Smith & Carlson, 1997).
= .38, t(119) = 4.48, p < .001. Anger control out
also explained a significant portion of variance in In this particular study, the researcher chose
the resilience scores, R =.143, F(1,120) = 20.07, p to focus on the relationship of resilience to the ex-
< .001. Resilience was also associated with anger pression of anger. This included the expression of
control in, = .39, t(119) = 4.63, p < .001. Anger anger outwardly and inwardly as well as controlling
control in also explained a significant portion of anger, both internally and externally. According to
variance in the resilience scores, R = .151, F Spielberger et al. (1995), the outward expression of
(1,120) = 21.42, p < .001. Finally, resilience was anger could be manifested as overt aggression while
associated with anger expression index scores, = - the inward expression of anger could include sup-
.35, t(115) = -4.02, p < .001. The anger expression pression or withdrawal from others. More posi-
index also explained a significant portion of vari- tively, internal anger control could include con-
ance in resilience scores, R = .122, F(1,116) = sciously trying to calm down and relax and external
16.13, p < .001. From these analyses, it can be con- anger control might mean remaining patient and
cluded that resilience in an individual significantly controlling angry behaviors (Spielberger et al.,
is associated with and may predict ones increased 1995).
ability to control their anger, both inward and out- When further exploring the results of this
ward. It can also be predicted that a resilient indi- study, the concepts of resilience and anger expres-
vidual is less likely to express their anger overall. sion (both inward and outward) are not significantly
Discussion related. Moreover, resilience was significantly re-
lated to the ability to control anger, both internally
The results of this investigation add to the and externally. These findings may indicate that
extant literature in a number of ways. First, it ad- those who are more resilient may be less likely to
dresses a gap in the resilience literature regarding express their emotions in a maladaptive or unhealthy
the connection between resilience and emotional manner. For example, resilient individuals may be
expression and control, specifically anger expres- less likely to respond outwardly in anger with vio-
sion and control. Secondly, the findings support lence or aggression towards others. Internally, resil-
that resilience is positively related to the control of ient individuals are probably less likely to socially
anger and negatively related to anger expression. withdraw, hold grudges, and/or pout and sulk.
This can lead to the conclusion that individuals who
are more resilient are less apt to respond to situa- Those who are resilient may also be more
tions and challenges with anger towards others or able to control their emotions and express them in a
themselves. constructive way, and move past the negative feel-
ing. When referring to outward anger control, this
As addressed previously, resilience can be could include sharing feelings with others, remain-
defined in many ways. Generally speaking, resil- ing patient, and controlling overtly angry or aggres-
ience can be defined as the ability to cope in a

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 34
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

sive behaviors. Inward anger control refers to resil- ently more resilient due to characteristics such as
ient individuals possibly being more adept at con- determination, intelligence, and motivation that are
sciously endeavoring to calm down, relax, and ac- associated with increased resilience (Smith & Carl-
tively reducing angry feelings. son, 1997; Smokowski et al., 1999). This could
have had a significant impact on the current study.
In this study and according to Spielberger
and Reheiser (2003), the anger expression index Additional limitations to consider include a
measures ones overall expression of anger, both limited sample size and a number of participants not
inwardly and outwardly. This refers to how much completing the survey. A number of participants
one suppresses angry emotions or behaves with failed to complete the back page of the survey that
hostility when angry as opposed to healthier ways included a majority of the questions involving the
of coping with anger. The findings of this study STAXI-2. Therefore, the overall N for the measures
indicate a negative relationship between resilience of Anger Control In/Out and Anger Expression In/
and overall anger expression. This could indicate Out ranged from 121-126 while the overall N was
that those who are resilient express their anger less 140. This limits the conclusions that can be drawn
frequently and with more adaptive skills. Those regarding the predictive relationship between resil-
who are not as resilient may be more likely to feel ience and anger expression to some degree.
angry, both internally and externally, and may not
cope with these feelings as effectively. Finally, the social desirability of participants
responses should be considered when examining the
The first hypothesis for this study, that resil- results of the current research. The CD-RISC in-
ience would be inversely related to anger, was con- volves participants potentially disclosing low levels
firmed. Resilience was found to have a significant of self-esteem, motivation, and hope for the future.
and negative relationship with the overall level of Also, the STAXI-2 may require participants to admit
anger experienced by an individual. The second that they have anger outbursts, treat others poorly,
hypothesis postulated that resilience would have a or, in general, do not cope well with negative emo-
significant relationship to the control of anger, both tions. Due to these mitigating factors, participants in
inwardly and outwardly. This was confirmed as this study may have felt pressure to misrepresent
well. Finally, the researcher proposed that resil- themselves when completing the survey for this
ience would have an inverse relationship with anger study despite the anonymity of their responses.
expression outward. This hypothesis was not con-
firmed due to a lack of a significant relationship Future Directions
between the Anger Expression Out scale and resil- When investigating the important concept of
ience. resilience in the future, it may be useful to continue
Limitations looking at how this characteristic affects anger ex-
pression. More specifically, the current study could
When considering the results of this study, a be expanded to include more diverse populations in
few limitations should be kept in mind. First of all, age, cultural identity, and socioeconomic status. In
the results can only be generalized to other under- general, those who are in the undergraduate and/or
graduate and graduate student populations. While graduate student populations may be considered to
the average age of the participants was older than be more resilient in general due to the standards re-
the typical college student, the majority of partici- quired for acceptance and success in the college en-
pants were still between the ages of 18 and 22. Ad- vironment.
ditionally, a majority of the participants were fe-
male and of European American descent which From the results of this study, we can see
may also affect the generalizability of the results. that resilience has a significant impact on how indi-
Moreover, those who have been successful enough viduals experience and express anger. This is an
to enter college or graduate school may be inher- important finding, but it would also be beneficial to

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 35
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

researchers and practitioners alike to have a better gression and anger directed inward as manifested by
understanding of how resilience affects emotional anxiety and depression.
expression in general. Additionally, examining
specific emotions and their relationship to resilience While it is important that we deepen and fur-
(i.e. sadness, depression anxiety) could be useful in ther clarify our understanding of the concepts of re-
helping the mental health field have a better under- silience and anger expression and control, it is also
standing of how and why individuals cope in vari- vital that we use this knowledge as a means of edu-
ous ways. Another important step that could be cation and prevention. It can be seen from the re-
carried out based on the current study includes sults of this study that an increase in resilience can
learning about how resilience can be fostered or serve to significantly decrease unhealthy expressions
improved in individuals. If mental health practitio- of anger. This knowledge can be used both by men-
ners can gather more information about increasing tal health professionals and the general public to
resilience, this could contribute to increased coping work towards the prevention of many of the difficul-
skills and emotional health in clients seeking vari- ties that can afflict the less resilient population such
ous types of mental health services. as anxiety, depression, aggression, etc. Preventions
focused on facilitating resilience in the general pub-
Conclusion lic, in addition to clinical samples can also lead to
lower overall costs of treating mental disorders, as
Violence and aggression, as well as mental well as the emotional repercussions that aggression
health problems, such as anxiety and depression, and violence can have on the general public.
have become significant problems in our country.
More and more mental health professionals are More specifically, if individuals can learn to
called to work with individuals experiencing these reduce their inward expression of anger, this would
issues or to develop research projects and preventa- result in a reduction of such behaviors as pouting or
tive programs to curtail such problems. Resilience sulking, holding grudges, or ignoring ones angry
is an important part of prevention and has been feelings. Unhealthy anger behaviors can lead to
found to be a protective factor against the afore- lower self-esteem, depression, and anxiety as well as
mentioned hardships, as well as, a way for people health problems related to the stress of not express-
to overcome struggling with these particular issues ing emotions (Staxi et al., 1995). The reduction of
(Lundenberg et al., 2004; Maddi et al., 2006). This outward expression of anger can lead to fewer rela-
study helps to further explain the link between the tionship conflicts, acts of interpersonal aggression,
expression of anger in various ways to the concept and violence. The unhealthy expression, or lack
of resilience. thereof, of anger and other emotions plays a large
role in each of psychological and interpersonal is-
From the findings presented here, it was sues mentioned.
shown that resilience plays an important role in
how well one controls their anger, both internally As for fostering resilience to reduce overall
and with other people. Additionally, resilience also anger expression, this can be done in multiple ways.
is related to lower levels of overall anger expression It could be accomplished in a school, community, or
in individuals. An inverse relationship to outward mental health setting. Psycho-education on the de-
anger expression was not found. Further research velopment of active coping strategies and ways of
in this area is warranted. Resilience can play a thinking that are a part of resilience would be useful
large part in the reduction of anger, both directed for individuals who are struggling with such issues
toward the self and toward others, by tapping into as anger, depression, or anxiety before these prob-
more adaptive emotional responses. Therefore, the lems become difficult to manage. Likewise, such
concept of resilience could be fostered and devel- skill development could also be introduced to the
oped in order to prevent or reduce instances of an- general public as helpful ways to reduce stress, an-
ger directed outward in the form of violence or ag- ger, depression, etc. in everyday life situations.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 36
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

Skills that would be important to teach in order to a model for youth health promotion. Journal
increase resilience might include identifying and of Adolescent Health, 22, 368-375.
relying on social support systems, developing an
internal locus of control, learning how to manage Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2007, January 21). Key
ones environment in order to have more of an in- facts at a glance: Correctional populations.
ternal locus of control as opposed to external when Retrieved March 31, 2007, from the World
facing stressful situations, developing long term Wide Web: http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/
goals, and finding positive role models. glance/tables/corr2tab.htm.

The knowledge gained from this study has Christiansen, E.J. & Evans, W.P. (2005). Adoles
cent victimization: testing models of
the potential to lead to fewer reactive interventions
resiliency by gender. Journal of Early
and more proactive programs and therapies being Adolescence, 25(3), 298-316.
utilized. Such proactive programs can be helpful in
children, adolescents and adults. Prevention can Connell, J.P., Spencer, M.B., & Aber, J.L. (1994).
begin by identifying individuals with anger control Educational risk and resilience in
African-American youth: context, self,
difficulties early in therapy or within the school
action, and outcomes in school. Child
system. If such individuals can be identified, men- Development, 65, 493-506.
tal health professionals can begin to foster resil-
ience and more effective anger control techniques Connor, K.M. & Davidson, J. (2003). Development
of a new resilience scale: The
in order to decrease the likelihood of unhealthy an-
Connor-Davidson resilience scale (cd-risc).
ger expression, both inward and outward; thus lead- Depression and Anxiety, 18, 76-82.
ing to lower levels of violence and aggression in
our society, as well as depression and anxiety in Connor, K.M., Davidson, J.T., Lee, L.C. (2003).
individuals. Spirituality, resilience, and anger in
survivors of violent trauma. Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 16(5), 487-494.

Correspondence may be addressed to Rachel Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I.K., Fabes, R.A., Reiser,
Kirkpatrick, 615 E. 52nd St. Ste. 215, Kansas City, M., Murphy, B.C., Holgren, R., Maszk, P.,
MO, 64133. Electronic mail may be addressed to: & Losoya, S. (1997). The relations of
regulation and emotionality to resiliency
rbk346@umkc.edu, Phone: (405) 833-6108 Fax: and competent social functioning in
(816) 235-5270 elementary school children. Child
Development, 68(2), 295-311.
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T.L., Fabes, R.A., Reiser,
References M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S.A.,
Valiente, C., Losoya, S.H., Guthrie, I.K., &
Belgrave, F.Z., Chase-Vaughn, G., Gray, G., Addi Thompson, M. (2004). The relations of
son, J.D., & Cherry, V.R. (2000). The effect effortful control and impulsivity to
tiveness of a culture and gender specific in childrens resiliency and adjustment.
tervention for increasing resiliency among Child Development, 75(1), 25-46.
African American preadolescent females.
Journal of Black Psychology, 26(2), 133- Gall, M.D., Gall, J.P., & Borg, W.R. (2007).
147. Educational Research. Boston: Pearson
Education, Inc. Hall, C.W. (2006). Self-
Blum, R.W. (1997). Healthy youth development as reported aggression and the perception of

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 37
Resilience and anger Kirkpatrick & Freeman

anger in facial expression photos. The Measuring anxiety, anger, depression, and
Journal of Psychology, 140(3), 255-267. curiosity as emotional states and personality
traits with the STAI, STAXI, and STPI. In
Jacobs, G.A., Latham, L.E., & Brown, M.S. (1988). Hersen, M., Hilsenroth, M.J., & Segal, D.L.
Test-retest reliability of the State-Trait (Eds.), Comprehensive Handbook of
Personality Inventory and the Anger Psychological Assessment (Vol. 2, pp. 70-
Expression Scale. Anxiety Research, 1, 263- 86). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
265.
Spielberger, C.D., Reheiser, E.C., & Sydeman, S.J.
Lundenberg, K., Stith, S.M., Penn, C.E., & Ward, (1995). Measuring the experience,
D.B. (2004). A comparison of nonviolent, expression, and control of anger. In
psychologically violent, and physically Kassinove, H. (Ed.), Anger Disorders:
violent male college daters. Journal of Definitions, Diagnosis, and Treatment. (pp.
Interpersonal Violence, 19(10), 1191-1200. 49-76). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
Maddi, S.R., Brow, M., Khoshaba, D.M., & Spielberger, C.D., Ritterband, L.M., Sydeman, S.J.,
Vaitkus, M. (2006). Relationship of Reheiser, E.C., & Unger, K.K.(1995).
hardiness and religiousness to depression Assessment of emotional states and
and anger. Consulting Psychology Journal: personality traits: Measuring psychological
Practice and Research, 58(3), 148-161. vital signs. In Butcher, J.N. (Ed.) Clinical
National Institute of Mental Health. (2006, Personality Assessment: Practical
December 26). NIMH: The numbers count. Approaches. (pp. 42-58). New York: Oxford
Retrieved March 31, 2007 from the World University Press.
Wide Web: http://www.nimh.nih.gov/ Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Ad
publicat/numbers.cfm ministration. (2007, January 9). 2005 Tables:
Ripple, C.H. & Luthar, S.S. (1998). Academic risk Tobacco and alcohol use. Retrieved March
among inner-city adolescents: the role of 31, 2007, from the World Wide Web: http://
personal attributes. Journal of School oas.samhsa.gov/NSDUH/2k5NSDUH/tabs/
Psychology, 38(3), 277-298. Sect2peTabs1to57.htm#Tab2.1B

Rodgers, B.K. & Rose, H.A. (2001). Personal, Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (2007). Using
family, and school factors related to Multivariate Statistics. Boston: Pearson
adolescent academic performance: a Education, Inc. Waller, M.A., Okamoto,
comparison by family structure. Marriage & S.K., Miles, B.W., & Hurdle, D.E. (2003).
Family Review, 33(4), 47-61. Resiliency factors related to substance use/
resistance: perceptions of native adolescents
Smith, C. & Carlson, B.E. (1997). Stress, coping, of the southwest. Journal of Sociology and
and resilience in children and youth. Social Social Welfare, 30(4), 79-94.
Service Review, 231-256.
Waller, M.A., Okamoto, S.K., Miles, B.W., &
Smokowski, P.R., Reynolds, A.J., & Bezruczko, N. Hurdle, D.E. (2003). Resiliency factors
(1999). Resilience and protective factors in related to substance use/resistance:
adolescence: an autobiographical perceptions of native adolescents of the
perspective from disadvantaged youth. southwest. Journal of Sociology and Social
Journal of School Psychology, 37(4), 425- Welfare, 30(4), 79-94.
448.
Spielberger, C.D. & Reheiser, E.C. (2003).

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 38
Examination of Perceived Drinking Norms and Binge Drinking
Attitudes in College

Jessica M. Bendjebar, Robert G. Riedel, Debra L. Ainbinder


Lynn University

Alcohol misuse has become one of the most 1986).


immediate and potentially harmful factors associ-
ated with the overall health and well-being of col- According to a systematic literature review
lege students (Baer, 1994). Nationwide studies ex- by Sullivan and Wodarski (2004), intervention pro-
amining alcohol use in college and the related nega- grams at many colleges were once implemented
tive consequences propose that nearly 44% of col- based on a common-knowledge based idea that
lege students report engaging in binge drinking at binge drinking was a normal part of the develop-
least once in a two week period (Wechsler, Daven- mental process entering college and therefore drink-
port, Dowdall, Moeykens, & Castillo, 1994). Heavy ing and its associated problems could not be pre-
drinking on college campuses has become a wide- vented. According to Strano, Cuomo, & Venable,
spread characteristic of the culture at many univer- (2004), interventions targeting specific audiences
sities. First year students experience a significant sharing similar attitudes and expectancies towards
change in the transition from high school to college. drinking are more likely to yield better results than
It is during this transition that students form social the great majority of the campus-based alcohol inter-
networks and peer groups, many of which are fa- ventions programs being widely used. Peer drinking
cilitated when freshmen students are put into on- norms, especially for women, have been found to be
campus housing and dormitories. For some stu- strong predictors in their own alcohol use (Lewis &
dents, excessive alcohol consumption and the re- Neighbors, 2004). Support for the use of peers
lated negative consequences emerge after matricu- within the intervention process has shown efficacy
lation to college (Turrisi, Mallet, Mastroleo & within certain populations (Dunleavy & Campbell,
Larimer, 2006). 2006).

Perceptions of campus alcohol use norms Borsari and Carey (2000) proposed that per-
continually emerge in the literature as one of the ceived drinking norms and alcohol expectancies
greatest determinants of undergraduates drinking were the missing factors in the majority of campus
behaviors (Novak & Crawford, 2001). Both active, based intervention and alcohol awareness programs.
which includes actual offers of alcohol and passive, Numerous studies have approached this challenge in
which refers to perceived norms of peer influences a variety of ways. Support for parent-based interven-
have been identified as being associated with heavy tions is strong and has shown efficacy in many col-
episodic drinking (Wood, Read, Mitchell, & Brand, lege samples. Recent indications that parents can
2004). Among the first studies to propose and iden- also communicate the importance of expectancies
tify individual characteristics that affect drinking about alcohol and social experiences could provide
tendencies based upon social drinking norms con- another means of correcting attitudes and social
cluded that compared to those who drank moderate norms (Turrisi, Jaccard, Taki, Dunnam, & Grimes,
quantities of alcohol, students who consumed ex- 2001). By encouraging social skills training, increas-
cessive and large quantities of alcohol perceived ing student awareness of the dangerous conse-
their peers to have attitudes that were lenient with quences of binge drinking, and thoughtful planning
regards to excessive use (Berkowitz & Perkins, and decisions prior to drinking, overall drinking

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 39
Drinking norms and attitudes Bendjebar, Riedel, Ainbinder

rates have been reduced on many campuses tification number that was used to gain access to the
(Ichiyama & Kruse, 1998). online self-report. Participants were did not com-
plete the first wave of the survey were given the op-
The purpose of the current investigation was portunity to complete the second wave which in-
to evaluate the accuracy of perceptions of alcohol cluded the additional information from the first
use and drinking norms in the sample population wave.
and to examine the role of attitudes regarding binge
drinking as they relate to ones individual alcohol Perceived Drinking Norms. Included in the
use. It was hypothesized that students would sig- second wave of data collection, perceived drinking
nificantly overestimate the number of drinks con- norms were measured through a seven part question
sumed by the average freshmen student of their instructing participants to enter an estimation of how
same sex. It was also hypothesized that permissive many drinks a typical freshmen student of their same
attitudes regarding binge drinking and the related sex consumes on each day of the week. Responses
negative consequences would be predictive of were measured on a continuous scale. Participants
greater alcohol consumption. were asked to enter this estimation to the nearest
whole number (see Appendix A).
Methods
Binge Drinking Attitudes. Binge drinking
Participants attitudes were assessed using five statements regard-
Participants were obtained from the popula- ing beliefs and attitudes about alcohol use and binge
tion of undergraduate freshmen students at a small drinking-related consequences. Responses to these
private university in south Florida. Approximately statements were provided on a 4-point scale ranging
600 students were enrolled in the fall semester of from strongly agree to strongly disagree with no
2007. A total of 162 students completed an ade- neutral response available. A total score based on the
quate percentage of the survey within the second responses to the five statements was calculated to
wave of data collection to have results included in provide a score that indicated their general attitudes.
data analysis. Male participants made up 45.6% of This score was interpreted to indicate that higher
the sample; female participants represented 54.4%. scores were representative of more permissive atti-
Based on participants reported ages at the time of tudes about binge drinking and excessive alcohol
data collection, less than 3% of the sample was le- consumption (see Appendix B).
gally allowed to consume alcohol. Over 90% of the Individual Alcohol Use. The dependent vari-
freshmen participants reside in an on-campus resi- able was assessed using self-report questions of al-
dence hall or dormitory. Unique to this sample, cohol consumption. Similar to the format of the
89.4% of the sample reported the economic status question assessing perceived drinking norms, par-
of their family to be above average or moderately ticipants were asked to estimate the number of
higher than most families. drinks they consume each day of the week. Re-
Procedure sponses were recorded on a ratio or continuous
scale. Two other questions also asked participants to
All students enrolled as freshmen in the fall recall a two week period and report how many times
semester were informed of the opportunity to par- they had engaged in binge drinking behaviors and
ticipate prior to the beginning of the fall semester. also how many times participants had gotten drunk
Upon arrival at freshmen orientation, students were or very high from alcohol over the last 30 days (see
invited to complete the first wave of the survey Appendix C).
which included demographic information and ques-
tions about personal alcohol consumption. Confi-
dentiality was protected throughout using a secure
server and participants were issued a personal iden-

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 40
Drinking norms and attitudes Bendjebar, Riedel, Ainbinder

Results Binge Drinking Attitudes


Perceived Drinking Norms Linear regression analyses between drinking
norm attitude scores and estimated number of drinks
Results supported the hypothesis that par- consumed on each day of the week, excluding Sun-
ticipants overestimated the amount of alcohol con- day, demonstrated statistical significance. Regres-
sumed by their peers for all seven days of the week. sion analyses for Monday, (R 2 = .06), F (1, 150) =
Mean comparisons between average number of 10.22, p < .01. For Tuesday, (R 2 = .04), F (1, 150) =
drinks consumed by the participant and partici- 6.22, p < .05. For Wednesday, (R 2 = .09), F (1, 151)
pants perception of average student alcohol con- = 14.75, p < .001. For Thursday, (R 2 = .09), F (1,
sumption were all statistically significant. The 150) = 15.91, p < .001. For Friday, (R 2 = .13), F (1,
mean number of drinks consumed on a typical 151) = 22.00, p < .001. For Saturday, (R 2 = .06), F
Monday and estimation of average number of (1, 150) = 10.01, p < .01.
drinks consumed by peers on that day was statisti-
cally significant, t (160) = 3.07 p = < .01. On Tues- Discussion
day, t (161) = 3.98, p = < .001. The number of
drinks consumed on a typical Wednesday was also Results from the current investigation sup-
significant, t (160) = 4.24 p = < .001. For Thursday, ported the hypotheses and provided evidence to sup-
t (161) = 5.89, p = < .001, and Friday, t (161) = port the future application of norm-correcting inter-
11.60, p = < .001. For Saturday, t (161) = 11.56, p = ventions. Participants overestimated the number of
< .001. Finally, the t-test on Sunday indicated sig- drinks consumed by their peers each day by an aver-
nificance between average number of drinks con- age of 2.29 drinks more than the actual average
sumed by the individual participant and perception number of drinks participants reported they con-
of peers mean number of drinks consumed, t (160) sumed. Further examination of the participants av-
= 4.13, p = < .001 (see figure 1). erage number of drinks consumed on each night of
the week and the estimation of the number of drinks

Figure 1. Perceived versus actual number of drinks consumed

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 41
Drinking norms and attitudes Bendjebar, Riedel, Ainbinder

consumed by the average student revealed signifi- and university setting involved. The sample of fresh-
cant inconsistencies between perceived and actual men students were collected at a school with signifi-
alcohol consumption. Results from this study also cantly higher proportion of international students
suggest that a permissive attitude towards binge than that of many universities around the country.
drinking is predictive of increased alcohol use. This
suggests that when binge drinking and excessive Based on the results of this sample, further
alcohol consumption is perceived as normative and research will be aimed at correcting inaccurate
acceptable behavior, students are more likely to en- drinking norms and expectancies and attempt to ad-
gage in binge drinking despite the risk of negative dress attitudes about binge drinking. Implications
consequences. from the study include support for the use of norm-
correcting strategies using peer education of factual
Limitations to this study include the sample drinking norms. Interventions applying a social-
size as well as the sampling method. Convenience norms theory approach could be explored in an at-
sampling was used based on the online format and tempt to correct inaccurate perceptions.
participant recruitment procedures may not be rep-
resentative of the population of undergraduate
freshmen students. Another limitation involved in
this study relates to the uniqueness of the sample

Appendix A. Perceived Drinking Norms


Consider a typical week during the last month. How much alcohol, on average (measured in number of
drinks *), do you think a typical college student (of your same sex) at your university drinks on each day
of a typical week?

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 42
Drinking norms and attitudes Bendjebar, Riedel, Ainbinder

Appendix B. Binge Drinking Attitudes


Please indicate your attitude towards the following statements in general.
Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree Strongly Agree

I know that there are policies/ rules


about alcohol use on campus, but
most students disregard or choose
not follow them.
I do not consider heavy alcohol use,
five or more drinks in a row
(males) or four or more drinks in a
row (females), to be a problem
among my peers.
I often hear my peers making jokes
or laughing about stories of foolish
or dangerous activities as a result of
a friends alcohol use.
Excessive alcohol use is not as bad
as using other illegal drugs or legal
drugs without prescription.

Drinking large amounts of alcohol


over the course of a few hours is a
normal part of the college experi-
ence.

Appendix C. Individual Alcohol Use


On a typical Monday, I have ____ drinks.
On a typical Tuesday, I have ____ drinks.
On a typical Wednesday, I have ____ drinks.
On a typical Thursday, I have ____ drinks.
On a typical Friday, I have ____ drinks.
On a typical Saturday, I have ____ drinks.
On a typical Sunday, I have ____ drinks.
Think back over the last two weeks. How many times have you had 5 or more drinks within a 2 hour
period?
During the past 30 days (about 1 month), how many times have you gotten drunk, or very high from al-
cohol? (Please give your best estimate)

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 43
Drinking norms and attitudes Bendjebar, Riedel, Ainbinder

References impact on alcohol abuse: A nationwide


study. Journal of Drug Issues, 26(4), 961-
Baer, J. S. (1994). Effects of college residence on 975.
perceived norms for alcohol consumption:
An examination of the first year in college. Strano, D. A., Cuomo, M. J., & Venable, R. H.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8(1), 43 (2007). Predictors of undergraduate student
- 50. binge drinking. Journal of College
Counseling, 7(1), 50-64.
Baer, J. S., Stacy, A., & Larimer, M. (1991). Biases
in the perception of drinking norms among Sullivan, M., & Wodarski, J. (2004). Rating college
college students. Journal of Studies on students substance abuse: A systematic
Alcohol, 52(2), 580-586. literature review. Brief Treatment and Crisis
Intervention, 4(1), 71-91.
Berkowitz, A. D., & Perkins, H. W. (1986).
Problem drinking among college students: Turrisi, R., Jaccard, J., Taki, R., Dunnam, H., &
A review of recent research. Journal of Grimes, J. (2001). Examination of the short-
American College Health, 35(1), 1-28. term efficacy of a parent intervention to
reduce college student drinking tendencies.
Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2000). Effects of a Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(4),
brief motivational intervention with college 366-372.
student drinkers. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 728-733. Turrisi, R., Mallett, K. A., Mastroleo, N. R., &
Larimer, M. E. (2006). Heavy drinking in
Dunleavy, V., & Campbell, K. (2006). The role of college students: Who is at risk and what is
interpersonal influence as a college alcohol being done about it? The Journal of General
prevention strategy. College Student Psychology, 133(4), 401-421.
Journal, 40(3), 654-664.
Wechsler, H., Davenport, A., Dowdall, G.,
Ichiyama, M. A., & Kruse, M. I. (1998). The social Moeykens, B., & Castillo, S. (1994). Health
contexts of binge drinking among private and behavioral consequences of binge
university freshmen. Journal of Alcohol and drinking in college: A national survey of
Drug Education, 44(1), 18-34. students at 140 campuses. Journal of the
Lewis, M. A., & Neighbors, C. (2004). American Medical Association, 272(2),
Gender-specific misperceptions of college 1672-1677.
student drinking norms. Psychology of Wechsler, H., & Nelson, T. F. (2001). Binge
Addictive Behaviors, 18(4), 334-339. drinking and the American college student:
Miley, W. M., & Frank, M. (2006). Binge and Whats five drinks? Psychology of Addictive
non-binge college students perceptions of Behaviors, 15(4), 287-291.
other students drinking habits. College Wood, M. D., Read, J. P., Mitchell, R. E., & Brand,
Student Journal, 40(2), 259-263.
N. H. (2004). Do parents still matter? Parent
Novak, K. B., & Crawford, L. A. (2001). Perceived and peer influences on alcohol involvement
drinking norms, attention to social among recent high school graduates.
comparison information, and alcohol use Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18(1),
among college students. Journal of Alcohol 19-30.
and Drug Education, 46(3), 18-27.
Perkins, H. W., & Wechsler, H. (1996). Variation
in perceived college drinking norms and its

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 44
2008 Awards and acknowledgements

In 2008 at the APA National Conference, Student Awards:


the Prevention Section of the Society for Counsel-
Counseling Psychology Graduate Student Prevention
ing Psychology (Division 17) presented awards to Research Award
members and new professionals in the field based
on their proven interest and quality work. The fol- For prevention research in counseling psy-
chology conducted by a student as part of his/ her
lowing are the Award Recipients for 2008:
graduate training. The recipient of the award re-
George Albee Award ceived $200 for research on prevention.

The George Albee Award was given this The award this year went to Stephanie Chapman,
year to the University of Vermont in memory of Dr. University of Houston
George Albee for his extensive contributions to the Counseling Psychology Dissertation of the Year-
field of prevention over his lifetime. He was a ma- Award in Prevention.
jor provider of theoretical, research, and interven-
tions in the prevention field. The Prevention Sec- This award is awarded for a dissertation pro-
tion takes great pride in recognizing his enormous ject demonstrating outstanding contributions to field
impact and influence, and we are thankful for the of prevention. The recipient of this award received a
excellent leadership and creativity he showed in plaque acknowledging the award.
being a developer of the field. The award was pre-
The award this year went to Erica L. Medlock of the
sented to a representative from the University of
University of Oregon
Vermont at the Business Meeting.
The Prevention Section was allotted two poster
Counseling Psychologist Life Time Achievement
presentations at the National Conference. The
Award in Prevention
chosen papers were:
For counseling psychologists who have en-
Evaluation of a Teen Sexual Assault Awareness and
gaged in more than ten years of prevention activi-
Prevention Program
ties, including theory building, research, practice,
Authored by: Sulzner, J.M., Donovan, A.,
training, and/or leadership.
& Israel, T. University of California, Santa Barbara
The award this year went to Dr. Michael Waldo,
Comprehensive School Counseling Program and
New Mexico State University.
Perceived School Climate
Authored by: Tu, C.C., & Wahl, K.H.
University of Minnesota, Twin Cities

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 45
2008 Awards and acknowledgements

Spotlight On:

Michael Waldo, Award Recipient: Counsel-


ing Psychologist Life Time Achievement Award in
Prevention

Michael Waldo (miwaldo@nmsu.edu)


earned degrees from the University of California
and University of Utah, and held faculty positions
at the University of Maryland, Montana State Uni-
versity and, currently, New Mexico State Univer-
sity. His practice and research interests focus on
design and evaluation of preventive interventions
that employ group work to improve interpersonal
relations. He is a Fellow in the American Psycho-
logical Association and Chaired the Division 17
Prevention Section. He currently serves as chair of
the Editorial Board for the Sections publication,
Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory,
Research, Practice and Training.

In the nomination materials for Michael


Waldos award, Arthur Horne clearly articulates
Waldos many virtues and the accomplishments
which made him the ideal choice for the Counseling
Psychologist Life Time Achievement Award in Pre-
vention. Waldo was responsible for elevating the
prevention special interest group into an acknowl-
edged Section of Division 17 and promoting the
work of the section in APA through dedicated pres-
entations held by the section.

Horne states of Waldo, In short, he has in-


fluenced all aspects of the prevention work field
within counseling psychology. It is imperative to
recognize that the progress prevention work has
made in the last two decades has been strongly in-
fluenced by his scholarship, research, teaching, su-
pervision, and clinical work. In short, he has had a
strong personal influence in one of the major move-
ments of our time.

Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice, & Training Volume 2 Issue 1 2008 46

You might also like