Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Review of General Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association

2006, Vol. 10, No. 4, 346 364 1089-2680/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.4.346

The Religious Mind and the Evolution of Religion


Matt J. Rossano
Southeastern Louisiana University

This article summarizes the literature on the religious mind and connects it to archeological
and anthropological data on the evolution of religion. These connections suggest a three
stage model in the evolution of religion: One, the earliest form of religion (pre-Upper
Paleolithic [UP]) would have been restricted to ecstatic rituals used to facilitate social
bonding; two, the transition to UP religion was marked by the emergence of shamanistic
healing rituals; and, three, the cave art, elaborate burials, and other artifacts associated with
the UP represent the first evidence of ancestor worship and the emergence of theological
narratives of the supernatural. The emergence of UP religion was associated with the move
from egalitarian to transegalitarian hunter-gatherers.

Keywords: evolution of religion, Upper Paleolithic, religious mind, shamanism

William James (1902/1961) argued that the the other) provide reasonable boundaries for
mystical experience was the foundation for re- framing theoretical models.
ligion. Nearly a century after James, research The purpose of this article is to summarize the
into the neurological and cognitive basis of re- current research on the religious mind and connect
ligion has advanced rapidly (Barrett, 2000; it to archeological and anthropological data on the
Boyer & Ramble, 2001; dAquili & Newberg, evolution of religion. The first section reviews the
1999; Lewis-Williams, 2002; Newberg, dAquili literature on and builds a general framework of the
& Rause, 2001; Norenzayan & Atran, 2002; religious mind. The second section uses this
Ramachandran & Blakslee, 1998, chap. 9). Par- framework, in conjunction with archeological and
alleling this work has been that addressing the anthropological data, to construct a model of the
evolutionary origins of religion (Atran, 2002; evolution of religion. A three-stage model is pro-
Boyer, 2001; Hayden, 2003; Sosis & Alcorta, posed with supportive evidence and hypotheses
2003; Wilson, 2002). While archeological evi- for future testing outlined.
dence and anthropological analogies can be
used to formulate evolutionary models of reli-
The Evolved Religious Mind: Mental
gion, these models are limited by the fact that
behavior and belief do not fossilize, and current
Attributes Giving Rise to Religion
hunter-gatherers cannot be uncritically accepted Defining religion is notoriously controversial.
as relics of our hominid past. Using the reli- For present purposes, religion is defined as be-
gious mind as a basis for formulating evolution- liefs or actions predicated on the existence of
ary models provides another distinct and con- supernatural entities or forces with powers of
verging evidentiary line complementing those agency that can intervene in or otherwise affect
of archeology and anthropology. Though the human affairs. This definition is a slightly mod-
mental lives of our hominid ancestors cannot be ified version of one used by sociologist Steve
known with certainty, the cognitive capacities Bruce (2002, p. 2), which follows comfortably
of chimpanzees (at one extreme) and humans (at in the well-established conceptual tradition of
religious anthropologists such as Edward B. Tylor
(1871) and James George Frazer (1890/1941).
Recently, considerable research has focused
The author thanks Dr. David Sloan Wilson and two anon- on the mental attributes that incline humans
ymous reviewers for thoughtful critiques of the manuscript. toward religious beliefs (Alcorta & Sosis, 2006;
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Matt J. Rossano, Department of Psychology, P.O.
Arglye, 2000; Atran, 2002; Boyer, 2001; Hay-
Box 10831, Southeastern Louisiana University, Hammond, den, 2003; Hinde, 1999; Lewis-Williams,
LA 70402. E-mail: rossano@selu.edu 2002). Though investigators vary in their per-
346
RELIGIOUS MIND 347

spectives, they converge on four mental at- goals. Tomasello and Call (1997) have, in fact,
tributes that appear to provide the cognitive argued that this is the critical cognitive distinc-
foundations for the emergence of religion: one, tion separating humans from other primates.
agency detection and causal attribution; two, the For our hominid ancestors, the ability to at-
social and emotional commitments of group tribute agency to another would have been highly
living; three, narrative formation and the emer- adaptive. From the time of Homo erectus/ergaster
gence of existential anxieties; and, four, the (about 1.8 million years ago), hominids were of a
ecstatic or mystical experience. Each of these physical size and a level of technical and social
attributes is an evolutionary extension, built sophistication such that their primary competitors
upon precursors found in many other species, were most likely other hominids (Alexander,
especially our nonhuman primate relatives. 1989; Flinn, Geary, & Ward, 2005; Lewin, 1998).
This competition would have encouraged the for-
Agency Detection and Causal Attribution mation of larger groups with increasing numbers
of non-kin, as well as the formation of coalitions
Determining cause-effect relations is an and alliances both within and between groups
adaptive mental function found in many crea- (Aiello & Dunbar, 1993; McBrearty & Brooks,
tures (e.g., Hunt, 1996; OConnell & Dunbar, 2000). In this vastly complicated social world, the
2005; Premack, 1976; Premack & Premack, ability to read the goals and intentions of others
1994). Agency detection (or theory of mind) is would have been crucial. A distinct advantage
a form of causal attribution whereby an organ- would have gone to those of our ancestors who
ism assigns anothers actions to an internal state could quickly and accurately recognize the signals
such as a belief, desire, or intention. A rudimen- associated with others who intended to harm or
tary form of this mental capacity was likely help. Thus, we would expect predator/protector
present in our earliest hominid ancestors and agency detection mechanisms to be trip-wired
may be present in great apes today (Byrne & such that only partial or scant input is required to
Whiten, 1991; Gallup, 1970, 1982; Hare, Call, engage the mechanism. It makes considerable
& Tomasello, 2001; however, see also Pon- adaptive sense to overassign agency given that the
vinelli & Eddy, 1996; Ponvinelli & Prince, false alarm cost would be far outweighed by the
1998 for a more cautious assessment). benefit of the occasional hit (Guthrie, 1993). For
Developmental studies have shown that the example, rustling leaves usually signal nothing
ability to attribute agency to others develops more than the wind, but a single failure to recog-
incrementally over the first five or six years of nize them as signaling the stealthy approach of a
life (see Baron-Cohen, 2005; Gopnik, 1999, for malicious enemy or hungry predator could be life
reviews). By around 12 months, infants show threatening. Survival is better served by overas-
dishabituation to the actions of agents who vi- signing agency than underassigning.
olate goal-directedness and, throughout most of Since it is by evolutionary design prone to
infancy, youngsters appear to expect that human overassignment, the human agency-detecting
actions will be goal-directed (Baldwin, Baird, capacity was ripe for cultural manipulation and
Saylor, & Clark, 2001; Meltzoff, 1995). At 14 supernatural extension. Natural processes with
months, infants work to establish and maintain no obvious explanationstorms, illness, animal
joint attention and begin to develop an under- behavior, and so forthwere all prime candi-
standing of pretence. Two-year-old children dates for the actions of a supernatural agent.
show evidence of using mental state words and The notion of powerful natural, animal, and
by age three they appear to understand the re- human/animal spirits is nearly universal in tra-
lationship between that act of seeing and the ditional, shamanistic religions (Hayden, 2003,
state of knowing. By age four or five, children pp. 57 60; McClenon, 1997; Winkelman,
have a sensitivity to anothers mind in that they 1990). Evidence from Upper Paleolithic (UP)
understand that another can have ideas that are cave paintings suggests that these ideas may
incongruent with reality (Wimmer & Perner, extend back tens of thousands of years (Clottes
1983). These findings suggest that from a very & Louis-Williams, 1998; Dickson, 1990; Leroi-
early age humans treat other humans as inten- Gourhan, 1968). Animal or ancestral spirits
tional agents, whose actions are directed by who monitor peoples actions for moral integ-
mental constructs such as beliefs, desires, and rity and punish those who offend (often through
348 ROSSANO

acts of nature) are equally common motifs (At- Religious rituals can also serve as costly-to-
ran, 2002; Boyer, 2001; Hayden, 2003). fake signs of social commitment (Irons, 1996,
2001; Sosis, 2000; Sosis & Bressler, 2003; So-
Social/Emotional Commitments sis & Ruffle, 2003). For example, the initiation
rituals of traditional societies frequently involve
A second important aspect of the evolved rigorous physical and emotional trials (Atran,
religious mind is the capacity for social com- 2002, p. 153; Boyer, 2001, pp. 243246; Gluck-
mitmenta capacity that in humans has reach lick, 2001; Young, 1965). These trials publicly
the unprecedented level of subjective commit- signal an individuals commitment to the group,
ment to social norms (Dugatkin, 2001). A num- its values, and to the spiritual forces governing
ber of researchers have argued that certain emo- it. Failure to live up to the groups standards
tions may be unique to humans and most likely brings both profound shame and potential retri-
evolved to enhance social cohesion (Barrett, bution (both divine and human; see Hayden,
1995; Parker, 1998; Trivers, 1985). Self- 2003, p. 104).
conscious emotions such as embarrassment, Religions signaling function remains relevant
pride, envy, guilt, and shame require cognitive even in more contemporary settings. A person
evaluation of our behavior against an abstract who regularly attends church and insists on a
(group-based) standard (Parker, 1998). These religious wedding ceremony may be viewed as
emotions inform us about the quality of our committed to the principles of monogamy and
interpersonal relationships and often drive us to fidelity and therefore more desirable as a mate.
behave in ways that further the establishment, Irons (2001, p. 304) notes that Honduran men
maintenance, and/or reparation of social bonds. whose work requires prolonged separation from
Humans also appear to posses an irrational ten- their wives, frequently express a preference for a
dency toward moralistic aggression, which also religious wife whom they believe will be less
serves to strengthen social cooperation (Bingham, likely to cheat. Individuals who unambiguously
1999). Erst Fehr (Fehr & Gachter, 2002; Fehr & demonstrate a belief in ever-vigilant supernatural
Tyran, 1996) has shown that humans are quite agents who monitor the morality of their actions
willing to engage in moralistic punishment of per- are often perceived as more reliable by other
ceived freeloaders even when doing goes against group members and therefore worthy of enhanced
ones self interest. Thus it appears that empathiz- trust and cooperation. Studies comparing religious
ing with others, feeling guilty when letting others communities to analogous secular ones confirm
down or prideful when having fulfilled or ex- that, in general, the religiously based ones tend to
ceeded others expectations, and raging with righ- be more socially cohesive and enduring (Sosis,
teous indignation against perceived cheaters are 2000; Sosis & Bressler, 2003; Sosis & Ruffle,
all essential elements of the human social regula- 2003). This supports the notion that religious sig-
tory psyche. These emotions help to build and naling builds a level of intragroup trust and soli-
maintain strong social bonds while curbing the darity exceeding that typically found in secular
self-interested behavior that can erode community settings.
spirit.
In the past (as is true today), religion very likely Episodic Memory and Narrative
harnessed powerful social emotions to reinforce Construction
social unity and ostracize deviants. The religious
rituals and ceremonies of traditional societies of- Endel Tulving (1983) has argued for a dis-
ten involve rhythmic dancing, disturbing imagery, tinction between episodic and semantic mem-
and the ingestion of psychotropic substances pro- ory. Semantic memory refers to ones stored
ducing ecstatic emotional states that enhance so- knowledge of facts, concepts, and general prin-
cial bonding (Atran, 2002, pp. 163164; Hayden, ciples of how the world operates. Episodic
1987, p. 30, 2003, pp. 30 32; Rappaport, 1999, memory is an autobiographic store of life expe-
pp. 222, 228). Rituals of this nature have been riences. It is context and time sensitive and
shown to promote the release of brain opiates, allows one to mentally travel back in time to
which facilitate the formation of strong social and past personal events and to project into the
emotional bonds among participants (Frecska & futurea form consciousness known as auto-
Kulcsar, 1989). noetic consciousness. Evidence for episodic
RELIGIOUS MIND 349

memory in animals is questionable and falls category: human females. She eats, sleeps, mar-
short of human capabilities, suggesting that it ries, gets pregnant, gives birth, worries, visits rel-
may be unique to humans (Roberts, 2002; Tulv- atives, works around the house, and so forth. She
ing & Lepage, 2000). does, however, have one striking categorical vio-
Autonoetic consciousness, in conjunction lation: she can become pregnant without sex.
with language, allows humans the capacity to Oddities, on the other hand, are just exceptionally
produce two types of narratives integral to the strange category members, such as a man with
religious mind: personal and theological. The seven toes or a woman who has 14 offspring.
personal narrative is a subjective, autobiograph- Counterintuitives, as opposed to mere oddi-
ical story that unifies experience and provides a ties, are good candidates for religious concepts
stable, coherent sense of self that penetrates by virtue of the fact that they are memorable yet
across time. It also, however, brings with it a comprehensible, thereby giving them an advan-
somber companion existential anxiety or the tage in cultural transmission and preservation. It
knowledge of inevitable self-suffering and appears that the religious myths with the great-
death. To some, religion represents an adapta- est likelihood of preservation are those that re-
tion to mollify this anxiety (e.g., Bloom, 1992; late largely mundane and intuitive ideas with
Feuerbach, 1843/1972; Geertz, 1966: Mali- just a few counterintuitives mixed in to arouse
nowski, 1922/1961). Indeed, Atran (2002, pp. attention and create anchors for recall.
177181) has shown that religious sentiments
are heightened when subjects are primed using Ecstatic States and Mystical Experiences
stories that evoke existential anxiety. However,
as Boyer (2001, pp. 19 22) has pointed out, An ecstatic state is an altered state of conscious-
religious beliefs are often as anxiety provoking ness typically brought on by some form of sensory
(eternal damnation, curses, demonic possession, deprivation, overstimulation, physical or emo-
etc.) as they are comforting, and a blissful af- tional stress, and/or ingestion of psychotropic sub-
terlife is not always (and has not always been) a stances. It lies at the end of a spectrum of inten-
part of religious doctrine (e.g., the Greek con- sified conscious states that appear to be universal
cept of Hades or Jewish notion of Sheol). While to humans (Lewis-Williams, 2002, pp. 121130).
religion and existential stress are intertwined, Similar altered states of consciousness (although
their relationship is complex and may have as of lesser intensity, most likely) may be present in
much to do with the ambiguities surrounding nonhuman animals as a means of coping with
death and dead bodies as with the straightfor- deprivation and stress (Hoskovec & Svorad, 1969;
ward need to assuage mortal fears (see Boyer, Marcuse, 1951; see also review in McClenon,
2001 pp. 222228). 2002, pp. 2328). Animals from rats to chimpan-
Theological narratives are the culturally derived zees have been shown to be susceptible to hyp-
concepts, myths, and stories that communities cre- notic procedures that appear to induce altered
ate and transmit in order to understand the rela- states (Hoskovec & Svorad, 1969; Siegel & Jar-
tionship between the supernatural and earthly vik, 1975; Volgyesi, 1969). However, it appears
worlds. Recently, a number of scholars have elu- that no animal compares to humans when it comes
cidated the parameters guiding how the mind con- to voluntarily inducing ecstatic states though med-
structs and communicates religious myths and itative practices or religious rituals. Achiev-
concepts (Atran, 2002, pp. 100 107; Barrett, ing this state is a key feature of shamanistic
2000; Boyer, 2001, pp. 79 81; Boyer & Ramble, religions (see Hayden, 2003, chap. 3).
2001; Kelly & Keil, 1985; Norenzayan & Atran, James (1902/1961) identified four qualities of
2002). One important parameter is that religious the ecstatic or mystical experience. One, inef-
concepts tend to be minimally counterintuitive fabilitythe fact that the mystical experience
rather than just odd. Counterintuitives are defined cannot be adequately described in words. Two,
as exemplars that retain nearly all of their onto- noetic quality or enlightenmentthe fact that
logical assumptions with the exception of one (or while mystical experiences are profoundly emo-
a few) critical violation(s). This violation makes tional, they also provide deep insights to the
the exemplar distinctively interesting, but pre- individual involved. The individual believes
serves its comprehensibility Thus, the Virgin that authoritative truths have been revealed
Mary is in most respects a typical member of her through the mystical experience. Often these
350 ROSSANO

insights involve an awakening to the unity and hanced opportunities for cooperative arrange-
of all things and a deep sense of oneness with ments and access to important group resources.
the divine. Three, transiencerefers to the Our narrative capacity awakens in us both the
fleeting and temporary nature of the experience, disturbing awareness of inevitable suffering and
rarely do they last longer than a few moments. death and the ability to formulate and transmit
Four, passivitythough mystical experiences supernatural concepts about what might lie be-
can be facilitated through meditative practices, yond them. Finally, ecstatic experiences offer
rituals, and other techniques, their actual occur- not only compelling validation of the supernat-
rence is spontaneous and unpredictable. The ural realm that we envision, but also a social
person accepts the experience and cannot will it bonding mechanism among those who share its
to occur. Since Jamess work other qualities affects.
such as realness and unusual percepts have been
proposed as additional qualities (Farthing, The Evolution of Religion
1992, pp. 442 443). Mystical states may occur
in purely secular settings as well as in sacred We will likely never know when the first
rituals and are open to a variety of culturally and religious idea was born, but a substantial num-
personally influenced interpretations (see ber of researchers have concluded that the art,
Hinde, 1999, pp. 185198, for a review). artifacts, and burial practices of the Upper Pa-
Religious rituals that bring about ecstatic leolithic reflect religious sentiments (Breuil,
states, such as those involving rhythmic danc- 1952/1979; Dickson, 1990; Dowson & Porr,
ing, chanting, stressful initiations, and the in- 2001; Hayden, 2003; Leroi-Gourhan, 1982;
gestion of intoxicants are commonly used to Lewis-Williams, 2002; Lommel, 1967; Mithen,
establish social bonds among hunter-gatherers 1996; Winkelman, 2002; see Hamayon &
(Hayden, 1987). The release of brain opioids Francfort, 2001, for a contrary view). Given that
during ecstatic states may be integral to this the Upper Paleolithic peoples were anatomi-
process (Hayden, 2003, p. 31; Frecska & cally modern humans and that religion is a
Kulcsar, 1989). All of this opens up the possi- human universal, it seems safe to conclude that
bility that ritually induced ecstatic states pro- religion was present by at least the time of the
vide not only enlightenment to the individual Upper Paleolithic in Europe. A key question
involve, but also help to form strong social becomes whether religion existed prior to the
alliances within and among groups. Upper Paleolithic and, if so, what form did it
take? The next section reviews evidence for the
A Model of the Evolved Religious Mind existence of religion in pre-Upper Paleolithic
times. It is argued that religion (or proto-
What kind of mind does it take to be reli- religion) existed, but was restricted to ecstatic
gious? The attributes just discussed provide a rituals used for social bonding.
potential outline of just such a mind. The human
mind is, first and foremost, a highly social mind Pre-Upper Paleolithic (UP) Religion
with an elaborated set of emotions that promote
interpersonal bonding. For good evolutionary Nonhuman primates. There is no compel-
reasons, the human mind promiscuously assigns ling evidence that nonhuman primates have re-
agency to natural forces and objects giving rise ligion. However, some foundational elements of
to supernatural agency. Supernatural agency religion such as ritual behavior and social emo-
combined with our powerful social emotions tions are present in our primate cousins. For
incline us to envision ever-vigilant spiritual example, rituals for establishing trust and build-
monitors. As societies grow larger and more ing community are common among nonhuman
complex, moralizing gods become increasingly primates. Male baboons engage in a stereotyped
prevalent and appear to help in maintaining scrotum grasp which (apparently) serves as a
social stability (Roes & Raymond, 2003). Ex- trust-building signal (Watanabe & Smuts,
hibiting costly-to-fake signs of belief in these 1999). Chimpanzees, bonobos, and spider mon-
ever-vigilant gods and commitment to the keys live in fission-fusion societies where the
group-based moral standards they represent can larger community often breaks into a series of
provide individual benefits in the form of en- smaller foraging parties. When these parties en-
RELIGIOUS MIND 351

counter each other after a period of separation, Homo erectus skulls unearthed from the
members engage in ritualized greeting behav- Zhoukoudien site near Bejing suggest that can-
iors such as mutual embraces, kissing, group nibalism among hominids may date back as far
pant-hooting, and grooming (Goodall, 1986; as half a million years. Many of these skulls
van Roosmalen & Klein, 1988, p. 515). Among were apparently broken open from the bottom in
bonobos, sexual stimulation is often used to a manner similar to practices employed by more
reduce tensions and allow food sharing even recent cannibals, suggesting that extraction and
among members of different communities (de consumption of the brain might have been the
Waal & Lanting, 1997). goal (Hayden, 2003, pp. 96 97; Weidenreich,
These ritualized welcoming, trust-building, 1943). Even more intriguing is an erectus skull
and tension-reducing behaviors may induce a found in Bodo, Ethiopia dated to around
mental state conducive to social bonding that 600,000 years ago. This skull contains 25 stone-
shares features in common with the ecstatic tool cut marks, presumably incurred when the
state. Grooming, for example, is known to cause skin was removed (White, 1986). Facial skin
the release of endogenous opiates, which act as removal would seem to have little practical pur-
a primary reinforcer contributing to social pose and therefore hints at possible symbolic or
bonding (Keverne, Martinez, & Tuite, 1989). ritual significance. A skull fragment with evi-
The origins of similar human greeting and re- dence of defleshing was also uncovered at the
integration rituals are likely found in these non- roughly 100,000-year-old Klasies River site in
human primate rituals. South Africa (Klein & Edgar, 2002, p. 16;
The foundations of the social emotions inte- Singer & Wymer, 1982). Similar finds strongly
gral to human religion are probably also present indicative of cannibalism and possible ritual
in nonhuman primates. De Waal (de Waal, (i.e., multiple skulls with broken bases and/or
1996; de Waal & Berger, 2000) has documented cut marks) have been documented at Neander-
evidence for empathy, pride, and a primitive thal sites in Kapina, Croatia, and Abri Moula in
sense of fairness in monkeys and apes, while southeastern France (DeFluer, Dutour, Valla-
Goodall (1986) has reported that chimpanzees das, & Vandermeersch, 1993; DeFluer, White,
occasionally engage in extended and emotive Valensi, Slimak, & Cregut-Bonnoure, 1999). In
rain dance or waterfall displays. All of this many of these instances, cranial fracturing from
suggests that the foundations of religion and natural processes or cannibalism resulting from
religious behavior were laid deep in the primate nutritional stress cannot be ruled out (Arsuaga
brain possibly prior to the onset of hominid et al., 1997; Binford & Ho, 1985). Even so, the
evolution. possibility remains that some of this cannibal-
Three other lines of evidence suggest the ism represents a spiritual sense that is hundreds
presence of pre-Upper Paleolithic religion as of thousands of years old.
well: (a) cannibalism, (b) early symbols includ- Red ochre and other early symbols. Red
ing the use of red ochre and, (c) deep cave ochre is a soft iron oxide that easily pigments
exploration. Each shall be discussed in turn. other surfaces. Among hunter-gatherers, red
Cannibalism. Among many hunter-gatherer ochre has little practical value, but is used ex-
societies, eating the flesh, especially the brain, tensively in rituals and carries significant sym-
of another was (is) tantamount to ingesting his bolic meaning (Power & Watts, 1996). Some
or her spirit (Hogg, 1966; Sanday, 1986). This have argued that reds symbolic importance
practice applied not just to enemies, but also rises to the level of a universal human archetype
(and more importantly) to relatives and fellow for such things as blood, sex, life, and death
tribe members. A family or fellow tribe member (James, 1957; Marshak, 1981; Wreschner,
might be consumed so that his or her strength 1980). The first indications of red ochre use date
and skills would forever remain within the tribe. back to nearly one million years ago where its
For example, among the Wari of South Amer- presence has been found at early Oldowan sites
ica, it is considered far more respectful and and South African caves (Bednarik, 1994;
compassionate to consume a loved one so that Leakey, 1971; Lorblancher, 1999). Red ochre is
he or she is literally reincorporated into the tribe not uncommon at hominid sites dated between
than to permit the remains to decay into the 200,000 100,000 years ago and was apparently
ground (Conklin, 2001). being purposely mined and transported to them
352 ROSSANO

(Knight, Power, & Watts, 1995; McBrearty & case for bear-cult rituals at the Neanderthal site
Brooks, 2000). of Regourdou cave in southwestern France. Re-
Evidence that red pigmentation was added to mains of a bear skull, bones, and other possibly
artifacts to enhance their appearance and possi- intentionally arranged and manipulated materi-
bly their meaningfulness can be found in the als dated to between 60,000 70,000 years old
form of a 100,000-year-old mammoth-tooth have been found here. Other deep cave sites
plaque covered with red ochre unearthed in associated with Neanderthals, such as Galerie
Tata, Hungary (Bednarik, 1995). Additionally, Schoepflin at Arcy-sur-Cure in France (Baffier
a red ochre cobble, carefully engraved with & Girard, 1998; Farizy, 1990), Congnac, and
crosses and dated to 77,000 years old, was Grotta della Barura (Lorblancher, 1999) may
found in the Blombos cave of South Africaa also have been locations of ritual activities.
find that some have referred to as the oldest
piece of art on record (Henshilwood & Sealy,
1997; Lewis-Williams, 2002, pp. 98 99). The Defining Pre-Upper Paleolithic (UP)
presence of red ochre in the archeological Religion
record coupled with the effort being expended
for its acquisition suggest that it was a valued None of the evidence reviewed is conclusive
commodity despite its nonutilitarian nature. regarding the presence of religion in pre-Upper
Apart from the presence of ochre, there are Paleolithic times. However, taken in total, it
but a few, scattered examples of what might be strongly suggests some manner of behavior
symbolic artifacts dating from 100,000 years transcending utilitarian concernsa proto-
ago or more. A three-million-year-old jasper religion of sorts. The challenge becomes one of
cobble in the form of a human face was uncov- setting the possible parameters for such a proto-
ered from a site near Makapansgat, South Af- religion. The current model contends that these
rica. The nearest source for this item was 10 km parameters can be found by identifying the
from the recovery site, leaving open the possi- likely social and cognitive context of late Lower
bility that it was intentionally transported pre- and Middle Paleolithic peoples. Socially, while
sumably by a hominid who was intrigued by its there is evidence showing that population den-
anthropomorphic quality (Dart, 1974). A simi- sities may have been increasing through the
lar anthropomorphized artifact was found at Be- Middle Paleolithic, it is only after the transition
rekhat Ram in the Golan Heights. This was a to the Upper Paleolithic that social stratification
233,000-year-old human-figure-shaped stone and inequality becomes evident (Dunbar, 1996,
that showed evidence of intentional modifica- p. 114; Hayden, 2003, p. 122131; McBrearty
tionpossibly the earliest known example of & Brooks, 2000; OShay & Zvelebil, 1984;
sculpture (dErrico & Nowell, 2000; Marshak, Vanhaerena & dErrico, 2005). Therefore, we
1997). Finally, many items recovered from late should expect that pre-UP religion falls within
Lower and Middle Paleolithic sites (from the social context of egalitarian hunter-
300,000 to 35,000 years ago) contain what ap- gatherers of increasing group size.
pear to be intentional zigzagging, parallel, or Cognitively, pre-UP religion most likely falls
radiating markings that may represent the ent- somewhere between the ape mind and the Ne-
optic experiences of altered states of conscious- anderthal mind. Evidence reviewed earlier indi-
ness (Bednarik, 1995; Horowitz, 1964; Kluver, cates that apes have a variety of rituals used for
1942). social bonding and these rituals may bring about
Deep cave rituals. A few Neanderthal deep a mental state partially overlapping with the
cave sites provide possible evidence for ritual ecstatic state of humans. As for Neanderthals,
behavior. About a quarter of a kilometer deep in no firm consensus exists concerning their cog-
the Bruniquel Cave of southwestern France, Ne- nitive capacities. The Neanderthal brain was
anderthals apparently arranged broken stalag- slightly larger than that of modern humans and
mites into two circles, one of which surrounded some remains suggest highly intelligent behav-
the remains of a fire (Rouzaud, Soulier, & Lig- ior and possibly symbolic thinking (dErrico,
nereaux, 1996). These remains have been dated Zilhao, Julien, Baffier, & Pelegrin, 1998; Gray-
to around 50,000 years old. Additionally, Hay- son & Delpech, 2003; Trinkhaus & Shipman,
den (2003, pp. 108 115) has made a spirited 1992).
RELIGIOUS MIND 353

On the other hand, the wealth of differences The cognitive context of pre-UP religion,
in tools, grave goods, tailored clothing, built therefore, appears to be one where rituals of
shelters, hunting technologies, and artistic re- social bonding were present, but without an
mains suggest important cognitive differences overarching supernatural framework. As group
between Neanderthals and Homo sapiens sizes increased over the course of the Middle
(Chase & Dibble, 1987; Gargett, 1989, 1999; Paleolithic, Neanderthals and other hominids
Hoffecker, 2002; Lieberman & Shea, 1994; faced the increasingly difficult challenge of
Mellars, 1996, pp. 366 391). Lewis-Williams maintaining stability in larger groups with
(2002, pp. 89 96) has recently reviewed the greater numbers of distantly related and nonre-
archeological evidence associated with the lated individuals. This leads to the following
Chatelperronian Neanderthals who, for thou- proposal for stage one of the evolution of reli-
sands of years, shared space in southwestern gion: the earliest form of religion would have
France and northern Spain with Homo sapiens. involved putting the ecstatic mental state in
The Chatelperronians showed evidence of tool service to group cohesion using rituals of social
technologies, personal adornments, and some bonding. This proposal is similar to that of
burial practices similar to their Cro-Magnon Hayden (1987, 2003, pp. 3134) who has ar-
neighbors (which Lewis-Williams attributes to gued that the original function of religion and
Neanderthal borrowing). However, unlike their religious ritual was to create reciprocal alliances
neighbors, the Chatelperronians showed no ev- between different groups as a buffer against
idence of image-making, burials with elaborate resources shortages. Hundreds of thousands of
grave goods, or sophisticated hunting strategies. years ago, those hominids who managed to cre-
For Lewis-Williams, this marks a key cognitive ate more socially cohesive groups and more
difference in memory capacity and conscious- reliable intergroup alliances may very well have
ness between the two populations. had a distinct selective advantage over their
According to Lewis-Williams (2002, pp. 89 more isolated, individualistic counterparts. Ec-
96), Neanderthals, unlike their human counter- static rituals may have been a critical mecha-
parts, could not intentionally recall, cognitively nism for constructing those groups.
restructure, and socially manipulate the images These ecstatic rituals need not have involved
or experiences they had in altered states of any supernatural elements. Instead, they might
consciousness. In other words, the images seen find their modern analogues in purely mimetic
in dreams, hallucinations, and other forms of community activities such as coordinated group
contemplation could not be recalled later and dance (square dancing, e.g.), marching, drill
constructed into an alternative view of reality. team, some team sports, participatory spectator
This deeply abstract manner of concept forma- events (rock concerts, football games, etc.), and
tion was the mode of consciousness that sup- possibly some fraternity/sorority rituals
ported image-making, elaborate burial rituals, (Donald, 1991). McNeill (1995, p. 1) contends
and even more sophisticated hunting practices that such rhythmic, group-coordinated actions
(numerous studies have shown that the anthro- bring about a euphoric state and a muscular
pomorphizing practices of hunter-gatherers are bonding among participants that serve to en-
as effective a means of predicting animal be- hance group cohesion and cooperation. Physi-
havior as scientific models, see Gubser, 1965; cally and emotionally engaging group-coordi-
Marks, 1976; Mithen, 1996, p. 168; Silberbauer, nated activities were the essence of pre-UP re-
1981). Thus, Neanderthals did not envision (and ligion. From the perspective of the religious
therefore create images of) a supernatural mind, pre-UP religion involved two of its four
world. aspects: the ecstatic state and social emotions.
The point of this discussion, however, is not Testing stage one. A number of testable
to pronounce final judgment on the Neanderthal hypotheses emerge from stage one, some of
capacity for symbolic thinking or religious which already have supportive evidence. First,
imagination. Instead, it is this: if these cognitive relative to subsequent stages, this stage should
attributes are at least questionable in Neander- be associated with the evolutionarily oldest
thals then it seems most prudent to conclude brain areas. Evidence to date is consistent with
that, by and large, pre-UP religion did not in- this prediction, as areas of the limbic system
volve a conception of the supernatural. have been implicated in both emotional func-
354 ROSSANO

tioning and ecstatic experiences (Mandell, tices, and symbolism seem to appear sud-
1980; Ramachandran & Blakeslee, 1998, pp. denly in the archeological record in Europe
177179; Winkelman, 1997, 2002). Winkelman beginning about 40,000 years ago, evidence
(1997, 2002) for example, attributes the altered from Africa indicates a more gradual transition
state of consciousness associated with ritual to dating back to about 250,000 years ago (Klein
high-voltage, slow-frequency activity of the & Edgar, 2002; McBrearty & Brooks, 2000;
hippocampal-septal region acting as a driver Mithen, 1996).
synchronizing activity across the frontal lobe. The emergence of anatomically modern hu-
The fact that many nonhuman animals also ap- mans brought with it the modern human brain
pear to experience altered states of conscious- and its full range of conscious experience. The
ness adds further support to notion that evolu- intensified altered states of the Homo sapiens
tionarily older brain structures are involved brain combined with its (most likely) unique
(Goodall, 1986; Hoskovec & Svorad, 1969; capacities for remembering, manipulating, and
Keverne, Martinez, & Tuite, 1989; Marcuse, interpreting the experiences of those states
1951; Volgyesi, 1969). A second related hy- brought forth something new on the evolution-
pothesis is that the brain areas activated by ary landscape: the supernatural. The current
group-based religious ritual (such as rhythmic model contends that these new supernatural vi-
chanting and dancing) would be similar to those sions marked the emergence of shamanism.
associated with secular group-based activities While attaching a precise definition to shaman-
such as square dancing and marching band, ism can spark controversy (see, e.g., Vitebsky,
especially where it involves older, subcortical 2000), in the current context the term is meant
structures. to indicate the attainment of altered states of
Finally, this model predicts that the psycho- consciousness for the purpose of interacting
logical effects of group-based religious rituals with the spirit world for the benefit ones com-
and secular group-coordinated activities are the munity (Hultkranz, 1973; Townsend, 1999).
same. Evidence confirming this can be found in Furthermore, the ability to achieve and manip-
the fact that both community religious rituals ulate altered states may very well have been
and secular group-coordinated activities can under selection pressure. Religious visions may
bring about deindividuation or the loss of a have been adaptive.
sense of personal identity as one becomes en- McClenon (1997, 2002) has marshaled con-
meshed in the crowd (dAquili & Newberg, siderable evidence in support of the notion that
1999, pp. 95103; Watson, 1973). Community- shamanistic healing rituals were fitness enhanc-
based rituals, whether secular or religious, en- ing in our ancestral past. Those of our ancestors
courage the release of brain opiates which pro- who were most susceptible to the beneficial
mote social bonding (Frecska & Kulcsar, 1989; physical and psychological effects of shaman-
Prince, 1982). Moreover, Levenson (2003) has isitic rituals had a selective advantage over oth-
found that activities that synchronize autonomic ers in surviving illness, overcoming debilitating
functions among individuals (whether secular emotional states, and enduring the rigors of
or religious in nature) correlate positively with childbirth. (Note: This would have been an in-
measures of empathy, thus providing a pathway dividual fitness advantage, which in this context
for enhanced social bonding. means an advantage over others in the same
group. This, over time, could have produced a
The Transition to Upper Paleolithic group fitness advantage in the sense that one
Religion group with a larger proportion of individuals
susceptible to shamanistic healing may have
Genetic and fossil evidence indicate that had higher overall fitness compared to another
modern humans arose in Africa about 150,000 with a lower proportion; see Wilson, 2002, pp.
years ago (Cann, Stoneking, & Wilson, 1987; 1225 for discussion).
Ingman, Kaessmann, Paabo, & Gyllensten, McClenons ritual healing theory is based
2000; Lewin, 1998, pp. 385 411; Ruvolo, on a number of converging lines of evidence.
1996). As to when human-like culture and be- This evidence includes:
havior emerged, the situation is murkier. While
evidence for sophisticated tools, hunting prac- 1. The universality (or near universality) of
RELIGIOUS MIND 355

ritual healing practices across traditional with them to Europe, which itself began to
societies; transform under the dynamic social and ecolog-
ical conditions of the Upper Paleolithic.
2. The fact that ritual healing always in- Religious healing rituals would appear to ne-
volves hypnotic processes and altered cessitate notions of supernatural agency. The
states of consciousness; supernatural world not only exists, but it exerts
willful causal force. This spiritual force is ac-
3. The finding that ritual healing is often cessed and directed by a prescribed, ritualized
highly effective for a range of maladies mode of encounter. Apart from the need for
where psychological factors are involved ritual, however, it seems unlikely that any
such as chronic pain, burns, bleeding, deeper theological understanding of the super-
headaches, skin disorders, gastrointestinal natural would be required (at least not initially).
disorders, and the discomforts and com- Only minimal linguistic skills would be needed
plications of childbirth; (if at all) to add to the persuasive impact of the
ritual (relax, heal, etc.). Furthermore, both
4. The evidence from comparative and ar-
anecdotal accounts and experimental evidence
cheological studies indicating the exis-
indicate that specific ideologies are unnecessary
tence of ritual, altered states of conscious-
to the physical and psychological affects of
ness and care of the sick among our pri-
healing rituals (McClenon, 2002, pp. 10, 79
mate cousins and hominid ancestors;
83). Among the !Kung hunter-gatherers, ritual
5. The fact that the earliest medical texts healing is caused by a powerful, but mysterious
(from Mesopotamian and Egyptian civili- spiritual energy call n/um (Katz, 1982, p. 34).
zations) closely connect healing with reli- N/um itself bears some resemblance to the very
gious ritual; ancient and widespread notion of a pervasive,
but equally mysterious spiritual force called
6. The finding that anomalous events associ- mana (Smart, 1976). This transition then, nec-
ated with ritual, such as miraculous essarily entails the emergence of the supernat-
healing, are effective in inducing super- ural agency aspect of the religious mind, but not
natural beliefs. necessarily narrative capacity.
Testing stage two. Two lines of testing arise
The connection between shamanism and the from stage two of the proposed model. First,
emergence of modern Homo sapiens also finds this model predicts that shamanism has more
support in Upper Paleolithic cave art. Following direct individual fitness benefits compared to
in the tradition of Lommel (1967), a number of later-emerging religious elements such as myths
researchers have argued that Upper Paleolithic and ancestor worship. A number of observa-
cave art reflects the experiences and rituals of tions offer support for this hypothesis. For in-
early shamanism (Dowson & Porr, 1999; stance, though shamanistic practices vary
Eliade, 1972; Halifax, 1982; Hayden, 2003; widely across the globe, they always include
Lewis-Williams, 1986, 2002; Winkelman, some type of healing (both individual and com-
2002). Though speculative, this suggests that munity based). This is consistent with the idea
shamanism predates the art since the depictions that in its primordial form it directly impacted
represent an already present system. fitness (McClenon, 2002; Winkelman, 1992).
Collectively, these observations lead to the Furthermore, though today the shaman is
following proposal regarding stage two in the seen as a rare, gifted individual, there is reason
evolution of religion: shamanistic healing ritu- to suspect that they were far more prevalent in
als constituted the bridging step from pre- the past. Among the !Kung, for example, it is
Upper Paleolithic religion to Upper Paleolithic estimated that half the men and a third of the
religion. In the millennia between the emer- women are (or were) shamans (Lewis-Williams,
gence of modern humans and the Upper Paleo- 1982). Among the Crow and Cheyenne of North
lithic in Europe, ecstatic rituals for social bond- America, spiritual visions were considered es-
ing evolved into (but were not entirely sup- sential to success in hunting and warfare, so
planted by) shamanistic healing rituals. This much so that those gifted with spiritual insight
was the religion that modern humans brought sometimes sold portions of their prowess to
356 ROSSANO

their less talented brethren (Lowie, 1963, p. plexity, and cultural and religious evolution
175). Finally, its prevalence and value suggest (e.g., Dickson, 1990, pp. 84 92, 199 20; Hay-
that altered states bestowed real physical and den, 2003; Klein & Edgar, 2002, pp. 166 167,
psychological benefits to their practitioners, and 230 232, 237240). Archeological evidence in-
recent studies of meditation appear to confirm dicates that not only were Cro-Magnon Homo
this (Davidson et al., 2003; Orme-Johnson & sapiens (in all likelihood) cognitively more so-
Herron, 1997). The fitness benefits of subse- phisticated than their Neanderthal counterparts,
quent elements of religion (if present at all) but their communities were larger and socially
were likely more indirect, relating to increases more sophisticated as well (see review in Dick-
in group stability and cooperation. son, 1990, pp. 84 92, 180 189). In Upper Pa-
A second line of testing pertains to the brain leolithic Europe, one of the important factors
mechanisms involved in agency detection and driving increasing populations and social com-
supernatural agency attribution. This model pre- plexity was resource abundance. Bone remains
dicts that agency detection is an evolutionarily indicate that large game animals such as deer,
newer function (relative to ecstatic states and horses, and mammoth were prevalent and tooth-
social emotions) restricted to larger-brained an- wear studies confirm that Upper Paleolithic
imals such as primates. Furthermore, it predicts peoples were consuming large amounts of
that newer brain structures, very likely in the hunted meat (Butzer, 1971; p. 463, Dahlberg &
cerebral cortex, are involved in these functions. Carbonell, 1961). Dickson (1990, p. 182) ar-
A number of lines of research provide evidence gues that in terms of resource availability and
for these predictions. acquisition, Upper Paleolithic peoples were
There is controversy over whether apes as- more analogous to the equestrian bison hunters
cribe internal mental states as causal factors for of the American Plains or the medieval rein-
others behavior (Byrne & Whiten, 1990; Gal- deer-hunting Saami (Lapps) of Scandinavia
lup, 1970, 1982; Hare, Call, & Tomasello, than pedestrian hunter-gathers such as the
2001; Ponvinelli & Eddy, 1996; Ponvinelli & !Kung of southern Africa.
Prince, 1998). Human infants, however, seem Thus, Upper Paleolithic societies had moved
predisposed to interpreting behavior in terms of from being generalized, egalitarian hunter-
intentional agents (Baldwin, Baird, Saylor, & gatherers to complex, transegalitarian ones
Clark, 2001; Meltzoff, 1995). Furthermore, su- (Hayden, 2003, pp. 122131; OShay & Zvele-
pernatural agency appears to require an ability bil, 1984; Vanhaerena & dErrico, 2005). Tran-
to engage in second-order theory of mind rea- segalitarian or complex hunter-gatherers typi-
soning (I know that you know that I know), cally use more sophisticated technologies for
something that emerges only in later childhood harvesting and storing seasonally abundant re-
(seven year olds as opposed to five year olds; sources (e.g., nets or traps to catch large quan-
see Bering, 2005, p. 433). Finally, Baron-Cohen tities of fish during spawning season). This
(1995, p. 91, 1997, pp. 9195) has reviewed leads to a more sedentary lifestyle with greater
studies using both brain damaged patients and private ownership of resources and increasing
neuroimaging of healthy subjects that implicate social inequalities. As will be shown shortly, the
the orbital frontal cortex in theory of mind. All move to a more transegalitarian society in Up-
of this is consistent with the idea that agency per Paleolithic times brought with it more ex-
detection and its extension to supernatural clusive religious rituals and an increasing em-
agency are likely to have emerged after ecstatic phasis on the ancestors. Exclusive rituals and
states and social emotions and may be restricted ancestor worship required the construction of a
to humans. coherent narrativea religious mythos about
how the supernatural world related to the
Upper Paleolithic Religion earthly one. Social complexity, therefore,
brought the fourth aspect of the religious mind
The last stage in the evolution of religion fully into view: narrative formation and existen-
emerges from the interaction of two elements: tial concerns.
human minds and complex societies. A number Ancestor worship and narrative formation.
of researchers have discussed the close connec- As with shamanism, ancestor worship is a uni-
tion between population density, social com- versal or near universal trait of traditional reli-
RELIGIOUS MIND 357

gions (Harvey, 2000; Lee & Daly, 1999; Par- supernatural constraint and ritual control on
rinder, 1976, p. 24). The ancestors played a their privilegesthus making their positions
prominent role in Greco-Roman paganism and more palatable to lower-status community
continue to be of central importance to Eastern members. Mayan priests and kings, for exam-
religions such as Confusionism and Shinto ple, were required to engaged in ritualized acts
(Ching, 1993; Shelton, 1998; Tsundona, 1986). of brutal self-mutilation, puncturing their pe-
As will be discussed shortly, ancestor worship nises or tongues and tugging objects through the
also entails the construction of a coherent, col- open wounds, as signals of their divine worthi-
lective narrative that is essential to social cohe- ness (Schele & Freidel, 1990). The divine an-
sion and relevant to existential concerns. The cestors fractured the community and held it
ancestors represent a continuity of existence together at the same time. To accomplish this
that extends beyond earthly death and intercon- would seem to require a compelling, unifying
nects the supernatural and human worlds. myth.
The private ownership of resources present in The veneration of elite ancestors entails the
transegalitarian societies includes the resource- construction of a conceptual framework for the
rich territories (e.g., streams abundant with fish, supernatural, or put another way, a coherent
certain migration routes, etc.) responsible for theological narrative connecting the ancestors,
the entire communitys well-being and prosper- the supernatural, and the earthly realm. Its ele-
ity. Claims to these critical territories typically ments would go something like this: (a) the
belong to certain families within the tribe by supernatural world exists (a holdover from early
virtue of their ancestral lineage. These families shamanism), (b) the ancestors reside in the su-
form the elite strata of an increasingly differen- pernatural world after death, (c) the ancestors
tiated society. For example, among the Tlingit can affect conditions in their former earthly
of North Americas northwest coast, the anyeti tribe, (d) the ancestors maintain a special rela-
or nobles of society are typically from clans tionship with their kin descendents in the tribe,
who claim ownership to the fishing weirs (traps) (e) the ancestors must be placated with certain
in the most prized territories (Oberg, 1973). rituals and sacrifices by their kin descendents so
Social elites typically justify their privileged as to maintain their good favor, which is bene-
status through their filial link to powerful an- ficial for both their direct kin and the tribe as a
cestors in the supernatural realm, whose benev- whole. From this skeletal framework, more
olent proprietorship over resource-rich territo- elaborate stories and myths explaining why all
ries generously provides for the entire tribe. this is true, how it originated in the history of
Special rituals and sacrifices open only to elites the tribe, and other important cultural lessons
are necessary to intercede with and properly and facts can be constructed to create a com-
placate these powerful ancestors so that the pelling story passed on from generation to gen-
entire tribes prosperity and security can be eration (the mythic stage in cultural evolution;
ensured (Woodward, 2000). see Donald, 1991). The full flowering of the
This divine justification may have served an narrative element in religion provides the cul-
essential dual purpose in establishing and main- tural myth that sustains the tribe against social
taining the elites privilege status within the disintegration and buffers individuals against
community. Boehm (1999) points out how the inevitable travails of life.
hunter-gatherer egalitarianism is aggressively The first evidence of ancestor worship. Ev-
enforced by coalitions that vigorously resist at- idence from the Upper Paleolithic is consistent
tempts at individual domination. Furthermore, with the notion of an elite social class increas-
Turner (1969) argues that community rituals ingly concerned with ancestry and legacy. For
often serve as a restraining force against high example, elaborate burial sites such as Sungir
status individuals exploiting others within the (White, 1993), Dolni Vestonice (Klima, 1988),
group. By appealing to the supernatural, re- and Saint-Germain-la-Riviere (Vanheren &
source-holding elites cloaked their status in di- dErrico, 2005) attest to the presence of an elite
vine legitimacy. This divine legitimacy may class whose members were laid to rest with
have functioned as an effective countermeasure great ceremony and lavish grave offerings.
against aggressive egalitarianism while at the Among more recent complex hunter-gatherers,
same time forcing elites to incur a degree of funeral rites of this character typically occur
358 ROSSANO

under the expectation that the deceased is soon Often these deep caves sites contain the foot-
to take his or her place as a powerful ancestor in prints or handprints of adolescents and children
the supernatural realm (Hayden, 2003, p. 239; (see Clottes, 1992). Owens and Hayden (1997)
Sandarupa, 1996). provide ethnographic evidence showing that
While the ancestors provided wealth, descen- among complex hunter-gatherers ecstatic rituals
dants were necessary to insure it. Thus, as might involving young people are often for the pur-
be expected, the Upper Paleolithic also provides pose of initiation into elite secret societies.
evidence of an increasing concern with progeny These individual or exclusive rituals represent
and fertility. Nearly 200 Venus figurines an important change in religion in the Upper
small artifacts carved in ivory, bone, or stone Paleolithic.
depicting naked, headless, female forms with Popular versus elite religion. Deep cave
exaggerated breasts, buttocks, and abdomen sites in combination with the evidence for elab-
have been unearthed and dated to Upper Paleo- orate burials and fertility concerns suggest that
lithic times. Other sculptured, carved, and the Upper Paleolithic marks the emergence of
graphic depictions of naked females and female something new in religion: exclusive rituals that
body parts as well as phallic symbols have also existed alongside community-wide ones. This
been found (Hayden, 2003, p. 154; Hutton, leads to the following proposal regarding the
1991; Soffer, 1985). Given the amount of effort third stage of the evolution of religion: Upper
and resources required to create these images, Paleolithic religion marks the beginning of a
the likelihood that they simply represent Stone divergence of religion into a community-wide
Age pornography seems remote. Instead, as nu- popular form and an exclusive ancestor-based
merous researchers have argued, they more elite form. This proposal is consistent with that
likely reflect concerns over fertility and child- of Dickson (1990, p. 199), who argues that the
bearing (Eliade, 1958; Wymer, 1982; see, how- Upper Paleolithic ushered in a move from sha-
ever, Harding, 1976 and Rice, 1982 for alterna- manistic cults to communal cults of greater so-
tive interpretations). Furthermore, the Upper cial complexity. Hayden (2003, pp. 209 211),
Paleolithic marks the first time in history where as well, has argued for a popular/elite distinc-
a substantial number of females were buried tion emerging in Upper Paleolithic religion.
with elaborate grave goods, indicating an ele- This division laid the groundwork for future
vation in their social status (Harrold, 1980). ones characterizing classic paganism such as the
This would be expected if fertility and progeny high gods or state gods of the ruling classes
were becoming increasingly valued. versus the minor gods of the field and forest,
Exclusive rituals. One of the unique and exclusive temple-based rituals versus public
puzzling aspects of Upper Paleolithic parietal feasts and festivals, state sponsored cults versus
art is the fact that much of it is located in familial or domestic ones, and so forth. The
remote, isolated cave chambers. While many highly mythologized and socially stratified reli-
spectacular examples of cave art are in rela- gion that would characterize the first great civ-
tively open, accessible areas; it is, nevertheless, ilizations was taking shape.
intriguing how frequently cave artists intention- Testing stage three. This model predicts
ally selected tiny, tight, difficult-to-reach re- that narrative formation and existential con-
cesses for their work. At various cave sites, cerns are uniquely human attributes. Evidence
some painted chambers can only be accessed to date appears to confirm this. Brain areas
after: a mile-long trek through frigid waters involved in linguistic abilities, episodic mem-
(Montespan Cave), a harrowing climb up a ory, and other related functions appear to be
sheer rock face (Nerja cave in Spain), a 16 foot part of the executive brain of the frontal lobe
rope decent (the shaft at Lascaux), or a 200 m where many of our highest abstract reasoning
climb after traversing a 450 m passage (Salon functions take place. For example, according to
Noir at Niaux cave in France). While the larger, the HERA model of memory (Nyberg, Cabeza,
more accessible cave sites were likely used for & Tulving, 1996; Tulving, Kapur, Craik,
community-wide rituals, these remote sites Moscovitch, & Houle, 1994), the left prefrontal
were probably used for exclusive rituals and/or cortex is critical for the encoding of information
individual vision quests (Hayden, 2003, pp. into episodic memory, while the right prefrontal
142153; Lewis-Williams, 2002, pp. 228 267). cortex is critical for retrieval from episodic
RELIGIOUS MIND 359

memory. In discussing the role of the frontal In terms of the religious mind, ecstatic states
lobes in episodic memory Tulving and Lepage and social emotions (present beginning with
(2000) state the following: pre-UP proto-religion) are hypothesized to form
The point is that the establishment, maintenance, and
the foundational basis for the emergence of
switching of these frontal sets require massive and religion. Sometime later, supernatural agency
presumably highly complex neuronal activity of a kind (present in transitional religion) and then, fi-
possible only for highly developed brains, such as that nally, narrative capacity (present in UP religion
of humans and possibly other higher primates. This is and onward) were added to this foundation.
where the story of episodic memory as a very recent
evolutionary adaptation comes in. (Tulving and Lep- This model falls generally in line with other
age, 2000, p. 222) proposals of human cognitive evolution such as
that of Donald (1991) and Nelson (2004) where
The construction of a coherent personal nar- mimesis precedes mythic or narrative capacity
rative that integrates experience and accounts both ontogenetically and phylogenetically. It
for our sense of self appears to be related to left also accommodates well with that of Mithen
hemisphere linguistic functions (Gazzinga, (1996), where social intelligence comprises the
1995; Ramachandran & Blakeslee, 1998, pp. bulk of stage one, with interactions between
134 137). Structures of the left hemisphere of social, technical intelligence and natural history
the cerebral cortex involved in memory and intelligence occurring in stage two and linguis-
language weave experiences together into a sen- tic intelligence being added in the last stage.
sible story about the self, its actions and moti- Continued progress in unraveling the evolu-
vations. Gazzinga, Irvy, and Mangun (1998, p. tionary origins of religion will very likely de-
543) refer to these functions as the interpreter pend on cross-fertilization among varied disci-
those aspects of the brain that collectively make plines. The current model offers an approach
sense of our actions and experiences. Using framed on the religious mind. Interaction with
language as a vehicle for constructing a per- models from other disciplines and perspectives
sonal narrative and a coherent framework for are necessary to further illuminate the human
integrating experience appears to be a uniquely penchant for the supernatural.
human attribute. Language trained apes are far
more utilitarian in their use of language. Kanzi,
the most language-skilled ape to date, uses 96% References
of his linguistic utterances to make requests
Aiello, L. C., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Neocortex
(Greenfield & Savage-Rumbaugh, 1991). Turn-
size, group size, and the evolution of language.
ing rituals into supernatural stories was the last Current Anthropology, 34, 184 193.
decisive step in the evolution of religion from Alcorta, C. S., & Sosis, R. (2006). Ritual, emotion,
its primordial origins and the first step in the and sacred symbols: The evolution of religion as
creation of book religions as we know them an adaptive complex. Human Nature, 16, 323359.
today. Alexander, R. (1989). Evolution of the human
psyche. In P. Mellars & C. Stringer (Ed.), The
Summary human revolution (pp. 455513). Edinburgh: Uni-
versity of Edinburgh Press.
The model being proposed can be summa- Argyle, M. (2000). Psychology and religion: An in-
rized succinctly in three stages. troduction. London: Routledge.
Arsuaga, J. L., Martinez, I., Gracia, A., Carretero,
1. Pre-UP proto-religion dating roughly from J. M., Lorenzo, C., Garcia, N., et al. (1997). Sima
de los Huesos (Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain). The
300,000 ya and involving ecstatic states/ site. Journal of Human Evolution, 33, 109 127.
rituals for social bonding. Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
2. Transitional religion dating from roughly Baffier, D., & Girard, M. (1998). Les cavernes
150,000 ya and involving pre-UP religion DArcy-sur-Cure. Paris: La Maison des Roches.
plus shamanistic healing rituals. Baldwin, D. A., Baird, J. A., Saylor, M. M., & Clark,
M. A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action. Child
3. UP religion dating from roughly 35,000 Development, 72, 708 717.
ya and involving all past forms plus elite Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge,
rituals and ancestor worship. MA: MIT Press.
360 ROSSANO

Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). How to build a baby that Chase, P. G., & Dibble, H. L. (1987). Middle Paleo-
can read minds. In S. Baron-Cohen (Ed.). The lithic symbolism: A review of current evidence
maladapted mind (pp. 207239). East Sussex, UK: and interpretations. Journal of Anthropological
Psychology Press. Archeology, 6, 263296.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Autism and the origins of Ching, J. (1993). Chinese religions. Maryknoll, NY:
social neuroscience. In A. Easton & N. Emery Orbis Books.
(Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of social behav- Clottes, J. (1992). Larcheologie des grottes ornees.
ior (pp. 239 256). East Sussex, UK: Psychology La Recherche, 239, 23, 52 61.
Press. Clottes, J., & Lewis-Williams, D. (1998). The sha-
Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural founda- mans of prehistory: Trance and magic in painted
tions of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, caves. New York: Abrams Press.
29 34. Conklin, B. A. (2001). Consuming grief: Compas-
Barrett, K. C. (1995). A functionalist approach to sionate cannibalism in an Amazonian society.
shame and guilt. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
(Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of dAquili, E., & Newberg, A. B. (1999). The mystical
shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 25 mind. Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press.
63). New York: The Guilford Press. dErrico, F., & Nowell, A. (2000). A new look at the
Bednarik, R. (1994). Art origins. Anthropos, 89, Berekhat Ram figurine: Implications for the origin
169 180. of symbolism. Cambridge Archeological Jour-
Bednarik, R. (1995). Concept-mediated marking in nal, 10, 123167.
the Lower Paleolithic. Current Anthropology, 36, dErrico, F., Zilhao, J., Julien, M., Baffier, D., &
605 634. Pelegrin, J. (1998). Neanderthal acculturation in
Bering, J. M. (2005). The evolutionary history of an Western Europe? A critical review of the evidence
illusion: Religious causal beliefs in children and and its interpretation. Current Anthropology,
adults. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklung (Eds.). 39(Suppl.), 1 44.
Dahlberg, A. A., & Carbonell, V. M. (1961). The
Origins of the social mind (pp. 411 437). New
dentition of the Magdalenian female from Cap
York: The Guilford Press.
Blanc, France. Man, 61, 49 50.
Binford, L. R., & Ho, C. K. (1985). Taphonomy at a
Dart, R. A. (1974). The waterworn Australopithecene
distance: Zhoukoudian, the cave home of Beijing
pebble of many faces from Makapansgat. South
man? Current Anthropology, 26, 413 442.
African Journal of Science, 70, 167169.
Bingham, P. M. (1999). Human uniqueness: A gen-
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J.,
eral theory. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 74,
Rosenkrantz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F. et al.
133169. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function
Bloom, H. (1992). The American religion. New produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychoso-
York: Simon & Schuster. matic Medicine, 65, 564 570.
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evo- Defleur, A., Dutour, O., Valladas, H., & Vander-
lution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: meersch, V. (1993). Cannibals among the Nean-
Harvard University Press. derthals. Nature, 362, 214.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. New York: Defleur, A., White, T., Valensi, P., Slimak, L., &
Basic Books. Cregut-Bonnoure, E. (1999). Neanderthal canni-
Boyer, P., & Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates balism at Moula-Guercy, Ardeche, France. Sci-
for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for ence, 286, 128 131.
recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cogni- de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured. Cambridge,
tive Science, 25, 535564. MA: Harvard University Press.
Breuil, A. B. (1979). Four hundred centuries of cave de Waal, F. B. M., & Berger, M. L. (2000). Payment
art (M. E. Boyle, Trans.). New York: Hacker Art for labour in monkeys. Nature, 404, 563.
Books. (Original work published 1952) de Waal, F. B. M., & Lanting, F. (1997). Bonobo:
Bruce, S. (2002). God is dead: Secularization in the The forgotten ape. Berkeley: University of Cali-
West. London: Blackwell. fornia Press.
Butzer, K. W. (1971). Environment and archaeology. Dickson, B. (1990). The dawn of belief. Tucson:
Chicago: Aldine Atherton. University of Arizona Press.
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1991). Tactical decep- Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind.
tion in primates: The 1990 database. Primate Re- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
port, 27, 1101. Dowson, T., & Porr, M. (2001). Special objects
Cann, R. L., Stoneking, M., & Wilson, A. C. (1987). special creatures: Shamanic imagery and Aurigna-
Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature, cian art. In N. Price (Ed.), The archaeology of
325, 3136. shamanism (pp. 165177). London: Routledge.
RELIGIOUS MIND 361

Dugatkin, L. A. (2001). Subjective commitment in Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cam-
nonhumans: What should we be looking for, and bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
where should we be looking? In R. Nesse (Ed.), Gopnik, A. (1999). Theory of mind. In R. Wilson &
Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. F. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cog-
120 137). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. nitive sciences (pp. 838 840). Cambridge, MA:
Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolu- MIT Press.
tion of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- Grayson, D. K., & Delpech, F. (2003). Ungulates and
versity Press. the Middle-to-Upper Paleolithic transition at
Eliade, M. (1958). Patterns in comparative religion. Grotte, XVI (Dordogne, France). Journal of Ar-
London: Sheed and Ward. chaeological Science, 30, 16331648.
Farizy, C. (1990). Du Mousterien au Chatelperronien Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S.
a Arcy-sur-Cure. Memoires du Musee de Prehis- (1991). Imitation, grammatical development and
toire, Ile-de-France, 3, 281289. the invention of protogrammar by an ape. In N. A.
Farthing, G. W. (1992). The psychology of conscious- Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schiefelbusch,
ness. New York: Prentice Hall. & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and
Fehr, E., & Gachter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment behavioral determinants of language development
in humans. Nature, 415, 137140. (pp. 235258). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fehr, E., & Tyran, J.-R. (1996). Institutions and re- Gubser, N. J. (1965). The Nunamiut Eskimos: Hunt-
ciprocal fairness. Nordic Journal of Political ers of caribou. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Economy, 67, 133144. Press.
Feuerbach, L. (1973). The fiery book: Selected writ- Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory
ings of Ludwig Feuerbach. Garden City, NY: An- of religion. New York: Oxford University Press.
chor Books. (Original work published 1843) Halifax, J. (1982). Shaman: The wounded healer.
Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C., & Ward, C. V. (2005). London: Thames & Hudson.
Ecological dominance, social competition, and co- Hamayon, R., & Francfort, H.-P. (2001). The concept
alitionary arms races: Why humans evolved ex- of shamanism: Uses and abuses. Budapest: Aka-
traordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Be- demiai Kiado.
havior, 26, 10 46. Harding, J. R. (1976). Certain Upper Paleolithic Ve-
Frazer, J. G. (1941). The golden bough: A study of nus statuettes considered in relation to the patho-
magic and religion. New York: Macmillian. (Orig- logical condition known as massive hypertrophy of
inal work published 1890) the breasts. Man, 11, 271272.
Frecska, E., & Kulscar, Z. (1989). Social bonding in Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chim-
the modulation of the physiology of ritual dance. panzees know what conspecifics know? Animal
Ethos, 17, 70 87. Behaviour, 61, 139 151.
Gallup, G. (1970). Chimpanzee self-recognition. Sci- Harrold, F. B. (1980). A comparative analysis of
ence, 167, 86 87. Eurasian Paleolithic burials. World Archaeol-
Gallup, G. (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence ogy, 12, 195211.
of mind in primates. American Journal of Prima- Harvey, G. (2000). Indigenous religions. London:
tology, 2, 237248. Cassell.
Gargett, R. H. (1989). Grave shortcomings: The ev- Hayden, B. (1987). Alliance and ritual ecstasy: Hu-
idence for Neanderthal burial. Current Anthropol- man response to resource stress. Journal for the
ogy, 30, 157190. Scientific Study of Religion, 26, 8191.
Gargett, R. H. (1999). Middle Paleolithic burial is not Hayden, B. (2003). Shamans, sorcerers, and saints.
a dead issue. Journal of Human Evolution, 37, Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Books.
2790. Henshilwood, C. S., & Sealy, J. C. (1997). Bone
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). Consciousness and the ce- artifacts from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos
rebral hemispheres. In M. S. Gazzaniga, (Ed.), The Cave, southern Cape, South Africa. Current An-
cognitive neurosciences (pp. 13911399). Cam- thropology, 38, 890 895.
bridge, MA: MIT Press. Hinde, R. A. (1999). Why gods persist. London:
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. Routledge Press.
(1998). Cognitive neuroscience. New York: Nor- Hoffecker, J. F. (2002). Desolate landcapes: Ice-age
ton. settlement in Europe. New Brunswick, N. J.: Rut-
Geertz, C. (1966). Religion as a cultural system. In gers University Press.
M. Banton (Ed.), Anthropological approaches to Hogg, G. (1966). Cannibalism and human sacrifice.
the study of religion (pp. 1 46). London: Tavi- New York: Citadel Press.
sock. Horowitz, M. J. (1964). The imagery of visual hal-
Glucklich, A. (2001). Sacred pain. Oxford: Oxford lucinations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
University Press. eases, 138, 513523.
362 ROSSANO

Hoskovec, J., & Svorad, D. (1969). The relationship Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1982). The Dawn of European
between human and animal hypnosis. American Art: An introduction to Paleolithic cave art. Cam-
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 11, 180 182. bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hultkrantz, A. (1973). A definition of shamanism. Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears:
Temenos, 9, 2537. The autonomic architecture of emotion. In P. Ek-
Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture of hook-tools by man, J. J. Campos, R. J. Davidson, & F. B. M. de
New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249 251. Waal (Eds.), Emotions inside out (pp. 348 366).
Hutton, R. (1991). The pagan religions of the ancient New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
British Isles. Oxford: Blackwell Press. Lewin, R. (1998). Principles of human evolution.
Ingman, M., Kaessman, H., Paabo, S., & Gyllensten, London: Blackwell Science.
U. (2000). Mitochondrial genome variation and the Lewis-Williams, D. (2002). The mind in the cave.
origin of modern humans. Nature, 408, 708 713. London: Thames & Hudson.
Irons, W. (1996). Morality, religion and human evo- Lewis-Williams, J. D. (1982). The economic and
lution. In W. Mark Richardson, & W. J. Wildman social context of southern San rock art. Current
(Eds.), Religion and science: History, method and Anthropology, 23, 429 449.
dialogue (pp. 375399). New York: Routledge. Lewis-Williams, J. D. (1986). The last testament of
Irons, W. (2001). Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of the Southern San. South African Archaeological
commitment. In R. Nesse (Ed.), Evolution and the Bulletin, 41, 10 11.
capacity for commitment (pp. 292309). New Lieberman, D. F., & Shea, J. J. (1994). Behavioral
York: Russell Sage Foundation. differences between archaic and modern humans
James, E. O. (1957). Prehistoric religion. New York: in the Levantine Mousterian. American Anthropol-
Frederick A. Praeger. ogist, 96, 300 332.
James, W. (1961). Varieties of religious experience. Lommel, A. (1967). Shamanism: The beginning of
New York: MacMillan. (Original work published art. New York: McGraw-Hill.
1902) Lorblancher, M. (1999). La Naissance de LArt.
Katz, R. (1982). Boiling energy: Community healing Paris: Editions Errance.
among the Kalahari Kung. Cambridge, MA: Har- Lowie, R. H. (1963). Indians of the plains. New
vard University Press. York: The American Museum of Natural History.
Kelly, M., & Keil, F. (1985). The more things Malinowski, B. (1961). Argonauts of the western
change . . . : Metamorphoses and conceptual struc- Pacific. New York: Dutton. (Original work pub-
ture. Cognitive Science, 9, 403 416. lished 1922)
Keverne, E. B., Martinez, N. D., & Tuite, B. (1989). Mandell, A. (1980). Toward a psychobiology of tran-
Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal scendence: God in the brain. In D. Davidson & R.
fluid of moneys influenced by grooming relation- Davidson (Eds.), The psychobiology of conscious-
ships. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14, 155161. ness (pp. 379 464). New York: Plenum Press.
Klein, R. G., & Edgar, B. (2002). The dawn of human Marcuse, F. L. (1951). Individual differences in an-
culture. New York: Wiley. imal hypnosis. British Journal of Medical Hypno-
Klima, B. (1988). A triple burial from the Upper sis, 3, 1720.
Paleolithic of Dolni Vestonice, Czechoslovakia. Marks, S. A. (1976). Large mammals and a brave
Journal of Human Evolution, 16, 831 835. people: Subsistence hunters in Zambia. Seattle:
Kluver, H. (1942). Mechanisms of hallucinations. In University of Washington Press.
Q. McNemar & M. A. Merrill (Eds.), Studies in Marschack, A. (1981). On Paleolithic ochre and the
personality (pp. 175207). New York: McGraw- early uses of color and symbol. Current Anthro-
Hill. pology, 22, 188 191.
Knight, C. D., Power, C., & Watts, I. (1995). The Marschack, A. (1997). The Berekhat Ram figuirine:
human symbolic revolution: A Darwinian account. A late Acheulian carving from the Middle East.
Cambridge Archeological Journal, 5, 75114. Antiquity, 71, 327337.
Leakey, M. D. (1971). Olduvai Gorge volume III: McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. (2000). The revolution
Excavations in beds I and II, 19601963. Cam- that wasnt: A new interpretation of the origin of
bridge: Cambridge University Press. human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39,
Lee, R. B., & Daly, R. (1999). Introduction: Foragers 453 463.
and others. In R. B. Lee and R. Daly (Eds.), The McClenon, J. (1997). Shamanic healing, human evo-
Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers lution and the origin of religion. Journal for the
(pp. 122). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univer- Scientific Study of Religion, 36, 345354.
sity Press. McClenon, J. (2002). Wondrous healing: Shaman-
Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1968). The evolution of Paleo- ism, human evolution and the origin of religion.
lithic art. Scientific American, 218, 58 70. DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University Press.
RELIGIOUS MIND 363

McNeill, W. (1995). Keeping Together in Time: Power, C., & Watts, I. (1996). Female strategies and
Dance and Drill in Human History. Cambridge, collective behavior. The archeology of earliest Homo
MA: Harvard University Press. sapiens sapiens. In J. Steele and S. Shennan (Eds.),
Mellars, P. (1996). The Neanderthal legacy. Prince- The archeology of human ancestry. Power, sex and
ton, NJ: Princeton University Press. tradition (pp. 306 330). London: Routledge.
Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of Premack, D. (1976). Intelligence in ape and man.
others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
month-old children. Developmental Psychol- Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1994). Levels of
ogy, 24, 838 850. causal understanding in chimpanzees and children.
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind. Lon- Cognition, 50, 347362.
don: Thames & Hudson. Prince, R. (1982). The endorphins: A review for psycho-
Nelson, K. (2004). Evolution and development of logical anthropologists. Ethnos, 10, 299302.
human memory systems. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Ramachandran, V. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phan-
Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the Social Mind (pp. toms in the brain. New York: Morrow.
354 382). New York: The Guilford Press. Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the
Newberg, A. B., dAquili, E., & Rause, V. (2001). Why making of humanity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
God wont go away. New York: Ballantine Books. University Press.
Norenzayan, A., & Atran, S. (2002). Cognitive and Rice, P. C. (1982). Prehistoric Venuses: Symbols of
emotional processes in the cultural transmission of motherhood or womanhood? Journal of Anthropo-
natural and nonnatural beliefs. In M. Shaller & C. logical Research, 37, 402 414.
Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are animals stuck in time?
culture (pp. 149 169). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473 489.
Nyberg, L. Cabeza, R., & Tulving, E. (1996). PET studies Roes, F. L., & Raymond, M. (2003). Belief in mor-
of encoding and retrieval: The HERA model. Psy- alizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24,
chonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 135148. 126 135.
OConnell, S. O., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2005). The
Rouzaud, F., Soulier, M., & Lignereux, Y. (1996). La
perception of causality in chimpanzees (Pan spp.).
Grotte de Bruniquel. Spelunca, 60, 28 34.
Animal Cognition, 8, 60 66.
Ruvolo, M. D. (1996). A new approach to studying
OShea, J., & Zvelebil, M. (1984). Oleneostrovski
modern human origins. Molecular Phylogenetics
mogilnik: Reconstructing the social and economic
and Evolution, 5, 202219.
organization of prehistoric foragers in northern
Sandarupa, S. (1996). Life and death in Toraja.
Russia. Journal of Anthropological Archaeol-
ogy, 3, 1 40. Ujung Pandang, Indonesia: Tiga Taurus Ujung
Oberg, K. (1973). The social economy of the Tlinglit Pandang.
Indians. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Sanday, P. R. (1986). Divine hunger. Cambridge:
Orme-Johnson, D. W., & Herron, R. E. (1997). An Cambridge University Press.
innovative approach to reducing medical care uti- Schele, L., & Freidel, D. (1990). A Forest of Kings.
lization and expenditures. American Journal of New York: Morrow.
Managed Care, 3, 135144. Seigel, R. K., & Jarvik, M. E. (1975). Drug-induced
Owens, D., & Hayden, B. (1997). Prehistoric rites of hallucinations in animals and man. In R. K. Siegel &
passage: A comparative study of Transegalitarian L. J. West (Eds.), Hallucinations: Behavior, experi-
hunter-gatherers. Journal of Anthropological Ar- ence, and theory (pp. 81161). New York: Wiley.
chaeology, 16, 121161. Shelton, J. (1998). As the Romans did. New York:
Parker, S. T. (1998). A social selection model for the Oxford University Press.
evolution and adaptive significance of self- Silberbauer, G. (1981). Hunter and habitat in the
conscious emotions. In M. Ferrari, & R. J. Stern- central Kalahari Desert. Cambridge, UK: Cam-
berg, (Eds.), Self-awareness: Its nature and devel- bridge University Press.
opment (pp. 108 134). New York: Guilford Press. Singer, R., & Wymer, J. J. (1982). The Middle Stone
Parrinder, G. (1976). African traditional religions. Age at Klasies River mouth in South Africa. Chi-
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. cago: University of Chicago Press.
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young Smart, N. (1976). The religious experience of man-
chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of kind. New York: Scribners.
the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, Soffer, O. (1985). The Upper Paleolithic of the cen-
(2, serial No. 247). tral Russian plain. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Povinelli, D. J., & Prince, C. G. (1998). When self Sosis, R. (2000). Religion and intragroup coopera-
met other. In M. Ferrari, & R. J. Sternberg, (Eds.), tion: Preliminary results of a comparative analysis
Self-awareness: Its nature and development (pp. of Utopian communities. Cross-Cultural Re-
37107). New York: Guilford Press. search, 34, 71 88.
364 ROSSANO

Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. (2003). Signaling, solidarity, Watanabe, J. M., & Smuts, B. B. (1999). Explaining
and the sacred: The evolution of religious behav- religion without explaining it away: Trust, truth,
ior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 264 274. and the evolution of cooperation in Roy A. Rap-
Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. (2003). Cooperation and com- paports the obvious aspects of ritual. American
mune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory Anthropologist, 101, 98 112.
of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 211239. Watson, R. I. (1973). Investigation into deindividu-
Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. (2003). Religious ritual and ation using cross-cultural techniques. Journal of
cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 342345.
religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthro- Weidenreich, F. (1943). The skull of Sinanthropus
pology, 44, 713722. Pekinensis. Pehpei, Chungking: Geological Survey
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. of China.
New York: Oxford University Press. White, R. (1993). Technological and social dimen-
Townsend, J. B. (1999). Shamanism. In S. D. Glazier sions of Aurignacian Age body ornaments
(Ed.), Anthropology of Religion, (pp. 429 469). across Europe. In H. Knecht, A. Pike-Tay, & R.
Westport, CT: Praeger. White (Eds.), Before Lascaux (pp. 27799). Boca
Trinkaus, E. & Shipman, P. (1992). Neanderthals: Of Raton, FL: CRC Press.
skeletons, scientists, and scandal. New York: Vin- White, T. D. (1986). Cut marks on the Bodo Cra-
tage Press. nium: A case of prehistoric defleshing. American
Trivers, R. L. (1985). Social evolution. Merlo Park, Journal of Physical Anthropology, 69, 503509.
CA: Benjamin/Cummings. Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwins cathedral: Evolution,
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal religion and the nature of society. Chicago: Uni-
altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557. versity of Chicago Press.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about be-
Tsunoda, R. (1986). Sources of Japanese tradition.
liefs: Representational and constraining function
New York: Columbia University Press.
of wrong beliefs in young childrens understanding
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory.
of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Winkelman, M. (1990). Shamans and other magico-
Tulving, E., Kapur, S., Craik, F. I. M., Moscovitch, M.,
religious healers: A cross-cultural study of their
& Houle, S. (1994). Hemispheric encoding/retrieval origins, nature, and social transformations.
asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission Ethos, 18, 308 352.
tomography findings. Proceedings of the National Winkelman, M. (1992). Shamans, priests, and witches:
Academy of Sciences, USA, 91, 20122015. A cross-cultural study of magico-religious practitio-
Tulving, E., & Lepage, M. (2000). Where in the brain is ners. Anthropological Research Papers #44. Tempe,
the awareness of ones past? In D. L. Schacter & E. AZ: Arizona State University.
Scarry (Eds.), Memory, brain, and belief (pp. 208 Winkelman, M. (1997). Altered states of conscious-
228). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ness and religious behaviour. In S. Glazier (Ed.).
Turner, V. W. (1969). The ritual process: Structural Anthropology of religion (pp. 393 428). Westport,
and anti-structure. Chicago: Aldine Publishing. CT: Greenwood Press.
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture. London: John Winkelman, M. (2002). Shamanism and cognitive evolu-
Murray. tion. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 12, 71101.
Vanhaeren, M., & dErrico, F. (2005). Grave goods Woodward, M. R. (2000). Gifts for the sky people: Animal
from the Saint-Germain-la-Riverere burial: Evi- sacrifice, head hunting and power among the Naga of
dence for social inequality in the Upper Paleo- Burma and Assam. In G. Harvey (Ed.), Indigenous
lithic. Journal of Anthropological Archaeol- religions (pp. 219229). London: Cassell.
ogy, 24, 117134. Wreschner, E. E. (1980). Red ochre and human evo-
van Roosmalen, M. G. M., & Klein, L. L. (1988). The lution: A case for discussion. Current Anthropol-
spider monkeys, genus Ateles. In R. Mittermeier, ogy, 21, 631 644.
A. B. Rylands, A. F. Coimbra-Filho, & G. A. B. da Wymer, J. (1982). The Paleolithic Age. London:
Fonesca, (Eds.), Ecology and behavior of neotro- Croom Helm.
pic primates, (pp. 445537), Washington, DC: Young, F. (1965). Initiation ceremonies. New York:
World Wildlife Fund. Bobbs-Merrill.
Vitebsky, P. (2000). Shamanism. In G. Harvey (Ed.),
Indigenous religions (pp. 55 67). London: Cassell. Received September 5, 2005
Volgyesi, F. A. (1969). Hypnosis of man and ani- Revision received February 14, 2006
mals. Baltimore: Williams and Williams. Accepted February 28, 2006

You might also like