Teachers Know Comprehension PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

L A U R A S .

PA R D O

What every teacher needs to know


about comprehension
Once teachers understand what is involved to the literacy event, the text has certain features,
and yet meaning emerges only from the engage-
in comprehending and how the factors of ment of that reader with that text at that particular
reader, text, and context interact to create moment in time. Figure 1 below presents a visual
model of this process. Each of the elements in the
meaning, they can more easily teach their
model (reader, text, context, and transaction) is de-
students to be effective comprehenders. scribed in more detail later in this article, along
with specific suggestions for how teachers can in-

C
omprehension is a complex process that has teract with the model to help children become
been understood and explained in a num- strong comprehenders, beginning in kindergarten.
ber of ways. The RAND Reading Study
Group (2002) stated that comprehension is the
process of simultaneously extracting and con-
structing meaning through interaction and involve-
The reader
ment with written language (p. 11). Duke (2003) Any literacy event is made up of a reader en-
added navigation and critique to her definition gaging with some form of text. Each reader is
because she believed that readers actually move unique in that he or she possesses certain traits or
through the text, finding their way, evaluating the
accuracy of the text to see if it fits their personal
agenda, and finally arriving at a self-selected loca- FIGURE 1
Model of comprehension
tion. A common definition for teachers might be
that comprehension is a process in which readers
construct meaning by interacting with text through
the combination of prior knowledge and previous
experience, information in the text, and the stance Social Context
the reader takes in relationship to the text. As these Cultural
different definitions demonstrate, there are many Reader
interpretations of what it means to comprehend
text. This article synthesizes the research on com- Transaction
prehension and makes connections to classroom Meaning
practice. I begin by introducing a visual model of happens here
comprehension.
Text

How comprehension works


Comprehension occurs in the transaction be-
tween the reader and the text (Kucer, 2001;
Rosenblatt, 1978). The reader brings many things

272 2004 International Reading Association (pp. 272280) doi:10.1598/RT.58.3.5


characteristics that are distinctly applied with each Comprehension is affected by a readers culture,
text and situation (Butcher & Kintsch, 2003; based on the degree to which it matches with the
Fletcher, 1994; Narvaez, 2002). The most impor- writers culture or the culture espoused in the text.
tant of these characteristics is likely the readers Readers also read in particular ways depending on
world knowledge (Fletcher, 1994). The more back- the purpose for reading. Another individual differ-
ground knowledge a reader has that connects with ence that exists in readers is motivation. Motivation
the text being read, the more likely the reader will can influence the interest, purpose, emotion, or per-
be able to make sense of what is being read sistence with which a reader engages with text
(Butcher & Kintsch, 2003; Schallert & Martin, (Butcher & Kintsch, 2003; Schallert & Martin,
2003). The process of connecting known informa- 2003). More motivated readers are likely to apply
tion to new information takes place through a series more strategies and work harder at building mean-
of networkable connections known as schema ing. Less motivated readers are not as likely to work
(Anderson & Pearson, 1984; Narvaez, 2002). In as hard, and the meaning they create will not be as
schema theory, individuals organize their world powerful as if they were highly motivated.
knowledge into categories and systems that make
retrieval easier. When a key word or concept is en-
countered, readers are able to access this informa-
tion system, pulling forth the ideas that will help
Teachers support readers
them make connections with the text so they can If readers have all these individual differences,
create meaning. Schema theory involves the stor- how do teachers best support elementary-age read-
age of various kinds of information in long-term ers to become competent comprehenders? They
memory. Because long-term memory appears to teach decoding skills, help students build fluency,
have infinite capacity (Pressley, 2003), it is likely build and activate background knowledge, teach
that readers have many ideas stored in long-term vocabulary words, motivate students, and engage
memory. When a key word or concept is presented them in personal responses to text.
to the reader (through a title, heading, or someone
who has recommended the text), some of this Teach decoding skills. In order to comprehend,
stored information is brought forward and tem- readers must be able to read the words. Some level
porarily placed into short-term memory so that the of automatic decoding must be present so that
reader can return to it quickly as he or she reads. short-term memory can work on comprehending,
Short-term memory has limited capacity, and of- not on decoding, words. Teachers help students get
ten the information pulled from long-term memo- to this level of automatic decoding by providing in-
ry prior to or during reading is only available for a struction in phonemic awareness and phonics at all
short time and then is placed back in long-term grade levels. If students put too much mental ener-
memory. Short-term memory shifts and juggles in- gy into sounding out the words, they will have less
formation, using what is immediately pertinent and mental energy left to think about the meaning.
allowing less pertinent information to slip back into While teachers in the primary grades work with
long-term memory (Schallert & Martin, 2003). phonemic awareness and phonics, teachers in the
The amount and depth of a readers world intermediate grades support students continued de-
knowledge vary as do other individual characteris- velopment of automatic decoding through spelling,
tics. Readers vary in the skills, knowledge, cogni- vocabulary, and high-frequency word activities.
tive development, culture, and purpose they bring
to a text (Narvaez, 2002). Skills include such things Help students build fluency. As word reading be-
as basic language ability, decoding skills, and high- comes automatic, students become fluent and can
er level thinking skills. Knowledge includes back- focus on comprehension (Rasinski, 2003). Teachers
ground knowledge about content and text and help students become more fluent by engaging them
relates to the available schema a reader has for a in repeated readings for real purposes (like perform-
particular text. A readers cognitive development ances and Readers Theatre). Teachers also model
causes that reader to evaluate text in different fluent reading by reading aloud to students daily so
waysfor example, to make moral judgments. that they realize what fluent reading sounds like.

What every teacher needs to know about comprehension 273


Some research indicates that reading aloud to stu- Teach vocabulary words. If there are too many
dents is the single most effective way to increase words that a reader does not know, he or she will
comprehension (see Morrow & Gambrell, 2000, for have to spend too much mental energy figuring out
a review of this literature). the unknown word(s) and will not be able to un-
derstand the passage as a whole. Teachers help stu-
Build and activate prior knowledge. Background dents learn important vocabulary words prior to
knowledge is an important factor for creating mean- reading difficult or unfamiliar texts. When teaching
ing, and teachers should help students activate prior vocabulary words, teachers make sure that the se-
knowledge before reading so that information con- lected words are necessary for making meaning
nected with concepts or topics in the text is more with the text students will be reading and that they
easily accessible during reading (Keene & help students connect the new words to something
Zimmermann, 1997; Miller, 2002). If students do they already know. Simply using the word lists sup-
not have adequate background knowledge, teachers plied in textbooks does not necessarily accomplish
can help students build the appropriate knowledge. this task (Blachowicz & Fisher, 2000). Many teach-
Duke (2003) suggested that one way to add to world ers consider the backgrounds and knowledge levels
knowledge is to use informational books with all stu- of their students and the text the students will be
dents, particularly very young students. By using engaging in and then select a small number of
information books, students build world knowledge words or ideas that are important for understanding
so that they will have the appropriate information to the text. Once teachers have decided on the appro-
activate at a later time. Teachers also support stu-
priate vocabulary words to use, students must ac-
dents acquisition of world knowledge by establish-
tively engage with the wordsuse them in written
ing and maintaining a rich, literate environment, full
and spoken languagein order for the words to be-
of texts that provide students with numerous oppor-
come a part of the students reading and writing vo-
tunities to learn content in a wide variety of topics.
cabularies. For example, asking students to create
Another way teachers help students build back-
graphic organizers that show relationships among
ground knowledge is to create visual or graphic or-
ganizers that help students to see not only new new words and common and known words helps
concepts but also how previously known concepts them assimilate new vocabulary. Asking students to
are related and connected to the new ones (Keene look up long lists of unrelated, unknown words is
& Zimmermann, 1997; Miller, 2002). Teachers unlikely to help students access the text more ap-
teach students how to make text-to-text, text-to- propriately or to increase personal vocabularies.
self, and text-to-world connections so that readers
can more easily comprehend the texts they read. Motivate students. Many individual reader fac-
Reading aloud and teacher modeling show stu- tors (e.g., cognitive development, culture) are not
dents how to activate schema and make connec- within a teachers control. However, teachers can
tions. For example, a first-grade teacher read aloud motivate students by providing them with interest-
from Ira Says Goodbye (Waber, 1991). She began ing texts, allowing them choices in reading and
the lesson by thinking aloud about the title and writing, and helping students set authentic purpos-
cover of the book. Oh I see that the author is es for reading (e.g., generating reports, writing let-
Bernard Waber and the title is Ira Says Goodbye. I ters, demonstrating some new ability or skill;
think this book is about the same Ira as in Ira Pressley & Hilden, 2002). Many teachers actively
Sleeps Over (Waber, 1973). I can activate my seek out students interests so that they can select
schema from that book. I am making a text-to-text texts, topics, themes, and units that will more like-
connection. I remember that.... She continued ly engage students. Teachers also provide and pro-
modeling for her students how to activate schema mote authentic purposes for engaging in reading
and make connections that helped her make mean- and writing. Authentic literacy events are those that
ing from this text. As she read the book to her stu- replicate or reflect reading and writing purposes
dents, she stopped occasionally to model and think and texts that occur in the world outside of schools.
aloud how she activated her own schema to make Some teachers do this by providing pen pals, us-
connections. ing students authentic questions for in-depth study,

274 The Reading Teacher Vol. 58, No. 3 November 2004


responding to community needs, or having students The authors intent in writing the text can in-
solve problems. fluence how a reader interacts with that text, par-
ticularly if this intent is made known through a
Engage students in personal responses to text. foreword, back-cover biography, or knowledgeable
Teachers encourage students to read both efferently other (as in the case of teachers in schools). Some
and aesthetically (Rosenblatt, 1978). Researchers texts are promoted as carrying a certain message
(McMahon, Raphael, Goatley, & Pardo, 1997) or theme by those who have encountered the book
building on the ideas of Rosenblatt developed a previously (Rosenblatt, 1978). The inherent mes-
literature-based approach to teaching reading com- sage that some texts carry with them, often related
prehension through the Book Club program. In this to the authors intent, is referred to as gist and has
instructional approach students read authentic liter- been defined as what people rememberthe main
ature; write personal, critical, and creative respons- ideas in the text (Pressley, 1998, p. 46). Gist is fre-
es; and talk about books with their classmates quently assessed through basal workbooks and
(Pardo, 2002). Teachers help students learn and ap- standardized reading tests; therefore, the authors
ply comprehension strategies while reading, intent is a key feature of text.
through writing, and during student-led discussion
groups called Book Clubs, where students explore
the individual meanings that have emerged as they Teachers support texts
engage with the text over a period of time. While Because certain features make some texts more
this program initially focused on the intermediate easily comprehensible, teachers help young readers
grades, many teachers have found that students in understand those features so they can comprehend
first and second grades are successful comprehen- effectively. Teachers teach text structures, model
ders when they read and engage in Book Clubs appropriate text selection, and provide regular in-
(Grattan, 1997; Raphael, Florio-Ruane, & George, dependent reading time.
2001; Salna, 2001).
Teach text structures. Because features of the
text are beyond a teachers control, teachers select
The text texts that have an obvious structure. They teach a
variety of narrative genres and some expository
Understanding the reader is one important text structures. With narrative works teachers help
piece of the comprehension puzzle, but features of students understand basic story grammar, including
the text also influence the transaction where com- the literary elements that are common across narra-
prehension happens. The structure of the textits tive pieces, such as plot, characters, and setting.
genre, vocabulary, language, even the specific word They teach specific elements that make each genre
choicesworks to make each text unique. Some unique (e.g., talking animals in folk tales). By do-
would even argue that it is at the word or micro- ing this, students will be able to access a schema
structure level that meaning begins (Butcher & for a certain narrative genre when they begin to
Kintsch, 2003). How well the text is written, read a new text and can begin to make text-to-text
whether it follows the conventions of its genre or connections for a particular story genre, which will
structure, and the language or dialect it is written in help them more easily make meaning. Likewise,
are all factors of the text. The content of a specific teachers share some common expository text struc-
text, the difficulty or readability of it, and even the tures with students, such as sequence, description,
type font and size are factors of a text that can in- comparison, and cause and effect. Teachers discuss
fluence a readers interaction. These features col- the idea of inconsiderate texts (Armbruster,
lectively are referred to as surface features, and 1984) with students and show them how to use
studies have shown that the quality of the text at the cues when reading nonfiction (such as reading
surface level is important for readers to be able to tables, charts, graphs, and the captions under pic-
make meaning effectively (Tracey & Morrow, tures; using bold print and italics to determine big
2002). or important ideas). Inconsiderate texts do not

What every teacher needs to know about comprehension 275


adhere strictly to one structure, but might be a com- also involves the activity that occurs around the
bination of several structures. Many textbooks have transaction. If a teacher assigns his or her students
a varied and mixed set of structures, and teachers to read a certain text for a specific reason, the trans-
can address specific features and demands of infor- action that occurs will be based on this context. If
mational text so that students are more likely to students are asked to discuss a text, generate ques-
engage in informational text with a repertoire of tions from it, or come up with a big idea, these
strategies and schema to help them construct mean- kinds of activities form a context within which the
ing (Duke, 2003). reader and text interact for a specific reason, one
that is unlikely to occur in exactly the same manner
Model appropriate text selection. Teachers teach ever again. Teachers create contexts and learning
students how to select appropriate texts by showing opportunities that will support the construction of
them what features to consider. Some teachers use meaning. Environments that value reading and
the Goldilocks approach (Tompkins, 2003), while writing, that contain a wide variety of texts, that
others suggest that teachers level books and tell stu- allow students to take risks, and that find time for
dents which level books they may select (Fountas reading aloud and reading independently are con-
& Pinnell, 1996). In the Goldilocks approach, read- texts that effectively promote the construction of
ers look for books that are not too hard or too easy, meaning (Keene & Zimmermann, 1997; Miller,
but just right. Just-right books are those that look 2002; Pardo, 2002).
interesting, have mostly decodable words, have
been read aloud previously, are written by a famil-
iar author, or will be read with a support person
nearby (Tompkins, 2003). Teachers have a wide va-
The transaction
riety of genres and levels of books available for stu- As we consider the readers individual and
dents to select for independent reading, and they unique differences, the characteristics of the con-
support students throughout the year with appro- text, and the features of the text, we are left to won-
priate book selection. der exactly what happens when these three come
together. At the most basic level microstructures
Provide regular independent reading time. (words, propositions) are being decoded and rep-
Teachers can make sure they provide students with resented by mental images (Butcher & Kintsch,
time to read independently every day. Reading be- 2003). This is most likely happening quickly, au-
comes better with practice, and comprehending be- tomatically, and in short-term memory. These men-
comes better with more reading practice (Pressley, tal images are calling forth ideas and information
2003). Many teachers use programs such as DEAR stored in long-term memory to assist the reader in
(Drop Everything And Read) or SSR (Sustained building a series of connections between represen-
Silent Reading) to ensure that students read inde- tations (van den Broek, 1994). These connections
pendently every day. occur between the reader and the text and between
different parts of the text. This representation is
fine-tuned by the reader as more information is en-
Teachers create and support countered in the text and more connections are
made. Readers exit the transaction maintaining a
a sociocultural context mental representation or gist of the text.
Reading takes place somewhere between a How do these connections lead to mental rep-
specific reader and a specific text. A sociocultural resentations? One way is through making infer-
influence likely permeates any reading activity ences. A reader is quite intentional as he or she
(Kucer, 2001; Schallert & Martin, 2003). engages with the text, asking, What is it Im look-
Depending on the place, the situation, and the pur- ing at here? Readers are searching for coherence
pose for reading, the reader and the text interact in and for a chain of related events that can lead them
ways that are unique for that specific context. The to infer or make meaning. As readers continue
same reading at another time or in a different place moving through the text, they continue to build in-
might result in a different meaning. The context ferences, drawing from long-term memory specific

276 The Reading Teacher Vol. 58, No. 3 November 2004


ideas that seem to create coherence and answer the alizing, and organizing (Keene & Zimmermann,
question posed earlier, What is it Im looking at 1997; Miller, 2002; Pardo, 2002). Teachers are ex-
here? As this answer emerges, meaning is real- plicit and direct in explaining what these strategies
ized. Inferencing is most likely done automatical- are and why good readers use them (Duffy, 2002;
ly and is one of the most important processes that Pressley & McCormick, 1995). They model the
occur during comprehension (Butcher & Kintsch, strategies (often by thinking aloud) for the students
2003; van den Broek, 1994). and provide them with numerous opportunities to
The mental representation needs to make sense practice and apply the strategies. In order for strate-
to the reader as it emerges; therefore, readers mon- gies to transfer so that students use them on their
itor the emerging meaning as they read, using own or in assessment situations, contexts need to
metacognitive and fix-up strategies, sometimes dis- remain similar. Therefore, teachers use texts and
carding ideas in the text if they do not add to the co- classroom structures that are easily maintained for
herence that the reader is trying to build (Pressley teaching, practicing and applying independently, and
& Afflerbach, 1995). If the readers background assessing. Teachers help students think metacogni-
knowledge or personal experiences agree with the tively about strategies, considering when and where
text, the reader assimilates this new information to apply each strategy, how to use it, and the impact
and creates new meaning. If, however, the readers it can have. In addition, teachers occasionally pro-
background knowledge and personal experiences vide students with difficult text. If students en-
do not agree with the new information presented counter only texts that they can read easily, there will
in the text, readers either adjust the information to be no reason to practice and apply strategies. It is
make it fit (accommodation), or they reject that in- when readers encounter challenging texts that they
formation and maintain their previous understand- put strategies to use (Kucer, 2001).
ing (Kucer, 2001). Readers apply a variety of
strategies throughout this process to support their Teach students to monitor and repair. Knowing
construction of meaning such as summarizing, what is understood and not understood while read-
clarifying, questioning, visualizing, predicting, and ing and then applying the appropriate strategy to
organizing. It is through the application of these repair meaning are vital for comprehension to oc-
strategies at various moments throughout the in- cur. Good readers monitor while reading to see if
teraction that meaning emerges. things make sense, and they use strategies to re-
pair the meaning when things stop making sense
(Duke, 2003; Pressley & Hilden, 2002). While
some studies support that monitoring is important
Teachers support transaction (Baker, 2002; Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995), other
At this point, it seems fairly obvious that com- studies indicate that readers often mismonitor
prehension occurs in the transaction between a (Baker, 1989; Baker & Brown, 1984; Kinnunen,
reader and a text within a sociocultural context. Vauras, & Niemi, 1998). Readers have been found
That makes the transaction crucial to comprehen- to both over- and underestimate their comprehen-
sion and the teachers role within this transaction sion of text. So, while monitoring is important and
very important. Teachers provide explicit instruc- good readers seem to monitor successfully, effec-
tion of useful comprehension strategies, teach stu- tive teachers realize that mismonitoring can affect
dents to monitor and repair, use multiple strategy meaning for less able students, and they provide
approaches, scaffold support, and make reading additional support as needed so that all readers
and writing connections visible to students. comprehend text successfully.

Provide explicit instruction of useful comprehen- Use multiple strategy approaches. Researchers
sion strategies. Good readers use strategies to sup- have found that teaching multiple strategies simul-
port their understanding of text. Teachers help taneously may be particularly powerful (Trabasso
students become good readers by teaching them how & Bouchard, 2002; National Institute of Child
to use the strategies of monitoring, predicting, in- Health and Human Development, 2000; Pressley,
ferring, questioning, connecting, summarizing, visu- 2000).

What every teacher needs to know about comprehension 277


There is very strong empirical, scientific evidence that the text, and the strategy. While adaptations may be
the instruction of more than one strategy in a natural made with students of different ages, teachers use
context leads to the acquisition and use of reading this model with students in all elementary grades.
comprehension strategies and transfer to standardized
comprehension tests. Multiple strategy instruction fa-
cilitates comprehension as evidenced by performance
Make reading/writing connections visible.
on tasks that involve memory, summarizing, and iden- Teachers help students see that reading and writ-
tification of main ideas. (Trabasso & Bouchard, 2002, ing are parallel processes and that becoming good
p. 184) writers can help them become good readers (Kucer,
2001). Composing a text can be thought of as writ-
Perhaps the most frequently used multiple ing something that people will understand. Writing
strategies approach is transactional strategy instruc- can bring understanding about a certain topic to the
tion (TSI), created and studied by Pressley and col- writer, who will have to be clear about the topic he
leagues (Brown, Pressley, Van Meter, & Schuder, or she is writing about. Meaning matters in com-
1996; Gaskins, Anderson, Pressley, Cunicelli, & prehending, and becoming a clear writer is all
Satlow, 1993). TSI teachers encourage readers to about how the reader will make meaning of the text
make sense of text by using strategies that allow that is being created. Recalling the earlier discus-
them to make connections between text content and sion of authentic purposes is important here as
prior knowledge. Teachers and students work in well; students will likely become engaged with the
small reading groups to collaboratively make mean- task of writing if asked to write for authentic and
ing using several teacher-identified strategies. important purposes.
Teachers model and explain the strategies, coach
students in their use, and help students use them
flexibly. Throughout the instruction, students are
taught to think about the usefulness of each strate- Closing comments
gy and to become metacognitive about their own Comprehending is a complicated process, as
reading processes. we have discovered and explored in this article.
Yet it is one of the most important skills for stu-
Scaffold support. When teaching strategies to dents to develop if they are to become successful
elementary-age students, teachers gradually release and productive adults. Comprehension instruction
responsibility for comprehending to students. An in schools, beginning in kindergarten, is therefore
effective model that has been used by some teach- crucial. Teachers use their knowledge and under-
ers is the Gradual Release of Responsibility model standings of how one learns to comprehend to
(Pearson & Gallagher, 1983). In this model, teach- inform classroom practices so they can most ef-
ers take all the responsibility for applying a newly fectively help readers develop the abilities to com-
introduced strategy by modeling, thinking aloud, prehend text. It is hoped that the discussion in this
demonstrating, and creating meaning. As time article can open a dialogue with teachers and
passes and students have more exposure to and teacher educators toward this end.
practice with using the strategy, teachers scaffold
students by creating activities within students
Pardo is a doctoral candidate in Teaching,
Zone of Proximal Development (Vygotsky, 1978)
Curriculum, & Educational Policy at Michigan
and slowly withdrawing more and more responsi-
State University (118 #36 Erickson Hall, East
bility. Teachers work collaboratively with the stu-
dents and the strategy, giving and taking as much as Lansing, MI 48824, USA). E-mail
necessary to create meaning. Eventually, students pardolau@msu.edu.
take on more and more responsibility as they be-
References
come more confident, knowledgeable, and capable.
Anderson, R.C., & Pearson, P.D. (1984). A schema-thematic
Finally, students are able to work independently. view of basic processes in reading comprehension. In
Teachers and students do not always progress in a P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.),
linear way, but often slip back and forth between Handbook of reading research (pp. 255291). New York:
more and less responsibility depending on the task, Longman.

278 The Reading Teacher Vol. 58, No. 3 November 2004


Armbruster, B.B. (1984). The problem of inconsiderate Kucer, S.B. (2001). Dimensions of literacy: A conceptual
texts. In G.G. Duffy, L.R. Roehler, & J. Mason (Eds.), base of teaching reading and writing in school settings.
Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
202217). New York: Longman. McMahon, S.I., Raphael, T.E., Goatley, V.J., & Pardo, L.S.
Baker, L. (1989). Metacognition, comprehension monitoring, (1997). The Book Club connection: Literacy learning and
and the adult reader. Educational Psychology Review, 1, classroom talk. New York: Teachers College Press.
338. Miller, D. (2002). Reading with meaning: Teaching compre-
Baker, L. (2002). Metacognition in comprehension instruc- hension in the primary grades. Portland, ME: Stenhouse.
tion. In C.C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension Morrow, L.M., & Gambrell, L.B. (2000). Literature-based
instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 7795). reading instruction. In M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D.
New York: Guilford. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research
Baker, L., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and (Vol. 3, pp. 563586). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
reading. In P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. Kamil, & P. Narvaez, D. (2002). Individual differences that influence
Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. reading comprehension. In C.C. Block & M. Pressley
353394). New York: Longman. (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-based best
Blachowicz, C.L.Z., & Fisher, P. (2000). Vocabulary instruc- practices (pp. 158175). New York: Guilford.
tion. In M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D. Pearson, & R. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching
503523). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the
Brown, R., Pressley, M., Van Meter, P., & Schuder, T. (1996). scientific research literature on reading and its implica-
A quasi-experimental validation of transactional strate- tions for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-
gies instruction with low-achieving second-grade stu- 4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
dents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 1837. Pardo, L.S. (2002). Book Club for the twenty-first century.
Butcher, K.R., & Kintsch, W. (2003). Text comprehension
Illinois Reading Council Journal, 30(4), 1423.
and discourse processing. In A.F. Healy & R.W. Proctor
Pearson, P.D., & Gallagher, M. (1983). The instruction of
(Vol. Eds.) & I.B. Weiner (Ed.-in-Chief), Handbook of psy-
reading comprehension. Contemporary Education
chology, Volume 4, Experimental psychology (pp.
Psychology, 8, 317344.
575595). New York: Wiley.
Pressley, M. (1998). Reading instruction that works: The
Duffy, G.G. (2002). The case for direct explanation of strate-
case for balanced teaching. New York: Guilford.
gies. In C.C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruc-
instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 2841).
tion be the instruction of? In M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal,
New York: Guilford.
P.D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading re-
Duke, N. (2003, March 7). Comprehension instruction for
search (Vol. 3, pp. 545561). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
informational text. Presentation at the annual meeting of
the Michigan Reading Association, Grand Rapids, MI. Pressley, M. (2003, March 7). Time to revolt against reading
Fletcher, C.R. (1994). Levels of representation in memory instruction as usual: What comprehension instruction
for discourse. In M.A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of could and should be. Presentation at the annual meet-
psycholinguistics (pp. 589607). San Diego: Academic ing of the Michigan Reading Association, Grand Rapids,
Press. MI.
Fountas, I.C., & Pinnell, G.S. (1996). Guided reading: Good Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of
first teaching for all children. Portsmouth, NH: reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading.
Heinemann. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gaskins, I.W., Anderson, R.C., Pressley, M., Cunicelli, E.A., & Pressley, M., & Hilden, K. (2002). How can children be taught
Satlow, E. (1993). Six teachers dialogue during cogni- to comprehend text better? In M.L. Kamil, J.B. Manning,
tive process instruction. The Elementary School Journal, & H.J. Walberg (Eds.), Successful reading instruction:
93, 277304. Research in educational productivity (pp. 3351).
Grattan, K.W. (1997). They can do it too! Book Club with Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
first and second graders. In S.I. McMahon, T.E. Raphael, Pressley, M., & McCormick, C. (1995). Advanced education-
V.J. Goatley, & L.S. Pardo (Eds.), The Book Club connec- al psychology for researchers, educators, and policy-
tion: Literacy learning and classroom talk (pp. 267283). makers. New York: HarperCollins.
New York: Teachers College Press. RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for under-
Keene, E.O., & Zimmermann, S. (1997). Mosaic of thought: standing: Toward a research and development program
Teaching comprehension in a readers workshop. in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Office of
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Education Research and Improvement.
Kinnunen, R., Vauras, M., & Niemi, P. (1998). Comprehension Raphael, T.E., Florio-Ruane, S., & George, M. (2001). Book
monitoring in beginning readers. Scientific Studies of Club Plus: A conceptual framework to organize literacy
Reading, 2, 353375. instruction. Language Arts, 79, 159168.

What every teacher needs to know about comprehension 279


Rasinski, T. (2003, March 7). Fluency: Chasing the illusive Trabasso, T., & Bouchard, E. (2002). Teaching readers how
reading goal. Presentation at the annual meeting of the to comprehend text strategically. In C.C. Block & M.
Michigan Reading Association, Grand Rapids, MI. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-
Rosenblatt, L.R. (1978). The reader, the text, the poem: The based best practices (pp. 176200). New York: Guilford.
transactional theory of the literary work. Carbondale: Tracey, D.H., & Morrow, L.M. (2002). Preparing young learn-
Southern Illinois University Press. ers for successful reading comprehension. In C.C. Block
Salna, K. (2001). Book Clubs as part of a balanced curricu- & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction:
lum. Illinois Reading Council Journal, 29(4), 4047. Research-based best practices (pp. 319333). New York:
Schallert, D.L., & Martin, D.B. (2003). A psychological analy- Guilford.
sis of what teachers and students do in the language arts van den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory of
classroom. In J. Flood, D. Lapp, J.R. Squire, & J.M. Jensen narrative texts: Inferences and coherences. In M.A.
(Eds.), Handbook of research on teaching the English lan- Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp.
guage arts (pp. 3145). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 539588). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Tompkins, G.E. (2003). Literacy for the 21st century: Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA:
Teaching reading and writing in pre-kindergarten Harvard University Press.
through grade 4. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Waber, B. (1973). Ira sleeps over. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Hall. Waber, B. (1991). Ira says goodbye. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

280 The Reading Teacher Vol. 58, No. 3 November 2004

You might also like