Charmaz 2008 A

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

CHAPTER 20

Constructionism and the


Grounded Theory Method

Kathy Charmaz

n the introduction to this Handbook, James is grounded theory? The term refers to both

I A. Holstein and Jaber F. Gubrium suggest


that a social constructionist approach
deals best with what people construct and
the research product and the analytic
method of producing it, which I emphasize
here. The grounded theory method begins
how this social construction process unfolds. with inductive strategies for collecting and
They argue that the constructionist vo- analyzing qualitative data for the purpose of
cabulary does not as readily address the why developing middle-range theories. Exam-
questions that characterize more positivistic ining this method allows us to rethink ways
inquiry.1 In their earlier methodological of bringing why questions into qualitative re-
treatise, The New Language of Qualitative search.
Method (Gubrium & Holstein, 1997), they A social constructionist approach to
proposed that naturalistic qualitative re- grounded theory allows us to address why
searchers could address why questions by questions while preserving the complexity of
considering the contingent relations be- social life. Grounded theory not only is a
tween the whats and hows of social life method for understanding research partici-
(p. 200). To date, however, most qualitative pants social constructions but also is a
research has not addressed why questions. method that researchers construct through-
In contrast, the grounded theory method out inquiry. Grounded theorists adopt a few
has had a long history of engaging both why strategies to focus their data gathering and
questions and what and how questions. What analyzing, but what they do, how they do it,

397
398 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

and why they do it emerge through interact- Objectivist grounded theory (Glaser, 1978,
ing in the research setting, with their data, 1992, 1998) has roots in mid-20th-century
colleagues, and themselves. positivism. It explicitly aims to answer why
How, when, and to what extent grounded questions. Objectivist grounded theorists
theorists invoke social constructionist pre- seek explanation and prediction at a general
mises depends on their epistemological level, separated and abstracted from the spe-
stance and approach to research practice. cific research site and process. Unlike my
From its beginnings, grounded theory has version of grounded theory, which I have
offered explicit guidelines that promise flex- previously called constructivist grounded
ibility and encourage innovation. Paradoxi- theory (Charmaz, 2000, 2006), 20th-century
cally, these guidelines also provided suffi- constructionism treated research worlds as
cient direction such that some researchers social constructions, but not research prac-
have treated the method as a recipe for tices.
stamping out qualitative studies. These re- The two respective emphases on under-
searchers emphasize application of the standing and explanation are not entirely
methodoften a narrow and rigid applica- mutually exclusive. An abstract understand-
tion at that. Such application limits the po- ing of particular sites and situations can al-
tential of grounded theory and fosters the low social constructionists to move from lo-
production of superficial studies. In con- cal worlds to a more general conceptual
trast, a social constructionist approach en- level. The close attention that social con-
courages innovation; researchers can de- structionist grounded theorists give their re-
velop new understandings and novel search problems builds the foundations for
theoretical interpretations of studied life. generic statements that they qualify accord-
The value of social constructionism for ing to particular temporal, social, and situa-
grounded theory studies has only begun to tional conditions.
be mined. In this chapter, I show how a grounded
Distinguishing between a social construc- theory informed by social constructionism
tionist and an objectivist grounded theory can lead to vibrant studies with theoretical
(Charmaz, 2000, 2002, 2006) provides a heu- implications that address why questions. To
ristic device for understanding divisions and provide a backdrop for the discussion, I out-
debates in grounded theory and indicates line the development of grounded theory
ways to move the method further into social and delineate distinctions among pro-
constructionism. The form of construction- ponents. By distinguishing between objec-
ism I advocate includes examining (1) the tivism and constructionism in grounded
relativity of the researchers perspectives, theory, I explicate their underlying assump-
positions, practices, and research situation, tions and point out the tensions between ex-
(2) the researchers reflexivity; and (3) depic- planation and understanding. How might
tions of social constructions in the studied grounded theorists resolve these tensions?
world.2 Consistent with the larger social con- How might the ways in which they construct
structionist literature, I view action as a cen- their studies foster developing explanations
tral focus and see it as arising within socially and understandings and thus attend to both
created situations and social structures. the particular and the general? What princi-
Constructionist grounded theorists attend ples might researchers adopt? To address
to what and how questions. They emphasize these questions, I offer several guidelines
abstract understanding of empirical phe- and look at how two grounded theorists, Su-
nomena and contend that this understand- san Leigh Star (1989) and Monica Casper
ing must be located in the studied specific (1998), constructed their respective analy-
circumstances of the research process. ses.
The Grounded Theory Method 399

Reconstructing Contested Logics ory tools to researchers who had not studied
of Grounded Theory with either Glaser or Strauss or their stu-
dents.5 Many qualitative researchers relied
Barney G. Glaser and Anselm L. Strausss solely on the justificatory ammunition that
(1967) original conception of grounded the- Glaser and Strauss (1967) had fired in de-
ory assumed a social constructionist ap- fense of qualitative research; however, other
proach to the empirical world. Like other so- researchers sought specific analytic guide-
cial scientists of the time, they adopted a lines. Strauss and Corbin (1990, 1998) did
more limited form of social constructionism not simply offer guidelines; they prescribed
than what I advocate here. Glaser and procedures as a path to qualitative success.
Strauss did not attend to how they affected Basics of Qualitative Research became some-
the research process, produced the data, thing of a bible for novices, who often inter-
represented research participants, and posi- preted the method in concrete ways that
tioned their analyses.3 Their research re- muted the social constructionist elements in
ports emphasized generality, not relativity, the method.6
and objectivity, not reflexivity. Meanwhile, the qualitative revolution
Nonetheless, Glaser and Strauss laid the that Denzin and Lincoln (1994, p. ix) pro-
foundation for constructing sound meth- claimed had grown exponentially in and
ods, as well as analyses. By adopting a few across fields. As I (Charmaz, 2000, 2006)
flexible guidelines, grounded theorists have argued previously, the entire qualita-
could construct their specific methodologi- tive revolution owed much to Glaser and
cal strategies, as well as the content of their Strausss (1967) initial statement. Glaser
research.4 Both method and content then and Strauss made qualitative research
emerge during the research process rather defensibleeven respectableat a time
than being preconceived before empirical when quantitative researchers had con-
inquiry begins. trolled the framing definitions of what
Until 1990, most scholars saw grounded counted as research: that is, only what these
theory as a single method based on a shared methodologists could count. Glaser and
logic. As both the originators and their stu- Strauss provided a strong justification for in-
dents worked with the method, changes ductive qualitative inquiry that many re-
emerged and debates ensued about what searchers seized to legitimize their own
grounded theory entails, whose version is work; but these researchers only loosely
correct, and which direction the method adopted the strategies, if at all.
should take. How did these discussions un- Still, Glaser and Strauss (1967) inspired
fold? What are their implications for a the democratization of qualitative research
grounded theory founded in social con- and of theorizing itself. No longer must a
structionism? To understand these issues, I qualitative researcher have the analytic acu-
take a brief look back at the emergence of men of an Erving Goffman or Anselm
contested logics of the method(s). Strauss. No longer must qualitative research
Glaser had supplied much of the original be a mysterious endeavor conducted by
logic and form of grounded theory. Theoreti- anointed elites. Qualitative research could
cal Sensitivity (1978) depicted his concept- spread beyond the confines of Chicago and
indicator logic and focus on core variables. its reach. Moreover, all qualitative research-
Beyond Glaser and Strausss (1967) original ers could aspire to theorizing and achieve
statement, however, Strausss Qualitative their goals by following a handful of flexible
Analysis for Social Scientists (1987) and guidelines.
Strauss and Corbins Basics of Qualitative Re- Because grounded theory was decidedly
search (1990, 1998) brought grounded the- inductive, scholars commonly viewed it as a
400 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

social constructionist method. Yet was it? ternal reality, the discovery of provisional
Certainly its emphasis on building an analy- truths in this reality, the role of the observer,
sis, studying processes, and attending to and an unproblematic representation of re-
how people create and view their worlds had search participants. Neither belabored accu-
strong social constructionist leanings. racy, but Strausss empirical studies with
Strausss Chicago roots made the method Corbin (Corbin & Strauss, 1984, 1988) dem-
compatible with symbolic interactionist, so- onstrate thorough description and data col-
cial constructionist currents in the disci- lection in the social constructionist tradi-
pline. Both Glaser and Strauss emphasized tion.
emergence, but subtle differences between By the early 1990s, qualitative inquiry in
them may be discerned. Glaser emphasized general and grounded theory in particular
the emergence of the grounded theorists had gained credibility in numerous disci-
ideas through studying the data. Strausss plines. It was a short-lived victory. Contested
use of the term also suggests the influence of views continued to develop as postmod-
George Herbert Meads (1932) analysis of ernists challenged assumptions in social
time. Fundamentally social and temporal theory and qualitative research (see, e.g.,
processes result in the present emerging as Clough, 1992; Daly, 1997; Denzin, 1992).
new and different from the past. Grounded theory came to exemplify the crit-
By 1990, grounded theory had become icisms these scholars leveled at ethnography
something of an orthodoxy (see Bryant & and qualitative research more generally.
Charmaz, 2007). Strauss and Corbins (1990, Traditional qualitative research had roots in
1998) book fostered an orthodox viewbut Enlightenment values, including beliefs in
it differed from Glaser and Strausss original reason, objectivity, scientific authority, and
statement and undermined Glasers empha- notions of progress through science.
sis on emergent codes and categories and, in Grounded theory became known as the
his view, diminished his considerable contri- most realist and positivist of the modernist
bution to the classic statement of grounded qualitative methods (Van Maanen, 1988).
theory. Glaser (1992) objected and asked For postmodernists, grounded theory epito-
for retraction of the book. Other scholars mized distanced inquiry by objective experts
framed the differences between Glaser and who assumed their training licensed them to
Strauss and Corbin as a debate, although define and represent research participants.
the latter two did not respond publicly to Glaser (1992) reappeared in methodological
Glasers charges. No debate followed from discussions and reaffirmed his objectivist
Strauss and Corbin. To date, perhaps the stance; however, his views have exerted
closest statement to a response came from more influence in professional disciplines
Corbin (1998) after Strausss death. Other such as nursing and management than in the
scholars (Atkinson, Coffey, & Delamont, social sciences.
2003; Charmaz, 2000; La Rossa, 2005; The postmodernist turn renewedand in-
Locke, 1997; Kelle, 2005), however, gave the tensified and generalizedepistemological
differences between the two versions sub- critiques that theorists and several qualita-
stantial discussion and debate from the tive sociologists had made in the 1960s
1990s to the present, particularly in nursing (Berger & Luckmann, 1966; Bruyn, 1966;
(see, e.g., Boychuk Duchscher & Morgan, Cicourel, 1964).7 Postmodernist critiques
2004; May, 1996; Melia, 1996; Stern, 1994; challenged positivist assumptions in classic
Wilson & Hutchinson, 1996). grounded theory statements and questioned
Although Glasers version of grounded its continued relevance. As a form of natu-
theory differed from that of Strauss and ralist inquiry (Gubrium & Holstein, 1997;
Corbin in conception and concrete strate- Lincoln & Guba, 1985; Lofland & Lofland,
gies, they shared basic premises about an ex- 1995), critics included grounded theory
The Grounded Theory Method 401

among those approaches castigated as The Constructionist Renewal


epistemologically naive, voyeuristic, and in- of Grounded Theory
trusive in the lives of the research partici-
pants (see, e.g., Clough, 1992). From post-
modernist perspectives, the underlying Postmodern challenges from without com-
assumptions in earlier grounded theory bined with positivistic inclinations from
statements mirrored a modernist epistemol- within grounded theory spurred efforts to
ogy. Simultaneously, the narrative turn theo- reclaim its strategies for social construction-
rized and valorized respondents full stories, ist inquiry. Those of us who adhered to a rel-
unlike the grounded theory strategy of using ativist epistemology never concurred with
excerpts of their stories to build theoretical grounding grounded theory in Glasers mid-
statements. Not surprisingly, some sociolo- 20th-century positivism. Strausss students
gists who had previously adopted grounded and colleagues (see, e.g., Charmaz, 1991,
theory methods (Ellis, 1995; Richardson, 2000; Clarke, 1998, 2005; Lempert, 1997;
1993; Riessman, 1990) sought new ap- Maines, 1984; Reif, 1975) particularly im-
proaches. bued grounded theory with social construc-
Other critics either misunderstood or re- tionism, whether or not they articulated
jected grounded theory emphases on theory epistemological reasons for their actions.
building rather than storytelling and on a No doubt, for some, grounded theory was
particular process or problem rather than inherently social constructionist; yet, para-
on the whole of research participants lives. doxically, Strauss and Corbins methodolog-
In actuality, few grounded theory studies ical procedures gave grounded theory an
build theory, but many provide an analytic objectivist cast.
handle on a specific experience. Still, the
growing emphasis on storytelling caused The ObjectivistConstructionist Dichotomy
some critics to question grounded theorists
use of data and their representation of re- Those grounded theorists who endorse a so-
search participants, and other critics dis- cial constructionism informed by recent
dained grounded theory analytic practices epistemological critiques have made explicit
and claims to scientific authority. efforts to distinguish between key grounded
Most critics could not see beyond Glaser theory strategies and their positivist anteced-
and Strausss (1967) early statements of the ents (see, e.g., Bryant, 2002, 2003;
grounded theory methodand other critics Castallani, Castallani, & Spray, 2003;
still cannot (Dey, 1999, 2004; Layder, 1998). Charmaz, 2000, 2002, 2005, 2006; Clarke,
As a result, until recently (Bryant, 2002; 2003, 2005; Henwood & Pidgeon, 2003;
Charmaz, 2000, 2002, 2005, 2006; Clarke, Mills, Bonner, & Francis, 2006; Seale, 1999).
2003, 2005, 2006; Henwood & Pidgeon, Numerous scholars have merged grounded
2003; Willig, 2001) the flexibility and po- theory strategies with the positivism in-
tential versatility of the method remained herent in Glasers (1978, 1992, 1998) and
hiddenand its promise for innovative so- Strauss and Corbins (1990, 1998) versions
cial constructionist study remained unful- of the method.
filled. By fusing grounded theory strategies Grounded theory strategies are just that
with the way Glaser and Strauss had used the strategies for creating and interrogating our
method, critics had relegated grounded the- data, not routes to knowing an objective
ory to being an outdated modernist method. external reality. Objectivist versions of
Discarding grounded theory guidelines, grounded theory assume a single reality
along with Glaser and Strausss objectivist that a passive, neutral observer discovers
assumptions, precluded revitalizing the through value-free inquiry. Assumptions of
method through social constructionism. objectivity and neutrality make data selec-
402 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

tion, collection, and representation unprob- nize and conceptualize data but does not
lematic; they become givens, rather than render the data objective, as Glaser (2003)
constructions that occur during the research asserts. From my constructionist view, objec-
process, and they shape its outcome. A naive tivity is a questionable goal, and what re-
empiricism results. Objectivists assume that searchers define as objective still reflects
data are self-evident and speak for them- partial knowledge and particular perspec-
selves. Possibilities of partial, limited, or tives, priorities, and positions. Subjectivities
missing data and multiple readings of them are embedded in data analysis, as well as in
remain unseen (see also Clarke, 2005, 2006). data collection. Methodological procedures
Objectivists aim to generalize through ab- neither make research objective nor pre-
stractions that separate the completed clude responsibility to locate research rela-
grounded theory from the conditions and tive to time, place, and situation. Grounded
contingencies of its data collection and anal- theorists awareness of the relativism in re-
ysis (see Glaser, 1998, 2001). As abstraction search practice fosters their reflexivity about
increases, so does decontextualization of the how they construct their actions. Both
research that gave rise to this abstraction. constructionist and objectivist versions of
Objectivists seek generalizations that pro- grounded theory adopt a realist position,
vide explanations and predictions. The com- but constructionists view learning about and
pleted grounded theory aims for fit, work, portraying the studied world as problematic.
relevance, and modifiability (Glaser, 1978). This constructionist version of grounded
My constructionist approach makes the theory redirects the method from its ob-
following assumptions: (1) Reality is multi- jectivist, mid-20th-century past and aligns it
ple, processual, and constructedbut con- with 21st-century epistemologies (Charmaz,
structed under particular conditions8; (2) 2000, 2006).9 Rather than assuming that the-
the research process emerges from inter- ory emerges from data, constructionists as-
action; (3) it takes into account the re- sume that researchers construct categories
searchers positionality, as well as that of the of the data. Instead of aiming to achieve par-
research participants; (4) the researcher and simonious explanations and generalizations
researched coconstruct the datadata are a devoid of context, constructionists aim for
product of the research process, not simply an interpretive understanding of the studied
observed objects of it. Researchers are part phenomenon that accounts for context. As
of the research situation, and their posi- opposed to giving priority to the re-
tions, privileges, perspectives, and interac- searchers views, constructionists see partici-
tions affect it (Charmaz, 2000, 2006; Clarke, pants views and voices as integral to the
2005, 2006). In this approach, research al- analysisand its presentation.
ways reflects value positions. Thus the prob- These differences between objectivist and
lem becomes identifying these positions and constructionist grounded theory offer re-
weighing their effect on research practice, searchers a frame to clarify their starting as-
not denying their existence. Similarly, social sumptions and research actions. In practice,
constructionists disavow the idea that re- however, grounded theory inquiry ranges
searchers can or will begin their studies with- between objectivist and constructionist
out prior knowledge and theories about approaches and has elements of both.
their topics. Rather than being a tabula rasa, Objectivist grounded theory strategies en-
constructionists advocate recognizing prior courage researchers to be active analysts of
knowledge and theoretical preconceptions their data. The reflexivity and relativity in
and subjecting them to rigorous scrutiny. this constructionist approach fosters taking
The comparative method inherent in researchers several steps further through
grounded theory helps researchers to scruti- critically examining their construction of the
The Grounded Theory Method 403

research process as they seek to analyze how The second principle follows. To make
their research participants construct their these kinds of decisions, researchers must
lives (Charmaz, 2006). think through what they are doing and how
and why they are doing it. Such thinking im-
plicates the researcher, who does not stand
Enacting 21st-Century
outside the studied process but is a part of it,
Constructionist Principles
as I detail subsequently. Reflexivity is central
Reconstructing grounded theory with 21st- to this constructionist revision and renewal
century methodological sensibilities can pre- of grounded theory. The scrutiny that
serve a grounded theory while simulta- grounded theorists give their method and
neously answering varied criticisms of the by extensionthemselves leads to the third
method. When stripped of their epistemo- principle: improvising their methods and
logical clothing, Glaser and Strausss (1967) analytic strategies.
original flexible strategies still make for The fourth principle assumes that in or-
sound research practice that researchers can der to understand how research participants
invoke to produce usefuland innovative construct their world, researchers need to
social constructionist analyses. know that world from their participants
A 21st-century social constructionist standpoints (Blumer, 1969; Goffman, 1989).
grounded theory rests on certain principles, Invoking grounded theory as a quick and
as I have implied earlier. Thus grounded dirty method impedes gaining this under-
theorists who adhere to this position: standing because achieving it includes defin-
ing tacit meanings and implicit actions, as
Treat the research process itself as a social well as what is directly observable and explic-
construction itly stated. Obtaining thorough, rich data, in
contrast, facilitates seeking and seeing tacit
Scrutinize research decisions and directions
meanings and actions and constructing use-
Improvise methodological and analytic ful grounded theories, as the subsequent re-
strategies throughout the research pro- search accounts attest.
cess
Collect sufficient data to discern and doc-
ument how research participants con- Social Constructionism
struct their lives and worlds. in Grounded Theory
Explicating a Basic Social Process
In brief, the first principle means that us-
ing grounded theory involves more than ap- Studying a basic social process isor wasa
plying a recipe for qualitative research. This fundamental objective of classic grounded
principle belies the current notion of treat- theory method.10 How do grounded theo-
ing the grounded theory method as some- rists go about it? How might a social con-
thing to apply and then treating the analysis structionist approach inform their research?
as something a computer program com- Several studies in the sociology of science
piles. Using grounded theory strategies exemplify adopting a social constructionist
means responding to emergent questions, approach in grounded theory (see, e.g.,
new insights, and further information and Baszanger, 1998; Bowker & Star, 1999;
simultaneously constructing the method of Clarke, 1998). As a case in point, I analyze
analysis, as well as the analysis. No set of Susan Leigh Stars (1989) grounded theory
rules can dictate what a researcher needs to in Regions of the Mind: Brain Research and the
do and when he or she needs to do it (see Quest for Scientific Certainty. In this book, she
Sanders, 1995). adopts social constructionist logic in her ar-
404 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

gument about how scientific theories be- that medical researchers and clinicians de-
come entrenched. manded accurate textbooks and atlases
By looking at scientific work in a specific of typical neurological conditions. Star
area and era, Star (1989) reconstructs what (pp. 8990) writes:
happened and how it occurred and simulta-
neously constructs a theoretical argument In the process of resolving taxonomic uncer-
about scientific theorizing. She pieces to- tainty, researchers thus created typical pictures
gether how 19th-century brain researchers, of diseases that were eagerly adopted by the
the localizationists, constructed certainty medical community. These representations in-
about their theory. These early brain re- clude functional anatomical mapssuch as
searchers earned the name localizationists be- maps that could indicate the anatomical point
in the brain that was the source of loss of
cause they contended that local areas of the
speech. These maps became substitutes, in the
brain controlled specific neurological func-
building of localization theory, for case data
tions. Consistent with classical grounded that contained irregular or anomalous find-
theory (Glaser, 1978; Glaser & Strauss, ings. The demand for functional anatomical
1967), Star defines a process, creating and representations in medical education, diagno-
maintaining certainty (1989, p. 87), and sis, and texts represented a market intolerant
identifies subprocesses constructed through of ambiguity and of individual differences. The
individual and collective actions that con- theory became unambiguously packaged into
stitute the major process. Localizationists the atlas. The ideal types represented in such
transformed the uncertainty that they wit- maps were presented as context-independent
nessed in their laboratories and clinics into (that is, as the brain, not a brain).
what Star calls global certainty at the institu-
tional level (p. 87). She addresses what and In the preceding excerpt, the relationship
how questions here. In examining the mech- between interaction and action with the sub-
anisms of transformation, Star scrutinizes sequent result is clear. The demand came
what localizationists dida processand first; a neurology textbook followed that
how they did itactions. Thus, she analyzes contained functional atlases, which erased
how localizationists ordinary actions accom- anomalies and ambiguities. The subsequent
plished this institutional transformation widespread adoption of the textbook made
and, simultaneously, rendered local contra- the localizationists views the standard in the
dictions invisible. field. The ideal type had become more than
Through studying her data, Star (1989) a source of comparison; it became the only
defines a set of actions that, taken together, serious measure. Thus Star (1989) implies
accomplished the hegemony of localization that these early neurologists had accom-
theory of the brain. To create and maintain plished significant boundary work that pre-
certainty, localizationists engaged in the fol- vented other theories of brain function to be
lowing actions: borrowing evidence from entertained.
other fields, evaluating their operational Stars attention to the sequencing of ac-
procedures rather than actual technical fail- tion reveals the interconnections between
ures, substituting ideal clinical pictures for knotty work problems and localizationists
anomalous findings, generalizing from case attempts to resolve them. Establishing an
results, and reducing epistemological ques- ideal typical clinical picture through the text-
tions to debates about technique (Star, 1989, book atlas is just one kind of action the
pp. 8793). Stars depiction of how localiza- localizationists undertook. Star similarly
tionists substituted ideal types for irregular traces how localizationists routinely con-
cases exemplifies key dimensions of her re- structed each kind of the aforementioned
construction of their emergent construc- actions in which they engaged. These ac-
tions of views and actions. She points out tions arose in the exigencies of problem solv-
The Grounded Theory Method 405

ing at work. Localizationists other actions synthesizes what localizationists did and how
reflected how they acted on their profes- they did it in one clear, direct statement:
sional ideologies by explicitly constructing Localizationists eventually intertwined
strategies to defeat brain diffusionists op- questions about the nature of phenomena,
posing theory of brain function. the strategies for organizing information
Note how Star (1989) moves from action and resources, and political commitments
to outcome in the excerpt. Earlier in the (p. 196). Then, to end her book, she raises
book, she provides the historical, profes- why questions and answers them in the fol-
sional, and work contexts in which the lowing discussion of the implications of ana-
reader can situate the actions she describes lyzing scientific work:
in this section. Hence she can move directly
to delineating the conditions under which The Implications of Analyzing Science
as Work
actions arose. Clinicians urgently needed to
Research on scientific theories has rarely
make definitive diagnoses. Brain research-
taken into account the processes in dimen-
ers needed to categorize diseases accurately. sions described above, especially the degree
They both sought certainty. The lack of tol- with which these complex multiple dimensions
erance for ambiguity made localization the- are interactive and developmental. What are
ory appealing. Later, Star tells us that local- the implications of looking at theories in this
izationists financial sponsors also pressed way? A conversation with Anselm Strauss pro-
for generality and standardization. When vided a partial answer to this question. As I was
the sponsors referees found irregular find- describing to him the many participants in the
ings in localizationists experimental re- debate about localization, and the various
ports, they requested that the localizationists kinds of work and uncertainties faced by par-
ticipants, I began to frame the concept, iner-
standardize their existing results rather than
tia. I saw the questions becoming extraordi-
redo the experiments. Here, significant ex-
narily complex and, at the same time, taken for
ternal bodies buttress the construction of granted by participants. In the middle of ex-
facts, and subsequently having their impri- plaining this, and when I was feeling over-
matur on the written reports serves to reify whelmed with the complexity and interdepen-
this construction. dence of all the issues, Strauss asked me: what
Star (1989) makes a strong case for accept- would it have taken to overthrow the theory?
ing her interpretation of what localization- (p. 196)
ists did and how they did it. She weaves spe-
cific evidence and telling incidents through By addressing what overthrowing the the-
her narrative that support her assertions. ory would have taken and when it could have
The range and thoroughness of her evi- occurred, Star answers why it did not. More-
dence make her argument compelling. She over, by showing how localization became
specifies how actions construct processes and remained entrenched, she offers a new
and answers what and how questions. Stars explanation of change and stability in scien-
use of grounded theory logic and construc- tific theorizing. Stars strong answers to how
tion of categories is transparent at this level. questions provide the foundation for
However, Star does not stop with what and advancing why questions. Throughout the
how questions. As she merges processes into book, she pieces together diverse sources of
major categories and chapter titles, she evidence that permit her to trace chronol-
brings the reader back to her major topics ogy and to make connections between ac-
and places them on center stage. Subse- tions, incidents, and outcomes.
quently, the grounded theory style and logic Star (1989) presents an analysis thor-
recede to the backstage. Rather than pro- oughly grounded in data. Her sorting and
vide a parsimonious statement of relation- categorizing of data make sense. She takes
ships between abstracted categories, Star simple, direct, but intermediate categories
406 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

as her headings and subheadings such as approach is an equation between knowing and
Diplomacy (p. 134), Compiling Credibil- working. These two kinds of events do not pro-
ity (p. 138), Manipulating Hierarchies of ceed in parallel: they are the same activity, but
Credibility (p. 140), Organizational Tac- differently reported. (1989, p. 197)
tics (p. 144), Controlling the Focus of the
Debate (p. 145), and Modes of Debate and Adapting Constructionist Grounded Theory
Tacit Debates (p. 152) to build an abstract for General Audiences
analysis. Star describes and explains each
category and often details a series of actions Grounded theory, particularly in its con-
that constitutes the category, as she did with structionist versions, can serve audiences in
Creating and Maintaining Certainty, dis- multiple disciplines and beyond the acad-
cussed earlier. Most of these intermediate emy. As many critics have observed, authors
categories are gerunds; they depict actions. often claim that grounded theory guided
As such, the categories not only give the their inquiry, but their work bears no resem-
reader a sense of peoples intentions and blance to it. Other authors use the method
concerns, but they also specify and anchor but do not claim it.12 And numerous others
the analysis. When Star uses gerunds, her adopt a couple of strategies, such as coding
categories provide more information and a and some kind of memo writing, but do not
clearer point of view than her other catego- engage in theoretical sampling or explica-
ries. They enliven her narrative and inform tion of a major category.
the reader of its direction. Taken together, Monica J. Caspers (1998) book on fetal
Stars intermediate categories outline her surgery, The Making of the Unborn Patient: A
chapters and organize her argument. Social Anatomy of Fetal Surgery, acknowledges
Like other qualitative researchers, the influence of grounded theorists Clarke
grounded theorists are often deservedly crit- and Strauss, but its grounded theory origins
icized for moving too quickly from the spe- are less clear than Stars. Nonetheless, Cas-
cific study to a general level.11 The strength per based her book on her dissertation,
of Stars analysis permits her to move from which used grounded theory. Like many au-
the particular case of localization theory to thors, Casper outlines her diverse sources of
considering why scientific theories do or data for her multisite ethnography, but she
do not change. Star challenges Thomas S. does not claim grounded theory analytic
Kuhns (1970) explanation that a critical strategies.
mass of anomalous findings forces a para- The social constructionism stands out in
digm change. In contrast, she shows that Caspers book, from the title through the
practical negotiations with and about analysis. Proponents have created fetal sur-
anomalous events are constitutive of science gery and, with surgical techniques, have cre-
at every level of organization (Star, 1989, ated the unborn patient. Making the unborn
p. 64). Star closes her book with the follow- into viable patients deserving of surgical in-
ing explanation of the significance of her terventions took sustained effort, which con-
study: tinues to be subject to disagreement and de-
bate. Fetal surgery is not simply a natural
sequel of medical progress; rather, it
The study of how theories take hold and be-
emerges from political advocacy, collective
come seen as natural is important in answer-
support, creation of a market, and cultural
ing some basic questions in the sociology
of knowledge and epistemology. This book values. The notion of the unborn patient
argues that problems/theories/facts/perspec- and the legitimacy of fetal surgery are both
tives are a form of collective behavior, and I crafted social constructions that occurred
have provided some data about the processes within a particular historical moment and
and conditions of that behavior. Implicit in this entered into larger public debates about re-
The Grounded Theory Method 407

productive politics. Informed consent is not took a critical stance toward their work.
simply signed and documented. The con- Contested positions surrounding a topic
sent form itself is manufactured after many such as fetal surgery, however, can force the
discussions and iterations, but it often im- researcher to maintain a problematic view of
plies that the procedure represents the last the data and not uncritically accept one or
hope and understates its risks and conse- another position, including ones own.
quences. Several commitments shaped Caspers
Casper (1998) builds a detailed construc- work. She locates her work as contributing
tionist story and places herself and her mul- to the dialogue of feminist scholars who had
tiple positions and situations in it. She ac- begun to theorize the fetus and to keep
knowledges multiple actors and contested women in their theories. Thus this perspec-
realities, her struggles with rendering them, tive leads her to keep women at the center of
and the relativity of her analysis.13 She began attention. As a result she takes into account
her study as an engaged feminist and argues how fetal surgery affected their lives; she
that no workwhether of fetal surgeons or does not reduce women to passive objects
of sociologistsstands outside of its contexts who were acted upon. Casper acknowledges
(p. 20). She states: that some critics might see her stance as bi-
ased. True, but her work implicitly conveys
I care too much about the issues raised by fetal an alternative interpretation of the conse-
surgery and the unborn patient to assume a po- quences of her perspective. She did not limit
lite, reasonable distance, and instead embrace her study to the boundaries of inquiry set by
a politics of engagement that recognizes my fetal surgeons because theirs erased women
own immersions in the worlds I study. I have as central participants and, by extension,
been moved and transformed by this research
erased questions of the effects of fetal sur-
in multiple ways, and fetal surgery is some-
thing I shall continue to think and talk about
gery on their health.
long after this book is published. My politics Feminist theory and practice gave Casper
and intellectual assumptions have been shaken a series of sensitizing concepts from which
time and again, precisely because fetal surgery to develop. Starting points frame but do not
evokes persistent debates about fetuses, abor- determine the content of constructionist
tion, womens roles, the health-care system, grounded theory. Thus Casper remains at-
and rescued technologies. (p. 25) tuned to cultural practices, conceptions of
personhood, and the place of womens bod-
Note how Caspers statement corre- ies and health in the unfolding scenarios
sponds with constructivist assumptions. She that she witnessed. Caspers feminist per-
acknowledges her starting points and con- spective no doubt informed her of earlier
tinued immersion in this world as a social ac- lengthy debates between prochoice and anti-
tor. Yet Casper also became immersed as a abortion activists about establishing if or
researcher and subsequently found her when a fetus had human qualities and whose
views challenged and changed. Like the rightsthe mothers or the unbornstook
studied phenomenon, the research process precedence. She detects meanings attached
itself is never neutral or without context. It, to representations of the fetus as a free
too, is an emergent social construction. The agent with its own needs and interests, a
political weight of Caspers topic magnified unique, autonomous individual, a visible
this social construction of the research pro- presence, a separate being from the mother,
cess. Respondents and gatekeepers alike and worthy of protection (Casper, 1998,
quizzed her about her views and commit- p. 16). In keeping with sociological treat-
ments. Some gatekeepers stalled, limited, or ment of work, Casper aims to show how fetal
refused access to data. Others welcomed surgery is a particular type of work that oc-
Casper into their worlds knowing that she curred in special work sites.
408 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

Caspers book tells a complex tale and in- Summary and Conclusion
volves multiple types of data, ranging from
documents to oral histories to firsthand ob- Throughout this chapter, I have built an ar-
servations. How might its grounded theory gument explaining how and why social con-
underpinnings be discerned? First, Casper structionists can adopt grounded theory
sees the history of fetal surgery as a socially guidelines to deepen and broaden their
constructed process and titles a chapter analyses and thus address why questions. A
Breaching the Womb. Second, she inserts social constructionist approach to grounded
telling in vivo codes into the headings and theory encourages researchers to make mea-
subheadings of her chapters. Among them sured assessments of their methods and of
are: A Bona Fide Patient (p. 51), Not themselves as researchers. A close attention
Gods Will (p. 67), A Spirit of Coopera- to what and how questions builds the founda-
tion (p. 110), Folks Are Always Rubbing tion for moving to why questions, as Stars
Shoulders (p.115), and Its a Reality (1989) analysis demonstrates. Thus social
Dump (p.151). Third, Casper shows how constructionists can invoke the generalizing
actions, conditions, and contingencies con- logic of objectivist grounded theory but do
tribute to the larger processes of conducting so in full view of their measured assess-
and legitimizing fetal surgery. ments, not in absence of them. The result
Does Casper develop complex grounded promises to be a nuanced analysis that ac-
theory categories? Does she explain one knowledges and analyzes positionality and
core variable? Does she offer precise gener- partiality, as Caspers (1998) analysis testi-
alizations abstracted from their sources? No. fies. The subsequent social constructionist
Casper skillfully constructs the social con- analysis resists the tendency in objectivist
struction of the unborn patient; her theoriz- grounded theory to oversimplify, erase dif-
ing remains embedded in the narrative. She ferences, overlook variation, and assume
presents a complex analysis of complicated neutrality throughout inquiry. Simulta-
worlds, and does so in accessible terms. Al- neously, this analysis grapples with why ques-
though Caspers use of grounded theory tions and offers qualified explanations.
bears little resemblance to objectivist Grounded theory is a method of explica-
grounded theory, it contains crucial ele- tion and emergence. The method itself ex-
ments of social constructionism consistent plicates the kinds of analytic guidelines that
with my approach. These elements include many qualitative researchers implicitly
(1) the attention to context; (2) the locating adopt. It also fosters explicating analytic and
of actors, situations, and actions; (3) the as- methodological decisionseach step along
sumption of multiple realities; and (4) the the way. By explicating their decisions,
subjectivity of the researcher, noted previ- grounded theorists gain control over their
ously. Casper produces an interpretive un- subject matter and their next analytic or
derstanding of the arenas she entered and methodological move. The construction of
points out that both her interpretations and the process, as well as the analytic product, is
the studied scenes could change as emer- emergent. As I stated earlier, immediate exi-
gent contingencies unfold. gencies in the field and concerns of gate-
We must look at Caspers purposes to un- keepers and participants affect this construc-
derstand her strategies. From the start, she tion, and the contextual positioning of the
aimed to write a book free from the esoteric research frames it. All become grist for anal-
obscurity of academic discourse. Thus she ysis. In short, when social constructionists
intended to make her book a vibrant specific combine their attention to context, action,
sociological story anchored to a larger story and interpretation with grounded theory an-
of contemporary politics and culture, and alytic strategies, they can produce dense
she fulfilled her goal. analyses with explanatory power, as well as
The Grounded Theory Method 409

conceptual understanding. Simultaneously, lished Studies in Ethnomethodology in 1967. Peter


their analyses attest to how furthering the so- Berger and Thomas Luckmanns The Social Con-
cial constructionist elements in grounded struction of Reality (1966) came out almost simulta-
theory strengthen the method. neously with The Discovery of Grounded Theory
(Glaser & Strauss, 1967) and built on the phe-
nomenological tradition of Alfred Schutz (1967).
In contrast, Strausss social constructionism drew
Acknowledgments on the pragmatist and symbolic interactionist tra-
ditions of Blumer (1969), Dewey (1958), Mead
I thank Jay Gubrium and Jim Holstein for their care- (1932, 1934), and Peirce (1958). These three de-
ful reviews of an earlier draft and Tina Balderrama velopments remained relatively independent of
for helping with the references. each other. Neither Strauss nor Glaser was influ-
enced by the other developments, but Strauss re-
mained in frequent contact with his Chicago
Notes school colleagues. Much of Strausss (1993) and
Corbin and Strausss (1984, 1988) subsequent re-
1. David Silverman (2005) has made a similar search and writing contained strong construc-
argument about qualitative research. He con- tionist elements; Glasers much less so.
tends that by studying phenomena that occur nat- 4. A number of works describe the method
urally, qualitative researchers can define how and its variations, so I do not detail them here.
interaction ensues and what meanings it holds. See Charmaz (2000, 2003, 2006); Clarke (2005,
For Silverman, answering the how and what 2006); Glaser (1978, 1998, 2003); Glaser and
questions must precede the why questions. Strauss (1967); Strauss (1987); Strauss and
2. In earlier works, I have referred to my ap- Corbin (1990, 1998).
proach as constructivist grounded theory to distin- 5. Their students locations also influenced
guish it from objectivist iterations. The present the dissemination of grounded theory. Many
chapter continues my earlier approach but more of the University of California, San Fran-
frames the discussion under the more general ru- cisco, nursing doctoral students of the early years
bric of social constructionism to be consistent later took positions in doctoral training programs
with the purpose of this volume. Constructivist in their profession than did the sociology stu-
grounded theory assumes relativity, acknowl- dents of the same era. Graduate programs in
edges standpoints, and advocates reflexivity. My nursing emerged and expanded from the mid-
use of constructivism assumes the existence of an 1970s through the 1980s, whereas positions in
obdurate, real world that may be interpreted in graduate sociology programs shrunk.
multiple ways. I do not subscribe to the radical 6. Paradoxically, the social constructionist
subjectivism assumed by some advocates of logic of Corbin and Strausss (1988) empirical
constructivism. Consistent with Marx, I assume work often is apparent.
that people make their worlds but do not make
7. See Bryant and Charmaz (2007) for a dis-
them as they please. Rather, worlds are con-
cussion of the epistemological climate of the mid-
structed under particular historical and social
1960s.
conditions that shape our views, actions, and col-
lective practices. Constructivist grounded theory 8. I come close to the Marxist view of history
(Bryant, 2002; Charmaz, 2000, 2002, 2005, 2006; here because I acknowledge human agency but
Clarke, 2003, 2005, 2006) has fundamental assert that it always occurs within a preexisting so-
epistemological roots in sociological social cial frame with its constraintsof which we may
constructionism. My position on social construc- be unaware and which may not be of our choos-
tionist grounded theory in this chapter relies on ing (see also Charmaz, in press).
the preceding definition and its premises. 9. My subsequent comparisons draw on
3. They did claim that their method was phe- Charmaz (in press).
nomenological (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Social 10. Now Glaser (2003) disavows his earlier in-
constructionist approaches had a long and varied sistence on finding and studying a basic social
history but moved to the forefront of qualitative process. I have long argued that the quest for a
sociology in the late 1960s. Harold Garfinkel pub- basic social process can mislead the researcher or
410 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

mask many processes, and therefore I agree with Bryant, A., & Charmaz, K. (2007). Grounded theory in
his recent view (see also Clarke, 2005). historical perspective: An epistemological account.
In A. Bryant & K. Charmaz (Eds.), The Sage handbook
11. Any qualitative study without extensive
of grounded theory. London: Sage.
data can make only limited claims; small inter-
Casper, M. (1998). The making of the unborn patient: A so-
view studies that make general claims stand on cial anatomy of fetal surgery. New Brunswick, NJ:
shaky ground. The generality of the claims needs Rutgers University Press.
to be proportionate to the thoroughness of the Castallani, B., Castallani, J., & Spray, L. (2003).
data collection. Grounded neural networking: Modeling complex
12. The genre matters here. Academic disci- quantitative data. Symbolic Interaction, 23, 577589.
plines and journals vary in their prescriptions for Charmaz, K. (1991). Good days, bad days: The self in
methodological detail. Many require authors to chronic illness and time. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
University Press.
specify their logic of sampling and data collec-
Charmaz, K. (2000). Constructivist and objectivist
tion, but not their analytic strategies. Books differ
grounded theory. In N. K. Denzin & Y. Lincoln
markedly in the amount and complexity of meth- (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed.,
odological explanation, depending on the pub- pp. 509535). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
lisher and projected audience. Trade and cross- Charmaz, K. (2002). Grounded theory analysis. In J. F.
over books (those published as scholarly works Gubrium & J. A. Holstein (Eds.), Handbook of inter-
that will reach general educated audiences) sel- view research (pp. 675694). Thousand Oaks, CA:
dom provide more than minimal information Sage.
and may not include a methodological section or Charmaz, K. (2003). Grounded theory. In J. A. Smith
appendix. (Ed.), Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to re-
search methods (pp. 81110). London: Sage.
13. These dimensions of Caspers work align
Charmaz, K. (2005). Grounded theory in the 21st cen-
her with the constructivist grounded theory that I tury: A qualitative method for advancing social jus-
have previously delineated. tice research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. E. Lincoln (Eds.)
Handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 507
535). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
References Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A prac-
tical guide through qualitative analysis. London: Sage.
Atkinson, P., Coffey, A., & Delamont, S. (2003). Key Charmaz, K. (in press). Reconstructing grounded the-
themes in qualitative research: Continuities and changes. ory. In L. Bickman, P. Alasuutari, & J. Brannen
New York: Rowman & Littlefield. (Eds.), Handbook of social research. London: Sage.
Baszanger, I. (1998). Inventing pain medicine: From the Cicourel, A. V. (1964). Method and measurement in sociol-
laboratory to the clinic. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers ogy. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
University Press. Clarke, A. E. (1998). Disciplining reproduction: Modernity,
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social con- American life sciences, and the problems of sex. Berkeley:
struction of reality. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. University of California Press.
Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism. Englewood Clarke, A. E. (2003). Situational analyses: Grounded
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. theory mapping after the postmodern turn. Symbolic
Bowker, G. C., & Star, S. L. (1999). Sorting things out: Interaction, 26, 553576.
Classification and its consequences. Cambridge, MA: Clarke, A. E. (2005). Situational analysis: Grounded the-
MIT Press. ory after the postmodern turn. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Boychuk Duchscher, J. E., & Morgan, D. (2004). Sage.
Grounded theory: Reflections on the emerging vs. Clarke, A. E. (2006). Feminisms, grounded theory, and
forcing debate. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 48(6), situational analysis. In S. Hess-Biber & D. Leckenby
605612. (Eds.), Handbook of feminist research methods (pp. 345
Bruyn, S. T. (1966). The human perspective in sociology: 370). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
The methodology of participant observation. Englewood Clough, P. T. (1992). The end(s) of ethnography: From real-
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. ism to social criticism. Newbury Park: Sage.
Bryant, A. (2002). Re-grounding grounded theory. Jour- Corbin, J. (1998). Comment: Alternative interpretations
nal of Information Technology Theory and Application, valid or not? Theory and Psychology, 8(1), 121128.
4(1), 2542. Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. L. (1984). Collaboration: Cou-
Bryant, A. (2003, January). A constructive/ist re- ples working together to manage chronic illness. Im-
sponse to Glaser. FQS: Forum for Qualitative Social age, 4, 109115.
Research, 4(1). Retrieved March 14, 2003, from Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. L. (1988). Unending work and
www.qualitative-research.net/fqs/-texte/1-03/1-03bryant-e. care: Managing chronic illness at home. San Francisco:
htm Jossey-Bass.
The Grounded Theory Method 411

Daly, K. (1997). Replacing theory in ethnography: A Layder, D. (1998). Sociological practice: Linking theory and
postmodern view. Qualitative Inquiry, 3(3), 343365. social research. London: Sage
Denzin, N. K. (1992). Symbolic interactionism and cultural Lempert, L. (1997). The other side of help: The nega-
studies: The politics of interpretation. Oxford, UK: Basil tive effects of help seeking processes of abused
Blackwell. women. Qualitative Research, 20, 289309.
Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Preface. In N. K. Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry.
Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
research (pp. ixxii). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Locke, K. (1997). Rewriting the discovery of grounded
Dewey, J. (1958). Experience and nature. New York: Do- theory after 25 years? Journal of Management Inquiry,
ver. 5(1), 239245.
Dey, I. (1999). Grounding grounded theory. San Diego, Lofland, J., & Lofland, L. (1995). Analyzing social set-
CA: Academic Press. tings: A guide to qualitative observation and analysis.
Dey, I. (2004). Grounded theory. In C. Seale, G. Gobo, Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
J. F. Gubrium, & D. Silverman (Eds.), Qualitative re- Maines, D. (1984). The social arrangements of diabetic
search practice (pp. 8093). London: Sage. self-help groups. In A. L. Strauss, J. Corbin, S.
Ellis, C. (1995). Emotional and ethical quagmires of re- Fagerhaugh, B. G. Glaser, D. Maines, B. Suczek, et al.
turning to the field. Journal of Contemporary Ethnogra- (Eds.), Chronic illness and the quality of life (2nd ed.,
phy, 24(1), 6898. pp. 111126). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. May, K. (1996). Diffusion, dilution or distillation? The
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. case of grounded theory method. Qualitative Health
Glaser, B. G. (1978). Theoretical sensitivity. Mill Valley, Research, 6(3), 309311.
CA: Sociology Press. Mead, G. H. (1932). The philosophy of the present. La Salle,
Glaser, B. G. (1992). Basics of grounded theory analysis. IL: Open Court.
Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: Uni-
Glaser, B. G. (1998). Doing grounded theory: Issues and dis- versity of Chicago Press.
cussions. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press. Melia, K. M. (1996). Rediscovering Glaser. Qualitative
Glaser, B. G. (2001). The grounded theory perspective: Con- Health Research, 6(3), 368378.
ceptualization contrasted with description. Mill Valley, Mills, J., Bonner, A., & Francis, K. (2006). The develop-
CA: Sociology Press. ment of constructivist grounded theory. Interna-
Glaser, B. G. (2002). Constructivist grounded theory? tional Journal of Qualitative Methods, 5(1), 110.
Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 3(3). Retrieved Peirce, C. S. (1958). Collected papers. Cambridge, MA:
March 15, 2007, from http://www.qualitative-research. Harvard University Press.
net/fqs-texte/3-02/3-02glaser-e-htm Reif, L. (1975). Ulcerative colitis: Strategies for manag-
Glaser, B. G. (2003). Conceptualization contrasted with de- ing life. In A. L. Strauss & B. G. Glaser (Eds.), Chronic
scription. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press. illness and the quality of life (pp. 8188). St. Louis, MO:
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of Mosby.
grounded theory. Chicago: Aldine. Richardson, L. (1993). Interrupting discursive spaces:
Goffman, E. (1989). On fieldwork. Journal of Contempo- Consequences for the sociological self. In N. K.
rary Ethnography, 18, 123132. Denzin (Ed.), Studies in symbolic interaction (Vol. 13,
Gubrium, J. F., & Holstein, J. A. (1997). The new lan- pp. 7784). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
guage of qualitative method. New York: Oxford Univer- Riessman, C. K. (1990). Divorce talk. New Brunswick,
sity Press. NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Henwood, K., & Pidgeon, N. (2003). Grounded theory Sanders, C. R. (1995). Stranger than fiction: Insights
in psychological research. In P. M. Camic, J. E. and pitfalls in post-modern ethnography. In N. K.
Rhodes, & L. Yardley (Eds.), Qualitative research in Denzin (Ed.), Studies in symbolic interaction (Vol. 17,
psychology: Expanding perspectives in methodology and pp. 89104). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
design (pp. 131155). Washington, DC: American Schutz, A. (1967). The phenomenology of the social world.
Psychological Association. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Kelle, U. (2005, May). Emergence vs. forcing of Seale, C. (1999). The quality of qualitative research. Lon-
empirical data?: A crucial problem of grounded don: Sage.
theory reconsidered. Forum: Qualitative Social Re- Silverman, D. (2005, August). Instances or sequences?:
search, 6(2), Art. 27. Retrieved May 30, 2005, from Improving the state of the art of qualitative research.
http/www.qualitative-research.net/fqs.texte-2-05/05-2-27-e. Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 6(3), Art. 30. Re-
htm trieved October 15, 2006, from http://www.
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions qualitative-research.net/fqs-texte/3-05/05-3-30-e.htm
(2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Star, S. L. (1989). Regions of the mind: Brain research and
LaRossa, R. (2005, November). Grounded theory meth- the quest for scientific certainty. Stanford, CA: Stanford
ods and qualitative family research. Journal of Mar- University Press.
riage and Family, 67, 837857. Stern, P. N. (1994). Eroding grounded theory. In J.
412 STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES

Morse (Ed.), Critical issues in qualitative research meth- Strauss, A. L. (1993). Continual permutations of action.
ods (pp. 212223). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative re- Van Maanen, J. (1988). Tales of the field: On writing eth-
search: Grounded theory procedures and techniques. nography. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Willig, C. (2001). Introducing qualitative research in psy-
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative re- chology: Adventures in theory and method. Buckingham,
search: Grounded theory procedures and techniques (2nd UK: Open University Press.
ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Wilson, H. S., & Hutchinson, S. A. (1996).
Strauss, A. L. (1987). Qualitative analysis for social scien- Methodologic mistakes in grounded theory. Nursing
tists. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Research, 45(2), 122124.

You might also like