Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/13739273

Mental Models of Attachment and Coping with


Abortion

Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology March 1998


DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.2.453 Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

57 60

3 authors, including:

Nebi Smer Brenda Major


Middle East Technical University University of California, Santa Barbara
61 PUBLICATIONS 1,222 CITATIONS 156 PUBLICATIONS 12,434 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Nebi Smer on 19 October 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1998, Vol. 74, No. 2, 453-467 0022-3514/98/$3.00

Mental Models of Attachment and Coping With Abortion


Catherine Cozzarelli and Nebi Sumer Brenda Major
Kansas State University University of California, Santa Barbara

This study explored the relationships between mental models of attachment and adjustment to abortion
in 408 women undergoing a lst-trimester abortion at a large free-standing abortion clinic. As expected,
mental models of attachment were related to postabortion distress and positive well-being. These
relationships were mediated by feelings of self-efficacy for coping with abortion, perceived support
from a woman's male partner, and perceived conflict from this same source. Model of self and model
of others interacted only in predicting perceived conflict and positive well-being. Model of self was
more strongly related to the mediator and outcome variables than was model of others. The effects
of model of self, however, were largely a reflection of the overlap between model of self and self-
esteem.

In recent years, mental models of attachment have been shown in romantic relationships are also governed by attachment pro-
to be related to behavior and functioning within romantic rela- cesses and may be similar to those characterizing attachment to
tionships and to affect regulation in times of distress. In the primary caregivers during childhood.
current study, we explored the relationships between mental Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) proposed a model of adult
models of self and others and adjustment to abortion. We ex- attachment based on Bowlby's suggestion that infant-caregiver
pected that mental models would influence postabortion adjust- interactions result in mental models of the self (which reflect
ment by affecting three mediators: perceived social support from the extent to which one views the self as worthy of love) and
a male partner, perceived social conflict from this same source, mental models of others (which reflect the extent to which others
and self-efficacy for coping with abortion. A second goal of this are seen as trustworthy or dependable). Both of these mental
study was to examine the extent to which the impact of model models are thought to vary along a positive-negative continuum
of self on the adjustment process could be attributed to its and, when crossed, to result in four attachment classifications
overlap with self-esteem. (as opposed to the three described in earlier work by Hazan &
Shaver, 1987): Fearful avoidants (who have negative models of
Attachment Theory both self and others) are hypothesized to avoid close relation-
ships because of a fear of rejection; dismissing avoidants (who
According to Bowlby's (1969/1982) infant attachment the- have a positive model of self but a negative model of others)
ory, as a function of interactions with their primary caregivers, are hypothesized to stress the importance of independence and
infants develop stable beliefs or expectations ("mental mod- self-reliance over close relationships; secure individuals (who
els") about their caregiver's responsiveness and about them- have positive models of both self and others) are hypothesized
selves as being worthy or unworthy of love. Attachment theorists to experience a general comfort with closeness and trust in
have conceptualized these beliefs as mental representations of others; and preoccupied individuals (who have a positive model
experience-based knowledge about the self and attachment fig- of others but a negative model of self) are hypothesized to have
ures (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1985; Main, Kaplan, & Cas- an intense desire for emotional intimacy coupled with a general
sidy, 1985). On the basis of the assumption that mental models distrust in others' availability. Research has confirmed that two
are relatively stable throughout the life span, Hazan and Shaver dimensions underlie differences in patterns of adult attachment
(1987) proposed that adults' thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (e.g., Brennan & Shaver, 1995; Brennan, Shaver, & Tobey, 1991;
Collins & Read, 1990; Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992) and
has provided strong support for the validity of Bartholomew and
Horowitz's operationalization of mental models (e.g., Griffin &
Catherine Cozzarelli and Nebi Sumer, Department of Psychology,
Bartholomew, 1994a, 1994b; Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1994).
Kansas State University; Brenda Major, Department of Psychology, Uni-
versity of California, Santa Barbara. Bowlby (1969/1982, 1988) suggested that stressful experi-
Preparation of this article was supported by National Institute of ences are especially likely to prime the attachment system and
Mental Health Grant 5R01MH47989. make attachment processes focal. Research using adult partici-
We thank Marilynn Buckham and the staff of Buffalo GYN Women- pants has corroborated that the attachment system is most
services Clinic, Jill Beck and the staff of Erie Medical Center, and
strongly activated under conditions of high stress (Simpson &
Shalom Press and his medical staff. Without their help and goodwill,
Rholes, 1994; Simpson et al., 1992). Theoretically, once this
this study could not have been conducted.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Cather- system has been activated, individuals with different mental
ine Cozzarelli, Department of Psychology, Bluemont Hall, Kansas State models of attachment will attempt to regulate affect and cope
University, Manhattan, Kansas 66502. Electronic mail may be sent via with a given stressor in ways that reflect the patterns that have
the Internet to psycozz@ksu.edu. been learned or reinforced throughout their relationship histories

453
454 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

(B. C. Feeney & Kirkpatrick, 1996; Kobak & Sceery, 1988; or dismissing) would report higher feelings of self-efficacy for
Mikulincer, Florian, & Tolmacz, 1990; Simpson & Rholes, coping with abortion than those with a negative model of self
1994) and that reflect their basic beliefs about themselves and (those classified as preoccupied or fearful) and that self-efficacy
others. would mediate the relationship between model of self and post-
Although researchers have begun to explore how attachment abortion adjustment (see Major, Richards, Cooper, Cozzarelli, &
processes influence adjustment to negative life events, to date Zubek, 1997, for more on the relationship between self-efficacy
no study has examined the extent to which mental models of the and postabortion adjustment).
self and others (as operationalized by Bartholomew & Horowitz,
1991) impact adjustment within the context of a specific, real-
Social Support
life stressor. (See Mikulincer & Florian, 1995, and Mikulincer,
Florian, & Weller, 1993, for such research using Hazan & Perceived social support can be defined as information or
Shaver's 1987, three-category measure of attachment styles.) actions (real or potential) leading individuals to believe they
Thus, one goal of the present study was to examine the impact are cared for, valued, or in a position to receive help when they
of mental models of the self and others on the process of adjust- need it. Perceived support has a direct, positive effect on health
ment to a real-life stressful event (abortion). Furthermore, al- and can serve a protective or ' 'buffering'' function for individu-
though it has often been acknowledged that model of self and als experiencing high levels of stress (see Cohen & Wills, 1985,
self-esteem are conceptually quite similar, prior research has for a review). In addition, research has shown that perceived
not examined the extent to which these two variables overlap. support, particularly from a woman's male partner, is an im-
Thus, the second major goal of this study was to explore the portant predictor of postabortion adjustment (Major & Cozza-
extent to which the impact of model of self on adjustment to a relli, 1992; Major et al., 1990; Shusterman, 1979). Our prior
stressful event could be attributed to its overlap with self-esteem. research based on the current sample demonstrated that per-
ceived social support from a woman's male partner was associ-
ated with increased levels of postabortion positive well-being,
Mental Models of Attachment and Adjustment
although it was unrelated to postabortion distress (see Major,
to Abortion
Zubek, Cooper, Cozzarelli, & Richards, 1997, for more
The stressful life event examined in this study was an un- information).
wanted pregnancy terminated by abortion, a stressor that is Perceptions of support are based not only on an assessment
experienced by approximately lV2 million American women of the behaviors or intentions of one's social network but also
each year (Russo, Horn, & Schwartz, 1992). Although most on the personality characteristics of the person in need of sup-
women do not appear to experience severe, negative, postabor- port (e.g., Cutrona & Russell, 1990; Dunkel-Schetter & Bennett,
tion psychological responses (see Adler et al., 1992, and Ma- 1990; Hobfoll & Freedy, 1990; Hobfoll, Nadler, & Leiberman,
jor & Cozzarelli, 1992, for reviews), abortion is perceived as 1986). Thus, in general, one would expect that individuals who
an acute stressor by a large number of women. Accordingly, have a positive view both of themselves and of their social
confronting an unintended pregnancy and impending abortion network would be more likely to perceive high levels of social
would be expected to prime the attachment system in the women support than those with one or two negative mental models.
experiencing this event. In addition, given that mental models Only one study, however, has examined this issue. Blain,
of attachment reflect beliefs about both the positive or negative Thompson, and Whiffen (1993) found that secure individuals
characteristics of the self and the likely availability of others perceived more global support from their social networks than
as social resources, mental models of attachment should affect individuals from all three insecure attachment groups. However,
numerous facets of the coping process. Three specific aspects no study has yet examined the impact of mental models of self
of the coping process were examined in the current study: self- and others on perceptions of support within the context of a
efficacy for coping, perceived social support from a male part- specific, real-life stressor (e.g., abortion). Given that positive
ner, and perceived social conflict from this same source. Each models of both self and others underlie the secure attachment
of these is briefly reviewed next. style, we predicted that women who were classified as secure
would perceive higher levels of social support from their male
partners than women in the three insecure attachment groups,
Self-Efficacy each of which are based on at least one negative mental model.
Self-efficacy can be defined as the conviction that one can Furthermore, we hypothesized that perceived support would me-
execute the behaviors required to produce a desired outcome in diate the effects of mental models on postabortion positive well-
a specific situation (Bandura, 1977). In general, individuals who being.
feel efficacious have been found to handle stressful life events
more effectively (Bandura, 1977; Cutrona & Troutman, 1986; Social Conflict
Schiaffino & Revenson, 1992; Smith & Coyne, 1988), including
abortion (Major & Cozzarelli, 1992; Mueller & Major, 1989). Although social relationships can be important sources of
Furthermore, self-efficacy beliefs have been found to mediate warmth and nurturance, in many cases relationships with others
the positive effects of more global personal resources such as can also have negative effects (e.g., be conflicted, time consum-
self-esteem, optimism, and locus of control on adjustment (Coz- ing, or psychologically threatening). Research suggests that con-
zarelli, 1993). In the current study, we hypothesized that individ- flictive interactions (even though they are less frequent) may be
uals with a positive model of self (women classified as secure at least as important as supportive interactions in determining
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 455

adjustment to a variety of life events or may even have a stronger self contributes to the process of adjusting to a negative life
effect than social support on emotional functioning (e.g., event above and beyond the impact of self-esteem. Accordingly,
Coyne & DeLongis, 1986; Lepore, 1992; Rook, 1984; Shinn, the second major aim of the current study was to address this
Lehmann, & Wong, 1984). Our prior research based on the issue. To do so, we examined the effects of model of self on
current sample demonstrated that perceived social conflict from our proposed mediator variables and postabortion adjustment
a woman's male partner was associated with increased levels of with and without self-esteem included in the model as a pre-
postabortion distress, although it was unrelated to postabortion dictor variable. We expected to find that, with self-esteem in-
positive well-being (see Major, Zubek, et al., 1997). cluded in the model, the effects of model of self would be
No prior study has explored the effects of mental models of substantially reduced if not eliminated.
attachment on perceived social conflict during a specific, stress-
ful life event. However, prior research has demonstrated that the Measurement Issues
secure attachment style is associated with more constructive
and adaptive responses to conflict and stress than the insecure Like other measures of attachment, the Bartholomew and Ho-
attachment styles and that secure individuals are the most likely rowitz (1991) measure we used in the current study can be
to respond to stress in ways that elicit constructive or noncon- used either as a categorical measure of attachment "styles" or
frontational behavior from their partners (J. Feeney, Noller, & as an indirect measure of the mental models underlying these
Callan, 1994; Kobak & Hazan, 1991; Levy & Davis, 1988; attachment classifications. Although past researchers have typi-
Pistole, 1989; Simpson & Rholes, 1994; Simpson, Rholes, & cally focused on the effects of attachment "styles," it is our
Phillips, 1996). Thus, consistent with our prediction for social opinion that more weight should be given to analyses based on
support, we hypothesized that secure individuals would perceive mental models. First, we do not agree that individuals come in
lower levels of social conflict from their male partners than discrete ' 'types,'' although attachment categories may serve as
women in the three insecure groups. Furthermore, we hypothe- useful heuristic devices. Second, many of the predictions that
sized that perceived social conflict would mediate the effects of researchers make about attachment styles are based on an analy-
mental models on postabortion distress. sis of the continuous mental models underlying them. Thus, we
believe that testing hypotheses in terms of mental models (rather
To summarize, we hypothesized that mental models would
than styles) allows researchers to more closely map their analyti-
affect how supportive and conflictive a woman perceives her
cal strategy to the conceptual logic underlying their hypotheses.
male partner to be and her self-efficacy for coping with her
Operationalizing attachment patterns as mental models also
abortion. Furthermore, we hypothesized that social support, so-
allows the researcher to contrast more clearly the relative contri-
cial conflict, and self-efficacy would function as mediators of
butions of the two types of mental models to the obtained results.
the relationships between mental models and postabortion
Accordingly, although we present the effects of categorical at-
adjustment.
tachment styles on the mediator and postabortion adjustment
variables, we focus our discussion mainly on analyses in which
Overlap of Mental Model of the Self and Self-Esteem the attachment variables are operationalized as mental models
of the self and others (and their interaction).
The common characterization of model of self as a sense of
worthiness or lovability is quite similar to many definitions of
self-esteem (e.g., Rosenberg, 1965). Not surprisingly, several Overview of Our Hypotheses
empirical studies have found that higher levels of global self-
To recapitulate our hypotheses:
esteem are reported by those with a positive rather than a
negative model of self (e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; 1. Women with a positive model of self (secures and dismissings)
Bylsma, Cozzarelli, & Sumer, in press). In addition, one study were expected to be higher in self-efficacy for coping with abortion
(Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994b) found that a latent variable than those with a negative model of self (those classified as preoccu-
representing model of self overlapped almost perfectly with a pied or fearful).
latent variable representing the self-concept (for which self- 2. Women with positive models of both self and others (those clas-
esteem was a marker variable). Prior research on stress and sified as secure) were expected to perceive higher levels of social
coping has also demonstrated that self-esteem is an important support and lower levels of social conflict from their male partners
predictor of adjustment to negative life events (e.g., Cozzarelli, than those with at least one negative mental model (those in the
1993; Hobfoll & Leiberman, 1987; Hobfoll & London, 1986; three insecure attachment groups).
Pearlin & Schooler, 1978) and that those who are high in self-
3. We expected that perceived support, perceived conflict, and self-
esteem are more likely to feel efficacious (Cozzarelli, 1993)
efficacy for coping with abortion would each mediate the effects
and to perceive high levels of social support from significant of mental models of attachment on adjustment to abortion.
others (e.g., Harter, 1990, 1993) than those who are low in
self-esteem. These findings raise the distinct possibility that the 4. We expected to find that, in large part, the effects of model of
impact of model of self on the stress and coping process derives self could be explained by its conceptual and empirical overlap
primarily from its overlap with self-esteem rather than from any with self-esteem. Thus, we hypothesized that when self-esteem was
added to the path model just described, the effects of model of self
unique properties of a mental model of attachment per se. In-
would be substantially reduced if not eliminated.
deed, although various attachment researchers have noted that
self-esteem and model of self show significant overlap, no study To test Hypotheses 1 to 3, we conducted a path analysis that
to date has explicitly examined the extent to which model of tested the effects of all three mediators simultaneously (see
456 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

Model of Self
Self-Efficacy

Model of Others Support

Interaction Conflict

Figure 1. Proposed path model.

Figure 1 for a diagram of this model). Note that in the model median = 23 years). Sixty-four percent were White and 30.4% were
the predicted secure versus insecure split in social support and African-American; 39.8% were Catholic and 37.6% were Protestant.
social conflict is operationalized as an interaction between Thirty-seven percent of the women were students, 51.3% were employed,
model of self and model of others. Although we made no specific and 17.1% were homemakers. The majority of the women were single
(79.6%), and most were currently raising at least one child (61.1%).
predictions in this regard, we also thought it probable that one
About half of the women were having a first abortion (50.3%). Our
or both of the mental models (or their interaction) would show
sample resembled the population of women obtaining abortions in the
a direct, unmediated effect on postabortion adjustment, because United States (Koonin, Smith, & Ramick, 1993) in terms of race and
it is unlikely that the attachment-adjustment relationship is en- marital status. However, the number of women in the 20- to 24-year age
tirely mediated by social support, social conflict, and self- group was somewhat higher in our sample (39.5%) compared with the
efficacy. national norm (33.2%).

Method
Procedure
Participants
In keeping with clinic procedure, women arriving at the clinic com-
The participants were 615 women who underwent first-trimester vac- pleted a medical history form and were given a blood test. After this,
uum aspiration abortions at one of three free-standing abortion facilities women met individually with a clinic counselor to review their medical
in Buffalo, New York, between February 1993 and September 1993. history and their reasons for having an abortion. After these individual
During this time, 1,177 women who arrived at these clinics to obtain sessions, women participated in counseling sessions in groups of 5 or
an abortion were approached and invited to participate in a larger study 6, during which the abortion procedure, reproduction, and birth control
of women's reactions to abortion (see Major, Zubek, et al., 1997, for were discussed and abortion consent forms were signed. During the
other data based on this sample). Of these, 134 were not eligible to waiting period between the two counseling sessions, the women were
participate in the study (because they were in their second trimester, asked to participate in the study by a trained researcher, who explained
were not pregnant, were lost due to procedural mix-ups, and so on). that a study was being conducted, with the clinic's endorsement, to learn
Of the 1,043 women who were eligible, 882 (84.5%) agreed to partici- about women's reactions to having an abortion. They were assured that
pate. A comparison of the demographic characteristics of women who their responses would be kept confidential and that if they refused to
agreed to participate in the study with those who refused revealed that participate, their treatment at the clinic would not be affected. Con-
the two groups were similar on all variables except age. Women who senting women were given a preprocedure questionnaire, which they
agreed to participate were younger (M = 23.68 years) than those who completed either individually or in small groups. After the abortion and
refused (M = 25.92 years), F ( l , 1021) = 19.964, p < .001. a 30-minute recovery period, women completed the immediate postabor-
All eligible women who agreed to participate completed a preabortion tion adjustment measures either privately or in groups of 2 or 3.
questionnaire and a postabortion questionnaire in the clinic on the day Follow-up data were collected from the women at a subsequent point
of their abortion procedure. Approximately 1 month after the abortion, in time. Women who returned to the clinic for their postabortion follow-
615 (69.7%) of these women also completed a follow-up questionnaire. up visit (27%) were met by a trained interviewer, who administered the
These 615 women constitute our initial sample. Comparisons of the follow-up questionnaire. Women who did not return to the clinic for their
demographic and psychological characteristics of the 615 women who follow-up visit were interviewed either at the university's Psychological
participated at the follow-up with the 267 women who did not participate Services Center or at another agreed-on location (1 % ) . If an interview
in the follow-up revealed no significant differences between the two site could not be arranged, the women returned the questionnaire by
groups. mail (72%). We compared the responses of women who completed the
Participants ranged in age from 14 to 40 years (mean = 23.9 years, postabortion questionnaire at the clinic with those who mailed it to the
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 457

researchers on all of the variables in this study. Only one of these self-efficacy scale was reduced from five to four by selecting the four
comparisons was significant: Non-White women (M = 1.68) were more items with the highest item-total correlations in a reanalysis of Cozza-
likely than White women (M = 1.53) to mail back the questionnaire, relli's (1993) data. The retained items asked women how sure they were
F ( l , 608) = 13.17, p < .001. In addition, women who completed the that within the next 2 months they would be able to think about children
measure at the clinic (M = 3.85) were marginally higher in self-efficacy or babies comfortably, spend time around children or babies comfortably,
(M = 3.68) than those who mailed back the questionnaire, F(\, 603) have good sexual relations, and watch television shows or read newspa-
= 3.58, p < .06. per or magazine stories about abortion. The coefficient alpha for this
Women were paid $20 for participating in the follow-up. The average reduced scale was .82, which is comparable to the alpha for the longer,
time that elapsed between the abortion and completion of the follow- five-item version (,86). 2
up questionnaire was a little more than 1 month (mean = 39 days,
median = 28 days). The range for the follow-up span was 10 days to
Variables Assessed After Abortion
8 months; 85% of the women completed the follow-up within 2 V2 months
after their abortions. The exact length of time that elapsed between the Attachment styles and mental models of attachment. We used Bar-
abortion and completion of the follow-up assessment did not have an tholomew and Horowitz's (1991) Relationship Questionnaire (RQ) to
effect on any of the outcome variables in this study. Thus, this variable assess attachment styles and mental models. The RQ consists of four
was not included as a covariate in any of the analyses in this paper. short paragraphs, each describing one of the four attachment styles
The present study focuses on women's adjustment at the follow-up (secure, dismissing, preoccupied, and fearful). Respondents were first
period and not on adjustment immediately after the abortion. asked to rate the extent to which each prototype described them and
their typical style in emotionally close relationships on separate 7-point
scales (1 = not at all like me; 1 = very much like me). They were then
Measures asked to endorse the one prototype that described them best.
Variables Assessed Before Abortion Our main predictions were made specifically in terms of mental mod-
els rather than attachment styles. Because there is no currently available
Demographic variables. Women were asked to respond to a series direct measure of mental models of attachment, we followed procedures
of demographic variables (e.g., age, race, marital status, highest level outlined by Griffin and Bartholomew (1994a, 1994b) to calculate mental
of education obtained, number of prior abortions, and religion). For model scores using the RQ. Specifically, a model of self score was
the purposes of this article, these demographic variables were used as obtained by summing participants' ratings on the two attachment patterns
covariates.1 The complete procedure that was used to select covariates with positive self-models (secure and dismissing) and subtracting their
is described more fully in the analysis section later. ratings on the two patterns with negative self-models (fearful and preoc-
Perceived social support from the male partner. A seven-item scale cupied). In similar fashion, a model of others score was obtained by
was used to assess perceptions of preabortion social support from a summing the scores on the two attachment patterns with positive models
woman's male partner. On scales ranging from 1 ("didn't do this at of others (secure and preoccupied) and subtracting ratings on the two
all") to 5 ("did this a great deal"), each woman rated the extent to attachment patterns that have negative models of others (fearful and
which her male partner performed a series of supportive behaviors since dismissing). Mental models scores derived in this fashion have been
first learning that she was pregnant. Sample items include "Let you shown to load adequately on latent variables reflecting models of self
know he'd be there no matter what you decided to do" and "Boosted and others (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994b).
your spirits if you were feeling low." The items on this scale were In using these attachment variables we encountered two types of
adapted from Russell and Cutrona's (1984) Social Provisions Scale. missing data. First, 45 women neglected to endorse any of the attachment
The women were instructed to circle "does not know" if their partner prototypes as being the most self-descriptive (thus, they could not be
did not know about their pregnancy. The coefficient alpha for this scale assigned to an attachment style). We suspect that this was a result of
was .94. confusion resulting from the formatting of our questionnaire. Second,
Preabortion social conflict from the male partner was assessed using 14 women did not respond to all four of the continuous attachment items
an eight-item scale. On scales ranging from 1 ("didn't do this at all")
to 5 ("did this a great deal"), each woman rated the extent to which
1
her male partner engaged in a variety of negative behaviors since learning We also examined whether any of the demographic variables inter-
of her pregnancy. Sample items include "Said cruel or angry things to acted with mental models of attachment in predicting any of the mediator
you" and "Fought or argued with you more than usual." These social or postabortion adjustment variables. There was only one significant
conflict items were created for the purposes of this study. As with the interaction. Specifically, model of self interacted with marital status in
social support measure, women were instructed to circle "does not predicting postabortion distress. A plot of this interaction revealed that,
know'' if their partner was not aware of the pregnancy. Coefficient alpha among women with a positive self-model, marital status was unrelated
for this measure was .90. (Additional information about the perceived to postabortion distress. However, among those with a negative self-
social support and social conflict items is provided in Major, Zubek, et model, unmarried women were more distressed than those who were
al., 1997.) married.
2
Because 23% of our sample (141 women) did not tell their partners Self-efficacy for coping is conceptually quite similar to secondary
about their abortions, these women could not provide information about appraisal, which has been defined as a cognitive assessment of the extent
perceived partner support and conflict. We compared women who did to which one has the resources necessary to cope with a given negative
not tell their partners about the abortion with those who did provide life event (Lazarus & Fblkman, 1984). In the present study, we assessed
such data in order to test for differences on the demographic, attachment, secondary appraisal with four items asking women to what extent they
self-efficacy, and outcome variables. There were no significant differ- felt they had the resources to deal with their abortion effectively (e.g.,
ences between the two groups on any of these measures. "I personally have what it takes to deal with this abortion," "I have
Preabortion self-efficacy for coping with abortion. The measure of the resources I need to successfully handle this abortion experience'').
self-efficacy was a slightly shortened version of a scale that has been The coefficient alpha for this scale was .85. We re-ran all the major
used in prior studies of self-efficacy and abortion (e.g., Cozzarelli, analyses presented in this article, substituting secondary appraisal for
1993; Major et al., 1990). In light of severe time constraints governing self-efficacy. The results were almost identical and did not yield any
questionnaire administration in this study, the number of items in the substantively different-conclusions.
458 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

(thus, we could not derive mental models scores for these participants). Results
Consequently, a total of 59 women had missing data on the attachment
variables. To determine whether participants who were missing attach- General Description of Responses
ment data differed from those who provided attachment data, we com-
pared the two groups on all the demographic, mediator, and outcome Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, and
variables. Although the two groups did not differ significantly on any ranges) for the major variables in the study are presented in
of the mediator or outcome variables, more non-White (M = 1.42) than Table 1. Correlations and partial correlations (controlling for
White(M= 1.67) women provided attachment data, F ( l , 613) = 15.22, age, marital status, and number of previous abortions) among
p < .001. In addition, nonmarried women (M = 1.10) were more likely these variables are presented in Table 2. To ensure that the same
to provide attachment data than married (M = 1.23) women, F ( 1, 596) sample of women was used across all of the various analyses
= 4.17, p < .05. reported here, we omitted all participants who had missing data
Self-esteem was measured via Rosenberg's (1965) 10-item Self-Es- on any of the variables assessed in the study. Thus, the final
teem Inventory, a well-validated, frequently used measure of trait self- sample size for all of the analyses reported next was 408 (141
esteem. Women indicated the extent of their agreement with each item
participants did not provide support-conflict data because their
on scales ranging from 1 (.strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The
partners did not know of the abortion, 59 had missing data on
internal consistency coefficient for this scale was .89.
Postabortion adjustment. Follow-up adjustment measures assessed the attachment variables, and 7 had missing data on other vari-
psychological distress and positive well-being. ables in the study).
Psychological distress was assessed with 42 items from the SCL-90- Of the 408 women, 40% (n = 164) endorsed the secure
R (Derogatis, 1983). These items comprised the Depression (14 items), attachment prototype as the most self-descriptive; 32% (n =
Anxiety (10 items), Hostility (6 items), and Somatization (12 items) 130) classified themselves as fearful, 9% (n = 36) as preoccu-
subscales. Women used 5-point scales to rate the extent to which they pied, and 19% (n = 78) as dismissing. Although the fearful
were bothered by each complaint since having the abortion (0 = not at attachment category was slightly overrepresented in this sample,
all; 4 = a great deal). Correlations among the four subscales ranged
this distribution of attachment styles is similar to the distribu-
from .53 to .82, with an average correlation of .68. A higher order factor
tions reported by other researchers who have used the four-
analysis revealed that the four subscales loaded on a single factor. (The
factor loadings were .92, .91, .81, and .68 for Anxiety, Depression, category model of attachment (e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz,
Hostility, and Somatization, respectively). Thus, we created a composite 1991; Bylsma et al., in press; Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1994).
measure of psychological distress by averaging scores on the four sub- In addition, although women had a relatively more positive
scales. Coefficient alpha for the composite distress measure was .90. model of self than model of others, the means for both mental
Positive well-being was assessed with an 18-item index developed by models were close to the midpoints of the scales.
Ryff (1989). The measure consists of six 3-item subscales, each as- As illustrated in Table 2, self-esteem was moderately corre-
sessing an aspect of well-being: Autonomy, Environmental Mastery, Per- lated with model of self (r = .52, p < .001) but was unrelated
sonal Growth, Positive Relations With Others, Purpose in Life, and Self- to model of others. In addition, both model of self and self-
Acceptance. Respondents used 6-point agree-disagree scales to indicate
esteem were positively correlated with perceived social support
how they "usually" feel and behave. Sample items include "When I
(rs = .22, p < .001, and .28, p < .001, respectively) and self-
look at the story of my life, I am pleased with how things have turned
out'' and ' 'I live life one day at a time and don't really think about the efficacy ( r s = .21, p < .001, and .33, p < .001, respectively)
future" (reverse scored). Given that we were most interested in pre- and negatively correlated with perceived social conflict ( r s =
dicting overall well-being, we combined scores on the six subscales - . 1 6 , p < .001, and - . 2 7 , p < .001). However, model of
and created one well-being index. The coefficient alpha for this index others was not significantly correlated with any of the mediator
was .85. variables.

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Ranges for the Major Variables

Variable M SD Possible range Obtained range

Continuous measures of attachment styles


Secure 4.23 1.77 1.0 to 7.0 1.0 to 7.0
Fearful 3.84 2.01 1.0 to 7.0 1.0 to 7.0
Preoccupied 2.85 1.78 1.0 to 7.0 1.0 to 7.0
Dismissing 3.42 1.84 1.0 to 7.0 1.0 to 7.0
Model of self 0.96 4.24 -12.0 to 12.0 -12.0to12.0
Model of others -0.18 4.13 -12.0 to 12.0 -12.0to10.0
Self-esteem 3.89 0.71 1.0 to 5.0 1.1 to 5.0
Mediator variables
Perceived support 4.02 1.19 1.0 to 5.0 1.0to5.0
Perceived conflict 1.39 0.77 1.0 to 5.0 1.0 to 5.0
Self-efficacy 3.82 0.94 1.0 to 5.0 1.0 to 5.0
Postabortion measures
Psychological distress 0.61 0.63 0.0 to 4.0 0.0 to 2.99
Positive well-being 4.58 0.69 1.0 to 6.0 2.3 to 6.0

Note. Scales are scored such that higher means represent higher levels of perceived support, perceived
conflict, self-efficacy, self-esteem, distress, and positive well-being.
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 459

Table 2
Correlations and Partial Correlations for Variables Used in Path Analyses

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Model of self .18 .52 .21 .22 -.16 -.32 .55


2. Model of others .17 .07 -.01 .09 -.04 -.10 .15
3. Self-esteem .52 .06 .34 .28 -.27 -.41 .79
4. Self-efficacy .22 .01 .35 .15 -.20 -.38 -.32
5. Perceived support .22 .06 .28 -.18 -.52 -.17 .32
6. Perceived conflict -.15 -.02 -.27 .21 -.51 .28 -.24
7. Distress -.31 -.09 -.41 -.36 -.18 .27 -.43
8. Positive well-being .54 .15 .79 .33 .31 -.22 -.43

Note. Zero-order correlations are presented above the diagonal, and partial correlations controlling for
age, whether or not this was a first abortion, and marital status are presented below the diagonal. Scales
are scored such that higher numbers represent a more positive model of self and others, higher self-esteem,
perceived support, perceived conflict, self-efficacy, levels of distress, and positive well-being. Correlations
greater than .09 are significant a t p < .05, .12 a t p < .01, and .15 a t p < .001.

Model of self was positively and significantly correlated with self reported higher levels of self-efficacy for coping with abor-
postabortion positive well-being (r = .55, p < .001), as was tion than women with a negative model of self, F ( l , 401) =
self-esteem (r = .79, p < .001). Model of self and self-esteem 15.08, p < .01.
were also negatively correlated with postabortion distress (rs ANCOV\s on the postabortion measures revealed a signifi-
= - . 3 2 , p < .001, and - . 4 1 , p < .001, respectively). A positive cant effect of attachment styles on both psychological distress,
model of others was weakly, albeit significantly, related to both F(3, 402) = 4.51, p < .001, and positive well-being, F ( 3 , 401)
increased positive well-being ( r = . 1 5 , p < . 0 0 1 ) and decreased = 24.24, p < .001. Newman-Keuls post hoc tests revealed that
postabortion distress (r = .10, p < .05). secures were significantly less distressed than fearfuls and that
secures and dismissings were significantly higher in positive
Selection of Covariates for All Analyses well-being than preoccupieds and fearfuls. (No other differences
between groups were significant.)
Several demographic variables were covaried in the analyses
presented later. These covariates were selected by entering all
of the available demographic variables as a block in separate Effects of Mental Models on Postabortion Adjustment
regression analyses predicting each of the major independent
and dependent variables in the study (attachment styles, mental We hypothesized that the effects of mental models of attach-
models, self-esteem, perceived partner support and conflict, self- ment on postabortion distress and positive well-being would be
efficacy, and the two adjustment variables). Age, whether or not mediated by perceived social support, perceived social conflict,
this was a first abortion, and marital status emerged as significant and self-efficacy for coping with abortion (see Figure 1). These
predictors in one or more of these regression analyses. Accord- hypotheses were tested in the form of a single path model,
ingly, these three variables were held constant in all of the which was estimated using the LISREL 8 program (Joreskog &
analyses described later. Sorbom, 1993).
We followed a standard two-step approach in conducting this
analysis. In a first test of the model, we estimated all of the
Differences by Attachment Type
possible direct and indirect paths between the attachment and
To test for differences by categorical attachment style, a series outcome variables to allow for the possibility that the attachment
of one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOV\s) were performed variables would have direct effects on postabortion adjustment
on the mediator and postabortion measures (Table 3). We hy- in addition to the hypothesized mediated effects. This analysis
pothesized that secure women would perceive higher levels of revealed that several of these paths were not significant (as
social support and lower levels of social conflict from their male would be expected, given our hypotheses). Inspection of modi-
partners than all three types of insecure women. As expected, fication indexes also suggested that the fit of the model to the
there was a significant effect of attachment style on both per- data would be improved by adding two correlated error terms:
ceived support, F ( 3 , 401) = 2.91,/? < .05, and perceived con- the first between the social support and social conflict variables
flict, F(3, 402) = 3.90, p < .01. Planned contrasts comparing (magnitude = .47) and the second between the two adjustment
secures to the other three groups revealed the expected secure variables (magnitude = - . 0 8 ) . (These correlated errors are
versus insecure split on both variables, F( 1, 401) = 5.35, p < consistent with the modest correlations between each pair of
.05, for social support and F ( l , 401) = 10.99, p < .01, for variables and with the conceptual similarity of the correlated
social conflict. The ANCOV\ on self-efficacy for coping with constructs.) Thus, in the second step of this analytical procedure,
abortion also revealed a significant effect of attachment style, we re-ran the model after eliminating the paths that were nonsig-
F ( 3 , 402) = 5.37, p < .001. In support of our hypothesis, a nificant in the first path analysis and after adding the suggested
planned contrast revealed that women with a positive model of correlated error terms. The path coefficients that are depicted
460 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

Table 3
Attachment Style Differences on Mediator and Postabortion Measures

Attachment styles

Mental model Secure Dismissing Preoccupied Fearful

Self positive positive negative negative


Other positive negative positive negative F(3, 401)

Mediator variables
Perceived support 4.20, 4.10b 3.81b 3.83b 2.91*
Perceived conflict 1.25, 1.47b 1.59b 1.49b 3.90**
Self-efficacy 3.93. 4.01, 3.48b 3.64b 5.37***
Postabortion measures
Psychological distress 0.50, 0.57,,b 0.72.,b 0.77b 4.51***
Positive well-being 4.84, 4.73, 4.22b 4.27b 24.24***

Note. Differences between means were probed via planned contrasts for the three mediator variables and
via Newman-Keuls tests for the two adjustment variables. Measures are scored such that higher means
represent higher levels of perceived support, conflict, self-efficacy, psychological distress, and positive well-
being. Values with the same subscript are not significantly different from each other.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

in Figure 2 represent the results of running this second model. cate a good fit. However, because the chi-square statistic is very
(These coefficients are nearly identical to those obtained in the sensitive to sample size, we also examined other indexes that
model with the nonsignificant paths included.) are not so affected: the Adjusted Goodness-of-Fit Index (AGFI)
The goodness of fit of the model was assessed by evaluating and the Non-normed Fit Index (NNFI; Hu & Bentler, 1995).
several goodness-of-fit statistics. First, we examined the chi- Values of both of these indexes can range from 0 to 1.0; values
square statistic. Nonsignificant or small chi-square values indi- closer to 1.0 indicate a better fit. The model depicted in Figure

-.22

Figure 2. Path model relating mental models of attachment to the mediator and postabortion adjustment
variables. All of the path parameters are significant at .05.
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 461

2 demonstrated a good fit to the data: x 2 ( 7 , N = 408) = 17.30, score on model of self was represented by a score that was one
p < .05; AGFI = .95; NNFI = .94. Although the chi-square standard deviation above the mean model of self score, and a
was significant (most likely because of our large sample size), low score on model of self was represented by a score that was
it has been suggested that chi-square-degrees of freedom ratios one standard deviation below the mean model of self score.
between 2:1 and 5:1 indicate an acceptable fit (Marsh & Ho- Given that the mean score on the model of self variable was
cevar, 1988). In this case, the chi-square-degrees of freedom close to zero, a "high" score on this variable reflects a positive
ratio fell within this interval. model of self and a " l o w " score, a negative model of self.
Inspection of the path coefficients in Figure 2 reveals that Inspection of Figure 3 reveals that the relationship between
model of self had significant direct effects on postabortion dis- model of others and positive well-being varied for women with
tress and positive well-being, such that the more positive a wom- a positive versus a negative model of self. Specifically, among
an's model of self, the less distress and more positive well- women with a negative self-model, model of others was unre-
being she reported. Furthermore, the more positive a woman's lated to positive well-being; the simple slope was not significant,
model of self, the higher was her self-efficacy for coping, the f(407) = 1.18. Overall, these women experienced lower levels
more support she perceived from her partner, and the less conflict of positive well-being than women with a positive model of self.
she perceived from this same source. Because both self-efficacy In contrast, among women with a positive model of self, as
and perceived conflict were also significant predictors of dis- the positivity of model of others increased, positive well-being
tress, model of self thus also had indirect effects on postabortion increased as well; the simple slope was significant, f(407) =
distress via these two mediators. In addition, because both self- 2.71, p < .01. Figure 3 clearly suggests that the combination
efficacy and social support were related to positive well-being, of two positive mental models is uniquely related to higher
model of self also had indirect effects on positive well-being. positive well-being.
In contrast to the effects for model of self, model of others Finally, we also hypothesized that the interaction between the
was not directly related to postabortion distress or positive well- two types of mental models would predict levels of both per-
being, nor was it related to any of the three mediator variables. ceived support and perceived conflict. However, this interaction
However, the interaction between the two mental models had a was significant only as a predictor of perceived conflict. The
small direct effect on positive well-being. We plotted this inter- interaction was plotted in the same way as described earlier for
action using the procedures outlined by Aiken and West (1991). the interaction on positive well-being. Inspection of Figure 4
This involved plotting two simple regression lines showing the suggests that the effect of model of others on perceived conflict
regression of positive well-being on model of others for women varied according to whether women had a positive or negative
with high and low scores on the model of self variable. A high model of self. Specifically, among women with a positive model

5.5

5.0
O)
0)
GO

Pos. Model of Self


I 4.5
' Neg. Model of Self
>
CO
o
4.0

3.5
Negative (-12) Positive (+12)
Model of Others
Figure 3. Interaction between mental models of attachment on positive well-being. Note: A higher score
equals more positive well-being. Pos. = positive; Neg. = negative.
462 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

2.5

2.0

o
>^
c
o Pos. Model of Self
O 1.5
CO
Neg. Model of Self
o
o
CO

1.0

0.5
Negative (-12) Positive (+12)
Model of Others
Figure 4. Interaction between mental models of attachment on perceived social conflict. Note: A higher
score equals greater perceived conflict. Pos. = positive; Neg. = negative.

of self, as the positivity of model of others increased, perceived run of the model, with the nonsignificant paths removed and
conflict decreased; the simple slope was significant, (407) = the correlated error terms (magnitudes = - .43 and - .08) added.
2.88, p < .01. Thus, women with positive models of both self The model presented in Figure 5 fits the data very well: x 2 ( 12,
and others (the combination of mental models, which underlies N = 408) = 20.88, p < .05; AGFI = .96; NNFI = .98.
the secure attachment style) perceived the lowest levels of con- A comparison of Figures 2 and 5 reveals that the impact of
flict from their partner. In contrast, among women with a nega- model of self was greatly altered in the new model. Most strik-
tive model of self, as the positivity of model of others increased, ingly, with self-esteem in the model, model of self was no longer
perceptions of conflict increased; the simple slope was signifi- related to any of the mediator variables, although self-esteem
cant, f(407) = 2.63, p < .01. The combination of a negative emerged as a significant predictor of all three. The magnitudes
model of self and a positive model of others (characteristic of of the coefficients linking self-esteem to each of the three media-
the preoccupied attachment style) was associated with the high- tors were all somewhat larger than the coefficients linking model
est levels of perceived conflict. Because perceived conflict was
of self to these same mediators in Figure 2. Because each of
significantly related to postabortion distress, this interaction was
these mediators was related to one of the postabortion outcome
also indirectly related to distress.
variables in Figure 5 (self-efficacy was no longer a significant
All told, as a group, the attachment variables accounted for predictor of positive well-being in this model), self-esteem also
5% of the variance in self-efficacy, 6% of the variance in social had indirect effects on postabortion distress and positive well-
support, and 6% of the variance in social conflict. The combina- being. (Model of self, in contrast, had no indirect effects.) Model
tion of the attachment and the mediator variables accounted for of self did, however, continue to have direct effects on postabor-
23% of the variance in postabortion distress and 38% of the tion distress and positive well-being with self-esteem in the
variance in postabortion positive well-being. model, although the magnitudes of these relationships were con-
siderably reduced. Finally, self-esteem also had direct effects
Effects of Mental Models on Postabortion Adjustment on both postabortion adjustment variables. The magnitude of
With Self-Esteem Included in the Model the direct relationship between self-esteem and positive well-
being was relatively large, which is not surprising given the
To examine the extent to which the effects of model of self
conceptual overlap between these two constructs.
could be attributed to its overlap with self-esteem, we re-ran
the model described previously (following identical procedures) Although the impact of model of others was unaffected by
but adding self-esteem as a predictor variable (Figure 5 ). Again, the addition of self-esteem to the model, the interaction between
the coefficients presented in Figure 5 are derived from a second the two mental models was no longer a significant predictor of
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 463

.17

Interaction

Self-Esteem

Figure 5. Path model including self-esteem as a predictor variable. All of the path parameters are significant
at .05.

positive well-being. In contrast, this interaction remained combination with model of self) was perhaps most surprising
a significant predictor of perceived conflict from a woman's with regard to perceived social support. We had hypothesized
male partner. (The pattern of this interaction also remained that the two types of mental models would interact such that
unchanged.) secure women (who have two positive mental models) would
All told, in the new model, the attachment variables and self- report perceiving higher levels of social support from their male
esteem accounted for 12% of the variance in self-efficacy, 8% partners than would women in the three insecure groups. Al-
of the variance in social support, and 9% of the variance in though secure women did, in fact, report the highest mean level
social conflict. Self-esteem and the attachment and mediator of social support, the results of the path analysis were more
variables accounted for 24% of the variance in postabortion suggestive of a model of self effect. This finding is consistent
distress and 65% of the variance in positive well-being.3 with other research that demonstrates that having a positive self-

Discussion
3
We also conducted a few supplemental analyses on the data presented
Effects of Mental Models of Attachment on the Process in this article. First, we examined whether mental models of attachment
of Coping With an Abortion interacted with any of the mediator variables in predicting postabortion
adjustment. There were no significant interactions. Second, negative af-
On the basis of reasoning derived from the attachment litera- fectivity or the tendency to experience negative emotional states across
ture, we predicted that mental models of attachment would have situations was also assessed in the preabortion questionnaire using the
an impact on postabortion distress and positive well-being via Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clarke, & Tellegen,
the mediators of perceived social support, perceived social con- 1988). Although we would argue that conceptually attachment styles
flict, and self-efficacy for coping with abortion. The results of are to some extent inherently confounded with the tendency to experience
our analyses were consistent with this hypothesis and demon- certain affective patterns, we re-ran all of the major analyses in this
article, holding negative affectivity scores constant to examine the extent
strated that mental models of attachment had an impact on many
to which negative affectivity might account for some of our findings. In
facets of the process of coping with this stressful life event. In some cases, the coefficients we obtained when negative affectivity was
addition, however, our results clearly indicated that model of held constant were slightly reduced compared with the coefficients we
self was a more important predictor of postabortion adjustment obtained without negative affectivity in the analysis, but the overall
than was model of others. conclusions suggested by the two sets of analyses did not differ in any
The lack of an effect for model of others (either alone or in substantive way.
464 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

view is critical for successfully mobilizing and taking advantage poses, model of self and self-esteem typically perform almost
of one's social network in times of stress (e.g., Cozzarelli, identically.
Karrasch, Sumer, & Major, 1994; Hobfoll, Nadler, & Leiberman, Other results obtained in the current study, however, clearly
1986). Thus, the preoccupied women in the present study (who suggest that model of self and self-esteem do not overlap com-
have a positive view of others but a negative view of themselves) pletely. For example, the correlation between these variables
may indeed have sought social support from their male partners, was .52, which suggests overlap but not complete redundancy.
but because of their negative self-views, may have used ineffec- In addition, some effects of the attachment variables remained
tive or inappropriate strategies for obtaining this support. Fear- significant even with self-esteem included in the path model
fuls, who have two negative models, may simply not have sought (e.g., the interaction between the two types of mental models
support. on perceived conflict). Most strikingly, and in contrast to the
We also expected to find that the two types of mental models results obtained for the three mediator variables, both self-es-
would interact in predicting levels of perceived social conflict teem and model of self had significant direct effects on the two
from a woman's male partner. We did indeed obtain such an postabortion adjustment variables. Apparently, when predicting
interaction in the path analysis, and the pattern of means in postabortion adjustment, self-esteem and model of self each
the ANCO\A was also consistent with this hypothesis. Taken appear to retain some unique predictive capacity (although the
together, the results of these analyses suggested that secure effects of self-esteem were clearly stronger). Given that the
women perceived the lowest levels of conflict and preoccupied attachment literature has not detailed how self-esteem and model
women, the highest. Although admittedly speculative, it is tempt- of self differ, our explanation of these findings must, of necessity,
ing to conclude that this effect reflects the fact that both of these be speculative. We suspect that, although both of these con-
groups of women were more likely to interact with their partners structs may reflect the valence of individuals' global self-evalua-
in the context of the abortion than were the two types of avoid- tions, measures of model of self may diverge from measures of
ants. However, consistent with their positive model of self, se- self-esteem in that they also index the extent to which individu-
cures may have been able to do so in more constructive and als have an internalized sense of self-worth (as opposed to
adaptive ways than preoccupieds, who have a negative self- depending on others for validation) or experience anxiety in
model. This supposition is also consistent with literature sug- interpersonal contexts or both. This argument is consistent with
gesting that secures are more likely to respond to stress or Bartholomew and Horowitz's (1991) original conceptualization
potential conflict in ways that elicit nonconfrontational behavior of model of self and also with the findings that model of self
from others, whereas the opposite is likely to be the case for is very strongly correlated with the anxiety dimension of both
those who are preoccupied (Kobak & Hazan, 1991; Simpson & Collins and Read's (1990) and Simpson et al.'s (1992) attach-
Rholes, 1994; Simpson et al., 1992). ment measures (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994b). It is quite
plausible that dependence or anxiety, or both, could have a direct
effect on global measures of psychological adjustment, such as
those used in the current study. Future research is needed to
Overlap Between Model of Self and Self-Esteem
clarify the extent to which self-esteem and model of self diverge
in their overlap with anxiety and dependence.
Both model of self and self-esteem are dispositional variables
that reflect the extent to which an individual enjoys positive In any case, given that there already exists a large volume of
self-evaluations, and both initially emerge in the context of early literature detailing the beneficial effects of high self-esteem
childhood interactions with important others. Although attach- (both in general and as a precursor to more adaptive coping
ment researchers have routinely noted that self-esteem and efforts), the field of attachment would benefit from a more
model of self overlap (e.g., Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994b; systematic exploration of the extent to which model of self is
Mikulincer, 1995), almost no research has been conducted that a proxy variable for high self-esteem both in general and more
seeks to distinguish these two constructs explicitly or to detail specifically, in situations requiring coping with stressful life
more precisely the nature or extent of this overlap (cf. Bylsma events. Exploring this issue requires a detailed conceptual analy-
et al., in press). The results of the current study make it clear sis of the ways in which self-esteem and model of self both
that empirically these constructs overlap to quite a large extent. mirror and diverge from each other. Unfortunately, not all attach-
The most striking overlap occurred with regard to the three ment researchers define model of the self in the same manner,
mediator variables. For example, a comparison of our two path and there currently appears to be no consensus regarding the
analyses strongly suggests that the relationship we obtained be- precise content of mental models of attachment. Thus, clarifying
tween model of self and self-efficacy is a function of the overlap the mental models construct is a necessary preliminary step in
between model of self and self-esteem. Indeed, self-esteem has addressing the overlap between model of self and self-esteem.
previously been established as a contributor to feelings of self-
efficacy for coping with abortion (Cozzarelli, 1993). All told,
Future Research
when self-esteem was included in the path model, model of self
was no longer a significant predictor of any of the mediators. Future research exploring the effects of mental models of
Self-esteem, in contrast, was significantly related to all three. attachment on adjustment to abortion would be enhanced by a
Clearly, model of self and self-esteem overlap in their ability to more thorough investigation of several issues surrounding social
predict these variables, and when entered together self-esteem support and social conflict. First, gathering information from
emerges as the stronger predictor. Relatedly, in other analyses women's male partners concerning their provision of support-
that we have conducted in our laboratory for a variety of pur- conflict could prove to be quite instructive. (We did not collect
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 465
this information in the current study because of time and finan- Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York:
cial constraints.) In particular, given that the support and conflict Basic Books.
measures in the current study were based solely on the subjective Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
reports of our female participants, it was impossible to tell Basic Books. (Original work published 1969)
whether these perceptions accurately reflected the behavior of Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy
their partners. Second, we found it surprising that secure women human development. New \fork: Basic Books.
Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult attachment
were not more likely than insecure women to have told their
and the dynamics of romantic relationships. Personality and Social
partners about the abortion. One possible explanation for this
Psychology Bulletin, 21, 267-283.
finding is that telling one's partner about the abortion may be
Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., & Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles,
perceived by many women as a necessity (e.g., to receive finan- gender, and parental problem drinking. Journal of Social and Personal
cial help, transportation to the clinic) or as an obligation rather Relationships, 8, 451-466.
than as a means of gaining social support. Thus, for example, Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect.
fearful women may have told their partners about the abortion Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50,
in order to gain instrumental resources, but they may not have 3-35.
expected to receive or have actively solicited emotional aid. Bylsma, W. H., Cozzarelli, C , & Sumer, N. (in press). The relationship
As was noted earlier, model of others had surprisingly weak between attachment styles and global self-esteem. Basic and Applied
effects on the mediator and outcome variables in our model. We Social Psychology.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buff-
were most surprised by the lack of an effect for model of others
ering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357.
on perceived social support (in the path analyses) and by the
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. (1990). Adult attachment, working models,
small magnitude of the interaction between the two mental mod-
and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and
els on perceived social conflict. We had expected that women's Social Psychology, 58, 644-663.
views about the trustworthiness of others would be more Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1994). Cognitive representations of attach-
strongly relevant to these judgments. One possible explanation ment: The structure and function of working models. In K. Bartholo-
for the small magnitude of these effects is suggested by the mew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol.
results of a study conducted by Pierce, Sarason, and Sarason 5, pp. 53-75). London: Jessica Kingsley.
(1992), in which students' ratings of the supportiveness of two Coyne, J. C , & DeLongis, A. (1986). Going beyond social support:
notes they received from their mothers in the context of a stress- The role of social relationships in adaptation. Journal of Consulting
ful event were influenced by relationship-specific support expec- and Clinical Psychology, 54, 454-460.
tations (i.e., the amount of support they expected to receive from Cozzarelli, C. (1993). Personality and self-efficacy as predictors of
their mothers, in particular) but not by their support expectations coping with abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
about people in general. This suggests that a relationship-spe- 65, 1224-1236.
cific measure of attachment styles might better predict the level Cozzarelli, C , Karrasch, A., Sumer, N., & Major, B. (1994). The mean-
ing and impact of partner's accompaniment on women's adjustment
of support or conflict an individual perceives from a particular
to abortion. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 2028-2056.
intimate than a more global attachment measure such as the one
Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. W. (1990). Type of social support and
we used in the present study (which asked about women's close
specific stress: Toward a theory of optimal matching. In B. R. Sarason,
relationships, in general). Although attachment styles are typi- I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional
cally assumed to reflect stable patterns of beliefs about the self view (pp. 319-366). New York: Wiley.
and generalized others, it is intuitively possible for an individual Cutrona, C. E., & Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social support, infant tem-
to feel quite differently about the trustworthiness of various perament, and parenting self-efficacy: A mediational role of post-
intimates (see also Collins & Read, 1994; Lewis, 1994, on this partum depression. Child Development, 57, 1507-1518.
point). Further research is needed to clarify the differential Derogatis, L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R manual I. Baltimore, MD: Johns
effects of general versus specific mental models of attachment Hopkins University School of Medicine.
on a wide variety of processes. Dunkel-Schetter, C , & Bennett, T. L. (1990). Differentiating the cogni-
tive and behavioral aspects of social support. In B. R. Sarason, I. G.
Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional view
References (pp. 267-296). New York: Wiley.
Feeney, B. C , & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1996). Effects of adult attachment
Adler, N. E., David, H. P., Major, B. N., Roth, S. H., Russo, N. E, & and presence of romantic partners on physiological responses to stress.
Wyatt, G. E. (1992). Psychological responses after abortion. Science, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 255-270.
248, 41-44. Feeney, J., Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1994). Attachment style, communi-
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and cation and satisfaction in the early years of marriage. In K. Bartholo-
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. mew & D. Perlman (Eds.). Advances in personal relationships, Vol. 5:
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behav- Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 269-308). London: Jessica
ioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Kingsley.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994a). The metaphysics of measure-
young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality ment: The case of adult attachment. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman
and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244. (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 5, pp. 17-52). Lon-
Blain, M. D., Thompson, J. M., & Whiffen, V. E. (1993). Attachment don: Jessica Kingsley.
and perceived social support in late adolescence: The interaction be- Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994b). Models of the self and other:
tween working models of self and others. Journal of Adolescent Re- Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment.
search, 8, 226-241. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 430-445.
466 COZZARELLI, SUMER, AND MAJOR

Harter, S. (1990). Adolescent self and identity development. In S. S. (1997). Mixed messages: Implications of social conflict and social
Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing support within close relationships for adjustment to a stressful life
adolescent (pp. 352-387). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1349-1363.
Press. Marsh, H. W, & Hocevar, D. (1988). A new, more powerful approach
Harter, S. (1993). Causes and consequences of low self-esteem in chil- to multitrait-multimethod analyses: Application of second-order con-
dren and adolescents. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The firmatory factor analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 107
puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 87-116). New \brk: Plenum. 117.
Hazan, C , & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an Mikulincer, M. (1995). Attachment style and the mental representation
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, of the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1203
52, 511-524. 1215.
Hobfoll, S. E., & Freedy, J. R. (1990). The availability and effective Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1995). Appraisal of and coping with
use of social support. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, a real-life stressful situation: The contribution of attachment styles.
91-103. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 406414.
Hobfoll, S. E., & Leiberman, J. R. (1987). Personality and social re- Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles
sources in immediate and continued stress resistance among women. and fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 18-26. of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273280.
Hobfoll, S. E., & London, P. (1986). The relationship of self-concept Mikulincer, M., Florian, V, & Weller, A. (1993). Attachment styles,
and social support to emotional distress among women during war. coping strategies and posttraumatic psychological distress: The impact
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 189-203. of the Gulf War in Israel. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Hobfoll, S. E., Nadler, A., & Leiberman, J. (1986). Satisfaction with ogy, 64, 817-826.
social support during crisis: Intimacy and self-esteem as critical deter- Mueller, P., & Major, B. (1989). Self-blame, self-efficacy, and adjust-
minants. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 296-304. ment to abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle 1059-1068.
(Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applica- Pearlin, L., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of
tion (pp. 76-100). London: Sage. Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337-356.
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation Pierce, G. R., Sarason, B. R., & Sarason, I. G. (1992). General and
modeling with the SIMPL1S command language. Hillsdale, NJ: specific support expectations and stress as predictors of perceived
Erlbaum. supportiveness: An experimental study. Journal of Personality and
Kobak, R. R., & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in marriage: The effects Social Psychology, 63, 297-307.
of security and accuracy of working models. Journal of Personality Pistole, C. (1989). Attachment in adult romantic relationships: Style of
and Social Psychology, 60, 861-869. conflict resolution and relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and
Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Personal Relationships, 6, 505-510.
Working models, affect regulation and representations of self and Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: Impact on
others. Child Development, 59, 135-146. psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Koonin, L. M., Smith, J. C , & Ramick, M. (1993). Abortion surveil- ogy, 46, 1097-1108.
lanceUnited States, 1990. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Reports,
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
42, 29-57.
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.
Russell, D., & Cutrona, C. (1984, August). The provision of social
New \fork: Springer.
relationships and adaptation to stress. Paper presented at the 92nd
Lepore, S. J. (1992). Social conflict, social support, and psychological
Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, To-
distress: Evidence and cross-buffering effects. Journal of Personality
ronto, Ontario, Canada.
and Social Psychology, 63, 857-867.
Russo, N. F , Horn, J. D., & Schwartz, R. (1992). U.S. abortion in
Levy, M. B., & Davis, K. E. (1988). Love styles and attachment styles
context: Selected characteristics and motivations of women seeking
compared: Their relations to each other and to various relationship
abortions. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 183-202.
characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 4 3 9 -
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on
471.
the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and
Lewis, M. (1994). Does attachment imply a relationship or multiple
Social Psychology, 57, 839-852.
relationships? Psychological Inquiry, 5, 4 7 - 5 1 .
Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Reliability and stability of adult
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, child-
attachment patterns. Personal Relationships, 1, 23-43.
hood, and adulthood. A move to the level of representation. In I.
Schiaffino, K. M., & Revenson, T. A. (1992). The role of perceived self-
Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory
efficacy, perceived control, and causal attributions in adaptation to
and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
rheumatoid arthritis: Distinguishing moderator from mediator effects.
opment, 50, 66-106.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 709-718.
Major, B., & Cozzarelli, C. (1992). Psychological predictors of adjust-
ment to abortion. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 121-142. Shinn, M., Lehmann, S., & Wong, N. (1984). Social interactions and
Major, B., Cozzarelli, C , Sciacchitano, A. M., Cooper, M. L., lesta, social support. Journal of Social Issues, 40, 55-76.
M., & Mueller, P. M. (1990). Perceived social support, self-efficacy Shusterman, L. (1979). Predicting the psychological consequences of
and adjustment to abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- abortion. Social Science and Medicine, 13A, 683-689.
chology, 59, 452-463. Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (1994). Stress and secure base relation-
Major, B., Richards, M. C , Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C , & Zubek, J. ships in adulthood. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Attach-
(1997). Personal resilience, cognitive appraisals, and coping: An ment processes in adulthood: Advances in personal relationships (Vol.
integrative model of adjustment to abortion. Manuscript submitted 5, pp. 27-52). London: Jessica Kingsley.
for publication. Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support-seeking
Major, B., Zubek, J. M., Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C , & Richards, C. and support-giving within couples in an anxiety provoking situation:
MENTAL MODELS AND COPING 467

The role of attachment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
chology, 62, 434 7 446. validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
relationships: An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and 1063-1070.
Social Psychology, 71, 899-914.
Smith, D. A. E, & Coyne, J. (1988, August). Patient self-efficacy and
recovery from myocardial infarction: A contextual perspective. Pre- Received July 21, 1995
sented at the 96th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Revision received February 25, 1997
Association, Atlanta, GA. Accepted March 9, 1997

Low Publication Prices for APA Members and Affiliates


Keeping you up-to-date. All APA Fellows, Members, Associates, and Student Affiliates
receiveas part of their annual duessubscriptions to the American Psychologist and
APA Monitor. High School Teacher and International Affiliates receive subscriptions to
the APA Monitor, and they may subscribe to the American Psychologist at a significantly
reduced rate. In addition, all Members and Student Affiliates are eligible for savings of up
to 60% (plus a journal credit) on all other APA journals, as well as significant discounts on
subscriptions from cooperating societies and publishers (e.g., the American Association for
Counseling and Development, Academic Press, and Human Sciences Press).

Essential resources. APA members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases of
APA books, including the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association,
and on dozens of new topical books each year.

Other benefits Of membership. Membership in APA also provides eligibility for


competitive insurance plans, continuing education programs, reduced APA convention fees,
and specialty divisions.

More information. Write to American Psychological Association, Membership Services,


750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242.

You might also like