The Neuroscience of Musical Perception

You might also like

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 4

The Neuroscience Of Musical Perception,

Bass Guitars And Drake


December 19, 2015 7:00 AM ET, by Bret Stetka www.npr.org

In June of 2001 musician Peter Gabriel flew to Atlanta to make music with two apes. The
jam went surprisingly well.

At each session Gabriel, a known dabbler in experimental music and a founding member
of the band Genesis, would riff with a small group of musicians. The bonobos one
named Panbanisha, the other Kanzi were trained to play in response on keyboards and
showed a surprising, if rudimentary, awareness of melody and rhythm.

Since then Gabriel has been working with scientists to help better understand animal
cognition, including musical perception. Plenty of related research has explored whether
animals other than humans can recognize what we consider to be music whether they
can they find coherence in a series of sounds that could otherwise transmit as noise. Many
do, to a degree. And it's not just apes that respond to song.

Parrots reportedly demonstrate some degree of "entrainment," or the syncing up of brain


wave patterns with an external rhythm; dolphins may and I stress may respond to
Radiohead; and certain styles of music reportedly influence dog behavior (Wagner
supposedly honed his operas based on the response of his Cavalier King Charles Spaniel).
But most researchers agree that fully appreciating what we create and recognize as music
is a primarily human phenomenon.

How Brain Waves


Entrain To Music

This image, taken from an


MEG reading, shows a
musician's brain more
strongly entraining to
Beethoven's unhurried
Sonata No. 23.

Courtesy of Keith Doelling

Recent research hints at


how the human brain is
uniquely able to recognize and enjoy music how we render simple ripples of vibrating
air into visceral, emotional experiences. It turns out, the answer has a lot to do with
timing. The work also reveals why your musician friends are sometimes more tolerant of
really boring music.
Past work has shown that human brain rhythms entrain to speech patterns. In other
words, our brain waves oscillate in unison with the rhythm of spoken word. Building on
this and other prior research, a recent study published in the Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences confirms that perhaps intuitively, and likely an evolutionary
byproduct of speech processing the same is true for music.

Using a technology called magnetoencephalography, or MEG, which measures magnetic


fields produced by electric currents in the brain, NYU psychology professor David Poeppel
and his graduate student Keith Doelling analyzed the brain wave patterns of 39 people as
they listened to music. Specifically, volunteers were played snippets of the "Three Bs,"
classical shorthand for Brahms, Bach and Beethoven. The study subjects' brain waves
were traced while the music played, and the researchers found that the brain activity
synced up with the music, particularly, as would be expected, in the auditory cortex the
region of the brain that processes sound. Their brains were on beat.

Next, Poeppel and Doelling assessed how brain wave patterns responded to music in
musicians compared with nonmusicians, defined as, for the purposes of the study, those
with at least six years of musical training who actively play music and those who don't
currently play and who have less than three years of training, respectively.

They found that the brain wave rhythms of nonmusicians didn't entrain to music with a
tempo less than one note per second, or what would be considered slow music. However
the musicians' brain waves buzzed in synchrony to music as slow as 0.7 notes per second.
"It looks like musicians can take longer, more drawn out musical phrases and group them
into a pattern of some kind," says Poeppel. "The response to what some people perceive
as just a series of beeps and blips turns on much more sharply in musicians."

Devin McAuley, a professor of cognitive science at Michigan State University who wasn't
involved with the study, says, "These are exciting findings that fit into a broader body of
work. It's been previously shown that musicians tend to be able to produce slower
rhythms compared with nonmusicians. It's also been shown that children with musical
training have a slower preferred tempo than kids who haven't had training."

The findings might help explain why in my completely unscientific sampling of friends
and acquaintances musicians tend to be more tolerant of slow, ambient, droning music
(my pal Kevin thrives at a dreamy 0.6 notes per second).

They might also explain why musicians appear to be more attuned to environmental
sounds, like picking out a single voice in the din of a crowded room. Research shows that
this may be because of musicians' heightened ability to discriminate between different
frequencies.

Poeppel is quick to acknowledge that his work by no means proves that entrainment
equates with being a good musician. "Maybe you're still a really bad piano player despite
your brain rhythms locking on, but most of our musicians were conservatory-trained so
presumably they're pretty good," he jokes. "And I don't plan on sitting them down and
saying, 'OK, now play something!' "
A self-described "hardcore nativist," Poeppel says that much of musical ability and
appreciation probably comes genetically hard-wired, but, as his study also showed, the
number of years of musical training someone has correlates with entrainment. Whatever
it means artistically speaking, we can train our brains to respond more precisely to music
in a measurable, biologic way.

A study from 2014 supports Poeppel's findings and may reveal a nuance that helps explain
how entrainment influences what we perceive as music. Using an electroencephalogram,
or EEG, to measure electrical activity in the brain, psychologist Laurel Trainor and her
colleagues from McMaster University in Hamilton, Ontario, found that the human brain
is far more sensitive to low notes being off tempo than higher notes. Or, put another way,
our brains entrain more to low frequencies.

The authors speculate that this could be why the rhythm sections of so many music styles
around the world rely on bass, drums and lower tones, and why discordant guitar solos
and blaring saxophone leads can sound just fine when off-beat bass guitar doesn't.

Poeppel cautions that this research is in its early stages, but he speculates that if in fact
we are more likely to entrain to lower frequencies it could help explain another critical
component of musical perception: movement.

Music brings with it a subconscious physicality. Dancing, foot-tapping and head-bobbing


or headbanging are often inseparable from the rhythm and beat of song itself. "Listening
to music doesn't just activate the auditory regions of the brain," Poeppel explains.
"Rhythmic input also automatically activates your motor cortex too. Call it 'predancing.'

According to McAuley, the human brain looks to find, or really assign, music in
movement, which could explain why the Internet exploded with musical mashups of
Drake's "Hotline Bling" video in October. Though The Cosby Show theme song, Vince
Guaraldi's "Linus and Lucy" and Justin Bieber's "What Do You Mean?" all vary in tempo,
they each appear to sync up with the rapper's moves just fine. "Our brains can lay down
beats anywhere on a continuously moving visual stimulus," says McAuley. It seems that
though we entrain accurately to an auditory beat, we appear to allow some rhythmic
leeway with visual input.

How humans perceive music is, of course, far more complicated than simply tuning in to
tempo. Music draws up and draws from memories, emotions and pleasure and
reward activity in the brain. Other acoustic qualities like melody, harmony and timbre
also play important roles. And our conscious ability to apply symbolic meaning to sounds,
lyrics and song and to recognize when listening to music that what we're listening to is
supposed to be music also certainly influences human musical perception.

But the work of Poeppel and others suggests that a tonal time perception hinted at in apes
and more fully formed in man could be a prerequisite for discerning a song. Musical
appreciation might begin with our brains simply staying on beat.
1) Were there any new and/or unfamiliar vocabulary words in the article? What were
they? Discuss with your neighbor and check in with your teacher, what do they mean?

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

1) Were there any new and/or unfamiliar vocabulary words in the article? What were
they? Discuss with your neighbor and check in with your teacher, what do they mean?

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

____________________________________________________________

You might also like