Blinded by The Starlight: An Evolutionary Framework For Studying Celebrity Culture and Fandom

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Review of General Psychology 2012 American Psychological Association

2012, Vol. 16, No. 2, 144 151 1089-2680/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027909

Blinded by the Starlight: An Evolutionary Framework for Studying


Celebrity Culture and Fandom

Charlotte J. S. De Backer
University of Antwerp

Seeing is believing. Historically, print media has used visual illustrations to add credibility to their texts.
The power of visuals is indeed stronger than the power of words. Even though pictures can be
manipulated to not portraying any truth, we more easily believe what we see, compared with what we
hear or what we read. Visual representations of reality create the false belief as if we witness the situation;
an eye-witness perception translates into an I-witness emotional response. From an evolutionarily point
of view, this can be labeled as a mismatch outcome of evolved psychological mechanisms. Adaptations
to deal with situations that existed in our evolutionary past are being triggered by modern artifacts that
might not be fitness-relevant themselves, but look a lot like fitness-relevant cues from the past. A classic
example is, for instance, a fear reaction upon encountering a water hose that looks a lot like a snake. In
a similar fashion, pictures, being visual representations of the world, look a lot like personal perceptions
of the real world and might trigger mental mechanisms to deal with problems associated with this world.
Applying this to understand the worship of stars, the author explains how mainly visual stimuli contribute
to the formation of parasocial (one way) bonds between stars and their audience. The author draws on
theories from the social sciences (communication studies, politics, and sociology), psychology, econom-
ics, and evolutionary biology, first to explain the field of celebrity or star studies, second to address the
importance of visuals in the production of stars, and finally to explore why audiences worldwide tend to
become emotionally involved with individuals they encounter on screens, with whom any form (or
possibility of) physical interaction is highly unlikely.

Keywords: celebrity, stardom, visual, parasocial relationships, imagery

Video killed the radio star. Pictures came and broke your heart.The methodological suggestions for translating this theoretical frame-
Buggles, 1979 work to empirical research.
In 1979 the British band The Buggles released their hit song,
Video Killed the Radio Star, telling the story of a singer whose Celebrities, Stars, and Your Renowned Neighbor:
career is cut short by the rise of TV. In reality, radio presenters Unraveling the World of Stardom
have not yet been replaced by TV presenters, and may never be,
but if both types of stars were placed on a scale of fame, the First, it is important to define what a celebrity is. Studies on
video stars would indeed outscore the radio stars. The majority of stardom cover a wide range of topics and different types of
TV characters become bigger stars than many radio presenters. In individuals; several different types of stars fall under this umbrella.
what follows, I outline an interdisciplinary theoretical framework Although Charles Darwin, for example, is considered to be a
that explains this phenomenon. person of great importance and influence, we would hesitate to
I draw on theories from the social sciences (communication define him as a celebrity. A scientist may be a star within a certain
studies, politics, and sociology), psychology, economics, and evo- academic discipline but would not be called a celebrity. Similarly,
lutionary biology, first to explain the field of celebrity or star Vincent Van Gogh has been world famous for over a hundred
studies, second to address the importance of visuals in the produc- years but is not referred to as a celebrity. The same applies for
tion of stars, and finally to explore why audiences worldwide tend many important historical figures, such as Julius Caesar and Al-
to become emotionally involved with individuals they encounter exander the Great. When thinking about celebrities, we think about
on screens, with whom any form (or possibility of) physical people from the modern entertainment industry who have become
interaction is highly unlikely. well-known for their work or actions, such as Angelina Jolie or
No new empirical research will be used to support this work, but Justin Bieber (at least at the time of writing). What defines a
the article concludes with a number of specific hypotheses and celebrity, though?
In order to answer this question we should start at a more
general level, by first explaining stars and stardom, driven by a
process of fame. Therefore, in what follows I will discuss (a) the
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Charlotte reason for fame (for what a celebrity is known), (b) the level of
J. S. De Backer, Department of Communication Studies, University of fame (how famous a celebrity is), and (c) the duration of fame (for
Antwerp, De Meerminne, Sint Jacobstraat 2, 2000 Antwerp, Belgium. how long a celebrity is famous) and show how all of these factors
E-mail: charlotte.debacker@ua.ac.be determine whether a person can be defined as a celebrity. This

144
BLINDED BY THE STARLIGHT 145

introduction will outline a number of evolutionary theories, as this that may explain the underlying fundamentals of this idea is the
perspective will become important in the next section where an recognition heuristic.
explanation of why we admire stars is attempted. An initial level Humans have a natural tendency to attribute higher status to
of background knowledge regarding the general field of star stud- people and objects they recognize compared with people and
ies will be helpful in understanding the range of people to which objects they do not recognize. Goldstein and Gigerenzer (1999)
this framework can be applied. illustrated this with a number of experiments in which respondents
As far as the reason for fame is concerned, it appears that almost were asked to rank cities according to their population size. Their
anything is possible. Some stars are famous because they can sing, results showed that people rank cities they recognize higher than
others because they are confused as to whether tuna is a kind of cities of which they had not previously heard. Using recognition to
fish or a kind of chickenthis is how Jessica Simpson became a rank objects was found to be a fast and economical way to decide
celebrity in 2003 in the show Newlyweds: Nick and Jessica. which of two items had a higher value. The recognition heuristic
Generally speaking, three key paths to stardom have been estab- for such tasks is simply stated: If one of two objects is recognized
lished in the literature (Rojek, 2001): Fame comes by way of and the other is not, then infer that the recognized object has the
inheritance; some are born famous, such as royals, and possibly higher value (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 1999, p. 41).
also the children of famous parents. Second, fame can be achieved, In a similar sense, this recognition heuristic can be applied to
through excelling at a certain skill; these individuals may play human beings: Individuals attribute higher status to other individ-
football, sing, dance, talk, cook, swim, write, and so forth better uals they recognize compared with individuals they do not recog-
than the majority of other people. Third, fame may be ascribed to nize. We also attribute more status, or prestige, to those who are
certain individuals, simply because a crowd of peoplean audi- known by many, compared to those who are known by fewer
encepays attention to them or at least to their actions. Many people. This may explain why higher levels of prestige are often
reality TV stars fall into this last category. The difference between attributed to international stars than to national stars.
achieved and ascribed stardom is that fame is a by-product in the Finally, to ensure definition as a celebrity, an individual should
first case and a goal in the latter. Stars who become famous be well-known for a sufficient amount of time. An individual may
through their achievements do not aim to become famousthey receive significant media attention for several days, for example, if
simply excel in a certain domain, and fame is the result. Stars who
they win the lottery, but if this attention does not continue the
become famous through audience attention, however, tend to have
individual will not become a celebrity. On the basis of the duration
fame as a goal and appear to be willing to go to great lengths to
of well-knownness, I further divide the group of well-known
attain it, such as by potentially embarrassing participation in a
people into three subgroups: celetoids, celebrities, and icons. Cele-
reality TV show, for instance.
toids have achieved, or have been attributed, a certain amount of
As we have seen, stardom can therefore be achieved through
fame, but this fame is short-lived (Rojek, 2001). Examples of
inheritance, hard work, or through attracting public attention. It is
celetoids may include a man who has won the lottery, a woman
also true, however, that an individual may be born in a locally
who has killed her husband and lover, or a girl who has fallen from
renowned family, be the best singer in the neighborhood and
a rooftop and survived; they are ordinary people who become
attract high levels of attention but still never become a celebrity.
famous for a short period of time. Celebrities are people who are
This person would very likely be renowned in his or her neigh-
borhood and social network, but to become a celebrity, a further very well-known for longer periods of time, most often throughout
level of recognition must be achieved beyond the limits of the their lifetimes. For the sake of this definition, however, a celebrity
personal social network. As Rojek (2001) explained, the term is a living famous person; after death, a celebrity becomes an icon.
renown refers to a form of localized fame that relies on the An icon embodies only the public image of the celebrity as they
recognition of an audience at only a small distance socially from were during their lifetime. Living celebrities are both the public
the renowned individual. An encounter, or at least the possibility image, which can exist beyond death, and the physical body that
of an encounter, between stars and their audience is possible. Only feeds this public image, and that is constrained by time.
when recognition is attained beyond the borders of low social As sociologist George Herbert Mead explained, every individual
distance can a person of renown be redefined as a celebrity. For consists of an I and one or more mes. The I refers to the
reasons I outline below, however, I label the latter group as subjective self, and the me refers to the individual as perceived
well-known people for now. For well-known people, compared by others (Reck, 1964). Applying this to the concept of what a
with renowned people, the number of people who would recognize celebrity is, celebrities embody both the public self, constructed by
them without having met them face to face or even having the themselves as well as by the entertainment industry and their
opportunity to do so is significantly larger than the number of audience (me), and also the person behind that image (I). In de
people who know and (potentially) meet them in real lifethat is, Saussures (1960) semiotic terms celebrities consist both of the
the people from their local social network. signified, which refers to the actual physical person, which we
Recognition is key factor in the phenomenon of celebrities, as is term the celebrified, and the signifierthe public image, or ce-
neatly summed up in Boorstins (1961) famous statement that the lebrifier, created both by the media and the audience. As regards
celebrity is a person who is well-known for their well-knownness icons, the celebrifier is what remains when the celebrified has
(p. 58). Although it could be argued that this circular definition passed away. Lady Diana Spencer, for example, was a worldwide
does not clarify exactly what a celebrity is, it does capture the celebrity when she was alive, and she retained her status as a star
essence of what makes a person a celebrity: recognition. The more even after she died. The celebrifier was gone, but the celebrified
people recognize an individual, the more fame can be attributed to prevailed. Lady Di became an icon, resembling what Diana had
that person and the bigger star that individual becomes. A theory been when she was alive, as a celebrity.
146 DE BACKER

In conclusion, as far as defining celebrities and other types of nowned is undoubtedly the most important factor. Being recog-
famous individuals is concerned, I propose the use of the term star nized by large numbers of people is an indicator of fame, and this
to describe an individual who has reached a given level of fame. can be achieved faster and more permanently for video stars than
Their stardom may have been inherited, achieved, or attributed. radio stars. To understand why video has lessened the recognition
These individuals can be active in various domains, such as in of the radio star, I turn now to the power of visuals and examine
sports, movies, music, and so on (Principle 1). Their level of fame, the relevant evolutionary theory.
determined by the number of people who recognize them and have
the opportunity to meet them face to face as opposed to the number Eye-WitnessI-Witness: The Effect of Visuals on Our
of people who recognize them but have no opportunity to meet Stone-Age Brains
them face to face, further divides the group of stars into renowned
and well-known people. The first type of star is only known among With Baby Suri, Seeing Is Believing (MSNBC;
individuals within his or her local social network, whereas the
September 12, 2006)
latter type of star is known beyond this level, by individuals at a
greater social distance from the star (Principle 2). On the basis of In 2006, Tom Cruise and Katie Holmes released a written
life stage criteria, I further separate well-known people into the statement confirming that their daughter Suri had been born on
categories of celetoids, celebrities, and icons (Principle 3). Every April 18, but for months the couple refused to release a picture of
well-known star starts as a celetoid, by achieving or inheriting her. Worldwide media speculation over the existence or nonexis-
fame, or by having fame attributed to them. During this initial tence of the baby followed, as people seemed reluctant to believe
stage, it could be argued that their own concept of self, the that the baby was real without having seen photographic evidence.
celebrifier, is more important than their public image, the celebri- In September the proof (and relief) finally came, when the first
fied. If, over time, the celebrified becomes more significant than pictures of baby Suri appeared on the cover of Vanity Fair. A
the celebrifier, celetoids become celebrities. This may best be picture is worth a thousand words, and in this and many other
considered as a continuous process, due to the difficulty of pin- celebrity gossip cases, one picture sells (or sold) more than a
pointing when exactly this transformation takes place. When the thousand words ever could (Vanity Fair allegedly paid millions of
celebrifier passes away and only the celebrified remains, the ce- dollars to obtain the rights to publish baby Suris first pictures, and
lebrity becomes an icon. An overview of this categorization is their website received a record 4.3 million hits in just one day, far
given in Figure 1. exceeding their daily average of 60,000 hits).
When it comes to defining who is a celebrity, the increase in Throughout history, people have attempted to boost their rec-
recognition needed to become well-known as opposed to re- ognition by means of visual communication. Even in ancient

Figure 1. A classification of stars.


BLINDED BY THE STARLIGHT 147

times, long before video or photography was invented, means but a blink of an eye in evolutionary terms. When we also bear in
other than word of mouth were sought to elevate recognition. mind that selection is a slow process that requires many genera-
Alexander the Great, for example, who has often been referred to tions to produce changes in the way our mind processes informa-
as the first true celebrity (Giles, 2002), boosted his recognition by tion (Cosmides & Tooby, 1994), we can assume that our minds as
having his face reproduced on ornaments. Statues of Alexander the they exist today are not sufficiently adapted to interpret the mass-
Great were strategically placed in public places; his image was mediated environment we live in properly. The evolutionary en-
stamped on bracelets, dinnerware, ornaments, and coins (Holt, vironment of adaptedness (EEA) refers to the environment to
2003). People who had never met him recognized his face, and he which a species is adapted. Every species faced specific reproduc-
became familiar, and it was this physical recognition that contrib- tive problems, different from other species, because they lived in
uted to the first enormous claim to fame in history. The depiction different environments or because their way of living was differ-
of famous faces on coins and banknotes is still widespread today, ent. Reproductive problems are tasks a species must undertake to
but the visual representation of individuals occurs on a much larger secure its survival and reproductive success, such as finding food,
scale in various highly visual mass media formats. finding mates, and so forth. The EEA is important in the context of
Today, the media reproduces interactions between media char- explaining functional properties, such as psychological mecha-
acters and their audiences on a very large scale, and it is this nisms, because these arise during the process of natural selection
mass-mediated production of culture that has created the age of (see Hagen, 1999 2002). The EEA of our psychological mecha-
celebrity we live in, according to Braudy (1997). Celebrities are no nisms is best located in the Pleistocene, when our ancestors lived
recent phenomenon, but their numbers, and the extent to which as hunter gatherers. Our ancestors lived in this way for approxi-
they are known, have increased dramatically since the introduction mately 99% of our evolutionary past (Hawkes, OConnell &
of mass-mediated visuals. Despite the great physical distances Rogers, 1997), having inevitable repercussions for our minds
between contemporary stars and the majority of their audiences, current mechanisms. Modern societies inevitably differ dramati-
the emotional connection that audiences feel with celebrities re- cally from this Pleistocene environment. Mass-mediated visual
mains strong. stimuli (pictures, videos), for example, simply did not exist in the
As soon as pictures of a star enter our daily lives, we may have environment that shaped our psychological mechanisms, which
the impression that he or she belongs to our personal social may go some way toward explaining the difficulties we encounter
network despite living on the other side of the world. Video did not with interpreting and responding to them appropriately.
kill the radio star: Video gave birth to the age of celebrity as we Even though many modern phenomena did not exist in the
know it today. The question remains, however: Why did visuals Pleistocene, some of them do resemble objects or stimuli that were
became so important from a consumer perspective? Those who present in that era, such as plastic animals made to appear as
aim to become well-known might use pictures to boost their realistic as possible. Consequently, it may be the case that certain
visibility and gain more prestige. Why, though, do we as an psychological mechanisms, shaped by the Pleistocene environ-
audience respond more strongly to pictures than to nonvisual ment, are triggered (activated) by modern phenomena. Such mis-
information about stars? matches between our psychological mechanisms, rooted in the
The proximate explanation for this is that visuals add credibility: past, and cues we encounter in the modern environment may help
Seeing is believing. Before photography was developed in the to explain behaviors that would otherwise make no sense in
early 1800s, engravings were used to illustrate texts in newspapers. evolutionary, fitness-enhancing terms. Such mismatches may have
From the middle of the 19th century onward, picture papers were irrelevant or maladaptive outcomes.
enjoying popularity across Europe and in North and South Amer- An example of a maladaptive outcome of the activation of our
ica (Goldberg, 1993). Pictures add credibility to a text because psychological mechanisms in modern society is our craving for
they enhance the audiences experience of an event, perhaps even sugar and fatty foods. The difference in access to calories in the
allowing them to feel that they personally witnessed it. According modern environment and access to calories in our ancestors
to Bird (1992), this credibility factor is the reason why tabloids and environment is, undeniably, significant. In the Pleistocene, food
gossip magazines in particular have very high proportions of visual was sometimes scarce, and our ancestors strategy of consuming
content: Pictures are needed to increase the trustworthiness of all or most of the food they came across was adaptive: It secured
many of their less accurate stories. For similar reasons, tabloid and their survival and reproductive success. Today, calories are easy
gossip magazine stories are often presented in interview format, so accessible: All we have to do is enter a supermarket. Consuming
that readers have the impression that they are receiving the infor- all or most of the calories available to us has become a maladaptive
mation firsthand. Another technique is to state that the information strategy: It results in poor health. Obesity is a major health prob-
comes from sources close to the celebrity. The truth can easily be lem in modern societies and is an example of a maladaptive
distorted with words, and although pictures can beand are outcome of an adaptation that was once beneficial (Gaulin &
manipulated, they retain a sense of authenticity that leads audi- McBurney, 2004).
ences to believe that what they are seeing is reality. Our brains may An example of an irrelevant outcome of the triggering of a
interpret mediated visual information in a similar way to real-life psychological mechanism by a modern object is the fear we
visual information: We experience what we see as if it really experience when we see a plastic spider or snake. This fear is
happened, as if I witnessed the situation, as if I was there. irrelevant, because plastic spiders, snakes, and other predators are
Eye-witness observation equates to an I-witness feeling. harmless. However, the fear makes sense when we consider the
From an evolutionary psychological perspective, this eye- EEA that shaped our psychological mechanisms: Snakes, for ex-
witnessI-witness phenomenon is not entirely surprising. Though ample, were a common threat to the survival and reproduction of
mediated visual information has existed for over 200 years, this is our ancestors, and it is perhaps not surprising that we still possess
148 DE BACKER

psychological adaptations that lead us to react to snake-like objects lives to the stars, and emotional responses of stars toward their
in a rather extreme way. The psychological mechanisms that cause audiences, again, are rather exceptional. Because of the one-way
a fight-or-flight reaction in the context of a perceived threat (see nature of information and emotion in these interactions, they are
Ledoux, 1998) can be compared to a smoke alarm: As Nesse and termed parasocial (e.g., Caughey, 1984; Giles, 2002; Horton &
Williams (1994) pointed out, the principle of a smoke alarm is to Wohl, 1956; Rubin, Perse, & Powell, 1985).
be oversensitive to danger. In other words, they are designed along Research suggests that parasocial relationships should be seen
the principle that it is better to react to large numbers of false as an extension of consumers social lives, rather than compensa-
alarms than not to react to actual danger. It is assumed that many tion for a lack thereof. If the latter were true, people who lack
of our psychological mechanisms also work in this waythat it is intimacy or have trouble with intimacy in their real lives should
better that our fight-or-flight mechanisms are triggered too often have strong parasocial bonds as compensation, but studies inves-
than not at all. In threatening situations, this would have been an tigating how parasocial relationships function have shown that the
advantage for our ancestors and explains why certain associations first case (extension) is more likely (Cohen, 2004). However,
are learned more easily than others, known as preparedness although they do not replace real interactions, parasocial relation-
(hman & Mineka, 2001). ships do help to boost self-esteem among consumers with low
Similarly, it may be that mass-mediated visuals trigger evolved self-esteem (Derrick, Gabriel, & Trippin, 2008). Favorite TV pro-
psychological mechanisms that were developed to deal with real- grams also decrease feelings of loneliness and low mood among
life visual input, just as plastic snakes might trigger an evolved fear average individuals (Derrick, Gabriel, & Hugenberg, 2009).
reaction to real snakes. In the same way that a plastic snake looks In fact, the one-way processes of parasocial relationships feature
as if it were a real snake, mass-mediated visuals look as if they dynamics that are very similar to patterns typical of real-life
were real, leading to the experience of eye-witnessed information relationships. Just as we feel distressed by the thought that we
as an I-witnessed situation. This may explain why people become might lose a friend, we tend to feel the same emotion when our
aroused by pornography (e.g., Salmon, 2005), and why people feel favorite TV characters are about to be taken off air, for example,
emotions associated with friendship, or even rivalry, when regu- when a show is about to stop production (Cohen, 2004; Eyal &
larly exposed to pictures of stars. Cohen, 2006; Lather, 2011). Furthermore, the level of intimacy
To sum, visuals are added to written data to increase the believ- established with same-sex friends in real life correlates with the
ability of the information given, and this is the proximate expla- level of intimacy experienced with parasocial friends, which
nation for their abundant presence in many contemporary media means that the closer a persons real-life relationships are, the
formats. The advent of visual stimuli introduced representations of closer their parasocial relationships will be (Greenwood & Long,
reality that unintentionally constituted a new environment for their 2011). Remark, however, that this does not hold for romantic
audiences one that activates our emotions. Pictures seem to relationships, where individuals who are single report greater
trigger ancient psychological mechanisms that were evolved to imagined intimacy with media characters than individuals who are
deal with the world as observed through our eyes. A key factor in in a relationship (see Greenwood & Long, 2011).
this new environment is the individuals we begin to regard as If we consider the parasocial relations between stars and their
friends: stars. audience within the framework of the eye-witnessI-witness the-
ory described above, a possible explanation for why this phenom-
enon occurs is outlined by Barkow as follows:
I Witness, I Feel: How Visuals Add (Parasocial)
Friends to Our Social Network The mass media may activate the psychological mechanisms that
evolved in response to selection for the acquisition of social informa-
tion. [. . .] We see them in our bedrooms, we hear their voices when
TV Is Providing Artificial Friends and Relatives to we dine: If this hypothesis is correct, how are we not to perceive them
Lonely PeopleKurt Vonnegut as our kin, our friends, perhaps even our rivals? As a result, we
automatically seek information about their physical health, about
I agree that when Diana died it touched the world, and as I told changes in their relative standing, and above all about their sexual
my son, when Diana died the world cried (Lorraine Clifford, relationships. (Barkow, 1992, pp. 629 630)
Australia; BBC News; January 16, 1998). The death of Diana,
Princess of Wales in 1997 caused an outpouring of emotion around Our minds process visuals of stars as if they are proof that these
the world. Investigating this phenomenon, Brown, Basil, and Bo- individuals are part of our social networks. We meet stars every
carnea (2003) noticed that peoples level of involvement with day, and though the encounters are not real, our minds process the
Diana prior to her death influenced the effect that media messages encounters in the same way as if they were real.
about her death had on them. In the conclusion of their study, they In my opinion, visuals play the most important role in this
suggested that future research was necessary to further analyze process. Of course, hearing and reading about stars may produce
cases such as this because of the growing impact of celebrities on similar results, also triggering our minds to interpret stars as if they
their audiences. were members of our social networks. Readers often establish
The relation between stars and their audiences is typically bonds with fictitious characters in books, and Zunshine (2007)
characterized by a lack of feedback in interactions. Stars play out argued that the reason we read fiction might be created and driven
their private lives in public (often unwillingly), and the public by the cognitive cravings of our stone-age minds, similar to the
produces an emotional reaction to this, such as the period of public phenomena outlined above. Still, the power of the mass media
mourning following Dianas death. Feedback is lacking in the visuals seems to have stronger effects. From an evolutionary
sense that the public or audience cannot easily reveal their private perspective, people who our ancestors heard about but never saw
BLINDED BY THE STARLIGHT 149

were not members of their personal social network. These were our psychological mechanisms to function in a similar way to
part of their extended social network (friends of friends). People smoke alarms: It is better that they are activated too often than too
they had heard of but never seen were, therefore, not as close to little. Of course, there are still limits. Smoke detectors will respond
them as people whose faces were familiar. to smoke from a cigarette, but if they were to be activated by the
Even today, in the realm of real interpersonal connections, the smoke of a candle, for example, they would be too sensitive to use.
bonds we have with close friends with who we interact and meet In a similar sense, we could argue that realistic pictures of people
face to face are much stronger than the bonds we have with people might activate emotional mechanisms developed to operate in
we have heard about or read about but never met face to face. face-to-face interactions, but cartoons of people might no longer
People go to great lengths to see each other when interacting: They have the same effect on our minds. Future research should inves-
fly to the other side of the world for conferences, meetings, and tigate where the boundaries lie between visuals that have the
personal visits. In addition, when we communicate through a power to affect us and those that do not.
medium such as the telephone or Internet, we tend to add visual In conclusion, it is my belief that an evolutionary framework
cues to these interactions such as pictures or video, simply because offers a useful platform for developing hypotheses and frame-
images imply closeness (Kirk, Sellen, & Cao, 2010). Visuals works for studying parasocial relationships. Relevant areas of
create the illusion of an encounter. study for these relations are growing every day. As we now live in
In this context, it is interesting to consider David Giless (2002) an age of cyberspace and Web 2.0, it will also be important to
framework for studying parasocial relationships. Giles pointed out extend the above theory to relationships with online characters
that a distinction must be made between different types of para- who may require entirely different categorization to stars. If visual
social bonds, based on the potential for encountering the media information is present, the people we meet online or at least people
characters involved. What he labeled first-order parasocial inter- renowned in the online community should elicit similar responses.
action involves media characters who address the audience di- It has been illustrated that parasocial bonds can even occur be-
rectly, such as talk show hosts. Linking this to what I discussed in tween individuals and their personal avatar in online games. Peo-
the introduction, the celebrified and celebrifier in this case do not ple who score highly on interdependent self-construal (an individ-
differ significantly from each other. Second-order parasocial in- uals sense of self in relation to others), in particular, tend to
teractions occur with media characters who are to some degree establish stronger parasocial bonds with their virtual self, as rep-
inauthentic (such as fictitious soap characters) but who have a resented by an avatar (Jin & Park, 2009).
real-life counterpart nevertheless. Here the celebrified and celebri-
fier are distinct, but the celebrifier guarantees potential for encoun- Discussion
tering the star. Third-order parasocial interactions, on the other
hand, involve fantasy or cartoon characters who have no real-life Seeing is believing. The media have long used visuals to illus-
counterpart at all. Here, no celebrifier exists only the celebrified. trate written information and add credibility to the content of the
Technically speaking, icons would therefore fall under this cate- message, and the power of visuals remains strong. Despite that
gory. pictures can now be manipulated, we continue to believe what we
Giles (2002) expected parasocial interactions to be strongest see more easily than what we hear or read. Visuals create the false
with the first group, and weakest with the fantasy characters and belief that we have actually witnessed an event. From an evolu-
proposed further investigation of this hypothesis. However, Nabi, tionary psychological perspective, which states that our human
Stitt, Halford, and Finnerty (2006) compared audiences emotional minds were shaped in a context in which visuals did not yet exist,
reactions to reality shows and fictional programming, and they I put forward the eye-witnessI-witness theory to explain the
noticed that parasocial relationships were stronger for fictional power of visuals. Visual representations of life closely resemble
programming. Their results might contradict Giles assumptions, real life, as directly perceived with our eyes, in the same way that
but they do not appear particularly strange in the context of the pictures of people closely resemble real people directly perceived
eye-witnessI-witness perspective outlined above. The evolution- with our eyes. Given that our minds have been shaped by the
ary framework supports the idea that parasocial interactions may process of evolution and are not yet sufficiently adapted to process
be equally strong among first-order, second-order, and third-order visual stimuli as different from real-life input, it is perhaps not
interactions, as long as the characters involved are all portrayed surprising that we are led to believe that we have really experi-
visually in the media, that is, as long as the celebrified is main- enced the events portrayed in the images we consume. This phe-
tained, because it is that cue that triggers our emotions. Moreover, nomenon may explain why audiences become emotionally at-
the eye-witnessI-witness paradigm allows us to predict that a tached to celebrities and other stars they encounter on screen.
second-order or even third-order parasocial interaction with a This article presented a theoretical framework for examining the
visual media character (such as a fictitious movie character) where field of star studies, for explaining why various media formats are
visuals are present may be stronger than a first-order parasocial so rich in visual information, and for clarifying why an abundance
interaction (e.g., with a real radio newscaster) where visuals are of images portraying humanlike beings may lead us to interpret
absent. such media characters as part of our social networks. Further
I agree with Giles that we can expect certain third-order para- research is needed to empirically test the ideas. Some specific
social interactions to be lower in intensity because of the unreal- assumptions that should be tested are: If in situations with no
istic portrayal of these characters. Although realistic visuals might visual stimuli, the bonds between stars and their audiences will be
trigger our stone-age minds to perceive media characters as part of less strong than in situations where visual stimuli are present
our social networks, cartoons and other unrealistic images are (Assumption 1). Because, if human minds process visual input
unlikely to have the same effect. As explained earlier, selection led according to the general idea put forward, that people we see are
150 DE BACKER

people we meet, then we might expect that repetitive visual en- may provide insight into how close people feel to potential part-
counters with media characters will result in the establishment and ners on the basis of the information they have received about them.
development of strong parasocial bonds between stars and their In the control condition, profiles could provide both visual and
audiences. On an emotional level, audiences will perceive stars as written information, whereas in the experimental conditions the
being part of their closest social networks. Moreover, although profiles could provide either written or visual information. If the
these parasocial bonds may also be created by regular nonvisual eye-witnessI-witness framework is correct, then we may expect
audio or written input, this latter type of bond, based on nonvisual people to feel closer to new contacts who provided only visual
input, is more likely to resemble the indirect relationships our information than to new contacts who provided only written in-
ancestors had with members of their extended social networks. formation, because the first group would trigger emotions related
This assumption could easily be tested by investigating the to close, personal social networks whereas the latter would trigger
differences and similarities among parasocial bonds between (a) emotions related to extended social networks.
audience members and visually represented media characters and A range of existing scales can be used to measure parasocial
(b) audience members and nonvisually represented media charac- bonds (see Giles, 2002; McCutcheon, Lange & Houran, 2002),
ters. However, because parasocial relationships mimic real-life many of which are based on Rubin et al.s (1985) Parasocial
relationships and we know the degree of closeness experienced in Interaction Scale. Because this scale was designed to measure the
interpersonal relationships is influenced by many factors, the study relation between audiences and TV characters, it has largely been
design must guarantee that all other influencing factors are con- used to further investigate parasocial interactions with visually
trolled as far as possible. For example, it would be interesting to represented characters, such as newscasters (e.g., Rubin et al.,
test whether parasocial bonds were stronger for TV newscasters or 1985) and soap opera characters (e.g., Rubin & Perse, 1987). An
for radio newscasters, or whether parasocial bonds between a adapted version of this scale has also been successfully applied to
reader and a character from a book were weaker than the paraso- investigate the relation between audiences and radio characters
cial bonds between the same character portrayed in a movie and a (Rubin & Step, 2000). More recently McCutcheon, Lange, and
consumer of that movie (given that both consumers were equally Houran (2002) developed a similar measurement tool. Their Ce-
interested in this character). lebrity Attitude Scale measures peoples admiration for celebrities,
An alternative method for testing this hypothesis would be in a focusing on three aspects of the nature and motivation of fans
more controlled experimental setting, where subjects have no admiration for media characters of all kinds. The intense-personal
access to visual input. This could be achieved in practice by dimension, which measures strong, compulsivealmost obses-
investigating the parasocial bonds between media characters and sivefeelings about stars, would be especially interesting to use in
(a) nonvisually impaired audience members and (b) visually im- this context. The scale has also been used in a range of psycho-
paired audience members. Because the latter group lack the visual logical studies, showing that correlations exist between the forma-
stimuli, their media experience may be an ideal natural experimen- tion and strength of parasocial interactions and (a) changes in
tal setting for investigating the impact of (a lack of) visual stimuli lifestyle (Boon & Lomore, 2001), (b) autonomy and attachment
on the formation and evolution of bonds between audience mem- changes in adolescence (Giles & Maltby, 2004), (c) self-reports of
bers and media characters. To be more specific, a new (real or mental health (Maltby et al., 2004), and (d) personal body image
fictitious) character could be created and presented in an audiovi- (Maltby, Giles, Barber, & McCutcheon, 2005).
sual movie clip to both visually impaired and nonvisually impaired
audiences, and, controlling for individual differences in attachment References
styles, the extent to which the two groups form parasocial rela-
tionships could then be investigated. It may be interesting to Barkow, J. H. (1992). Beneath new culture is old psychology: Gossip and
initiate such research when a new TV series is launched, especially social stratification. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby (Eds.),
when the actors are not well-known, so that no previous interac- The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of
tions between the characters and the consumers biases the results. culture (pp. 627 637). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Second, it would also be interesting to test the predictive value BBC News, 16 January 1998, Has Dianas death changed Britain? Your
reaction, http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/talking_point/43461.stm
of this new framework against an existing, applied framework,
Bird, E. S. (1992). For enquiring minds. A cultural study of supermarket
such as the one of Giles (2000). For instance, the idea that tabloids. Knoxville, TN: University of Tennessee Press.
parasocial bonds between visually represented fictitious characters Boon, S. D., & Lomore, C. D. (2001). Admirer-celebrity relationships
and their audiences will be stronger than the parasocial bonds among young adults. Explaining perceptions of celebrity influence on
between nonvisually represented reality characters and their audi- identity. Human Communication Research, 27, 432 465. doi:10.1093/
ences (Assumption 2) would be supported by this framework but hcr/27.3.432
not by Giless framework. This could be tested by comparing the Boorstin, D. (1961). The image. London, England: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.
strength of the parasocial bonds between (a) audience members Braudy, L. (1997). The frenzy of renown: Fame and its history. New York,
and fictional characters in movies, TV shows, virtual worlds, and NY: Vintage Books.
so on; and (b) audience members and characters from nonvisual Brown, W. J., Basil, M. D., & Bocarnea, M. C. (2003). Social influence of
an international celebrity: Responses to the death of Princess Diana.
reality programming, such as radio dramas. Such radio dramas are
Journal of Communication, 53, 587 605. doi:10.1111/j.1460-
still produced in the United Kingdom and would offer great 2466.2003.tb02912.x
potential for research. Caughey, J. (1984). Imaginary social worlds. Lincoln, NE: University of
Finally, as a complement to the main field of star studies, it may Nebraska Press.
also be interesting to test the framework by using data from social Cohen, J. (2004). Parasocial break-up from favorite television characters:
networking sites or online dating sites. The latter, in particular, The role of attachment styles and relationship intensity. Journal of
BLINDED BY THE STARLIGHT 151

Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 187202. doi:10.1177/ Mediating closeness. In proceedings of ACM Conference on Com-
0265407504041374 puter Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW 2010). ACM, New York,
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Better than rational: Evolutionary 135144.
psychology and the invisible hand. American Economic Review, 84, Lather, J. (2011). How do we react when our favorite characters are taken
327332. away? An examination of a temporary parasocial breakup. Mass Com-
Derrick, J. L., Gabriel, S., & Hugenberg, K. (2009). Social surrogacy: How munication and Society, 14, 196 215. doi:10.1080/15205431003668603
favored television programs provide the experience of belonging. Jour- Ledoux, J. (1998). The emotional brain. The mysterious underpinnings of
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 352362. doi:10.1016/ emotional life. London, England: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
j.jesp.2008.12.003 Maltby, J., Day, L., McCutcheon, L. E., Gillett, R., Houran, J., & Ashe,
Derrick, J. L., Gabriel, S., & Trippin, B. (2008). Parasocial relationships D. D. (2004). Personality and coping: A context for examining celebrity
and self-discrepancies: Faux relationships have benefits for low self- worship and mental health. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 411 428.
esteem individuals. Personal Relationships, 15, 261280. doi:10.1111/ doi:10.1348/0007126042369794
j.1475-6811.2008.00197.x Maltby, J., Giles, D. C., Barber, L., & McCutcheon, L. E. (2005). Intense-
de Saussure, F. (1960). Course in general linguistics. London, England: personal celebrity worship and body image: Evidence of a link among
Peter Owen. female adolescents. British Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 1732.
Eyal, K., & Cohen, J. (2006). When good friends say goodbye: A paraso- doi:10.1348/135910704X15257
cial breakup study. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 50, McCutcheon, L. E., Lange, R., & Houran, J. (2002). Conceptualization and
502523. doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem5003_9 measurement of celebrity worship. British Journal of Psychology, 93,
Gaulin, S., & McBurney, D. H. (2004). Evolutionary psychology (2nd ed.). 67 87. doi:10.1348/000712602162454
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Nabi, R. L., Stitt, C. R., Halford, J., & Finnerty, K. L. (2006). Emotional
Giles, D. (2002). Parasocial interaction: A review of the literature and a and cognitive predictors of the enjoyment and reality-based and fictional
model for future research. Media Psychology, 4, 279 305. doi:10.1207/ television programming: An elaboration of the uses and gratifications
S1532785XMEP0403_04 perspective. Media Psychology, 8, 421 447. doi:10.1207/
Giles, D., & Maltby, J. (2004). The role of media figures in adolescent s1532785xmep0804_5
development: Relations between autonomy, attachment, and interest in Nesse, R. M., & Williams, G. C. (1994). Why we get sick? The new science
celebrities. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 813 822. doi: of Darwinian medicine. New York, NY: Times Books, Random House.
10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00154-5 hman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness. To-
Goldberg, V. (1993). The power of photography: How photography ward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Re-
changed our lives. New York, NY: Abbeville Press. view, 108, 483522. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The recognition heuristic: How Reck, A. J. (1964). Selected writings: George Herbert Mead. New York,
ignorance makes us smart. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, & The ABC NY: Bobbs-Merrill.
Research Group (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 37 Rojek, C. (2001). Celebrity. London, England: Reaktion Books.
58). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Rubin, A. M., & Perse, E. M. (1987). Audience activity and soap opera
Greenwood, D. N., & Long, C. R. (2011). Attachment, belongingness involvement: A uses and effects investigation. Human Communication
needs, and relationship status predict imagined intimacy with media Research, 14, 246 268. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1987.tb00129.x
figures. Communication Research, 38(2), 278 297. doi:10.1177/ Rubin, A. M., & Step, M. M. (2000). Impact of motivation, attraction, and
0093650210362687 parasocial interaction on talk radio listening. Journal of Broadcasting and
Hagen, E. (1999 2002). The evolutionary psychology FAQ. http:// Electronic Media, 44, 635654. doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem4404_7
www.anth.ucsb.edu/projects/human/evpsychfaq.html Rubin, R. B., Perse, E. M., & Powell, R. A. (1985). Loneliness, parasocial
Hawkes, K., OConnell, J. F., & Rogers, L. (1997). The behavioral ecology interaction, and local television news viewing. Human Communication
of modern hunter-gatherers, and human evolution. Trends in Ecology & Research, 12, 155180. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1985.tb00071.x
Evolution, 12, 29 32. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(96)10060-4 Salmon, C. (2005). Crossing the abyss: Erotica and the intersection of
Holt, F. (2003). Alexander the Great and the mystery of the elephant evolutionary psychology and literary studies. In J. Gottschall & D. S.
medallions. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Wilson (Eds.) The literary animal: Evolution and the nature of narrative
Horton, D., & Wohl, R. R. (1956). Mass communication and para-social (pp. 244 257). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
interaction: Observations on intimacy at a distance. Psychiatry, 19, Zunshine, L. (2007). Fiction and theory of mind: An exchange. Philosophy
215229. and Literature, 31, 189 196. doi:10.1353/phl.2007.0019
Jin, S. A. A., & Park, N. (2009). Parasocial Interaction with my avatar:
Effects of interdependent self-construal and the mediating role of self-
presence in an avatar-based console game, Wii. CyberPsychology & Received February 8, 2012
Behavior, 12, 723727. doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0289 Revision received February 8, 2012
Kirk, D. S., Sellen, A., & Cao, X. (2010). Home video communication: Accepted February 22, 2012

You might also like