IWOHandbookapplicantreactionschapter November 15

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 62

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/277000722

Applicant Perspectives on Employee Selection


Systems (in press)

Chapter · January 2016

CITATIONS READS

3 848

5 authors, including:

Julie Mccarthy Neil Robert Anderson


University of Toronto Brunel University London
34 PUBLICATIONS 608 CITATIONS 229 PUBLICATIONS 9,766 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Sara Mana Ahmed


University of Surrey
15 PUBLICATIONS 20 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Team Trust in NASA teams View project

MSc Project: Occupational Psychology - Strategic Resource or Purveyor of Tests View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Talya N. Bauer on 09 February 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 1

APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES ON EMPLOYEE SELECTION SYSTEMS

Donald M. Truxillo
Portland State University

Talya N. Bauer
Portland State University

Julie M. McCarthy
University of Toronto

Neil Anderson
Brunel University

Sara M. Ahmed
Brunel University
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 2

ABSTRACT

The field of applicant reactions has grown since the introduction of the original applicant

reactions models over 20 years ago. In this chapter, we describe the theoretical basis for

applicant reactions research. We further summarize the empirical findings about the

antecedents and moderators of applicant reactions in affecting a range of outcomes and which

selection procedures are preferred by applicants. We also describe similarities and differences

in applicant reactions cross-nationally and summarize the findings on test-taking dispositions.

We conclude with a discussion of future research directions in light of changes in selection

practice and provide recommendations for organizations.

Keywords: Applicant reactions, applicant attitudes, test-taking attitudes, test-taker

dispositions.
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 3

APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES ON EMPLOYEE SELECTION SYSTEMS

INTRODUCTION

Historically, personnel selection has investigated how to select the right candidate for

the job to benefit the functioning of the organization. The needs of the organization – in terms

of the human capital needed to fill a particular job opening – were the primary focus, with the

predictive validity of selection procedures being paramount. Although some mention was

made regarding concepts like “face validity,” that is, the degree to which the selection

process looks job-related to the applicant, the viewpoint of the applicant was rarely

considered.

Over time, greater attention was placed on personnel selection from the perspective of

job applicants. More specifically, researchers began to investigate how applicants perceive

and react to the hiring process. The source of this interest varied to some degree depending

on the national culture of the researcher. For instance, while the primary motive for interest in

applicant reactions in the US stemmed from a desire to avoid litigation on the part of

applicants, some European researchers (e.g., Schuler, 1993) focused on concepts like “social

validity”, or ensuring applicant dignity and respect. Whatever the motivation, this research

interest was accelerated by the development of theoretical and conceptual models to explain

applicant reactions (e.g., Arvey & Sackett, 1993; Gilliland, 1993; Schuler, 1993). Not only

did these models aim to explain how applicant reactions develop, but the hope was that such

reactions could also explain a number of outcomes, such as perceptions of the organization,

later behavior on the job, and applicant litigation.

Over the past two decades, a rich literature on applicant reactions (and the related

topic of test-taking predispositions such as applicant motivation) has developed. This

literature has provided a number of consistent findings regarding which selection procedures

applicants prefer. It has also shown that applicant reactions affect a number of applicant
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 4

attitudes and perceptions that are within the selection arena. Although the original hope was

that applicants’ treatment and perceptions during the hiring process would also affect more

distal outcomes on the job, the effects of applicant reactions on more distal outcomes such as

job performance have not been borne out in the research (McCarthy, Van Iddekinge, Lievens,

Kung, Sinar, & Campion, 2013), and the effects on job attitudes and actual litigation remain

largely unexplored.

Some researchers have thus discounted the ultimate value of applicant reactions

research, but we think that this is too pessimistic, given that applicant reactions clearly do

affect a number of important attitudes and behaviors. For example, perceptions of the hiring

process have a significant effect on organizational attractiveness; certain types of selection

procedures (e.g., interviews) appear to be preferred over others (e.g., cognitive tests) by

applicants around the world; and test-taking predispositions relate to test performance (e.g.

Anderson & Witvliet, 2008; Anderson, Ahmed, & Costa, 2012; Anderson, Salgado, &

Hülsheger, 2010; McCarthy et al., 2013). Moreover, many issues still need to be examined,

such as the way that selection may affect applicants’ self-perceptions, and how reactions to

promotional processes may affect job attitudes and performance.

In this chapter, we provide an overview of the applicant reactions literature, starting

with an in-depth history of this area along with a description of the dominant conceptual and

theoretical models. We next review the selection procedures that applicants tend to prefer,

the outcomes that have been found to relate to applicant reactions, and the international and

cross-cultural studies of applicant reactions. We next move to the antecedents of applicant

reactions as well as factors that may moderate the effects of these reactions. We then review

the growing field of test-taking predispositions. We conclude the chapter by reviewing the

implications for organizations and applicants as well as the many unanswered questions to be

addressed by future research.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 5

RESEARCH HISTORY OF THE APPLICANT REACTIONS FIELD

From its inception, research on selection has predominantly been focused on the

needs of the organization. Questions such as “How can we select the best applicants?” “How

can we make our tests more valid?” “How can we save time and money with our selection

procedures?” had dominated the literature. It was established that work samples and

simulations are rated more favorably than other types of selection procedures (Chan &

Schmitt, 2004). Starting in the 1990s, however, researchers began to adopt a more nuanced

approach to examining applicants’ points of view. At this point, the conversation was slowly

expanded to include questions like “How are applicants feeling about our selection

procedures, and what do they like or dislike?” “Are the best applicants staying in our

selection process?” “Do applicant reactions matter to recruitment outcomes?” Early work

illustrated that applicant reactions did matter to a number or outcomes. However, as

mentioned previously, it wasn’t until the development of models by Arvey and Sackett

(1993), Schmitt and Gilliland (1992), Gilliland (1993), and Schuler (1993) that research in

this area became more theoretically rich and took large strides forward.

Subsequent work has primarily adopted Gilliland’s (1993) fairness approach, perhaps

because of its strong theoretical basis in organizational justice theory. Gilliland argued for

the need to differentiate between procedural and distributive justice (e.g., a fair process

versus fair selection decisions/whether a job offer was made), advanced the idea that different

distribution rules might be used depending upon contextual issues, spearheaded the

development of a list of procedural justice rules that map onto the selection context (e.g., job-

relatedness of procedures; consistency of administration), highlighted the importance of

interactional justice (e.g., treatment at the test site), and proposed that procedural justice

might compensate for lower distributive outcomes.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 6

Gilliland’s (1993) model included 10 procedural justice rules under three categories.

Formal characteristics included job relatedness, opportunity to perform, reconsideration

opportunity, and consistency of administration. Explanations include feedback, selection

information, and honesty. Interpersonal Treatment includes interpersonal effectiveness of

the administrator, two-way communication, and propriety of questions. These 10 “rules” are

defined as follows: Job relatedness is defined as the degree to which the selection process is

(or appears to be in the case of face valid job relatedness) relevant for the job the applicant is

hoping to earn. Opportunity to perform is defined as the ability to express one’s true

ability. Reconsideration is defined as the chance to challenge or modify a decision made

during the selection process. Consistency is defined as the standardization of the process so

that every applicant is treated in the same manner. Feedback refers to the provision of

timely and informative feedback about an applicant’s performance on either their place in the

process or a final decision being made about that selection hurdle or a job offer. Selection

information is defined as how advanced information regarding the selection process they are

going through with an organization. Honesty is defined as truthfully communicating with

applicants. Interpersonal effectiveness is defined as the extent the test administrator treats

applicants with warmth and respect. Two-way communication is defined as the opportunity

for applicants to have their opinions and views known during the selection process and/or to

ask questions about the process. And finally, propriety of questions is defined as the

avoidance of questions which invade the privacy of an applicant. In addition, distributive

justice refers to the perceived fairness of the actual selection outcome, such as whether a

positive outcome was perceived, or what type of outcome was received compared to others.

Bauer, Truxillo, Sanchez, Craig, Ferrara, and Campion (2001) tested and validated a measure

(the Selection Procedural Justice Scales or SPJS) of these 10 rules which supported

Gilliland’s justice model and outcomes and which has subsequently seen wide use. To further
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 7

understand the development of this research area, we next turn our attention to the theoretical

foundations of applicant reactions.

THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS OF THE APPLICANT REACTIONS FIELD

To date, the applicant reactions field has been primarily driven by the theoretical

foundation of organizational justice theory, as can be evidenced by its historical roots in

Gilliland’s (1993) justice-based model. Indeed, it is safe to say that a majority of the work

we will review in this chapter is grounded in some form of organizational justice theory, and

research in this area has begun to take a more sophisticated examination of relationships to

examine boundary conditions, such as key interaction effects for applicant reactions. A

recent example by Sumanth and Cable (2011) established that the status of an organization

and applicant interact such that organizations with rigorous procedures allow them to select

the best applicants but that the best applicants are more likely to find such procedures unjust

than low-status applicants. In this section we describe some additional theoretical approaches

beyond justice that have been used to explain applicant reactions.

Attributions. Ployhart and Ryan (1997) describe attributions as a fundamental part of

social perception and are considered the basis for further judgments, emotions, and behaviors.

Ployhart and Harold (2004) further argue that applicants make attributions about the selection

procedures and outcomes they experience. This perspective is different from the theoretical

basis of fairness. As they note, “attributional processing occurs between the presence of an

event… and formation of perceptions (e.g., fairness, attitudes) and behaviour (e.g.,

motivation, test performance)” (p. 89). The theory argues that explanations given to

applicants will vary on the richness of information they convey in terms of consistency,

consensus, and distinctiveness as originally described in work by Kelley (1967). This

cognitive appraisal, which is based on understanding an applicant’s attributional style, has the

potential to expand the theoretical basis and domain of research in the applicant reactions
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 8

arena. Although Ployhart and Ryan (1997) were able to establish initial support for the

attribution framework, much more empirical work is needed to confirm this framework. We

support the idea of additional work applying the attribution model as a way to expand our

understanding of this framework.

Self-serving bias. Yet another research stream is embedded in the concept of a self-

serving bias, or social identity. This work is built upon empirical evidence suggesting that

perceived test performance has an influence on applicant reactions (Chan & Schmitt, 2004).

As a result, individuals may protect themselves from threatening situations, such as being

rejected for a job or failing a test, by evaluating the test as unfair because, if the test is faulty,

the outcome is not deemed as that important to worry about. Indeed, applicants may go to

great lengths to protect their identities in a self-serving bias. As Herriot (2004) notes, “It is

argued that this theory [social identity theory] enables an understanding of how applicants’

social identities interact with their perceptions of selection episodes to predict their exit from

the process.” (p. 75). These findings also raise a potential paradox, as a fair procedure may

result in positive attitudes about the organization, but may make the applicant feel even worse

about themselves if they are unsuccessful in obtaining the job.

As these researchers note, studies which combine or examine the relative effects of a

self-serving bias perspective with organizational justice are sorely needed, yet seldom done.

A notable exception is a study conducted by Schmitt, Oswald, Kim, Gillespie, and Ramsay

(2004) which found that both the self-serving bias and organizational justice appeared to

matter in the perceptions of college freshman taking the ACT and SAT. A provocative

finding in this area is that following a negative selection decision where all applicants were

told they had already scored in the top 20% of performers, those participants who were

rejected and received performance feedback suffered lowered core self-evaluations versus

those only being told they were rejected (Schinkel, van Dierendonck, & Anderson, 2004).
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 9

This brings into question the advice about how much feedback to provide applicants with, as

well as when to provide it.

In addition, new theoretical streams related to justice and self-concepts have begun

which examine justice expectations. Bell, Ryan, and Wiechmann (2004) theorize that

expectations from direct experience, indirect experience, and other beliefs will influence

attitudes, thoughts, and actual behaviors. More recently, a study by Bell, Wiechmann, and

Ryan (2006) revealed that applicant with higher justice expectations reported higher levels of

pre-test motivation and self-efficacy and more positive intentions to recommend and accept

the job and positive justice perceptions in testing process. Again, although this justice

expectations approach seems promising, far more work is needed.

Justice refinements: Fairness heuristic theory (FHT) and fairness theory.

Beyond traditional organizational justice theory approaches, some refinements and new

perspectives have been introduced. For instance, while still an organizational justice theory,

work by Lind and others (e.g., Lind, 2001) focuses on fairness heuristic theory, which argues

that individuals tend to use the information which they have readily available to them. More

specifically, FHT argues that people normally perceive some risk in giving power to others

and therefore seek to understand where they fit into the situation. In a selection situation this

power is that of rejection or acceptance by a key entity, the organization. Given this, upon

finding themselves in such a situation, individuals immediately begin to assess whether or not

the entity is trustworthy, fair, and respectful (van den Bos, Vermunt, & Wilke, 1997), and,

barring later events, this judgment that developed early in the relationship remains as the lens

through which the applicant views the organization thereafter. Further, these judgments are

most salient at the start of interactions because it is at that point when the least amount of

information is known. In this way, an applicant’s fairness perceptions develop as a type of

sense-making about how the organization treats people and, by extension, will treat the
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 10

applicant. Although FHT has only received scant attention in the applicant reactions

literature, its focus on the development of the applicant’s relationship with the employer

seems a natural fit for the study of applicant reactions.

Another organizational justice approach is fairness theory, which describes how

persons (in this case, applicants) react to negative outcomes (e.g., Folger & Cropanzano,

2001). Applicants can perceive that the employer could have done something different or

should have done something different; or that the employer would have done something

different if such an alternative had been available. This fairness theory approach has been

particularly useful in understanding how applicants process different types of explanations

given to them when they receive a negative result from a selection procedure (e.g., Gilliland,

Groth, Backer IV, Dew, Polly, & Langdon, 2001; Truxillo, Bodner, Bertolino, Bauer, &

Yonce, 2009). Using this approach, explanations can be broken down into justifications (e.g.,

“we used a good, valid procedure”) versus excuses (e.g., “we had no other choice but to do

what we did because there were too many applicants for the number of jobs”). Although

fairness theory shows promise for understanding a range of explanations that organizations

give to employees (e.g., Shaw, Wild, & Colquitt, 2003), a meta-analysis of the effects of

justifications versus excuses shows that the effects of these different types of explanations on

applicant reactions are still unclear (Truxillo et al., 2009). However, this seems to be a

promising area for future research.

WHICH SELECTION PROCEDURES DO APPLICANTS PREFER?

Numerous studies in many countries have examined which selection techniques job

applicants prefer. Meta-analytic summaries of these studies reveal remarkable consistency

across cultures, such that: interviews and work samples are judged the highest in fairness;

résumés, cognitive ability tests, references, biodata and personality tests are judged moderate

in fairness; and honesty tests, personal contacts and graphology are judged the lowest in
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 11

fairness (Anderson et al., 2010; Hausknecht, Day, & Thomas, 2004). Table 1 presents meta-

analytic estimates of favorability ratings for the most common selection procedures. We note,

however, that much of this research on applicant preferences has asked respondents to give

their reactions to different selection procedures simultaneously (e.g., asked their opinion

about a cognitive ability test and also an interview). This raises a question about whether

applicants’ preferences for different types of assessments are quite so clear-cut when they

consider individual assessments (as would be more common in most selection contexts) and

not in comparisons to others. Further, we note that all assessments with a particular label are

not equivalent (e.g., the use of different media in simulations; Bruk-Lee, Drew, & Hawkes,

2013), with considerable variability in content and fidelity. For instance, a non-job-related

structured interview may be viewed less favourably by applicants than a cognitive test

containing job-related questions.

____________________________

Insert Table 1 About Here


____________________________

There are also important findings when it comes to applicant preferences within

specific selection techniques. This is particularly evident with respect to job interviews, with

research indicating that candidates prefer unstructured, as opposed to structured, interviews

(Chapman & Zweig, 2005; Latham & Finnegan, 1993). This is not surprising, as candidates

have more opportunity to voice their opinions and have stronger feelings of control in

unstructured interviews (Madigan & Macan, 2005). Moreover, the standardized nature of

structured interviews may make it more difficult for candidates to manage impressions

(Posthuma, Morgeson, & Campion, 2002). Research also suggests that job applicants prefer

face-to-face interviews over videoconferencing or telephone-based interviews (Bauer,

Truxillo, Paronto, Weekley, & Campion, 2004; Chapman, Uggerslev, & Webster, 2003;

Silvester, Anderson, Haddleton, Cunningham-Snell, & Gibb, 2000). For example, Chapman
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 12

and colleagues (2003) found that face-to-face interviews were associated with higher levels

of procedural justice and job acceptance intentions. Similarly, Bauer et al. (2004) found that

face-to-face interviews were associated with higher levels interpersonal justice,

organizational attractiveness, and lower litigation intentions than questions asked and

answered via technology. These findings may be due, in part, to the fact that interviews

which are conducted in person are less contrived and may thus provide a more comfortable

platform for interpersonal interaction.

Research also suggests that certain groups have preferences for specific selection

tools. For example, Nikolaou and Judge (2007) found that a group of students exhibited

stronger preferences for ability, personality, and honesty tests than a group of employees.

However, additional work is needed to assess whether the mechanism underlying the effect is

due to role differences, age differences, and/or other factors. For example, the students in this

sample were an average of ten years younger than the employees, and the observed

differences may be due to different attitudes across the two groups. Cultural differences may

also play a role in specific preferences. Indeed, Hoang, Truxillo, Erdogan, and Bauer (2012)

found that individuals from the United States believe that employers have a greater right to

obtain information from interviews, personal references, and personal contacts than do

individuals from Vietnam.

As technological advances enable an impressive amount of flexibility, creativity, and

realism to be built into computerized tools (Tippins, 2009), the newest online assessments

need further examination for how applicants will react to them. To date, findings indicate that

computerized ability tests are perceived equally or more positively than their paper-and-

pencil counterparts (see Potosky & Bobko, 2004; Salgado & Moscoso, 2003). This finding

holds true for situational judgment tests (see Chan & Schmitt, 1997; Lievens & Sackett,

2006) and video-based résumés (Hiemstra, Derous, Serlie, & Born, 2012). There are,
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 13

however, a number of important factors that may influence applicant reactions to

computerized tests, including test anxiety (Oostrom, Born, Serlie, & Van Der Molen, 2010),

computer anxiety (Wiechmann & Ryan, 2003), and computer efficacy (Potosky & Bobko,

2004; Sylva & Mol, 2009). The development of techniques to reduce these potential

impediments is likely to have significant implications for job applicants.

Finally, researchers have started to examine different reactions to assessments

administered via the Internet (see Bauer, Truxillo, Tucker, Weathers, Bertolino, Erdogan, &

Campion, 2006; Konradt, Warszta, & Ellwart, 2013) which allow organizations to test large

numbers of job applicants in a timely and cost-effective manner (Oostrom et al., 2013). In

terms of applicant preferences, findings indicate that internet-based assessments are preferred

by external, as opposed to internal, candidates, and are also preferred by internet-savvy

individuals (Sylva & Mol, 2009). A primary concern with this type of assessment, however,

is privacy (see Bauer et al., 2006). Privacy concerns are particularly salient given recent

trends to use social networking sites for selection purposes (Roth, Bobko, Van Iddekinge, &

Thatcher, 2013; Van Iddekinge, Lanivich, Roth, & Junco, 2013), as well as recent trends

towards data sharing across employers (Bauer et al., 2006). We strongly encourage additional

research that focuses on privacy concerns with respect to Internet-based assessments. The

conceptual model advanced by Bauer and colleagues (2006) provides a solid framework for

additional work in this important area.

THE EFFECTS OF APPLICANT REACTIONS:

INDIVIDUAL AND ORGANIZATIONAL OUTCOMES

Research has demonstrated that applicant reactions to selection processes can

influence a range of individual and organizational outcomes. Gilliland (1993) proposed that

perceptions of treatment during selection procedures can have an impact in three ways:

during the hiring process, after hiring, and on applicants’ self-perceptions. It has further been
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 14

argued that selection process fairness (as opposed to outcome fairness) has been central to

applicant reactions research because organizations have relatively little control over what

outcomes applicants receive (Truxillo & Bauer, 2011). In the following section, we provide a

summary of the empirical research on the relationship between applicant fairness reactions

and a variety of individual and organizational outcomes.

Individual Outcomes

Self-perceptions. Gilliland (1993) proposed that procedural fairness and selection

outcomes interact to affect self-perceptions (e.g., self-efficacy, self-esteem, and core self-

evaluations). Research supports the relationship between applicant reactions and several

types of self- perceptions. For example, Bauer et al. (2001) reported that fairness reactions

were positively related to self-esteem. Hausknecht et al. (2004) found a weak positive

relationship between selection fairness and self-efficacy. A meta-analysis by Truxillo et al.

(2009) showed that providing explanations affected applicants’ fairness and self-perceptions.

Further, several studies have found that fairness reactions were positively related to core self-

evaluation (Anderson et al., 2012; Nikolaou & Judge, 2007). Research also supports the

relationship between selection decisions and self-perceptions. A study by Fletcher (1991)

examined the effects of selection decisions on applicants’ self-esteem longitudinally and

found that assessment decisions had a significant effect, with unsuccessful applicants

showing a drop in self-esteem. Finally, research has found support for the moderating effects

of process fairness and selection outcome on self-efficacy (e.g., Gilliland, 1994; Ployhart &

Ryan, 1997). In summary, therefore, it is apparent that fairness perceptions among applicants

have a number of effects upon self-perceptions such as self-efficacy, and core self-

evaluations.

Positive and negative psychological effects. Anderson (2004) suggested the need for

research related to the negative psychological effects of selection methods, most notably for
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 15

those who received a rejection decision given the high ratio of rejected to selected candidates

typical of selection situations. He argued that unsuccessful candidates – but especially those

in internal promotion procedures – could, potentially, claim causal harmful effects upon their

psychological well-being. Indeed several researchers have called for fundamental research

into the mental health outcomes (e.g., well-being) of selection methods upon applicants

(Ford, Truxillo, & Bauer, 2009; Truxillo & Bauer, 2011). However, only limited studies

have been done in this area. Anderson and Goltsi (2006) explored the potential negative

psychological effects of a managerial entry-level assessment center longitudinally. They

found no evidence of negative psychological effects for rejected applicants. On the other

hand, they found that positive effects and psychological well-being declined slightly for

successful applicants. Schinkel et al. (2004) showed that the affective well-being of rejected

participants who received performance feedback was significantly decreased compared to

those who were rejected with no feedback. Bell et al. (2006) found that justice expectations

have a moderating influence, such that perceptions of justice have a greater influence on

negative effects and psychological withdrawal when justice expectations are high.

Selection performance. Research has suggested that applicant reactions influence

applicants’ subsequent selection/test performance (actual and self-evaluation) (e.g., Chan,

1997; Chan & Schmitt, 1997; Gillespie & Ryan, 2012; Hausknecht et al., 2004). An

interaction between process fairness and outcome fairness for self-assessed selection

performance has also been observed (e.g., Ployhart & Ryan, 1997). Similarly, Ployhart and

Ryan (1998) found that the relationship between outcome fairness and performance

expectations was positive when the selection procedure was perceived as fair and negative

when the selection procedure was perceived as unfair. At the same time, several studies

found that selection fairness is influenced by perceived performance, providing evidence for
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 16

a negative feedback loop, or a reciprocal relation between the two variables (e.g., Chan,

Schmitt, Jennings, Clause, & Delbridge, 1998).

Organizational Outcomes

Job performance and criterion-related validity. Earlier reviews were critical of the

lack of studies investigating whether applicant reactions had longer-term effects upon the

critically important factor of job performance (Anderson, Born, & Cunningham-Snell, 2001;

Hausknecht et al., 2004). This was a limitation in that applicant perspectives research had not

unambiguously demonstrated the effect that applicant reactions could ultimately affect the

most important of all outcomes for an organization – job performance. An early study by

Gilliland (1994) reported that job-relatedness perceptions influenced job performance but no

relationship was found between fairness perceptions and job performance. More recently, a

study by McCarthy et al. (2013) found that perceptions of fairness across three samples in

two continents were not related to job performance. Further, no effect was found either

positively or negatively for applicant reactions upon criterion-related validity of test scores.

This research raises the question of whether there are any circumstances by which applicant

reactions may affect later job performance. One possibility is the consideration of internal

applicant reactions and work performance in promotional selection contexts (cf. Ford et al.,

2009; Schaubroeck & Lam, 2004; Truxillo & Bauer, 2011), as the relationship between

reactions and later performance are likely to be stronger among current employees than

among external candidates.

Organizational attractiveness. A number of studies have found that selection

fairness is positively related to organizational attractiveness (e.g., Ababneh, Hackett, &

Schat, 2013; Anderson et al., 2012; Hausknecht et al., 2004; Schreurs, Derous, Proost, &

Witte, 2010; Walsh, Tuller, Barnes-Farrell, & Matthews, 2010), and that this effect lasts over

time (Bauer, Maertz, Dolen, & Campion, 1998). In their meta-analysis, Hausknecht et al.
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 17

(2004) found that perceptions of procedural (r=.44) and distributive justice (r=.34) and

organizational attractiveness were correlated. Furthermore, Lazer, Zinger, and Lachterman

(2007) found that social fairness has an influence on job attractiveness. However, selection

fairness may not affect applicants’ organizational attractiveness when they are already highly

attracted to the organization (Truxillo, Bauer, Campion, & Paronto, 2002).

Organizational commitment and job satisfaction. Gilliland (1993) proposed

organizational commitment and job satisfaction as outcomes of selection fairness perceptions.

Bauer et al. (2001) showed that social and structural fairness were positively related to

organizational commitment. Robertson, Iles, Gratton, and Sharpley (1991) demonstrated that

applicants who passed exhibited higher organizational commitment. Ryan, Sacco,

McFarland, and Kriska (2000) found that applicants who self-selected out of the process had

more negative perceptions of the organization and lower job commitment. In terms of job

satisfaction, García-Izquierdo, Moscoso, and Ramos-Villagrasa (2012) found that procedural

justice was positively related to job satisfaction in a promotional selection context. The most

compelling study in this topic is perhaps the longitudinal field study by Ambrose and

Cropanzano (2003). They found that the perceived fairness of promotional procedure was

related to job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Additional systematic work

examining the effects of fairness reactions on job satisfaction and organizational commitment

is needed (Truxillo & Bauer, 2011). To be specific, these outcomes may be best examined

longitudinally as an outcome of internal applicant reactions to promotion, where these

outcomes are more proximal to the selection situation, rather than an entry-level selection

context.

Recommendation intentions. Research has supported the positive relationship

between applicant fairness reactions and intentions to recommend the organization to others

as a potential employer (e.g., Ababneh et al., 2013; Bell et al., 2006; Geenen, Proost, van
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 18

Dijke, de Witte, & von Grumbkow, 2012; Giumetti & Sinar, 2012; Hausknecht et al., 2004;

Schreurs et al., 2010). Perceptions of fairness have also been found to influence

recommendation intentions among internal candidates applying for promotions (McCarthy,

Hrabluik, & Jelly, 2009). However, some research has found that the relationship may

weaken over time (Bauer et al., 1998; Gilliland, 1994), suggesting that such recommendation

intention effects may be rather short-lived.

Litigation intentions and actual legal challenges. Gilliland (1993) proposed legal

battles as an outcome of perceived fairness. Anderson (2011a) proposed a model and process

of ‘perceived job discrimination’ to suggest that applicants who perceive extremely unfair

treatment or outcomes may, in certain circumstances, be minded to lodge complaints and

even initiate expensive legal action against a recruiter organization. The legal ramification of

applicant reactions might be the most important reasons that many organizations are

interested in applicant reactions to selection procedures. Empirical research in laboratory

settings has supported the negative relationship between selection fairness and litigation

intentions (Ababneh et al., 2013; Bauer et al., 2001; Bauer et al., 2004), and likelihood of

complaints (Thibodeaux & Kudisch, 2003). In a field sample of actual job applicants, Geenen

et al. (2012) found that distributive justice expectations were negatively related to litigation

intentions, particularly in the presence of negative effects. However, no empirical studies

have explored the relationship between applicant fairness reactions and actual legal

outcomes, which remain the weakest link in applicant reactions research (Anderson, 2011b;

Truxillo & Bauer, 2011). Moreover, litigation intentions and taking actual legal actions are

very different, at least partially because proceeding with a lawsuit requires a great effort from

the applicant. This is a shortcoming in our understanding of negative applicant reactions at

the extremes of the spectrum and given the costs to an organization, both financial and
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 19

reputational, of having to defend such legal actions, this is an area where we feel much more

research is needed.

Process satisfaction. Research on the relationship between selection fairness and

process satisfaction has suggested that applicant fairness reactions are positively related to

satisfaction with the selection process and perceptions of selection system, and that this

relationship lasts over time (Macan, Avedon, Paese, & Smith, 1994; Truxillo, Bauer, &

Sanchez, 2001). Giumetti and Sinar (2012) reported that procedural fairness had a strong

relationship with process satisfaction. Also, Hausknecht et al.’s (2004) meta-analysis found

that fairness dimensions were positively correlated (r=.36 for perceived predictive validity

and r=.39 for face validity) with attitudes about selection processes.

Turnover intentions. Surprisingly, rather limited research has been done into the

effects of applicant reactions upon intention to quit. Truxillo et al. (2002) found no

relationship between providing fairness information and later turnover intentions among

those hired. These findings may be due the nature of the sample (police applicants), as they

were already highly attracted to the organization. Another study by Ambrose and Cropanzano

(2003) showed that fairness perceptions regarding a promotional procedure are significantly

related to turnover intentions over time. Hausknecht et al. (2004) were not able to include

work behavioral outcomes, such as applicant turnover, withdrawal, and organizational

commitment, in their meta-analysis due to the lack of studies into these outcomes. The reason

is that these behaviors are not applicable in entry-level contexts, where most applicant

reactions research has been conducted, since external applicants often have little at stake and

minimal knowledge about the organization (Truxillo, Steiner, & Gilliland, 2004). Again, we

propose that turnover intentions resulting from early applicant reactions is a potentially

fruitful area for additional future research.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 20

Withdrawal. A review of the few existing studies on this topic provides equivocal

results. Ployhart and Ryan (1998) found that process fairness was related to withdrawal from

the selection process. Another study by Schmit and Ryan (1997), based on interviews with

applicants who withdrew from the police selection process, reported that about 12% of

applicants mentioned the procedural injustice as a reason for withdrawing. On the other hand,

Ryan et al. (2000) found that applicant perceptions were generally unrelated to withdrawal

behavior. Also, Truxillo et al. (2002) found no relationship between selection information

and continuation in the selection process; Truxillo et al. noted that this may be due to the

nature of the sample (i.e., police applicants) as they were highly attracted to the organization;

thus, these results may not be applicable to all applicant pools.

Reapplication intentions. In one of the few studies on reapplication intentions,

Ployhart and Ryan (1997) found interactions between process and outcome fairness

influencing reapplication intentions, such that when individuals perceive a fair process, there

is essentially no relationship between outcome fairness and reapplication intentions.

However, when individuals perceive an unfair process, there is a strong negative relationship

between reapplication intentions and outcome fairness. LaHuis, MacLane, and Schlessman

(2007) also found a relationship between justice and reapplication intentions, such that the

relationship was stronger when perceived performance was high. Furthermore, Ryan, Boyce,

Ghumman, Jundt, and Schmidt (2009) found that intention to apply for a job was influenced

by perceptions of fairness. Finally, a study by Gilliland et al. (2001) reported that applicants

receiving an explanation based on fairness in a rejection letter were at least twice as likely to

reapply to the organization than those who received a standard rejection letter. In other

words, the fairness explanation appeared to improve actual reapplication behavior.

Customer purchase intentions. Some research has suggested that there is a

relationship between applicant reactions and both purchase intentions (Macan et al., 1994;
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 21

Wiechmann & Ryan, 2003) and service intention (Maertz, Mosley, Bauer, Posthuma, &

Campion, 2004). This positive relationship between selection fairness (job relatedness) and

purchase intentions (r=.14) was generally supported by Hausknecht et al.’s (2004) meta-

analysis. Also, Rolland and Steiner (2007) reported that improving selection fairness by

providing an explanation improved intention of rejected applicants to use the organization’s

services. We encourage future research to examine the impact of reactions on applicant’s

actual purchasing behaviors and brand image perceptions, particularly given the vast

applicant pools that technology-based selection systems have enabled organizations to

examine.

Job acceptance. The relationship between applicant reactions and job acceptance

intentions has received considerable support. For example, the meta-analytic results of

Hausknecht et al. (2004) and several studies have found a positive relationship between

selection fairness, selection expectations, and job acceptance intentions (e.g., Bell et al.,

2006; LaHuis, 2005; Ryan et al., 2009; Schreurs et al., 2010; Truxillo et al., 2002). With

regard to the actual job acceptance decision as an outcome, however, studies have produced

more mixed findings. For example, Carless (2006) reported that applicants’ job acceptance

decision was unrelated to applicant reactions to selection characteristics; on the other hand, a

study by Walsh et al. (2010) showed that fairness perceptions were positively related to

actual job acceptance.

Summary. Our review of the applicant reactions literature has suggested that their

reactions to selection process and fairness relate to various individual and organizational

outcomes – several of which are of considerable importance. Although the effects of

applicant reactions on many of these outcomes seem clear, these effects appear to vary over

time and context, particularly whether the study concerns external applicants versus

promotions where emotions can run high. Applicant perspective research has come of age in
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 22

the sense that over recent years major advances have included several meta-analyses

summarizing individual primary studies and research issues (e.g., Anderson et al., 2010;

Hausknecht et al., 2004; Truxillo et al., 2009), and a number of narrative reviews have been

published summarizing the literature to date.

ANTECEDENTS AND MODERATORS OF APPLICANT REACTIONS

Other factors besides the hiring process have been shown to be antecedents of

applicant reactions, and a number of factors have been shown or hypothesized to moderate

these reactions. We describe these below.

Antecedents. One important consideration is that despite what organizations may try

to do to affect applicant reactions, such reactions are a function of variables outside of the

organization’s control. An example of such individual differences is applicant personality.

Truxillo, Bauer, Campion, and Paronto (2006) examined whether applicant Big Five

personality measured at baseline affected later applicant reactions in terms of fairness, self-

perceptions, and organizational attractiveness. They found that personality variables related

to applicant perceptions after controlling for the applicant’s test score: Neuroticism had a

negative relationship with reactions, and agreeableness had a positive relationship.

Honkaniemi, Feldt, Metsapelto, and Tolvanen (2013) found that certain personality profiles

(e.g., “resilient”) had more positive reactions than others (e.g., “overcontrolled”). Similarly,

Viswesvaran and Ones (2004) found that individual differences predicted the importance of

various selection system characteristics, for example, that those higher in cognitive ability

emphasized the content of the selection procedure, and extraverts valued the process of

developing the selection procedure. Taken together, these studies suggest that applicant

individual differences may dispose them to react in certain ways to the hiring process,

regardless of what the process itself involves. However, a range of other individual

differences, such as core self-evaluations, emotional intelligence, and integrity (all of great
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 23

interest in the W/I/O psychology literature) remain relatively unexplored in the realm of

applicant reactions.

Moderators. There are a number of moderators that can be considered in the study of

applicant reactions and test-taking predispositions. For example, external job applicants

versus internal candidates up for promotion may have different reactions and predispositions.

Although external candidates have received the overwhelming degree of research attention

in the applicant reactions literature, internal candidates for promotion appear to have

relatively strong reactions (e.g., Ambrose & Cropanzano, 2003; Truxillo & Bauer, 1999),

suggesting that this is an important moderator to consider. Moreover, internal candidates

obviously have much more information about the organization, making their reactions more

stable; on the other hand, such an established relationship with the organization may also

make disappointing results a form of contract breach – a serious concern to the organization.

Despite calls in the literature for increased empirical examination of internal

candidates’ reactions, there have been relatively few studies of this issue in recent years (for

exceptions see Giumetti & Sinar, 2012; McCarthy et al., 2009). However, Ford et al. (2009)

make a compelling argument that the effects of either rejection (low outcome favorability) or

negative reactions among internal candidates can have serious negative performance

consequences for the organization, and can also affect a number of outcomes including

applicant stress outcomes. In other words, the differential effects of applicant reactions

among internal candidates is a potentially critical concern for organizations, and its potential

effects on stress and well-being outcomes may have societal consequences as well.

Another moderator that may be important to understanding reactions is differences in

the national and cultural context, with differences in applicant expectations for the hiring

process and for employer behavior, and differences in terms of legal protections during the

hiring process. Although such international differences are discussed elsewhere in this
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 24

chapter (and generally show few differences in which selection procedures applicants prefer),

we note that research has found international differences in the effects of justice across

cultures (Shao, Rupp, Skarlicki, & Jones, 2013). Such findings suggest that future cross-

cultural research may want to focus less on differences in reactions to specific selection

procedures and more on how applicant perceptions of selection affect their later attitudes

towards themselves and the employer, as well as actual behavioral differences.

CROSS-NATIONAL SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES IN APPLICANT

REACTIONS

There has been considerable on-going debate over whether applicant reactions are

primarily caused by within-country cultural effects (so-called ‘situational specificity’) or

whether, conversely, reactions are more a function of a general pattern of responses that is

common across countries and cultures (so-called ‘reaction generalizility’ by Anderson et al.,

2010). Certainly, the role of cultural values in applicant perceptions is a concern for

multinational organizations seeking to standardize selection practices, especially with the

growing number of multinational organizations. This has arguably attracted the attention of

many researchers to the issue applicant reactions across various countries (e.g., Anderson et

al., 2012; Anderson & Witvliet, 2008; Hoang et al., 2012).

A recent meta-analysis by Anderson et al. (2010) using 38 independent samples and

covering 10 popular selection procedures found considerable similarity in reaction

favorability. They found that applicants generally prefer certain selection methods (e.g.,

interview and work sample test), whereas other methods are least preferred (graphology,

honesty tests, and personal contacts) across cultures. Moreover, Ryan and colleagues (2009)

examined applicant reactions to 8 selection methods across 21 countries. They found that

perceptions of selection methods were mostly similar across individuals holding different

cultural values, showing greater convergence than divergence regarding applicant perceptions
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 25

globally. Interestingly, they also found that a country’s gross domestic product was

negatively related to both applicants’ predictive validity and fairness perceptions.

Steiner and Gilliland (2001) suggest that some cultural dimensions are most likely to

influence the salience of various distributive and procedural justice rules. One example is

Hofstede’s (1980; 2001) notion of uncertainty avoidance and its relation to the legal and

political systems’ characteristics. As the laws against discrimination in the US are explicit,

American applicants might be more aware of their rights and opportunities to act in response

to the perceived unfairness of discrimination (Truxillo et al., 2004). Another study by Walsh

et al. (2010) examined the moderating role of societal culture (performance orientation and

uncertainty avoidance) on the effects of selection fairness perceptions. These authors found

that performance orientation moderates the effect of structural fairness perceptions on

organizational attractiveness with a stronger relationship seen among applicants from more

performance-oriented countries. Also, Harris, Van Hoye, and Lievens (2003) examined

cross-cultural differences in privacy perceptions of an Internet-based selection system and

found a few significant differences between the US and Belgian respondents. However, the

role of culture on applicant reactions to selection procedures warrants further investigation. In

Table 2, we provide a summary of research linking Hofstede’s cultural dimensions to

procedural justice of personnel selection based on the applicant reactions literature (i.e.,

Bertolino & Steiner, 2007; Phillips & Gully, 2001; Steiner & Gilliland, 2001; Walsh et al.,

2010).

____________________________

Insert Table 2 About Here


____________________________

We suspect that applicant reactions will have ‘core’ elements of a basic response that

are internationally generalizable, but also that there will be more local, culturally-specific
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 26

‘surface reactions’ that need to be considered by employers. One meta-analytic finding in

particular is important here – that applicant reactions have been found to correlate positively

with the criterion-related validity of popular selection predictor methods (Anderson et al.,

2010) – suggesting that organizations using more valid methods are likely to also elicit more

positive reactions from candidates.

TEST-TAKING DISPOSITIONS AND REACTIONS

Test-taking dispositions are related to stable individual differences, such as

personality traits, and include test-taking anxiety, test-taking motivation, and test-taking self-

efficacy (Arvey, Strickland, Drauden, & Martin, 1990; Chan & Schmitt, 2004; Ryan &

Ployhart, 2000). For example, test-taking anxiety and test-taking efficacy have been linked to

neuroticism (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic, Ahmetoglu, & Furnham, 2008), while test-taking

motivation has been linked to conscientiousness (e.g., Salgado, Remeseiro, & Iglesias, 1996).

Research on test-taking dispositions originated in the educational realm, with studies

examining test-taking anxiety among high school and university students (see meta-analyses

by Hembree, 1998; Seipp, 1990; Speilberger & Vagg, 1995). The majority of these studies

assessed general measures of anxiety by administering the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

(Speilberger, 1983). Findings were unequivocal, with meta-analytic reviews demonstrating

that test-taking anxiety has significant negative effects on test performance (see Hembree,

1998; Seipp, 1991). Later research in this area introduced the concepts of test-taking

motivation (e.g., Sundree & Kitsantas, 2004; Wolf & Smith, 1995) and test-taking self-

efficacy (e.g., Díaz, Glass, Arnkoff, & Tanofsky-Kraff,, 2001; Smith, Arnkoff, & Wright,

1990), each of which demonstrated positive effects with test performance.

The concept of test-taking dispositions was introduced to the domain of personnel

selection by Arvey and collegues (1990), whose seminal work included the development of

an instrument for assessing applicant reactions to employment tests. Their instrument, the
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 27

Test Attitude Survey (TAS), contains nine dimensions: motivation, lack of concentration,

belief in tests, comparative anxiety, test ease, external attribution, need for achievement,

future effects and test preparation. The TAS provided the foundation for a number of studies

on the test-taking dispositions of job applicants, the majority focusing on test anxiety and

motivation – the scales with the strongest psychometric properties (McCarthy & Goffin,

2003). The work by Arvey and colleagues also triggered a number of studies that

operationalized test-taking dispositions in unique ways. In the sections below, we review this

research, beginning with the theories and findings for test-taking anxiety, followed by test-

taking motivation, and concluding with test-taking self-efficacy.

Test-taking anxiety. Many applicants experience feelings of nervousness when

confronted with selection tests because a selection decision can be a life-changing event that

may require a major career and life shift. Unfortunately, high levels of anxiety among

applicants can result in applicant withdrawal from the selection process, lower levels of

organizational attractiveness, and reduced intentions to recommend the organization to others

(Arvey et al., 1990; Hausknecht et al., 2009; McCarthy et al., 2009; McCarthy & Goffin,

2004; Ryan, 2001; Schmit & Ryan, 1997).

Test-taking anxiety is also directly related to lower scores on selection tests

(Hausknecht et al., 2004; McCarthy et al., 2009). Cognitive interference theories provide the

foundation for understanding this relation (e.g., Processing Efficiency Theory; Eysenck &

Calvo, 1992; Attentional Control Theory; Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007;

Integrative Resource Theory; Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989). These theories assert that anxiety

interferes with an individual’s ability to attend to and process performance-relevant

information, resulting in lower levels of performance. Applied to selection contexts, this

means that high levels of anxiety may inhibit an applicant’s ability to process and understand

selection procedure content, resulting in lower levels of performance and a reduced


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 28

probability that they will be accepted for the job. Consistent with this proposition, high levels

of test-taking anxiety have been found to be positively related to cognitive interference in

selection (Ryan & Chan, 1999) and promotional (McCarthy et al., 2009) contexts. Test-

taking anxiety has also been found to exhibit negative relations with test performance (e.g.,

Arvey et al., 1990; Hausknecht et al., 2004; McCarthy et al., 2013), job interview

performance (Cook, Vance, & Spector, 2000; McCarthy & Goffin, 2004) and performance on

the job (McCarthy et al., 2013).

These findings have led several researchers to suggest that anxiety may reduce the

criterion-related validity of selection tests, such that high levels of anxiety may introduce

extraneous variance into test scores and reduce the extent to which the scores reflect

applicant’s true ability levels (e.g., Arvey et al., 1990; Ryan & Ployhart, 2000). In line with

this proposition, there is evidence that a composite measure of test attitudes, which included

test-taking anxiety, differentially moderated the validity of a cognitive ability test (more

valid) and a personality test (less valid) in a lab setting (Schmit & Ryan, 1992). In contrast, a

recent field study with job applicants found that taking anxiety did not have any effect on the

validity of selection tests (McCarthy et al., 2013). This latter finding is consistent with

research by Reeve and Bonaccio (2008) who found that test anxiety has no significant

influence on observed test scores.

Combined, these findings demonstrate that test anxiety impacts both test performance

and job performance and is therefore an important consideration for both applicants and

organizations. This highlights the value of training programs to help applicants minimize test

anxiety. At the same time, findings generally indicate that test validity is not affected by

anxiety, that is, test scores predict who will do well on the job regardless of applicant levels

of anxiety, providing additional support for the use of standardized tests in selection contexts.

Research also finds evidence of gender differences in test and interview anxiety, with
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 29

females typically scoring higher than males (Cassaidy & Johnson, 2002; Sieverding, 2009).

At the same time, findings indicate that anxiety is actually more detrimental to the

performance of male candidates (Feiler & Powell, 2013; McCarthy & Goffin, 2005). As

detailed by McCarthy and Goffin (2005), this is likely due to the fact that females are more

efficacious in coping with the stress experienced during testing situations. Thus, while

females experience higher levels of test-taking anxiety, results suggest that this does not put

them at a disadvantage in test-taking situations. Future research that focuses on coping

strategies for male applicants may prove particularly helpful. Job applicants from minority

groups are also more likely to experience high levels of test anxiety due to stereotype threat

(Ployhart, Ziegert, & McFarland, 2003). This may be because minority candidates are

expected to perform worse on tests given decades of research findings which indicate that

Blacks and Hispanics consistently score lower than Whites (see Nguyen & Ryan, 2008).

Minority candidates experience stereotype threat (see Steele & Aronson, 1995) when they

worry that their test performance will validate the negative stereotype (Ployhart et al., 2003).

Stereotype threat has been found to be related to higher levels of test-taking anxiety, and

lower levels of test-taking motivation, test fairness, belief in tests, and test-taking efficacy

(Arvey et al., 1990; Chan, Schmitt, DeShon, Clause, & Delbridge, 1997; Ployhart et al.,

2003; Schmit & Ryan, 1997). There is minimal evidence, however, that stereotype threat

actually accounts for sub-group differences in actual selection test performance (see Ployhart

& Harold, 2004; Sackett & Ryan, 2012). Thus, future research is needed on the mechanism

by which stereotype threat may influence on test-taking reactions and test performance in

personnel selection samples.

Finally, there has been considerable theoretical and empirical work on the

conceptualization and measurement of anxiety in selection contexts. With respect to job

interviews, McCarthy and Goffin (2004) developed a model of anxiety which contains five
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 30

dimensions: Appearance Anxiety, Communication Anxiety, Social Anxiety, Behavioral

Anxiety and Performance Anxiety. Findings indicated that candidates experience varying

levels of the five interview anxiety types, and that overall interview anxiety is negatively

related to interview performance. With respect to selection tests, Proost, Derous, Schreurs,

Hagtvet, and De Witte (2008) developed a multi-dimensional instrument called the Self-

versus Other-Referenced Anxiety Questionnaire (SOAQ). Self-Referenced Anxiety reflects

concerns that originate from internally-driven pressures (e.g., the candidate is worried about

obtaining a low score because s/he will blame themself for poor performance), while Other-

Referenced Anxiety, reflects concerns that originate from externally-driven pressures (i.e.,

the candidate is worried about obtaining a low score because others will blame them for poor

performance). Findings indicated that self-referenced test anxiety demonstrated a positive

relation with test performance, while other-referenced test anxiety demonstrated a negative

relation with test performance.

Combined, these studies provide a more nuanced perspective into the nature and

effects of anxiety in selection contexts and answer calls for increased focus on the

dimensions of applicant reactions (Chan & Schmitt, 2004). They also have important

implications for future research on applicant anxiety, as different types of anxiety may

exhibit unique relations with core outcomes, such as test performance, job performance, and

test-taking attitudes. Understanding how the various dimensions of anxiety operate is also

particularly useful for the development of training interventions that can focus on more

specific strategies and techniques.

Test-taking motivation. Applicants also vary in the extent to which they are

motivated to do well on selection tests. Consistent with Gilliland’s (1993) model, results

generally indicate that test-taking motivation is an outcome of process fairness (e.g., Bauer et

al., 2006; Hausknecht et al., 2004). Findings also reveal that job applicants with high levels
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 31

of test-taking motivation are less likely to withdraw from the selection process (Schmit &

Ryan, 1992; 1997), more likely to achieve high test scores in selection contexts (Hausknecht

et al., 2004), and more likely to achieve high scores in promotion contexts (McCarthy et al.,

2009).

As with test-taking anxiety, these findings have led to concerns that differences in

applicant motivation to perform on selection tests may result in biased estimates of their true

qualifications (Ryan & Ployhart, 2000; Smither, Reilly, Millsap, Pearlman, & Stoffey, 1993).

Evidence examining this issue is mixed. Specifically, a study by Schmit and Ryan (1992)

found that a composite measure of test-taking attitudes (which included test motivation)

moderated the validity of a cognitive ability and personality test in a lab setting. Similarly,

O’Neill, Goffin, and Gellatly (2010) found that test-taking motivation moderated the validity

of a test assessing the trait of steadfastness in an applied context. More recently, McCarthy

and colleagues (2013) found no evidence that test-taking motivation influenced the criterion-

related validity of selection tests. Future research that aligns test-taking motivation with the

constructs assessed in the selection test (e.g., tests assessing achievement and/or drive;

cognitive skills) and the constructs assessed in job performance (e.g., core task performance,

extra-role behaviors) may provide additional insight into motivation’s role as a moderator.

From a psychometric perspective, one of the most popular instruments for assessing

test-taking motivation is the motivation subscale of the TAS, which was developed by Arvey

and colleagues in 1990. More recently, Sanchez, Truxillo, and Bauer (2000) developed a

measure of test-taking motivation based on expectancy theory. This was the first study to link

test-taking motivation to an established motivation theory, as well as the first to

conceptualize test-taking motivation as a multidimensional construct. Their measure, the

Valence, Instrumentality, Expectancy Scale (VIEMS), boasts strong psychometric properties

and contains three subscales: Valence, or the attractiveness of obtaining a position;


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 32

Instrumentality, or the extent to which applicants feel that high test performance will result in

a job offer; and Expectancy, or the probability that effort will lead to a high test score. The

three dimensions of the VIEMS have been found to explain variance in actual test

performance, perceived test performance and test-taking experience (Sanchez et al., 2000).

Moreover, the valence and instrumentality scales have been found to exhibit positive

relations with applicant emotional stability, extraversion and positive affect (Bott, Snell,

Darhling, & Smith, 2010). Ultimately, the VIEMS enables more nuanced theory-based

assessments of the antecedents, consequences and processes associated with the components

of test-taking motivation. Future research should examine the role that the three components

of test-taking motivation have with respect to different types of applicants, different types of

selection tests, and/or different types of jobs. Moreover, Ryan (2001) notes the dynamic

nature of dispositions such as motivation (an issue that is likely true for most applicant

reactions; Chan & Schmitt, 2004), but this area has received little attention.

Test-taking self-efficacy. Self-efficacy refers to the belief that one can succeed on a

certain task (Bandura, 1982) and has been found to have a strong relation with job

performance (Sadri & Robertson, 1993). This holds true in selection contexts, as applicants

who perceive that they may not do well on a selection test, or who demonstrate low levels of

test-taking efficacy, typically obtain lower test scores (Bauer et al., 1998; McCarthy et al.,

2013; Ryan, Ployhart, Greguras, & Schmit, 1998). Individuals with low levels of test-taking

efficacy are also more likely to have high test-taking anxiety, low levels of test-taking

motivation, and reduced belief in tests (McCarthy et al., 2013; Ryan et al., 1998).

Gilliland (1993) suggested that perceptions of justice are likely to influence self-

efficacy perceptions. In support of this proposition, Bauer et al. (1998) found that perceptions

of procedural fairness explained variance in test-taking self-efficacy beyond outcome

favorability (i.e., whether candidates passed or failed). At least three additional studies have
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 33

found that selection fairness is related to self-efficacy (Maertz, Bauer, Mosley, Posthuma, &

Campion, 2005; Truxillo et al., 2002; Truxillo, Bauer, & Sanchez, 2001). These findings

highlight the value of ensuring that selection tests are viewed in as fair, as fair selection

systems can serve to boost candidate efficacy. Other potential antecedents of efficacy that

have been empirically supported are: being male, being hired with a similar test in the past,

and general levels of self-efficacy (Maertz et al., 2005). Thus, future work that focuses on

gender differences with respect to test-taking efficacy, as well as specific techniques for

improving test efficacy is likely to prove valuable.

Dispositional versus situational reactions. In contrast to dispositional reactions,

situational reactions reflect more transient, time-bound experiences. Given that fairness

perceptions are more specific to the characteristics of the selection process than are test-

taking anxiety, motivation and self-efficacy, they are more situational in nature (Chan &

Schmitt, 2004; McCarthy et al., 2013. Theoretically, dispositional and situational reactions

are expected to have differential relations with key variables. Specifically, reactions that are

more situationally-based (e.g., perceptions of fairness) are likely to exhibit significant

relations with proximal outcomes (e.g., test-taking performance, applicant withdrawal

behaviors), but non-significant relations with distal outcomes (e.g., job performance,

turnover) because the situation-specific nature of these reactions makes it unlikely that they

will influence how applicants behave on the job. In contrast, dispositional test-taking

reactions (e.g., test-taking anxiety) are likely to exhibit significant relations with both

proximal (e.g., test-taking performance, withdrawal behaviors), and distal (e.g., job

performance, turnover) outcomes. These propositions are consistent with affective events

theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), the theory of attitude–behaviour relations (Ajzen &

Fishbein, 1977), and models of job performance (e.g., Campbell, McCloy, Oppler, & Sager,

1993), each of which holds that attitude-behavior relations are strongest when they are
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 34

proximal in duration and focus.

Empirical research findings support these propositions, as perceptions of fairness have

been found to be related to test-taking performance and recommendation intentions, while

test-taking anxiety and motivation have been found to be related to test-taking performance,

applicant withdrawal, and performance on the job (see Hauscknecht et al., 2004; McCarthy et

al., 2013). Future research is needed to examine whether situational reactions also exhibit

stronger relations with proximal antecedents (e.g., procedural characteristics), and whether

dispositional reactions also exhibit stronger relations with distal antecedents (e.g.,

organizational image).

DESIGN ISSUES: RECOMMENDED DESIGNS FOR APPLICANT PERSPECTIVE

RESEARCH

From early in is development, the applicant reactions literature had begun to

emphasize longitudinal and time-lagged designs. The primary reason for this approach is that

it is not enough to assess applicant perceptions after a selection procedure. Rather, it is

important whether or not to attribute any post-selection reactions to the selection procedure,

or to pre-existing perceptions on the part of the applicant. Put differently, the question is

whether applicant perceptions changed as a result of the selection procedure, or were they

already developed when the applicant came into the selection system. That said, many

applicant reactions studies have been conducted in laboratory settings, or have not measured

applicant reactions at the appropriate time (e.g., after feedback). Accordingly, studies have

generally not examined the dynamic nature of applicant reactions (see Chan & Schmitt,

2004) and how applicant reactions and affect may change over time. Below we describe the

contexts within which applicant reactions studies are typically or can be conducted and some

typical design issues associated with each.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 35

Laboratory studies. Much of the applicant reactions research has used laboratory

studies of undergraduate applicants, either reacting to selection scenarios (e.g., Steiner &

Gilliland, 1996) or subjected to different selection procedures (e.g., Gilliland, 1994). Such

samples are attractive for a number of reasons: Participants can legally and ethically be

assigned to different selection conditions and any differences in their treatment can be

attributed to the selection procedure. On the other hand, students in simulated conditions are

not actual applicants and have little at stake in the hiring process, and real applicants often

have numerous factors that could affect their reactions to an organization (e.g., perceptions of

an employer’s “brand”, their own need for employment) besides the selection procedure. In

fact, it is unclear from meta-analytic studies whether the results of laboratory studies over-

estimate the effects of applicant reactions or under-estimate them (Hausknecht et al., 2004;

Truxillo et al., 2009).

Applicant samples in field contexts. One of the primary reasons for the development

of longitudinal designs in applicant reactions research is particular to the challenge of

conducting field studies with actual applicants: Participants cannot be randomly assigned to

experimental conditions. That is, it would be unethical and impractical, and in many

countries illegal, to expose different applicants within the same cohort to different selection

procedures or to differential treatment that may in some way affect their performance in the

hiring process. For that reason, longitudinal designs allow for the measurement of baseline

perceptions prior to the hiring process so that applicants can act as their own control in the

hiring process. This is particularly true when assessing outcomes that focus on the individual

(e.g., self-efficacy) or perceptions of the organization (e.g., organizational attractiveness):

Applicant self-efficacy or organizational attractiveness would be measured prior to testing as

used as control variables in assessing post-test reactions. However, when the outcome of

interest is focused on the selection procedure itself – most typically, fairness perceptions of
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 36

the hiring process – the approach is slightly different, because applicants cannot have

developed perceptions of the hiring process prior to the process when know little about it.

Instead, surrogate measures are often used, such as general perceptions of how fair it is to

test applicants.

It is possible under certain conditions to assign applicants to conditions in field

settings, with the conditions based on pre-existing groups. For example, Truxillo et al. (2002)

used a quasi-experimental design to assign police applicants to “no explanation” and

“explanation” conditions, with applicants in each condition coming from separate cohorts of

applicants six months apart. Or, naturally occurring groups of applicants who failed or who

passed a selection procedure could also be compared. With these sorts of quasi-experimental

designs (see Cook, Campbell, & Peracchio, 1990), baseline (pre-selection) measures become

particularly important to account for any pre-existing differences in applicants and account

for any differences statistically (e.g., as control variables in a regression analysis). Although

these quasi-experimental designs have not been used much in the applicant reactions

literature, we encourage their use in future because of their ability to compare how the

differential treatment of applicants affects applicant outcomes. Alternatively, when the

research questions is appropriate, the assignment of actual applicants to experimental

conditions pre-selection (e.g., using a quasi-experiment; Truxillo et al., 2002) or post-

selection (e.g., providing different treatment to applicants after a selection decision; e.g.,

Gilliland et al., 2001) is sometimes possible and can also allow for comparisons between

applicants exposed to different experimental treatments. Such studies would eliminate any

question as to whether any results are attributable to applicant reactions or from some artifact

of the unrealistic settings presented to student samples. Further, it is through such field

studies that we can come to identify the factors in the hiring process that can affect individual

applicants, organizations, and society as a whole.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 37

Summary. A large number of applicant reactions studies have been conducted in

laboratory settings using undergraduates, primarily because of the greater control provided as

well as the ability to manipulate factors (e.g., unfair procedures; Ployhart & Ryan, 1998) that

might not be possible in the field. Field studies provide greater realism and thus greater

generalizability to the real world, primarily because they reflect that actual factors (e.g.,

applicant motivation) that may affect applicant perceptions. At the same time, field studies

raise challenges in terms of requirements that baseline (pre-selection) perceptions be

measured and statistically controlled.

GUIDELINES FOR HRM/SELECTION PRACTITIONERS

Throughout this chapter we have highlighted the abundant academic research which

has been conducted to date on applicant reactions. This is a critical part to our understanding

of the complex and important aspects of IWO. However, as Rynes (2007) wrote in her

Academy of Management Journal Editorial “What academics and practitioners can do, alone

and together,” we agree that there is a great need for research translation and knowledge

transfer. To this end, this section of our chapter is focused on sharing such a research

translation. We do this by building upon a recently and jointly-published set of professional

society guidelines for selection practitioners which provide pragmatic and easy-to-understand

evidence-based advice drawing upon applicant reactions research and how these findings can

best be integrated into employee selection procedures by organizations (Bauer, McCarthy,

Anderson, Truxillo, & Salgado, 2012). The guidelines were published jointly by SIOP and

SHRM and give an application-orientated summary research on the applicant’s perspective

and especially the implications arising from research by IWO psychologists and crucial

implications for the design of recruitment and selection procedures.

Overall, organizations have goals around applicant reactions which relate to avoiding

negative reactions so that legal actions (e.g., lawsuits, formal complaints) and bad PR (e.g.,
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 38

negative word of mouth, low online ratings) are avoided or at least ameliorated. This is

important because in today’s competitive selection climate, only a small fraction of those

who apply for most jobs will ultimately be hired. On the flipside, organizations are also

motivated to maximize the potentially positive reactions of applicants so that the best

candidates will stay in the selection process, will accept potential job offers, and will be more

effective once they are on the job. Taken together, organizations can benefit from a

translation of applicant reactions research to actual employment settings.

Organizational Best Practices and Recommendations

To summarize some of the key points made in a recent white paper (Bauer et al.,

2012), eight recommendations for practitioners are offered. Give explanations and/or

justifications about selection procedures. Be sure to keep this relatively non-technical, but do

share what the organization does, why it’s valid, and how it is used by the organization.

While the outcome may not be the one that applicants desired, understanding “why” is

helpful. Relatedly, use job-related selection procedures. As has been shown throughout the

current chapter, applicants care whether or not the selection methods they experience appear

and are job-related. This means that it’s important to examine the organization’s selection

procedures to make sure they are scientifically sound (valid) and practically sound. This is a

basic point but one that is a starting point for good applicant reactions. Provide job

applicants with an opportunity to perform. Doing so helps them see the fairness of the

process regardless of how their actual performance on the selection procedure or their actual

outcome. Provide timely feedback to job candidates about their application status. No news

is bad news in the mind of most job applicants. Avoid generating undue anxiety by giving

applicants at least some degree of information about your timeline. Deal honestly with

applicants. This avoids a lot of potential problems down the line. Treat candidates with

respect. This recommendation holds for all potential interactions in the workplace. Respect
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 39

goes a long way toward mutual understanding and avoiding potential conflict. And finally,

encourage two-way communication. To the degree that you are able to allow candidates some

voice during the process, their reactions will be more strongly positive. In addition, Table 3

contains a “Best Practice Checklist” which may be used by researchers and organizational

practitioners to self-evaluate how they are doing to positively influence positive reactions for

the applicants they encounter (Bauer et al., 2012).

____________________________

Insert Table 3 about here


____________________________

Applicant Best Practices and Recommendations

For applicants, there are important points which emerge from the research done on

applicant reactions. The following is a list of “do’s” and “don’ts” for applicants. Do prepare

(both mentally and physically) for selection procedures so you can perform your best and

lessen test anxiety. Find out all you can online and from the organization if there are ways

you can prepare such as past tests, sample interview questions, etc. In addition, find out all

you can about the organization’s culture and procedures by doing your homework about the

organization. Finally, get yourself into the best shape: eat well, get a good night’s sleep, and

take deep breaths to help you stay focused during long days of selection procedures. Do ask

for feedback if you advance to late rounds in the selection process before being rejected.

Learning why you were not selected this time can pay valuable dividends in future selection

situations. It also signals a learning orientation and interest in the organization. Do be open

to feedback. If you do receive feedback, it may or may not be positive, but be open to it and

try to learn as much as possible. Don’t allow your self-image (self-esteem, self-efficacy) to be

damaged by an unpleasant selection process. Not every selection process is a healthy one,

nor does everyone get an interview or a job. Do not let disappointment or frustration

influence how you feel about yourself. There’s simply not enough information to “take it
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 40

personally.” Don’t over-react or take personally time delays. It is hard to wait for news

about how you did during a job application but that’s part of the process. Reacting

emotionally doesn’t do any good. And finally, don’t let a negative outcome such as not

getting a job outweigh a positive experience. If you managed to get an interview or if you

learned more about an industry you’d be interested in working in at some future date, those

are good things in themselves. While you may not have secured a job you were interested in,

learning what you can from the experience and then “moving on” is a healthy attitude which

can serve you well.

FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS

As we have reviewed this literature, we’ve identified a number of areas for future

research: The application of fairness heuristic theory and fairness theory to applicant

reactions research; the systematic expansion of the applicant reactions field into the domain

of promotions; and the exploration of the dynamic nature of applicant reactions and

dispositions. In this section, we summarize what we consider to be the biggest gaps in this

literature, as well as some ideas for where the literature should go to stay relevant to the

current selection context.

Address issues relevant to current hiring practices. The primary applicant

reactions models are now over 20 years old. That in itself is not a problem: Gilliland’s (1993)

model, for instance, is rooted in a strong theoretical base and does a good job of fixing

selection concepts within organizational justice theory. However, the last 20 years have seen

significant changes in the way selection practice is done, including the use of Internet testing

and mobile technology. Moreover, organizations have far greater data available on applicants

and can link it to additional data sources (e.g., social media), factors that were not considered

when these applicant reactions models were developed. For example, some employers now

use (or are considering the use of) applicants’ social media pages for selection decisions.
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 41

Although there are legal reasons why employers should be wary of such practices, there are

also issues around how applicants perceive such practices. For example, Baysinger, Krauss,

Truxillo, Bauer, and Rineer (2014) found that on-line applicants’ privacy concerns (concern

about how their data might be used or shared) at baseline had a significant effect on later

applicant reactions – a far greater effect than other applicant concerns. In short, the applicant

reactions literature is well prepared to shed light on the viability of many new selection

practices and should move in this direction, as the concerns of societies and their citizens

continue to evolve.

Employer practices for improving reactions and dispositions. The applicant

reactions literature has determined that applicants prefer certain selection procedures to

others. However, the procedures preferred by applicants are not always the most practical for

organizations to use. For this reason, there has been research into explanations as an

inexpensive way to improve applicant reactions (e.g., Truxillo et al., 2009). In addition to a

need to examine a range of explanation types to understand which explanations are most

effective and when, we see several other areas for future research on improving reactions.

First, research has shown that respectful treatment during selection can affect later applicant

reactions (e.g., Feys & Anseel, 2012). Future research should examine ways to improve such

treatment by training HR professionals about how to treat applicants well. This includes ways

to treat applicants as individuals despite large applicant pools. Some large organizations such

as Google and Marriott continue to apply applicant reactions research findings in practical

ways to help them treat applicants better despite the large numbers of applicants they see

each year (McCarthy, 2013).

Second, researchers may want to consider how providing information to applicants

(or to test-takers in general) can affect test performance and perhaps even test validity.

Truxillo et al. (2009) found that explanations affected test-taking motivation, which in turn
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 42

affected test performance. These findings suggest that explanations and other interventions

(providing different instructions) may affect test-taker performance, which may have

important implications for how to treat test-takers during test validation processes.

Stop chasing implausible outcomes and examine the ones that matter. One hope

of early applicant reactions research was that such reactions might affect a number of post-

hire outcomes such as performance. The effects on distal outcomes such as job performance

appear to be somewhat limited (McCarthy et al., 2013). More important, though, the

likelihood of reactions to a hiring procedure affecting outcomes like behavior later on the job

seems a bit of a stretch, and the inability for applicant reactions research to “deliver” on these

promised outcomes has likely caused many researchers to dismiss the field entirely.

However, we argue that such an approach is “throwing out the baby with the bathwater.”

Rather, there are many viable outcomes of applicant reactions that need to be examined and

that are at least as relevant. First, as we’ve mentioned, applicant reactions to the promotion

procedures have received relatively little examination, but the research thus far is promising

(Ambrose & Cropanzano, 2003; Truxillo & Bauer, 1999). Internal candidates for promotion

have far more investment than external candidates and do not simply go away once rejected;

their reactions could be critical for organizational performance. Second, the effects of testing

procedures on self-perceptions and even employee health and well-being has important

implications for the person, the organization, and for society. In short, we should stop

considering just the outcomes described in the original applicant reactions models and instead

examine outcomes that are more likely to be affected by the selection context – and which are

relevant to applicants, employers, and to society.

CONCLUSION

The field of applicant reactions began in earnest with a consideration of the fair

treatment of applicants (Gilliland, 1993) but has since become quite broad, with a number of
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 43

implications for selection practice. As we have identified in this chapter, a large literature

has developed, examining many important, yet distinct, questions about applicants’

dispositions, experiences, and reactions to the hiring process. Despite these advances over the

last 20 years, more work is needed to understand when applicant reactions matter, in terms of

both organizational and individual outcomes. As a research field, applicant reactions has

developed a strong theoretical footing and has important implications for the design of

selection practices internationally. As the practice of selection has profoundly changed in

recent years via the use of social media and so forth, and this change is set to continue in the

coming years, our expectation is that applicant perspectives research will continue to have

important practical consequences, . For these reasons, the field of applicant reactions is

poised for new advances. We look forward to this next wave of applicant reactions research

and what it will bring.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 44

References

Ababneh, K. I., Hackett, R. D., & Schat, A. C. (2014). The role of attributions and fairness in

understanding job applicant reactions to selection procedures and decisions. Journal of

Business and Psychology, 29, 111-129.

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review

of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888-918.

Ambrose, M. L., & Cropanzano, R. (2003). A longitudinal analysis of organizational fairness:

An examination of reactions to tenure and promotion decisions. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 88, 266–275.

Anderson, N. (2004). Editorial – The dark side of the moon: Applicant perspectives, negative

psychological effects (NPEs), and candidate decision making in selection. International

Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 1–8.

Anderson, N. (2011a). Perceived job discrimination: Towards a model of applicant propensity to

case initiation in selection. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 19, 229-244.

Anderson, N. (2011b). Perceived Job Discrimination Matters: Going Beyond a Science of Mean

Effects. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 19, 258-261.

Anderson, N., Ahmed, S., & Costa, A. C. (2012). Applicant reactions in Saudi Arabia:

Organizational attractiveness and core-self evaluation. International Journal of Selection

and Assessment, 20, 197-208.

Anderson, N., & Goltsi, V. (2006). Negative psychological effects of selection methods:

Construct formulation and an empirical investigation into an assessment center.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 14, 236-247.

Anderson, N., Born, M., & Cunningham-Snell, N. (2001) Recruitment and Selection: Applicant

Perspectives and Outcomes. In N. Anderson, D.S. Ones, H.K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran,
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 45

(2001), (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial, Work and Organizational Psychology, Volumes I

and II, First Edition, London/New York: Sage.

Anderson, N., Salgado, J. F., & Hülsheger, U. R., (2010). Applicant reactions in selection:

Comprehensive meta-analysis into reaction generalization versus situational specificity.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18, 291-304.

Anderson, N., & Witvliet, C. (2008). Fairness reactions to personnel selection methods: An

international comparison between the Netherlands, the United States, France, Spain.

Portugal, and Singapore. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 16, 1-13.

Arvey, R., & Sackett, P. (1993). Fairness in selection: Current developments and perspectives.

In: N. Schmitt, and W.C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel Selection on Organization (pp.171-

202). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Arvey, R. D., Strickland, W., Drauden, G., & Martin, C. (1990). Motivational components of test

taking. Personnel Psychology, 43, 695-716.

Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-

147.

Bauer, T. N., Maertz, C. P., Dolen, M. R., & Campion, M. A. (1998). Longitudinal assessment of

applicant reactions to employment testing and test outcome feedback. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 83, 892-903.

Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., Paronto, M., Weekley, J., & Campion, M. A. (2004). Applicant

reactions to different selection technology: Face-to-Face, interactive voice response, and

computer-assisted telephone screening interviews. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 12, 135-148.

Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., Sanchez, R. J., Craig, J., Ferrara, P., & Campion, M. A. (2001).

Applicant reactions to selection: Development of the selection procedural justice scale

(SPJS). Personnel Psychology, 54, 387-419.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 46

Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., Tucker, J., Weathers, V., Bertolino, M., Erdogan, B., & Campion,

M. A. (2006). Selection in the information age: The impact of privacy concerns and

computer experience on applicant reactions. Journal of Management, 32, 601-621.

Bauer, T. N., McCarthy, J. M., Anderson N., Truxillo, D. M., & Salgado, J. F. (2012). Candidate

Reactions to Selection. SIOP International Affairs Committee White Paper. Society for

Industrial – Organizational Psychology.

Baysinger, M., Krauss, A., Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., & Rineer, J. (2014). The impact of

explanations and fairness perceptions on applicant outcomes. In M. Baysinger

(Symposium Chair), Understanding applicant fairness perceptions: Moderators, mediators,

and contextual influences. SIOP Conference, Honolulu, HI.

Bell, B. S., Ryan, A. M., & Wiechmenn, D. (2004). Justice expectations and applicant

perceptions. Justice Expectations and Applicant Perceptions, 12, 24-38.

Bell, B. S., Wiechmann, D., & Ryan, A. M. (2006). Consequences of organizational justice

expectations in a selection system. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 455–466.

Bertolino, M., & Steiner, D. (2007). Fairness reactions to selection methods: An Italian study.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 15, 197-205.

Bott, J., Snell, A., Dahling, J., & Smith, B. N. (2010). Predicting individual score elevation in an

applicant setting: The influence of individual differences and situational perceptions.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 2774-2790.

Bruk-Lee, V., Drew, E., & Hawkes, B. (2013). Candidate reactions to simulations and media-

rich assessments in personnel selection. In M. S. Fetzer & K. A.Tuzinski (Eds.),

Simulations for personnel selection (pp.43-60). New York, NY: Springer

Campbell, J. P., McCloy, R. A., Oppler, S. H., & Sager, C. E. (1993). A theory of performance.

In. N. Schmitt and W.C. Borman, (Eds.), Personnel selection in organizations (pp. 35-70).

San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 47

Carless, S. (2006). Applicant reactions to multiple selection procedures for the police force.

Applied Psychology: An International Review, 55, 145-167.

Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance.

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270-295.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Ahmetoglu, G., & Furnham, A. (2008). Little more than personality:

Dispositional determinants of test anxiety (the big five, core self-evaluations, and self-

assessed intelligence). Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 258-263.

Chan, D. (1997). Racial subgroup differences in predictive validity perceptions on personality

and cognitive ability tests. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 311-320.

Chan, D., Schmitt, N., DeShon, R. P., Clause, C. S., & Delbridge, K. (1997). Reactions to

cognitive ability tests: The relationships between race, test performance, face validity

perceptions, and test-taking motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 300.

Chan, D., Schmitt, N., Jennings, D., Clause, C., & Delbridge, K. (1998). Applicant perceptions

of test fairness: Integrating justice and self-serving bias perspectives. International Journal

of Selection and Assessment, 6, 232-239.

Chan, D., & Schmitt, N. (1997). Video-based versus paper-and-pencil method of assessment in

situational judgment tests: Subgroup differences in test performance and face validity

perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 143-195.

Chan, D., & Schmitt, N. (2004). An agenda for future research on applicant reactions to selection

procedures: A construct-oriented approach. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 12, 9-23.

Chapman, D. S., Uggerslev, K. L., & Webster, J. (2003). Candidate reactions to face-to-face and

technology-mediated interviews: A field investigation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88,

944-953.

Chapman, D. S., & Zweig, D. I. (2005). Developing a nomological network for interview
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 48

structure: Antecedents and consequences of the structured selection interview. Personnel

Psychology, 58, 673-702.

Cook, T. D., Campbell, D. T., & Peracchio, L. (1990). Quasi-experimentation. In M. D. Dunnette

& L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 491-

576). Paolo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Cook, K. W., Vance, C. A., & Spector, P. E. (2000). The relation of candidate personality with

selection-interview outcomes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 867-885.

Díaz, R. J., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., & Tanofsky-Kraff, M. (2001). Cognition, anxiety, and

prediction of performance in 1st-year law students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93,

420-429.

Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency

theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409-434.

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive

performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336-353.

Feiler, A. R., & Powell, D. M. (2013). Interview anxiety across the sexes: Support for the sex-

linked anxiety coping theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 12-17.

Feys, M. L., Anseel, F., & Wille, B. (2012). A longitudinal study of candidates’ reactions in

“American Idol.” In G. W. Giumetti & P. B. Barger (Chairs), Moving beyond “Soft”

outcomes and single time points in reactions research. Paper presented at the 27th annual

Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology Conference, San Diego, CA.

Fletcher, C. (1991). Candidates' reactions to assessment centres and their outcomes: A

longitudinal study. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 64, 117-127.

Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (2001). Fairness theory: Justice as accountability. In J. Greenberg

& R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in Organizational Justice, pp. 1-55. Stanford, CA:

Stanford University Press.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 49

Ford, D. K., Truxillo, D. M., & Bauer, T. N. (2009). Rejected But still there: Shifting the focus in

applicant reactions to the promotional context. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 17, 402-416.

García-Izquierdo, A. L., Moscoso, S., & Ramos-Villagrasa, P. J. (2012). Reactions to the fairness

of promotion methods: Procedural justice and job satisfaction. International Journal of

Selection and Assessment, 20, 394-403.

Geenen, B., Proost, K., van Dijke, M., de Witte, K., & von Grumbkow, J. (2012). The role of

affect in the relationship between distributive justice expectations and applicants'

recommendation and litigation intentions. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 20, 404–413.

Gillespie, J. Z., & Ryan, A. M. (2012). Gender-based preferential selection: influences of

perceptions of procedurally unfair advantage on performance and self-evaluations. Journal

of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 150–179.

Gilliland, S. (1994). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to a selection

system. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 691-701.

Gilliland, S. W. (1993). The perceived fairness of selection systems: An organizational justice

perspective. Academy of Management Review, 18, 694-734.

Gilliland, S. W., Groth, M., Backer IV, R. C., Dew, A. F., Polly, L. M., & Langdon, J. C.

(2001). Improving applicants' reactions to rejection letters: An application of fairness

theory. Personnel Psychology, 54, 669-703.

Giumetti, G. W., & Sinar, E. F. (2012). Don't you know me well enough yet: Comparing

reactions of internal and external candidates to employment testing. International Journal

of Selection and Assessment, 20, 139-148.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 50

Harris, M. M., Van Hoye, G., & Lievens, F. (2003). Privacy and attitudes towards internet-based

selection system: A cross-cultural comparison. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 11, 230-236

Hausknecht, J., Day, D., & Thomas, S. (2004). Applicant reactions to selection procedures: An

updated model and meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 57, 639-684.

Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety. Review of

Educational Research, 58, 47-77.

Herriot, P. (2004). Social identities and applicant reactions. International Journal of Selection

and Assessment, 12, 75-83.

Hiemstra, A. M., Derous, E., Serlie, A. W., & Born, M. P. (2012). Fairness perceptions of video

resumes among ethnically diverse applicants. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 20, 423-433.

Hoang, T. G., Truxillo, D. M., Erdogan, B., & Bauer, T. N., (2012). Cross-cultural examination

of applicant reactions to selection methods: United States and Vietnam. International

Journal of Selection and Assessment, 20, 209-219.

Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture consequences: Comparing values, behaviours, institutions and

organizations across nations. 2nd Ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Motivation, leadership and organization: Do American theories apply

abroad. Organizational Dynamics, 9, 42-63.

Honkaniemi, L., Feldt, T., Metsäpelto, R., & Tolvanen, A. (2013). Personality types and

applicant reactions in real-life selection. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,

21, 32-45.

Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An

integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 74, 657-690.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 51

Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution in social action. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley,

R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds), Attribution: Perceiving the cause of behavior

(pp. 1-26). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

Konradt, U., Warszta, T., & Ellwart, T. (2013). Fairness perceptions in web-based selection:

Impact on applicants' pursuit intentions, recommendation intentions, and intentions to

reapply. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 21, 155-169.

LaHuis, D. M. (2005). Individual differences in applicant reactions: A job-search perspective.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 13, 150-159.

LaHuis, D. M., MacLane, C. N., & Schlessman, B. R. (2007). Do applicants’ perceptions matter?

Investigating reapplication behavior using fairness theory. International Journal of

Selection and Assessment, 15, 383-393.

Latham, G. P., & Finnegan, B. J. (1993). Perceived practicality of unstructured, patterned, and

situational interviews. In J. Schuler, J. L. Farr, & M. Smith (Eds.), Personnel selection and

asssessment: Individual and organizational perspectives (pp. 41-55). Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lazer, A., Zinger, A., & Lachterman, B. (2007). The influence of prefeedback selection justice

on perceptions of overall procedural justice and organizational attractiveness in a real-life

selection procedure. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 16, 94-

109.

Lievens, F., & Sackett, P. R. (2006). Video-based versus written situational judgment tests: A

comparison in terms of predictive validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1181-1188.

Lind, E. A. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in

organizational relations. Advances in organizational justice, 56, 88.

Madigan, J., & Macan, T. H. (2005). Improving candidate reactions by altering test

administration. Applied H.R.M. Research, 10, 73-88.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 52

Macan, T., Avedon, M., Paese, M., & Smith, E. (1994). The effects of applicants' reactions to

cognitive ability tests and an assessment center. Personnel Psychology, 47, 715-738.

Maertz, C. P., Jr., Bauer, T. N., Mosley, D. C., Jr., Posthuma, R. A., & Campion, M. A. (2005).

Predictors of self-efficacy for cognitive ability employment testing. Journal of Business

Research, 58, 160-167.

Maertz, C. P., Mosley, D. C., Bauer, T. N., Posthuma, R. A., & Campion, M. A. (2004). Do

procedural justice perceptions in a selection testing context predict applicant attraction and

intention toward the organization? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 125-145.

McCarthy, J. M. (2013). When it comes to practice, do applicant reactions theories matter?

Exploring theory-practice connects and disconnects from the front lines. J. McCarthy

(Chair) with panelists Talya Bauer (Portland State University), Todd Carlisle (Google),

Victoria Davis (Marriott International), & Donald Truxillo (Portland State University) and

Wayne Cascio (Discussant). Panel discussion at SIOP’s Annual Meeting, Houston, TX.

McCarthy, J. M., & Goffin, R. D. (2003). Is the test attitude survey psychometrically sound?

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 446-464.

McCarthy, J. M., & Goffin, R. D. (2004). Measuring job interview anxiety: Beyond weak knees

and sweaty palms. Personnel Psychology, 57, 607-637.

McCarthy, J. M., & Goffin, R. D. (2005). Selection test anxiety: Exploring tension and fear of

failure across the sexes in simulated selection scenarios. International Journal of Selection

and Assessment, 13, 282-295.

McCarthy, J. M., Hrabluik, C., & Jelley, R. B. (2009). Progression through the ranks: Assessing

employee reactions to high-stakes employment testing. Personnel Psychology, 62, 793-832.

McCarthy, J. M., Van Iddekinge, C. H., Lievens, F., Kung, M.-C., Sinar, E. F., & Campion, M.

A. (2013). Do candidate reactions relate to job performance or affect criterion: related


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 53

validity? A multistudy investigation of relations among reactions, selection test scores, and

job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 701-719.

McFarland, L. (2003). Warning against faking on a personality test: Effects on applicant

reactions and personality test scores. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,

11, 265-276.

Nikolaou, I., & Judge, T. (2007). Fairness reaction to personnel techniques in Greece: The core

of self-evaluations. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 15, 206-219.

Nguyen, H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of

minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 93, 1314-1334.

O'Neill, T. A., Goffin, R. D., & Gellatly, I. R. (2010). Test-taking motivation and personality test

validity. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9, 117-125.

Oostrom, J. K., Born, M. P., Serlir, A. W., & Van Der Molen, H. T. (2010). Effects of individual

differences on the perceived job relatedness of a cognitive ability test and a multimedia

situational judgment test. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18, 394-406.

Phillips, J., & Gully, S. (2002). Fairness reaction to personnel selection techniques in Singapore

and United States. International Journal of Human Resource Management, 13, 1186-1205.

Ployhart, R. E., & Harold, C. M. (2004). The applicant attribution-reaction theory (AART): An

integrative theory of applicant attributional processing. International Journal of Selection

and Assessment, 12, 84-98.

Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. M., (1997). Toward an explanation of applicant reactions: An

examination of organizational justice and attribution frameworks. Organizational Behavior

and Human Decision Processes, 72, 308-335.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 54

Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. M. (1998). Applicants' reactions to the fairness of selection

procedures: The effects of positive rule violations and time of measurement. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 83, 3-16.

Ployhart, R. E., Ziegert, J. C., & McFarland, L. A. (2003). Understanding racial differences on

cognitive ability tests in selection contexts: An integration of stereotype threat and

candidate reactions research. Human Performance, 16, 231-231-259.

Posthuma, R. A., Morgeson, F. P., & Campion, M. A. (2002). Beyond employment interview

validity: A comprehensive narrative review of recent research and trends over time.

Personnel Psychology, 55, 1-81.

Potosky, D., & Bobko, P. (2004). Selection testing via the internet: Practical considerations and

exploratory empirical findings. Personnel Psychology, 57, 1003-1034.

Proost, K., Derous, E., Schreurs, B., Hagtvet, K. A., & DeWitte, K. (2008). Selection test

anxiety: Investigating applicants' self- vs. other-referenced anxiety in a real selection

setting. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 16, 14-26.

Reeve, C. L., & Bonaccio, S. (2008). Does test anxiety induce measurement bias in cognitive

ability tests? Intelligence, 36, 526-538.

Robertson, I. T., Iles, P. A., Gratton, L., & Sharpley, D. (1991). The impact of personnel

selection and assessment methods on candidates. Human Relations, 44, 565-606.

Rolland, F., & Steiner, D. D. (2007). Test-taker reactions to selection process: Effects of

outcome favorability, explanations, and voice on fairness perceptions. Journal of Applied

Social Psychology, 37, 2800-2826.

Roth, P. L., Bobko, P., Van Iddekinge, C. H., & Thatcher, J. B. (2013). Social media in

employee-selection-related decisions A research agenda for uncharted territory. Journal of

Management, 0149206313503018.

Ryan, A. M. (2001). Explaining the black-white test score gap: The role of test perceptions.
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 55

Human Performance, 14, 45-75.

Ryan, A. M., Boyce, A. S., Ghumman, S., Jundt, D., & Schmidt, G. (2009). Going global:

Cultural values and perceptions of selection procedures. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 58, 520-556.

Ryan, A. M., & Chan, D. (1999). Perceptions of the EPPP: How do licensure candidates view the

process? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 30, 519-530.

Ryan, A. M., & Ployhart, R. E. (2000). Candidates' perceptions of selection procedures and

decisions: A critical review and agenda for the future. Journal of Management, 26, 565-

606.

Ryan, A. M., Ployhart, R. E., Greguras, G. J., & Schmit, M. J. (1998). Test preparation programs

in selection contexts: Self-selection and program effectiveness. Personnel Psychology, 51,

599-621.

Ryan, A. M., Sacco, J. M., McFarland, L. A., & Kriska, D. (2000). Applicant self-selection:

Correlates of withdrawal from a multiple hurdle process. Journal of Applied Psychology,

85, 163-179.

Rynes, S. L. (2007). Let’s create a tipping point: What academics and practitioners can do, alone

and together. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 1046-1054.

Sackett, P. R., & Ryan, A. M. (2012). Concerns about generalizing stereotype threat research

findings to operational high-stakes testing (pp. 249-263). NY, NY: Oxford University

Press.

Sadri, G., & Robertson, I. T. (1993). Self-efficacy and work-related behaviour: A review and

meta-analysis. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 42, 139-152.

Salgado, J. F., Moscoso, S., & Lado, M. (2003). Test-retest reliability of ratings of job

performance dimensions in managers. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,

11, 98-101.
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 56

Salgado, J. F., Remeseiro, C., & Iglesias, M. (1996). Personality and test-taking motivation.

Psicothema, 8, 553-562.

Sanchez, R. J., Truxillo, D. M., & Bauer, T. N. (2000). Development and examination of an

expectancy-based measure of test-taking motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85,

739-750.

Schaubroeck, J., & Lam, S. S. (2004). Comparing lots before and after: Promotion rejectees'

invidious reactions to promotees. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,

94, 33–47.

Schinkel, S., Dierendonck, D. V., & Anderson, N. (2004). The impact of selection encounters on

applicants: An experimental study into feedback effects after a negative selection decision.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 197-205.

Schmitt, N., Oswald, F. L., Kim, B. H., Gillespie, M. A., & Ramsay, L. J. (2004). The impact of

justice and self-serving bias explanations of the perceived fairness of different types of

selection tests. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 160-171.

Schmit, M. J., & Ryan, A. M. (1992). Test-taking dispositions: A missing link? Journal of

Applied Psychology, 77, 629-637.

Schmit, M. J., & Ryan, A. M. (1997). Applicant withdrawal: The role of test-taking attitudes and

racial Differences. Personnel Psychology, 50, 855–876.

Schmitt, N., & Gilliland, S.W. (1992). Beyond differential prediction: Fairness in selection. In

D. M. Saunders (Ed.), New approaches to employee management: Fairness in employee

selection. Vol. 1( pp. 21-46). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Schreurs, B., Derous, E., Proost, K., & Witte, K. D. (2010). The relation between selection

expectations, perceptions and organizational attraction: A test of competing model.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18, 447-452.

Schuler, H. (1993). Social validity of selection situations: A concept and some empirical results.
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 57

In J. Schuler, J. L. Farr, & M. Smith (Eds.), Personnel selection and asssessment:

Individual and organizational perspectives (pp. 41-55). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings. Anxiety

Research, 4, 27-41.

Shao, R., Rupp, D. E., Skarlicki, D. P., & Jones, K. S. (2013). Employee justice across cultures:

A meta-analytic review. Journal of Management, 39, 263-301.

Shaw, J. C., Wild, E., & Colquitt, J. A. (2003). To justify or excuse? A meta-analytic review of

the effects of explanations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 444-458. doi: http://

10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.444

Sieverding, M. (2009). 'Be cool!': Emotional costs of hiding feelings in a job interview.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 17, 391-401.

Silvester, J., Anderson, N., Haddleton, E., Cunningham-Snell, N., & Gibb, A. (2000). A cross-

modal comparison of telephone and face-to-face selection interviews in graduate

recruitment. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 8, 16-21.

Smith, R. J., Arnkoff, D. B., & Wright, T. L. (1990). Test anxiety and academic competence: A

comparison of alternative models. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 313-321.

Smither, J., Reilly, R., Millsap, R., Pearlman, K., & Stoffey, R. (1993). Applicant reactions to

selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 46, 49-76.

Spielberger, G. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Spielberger, C. D., & Vagg, P. R. (1995). Test anxiety: A transactional process model (pp. 3-14)

Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.

Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of

African-Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797-811.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 58

Steiner, D., & Gilliland, S. (2001). Procedural justice in personnel selection: International and

cross-culture perspectives. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 81, 124-137.

Sumanth, J. J., & Cable, D. M. (2011). Status and organizational entry: How organizational and

individual career status affect justice perceptions of hiring systems. Personnel Psychology,

64, 963-1000.

Sundre, D. L., & Kitsantas, A. (2004). An exploration of the psychology of the examinee: Can

examinee self-regulation and test-taking motivation predict consequential and non-

consequential test performance? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 6-26.

Sylva, H., & Mol, S. T. (2009). E-recruitment: A study into applicant perceptions of an online

application system. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 17, 311-323.

Thibodeaux, H. F., & Kudisch, J. D. (2003). The relationship between applicant reactions, the

likelihood of complaints, and organization attractiveness. Journal of Business and

Psychology, 18, 247-257.

Tippins, N. T. (2009). Internet alternatives to traditional proctored testing: Where are we now?

Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 2, 2-10.

Truxillo, D. M., & Bauer, T. N. (1999). Applicant reactions to test scores banding in entry-level

and promotional contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 322-339.

Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., Campion, M. A., & Paronto, M. E. (2002). Selection fairness

information and applicant reactions: A longitudinal field study. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 87, 1020-1031.

Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., Campion, M. A., & Paronto, M. E. (2006). A field study of the

role of big five personality in applicant perceptions of selection fairness, self, and the hiring

organization. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 14, 269-277.

Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., & Sanchez, R. J. (2001). Multiple dimensions of procedural

justice: Longitudinal effects on selection system fairness and test-taking self-efficacy.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 59

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 336-349.

Truxillo, D. M., Bodner, T. B., Bertolino, M., Bauer, T. N., & Yonce, C. (2009). Effects of

explanations on applicant reactions: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of

Selection and Assessment, 17, 346-361.

Truxillo, D. M., & Bauer, T. N., (2011). Applicant reactions to organizations and selection

systems. In: S. Zedeck (Ed.), APA Handbook of I/O Psychology, Volume II (pp. 379–397).

Washington, DC: APA Press.

Truxillo, D. M., Steiner, D., & Gilliland, S. (2004). The importance of organizational justice in

personnel selection: Defining when selection fairness really matter. International Journal

of Selection and Assessment, 12, 39-53.

Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive justice:

What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 95-104.

Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Importance of perceived personnel selection system

fairness determinants: Relations with demographic, personality, and job characteristics.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12, 172-186.

Walsh, B. M., Tuller, M. D., Barnes-Farrell, J. L., & Matthews, R. A. (2010). Investigating the

moderating role of cultural practices on the effect of selection fairness perceptions.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18, 365-379.

Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the

structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work (pp. 1-74). Elsevier

Science/JAI Press.

Wiechmann, D., & Ryan, M. A. (2003). Reactions to computerized testing in selection contexts.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 11, 215-229.

Wolf, L. F., & Smith, J. K. (1995). The consequence of consequence: Motivation, anxiety, and
APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 60

test performance. Applied Measurement in Education, 8, 227-242.


APPLICANT PERSPECTIVES 61

Table 1. Meta-analytic estimates of favorability ratings for ten selection procedures.

Hauschnecht et al. (2004) Anderson et al. (2010)


Selection tool studied Mean K Mean K

Interviews 3.70 10 3.96 25


Work Samples 3.61 10 3.84 23

Resume’s 3.57 5 3.55 19


References 3.29 7 3.11 21
Cognitive Ability 3.11 10 3.28 26
Biodata 2.81 8 3.06 19
Personality Tests 2.83 10 2.91 25

Honesty Tests 2.47 6 2.64 19


Personal Contacts 2.51 6 1.85 22
Graphology 1.69 6 1.66 21

Note. Haushnecht et al. (2004) meta-analytic estimates are based on a 5-point scale; Anderson et
al. (2010) meta-analytic estimates are calibrated to a 5-point scale.

View publication stats

You might also like