Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

^ Academy oi Management Review

1999. Vol. 24. No. 2. 286-307.

MULTILEVEL THEORIZING ABOUT CREATIVITY


IN ORGANIZATIONS: A SENSEMAKING
PERSPECTIVE
ROBERT DRAZIN
MARY ANN GLYNN
ROBERT K. KAZANIIAN
Emory University

In this article we explore assumptions about the levels oi analysis embedded in the
extant literature on creativity in organizations. Uncovering and then relaxing these
assumptions allow us to extend the literature with an alternative but complementary
model of how creativity unfolds in complex, large-scale, and long-duration organiza-
tional projects. We build on the paradigm of sensemaking and propose a multilevel
model of creativity that, as its defining feature, examines how periodic organizational
crises reframe the negotiated order of belief structures about creativity.

Early research on creativity centered, to a tional) affect creativity. Drazin and Schoon-
large extent, on discovering and describing the hoven (1996) urged the development of multi-
nature of creative people (e.g., Barron, 1955; level models that link a firm's strategic focus to
MacKinnon, 1965). While noteworthy in its own the behavior of its senior managers and, in turn,
right, its nearly exclusive focus on the individ- to individuals' commitment to the creative pro-
ual level of analysis eclipsed more macro expla- cess.
nations of creativity (Slappendel, 1996). Am- Given the recent focus on multilevel ap-
abile, working with her colleagues (e.g., proaches to creativity, a timely question is one
Amabile, 1983, 1996; Amabile, Goldfarb, & Brock- concerning how level-of-analysis (LOA) issues
field, 1990), enlarged the scope of creativity re- have been modeled. The choice of focal levels of
search from its origins at the individual level to analysis is profound and central to the develop-
the group or social-psychological level and, ment of any model; it affects the conceptual
eventually, to the organizational level (Amabile, framework, research methods, locus of interest,
Conti, Coon, Lazenby, & Herron, 1996). In gen- and, consequently, the full measure of a theoret-
eral, scholars in the field have followed this ical and empirical approach to a phenomenon.
approach, and multilevel models of creativity in All too often, however, institutionalized, taken-
organizations are now emerging. for-granted assumptions about LOA issues be-
Woodman, Sawyer, & Griffin (1993) proposed come incorporated, unquestioned, into a model.
one of the first multilevel models; they linked Because these assumptions are the foundation
individual-, group-, and organizational-level upon which theory rests, they both enable and
variables to creative outcomes. Glynn (1996) pre- constrain further theory development. Exposing
sented a theoretical model that related intelli- these LOA assumptions allows for their modifi-
gence at both the individual and organizational cation and, thus, for new and alternative ways
levels to creativity, and Ford (1996) integrated of theorizing about organizational creativity.
multiple levels of analysis to explain engage- Drawing from the works of multilevel theorists
ment in creative behavior. Oldham and Cum- (DiMaggio, 1991; Giddens, 1994; House, Rous-
mings (1996). in a rare and important empirical seau, & Thomas-Hunt, 1995; Klein, Dansereau, &
test, demonstrated that factors at multiple levels Hall, 1994; Rousseau, 1985), we examine how the
of analysis (i.e.. individual, job. and organiza- creativity literature addresses LOA issues. We
review relevant creativity research to reveal
theoretical assumptions about the analytical
levels to which generalizations are made; ag-
Authors are listed alphabetically. All authors contributed
equally to this project. We thank Katherine Klein and the
gregation and composition issues across levels
anonymous AMR reviewers for their help. of analysis; treatment of subunits as heteroge-
286
1999 Drazin, Glynn, and Kazanjian 287

neous, homogeneous, or independent of higher- theoretical significance, this setting has practical
level influences; and dynamic change over time. importance as well. Projects marked by interde-
Following this, we explore the consequences of pendence, long duration, and large scale have
altering LOA assumptions and propose an illus- become increasingly common in practice (e.g..
trative model. In our review of the literature, we Clark & Fujimoto, 1988; DeMaio, Verganti, & Corso,
find that a particular set of assumptions domi- 1994; Hoffman, 1997; Ouinn & Pacquette, 1988; Sab-
nates the current literature on creativity. These bagh, 1996) but, to date, have been relatively un-
assumptions center on a definition of creativity derstudied in creativity research.
as an oufcome and generalization primarily to Thus, a model of the creative processes in
the small group or project level of analysis. Our such a setting can coexist as a companion to the
goal is to address the gap in multilevel theoriz- individual and small group models that have
ing that such assumptions may have inadver- dominated the literature. Our setting allows us
tently created. to make a set of logical arguments that are in-
In contrast to existing models, we define cre- ternally consistent with respect to this exemplar
ativity as the process of engagement in creative (Doty & Glick, 1994). Other contexts may vary
acts, regardless of whether the resultant out- systematically from the one we describe, but we
comes are novel, useful, or creative (Amabile, believe that similar processes may operate in
1988, 1996; Ford, 1996). This process orientation other cells of a typology that describe related
focuses our inquiry on how individuals attempt creative processes in organizations.
to orient themselves to, and take creative action We begin by reviewing the literature on cre-
in, situations or events that are complex, ambig- ativity to answer the question "How have cre-
uous, and ill defined. In other words, this is an ativity researchers approached the LOA issue in
issue of how individuals engage in sensemak- developing their models?" We analyze the cur-
ing in organizations (Greenberg, 1995; Louis, rent literature to extract underlying themes and
1980; Volkema, Farquhar, & Bergmann, 1996; assumptions about levels of analysis. From the
Weick. 1995). outset, we recognize that not all creativity stud-
A sensemaking approach to creativity affords ies are of the same bent; our objective is to
a fresh perspective on LOA issues. Tradition- identify the dominant approaches to LOA issues
ally, creativity research has depicted the key that have framed the study of creativity, to un-
levels of analysis as being individual, group, cover their limitations, and to suggest alterna-
and organizational, with creativity at higher lev- tives that might extend multilevel theorizing.
els typically being an aggregation of creative
output at lower levels (e.g., Glynn, 1996; Wood-
man et al., 1993). A sensemaking perspective
LOA ISSUES IN THE CREATIVITY UTERATURE
enriches this LOA perspective by pointing to
cross-level, systemic, and embedded effects that Creative behavior often is modeled as the re-
may arise from idiosyncratic and/or communal sult of individual characteristics and propensi-
interpretations of what it means to be creative. ties (Amabile, 1996; Mumford & Gustafson, 1988),
This implies that conflict, political influence, including personality factors (Barron & Har-
and negotiated order may operate at more rington, 1981; Singh, 1986) and cognitive skills,
macro-organizational levels (Walsh & Fahey, such as linguistic ability, expressive fluency,
1986; Weick, 1995) and over time in organizations convergent and divergent thinking, and intelli-
to influence creative processes. gence (Barron & Harrington, 1981; Basadur &
A process-orientation and sensemaking per- Finkbeiner, 1985; Basadur, Graen, & Green, 1982;
spective leads us to contexts that are expansive Gardner, 1993; Glynn, 1996; Helson, Roberts, &
enough to allow a full exploration of multiple and Agronick, 1995; Sternberg. 1988). Scholars have
different levels of analysis. We examine how cre- found individual creativity to be highest when
ative thought and action unfold in an exemplar individuals are motivated by intrinsic engage-
setting: an interdependent, complex, large-scale, ment; challenge; task satisfaction; and goal-
long-duration organizational project. These oriented, self-regulatory mechanisms (Amabile.
projects present an "extreme situation" in which 1988; Amabile, Hill, Hennessey, & Tighe, 1994;
the "processes of interest are transparently ob- Glynn & Webster, 1993; Kanfer. 1990; Kanfer &
servable" (Eisenhardt, 1989: 537). In addition to its Ackerman. 1989).
288 Academy of Management Review April

From this individual level of inquiry, creativ- treated as factors to be manipulated in order to
ity researchers have extended their perspective improve these outcomes. Rousseau (1985) terms
to include contextual variables. They have this approach to multilevel theorizing function-
found that settings that provide opportunities, alist-reductionist. Functionalist and reductionist
absence of constraints (Amabile, 1988; Amabile arguments allow researchers to model the func-
& Gryskiewicz, 1987; Oldham & Cummings. tional contribution of units at lower hierarchical
1996). and rewards (Mumford & Gustafson. 1988) levels to outcomes at higher levels.
foster creativity. A number of multilevel studies This functionalist perspective dominates both
have revealed that leader style, cohesiveness, the creativity and innovation literature (Drazin.
group tenure, and degree of cooperation are an- 1990). For instance. Amabile (1988) defines cre-
tecedents to creativity (King & Anderson, 1990) ativity as the production of novel and useful
and research team effectiveness (Payne, 1990). ideas. Similarly, Oldham and Cummings (1996)
Further, Andrews (1979) has found that the define creativity as products, ideas, or proce-
composition of groups is a determinant of dures that are (1) novel or original and (2) rele-
recognition and effectiveness, as well as of pub- vant and useful. Ford (1996) also adopts an out-
lications, for research and development teams. put orientation and views creative outcomes as
This work notwithstanding, organizational- those that are novel and valuable. Woodman et
level variables have been incorporated into rel- al. (1993) define creativity as the generation of a
atively few models of creativity. Some studies valuable, useful new product, service, idea, pro-
demonstrate that organizational policies, struc- cedure, or process by individuals working to-
tures, climate (Burkhardt & Brass, 1990; Tushman gether in a complex social system. Following
& Nelson, 1990), and training (Basadur, Graen, & the functional tradition, the research question
Scandura, 1986; Wheatley, Anthony, & Maddox, posed explicitly or implicitly in most creativity
1991) affect creative output. To date. Woodman studies is "How do you increase creative outputs
et al. (1993) offer the most comprehensive theo- in organizations?"
retical model, linking culture, resources, tech- The level of generalization (to small groups)
nology, strategy, and rewards to organizational and the definition of creativity (as a novel and
creativity. We concur with Oldham and Cum- useful outcome) govern other critical LOA as-
mings' statement: "Unfortunately, little is known sumptions in creativity research. Rousseau
about the conditions that promote the creative (1985) offers a framework that categorizes
performance of individual employees in organi- theories into three general types of multilevel
zations" (1996: 607; emphasis added). models: (1) compositional, (2) cross-level, and
From this overview, we observe that the levels (3) multilevel. Compositional models are con-
of generalization and construct definition em- cerned with specifying the similarity of a pro-
ployed by researchers are the most consequen- cess across multiple levels (Rousseau, 1985) or
tial factors governing the composition of creativ- determining whether macrolevel processes are
ity theories. The level of generalization is consistent with underlying microlevel processes
important because it specifies the focal unit to (DiMaggio, 1991). In the creativity literature,
which the theoretical and empirical statements there seems to be an implicit assumption that
of the research apply (Rousseau, 1985). Tradi- the creative process is alike at all levels of anal-
tionally, creativity researchers have concen- ysis—that is, for individuals, groups, and organ-
trated on the small group (or independent izational systems (Amabile. 1988; Woodman et
project team) as their focal level of analysis and al.. 1993). Creativity researchers readily ac-
have emphasized project-level outcomes that knowledge that there are differences among in-
combine the talents of team members into dividuals that can impact creativity, but they
project-level efforts. With some exceptions tend to adopt a set of assumptions that allow
(Glynn, 1996; Woodman et al., 1993), little has them to treat cross-level effects (between indi-
been done to extend research beyond the level vidual and organizational levels, for instance)
of the small-group project. as homogeneous in nature (Klein et al., 1994).
Similarly, we see construct definition as con- For example, Amabile assumes that "major
sequential for theory building. In general, schol- corporations select individuals who exhibit rel-
ars have defined creativity as an important out- atively high levels of these personal qualities,
come to a system, with independent variables [and that] the variance above this baseline may
1999 Drazin, Giynn. and Kazanjian 289

well be accounted for primarily by factors in the (Oldham & Cummings, 1996)—occasionally with
work environment" (1988: 128). Amabile later af- children as subjects (Koestner. Ryan, Bernieri, &
firms this view in her statement that "whatever Holt, 1984; Stahl & Koser, 1978). Although some
an individual's talents . . . the conditions under models include variables such as job design
which he or she works . . . can significantly in- (Amabile. 1988; Hackman & Oldham. 1980;
crease or decrease the level of creativity pro- Kanter. 1988), supervisory style (Stahl & Koser.
duced" (1996: 17). Support for this notion can be 1978; West, 1989), or performance feedback (Car-
found in Oldham and Cumming's (1996) study. son & Carson, 1993), most tend to assume total
These authors hypothesized a person-by-situa- inclusiveness of an individual in a small group.
tion interaction effect on creativity and found Consistent with this LOA focus, scholars tend
statistical support for an ordinal relationship. to model creativity as a discrete task, conducted
That is, individuals whose dispositions make by individuals or small groups who are isolated
them more likely to be affected by a favorable from broader organizational and occupational
work environment are more creative, but the pressures. This perspective applies to some or-
creativity of all individuals is raised by a sup- ganizational situations, particularly for "skunk-
portive environment. Thus, one assumption in works" types of small, team-based new-product
creativity research, made explicitly or implic- development that can be found in such compa-
itly, is the homogeneity of higher-level (or situ- nies as 3M, Sony, or Thermos (Dumaine, 1993;
ational) effects on individuals. Peters, 1988). However, large, complex initia-
The degree of inclusion of individuals in a tives, characterized by long time horizons and a
hierarchy of levels is an important construct in project management structure composed of mul-
cross-level research (House et al., 1995; Rous- tifunctional, interdependent teams, are increas-
seau, 1985). Total inclusion implies that only the ingly more common venues for creative action
group in which an individual has formal mem- (DeMaio et al., 1994). Examples include the de-
bership is influential (e.g., a functional depart- velopment of new aircraft by Boeing (Sabbagh.
ment or an assigned project team). Partial inclu- 1996), new automobiles (Clark & Fujimoto, 1988;
sion means that an individual occupies multiple Ouinn & Pacquette, 1988), space projects at
organizational roles and is influenced by mem- NASA (Hoffman, 1997; Sayles & Chandler, 1971).
bership in all of them; situational attributes can and defense contracting (Scudder. Schroeder.
cue or make salient membership in a particular Van De Ven, Seiler, & Wiseman, 1989). In all of
group to the exclusion of other groups (Ashforth these cases, creativity was critical to the suc-
& Mael, 1996). As Fine observes, "The assump- cess of the effort, but the organizational context
tion of a dominant [occupational] identity overly was not that of an isolated individual or a small
limits people's choices in constructing their group unit—rather, a large-scale, long-term
work relations" (1996: 92). project. The assumption of total inclusiveness of
individuals in these large, complex organization-
Although individuals may be members of an al systems seems questionable.
organizational work unit and a cross-functional
team, they may also situate themselves in terms Finally, we note that much of the creativity
of an occupational identity (Trice, 1993; Van literature has ignored the dimension of time.
Maanen & Barley, 1984). Partial inclusiveness This is likely the result of a tendency of re-
complicates cross-level research because ef- searchers to define creativity as an outcome,
fects can no longer be attributed to membership rather than a process (see Ford. 1996. for an
in a single hierarchical group; researchers need important exception). This definition leads re-
to account for multiple, and often competing, searchers toward static models that emphasize
influences that cause individuals to situate explaining variance in the dependent variable
themselves in accord with this complexity (Ash- (creative outcomes), rather than examining how
forth & Mael, 1996). The assumptions adopted by the dynamic process of creativity unfolds over
most creativity researchers seem to imply total time (Mohr, 1982).
inclusion; in many ways, this may have been a The most explicit consideration of the role of
natural outgrowth of the contexts in which they time in understanding micro-macro relation-
have studied creativity. ships is contained in Giddens' (1994) theory of
Much of the research on individual creativity structuration. Central to his model is the concept
has been conducted in behavioral laboratories of the duality of structure. His thesis is that hu-
290 Academy of Management Review April

man agency and structure cannot be understood ing construct definition and level of generaliza-
apart from each other. tion.
Knowledgeable individuals understand the
workings of their society; in turn, they both react
to and participate in their reproduction and re-
Construct Definition: Creativity As a Process
direction. Because individuals can intervene in
events and take action to influence events, time We define creativity as a process, rather than
is accorded a central role in understanding the an outcome. This distinction is not unique to us
interplay between individual and structure (Mohr, 1982); Amabile (1988) has modeled cre-
(Peterson, 1998). In the organizational literature, ativity as an individual-level cognitive process
Klein et al. (1994) suggest that individuals may consisting of multiple stages. To Torrance (1988),
react homogeneously to the influence of mac- individual creativity is a process of sensing
rolevel factors within one period of time, but problems, making guesses, formulating hypoth-
they react heterogeneously across multiple pe- eses, communicating ideas to others, and con-
riods of time. tradicting conformity or "what is expected."
Where Giddens differs from Klein and col- At the individual level, we define creativity as
leagues is in allowing for the possibility that the engagement of an individual in a creative
individuals will use time to influence and act (Ford, 1996; Torrance. 1988). Creative engage-
change structure. This suggests that creativity ment is a process in which an individual behav-
may not proceed in linear, hierarchical paths iorally, cognitively, and emotionally attempts to
but, rather, in uneven, chaotic bursts that are produce creative outcomes (Kahn, 1990). For ex-
responses to problems that erupt over time (Ka- ample, engineers working on a project may at-
zanjian, 1988; Peterson, 1998; Quinn. 1985). As tempt to design an apparatus that is creative;
such, a model of creativity in these contexts they may collect data, consult past solutions,
would acknowledge the interplay between indi- contemplate alternatives, propose inventive
viduals and structure over time (Giddens, 1994). ideas, and become emotionally invested in their
Uncovering such processes and understanding work. Their ideas may or may not be considered
how they shape patterns of creative effort in by others as creative, but the process of gener-
large-scale projects is the impetus for our pro- ating those ideas logically can be called "cre-
posing a multilevel, sensemaking perspective ativity;" in effect, creativity as a process is a
on creative processes in organizations. necessary, but not sufficient, condition for cre-
ative outcomes. In the creativity literature,
scholars generally assume, but do not explicitly
CREATIVITY AS A SENSEMAKING PROCESS model, the occurrence of such a creative pro-
Our review reveals that two significant as- cess. Extant models tend to adopt a variance
analysis framework (Mohr, 1982) that seeks to
sumptions have governed existing multilevel
explain the relationship between context and
approaches to creativity. First, creativity has
outcomes, presuming that a creative process un-
been defined as an outcome judged by others to
derlies the generation of creative outcomes.
be novel and useful. Second, creativity theories
have been primarily built at one level of gener- Creativity is a choice made by an individual
alization—that of the small project group or to engage in producing novel ideas; the level of
team—with the assumption of individual inclu- creative engagement can vary from person to
siveness in a hierarchy of work units embedded person and from situation to situation. An indi-
in an organization. Individual and small group vidual may choose minimal engagement, pro-
creativity have been assumed to be similar in posing simple solutions that may not be novel or
composition (Amabile. 1988. 1996). and organiza- useful—a behavior Ford (1996) refers to as "ha-
tional creativity typically has been considered bitual action." Alternatively, an individual may
to be the accumulation of individual or small choose to engage in a full manner, using all of
group creativity (Glynn. 1996). These assump- his or her abilities in an effort to produce cre-
tions have driven a prototypical treatment of ative outcomes. To Kahn (1990), such processes
other LOA issues. In this section we propose an of engagement (and disengagement) vary over
alternative multilevel model of creativity that time, ebbing and flowing from moment to mo-
builds on a different set of assumptions regard- ment and from day to day.
1999 Drazin, Glynn, and Kazan;ian
291
Creativity also can be defined as a group- cal level of analysis and the context in which we
level process. The complex, creative projects examine creative processes.
taken on by large organizations require the con-
certed engagement of many individuals, rather
than just one or a few. Amabile (1988, 1996) con- Level of Generalization: Large-Scale, Long-
siders group and individual processes of cre- Duration Creative Projects
ativity to be of similar composition, because Creativity increasingly occurs in the context
both involve cognitive processes of idea gener- of large-scale, long-term projects that employ
ation and idea testing. In settings where the multiple, interdependent teams. Sabbagh's
object of creativity is complex and requires (1996) account of the development of the Boeing
skills from multiple bases of expertise, it may be 777 aircraft provides a detailed example. The
difficult to separate out individual- from group- project was large and of long duration: the 777
level contributions. For example, in their study ultimately required the development of over four
of brain storming at a product design firm, Sut- million discrete components and was priced at
ton and Hargadon (1996) found that when groups $100 million per aircraft; it took 5V2 years to com-
address complex problems, many individuals plete.
contribute to the process, and it is difficult to The organization design used to complete the
assign credit to any one individual for the de- Boeing 777 was equally complex. A hierarchy of
sign outcome. Groups also go through stages teams, embedded in a complex project manage-
that mirror the processes of individuals—that is, ment structure, designed and built the aircraft.
developing criteria, generating alternatives, Each of the major systems of the plane, such as
modifying those alternatives, and amplifying the wings, empennage, and fuselage, had a ded-
and extending original ideas. icated "design-build" system team. These teams
Individuals and groups participate in creative were further broken down into smaller teams
responsible for each subsystem, resulting in 250
processes in an iterative fashion. Individuals
separate but highly interdependent design-
develop ideas, present them to the group, learn
build subteams.'
from the group, work out issues in solitude, and
then return to the group to further modify and We use large. long-term, complex organiza-
enhance their ideas. The iterative, interactive tional projects as an exemplar setting for sev-
eral reasons. First, they represent a frequently
nature of group creativity requires that individ-
occurring, real-world phenomenon, operating at
uals first choose to engage in individual-level
a level of analysis in between that of the project
creativity. We make the assumption that indi- team and the entire organization (House et al..
viduals act homogeneously within groups as 1995). Creativity at this level consists of the ebb
they engage in creative behavior (Klein et al., and flow of creative engagement among differ-
1994). ent communities of individuals involved in the
Creative processes at the organizational level project. Second, because organizations are mov-
may not simply aggregate from individual or ing away from traditional hierarchical struc-
group efforts; rather, they may emerge from a tures, multilevel research needs to examine or-
process of negotiating multiple and potentially ganizational forms that exhibit more complex
competing interests between different commu- types of interdependence (House et al., 1995). In
nities or groups within the organization (e.g., these settings, communication and coordination
those technically responsible for creative activ-
ity and those managing the creative process). At
the organizational level, creativity can be de- ' Projects similar in scope and complexity, if not size,
occur in the auto industry as well (Clark & Fujimoto, 1988).
scribed in terms of a process that maps when
Quinn and Pacquette's (1988) case study of the development
creative behavior occurs and who engages in of the Ford Taurus captures a similar challenge in the de-
creative behavior. Such issues are pursued to velopment of a new car. Here again, a nested hierarchy with
best advantage in contexts in which there are a large number of interdependent teams pursued the design
communities of actors engaged interdepen- in parallel. Similar examples can be found at NASA (Ring &
Rands, 1989: Sayles & Chandler, 1971), with the SST (Hor-
dently in large-scale creative pursuits over time. witch, 1982), or more currently with the development of en-
Thus, situations that allow us to relax the as- terprise-wide software conversions by such firms as SAP
sumption of total inclusiveness become our fo- and PeopleSoft.
292 Academy oi Management Review April

are handled directly between communities of formation (Dutton & Dukerich. 1991; Dutton &
individuals assigned to project teams. Third, Jackson. 1987). strategy formation in top man-
large-scale, long-time-duration projects may be agement teams (Porac, Thomas, & Baden-Fuller,
characterized by considerable situational ambi- 1989), change management (Gioia & Chittipeddi.
guity; the ability of a new product or service to 1991; Poole, Gioia, & Gray, 1989), and technology
perform as anticipated may not be known well diffusion (Barley, 1986). Researchers have stud-
in advance, and there may be considerable ied the general innovation process using a sen-
change to product specifications as the project semaking framework (Dougherty. 1992; Hill &
progresses and reveals untoward obstacles. Levenhagen, 1995; Ring & Rands, 1989), but we
Further, the horizontal and highly interactive know of only limited work that applies an inter-
nature of project work may break down individ- pretive framework specifically to the study of
uals' inclusiveness in their home functional de- creativity (Ford, 1996; Ford & Gioia, 1995).
partments, in response to numerous subproject Those with a sensemaking perspective ap-
assignments over the long life of a large project. proach LOA issues somewhat differently from
Situations characterized by high levels of am- those with traditional perspectives. First, al-
biguity and low levels of inclusiveness provide though researchers recognize that individuals
individuals little in the way of guidance con- are the center of organizational life, those indi-
cerning what is correct behavior. According to viduals are accorded a different role: they create
House et al., such psychologically weak situa- meanings about their social setting through in-
tions "stimulate groups to engage in collective teractions with others (Weick, 1979). But individ-
sense making and construct their own version of uals also have agency and take actions that
reality" (1995: 94). Different communities come to shape their environments (Giddens, 1994; Gioia
a project team with different professional & Pitre, 1990); thus, the interpretation process is
frames; in turn, these influence team members' inherently dynamic.
interpretations of events that occur during the Because individuals develop and maintain
process of project work. In the face of situational subjective interpretations of their roles in organ-
ambiguity, individuals within different commu- izations, different levels of analysis emerge
nities will consult one another to develop an (Weick, 1995):
interpretation of events in lieu of crossing com-
munal boundaries. 1. an infrasuhjecfive (or individual) level;
2. an intersubjective level, between two or
more individuals, that represents shared
frames of reference (which may transcend
A Sensemaking Perspective on Creativity formal groups or subunits): and
The goal of theory building in the interpretive 3. a collective level that represents the unfold-
ing of change across intersubjective levels.
or sensemaking perspective is to describe organ-
izational life. The focus is less on understanding We examine how each of these levels, indepen-
how to manipulate a system (so as to increase dently and interactively, affects creative pro-
the level of creativity) than it is on understand- cesses over time in large-scale organizational
ing the processes through which individuals projects. In particular, we allow for the possibil-
and organizations develop systems of meaning ity that events, which are viewed as crises, oc-
about creative action. With its focus on the de- cur and how these, in turn, reframe meanings so
velopment of meanings and how they motivate as to shift the balance of power to favor creative
engagement and action, a sensemaking per- activity by certain groups over others.
spective is well suited to our focus on creativity
as a process. Thus, our central research ques- The Intrasubjective Level: The Development of
tion becomes "How does the process of creativ- Sensemaking Frames
ity unfold in organizations?"
Although functionalist perspectives dominate When individuals are confronted with an
organizational research (Gioia & Pitre. 1990). a equivocal set of events, they struggle to make
sensemaking perspective has made significant sense of them (Dutton & Jackson, 1987; House et
headway (Burrell & Morgan. 1979). A sensemak- al., 1995; Peterson. 1998; Weick, 1979). According
ing approach has been invoked to explain a to Goffman (1974). individuals pose to them-
diversity of topics, including issue and agenda selves the question "What is it that is going on
1999
Drazin, Giynn, and Kazanjian 293

here?" Their answer determines how they will Frames include beliefs about how situations
engage in that situation (Kahn. 1990). Mean- and individual actions are tied together to
ing— or sense—develops about the situation, achieve goals (Ford, 1996). These goals can
which allows the individual to act in some ra- include intrinsic satisfaction with the task (Am-
tional fashion; thus, meaning—or sensemak- abile, 1996) or extrinsic rewards associated with
ing—is a primary generator of individual action. creative outcomes, including recognition. Indi-
Scholars have labeled the meanings that in- viduals respond to situations with intentionali-
dividuals hold as frames (Bateson, 1972; Goff- ty—that is. they predict how their actions will
man, 1974), enacfmenfs (Weick, 1979), schemata affect them. If the situation and desired out-
(Poole et al., 1989), and cognitive maps (Porac et comes come together in a meaningful way, a
al.. 1989). What is central to all of these defini- person will respond by engaging in creative
tions is that an individual (1) develops an intra- acts (Ford, 1996; Kahn, 1990); otherwise, the per-
subjective cause-and-effect map of events, ac- son will limit or refrain from engaging in cre-
tions, and consequences; (2) places himself or ative behavior. Thus, we posit the following:
herself in this map; and (3) takes action accord-
ing to this map as events unfold. Frames organ- Proposifion 1: An individual forms an
ize meaning, motivation, and subsequent in- intrasubjective frame of reference for
volvement and action; during any experience of creativity: this frame mediates be-
work activity, an individual not only develops a tween events and engagement (or dis-
sense of what is going on but also a sense of engagement) in creative acts.
how to engage.
According to Goffman (1974), all primary The Intersubjective Level: The Construction of
frames play an important role in governing the Shared Meaning Within Communities
extent to which an individual becomes involved
in the activity organized by the frame; frames An individual's development of a creative
link events to meanings and meanings, in turn, frame of reference does not take place in social
to ensuing action. Involvement can range from isolation; it is shaped by interactions with oth-
complete boredom and abstention to full en- ers who are engaged directly or indirectly in
gagement in the activities at hand. Thus, similar endeavors. When faced with an equivo-
cal situation, an individual reduces equivocality
frames, and the process of framing, can impact
by seeking out the interpretations of others
significantly the extent to which an individual
(Volkema et al., 1996). It is through these inter-
engages in the process of creativity. For exam- actions that schemata, scripts, and categoriza-
ple, testing the performance of a product com- tions diffuse throughout communities (Poole et
ponent could be construed to mean different al., 1989; Poole, Gray, & Gioia, 1990), occupa-
things to an engineer. Depending upon his or tional groups (Trice, 1993), third parties in re-
her frame of reference, the test might be re- lated work, or networks of weaker ties
garded as a critical step in developing an im- (Granovettor, 1973). When interdependence is
portant prototype and, thus, worthy of creative high, a collective mind can emerge (Weick &
engagement; conversely, it might be viewed as Roberts, 1993) and, with it, a communal sense of
"make work" imposed by management and, what makes sense. The result is a shift from the
thus, less inviting of creative engagement. self-referential "I" to the more inclusive "we"
Individuals "bracket" the unpunctuated expe- (Weick, 1995).
riences of the moment using their best available In the organizational literature, authors cap-
frames; however, events, actions, and frames ture intersubjective frames in such constructs as
are linked together in an iterative cycle of per- organizational memory (Walsh & Ungson, 1991).
ception (Weick, 1979). Frames are subject to organization mind (Sandelands & Stablein.
modification and renewal in the face of actions 1987), collective mind (Weick & Roberts, 1993).
taken by an individual. Accordingly, they medi- and organizational intelligence (Glynn, 1996). In
ate between the stream of behavior that con- recognizing that mental maps are shared and
fronts an individual and his or her actions; as belief systems converge, scholars assume an
mediators, they determine how an individual intersubjective level of analysis that differs from
responds to a context. traditional conceptualizations of the individual.
294 Academy oi Management Review April

group, or organizational levels (Daft & Weick, sonnel) and (2) administrative specialists who
1984). plan, manage, and implement the design (i.e..
However, although shared frames of reference those responsible for budgeting, for scheduling,
may develop within communities, they can dif- and for interfacing with clients). These two com-
fer across communities. As Goffman (1974) notes, munities—technical and managerial—likely
even though two actors share a similar set of have different views of what makes sense and,
experiences, their frames of reference may differ therefore, different views of what constitutes
based on their positions with respect to that creativity.
activity. As a result, sensemaking may not be Similarly, Trice (1993) views organizations as
neat, tidy, and polite but, rather, may be marked collections of diverse subcultures that subscribe
by divergent and sometimes antagonistic to different systems of meanings. For him, there
frames of reference, as different communities are two dominant subcultures—administrative
argue their respective beliefs (Trice, 1993; Walsh and occupational—that compete over the con-
& Fahey, 1986; Walsh. Henderson. & Deighton. trol of work, access to power and resources, and
1988; Weick, 1995). credit; their inevitable clashes are resolved
Fine explains: through adaptation and negotiation.
By the placement of an occupation within an or- Daft and Becker (1978) demonstrate explicitly
ganizational field, workers provisionally create the link between different communities of mean-
occupational meanings, given the real con- ing and creative endeavors. In their study of
straints under which they work and in light of the
evaluations of other actors who impinge on their high schools, they found that 71 percent of the
claimed expertise (1996: 111) administrative innovation proposals (e.g.,
changes in community/school relationships,
Thus, even in a single organization, a multitude registration systems, human resource manage-
of diverse frames can exist, arising from and ment systems, and systems to control costs) orig-
characterizing different job categories, occupa- inated from administrators and, conversely, 70
tions, positions, status, ideologies, and para- percent of the teaching innovation proposals
digms (Trice. 1993; Weick. 1995). Creative activ-
(e.g., curriculum changes, new teaching tech-
ity, undertaken in large-scale organizations, is a
niques, and equipment to support instruction)
particularly ripe context for the development of
were initiated by teachers. Daft's (1978) finding
divergent frames of reference. Creativity typi-
that different groups promulgated different
cally involves tension between innovation and
types of innovations led him to conclude that
control (Weick. 1995) and. thus, can engender a
there was a dual-core nature to the innovation
natural dialectic between different, and perhaps
process.
opposing, intraorganizational communities
(Huff. 1988). Dougherty (1992) and Trice (1993) are Differences in occupational frames are likely
instructive to understanding how different inter- to lead to differences in the creative solutions
pretive frameworks embed and affect creativity. proposed by each community. Technical staffs
Dougherty (1992) notes that innovation re- are likely to emphasize technical creativity as
quires the insights of multiple "thought critical to the success of the project. Technical
worlds"—that is. the interpretive schemas of dif- creativity involves the proposal of solutions that
ferent communities of specialists who literally have technical foundations; it also involves the
think differently from each other. She identifies use and extension of the knowledge base held
four competing thought worlds—technical, field, by technical personnel. For example, a materi-
manufacturing, and planning—and argues that, als engineer might propose the use of a new
at best, each thought world views the other as composite material, and a software engineer
esoteric and. at worst, meaningless. The cre- might propose the development of a computer-
ative setting that serves as our exemplar (com- ized system to automate tasks previously done
plex, interdependent, large-scale projects) cre- by system operators. Alternatively, creative
ates a context in which there is likely to be project administrators might propose novel sys-
conflict between two of the thought worlds tems in any of a series of project managerial
Dougherty (1992) describes: (1) technical special- domains, including managing risk, satisfying
ists who create product design (e.g.. engineers, customers, building political skills, staffing
scientists, marketers, and manufacturing per- project teams, estimating costs, measuring
1999 Drazin, GJynn, and Kazanjian 295

project performance, and developing project Proposition 2a: The frames of techni-
feedback and evaluation systems (Frame, 1994). cal staff will emphasize the need to
In large-scale organizational projects, there is experiment and the centrality of tech-
an inherent contest between the sensemaking nical creativity to the success of the
frames of the administrative and technical com- project.
munities, as each invokes their own referent
framework to make sense of tasks or events Proposition 2b: The frames of project
(Thomas & McDaniel, 1990; Trice, 1993). Techni- managers will emphasize the need to
cal staff members, because they deal with tasks satisfy senior management and cli-
that have both heavy mental and material com- ents and the centrality of administra-
ponents (Barley, 1996), have a vested interest in tive creativity to the success of the
developing reputations of creativity in science project.
and engineering. In their study of a product-
design consulting firm, Sutton and Hargadon
(1996) give a vivid example of how technicians The Collective Level: Negotiated Order Among
seek reputational capital in brainstorming ses- Differing Communities
sions. To engineers, brainstorms are "status Because the referent frames of the managerial
auctions" in which they bid for prestige based and technical communities can differ from each
on their ideas; as one industrial designer put it, other, discord can ensue. Ambiguity over the
"You are probably going to ask me about how relative authority of managers and technicians
brainstorms lead to creative products, but what in a single creative project is a manifestation of
strikes me is that those engineers treat it as a deeper conflict between the two occupational
competition. It's a competition!" (Sutton & Har- subcultures (Barley, 1996; Trice, 1993). The issue
gadon, 1996: 706). is fundamentally one of the separation of posi-
Conversely, project managers compete for a tion from expertise (Barley, 1996). Managers may
different sort of reputational capital. In his study accept technicians (begrudgingly) because they
of occupational "rhetorics," Fine notes that the solve problems useful to the manager's goals;
ideal of "business" is linked to "images of ra- conversely, technicians may accept managers
tionality. . .particularly as linked to control over (begrudgingly) because they can provide the re-
the work process, long- and short-term planning, sources needed to create technical designs.
and an awareness of the economic placement of Because they face different goals and tasks
the workplace" (1996: 104). Project management and value different forms of creative engage-
is responsible for meeting preestablished goals ment, project managers and technical staff are
of cost, schedule, and functionality imposed by likely, at least on occasion, to have opposing
senior managers; their creativity takes the form views of creativity. These views clash in organ-
of finding ways to impress these executives and izational settings, with each group representing
develop reputations of meeting goals success- its opinions to the other in the form of an argu-
fully. ment that stands for its unique model of what
Finally, because the technical and manage- makes sense. These argumeiits reveal to the
rial communities have higher within-group than other parties the sensemaking perspective each
between-group contact, their referent frames are holds. Although one outcome might be mutual
supported by behavioral interaction patterns accommodation (Poole et al., 1989), another
that reinforce one belief system over the other. might be that each party sees the other as not
Thus, we propose that intersubjective frames making any sense (Dougherty, 1992).
will likely be homogeneous within a given com- How conflicts are adjudicated between the
munity but heterogeneous across different com- technical and managerial staffs determines the
munities. process and direction of project creativity. Dis-
putes are resolved politically (Walsh & Fahey.
Proposifion 2: Technical staffs and 1986), and whoever has the power to resolve a
project management will differ in the dispute owns the belief system that carries the
frames of reference they use to under- day (Hickson, Hinings, Lee, Schneck. & Pen-
stand and engage in a large-scale cre- nings, 1971; Pfeffer &. Salancik. 1978). The cumu-
ative project. lative resolutions of many disputes, in turn, be-
296 Academy oi Management Review April

come events for interpretation themselves. As ness to consider the need to reframe. When a
managerial and technical staffs witness a dis- disruptive event occurs, a frame may be broken
pute resolved in favor of one party over another, and what once made sense no longer does; the
interpretations develop about how the organiza- individual experiences temporary disorganiza-
tion's political system affects creativity. These tion (Goffman, 1974; Poole et al., 1989). If, be-
interpretations guide the extent to which each cause of new information, enough individuals
group sets creative goals for itself and the de- experience interpretative disorganization, then
gree to which the groups become engaged in an entire intersubjective frame may shift. Thus,
their own form of creative process. there is the potential for a new set of frames to
A negotiated order is likely to emerge from the emerge, at least temporarily.
opposing frames of managers and technicians When a project experiences an event signifi-
(Walsh & Fahey, 1986), with occupational spe- cant enough to be called a "crisis" (Dutton &
cialists doing "whatever is necessary within Jackson, 1987; Peterson, 1998; Tyre 8f Orlikowski,
their ability to achieve the ends of the organiza- 1994), the negotiated order between managerial
tion" (Fine, 1996: 111). Consistent with Weick and technical staff may shift to establish a new
(1995), we use the term collective structure to collective order to resolve the crisis. We draw
reflect the simultaneous cognitive and sociopo- upon Habermas (1975) to develop our definition
litical underpinnings of the process. Sensemak- of crisis; to us, a crisis occurs when the structure
ing may not be completely shared, but political of a social system allows for fewer possibilities
compromise between opposing groups will for problem solving than are necessary for the
nonetheless guide creative behavior. We find it continued existence of the system. Crises arise
useful to regard collective structure as a sliding from exogenous environmental changes but
scale that shifts to reflect the relative balance of also in "structurally inherent system-impera-
power between the project management and tives that are incompatible and cannot be inte-
technical staffs. For a particular point in time, grated" (Habermas, 1975: 2). A crisis occurs when
the scale indicates which interpretive scheme the negotiated order of a collective system does
controls the process of creativity. Following this not allow a problem to be resolved. Accordingly,
line of logic, we propose the following: we posit this:

Proposition 3: At any given point in Proposifion 4: During a crisis, mem-


the history of a project, a balance of bers of the technical and managerial
power will exist between project man- staffs of a creative project will feel
agers and the technical staff. This bal- cognitive disorganization in their re-
ance will determine the extent to spective sensemaking frameworks for
which each community engages in creativity.
creative behavior.
Crises have sociopsychological effects; Poole
et al. (1989: 273) term these critical incidents or-
Crises and the Possibility of Temporary ganizafionai breakdowns. When a crisis occurs,
Reframings it causes project members to suspend their ex-
Sensemaking frames are fragile and subject isting frames of reference and look at the world
to change. Sensemaking requires individuals differently; Goffman (1974) terms this set of
and communities to enact their environment—a events re-keying. Whereas keying involves a
process that is reinforced through interactions transformation or transcription of experience
with others in an attempt to reduce uncertainty. from one meaning into another, re-keying
But even the solidarity of intersubjectivity may means to reorganize meaning parenthetically
not be enough to sustain a frame in the face of by modifying (and not totally transforming) a
inconsistent information. When individuals are current frame. Opposing frames of reference
involved in a stream of behavior (such as cre- may become united through the common expe-
ativity), they always sustain some check upon rience of a crisis. Each side, usually carrying its
their total involvement (Goffman, 1974). At the own set of meanings, can alter or drop these
cognitive level, there is always a trace of hesi- meanings in recognition of the need to resolve
tancy about past interpretation and a willing- the crisis.
1999
Drazin, Giynn, and Kazanjian 297

In the life of a project, crises can occur that sis, project managers and technical staff might
can shift the negotiated order between project simultaneously engage in creative behavior.
management and technical staff to allow for Our overall point is that crises shift structures of
crisis resolution. The types of crisis are defined power and meaning. Although we focus on com-
by the major goals of the project: functionality, peting models, we recognize that the specifics of
cost, and schedule. A crisis of functionality oc- the setting will govern the direction of the shift.
curs when a major design goal of the project is Thus:
in jeopardy. Design goals, developed to meet
strategic objectives or the specific needs of a Proposition 5: The type of crisis a
client, spell out the performance of all or part of project encounters will shift the nego-
the product or service being developed. Exam- tiated order of the collective structure
ples of functionality crises are a plane that is to favor the referent frames of those
overweight or fails to meet efficiency goals, a capable of resolving the crisis.
car that does not pass a crash test, or an oper- Proposition 5a: When a crisis in func-
ating system that is unstable. If a project falls tionality occurs, the collective struc-
short in meeting a design criterion that is essen- ture of the project organization will
tial for the project's success, a crisis ensues. shift to favor the referent frames of the
If the crisis is framed as one of functionality, technical staff. The technical staff will
the effect will be to shift power to those capable engage more in creative behavior.
of solving the crisis, because those subunits
most capable of dealing with an organization's Proposition 5b: When a crisis in cost or
critical problems are the ones that acquire schedule occurs, the collective struc-
power (Hickson et al., 1971; Pfeffer & Salancik, ture of the project organization will
1978). The ability to solve a problem and, conse- shift to favor the referent frames of the
quently, to help the organization cope with un- project managers. The project man-
certainty creates dependence on the subunit. In agement staff will engage more in
the case of a functionality crisis, power accrues creative behavior.
to the technical staff, and the result is a shift in Following Goffman (1974) and Trice (1993), we
the negotiated order of the collective structure expect that crisis-engendered reframings are re-
so that the creative frame of the technical staff versible and temporary. They are likely to be
becomes primary. This results in management's bounded in time by recognition of the crisis and
willingness to search for and accept technically its subsequent resolution. The collective struc-
creative solutions that solve the design problem. ture may revert to its previous balance of power
Alternatively, crises can arise in response to and meaning (Trice, 1993), or a new collective
cost or schedule problems. These crises will structure may emerge that rewards the side that
shift power toward the project management resolved the crisis by according it relatively
staff, and, as a result, that staff's sensemaking greater status. Lasting change may occur when
frame will dominate during the period of crisis. repeated crises leave organizational members
Project managers may refocus the organiza- with altered interpretive schemes (Poole et al..
tion's attention on "the conservation of re- 1989). The effects may be cumulative over the
sources and tight control mechanisms" and the course of the life of a project, and the histories
"maintenance of the status quo" (Thomas, Clark, that develop may spill onto future projects.
& Gioia, 1993: 244). The effect on creative behav- Based on this logic, we propose the following:
ior will be to favor simpler cost-effective solu-
tions and to be biased against elaborate Proposition 6a: After a crisis has
searches for creative technical alternatives. passed, a project will revert to its pre-
vious negotiated order and the bal-
Such a classification may be oversimplified, ance of creative engagement (admin-
for a crisis might involve collaboration between istrative or technical) that order
administrative and technical staffs (Daft, 1978). implies.
A cost problem might be solved by alternative
technologies and a technical problem by a Proposition Gb: Repeated crises of one
project manager's creative interface with a cli- type will shift the negotiated order of
ent. Further, depending on the nature of the cri- the project permanently in favor of the
298 Academy oi Management Review April

affected group, and that group will creative solutions have been thwarted by their
engage more in creative behavior. organizations' dominant political order. Our
model of periodic crises fits the work of Tyre and
Crises may occur naturally, but they also may Orlikowoski (1994). Based on three intensive
be staged intentionally to suit the purposes of case studies, these authors conclude that effec-
an individual or group (Goffman, 1974; Poole et tive firms and managers take advantage of the
al., 1989). An actor may manipulate the organi- "lumpiness" of organizational change. They ex-
zation by reframing as crises events and issues ploit natural opportunities for change that ap-
that occur during the life of a project. Several pear in periodic, discontinuous bursts to intro-
studies in the sensemaking literature demon- duce ideas and keep the process of innovation
strate that how an organization labels an issue fresh. We summarize our expectations as fol-
determines how it responds to that issue (Dutton lows:
& Jackson. 1987; Staw, Sandelands, & Dutton,
Proposifion 7a: Individuals or commu-
1981; Thomas & McDaniel, 1990). Influential rep-
nities may stage crises fo shift the ne-
resentatives of the project or technical staff may
gotiated order in favor of the type of
use periodic episodes of sensemaking that occur
creative engagement (administrative
in a project (e.g., project milestones, interim
or technical) that order implies.
project test dates, and meetings with stakehold-
ers) to create a condition of crisis. For example, Proposition 7b: Individuals or commu-
during periodic project testing, the technical nities may time the introduction of
staff may seek to push its group's agenda by creative solutions to match the timing
demonstrating that a key part of the project is of periodic crises.
failing to meet its design criteria. If the refram-
ing is effective and a crisis is declared, the tech-
nical staff may be free to engage in the creative A Process Model of Creativity over Time
activities it wishes to pursue. Our framework on creativity is composed of
A second, less Machiavellian, possibility ex- four interrelated concepts: (1) individual sense-
ists for using crises to manage a community's making, (2) intersubjectively shared frames of
goals. It is likely that members of both the tech- reference, (3) a collective structure that repre-
nical and project staffs recognize and under- sents a negotiated belief structure between par-
stand the structure of meaning in which they ties that have different frames of reference, and
operate (Giddens, 1994). It may be that the cur- (4) a shift in the negotiated belief structure that
rent negotiated order would prevent them from results from crises. With this multilevel sense-
having an idea funded or accepted. Knowing making model of creativity, we seek to address
that crises occur periodically, these parties the question of how creativity unfolds over time.
would time the introduction of their idea to an We summarize our model graphically in Figure
occurring crisis that would favor its acceptance. 1 as a series of potential paths a creative project
The research of Burgelman and Sayles (1986) could take. We emphasize that these paths are
on internal corporate venturing provides some potential, because each project organization
support for this possibility. In their case study, would be subject to the influences of its own
the authors note that a research scientist had particular history of crises. The challenge to cre-
been working on new types of insulation for ativity researchers is to apply this model in
years but was having trouble getting the work studies of individual organizations.
supported. With the coming of oil embargoes in The figure depicts the history of one potential
the 1970s and the dramatic increase in the price project as it unfolds over time (shown on the
of energy, the scientist's company faced a crisis X-axis). The history is broken down into different
in developing new products. The scientist then time episodes, labeled A through E. The y-axis
used these events as an opportunity to introduce represents a sliding scale depicting the relative
a proposal for added research on insulation. The dominance of the technical or project manage-
proposal was accepted. ment staff in the negotiated order. Greater cre-
It is likely that individuals engage in staging ativity on the part of the technical staff is repre-
crises, or in timing the introduction of ideas to sented in the upper half of the figure, and
periodic crises, when their efforts to introduce greater creativity on the part of the project man-
1999
Drazin, Giynn, and Kazanjian
299

HGURE 1
A Representative Model of Creativity over Time in a Large-Scale Project
Technical staff
(Higher technical creativity)

C.
A. A crisis of
Negotiation functionality

Negotiated
order Time

Emergence of
negotiated order D. E.
A crisis of Repeated
time/budget time/budget
crises

Project management staff


(higher manager creativity)

agement staff is represented in the lower half of During a period of functional crisis (period C).
the figure. the negotiated belief structure abruptly
The outset of the project is marked by ambi- switches in favor of the technical staff. At this
guity and negotiation between competing belief time management is more solicitous of creativ-
systems (time period A). As individuals come ity from the technical staff. Technical solutions
together for the first time, they face great uncer- that previously might have been considered too
tainty and seek to reduce that uncertainty by risky, expensive, or aggressive management
making sense of the project and its creative de- now favors, and the technical staff engages in
mands (Huff, 1988). When the members of the creative behavior. When the crisis is resolved,
project experience sufficient time together, in- presumably owing to the generation of creative
tersubjective frames of reference begin to technical solutions, the negotiated order reverts
emerge. back to its prior level.
As a result of this period of vacillation and Similarly, in time period D the organization
uncertainty, a negotiated belief structure experiences a crisis of budget or schedule. This
emerges in time period B. This time period is results in a shift of the negotiated belief struc-
marked by relative stability, which will persist ture temporarily in favor of project managers.
until a crisis appears. The relative balance be- Project managers engage in creative behavior
tween the technical and managerial belief in their search for solutions to project-related
structures determines the extent to which each problems that may be solved by securing more
side engages in creativity. There is a parallel resources or extending deadlines, or by devel-
between the collective and individual levels oping administrative systems that help the or-
that determines the energy project members put ganization regain control over its targets. When
into creative action or habitual action (Ford, this crisis is resolved, the organization once
1996). again reverts to its prior negotiated order.
Time periods C and D represent crises of func- Time period E represents a series of repeated
tionality and time/budget, respectively. In both and frequent crises of budget/schedule. The
cases, when a crisis occurs, the organization project experiences a series of reframings in
experiences a discontinuous and sudden shift to quick succession, which gradually results in a
a different balance in the negotiated order. This new belief structure that permanently favors the
shift persists until the crisis is resolved, at belief systems of project managers. Should such
which point the organization reverts quickly to a permanent shift occur, the organization is
its prior negotiated order. likely to experience a pronounced reduction in
300 Academy oi Management Review April

creative engagement on the part of the technical multilevel theorizing. In doing so. our aim is not
staff and an accompanying increase in creativ- to criticize or supplant existing creativity theory
ity on the part of the project manager staff. but, rather, to illustrate the possibility of other,
We note that, at first glance, this model might perhaps complementary, theoretical ap-
be interpreted as a variant of a punctuated equi- proaches. As such, our ideas are generalizable
librium approach to change (Gersick, 1991). Our beyond the domain of creativity. In the following
model shares with Gersick's work the notion we describe three theoretical realms to which
that a crisis can have a critical impact on the our multilevel model might apply: (1) assump-
unfolding nature of a project. However, her work tions about inclusiveness. (2) process models
focuses on crises that occur as a result of the and the dimension of time, and (3) a sensemak-
passage of time; more specifically, she found ing perspective on defining levels of analysis.
that crises occur at roughly the midpoint of
projects and are generated by anxiety over
Assumptions About Inclusiveness
project completion.
Our model differs in that we propose that cri- In our review of the creativity literature, we
ses can occur periodically throughout the length found that most researchers adopt a common set
of a project and are initiated by events both of LOA assumptions concerning two key choic-
internal and external to the project. Gersick sug- es: (1) the definition of the creativity construct
gests that all members of a project are mobilized and (2) the level about which generalizations
by the midpoint milestone, whereas we focus on are made. We have shown that most creativity
how crises shift power, mobilize communities, researchers define creativity as an outcome that
and affect creativity throughout the life of a is judged by others as novel and useful and
project. Although we recognize that crises can focus on the level of the individual or work unit/
result in permanent change, as depicted in time small project team. Taken together, these
period E of the figure, our model allows and choices lead researchers to make further as-
even expects that the history of a project will be sumptions about the relationships of individu-
marked by multiple crises of varying types, als to project teams.
which result in only temporary reframings of a In general, we have discovered that creativity
stable negotiated order. Thus, our model has researchers tend to adopt a functionalist-
more in common with switching structure mod- reductionist approach (Rousseau, 1985),
els of change (Duncan, 1976) than with punctu- whereby individuals are modeled as entities
ated equilibrium models. that contribute to project or work group out-
comes, which, in turn, ultimately define the
overall organizational outcome. What each
DISCUSSION lower level of analysis contributes to the next
Socrates reportedly said at his trial that an higher level is creativity. Thus, individuals' cre-
unexamined life was not worth living. We be- ativity aggregates to become work group cre-
lieve that the same holds true for theories. All ativity, and work groups' creativity aggregates
theories contain assumptions; assumptions are to become organizational creativity. The result
necessary because they allow researchers to is that organizational creativity is defined in
proceed without constant introspection and jus- terms of a part-whole relationship so that
tification. But, at least occasionally, an exami- smaller or microlevel entities contribute to the
nation of the dominant assumptions of a theo- functioning of the whole macrolevel system
retical domain is warranted. It has been over 15 (Rousseau, 1985).
years since Amabile (1983) introduced her book A logical extension of the functionalist-
on the role of context in generating creativity. reductionist approach is to model individuals as
Since then, multilevel theories on organization- an inclusive part of the groups for which they
al creativity have appeared. work. That is, individuals are assumed to be
Our goals in this article have been twofold: influenced solely by the characteristics of the
(1) to examine assumptions about levels of anal- formal hierarchy to which they report. Because
ysis embedded in this literature and (2) to exam- the hierarchy has a captive influence on indi-
ine how relaxing or altering some of those as- viduals' creative behavior, researchers implic-
sumptions leads to alternative perspectives on itly assume homogeneous effects of higher-level
1999
Drazin, Giyjin, and Kazan;ian 301
units on individuals. Although individual differ- tive staff could easily be extended to other forms
ences are acknowledged, group or contextual of role and/or social identity overlap. These
effects are assumed to override those differ- might include work versus family roles, func-
ences. tional versus project team roles, and multiple
By focusing on more complex organizations, occupational and professional roles and identi-
characterized by high levels of horizontal inter- ties.
dependence, we have been able to relax the Organizations are complicated entities. An
prevailing assumption of inclusiveness. In- assumption of inclusiveness has allowed cre-
stead, we have proposed that individuals are ativity researchers to reduce complexity. This is
influenced significantly by their occupational particularly useful when the goals of research-
subculture. By using large-scale projects as an ers have normative implications; inclusiveness
exemplar, we can assume significant numbers implies influence and control over valued out-
of both administrators and technicians to allow comes. However, if the goal of the researcher is
for meaningful occupational subcultures to de- to describe complex organizational behavior,
velop. According to multilevel theorists (House then relaxing the assumption of inclusiveness
et al., 1995; Rousseau, 1985), this amounts to an seems warranted.
assumption of partial inclusiveness. Partial in-
clusion complicates multilevel theories; it sug-
gests that individuals are subject to influence Process Models and Time
from a multiplicity of groups in which they hold We have focused on creativity as a process,
membership (e.g., occupational, organizational, rather than as an outcome. At the intrasubjec-
or task). Specifically, we have proposed that or- tive (or individual) level, we have defined cre-
ganizational-level creativity can be described ativity as a person's psychological engagement
as an order negotiated between administrators in creative activity. At the level of an entire
and technicians that occurred at the level of the organizational project, we have shown that cre-
entire project. ativity involves not only individual engagement
One potential avenue for future research on but the emerging structuration of who engages
creativity might be to investigate the degree of and when they engage. At the project level, we
inclusiveness present in different organization- have illustrated that creativity consists of the
al or project contexts (House et al., 1995). We ebb and flow of creative engagement among
chose to focus our theory building on a setting different occupational subcultures—that is,
where membership in occupational groupings managerial versus technical staffs. We have
would have a substantial impact on the frames posited that the political dynamics of such cre-
held by individuals. It is possible that occupa- ative engagement leads to the emergence of a
tional membership would also have a similar negotiated order, with shifts in the balance of
impact on smaller project teams or work groups. power over the history of the project. Thus, given
However, in these settings the influence of for- this dynamic perspective, time and timing are
mal organizational membership may be stron- what we have sought to explain.
ger because cross-functional and cross-team in- One of the most salient ways in which we
terdependence and interaction would be have captured time is in noting how the organi-
minimized. Such small-group settings might be zation responds to crises, particularly those of
characterized by a higher degree of inclusive- functionality, cost, and schedule. When an event
ness. occurs that an organization (or its membership)
Our model also suggests that multilevel the- interprets as a crisis, the negotiated order of
ories should incorporate sociopolitical pro- creative engagement can shift from one occupa-
cesses. We maintain that various subcultures in tional subculture to another. This shift occurs
organizations compete with each other and that through multilevel dynamics that are systemic
this competition has significant effects on an and sociopolitical in nature. We have argued
organization's behavior. The principle of partial that the classification of an event as a crisis
inclusion allows for the influence of overlapping causes subcultures to reframe—at least tempo-
role membership and occupational identities rarily—their shared, intersubjective causal
(DiMaggio, 1991; Fine, 1996). Our discussion of maps. The crisis parenthetically alters the pri-
differences between technical and administra- mary frames of subcultures (and member indi-
302 Academy of Management Review April

viduals) in a profound way that shifts the bal- engender political contests over how the organ-
ance and determines which subculture (or ization defines and solves problems creatively.
individual) engages in creative behavior. Crises Thus, a primary contribution of a sensemak-
are time-bound events. Without explicitly in- ing perspective is that it alerts us to alternative
cluding time as a variable in our model, crises ways of modeling multilevel influences in organ-
would have no meaning. izations. A principal theme we uncovered in the
By explicitly considering the role of time, our creativity literature was the use of an aggrega-
model extends previous work on levels of anal- tion model to describe how individual actions
ysis, specifically that of Klein et al. (1994). We can produce patterns of behavior at the organi-
suggest that members of occupational subcul- zational level. An alternative view, emerging
tures (administrative and technical) are rela- from a sensemaking perspective, is that individ-
tively homogeneous in their frames of reference uals may influence and, in turn, be influenced
at any given point in time. However, when a by cross-level effects (Rousseau, 1985) to affect
crisis occurs, it has the effect of reframing these the intersubjectively derived sensemaking
primary frames of reference and causing them frames that subcultures can share. For example,
to shift. Within each occupational group, the an individual may impact creativity at the col-
resultant reframing is likely to be relatively ho- lective level by actively seeking to frame events
mogeneous. Thus, the interpretative schemas an as crises. If successful, a reframing may result,
occupational culture maintains are homoge- and the negotiated order may shift to motivate
neous and internally consistent within each subgroups, as well as individuals in those sub-
time period, but they may be considered heter- groups, to engage in creative acts.
ogeneous across time periods. At more macro levels, then, organizational
Time also plays a role in giving individuals creativity can result not only from activities born
agency. Giddens (1994) proposes that an integra- of a single individual but, rather, from dynamic
tion of macrolevel variables (structure) and mi- and changing processes of sensemaking that
crolevel variables (agency) can occur only in a emerge and establish themselves as a negoti-
dynamic framework. Our model allows individ- ated order for a point in time. Interpretive pro-
uals to understand the constraints under which cesses thus occur simultaneously at multiple
they operate and to take action to change those levels of analyses in organizations, with effects
constraints. This requires that time explicitly be at one level interacting with, and embed-
incorporated into a model to allow for interpre- ding, other levels of organizational analysis and
tation, action, and the emergence of a new or sensemaking.
modified interpretation. Incorporating time into
multilevel models increases the ability of a the-
orist to propose alternative, and possibly more
Implications for Research
robust, models.
Empirically testing the multilevel theory on
creativity we advance here calls for methods
that go beyond static or cross-sectional inquiry.
Sensemaking and Levels of Analysis Researchers interested in studying sensemak-
In constructing our multilevel model of cre- ing processes in organizations need to adopt "a
ativity, we adopted Weick's (1995) framework of set of methodological tactics that enables them
intrasubjective, intersubjective, and collective to deal with meanings rather than frequency
levels of analysis. Although this parallels the counts" (Weick, 1995: 173). As a starting point for
traditional tripartite classification of individual, future research, we offer ideas on operation-
group, and organization levels, it differs in the alizing three key features of our model: (1) as-
important respect of focusing on the processes sumptions about individual inclusiveness in or-
of sensemaking and interpretation in organi- ganizations; (2) the importance of frames of
zations. This focus on cognitive frameworks of reference and their shifts over time; and (3) the
sensemaking has allowed us to model the re- measurement of engagement in creative behav-
framing that occurs during crisis, and to model ior at the individual and collective levels. In all
how reframing shifts the negotiated order be- of these arenas, incorporating dynamic models
tween administrative and technical frames to of change, evolution, and responsiveness to or-
1999
Drazin, Glynn, and Kazanjian
303
ganizational events and crises are important models over time in response to key environ-
techniques.
mental events or shifts (Barr et al.. 1992), as well
Researchers could examine assumptions as managers' beliefs about technological inno-
about individuals' inclusiveness in occupa- vation (Swan & Newell, 1994). Building on work
tional, organizational, or other communities by in organizational decision making (Ford & He-
adopting methods from the burgeoning litera- garty, 1984) and strategy (Barr et al., 1992;
ture on organizational identification, which Daniels, deChernatony, & Johnson, 1995). those
Mael and Ashforth (1992) define as the "per- using cognitive mapping techniques might test
ceived oneness with or belongingness to" the our propositions on the development of and
organization of which one is a member. One change in referent frames for creativity in organ-
might posit that the degree to which an individ- izations. Of necessity, such study would benefit
ual identifies with a collective might be an in- from a field-based case study of a single organ-
dicator of the degree to which that person feels ization and the unfolding of processes over time;
included in, or influenced by, the collective. Dutton and Dukerich's (1991) seminal study of
Thus, the degree of individuals' inclusiveness sensemaking at the New York Port Authority is
might be gauged by the strength of their identi- an instructive model.
fication. Organizational identification can be
measured with the Mael and Ashforth (1988) To measure the creative engagement of indi-
scale, which has been adapted across different viduals and groups, researchers might employ
types of membership contexts (e.g.. Bhatta- procedures used by Kahn (1990) in his study of
charya, Rao, & Glynn, 1995). Shifts in individu- psychological engagement and disengagement.
als' identification over time could be detected by Adopting qualitative methodologies, Kahn used
assessing members' identification at different multiple strategies, including participant obser-
points in the life of a project team, in individu- vation, interviews, and content analysis of ar-
als' tenure with the organization, and/or in terms chival documentation. Such qualitative methods
of crises or other punctuation points. Research- might be employed, or paired with nonqualita-
ers, thus, could test whether individuals who tive methods (e.g., survey analysis), to study cre-
strongly identify with their occupational com- ative engagement at work.
munity might correspondingly be more respon- Another strategy for assessing engagement
sive to situational factors designed to affect might be the adaptation of the Experience Sam-
their creative engagement. pling Method (ESM): a "quasi-naturalistic
A central feature of our multilevel model of method that involves signaling research sub-
creativity is that cognitive frames shift over time jects at random times during the day, frequently
in response to crises. The successful testing of for a week or longer, and obtaining a report of
our propositions, therefore, requires that these the nature and quality of their experience"
referent frames be measurable. One demon- (Kubey, Larson, & Csikszentmihalyi, 1996: 99).
strated technique for examining individuals' Used extensively to assess whether a person is
causal maps and the extent to which they may in "flow"—a condition of high challenges and
be shared within a collective is that of cognitive skills (Csikszentmihalyi 8f LeFevre, 1989)—it is a
mapping (Barr, Stimpert, & Huff, 1992; Fiol & technique that can be harnessed to examine
Huff, 1992). This approach has been applied to whether an individual is in the process of cre-
process research on organizational innovation ative engagement. The ESM strategy yields data
(Swan, 1995). Cognitive maps represent individ- that can be used in hierarchical linear modeling
uals' schema or interpretations about concepts of experience (Moneta & Csikszentmihalyi, 1996)
and cause-and-effect relationships in their in- and can illuminate how and when individuals
formation environment (Fiol & Huff, 1992). Data engage in creativity, as well as the factors that
for cognitive maps can be generated through govern the process and its outcomes. Such qual-
interviews (Bougon, 1983); causal grids (Swan &. itative, in-depth analyses of how creative pro-
Newell, 1994); or the content analysis of text, cesses unfold and change over time, and in re-
interview transcriptions, or statements (see Huff, sponse to crises, can begin to explicate the
1990. for an overview of techniques). ecology of creative engagement in organiza-
tions and. thus, test some of the propositions
Cognitive mapping techniques have been advanced here.
used to study changes in managers' mental
304 Academy oi Management Review April

CONCLUSION J. Dutton (Eds.), Advances in strategic management, vol.


13: 17-72. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Multilevel theories are emerging as powerful Barley, S. R. 1986. Technology as an occasion for structuring:
models for researchers to employ in mapping Evidence from observations oi CAT scanners and the
organizational phenomena. Because they simul- social order of radiology departments. Administrative
taneously and interactively examine how Science Quarterly, 31: 78-108.
agency at one level of analysis can interact Barley, S. R. 1996. Technicians in the workplace: Ethno-
with, and influence, that at other levels, they graphic evidence for bringing work into organization
studies. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41: 404-441.
afford a means of describing the ever-more-
complex and ever-changing organizational Barr, P. S., Stimpert, J. L., & Huff, A. S. 1992. Cognitive change,
landscape. We have focused on how assump- strategic action, and organizational renewal. Strategic
Management Journal, 13: 15-36.
tions about levels of analysis, embedded in
models as institutionalized norms, can be Barron, F. 1955. The disposition toward originality. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51: 478-485.
changed so as to refocus and illuminate organ-
izational processes differently. We have pro- Barron, F., & Harrington, D. M. 1981. Creativity, intelligence,
and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32:
posed an illustrative model, to complement
439-476.
those in the extant literature, that emphasizes
how individuals, communities, and organization- Basadur, M., & Finkbeiner, C. T. 1985. Measuring preference
for ideation in creative problem solving training. Journal
al systems can create meanings that impact the of Applied Behavioral Science, 21: 37-49.
direction and flow of creativity in organizations.
Basadur, M., Graen, G. B., & Green, S. G. 1982. Training in
We now invite future organizational scholars to
creative problem solving: Effects on ideation and prob-
continue the Socratic tradition of leaving no lem finding and solving in an industrial research organ-
assumptions unquestioned—even ours; it is ization. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
such crises of meaning that propel the creative mance, 30: 41-70.
processes, for organizational practice and Basadur, M., Graen, G. B., & Scandura, T. A. 1986. Teaching
theorizing. effects on attitudes toward divergent thinking among
manufacturing engineers. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy. 71: 612-617.
Bateson, G. 1972 Steps to an ecology of mind. New York:
REFERENCES Ballantine Books.
Amabile, T. M. 1983. The social psychology of creativity. New Bhattacharya, C. B., Rao, H., & Glynn, M. A. 1995. Understand-
York: Springer-Verlag. ing the bond of identification: An investigation of its
correlates among art museum members. Journal of Mar-
Amabile, T. M. 1988. A model of creativity and innovation in
keting. 59(4): 46-57.
organizations. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.),
Research in organizational behavior, vol. 10: 123-167. Bougon, M. 1983. Uncovering cognitive maps: The Self-Q
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. technique. In G. Morgan (Ed), Beyond method; Strate-
gies for social research: 173-188. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Amabile, T. M. 1996. Creativity in context. New York: West-
view Press. Burgelman, R. A., & Sayles, L. R. 1986. Inside corporate inno-
vation: Strategy, structure and managerial skills. New
Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M.
1996. Assessing the work environment for creativity. York: Free Press.
Academy of Management Journal 39: 1154-1184. Burkhardt, M. E., & Brass, D. ]. 1990. Changing patterns or
Amabile, T. M., Goldfarb, P., & Brockfield, S. 1990. Social patterns of change: The effects of change in technology
influences on creativity: Evaluation, coaction, and sur- on social network structure and power. Administrative
veillance. Creativity Research Journal 3: 6-21. Science Quarterly. 35: 1-8.

Amabile, T. M., & Gryskiewicz, S. S. 1987. Creativity in the Burrell, G., & Morgan, G. 1979. Sociological paradigms and
R&D laboratory. Technical Report No. 10. Greensboro, organizational analysis. London: Heinemann.
NC: Center for Creative Leadership. Carson, P. P., & Carson, K. D. 1993. Managing creativity
Amabile, T. M., Hill. K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. enhancement through goal setting and feedback. Jour-
1994. The work preference inventory: Assessing intrinsic nal of Creative Behavior. 27(1): 36-45.
and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Per- Clark, K. B., & Fujimoto, T. 1988. Overlapping problem solv-
sonality and Social Psychology, 66: 950-967. ing in product development. Working paper. Harvard
Andrews, F. M. 1979. Scientific productivity. Cambridge, Business School, Cambridge, MA.
England: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, I. 1989. Optimal experience
Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. 1996. Organizational identity and in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social
strategy as a context for the individual. In J. Baum & Psychology, 56: 815-822.
1999
Drazin, Glynn, and Kazanjian
305
Daft, R. L., 1978. A dual-core model of organizational innova-
Ford, C. M., & Gioia, D. A. 1995. Creativity in organizations:
tion. Academy of Management Journal 21: 193-210. Ivory tower visions and real world voices. Newbury Park,
Daft, R. L., & Becker, S. W. 1978. Innovation in organizations. CA: Sage.
New York: Elsevier Science Publications.
Ford, J. D., & Hegarty, W. H. 1984. About the causes and
Daft, R. L., & Weick, K. E. 1984. Towards a model of organi- effects of structure: An exploratory study. Academy of
zations a s interpretation systems. Academy of Manage- Management Journal 27: 271-291.
ment Review, 9: 284-295.
Frame, J. D. 1994. The new project management. San Fran-
Daniels, K., deChematony, L., & Johnson, G. 1995. Validating cisco: Jossey-Bass.
a method for mapping managers' mental models of
Gardner, H. 1993. Frames oi mind. New York: Basic Books.
competitive industry structures. Human Relations, 48
975-991. Gersick, C. 1991. Revolutionary change theories: A multi-
level exploration of the punctuated equilibrium para-
DeMaio, A., Verganti, R., & Corso, M. 1994. A multi-project digm. Academy of Management Review, 16: 10-36.
framework for new product development. European
Journal of Operational Research, 78: 178-191. Giddens, A. 1994. Central problems in social theory: Action,
structure and contradiction in social analysis. Berkeley,
DiMaggio, P. 1991. The micro-macro dilemma in organiza- CA: University of California Press.
tional research: Implications of role-system theory. In
I. Huber (Ed.), Macro-micro linkages in sociology: 76-98. Gioia, D. A., & Chittipeddi, K. 1991. Sensemaking and sense-
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. giving in strategic change initiation. Strategic Manage-
ment Journal 12: 433-448.
Doty, D. H., & Glick, W. H. 1994. Typologies as a unique form
of theory building: Toward improved understanding and Gioia, D. A., & Pitre, E. 1990. Multiparadigm perspectives on
modeling. Academy oi Management Review. 19: theory building. Academy of Management Review, 4:
230-251. 584-602.

Dougherty, D. 1992. Interpretive barriers to successful prod- Glynn, M. A. 1996. Innovative genius: A framework for relat-
uct innovation in large firms. Organization Science, 3: ing individual and organizational intelligences to inno-
179-203. vation. Academy of Management Review, 21: 1081-1111.
Glynn, M. A., & Webster, J. 1993. Refining the nomological net
Dumaine, B. 1993. Payoff from the new management. For-
of the adult playfulness scale: Personality, motivational,
tune, December 13: 103-110.
and attitudinal correlates for highly intelligent adults.
Drazin, R. 1990. Professionals and innovation: Structural- Psychological Reports, 72: 1023-1026.
functional versus radical-structural perspectives. Jour-
Goffman, E. 1974. Frame analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
nal of Management Studies. 27: 245-263.
University Press.
Drazin, R., & Schoonhoven, C. B. 1996. Community, popula-
Granovetter, M. S. 1973. The strength of weak ties. American
tion, and organization effects on innovation: A multi-
Journal of Sociology. 78: 1360-1380.
level perspective. Academy of Management Journal 39:
1065-1083. Greenberg, D. N. 1995. Blue versus gray: A metaphor con-
straining sensemaking around a restructuring. Group &
Duncan, R. B. 1976. The ambidextrous organization: Design- Organization Management. 20: 183-209.
ing dual structures for innovation. In R. H. Kilmann, L. R.
Pondy, & D. P. Slevin (Eds.), The management of organi- Habermas, J. 1975. legitimation crisis. Boston: Beacon Press.
zation design: Strategies and implementation: 167-188. Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. 1980. Wori redesign. Read-
New York: Elsevier North-Holland. ing, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Dutton, J. E., & Dukerich, J. M. 1991. Keeping an eye on the Helson, R., Roberts, B., & Agronick, G. 1995. Enduringness
mirror: Image and identity in organizational adaptation. and change in creative personality' and the prediction
Academy of Management Journal. 34: 517-554. of occupational creativity. Journal of Personality and
Dutton, J. E., & Jackson, S. E. 1987. Categorizing strategic Social Psychology. 69: 1173-1183.
issues: Links to organizational action. Academy of Man- Hickson, D. J., Hinings, C. R., Lee, C. A., Schneck, R. E., &
agement Review, 12: 76-90. Pennings, J. M. 1971. A strategic contingencies theory of
inter-organizational power. Administrative Science
Eisenhardt, K. M. 1989. Building theories from case study
Quarterly. 16: 216-229.
research. Academy of Management Review, 14: 532-551.
Fine, G. A. 1996. Justifying work: Occupational rhetorics as Hill. R. C , & Levenhagen, M. 1995. Metaphors and mental
models: Sensemaking in innovative and entrepreneur-
resources in restaurant kitchens. Administrative Sci-
ial activities. Journal of Management. 21: 1057-1074.
ence Quarterly. 41: 90-115.
Hoffman, E. 1997. NASA project management: Modern strat-
Fiol, M., & Huff, A. S. 1992. Maps for managers: Where are
egies for maximizing project performance. Project Man-
we? Where do we go from here? Journal of Management
agement Journal 28(3): 4-6.
Studies. 29: 267-285.
Horwitch, M. 1982. Clipped wings: The American SST conflict.
Ford, C. M. 1996. A theory of individual creativity in multiple
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
social domains. Academy of Management Review, 21:
1112-1134. House, R., Rousseau, D. M., & Thomas-Hunt. M. 1995. The
306 Academy of Management Review April

meso paradigm: A framework for the integration of Mumford, M. D., & Gustafson, S. B. 1988. Creativity syndrome:
micro and macro organizational behavior. In B. M. Staw Integration, application, and innovation. Psychological
& L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational Bulletin, 103: 27-43.
behavior, vol. 17: 71-114. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. 1996. Employee creativity:
Huff, A. S. 1988. Politics and argument as a means of coping Personal and contextual factors at work. Academy of
with ambiguity and change. In L. R. Pondy, R. J. Boland, Management Journal, 39: 607-634.
& H. Thomas (Eds.), Managing ambiguity and change: Payne, R. 1990. The effectiveness of research teams: A re-
79-90. New York: Wiley. view. In M. A. West & J. L. Farr (Eds.), Innovation and
Huff, A. S. 1990. Mapping strategic thought. Chichester, creativity at work: 101-122. Chichester, England: Wiley.
England: Wiley. Peters, T. 1988. The mythology of innovation, or a skunk-
Kahn, W. A. 1990. Psychological conditions of personal en- works tale, part II. In M. L. Tushman & W. L. Moore (Eds.),
gagement and disengagement at work. Academy of Readings in the management of innovation: 138-147.
Management Journal 33: 692-724. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Kanfer, R. 1990. Motivation theory and industrial/organiza- Peterson, M. F. 1998. Embedded organizational event: The
tion psychology. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of units of process in organizational science. Organization
industrial and organizational psychology, vol. 1: 75-170. Science, 9: 16-33.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Pfeffer, J., & Salancik, G. R. 1978. The external control of
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. 1989. Motivation and cognitive organizations: A resource dependence perspective. New
abilities: An integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction York: Harper & Row.
approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psy- Poole, P., Gioia, D., & Gray, B. 1989. Influence modes, schema
chology Monograph. 74: 657-690. change, and organizational transformation. Journal of
Kanter, R. M. 1988. When a thousand flowers bloom: Struc- Applied Behavioral Science, 25: 271-289.
tural, collective, and social conditions for innovation in Poole, P., Gray, B., & Gioia, D. 1990. Organization script
organization. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), development through interactive accommodation.
Research in organizational behavior, vol. 10: 169-211. Group and Organization Studies. 15: 212-232.
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Porac, J. F., Thomas, H., & Baden-Fuller, C. 1989. Competitive
Kazanjian, R. K. 1988. Relation of dominant problems to groups as cognitive communities: The case of Scottish
stages of growth in technology-based new ventures. knitwear manufacturers. Journal of Management Stud-
Academy of Management Journal. 31: 257-279. ies, 26: 397-416.
King, N., & Anderson, N. 1990. Innovation in working groups. Ouinn, J. B. 1985. Managing innovation: Controlled chaos.
In M. A. West & J. L. Fan (Eds.), Innovation and creativity Harvard Business Review, 63(May-June): 78-84.
at work: 81-100. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Ouinn, J. B., & Pacquette, P. 1988. Ford: Team Taurus. Teach-
Klein, K. J., Dansereau, F., & Hall, R. J. 1994. Levels issues in ing case. Dartmouth, NH: Amos Tuck School, Dartmouth
theory development, data collection, and analysis. College.
Academy oi Management Review. 19: 195-229.
Ring, P. S., & Rands, G. P. 1989. Sensemaking, understand-
Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. 1984. Setting ing, and committing: Emergent interpersonal transac-
limits on children's behavior: The differential effects of tion processes in the evolution of 3M's microgravity re-
controlling vs. informational styles on intrinsic motiva- search program. In A. H. Van de Ven, H. L. Angle, & M. S.
tion and creativity. Journal of Personality. 52: 233-248. Poole (Eds), Research on the management of innovation:
Kubey, R. W., Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1996. Expe- 337-366. New York: Harper & Row.
rience sampling method applications to communication Rousseau, D. M. 1985. Issues of level in organizational re-
research questions. Journal of Communication. 46: search. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research
99-120. in organizational behavior, vol. 17: 1-37. Greenwich, CT:
Louis, M. R. 1980. Surprise and sense-making: What newcom- JAI Press.
ers experience in entering unfamiliar organizational Sabbagh, K. 1996. Twenty-first-century ;et: The making and
settings. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25: 226-251. marketing of the Boeing 777. New York: Scribner.
MacKinnon, D. W. 1965. Personality and the realization of Sandelands, L. E., & Stablein, R. E. 1987. The concept of
creative potential. American Psychologist, 20: 273-281. organization mind. In S. Bacharach & N. DiTimaso (Eds.),
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B. E. 1992. Alumni and their alma mater: Research in the sociology of organizations, vol. 5: 135-
A partial test of the reformulated model of organization- 161. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
al identification. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13: Sayles, L. R., & Chandler, M. K. 1971. Managing large sys-
103-123. tems: Organizations of the future. New York: Harper &
Mohr, L. 1982. Explaining organizational behavior. San Fran- Row.
cisco: Jossey-Bass. Scudder, G. D., Schroeder, R. G., Van de Ven, A. H., Seiler,
Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1996. The effect of G. R., & Wiseman, R. M. 1989. Managing complex inno-
perceived challenges and skills on the quality of sub- vations: The case of defense contracting. In A. H. Van de
jective experience. Journal of Personality, 64: 275-310. Ven, H. L. Angle, & M. S. Poole (Eds.), Research on the
1999 Drazin, Glynn, and Kazanjian 307
management o/innovation: 401-438. New York: Harper & Trice, H. M. 1993. Occupational subcultures in the workplace.
Row.
Ithaca, NY: ILR Press, Cornell University.
Singh, B. 1986. Role of personality versus biographical fac- Tushman, M. L., & Nelson, R. R. 1990. Introduction: Technol-
tors in creativity. Psychological Studies, 31: 90-92. ogy, organizations, and innovation. Administrative
Slappendel, C. 1996. Perspectives on innovation in organi- Science Quarterly, 35: 1-8.
zations. Organization Studies, 17: 107-124. Tyre, M J., & Orlikowski, W. J. 1994. Windows of opportunity:
Stahl, M. J., & Koser, M. C. 1978. Weighted productivity in Temporal patterns of technological adaptation in organ-
R&D: Some associated individual and organizational izations. Organization Science, 5: 98-118.
variables. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Manage-
Van Maanen, J., & Barely, S. R. 1984. Occupational commu-
ment, EM-25: 20-24.
nities: Culture and control in organizations. In. B. M.
Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., & Dutton, J. E. 1981. Threat- Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organization-
rigidity effects in organizational behavior: A multi- al behavior, vol. 6: 287-365. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
level analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26
Volkema, R. J., Farquhar, K., & Bergmann, T. J. 1996. Third-
501-524.
party sensemaking in interpersonal conflicts at work: A
Sternberg, R. J. 1988. A three-facet model of creativity. In R. J. theoretical framework. Human Relations, 49: 1437-1454.
Sternberg (Ed), The nature of creativity: Contemporary Walsh, J., & Fahey, L. 1986. The role of negotiated belief
psychological views: 125-147. Cambridge, England: structures in strategy making. Journal oi Management,
Cambridge University Press. 12: 325-338.
Sutton, R. I., & Hargadon, A. 1996. Brainstorming groups in Walsh, J., Henderson, C, & Deighton, J. 1988. Negotiated
context: Effectiveness in a product design firm. Admin- belief structures and decision performance: An empiri-
istrative Science Quarterly, 41: 685-718. cal investigation. Organizational Behavior and Human
Swan, J. A. 1995. Exploring knowledge and cognitions in Decision Processes, 42: 194-216.
decisions about technological innovation: Mapping Walsh, J., & Ungson, G. 1991. Organizational memory. Acad-
managerial cognitions. Human Relations, 48: 1241-1270. emy oi Management Review, 16: 57-91.
Swan, J. A., & Newell, S. 1994. Managers' beliefs about factors Weick, K. E. 1979. The social psychology oi organizing. Read-
affecting the adoption of technological innovation: A ing, MA: Addison-Wesley.
study using cognitive maps. Journal of Managerial Psy-
chology, 9(2): 3-11. Weick, K. E. 1995. Sensemalring in organizations. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Thomas, ]., Clark, S., & Gioia, C. 1993. Strategic sensemaking
and organizational performance: Linkages among scan- Weick, K. E., & Roberts, K. H. 1993. Collective mind in organ-
ning, interpretation, action, and outcomes. Academy of izations: Heedful interrelating on flight decks. Adminis-
Management Journal 36: 239-270. trative Science Quarterly, 38: 357-381.
Thomas, J., & McDaniel, R. 1990. Interpreting strategic issues: West, M. A. 1989. Innovation amongst healthcare profession-
Effect of strategy and the information-processing struc- als. Social Behavior. 4: 173-184.
ture of top management teams. Academy of Manage- Wheatley, W. J., Anthony, W. P., & Maddox, E. N. 1991. Select-
ment Journal, 33: 286-306. ing and training strategic planners with imagination
Torrance, E. P. 1988. The nature of creativity as manifest in and creativity. Journal oi Creative Behavior, 25: 52-60.
its testing. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativ- Woodman, R. W., Savsryer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. 1993. Toward
ity: Contemporary psychological views: 43-75. Cam- a theory of organizational creativity. Academy of Man-
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. agement Review. 18: 293-321.

Robert Drazin is an associate professor of organization and management at the


Goizueta Business School of Emory University. He received his Ph.D. from the Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania. His research interests focus on organizational innovation, organ-
ization design, and organizational creativity.
Mary Arm Glynn is an associate professor of organization and management at the
Goizueta Business School of Emory University. She received her Ph.D. from Columbia
University. Her primary research interests focus on creativity, innovation, organiza-
tional cognition, learning and intelligence, and organizational identity and identifi-
cation processes.
Robert K. Kazanjian is an associate professor of organization and management at the
Goizeuta Business School of Emory University. He received his Ph.D. from the Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania. His primary research focus is on the strategy and design of
growing high-technology firms.

You might also like