Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Personal Relationships, 9 (2002), 27–37. Printed in the United States of America.

Copyright # 2002 ISSPR. 1350-4126/02

Forgiveness in marriage: The role of relationship


quality, attributions, and empathy

FRANK D. FINCHAM,a F. GIORGIA PALEARI,b CAMILLO REGALIAb


AND
a b
University at Buffalo, The State University of New York; and Catholic University of Sacred Heart,
Milano

Abstract
Italian husbands (n = 79) and wives (n = 92) from long-term marriages provided data on the role of marital quality,
affective reactions, and attributions for hypothetical partner transgressions in promoting forgiveness. Structural equation
modeling revealed that, as hypothesized, positive marital quality was predictive of more benign attributions that, in turn,
facilitated forgiveness both directly and indirectly via affective reactions and emotional empathy. Unexpectedly, marital
quality did not account for unique variance in forgiveness. Compared to husbands, wives’ responsibility attributions were
more predictive of forgiveness, whereas empathy was a better predictor of forgiveness in husbands than in wives. The
findings are discussed in terms of their implications for the burgeoning therapeutic literature on forgiveness.

Intimate relationships satisfy our deepest forgiveness is one of the most important factors
affiliative needs and are also the source of contributing to marital longevity and satisfac-
some of our most poignant hurts. When the tion (Fenell, 1993). Studies of forgiveness have
hurt occurs, negative feelings (e.g., anger, recently mushroomed (for a bibliography see
resentment) are common, creating a potential McCullough, Exline, & Baumeister, 1998), but
disruption in the relationship. One means of little is known about forgiveness in marriage
meeting this challenge is through forgiveness, (but see Fenell, 1993; Fincham, 2000). The
a concept that has received remarkably little present manuscript draws on the growing body
attention in science despite its pervasiveness of forgiveness research to inform marital
across cultures and major religions (Worthing- research on the topic.
ton & Wade, 1999). Although it is a complex
construct without a consensual definition, at Forgiveness in social psychological research
the center of various approaches to forgiveness
is the idea of a transformation in which Several social psychological studies demon-
motivation to seek revenge and to avoid strate that forgiveness is shaped by social
contact with the transgressor is lessened and events, and by social-cognitive processes,
prosocial motivation toward the transgressor is following an offense. Specifically, confessions
increased. According to the psychotherapy and apologies accompanied by visible signs of
literature, forgiveness helps to restore relation- contrition foster forgiveness (Darby & Schlen-
ships, release bitterness and anger, and heal ker, 1982; McCullough, Worthington, &
inner emotional wounds (e.g., DiBlasio & Rachal, 1997; Ohbuchi, Kameda, & Agarie,
Proctor, 1993). Partners themselves acknowl- 1989; Weiner, Graham, Peter, & Zmuidinas,
edge that the capacity to seek and grant 1991). In the case of unintentional transgres-
sions, mitigating accounts by the offender,
particularly partial acceptance of responsibil-
ity, have positive effects on the victim’s
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- evaluative judgments and responses to the
dressed to Frank Fincham, Department of Psychology,
University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, transgression (Gonzales, Haugen, & Manning,
Buffalo, NY 14260-4110; e-mail: fincham@buffalo.edu. 1994). Attributing the transgression to external

27
28 F. D. Fincham, F. G. Paleari, and C. Regalia

circumstances and judging it as mild, uninten- rumination about the offense was unrelated to
tional, and unavoidable strengthen the will- empathy toward the offender,hence no evidence
ingness to forgive the offender (Bies & Tripp, was obtained to support the hypothesized ‘‘pre-
1996; Boon & Sulsky, 1997; Fincham, 2000), offense relational closeness! rumination!
whereas experiencing ‘‘hot’’ cognitions (an- empathy! forgiveness’’ (p. 1597) sequence.
ger, bitterness, disorientation) and ruminating Thus, the role of empathy as a facilitator of
about the offense (and the perpetrator’s forgiveness processes seemed to hold in relation
negative motives and characteristics) magnify to transgression-level and relationship-level
the willingness to seek revenge (Bies & Tripp, variables such as apology and pre-offense
1996; Worthington & Wade, 1999). Feeling closeness, but not with respect to social-
empathy for the offender also seems to have a cognitive variables such as rumination.
crucial role in promoting forgiveness (McCul-
lough et al., 1997).
Forgiveness in married couples
In light of such research, McCullough,
Rachal, et al. (1998) developed a social- Although the McCullough, Rachal, et al.
psychological model of determinants and (1998) study forms an important bridge
consequences of interpersonal forgiveness. between two areas of inquiry—the social-
According to the model, social-cognitive psychological mechanisms that control for-
variables related to the way the victim thinks giveness and the role of forgiveness in close
and feels about the offender and the offense relationships—that have been frequently sepa-
(e.g., attributions, ruminative thoughts, em- rated in analyses of interpersonal forgiveness,
pathic emotions) are the most proximal it has two important limitations. First, the
determinants of forgiving. A victim’s will- model of forgiveness was tested independent of
ingness to forgive is primarily affected by his gender. As men and women tend to differ in
or her empathy toward the offender and, less their responses to transgression (relative to
proximally, by the victim’s attributions and men, women reports greater levels of anger,
rumination about the offense. Compared with relationship damage, and difficulty of forgive-
social-cognitive variables, features of the ness; Gonzales et al., 1994), and men tend to
transgression, such as the perceived severity exhibit smaller effect sizes as a result of
of the offense and the extent to which the participating in forgiveness intervention stu-
offender apologizes and seeks forgiveness for dies (Worthington, Sandage, & Berry, 2000),
the offense, are viewed as less proximal exploring gender differences in forgiving is
determinants of forgiveness and thus shape called for. Second, McCullough, Exline, et al.
forgiveness, at least indirectly, via social- (1998) did not distinguish among different
cognitive variables. Even more distal than kinds of relationships. As most participants in
the social-cognitive and transgression-related their study reported incidents involving ro-
determinants of forgiveness are qualities of the mantic partners, the finding may be specific to
relationship in which the offense takes place, dating relationships but even this is uncertain.
such as level of intimacy, closeness, satisfac- The present study therefore examines
tion, and commitment. gender differences in forgiveness within a
McCullough, Rachal, et al. (1998) tested specific relationship, long-term marriage.
a mediational model in which pre-offense Although some studies examine forgiveness
relational closeness influenced forgiving by within romantic relationships (e.g., Boon &
(a) making apologies and ruminative thoughts Sulsky, 1997) and clinical or disrupted couples
more and less likely, respectively; and (e.g., Coyle & Enright, 1997; Dobash &
(b) facilitating, via apologies and ruminative Dobash, 1984; Mazor, Batiste-Harel, & Gam-
thoughts, offender-focused empathy. They pel, 1998), only Fenell (1993) and Fincham
found evidence consistent with the hypothe- (2000) have investigated forgiveness in mar-
sized ‘‘pre-offense relational closeness!apo- ried, community couples. By asking couples
logy! empathy! forgiveness’’ (p. 1597) from the community, married for over 20
sequence. However, contrary to prediction, years, to indicate what they view as the 10
Forgiveness in marriage 29

most important factors contributing to their 3; Karney, Bradbury, Fincham, & Sullivan,
own long-term marriage, Fenell (1993) found 1994). The causal attributions–responsibility
that willingness to forgive and to be forgiven attributions pathway is consistent with prior
was rated by the couples as one such factor. research testing a linear or entailment model of
More recently, Fincham (2000) demonstrated the relations among different types of attribu-
that forgiveness fully mediated the well- tions (Fincham & Bradbury, 1987; Lussier,
documented relation between responsibility Sabourin, & Wright, 1993).
attributions and reported behavior (Bradbury The model assumes that benign respon-
& Fincham, 1992). To complement these sibility attributions promote forgiveness both
initial findings, the present study investigates directly (path f ) and indirectly, through the
the role that relationship-level variables (such mediation of affective reactions (path d-h) and
as marital quality) and social-cognitive vari- emotional empathy (path e-i). The direct link
ables (such as causal and responsibility is supported by basic research (Boon &
attributions, affective reactions, and emotional Sulsky, 1997; Darby & Schlenker, 1982;
empathy1) have in promoting forgiveness in Fincham, 2000; Weiner et al., 1991) as well
long-term married couples. as the literature on therapeutic interventions
(Al-Mabuk, Dedrick, & Vanderah, 1998). In
both domains, willingness to forgive an
Linking relationship quality, attributions,
offender is enhanced by more benign respon-
affective reactions, and emotional empathy
sibility attributions (e.g., viewing the offense
to forgiveness
as less intentional and avoidable). The hypoth-
To date no study has investigated the extent to esized indirect links from benign responsibil-
which both relationship-level variables (e.g., ity attributions to forgiveness are primarily
marital quality) and social-cognitive variables based on Weiner’s (1995) theory. According to
(e.g., attributions, affective reactions, and Weiner, ‘‘perceptions of responsibility and
emotional empathy) predict forgiveness in nonresponsibility for events and states have
married couples. In view of this lack, and respective linkages to emotions of anger and
consistent with the social-psychological model sympathy’’ and ‘‘these emotions, rather than
of forgiving previously described (McCul- ‘cold’ cognitions, determine how to react to
lough, Exline, et al., 1998), we hypothesized others who have engaged in moral transgres-
that the associations among relationship qual- sions’’ (Weiner, 1995; pp. 21, 17). There are
ity, causal and responsibility attributions, data corroborating Weiner’s theory (e.g.,
affective reactions, emotional empathy, and Betancourt, 1990; Betancourt & Blair, 1992;
forgiveness could be conceptualized through Schimdt & Weiner, 1988; Zucker & Weiner,
the theoretical model shown in Figure 1. 1993).2 We therefore hypothesize that benign
According to the model, positive marital responsibility attributions for negative spouse
quality promotes less conflict-promoting and behaviors facilitate willingness to forgive by
more benign responsibility attributions both reducing negative affective reactions and by
directly (path c) and indirectly, via causal enhancing spouse-focused emotional empa-
attributions (path a-b). The hypothesized links thy. The amounts of negative affect and
between marital quality and attributions are emotional empathy experienced by married
supported by existing longitudinal (e.g., Finc- couples in reaction to negative spouse beha-
ham & Bradbury, 1993) and cross-sectional viors are hypothesized to be inversely asso-
data (e.g., Fincham & Bradbury, 1992, Study ciated (link g).
Moreover, marital quality is linked directly
to negative affective reactions (path j) and
1. Emotional empathy can be defined as a vicarious
emotion that is congruent with but not necessarily
identical to the emotion of another person (Batson &
Shaw, 1991). Empathy includes concepts such as 2. Fincham and Bradbury (1992) also provide empirical
sympathy, compassion, tenderness, caring (Batson, evidence supporting the causal attributions!responsi-
1991; Batson & Shaw, 1991). bility attributions!affective reactions sequence.
30 F. D. Fincham, F. G. Paleari, and C. Regalia

Figure 1. Hypothesized model of relations among marital quality, attributions, affect, and
forgiveness.

emotional empathy (path k) and indirectly Participants were predominantly in their


through the mediation of attribution processes. first marriages (98% of the husbands and 97%
These assumptions rest on empirical data of the wives), with mean age in the late 40s
showing that spouses in close and satisfying (M = 48.7 years, SD = 5.4 for the husbands and
relationships, relative to spouses in dissatisfy- M = 45.7, SD = 5.1 for the wives), and generally
ing marriages, are more likely to react to some high school education (M = 11.2 years,
negative partner behaviors by feeling empathy SD = 3.1, and M = 11.3, SD = 2.8). Couples
and experiencing few negative emotions averaged 21.2 years of marriage (SD = 4.3).
(Carstensen, Gottman, & Levenson, 1995;
Levenson, Carstensen, & Gottman, 1994; Procedure
McCullough, Exline, et al., 1998). Finally,
Marital quality, attributions, affective reactions,
consistent with theoretical writings that link
emotional empathy, and forgiveness were
relationship valence to forgiveness (e.g.,
assessed with questionnaires sent home with
Worthington & Wade, 1999), presumably
the adolescent child. Couples received the
because the reconciliation promoted by for-
materials together with two separate envelopes
giveness is necessary for a satisfying relation-
and a cover letter thanking them for their
ship, the model predicts that marital quality is
participation in the study and instructing them
connected directly to forgiveness (path i).
on their task. The importance of independent
However, in light of McCullough, Exline, et
completion of the materials was emphasized in
al.’s (1998) work, indirect links, via attribu-
the letter, and couples were asked to seal the
tions, affective reactions, and emotional em-
completed materials in separate envelopes
pathy, are also posited.
before talking about the study.

Method Materials
Participants filled out two sets of materials: a
Participants
relationship quality questionnaire and a rela-
One hundred and twenty-eight Italian married tionship events questionnaire.
couples with an adolescent child attending the
last three years of secondary school were Marital quality. Marital quality was assessed
contacted through the child’s school as a part using the Quality of Marriage Index (QMI,
of a larger project. They were sent letters Norton, 1983). The QMI is a six-item inventory
introducing the study as a survey on family that assesses marital quality using broadly
relations and inviting them to participate. Of worded, global items (e.g., ‘‘We have a good
the eligible couples, 72% agreed to participate; marriage’’). The respondent shows the degree
92 wives and 79 husbands returned completed of agreement with each of five items on a scale
questionnaires. ranging from 1 (very strong disagreement) to 7
Forgiveness in marriage 31

(very strong agreement) and with one item on a believed they would experience each of five
scale ranging from 1 (very strong dis- emotions on a 7-point scale (1 = not at all to 7
agreement) to 10 (very strong agreement). In = very much). Principal-components analyses
the present study, the QMI had high internal with oblique rotation were conducted to
consistency (alpha coefficients = .96 for both reduce these items to a smaller set of under-
husbands and wives). lying components. Two factors emerged: (a)
negative affective reactions (angry, sad, ner-
Relationship events questionnaire. This vous) and (b) emotional empathy (sympa-
booklet contained four negative spouse beha- thetic, softhearted). In both husbands and
viors (e.g., you and your spouse had arranged to wives, the negative affective reactions factor
spend the evening together, but when the possessed an eigenvalue of greater than 2.63
evening came, your spouse told you that he/ and explained more than 52% of the variance,
she had arranged to do something else by and the emotional empathy factor possessed an
himself/herself) and instructed respondents to eigenvalue of greater than 1.29 and explained
vividly imagine his or her spouse performing more than 25% of the variance. Coefficient
the behavior. After each event description, alphas for negative affective reactions and
participants answered questions about attrib- emotional empathy composites were .76 and
utions, emotions, and forgiveness. The order of .91, respectively, for the husbands and .86 and
the questions was counterbalanced across .85, respectively, for the wives.
participants. Responses to corresponding Forgiveness was assessed using four items
questions for the four events were averaged in from a five-item measure of forgiving used by
subsequent analyses. McCullough et al. (1997). The four items were
Marital attributions were assessed using ‘‘I would disapprove of my spouse,’’ ‘‘I would
items from the Relationship Attribution Mea- think favorably of my spouse,’’ ‘‘I would
sure (RAM) of Fincham and Bradbury (1992). condemn my spouse,’’ and ‘‘I would forgive
Spouses were asked to rate on 6-point scales my spouse.’’ The fifth item, ‘‘I wish him/her
the extent to which they agreed with six well,’’ was omitted because it did not lend itself
attribution statements made about each nega- to assessing forgiveness in marriage. Spouses
tive partner behavior. Three statements per- responded to each item using a 5-point scale (1
tained to causal attributions and three focused = not at all to 5 = completely) and the first and
on responsibility attributions. We formed third items reported above were reverse scored.
causal and responsibility attribution indices Principal-components analyses with oblique
by summing across individual dimensions. rotation yielded a single principal component
Higher causal attribution scores reflect less for husbands (eigenvalue = 2.84, percentage of
damaging or more benign causal attributions: variance = 71) and wives (eigenvalue = 2.62,
causes that are less likely to be located in the percentage of variance = 65.5). Coefficient
partner, more unstable or changing and alphas were .85 and .83, respectively.
specific or affecting few areas of the marriage The QMI, the RAM, and the forgiveness
(more benign causal attributions). Higher items were translated into Italian by the
responsibility attribution scores show that the second author. In order to check the transla-
partner behavior is seen as less intentional, tion, the Italian version of materials was
selfishly motivated, and blameworthy (more back-translated into English by a bilingual
benign responsibility attributions). Coefficient English-Italian speaker.
alphas for causal and responsibility indices
were .82 and .78, respectively, for the
Results
husbands, and .85 and .84, respectively, for
wives.
Descriptive statistics and correlations
Emotions were assessed by asking partici-
pants to imagine how they would feel if the Tables 1 and 2 show the means and standard
negative partner behavior occurred that day. deviations for the variables investigated
They then rated the extent to which they and the correlations among them, respectively.
32 F. D. Fincham, F. G. Paleari, and C. Regalia

Table 1. Means and standard deviations for marital quality, causal and responsibility
attributions, negative affective reactions, emotional empathy, and forgiveness for husbands
and wives

Husbands Wives
Variables M SD M SS
Marital quality 36.82 8.25 34.83 8.55
Causal attributions 42.04 11.95 39.83 12.92
Responsibility attributions 56.60 11.83 57.67 11.35
Negative affective reactions 40.61 15.32 47.33 15.38
Emotional empathy 31.90 10.77 25.30 8.26
Forgiveness 56.73 13.07 53.29 11.95

Modeling strategy Goodness of model fit was evaluated in


two ways. First, we used the chi-square
To test whether the relations among marital statistic (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1984), the
quality, causal and responsibility attributions, Comparative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990),
negative affective reactions, emotional empa- and the Root Mean Square Error of Approx-
thy, and forgiveness were consistent with the imation (RMSEA; Bentler, 1995) to assess
model described in Figure 1, we examined a the absolute goodness of fit of each model.
series of structural equation models using EQS Second, where there were nonsignificant path
Version 5 (Bentler, 1995). The analyses were coefficients we used chi-squared difference
carried out using a multiple groups modeling tests to assess the relative changes in good-
strategy that simultaneously estimated para- ness of fit associated with the removal of
meters for husbands and wives. We began by these paths from the model (Byrne, 1994).
constraining corresponding paths for husbands The chi-squared difference tests evaluate the
and for wives to be equal. When the Lagrange significance of the difference in chi-square
Multipler (LM) test indicated that a path was values for two competing models, the nested
not equivalent across husbands and wives, the model generated by dropping one path from a
equality constraint was released and the model given model and the given model itself. The
was reestimated in order to improve the model model having a significantly better fit is
fit (Byrne, 1994). retained as the best description of the

Table 2. Correlations among marital quality, causal and responsibility attributions, negative
affective reactions, emotional empathy, and forgiveness for husbands (above diagonal) and
wives (below diagonal)

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Marital quality – .25* .34** .20 .36** .37**
2. Causal attributions .40*** – .32** .29** .24* .31**
3. Responsibility attributions .31** .40*** – .36** .48*** .55***
4. Negative affective reactions .07 .23* .39*** – .56*** .66***
5. Emotional empathy .09 .19 .34** .28** – .75***
6. Forgiveness .25* .32** .68*** .52*** .53*** –

* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.


Forgiveness in marriage 33

observed data. If models are not significantly behaviors through more benign causal attribu-
different, parsimony dictates that the nested tions which, in turn, promote more benign
model (the one with fewer paths) is a better responsibility attributions for the same beha-
description of relations among the constructs. viors. The pathways among marital quality,
causal attributions, and responsibility attribu-
Structural relations tions were all equivalent across gender.
Consistent with our hypotheses, responsi-
The hypothesized model (see Figure 1) pro-
bility attributions were related to forgiving
vided a good fit to the data, 2 = 23.837 (df =
both directly and indirectly through negative
21, p = .30), CFI = .990, and RMSEA = .029,
affective reactions and emotional empathy.
but LM 2 statistics and related probability
Specifically, spouses making more benign
values suggested that the equality constraints
responsibility attributions were more likely to
related to paths f (from benign attributions to
react to their partner’s negative behaviors by
forgiveness) and i (from emotional empathy to
being empathic and not experiencing negative
forgiveness) were not appropriate. When the
affects. Although they covaried, negative
two constraints were released, the model fit
affective reactions and emotional empathy
increased significantly, 2 (2) = 7.61, p < .05
predicted forgiveness independently such that
(fit statistics were 2 (19) = 16.237, p = .64; CFI
spouses were more likely to forgive their
= 1.000; RMSEA = .000), indicating that beta
partner for negative behaviors when they felt
weights associated with paths from attributions
empathy or did not experience negative emo-
to forgiveness and from emotional empathy to
tional states. Not all pathways among respon-
forgiveness are reliably different for husbands
sibility attributions, affect reactions, empathy,
and wives. However, because many near-zero
and forgiveness were equivalent across gender.
paths remained in the reestimated model, we
In particular, responsibility attributions were
generated a series of nested models by remov-
more strongly related to forgiveness in wives
ing one nonsignificant or marginally significant
than in husbands, whereas emotional empathy
path at a time from it.3 Compared to the
was more strongly associated to forgiveness in
reestimated model, the nested model in which
husbands than in wives.
the paths j (from marital quality to negative
Finally, contrary to predictions, marital
affective reactions), k (from marital quality to
quality was not directly linked to forgiveness,
emotional empathy), and l (from marital quality
to affective reactions, or to emotional empathy.
to forgiveness) were progressively dropped
It related to these variables only indirectly
proved to be a more parsimonious and equally
through a causal chain in which responsibility
adequate description of the data (2 = 23.216,
attributions proved to play a pivotal role.
df = 22, p = .40; CFI = .996; RMSEA = .020).
Parameter estimates for this final model, which
accounted for a large amount of variance in Discussion
both husbands’ (R2 = .61) and wives’ (R2 = .64)
Interpersonal forgiveness has received growing
forgiveness, are presented in Figure 2. The
attention in the scientific literature, and research
figure shows path estimates for both husbands
is emerging that examines forgiveness in close
and wives (the latter in parentheses).
relationships. Social psychological studies
As predicted, marital quality was signifi-
provide evidence that relationship-related
cantly related to responsibility attributions
variables, such as pre-offense closeness
both directly and indirectly via causal attribu-
(McCullough, Exline, et al., 1998), and
tions. Specifically, spouses judging their
social-cognitive variables, such as offender-
marital relationship positively were more
focused attributions (Boon & Sulsky, 1997;
likely to explain their partner’s negative
Fincham, 2000) and emotional empathy
(McCullough et al., 1997; McCullough, Ex-
3. The resulting trimmed models should be interpreted with line, et al.,1998), play an important role in
caution as the sample size in this study favors trimming promoting forgiveness. Although McCul-
paths that might be significant with a larger sample. lough, Exline, et al.’s (1998) interesting
34 F. D. Fincham, F. G. Paleari, and C. Regalia

Figure 2. Model of obtained relations among marital quality, attributions, affect, and
forgiveness. Parameters for wives are in parentheses; for all paths p < .05, one-tailed.

theoretical model proposed that the quality of affective-cognitive variables contrasts with
the close relationship in which the offense McCullough, Exline, et al.’s (1998) previous
takes place affects forgiveness through both findings. We suspect that this inconsistency
attributions and empathy, no published study may reflect the different kind of social
has simultaneously investigated these variables psychological constructs investigated by the
within married couples. two studies (relationship quality, attributions,
The present study was designed to explore and negative affect reactions versus closeness,
the role that relationship quality, attributions, apology, and rumination) as well as the
affective reactions, and emotional empathy different samples in which data were col-
play in facilitating interpersonal forgiveness lected (Italian married couples versus U.S.
within long-term married couples. Consistent college students in introductory psychology
with the McCullough, Exline, et al. (1998) courses).
model, we hypothesized that marital quality Overall, the present findings demonstrate
would be a more distal determinant of forgive- that both attributions and emotional reactions
ness than affective-cognitive variables. In light that long-term spouses develop in response to
of Weiner’s (1995) attributional theory of their partner’s negative behaviors are crucial in
helping behavior, we also specified that, understanding their willingness to forgive the
compared to affective reactions and emotional partner. This result is consonant with research
empathy, attribution processes would operate indicating that partners’ attributions and emo-
more distally in the causal chain leading to tions shape their reactions to problematic
forgiveness. marital situations (Boon & Sulsky, 1997;
For both husbands and wives, results were Bradbury & Fincham, 1992; Fincham, 2000;
consistent with these hypotheses: Support was Geist & Gilbert, 1996; Miller & Bradbury,
obtained for a model where forgiveness was 1995). Indirect, marital quality also plays a
the result of a causal sequence in which role. By regulating emotional expressions and
positive relationship quality determined causal interactions between spouses, it provides the
and responsibility attributions, which, in turn, climate in which affective-cognitive processes
promoted forgiveness both directly and indir- influencing forgiveness are occasioned. As
ectly via affective reactions and emotional McCullough, Exline, et al. (1998) argued,
empathy. However, contrary to prediction, no spouses involved in satisfactory and close
evidence was obtained to support the hypothe- marriages typically experience a sense of well-
sized direct effect of marital quality on being and comfort in the relationship that
forgiveness, after controlling for the affective- enhances their ability to positively reinterpret
cognitive variables investigated. The fact that marital transgressions, to emotionally identify
the marital quality–forgiveness link is medi- with the offender partner, and, ultimately, to
ated completely, rather than partially, by forgive him or her.
Forgiveness in marriage 35

As anticipated, however, gender appears to natural course of marital relationships. How-


be an important factor for understanding ever, Fincham (2000) shows that proneness to
forgiveness. First, the direct effects of respon- forgive, assessed via hypothetical transgres-
sibility attributions on forgiveness turned out sions, does predict behavioral responses to an
to be stronger for wives than for husbands. actual transgression. Second, the transgres-
This pattern is congruent with literature sions investigated in this study were relatively
showing that attributions and behaviors ex- minor ones. Because transgression severity
hibited during problem-solving and support influences forgiveness (Boon & Sulsky, 1997),
discussions are more strongly related among it would be unwise to generalize these findings
wives than among husbands (Bradbury, to severe transgressions. Indeed, a challenge
Beach, Fincham, & Nelson, 1996; Bradbury for future research is to determine whether a
& Fincham, 1992; Miller & Bradbury, 1995), single process model can explain forgiveness
that wives engage in more attributional of both minor and severe transgressions.
activity than husbands do (see Holtzworth- Third, the structural model tested is one of
Munroe, & Jacobson, 1985), and with spec- several that could fit the data and models that
ulation that wives are more sensitive to include different parameters could also ac-
relationship events (see Worell, 1988). Sec- count for variance in forgiveness. Finally,
ond, the direct effects of emotional empathy although the data are consistent with the tested
on forgiveness are stronger for husbands than structural model, these data are correlational
for wives. Men display less intimate behavior, and cannot be used to draw conclusions about
including empathic behavior, in relationships causality. Thus, longitudinal research would
than women do (for a review see Prager & be useful in exploring more fully causal links
Fincham, 2001), and so when men are among the variables investigated.
empathic it may have a greater impact on Notwithstanding these limitations, the pre-
their relationship functioning, including their sent study is the first to explore the simulta-
willingness to forgive a partner. neous contributions of relationship and
The gender-related findings we obtained affective-cognitive variables to forgiveness
are best understood in light of the overall within long-term married couples. Much work,
(direct and indirect effects) impact of affec- however, remains to be done in illuminating
tive-cognitive variables on forgiveness. Speci- the impact that these same variables have on
fically, for husbands, emotional empathy is not forgiveness within different samples of cou-
more predictive of forgiveness than attribu- ples (e.g., spouses married for a longer/shorter
tions ( b s = .52, .23, and .52 for responsi- time) as well as in investigating their impact
bility attributions, negative affective reactions, after controlling for other determinants of
and empathy respectively), whereas for wives spouses’ forgiveness. As Fincham’s (2000)
attributions are clearly the most important and Worthington’s (1998) organizational frame-
predictor of forgiveness ( b s = .69, .23, and works point out, several broad classes of
.31 for responsibility attributions, affective variables that have received either little or no
reactions, and empathy respectively). Thus, prior attention in relation to forgiveness may
forgiveness appears to be driven by cognitive influence its occurrence. Clearly, much work
(attributions) and affective (empathy) vari- remains to achieve a more complete under-
ables in husbands but is primarily driven by standing of forgiveness in marriage.
cognitive variables (attributions) in wives. Forgiveness is receiving considerable atten-
Before turning to its implications, we tion in the psychotherapy literature (see
highlight a number of limitations to this study. Gordon, Baucom, & Snyder, 2000; Worthing-
First, because it uses hypothetical scenarios of ton, 1998) with a growing number of inter-
marital transgressions, the study does not vention programs focusing on forgiveness as
address actual forgiving but only the disposi- the major goal of the intervention or as
tion or willingness to forgive. As a result, the a component of a broader intervention.
current findings cannot be confidently ex- However, the clinical implementation of
tended to real forgiveness occurring in the forgiveness interventions has far outstripped
36 F. D. Fincham, F. G. Paleari, and C. Regalia

empirical data on forgiveness. We are thus in victim’s attributions for the hurtful event and
the position of attempting to induce forgive- by inducing the victim to feel empathy toward
ness without knowing a great deal about how the offender (e.g., Al-Mabuk et al., 1998;
forgiveness operates in everyday life or in Gordon & Baucom, 1998; Worthington,
close relationships. 1998). If replicated, the gender-related pattern
The present study speaks to the gap of findings for attributions and affective
between basic research on forgiveness and reactions have an important implication; they
the increased role of forgiveness in therapy by suggest that relatively more time be given to
offering empirical information on predictors of attributions for inducing forgiveness among
forgiveness in long-term marriages. In doing wives but that empathy and attributions might
so, it provides some empirical support for be equally effective routes for increasing
attempts to foster forgiveness by changing the forgiveness in husbands.

References
Al-Mabuk, R. H., Dedrick, C. V. L., & Vanderah, K. Darby, B. W., & Schlenker, B. R. (1982). Children’s
M. (1998). Attribution retraining in forgiveness reaction to apologies. Journal of Personality and
therapy. Journal of Family Psychotherapy, 9(1), Social Psychology, 43(4), 742–753.
11–30. DiBlasio, F. A., & Proctor, J. H. (1993). Therapists and
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question. Hillsdale, NJ: the clinical use of forgiveness. The American Journal
Erlbaum. of Family Therapy, 21(2), 175–184.
Batson, C. D., & Shaw L. L. (1991). Evidence for Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1984). The nature and
altruism. Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives. antecedents of violent events. British Journal of
Psychological Inquiry, 2, 107–122. Criminology, 24(3), 269–288.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural Fenell, D. (1993). Characteristics of long-term first
models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246. marriages. Journal of Mental Health Counseling,
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program 15(4), 446–460.
manual, multivariate software. Encino. Fincham, F. D. (2000). The kiss of the porcupines. From
Betancourt, H. (1990). An attribution-empathy model of attributing responsibility to forgiving. Personal Rela-
helping behavior: Behavioral intentions and judge- tionships, 7, 1–23.
ments of help-giving. Personality and Social Psychol- Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1987). Cognitive
ogy Bulletin, 16 (3), 573–591. processes and conflict in close relationships: An
Betancourt, H., & Blair, I. (1992). A cognition (attribu- attribution-efficacy model. Journal of Personality
tion)-emotion model of violence in conflict situations. and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1106–1118.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(3), Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1992). Assessing
343–350. attributions in marriage: The Relationship Attribution
Bies, R. J., & Tripp, T. M. (1996). Beyond distrust: Measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
‘‘Getting even’’ and the need for revenge. In R. M., ogy, 62(3), 457–468.
Kramer & T. R., Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1993). Marital
Frontiers in theory and research, (pp. 246–260) satisfaction, depression, and attributions: A long-
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. itudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Boon, S. D., & Sulsky, L. M. (1997). Attributions of Psychology, 64(3), 442–452.
blame and forgiveness in romantic relationships: A Geist, R., & Gilbert, D. G. (1996). Correlates of expressed
policy-capturing study. Journal of Social Behavior and and felt emotion during marital conflict: Satisfaction,
Personality, 12(1), 19–44. personality, process, and outcome. Personality and
Bradbury, T. N., Beach, S. R. H., Fincham, F. D., & Individual Differences, 21(1), 49–60.
Nelson, G. M. (1996). Attributions and behavior in Gonzales, M. H., Haugen, J. A., & Manning, D. J. (1994).
functional and dysfunctional marriages. Journal Victims as ‘‘narrative critics’’: Factors influencing
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(3), rejoinders and evaluative responses to offenders’
569–576. accounts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Attribution and 20(6), 691–704.
behavior in marital interaction. Journal of Personality Gordon, K., & Baucom, D. (1998). Understanding
and Social Psychology, 63(4), 128–613. betrayals in marriage: A synthesized model of
Byrne, B. M. (1994). Structural equation modeling with forgiveness. Family Process, 37(4), 425–449.
EQS and EQS/Windows. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gordon, K. C., Baucom, D. H., & Snyder, D. K. (2000).
Carstensen, L. L., Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. M. The use of forgiveness in marital therapy. In M. E.
(1995). Emotional behavior in long-term marriage. McCullough, K. Pargament, & C. Thoreson (Eds.),
Psychology and Aging, 10(1), 140–149. Forgiveness: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 203–
Coyle, C. T., & Enright, R. D. (1997). Forgiving 227). New York: Guilford Press.
intervention with post-abortion men. Journal of Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1985). Causal
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 1042– attributions of married couples: When do they search
1046. for causes? What do they conclude when they do?
Forgiveness in marriage 37

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, Miller, G. E., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). Refining the
1398–1412. association between attributions and behavior in
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1984). LISREL VI: marital interaction. Journal of Family Psychology,
Analysis of linear structural relationships by method of 9(2), 196–208.
maximum likelihood. Chicago: National Educational Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical
Resources. look at the dependent variable. Journal of Marriage
Karney, B. R., Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., & and the Family, 45, 141–151.
Sullivan, K. T. (1994). The role of negative affectivity Ohbuchi, K., Kameda, M., & Agarie, N. (1989). Apology
in the association between attribution and marital as aggression control: Its role in mediating appraisal of
satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- and response to harm. Journal of Personality and
chology, 66(2), 413–424. Social Psychology, 56(2), 219–227.
Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M. Prager, K., & Fincham, F. D. (2001). Intimacy in human
(1994). Influence of age and gender on affect, development and interpersonal relationships. Manu-
physiology, and their interrelations: A study of long- script submitted for publication.
term marriages. Journal of Personality and Social Schimdt, G., & Weiner, B. (1988). An attribution-affect-
Psychology, 67(1), 56–68. action theory of behavior: Replications of judgments of
Lussier, Y., Sabourin, S., &: Wright, J. (1993). On help-giving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
causality, responsibility, and blame in marriage: tin, 14(3), 610–621.
Validity of the entailment model. Journal of Family Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A
Psychology, 7(3), 322–332. foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York:
Mazor, A., Batiste-Harel, P., & Gampel, Y. (1998). The Guilford Press.
Divorcing spouses’ coping patterns, attachment bond- Weiner, B., Graham, S., Peter, O., & Zmuidinas, M.
ing and forgiveness processes in the post-divorce (1991). Public confession and forgiveness. Journal of
experience. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 29(3/ Personality, 59(2), 281–312.
4), 65–81. Worrel, J. (1988). Women’s satisfaction in close relation-
McCullough, M. E., Exline, J. J., & Baumeister, R. F. ships. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 477–498.
(1998). An annotated bibliography of research on Worthington, E. L., & Wade, S. (1999). The psychology of
forgiveness and related concepts. In E. L. Worthington unforgiveness and forgiveness and implications for
(Ed.), Dimensions of forgiveness: Psychological clinical practice. Journal of Social and Clinical
research and theological perspectives (pp. 193–317). Psychology, 18, 385–418.
Philadelphia: Templeton Press. Worthington, E. L. (1998). An empathy-humility-commit-
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., ment model of forgiveness applied within family
Worthington Jr., E. L., Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. dyads. Journal of Family Therapy, 20, 59–76.
(1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships: Worthington, E. L., Sandage, S. J., & Berry, J. W. (2000).
II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement. Journal Group interventions to promote forgiveness. In M. E.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(6), 1586– McCullough, K. Pargament, & C. Thoreson (Eds.),
1603. Forgiveness: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 228–
McCullough, M. E., Worthington Jr., E. L., & Rachal, K. 253). New York: Guilford Press.
C. (1997). Interpersonal forgiving in close relation- Zucker, G. S., & Weiner, B. (1993). Conservatism and
ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, perceptions of poverty: An attributional analysis.
73(2), 321–336. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23(12), 925–943.

You might also like