Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

GENERAL ARTICLES

The Amsterdam Preoperative Anxiety and Information


Scale (APAIS)
Nelly Moerman, MD*, Frits S. A. M. van Dam, rhq§, Martin J. Muller, MA& and
Hans Oosting, PhDt
* Department of Anesthesiology and t Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Academic Medical Centre,
University of Amsterdam; $ Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Amsterdam; and
Downloaded from https://journals.lww.com/anesthesia-analgesia by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3tjcLwhL8g9aoO2j1rfYWHbEn+E/g5+TC/R5DcUhib+Y= on 04/17/2018

+j Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

The purpose of the present study was to assess patients’ emerged that 32% of the patients could be considered as
anxiety level and information requirement in the pre- “anxiety cases” and over 80% of patients have a positive
operative phase. During routine preoperative screen- attitude toward receiving information. Moreover, the re-
ing, 320 patients were asked to assess their anxiety and sults demonstrated that 1) women were more anxious
information requirement on a six-item questionnaire, than men; 2) patients with a high information requirement
the Amsterdam Preoperative Anxiety and Information also had a high level of anxiety; 3) patients who had never
Scale (APAIS). Two hundred patients also completed undergone an operation had a higher information re-
Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI- quirement than those who had. The APAIS can provide
State). Patients were able to complete the questionnaire anesthesiologists with a valid, reliable, and easily applica-
in less than 2 min. On factor analysis, two factors emerged ble instrument for assessing the level of patients’ preoper-
clearly: anxiety and the need for information. The anxiety ative anxiety and their need for information.
scale correlated highly (0.74) with the STAIState. It (Anesth Analg 1996;82:445-51)

A
nxious patients respond differently than non- Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (13), which has been trans-
anxious patients to anesthesia. The insertion of lated into Dutch and validated by van der Ploeg et al.
an intravenous catheter in the preoperative (14). He also obtained norms for the Dutch population.
phase can be a difficult task as a result of anxiety- The questionnaire consists of two separate, 20-item,
related vasoconstriction (1,2). In anxious patients, self-report rating scales for measuring trait and state
larger doses of anesthetics are required to induce an- anxiety. The trait anxiety is a relatively stable person-
esthesia (3,4) and the anesthesia itself may be associ- ality disposition, while state anxiety is the situation-
ated with autonomic fluctuations (5,6). Although the related anxiety and this may differ depending on the
exact percentage of patients who are anxious preop- stress of the particular moment. The state scale is
eratively is not known, the literature suggests an inci- recommended for measuring patient anxiety in the
dence between 60% and 80% (7-10). Although a sed- preoperative phase (15) and has been used in several
ative drug is often given as premeditation to relieve anesthesiologic studies (16-19). Although this ques-
tionnaire is fairly short, it is still too long for use in
fear and anxiety, anxious patients might also benefit
busy outpatient clinics. Moreover, the questions are
from more attention and information from the anes-
not related to the specific situation with which the
thesiologist. In clinical practice, however, the anesthe-
patient is confronted.
siologist has very little time for preoperative consul-
A second aspect of preoperative care is the patient’s
tation to identify the patients who are anxious and need for information. Several studies (20-22) have
may benefit from extra attention. shown that information given to patients before sur-
There are many instruments for measuring the pa- gery may facilitate recovery. However, some patients
tient’s level of preoperative anxiety (11,12). The instru- like to shut themselves off from information, whereas
ment most commonly used is Spielberger’s State-Trait others want to be informed as fully as possible (23).
These different coping styles are almost never hon-
ored, as it is practically impossible for the anesthesi-
Accepted for publication September 15, 1995. ologist to discriminate between patients who would
Address correspondence to Nelly Moerman, MD, Department of
Anesthesiology, Academic Medical Centre, University of Amster- like to be informed as fully as possible from those who
dam, PO Box 22700,llOO DE Amsterdam, The Netherlands. want to know as little as possible. It would be greatly

01996 by the International Anesthesia Research Society


0003-2999/96/$5.00 Anesth Analg 1996;82:445-51 445
446 MOERMAN ET AL. ANESTH ANALG
THE AMSTERDAM PREOPERATIVE ANXIETY AND INFORMATION SCALE (APAIS) 1996;82:445-51

beneficial to clinical practice if anesthesiologists knew Table 1. The Amsterdam Preoperative Anxiety and
whether they were dealing with a patient who wanted Information Scale (APAIS)
more than basic information which is routinely given, 1. I am worried about the anesthetic.
or a patient who would rather not be given any extra 2. The anesthetic is on my mind continually.
information. 3. I would like to know as much as possible about
As we could not find a clinically applicable instru- the anesthetic.
ment in the literature which fulfilled all our require- 4. I am worried about the procedure.
5. The procedure is on my mind continually.
ments, i.e., short, specifically attuned to the preoper-
6. I would like to know as much as possible about
ative situation, and easy to interpret, we decided to the procedure.
develop a new instrument. Our point of reference was
The measure of agreement with these statements should be graded on a
the work of Miller and Mangan (24,25), who studied five-point Likert scale from 1 = not at all to 5 = extremely.
the way patients cope with the stress of a threatening
situation. They differentiated between “monitors” and (laryngectomy, reconstructive and transplantation
“blunters,” defining monitors as people who want to surgery).
know as much as possible and search actively for
information and blunters as those who have no need Statistical Analysis
for information and even try to avoid it. This instru-
Validity. To evaluate the validity of the APAIS (Ta-
ment should make it possible to distinguish anxious
ble 11, we performed several analyses. Attention was
from nonanxious patients and patients who want in-
devoted to some aspects of construct (content) validity
formation from those who do not.
and criterion validity, too.
Construct validity was evaluated by factor analysis.
Methods Factor analysis is a statistical approach to reduce data
A six-item questionnaire, the Amsterdam Preopera- by determining the relationships among variables and
tive Anxiety and Information Scale (APAIS) (Table 1) to determine the underlying structure which is formed
was developed in a previous studyi, covering both the by latent variables known as factors. The relation be-
tween variables and a certain factor is given by the
“monitor” and “blunting” aspects. Four items repre-
sented fear of anesthesia and fear of the surgical pro- so-called factor loadings, which indicate how much
cedure (Cronbach’s cx0.86). Two items represented the weight is assigned to each factor. Variables with high
loadings for a factor are closely related to that partic-
need for information (Cronbach’s (Y0.72). The internal
consistencies of both scales were sufficient for group ular factor. Rotation is the procedure used to make the
factor solution more interpretable (26). The results of
comparison.
the factor analysis should reflect the concepts we put
During a period of 3 mo, 320 consecutive patients
visiting the anesthesiology outpatient department (pa- into our scales and should thus concur with the results
tients who could not speak Dutch were excluded) of our previous study, i.e., two factors should emerge:
were asked by the nursing staff to fill out this ques- anxiety and a need for information.
tionnaire (Table 1). We noted the age and sex of the As a measure of concurrent validity we determined
patients and whether they had had surgery previ- the correlation of the APAIS with the STAI. We hy-
ously. To ascertain that our instrument really meas- pothesized that the State version of the STAI should
ured anxiety, the last 200 patients were also asked to correlate highly (>0.60) with the anxiety scale of the
fill out the State version of Spielberger’s STAI. This APAIS and should have a low correlation (<0.30) with
questionnaire consists of a 20-item self-report rating the need-for-information scale.
For clinical use it is important to be able to identify
scale for measuring state anxiety. In the latter group
we also examined the kind of procedure involved and those patients who can be considered as “anxiety cas-
es.” For this purpose we used Spielberger’s STAI as
the duration of the operation. We classified the oper-
ations as minor, intermediate, or major. “Minor” was the “gold standard.” Auerbach (27) divided a group of
defined as less invasive surgery of limited duration surgical patients on the basis of their preoperative
(minor orthopedic surgery, diagnostic procedures, ar- score on Spielberger’s trait anxiety scale into a high-
trait-anxiety group and a low-trait-anxiety group. The
throscopies, laparoscopies, inguinal hernia). Opera-
tions classified as “intermediate” had more impact for mean state anxiety score of the high-trait-anxiety
the patient (cholecystectomy, hysterectomy), and “ma- group was 46. We used this score on the state scale as
jor” were extensive operations with a high impact a reference point and considered patients with a score
~46 on the STAI-State as anxiety cases. Furthermore,
this point concurs 2 the 9th decile of a Dutch male
1 Moerman N, Dam van F, Boulogne-Abraham T, Hooff van M. reference group and 2 the 8th decile of a Dutch female
The patient’s need for information in the preoperative period. Pro-
ceedings of the 9th European Congress of Anaesthesiology. Jerusa- reference group (14). We determined for different cut-
lem, Israel, 1994:257. off points on the APAIS anxiety scale the sensitivity
ANESTH ANALG MOERMAN ET AL. 447
1996;82:445-51 THE AMSTERDAM PREOPERATIVE ANXIETY AND INFORMATION SCALE (APAIS)

(proportion of correctly identified cases), the specific- Table 2. Patient Characteristicsby Whole Group and
ity (proportion of correctly identified noncases), and Subgroup
the positive predictive value (probability of a high APAIS APAIS + STAI
scale score being a case) in relation to the STAI. (n = 320) (n = 200)
The validity was further evaluated by known-group Sex
comparison in three different ways. 1) We hypothe- Male 121 (37.8%) 85 (42.5%)
sized that women should have a higher score than Female 199 (62.2%) 115 (57.5%)
men on the APAIS anxiety scale. This hypothesis was Mean k SD a e 38.3 IL 13.6 (18-87) 38.8 2 13.9 (18-87)
(yr) (range $
based on data from the literature, where women are
Previous surgery
usually regarded as being more anxious than men Yes 242 (75.6%) 156 (78%)
(7,10,14,19,28). 2) From the work of Miller and Man- No 78 (24.4%) 44 (22%)
gan (24,25) it is known, that high monitors are also Kind of
operation”
anxious people. In other words, in a threatening situ-
Minor 145 (72.5%)
ation monitoring is mostly associated with higher anx- Intermediate 42 (21.0%)
iety and arousal than blunting (29). We therefore hy- Major 7 (3.5%)
pothesized that, in our instrument, patients with a AI’AIS = Amsterdam Preoperative Anxiety and Information Scale; STAI
high information requirement should have a higher = Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
score on the anxiety scale than patients with a low a Operations were classified for the last 200 patients only; 6 patients were
not operated on.
information requirement. 3) The effect of preanesthetic
information is less valuable for patients who have
previous anesthetic experience than for those who do Table 3. Factor Loadings in a Two-Factor Solution (After
not (30). We therefore hypothesized that patients with Oblique Rotation)
previous experience of anesthesia and surgery should Factor
have a lower information requirement than those who
1 2
had never had surgery. No specific hypothesis was
formulated regarding the difference between men and Anesthesia
women with respect to their information requirement. 1. Worried about 0.83 0.03
Data were analyzed using the SPSS version 4.0. An 2. Thinks about it continually 0.86 -0.04
analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used for group 3. Wishes to know as much as 0.01 0.87
possible
comparison. Student’s t-tests were used to compare Surgery
the mean scale scores for the subgroups at baseline. 4. Worried about 0.81 0.03
Statistical significance was considered at P < 0.05. 5. Thinks about it continually 0.85 -0.02
Reliability. Cronbach’s os were calculated as a meas- 6. Wishes to know as much as -0.01 0.87
ure for internal consistency of the scales. Reliability possible
was considered acceptable when Cronbach’s (YS were Eigenvalue 3.07 1.25
Percent of variance 51.1 20.8
270 (26).

Results Concurrent validity was determined by the correla-


tion with the STAI. The correlation between the anxi-
Of the 322 patients who were asked to participate, 2 ety items of the APAIS and the STAI-State was high
patients refused. Patient characteristics are presented (0.74) and the correlation between the information
in Table 2. Patients had no problem completing the items and the STAI-State was low (0.16).
APAIS and usually did so in less than 2 min. As was
predicted, we found in a factor analysis with oblique Anxiety Scale
rotation (see Table 3) two factors, which explained
72% of the variance: anxiety (questions 1, 2, 4, and 5, The anxiety scale consists of four items (questions 1,2,
Table 1) and the need for information (questions 3 and 4, 5), each of which could be scored from 1 to 5. The
6, Table 1). The correlation between both factors was score of the anxiety scale is the sum of these four
0.31. The following step was to convert the two factors questions, with a scoring range from 4 to 20. There
to scales and calculate Cronbach’s (Yfor the two scales was a highly significant difference (P = <O.OOl) be-
separately. Cronbach’s (Y for the four anxiety items tween men and women. The mean score of men was
(questions 1, 2, 4, 5) was 0.86. Cronbach’s cx for the 7.5 (SD 3.5) and the mean score of women was 9.9
need-for-information items (questions 3 and 6) was (SD 4.5). But an ANOVA indicated an interaction effect
somewhat lower (0.68), as was to be expected with a between previous experience of surgery and gender
scale consisting of only two items, but still sufficient (P = 0.02); t-test for differences between means
for group comparisons. showed that men who had been operated on before
448 MOERMAN ET AL. ANESTH ANALG
THE AMSTERDAM PREOPERATIVE ANXIETY AND INFORMATION SCALE (AI’AIS) 1996;82:445-51

Table 4. The Scores on the Anxiety Scale (Questions 1, 2, 4, 5) for Male and Female Related to Experience of Previous
Surgery (n = 320)
Male* Femalet
Previous surgery Mean” SD II Meana SD n P
Yes 6.8 2.9 98 9.7 4.4 144 <O.OOl
No 10.4 4.5 23 10.6 4.5 55 0.91
a t-test for differences between means.
*I' 5 0.001; tP = 0.23.

had a lower score on the anxiety scale than those Table 5. The Relationship Between the Score on the
without previous experience of surgery. In women Information Scale (Questions 3 and 6) and the Score on
there was no such difference between those who had the Anxiety Scale (Questions 1, 2, 4, 5) (n = 320)
previous experience of surgery and those who had not Score,
(Table 4). anxiety scale
There were no statistically significant relationships
between age, type of operation, and the scores on the Score, information scale n Mean SD

anxiety scale. As the number of patients who under- 24, no/little information requirement 54 7.1 3.6
went major surgery was low, the results regarding the 5-7, average information requirement 127 8.4 3.5
type of operation has to be interpreted with caution. 8-10, high information requirement 139 10.3” 4.8
“Significant difference with both other groups (Tukey HSD procedure
Need-for-Information Scale [P < 0.051).

The need-for-information scale consists of two items


(questions 3 and 6), each of which could be scored what score on the APAIS anxiety scale patients could be
from 1 to 5. The sum of the need-for-information scale considered anxious and would therefore benefit from
is the sum of these two questions, with a scoring range more attention. As mentioned in Methods, we used the
from 2 to 10. ANOVA indicated no interaction effect STAI-State as a “gold standard” and chose the score of 46
between type of operation and gender; only a statisti- as a reference point. Using this reference point, the sen-
cally significant main effect for previous experience of sitivity, specificity, and the predictive value were calcu-
surgery existed (P = 0.002). Patients with previous lated at different cutoff points on the anxiety scale
experience had a lower score (mean 6.6, SD 2.3) on the (APAIS). Table 6 shows that the cutoff point of 11 leads
information scale, than those who had not been oper- to a good balance. Sensitivity and specificity are good
ated on before (mean 7.5, SD 2.2). There were no sta- and the predictive value is 71%. At the score of 11, 37
tistically significant relationships between age, type of patients are misclassified (18 false-positives and 19 false-
operation, and the scores on the information scale. negatives). This means that 9% (n = 18) of the patients
In order to investigate whether there was a relation- are anxious on the anxiety scale (APAIS) but not on the
ship between the patient’s need for information and STAI-State, and 9.5% (n = 19) are not anxious on the
the level of anxiety, we divided patients according to APAIS although they are on the STAI-State. At the cutoff
their score on the information scale into three groups. score of 10 sensitivity increasesbut, becauseof the lower
Patients with a score of 2-4 on the information scale specificity, the predictive value is lower, resulting in a
can be classified as having no or little information higher number of false-positive patients (anxious on the
requirement and can be considered as blunters. Pa- APAIS but not on the STAI) than at the score of 11. At the
tients with a score of 5-7 can be classified as having an scores> 11 sensitivity decreasesand specificity increases.
average information requirement, and those with a The cutoff scores11-13 produce approximately the same
score of 8-10 as having a high information require- amount of misclassified patients (false-positives and
ment. The latter can therefore be considered “moni- false-negatives together), varying from 34 to 39. How-
tors.” Using this three-group classification for the in- ever, an increasing predictive value reduces the number
formation scale, it turned out that in the population of of false-positive patients.
320 patients the percentages of patients with low, me- Using the score of 46 on the STAI-State as a refer-
dium, and high information requirements were 16.9%, ence point, the prevalence of anxiety cases in the pop-
39.7%, and 43.4%, respectively. After correction for sex ulation (n = 200) was 32%.
and experience with previous operations, patients
with a high information requirement turned out to be Discussion
the ones who were most anxious (Table 5).
We also investigated whether we could use the APAIS The purpose of the study was to develop a screening
for detecting “anxiety cases.” We wanted to know at instrument for use in the preoperative period. For this
ANESTH ANALG MOERMAN ET AL. 449
1996;82:445-51 THE AMSTERDAM PREOPERATIVE ANXIETY AND INFORMATION SCALE (APAIS)

Table 6. Characteristicsof the Anxiety Scale(APAIS) at Different Cutoff Points with a Score of 46 on the STAI-State as a
ReferencePoint (n = 200)
Cutoff scoreon the anxiety scale
10 11 12 13
Sensitivity 75.0% 70.3% 59.4% 53.1%
Specificity 78.7% 86.8% 90.4% 97.1%
Positive predictive value 62.3% 71.4% 74.5% 89.5%
Patients, n (o/o)
a. True-positive 48 (24) 45 (22.5) 38 (19) 34 (17)
b. False-positive 29 (14.5) 18 (9) 13 (6.5) 4 (2)
c. False-negative 16 (8) 19 (9.5) 26 (13) 30 (15)
d. True-negative 107(53.5) 118(59) 123(61.5) 132 (66)
APAIS = Amsterdam Preoperative Anxiety and Information Scale; STAI = Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.

reason, a six-item questionnaire was developed: the be answered. Because the APAIS is specifically at-
Amsterdam Preoperative Anxiety and Information tuned to the preoperative situation, patients can com-
Scale (APAIS). The APAIS was easily and very quickly plete it without further explanation.
completed by patients. Two clear factors emerged: The APAIS can be used for clinical practice and for
anxiety and information requirements. The anxiety research purposes. The scores on the anxiety scale of
scale correlated highly with the standard question- the APAIS range from 4 (not anxious) to 20 (highly
naire for measuring anxiety: Spielberger’s STAI-State anxious). The cutoff points chosen depend on the pur-
(0.74). Both the anxiety and the need-for-information pose for which the scale is to be used, i.e., clinical use
scale showed good psychometric properties and were or research purpose. Based on a comparison with the
feasible in clinical practice. According to the literature STAI as a gold standard, it is clear from the results that
we found that 1) women have a higher score on the for clinical practice the cutoff score of 11 produces a
anxiety items than men, and 2) there is a positive good predictive value with an acceptable balance be-
relationship between anxiety and information require- tween false-positive and false-negative patients. So
ment. Patients with a greater need for information far, the score of 11 seems a useful and efficient score
were patients with a higher anxiety level than those for identifying anxious patients in clinical practice. A
with a low information demand. 3) Patients without score of 10 would result in a lower predictive value
previous experience of surgery had a higher informa- and a higher number of false-positive patients (anx-
tion requirement than those who had been operated ious on the APAIS but not on the STAI) than the score
on before. of 11 (14.5% vs 9%). Whether the anesthesiologist will
As already mentioned, there was a difference in accept a score of 10 as an indication for anxiety cases
anxiety levels between men and women. Women and accept a relatively high number of false-positive
scored higher on the anxiety scale than men. It was, patients, or prefers a score of 11 with a relatively low
however, striking that men who had not been oper- number of false-positive patients depends on the
ated on before were just as anxious as women. How- amount of time the anesthesiologist wants to devote to
ever, men who had undergone previous surgery a patient’s preoperative stress and anxiety. With
scored lower on the anxiety scale than men who had scores higher than 11 the predictive value increases
not. In other words men who have been operated on but because of the higher percentage of specificity the
before cope differently with their fear of anesthesia number of false-negative patients (not anxious on the
and surgery than women. This finding warrants fur- APAIS, but anxious on the STAI) also increases. For
ther research. the purposes of clinical practice, it is important to
In contrast to our expectations, it emerged that the identify the patients who are anxious, and a high
questionnaire did not distinguish well between fear of number of false-negative patients is not acceptable. On
anesthesia and fear related to surgery, which means the basis of these results, we recommend for the pur-
that feelings of anxiety in the preoperative period are poses of clinical practice that patients with a score of
diffuse and are not really focused on either surgery or 211 on the anxiety scale should be considered as
anesthesia. In this respect, the STAI-State should be a anxiety cases.Future research should be conducted to
good method for measuring preoperative anxiety, as clarify whether it is useful to distinguish between
noted by Spielberger et al. (15). A great advantage of anxiety casesand nonanxiety cases.
our questionnaire, however, is that it is much shorter. When the list is used for research purposes, the
The anxiety scale of the APAIS consists of only four number of false-positive patients is more important.
questions, while in the STAI-State 20 questions must The score of 11 produces 9% false-positives, that is to
450 MOERMAN ET AL. ANESTH ANALG
THE AMSTERDAM PREOPERATIVE ANXIETY AND INFORMATION SCALE (APAIS) 1996;82:445-51

say, 18 patients have a high score on the APAIS, but Academic Medical Centre for their assistance, Marjolein Porsius,
not on the STAI. With a score of 13 the number of research nurse, for collecting the data about the operations, and
Marion Alhadeff for her expertise and support as a translator.
false-positives decreases to 4. If the list is used for
research purposes, where anxiety reduction is an im-
portant outcome criterion, we recommend a score of
13. At the score of 13 there are hardly any false-
positives (2%) and thus the database is less polluted. References
The scores on the information scale of the APAIS 1. Thyer BA, Papsdorf JD, Davis R, Vallecorsa S. Autonomic cor-
range from 2 (no need for information) to 10 (high relates of the subjective anxiety scale. J Behav Ther Exp Psychi-
need for information). The results of our study show atry 1984;15:3-7.
2. Wallin BG. Neural control of human skin blood flow. J Auton
that over 80% of patients have a positive attitude Nerv Syst 1990;30:5185-90.
toward receiving information (score 2 5). This figure 3. Williams JGL, Jones JR. Psychophysiological responses to anes-
concurs with data from other countries (31,32). Given thesia and operation. JAMA 1968;203:127-9.
the implications for daily medical practice, this is an 4. Goldmann L, Ogg TW, Levey AB. Hypnosis and daycase
anaesthesia: a study to reduce pre-operative anxiety and intra-
important point of which every anesthesiologist
operative anaesthetic requirements. Anaesthesia 1988;43:466-9.
should be aware. It also emerged that the patients 5. Tolksdorf W, Schmollinger U, Berlin J, Rey ER. Das praopera-
with an extremely high information requirement tive psychische Befinden-Zusammenhlnge mit anasthesiere-
(score 2 8) are anxious patients. The relationship be- levanten psychophysiologischen Parametern (The preoperative
tween anxiety and information requirement has al- psychological state and its correlation to psychophysical para-
meters important to anaesthesia). Anasth Intensivther Not-
ready been underlined by Janis (33), who was the first fallmed (Stuttgart) 1983;18:81-7.
to conduct systematic research on preoperative anxi- 6. Tolksdorf W, Berlin J, Rey ER, et al. Der praoperative StreB:
ety. It is important to realize that anxious patients Untersuchung zum Verhalten psychischer und physiologischer
might derive great benefit from more attention and Stregparameter nichtpramedizierter Patienten in der praopera-
tiven Phase (Preoperative stress: investigation of psychological
information. However, extensive information is not
and physiological stress parameters in unpremeditated pa-
always useful and may even induce anxiety (34). Par- tients). Anaesthesist 1984;33:212-7.
ticularly patients with a “blunting” coping style may 7. Norris W, Baird WLM. Preoperative anxiety: a study of the
become anxious when confronted with extensive in- incidence and aetiology. Br J Anaesth 1967;39:503-9.
8. Ramsay MAE. A survey of pre-operative fear. Anaesthesia 1972;
formation. By contrast, patients with a monitoring
27396-402.
coping style become anxious when they are not pro- 9. Mackenzie JW. Daycase anaesthesia and anxiety: a study of
vided with as much information as they want (24,35). anxiety profiles amongst patients attending a day bed unit.
In our population almost 17% of the patients had a Anaesthesia 1989;44:437-40.
negative or uninterested attitude toward information 10. Shevde K, Panagopoulos G. A survey of 800 patients’ knowl-
edge, attitudes and concerns regarding anesthesia. Anesth
(score I 4). The law requires that patients be given
Analg 1991;73:190-8.
information but, as mentioned above, it is important 11. Johnston M. Pre-operative emotional state and post-operative
to realize that not everyone wants to be fully in- recovery. Adv Psychosom Med 1986;15:1-22.
formed. We therefore advise that patients with a score 12. Millar K, Jelecic M, Bonke B, Asbury AJ. Assessment of preop-
erative anxiety: comparison of measures in patients awaiting
of 5 and higher should be given information on the
surgery for breast cancer. Br J Anaesth 1995;75:180-3.
topics about which they wish to be informed and in 13. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. State trait anxiety
accordance to their score. A score below 5 should be a inventory manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
signal for providing no more information than is le- Press, 1970.
gally required. 14. Ploeg HM van der, Defares PB, Spielberger CD. Handleiding bij
zelfbeoordelingsvragenlijst STAI-DY. Lisse-Amsterdam: Swets
Consideration of patient’s preoperative fears and & Zeitlinger, 1980.
anxieties is of paramount importance in the quality of 15. Spielberger CD, Auerbach SM, Wadworth AI’, et al. Emotional
anesthesiologic care. However, devoting attention to a reactions to surgery. J Consult Clin Psycho1 1973;40:33-8.
patient’s fear takes time, and time is in short supply. 16. Lanz E, Schafter M, Briinisholz V. Midazolam zur oralen Pra-
medikation vor Regional-Anaesthesie (Oral premeditation with
Fortunately, not all patients are equally anxious and in
midazolam for local anesthesia). Anaesthesist 1987;36:197-202.
need of additional support. We have developed a 17. Antrobus JHL. Anxiety and informed consent: does anxiety
simple screening instrument which, if used during influence consent for inclusion in a study of anxiolytic premed-
preoperative assessment, may facilitate the identifica- ication? Anaesthesia 1988;43:267-9.
tion of those patients who are in need of extra support. 18. Arellano R, Cruise C, Chung F. Timing of the anesthetist’s
preoperative outpatient interview. Anesth Analg 1989;68:645-8.
The extent to which the APAIS will be useful in clin- 19. Badner NH, Nielson WR, Munk S, et al. Preoperative anxiety:
ical practice has to be verified in future research. detection and contributing factors. Can J Anaesth 1990;37:444-7.
20. Suls J, Wan CK. Effects of sensory and procedural information
on coping with stressful medical procedures and pain: a meta-
The authors wish to thank Benno Bonke, PhD, and Kommer analysis. J Consult Clin Psycho1 1989;57:372-9.
Sneeuw, MA, for their valuable and constructive comments, the 21. Webber GC. Patient education: a review of the issues. Med Care
nursing staff of the anesthesiologic outpatient department of the 1990;28:1089-1103.
ANESTH ANALG MOERMAN ET AL. 451
1996;82:445-51 THE AMSTERDAM PREOPERATIVE ANXIETY AND INFORMATION SCALE (APAW

22. Devine EC. Effects of psychoeducational care for adult surgical 29. Murris P. Monitoring and blunting: coping styles and strategies
patients: a meta-analysis of 191 studies. Patient Educ Couns in threatening situations [thesis]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands:
1992;19:129-42. Univ. of Amsterdam, 1994.
23. Miller SM, Combs C, Stoddard E. Information, coping and 30. Elsass I’, Eikard 8, Junge J, et al. Psychological effect of detailed
control in patients undergoing surgery and stressful medical preanesthetic information. Acta Anaesthesiol Stand 1987;31:
procedures. In: Steptoe A, Appels A, eds. Stress, personal con- 579-83.
trol and health. New York: Wiley, 1989;107-30. 31. Londsdale M, Hutchison GL. Patients’ desire for information
24. Miller SM, Mangan CE. Interacting effects of information and about anaesthesia: Scottish and Canadian attitudes. Anaesthesia
coping style in adapting to gynecologic stress: should the doctor 1991;46:410-2.
tell all? J Pers Sot Psycho1 1983;45:223-36. 32. Farnill D, Inglis S. Patients’ desire for information about
25. Miller SM. Monitoring and blunting: validation of a question- anaesthesia: Australian attitudes. Anaesthesia 1993;48:162-4.
naire to assess styles of information seeking under threat. J Pers 33. Janis IL. Psychological stress: psychoanalytic and behavioral
Sot Psycho1 1987;52:345-53. studies of surgical patients. New York: Wiley, 1958.
26. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. 3rd ed. New 34. Lankton JW, Batchelder BM, Ominsky AJ. Emotional responses
York: McGraw-Hill, 1994. to detailed risk disclosure for anesthesia: a prospective random-
27. Auerbach SM. Trait-state anxiety and adjustment to surgery. ized study. Anesthesiology 1977;46:294-6.
J Consult Clin Psycho1 1973;40:264-71. 35. Watkins LO, Weaver L, Odegaard V. Preparation for cardiac
28. Johnston M. Anxiety in surgical patients. Psycho1 Med 198O;lO: catheterization: tailoring the content of instruction to coping
14552. style. Heart Lung 1986;15:382-9.

You might also like