Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, JUNE 2016,

VOL. 0, NO. 0, 1–12

Research Report
Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with
SLI and their typically developing peers
Safiyyah Abdul Aziz†, Janet Fletcher† and Donna M. Bayliss‡
†School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
‡Neurocognitive Development Unit, School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
(Received December 2015; accepted June 2016)

Abstract
Background: Past research with children with specific language impairment (SLI) has shown them to have poorer
planning and problem-solving ability, and delayed self-regulatory speech (SRS) relative to their typically developing
(TD) peers. However, the studies are few in number and are restricted in terms of the number and age range
of participants, which limits our understanding of the nature and extent of any delays. Moreover, no study has
examined the performance of a significant subset of children with SLI, those who have hyperactive and inattentive
behaviours.
Aims: This cross-sectional study aimed to compare the performance of young children with SLI (aged 4–7 years)
with that of their TD peers on a planning and problem-solving task and to examine the use of SRS while performing
the task. Within each language group, the performance of children with and without hyperactive and inattentive
behaviours was further examined.
Methods & Procedures: Children with SLI (n = 91) and TD children (n = 81), with and without hyperactive and
inattentive behaviours across the three earliest school years (Kindergarten, Preprimary and Year 1) were video-taped
while they completed the Tower of London (TOL), a planning and problem-solving task. Their recorded speech
was coded and analysed to look at differences in SRS and its relation to TOL performance across the groups.
Main Contribution: Children with SLI scored lower on the TOL than TD children. Additionally, children
with hyperactive and inattentive behaviours performed worse than those without hyperactive and inattentive
behaviours, but only in the SLI group. This suggests that children with SLI with hyperactive and inattentive
behaviours experience a double deficit. Children with SLI produced less inaudible muttering than TD children,
and showed no reduction in social speech across the first three years of school. Finally, for children with SLI, a
higher percentage performed better on the TOL when they used SRS than when they did not.
Conclusions & Implications: The results point towards a significant delay in the development and internalization of
SRS in the SLI group, which should be taken into account when considering the planning and problem-solving
of young children with SLI.

Keywords: self-regulation, self-talk, private speech, specific language impairment (SLI), planning, hyperactive and
inattentive behaviours.

What this paper adds


It is known that children with planning and problem-solving. Deficits in planning have been attributed to poor
SRS. The limited research available suggests that children with SLI may have delayed SRS, however, the nature and
extent of this delay is unknown. This study compares the SRS and planning and problem-solving performance of
91 young children with SLI with that of 81 TD children. Results showed that children with SLI have delays in
planning and problem-solving that are evident from a young age, and delayed SRS in comparison with TD peers. In
addition, a subsample of children with SLI with hyperactive and inattentive behaviours showed a double deficit, that
is, they performed worse than children with SLI without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours on the planning and
problem-solving task.

Address correspondence to: Safiyyah Abdul Aziz, School of Psychology (M304), The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA 6009,
Australia; e-mail: safiyyah.abdulaziz@uwa.edu.au
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  C 2016 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists

DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12273
2 Safiyyah Abdul Aziz et al.
Introduction when the children were 9 years old and then declining
with age. In addition to age-related changes, microge-
Vygotsky theorized that language, especially ‘speech for
netic studies have shown that the amount of private
the self’, is essential for basic mental operations and
speech tends to increase linearly as the task gets more
executive functions including self-regulation, planning
challenging and then to reduce and be ineffective when
and problem-solving (Vygotsky 1987). Since then, re-
the task gets too difficult (Duncan and Pratt 1997).
searchers have expanded on Vygotsky’s work, examin-
Research linking language and cognition has lent
ing the importance of self-regulatory speech (SRS) for
support to the notion that language is paramount dur-
cognition and behaviour, with SRS typically defined in
ing cognitively demanding tasks. Positive associations
these recent studies as private speech (Winsler et al.
have been found between children’s use of SRS and per-
1997) or self-directed speech (Lidstone et al. 2012). For
formance on a variety of cognitive tasks such as theory of
the purpose of this paper, we will use the term ‘self-
mind (Fernyhough and Meins 2009), spatial problem-
regulatory speech’ (SRS) more broadly to refer to all
solving (Sturn and Johnston 1999), visuo-spatial pro-
speech SLI have delays in executive functioning, includ-
cessing and executive function tasks (Marton 2008).
ing that is used for the benefit of one’s self. This includes
More specifically, the importance of language for plan-
overt private and social speech that has a self-regulatory
ning and problem-solving has been demonstrated using
function during planning and problem-solving, as well
experimental studies. Research using articulatory sup-
as more covert inaudible muttering. While many re-
pression tasks, where SRS is prevented by asking partici-
searchers have examined SRS in typically developing
pants to repeat a word (e.g., Monday, Monday, Monday)
(TD) children and children with ADHD, there is lim-
while performing a task, has been shown to impair per-
ited research on its use by young children with specific
formance on several planning and problem-solving tasks
language impairment (SLI) compared with TD chil-
including the Tower of London (ToL) (Lidstone et al.
dren. Given that children with SLI have a language im-
2010) the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Baldo et al.
pairment and are often delayed on many cognitive and
2005) and Raven Progressive Matrices (Kim 2002). In
executive measures (e.g., Henry et al. 2012), an investi-
contrast, the concomitant performance of other tasks
gation of SRS in this population is particularly relevant
that do not involve language, such as foot tapping, does
and forms the basis of this study.
not have this effect (Lidstone et al. 2010), providing
Research investigating the development of SRS has
strong evidence that language is important for plan-
shown that preschool children often talk overtly through
ning and problem-solving. In sum, when language use is
their activities to regulate their thoughts and behaviours
prevented, cognitive performance during planning and
(Winsler et al. 2007). They use their language, both
self-regulation is negatively affected.
speech directed to others and to themselves (social and
The importance of language for successful planning
private speech respectively), to plan and problem solve
and problem-solving raises an important question as to
by talking through the required steps to keep themselves
what happens in children who have either delayed or
on track (Sturn and Johnston 1999). Vygotsky (1987)
disordered language development, such as children di-
theorized that speech-for-self has a curvilinear develop-
agnosed with SLI. SLI is used to describe delayed or dis-
mental trajectory. In the beginning, SRS involves both
ordered language acquisition and development in chil-
private and social speech. As children develop, their use
dren with at least average nonverbal intelligence, normal
of overt private speech increases up to the point where
hearing, and no neurological impairment (Bishop and
speech starts to become internalized, which is initially
Baird 2001). These children typically have deficits in
evident as inaudible muttering before speech becomes
expressive language, making it difficult for them to use
completely internal to form silent verbal thought. This
SRS. In addition, children with SLI often have a re-
trajectory has been supported in more recent develop-
ceptive language impairment (Bishop and Baird 2001)
mental studies with TD children (e.g., Berk and Spuhl
that may limit their comprehension of, interaction with,
1995; Winsler et al. 1997; Winsler and Naglieri 2003).
and verbal scaffolding by people around them. There-
For example, SRS has been shown to increase linearly
fore, children with SLI may have a double barrier to
with age in very young children (e.g., 2–3-year-olds,
SRS development. For this reason, it is vital to exam-
Furrow 1984; 3.5–5-year-olds, Berk and Spuhl 1995)
ine the development of SRS in children with SLI and
and to decline linearly with age in school-aged chil-
the impact of any delays or deficits on planning and
dren (e.g., 5–17-year-olds; Winsler and Naglieri 2003).
problem-solving performance.
Taken together, these studies show a curvilinear pattern
The current limited research in the area of SRS in
of overt SRS in children peaking at about 4–5 years of
children with SLI points towards a delay in its devel-
age then declining with age. In addition, Winsler and
opment compared with TD children (Lidstone et al.
Naglieri (2003) found that the use of covert whispers
2012; Sturn and Johnston 1999). This delay in the de-
and mutterings showed an inverted U shape, peaking
velopment of SRS may potentially explain, not only
Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with SLI and TD peers 3
the acknowledged deficits in cognition (e.g., Marton This deficit has been partly attributed to having less ef-
2008), but also emotional and psychosocial difficulties fective SRS during planning and problem-solving. Stud-
(e.g., Conti-Ramsden 2008) seen in children with SLI ies have shown that children with ADHD have more
through its impact on problem-solving. For example, overt SRS than TD peers in middle childhood (e.g.,
difficulties in using language for social problem-solving Corkum et al. 2008). Winsler (2009) suggests that this
may contribute to children with SLI misunderstanding is because children with ADHD need to use overt SRS
complex social situations, experiencing difficulties in the to regulate their hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
development and maintenance of intimate relationships, while performing challenging tasks, which necessarily re-
and displaying impairments in social cognition (Conti- duces the amount of planning related speech. Within the
Ramsden 2008). However, research specifically examin- SLI population, there is evidence to show that having hy-
ing SRS in children with SLI is scarce. To the best of peractivity and inattention, in addition to language dif-
our knowledge, there are only two studies examining ficulties, further reduces performance on executive func-
SRS in children with SLI. In the first study, Sturn and tioning tasks, including planning and problem-solving
Johnston (1999) observed a small group of preschoolers (e.g., Marton 2008). Therefore, it is vital to look not
with SLI and matched peers completing a construction only at the SRS of children with SLI in general, but also
task and recorded their overt problem-solving speech, to examine the population of children with SLI and hy-
both private and social. They found that children with peractive and inattentive behaviours more closely, as any
SLI produced less task-relevant problem-solving speech, difficulties they may have with planning and problem-
but also talked less overall, compared with TD children. solving may be intensified relative to children with SLI
Sturn and Johnston (1999) concluded that children with without such behaviours.
SLI are less likely than TD children to use language for
cognitive purposes. Interestingly, children with SLI who
The present study
were more verbose were less efficient on a measure of
cognitive style, the exact opposite of the pattern shown Taken together, the two studies on SRS in children with
by TD children. One potential explanation offered for SLI provide an excellent starting point in understand-
this finding was that some children with SLI do use ver- ing SLI and its effects on SRS and concomitant changes
bal modes of thought, but that this may actually impede in planning. However, there are a few issues that war-
their cognitive efficiency (Sturn and Johnston 1999). rant addressing. First, no studies have examined changes
However, as the authors indicated, the small numbers in SRS across age within the SLI group; Sturn and
in each group (N = 6) and considerable within group Johnston (1999) studied 4–5-year-olds and Lidstone
variance limited their ability to draw strong conclusions. et al. (2012) looked at 7–12-year-old children with SLI
In the second study to address SRS in children with as a single group. An examination of age-related differ-
SLI, Lidstone et al. (2012) assessed the self-directed ences is essential to understand the nature and extent
speech of children with SLI and TD children aged 7–11 of the delay in SRS experienced by children with SLI.
years during planning and problem-solving on the TOL. Thus, the first aim of this study was to examine the
The children completed the TOL tasks under normal planning and problem-solving performance (measured
conditions and under dual task conditions (articulatory by the TOL) and SRS of young children with SLI and
suppression and foot tapping). Lidstone et al. (2012) their TD peers across the first three years of school.
found that children with SLI performed more poorly Specifically, we expected that TD children would per-
than controls on the TOL and had less internalized form better on the TOL and have more mature SRS
and more overt self-directed speech than their age and than children with SLI.
nonverbal intelligence matched TD peers. This points A second issue to address comes from the evidence
toward a delayed developmental pattern of self-directed that both children with SLI and children with hyperac-
speech in children with SLI. tive and inattentive behaviours display poorer executive
Further complicating the study of SRS during plan- functioning. As there is a high proportion of children
ning and problem-solving in children with SLI, is the with SLI displaying these behaviours, it is pertinent to
documented comorbidity of hyperactive and inatten- examine whether any differences in performance can be
tive behaviours in a large number of these children. explained by the hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
Studies have reported that between 25% and 45% of experienced by a number of children with SLI. Thus,
children with SLI display high levels of hyperactive and the second aim of this study was to examine the impact
inattentive behaviours (Marton 2008). This is impor- of both SLI and hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
tant to note because there is converging evidence to on SRS and planning and problem-solving by compar-
suggest that hyperactive and inattentive behaviours are ing the SRS and TOL performance of children with SLI
consistently associated with poor planning and problem- with and without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
solving performance in children (Winsler et al. 2007). to the performance of TD children with and without
4 Safiyyah Abdul Aziz et al.
hyperactive and inattentive behaviours. Based on the average scores on this test. Six children did not meet the
findings from studies with children with ADHD, it was inclusion criteria: three children with SLI (two had lower
expected that children with hyperactive and inattentive than average nonverbal IQ and one could not complete
behaviours would perform worse on the TOL and have the TOL practice trials), and three TD children because
less mature SRS than children without hyperactive and their nonverbal IQ scores were > 125. In addition,
inattentive behaviours. for the SRS analysis, one child with SLI was excluded
A third issue that needs exploration is the link be- from the study due to technical difficulties. Descriptive
tween SRS and planning and problem-solving perfor- data are provided in table 1.
mance. Sturn and Johnston (1999) suggested that chil-
dren with SLI may be less effective in their use of SRS
during planning and problem-solving. However, neither Tasks and procedures
Sturn and Johnston (1999) nor Lidstone et al. (2012) Nonverbal ability
examined the direct relationship between SRS and item
by item performance during the planning task. Conse- A measure of nonverbal IQ was obtained from two
quently, it is important to assess whether children with screening subtests (Matrices and Recognition) from the
SLI are more likely to get an item correct when using WNV (Wechsler and Naglieri 2006). The WNV was
SRS as compared with when they are silent. This will not part of the psychometric testing previously carried
enable us to gauge the usefulness of SRS for children out with children in the LDC.
with SLI during planning and problem-solving. This
analysis will also enable us to see whether children with Language ability
SLI show a similar relationship between SRS and TOL
performance as TD children. Thus, the final aim was to The CCC-2 (Bishop 2003) was completed by parents of
examine the SRS produced on individual items of the participants and used to screen for pragmatic language
TOL and subsequent performance on these items. We impairment and SLI to ensure average communication
expected that using SRS during an item would increase ability in TD children and to confirm below average
the likelihood of getting that item correct. communication ability in children with SLI.

Behaviour rating
Method
The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ;
Participants Goodman 2001) is a 25-item behavioural screening
One hundred and seventy eight 4–7-year-old children questionnaire. The hyperactivity and inattentiveness
(age range: 48–80 months) were recruited from Primary subscale was the measure of interest in this study. High
schools and Language Development Centres (LDCs) in scores (7–10) indicate a substantial risk of clinically sig-
Perth, Western Australia. All children were in Kinder- nificant problems in this area (Goodman 2001) and
garten, Pre-primary or Year 1 at the time of recruitment children with scores in this range were categorized as
and data collection. having hyperactive and inattentive behaviours. How-
In this study, 94 children with SLI were recruited ever, being a screening tool, this does not amount to a
from LDCs in five different suburban areas. LDCs are diagnosis of ADHD.
specialist teaching schools for children in their first three
years of schooling (Kindergarten to Year 1) who have
Planning task
been diagnosed with SLI by speech pathologists and
psychologists, i.e., they have been assessed as having Shallice’s (1982) mechanical TOL test was used to mea-
significantly impaired language development in combi- sure planning and problem-solving abilities. This is an
nation with adequate adaptive behaviours and at least extensively used, easily demonstrable and engaging spa-
average nonverbal intelligence. Eighty-four TD children tial planning task presented in game-form, which can be
were recruited from the same school area as the children administered to children from 4 years of age onwards.
with SLI. Inclusion criteria required children to have av- This is helpful for our population of interest because
erage to high average nonverbal IQ (scores of 85–125) children with SLI, who have receptive language im-
on the Wechsler Non-Verbal Scale of Ability (WNV), pairments, find complex verbal instructions extremely
and to pass the practice items on the TOL. In addition, difficult to comprehend. The mechanical form of the
children with SLI had to score below average on the gen- TOL used a pair of identical wooden game boards, one
eral communication composite (GCC) of the Children’s each for the child and the examiner. The game boards
Communication Checklist—Second Edition (CCC-2), consisted of three pegs: short, medium and tall, which
while the TD children were required to have at least hold one, two or three balls of the same size, respectively.
Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with SLI and TD peers 5
Table 1. Means and standard deviations for age, nonverbal IQ and general language in TD and children with SLI

Group n Age, months (SD) WNV scores (SD) GCC scores (SD)
SLI 91 63.27 (9.18) 103.34 (10.83) 47.72 (11.96)
Male 62 63.54 (9.66) 103.08 (11.59) 47.47 (12.06)
Female 29 61.93 (8.25) 103.28 (10.90) 48.30 (11.92)
K 35 53.35 (2.33) 104.32 (12.01) 51.68 (9.96)
PP 24 64.21 (3.31) 104.08 (11.07) 47.13 (12.05)
Y1 32 73.36 (3.99) 101.06 (10.73) 43.70 (12.86)
Hyp 31 63.53 (9.82) 101.44 (11.93) 46.33 (15.96)
NonHyp 60 62.79 (8.98) 104.03 (10.98) 48.35 (9.74)
TDa 81 65.95 (10.25) 108.52 (9.32) 77.93 (14.55)
Male 48 66.00 (9.94) 108.26 (9.24) 78.17 (14.77)
Female 33 65.88 (10.80) 108.88 (9.55) 77.62 (14.47)
K 25 53.44 (4.01) 107.68 (9.12) 78.76 (12.92)
PP 24 65.67 (4.09) 110.62 (8.92) 80.96 (14.79)
Y1 32 76.26 (3.24) 107.58 (9.81) 74.94 (15.47)
Hyp 31 63.53 (9.82) 101.44 (11.93) 46.33 (15.96)
NonHyp 60 62.79 (8.98) 104.03 (10.98) 48.35 (9.74)
Notes: K = Kindergarten, PP = Preprimary, Y1 = Year 1, Hyp = children with hyperactive and inattentive behaviours, NonHyp = children with no hyperactive and inattentive
behaviour, GCC = General Communications Composite of the CCC-2.
a
Part of the TD children’s data have been published in Aziz et al. (2015), with permission from Springer.

Each ball was either yellow, blue or red. The children the second trial was coded. Timing for each item began
were asked to change the configuration of the balls on when the examiner presented the game boards to the
the pegs of their board to match the ball configuration child and ended when the child completed the item or
on the examiner’s board in the minimum number of when he/she gave up after trying. To reduce distress,
moves. testing was discontinued when a child failed three
In this study, the TOL consisted of four practice consecutive items or completed the last item.
items and 17 structurally unique test problems increas-
ing in complexity from problems that could be solved in
a minimum of three moves up to a minimum of seven General procedure
moves per item. This series was adapted from a comput-
Ethical approval was obtained from the Human Re-
erized TOL version by Berg and colleagues (Berg and
search Ethics Office of The University of Western
Byrd 2002). The TOL followed the procedures used in
Australia (Reference Number: RA/4/1/4738) prior to
a previous study by MacDonald and Berg (2005), in
participant recruitment. After informed consent and
which the examiner concluded the instructions by say-
screening measures, the TOL task was administered in-
ing, ‘Some children like to talk when playing this ball
dividually to each child by an examiner in a quiet area
puzzle game, if you want to talk, it is fine.’ In addition,
in the school during school hours. Two examiners tested
the child was asked to put his/her non-dominant hand
the children; both were young females with a graduate
in a glove and was instructed not to use the hand in
degree in psychology.
the glove to prevent him/her from moving two balls at
Children were audio and video taped for later scoring
one time.
of total number of moves made, total time taken, rule
Each child was given the four practice items. If the
breaks and SRS using a small, unobtrusive video camera
child failed the first trial of a practice item, teaching
that was positioned at a 30° angle facing the child.
was provided. All children were required to pass two
or more practice items before beginning the test items,
starting with the simplest. Each problem proceeded
Scoring the TOL
until either the child completed the problem, gave up,
or violated a rule. Each problem proceeded until either Each item was given a score out of 3. A score of 3 was
the child completed the problem, gave up, or violated given when a child solved the problem during the first
a rule. If the child gave up or violated a rule, a fail was trial in the minimum number of moves. A score of 2
recorded for the trial and the child was given a second was given when a child solved the problem during the
trial on the same test item. If the child failed both trials first trial but not in the minimum number of moves. A
of a given item, he/she was considered to have failed score of 1 was given when a child solved the problem
the item and was given the next item. When a second during the second trial of an item. A score of 0 was given
trial was given for a particular item, only the SRS for when the child failed both trials of an item. A total score
6 Safiyyah Abdul Aziz et al.
was derived by summing the scores across the completed that was not loud enough to be understood, muttering
items. or silent lip movements.

Coding of speech Inter rater reliability


Children’s speech during the TOL administration was A graduate student rater, trained with the coding criteria
coded in terms of utterances from audio and video by the first author, independently coded a random 25%
recordings. An utterance was defined as a length of con- of the video and audio recordings for task relevance,
tinuous speech with less than a 2 s gap between words. addressee, and inaudible muttering. This was then cor-
Mean utterances per item was calculated by averaging related with the video and audio recordings coded by
the total number of utterances across the total num- the first author. An intraclass correlation (ICC) was cal-
ber of TOL items attempted. The children’s utterances culated for private and social speech separately as well
were coded for task relevance (planning or non-planning as for inaudible muttering for each category of speech
speech), addressee (private or social speech) and inaudi- (i.e., formulating a plan, focusing and sustaining atten-
ble muttering. tion, evaluation or seeking assistance). The ICC for task
relevant private speech consisting of formulating a plan,
Task relevance/irrelevance focusing and sustaining attention, evaluation or seek-
ing assistance ranged from 0.70 to 0.99, while the ICC
Task relevant utterances were all utterances that were for task relevant social speech consisting of formulat-
directly related to solving the TOL. This could be ut- ing a plan, focusing and sustaining attention, evaluation
terances for formulating a plan, focusing and sustaining or seeking assistance ranged from 0.85 to 0.99, and the
attention, evaluation or seeking assistance. Examples of ICC for inaudible muttering was 0.90. The average ICC
task relevant utterances are: ‘the blue one goes here’, ‘I’m for SRS coding was 0.91.
looking carefully.’ Utterances were coded as task irrele-
vant if they did not relate to the TOL such as remarks
and descriptions of other objects, events or people out- Results
side the testing context such as, ‘I went to the library Data screening
just now.’ Only 2.5% of the total speech (private and so-
cial) was task irrelevant, therefore these utterances were TOL scores were log transformed to correct for unequal
removed from further analyses. variance across groups (Field 2013). Analyses were then
performed on both the raw scores and the transformed
scores. The same pattern of results emerged in both
Addressee analyses, and so, for ease of interpretation, the analysis
We followed Sturn and Johnston’s (1999) method of performed using raw scores is presented. All variables
coding both private and social speech to be able to cap- met criteria for normality.
ture the full content of children’s SRS. All utterances
were deemed social speech when the child was interact- Preliminary analyses
ing with the examiner. This was operationally defined
as having any one or a combination of these elements: The SLI and TD groups were matched in terms of age,
t(170) = 1.67, p = 0.10, Cohen’s d = 0.26, but they dif-
r eye contact or eye gaze; fered significantly in nonverbal IQ score t(170) = 3.37,
r using the examiner’s name; p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.52. Due to this difference,
r asking a question and waiting for a response from nonverbal IQ was entered as a covariate in all subsequent
the examiner; or analyses involving language group comparisons.
r initiating physical proximity such as touching the In addition, the children categorized as having hy-
examiner’s hand or leaning towards the examiner. peractive and inattentive behaviours obtained the same
language scores (as measured by the CCC-2) and non-
An utterance was coded as private speech after look- verbal ability scores (as measured by the WNV) as chil-
ing for evidence of social speech and finding no evidence dren without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
of social intent. within both the TD and SLI group (all p > 0.10)

Inaudible muttering Group comparisons on TOL performance


This was coded separately from private speech. Inaudible To examine the TOL performance of SLI and TD
muttering was operationally defined as any whispering children across grade levels, a 2 (language group: SLI,
Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with SLI and TD peers 7
Private speech
For private speech, the analysis revealed no significant
main effects and no significant interaction between lan-
guage group and grade level (all p > 0.10).

Social speech
For social speech, there was no difference in the mean
utterance per item between SLI and TD children,
F(1,164) = 0.40, p = 0.527, ηp 2 = 0.00. However, there
was a significant main effect of grade level, F(2,164) =
9.02, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.10, which was moderated
by a significant interaction between language group
and grade level, F(2,164) = 3.45, p = 0.034, ηp 2 =
Figure 1. TOL total score (mean and 95% CI) according to lan- 0.04. Further analysis revealed a non-significant effect
guage group (SLI and TD children) and grade level (Kindergarten, of grade level for the SLI group, F(2,87) = 2.09, p =
Preprimary, Year 1).
0.129, ηp 2 = 0.05, and a significant effect of grade level
for the TD group, F(2,78) = 11.33, p < 0.001, ηp 2 =
TD) × 3 (grade level: Kindergarten, Preprimary, Year 1) 0.23. For the TD children, Kindergarten children pro-
ANCOVA was conducted on the TOL total scores, with duced significantly more social speech than Preprimary
nonverbal IQ scores entered as a covariate. This analysis children, contrast estimate = 0.94, p = 0.001, but there
revealed that children with SLI performed worse than was no significant difference between Preprimary and
TD children, F(1,165) = 23.15, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.12. Year 1 children, contrast estimate = 0.24, p = 0.352.
There was also a significant main effect of grade level,
F(2,165) = 20.36, p< 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.20, with Prepri- Hyperactive and inattentive behaviours and
mary children performing significantly better than
performance on the TOL
the Kindergarten children, contrast estimate = –7.36,
p < 0.001, but no difference between Preprimary and To examine the TOL performance of SLI and TD
Year 1, contrast estimate = –1.43, p = 0.353. There was children with and without hyperactive and inattentive
no significant interaction between language group and behaviours, TOL total scores were collapsed across
grade level (p > 0.10), indicating that children with SLI grade level for each language group to ensure adequate
were impaired relative to TD children across all grade sample sizes within each group. A 2 (language group:
levels. Figure 1 presents the means and 95% confidence SLI, TD) × 2 (behaviour group: hyperactive/inattentive
intervals for each language group across grade level. and non-hyperactive/non-inattentive) ANCOVA was
performed on TOL total scores, with nonverbal IQ
entered as a covariate. Non-hyperactive/non-inattentive
Group comparisons on SRS during the TOL
children performed better than hyperactive/inattentive
To examine SRS during the TOL for TD children and children, F(1,167) = 4.68, p = 0.032, ηp 2 = 0.027.
those with SLI across grade levels, a 2 (language group: However, this main effect was moderated by a signifi-
SLI, TD) × 3 (grade level: Kindergarten, Preprimary, cant language group by behaviour group interaction,
Year 1) ANCOVA was conducted on the mean utterance F(1, 167) = 4.01, p = 0.047, ηp 2 = 0.02. As shown
per item for each type of SRS (inaudible muttering, in figure 2, within the SLI group, children with hyper-
private speech, social speech), with nonverbal IQ scores active/inattentive tendencies performed significantly
entered as a covariate. worse on the TOL than their non-hyperactive/non-
inattentive counterparts, t(89) = 3.20, p = 0.002,
d = 0.72. In contrast, for the TD group, there was
Inaudible muttering
no difference in TOL performance between children
For inaudible muttering, TD children produced more with or without hyperactive and inattentive tendencies,
inaudible muttering than children with SLI, F(1,164) = t(79) = 0.53, p = 0.599, d = 0.12. In addition, children
127.63, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.44. The main effect of with SLI without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
grade level and the interaction were non-significant (all performed significantly worse than TD children with-
p > 0.10), indicating that the effect of language group out hyperactive and inattentive behaviours, t(128) =
was the same across the three grade levels (refer to –3.97, p < 0.001, d = 0.70, and children with SLI
table 2 for means and standard deviations). and hyperactive and inattentive behaviours performed
8 Safiyyah Abdul Aziz et al.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations for mean utterance per item for inaudible muttering, private speech and social speech in TD
and children with SLI

Mean utterance inaudible muttering Mean utterance private speech Mean utterance social speech
Group n per item overall (SD) per item overall (SD) per item overall (SD)
SLI 90 0.05 (0.18) 0.94 (1.18) 1.08 (1.17)
K 35 0.04 (0.26) 0.85 (1.17) 1.19 (1.58)
PP 24 0.01 (0.05) 0.85 (1.00) 1.33 (0.91)
Y1 31 0.02 (0.09) 1.11 (1.31) 0.73 (0.64)
Hyp 30 0.05 (0.12) 1.38 (1.41) 1.00 (1.24)
NonHyp 60 0.05 (0.20) 0.72 (0.98) 1.10 (1.14)
TD 81 0.66 (0.45) 1.13 (1.59) 0.91 (1.08)
K 25 0.71 (0.50) 1.55 (1.86) 1.66 (1.34)
PP 24 0.62 (0.50) 0.81 (1.33) 0.72 (0.76)
Y1 32 0.65 (0.38) 1.01 (1.51) 0.47 (0.72)
Hyp 11 0.74 (0.44) 0.73 (0.63) 0.95 (0.72)
NonHyp 70 0.65 (0.45) 1.20 (1.68) 0.90 (1.13)
Note: K = Kindergarten, PP = Preprimary, Y1 = Year 1, Hyp = children with hyperactive and inattentive behaviours, NonHyp = children with no hyperactive and inattentive
behaviour.

SLI without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours


(M = 24.8 and 11.2 for children with SLI with
and without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
respectively), t(89) = –7.35, p < 0.001, d = –1.64.

Hyperactive and inattentive behaviours and SRS


during the TOL
We further explored the SRS of children with SLI with
and without hyperactive and inattentive behaviours by
conducting independent samples t-tests on the mean ut-
terance per item for inaudible muttering, private speech
and social speech. These analyses revealed no significant
differences between the two behaviour groups in inaudi-
ble muttering, t(88) = –0.05, p < 0.957, d = –0.01,
or social speech, t(88) = 0.17, p = 0.867, d = 0.05.
Figure 2. TOL total score (mean and 95% CI) according to language
group (SLI, TD) and behaviour group (with and without hyperactive
However, children with SLI with hyperactivity produced
and inattentive behaviours). significantly more private speech than those without hy-
peractivity, t(88) = –2.67, p < 0.009, d = –0.60. Means
and standard deviations are presented in table 2.
significantly worse than TD children with hyperactive
and inattentive behaviours, t(36.65) = –4.37, p = Relationship between SRS and item performance on
0.003, d = –1.14, indicating that the presence of SLI
the TOL
had a detrimental impact on TOL performance even in
the absence of hyperactive and inattentive behaviours. Lastly, we examined whether children do better or worse
Given that past research has found that children (or the same) on the TOL when they use SRS compared
with elevated levels of hyperactivity and inatten- with when they are silent, and whether this was related to
tiveness tend to commit more rules breaks than language group. Following Winsler and Naglieri (2003),
non-hyperactive/non-inattentive children (Culbertson we did this by calculating for each child two propor-
and Zillmer 1998), an analysis was conducted to exam- tions: (1) the proportion of items with SRS that were
ine whether rule breaks might explain the differences completed correctly, and (2) the proportion of items
in TOL performance found between children with completed silently that were completed correctly. The
SLI with and without hyperactive and inattentive averages of these within-child proportions are listed in
behaviours. This analysis revealed that children with table 3. For those children who sometimes used SRS and
SLI with hyperactive and inattentive behaviours com- sometimes did not, more TD children performed better
mitted significantly more rule breaks than children with without SRS than with SRS, and the opposite pattern
Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with SLI and TD peers 9
Table 3. Children’s SRS and performance during the TOL

Overall sample (n = 171) SLI group (n = 90) TD group (n = 81)


Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
TOL Performance∗∗ 31.19 (9.62) 27.57 (10.53) 35.24 (6.48)
Number of Items with SRS∗∗ 9.37 (5.18) 7.62 (5.30) 11.31 (4.29)
Percentage of children who had at least one SRS 80.70% 84.44% 76.54%
Percentage of children who had no SRS (always 19.30% 15.56% 23.46%
silent)
Proportion of items correct∗ .77 (.29) .61 (.16) .88 (.16)
Percentage of children who were never silent 9.94% 8.88% 11.11%
(always had SRS)
Proportion of items correct∗∗ .88 (.26) .75 (.34) 1.0 (0.00)
Percentage of children that sometimes have SRS 70.76% 75.56% 65.43%
and sometimes don’t
Proportion of items with SRS that were correct∗∗ .86 (.28) .78 (.32) .95 (.14)
Proportion of items correct when silent∗∗ .89 (.21) .81 (.27) .97 (.09)
Percentage of children whose performance was:
Better with SRS 24.60% 40.98% 9.23%
Better without SRS 25.40% 29.51% 21.58%
The same 50.00% 29.51% 69.23%
Notes: ∗∗ Group difference significant at p < 0.001, ∗ group difference significant at p < 0.05. SRS = self-regulatory speech.

was true for children with SLI (i.e., on average, children two years during the early years of schooling. Moreover,
with SLI were more likely to get an item right when previous research that has examined planning in children
they used SRS compared with when they were silent). with SLI during middle childhood (7–12 years) suggests
However, it should be noted that for TD children, the that these children may continue to lag behind their TD
majority performed comparably regardless of whether peers throughout primary school (Lidstone et al. 2012;
or not they used SRS. Marton 2008). The fact that such a gap in performance
is evident at such a young age is concerning and longi-
tudinal research tracking the developmental trajectory
Discussion
of planning and problem-solving skills in children with
The first aim of this study was to examine the planning SLI from the early years through to middle childhood is
and problem-solving performance and SRS of young warranted to investigate the extent and duration of this
children with SLI (4–7 years) compared with their TD delay.
peers, and in particular, the changes in children’s plan- In relation to SRS, an important finding from this
ning and problem-solving performance across the first study was that TD children exhibited significantly more
three years of schooling (Kindergarten, Preprimary and inaudible muttering compared with children with SLI
Year 1). As hypothesized the results showed that children who had almost no inaudible muttering. This shows
with SLI performed more poorly on the TOL than their that even at a very early age, TD children have inaudible
TD peers. This is consistent with the studies of Marton muttering, which is a sign of SRS internalization. Thus,
(2008) and Lidstone et al. (2012), showing that children not only are they using SRS but they are also show-
with SLI have problems with planning and problem- ing signs of internalizing their SRS to form complete
solving on complex cognitive tasks such as the TOL. Our verbal thought. In contrast, children with SLI seem to
study extended this previous research by demonstrating have a delay in inaudible muttering, which may be an
that the difficulties with planning and problem-solving indication of an overall delay in SRS development.
experienced by children with SLI are evident from a very Both TD children and those with SLI displayed
young age. The results also showed that although per- the same amount of private speech during the TOL.
formance improved across grade levels for both groups Consistent with the findings of Lidstone et al. (2012),
of children, children with SLI were impaired relative this indicates that children with SLI possess and are able
to their TD peers at each grade level. Figure 1 shows to use private speech for planning and problem-solving.
that the average performance of children with SLI in However, for social speech we saw an interesting pat-
Preprimary and Year 1 was similar to the performance tern. TD children followed the expected developmental
of TD children in Kindergarten. This suggests that the pattern, with Kindergarten children displaying more
planning and problem-solving ability of children with social speech than Preprimary and Year 1 children,
SLI may be lagging behind their TD peers by up to indicating that as TD children age, they use less social
10 Safiyyah Abdul Aziz et al.
speech (an immature form of SRS) and progress on to delayed self-regulation (Barkley 2001). Therefore, hav-
inaudible muttering. However, this was not the case for ing hyperactivity comorbid with a language impairment
children with SLI who had the same amount of social may result in greater problems with using language for
speech across the three grade levels. Taken together, the planning and sustaining attention on a complex cogni-
results point towards a delay in SRS development in tive task, leading to poorer performance for these chil-
children with SLI as they appear to rely on social speech, dren. Within the SLI group, children with hyperac-
a less mature form of SRS, throughout the three grade tive and inattentive behaviours produced more private
levels instead of using inaudible muttering, which they speech than children without hyperactive and inatten-
clearly lack. These results are in line with Lidstone et al. tive behaviours, but there were no differences in in-
(2012) showing that children with SLI have a delay in audible muttering or social speech. Higher amounts of
SRS development and internalization. Our study has private speech in children with SLI with hyperactive
extended this previous work by looking at the different and inattentive behaviours is in line with previous stud-
types of SRS, which provides further insight into the ies on SRS in TD children with ADHD, which show
nature of the SRS delay in young children with SLI. that these children have more private speech than those
The second aim of this study was to examine whether without hyperactivity and inattention, even though
planning performance was associated with levels of hy- their performance is still lower than children with-
peractivity and inattention in both children with SLI out ADHD (Winsler et al. 2007). Researchers (Barkley
and their TD peers. Our hypothesis was partially sup- 2001; Winsler et al. 2007) have argued that the rea-
ported. The results indicated that children with hyperac- son why hyperactive and inattentive children have more
tive and inattentive behaviours did perform more poorly SRS during planning and problem-solving is because
on the TOL than children without hyperactive and inat- they need to use SRS to regulate their behaviour so that
tentive behaviours, however, this was only the case for they are able to focus on the task at hand. Therefore,
children with SLI. Within the TD group, there was these children use SRS for two functions: behavioural
no difference in planning performance between chil- self-regulation and the cognitive task at hand. Thus,
dren with hyperactive and inattentive behaviours and despite having more private speech, their planning and
those without. This finding appears inconsistent with problem-solving performance may still be lower because
previous studies that have shown children with ADHD they need to divide their cognitive resources between
to have poorer planning than children without ADHD these two concurrent functions. Given that 25–45%
(e.g., Corkum et al. 2008), which has been attributed of children with SLI exhibit some degree of hyperac-
to delayed SRS (Winsler et al. 2007). However, it is tive and inattentive behaviours, more attention should
important to note that the children in our sample clas- be directed to this group to further our understanding
sified as having hyperactive and inattentive behaviours of this double deficit and its impact on their SRS and
were not diagnosed with ADHD, but were classified executive functioning.
on the basis of elevated levels of hyperactivity on the The final aim of this study was to examine whether
SDQ. Thus, the levels of hyperactivity and inattention SRS was related to performance on the TOL. The hy-
evident in the present study may be milder than those pothesis that children who used SRS would be, on av-
evident in studies of children diagnosed with ADHD, erage, more accurate on the TOL relative to the chil-
which may translate into milder cognitive sequelae for dren who were silent was partially supported. Children
the TD children in our sample. Nonetheless, the re- with SLI appeared to be most disadvantaged when they
sults clearly indicate that the presence of hyperactive were silent. However, TD children outperformed chil-
and inattentive behaviours further impedes the already dren with SLI regardless of whether they were talking
impaired planning and problem-solving performance of or silent. This observation raises an interesting question
children with SLI, and that this may be partially ex- as to whether TD children who were silent were using
plained by the greater number of rule breaks commit- silent verbal thought (i.e., not exhibiting signs of SRS
ted by these children during the TOL. These findings as it is fully internalized) while children with SLI who
suggest that having a SLI and hyperactive and inatten- were silent lack verbal thought to support performance
tive behaviours may result in a double deficit for these on the TOL. This question warrants further investiga-
children. tion as we observed that children with SLI displayed
The differential impact of hyperactivity and inat- virtually no inaudible muttering, which is typically an
tention in children with and without SLI is intrigu- indication of SRS being internalized. However, children
ing. One possible explanation is that children with SLI with SLI who were silent may not be using verbal medi-
and hyperactive and inattentive behaviours are lim- ation as a strategy during planning and problem-solving
ited in terms of the compensatory strategies available tasks compared with children with SLI who have private
to them to help them stay on track, plan and prob- speech or social speech. Some support for this sugges-
lem solve. We know that children with ADHD exhibit tion comes from the finding that of those children who
Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with SLI and TD peers 11
used SRS sometimes but not always, for children with developmental trajectory of planning performance in
SLI, a higher percentage performed better on the TOL young children with SLI to confirm that the apparent
when they used SRS than when they didn’t, whereas the delay in development is not simply a cohort effect. In
opposite pattern was observed for TD children. addition, it would be valuable to follow the children
The results of this study have several clinical beyond age 7. This would offer more insight into the
implications. First, it is essential that behavioural screen- developmental trajectory of the planning ability of
ing for hyperactivity and inattention for children with children with SLI across childhood. In addition, future
SLI is carried out in preschool. Given the combined im- studies in this area should include more participants
pact of SLI and hyperactive and inattentive behaviours, within both language groups to enable comparisons of
an assessment of behaviour would provide an important children with and without hyperactive and inattentive
insight that parents and teachers could use to guide in- behaviours within each grade level. Although the rela-
tervention and further assessment and diagnosis. It is tive numbers correspond to the proportion occurring in
important to note that we did not diagnose ADHD; the population, the small numbers limited our ability
this was not our aim. Our purpose was to determine to look at hyperactivity and inattention within each
whether, even at subclinical levels, having hyperactive grade level. This would be important to assess whether
and inattentive behaviours and SLI would impair plan- the impact of hyperactive and inattentive behaviours
ning and problem-solving performance to a greater ex- on SRS is reduced as children mature.
tent than having only SLI. This proved to be the case. As noted before, the children in our study were
Thus, future studies examining the effects of a comorbid younger than those in previous studies using the TOL.
diagnosis of ADHD and SLI and its impact on planning We also used a TOL set that ranged in difficulty from
and problem-solving in young children are warranted. 3- to 7-move problems. As we discontinued the test af-
Second, our study highlights the importance of con- ter three consecutive failures, which was necessary given
ducting early assessments of executive functioning, es- the age of the children, not all children completed all
pecially planning and problem-solving, in children with the TOL items. Children in the SLI group, particularly
SLI. This is particularly important in the early years to younger children and those with hyperactivity, had the
enable the provision of appropriate intervention pro- most difficulty and thus stopped earlier. This resulted in
grammes for children with SLI. Such interventions are children stopping at different levels of difficulty, and so,
warranted because executive function deficits, especially it was not possible for us to make meaningful inferences
a weakness with planning and problem-solving, may regarding the time taken to complete problems or the
be one underlying factor contributing to a number of number of moves.
the learning and social skills deficits that children with
SLI often demonstrate during primary and secondary Conclusions
school. Previous research on older children with SLI
indicates that they perform lower on virtually every aca- The present study makes a significant contribution to
demic task and display poorer emotion regulation and our understanding of SRS changes across age during
social skills (Botting and Conti-Ramsden 2000). Early a complex cognitive task that involves planning and
intervention and continued monitoring may assist chil- problem-solving ability in young children with SLI.
dren with SLI develop the planning and problem-solving Specifically, children with SLI show a delay in SRS devel-
skills needed for more complex social interactions as opment, and those with hyperactive and inattentive be-
they age. haviours may have a double deficit that exacerbates their
Third, when examining the relationship between difficulties with planning and problem-solving. These
SRS and performance on the individual items on the findings suggest that an intervention using SRS train-
TOL, we found that in children with SLI, those who had ing for children with SLI when planning and problem-
SRS did better than children who were silent, supporting solving may be useful. More broadly, the findings indi-
Vygotsky’s notion that SRS is paramount in cognitive cate that any assessment of executive functioning skills
tasks. SRS interventions and verbal scaffolding have also in children with SLI should take into account their be-
proven to be useful for TD children in enhancing their havioural issues, particularly hyperactivity and inatten-
planning and problem-solving abilities (Winsler 2009). tion, in addition to their language, including SRS.
Therefore, SRS training may be useful for children with
SLI to assist them in their SRS development. Acknowledgements
Our study has several limitations. First, it used a
cross-sectional design and did not follow the planning The authors acknowledge the contribution of W. Keith Berg for
providing the TOL item configurations that were used in this study.
performance of the same group of children over time. Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Consequently, the changes across age needs to be The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the
assessed using longitudinal studies to examine the paper.
12 Safiyyah Abdul Aziz et al.
References GOODMAN, R., 2001, Psychometric properties of the strengths and
difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy
AZIZ, S. A., FLETCHER, J. and BAYLISS, D. M., 2015, The of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345.
effectiveness of self-regulatory speech training for plan- doi:10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015
ning and problem-solving in children with specific lan- HENRY, L. A., MESSER, D. J. and NASH, G., 2012, Execu-
guage impairment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- tive functioning in children with specific language impair-
ogy, 43(8), doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0115-7. (available at: ment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 37–
http://woodm.myweb.port.ac.uk/stats). 45.
BALDO, J. V., DRONKERS, N. F., WILKINS, D., LUDY, C., RASKIN, P. KIM, H. S., 2002, We talk, therefore we think? A cultural analy-
and KIM, J., 2005, Is problem-solving dependent on language? sis of the effect of talking on thinking. Journal of Personal-
Brain and Language, 92(3), 240–250. ity and Social Psychology, 83, 828–860. doi:10.1037//0022-
BARKLEY, R. A., 2001, The executive functions and self- 3514.83.4.828
regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspec- LIDSTONE, J., MEINS, E. and FERNYHOUGH, C., 2010, The roles
tive. Neuropsychology Review, 11(1), 1–29. doi:10.1023/A: of private speech and inner speech in planning during
1009085417776 middle childhood: evidence from a dual task paradigm.
BERG, W. K. and BYRD, D. L., 2002, The Tower of London spatial Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 438–451.
problem-solving task: enhancing clinical and research imple- doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.002
mentation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol- LIDSTONE, J., MEINS, E. and FERNYHOUGH, C., 2012, Verbal me-
ogy, 24(5), 586–604. diation of cognition in children with specific language im-
BERK, L. E. and SPUHL, S. T., 1995, Maternal interaction, pairment. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 651–660.
private speech, and task performance in preschool chil- doi:10.1017/S0954579412000223
dren. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 145–169. MACDONALD, C. and BERG, W., 2005, MSc, University of Florida
doi:10.1016/0885-2006(95)90001-2 e-Theses Archive.
BISHOP, D. V. and BAIRD, G., 2001, Parent and teacher report of MARTON, K., 2008, Visuo-spatial processing and executive func-
pragmatic aspects of communication: use of the Children’s tions in children with specific language impairment. Inter-
Communication Checklist in a clinical setting. Developmental national Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
Medicine and Child Neurology, 43(12), 809–818. 43(2), 181–200. doi:10.1080/16066350701340719
BISHOP, D. V., 2003, The Children’s Communication Checklist (Vol. SHALLICE, T., 1982, Specific impairments of planning. Philo-
2): ASHA. sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B,
BOTTING, N. and CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 2000, Social and be- Biological Sciences, 298, 199–209. doi:10.1098/rstb.1982.
havioural difficulties in children with language impairment. 0082
Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 16(2), 105–135. doi: STURN, A. and JOHNSTON, J., 1999, Thinking out loud: an ex-
10.1191/026565900668565128. ploration of problem-solving language in preschoolers with
CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 2008, Heterogeneity of specific language and without language impairment. International Journal
impairment in adolescent outcomes. In C.F. Norbury, J.B. of Language and Communication Disorders, 34(1), 1–15.
Tomblin and D.V.M. Bishop (Eds), Understanding develop- doi:10.1080/136828299247586
mental language disorders: From theory to practice (New York, VYGOTSKY, L. S., 1987, The Collected Works of LS Vygotsky. Vol. 1,
NY, US: Psychology Press), pp. 115–129. Problems of General Psychology including the Volume Thinking
CORKUM, P., HUMPHRIES, K., MULLANE, J. C. and THERIAULT, F., and Speech (New York: Plenum).
2008, Private speech in children with ADHD and their typi- WECHSLER, D. and NAGLIERI, J. A., 2006, Wechsler Nonverbal Scale
cally developing peers during problem-solving and inhibition of Ability: WNV (PsychCorp).
tasks. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(1), 97–115. WINSLER, A., 2009, Still talking to ourselves after all these
doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.12.003 years: review of current research on private speech. Private
CULBERTSON, W. C. and ZILLMER, E. A., 1998, The Tower of Speech, Executive Functioning, and the Development of Verbal
LondonDX: a standardized approach to assessing executive Self-Regulation, 3–41. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511581533.
functioning in children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 003
13(3), 285–301. doi:10.1093/arclin/13.3.285 WINSLER, A., DIAZ, R. M. and MONTERO, I., 1997, The role of
DUNCAN, R. M. and PRATT, M. W., 1997, Microgenetic change private speech in the transition from collaborative to inde-
in the quantity and quality of preschoolers’ private speech. pendent task performance in young children. Early Child-
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20(2), 367– hood Research Quarterly, 12(1), 59–79. doi:10.1016/s0885-
383. doi:10.1080/016502597385388 2006(97)90043-0
FERNYHOUGH, C. and MEINS, E., 2009, Private speech and WINSLER, A., MANFRA, L. and DIAZ, R. M., 2007, ‘Should
theory of mind: evidence for developing interfunc- I let them talk?’: private speech and task performance
tional relations. Private Speech, Executive Functioning, and among preschool children with and without behavior prob-
the Development of Verbal Self-Regulation, 95–104. doi: lems. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22(2), 215–231.
10.1017/CBO9780511581533.008 doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.01.001
FIELD, A., 2013, Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics WINSLER, A. and NAGLIERI, J., 2003, Overt and covert ver-
(London: Sage). bal problem-solving strategies: developmental trends in use,
FURROW, D., 1984, Social and private speech at two years. awareness, and relations with task performance in chil-
Child Development, 55(2), 355–362. doi:10.1111/1467- dren aged 5 to 17. Child Development, 74(3), 659–678.
8624.ep7304525 doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00561

You might also like