Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 25

Psychological Inquiry, 21: 2–25, 2010

Copyright 
C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1047-840X print / 1532-7965 online
DOI: 10.1080/10478401003596626

TARGET ARTICLE

The Five-Factor Framing of Personality and Beyond: Some Ruminations


Jack Block
University of California, Berkeley

The five-factor conceptualization of personality has been presented as all-embracing


in understanding personality and has even received authoritative recommendation
for understanding early development. I raise various concerns regarding this popular
model. More specifically, (a) the atheoretical nature of the five-factors, their cloudy
measurement, and their inappropriateness for studying early childhood are dis-
cussed; (b) the method (and morass) of factor analysis as the exclusive paradigm for
conceptualizing personality is questioned and the continuing nonconsensual under-
standings of the five-factors is noted; (c) various unrecognized but successful efforts
to specify aspects of character not subsumed by the catholic five-factors are brought
forward; and (d) transformational developments in regard to inventory assessment of
personality are mentioned. I conclude by suggesting that repeatedly observed higher
order factors hierarchically above the proclaimed five may promise deeper biological
understanding of the origins and implications of these superfactors.

Key words: personality, five-factor model, neuroticism, extraversion, openness,


agreeableness, conscientiousness.

Introduction and Perspective they were crucially different in their methodology, fur-
ther development, and aspiration. The initial BF was
I have been a long, active participant in and an strictly phenotypic, empirically organizing the usage
attentive—even obsessive—observer of the scientific of single-word common-language personality descrip-
study of personality including its early development, tors via the method of factor analysis into five factors
its conceptual and empirical ups and downs, its fads (neutrally identified by the Roman numerals, I through
and fashions. In this perambulating and idiosyncratic V). The FFM has been far more ambitious, being ex-
article, I attempt to touch primarily upon the last three plicitly hierarchical in its two-level structure of factors
decades or so, expressing some personal recognitions and facets, and named its five primary factors as Neu-
and concerns regarding this thorny field as we go for- roticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and
ward further into the 21st century. Conscientiousness.1 In what follows, when I refer con-
Since the 1980s, the study of human personality jointly to these two related approaches, I collectively
has been largely, vigorously, and grandly centered refer to the BF and FFM as the Five-Factor-Approach
on delimited and foreclosed stated responses by lay (FFA). I think it is fair to say that, thus far, the FFM
adult participants to brief person-descriptive words or initiated by Costa and McCrae has achieved wider lit-
phrases—the lexical Big Five (BF) collection of ad- erature and applications prominence than Goldberg’s
jectives initially organized and promulgated largely by BF.
Goldberg (1981, 1990, 1993) and the later developed, Historically, the BF emerged as a consequence of
derivative variant, the Five-Factor-Model (FFM) ques-
tionnaire approach to personality study advocated by “one critical assumption. Those individual differences
Costa and McCrae (1992). Various additional versions that are most significant in the daily transactions of
of the lexical BF and of the FFM have appeared over
the years. 1 These factors have often been simply identified by their initial-
In important ways, the original BF and the FFM letter designations (N, E, O, A, C), mnemonically remembered more
are necessarily substantively similar, but in other ways easily as CANOE or OCEAN.

2
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

persons with each other will eventually become en- Subsequently, impressed by the increasingly
coded in their language . . . . A major objective . . . is influential lexical five-factor analyses of Goldberg’s
to develop a theoretically compelling structure, or tax- person-descriptors, Costa and McCrae saw larger pos-
onomy, for all personality-descriptive terms in En- sibilities in developing a questionnaire version of the
glish. . . ”. (Goldberg, 1982, p. 204) adjectival five-factors. They considered the inventory
format of short sentences as having wider usage poten-
This motivating conjecture about everyday lay lan- tial by the wide variety of psychologist-evaluators than
guages generated a number of studies involving various single-word, person-adjective descriptors. Further,
samples of adjectives, subjects, and languages, then and crucially, they interpreted their three pre-existing
evaluated with respect to the empirical usage of single- NEO questionnaire scales as substantively equivalent,
word, adjective person-descriptors. These usage cor- psychologically, to three of Goldberg’s adjective
relations among the adjectives, when subjected to the factor dimensions—Goldberg’s Emotional Stability
statistical method of factor analysis, repeatedly were adjective factor, if reflected, they viewed as equivalent
reported as issuing five adjective cluster-dimensions to their NEO Neuroticism factor, Goldberg’s Surgency
and were interpreted as supporting “the Lexical adjective factor they interpreted as tantamount to their
Hypothesis.”2 The BF dimensions emerging were la- NEO Extraversion factor, and Goldberg’s Intellect
beled (I) Surgency (or Extraversion), (II) Agreeable- adjective factor they claimed, arguably, could be
ness, (III) Conscientiousness (or Dependability), (IV) construed as rather similar to their NEO Openness to
Emotional Stability (vs. Neuroticism), and (V) Intel- Experience factor.4 But their initial three-dimensional
lect. For many BF advocates, the five clusters seemed NEO inventory lacked questionnaire versions of two
intuitively apt, perhaps even evolutionarily germane, of the five Goldberg person-adjective factors. They
and therefore the obtained factor structure was deemed therefore constructed questionnaires for their own
universally sufficient for use in describing persons in versions of inventory scales labeled Conscientiousness
general. and Agreeableness and “grafted” (Goldberg, 1993, p.
Separately, and quite independent of the BF, 31) these two new “domains,” that is, scales, onto what
Costa and McCrae (1985) had developed their own, they now called the NEO–PI (Costa & McCrae, 1985).
acronymically named three-factor NEO Personality These latter two factor-representing scales, each subse-
Inventory, limited to measuring their particular scale quently further subdivided as before by dyadic decision
versions of Neuroticism and Extraversion—both con- into six eight-item facet scales, in keeping with their
structs already well evaluated by a number of pre- previously established scheme, became part of what
vious questionnaire scales—and a relatively unusual was revisionally named the NEO–PI–R (Costa &
scale evaluating what they called Openness to Ex- McCrae, 1992).
perience.3 The questionnaire person-queries defining These person-measures—the initial BF and the
each of these three factor scales (which they charac- later-evolved FFM—are based on aggregations of ad-
terized as representing “domains”) were each further jectives or inventory-sentences responded to by the
subdivided thoughtfully but subjectively into exactly studied lay persons as descriptive of their selves or
six “facets”—short, eight-sentence-long subscales of of others with whom they have some acquaintance.
“person discriminants,” each subscale posited as rep- Subsequent factor analyses of these person-descriptors,
resenting an important feature of each factor do- variously formulated and expressed in a variety of lan-
main. The Costa and McCrae decisions reached in guages, have repeatedly reported the finding of five
“faceting” their three initial NEO personality scales largely similar factors. The reporting of this replicated
from “person-discriminants” remain essentially un- finding has been interpreted as attesting to the univer-
changed after three decades of usage and research. sality of a common, conceptually sufficient, orthogonal
structure underlying person variation. The emergence
of this consensus, enthusiastically and internationally
2 However, it is often unrecognized or unacknowledged that the embraced in the last 30 years, has been viewed as a
lexical hypothesis is not the only basis for its subsequent findings signal, fruitful, scientific achievement: the finding of
and the five-factor model. The nature of the algorithm of factor the five basic factors underlying the human personal-
analysis is that it will reveal lexical factor dimensions if and only if
ity. Why are there five factors? It was claimed by some
those dimensions have many synonyms or near-synonyms among the
trait terms being studied. Person qualities that are important but not as “an empirical fact, like the fact that that there are
abundantly referenced as adjectives in the common language (e.g., seven continents on earth and eight American Presi-
“effeminate” or “mannish”), were they to be included in the factor dents from Virginia” (McCrae & John, 1992, p. 194).
analysis, would tend to be missed by the factor analysis process. It is
not always the case that the number of synonyms for a word testifies
to the psychological importance of that word. Certain words—crucial
words—do not need to be redundantly expressed. 4 It is not without interest that, a few years later, McCrae argued—
3 Cattell (1970), Coan (1972), and Tellegen (1974), among others, I think correctly—that Openness was insufficiently represented by
had earlier ventured into this psychological domain. single word descriptors as in Goldberg’s BF.

3
BLOCK

A quick and doubtless incomplete Google search Child Psychology, titled “Personality Development”
reveals thousands of “five personality factor” articles (Caspi & Shiner, 2006). Handbook chapters in well-
in refereed journals (about 600 in the last 2 years). established psychological fields tend to be highly in-
Although many are of peripheral interest and some fluential, defining scope and providing ready access to
may even be strange, conveying unexplained, and un- au courant knowledge. Novitiates receive a rapid and
replicated connections between the FFA and available helpful introduction in the conceptual and empirical
other data (e.g., Guadagno, Okdie, & Eno, 2008, on domain; toilers in the field easily can update them-
blogging prediction; Tate & Shelton, 2008, on tattoo- selves. But, nevertheless, very much depends on the
ing and body piercing; Vermaes, Janssens, Mullaart, perspective or framing of the chapter. Topically equiv-
Vinck, & Gerris, 2008, on spina bifida), any student alent accounts by different authors or at different times
of the personality literature must acknowledge the pre- can sing very different tunes, in regard to both content
vailing temper of these times: The FFA, variously pred- and standards of coverage.
icated, has become central to contemporary personality Although this particular chapter in a prescriptive
research. volume is a lucid, well-organized, contemporizing ac-
I note that I am on record from years ago as count of the place of temperament within develop-
a “contrarian” with regard to then accepting the mental psychology, I was surprised by the FFA the-
questionnaire- or descriptor-based five-factor approach oretical perspective advocated therein with respect to
to portraying personality structure as a fundamental developmental psychology. In my view, the concep-
advance in the understanding of human personality tual orientation presented illustrates the far—and as
(Block, 1995a, 1995b, 2001). I am vain enough to yet empirically unwarranted—reaches of the adult-
suggest that present readers may find these earlier oriented FFA into the role of temperament within child
background articles informative, perspectivizing, and development.
of continuing current interest. Then, although I by no At its outset and thereafter, Caspi and Shiner (2006)
means questioned the substantial usefulness of ques- seem to have prestructured developmental issues of
tionnaires or descriptor-clusters, which I myself have emergent temperament in terms of the FFM perspec-
frequently used to advantage (e.g., Block, 1965), I par- tive as brought forward by factor-analysis-oriented per-
ticularly noted a variety of problems I saw plaguing the sonologists (e.g., Goldberg, 1990; John & Srivastava,
lay-response focused FFA as the sole and sufficient ba- 1999; McCrae & Costa, 1999). They suggest that devel-
sis for comprehensively conceptualizing the complex opmentalists “now have greater clarity about the adult
terrain of personality. personality traits that child studies should be trying to
Herein, I somewhat repeat but also further extend predict over time” (p. 307).
concerns regarding the FFA as the personality rubric However, as usually operationalized via a question-
for our time. I also acknowledge that there may be a naire for lay adults, the FFM cannot be readily inter-
deep underlying genetic basis to the empirical regular- preted or analogized to apply to the behavior and verbal
ity of the findings accruing to “the five-factors”; how- comprehension of individuals less than midadolescent,
ever, I continue to appreciably disagree with or find that is, infancy, toddlers, young, and middle children.
insufficient the psychological understandings usually To support their suggestion for considering the FFA as
emanating from this approach. intimated in the earlier years, Caspi and Shiner focus on
I recognize that I may be perceived by some as hav- one study, the statistical reprocessing (Measelle, John,
ing a penchant for disagreeing with fellow psycholo- Ablow, Cowan, & Cowan, 2005) of a previously re-
gists (Block, 1995a); however, I suggest—equally— ported puppet-based interview of a sample of children
that others may have an equivalent penchant for dis- aged 5 to 7 (Measelle, Ablow, Cowan, & Cowan, 1998).
agreeing with disagreement. Understandably, even in In their Table 6.1, Caspi and Shiner display selected
science, there often can be an overeagerness and pro- puppet-interview responses of these children as cor-
jectivity in pressing one’s viewpoint, even a latent pro- responding in their organization to the adult-oriented
crusteanism or lacunae in the interpretation of logic and five-factor structure. Thus, a child’s puppet response,
realities. I frankly own up to such susceptibilities and “If someone is mean to me, I don’t hit them,” is said
hope that, in what follows, I have sufficiently tethered to augur the adult factor Agreeableness; a child’s pro-
them. jected puppet response, “I’m a smart kid,” is claimed to
intimate later adult Openness to Experience; the puppet
response, “I get nervous when my teacher calls on me,”
The FFA and Developmental Psychology is anticipated to be predictive of adult-based Neuroti-
cism. Other psychologists may wish to await indepen-
The remarkable spread, reach, and sway of the dent replication of this secondary analysis and—more
FFA may be exemplified—by no means exclusively— important—the demonstration of a variety of concor-
by the recent, important chapter in the Handbook of dant studies before importantly relying upon the grand

4
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

suggestion of a downward applicability to young chil- Why the FFA Does Not Help Understanding
dren of the FFM. This downward age generalization of
the FFA should not rest only upon so fragile a one-time The FFA Is Atheoretical5
postinterpretation.
The term “theory” means very different things to
In the meanwhile, the question of the structure of
different people. Sometimes, the term “theory” is used
young children’s person-attributions is perhaps indi-
informally by psychologists to describe a hunch that
cated by an earlier study (Peevers & Secord, 1973)
research in a particular area will prove fruitful or that
on age developmental changes in person descriptions.
certain distinctions among variables are worth mak-
They found that children in kindergarten or the first
ing. Sometimes a relation between two variables is
grade do not characteristically invoke dispositional la-
proposed as a “theory.” More rigorously, however, it
bels along the lines of the FFM. The claimed general-
would be scientifically superior to define a model of
ization of the FFM to the earlier wordings employed
personality as a system within the individual of dy-
by young children in a puppet interview awaits further
namically interconnected, interdependent variables or-
and independent empirical demonstration.
ganized over time to have adaptive systemic effect. A
In proposing that the FFA applies to the early
proper model is more than a simple listing of person-
developmental years, this view accepts that five
ality variables or domains asserted to be sufficient; it
nominally orthogonal trait constructs emanating
should be an integrative model of organized person-
from factor analyses of quickly-responded-to self-
ality functioning. Given these prefatory remarks, the
or other-person questionnaires by lay adults provide
FFA can be recognized as descriptive only, statically
a cogent, sufficient, organizing framework for un-
so rather than a dynamic model.
derstanding personality and the long, prior years of
human development.
I suggest, alternatively, this way of framing de- The FFA Ignores Contemporary
velopmental issues of temperament from infancy on Evolutionary Thinking
in terms of the personality trait categories stemming Goldberg’s (1981) common-sense suggestion as
from the questionnaire responses of opportunely avail- to how the five trait descriptive categories lexically
able lay adults is premature and unresponsive to other emerged over ancestral time because of their crucial
developmental research. Certainly, the five-factor trait utility in evaluating inevitable social contexts is cer-
viewpoint is currently popularly employed. But popu- tainly suggestive as a loosely grounded evolutionary
larity has never been a scientific criterion of veracity. conjecture. But it was never seriously intended and
The lay adult-based, asserted as orthogonal, five-factor grounded in evolutionary terms. As Hubbard (1995)
structure did not derive in any way from achieved un- remarked,
derstandings in developmental psychology. Rather, al-
though the FFA orientation in its own terms—when Development is dialectical and not linear. What hap-
closely considered—remains semantically murky and pens to a person all along the way, and what that
contentious, it is offered in this handbook chapter on in- person makes happen, will continuously affect her or
born temperament as the generative conceptual scheme his biological, psychological and social growth and
for retrospectively considering the complexities of hu- development. We are not simply . . . readouts of our
man development from the very early years. I submit genes. (p. 8)6
that the nature of the neonate, the life-enveloping in-
fant, the developing child, the emerging adolescent, It is about time we recognized that heredity and
the subsequent prime years of the adult, and even the environment are not simply additively related; their
octogenarian—progressively and often fundamentally connection is complex and nonlinear, often synergistic.
changing over time and context—is not especially il-
luminated by a framing emerging from orthogonaliz- 5 The FFA view is quite explicit: “Description of personality
ing factor analyses of the responses offered by often- must precede, not follow, personality theory” (Costa & McCrae,
unrepresentative adult samples to quick inventories of 1992, p. 861). But compare with “Science walks forward on two feet,
convenience. To currently make a probative case for namely theory and experiment. . . . Sometimes it is one foot which
is put forward first, sometimes the other, but continuous progress is
the prematurity of settling upon the “five factors” of
only made by the use of both” (Millikan, 1923/1965, p. 54).
adult personality as a coherent and advancing struc- 6 Although Caspi and Shiner have offered a sophisticated discus-
ture for reflecting backward in time upon personality sion of the complicated field of molecular genetics, they acknowl-
development, it is necessary to indicate how this five- edge that this research area as yet offers little that is dependably
factor structuring “rooted” (McCrae & Costa, 1989, replicable to developmental knowledge and remains most difficult.
p. 108) in the mechanics of factor analysis is defi- In particular, they note that, “for behavioral prediction, it is often
a short-sighted strategy to rely exclusively on measures of broad
cient and perhaps even stultifying in generating under- superfactors (i.e. the FFA) . . . such exclusive reliance may limit re-
standing of core temperament and dynamically evolved search into the genetic etiology of personality differences” (Caspi &
personality. Shiner, 2006, p. 332).

5
BLOCK

The recent emergence of “evo-devo”7 and epigenesis the complexly interactive system characterizing an in-
have great potential for understanding these nonlinear dividual’s personality.
developments but are scientific fields “untouchable” by
rapidly emitted lay response to the FFA.
The Use of Factor Analysis as the Person
Paradigm
The FFA Has No Serious Ontogenetic
Implications The FFA is based on, and embraces, the mathemat-
ical model of factor analysis as a sufficient method
Beyond question, an absolute tenet of the study for recognizing and establishing a suitable taxonomy
of human development is that lives through time are of adult human character. There remain psychological
involved. The static FFA, quite unaided by unique reasons to be uneasy with this union.
theoretical principles or guidance, generates repeti- The FFA is pushed by prior substantive preference
tive, unsurprising (and, in a sense, tautological) slight, rather than pulled by pure mathematical truths. (As ex-
nonchance findings within large samples. It provides perienced factor analysts will acknowledge, there are
no empirical expectations regarding an infant’s devel- differences among them with respect to stopping rules,
opmental course or its adaptive strengths or suscepti- rotational preferences, and other witting or unwitting
bilities before becoming a literate late adolescent. manipulation of data.) Although the FFA has been pre-
sented as if the five obtained factors are orthogonal and
are each of equivalent behavioral importance, in fact
Some Inadequacies of the FFA the orthogonality is deliberately created rather than em-
pirically existent and the first two or three trait factors
Mathematical Orthogonality Is Unrelated to in the five-factor-structure are, empirically, apprecia-
Psychological Understanding bly more clear and consequential than the remaining
Effectively, the FFA considers and evaluates its five three or two.
dimensions as orthogonal. Many users of the FFA Loevinger (1993) called attention to the psycholog-
do not consider the implications of its assumption ically untenable assumption within factor analysis of
or creation of orthogonality. To exemplify, in three- mathematical rectilinearity, of linearity in general with
dimensional evaluation of the space of a room, a regard to understanding humans. For epistemological
layperson will know that the length of a room en- reasons, Meehl (1992) has cautioned that “No statisti-
tails nothing necessarily about its width or its ceil- cal procedure should be treated as a mechanical truth
ing height—length, width, and height are conceptually, generator” (p. 152). In particular, the factor analytic
logically unrelated, although they may well be, and of- method can generate a variety of “truths,” according to
ten are, functionally related. Treated only as a set of or- the bent or desires or avoidances of the particular fac-
thogonal dimensions, each dimension operates singly, tor analyst. Although the method of factor analysis can
separately,as unrelated to the others—rather than con- be extraordinarily illuminating, by itself the method
jointly or configurationally. alone should not be empowered to make paramount
However, in fact, for each of the five FFA dimen- and controlling decisions regarding the concepts to be
sions, the other four factors indubitably influence the used within the field of personality. Factor analysis re-
special ways the particular, focused-upon dimension is sults may certainly offer or confirm suggestions, but
expressed; that is, by virtue of controlling or not con- these should be based on conceptual and epistemic
trolling any one of the FFA factors, the correlates of an- considerations.
other factor will vary. It is the complex interplay of the In actuality, the diverse individuals employ-
various personality dimensions or aspects of character ing factor analysis are not passive recipients
(conjoined with the situational context) that truly ex- of what their favored algorithm happens to is-
presses the nature of an individual. In the FFA research sue. At various points during the factor analytic
world, however, each of the five factors is usually eval- process, analysts have an opportunity to sluice their
uated singly rather than as moderated by another or results in preferred directions—to have more or fewer
others of the five factors. The plethora of low, even if factors, orthogonalized or correlated factors, to fit a
statistically “significant,” relationships, often correla- preferred or previous conceptual scheme or to be sub-
tionally observed for a factor domain when scanning ject to the uncontrolled, happenstance lay of the data.
large samples, is at a far remove from understanding For these reasons, among others, we should bolster
our particular factoring results in complementary, quite
separate, and non-factor-analytic ways, seeking a va-
7 The short-hand phrase for evolutionary developmental biology:
riety of phenotypically diverse but theoretically con-
the study of how the dynamics of development may determine phe-
notypic variation arising from genetic variation and how that may vergent psychological resources and methodologies
subsequently affect the direction of phenotypic evolution (Ploeger, involving close conceptualizing, perceptive ob-
van der Maas, & Raaijmakers, 2008a, 2008b). servation, and unconfounding, clarifying, incisive
6
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

empiricism (Block & Block, 1980). Within the sci- chological theory to have a clarifying perspective on
entific tradition, nomological recognitions should the meaning of scale scores. In studying human de-
emerge; personal preferences or aspirations should not velopment, a differential strategy is able to specify an
alone shape a field. individual’s particular stage or place within the series
of life-transforming changes or life-phase transitions
undergone in the arc of one’s personality-structural de-
The Scaling Paradigm Underlying the FFA is
velopment (such as, e.g., establishing a self-referent
Importantly Ill-Suited for a Larger
rather than an other-referent conception of morality or
Psychological Understanding
in developing a personal “theory of mind” or in rec-
The measurement logic used in the FFA approach ognizing the possibilities and limitations inherent in
for constructing scales of measurement, although con- one’s age).
ventional and popular, does not reflect crucial devel- With respect to the FFA, in addition to the prob-
opmental, phase-transition changes and may be mis- lem of achieving perspective on the psychological
leading or obscuring of the developmental meaning of meaning of a score on the FFA Conscientiousness
FFA scores. factor, the remaining four FFA scales—when closely
Years ago, Loevinger (1948, 1957, 1993) concerned reconsidered—can also be recognized as warranting
with “objective tests as instruments of psycholog- usage of differential rather than cumulative scoring, as
ical theory,” recognized a psychologically strategic more informative when interpreted within a conceptual
distinction between what she called cumulative and or empirical norming perspective.
differential scale measurement in creating psychologi- For each of the remaining four FFA factor dimen-
cal test measures. sions, comparing scorers at either end of the contin-
In the cumulative (or monotone) measurement uum (extreme Extraversion, such as unbridled hyper-
model conventionally used, the more keyed answers activity vs. insufficient Extraversion, such as extreme
of the respondent to the putative trait, the higher interiority; extreme Neuroticism, such as hyperadverse
the individual’s score on that trait and the more reactivity to the quotidian day vs. insufficient Neuroti-
of that trait the person is assumed to intrinsically cism, such as an extreme unreactivity to the tenor and
possess. Each of the five primary FFA scales is eval- fluctuations of daily existence; extreme Agreeableness,
uated via the principle of cumulative scale measure- such as self-ignoring vs. insufficient Agreeableness,
ment. such as irascibility; and extreme Openness, such as ex-
However, a differential model (also known as a non- treme capitulation to the fleeting vagaries of thought
monotonic scale; Coombs, 1953) places an individual’s and perception vs. insufficient Openness, such as tram-
low, mid-range, or high score along an arcing, curvilin- meled thought) will certainly deliver findings of sub-
ear person dimension, with different positions on the stantive interest. But this simple, High-Low approach
dimension having quite different psychological impli- to analysis is oblivious to what it fails to reveal. When
cations. referenced against modal-or median-like or concep-
A cumulatively treated scale of the FFA provides tually, societally defined placements on each of the
a numerical score on its dimension but not a context five trait dimensions, the differential measurement ap-
of psychological meaning or implication—a frame of proach may reveal the existence of psychologically
reference—for that score. Conceptually, however, dif- crucial, nonlinear relationships—findings undetectable
ferential measurement is often necessary for conveying and unrecognizable via the use of cumulative scoring
the psychological implications or the “fitness” conse- and simplistic correlational analysis.
quent upon an individual’s place along the arc of a FFA For example, from long ago (Block & Thomas,
dimension. 1955), a measure of self-esteem, when evaluated cumu-
Thus, for example, does an individual’s particular latively, displays many personality differences between
Conscientiousness score on the NEO–PI–R imply ex- individuals at one extreme versus the other extreme of
cessive conscientiousness (such as extreme conformity, the self-esteem dimension. An individual with a very
capitulation to apparent consensus, being “plodding,” high level of self-esteem leads a more self-satisfying
“routinized,” “overly-diligent,” “obsessive,” etc.) or life than one with a very low level of self-esteem. How-
does it imply insufficient conscientiousness (being ever, when evaluated according to a differential model,
“undependable,” “careless,” “inattentive,” “breaking individuals with an appreciable self-esteem but also
of rules,” etc.), or imply an intermediate conscien- cognizant of certain personal imperfections prove to
tiousness level (being “persistent,” “organized,” “self- have personality qualities different from and psycho-
disciplined,” “self-controlled,” etc.), that is considered logically more favorable than those of individuals de-
normative, appropriate, and situationally adaptive? scribing themselves as absolutely without any personal
These distinctions are of consequence but are un- flaws or faults. Extreme, unqualified self-esteem seems
recognized and unspecified by conventional cumula- to reflect a narcissism and lack of self-reflection rather
tive scale analysis. We need to have norms or psy- than an achieved perfection.
7
BLOCK

Similarly, men with unwarranted confidence are im- ment with respect to a variety of what Meehl called
portantly different from men with unwarranted uncer- “surface traits” (“correlated response families”) and
tainty (a High/Low contrast), but both groups are each sometimes the “surface traits” of others. In the initial
interestingly different from men with confidence at- sample on which the FFM is based, the participants
tuned to the probabilistic realities of their external sit- tended to be relatively intelligent and of remarkably
uation (Block & Petersen, 1955). high education.8 Subsequently, the FFA was predom-
Another example: In the present cultural epoch, late inantly based on select,laypeople opportunely avail-
adolescents who are heavily into the drug scene are able or specially motivated and/or lured—college stu-
psychologically less internally and externally adap- dents or college proximate individuals whom Henrich,
tive than late adolescents totally abstaining from drugs. Heine, and Norenzayan (in press) called “weird” peo-
But total abstainers from drugs in the mid-1980s were ple: Participants from societies relatively W estern, rel-
psychologically less socially adaptive than adolescents atively Educated, relatively I ndustrialized, relatively
who have experimented somewhat with drugs (Shedler Rich, and relatively Democratized. Samples of rela-
& Block, 1990). tively uneducated, unskilled, culturally different, “sub-
As a last illustration of the value of differen- merged” individuals have rarely been studied.
tial analysis, comparing highly impulsive, relatively A central assumption of the FFA is that the use
uncontrolled individuals with highly restrained, rel- of everyday personality language can be used for sci-
atively overcontrolled individuals reveals many co- entific purposes. Hofstee (1990) thoughtfully contem-
gent personality differences but fails to reveal the plated this conjecture and concluded that “the natural
person-qualities characterizing resilient individuals, language of personality is difficult to define, to a great
that is, individuals averaging intermediate, situation- extent parochial, oversaturated with evaluative consid-
ally more adaptive places along this impulsivity- erations, unruly with respect to its internal structure”
constraint dimension (e.g., Block & Block, 1980). (p. 850). Elsewhere, he further remarked, “The over-
Although psychologically still too infrequently ap- whelming impression that arises from processing em-
plied, a differential rather than a cumulative approach pirical [language] data . . . is that substantive results
to personality measurement can provide crucial un- are fuzzy. Hardly any two persons using the same
derstandings not available by simple correlations or trait adjectives seem to mean precisely the same thing”
high–low comparisons. It also bespeaks provocatively (Hofstee, de Raad, & Goldberg, 1992, p. 162). Gold-
to evolutionary psychology in suggesting how herita- berg and Kilkowski (1985) also commented on the ap-
ble person variation has been maintained as a function preciable differences among respondents in their word
of the “fitness” disadvantages accruing to individuals understandings of common terms. To the extent ad-
at either extreme of a significant personality dimension jectives are indeed “fuzzily” interpreted, the obscuring
(Macdonald, 1995; Nettle, 2006). fuzziness also obviously applies to adjectival phrases
or the longer statements in questionnaires.

FFA Equivocality in Describing the Person


Continuing Personologist Differences in
Because of their research convenience and sub- Interpretation of the Five Factors
stantial usefulness, printed questionnaires or adjective
word lists administered to lay individuals have a long, The FFA represents an interspersing of ques-
long history in personality psychology. There are at tionnaire items or person-adjectives that can be
least two problems besetting the use of such question- personologist-organized into five orthogonal (unre-
naires and word lists—one regarding the commonal- lated) trait scales deemed crucial and presumed to be
ity of language discriminations among the laity and factorially sufficient. However, there prove to be large
a second regarding the conceptual “naming” language differences among psychologists in their understand-
employed by academic personologists attempting a rel- ings of these five scale domains when they are elab-
evant and apt nomenclature. There is also the separate orated as to their fuller psychological meanings. This
issue of the psychological authenticity of the state- is not surprising; as Meehl (1998) earlier despairingly
ments offered by the laypeople providing the data. noted, construct namings are usually problematic be-
cause they lack consensuality. The five named FFA
“Fuzziness” Among Laypersons in Their Usage domains, per se, have—without close consideration to
of Person-Descriptive Words resolve different construals—been largely presumed
The FFA is usually operationally realized through to be equivalently understood by all psychologists
investigator-convenient administration of question-
naires or word lists impersonally offered as understand- 8 In the original Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging, which
able by conveniently accessible or captive participants. Costa and McCrae first used to begin their questionnaire construc-
Participants respond via quickly indicating their place- tion, 25% of their participants held Ph.D.s

8
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

although often operationalized via a variety of FFA PI–R, necessarily six in number in accord with its com-
versions and language translations. mitted planned design, do not represent a coherent or
Even granting the current FFA, perplexities or advancing construct.10
questions or arguments remain with regard to the
interpretation of each of its offered five trait categories Conscientiousness. The dictionary offers two
and the trait assignment of various facets. From one conceptually very different—equally important and
of the representations of the FFA to another, the equally societally evaluative—meanings of the word:
psychological “flavorings” of the five factors can (i) Thorough or assiduous efforts to comply with ex-
differ substantially. Given these different connotative ternal or internalized regulations, and (ii) Guided by or
interpretations, the lay, societally evaluative namings in accordance with the ethical dictates of an internal,
of the five factors might have been better identified self-confronting conscience, being principled, person-
in more abstract, technical terms as in the Roman ally sensitive to issues of fairness and injustice.
numerals used in the original BF. FFA questionnaires completely ignore the ethical
aspect of the term Conscientiousness. They focus in-
Extraversion (initially called Surgency by Gold- stead and solely on the first meaning, as indicating the
berg). Historically, this factor has also been labeled respondent’s likely rule-abiding, diligence, assiduous-
as assertiveness, power, activity, positive emotion- ness, organization, perfectionism, whether the individ-
ality, and interpersonal involvement (Carroll, 2002, ual is a “good citizen.” However, pressed to an extreme,
p. 103). Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, (2006) such conscientious rule-abidingness—to learned rules
found it crucial to bifurcate Extraversion into the and prescribed behaviors—can be psychologically nar-
dimensions of social vitality and social dominance. rowing, lead to unquestioning conformance, stultified
Does this factor imply sociability as averred (Guilford, and highly controlled behavior eventuating in such per-
1977) or impulsivity (Eysenck, 1977) or a varying mix- son attributes as over-compliance, phlegmatism, pas-
ture of the two? Is the facet, impulsivity, better included sivity, and constraint. It might be more aptly labeled
within this FFA broad factor, as Goldberg preferred, or Constraint, as Tellegen (1985) suggested.11 It is of in-
better located within the Neuroticism factor, as fixed terest that Conscientiousness is often equated with the
in the facets of Costa and McCrae? Thus, there con- popular Rothbart developmental construct of Effortful
tinues to be confusion and contestation regarding var- Control (e.g., Caspi & Shiner, 2006; Evans & Rothbart,
ious construals of this factor. Different psychologists 2007).
project importantly differing views of this fundamen- As Loevinger (1994) trenchantly noted, the term
tal domain. The dictionary does not especially help: It Conscientiousness, when considered beyond its par-
is defined by the Merriam-Webster Collegiate Dictio- ticular interpretation within the FFA, has an essential,
nary (2003) as “the act, state, or habit of being pre- indisputable moral or ethical meaning—of principled
dominantly concerned with and obtaining gratification concern by the individual with what is right and what
from what is outside the self.” I submit that the word
extraversion, although by now having developed broad 10 I note that, crucially, the FFA is insensitive to two very different

colloquial usage, is not sufficiently univocal to permit ways respondents may earn high scores on Neuroticism. Respon-
serious scientific usage. dents high on N who conjointly score high on FFA Conscientious-
ness (which incorporates much of what earlier was called overcontrol
or overmonitoring of expressiveess) manifest a different pattern of
Neuroticism (called by the BF, when reversed, maladaptive response than respondents equivalently high on N but
Emotional Stability). As Loevinger (1957) early re- conjointly scoring low on the FFA Cscale (wherein low C scores im-
marked, “neurosis is far too variable in its manifesta- ply much of what earlier was called undercontrol or undermonitoring
tions to conform to a scale model” (p. 675), which, of expressiveness). Illustrations of these different response patterns
are reported in Dahlstrom, Welsh, and Dahlstrom (1960/1975). Some
however, the FFA indeed applies. It is also suggestive
items exemplifying Neurotic Overcontrol (NOC), all answered True,
that the American Psychiatric Association, beginning are “I am slow in making up my mind”; “I am afraid of deep water”;
with its Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Men- “I usually feel nervous and ill at ease at a party or dance.” Some items
tal Disorders (3rd ed., rev.), no longer diagnostically exemplifying Neurotic Undercontrol (NUC), all answered True, are
employs the term neurotic or variants thereof.9 This “I must admit I often try to get my own way regardless of what
others may want”; I often do whatever makes me feel cheerful here
FFA domain impresses me as an overinclusive, easy-
and now, even at the cost of some distant goal”; “There have been a
to-invoke, societally evaluative wastebasket label for few times when I have been very mean to another person.” The NOC
an unwieldy hodgepodge of quite different person- and NUC scales correlate only slightly with each other, but each
qualities. It can be understood globally as referring to is a strong indicator of susceptibility to personal distress. The six
susceptibility to personal distress, however induced. I a priori personally posited FFM Neuroticism facets do not provide
a fundamental, cleaving distinction between these two fundamental
suggest that the different N facets offered by the NEO–
modes of maladaptation.
11 Indeed, the hard-working, entirely dependable, organized, Holo-
9 Consider, if you will, that a chronically depressed individual
caust implementer Adolf Eichmann would likely have scored quite
might also be called Emotionally Stable! highly on the conventional FFA Conscientiousness measure.

9
BLOCK

is wrong, as experienced introspectively or manifested predominantly emphasize the intellectual basis, per se,
behaviorally. It depends on achieving what is, for too of this factor (Saucier, 1992).
many, often a developmentally unachieved life phase-
transition. It involves the existence of a developed inner The Restricted Understanding of
life, a self-awareness on which ethical, morally based Person-Differences by Perfunctionary
conscientiousness depends. Conscientiousness in its Questioning of Lay Respondents
ethical sense, although unrepresented within the FFA,
is psychologically essential and therefore warrants There is an unacknowledged or unweighted recog-
personological representation. The Honesty-Humility nition by FFA proponents of the inherent problems re-
sixth factor identified and pressed by Ashton and Lee siding with lay report over and beyond issues of word
(2005) to some extent responds to the ethical insensi- understanding.
tivity of the FFA. First, the FFA implicitly and sometimes explic-
itly assumes respondents are essentially veridical and
Agreeableness. This societally relevant FFA earnestly answer in good faith. Generally, FFA investi-
factor—perhaps better named Affiliativeness—reflects gators do not check seriously or adjust for the possible
an individual’s interpersonal behavioral tendencies. presence of respondent misrepresentation although it
Self- (or other-) awarded adjectives related to this has been repeatedly demonstrated that FFM question-
Agreeableness factor are forgiveness, gentleness, naire responses readily can be faked (e.g., Viswesvaran
friendliness, flexibility, patience (Graziano & Tobin, & Ones, 1999).12 Transparently obvious self-report
2002). Such ascribed adjectives likely have a certain measures can be quick, are certainly not necessarily
amount of validity even when self-attributed and also untrue, and are much improved if some subtly effec-
when attributed to others. However, individuals po- tive validity indices such as are in the MMPI–2–RF
litely and prudently responding to a FFA questionnaire (Tellegen & Ben-Porath, 2008) are also incorporated
may well have a tendency to be self-protective and to identify deliberately faking respondents. Accord-
carefully deferent and may erect a favorable social ingly, the contemporary FFA can be expected to be
facade. Because of such response bias, an experienced, afflicted to some extent by misrepresentations from
perhaps cynical questionnaire-user such as Eysenck the targeted individual of his or her latent “surface”
(1992) can view this factor (and self-awarded Con- characteristics and by the protective “glossings” of
scientiousness as well) as empirically and importantly familiars.
displaying much self-favoring impression manage- Second, besides the possibility of deliberate faking,
ment, and even downright lying. Of interest, among FFA lay respondents can be expected to be generally
additional characteristics attributed by others to lay society-abiding and socially prudent, unsurely coop-
individuals scoring high on this factor are compliance, erative, and of unknown psychological-mindedness in
submissiveness, and undiscriminating affiliativeness. their awareness of self and of others. In responding
to questionnaires, the reacting person—although “hon-
Openness. The Openness to Experience factor est” in response—may be unwittingly defensive or may
earlier had been called Intellect within the lexical BF. not be sufficiently self-observant and insightful to be
As embodied in somewhat transmogrified form for authentically revealing. In describing “familiar” others,
the purposes of the NEO–PI–R inventory (Costa & equivalent concerns apply—Is the informant truly pro-
McCrae, 1992), it has been richly described by viding his or her impressions of a mate or friend? Is the
McCrae and Costa (1997) as characterizing a literate, informant being protective in characterizing the other?
questing, omnivorous intelligence: individuals high on Is the informant sufficiently self- or other-observant
imaginativeness, aesthetically resonant, attentive to in- and insightful to offer usefully incisive information?
ner feelings, preferring variety, intellectually curious, Third, with increasing frequency, participants are
and (therefore) scoring higher on various intelligence encountering a five-factor measure from a distance,
measures. Empirically, it merits mention that in some via the Internet. There seems to be a compelling se-
samples, the Openness factor sometimes does not re- ductiveness to many investigators of convenience per
liably emerge in analyses (e.g., De Raad, 1998). Of se—the accumulation of computer-ready data from
importance, it appears to reflect a person-quality that happenstance online visitors to an Internet five-factor
often is developmentally unachieved or has atrophied measure.
over the years. When something akin to it empirically However, a recent study (Denissen & Penke, 2008,
does emerge, it perhaps can be more fundamentally p. 1291) illustrates some of the quirky sampling prob-
interpreted conceptually as an indication of percep- lems afflicting an impersonal Internet FFA approach.
tual and cognitive responsivity (not only behavioral Their online questionnaire (including the BFI) was no-
responsivity), of being exquisitely attentionally sensi- ticed by 3,909 Internet visitors over several months.
tive and proactively responsive to the affordances in
one’s environmental surround. But some continue to 12 But see Hirsch (2008) for a potential corrective.

10
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

Of these, 2,923 contributed some data. Of this reduced of competing, variously similar but often uncompared
sample, 5 did not show any variance in their responses, five-factor questionnaire versions, in different lan-
33 acknowledged they had not honestly responded, 112 guages, often uncertainly equivalent—all supportive
were excluded because of the remarkably excessive of the magical number, five.
time they took to complete the 5-min questionnaire,
81 were omitted because of name confusion, and 579
were deleted because they were younger than the re- Going Beyond the Person-Descriptions
quired age of 16, resulting in a factor analysis sample Afforded by the FFA
of 2,113—71% female, 48% without a high school
degree. For some more uncertain psychologists, as the FFA
It would seem the current research ethos is that came onto the stage and its popularity quickened, a
research with participants should not take much inves- gnawing question emerged: Were the BF and the subse-
tigator time and be especially designed for trouble-free quent FFM empirically entitled to their claims of suffi-
computer data processing. In the incessant search for cient encompassment of individual differences among
quicker research, some have proceeded from the 240 persons? Regarding the BF, was the developed lexi-
item NEO-PI–R to the more “efficient,” 5-min, 44- cal universe of common trait descriptors exhaustively
item Big Five Inventory (BFI; John, Donahue, & Ken- sampled? Regarding the FFM, was the NEO–PI–R all-
tle, 1991). There even has been offered simulacra of inclusive of those aspects of character personologists
the longer FFM questionnaire—1-min, 10-item short deemed crucial to consider?
forms (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003; Rammst-
edt & John, 2007). Rumor hath it that an even more
efficient 30-sec, 5-item form may be in the offing. Regarding the Inclusiveness of the BF
Fourth, for those professionally involved in close,
intensive, and conditional person-evaluation, much of At junctures in the procedural and evaluative pro-
the nominal information provided by lay respondents cess initially resulting in the BF, Goldberg and his
to the extant five-factor inventories is not especially coworkers had invoked various adjective selection cri-
illuminating. A layperson’s response to FFA inventory teria that some questioned. Certainly, the totality of
questions is not seen as bearing on deeply personal— factor analysis congruences in self- and peer-ratings
even threatening—issues such as, for example, nar- by available college students and lay samples was im-
cissism, hostility, sexuality, repression, jealousy, com- pressive. But only single-word adjective descriptors—
pulsivity, or highly private, embarrassing, unusual, taken alone and without context—were used and the
bizarre proclivities. In almost all circumstances, the sufficiency of single adjectives for the task of seri-
perfunctory, impersonal administration to polite or ous person evaluation could be questioned. For many
captive or otherwise induced lay respondents to FFA personologists, the adjectival approach was viewed as
questionnaires understandably activates a layperson’s providing “surface trait” description of persons rather
protective façade of socially appropriate self- or other- than “source trait” understanding of persons.13
presentation. For this reason, FFA self-report inven- In addition, some of the guiding decisions eventu-
tories are widely perceived as lacking a depth of ating in the then BF were troublesome to many. In par-
connection with respondents. Thus, there is a level ticular, consequential, subjective, a priori exclusion-
of psychological understanding that the FFA question- ary rules regarding what “lies outside the Big Five”
naire approach simply cannot reach and reveal. There framework were invoked. Thus, excluded from consid-
is more to the study and understanding of individual eration were common-language adjectives rarely em-
person-differences than gathering unthreatening and ployed (although rarity of usage of a word need not
foreclosed self-report or person-descriptions by ac- bely its great importance)–those with low empirical
quaintances. base rates, those not used in polite parlance, adjec-
In summary, despite the currently satisfied adher- tives decided as correlating too highly with the already
ents of the proclaimed FFA, the deeper, larger issues achieved five cluster-dimensions. Adjectives describ-
surrounding these five broad trait categories continue ing physical characteristics were ruled out as irrele-
for those less satisfied. For the five settled-upon factors, vant for understanding individual differences, although
there remain issues as to their psychological meanings, it is acknowledged more generally that such physical
the warrant of their labels, their denotative meaning and characteristics have important interpersonal (and there-
connotative implications, their conceptual incisiveness fore personality) implications (e.g., attractiveness–
and sufficiency as a frame for understanding the per- ugliness, tallness–shortness, obesity–skinniness, etc.).
son and the developmental progression of temperament
within the person. 13 Meehl talked of “surface traits” as “correlated response fami-
In the meanwhile, the claimed “emerging consen- lies” and “source traits” is Meehl’s term for diverse but psychologi-
sus” regarding the FFA has resulted in a cacophony cally related expressions of the “latent source”

11
BLOCK

Aware of the frequently expressed concerns that procedure they evaluated retrieve their FFM five-factor
the BF adjective domain may not be all-inclusive, structure and perhaps more than their five factors?
Saucier and Goldberg (1998) sought to further eval- Among the methods in which they reported
uate whether, indeed, “the lexical perspective provides successfully “recovering” their five-factor structure
an unusually strong rationale for the selection of vari- were Jackson’s Personality Research Form (Costa &
ables in personality research” (p. 499). They searched McCrae, 1988) embodying Murray’s needs, the widely
for the possible dimensions “beyond the Big Five” and and long-used Minnesota Multiphasic Personality In-
indeed did find many outlier clusters. However, the BF ventory (Costa, Busch, Zonderman, & McCrae, 1986),
adjective set remained unchanged by virtue of a conclu- the popular California Psychological Inventory (Mc-
sion that these outliers were “generally covering con- Crae, Costa, & Piedmont, 1993), and what they called
tent not conventionally defined . . . for describing per- the CQS, more widely known as the California Adult
sonality as personality is conventionally understood” Q-set (CAQ) (Block, 1961, 2008). For obvious filial
(p. 520). Saucier and Goldberg acknowledged only that reasons, I focus here on their CAQ analysis, first to
“potentially” (their italics), various independent adjec- describe the basis of their interpretation and then to
tive clusters may be warranted “when one wishes to suggest a contrary view.
extend variable, i.e., adjective, selection outside the The CAQ is a set of person-evaluating statements
domain of personality traits as conventionally defined” commensurately and independently used by experi-
(pp. 495–496). enced and trained clinical psychologists and psychi-
Unconvinced by these claims of how personality atrists, often aggregated, to assess the relative salience
should be “conventionally” defined, Paunonen and of person-qualities in a closely-scrutinized individual.
Jackson (2000), methodologically sophisticated in- Typically, in research, the qualities of a person are
vestigators, in their own later scrutiny of the very denoted by independently formulated, commensurate,
same data, identified “plenty” of trait descriptor out- subsequently aggregated assessments by clinicians or
liers not veritably describable by the adjectives within person-assessors. It is not intended for use by individ-
the established BF. Illustrative descriptor outliers they ual laypeople to describe themselves, their spouses,
found that were simply not available within the lexi- or their peer-acquaintances because it assumes that
cal BF were, for example, masculinity–femininity, hu- self- or spouse- or peer-descriptions are often not in-
morous and witty, honest and ethical, manipulative formationally trustworthy or psychologically percep-
and sly, variety- and experience-seeking, sensual and tive enough. Also, the nonproprietary CAQ is person
sexy.14 The Paunonen and Jackson analyses, using only oriented, not variable oriented. It is not so quick or
slightly different criteria, strongly suggested the need convenient or inexpensive to employ as are hand-out
for a more inclusive view of person-qualities than was questionnaires or adjective lists.
encountered via the self-descriptive adjectives or peer- McCrae, Costa, and Busch (1986) described their
descriptive adjectives within the BF. For very many, analysis of the CAQ as representing
the additional descriptors discerned by Paunonen and
Jackson appeared salient and necessary, personologi- a unique opportunity to test the generality of the
cally. five-factor model . . . because the content of the CQS
. . . was determined solely by the judgments from psy-
chodynamically oriented clinicians. . . . If the five-
factor model can be recovered from the items of the
Regarding the FFM and the Vaunted CQS, its claim to comprehensiveness will be consid-
“Recovery” of the Five-Factor Model in erably strengthened. (p. 432)
Alternative Person-Descriptive Procedures
In the McCrae et al. study, rather than having serious
Costa and McCrae, after assembling the five-factor clinicians each portray a commensurate evaluation of
NEO–PI, embarked on a wide-ranging research pro- a subject, participants were mailed the CAQ and were
gram to justify their five-factor approach. However, asked to describe themselves at home via the CAQ pro-
their strategy centrally depended on an interpretive cedure. They also responded, via mail, to an early ver-
asymmetry that is logically unappreciated. In their hec- sion of the NEO–PI and to adjectives representing the
tic early body of work, they sought to demonstrate the BF.
“validity” or universality of their five-factor model by In their factor analysis of the self-sorted CAQ items,
“recovering” their postulated five factors from a variety an orthogonal five-factor varimax solution was first
of preexistent person-evaluating procedures. They did sought. To better fit their data, however, and because, as
not ask the reverse question: Can the person-evaluating they acknowledged, “factor interpretation is a subjec-
tive process” (p. 439), it was decided to further rotate
14 I note that each of these “outliers” could readily become in- two of their factors by 30◦ . They concluded that the
disputable factors were each to be redundantly represented so as to content of the five CAQ factors resembles the content
build up their communalities. of the FFM-based and BF-based five factors. Subject
12
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

CAQ-based five-factor scores proved to correlate at- tivated and relatively qualified observers. Lanning’s
tractively with the FFM and BF factor scores of the par- major analysis used CAQ data from the Institute of Per-
ticipants and also with FFM and BF scores alternatively sonality Assessment and Research of the University of
derived from spouses and from participant-nominated California at Berkeley, based on five-observer compos-
friends. Even further, McCrae et al. reported that CAQ ites available for each of 940 participants (58% male).
five-factor descriptions by available watchers of a 40- Participants had been observed in groups of 10 over
min life-history videotape of each of more than 100 sustained, interactional weekends. Observer-judges
members of their participant pool also showed a con- were present during close participant interviews, lead-
vergence with the FFM and BF model. Watchers were erless discussion groups, a variety of social settings
described as technicians, psychologists (of unspecified (meals, coffee breaks, social games like charades, etc);
background), and a single psychiatrist. Later, a single observers were members of the Institute of Personality
second judge (of unspecified background) viewing the Assessment and Research professional staff and ad-
videotapes provided impressions of each of the more vanced graduate students in personality psychology. A
than 100 participants via the NEO–PI. total of 61 observer-judges provided CAQs, with no
I do not question that this “recovery program” judge serving on more than four weekends. This ardu-
educed the intended five factors; I would be distraught ous research arrangement represents an approach and
otherwise. I do question the widely referenced conclu- commitment by assessors at an entirely different level
sion in their abstract that “these findings strongly sup- than simply mailing the CAQ to available participants
port the claim to comprehensiveness of the five-factor for self-evaluation homework and return.
model” (p. 430). When Lanning submitted the accumulated CAQ
First, to comment more specifically on their study, composites to a five-factor analysis, CAQ factors like
note that in their CAQ item-factor analysis, McCrae Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Extraversion clearly
et al. reported obtaining as many as 32 eigenval- emerge, a CAQ factor somewhat similar to the Open-
ues greater than 1.0, the widely recognized mathe- ness factor is seen, and the last CAQ factor appears
matical threshold above which factors can be said to to connote self- or executive-or ego-control more than
exist. However, this 1.0 threshold is influenced by data the work-ethic typically central to Conscientiousness
noise, and so establishing the number of stable, replica- as expressed within the FFM.
ble factors within a particular factor analysis has long When an eight-factor CAQ solution was tried, three
presented a difficult problem to users of the method. additional factors emerged, reflecting Physical Attrac-
Beyond their five-factor solution, McCrae et al. tried tiveness (including heterosexual interest and charm),
an eight-factor solution of their CAQ item intercor- Insight, and Ambition (similar to the additional factor
relations. “In the eight-factor solution, three new fac- noted by McCrae et al., 1986).
tors emerged, along with versions of the original five.” When an 11-factor CAQ solution was computed,
One of these three new factors betokens introspec- additional factors representing Hostile Candor, Nar-
tion and a complicated inner life, a second new fac- cissism, and Humor were found. When a 15-factor
tor reflects forcefulness in social interaction, and the CAQ solution was sought, factors of Sensuality and
third acknowledges physical attractiveness as influ- Somatization appeared and the previous Insight factor
encing one’s personality. They also found Neuroticism split into factors of Social Acuity and Concern with
to be much more articulated than a summary label Motives.
would justify and found the emergence of erotic inter- An especially interesting—and implicative—
ests. Unfortunately, they did not pursue these findings; analysis by Lanning involved partialling the five-
perhaps increasing the number of factors would in- factors derived from analysis of a large sample (N =
crease the predictability of real-life criteria (Mershon 822) of his CAQs from the CAQs of the sample remain-
and Gorsuch, 1988). ing (N = 118). His Table 4 reveals 21 CAQ items that,
In their Discussion section, they acknowledge that after five-factor partialling, were reliable but could not
be represented within the common five-factor space
whether the five-factor model is judged to be adequate (e.g., physically attractive, skillful in social play, mas-
or not in part depends on what one chooses to include
culinity/femininity, heterosexual interest, initiates hu-
within the definition of personality . . . researchers and
mor, sees self as attractive) and therefore were not
clinicians should recognize that measurement of the
five factors themselves gives a complete [italics added] amenable to the FFA.
characterization of the person only at a global [italics Lanning noted a number of reasons for this greater
added] level. . . . The factors represent groups of traits complexity of results from the CAQ: First, there is a
that covary but are not necessarily interchangeable. great difference between single-word adjectives and
(pp. 443–444) simple self-statements as in the FFA and, alternatively,
CAQ items such as “is uncomfortable with uncer-
A later article (Lanning, 1994) offers further per- tainty and complexity,” “feels a lack of meaning in
spective on the CAQ, when used as intended by mo- life,” “has repressive or dissociative tendencies,” “is
13
BLOCK

self-dramatizing, histrionic.” Such CAQ items have a tily candorous, narcissistic, somatizing of problems,
clinical origin; equivalent single-word adjectives are socially acute, and concerned with motives. It appears
unlikely to emerge in lexical sampling and, accord- that lay characterizations in terms of the five broad
ingly, “the CAQ may include characteristics that are factors may not provide a rich description of an in-
poorly represented by trait adjectives” (p. 159).15 dividual person. As Allport (1961) warned, “common
Second, “CAQ items differ in breadth as well as in speech is a poor guide to psychological subtleties” (p.
content from single-word or self-statement descriptors. 356). It may be sufficient for expressing “folk con-
The use of conjunctions, qualifiers, elaborations, and cepts,” but there may well be insufficiencies in lay
context inherently reduces the scope of these items. common-language for characterizing the complexities
Trait adjectives, particularly those in common use, are and unfoldings of human nature. “As scientific con-
typically broader” (John, Hampson, & Goldberg, 1991, structs [personality conceptions] should be formulated
p. 159). The trait adjective, sensitive, for example, can with no necessary regard for their usability and under-
refer to a number of CAQ items, including “is thin- standability by laypersons” (Block, 1995a, p. 188).
skinned, sensitive to criticism,” “is esthetically reac-
tive,” and “seems to be aware of impression s/he makes Transformational Developments in Regard
on others.” to the FFA
Third, there is widespread acceptance in medi-
cal and other fields that specialists can validly dis- By the end of the 20th century, the proprietary FFM,
cern and effectively communicate to others distinc- although having become preponderant in published
tions not discernible by laypeople (see, e.g., Tanaka (and unpublished) research on personality, had also
& Taylor, 1991). It is a natural extrapolation from this been recognized as having insufficiencies, method-
well-accepted recognition that psychologically sophis- ological intertwined with the conceptual.
ticated, experienced, disinterested person-evaluators Consequent upon relying on the factor analytic
can be expected to provide more differentiated under- model, it had always been recognized that understand-
standings than opportunely available lay FFM or BF ings of the person may exist at several different hier-
responders. I suggest that lay respondents to imper- archically related levels. At the very lowest level is the
sonally administered, printed self- or other-inventories person-discriminant, the particular behavior or expres-
are often motivationally not deeply involved, are usu- sion by the person of his or her position with respect
ally socially prudent, and often are not psychologically to a matter of personological interest. If sufficiently
minded enough in their awareness of self or of others. covarying, a collection of discriminants can conceptu-
Fourth, there is no question that the repeatedly ob- ally warrant being aggregated to form what have been
served, famed five-factors indeed can be found within called a facet. Facets, in turn, if justified by their inter-
the CAQ and can be “recovered.” But their “necessity correlations, may further warrant further aggregation
does not imply sufficiency, despite inferences in the to constitute a factor-domain—one of the five factors.
literature that the five factors ‘underlie,’ ‘comprehen- (And, in turn, the factor-domains, to the extent they
sively characterize,’ ‘subsume,’ and ‘account . . . for’ correlate, may imply the more ultimate presence of
the CAQ factor structure” (Lanning, 1994, p. 159). “higher order” superfactors or “metatraits.”)
By reversing the research question posed by Mc- It will be remembered that, in developing the
Crae et al. (1986), it is clear from both their own anal- FFM initially, Costa and McCrae implemented a per-
yses and from the later Lanning study that although sonal, prior, unexpressed, but result-shaping theoreti-
the five-factor structure of the FFM or BF indeed can cal model that achieved wide usage in the field. Af-
be found within the CAQ, the CAQ’s extensity of con- ter evolving their three desired NEO factor scales,
tent importantly cannot be recovered solely from the which they characterized as representing broad “do-
five-factor structure. Many reliably positioned CAQ mains,” they partitioned each of these three factor
items are not redundant enough (quite deliberately) scales domains into six lower level subdimensions or
to emerge as factors and so provide little computer “facets,” with each facet consisting of eight NEO-
grist from which the factor analytic algorithm can inventory statements or “person discriminants.” Cru-
make them factors. As a consequence, the delimited cially, in faceting each of their three factor scales, they
FFA cannot express such psychologically important appear to have relied on their own subjective under-
person qualities, as noted by Paunonen, Jackson, and standings of the personality literature as supplemented
Lanning, as: masculine-feminine, humorous and witty, by their own dyadic decisions. It is unclear how much
honest and ethical, manipulative and sly, sensual and of the machinery of factor analysis was employed at
sexy, physically attractive, charming, ambitious, hos- this junction.
Subsequently—to enhance comparability and con-
15 I suggest that the short, simple sentences in the FFM ques- nection with Goldberg’s prior BF—Costa and McCrae
tionnaire often carry no more information than single-word trait constructed two new inventory scales analogous to the
descriptors. two BF factors they lacked and added them to their
14
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

three-factor NEO–PI. The two new scales—targeting last century—attributing it to the encumbering domi-
Conscientousness and Agreeableness—were also con- nance in the field by conveniently available but propri-
formed to their preestablished pattern of six facets, etary, insufficient instruments—he proposed a public
each consisting of eight inventory items. domain approach freely and extensively available to all.
The Costa and McCrae presentation of their now He further recognized that the most catholic approach
FFM into lower level facets was generally not ques- to person-assessment would be to focus on the full,
tioned and proved convenient and attractive to many perhaps ever-expanding range of narrow-bandwidth
subsequent NEO–PI–R users. However, although person-discriminants rather than focusing on preor-
based on thoughtful, considered judgment, the facets dained, broad band width factors (the current IPIP
specified for each domain were bothersome to many as includes far more than 2,000 items, many of them
well; the particular facets posited would have been due to the Gronigen contingent of Hofstee, de Raad,
importantly different had psychologists of different and Hendriks and has been translated into more than
persuasion done the positing and/or had a different 25 languages). He further acknowledged that single-
set of inventory statements been involved. Thus, there word person-descriptors, as prioritized earlier in his
have been conceptual, disputative, jangling questions “Lexical Hypothesis,” are importantly deficient when
regarding these prescribed FFM facets, their meaning, international usage is envisaged and that a more con-
and appropriate domain placement. The study seek- textualized, somewhat longer common item format
ing to demonstrate the discriminant validity of the of short verbal phrases is more desirable. Finally,
30 NEO–I–R facet scales (McCrae & Costa, 1992) he envisaged the enormous potential afforded by the
via their patterns of correlation with person-adjectives Internet: mutual research communication, easy data in-
found clear differences between the factors but unclear terchange, and cumulative, refining progress in ques-
distinctions among the posited facets of a factor. tionnaire construction. His cost-free, public domain,
In the intervening years, the proprietary NEO–PIR asymptotically growing IPIP item universe is readily
has not been subject to further change beyond some and flexibly available worldwide to investigators and
psychometric tunings within a few of its a priori facet already has catalyzed almost 100 publications.
scales, resulting in the issuing of a modestly improved The research possibilities afforded by Goldberg’s
NEO–PI–3 (McCrae, Costa, & Martin, 2005). Further IPIP may be exemplified by (but are not limited to) an
or radical changes in the NEO–PI do not seem to be early application by him employing data from his large
envisaged by its proponents. “The five factors do not Eugene-Springfield community sample (ESCS). The
exhaust the description of personality; they merely rep- participants had earlier been administered the NEO–
resent the highest [italics added] hierarchical level of PI–R questionnaire and individual scores on the 30
trait description” (Costa & McCrae, 1995). That is, al- NEO facets had been calculated. However, also and
though the facets exist at a lower level of the factor separately, the ESCS participants had been adminis-
hierarchy than the five factors, the FFM posits there is tered the then IPIP. Each item of the IPIP was corre-
not a higher level of person understanding above the lated with each of the 30 NEO–PI–R facet scores sep-
five factors. McCrae and Sutin (2007) recently have arately available, and the IPIP items relating to each
offered an extensive “preview” of possible future five- of the NEO facet scales were identified. This elaborate
factor research that is limited entirely to research with but now computer-easy analysis issued IPIP 10-item
the existing NEO–PI–R. scales faithfully representing each of the 30 NEO facet
This historical recounting of the FFM’s facets and scales. Indeed, psychometrically, the IPIP empirically
scales sets the stage for understanding some devel- based facet scales appear slightly superior to the ini-
opments in the new century. In particular, Goldberg tially proposed and now proprietary NEO facet scales,
(1999)—the BF progenitor but also a long contribu- correlating. 94 with them when corrected for attenua-
tor to and innovator of person-assessment by means of tion. However, and crucially, the IPIP 10-item scales
inventory responses—has proposed some fundamen- empirically indexing the 30 NEO–P–R facet scales are
tal changes in the way personality inventories might not proprietarily dependent on or limited to the NEO–
be better developed and further evolve. Goldberg’s PI–R and may be further modified or improved at will.
projected changes initially languished but more re- Many equivalent analyses exist and testify to the
cently have generated much attention and consequence fecundity of this enlarging public domain approach. It
(Goldberg et al., 2006). would appear that many kinds of already established
His vision involved establishing a freely avail- personality scales can be duplicated and likely bet-
able “broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality in- tered by Internet processing of the extensive, readily
ventory measuring the lower-level facets” of person- accessible resources of the IPIP and already available
behavior (which he called the International Personal- databases.
ity Item Pool, or IPIP). Goldberg’s specific sugges- As a further instance, a recent IPIP analysis claims
tions were several: Because he was dismayed at the attention (DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson, 2007). These
slow pace of progress by personality assessment in the investigators were motivated to factor—for each of the
15
BLOCK

five domains—the ordained facets of the NEO–PI–R. personality may exist at several different hierarchi-
In addition, because the AB5C two-dimensional cir- cally related factor levels. However, with respect to
cumplex structures generated by Hofstee et al. (1992) the FFM, the full implications of this recognition had
provided better coverage of the range of personality- been evaded. Thus, as noted earlier, Costa and McCrae
behaviors than the NEO–PI–R facets provided by noted that the five factors are not exhaustive of per-
Costa and McCrae, they also studied the scope of the sonality, but rather represent the highest level of trait
publically available AB5C–IPIP facets. description. The claim is that there is not a higher level
Referencing the large ESCS sample, each of the above the five factors.
five FFA inventory scales, when factor-analyzed, However, because of the empirically observed, re-
displayed two subfactors The Neuroticism items peated correlations among the five broad factors con-
suggested separation into two subfactors, labeled With- stituting the FFM, there arose an interest in factoring
drawal and Volatility,16 the Extraversion items sug- efforts going beyond the promoted five—to seek higher
gested subfactors of Enthusiasm and Assertiveness, the order, more abstract factors of wider, more general im-
Agreeableness items grouped into Compassion and Po- plication than those of the “basic” five-factor model.
liteness subfactors, the Conscientiousness items sug- The supposed orthogonality of the FFM had long
gested subfactors of Industriouness and Orderliness, been recognized as presumed rather than real; such
and the Openness/Intellect facets grouped into subfac- orthogonality had been imposed by the use of a par-
tors of Intellect and Openness. These subfactors were ticular method of analysis and rotation. Empirically,
hierarchically positioned above the ordained facets of factor dimensions ordinarily are not orthogonal and, in
the NEO–PI–R or established AB5C–IPIP facets but principle, they should not truly be expected to be. If
below the five FFA “domain” scales; they were more extracted by a mathematical algorithm that is psycho-
than facets but less than domain factors. Requiring a logically innocent, orthogonality may be assured, but it
distinctive name, these conceptually attractive subfac- is quickly lost when the mathematical abstractions are
tors were dubbed “aspects.” given psychological content, typically implemented by
Having established factor scores reflecting each of evaluating item summations selected to represent the
the 10 different “aspects,” it was easy enough—in these factors. Although FFM dimensional scores ostensibly
computer times—then to correlate each aspect with should be orthogonal, empirically they are not—each
each of the more than 2,000 public domain IPIP items of the five factor dimensions is indeed correlationally
administered to the ESCS. By so doing, and identi- linked with other of the five factors. This recurrent ob-
fying those items that were especially good markers servation obviously suggested to many the relevance
of an “aspect,” it proved possible to create an assess- and perhaps usefulness of seeking the consequence of a
ment instrument indexing each of the 10 BF aspects. higher order factor analysis than the earlier-proclaimed
With a preliminary version of their Big Five Aspect FFM.
Scales in hand, a quite different participant sample Digman (1997) was the first to act upon this recog-
substantially cross-validated and refined their effort. nition. He took 14 different correlation matrices each
Their Table 4 provides compelling evidence psycho- showing five-factor solutions. In various, reasonably
metrically and conceptually of the usefulness of func- sensible ways, he allowed the five factors of each ma-
tioning at the aspect level intermediate to the previ- trix to achieve its natural nonorthogonality. Sometimes
ously offered, dyadically based, narrower facet level he computed factor scores by factor loading weight-
and the overly broad BF factor level. It is the unusually ings, sometimes he took the intercosines of axes from
deep resources of the IPIP that allowed psychometric a nonorthogonal (promax) rotational solution, often he
creation of replicable, public domain scales to index took the correlations among trait scales designed to re-
the newly available 10 aspects of the BF. Conceptual, flect the five factors; his samples were of different ages
empirical aspects might well replace usage of the pre- and culture.
scribed, NEO–PI–R offered FFM facets. Digman’s analysis of these 14 five-factor correlation
matrices—each one based on the correlations among
the five nonorthogonal factors—indicated to him the
Converging Toward Higher Order Personality general existence of two robust higher order factors
Superfactors above the BF. These were interpreted by him in broad,
abstract terms. His first, repeatedly observed, higher
Deeper analyses of the FFA approach—particularly order factor, which he non specifically termed Alpha,
the FFM—have long been occurring. Theoretically, it he suggested as representing what he called “the social-
had always been recognized that understandings of ization process,” involving the FFM factors reflecting
Neuroticism (reversed), Conscientiousness, and Agree-
16 The two mid-1950 scales, NOC and NUC—by their ableness. These three converging factors implied to
construction—appear to make the same basic distinction with respect him an Emotional Stability he conjecturally viewed as
to Neuroticism as the recent Withdrawal and Volatility “aspects.” integral to the necessary basis for any stable society.
16
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

The second higher order factor of Digman, also non- it seems fair to suggest it is the Big Two higher order
specifically named, was Beta, and vaguely interpreted factors from which all lower order factors hierarchi-
by him as “personal growth” (vs. “personal constric- cally devolve.
tion”). He viewed it in more uncertain, groping terms In response to these accumulating, separate findings
such as Maslow’s (1962) notion of “self actualization,” of higher order factors existing above the FFM, a per-
Tellegen’s Positive Emotionality, a venturesome en- haps countering argument has appeared from McCrae
countering of life, and surgent imaginativeness. Again et al. (2008). They suggest that the repeated finding
referencing the prior established FFM five-factors, he of two higher order factors admits of quite another
found this higher order factor stems from Extraversion interpretation: “An alternative is that they are method
conjoined with Openness to Experience. artifacts and that the five factors are themselves orthog-
Becker (1999), in his own higher order analysis of onal” (p. 443). They suggest that the existence of higher
the FFA, came up with two similar higher order su- order factors derives from the existence of informant-
perfactors, which he labeled “mental health” and “be- specific effects and that “such informant specific ef-
havior control.” Carroll (2002), a well-respected factor fects are generally interpreted as artifacts, biases that
analyst but one not versed in personality issues, in a contribute to observed scores because of the method
posthumously published and largely unnoted hierar- used rather than as a reflection of the true score”
chical analysis of a previously unworked data set of (p. 443).
teacher ratings, found the usual five-factors at an in- As viewed in these terms, there is a contrast be-
termediate level but two higher order factors above the tween two sources of FFM questionnaire variance:
proclaimed “big five.” He chose to call them “general (a) “artifact,” informant-specific effects arising from
goodness of personality” and “personal growth” versus differently describing sources, such as self, parent,
“personal constriction.” DeYoung, Peterson, and their spouse, acquaintance, an aggregate of multiple in-
coworkers in a series of diverse studies (DeYoung, formants, and so on, and (b) “substance,” the “true”
Hasher, Djikic, Criger, & Peterson, 2007; DeYoung, or “real” scores revealed by the five factors. McCrae
2006; DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2002; DeYoung, et al. view informant-specific effects as an expression
Peterson, Seguin, & Tremblay, 2008), distilled their of “artifact” extraneously influencing five factor scores,
several five-factor matrices into two equivalent higher and therefore believe these effects should be excluded,
order factors—the Big Two—they chose to christen as thus allowing the true “substance” within the FFM to
“Stability” and “Plasticity.”17 emerge.
These several studies, all arising from different per- From this presumption—not uniformly accepted—
suasions and different sets of data, testify to the stur- that informant specific effects are generally interpreted
diness of the findings: They all agree in grouping to- as “artifacts,” they proceed through a complicated se-
gether Emotional Stability (i.e., Neuroticism, reversed), ries of analyses and conclude that “there are alternative
Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness as constituting interpretations of the present data” (p. 452). They ac-
the one superfactor or metatrait and the second super- knowledge that the two higher order factors repeatedly
factor or metatrait as being a fusion of Extraversion found by various investigators may be “real phenom-
and Openness.18 The recent, complex, comparative, ena that shape the intercorrelations of the Big Five
all-embracing study by Markon, Krueger, and Watson traits” (p. 452); that is, they are not completely arti-
(2005) also concludes, after evaluating various “Big factual. But they also suggest that the two higher or-
Trait” factor analyses: “The hierarchy does descend der factors “might be accounted for by widely shared
downward from the Big Two traits” (p. 153). Currently, schemas concerning the covariation of traits” (p. 452).
They conclude by reasserting that “the Big Five are in
17 Naming factors is a disputacious undertaking. The awarded practice the highest accessible level in the personal-
names of these two metatraits impress me as not quite apt, com- ity trait hierarchy” (p. 453). The McCrae et al. analysis
municatively. In the next section, I attempt a larger discussion of warrants further close consideration, wider replication,
these higher order factors that remains faithful to their essence and
and especially the broader consensual evaluation it has
connotations but escapes some unfortunate implications of their ini-
tial names. not yet received.19
18 Of interest, Musek (2007) and Rushton & Irwing, (2008) went Meanwhile, a spate of related studies has appeared
even further in their hierarchization. They suggested that one gen- that, taken together, may have moved personality un-
eral personality factor underlies the understanding of personality. So,
conceptually, the factor analytic hierarchical approach to personal-
19 Another argument against the existence of higher order factors
ity may be said to proceed from its very lowest level, personality
discriminants, to the next higher factor level, the organization of has appeared (Ashton, Lee, Goldberg, & de Vries, 2009). It is com-
discriminants into (nonarbitrary) facets, then further into a higher plicated also, in a different fashion than the McCrae argument, and
organization of these facets into a half dozen or so midlevel factors conjectures that self-evaluative bias might account for the appar-
(as represented by the FFA), a further grouping of these midlevel ent existence of higher order factors. However, analyses based on
factors into two higher order factors, and the culmination of every- multi-informant ratings also continue to issue higher order factors
thing into a solitary, apical general factor signifying only something (DeYoung, 2006), a finding that cannot be ascribed to self-evaluation
like fitness for collective living. bias.
17
BLOCK

derstandings beyond the FFM preference that the five Yet another article (DeYoung et al., 2007) relates
factors continue their reign as “basic.” The two higher circadian rhythm theoretically to the Stability metafac-
order factors repeatedly found have been demonstrat- tor via the dependence of both on serotonergic func-
ing unusual, deeper, far-afield relationships not readily tion. If replicable and extended, this may be another
construable in terms of the nominal five. In partic- herald of a neurobiological connection to personality.
ular, a neurobiological conjecture as to what under- Further assessing this possibility, Hirsh, DeYoung, and
lies the two metatraits has been especially provocative. Peterson (2009) provide an intriguing hypothesis and a
If further supported and extended, this development further test that behavioral differences are neurophar-
has the promise of grounding the study of individual macological expressions of the individual’s serotonin
differences within a biological context having person- and dopamine systems. Within the Eugene-Springfield
ality and interpersonal significance. In a first, intrepid community sample long and extensively studied by
article, DeYoung et al. (2002) did not only establish Goldberg, 307 participants and their multi-informant
that two higher order factors, tantamount to Alpha and peers had responded to the BFI about the subject. Also,
Beta, indeed subsumed the long-proclaimed “big five.” at a different time, participants had indicated the fre-
They further conjectured that their two higher order quency with which they engaged in a wide variety of
factors are expressive of the neural structures organiz- activities during the preceding year. These numerous
ing serotonergic and dopaminergic function, respec- activities subsequently were brought into a more sim-
tively. From an intriguing evolutionary rationale for ple form and further organized by means of cluster
the kinds of behaviors subserved by serotonergic and analysis.
dopaminergic function, they extrapolated ambitiously, From the BF scores of the participants, their meta-
suggesting an empirical test of their broad, innovative traits of Stability and Plasticity were readily derived
scheme: high scorers on Stability, a multi-interpreted and correlated with each of the behavioral clusters.
label (discussed shortly) they preferred to the prior la- The analyses were complex, aimed at forfending al-
beled Alpha, should show more conformity,20 as mea- ternative or complicating interpretations. As conjec-
sured by several social desirability measures (the Paul- tured, for the Stability metatrait, the 10 highest and
hus Impression Management Scale and the Eysenck significant behavioral cluster predictors reflected con-
Lie Scale), than low scorers. In two separate, sizable straint and were uniformly negative, including sex and
samples, in addition to replicating the existence of their laughter. For the Plasticity metatrait, the 10 highest
two higher order factors, findings supported their ex- and significant behavioral cluster predictors reflected
pectation that measures of conformity related impor- engagement and were uniformly positive, also includ-
tantly to their Stability factor and that, after controlling ing sex and laughter. These findings conjoining en-
for the scales that make up Stability, related negatively tirely separate and different data sets conform im-
to the Plasticity factor,their preferred name to Beta. pressively well to previously expressed interpretations
Another article (Peterson, Smith, & Carson, 2002), of the Stability metatrait as reflecting serotonergically
together with a successful replication, demonstrated mediated self-regulation and constraint whereas the
that what they called Plasticity characterizes individu- Plasticity metatrait reflects dopaminergically mediated
als with reduced Latent Inhibition (LI). LI is a precon- exploration and engagement. Serotonin has been reli-
scious gating mechanism in the brain, screening from ably associated with impulse or overcontrol (Carver &
conscious awareness ongoing stimuli earlier experi- Miller, 2006), whereas dopamine has been reliably rec-
entially established as task-irrelevant. The brain, by ognized as associated with exploratory, engaged, un-
ignoring stimuli previously identified as unimportant, constrained behaviors (Panksepp, 1998), what we ear-
reasonably tends subsequently to not attend to such lier called undercontrol of thought and behavior (e.g.,
stimuli. However, reduced LI (i.e., unattenuated per- Block, 2002).
ception) permits further attention to the ongoing stream The Peterson–DeYoung approach impresses me
of stimuli and has been associated with dopaminegic as having achieved appreciable significance and
neural function. The finding of great implicative inter- demonstrated unusual arenas of fertility for person-
est here is that individuals with higher levels of Beta characterization via inventory responses. Crucially, it
or Plasticity (Openness and Extraversion) characteris- has the adaptive virtues of conjoining both the imbed-
tically manifest reduced LI, a most unusual and theo- ded motivation toward stability with the imbedded mo-
retically suggested, suggestive finding. tivation toward responsiveness to novelty. This line of
research is thought-provoking and may well, if more
widely demonstrated and extended, move the under-
20 The concept of “conformity” also is better measured by a “dif-
standing of person-behaviors from inventory responses
ferential” model rather than a “cumulative” model. One can be exces- to the biological realm. This biological grounding of
sively conforming and insufficiently conforming. An intermediate,
curvilinear level of responsivity to the context has more adaptive
the two higher order factors may open up new, ad-
possibilities and occupies the more adaptive portion of the dimen- vancing, theoretical possibilities undreamed-of earlier
sion. within the FFA.
18
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

Observations on Where We Are Now and Some introduced by Hofstee et al. (1992), is intriguing and
Suggestions useful but, I suggest, not quite sufficient. In applica-
tion, it blends only two factors at a time, omitting the
Of course, I well recognize that the ever-changing undoubted interactive influences of additional factors.
field of personality psychology defies summary; Such restricted blending cannot represent the dynamic,
“wise” projections as to its future are rash indeed. Nev- sometimes disjunctive, interaction of all of the factors
ertheless, a few remarks after this long peregrination functioning within the particular personality system.
seem in order. Fourth, as models go, factor analysis and struc-
First, some suggestions are obvious and simply in- tural equation modeling are extremely useful per se,
volve better scientific practice. There must be effec- but this linear approach—useful as it is—hardly pro-
tive specific replication and extended wider, general- vides a model of the human being as a complex adap-
izing evaluation within the personology community tive system. Rather, the factors emanating from fac-
of attention-gaining results—by other than the original tor analysis represent separated, “purified” behavioral
investigators. This responsibility is absolutely required tendencies—each more or less dominant—in the or-
scientifically. More attention should also be addressed ganism. However, considered within the organized or-
to psychometrics per se as well as factor analysis and ganism, the individual’s characterological system is
structural equation modeling. There needs to be better better viewed as dynamically reactive and interactive
recognition of the weaknesses and strengths of various in accord with genetically ordained temperament, sub-
approaches to evaluating the dependability or quality of sequently evolved habituations, and the surrounding
measurement (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). For example, context. The goal of the study of personality is “to
a high coefficient alpha may sometimes be only a re- provide an integrative [italics added] framework for
flection of the remarkable redundancy of questionnaire understanding the whole person” (McAdams & Pals,
statements as in the still widely used Rosenberg (1965) 2006, p. 204).
measure; there is unawareness of the possibly influen- Prestructured by the BF (but not by the higher or-
tial, misdirecting effects of attenuation (Schmidt, in der factors subsuming them), Van Egeren (2009) of-
press), and there is neglect of the possibility of un- fers a more specific, system-adaptive conceptualization
toward effects of score range restriction/enhancement of BF functioning. He views each of the five broad
or the unusualness of the sample on one’s findings trait factors as having “adaptive propensities,” latent
(Schmidt, Shaffer, & Oh, 2008). Also, the person- system-regulatory behavioral rules responding to the
manifestations studied should be diverse, brought into embedded “pulls/pushes” of the particular factor. His
a comparative frame by standard or T-scoring, aggre- elucidation of cybernetic regulation for each of the
gated whenever feasible to enhance reliability (and five “adaptive propensities” offers “causal clarity” in
consequent validity), with analysis not limited solely to understanding how each of them influences behavior.
ineluctable personality questionnaires but also to dis- The work deserves wide and close reading although, in
parate, far-removed measures such as, for example, the particular, I disagree with Van Egeren’s construal of the
Stroop Test, the Rod-and-Frame Test, Reduced Latent Openness to Experience factor as focused on “reward
Inhibition, short-term memory, visual tracking ability, prediction.” And the grand, perhaps ever unachievable
breadth of visual attention, extent of the individual’s but luring question regarding the dynamic organiza-
“fundamental attribution error,” kind of moral reason- tion of these five separate “adaptive propensities” into
ing, and so on. The ultimate goal is achieving a broadly a complex adaptive system remains unconsidered.
ranging, coherence-suggesting nomological network. Fifth, in my view, the long-held BF factor called,
Second, the cumulating, Internet-oriented, pub- confoundedly and confusingly, Intellect/Openness to
lic domain approach catalyzed by Goldberg (1999, Experience, has been unfortunately treated. In certain
Goldberg et al., 2006) involving the IPIP has appre- personality-studied samples, this factor is empirically
ciably broadened and enhanced the scientific possibil- relatively small and variable in magnitude (even nonex-
ities of the inventory approach and has seen increasing istent on occasion). Reflection and living in the real
and widespread research usage in the larger person- world can be intrusively informing and more influential
ality field. His proposed changes have the promise of than muddled, distantly derived factorial results. Such
transforming accomplishment and practice in the field. reflection suggests that having a high Intellect does not
In my view, his suggestions can become a revolution- guarantee one’s being, therefore, Open to Experience,
ary, unifying, and scientifically cumulative approach to whereas Openness to Experience may not necessarily
the heretofore unmanageable, redundant, and lacunaed entail what is conventionally thought of as Intellect or
field of person-assessment. The changes he suggests “general mental ability.” Individuals with high intellect
have the promise of transforming inventory practice certainly may be smart but also so narrowly channeled
and accomplishment. they may be relatively insensitive to and unaware of cir-
Third, the tactic of “geometric blending” of fac- cumstances especially eliciting of experiential, expres-
tors according to two-dimensional circumplexes, as sive openness, emotionalized verbal or bodily language
19
BLOCK

like a piercing poetic phrase or the anticipations inher- tive behavioral systems, in the broadest sense, are really
ent in sinuous duo-dancing. Likewise, the indubitably emotional systems that motivate and, in a general way,
unusual restructurings and awarenesses of an individ- guide behavior in response to critical stimuli” (p. 48).
ual may not augur especial intellectual ability in the Accordingly, individual differences in mental ability or
“IQ” sense. Such unusually perceiving, kaleidoscopi- intellect intrinsically relate to personality development
cally attentive individuals may be naive, mundane, and and social functioning. It warrants unique study rather
even psychotic. The past empiricism showing an im- than a confounded focus.
portant correlation between measures of intelligence Sixth, if Intellect is respected but partitioned away
per se and measures of unusual perceptions, sensing, as separate, a purified Openness to Experience di-
and cognitions is due, I suggest, to the specialness of mension can be better understood and evaluated. The
the samples that happen to have been evaluated. articulated concept of Openness gradually emerged
Conceptually conjoining both Intellect and Open- from the course of various earlier studies (As, O’Hara,
ness to Experience impresses me as limiting. Indeed, & Munger, 1962; Cattell, Eber, & Tatsuoka, 1970;
in earlier analytic work (McCrae & Costa, 1985a, Coan, 1974; Fitzgerald, 1966; Kris, 1952; Tellegen &
1985b), McCrae and Costa considered Intelligence as Atkinson, 1974). The several articles by McCrae (e.g.,
a sixth dimension minimally related to Openness to 1993–1994; McCrae & Costa, 1985a, 1997) alluding
Experience (McCrae, 1993–1994, p. 47). Conjoining to the nature or essence of what has come to be known
general mental ability with openness combines two as Openness to Experience impress me as extraordinar-
very different person-capacities better left asunder. A ily apt and enlarging of understanding of the concept.
simple and simplifying solution to this confounding Some individuals are relatively open—behaviorally,
problem is to cleanly conceptually separate these two perceptually, cognitively; others may not register or
important, differential qualities presently conjoined. be sensitive to openness-eliciting, epiphanic circum-
With respect to Intellect (or general mental ability) per stances, to apercus.
se, personology needs to recognize and accept differ- I think it useful to further convey some additional in-
ences along this dimension as having definite, appre- stances of Openness or, as it frequently is alternatively
ciable, long, and diverse real-life personality implica- characterized, Absorption. The concept of Openness
tions for the lives people create and live (as personal to experience is uncertainly understood by many and
musing will immediately attest). General mental ability therefore further instances of what so often is felt but
has been viewed as “an all-purpose life-tool” (see, e.g., not easily verbalized may be useful to many.
Gottfredson, 1997; Lubinski, 2004, 2009); it should not But first, a vividly exemplifying instance of Open-
be relegated, as it has been in much of the history of ness: Travelers to France may have seen Picasso’s
psychology, exclusively to study of the separate, non- famous “sculpture,” Bull’s Head. Because he imme-
personological domain of the structure of intelligence diately visualized or sensed the possibility, Picasso
(Jensen, 1998). It is crucial for understanding the ways salvaged from a fortuitously encountered scrap heap
a personality develops and functions. two items—a discarded bicycle racing seat and dis-
Given the largely unspecifiable range and flow of ex- carded bicycle racing handlebars. He spontaneously
perience, contemporary evolutionary understandings positioned the inverted handlebars with the racing seat
suggest that individual variation in intelligence—by and, lo and behold, this bricollage became a remark-
which I mean adaptive resourcefulness—is predicated able, widely recognizable, soon-famous simulacrum of
on and emerges from an interactive set of domain- a bull’s head. Picasso’s flitting, fleeting attentional en-
general competencies (e.g., working memory, inhibi- gagement exemplifies the remarkable openness of his
tion, fluid intelligence, habituation, response latency, artistic creativity.
statistical learning, associative learning, metacogni- Openness or Absorption less paradigmatically may
tion, imitative ability, mind-attribution). The individ- be felt by an unrestrained, intensified, inpouring of
ual’s particular suite or repertoire of these interrelated feeling during perception, thought, or behavior; it facil-
domain-general abilities can be said to underpin what itates a sudden, intuitive understanding of the meaning
is often called “native intelligence,” the widely op- or possibilities of something. Certain communal situa-
portune mental ability to adaptively find coherence or tions or contexts are especially conducive of openness
possibility amidst the incoming onslaught of limitless or absorption. Some instances: observing a modern
environmental inputs. dancer’s fluid and opportune grace; deeply register-
Given the dynamics of complex adaptive regula- ing a suddenly transfixing poetic line or metaphor or
tion, individuals vary widely—innately and in their life trope; reverential awe when beholding nature (e. g.,
learnings—in their ability to adapt to their personal life greenery or vistas or sunsets); the pan-cultural, numi-
situation and the flux of their experience, to equilibrate nous “rhythm and modes” discerned by Dissanayake in
and re-equilibrate in response to their ever-changing her 2000 book, Art and Intimacy; introspections after
being and their ever-changing world. But, as Depue, giving birth or cradling a child one has fathered; the
Collins, and Luciana (1996) tellingly remark, “Adap- lability and intense perceptions within the mescaline
20
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

experience; true empathy (not sympathy) felt with an- responding to the two essential formal social system re-
other; the love/sex experience; the reach and touch quirements for enduring human adaptation. Likewise,
of art; piquant, nonlinear thinking that may go astray the various efforts by political scientists (e.g., Jost,
from its “sensible” path; fragile ikebana floral arrange- Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003) to character-
ments; the sensuality involved in transforming a lump ize individuals and political entities in terms of their
of clay into a self-made pot; the sheer joy suffusing motivational emphases on preservation versus innova-
moments of effective athleticism; the heightened ex- tion, stability versus change, order versus complexity,
oticism of foreign travel; creative efforts that anticipate familiarity versus novelty, conformity versus creativity
and then fit previously unverbalizable expectations; the also derives from recognition of the dual necessity of
absorptions of meditation; the need to add a unique per- an individual and his or her immediate societal sur-
sonalizing touch or flair to one’s surroundings—one’s round attaining and sustaining a stability or order with
attire, one’s “style,” one’s avocations; the evocative the complementary need of the individual and his or
memory of smells arising after a long-delayed rain; her societal surround to be able to adapt to change and
the emanation of improvisation in response to sinu- novelty.
ous jazz; playful, explorative, ready engagement; un- The naming of factors is inevitably a disputatious
hurried sentience; being captured by the strains and task. Without fundamentally disagreeing with these
cumulativeness of Beethoven’s Ninth Symphony or a most recent higher order factor interpretations, my
soaring Pavarotti aria. I also note that incidental learn- own psychological background leads me in a some-
ing may often also be an aspect or even a fundament what different theoretical (or terminological) direction.
of openness to experience. I am troubled by the uncertain connotations inherent
The experience of Openness or Absorption is per- in the primary, nonprocessual labels offered, Stability
haps best understood via Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi’s and Plasticity. Stability exemplifies societally exem-
notion of “psychological flow.” Flow, as defined in plary behavior necessary and desirable for the con-
Wikipedia, “is the mental state of operation in which tinuation of the social order, that is, conventionality.
the person is fully immersed in what he or she is doing But sometimes, an adaptive mode may go awry. The
by a feeling of energized focus, full involvement, and psychological processes underlying Stability—in the
success in the process of the activity.” Csikszentmiha- extreme—can lead to unyielding, stultifying confor-
lyi’s work warrants more psychological interest than it mity and constraint in behavior and outlook. Plastic-
has yet received. ity represents engagement, exploration, and adaptation
Seventh, it seems clear, from an accumulating num- to novelty, that is, individuality. But sometimes, the
ber of analyses, that the BF are hierarchically sub- psychological processes underlying Plasticity—in the
sumed by the higher order, progenitive Big Two fac- extreme—can lead to structureless, chaotic reactivity
tors. Although initially labeled, somewhat vaguely, as transitorily impinging on behaviors and percepts and
Alpha and Beta because of uncertainty as to what these cognitions. These “complexified” interpretations pro-
factors represented, more recently the more specific posed as underlying the factors strike me as going well
labels, Stability and Plasticity, may have gained cur- beyond single-word, nonontogenic labels. In addition,
rency. They are suggested from two rationales. The a neural modeling formulation conjoined with an illu-
neural network computer model of Grossberg (1987) minating account of the common themes historically
argues that an already sufficient (i.e., stable) computer underlying mythic narrative strikes me as distant in-
classification system would adaptively fail when it en- terpretively from immediately close psychological un-
countered novelty unless it was balanced by another derstanding of the complex adaptive system that is per-
system capable of adapting to novelty or further ad- sonality and personality developments.
justing categories (i.e., being “plastic”). Coming from Eighth, in my own thinking, the Stability and Plas-
a quite different but quite complementary direction, ticity factor meta-dimensions seem striking manifesta-
Peterson (1999), in a remarkably broad and percep- tions of an early, central developmental process and a
tive volume, proposed and elucidated two functionally subsequent life characteristic. The great Swiss psychol-
analogous, similarly named, Stability and Plasticity ogist, Jean Piaget, years ago introduced the concepts
themes as underlying historical and worldwide emer- of assimilation and accommodation as fundamental
gent cultural myths, religions, and the functioning of organismic processes in the development of the child.
contemporary societies. As conceptualized by him, they are the cardinal de-
These themes of logic and historical universality velopmental principles by which the child constructs
have also been recognized by other scholars originat- and reconstructs perceptual and action schemata for
ing their views from a different standpoint. Cassirer behaving adaptively—and achieves equilibration—in
(1944), a well-known philosopher of the last century, the world. Biologically grounded, with extraordinary
made a distinction between “reality” and “possibility.” range, Piaget’s genetic epistemology has transformed
This polarity between tradition/stability versus innova- our views of psychological development. Widely rec-
tion/ change can be seen as cognizant of and abstractly ognized (if not well understood), the assimilation and
21
BLOCK

accommodation concepts are integral to his, and many studies a restricted version of the panoply of individual
views. From my own perspective, the renderings in En- differences, providing a factor-analysis based empiri-
glish of Piaget’s views have been somewhat clouded, cal taxonomy. It is not concerned with the variety of life
and I have elsewhere attempted a closer view of the happenings that arise from the dynamic workings of the
dynamic sequencing involved (Block, 1982). personality system. What are such dynamic workings?
For Piaget (1970), “assimilation is the integration For example, under certain circumstances, individ-
of external elements into evolving or completed struc- uals experience anxiety. There are several, specifiable,
tures” (p. 706). Accommodation is “any modification intrapersonal ways anxiety can come about, and there
of an assimilatory scheme or structure by the elements are several, specifiable, intrapersonal ways anxiety can
it assimilates” (Piaget, 1970, p. 708). Both of these are be lessened. Anxiety can be managed in ways that can
conceived of as processes, not just the static issue of a be quite different from the way anxiety initially was
factor analysis. raised.
Whereas “assimilation is necessary in that it in-
sures continuity and the integration of new elements to Coda
these structures” (Piaget, 1970, p. 707), accommoda-
tion is necessary to permit structural change, i.e., the The understanding of personality, its myriad—
transformation of structures is a function of the new even innumerable—characterizations is an unending
elements encountered. Piaget presumes that the organ- conceptual task. But also it is an approachable, ever
ism’s neurology has been wired by evolution to bring attracting scientific goal. Personologists seek the fun-
to equilibrium what is not in equilibrium. It is the work damental parameters of personality, their consequent
of equilibration to modify or create structures and it is hierarchical downward relations, and sometimes the
the interplay of assimilation and accommodation that lawful connections that become a complex adap-
is known as “self-regulation.” tive system. That system—deeply considered—must
I suggest that the concepts, assimilation/ be evolutionarily suited for life continuance and for
accommodation, are inclusive of and more readily sys- constructively adapting to threatening and novel cir-
tem (and connotatively) implicative than the terms, Sta- cumstances. We need an adaptive paradigm for this
bility and Plasticity. The higher order factor labeled personality system, a model or point of view or pro-
Stability is equally (I would say, better) seen as ex- ductive principles that can, in Gell-Mann’s memorable
pressing assimilation, the ingrained tendency to locate and guiding phrase, generate sufficient “surface com-
new experience in terms of prior organized experience. plexity arising out of deep simplicity.” Although we
Likewise, the higher order factor, Plasticity may be are far from beginning, we have a long way to go.
better recognized as expressing accommodation, the
evolution-ingrained tendency to reorganize, reconcep-
tualize, and re-equilibrate experience but introducing Acknowledgments
wider and additional parameters.
Stability and Plasticity, thought of in neural mod- Preparation of this article was supported by National
eling terms or in culturally mythic terms certainly Institute of Mental Health Grant MH 16080 to Jack
projects a tenable interpretation. And the neurobiolog- Block.
ical reasoning conjectured as underlying Stability and
Plasticity is by no means questioned by this contrary
interpretation in terms of assimilation/accommodation. References
But the Piagetian conceptualization may well register
and resonate more with the immediate understandings Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York:
Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
of developmental psychologists. The process of as- As, A., O’Hara, J. W., & Munger, M. P. (1962). The measurement
similation attempts to conserve individual (and social) of subjective experiences presumably related to hypnotic sen-
structures already evolved in the service of continu- sibility. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 3, 47–64.
ing what has already proven to function; the process Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-Humility, the Big Five,
of accommodation, unfazed by novelty, attempts to in- and the Five-Factor Model. Journal of Personality, 73, 1321–
1353.
corporate the new into a restructured, further adaptive Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Goldberg, L. R., & de Vries, R. (2009).
mode of functioning. Higher order factors of personality: Do they exist? Personality
Finally, there needs to be a contemporary recon- and Social Psychology Review, 13, 79–91.
sideration by personologists of what the study of per- Becker, P. (1999). Beond the Big Five. Personality and Individual
sonality is all or centrally about. At one time, at least Differences, 26, 511–530.
Block, J. (1961). The Q-sort method in personality assessment and
as I understood the quest, personality psychology as- psychiatric research. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
pired to understanding the dynamics of intraindividual Block, J. (1965). The challenge of response sets: Unconfounding
functioning; it was not just the study of individual dif- meaning, acquiescence, and social desirability in the MMPI.
ferences, of which there can be no end. Now, the FFA New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

22
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

Block, J. (1982). Assimilation, accommodation, and the dynamics Dahlstrom, W. G., Welsh, G. S., & Dahlstrom, L. E. (Eds.). (1975).
of development. Child Development, 53, 283–295. An MMPI handbook. Volume II: Research applications. Min-
Block, J. (1995a). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach neapolis: University of Minnesota Press. (Original work pub-
to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187– lished 1960)
215. Denissen, J. J. A., & Penke, L. (2008). Motivational individual re-
Block, J. (1995b). Going beyond the five factors given: Rejoinder action norms underlying the Five-Factor model of personality:
to Costa and McCrae and Goldberg and Saucier Psychological First steps towards a theory-based conceptual framework. Jour-
Bulletin, 117, 226–229. nal of Research in Personality, 42, 1285–1302.
Block, J. (2001). Millennial contrarianism. Journal of Research in Depue, R. A., Collins, P. F., & Luciana, M. (1996). A model of
Personality, 35, 98–107. neurobiology-environment interaction in developmental psy-
Block, J. (2002). Personality as an affect-processing system. chopathology. In M. F. Lenzenweger & J. J. Haugaard (Eds.),
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Frontiers of developmental psychopathology (pp. 44–77). New
Block, J. (2008). The Q–sort in Character Appraisal: Encoding sub- York: Oxford University Press.
jective impressions quantitatively. Washington, DC: American De Raad, B. (1998). Five big, big five issues: Rationale, content,
Psychological Association. structure, status, and crosscultural assessment. European Psy-
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego- chologist, 3, 113–124.
resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in
Development of cognition, affect, and social relations (Vol. 13, a multi informationsample. Journal of Personality and Social
pp. 39–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology, 91, 1138–1151.
Block, J., & Petersen, P. (1955). Some personality correlates of DeYoung, C., Hasher, L., Djikic, M., Criger, B., & Peterson, J. B.
confidence, caution and speed in a decision situation. Journal (2007). Morning people are stable people: Circadian rhythm
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 34–41. and the higher-order factors of the Big Five. Personality &
Block, J., & Thomas, H. (1955). Is satisfaction with self a measure of Individual Differences, 43, 267–276.
adjustment? Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-
254–259. order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there
Carroll, J. B. (2002). The five-factor personality model: How com- neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences, 33,
plete and satisfactory is it? In H. I. Braun & D. N. Jackson & 533–552.
D. E. Wiley (Eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., Seguin, J. R., & Tremblay, R. E.
educational measurement (pp. 97–126). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (2008). Externalizing behavior and the Higher-Order Factors
Cartwright, D. (1973). Determinants of scientific progress: The case of the Big Five. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 947–
of research on the risky shift. American Psychologist, 28, 222– 953.
231. DeYoung, C., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets
Carver, C. S., & Miller, C. J. (2006). Relations of serotonin function and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality
to personality: Current views and a key methodological issue. and Social Psychology, 91, 880–896.
Psychiatry Research, 144, 1–15. Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal
Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In N. of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246–1256.
Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Dissanayake, E. (2000). Art and intimacy: How the Arts began.
child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality develop- Seattle: University of Washington Press.
ment (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 300–353). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Dunnette, M. D. (1966). Fads, fashions, and folderol in psychology.
Cassirer, E. (1944). An essay on man: An introduction to a philosophy American Psychologist, 1, 343–352.
of human culture. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for
Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsuoka, M. M. (1970). The hand- adult temperament. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 868–
book for the Sixteen Personality Factor (i6 PF) Questionnaire. 888.
Champagne, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Eysenck, H. J. (1977). Personality and factor analysis: A reply to
Coan, R. W. (1972). Measurable components of Openness to experi- Guilford. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 405–411.
ence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39, 346. Eysenck, H. J. (1992). Four ways five factors are not basic. Person-
Coan, R. W. (1974). The Optimal Personality: An empirical and ality and Individual Differences, 13, 667–673.
theoretical analysis. New York: Columbia University Press. Fitzgerald, E. T. (1966). Measurement of openness to experience:
Coombs, C. H. (1953). Theory and methods of social measurement. A stusy of regression in the service of the ego. Journal of
In L. Festinger & D. Katz (Eds.), Research methods in the Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 655–631.
behavioral sciences (pp. 471–535). New York: Dryden. Flow. (2010). In Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia. Retrieved from
Costa, P. T. J., Busch, C. M., Zonderman, A. B., & McCrae, R. R. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Flow (psychology)
(1986). Correlations of MMPI Factor Scales with measures of Goldberg, L. R. (1981). Language and individual differences: The
the Five Factor Model of Personality. Journal of Personality search for universals in personality lexicons. In L. Wheeler
Assessment, 50, 640–650. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 2,
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality pp. 141–160). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Re- Goldberg, L. R. (1982). From Ace to Zombie: Some explorations
sources. in the language of personality. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N.
Costa, P. T. J., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalog to classifi- Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 1, pp.
cation: Murray’s needs and the five-factor model. Journal of 203–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 258–265. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”:
Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inven- The Big Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social
tory (NEO–PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI) Psychology, 59, 1216–1229.
professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic traits. American
Resources. Psychologist, 48, 26–34.
Costa, P. T. J., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierar- Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, per-
chical personality assessment using the Revised NEO Person- sonality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several
ality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21–50. five-factor models. In I. D. I. Mervielde, F. DeFruyt, & F.

23
BLOCK

Ostendorf (Ed.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. Loevinger, J. (1948). The technic of homogenous tests compared
7–28). Tilburg, the Netherlands: Tilburg University Press. with some aspects of “scale analysis” and factor analysis. Psy-
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, chological Bulletin, 45, 507–529.
M. C., Cloninger, C. R., et al. (2006). The International Per- Loevinger, J. (1957). Objective tests as instruments of psychological
sonality Item Pool and the future of public-domain person- theory. Psychological Reports, 3, 635–694.
ality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84– Loevinger, J. (1993). Measurement of personality: True or false.
96. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 1–16.
Goldberg, L. R., & Kilkowski, J. M. (1985). The prediction of se- Loevinger, J. (1994). Has psychology lost its conscience? Journal of
mantic consistency in self-descriptions: Characteristics of per- Personality Assessment, 62, 2–8.
sons and of terms that affect the consistency of responses to Lubinski, D. (2004). Introduction to the special section on cogni-
synonym and antonym pairs Journal of Personality and Social tive abilities: 100 years after Spearman’s (1904) “ ‘General
Psychology, 48, 82–98. intelligence,’ objectively determined and measured.” Journal
Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. J. (2003). A very of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 96–111.
brief measure of the Big Five personality domains. Journal of Lubinski, D. (2009). Exceptional cognitive ability: The phenotype.
Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. Behavior Genetics, 39, 350–358.
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why “g” matters: The complexity of Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating
everyday life. Intelligence, 24, 79–132. the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integra-
Graziano, W. G., & Tobin, R. M. (2002). Agreeableness: Dimen- tive hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social
sion of personality or social desirability artifact? Journal of Psychology, 88, 139–157.
Personality, 70, 695–728. Macdonald, K. (1995). Evolution, the 5-factor model, and levels of
Grossberg, S. (1987). Competitive learning from interactive learning personality. Journal of Personality, 63, 525–567.
to adaptive resonance. Cognitive Science, 11, 23–63. Maslow, A. (1962). Toward a psychology of being. Princeton, NJ:
Guadagno, R. E., Okdie, B. M., & Eno, C. A. (2008). Who blogs? Van Nostrand.
Personality predictors of blogging. Computers in Human Be- McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new Big Fve: Fundamental
havior, 24, 1993–2004. principles for an integrative science of personality. American
Guilford, J. P. (1977). Will the real factor of extraversion– Psychologist, 61, 204–217.
introversion please stand up? A reply to Eysenck. Psychological McCrae, R. R. (1993–1994). Openness to experience as a basic
Bulletin, 84, 412–416. dimension of personality. Imagination, Cognition, and Person-
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (in press). The weirdest ality, 13, 39–55.
people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985a). Openness to experience.
Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Metatraits of In R. Hogan & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Perspectives in personality
the Big Five differentially predict Engagement and Restraint of (pp. 145–172). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Behavior. Journal of Personality, 77, 1085–1102. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. J. (1985b). Updating Norman’s “Ad-
Hofstee, W. K. B. (1990). The use of everyday personality language equate Taxonomy”: Intelligence and personality dimensions in
for scientific purposes. European Journal of Personality, 4, 77– natural language and in questionnaires. Journal of Personality
88. and Social Psychology, 49, 710–721.
Hofstee, W. K. B., de Raad, B., & Goldberg, L. R. (1992). Integration McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1989). Rotation to maximize the
of the Big Five and circumplex approaches to trait structure. construct validity of factors in the NEO Personality Inventory.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 146–163. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 24, 107–124.
Hubbard, R. (1995). Genomania and health. American Scientist, 83, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1992). Discriminant validity
8–10. of the NEO–PIR facet scales. Educational and Psychological
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of Meta-analysis: Measurement, 52, 229–237.
Correcting error and bias in research findings (2nd ed.). Thou- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Conceptions and correlates
sand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. of Openness to Experience. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, &
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g-Factor: The science of mental ability. S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp.
Westport, CT: Praeger. 826–848). New York: Academic Press.
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A five–factor theory of per-
Inventory Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: Institute of Personality sonality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of
and Social Research, University of California. personality (pp. 139–153). New York: Guilford.
John, O. P., Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (1991). The basic McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. J., & Busch, C. M. (1986). Evaluating
level in personality-trait hierarchies: Studies of trait use and comprehensiveness in personality systems: The California Q-
accessability in different contexts. Journal of Personality and Set and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 54, 430–
Social Psychology, 60, 348–561. 446.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: McCrae, R., Costa, P. T., Jr., & Martin, T. A. (2005). The NEO–PI–3:
History, measurement, and theoretical perspective. In L. A. A more readable revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal
Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality (pp. 102– of Personality Assessment, 84, 261–270.
138). New York: Guilford. McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. J., & Piedmont, R. L. (1993). Folk
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). concepts, natural language, and psychological constructs: The
Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psycho- California Psychological Inventory and the five-factor model.
logical Bulletin, 129, 339–375. Journal of Personality, 61, 1–26.
Kenrick, D. T., & Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the Five-
Lessons from the person-situation debate. American Psycholo- Factor Model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60,
gist, 43, 23–34. 175–215.
Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic explorations in art. New York: In- McCrae, R. R., & Sutin, A. R. (2007). New frontiers for the Five-
ternational Universities Press. Factor Model: A preview of the literature. Social and Person-
Lanning, K. (1994). Dimensionality of observer ratings on the Cal- ality Psychology Compass, 1, 423–440.
fornia Q-set. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, McCrae, R. R., Yamagata, S. R., Jang, K. L., Reimann, R.,
151–160. Ando, J., Ono, Y., et al. (2008). Substance and artifact in the

24
FIVE-FACTOR FRAMING

higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and sion of four major issues in psychology from an evolutionary
Social Psychology, 95, 442–455. development perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 19, 1–18.
Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. C., & Cowan, C. P. Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one
(1998). Assessing young children’s self-perceptions of their minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory
academic, social and emotional lives: An evaluation of the in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41,
Berkeley Puppet Interview. Child Development, 69, 1556– 203–212.
1576. Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of in
Measelle, J. R., John, O. P., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. C., & Cowan, mid-level change in personality traits across the life course: A
C. P. (2005). Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin,
self-reports on the big five dimensions? Journal of Personaliity 132, 1–25.
and Social Psychology, 89, 90–106. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent seolf-image.
Meehl, P. (1992). Factors and taxa, traits and types, differences of Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
degree and differences of kind. Journal of Personality, 60, 117– Rushton, J. P., & Irwing, P. (2008). A General Factor of Person-
174. ality (GPF) from two meta-analyses of the Big Five: Digman
Meehl, P. E. (1998, May). The power of quantitative thinking. Paper (1997) and Mount, Barrick, Scullen, and Rounds (2005). 45,
presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological 679–683.
Society, Washington, DC. Saucier, G. (1992). Openness versus intellect: Much ado about noth-
Merriam-Webster’s Collegiate Dictionary (11th ed.). (2003). Spring- ing? European Journal of Personality, 6, 381–386.
field, MA: Merriam-Webster Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1998). What is beyond the Big Five?
Mershon, B., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1988). Number of factors in the per- Journal of Personal496-495-ity, 66, 495–524.
sonality sphere: Does increase in factors increase predictability Schmidt, F. L. (in press). Detecting and correcting the lies that data
of real-life criteria? Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- tell. Perspectives on Psychological Science.
ogy, 55, 675–680. Schmidt, F. L., Shaffer, J. A., & Oh, I.–S. (2008). Increased accu-
Millikan, R. A. (1965). The electron and the light-quant from the racy for range restriction corrections: Implications for the role
experimental point of view. From Nobel Lectures, Physics of personality and general mental ability in job training and
1922–1941 (pp. 54–66). Amsterdam: Elsevier. (Original performance. Personnel Psychology, 61, 827–868.
work published 1923) Available from http://nobelprize.org/ Shedler, J., & Block, J. (1990). Adolescent drug use and psycholog-
nobel prizes/physics/laureates/1923/millikan-lecture.html ical health. American Psychologist, 45, 612–630.
Musek, J. (2007). A general factor of personality: Evidence for Tanaka, J. W., & Taylor, M. E. (1991). Object categorization and
the Big One in the five-factor model. Journal of Research in expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive
Personality, 41, 1213–1233. Psychology, 23, 457–482.
Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans Tate, J. C., & Shelton, B. L. (2008). Personality correlates of tattooing
and other animals. American Psychologist, 61, 622–671. and body piercing in a college sample: The kids are alright.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of hu- Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 281–285.
man and animal emotions: New York: Oxford University Press. Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their
Paunonen, S. V., & Jackson, D. N. (2000). What is beyond the Big relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report.
Five? Plenty! Journal of Personaliity, 68, 821–835. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety
Peevers, B. H., & Secord, P. (1973). Developmental changes in at- disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tribution of descriptive concepts to persons. Journal of Person- Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-
aliity and Social Psychology, 27, 120–128. altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic
Peterson, J. B. (1999). Maps of meaning: the architecture of belief. susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268–277.
New York: Routledge. Tellegen, A., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2008). Minnesta Multiphasic
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. H. (2002). Openness Personality Inventory-2, Retructured Form. Minneapolis: Uni-
and extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: versity of Minnesota Press.
replication and commentary. Personality and Individual Differ- Van Egeren, L. F. (2009). A cybernetic model of global personality.
ences, 33, 1137–1147. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 92–108.
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s Vermaes, I. P. R., Janssens, J. M. A. M., Mullaart, R. A., Vinck, A.,
manual of child psychology (Vol. 3, 3rd ed.). New York: Wiley. & Gerris, J. R. M. (2008). Parents’ personality and parenting
Ploeger, A., van der Maas, H. L. J., & Raaijmakers, Q. A. W. (2008). stress in families of children with spina bifida. Child: Care,
Is evolutionary development biology a viable approach to the Health and Development, 34, 665–674.
study of the human mind? Psychological Inquiry, 19, 41–48. Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1999). Meta-analyses of fakability
Ploeger, A., van der Maas, H. L. J., & Raijmakers, M. e. J. (2008). estimates: Implications for personality measurement. Educa-
Is evolutionary theory a metatheory for psychology? A discus- tional and Psychological Measurement, 59, 197–210.

25
Copyright of Psychological Inquiry is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like