Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2008, 49, 133–140 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00625.

Cognition and Neurosciences


Blackwell Publishing Ltd

Internal imagery training in active high jumpers


C.-J. OLSSON,1 BERT JONSSON2 and LARS NYBERG1,3
1
Department of Integrative Medical Biology (Physiology), Umeå University, Sweden
2
Department of Psychology, Umeå University, Sweden
3
Department of Radiation Sciences (Radiology) Umeå University, Sweden

Olsson, C.-J., Jonsson, B. & Nyberg, L. (2008). Internal imagery training in active high jumpers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 133–140.
The main purpose of this study was to examine whether the use of internal imagery would affect high jumping performance for active high
jumping athletes. Over a period of six weeks, a group of active high jumpers were trained with an internal imagery program for a total of 72
minutes. This group was compared to a control group consisting of active high jumpers that only maintained their regular work-outs during
the same time period. Four variables were measured; jumping height, number of failed attempts, take-off angle, and bar clearance. There was
a significant improvement on bar clearance for the group that trained imagery (p < 0.05) but not for the control group. No other differences
were found. The results suggest that internal imagery training may be used to improve a component of a complex motor skill. Possible
explanations and future recommendations are discussed.
Key words: Mental practice, internal imagery, training.
C.-J. Olsson, Department of Integrative Medical Biology (Physiology), Umeå University, S-901 87 Umeå, Sweden. Tel: +46 90786 5186-13;
fax: +46 90 786 6696; e-mail: cj.olsson@physiol.umu.se

INTRODUCTION shot. Thus, the participants in the physical + imagery group


received as much physical practice as the physical group but
Mental training is a broad term used in many situations. It imagery practice was added on top of that. The performance
was defined by Richardson (1967) as the mental rehearsal of measure was how close to the pin they were on a 50 m shot.
a task without any muscular movement and is often referred The results showed that the physical + imagery group
to as imagery. The question of whether a motor skill would improved significantly greater than the group that only
benefit from imagery training has been examined in several trained physically.
studies (for overviews see e.g. Driskell, Copper & Moran, Grouios (1992) investigated the effects of imagery on
1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983). Although the results have female springboard divers. They were first tested on a
varied across studies, the majority show that imagery training selected dive and then the participants were randomly
results in positive effects on subsequent performance. assigned to physical practice, imagery practice or a control
In one study, Mendoza and Wichman (1978) examined group. The imagery practice group imagined themselves
the effects of imagery training on dart-throwing performance. performing the task 10 times each day for 21 days. The
Participants were scored on their dart-throwing ability and physical practice group physically performed the springboard
then assigned either to a control group that did nothing, to dive for the same amount of time. The control group solved
a group that trained imagery, to a group that trained both crossword puzzles for approximately the same time. The
imagery and physically or to a group that only trained physically. results showed that imagery training had positive effects on
The training interventions lasted for six weeks with daily springboard performance. In addition to these examples
training twice for 15 minutes. After the training sessions the there are several other studies showing positive effects of
participants were again tested on their dart-throwing ability. imagery training on sport performance (e.g., Mamassis &
The conclusions were that physical practice was most effec- Doganis, 2004; Peynircioglu, Thompson & Tanielian, 2000;
tive and that imagery training either combined with physical Smith & Holmes, 2004; Wrisberg & Anshel, 1989).
practice or by itself was more effective than no practice. Although most studies show positive effects of imagery
Another study examined how imagery training combined training, there are some that do not demonstrate such effects.
with physical training would affect the approach shot for In a study by Mumford and Hall (1985), figure skaters’ ability
golfer beginners (Brouziyne & Molinaro, 2005). The par- to use imagery training to improve a figure was investigated.
ticipants either practiced imagery combined with physical Three groups received imagery training and a control group
training, practiced physically, or had no practice (control) did not receive any imagery training. All three treatment
for five weeks. The imagery practice consisted of mentally groups received four training sessions for one hour on separate
representing (imagining) a scene close to reality before each days and were then tested on the figure again. The results

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600
Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
134 C.-J. Olsson et al. Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

showed no difference in performance between the four groups 1977; Murphy, 1994; Rotella, Gansneder, Ojala & Billing,
after the imagery training (for other studies with null effects 1980). It is also known that internal imagery produces
from mental training see Corbin, 1967; Gordon, Weinberg & greater physiological responses (Hale, 1982; Perry & Morris,
Jackson, 1994; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Taylor & Shaw, 2002). 1995) and this is argued to be more effective on performance
A possible explanation for the different outcomes in studies outcomes (Holmes & Collins, 2001). In a study by Smith
of mental imagery could be the type of sport examined. (1987) in which specific conditions that help imagery training
Poulton (1957) categorized different sports into either closed in sport are outlined, the use of external imagery is simply
or open. In a closed sport, the environment never changes, seen as a motivational factor that should not be discounted
it is predictable, and every move can be planned in advance. for, although, as a performance enhancer the use of internal
Examples of closed sports are golf and springboard diving. imagery is more beneficial. In three experiments, Hardy and
In sports such as basketball, except for certain closed elements Callow (1999) investigated external and internal imagery
such as free-throw shooting, the environment changes perspectives on motor performance. They used experienced
constantly and little is known about the next move. Therefore athletes in karate learning a new kata, students learning a
these sports are classified as open. Many individual sports simple gymnastics floor routine, and high-ability rock climbers
are closed and most team sports are open. performing new boulder problems. They found that for the
Weinberg, Butt, Knight, Burke and Jackson (2003) inves- acquisition of a task, external imagery was superior to internal.
tigated the relationship between the use and effectiveness of However, internal imagery was more effective when the
imagery in athletes from both individual and team sports. athlete already had reached a certain level of expertise. Thus,
They concluded that for a closed sport, such as high jump, for active athletes, the internal view should be more effective
it is much easier to know what to imagine since there is the as a performance enhancer than the external view.
possibility to prepare and imagine exactly what will take Type of outcome measures is a third, and much overlooked,
place. This is in contrast to an open sport in which athletes explanation for variability across studies of the effectiveness
must be concerned with several factors that may impede the of imagery. In studies of imagery training the measures are
performance. They also found that athletes in closed sports, generally in terms of hit or miss (e.g. when examining basket-
mostly individual sports, tended to find imagery more ball shooting) or in total performance (e.g. in gymnastics
effective than athletes in open sports. In a study comparing routines). Having only one type of sub-optimal outcome
individual and team sports use of imagery, Hall, Rodgers, measure may be a reason why some studies of mental
and Barr (1990) examined 381 female and male athletes imagery failed to show any effects. Support for this notion
using a 37-item questionnaire. The participants were competi- comes from studies of physical performance, as in a study of
tive athletes from recreational/house league up to the inter- golf (Jagacinski, Greenberg & Liao, 1997). When looking at
national level. The results showed that athletes in closed golf tempo, which is the overall duration of a golf swing,
sports tended to use internal imagery more often than athletes they found no significant difference between older and younger
in open sports and also that higher level athletes tended to players. However, when looking at another outcome
use imagery to a greater extent than lower level athletes. measure, the rhythm, which is the speeding up and slowing
When examining the use of mental imagery among elite down within a shot, a significant difference was found. To
athletes it was found that between 70 and 90% of elite have appropriate outcome measures may depend on having
world-class athletes were using imagery training as a way to appropriate equipment to estimate the outcomes. In studies
enhance performance (for review see Jones & Stuth, 1997). of children’s motor performance, the effects of additional
A second possible explanation for different outcomes in outcome measures and more advanced equipment is seen
studies of mental imagery could be based on the distinction clearly. Using film cameras, von Hofsten and Lindhagen
between external and internal imagery. Mahoney and (1979) showed that children at about 4 months of age were
Avener (1977) classified the internal view of imagery as a able to make a successful reach for a moving object. When
first person’s visual and kinesthetic view, experienced from further decomposing children’s reaching trajectories into
within and where the agent feels as if she/he is performing movement elements, von Hofsten (1979) found that children
the action. This should be distinguished from the external between 4 months and 6 months not only could make a
view, where the agent takes the view of a spectator watching successful reach, but their reaching pattern was organized
the action as on a video. The common view seems to be that into acceleration and deceleration phases. Using an optoe-
internal imagery is more effective as performance enhance- lectronic device, which is a technology based on monitoring
ment than is external imagery. For example, studies using the movements using infrared light attached to the children,
self-reported questionnaires among Olympic, national, and von Hofsten (1991) showed that reaching trajectories become
university level athletes show that elite athletes tend to rely straighter over time and, thus, have a structure similar to
more on internal imagery than non-elite athletes (Mahoney, adult reaching at approximately 6 months of age. One example
Gabriel & Perkins, 1987). Other studies support the view of where type of outcome measure made a difference when
internal imagery being more effective than external imagery studying the effects of mental imagery was a study by
(e.g., Fery & Morizot, 2000; Hale, 1982; Mahoney & Avener, Ranganathan, Siemionow, Liu, Sahgal, and Yue (2004). In

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Internal imagery training 135

this study the effects of mental imagery on a strength task the final sample included ten athletes in the motor group, with a
were examined. When comparing muscle volume before and mean age of 20.2 years (SD = 4.1) and nine athletes in the motor +
imagery group, with a mean age of 18.1 years (SD = 2.6). The
after imagery, as a measure of strength gain, they did not
participation was voluntary and the participants could drop out at
find any effects of imagery training. However, when comparing any time. The participants received a monetary reward of $125 after
the total force exerted from the muscle, which also would be completing all stages of the study.
a measure of strength, before and after imagery training,
there was a significant improvement.
The present study examined the effectiveness of internal Procedure
imagery on high jumping performance after taking into The study consisted of three main components: (I) pretest, (II)
account the above-mentioned factors. The recruited parti- intervention, and (III) post-test.
cipants, active high jumpers, were chosen because of high
I. Pretest. The study started with pretest jumping (i.e. how high the
jump being a closed sport, appropriate for imagery training.
participants jumped before the intervention). For an overview of the
Internal imagery was used because of its apparent advantage set-up for the pretest, see Fig. 1. They were asked to jump as high
over external imagery for athletes that are on a higher level. as possible as if they were in a competition. The participants had a
When constructing the internal imagery program several maximum of three attempts on each height. The participants were
top-level high jumping coaches were contacted. They stressed free to choose a starting height. They started to jump and after each
cleared height the bar was raised. To make sure that the participants
two critical technical components that influence jumping
were going to get a chance to jump as high as possible they freely
height. These were take-off angle, which is the angle of the decided the next jumping height. When the participant made three
lean of the athlete away from the bar, and bar clearance, failed attempts on a height the pretest was completed. The heights for
which is the bend over the bar where the athlete arches the all jumps including the number of failed attempts were written down.
back by pulling their feet towards their head in order to
more efficiently clear the bar. The internal imagery program II. Intervention. The second part of the study was the intervention
or training phase. When all the participants were done with the
included imagining of take-off as well as bar clearance. The
pretest, they were randomly assigned to one of two groups; either
importance of appropriate outcome measures was considered the motor group or the motor + imagery group. The length of the
by using video analysis to analyze and quantify changes in intervention phase was based on earlier studies on physical training
bar clearance and take-off angle. The main purpose of the in which six weeks of practice led to positive effects on motor per-
study was to investigate the effects of internal imagery training formance (Karni, Meyer, Jezzard, Adams, Turner & Ungerleider,
1995). Each participant met individually with the experimenter for
on actual jumping performance and on the motor parameters
12 sessions over a six-week period. Participants in the motor group
take-off angle and bar clearance. Of the latter two, the bar were asked to do a finger tapping task (not reported in this paper)
clearance component is arguably the more complex process and a cognitive task (not reported in this paper). None of the tasks
from a cognitive/internal imagery point of view, involving a had anything to do with high jumping and the session took approx-
complex series of movements. Thus, as internal imagery imately 15 minutes. When participants in the motor + imagery
group met with the experimenter they did the same two tasks as
tends to more easily benefit activities of high cognitive
the motor group (tapping and cognitive). These tasks were added to
complexity (Martin, Moritz & Hall, 1999; White & Hardy, the experiment in order for both groups to spend approximately the
1995), we hypothesized that the bar clearance will benefit the same amount of time with the experimenter minimizing the possibility
most from the internal imagery training. A related purpose of experimenter effects. In addition, at the beginning of each session
was to evaluate the relation between take-off angle, bar they were asked to imagine high jump according to an internal
imagery instruction. This instruction was based on information
clearance and jumping height. To the extent that jumping
about high jump given by three high jumping coaches who trained
height is related to take-off angle and bar clearance, a athletes on the top international level. Information was also taken
positive relation was hypothesized. from international web-pages describing what is important when
performing and training high jump: www.brianmac.demon.co.uk/
highjump (16 August 2006); www.todd.acheson.com (16 August
METHOD 2006). Before each imagery session it was made sure that the athletes
understood the concept of internal imagery and that it was important
that they tried to feel like they were executing the high jump when
Participants
imagining it.
Twenty-four participants were recruited for this study. They ranged The high jumping instruction for the internal imagery stated:
in age from 16 to 29 years (mean age 19.3 years; SD 3.4 years).
There were 13 males and 11 females. Inclusion criteria for this study Imagine that you are running towards the bar in a calm pace. At the
were that the participants had to be competitive track and field curve you are slightly leaning inwards from the bar. In the last two
athletes competing at the national elite level. The participants were steps the legs run past the body and you lean slightly backwards. In
recruited from a national track and field high school in Umeå, the take-off you plant the whole foot, you feel that the knee is
Sweden. Also, active track and field athletes living in Umeå were straight, and you lean away from the bar. The lead leg bends and is
recruited. All the participants included had been training for a parallel with the bar, the arms help you up. The take-off foot leaves
substantial amount of time, thus there were no novel high jumpers the ground; you rotate so that your back is against the bar. You pull
included in this study. Four participants dropped out from the study your heals towards your head so your back bends. You push your
because of injuries and leaving the city. The data was initially hip up and lean your head back and pull your legs over the bar. You
screened for outliers and one participant had to be excluded. Therefore, land on the pit and you made the jump.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
136 C.-J. Olsson et al. Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

Fig. 1. The set up was according to the figure and was the same for every participant. Two cameras were used to record the outcome measures
take-off angle and bar clearance. In the figure the runway path is from the right. For two of the participants the set-up was mirrored because
they jumped from the left side.

At each session, the participants in the motor + imagery group Adobe Premiere Pro. To estimate the take-off angle, the video was
imagined for 6 mins. They imagined repeatedly four times for 1.5 stopped at the first frame when the take-off foot was completely
minutes with 30 s of rest in between. They were given the same planted on the ground. That particular frame was exported into
instruction each session and when there was 5 s left of rest Adobe Illustrator CS2 where a line was drawn from the foot of the
in between the imagery sessions they were told to get ready for participant through the middle of the head (see Fig. 2). The program
imagery. The participants were told to do their best at all times. The provided us the coordinates for this line and the angle for the take-off
total time the participants spent imagining high jump in the study was calculated using basic trigonometry. From the recordings of bar
was 12 sessions × 6 mins. Some of the participants imagined with their clearance, the frame when the participant’s hip was over the bar was
eyes closed and some imagined with their eyes open. When asking the exported to Adobe Illustrator CS2. In this context, the bar clearance
participants how many jumps they did when imagining they reported is defined as the distance between shoulder and foot. A line was
that it took approximately 10 s to complete one jump; hence the drawn from the foot to the left shoulder (see Fig. 2). The coordinates
total number of jumps was about 8 for each 1.5 minute period. were given by the program and the distance of the line was calculated
using trigonometry. In some jumps the bar clearance measure was
III. Post-test. The third and last part of the study was the post-test. not possible to estimate. This was the case when the participants did
The set-up was the same as during pretest (Fig. 1). The participants not follow through with the jump because they interrupted the
started to jump on the same height as they did during the pretest. attempt shortly after take-off, pushing the bar down with their
In addition, when raising the bar it was also made sure that it was hands. Another exclusion possibility was when the participants
raised to the same height as in the pretest. If the participants cleared jumped much higher than the bar, such that bending of the back
the height they stopped at during the pretest they kept on jumping was unnecessary for the participants in order to clear the jump.
on higher heights until they made three false jumps. As in the These jumps were excluded from the statistical analysis. Fifteen
pretest, each jump was written down as a cleared jump or as a jumps from the take-off and the bar clearance were randomly
false jump. selected for an inter-rater reliability test. The inter-class correlation
coefficient was 0.962, p < 0.01.
Improvements in the technique after the training sessions could be
Instruments shown in the take-off angle and the bar clearance. The imagery pro-
gram emphasized leaning away from the bar at take-off, and pulling the
Two cameras were used to film the high jumps. One camera was set feet towards the head during bar clearance. Therefore a performance
up in line with the position for bar clearance and the other where increase should result in a greater take-off angle and a smaller distance
the participants made their take-off (see Fig. 1). A Panasonic NV- between head and foot in bar clearance. A smaller distance would
DS65 digital camera and a Sony DCR-TRV33E PAL digital camera improve the athletes’ jumping height because they would be able to
were used for the recordings. The films were analyzed using Adobe clear the bar more easily. In addition to these two parameters, improve-
Premiere Pro 1.5 and Adobe Illustrator CS2. ment was qualified as maximum height and number of false jumps.

Outcome measures Statistical analysis


All the heights that the athletes were jumping on, both cleared and After excluding the jumps where bar clearance was not measurable
false jumps were marked. The video recordings were first taken over at pretest, 19 participants performed a total of 46 jumps showing
from the video cameras into a data program for video editing, bar clearance and 68 jumps showing take-off angle from. Each

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Internal imagery training 137

Fig. 2. The take-off angle was estimated by calculating the angle between the two lines (left picture). The bar clearance was estimated by
calculating the length of the line from the tip of the shoe to the left shoulder (right picture).

Table 1. Outcome measures for the motor (N = 10) and for the motor + mental group (N = 9) showing performance before and after the
imagery training as well as change in performance and statistical values

Group Outcome measure Pretest Posttest Change SD Statistics

Motor Height (cm) 160.3 162.2 1.9 5.9 t(9) = 0.91, p > 0.05
Take off (°) 14.4 15.0 0.6 1.6 t(9) = 1.45, p > 0.05
Bar clearance (mm) 68.7 65.9 −2.8 6.6 t(9) = 1.25, p > 0.05
False jumps 1.3 0.8 −0.5 1.9 t(9) = 1.46, p > 0.05
Motor Height (cm) 161.9 163.4 1.5 3.8 t(8) = 0.12, p > 0.05
+ Take off (°) 15.4 15.7 0.3 2.2 t(8) = 0.29, p > 0.05
Mental Bar clearance (mm) 74.5 70.3 − 4.2 7.1 t(8) = 2.31, p < 0.05
False jumps 2.1 1.1 −1.0 2.2 t(8) = 0.69, p > 0.05

height and corresponding take-off angle and bar clearance from the There was also a significant positive correlation between bar
pretest were correlated to validate the take-off angle and the bar clearance and height (r (45) = 0.574, p < 0.01).
clearance measures as variables that are related to high jump
There were no significant differences between the two
performance. If this shows a positive correlation it would strengthen
the coaches’ view of these variables being important for jumping groups before the intervention, p > 0.05. Thus, any differences
height and, thus, validating these measures. Independent samples between the groups after the intervention should be attributable
t-tests were done on the pretest and post-test performance for the to the imagery training.
outcome measures height, take-off angle, bar clearance and false Table 1 shows the pretest values, the post-test values as
jumps to see if the groups differed before and after the training
well as the change in the outcome measures used in this
sessions. Within-group changes in performance were assessed using
paired samples t-tests. Also, a group by session (pre/post) mixed study for the motor group and for the motor + imagery group.
ANOVA was done to see whether there was an interaction which Both groups tended to show positive improvements on all
could be an indication of a selective improvement after imagery the outcome measures.
training. Non-parametric tests for independent samples, Sign test, Similar to the pretest results, there were no differences
were also computed to evaluate any differences between the two
between the two groups after the intervention phase. However,
groups after the training sessions in terms of the number of participants
that showed any improvement. The statistical calculations were a comparison of the pretest results with the post-test results
done using SPSS 13.0 and the alpha level was set to p < 0.05. revealed a positive trend for both groups for bar clearance.
A group by session (pre/post) mixed ANOVA revealed a
tendency for an interaction on the bar clearance measure
RESULTS (F(2, 17) = 3.22; p = 0.091). As expected the strongest effect
At pretest the correlation between take-off angle and height, was for bar clearance, where an improvement was seen for
across groups, was significant (r (67) = 0.478, p < 0.01). all participants in the intervention group. We further analyzed

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
138 C.-J. Olsson et al. Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

this interaction by t-tests of the pre-post difference for each and using more appropriate outcome measures to detect small
group. We found that the difference was significant for the but potentially important changes.
mental + imagery group. For the motor group, the correspond- It was hypothesized that the bar clearance would benefit
ing change was not significant. No other outcome measures most from the internal imagery program. The bar clearance
showed a significant change, either for the motor + imagery is the most complex component of the high jump. During
or the motor group. Also, there were no between-group the bar clearance the athlete needs to rotate the body in the
differences in the number of participants that improved after air in order for the back to turn towards the bar. Also, the
the intervention. athlete needs to push the hip up in order to arch the back
and, thus, clear the bar. Therefore it is argued that this com-
ponent is more complex, and has a lot of degrees of freedom
DISCUSSION when executing it. As a result, it is in need of more cognitive
The main purpose of this study was to examine whether work than, for example, the take-off, which is more dependent
internal imagery would enhance high jumping performance on physical power and thus have less degrees of freedom
after six weeks of intervention. The control group, that when performing it. This complexity may be a reason for
maintained their regular physical training program, showed the benefit of internal imagery on the bar clearance. This is
positive trends on all measured variables, although no supported from findings that internal imagery is beneficial
increase was statistically significant. A similar pattern was for skills that require complex movements (White &
seen for the intervention group, but for this group the Hardy, 1995).
improvement was significant for one of the measured outcome An important aspect of human research is individual
variables; bar clearance. For this variable, the intervention differences. Developmental theories explain individual dif-
group showed a 50% greater gain than in the control group, ferences to be associated with individual development,
suggesting that the internal imagery training program helped where genetic activity, neural activity, and behavior together
to improve this component of the high jump. The internal with environmental factors such as physical, social and
imagery program emphasized bar clearance by encouraging cultural, all together determine the individual development
the athletes to visualize bending the back and pulling the and cause individual differences (Gottlieb, Wahlsten &
feet towards the head. The significant result in the bar clearance Lickliter, 1998). This is also likely to be a factor to take into
variable for the intervention group suggests that internal account when explaining performance effects from imagery
imagery combined with regular physical practice improved training. Everybody has the ability to use imagery; however
bar clearance greater than only maintaining physical practice. there are individual differences in how well one can create
Thus, internal imagery may help to improve a critical com- these images (cf. Martin et al., 1999). It has been shown that
ponent of high jumping. imagery is associated with sport performance, and also that
The present study shows a relation between take-off angle, high confident athletes, who in addition were associated
bar clearance and height, thus confirming that bar clearance with being top-level athletes, tend to have a higher ability to
and take-off angle are significant parameters for active high perform imagery (Moritz, Hall, Martin & Vadocz, 1996).
jumpers to improve in order to jump higher. As a result, Even though all the participants were advanced athletes in
these parameters are valid measures when investigating the this study they were on different achievement levels. It is
effects of imagery training. We observed no greater gain in therefore likely that there were differences of the imagery
the height outcome measure after the intervention. On ability. Nonetheless all participants in the intervention group
average, both groups increased their height by less than 2 cm. increased their performance on the bar clearance measure. It
Given the limited length of the training program (6 weeks) is suggested that if an athlete has a good imagery ability, the
it is probably unrealistic to expect greater gains. One would imagery is more likely to have greater performance effects
expect that enhanced technique improves performance and compared to if the athlete has low imagery ability (Hall,
since these results confirm the relationship among the 1998; Martin et al., 1999). Further, it seems to be a relationship
measured variables, over time, it is predicted that improved between the use of imagery and imagery ability, thus there is
bar clearance will lead to higher jumping height. Longer-term a possibility to improve your imagery skill (Rodgers, Hall &
studies are needed to address this issue. Buckolz, 1991; Vadocz, Hall & Moritz, 1997). Improving
It was possible to detect the changes in performance due your imagery ability will then improve the results of the
to the video analysis. The reason why some of the previously imagery training. Therefore, taking into account the individual
done studies failed to show effects from mental imagery may differences, in the present study it is likely that those athletes
be due to not considering small improvements in technique. that had high imagery ability might have been able to use the
If we had relied only on results categorized as hit or miss it imagery for the intended performance improvement at the
would be more difficult to determine whether an individual start of the training program. Those athletes that had a
has improved or not (cf. Schmidt & Lee, 1999). Thus, in low imagery ability first had to increase their imagery
order to better understand the effects of mental imagery, future ability and then were able to use the imagery to improve the
research should emphasize both measuring motor performance performance.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Internal imagery training 139

There are some limitations with this study. First, there rhythm, and aging in golf. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29(2),
were a limited number of high jumpers available for this 159–173.
Jones, L. & Stuth, G. (1997). The uses of mental imagery in athletics:
study which resulted in small groups. Second, even though
An overview. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 6, 101–115.
the participants were advanced athletes, for internal imagery Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M., Turner, R. & Ungerleider,
to be effective, the participants must have a strong motor L. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex
representation of what they imagine. There is a possibility plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature, 377, 155–158.
that the participants in this study were not skilled enough Mahoney, M. J. & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the elite athlete:
An exploratory study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1(2),
for the internal imagery to be very effective, which may have
135 –141.
affected the outcome. However, with these caveats in mind, Mahoney, M. J., Gabriel, T. J. & Perkins, T. S. (1987). Psychological
the conclusion from this study is that mental training can skills and exceptional athletic performance. The Sport Psychologist,
improve a complex motor component in high jumping. 1, 181–199.
Mamassis, G. & Doganis, G. (2004). The effects of a mental training
program on juniors pre-competitive anxiety, self-confidence, and
This research was supported by a grant to Lars Nyberg from CIF
tennis performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16,
(Centrum för Idrottsforskning), Sweden. The authors would also
118 –137.
like to thank the coaches for their help in making the imagery training
Martin, K. A., Moritz, S. E. & Hall, C. R. (1999). Imagery use in
program and the athletes for participating in this study.
sport: A literature review and applied model. The Sport Psy-
chologist, 13, 245 –268.
Mendoza, D. & Wichman, H. (1978). “Inner” darts: Effects of mental
REFERENCES practice on performance of dart throwing. Perceptual and Motor
Brouziyne, M. & Molinaro, C. (2005). Mental imagery combined Skills, 47, 1195 –1199.
with physical practice of approach shots for golfer beginners. Moritz, S. E., Hall, C. R., Martin, K. A. & Vadocz, E. (1996). What
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101, 203 –211. are confident athletes imaging? An examination of image con-
Corbin, C. B. (1967). Effects of mental practice on skill development tent. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 171–179.
after controlled practice. Research Quarterly, 38, 534 –538. Mumford, B. & Hall, C. (1985). The effects of internal and external
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C. & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice imagery on performing figures in figure skating. Canadian Journal
enhance performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), of Applied Sport Sciences, 10, 171–177.
481–492. Murphy, S. M. (1994). Imagery interventions in sport. Medicine and
Feltz, D. L. & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice Science in Sports and Exercise, 26, 486 – 494.
on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal Nordin, S. M. & Cumming, J. (2005). More than meets the eye:
of Sport Psychology, 5, 25–57. Investigating imagery type, direction, and outcome. The Sport
Fery, Y.-A. & Morizot, P. (2000). Kinesthetic and visual image in Psychologist, 19, 1–17.
modeling closed motor skills: The example of the tennis serve. Perry, C. & Morris, T. (1995). Mental imagery in sport. In T. Morris
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 707–722. & J. Summers (Eds.), Sport psychology: Theory, applications and
Gordon, S., Weinberg, R. & Jackson, A. (1994). Effect of internal issues. Queensland: John Wiley.
and external imagery on cricket performance. Journal of Sport Peynircioglu, Z. F., Thompson, J. L. W. & Tanielian, T. B. (2000).
Behavior, 17, 60–66. Improvement strategies in free-throw shooting and grip-strength
Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D. & Lickliter, R. (1998). The significance tasks. The Journal of General Psychology, 127(2), 145 –156.
of biology for human development: A developmental psycho- Poulton, E. C. (1957). On prediction in skilled movements. Psycho-
biological systems view. In W. Damon (Ed.) & R. M. Lerner logical Bulletin, 54(6), 467– 478.
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1. (5th edn, Ranganathan, V. K., Siemionow, V., Liu, J. Z., Sahgal, V. & Yue, G.
pp. 233–273). New York, John Wiley & Sons. H. (2004). From mental power to muscle power – gaining
Grouios, G. (1992). The effect of mental practice on diving per- strength by using the mind. Neuropsychologia, 42, 944 –956.
formance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 60 – Richardson, A. (1967). Mental practice: A review and discussion
69. (Part II). Research Quarterly, 38(2), 263 –273.
Hale, B. D. (1982). The effects of internal and external imagery on Rodgers, W., Hall, C. & Buckolz, E. (1991). The effect of an imagery
muscular and ocular concomitants. Journal of Sport Psychology, training program on imagery ability, imagery use, and figure
4, 379–387. skating performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3,
Hall, C. R. (1998). Measuring imagery abilities and imagery use. In 109 –125.
J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measure- Rotella, R. J., Gansneder, B., Ojala, D. & Billing, J. (1980). Cognitions
ment (pp. 165–172). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information and coping strategies of elite skiers: An exploratory study of
Technology. young developing athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 350–
Hall, C. R., Rodgers, W. M. & Barr, K. A. (1990). The use of 354.
imagery by athletes in selected sports. The Sport Psychologist, 4, Schmidt, R. A. & Lee, T. D. (1999). Motor control and learning: A
1–10. behavioral emphasis, 3rd edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Hardy, L. & Callow, N. (1999). Efficacy of external and internal Smith, D. (1987). Conditions that facilitate the development of
imagery perspectives for the enhancement of performance on sport imagery training. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 237–247.
tasks in which form is important. Journal of Sport and Exercise Smith, D. & Holmes, P. (2004). The effect of imagery modality on
Psychology, 21, 95–112. golf putting performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
Holmes, P. S. & Collins, D. J. (2001). The PETTLEP approach to 26(3), 385 –395.
motor imagery: A functional equivalence model for sport Taylor, J. A. & Shaw, D. F. (2002). The effects of outcome imagery
psychologists. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13(1), on golf-putting performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20,
60–83. 607 – 613.
Jagacinski, R. J., Greenberg, N. & Liao, M.-J. (1997). Tempo, Vadocz, E. A., Hall, C. R. & Moritz, S. E. (1997). The relationship

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
140 C.-J. Olsson et al. Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

between competitive anxiety and imagery use. Journal of Applied on the learning and performance of different motor skills.
Sport Psychology, 9, 241–253. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 169–180.
von Hofsten, C. (1979). Development of visually directed reaching: Weinberg, R., Butt, J., Knight, B., Burke, K. L. & Jackson, A.
The approach phase. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, (2003). The relationship between the use and effectiveness of
160–178. imagery: An exploratory investigation. Journal of Applied Sport
von Hofsten, C. (1991). Structuring of early reaching movements: A Psychology, 15, 26 – 40.
longitudinal study. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23(4), 280 –292. Wrisberg, C. A. & Anshel, M. H. (1989). The effect of cognitive
von Hofsten, C. & Lindhagen, K. (1979). Observations on the develop- strategies on the free throw shooting performance of young
ment of reaching for moving objects. Journal of Experimental athletes. The Sport Psychologist, 3, 95 –104.
Child Psychology, 28, 158–173.
White, A. & Hardy, L. (1995). Use of different imagery perspectives Received 25 April 2007, accepted 13 July 2007

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

You might also like