Adolescence

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 609

ste32118_fm_i-xxi.

indd Page i 19/04/13 1:52 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Adolescence
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page ii 19/04/13 1:52 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page iii 19/04/13 1:52 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Tenth Edition

Adolescence
Laurence Steinberg
Temple University
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page iv 19/04/13 1:52 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

ADOLESCENCE, TENTH EDITION


Published by McGraw-Hill, a business unit of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., 1221 Avenue of the Americas,
New York, NY, 10020. Copyright © 2014 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved. Printed in the
United States of America. Previous editions © 2011, 2008, and 2005. No part of this publication may be reproduced or
distributed in any form or by any means, or stored in a database or retrieval system, without the prior written consent of
The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., including, but not limited to, in any network or other electronic storage or transmission,
or broadcast for distance learning.
Some ancillaries, including electronic and print components, may not be available to customers outside the United States.
This book is printed on acid-free paper.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 DOW/DOW 1 0 9 8 7 6 5 4 3
ISBN 978-0-07-353211-0
MHID 0-07-353211-8

Senior Vice President, Products & Markets: Kurt L. Strand


Vice President, General Manager: Michael Ryan
Vice President, Content Production & Technology Services: Kimberly Meriwether David
Managing Director: William Glass
Brand Manager: Allison McNamara
Development Editor: John Haley
Senior Director of Development: Dawn Groundwater
Editorial Coordinator: Sarah Kiefer
Marketing Manager: Ann Helgerson
Director, Content Production: Terri Schiesl
Project Manager: Erin Melloy
Buyer: Susan K. Culbertson
Cover Image: Robert Kohlhuber/Getty Images/RF
Typeface: 10/12 Minion
Compositor: Aptara®, Inc.
Printer: R. R. Donnelley
All credits appearing on page or at the end of the book are considered to be an extension of the copyright page.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Steinberg, Laurence D., 1952–


Adolescence/Laurence Steinberg.—Tenth Edition.
pages cm
ISBN-13: 978-0-07-353211-0 (alk. paper)
ISBN-10: 0-07-353211-8
1. Adolescent psychology—Textbooks. I. Title.

BF724.S75 2013
305.235—dc23 2013005384

The Internet addresses listed in the text were accurate at the time of publication. The inclusion of a website does not indicate an
endorsement by the authors or McGraw-Hill, and McGraw-Hill does not guarantee the accuracy of the information presented at
these sites.

www.mhhe.com
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page v 19/04/13 1:52 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

For Wendy and Ben


ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page vi 22/04/13 5:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

About the Author

LAURENCE STEINBERG, Ph.D., is the Distinguished University


Professor and Laura H. Carnell Professor of Psychology at Temple
University. He graduated from Vassar College in 1974 and from
Cornell University in 1977, where he received his Ph.D. in human
development and family studies. He is a Fellow of the American
Psychological Association and the Association for Psychological Science
and former President of the Society for Research on Adolescence and
the Division of Developmental Psychology of the American Psycho-
logical Association. Dr. Steinberg has been on the editorial boards of
many major journals, including Developmental Psychology and Child
Development, where he served as Associate Editor. He chaired the
National Academies’ Committee on the Science of Adolescence, and
has been a frequent consultant to state and federal agencies and
lawmakers on child labor, secondary education, and juvenile justice
policy. His work was cited numerous times by the U.S. Supreme Court
in its landmark decisions that abolished the juvenile death penalty and
mandatory sentences of life without parole for juveniles.
Dr. Steinberg is one of the most highly cited scholars in the field
of developmental psychology. His own research has focused on a
range of topics in the study of contemporary adolescence, including
parent–adolescent relationships, risk taking and decisionmaking,
mental health, adolescent brain development, school-year employ-
ment, academic achievement, and juvenile crime and justice. He has
been the recipient of numerous honors, including the John P. Hill
Award for Outstanding Contributions to the Study of Adolescence,
given by the Society for Research on Adolescence; the Society for
Adolescent Medicine’s Gallagher Lectureship; and, from the American
Psychological Association, the Urie Bronfenbrenner Award for
Lifetime Contribution to Developmental Psychology in the Service of
Science and Society; the Award for Distinguished Contributions to
Research in Public Policy; and the APA Presidential Citation. In 2009,
he was named as the first recipient of the Klaus J. Jacobs Research
Prize for Productive Youth Development.
Dr. Steinberg also has been recognized for excellence in research and
teaching by the University of California, the University of Wisconsin,
and Temple University, where he was honored in 1994 as one of
that university’s Great Teachers. He has taught undergraduate and
graduate courses in adolescence for nearly 40 years and has served as
the doctoral advisor to more than 35 students, many of whom have
gone on to become influential scholars in their own right in the field
of adolescence.
vi
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page vii 19/04/13 1:52 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e About the Author vii

In addition to Adolescence, Dr. Steinberg is the author or co-author


of more than 350 scholarly articles on growth and development during
the teenage years, as well as the books You and Your Adolescent; When
Teenagers Work: The Psychological and Social Costs of Adolescent
Employment (with Ellen Greenberger); Crossing Paths: How Your
Child’s Adolescence Triggers Your Own Crisis (with Wendy Steinberg);
Beyond the Classroom: Why School Reform Has Failed and What Parents
Need to Do (with B. Bradford Brown and Sanford Dornbusch); The
10 Basic Principles of Good Parenting (which has been published in
10 languages); Rethinking Juvenile Justice (with Elizabeth Scott); and
the textbooks Development (with Deborah Lowe Vandell and Marc
Bornstein) and Lifespan Development (with Marc Bornstein, Deborah
Lowe Vandell, and Karen Rook). He is co-editor of Studying Minority
Adolescents: Conceptual, Methodological, and Theoretical Issues (with
Vonnie McLoyd) and the Handbook of Adolescent Psychology (with
Richard Lerner).
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page viii 15/05/13 12:22 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Brief Contents

About the Author vi


A Note from the Author xv

Introduction The Study of Adolescent Development 3

PART 1
The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence 19
1 Biological Transitions 21
2 Cognitive Transitions 55
3 Social Transitions 85

PART 2
The Contexts of Adolescence 117
4 Families 119
5 Peer Groups 151
6 Schools 187
7 Work, Leisure, and Media 219

PART 3
Psychosocial Development During Adolescence 251
8 Identity 253
9 Autonomy 285
10 Intimacy 315
11 Sexuality 349
12 Achievement 383
13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 415

Glossary G-1
References R-1
Credits C-1
Name Index I-1
Subject Index I-24

viii
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page ix 15/05/13 12:22 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Chapter Title ix


Contents
About the Author vi
A Note from the Author xv

Introduction
The Study of Adolescent
Development 3
A MULTIDISCIPLINARY APPROACH TO
ADOLESCENCE 4
THE BOUNDARIES OF ADOLESCENCE 5
Early, Middle, and Late Adolescence 6
A FRAMEWORK FOR STUDYING ADOLESCENT
DEVELOPMENT 7
The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence 7
The Contexts of Adolescence 9
Psychosocial Development of Adolescence 11
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON ADOLESCENCE 13
Biosocial Theories 13
Organismic Theories 14
Learning Theories 14
Sociological Theories 15
Historical and Anthropological Theories 16
STEREOTYPES VERSUS SCIENTIFIC STUDY 17

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL AND SOCIAL IMPACT


PART 1
OF PUBERTY 35
The Fundamental Changes
The Immediate Impact of Puberty 36
of Adolescence 19
The Impact of Specific Pubertal Events 40
Chapter 1 The Impact of Early or Late Maturation 41
Biological Transitions 21 OBESITY AND EATING DISORDERS 45
PUBERTY: AN OVERVIEW 22 Obesity 45
The Endocrine System 22 Anorexia Nervosa, Bulimia, and Binge Eating
Disorder 48
What Triggers Puberty? 24
How Hormones Influence Adolescent PHYSICAL HEALTH AND HEALTH CARE IN
Development 25 ADOLESCENCE 51
The Paradox of Adolescent Health 51
SOMATIC DEVELOPMENT 26
Causes of Mortality in Adolescence 52
Changes in Stature and the Dimensions
of the Body 26 Promoting Adolescent Health 53
Sexual Maturation 29
Chapter 2
THE TIMING AND TEMPO OF PUBERTY 32
Cognitive Transitions 55
Variations in the Timing and Tempo
of Puberty 32 CHANGES IN COGNITION 56
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Pubertal Thinking About Possibilities 56
Timing 32 Thinking About Abstract Concepts 58
ix
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page x 19/04/13 1:53 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

x Contents

Thinking About Thinking 58 THE INFLUENCE OF NEIGHBORHOOD


Thinking in Multiple Dimensions 59 CONDITIONS ON ADOLESCENT
Adolescent Relativism 60 DEVELOPMENT 110
Processes of Neighborhood Influences 112
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON ADOLESCENT
THINKING 61
The Piagetian View of Adolescent Thinking 61
The Information-Processing View of Adolescent
Thinking 62
THE ADOLESCENT BRAIN 65
What Changes in Adolescence? 66
Implications for Adolescent Behavior 71
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN INTELLIGENCE
IN ADOLESCENCE 72
The Measurement of IQ 73
Types of Intelligence 73
Intelligence Test Performance in Adolescence 74
Culture and Intelligence 75
ADOLESCENT THINKING IN CONTEXT 76
Social Cognition in Adolescence 76
Adolescent Risk Taking 78

Chapter 3
Social Transitions 85
SOCIAL REDEFINITION AND PSYCHOSOCIAL
PART 2
DEVELOPMENT 86 The Contexts of Adolescence 117
THE ELONGATION OF ADOLESCENCE 87
ADOLESCENCE AS A SOCIAL INVENTION 88 Chapter 4
The “Invention” of Adolescence 89 Families 119
Emerging Adulthood: A New Stage of Life or a IS CONFLICT BETWEEN TEENAGERS AND PARENTS
Luxury of the Middle Class? 91 INEVITABLE? 120
CHANGES IN STATUS DURING ADOLESCENCE 95 The Generation Gap: Fact and Fiction 120
Drawing a Legal Boundary 95 What Do Adolescents and Parents Usually Fight
About? 121
Inconsistencies in Adolescents’ Legal Status 97
THE PROCESS OF SOCIAL REDEFINITION 97 FAMILY RELATIONSHIPS AT ADOLESCENCE 122
Common Practices in the Process of Social A Time of Reorganization and Change 123
Redefinition 98 The Adolescent’s Parents at Midlife 123
VARIATIONS IN SOCIAL TRANSITIONS 99 Changes in Family Needs and Functions 124
Variations in Clarity 100 Transformations in Family Relations 125
Variations in Continuity 103 Sex Differences in Family Relationships 127

THE TRANSITION INTO ADULTHOOD IN FAMILY RELATIONSHIPS AND ADOLESCENT


CONTEMPORARY SOCIETY 107 DEVELOPMENT 128
Special Transitional Problems of Poor and Parenting Styles and Their Effects 129
Minority Youth 108 Ethnic Differences in Parenting Practices 132
The Effects of Poverty on the Transition Into Autonomy and Attachment in the Adolescent’s
Adulthood 109 Family 133
What Can Be Done to Ease the Transition? 109 Adolescents’ Relationships With Siblings 134
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xi 14/05/13 12:50 PM user1 /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Contents xi

BEHAVIORAL GENETICS AND ADOLESCENT Relational Aggression 177


DEVELOPMENT 135 Victimization and Harassment 179
Genetic and Environmental Influences on
THE PEER GROUP AND PSYCHOSOCIAL
Adolescent Development 136
DEVELOPMENT 183
Why Are Siblings Often So Different? 137
THE ADOLESCENT’S FAMILY IN A CHANGING Chapter 6
SOCIETY 138
Schools 187
The Changed and Changing Nature of
Family Life 138 THE BROADER CONTEXT OF U.S. SECONDARY
EDUCATION 189
Adolescents and Divorce 140
The Origins of Compulsory Education 189
The Specific Impact of Marital Conflict 142
The Rise of the Comprehensive
The Longer-Term Effects of Divorce 143
High School 190
Custody, Contact, and Conflict Following
School Reform: Past and Present 190
Divorce 143
What Should Schools Teach? 192
Remarriage 144
Education in the Inner Cities 193
Economic Stress and Poverty 145
Special Family Forms 147 THE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF SCHOOLS 194
School Size and Class Size 194
THE IMPORTANCE OF THE FAMILY IN ADOLESCENT
DEVELOPMENT 149 Age Grouping and School Transitions 196
How Secondary Schools Differ From Elementary
Schools 197
Chapter 5
Tracking 199
Peer Groups 151
Ethnic Composition 203
THE ORIGINS OF ADOLESCENT PEER GROUPS
Alternatives to Public School 203
IN CONTEMPORARY SOCIETY 153
Age Grading and Compulsory Education 153 CLASSROOM CLIMATE 205
Changes in the Size of the Youth Population 153 The Best Classroom Climate for
Adolescents 205
THE ADOLESCENT PEER GROUP: A PROBLEM OR
Teacher Expectations and Student
A NECESSITY? 154
Performance 206
Is There a Separate Youth Culture? 156
The Importance of Student Engagement 207
THE NATURE OF ADOLESCENT PEER GROUPS 157 School Violence 209
Changes in Peer Groups During Adolescence 157
BEYOND HIGH SCHOOL 211
Cliques and Crowds 158
The College-Bound 211
Changes in Clique and Crowd Structure Over
The Non-College-Bound 213
Time 160
SCHOOLS AND ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT 214
ADOLESCENTS AND THEIR CROWDS 163
Characteristics of Good Schools 214
The Social Map of Adolescence 163
The Effects of School on Adolescent
Crowds as Reference Groups 164
Development 215
ADOLESCENTS AND THEIR CLIQUES 166
Similarity Among Clique Members 166 Chapter 7
Common Interests Among Friends 168 Work, Leisure, and Media 219
Similarity Between Friends: Selection or ADOLESCENTS’ FREE TIME IN CONTEMPORARY
Socialization? 171 SOCIETY 220
POPULARITY AND REJECTION IN ADOLESCENT Patterns of Time Use in Contemporary
PEER GROUPS 174 America 220
Determinants of Popularity and Patterns of Time Use in Other
Rejection 174 Countries 222
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xii 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

xii Contents

ADOLESCENTS AND WORK 222


School and Work in the Early 20th Century 223
The Rise and Fall of the Student Worker 223
Teenage Employment in Other Nations 224
The Adolescent Workplace Today 225
Employment and Adolescent Development 226
Youth Unemployment 228
ADOLESCENTS AND LEISURE 229
Adolescents’ Free Time and Their Moods 229
Structured Leisure Activities 231
Unstructured Leisure Time 233
Promoting Positive Youth Development 235
ADOLESCENTS, MEDIA, AND THE INTERNET 237
Patterns of Media Use 237
Theories of Media Influence and Use 239
The Impact of Adolescents’ Exposure to
Controversial Media Content 241
Electronic Media and Adolescent
Development 244
Mass Media and Adolescent Girls’ Body
Image 247
The Adolescent Consumer 248
FREE TIME AND ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT 249

PART 3
Psychosocial Development
During Adolescence 251
The Social Context of Identity
Development 266
Chapter 8 Resolving the Identity Crisis 268
Identity 253 Problems in Identity Development 268
IDENTITY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE 254 RESEARCH ON IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT 269
CHANGES IN SELF-CONCEPTIONS 256 Determining an Adolescent’s Identity
Changes in the Content and Structure of Status 269
Self-Conceptions 256 Studying Identity Development Over Time 271
Dimensions of Personality in Adolescence 258
THE DEVELOPMENT OF ETHNIC IDENTITY 273
CHANGES IN SELF-ESTEEM 259 The Process of Ethnic Identity
Stability and Changes in Self-Esteem 259 Development 273
Group Differences in Self-Esteem 262 Discrimination and Its Effects 276
Antecedents and Consequences of High Multiethnic Adolescents 279
Self-Esteem 264
GENDER-ROLE DEVELOPMENT 280
THE ADOLESCENT IDENTITY CRISIS 265 Gender-Role Socialization During
Erikson’s Theoretical Framework 266 Adolescence 280
Identity Versus Identity Diffusion 266 Masculinity and Femininity 282
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xiii 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Contents xiii

Chapter 9 DATING AND ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS 335


Autonomy 285 Dating and the Development of Intimacy 337
AUTONOMY AS AN ADOLESCENT The Development of Dating Relationships 340
ISSUE 287 The Impact of Dating on Adolescent
Development 342
THE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTIONAL
AUTONOMY 288 INTIMACY AND PSYCHOSOCIAL
DEVELOPMENT 346
Emotional Autonomy and Detachment 289
Emotional Autonomy and Individuation 290
Research on Emotional Autonomy 290 Chapter 11
Emotional Autonomy and Parenting Sexuality 349
Practices 292 SEXUALITY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE 350
THE DEVELOPMENT OF BEHAVIORAL SEXUAL ACTIVITY DURING ADOLESCENCE 351
AUTONOMY 294
Stages of Sexual Activity 352
Changes in Decision-Making Abilities 294
Sexual Intercourse During Adolescence 353
When Do Adolescents Make Decisions as Well as
Changes in Sexual Activity Over Time 355
Adults? 296
Changes in Susceptibility to Influence 297 THE SEXUALLY ACTIVE ADOLESCENT 356
Ethnic and Cultural Differences in Expectations Psychological and Social Characteristics of
for Autonomy 300 Sexually Active Adolescents 356
Changes in Feelings of Self-Reliance 301 Hormonal and Contextual Influences on Sexual
Activity 358
THE DEVELOPMENT OF COGNITIVE
Parental and Peer Influences on Sexual
AUTONOMY 302
Activity 359
Moral Development During Adolescence 302
Sex Differences in the Meaning of Sex 363
Prosocial Reasoning, Prosocial Behavior, and
Sexual Orientation 364
Volunteerism 306
Sexual Harassment, Rape, and Sexual Abuse
Political Thinking During Adolescence 308
During Adolescence 367
Religious Beliefs During Adolescence 309
RISKY SEX AND ITS PREVENTION 369
Contraceptive Use 369
Chapter 10 AIDS and Other Sexually Transmitted
Intimacy 315 Diseases 371
INTIMACY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE 317 Teen Pregnancy 372
Adolescent Parenthood 375
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON ADOLESCENT
INTIMACY 318 Sex Education 379
Sullivan’s Theory of Interpersonal
Development 318
Chapter 12
Interpersonal Development During Achievement 383
Adolescence 319
Attachment in Adolescence 320 ACHIEVEMENT AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE 384

THE DEVELOPMENT OF INTIMACY ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVES AND BELIEFS 386


IN ADOLESCENCE 324 Achievement Motivation 386
Changes in the Nature of Friendship 324 The Importance of Beliefs 388
Changes in the Display of Intimacy 325 ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON
Sex Differences in Intimacy 326 ACHIEVEMENT 392
Changes in the Targets of Intimacy 328 The Influence of the Home Environment 392
Friendships With the Other Sex 333 The Influence of Friends 395
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xiv 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

xiv Contents

EDUCATIONAL ACHIEVEMENT 397 Prevention and Treatment of Substance Use and


The Importance of Socioeconomic Status 398 Abuse 430
Ethnic Differences in Educational EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 431
Achievement 399 Categories of Externalizing Problems 431
Changes in Educational Achievement Over Developmental Progression of Antisocial
Time 402 Behavior 434
Dropping Out of High School 405 Changes in Juvenile Offending Over
OCCUPATIONAL ACHIEVEMENT 407 Time 434
The Development of Occupational Plans 408 Causes of Antisocial Behavior 437
Influences on Occupational Choices 408 Prevention and Treatment of Externalizing
Problems 440
INTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 442
Chapter 13 The Nature and Prevalence of Depression 442
Psychosocial Problems in
Sex Differences in Depression 444
Adolescence 415
Suicide 445
SOME GENERAL PRINCIPLES ABOUT PROBLEMS
Causes of Depression and Internalizing
IN ADOLESCENCE 416
Disorders 447
PSYCHOSOCIAL PROBLEMS: THEIR NATURE AND Treatment and Prevention of Internalizing
COVARIATION 418 Problems 448
Comorbidity of Externalizing Problems 419 STRESS AND COPING 449
Comorbidity of Internalizing Problems 420
Glossary G-1
SUBSTANCE USE AND ABUSE 421
References R-1
Prevalence of Substance Use and Abuse 422 Credits C-1
Causes and Consequences of Substance Use and Name Index I-1
Abuse 426 Subject Index I-24
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xv 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

A Note from the Author

Two psychopathic killers persuaded me to abandon my The reminiscence bump makes teaching adoles-
dreams to someday become a comedy writer and study cence both fun and frustrating. Fun, because it isn’t hard
psychology instead. I did not enter college intending to to get students interested in the topic. Frustrating,
become either a psychologist or a professor. I majored though, because it’s a challenge to get students to look
in English, hoping to study creative writing. I became at adolescence from a scientific, as well as personal,
interested in psychology during the second semester of perspective. That, above all, is my goal for this book. I
my freshman year, because of an introductory course in don’t want you to forget or set aside your own experi-
personality theory. My professor had assigned the ence as an adolescent. (I couldn’t make that happen,
book, In Cold Blood, and our task was to analyze the anyway.) But what I hope I can do is to help you under-
personalities of Dick and Perry, the two murderers. I stand adolescence—your own adolescence as well as
was hooked. I followed this interest in personality de- the adolescence that is experienced by others around
velopment to graduate school in developmental psy- the world—more deeply and more intelligently, by in-
chology, where I learned that if you really wanted to troducing you to the latest science on the subject. I still
understand how we develop into the people we ulti- maintain a very active program of research of my own,
mately become, you have got to know something about and that necessitates staying on top of the field’s most
adolescence. That was nearly 40 years ago, and I’m still recent and important developments. There is a lot of
as passionate about studying this period of life as I was exciting work being done on adolescence these days
then. (one of my interests is the adolescent brain), and I want
I hope that this book gets you more excited about to share this excitement with you. Who knows, maybe
adolescence, too. you’ll become hooked, too.
One reason I like teaching and writing about adoles- I’ve tried to do my best at covering the most impor-
cence is that most students find it inherently interest- tant topics and writing about them in a way that is not
ing, in part because pretty much everyone has such only informative, but fun and interesting to read. If
vivid recollections of what it was like to be a teenager. there’s something I could have done better, please let
In fact, researchers have discovered that people actu- me know.
ally remember events from adolescence more intensely
Laurence Steinberg
and more accurately than events from other times,
Temple University
something that has been referred to as the “reminis-
laurence.steinberg@temple.edu
cence bump.”

xv
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xvi 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Preface

Studying Adolescence
in Context
The key goal of this text is to help students under-
stand how the context in which adolescents come of
age shapes the way in which they develop. Adolescent
development cannot be understood apart from the
context in which young people live and grow up—
families, peer groups, schools, neighborhoods, and
work and leisure settings. Perhaps the greatest expan-
sion of knowledge during the past two decades has
been about adolescents from ethnic minority groups,
from families that have recently immigrated to a new
culture, and from parts of the world other than North
America. The tenth edition of Adolescence integrates
discussions of ethnicity and culture throughout
every chapter, focusing not only on ethnic differences
in development but also on similarities that cut
across adolescents from different social, economic,
and cultural backgrounds. A new feature called
“Making the Cultural Connection” asks students to
contemplate how particular findings might (or might Thinking Critically to Make
not) change if the research were carried out in a dif-
ferent cultural context. Connections
In addition to “Making the Cultural Connection,” three
other sets of questions added throughout the text ask
students to think more deeply about particular research
findings. “Making the Personal Connection,” asks stu-
dents to think about their own adolescent experience in
the context of the research. “Making the Scientific Con-
nection” asks students to contemplate a finding’s scien-
tific implications. “Making the Practical Connection”
challenges students to think about how a finding might
inform policy or practice. Many instructors may want
to use these questions as a launching pad for class dis-
cussions or as essay questions on examinations.

Analyzing the Latest


Research
Adolescence strives to provide students with the most cur-
rent, most thorough coverage of the scientific literature on
adolescent development. The tenth edition includes more
xvi
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xvii 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e xvii

concerns that surface across, and are affected by, differ-


ent settings.
This book contains an Introduction and 13 chapters,
which are grouped into three parts: the fundamental
biological, cognitive, and social changes of the period
(Part 1); the contexts of adolescence (Part 2); and psy-
chosocial development during the adolescent years
(Part 3). The Introduction presents a model for study-
ing adolescence that serves as both the organizational
framework for the text and an overview of some of the
basic disciplinary perspectives on the period. I have
found the framework to be extremely helpful in teach-
ing adolescent development, and I highly recommend
using it. However, if the model does not fit with your
course outline or your own perspective on adolescence,
it is possible to use the text without using the frame-
work. Each chapter is self-contained, and so it is not
necessary to assign chapters in the sequence in which
they are ordered in the text. Most users of this book as-
sign the chapters in the order in which they appear, but
than 800 new studies from over 60 scientific journals from some assign the chapters in a sequence that pairs an
the fields of psychology, education, neuroscience, sociol- aspect of psychosocial development with the context
ogy, psychiatry, criminology, economics, law, medicine, that most influences it (for example, “Schools” with
and public health. Studies that break new ground (like “Achievement,” or “Peer Groups” with “Intimacy”), and
studies of brain development), change the way the field that has worked well for them.
thinks (like studies of why aggressive adolescents are often
popular), or update existing findings with more recent
samples or newer methods (like studies of Internet use) Theory and Methods
are included to give students the opportunity to review Although the Introduction reviews how different disci-
and analyze the latest information the field has to offer. plines (such as psychology, sociology, anthropology,
and history) approach the study of adolescence, it does
not provide detailed examinations of particular theories
About the Tenth Edition or research methods. My preference is to integrate ma-
The material in each chapter has been thoroughly up- terial on theory and methods when it is most relevant,
dated. Several years ago, I came to the conclusion that it in a way that shows students how research and theory
was unwise to include any important material in a boxed are related. At the beginning of the chapter on intimacy,
insert, because instructors told me that students often for instance, several perspectives on close relationships
skipped over these. I haven’t seen any evidence since (for example, attachment theory and Sullivan’s per-
then that has persuaded me to return to using boxes, and spective on psychosocial development) are presented,
in fact I’ve heard that today’s instructors are even more and then the relevant research is examined. Similarly,
negative about them than they had been in the past. the research methods and tools employed in the study
Based on positive feedback I’ve received from users, I’ve of adolescence are discussed in the context of specific
retained the use of interim summaries—called “Recaps.” studies that illustrate the powers—or pitfalls—of cer-
tain strategies.

Organization and Learning


Aids Chapter-by-Chapter
The overall organization of this book has not changed
Changes
since the previous edition. Specifically, the chapters In addition to over 800 new research citations, the
about psychosocial development during adolescence tenth edition of Adolescence features updated and ex-
are separate from those about the contexts of adoles- panded coverage of key issues in development in every
cence. In this way, the psychosocial concerns of adoles- chapter. A new feature, “Making the Connection,” re-
cence—identity, autonomy, intimacy, sexuality, and places the “Food for Thought” feature and focuses on
achievement—are presented as central developmental the application of research findings to everyday life,
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xviii 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

xviii

social policy, and practice, and on the ways in which Chapter 6


adolescent development may be affected by cultural Thorough update of all content (more than 40 new
context. Below we present a complete list of changes in citations)
each chapter:
Expanded discussion of changes in No Child Left
Chapter 1 Behind under President Obama
Thorough update of all content (more than 70 new Expanded discussion of learning disabilities
citations) Addition of material on ADHD
Expanded discussions of causes of the decline in the age Chapter 7
of puberty
Thorough update of all content (more than 60 new
Expanded discussion of adolescent sleep
citations)
Expanded discussion of adolescent obesity Added discussion of why student employment has de-
Expanded discussion of eating disorders clined in the last decade
Addition of discussion of the impact of puberty on Revised discussion of youth unemployment to reflect
brain development the recent recession
Addition of material on body dissatisfaction among Expanded discussion of impact of violence in the media
boys on adolescent behavior
Updating of statistics on Internet use
Chapter 2
Addition of extensive discussion of the impact of
Thorough update of all content (more than 60 new the Internet on adolescent development, including
citations) Internet addiction
Expanded discussion of structural and functional brain Expansion of discussion of the impact of social net-
development in adolescence working sites
Addition of material on “the social brain”
Chapter 8
Addition of discussion of dual systems models of
decision-making Thorough update of all content (more than 60 new
citations)
Chapter 3 Extended discussions of ethnic identity development
Thorough update of all content (more than 40 new and discrimination
citations) Material on gender role development has been modi-
Addition of discussion of the impact of Hurricane fied to be consistent with newer research indicating
Katrina on mental health that adolescent males and females are not all that
Expanded discussion of neighborhood effects different
Expanded discussion of impact of exposure to violence Chapter 9
Chapter 4 Thorough update of all content (more than 40 new
Thorough update of all content (more than 70 new citations)
citations) Added discussion of the brain science of peer influence
Expanded discussion of familism Added material on civic engagement
Addition of material on adolescents with lesbian or gay Expansion of coverage of religious development
parents
Chapter 10
Addition of material on adolescents in foster care
Thorough update of all content (more than 50 new
Chapter 5 citations)
Thorough update of all content (more than 60 new Addition of material on the neuroscientific bases for
citations) individual differences in interpersonal functioning
Expanded discussion of bullying Addition of material on social networking and the
Expanded discussion of cyberbullying quality of friendships
Expanded discussion of the causes and consequences of Extended discussion of co-rumination
having antisocial friends Expansion of discussion of attitudes toward dating
Addition of material on victims’ responses to bullies, relationships
and their effectiveness Expanded discussion of dating violence
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xix 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e xix

Chapter 11
Supplements
Thorough update of all content (more than 70 new
citations) FOR THE INSTRUCTOR
Dropped material on restrictive and permissive societies The supplements for the tenth edition have been care-
Added material on genetic influences on sexual risk fully revised and updated. The instructor resources
taking for the new edition include an Instructor’s Manual,
Added material on transgender youth Test Bank, and PowerPoint presentations for each
chapter. All of these supplements can be found on the
Expanded discussion of LGBT youth
text’s password-protected Online Learning Center at
www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e. Ask your McGraw-Hill
Chapter 12
representative for access information.
Thorough update of all content (more than 40 new
citations) FOR THE STUDENT
Added discussion of social and personality contributors
The student Online Learning Center features chapter-
to academic success
by-chapter multiple choice, true/false, and essay ques-
Added discussion of importance of parental expectations tions that allow students to test their understanding of
Added table showing results of international achieve- the text. For access go to www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e.
ment comparisons
❚ CourseSmart eBook This
Chapter 13 book is available as a Course-
Thorough update of all content (more than 130 new Smart eBook. CourseSmart is
citations) a new way to find and buy eTextbooks. At CourseSmart
Updated all statistics on prevalence and demo- you can save up to 50% off the cost of a print textbook,
graphic differences in substance abuse, crime, and reduce your impact on the environment, and gain access
depression to powerful web tools for learning. CourseSmart has the
largest selection of eTextbooks available anywhere, offer-
Updated diagnostic criteria tables to be consistent with
ing thousands of the most commonly adopted textbooks
the new DSM-V
from a wide variety of higher education publishers.
Added discussion of callous-unemotional traits and CourseSmart eTextbooks are available in one standard
conduct disorder online reader with full text search, notes and highlighting,
Expanded discussion of the biological underpinnings of and e-mail tools for sharing notes between classmates.
chronic antisocial behavior For further details contact your sales representative or
Added discussion of non-suicidal self-injury go to www.coursesmart.com.
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xx 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Acknowledgments

Revising a textbook at a time when so much new informa- Cheryl Somers, Wayne State Univ
tion is available is a challenge that requires much assis- Elizabeth Morgan, Boise State Univ
tance. Over the years, my students (as well as many who Dan Fuller, Northeastern State Univ
have written to me from other institutions) have suggested
Mary O’Sullivan, Southern Conn Univ
numerous ways in which the text might be improved, and
I have learned a great deal from listening to them. I am Paula Manley, Univ of Wisc Green Bay
especially grateful to Ashley Smith, who ably tracked down Janice Stapley, Monmouth State Univ
and organized much of the new research published in the Jeannine Stamatakis, Diabalo Valley College
three years between editions. I also acknowledge with ap-
I also wish to thank my colleagues at McGraw-Hill,
preciation the following colleagues, whose feedback on
including Mike Sugarman, Director; Allison McNa-
the ninth edition helped make this one better:
mara, Senior Brand Manager; John Haley, Developmen-
Rebecca Shlafer, Univ of Minnesota Minneapolis tal Editor; Ann Helgerson, Marketing Manager; and
Paige Goodwin, Western Illinois Univ Erin Melloy, Project Manager.
Sara Villanueva, Saint Edwards Univ In addition, I am grateful to the many colleagues and
students across the country who took the time during
Kristi Cordell-McNulty, Angelos State Univ
the past 30 years to send me comments and suggestions
Dorothy Shedlock, State Univ of NY-Oswego based on their firsthand experiences using Adolescence
Omar Jamil, DePaul Univ in the classroom. They have improved the text with
Ravisha Mathur, San Jose State Univ each edition.
Janay Sander, Univ of Texas at Austin
Laurence Steinberg
Robyn Rogers, Texas State Univ San Marcos

xx
ste32118_fm_i-xxi.indd Page xxi 19/04/13 1:55 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Adolescence
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-1 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

References
Abecassis, M., Hartup, W. W., Haselager, G. T., use. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, and the creation of a Black achievement ide-
Scholte, R. J., & Van Lieshout, C. M. (2002). 311–318. ology. Sociology of Education, 76, 305–325.
Mutual antipathies and their significance in Adams, R. E., Bukowski, W. M., & Bagwell, C. Aksglaede, L., Sørensen, K., Petersen, J. H.,
middle childhood and adolescence. Child (2005). Stability of aggression during early Skakkebæk, N. E., & Juul, A. (2009). Recent
Development, 73, 1543–1556. adolescence as moderated by reciprocated decline in age at breast development: The
Abela, J. R. Z., & Hankin, B. L. (2011). Rumina- friendship status and friend’s aggression. Copenhagen Puberty Study, Pediatrics, 123,
tion as a vulnerability factor to depression International Journal of Behavioral Develop- 932–939.
during the transition from early to middle ment, 29, 139. Alan Guttmacher Institute. (1994). Sex and
adolescence: A multiwave longitudinal Adams, R., & Laursen, B. (2001). The organiza- America’s teenagers. New York: Author.
study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, tion and dynamics of adolescent conflict Alasker, F., & Flammer, A. (1999). Time use by
259–271. with parents and friends. Journal of Marriage adolescents in an international perspec-
Ablard, K., & Mills, C. (1996). Implicit theories and the Family, 63, 97–110. tive. II: The case of necessary activities. In
of intelligence and self-perceptions of aca- Adegoke, A. (1993). The experience of sper- F. Alasker & A. Flammer (Eds.), The adoles-
demically talented adolescents and chil- marche (the age of onset of sperm emis- cent experience: European and American ado-
dren. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, sion) among selected adolescent boys in lescents in the 1990s (pp. 61–83). Hillsdale,
137–148. Nigeria. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, NJ: Erlbaum.
Abma, J., Driscoll, A., & Moore, K. (1998). Young 22, 201–209. Albers, A. B., & Biener, L. (2003). Adolescent
women’s degree of control over first inter- Adelson, J. (1972). The political imagination of participation in tobacco promotions: The
course: An exploratory analysis. Family Plan- the young adolescent. In J. Kagan & R. Coles role of psychosocial factors. Pediatrics, 111,
ning Perspectives, 30, 12–18. (Eds.), Twelve to sixteen: Early adolescence. 402–406.
Abrahamson, A., Baker, L. A., & Caspi, A. (2002). New York: Norton. Albert, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011b). Judgment
Rebellious teens? Genetic and environmen-
Adler, N., Boyce, T., Chesney, M., Cohen, S., and decision making in adolescence. Journal
tal influences on the social attitudes of ado-
Folkman, S., Kahn, R., & Syme, S. (1994). of Research on Adolescence, 21, 211–224.
lescents. Journal of Personality and Social
Socioeconomic status and health: The Alberts, A., Elkind, D., & Ginsberg, S. (2007). The
Psychology, 83, 1392–1408.
challenge of the gradient. American Psy- personal fable and risk-taking in early adoles-
Aceves, M. J., & Cookston, J. T. (2007). Violent vic- chologist, 49, 15–24. cence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36,
timization, aggression, and parent–adolescent
Adler, N., Ozer, E. J., & Tschann, J. (2003). Abor- 71–76.
relations: Quality parenting as a buffer for vi-
tion among adolescents. American Psycholo- Albrecht, A. K., Galambos, N. L., & Jansson, S. M.
olently victimized youth. Journal of Youth and
gist, 58, 211–217. (2007). Adolescents’ internalizing and ag-
Adolescence, 36, 635–647.
Agliata, D., & Tantleff-Dunn. (2004). The impact gressive behaviors and perceptions of par-
Aceves, M. J., Hinshaw, S. P., Mendoza-Denton,
of media exposure on males’ body image. ents’ psychological control: A panel study
R., & Page-Gould, E. (2010). Seek help from
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, examining direction of effects. Journal of
teachers or fight back? Student perceptions
7–22. Youth and Adolescence, 36, 673–684.
of teachers’ actions during conflicts and re-
sponses to peer victimization. Journal of Agoston, A. M., & Rudolph, K. D. (2011). Trans- Albrecht, C., & Teachman, J. D. (2003). Child-
Youth and Adolescence, 39, 658–669. actional associations between youths’ re- hood living arrangements and the risk of
sponses to peer stress and depression: The premarital intercourse. Journal of Family
Achenbach, T., & Edelbrock, C. (1987). The
moderating roles of sex and stress exposure. Influence, 24, 867–894.
manual for the Youth Self-Report and Profile.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, Alex Mason, W., & Spoth, R. L. (2011). Longitu-
Burlington: University of Vermont.
159–171. dinal associations of alcohol involvement
Adalbjarnardottir, S., & Hafsteinsson, L. G. (2001).
Adolescents’ perceived parenting styles and Aguilar, B., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, with subjective well-being in adolescence
their substance use: Concurrent and longitu- E. (2000). Distinguishing the early onset/ and prediction to alcohol problems in early
dinal analyses. Journal of Research on Adoles- persistent and adolescence-onset antiso- adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
cence, 11, 401–423. cial behavior types: From birth to 16 years. 40, 1215–1224.
Development and Psychop a thology, 12, Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Kabbani, N. S.
Adam, E., & Chase-Lansdale, P. (2002). Home
109–132. (2001). The dropout process in life course
sweet home(s): Parental separations, resi-
dential moves, and adjustment problems in Ahn, N. (1994). Teenage childbearing and high perspective: Early risk factors at home and
low-income adolescent girls. Developmental school completion: Accounting for individ- school. Teachers College Record, 103, 760–822.
Psychology, 38, 792–805. ual heterogeneity. Family Planning Perspec- Alexander, K., Natriello, G., & Pallas, A. (1985).
tives, 26, 17–21. For whom the cognitive bell tolls: The im-
Adam, E., Chyu, L., Hoyt, L. T., Doane, L. D.,
Boisjoly, J., Duncan, G., . . . McDade, T. Ainsworth, J. W. (2002). Why does it take a pact of dropping out on cognitive perfor-
(2011). Adverse adolescent relationship his- village? The mediation of neighborhood ef- mance. American Sociological Review, 50,
tories and young adult health: Cumulative fects on educational achievement. Social 409–420.
effects of loneliness, low parental support, Forces, 81, 117–152. Allen, J., Hauser, S., O’Connor, T., & Bell, K. (2002).
relationship instability, intimate partner vi- Ainsworth-Darnell, J., & Downey, D. (1998). As- Prediction of peer-rated adult hostility from
olence, and loss. Journal of Adolescent sessing the oppositional culture explanation autonomy struggles in adolescent–family
Health, 49, 278–286. for racial/ethnic differences in school perfor- interactions. Development and Psychopathol-
Adamczyk-Robinette, S. L., Fletcher, A. C., & mance. American Sociological Review, 63, ogy, 14, 123–137.
Wright, K. (2002). Understanding the author- 536–553. Allen, J., & McElhaney, K. (March, 2000). Auton-
itative parenting–early adolescent tobacco Akom, A. A. (2003). Reexamining resistance as omy in discussions vs. autonomy in decision-
use link: The mediating role of peer tobacco oppositional behavior: The nation of Islam making as predictors of developing close

R-1
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-2 18/04/13 6:07 AM user-1 /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-2 References

friendship competence. Paper presented at the distress among LGBT youth: The influence Anderson, A. L., & Hughes, L. A. (2009). Expo-
biennial meetings of the Society for Research of perceived discrimination based on sexual sure to situations conducive to delinquent
on Adolescence, Chicago. orientation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, behavior: The effects of time use, income,
Allen, J., McElhaney, K., Kuperminc, G., & Jodl, 38, 1001–1014. and transportation. Journal of Research in
K. (2004). Stability and change in attach- Althaus, F. (1997). Female circumcision: Rite of Crime and Delinquency, 46, 5–34.
ment security across adolescence. Child passage or violation of rights? International Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., & Eubanks, J.
Development, 75, 1792–1805. Family Planning Perspectives, 23, 130–133. (2003). Exposure to violent media: The ef-
Allen, J., Philliber, S., Herrling, S., & Kuperminc, Altschul, I., Oyserman, D., & Bybee, D. (2006). fects of songs with violent lyrics on aggressive
G. (1997). Preventing teen pregnancy and Racial-ethnic identity in mid-adolescence: thoughts and feelings. Journal of Personality
academic failure: Experimental evaluation of Content and change as predictors of aca- and Social Psychology, 84, 960–971.
a developmentally based approach. Child demic achievement. Child Development, 77, Anderson, M., Kaufman, J., Simon, T. R., Barrios,
Development, 64, 729–742. 1155–1169. L., Paulozzi, L., Ryan, G., . . . School Associated
Allen, J., Pianta, R., Gregory, A., Mikami, A., & Alva, S. (1993). Differential patterns of achieve- Violent Deaths Study Group. (2001). School-
Lun, J. (2011). An interaction-based ap- ment among Asian-American adolescents. associated violent deaths in the United States,
proach to enhancing secondary school in- Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 407–423. 1994–1999. Journal of the American Medical
struction and student achievement. Science, Association, 286, 2695–2702.
Amato, P., & Booth, A. (1996). A prospective
333, 1034–1037. study of divorce and parent–child relation- Anderson, S. E., Dallal, G. E., & Must, A. (2003).
Allen, J., Porter, M. R., & McFarland, F. C. (2006). ships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, Relative weight and race influence average
Leaders and followers in adolescent close 58, 356–365. age at menarche: Results from two nationally
friendships: Susceptibility to peer influence representative surveys of U.S. girls studied
Amato, P., & Cheadle, J. (2008). Parental di-
as a predictor of risky behavior, friendship 25 years apart. Pediatrics, 111, 844–891.
vorce, marital conflict, and children’s be-
instability, and depression. Development and Andersson, H., & Bergman, L. R. (2011). The
havior problems: A comparison of adopted
Psychopathology, 18, 155–172. role of task persistence in young adolescence
and biological children. Social Forces, 86,
Allen, J., Porter, M., McFarland, F., Marsh, P., & for successful educational and occupational
1139–1161.
McElhaney, K. (2005). The two faces of attainment in middle adulthood. Develop-
Amato, P., & Fowler, F. (2002). Parenting prac- mental Psychology, 47, 950–960.
adolescents’ success with peers: Adolescent
tices, child adjustment, and family diver-
popularity, social adaptation, and deviant Ando, M., Asakura, T., & Simons-Morton, B.
sity. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 64,
behavior. Child Development, 76, 747–760. (2005). Psychosocial influences on physical,
703–716.
Allen, J., Porter, M., McFarland, C., McElhaney, verbal, and indirect bullying among Japanese
Amato, P., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce early adolescents. Journal of Early Adoles-
K. B., & Marsh, P. (2007). The relation of
and the well-being of children: A meta- cence, 25, 268–297.
attachment security to adolescents’ pater-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 26–46.
nal and peer relationships, depression, and Andrews, J., Alpert, A., Hops, H., & Davis, B.
externalizing behavior. Child Development, Amato, P., & Sobolewski, J. M. (2001). The ef- (1996, March). The relation of competence in
78, 1222–1239. fects of divorce and marital discord on middle adolescence to depression and antiso-
adult children’s psychological well being. cial behavior: A multi-method assessment. Pa-
Allen, M. L., Elliott, M. N., Morales, L. S., Diamant,
American Sociological Review, 66, 900–921. per presented at the biennial meetings of the
A. L., Hambarsoomian, K., & Schuster, M. A.
(2007). Adolescent participation in preven- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diag- Society for Research on Adolescence, Boston.
tive health behaviors, physical activity, and nostic and statistical manual of the American Andrews-Hanna, J., Seghete, K., Claus, E., Burgess,
nutrition: Differences across immigrant gen- Psychiatric Association (DSM-V). Washington: G., Ruzic, L., & Banich, M. (2011). Cognitive
erations for Asians and Latinos compared Author. control in adolescence: Neural underpinnings
with Whites. American Journal of Public American Psychological Association Zero Toler- and relation to self-report behaviors. PLoS
Health, 97, 337–343. ance Task Force. (2008). Are zero tolerance ONE, 6, e21598.
Allen, R., & Mirabell, J. (1990, May). Shorter policies effective in the schools?: An eviden- Andrinopoulos, K., Kerrigan, D., & Ellen, J. M.
subjective sleep of high school students from tiary review and recommendations. American (2006). Understanding sex partner selection
early compared to late starting schools. Paper Psychologist, 63, 852–862. from the perspective of inner-city Black ado-
presented at the second meeting of the So- Analitis, F., Velderman, M. K., Ravens-Sieberer, lescents. Perspectives on Sexual and Repro-
ciety for Research on Biological Rhythms, U., Detmar, S., Erhart, M., Herdman, M., . . . ductive Health, 38, 132–138.
Jacksonville, FL. European Kidscreen Group. (2009). Being Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Erkanli, A., & Worthman,
Alli, M. M., Amialchuk, A., & Dwyer, D. S. (2011). bullied: Associated factors in children and C. M. (1999). Pubertal changes in hormone
The social contagion effect of marijuana use adolescents 8 to 18 years old in 11 European levels and depression in girls. Psychological
among adolescents. PLoS ONE, 6, e16183. countries. Pediatrics, 123, 569–577. Medicine, 29, 1043–1053.
Allison, K., Crawford, I., Leone, P., Trickett, E., Anderman, E. (2002). School effects on psycho- Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Worthman, C.
Perez-Febles, A., Burton, L., & Le Blanc, R. logical outcomes during adolescence. Journal (1998). Puberty and depression: The roles
(1999). Adolescent substance use: Preliminary of Educational Psychology, 94, 795–809. of age, pubertal status, and pubertal tim-
examinations of school and neighborhood Anderman, E., Cupp, P. K., Lane, D. R., Zimmer- ing. Psychological Medicine, 28, 51–61.
context. American Journal of Community Psy- man, R., Gray, D. L., & O’Connell, A. (2011). Angold, A., Worthman, C., Costello, E., Stangl, D.,
chology, 27, 111–141. Classroom goal structures and HIV and Messer, S., & Tweed, D. (1996, March). Pu-
Allwood, M., & Bell, D. (2008). A preliminary pregnancy prevention education in rural berty and depression. Paper presented at the
examination of emotional and cognitive high school health classrooms. Journal of Re- Society for Research on Adolescence, Boston.
mediators in the relations between vio- search on Adolescence, 21, 904–922. Ansary, N. S., & Luthar, S. S. (2009). Distress and
lence exposure and violent behaviors in Anderman, E., & Midgley, C. (1996, March). academic achievement among adolescents
youth. Journal of Community Psychology, Changes in achievement goal orientations af- of affluence: A study of externalizing and
36, 989–1007. ter the transition to middle school. Paper pre- internalizing problem behaviors and school
Almeida, J., Johnson, R. M., Corliss, H. L., sented at the biennial meetings of the Society performance. Development and Psychopa-
Molnar, B. E., & Azrael, D. (2009). Emotional for Research on Adolescence, Boston. thology, 21, 319–341.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-3 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-3

Antshel, K., & Barkley, R. (2011). Attention outcomes of early puberty. Journal of Youth occupational destinations (pp. 471–510). Ox-
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. (2011). In and Adolescence, 40, 1423–1435. ford: Clarendon Press.
B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclope- Armor, D. (1972). School and family effects on Arunkumar, R., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (1999).
dia of adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 56–61). New Black and White achievement: A reexamina- Perceiving high or low home–school disso-
York: Academic Press. tion of the USOE data. In F. Mosteller & D. nance: Longitudinal effects on adolescent
Apel, R., Bushway, S., Brame, R., Haviland, A. M., Moynihan (Eds.), On equality of educational emotional and academic well-being. Journal
Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (2007). Unpack- opportunity. New York: Random House. of Research on Adolescence, 9, 441–466.
ing the relationship between adolescent em- Armour, S., & Haynie, D. L. (2007). Adolescent Asakawa, K., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1998).
ployment and antisocial behavior: A matched sexual debut and later delinquency. Journal The quality of experience of Asian American
samples comparison. Criminology, 45, 67–97. of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 141–152. adolescents in academic activities: An explo-
Apel, R., & Kaukinen, C. (2008). On the relation- Armstrong, T. D., & Costello, E. (2002). Com- ration of educational achievement. Journal
ship between family structure and antisocial munity studies on adolescent substance of Research on Adolescence, 8, 241–262.
behavior: Parental cohabitation and blended use, abuse, or dependence and psychiatric Asakawa, K., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000).
households. Criminology: An Interdisciplinary comorbidity. Journal of Consulting and Feelings of connectedness and internalization
Journal, 46, 35–69. Clinical Psychology, 70, 1224–1239. of values in Asian American adolescents.
Apfel, N., & Seitz, V. (1997). The firstborn sons of Arndorfer, C. L., & Stormshak, E. A. (2008). Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 121–145.
African American teen mothers: Perspectives Same-sex versus other-sex best friendship Asato, M., Terwilliger, R., Woo, J., & Luna, B.
on risk and resilience. In S. Luthar, J. Burack, in early adolescence: Longitudinal predic- (2010). White matter development in ado-
D. Cicchetti, & J. Weisz (Eds.), Developmental tors of antisocial behavior throughout ado- lescence: a DTI study. Cerebral Cortex, 20,
psychopathology: Perspectives on risk and dis- lescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 2122–2131.
order (pp. 484–506). New York: Cambridge 37, 1059–1070. Aseltine, R., & Gore, S. (2005). Work, post-
University Press. secondary education, and psychosocial func-
Arnett, J. (1994). Are college students adults? Their
Aquilino, W. (2005). Impact of family structure conceptions of the transition to adulthood. tioning following the transition from high
on parental attitudes toward the economic Journal of Adult Development, 1, 213–224. school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20,
support of adult children over the transition 615–639.
Arnett, J. (1998). Learning to stand alone: The
to adulthood. Journal of Family Influence, 26, Aseltine, R., Gore, S., & Colten, M. (1994).
contemporary American transition to
143–167. Depression and the social developmental
adulthood in cultural and historical con-
Aquilino, W., & Supple, A. J. (2001). Long- text. Human Development, 41, 295–315. context of adolescence. Journal of Person-
term effects of parenting practices during ality and Social Psychology, 67, 252–263.
Arnett, J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A the-
adolescence on well-being: Outcomes in Aseltine, R., Jr., Gore, S., & Colten, M. (1998).
ory of development from the late teens
young adulthood. Journal of Family Issues, The co-occurrence of depression and sub-
through the twenties. American Psycholo-
22, 289–308. stance abuse in late adolescence. Develop-
gist, 55, 469–480.
Arbeton, A., Eccles, J., & Harold, R. (1994, February). ment & Psychopathology, 10, 549–570.
Parents’ perceptions of their children’s competence: Arnett, J. (2001). Adolescents’ responses to ciga-
rette advertisements for five “youth brands” Aseltine, R. H., Schilling, E. A., James, A.,
The role of parent attributions. Paper presented Glanovsky, J. L., & Jacobs, D. (2009). Age
at the biennial meetings of the Society for Re- and one “adult brand.” Journal of Research on
Adolescence, 11, 425–443. variability in the association between heavy
search on Adolescence, San Diego. episodic drinking and adolescent suicide
Archibald, A., Graber, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. Arnett, J. (2002). Adolescents in Western countries
attempts: Findings from a large-scale,
(1999). Associations among parent–adolescent on the threshold of the 21st century. In B.
school-based screening program. Journal of
relationships, pubertal growth, dieting, and Brown, R. Larson, & T. Saraswathi (Eds.), The
the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
body image in young adolescent girls: A short- world’s youth: Adolescence in eight regions of the
cent Psychiatry, 48, 262–270.
term longitudinal study. Journal of Research on globe. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Asher, S., Parker, J., & Walker, D. (1996). Distin-
Adolescence, 9, 395–415. Arnett, J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The wind-
guishing friendship from acceptance: Impli-
Archibald, A., Linver, M., Graber, J., & Brooks- ing road from the late teens through the twen-
cations for intervention and assessment. In
Gunn, J. (2002). Parent–adolescent relation- ties. New York: Oxford University Press.
W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup
ships and girls’ unhealthy eating: Testing Arnett, J. (2007). Suffering, selfish, slackers? Myths (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in
reciprocal effects. Journal of Research on Ado- and reality about emerging adults. Journal of childhood and adolescence (pp. 366–405).
lescence, 12, 451–461. Youth and Adolescence, 36, 23–29. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ardelt, M., & Day, L. (2002). Parents, siblings, Arnett, J., & Balle-Jensen, L. (1993). Cultural Asmussen, L., & Larson, R. (1991). The quality
and peers: Close social relationships and ad- bases of risk behavior: Danish adolescents. of family time among young adolescents in
olescent deviance. Journal of Early Adoles- Child Development, 64, 1842–1855. single-parent and married-parent families.
cence, 22, 310–349. Aronson, J., Jannone, S., McGlone, M., & Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53,
Arens, A., Yeung, A., Craven, R., & Hasselhorn, Johnson-Campbell, T. (2009). The Obama 1021–1030.
M. (2011). The twofold multidimensionality effect: An experimental test. Journal of Ex- Aspy, C. B., Vesely, S. K., Oman, R. F., Rodine, S.,
of academic self-concept: Domain specificity perimental Social Psychology, 45, 957–960. Marshall, L., & McLeroy, K. (2007). Parental
and separation between competence and af- Arseneault, L., Tremblay, R. E., Boulerice, B., & communication and youth sexual behaviour.
fect components. Journal of Educational Psy- Saucier, J. (2002). Obstetrical complications Journal of Adolescence, 30, 449–466.
chology, 103, 970–981. and violent delinquency: Testing two devel- Astor, R. (1994). Children’s moral reasoning
Arias, D. F., & Hernández, A. M. (2007). Emerg- opmental pathways. Child Development, 73, about family and peer violence: The role of
ing adulthood in Mexican and Spanish youth: 496–508. provocation and retribution. Child Develop-
Theories and realities. Journal of Adolescent Arum, R., & Hout, M. (1998). The early returns: ment, 65, 1054–1067.
Research, 22, 476–503. The transition from school to work in the Atkins, R., Hart, D., & Donnelly, T. M. (2005).
Arım, R. G., Tramonte, L., Shapka, J. D., Susan United States. In Y. Shavit & W. Mueller The association of childhood personality
Dahinten, V., & Douglas Willms, J. (2011). (Eds.), From school to work: A comparative type with volunteering during adolescence.
The family antecedents and the subsequent study of educational qualifications and Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 2, 145–162.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-4 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-4 References

Attie, I., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1989). The develop- Bailey, P. E., Bruno, Z. V., Bezerra, M. F., Bank, L., Burraston, B., & Snyder, J. (2004). Sibling
ment of eating problems in adolescent girls: Queiroz, I., Oliveira, C. M., & Chen-Mok, M. conflict and ineffective parenting as predic-
A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychol- (2001). Adolescent pregnancy 1 year later: tors of adolescent boys’ antisocial behavior
ogy, 25, 70–79. The effects of abortion vs. motherhood in and peer difficulties: Additive and interac-
Auerbach, R. P., Eberhart, N. K., & Abela, J. R. Z. Northeast Brazil. Journal of Adolescent tional effects. Journal of Research on Adoles-
(2010). Cognitive vulnerability to depression Health, 29, 223–232. cence, 14, 99–125.
in Canadian and Chinese adolescents. Journal Baird, A., Fugelsang, J., & Bennett, C. (2005, April). Bankston, C. L., III, & Caldas, S. (1998). Family
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 57–68. “What were you thinking”?: An fMRI study of structure, schoolmates, and racial inequali-
Aunola, K., Stattin, H., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2000). adolescent decision making. Poster presented ties in school achievement. Journal of Mar-
Parenting styles and adolescents’ achieve- at the 12th Annual Cognitive Neuroscience riage & the Family, 60, 715–723.
ment strategies. Journal of Adolescence, 23, Society (CNS) Meeting, New York. Banny, A. M., Heilbron, N., Ames, A., & Prinstein,
205–222. Bakan, D. (1972). Adolescence in America: From M. J. (2011). Relational benefits of relational
Austin, S. B., & Gortmaker, S. L. (2001). Dieting idea to social fact. In J. Kagan & R. Coles aggression: Adaptive and maladaptive asso-
and smoking initiation in early adolescent (Eds.), Twelve to sixteen: Early adolescence. ciations with adolescent friendship quality.
girls and boys: A prospective study. American New York: Norton. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1153–1166.
Journal of Public Health, 91, 446–450. Baker, L., Tuvblad, C., Reynolds, C., Zheng, M., Barber, B. (1996). Parental psychological con-
Avenevoli, S., & Steinberg, L. (2001). The con- Lozano, D. I., & Raine, A. (2009). Resting trol: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child
tinuity of depression across the adolescent heart rate and the development of antiso- Development, 67, 3296–3319.
transition. In H. Reese & R. Kail (Eds.), cial behavior from age 9 to 14: Genetic and Barber, B., & Eccles, J. (1992). Long-term influ-
Advances in child development and behavior environmental influences. Development and ence of divorce and single parenting on ad-
(Vol. 28, pp. 139–173). New York: Aca- Psychopathology, 21, 939–960. olescent family- and work-related values,
demic Press. Baker, T., & Velez, W. (1996). Access to and behaviors, and aspirations. Psychological
Averett, S. L., Rees, D. I., & Argys, L. M. (2002). opportunity in postsecondary education Bulletin, 111, 108–126.
The impact of government policies and in the United States: A review. Sociology of Barber, B., Eccles, J., & Stone, M. (2001). What-
neighborhood characteristics on teenage Education, Extra Issue, 69, 82–101. ever happened to the jock, the brain, and the
sexual activity and contraceptive use. Bakken, J., & Brown, B. (2010). Adolescent se- princess? Young adult pathways linked to
Amer i can Jou r nal of Public Health, 92 , cretive behavior: African American and adolescent activity involvement and social
1773–1778. Hmong Adolescents “strategies and justifi- identity. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16,
Azmitia, M., Cooper, C. R., & Brown, J. R. (2009). cations for managing parents” knowledge 429–455.
Support and guidance from families, friends, about peers. Journal of Research on Adoles- Barber, B., & Olsen, J. (1997). Socialization in
and teachers in Latino early adolescents’ cence, 20, 359–388. context: Connection, regulation, and au-
math pathways. Journal of Early Adolescence, Bakker, M. P., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Olde- tonomy in the family, school, neighbor-
29, 142–169. hinkel, A. J. (2010). Peer stressors and gender hood, and with peers. Journal of Adolescent
Babalola, S. (2004). Perceived peer behavior and differences in adolescents’ mental health: Research, 12, 287–315.
the timing of sexual debut in Rwanda: A sur- The TRAILS study. Journal of Adolescent Barber, B., & Olsen, J. (2004). Assessing the tran-
vival analysis of youth data. Journal of Youth Health, 46, 444–450. sitions to middle and high school. Journal of
and Adolescence, 33, 353–363. Balfanz, R., Legters, N., West, T. C., & Weber, L. M. Adolescent Research, 19, 3–30.
Bachman, J. (1983, Summer). Premature affluence: (2007). Are NCLB’s measures, incentives, and Barber, B., Stone, M., Hunt, J., & Eccles, J.
Do high school students earn too much? improvement strategies the right ones for the (2005). Benefits of activity participation:
Economic Outlook USA, 64–67. nation’s low-performing high schools? Ameri- The roles of identity affirmation and peer
Bachman, J., & Schulenberg, J. (1993). How can Education Research Journal, 44, 559–593. norm sharing. In J. Mahoney, R. Larson, &
part-time work intensity relates to drug use, Balsa, A., Homer, J., French, M., & Norton, E. J. Eccles (Eds.), Organized activities as con-
problem behavior, time use, and satisfaction (2011). Alcohol use and popularity: Social texts of development (pp. 185–210). Hills-
among high school seniors: Are these conse- payoffs from conforming to peers’ behav- dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
quences or merely correlates? Developmental ior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, Barboza, G. E., Schiamberg, L. B., Oehmke, J.,
Psychology, 29, 220–235. 559–568. Korzeniewski, S. J., Post, L. A., & Heraux, C.
Bachman, J. G., Staff, J., O’Malley, P. M., Schul- Balsano, A., Theokas, C., & Bobek, D. (2009). A G. (2009). Individual characteristics and the
enberg, J. E., & Freedman-Doan, P. (2011). shared commitment to youth: The integra- multiple contexts of adolescent bullying: An
Twelfth-grade student work intensity tion of theory, research, practice, and policy. ecological perspective. Journal of Youth and
linked to later educational attainment and In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook Adolescence, 38, 101–121.
substance use: New longitudinal evidence. of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. Bardone, A., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., Dickson, N.,
Developmental Psychology, 47, 344–363. 623–650). New York: Wiley. & Silva, P. (1996). Adult mental health and
Baer, J. (2002). Is family cohesion a risk or pro- Bámaca, M. Y., & Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2006). social outcomes of adolescent girls with de-
tective factor during adolescent develop- Testing a model of resistance to peer pressure pression and conduct disorder. Development
ment? Journal of Marriage and the Family, among Mexican-origin adolescents. Journal and Psychopathology, 8, 811–829.
64, 668–675. of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 631–645. Barnes, G., & Farrell, M. (1992). Parental sup-
Baer, J., & Schmitz, M. F. (2007). Ethnic differ- Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G., & port and control as predictors of adoles-
ences in trajectories of family cohesion for Pastorelli, C. (1996). Multifaceted impact of cent drinking, delinquency, and related
Mexican American and non-Hispanic White self-efficacy beliefs on academic function- problem behaviors. Journal of Marriage and
adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, ing. Child Development, 67, 1206–1222. the Family, 54, 763–776.
36, 583–592. Bandura, A., & Walters, R. (1959). Adolescent ag- Barnes, G. M., Hoffman, J. H., Welte, J. W., Farrell,
Bagwell, C., Newcomb, A., & Bukowski, W. (1998). gression. New York: Ronald Press. M. P., & Dintcheff, B. A. (2006). Effects of pa-
Preadolescent friendship and peer rejection as Banister, E. (1999). Women’s midlife experience rental monitoring and peer deviance on sub-
predictors of adult adjustment. Child Devel- of their changing bodies. Qualitative Health stance use and delinquency. Journal of Marriage
opment, 69, 140–153. Research, 9, 520–537. and Family, 68, 1084–1104.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-5 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-5

Barnes, R. (2007, June 29). Divided court limits high school and adaptation in the transition friends. Journal of Adolescent Research, 11,
use of race by school districts. Washington to young adulthood among African American 325–346.
Post, p. A01. youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, Beaver, K. M., DeLisi, M., Wright, J. P., & Vaughn,
Barnett, T. A., O’Loughlin, J., Sabiston, C. M., 877–890. M. G. (2009). Gene–environmental interplay
Karp, I., Belanger, M., Van Hulst, A., & Bauermeister, J. A., Zimmerman, M., Xue, Y., and delinquent involvement: Evidence of di-
Lambert, M. (2010). Teens and screens: The Gee, G. C., & Caldwell, C. H. (2009). Work- rect, indirect, and interactive effects. Journal
influence of screen time on adiposity in ing, sex partner age differences, and sexual of Adolescent Research, 24, 147–168.
adolescents. American Journal of Epidemi- behavior among African American youth. Beaver, K. M., & Wright, J. P. (2007). A child ef-
ology, 172, 255–262. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 802–813. fects explanation for the association between
Barnett, T., Rowley, S., Zimmerman, M., Vansa- Baumer, E. P., & South, S. J. (2001). Community family risk and involvement in an antisocial
dia, P., & Howard Caldwell, C. (2011). A effects on youth sexual activity. Journal of lifestyle. Journal of Adolescent Research, 22,
longitudinal study of household change on Marriage and the Family, 63, 540–554. 640–664.
African American adolescents. Journal of Baumrind, D. (1978). Parental disciplinary pat- Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse
Community Psychology, 39, 303–315. terns and social competence in children. control and loss of willpower to resist drugs:
Barratt, M., Roach, M., Morgan, K., & Colbert, K. Youth and Society, 9, 239–276. A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuro-
(1996). Adjustment to motherhood by single science, 8, 1458–1463.
Bauserman, R., & Rind, B. (1997). Psychological
adolescents. Family Relations, 45, 209–215. Becker, B. E., & Luthar, S. S. (2007). Peer-
correlates of male child and adolescent sexual
Barrett-Singer, A. T., & Weinstein, R. S. (2000). experiences with adults: A review of the non- perceived admiration and social preference:
Differential parent treatment predicts clinical literature. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Contextual correlates of positive peer re-
achievement and self-perceptions in two cul- 26, 105–141. gard among suburban and urban adoles-
tural contexts. Journal of Family Psychology, cents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17,
Bayer, P., Hjalmarsson, R., & Pozen, D. (2009).
14, 491–509. 117–144.
Building criminal capital behind bars: Peer
Barrocas, A. L., & Hankin, B. L. (2011). Develop- effects in juvenile corrections. Quarterly Beckwith, L., Cohen, S., & Hamilton, C. (1999).
mental pathways to depressive symptoms in Journal of Economics, 124, 105–147. Maternal sensitivity during infancy and sub-
adolescence: A multi-wave prospective study sequent life events relate to attachment repre-
Bayley, N. (1949). Consistency and variability in
of negative emotionality, stressors, and anxi- sentation at early adulthood. Developmental
the growth of intelligence from birth to eigh-
ety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Psychology, 35, 693–700.
teen years. Journal of General Psychology, 75,
39, 489–500. Beets, M. W., Flay, B. R., Vuchinich, S.,
165–196.
Barry, C. M., & Nelson, L. J. (2005). The role of Snyder, F. J., Acock, A., Li, K. K., . . . Durlack,
Beal, S. J., & Crockett, L. J. (2010). Adolescents’
religion in the transition to adulthood for J. (2009). Use of a social and character
occupational and educational aspirations
young emerging adults. Journal of Youth and development program to prevent substance
and expectations: Links to high school ac-
Adolescence 34, 245–255. use, violent behaviors, and sexual activity
tivities and adult educational attainment.
Barry, C. M., & Wentzel, K. R. (2006). Friend among elementary-school students in
Developmental Psychology, 46, 258–265.
influence on prosocial behavior: The role Hawaii. American Journal of Public Health,
of motivational factors and friendship Beam, M. R., Chen, C., & Greenberger, E. (2002). 99, 1438–1445.
characteristics. Developmental Psychology, The nature of adolescents’ relationship with Bell, A., Weinberg, M., & Hammersmith, S.
42, 153–163. their “very important” nonparental adults. (1981). Sexual preference: Its development in
American Journal of Community Psychology, men and women. Bloomington: Indiana
Bartko, W. T., & Eccles, J. (2003). Adolescent
30, 305–325. University Press.
participation in structured and unstruc-
tured activities: A person-oriented analy- Bean, R. A., Barber, B. K., & Crane, D. R. (2006). Bell, J., & Bromnick, R. (2000, March). A grounded
sis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, Parental support, behavioral control, and psy- approach to understanding modern dilemmas
233–241. chological control among African American of individuality. Paper presented at the eighth
youth: The relationships to academic grades, biennial meeting of the Society for Research
Basile, K. C., Black, M. C., Simon, T. R., Arias, I.,
delinquency, and depression. Journal of Fam- on Adolescence, Chicago.
Brener, N. D., & Saltzman, L. E. (2006). The
ily Issues, 27, 1335–1355.
association between self-reported lifetime Bellmore, A. (2011). Peer rejection and unpopu-
history of forced sexual intercourse and re- Bean, R. A., Bush, K. R., McKenry, P. C., & Wilson, larity: Associations with GPAs across the
cent health-risk behaviors: Findings from S. M. (2003). The impact of parental support, transition to middle school. Journal of Edu-
the 2003 national youth risk behavior survey. behavioral control, and psychological control cational Psychology, 103, 282–295.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, e1–e7. on the academic achievement and self-esteem
Bellmore, A., Villarreal, V. M., & Ho, A. Y. (2011).
of African American and European American
Bassi, M., Steca, P., Delle Fave, A., & Caprara, G. V. Staying cool across the first year of middle
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research,
(2007). Academic self-efficacy beliefs and school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40,
18, 523–541.
quality of experience in learning. Journal of 776–785.
Youth and Adolescence, 36, 301–312. Bearman, P. S., & Brückner, H. (2001). Promising Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991).
Batterink, L., Yokum, S., & Stice, E. (2010). Body the future: Virginity pledges and first inter- Childhood experience, interpersonal devel-
mass correlates inversely with inhibitory course. American Journal of Sociology, 106, opment, and reproductive strategy: An
control in response to food among adoles- 859–912. evolutionary theory of socialization. Child
cent girls: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 52, Bearman, P., & Moody, J. (2004). Suicide and Development, 62, 647–670.
1696–1703. friendships among American adolescents. Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., Houts, R., Friedman, S.,
Battin-Pearson, S., Newcomb, M. D., Abbott, R. American Journal of Public Health, 94, 89–96. DeHart, G., . . . Cauffman, E. (2007). Family
D., Hill, K. G., Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. Bearman, S. K., & Stice, E. (2008). Testing a gen- rearing antecedents of pubertal timing. Child
(2000). Predictors of early high school der additive model: The role of body image Development, 78, 1302–1321.
dropout: A test of five theories. Journal of in adolescent depression. Journal of Abnor- Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., Houts, R. M., Halpern-
Educational Psychology, 92, 568–582. mal Child Psychology, 36, 1251–1263. Felsher, B. L., & NICHD Early Child Care
Bauermeister, J. A., Zimmerman, M. A., Barnett, Beaumont, S. (1996). Adolescent girls’ percep- Research Network. (2010). The development
T. E., & Caldwell, C. H. (2007). Working in tions of conversations with mothers and of reproductive strategy in females: Early
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-6 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-6 References

maternal harshness → earlier menarche → competence. Journal of Educational Psychol- pledges and adolescent sexual behavior. Jour-
increased sexual risk taking. Developmental ogy, 99, 140–153. nal of Adolescent Health, 36, 428–436.
Psychology, 46, 120–128. Ben-Zur, H., & Reshef-Kfir, Y. (2003). Risk tak- Berzonsky, M. (2004). Identity style, parental au-
BeLue, R., Francis, L. A., & Colaco, B. (2009). ing and coping strategies among Israeli ado- thority, and identity commitment. Journal of
Mental health problems and overweight in a lescents. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 255–265. Youth and Adolescence, 33, 213–220.
nationally representative sample of adoles- Berge, J., Wall, M., Loth, K., & Neumark-Sztainer, Best, A. L. (2006). Freedom, constraint, and fam-
cents: Effects of race and ethnicity. Pediatrics, D. (2010). Parenting style as a predictor of ad- ily responsibility: Teens and parents collab-
123, 697–702. olescent weight and weight-related behaviors. oratively negotiate around the car, class,
Benda, B., & Corwyn, R. (1998). Race and gender Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 331–338. gender, and culture. Journal of Family Issues,
differences in theories of sexual behavior Berkel, C., Knight, G., Zeiders, K., Tein, J., Roosa, 27, 55–84.
among rural adolescents residing in AFDC M., Gonzales, N., & Saenz, D. (2010). Dis- Beyers, J. M., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K.
families. Youth and Society, 30, 59–88. crimination and adjustment for Mexican A. (2003). Neighborhood structure, parent-
Bender, H., Allen, J., McElhaney, K., Antonishak, J., American adolescents: A prospective exami- ing processes, and the development of
Moore, C., O’Beirne Kelly, H., & Davis, S. nation of the benefits of culturally related youths’ externalizing behaviors: A multilevel
(2007). Use of harsh physical discipline and values. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, analysis. American Journal of Community
developmental outcomes in adolescence. De- 893–915. Psychology, 31, 35–53.
velopment and Psychopathology, 19, 227–242. Berkel, C., Murry, V. M., Hurt, T. R., Chen, Y., Beyers, J. M., Loeber, R., Wikström, P. H., &
Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. New Brody, G. H., Simons, R. L., . . . Gibbons, F. Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2001). What pre-
York: Houghton Mifflin. (2009). It takes a village: Protecting rural dicts adolescent violence in better-off neigh-
African American youth in the context of borhoods? Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Benenson, J. F., & Christakos, A. (2003). The
racism. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 29, 369–381.
greater fragility of females’ versus males’
175–188. Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (2008). Dynamics of
closest same-sex friendships. Child Develop-
ment, 74, 1123–1129. Berkowitz, R. I., Wadden, T. A., Tershakovec, perceived parenting and identity formation
A. M., & Cronquist, J. L. (2003). Behavior in late adolescence. Journal of Adolescence,
Benhorin, S., & McMahon, S. (2008). Exposure
therapy and sibutramine for the treatment of 31, 165–184.
to violence and aggression: Protective roles
adolescent obesity. Journal of the American Beyers, W., Goossens, L., Vasant, I., & Moors, E.
of social support among urban African
Medical Association, 289, 1805–1812. (2003). A structural model of autonomy in
American youth. Journal of Community Psy-
Berlan, E. D., Corliss, H. L., Field, A. E., Goodman, middle and late adolescence: Connectedness,
chology, 36, 723–743.
E., & Bryn Austin, S. (2010). Sexual orienta- separation, detachment, and agency. Journal
Benjet, C., & Hernandez-Guzman, L. (2002). A tion and bullying among adolescents in the of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 351–365.
short-term longitudinal study of pubertal growing up today study. Journal of Adolescent Beyth-Marom, R., Austin, L., Fischoff, B.,
change, gender, and psychological well-being Health, 46, 366–371. Palmgren, C., & Jacobs-Quadrel, M. (1993).
of Mexican early adolescents. Journal of
Berliner, D., & Biddle, B. (1995). The manufactured Perceived consequences of risky behaviors:
Youth and Adolescence, 31, 429–442.
crisis: Myths, fraud, and attack on America’s Adults and adolescents. Developmental Psy-
Benner, A. D. (2011). Latino adolescents’ loneli- public schools. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. chology, 29, 549–563.
ness, academic performance, and the buffer-
Berndt, T. (1996). Exploring the effects of Biddlecom, A. E., Awusabo-Asare, K., & Bankole,
ing nature of friendships. Journal of Youth
friendship quality on social development. A. (2009). Role of parents in adolescent sex-
and Adolescence, 40, 556–567.
In W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup ual activity and contraceptive use in four
Benner, A. D., & Graham, S. (2009). The transi- (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship African countries. International Perspectives
tion to high school as a developmental pro- in childhood and adolescence (pp. 346–365). on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 35, 72–81.
cess among multiethnic urban youth. Child New York: Cambridge University Press. Biehl, M., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2001). Adoles-
Development, 80, 356–376. Berndt, T., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influence cents’ and adults’ understanding of probabil-
Benner, A. D., & Graham, S. (2011). Latino ado- on adolescents’ adjustment to school. Child ity expressions. Journal of Adolescent Health,
lescents’ experiences of discrimination across Development, 66, 1312–1329. 28, 30–35.
the first 2 years of high school: Correlates Berndt, T., & Perry, T. (1990). Distinctive features Biehl, M. C., Natsuaki, M. N., & Ge, X. (2007).
and influences on educational outcomes. and effects of early adolescent friendships. In The influence of pubertal timing on alcohol
Child Development, 82, 508–519. R. Montemayor, G. Adams, & T. Gullota use and heavy drinking trajectories. Journal
Benner, A. D., Graham, S., & Mistry, R. S. (2008). (Eds.), Advances in adolescence research (Vol. of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 153–167.
Discerning direct and mediated effects of 2, pp. 269–287). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Biener, L., & Siegel, M. (2000). Tobacco market-
ecological structures and processes on ado- Berninger, V., & Miller, B. (2011). Adolescent ing and adolescent smoking: More support
lescents’ educational outcomes. Developmen- specific learning disabilities. In B. Brown & for a causal inference. American Journal of
tal Psychology, 44, 840–854. M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- Public Health, 90, 407–411.
Benner, A. D., & Kim, S. Y. (2009). Experiences of cence (Vol. 3, pp. 21–29). New York: Aca- Bierman, K., & Furman, W. (1984). The effects of
discrimination among Chinese American demic Press. social skills training and peer involvement
adolescents and the consequences for socio- Berns, G., Capra, C., Moore, S., & Noussair, C. on the social adjustment of preadolescents.
emotional and academic development. De- (2010). Neural mechanisms of the influence Child Development, 55, 151–162.
velopmental Psychology, 45, 1682–1694. of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. Bierman, K., & Wargo, J. (1995). Predicting the
Benner, A. D., & Kim, S. Y. (2010). Understand- Neuroimage, 49, 2687–2696. longitudinal course associated with aggressive-
ing Chinese American adolescents’ develop- Bersamin, M., Fisher, D., Walker, S., Hill, D., & rejected, aggressive-nonrejected, and rejected
mental outcomes: Insights from the family Grube, J. (2007). Defining virginity and ab- non-aggressive status. Development and Psy-
stress model. Journal of Research on Adoles- stinence: Adolescents’ interpretations of sex- chopathology, 7, 669–682.
cence, 20, 1–12. ual behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, Bijvank, M., Konijn, E., Bushman, B., & Roelofsma,
Benner, A. D., & Mistry, R. S. (2007). Congru- 41, 182–188. P. (2009). Age and violent-content labels make
ence of mother and teacher educational ex- Bersamin, M., Walker, S., Waiters, E., Fisher, D., & video games forbidden fruits for youth. Pedi-
pectations and low-income youth’s academic Grube, J. (2005). Promising to wait: Virginity atrics, 123, 870–876.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-7 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-7

Bills, D., Helms, L., & Ozcan, M. (1995). The im- Brookings papers on educational policy. Blos, P. (1967). The second individuation pro-
pact of student employment on teachers’ Washington, DC: Brookings Institution. cess of adolescence. In R. S. Eissler et al.
attitudes and behaviors toward working Bishop, J., Bishop, M., Gelbwasser, L., Green, S., (Eds.), Psychoanalytic study of the child (Vol.
students. Youth and Society, 27, 169–193. & Zuckerman, A. (2003). Why do we harass 15). New York: International Universities
Bimmel, N., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans- nerds and freaks?: Towards a theory of stu- Press.
Kranenburg, M., Juffer, F., & De Geus, E. dent culture and norms. In D. Ravitch Blos, P. (1979). The adolescent passage. New York:
(2008). Problem behavior and heart rate re- (Ed.), Brookings papers on education policy. International Universities Press.
activity in adopted adolescents: Longitudinal Washington, DC: Brookings Institution. Blum, R., Beuhring, T., Wunderlich, M., & Resnick,
and concurrent relations. Journal of Research Black, M., Oberlander, S. E., Lewis, T., Knight, E. D., M. (1996). Don’t ask, they won’t tell: The qual-
on Adolescence, 18, 201–214. Zolotor, A. J., & Litrownik, A. J., . . . ity of adolescent health screening in five prac-
Bingenheimer, J. B. (2005). Firearm violence English, D. (2009). Sexual intercourse among tice settings. American Journal of Public Health,
exposure and serious violent behavior. adolescents maltreated before age 12: A pro- 86, 1767–1772.
Science, 308, 1323–1326. spective investigation. Pediatrics, 124, 941–949. Blum, R., & Nelson-Mmari, K. (2004). The health
Bingham, C., & Crockett, L. (1996). Longitudinal Black, M., Ricardo, I., & Stanton, B. (1997). So- of young people in a global context. Journal of
adjustment patterns of boys and girls cial and psychological factors associated with Adolescent Health, 35, 402–418.
experiencing early, middle, and late sexual AIDS risk behaviors among low-income, Blum, R., Resnick, M., & Stark, T. (1990). Factors
intercourse. Developmental Psychology, 32, urban, African American adolescents. Jour- associated with the use of court bypass by
647–658. nal of Research on Adolescence, 7, 173–195. minors to obtain abortions. Family Planning
Bingham, C., & Shope, J. T. (2004). Adolescent Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, Perspectives, 22, 158–160.
problem behavior and problem driving in C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence Blum, R., & Rinehart, P. (2000). Reducing the
young adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Re- predict achievement across an adolescent risk: Connections that make a difference in the
search, 19, 205–223. transition: A longitudinal study and an in- lives of youth. Minneapolis: Division of Gen-
Birkett, M., Espelage, D. L., & Koenig, B. (2009). tervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263. eral Pediatrics and Adolescent Health, Uni-
LGB and questioning students in schools: Blair, J. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial versity of Minnesota.
The moderating effects of homophobic bul- prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Blumenthal, H., Leen-Feldner, E. W., Babson,
lying and school climate on negative out- Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 387–392. K. A., Gahr, J. L., Trainor, C. D., & Frala, J. L.
comes. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, Blais, J. J., Craig, W. M., Pepler, D., & Connolly, J. (2011). Elevated social anxiety among early
989–1000. (2008). Adolescents online: The importance maturing girls. Developmental Psychology, 47,
Birmaher, B., Rabin, B., Garcia, M., Jain, U., of Internet activity choices to salient rela- 1133–1140.
Whiteside, T., Williamson, D., . . . Ryan, N. tionships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Blyth, D., Simmons, R., & Zakin, D. (1985). Sat-
(1994). Cellular immunity in depressed, 37, 522–536. isfaction with body image for early adoles-
conduct disorder, and normal adolescents: Blake, S. M., Ledsky, R., Goodenow, C., Sawyer, R., cent females: The impact of pubertal timing
Role of adverse life events. Journal of the Lohrmann, D., & Windsor, R. (2003). Condom within different school environments. Jour-
American Academy of Child and Adolescent availability programs in Massachusetts high nal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 227–236.
Psychiatry, 33, 671–678. schools: Relationships with condom use and Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J.
Biro, F., Galvez, M. P., Greenspan, L. C., sexual behavior. American Journal of Public (2010). Risk factors for conduct disorder and
Succop, P. A., Vangeepuram, N., . . . Pinney, Health, 93, 955–962. oppositional/defiant disorder: Evidence
S. M., (2010). Pubertal assessment method Blakemore, S-J. (2012). Imaging brain development: from a New Zealand birth cohort. Journal of
and baseline characteristics in a mixed lon- The adolescent brain. Neuroimage., 61, 397-406. the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
gitudinal study of girls, Pediatrics, 126, Blanc, A. K., Tsui, A. O., Croft, T. N., & Trevitt, Psychiatry, 49, 1125–1133.
583–590. J. L. (2010). Patterns and trends in adoles- Boden, J., Fergusson, D., & Horwood, J. (2011).
Biro F., Huang, B., Crawford, P., Lucky, A., Striegel- cents’ contraceptive use and discontinuation Age of menarche and psychosocial outcomes
Moore, R., . . . Barton, B. (2006). Pubertal cor- in developing countries and comparisons in a New Zealand birth cohort. Journal of the
relates in Black and White girls. Journal of with adult women. International Perspectives American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Pediatrics, 148, 234–240. on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 35, 63–71. Psychiatry, 50, 132–140.
Biro, F., Striegel-Moore, R., Franko, D. L., Padgett, Bleakley, A., Hennessy, M., Fishbein, M., & Jordan, Bogenschneider, K. (1997). Parental involvement
J., & Bean, J. A. (2006). Self-esteem in adoles- A. (2008). It works both ways: The relation- in adolescent schooling: A proximal process
cent females. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, ship between sexual content in the media and with transcontextual validity. Journal of Mar-
501–507. adolescent sexual behavior. Media Psychology, riage and the Family, 59, 1–16.
Bishai, D., Mercer, D., & Tapales, A. (2005). Can 11, 443–461. Bogin, B. (2011). Puberty and adolescence: An
government policies help adolescents avoid Blinn-Pike, L. (1999). Why abstinent adolescents evolutionary perspective. In B. Brown &
risky behavior? Preventive Medicine, 40, report they have not had sex: Understanding M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence
197–202. sexually resilient youth. Family Relations, 48, (Vol. 1, pp. 275–286). New York: Academic
Bishop, D., Hardiman, M., Uwer, R., & von 295–301. Press.
Suchodoletz, W. (2007). Maturation of the Blodgett Salafia, E., Gondoli, D., & Grundy, A. Bohnert, A. M., Kane, P., & Garber, J. (2008). Or-
long-latency auditory ERP: Step function (2009). The longitudinal interplay of mater- ganized activity participation and internal-
changes at start and end of adolescence. nal warmth and adolescents’ self-disclosure izing and externalizing symptoms: Reciprocal
Developmental Science, 10, 565–575. in predicting maternal knowledge. Journal of relations during adolescence. Journal of
Bishop, J. (1999). Nerd harrassment, incentives, Research on Adolescence, 19, 654–668. Youth and Adolescence, 37, 239–250.
school priorities and learning. In S. Mayer & Blomfield, C. J., & Barber, B. L. (2011). Develop- Boldero, J., & Fallon, B. (1995). Adolescent help-
P. Peterson (Eds.), Earning and learning. mental experiences during extracurricular seeking: What do they get help for and from
Washington, DC: Brookings Institution. activities and Australian adolescents’ self- whom? Journal of Adolescence, 18, 193–209.
Bishop, J. (2001). The role of end-of-course ex- concept: Particularly important for youth Bolland, J. M., Lian, B. E., & Formichella, C. M.
ams and minimum competency exams in from disadvantaged schools. Journal of Youth (2005). The origins of hopelessness among
standards-based reforms. In D. Ravitch (Ed.), and Adolescence, 40, 582–594. inner-city African American adolescents.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-8 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-8 References

American Journal of Community Psychology, Borzekowski, D., Fobil, J., & Asante, K. (2006). perspective on bullying-related attitudes and
36, 293–305. Online access by adolescents in Accra: behaviors: The influences of school context.
Bolling, D., Pitskel, N., Deen, B., Crowley, M., Ghanaian teens’ use of the Internet for American Journal of Community Psychology,
Mayes, L., & Pelphrey, K. (2011). Develop- health information. Developmental Psy- 43, 204–220.
ment of neural systems for processing social chology, 42, 450–458. Brady, S., & Halpern-Felsher, B. (2007). Adoles-
exclusion from childhood to adolescence. Bos, H., Sandfort, T., de Bruyn, E., & Hakvoort, E. cents’ reported consequences of having oral
Developmental Psychology, 14, 1431–1444. (2008). Same-sex attraction, social relation- sex versus vaginal sex. Pediatrics, 119, 229–236.
Bomar, J., & Sabatelli, R. (1996). Family system ships, psychosocial functioning, and school Brahmbhatt, S. B., McAuley, T., & Barch, D. M.
dynamics, gender, and psychosocial maturity performance in early adolescence. Develop- (2008). Functional developmental similari-
in late adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Re- mental Psychology, 44, 59–68. ties and differences in the neural correlates
search, 11, 421–439. Botticello, A. L. (2009). School contextual influ- of verbal and nonverbal working memory
Boney-McCoy, S., & Finkelhor, D. (1995). The ences and the risk for adolescent alcohol tasks. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1020–1031.
psychosocial impact of violent victimization misuse. American Journal of Community Psy- Branch, C. (1995, August). Gang bangers: Ethnic
in a national youth sample. Journal of Con- chology, 43, 85–97. variations. Paper presented at the American
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 726–736. Botvin, M., & Vitaro, F. (1995). The impact of Psychological Association, New York.
Bong, M. (2009). Age-related differences in peer relationships on aggression in child- Brand, S., Gerber, M., Beck, J., Hatzinger, M.,
achievement goal differentiation. Journal of hood: Inhibition through coercion or pro- Puhse, U., & Holsboer-Trachsler, E. (2010).
Educational Psychology, 101, 879–896. motion through peer support. In J. McCord High exercise levels are related to favorable
Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., & (Ed.), Coercion and punishment in long-term sleep patterns and psychological functioning
Verhulst, F. C. (2004). Developmental trajec- perspectives (pp. 183–197). New York: Cam- in adolescents: A comparison of athletes and
tories of externalizing behaviors in child- bridge University Press. controls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46,
hood and adolescence. Child Development, Bouchey, H. A., Shoulberg, E. K., Jodl, K. M., & Ec- 133–141.
75, 1523–1537. cles, J. S. (2010). Longitudinal links between Branstetter, S. A., Furman, W., & Cottrell, L.
Bontempo, D. E., & D’Augelli, A. R. (2002). Ef- older sibling features and younger siblings’ aca- (2009). The influence of representations of
fects of at-school victimization and sexual demic adjustment during early adolescence. attachment, maternal–adolescent relation-
orientation on lesbian, gay, or bisexual Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 197–211. ship quality, and maternal monitoring on
youths’ health risk behavior. Journal of Ado- Bowen, N. K., & Bowen, G. L. (2002). Neighbor- adolescent substance use: A 2-year longitudi-
lescent Health, 30, 364–374. hood social disorganization, families, and nal examination. Child Development, 80,
Boone, E., & Leadbeater, B. (2006). Game on: the educational behavior of adolescents. 1448–1462.
Diminishing risks for depressive symptoms Journal of Adolescent Research, 17, 468–490. Bratt, C. (2008). Guardians to counter adolescent
in early adolescence through positive in- Bowker, A. (2004). Predicting friendship stability drug use? Limitations of a routine activities
volvement in team sports. Journal of Research during early adolescence. Journal of Early approach. Youth and Society, 39, 385–405.
on Adolescence, 16, 79–90. Adolescence, 24, 85–112. Braun, H., Jenkins, F., & Grigg, W. (2006). Com-
Booth, A., Johnson, D., Granger, D., Crouter, A., Bowker, J. C. (2011). Examining two types of best paring private schools and public schools using
& McHale, S. (2003). Testosterone and child friendship dissolution during early adolescence. hierarchical linear modeling. U.S. Depart-
and adolescent adjustment: The moderating Journal of Early Adolescence, 31, 656–670. ment of Education, National Center for
role of parent–child relationships. Develop- Bowker, J. C., & Spencer, S. V. (2010). Friendship Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S.
mental Psychology, 39, 85–98. and adjustment: A focus on mixed-grade Government Printing Office.
Booth, A., Scott, M., & King, V. (2010). Father friendships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Brausch, A. M., & Gutierrez, P. M. (2010). Differ-
residence and adolescent problem behav- 39, 1318–1329. ences in non-suicidal self-injury and suicide
ior: Are youth always better off in two- Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. attempts in adolescents. Journal of Youth and
parent families? Journal of Family Issues, 31, Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Adolescence, 39, 233–242.
585–605. Boyer, T. W. (2006). The development of risk- Bray, J., Adams, G., Getz, J., & McQueen, A. (2003).
Booth, M. Z., & Sheehan, H. C. (2008). Percep- taking: A multi-perspective review. Develop- Individuation, peers, and adolescent alcohol
tions of people and place: Young adolescents’ mental Review, 26, 291–345. use: A latent growth analysis. Journal of Con-
interpretation of their schools in the United Boyle, M. H., Georgiades, K., Racine, Y., & Mustard, sulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 553–564.
States and the United Kingdom. Journal of C. (2007). Neighborhood and family influ- Bray, J., Adams, J., Getz, J., & Baer, P. E. (2001).
Adolescent Research, 23, 722–744. ences on educational attainment: Results Developmental, family, and ethnic influ-
Borelli, J. L., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Recipro- from the Ontario Child Health Study follow- ences on adolescent alcohol usage: A growth
cal, longitudinal associations among adoles- up 2001. Child Development, 78, 168–189. curve approach. Journal of Family Psychology,
cents’ negative feedback-seeking, depressive Brack, C., Brack, G., & Orr, D. (1996). Adolescent 15, 301–314.
symptoms, and peer relations. Journal of Ab- health promotion: Testing a model using Brechwald, W., & Prinstein, M. (2011). Beyond
normal Child Psychology, 34, 159–169. multidimensional scaling. Journal of Re- homophily: A decade of advances in under-
Borkowski, J., & Sneed, M. (2006). Will NCLB search on Adolescence, 6, 139–149. standing peer influence processes. Journal of
improve or harm public education? Harvard Bradford, K., Vaughn, L. B., & Barber, B. K. (2008). Research on Adolescence, 21, 166–179.
Educational Review, 76, 503–525. When there is conflict: Interparental conflict, Brendgen, M., Lamarche, V., Wanner, B., & Vitaro,
Born this Way Foundation. (2012). Website ac- parent–child conflict, and youth problem be- F. (2010). Links between friendship relations
cessed at http://bornthiswayfoundation.org/ haviors. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 780–805. and early adolescents’ trajectories of depressed
pages/our-mission/ on July 2, 2012. Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., Pipes McAdoo, H., mood. Developmental Psychology, 46, 491–501.
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. & Garcia-Coll, C. (2001). The home environ- Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. (2000).
(2010). Social competence, externalizing, ments of children in the United States, part Deviant friends and early adolescents’ emo-
and internalizing behavioral adjustment 1: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty tional and behavioral adjustment. Journal of
from early childhood through early adoles- status. Child Development, 72, 1844–1867. Research on Adolescence, 10, 173–189.
cence: Developmental cascades. Develop- Bradshaw, C. P., Sawyer, A. L., & O’Brennan, Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Doyle, A., Markiewicz,
ment and Psychopathology, 22, 717–735. L. M. (2009). A social disorganization D., & Bukowski, W. (2002). Same-sex peer
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-9 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-9

relations and romantic relationships during competence among rural African American (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing
early adolescence: Interactive links to emo- youths. Journal of Marriage and the Family, adolescent (pp. 16–23). Cambridge, MA:
tional, behavioral, and academic adjustment. 57, 567–579. Harvard University Press.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 77–103. Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., & Flor, D. (1996). Pa- Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. (1985). The ef-
Brenhouse, H. C., Sonntag, K. C., & Andersen, rental religiosity, family processes, and youth fects of delayed menarche in different con-
S. L. (2008). Transient D-sub-1 dopamine re- competence in rural, two-parent African- texts: Dance and nondance students. Journal
ceptor expression on prefrontal cortex pro- American families. Developmental Psychol- of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 285–300.
jection neurons: Relationship to enhanced ogy, 32, 696–706. Brown, B. (1990). Peer groups. In S. Feldman &
motivational salience of drug cues in adoles- Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., & McCoy, J. (1994). G. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The devel-
cence. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 2375–2382. Forecasting sibling relationships in early oping adolescent (pp. 171–196). Cambridge,
Brennan, P. A., Hall, J., Bor, W., Najman, J. M., & adolescence from child temperaments and MA: Harvard University Press.
Williams, G. (2003). Integrating biological family processes in middle childhood. Child Brown, B. (1996). Visibility, vulnerability, devel-
and social processes in relation to early-onset Development, 65, 771–784. opment, and context: Ingredients for a fuller
persistent aggression in boys and girls. De- Broh, B. A. (2002). Linking extracurricular pro- understanding of peer rejection in adoles-
velopmental Psychology, 39, 309–323. gramming to academic achievement: Who cence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 16, 27–36.
Brent, D., Emslie, G., Clarke, G., Wagner, K. D., benefits and why? Sociology of Education, 75, Brown, B. (1999). “You’re going out with who?”
Asarnow, J. R., Keller, M. . . . Zelazny, J. 69–95. Peer group influences on adolescent roman-
(2008). Switching to another SSRI or to ven- Broidy, L., Nagin, D., Tremblay, R., Bates, J., tic relationships. In W. Furman, B. Brown, &
lafaxine with or without cognitive behavioral Brame, B., Dodge, K., . . . Vitaro, F. (2003). C. Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives
therapy for adolescents with SSRI-resistant Developmental trajectories of childhood dis- on adolescent romantic relationships (pp. 291–
depression: The TORDIA randomized con- ruptive behaviors and adolescent delin- 329). New York: Cambridge University Press.
trolled trial. JAMA: Journal of the American quency: A six-site, cross-national study. Brown, B. (2004). Adolescents’ relationships
Medical Association, 299, 901–913. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222–245. with peers. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.),
Bridges, G., & Steen, S. (1998). Racial disparities Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human Handbook of adolescent psychology. New
in official assessments of juvenile offenders: development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- York: Wiley.
Attributional stereotypes as mediating mech- versity Press. Brown, B., & Bakken, J. (2011). Parenting and
anisms. American Sociological Review, 63, Bronstein, P., Duncan, P., Clauson, J., Abrams, C. peer relationships: Reinvigorating research
554–570. L., Yannett, N., Ginsburg, G., & Milne, M. on family–peer linkages in adolescence. Jour-
Brinch, C., & Galloway, T. (2012). Schooling in (1998). Preventing middle school adjust- nal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 153–165.
adolescence raises IQ scores. Proceedings of the ment problems for children from lower-in- Brown, B., Herman, M., Hamm, J. V., & Heck, D.
National Academy of Sciences, 109, 425–430. come families: A program for aware (2008). Ethnicity and image: Correlates of mi-
Brody, G., Beach, S., Philibert, R., Chen, Y.-F., Lei, parenting. Journal of Applied Developmental nority adolescents’ affiliation with individual-
M.-K., Murry, V., & Brown, A. (2009). Psychology, 19, 129–152. based versus ethnically defined peer crowds.
Parenting moderates a genetic vulnerability Bronstein, P., Ginsburg, G., & Herrera-Leavitt, I. Child Development, 79, 529–546.
factor in longitudinal increases in youths’ (2000). Parental predictors of motivational ori- Brown, B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships
substance use. Journal of Consulting and entation and academic performance in early in adolescence. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg
Clinical Psychology, 77, 1–11. adolescence: A longitudinal study. Unpub- (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology
Brody, G., Beach, S., Philibert, R., Chen, Y., & lished manuscript. Burlington, VT: Depart- (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 74–103). New York: Wiley.
Murry, V. (2009). Prevention effects moder- ment of Psychology, University of Vermont. Brown, B., Mory, M., & Kinney, D. (1994). Cast-
ate the association of 5-httlpr and youth risk Brookmeyer, K. A., Henrich, & Schwab-Stone, M. ing crowds in a relational perspective: Carica-
behavior initiation: G3E hypotheses tested (2005). Adolescents who witness community ture, channel, and context. In R. Montemayor,
via a randomized prevention design. Child violence: Can parent support and prosocial G. Adams, & T. Gullotta (Eds.), Advances in
Development, 80, 645–661. cognitions protect them from committing adolescent development: Vol. 5. Personal rela-
Brody, G., Chen, Y., Murry, V., Ge, X., Simons, R., violence? Child Development, 76, 917–929. tionships during adolescence. Newbury Park,
Gibbons, F., . . . Cutrona, C. (2006). Perceived Brooks-Gunn, J., & Donahue, E. (Eds.) (2008). CA: Sage.
discrimination and the adjustment of Afri- Children and electronic media. The Future of Brown, B., & Mounts, N. (1989, April). Peer
can American youths: A five-year longitudi- Children, 18. group structures in single versus multiethnic
nal analysis with contextual moderation Brooks-Gunn, J., Graber, J., & Paikoff, R. (1994). high schools. Paper presented at the biennial
effects. Child Development, 77, 1170–1189. Studying links between hormones and nega- meetings of the Society for Research in Child
Brody, G., Ge, X., Kim, S., Murry, V., Simons, R., tive affect: Models and measures. Journal of Development, Kansas City.
Gibbons, F., . . . Conger, R. (2003). Neighbor- Research on Adolescence, 4, 469–486. Brown, B., Mounts, N., Lamborn, S., & Steinberg,
hood disadvantage moderates associations of Brooks-Gunn, J., Newman, D., Holderness, C., & L. (1993). Parenting practices and peer group
parenting and older sibling problem attitudes Warren, M. (1994). The experience of breast affiliation in adolescence. Child Development,
and behavior with conduct disorders in development and girls’ stories about the pur- 64, 467–482.
African American children. Journal of Consult- chase of a bra. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Brown, B., Von Bank, H., & Steinberg, L. (2008).
ing and Clinical Psychology, 71, 211–222. cence, 23, 539–565. Smoke in the looking glass: Effects of discor-
Brody, G., Murry, V., Kogan, S., Gerrard, M., Gib- Brooks-Gunn, J., & Paikoff, R. (1993). “Sex is a dance between self- and peer-rated crowd
bons, F., Molgaard, V., . . . Wills, T. (2006). gamble, kissing is a game”: Adolescent sexu- affiliation on adolescent anxiety, depression,
The Strong African American Families Pro- ality and health promotion. In S. Millstein, A. and self-feelings. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
gram: A cluster-randomized prevention trial Petersen, & E. Nightingale (Eds.), Promoting cence, 37, 1163–1177.
of long-term effects and a mediational the health of adolescents: New directions for Brown, C. S., Alabi, B. O., Huynh, V. W., & Masten,
model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical the twenty-first century (pp. 180–208). New C. L. (2011). Ethnicity and gender in late
Psychology, 74, 356–366. York: Oxford University Press. childhood and early adolescence: Group
Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., & Flor, D. (1995). Brooks-Gunn, J., & Reiter, E. (1990). The role of identity and awareness of bias. Developmental
Linking family processes and academic pubertal processes. In S. Feldman & G. Elliott Psychology, 47, 463–471.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-10 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-10 References

Brown, J., & Bobkowski, P. (2011a). Media, influ- Bruvold, W. (1993). A meta-analysis of adoles- Buehler, C., & Welsh, D. P. (2009). A process
ence of. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), cent smoking prevention programs. Ameri- model of adolescents’ triangulation into par-
Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 189– can Journal of Public Health, 83, 872–880. ents’ marital conflict: The role of emotional
195). New York: Academic Press. Bryant, A., Schulenberg, J., O’Malley, P., Bachman, reactivity. Journal of Family Psychology, 23,
Brown, J., & Bobkowski, P. (2011b). Older and J., & Johnston, L. (2003). How academic 167–180.
newer media: Patterns of use and effects on achievement, attitudes, and behaviors relate Buhi, E. R., & Goodson, P. (2007). Predictors of
adolescents’ health and well-being. Journal of to the course of substance use during adoles- adolescent sexual behavior and intention: A
Research on Adolescence, 21, 95–113. cence: A 60-year, multiwave national longitu- theory-guided systematic review. Journal of
Brown, J., Cohen, P., Chen, H., Smailes, E., & dinal study. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Adolescent Health, 40, 4–21.
Johnson, J. (2004). Sexual trajectories of abused 13, 361–397. Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship,
and neglected youths. Journal of Developmental Bryant, A., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2002). Exam- interpersonal competence, and adjustment
& Behavioral Pediatrics, 25, 77–82. ining the effects of academic beliefs and be- during preadolescence and adolescence.
Brown, J., & L’Engle, K. (2009). X-Rated: Sexual haviors on changes in substance use among Child Development, 61, 1101–1111.
attitudes and behaviors associated with urban adolescents. Journal of Educational Buhrmester, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, inter-
U.S. early adolescents’ exposure to sexually Psychology, 94, 621–637. personal competence, and the developmen-
explicit media. Communication Research, Bryant, A., & Zimmerman, M. (2003). Role mod- tal contexts of early adolescent friendship. In
36, 129–151. els and psychosocial outcomes among African W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup,
Brown, J. D., L’Engle, K. L., Pardun, C. J., Guo, G., American adolescents. Journal of Adolescent (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in
Kenneavy, K., & Jackson, C. (2006). Sexy me- Research, 18, 36–67. childhood and adolescence (pp. 158–185).
dia matter: Exposure to sexual content in Buchanan, C., Eccles, J., & Becker, J. (1992). Are New York: Cambridge University Press.
music, movies, television, and magazines adolescents the victims of raging hormones? Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The de-
predicts Black and White adolescents’ sexual Evidence for activational effects of hormones velopment of companionship and intimacy.
behavior. Pediatrics, 117, 1018–1027. on moods and behavior at adolescence. Psy- Child Development, 58, 1101–1113.
Brown, J. S., Meadows, S. O., & Elder, G. H., Jr. chological Bulletin, 111, 62–107. Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1990). Percep-
(2007). Race-ethnic inequality and psycho- Buchanan, C., & Maccoby, E. (1993, March). Rela- tions of sibling relationships during middle
logical distress: Depressive symptoms from tionships between adolescents and their nonresi- childhood and adolescence. Child Develop-
adolescence to young adulthood. Develop- dential parents: A comparison of nonresidential ment, 61, 1387–1396.
mental Psychology, 43, 1295–1311. mothers and fathers. Paper presented at the Buhrmester, D., & Yin, J. (1997, April). A longitu-
Brown, K., McMahon, R., Biro, F., Crawford, P., biennial meetings of the Society for Research dinal study of friends’ influence on adolescents’
Schreiber, G., Similo, S., . . . Striegel-Moore, S. in Child Development, New Orleans. adjustment. Paper presented at the biennial
(1998). Changes in self-esteem in Black and Buchanan, C., Maccoby, E., & Dornbusch, S. meetings of the Society for Research in Child
White girls between the ages of 9 and 14 (1996). Adolescents after divorce. Cambridge, Development, Washington, DC.
years: The NHLBI Growth and Health Study. MA: Harvard University Press. Bukowski, W., Gauze, C., Hoza, B., & Newcomb,
Journal of Adolescent Health, 23, 7–19. Buchanan, M., & Robbins, C. (1990). Early adult A. (1993). Differences and consistency be-
Brown, L. K., Tolou-Shams, M., Lescano, C., psychological consequences for males of ad- tween same-sex and other-sex peer relation-
Houck, C., Zeidman, J., Pugatch, D., . . . Proj- olescent pregnancy and its resolution. Jour- ships during early adolescence. Developmental
ect SHIELD Study Group. (2006). Depressive nal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 413–424. Psychology, 29, 255–263.
symptoms as a predictor of sexual risk Buchmann, C., & Dalton, B. (2002). Interper- Bukowski, W., Sippola, L., & Hoza, B. (1999). Same
among African American adolescents and sonal influences and educational aspirations and other: Interdependency between partici-
young adults. Journal of Adolescent Health, in 12 countries: The importance of institu- pation in same- and other-sex friendships.
39, e1–e8. tional context. Sociology of Education, 75, Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 439–459.
Brown, S., & Rinelli, L. (2010). Family structure, 99–122. Bukowski, W., Sippola, L., & Newcomb, A. F. (2000).
family processes, and adolescent smoking Buechel, F., & Duncan, G. (1998). Do parents’ Variations in patterns of attraction of same-
and drinking. Journal of Research on Adoles- social activities promote children’s school at- and other-sex peers during early adolescence.
cence, 20, 259–273. tainments? Evidence from the German So- Developmental Psychology, 36, 147–154.
Brown, W. T., & Jones, J. M. (2004). The sub- cioeconomic Panel. Journal of Marriage and Bulcroft, R., Carmody, D., & Bulcroft, K. (1996).
stance of things hoped for: A study of the the Family, 60, 95–108. Patterns of parental independence giving to
future orientation, minority status percep- Buehler, C. (2006). Parents and peers in relation adolescents: Variations by race, age, and gen-
tions, academic engagement, and academic to early adolescent problem behavior. Jour- der of child. Journal of Marriage and the
performance of Black high school students. nal of Marriage and Family, 68, 109–124. Family, 58, 866–883.
Journal of Black Psychology, 30, 248–273. Buehler, C., Benson, M., & Gerard, J. (2006). In- Bullock, B., & Dishion, T. J. (2002). Sibling collu-
Browning, C. R., Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. terpersonal hostility and early adolescent sion and problem behavior in early adoles-
(2005). Sexual initiation in early adolescence: problem behavior: The mediating role of cence: Toward a process model for family
The nexus of parental and community control. specific aspects of parenting. Journal of Re- mutuality. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
American Sociological Review, 70, 758–778. search on Adolescence, 16, 265–292. chology, 30, 143–153.
Brubacher, J., & Rudy, W. (1976). Higher educa- Buehler, C., Franck, K., & Cook, E. (2009). Ado- Bumpass, L., & McLanahan, S. (1987, April).
tion in transition (3rd ed). New York: Harper lescents’ triangulation in marital conflict and Unmarried motherhood: A note on recent
& Row. peer relations. Journal of Research on Adoles- trends, composition and Black-White differ-
Brückner, H., & Bearman, P. (2005). After the cence, 19, 669–689. ences. Paper presented at the annual meeting
promise: The STD consequences of adoles- Buehler, C., Krishnakumar, A., Stone, G., Anthony, of the Population Association of America,
cent virginity pledges. Journal of Adolescent C., Pemberton, S., Gerard, J., & Barber, B. Chicago.
Health, 36, 271–278. (1998). Interparental conflict styles and youth Bumpus, M. F., Crouter, A., & McHale, S. (2001).
Brumberg, J. (1997). The body project: An inti- problem behaviors: A two-sample replication Parental autonomy granting during adoles-
mate history of American girls. New York: study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, cence: Exploring gender differences in context.
Random House. 119–132. Developmental Psychology, 37, 163–173.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-11 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-11

Burdette, A., & Needham, B. (2012). Neighbor- and moderation. Developmental Psychology, century: A global perspective. Journal of Re-
hood environment and body mass index tra- 44, 69–80. search on Adolescence, 12, 69–98.
jectories from adolescence to adulthood. Bussing, R., Mason, D. M., Bell, L., Porter, P., & Callahan, R. (2005). Tracking and English learn-
Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 30–37. Garvan, C. (2010). Adolescent outcomes of ers: Limiting opportunity to learn. American
Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2012). Current popu- childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- Educational Research Journal, 42, 305–328.
lation survey. Washington: Author. order in a diverse community sample. Journal Calvert, S. (2008). Children as consumers: Ad-
Burg, S., Mayers, R., & Miller, L. (2011). Spiritu- of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- vertising and marketing. The Future of Chil-
ality, religion, and healthy development in cent Psychiatry, 49, 595–605. dren, 18, 205–234.
adolescents. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein Butler, K. (2006, July 4). The grim neurology of Calvete, E., & Cardeñoso, O. (2005). Gender dif-
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. teenage drinking. The New York Times. ferences in cognitive vulnerability to depres-
353–359). New York: Academic Press. Buzy, W. M., McDonald, R., Jouriles, E. N., sion and behavior problems in adolescents.
Burke, J. D., Pardini, D. A., & Loeber, R. (2008). Swank, P. R., Rosenfield, D., Shimek, J. S., & Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33,
Reciprocal relationships between parent- Corbitt-Shindler, D. (2004). Adolescent girls’ 179–192.
ing behavior and disruptive psychopathol- alcohol use as a risk factor for relationship Campa, M. I., & Eckenrode, J. J. (2006). Pathways
ogy from childhood through adolescence. violence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, to intergenerational adolescent childbearing
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 14, 449–470. in a high-risk sample. Journal of Marriage
679–692. Byrd, C., & Chavous, T. (2011). Racial identity, and Family, 68, 558–572.
Burnett, S., Bault, N., Coricelli, G., & Blakemore, school racial climate, and school intrinsic mo- Campbell, B. (2011). Adrenarche and middle
S.-J. (2010). Adolescents’ heightened risk- tivation among African American youth: The childhood. Human Nature, 22, 327–349.
seeking in a probabilistic gambling task. Cog- importance of person–context congruence. Campbell, B. C., Prossinger, H., & Mbzivo, M.
nitive Development, 25, 183–196. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 849–860. (2005). Timing of pubertal maturation and
Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Kadosh, K., & Blakemore, Byrne, B., & Shavelson, R. (1996). On the structure the onset of sexual behavior among Zimbabwe
S-J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: of social self-concept for pre-, early, and late school boys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34,
Evidence from functional magnetic resonance adolescents: A test of the Shavelson, Hubner, 505–516.
imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience and Stanton (1976) model. Journal of Person- Campbell, F., Pungello, E., Miller-Johnson, S.,
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1654–1664. ality and Social Psychology, 70, 599–613. Burchinal, M., & Ramey, C. (2001). The de-
Burrell, G., & Roosa, M. (2009). Mothers’ eco- Byrnes, J. (2011). Academic achievement. In velopment of cognitive and academic abili-
nomic hardship and behavior problems in B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia ties: Growth curves from an early childhood
their early adolescents. Journal of Family Is- of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 1–9). New York: educational experiment. Developmental Psy-
sues, 30, 511–531. Academic Press. chology, 37, 231–242.
Burt, K. B., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). Competence Cairns, R., Leung, M., Buchanan, L., & Cairns, B. Campbell, F., & Ramey, C. (1995). Cognitive and
and psychopathology: Cascade effects in the (1995). Friendships and social networks in school outcomes for high-risk African-
NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth childhood and adolescence: Fluidity, reliabil- American students at middle adolescence:
Development. Development and Psychopa- ity, and interrelations. Child Development, Positive effects of early intervention. American
thology, 22, 557–567. 66, 1330–1345. Educational Research Journal, 32, 743–772.
Burt, K., Obradović, J., Long, J., & Masten, A. Caldwell, C. H., Kohn-Wood, L. P., Schmeelk- Campbell, M. A., Porter, S., & Santor, D. (2004).
(2008). The interplay of social competence Cone, K. H., Chavous, T. M., & Zimmerman, Psychopathic traits in adolescent offenders:
and psychopathology over 20 years: Testing M. A. (2004). Racial discrimination and ra- An evaluation of criminal history, clinical,
transactional and cascade models. Child De- cial identity as risk or protective factors for and psychosocial correlates. Behavioral Sci-
velopment, 79, 359–374. violent behaviors in African American young ences and the Law, 22, 23–47.
Burt, S. A., Barnes, A. R., McGue, M., & Iacono, adults. American Journal of Community Psy- Campione-Barr, N., & Smetana, J. G. (2004). In the
W. G. (2008). Parental divorce and adoles- chology, 33, 91–105. eye of the beholder: Subjective and observer
cent delinquency: Ruling out the impact of Caldwell, C. H., Sellers, R. M., Bernat, D. H., & ratings of middle-class African American
common genes. Developmental Psychology, Zimmerman, M. A. (2004). Racial identity, mother–adolescent interactions. Developmen-
44, 1668–1677. parental support, and alcohol use in a sample tal Psychology, 40, 927–934.
Burt, S. A., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2009). of academically at-risk African American Campione-Barr, N., & Smetana, J. G. (2010).
Nonshared environmental mediation of the high school students. American Journal of “Who said you could wear my sweater?” Ad-
association between deviant peer affiliation Community Psychology, 34, 71–82. olescent siblings’ conflicts and associations
and adolescent externalizing behaviors over Caldwell, L., & Darling, N. (1999). Leisure con- with relationship quality. Child Development,
time: Results from a cross-lagged monozy- text, parental control, and resistance to peer 81, 464–471.
gotic twin differences design. Developmental pressure as predictors of adolescent partying Cantillon, D. (2006). Community social organi-
Psychology, 45, 1752–1760. and substance use: An ecological perspective. zation, parents, and peers as mediators of
Burt, S. A., McGue, M., Krueger, R. F., & Iacono, Journal of Leisure Research, 31, 57–77. perceived neighborhood block characteris-
W. G. (2007). Environmental contributions Caldwell, M. S., Rudolph, K. D., Troop-Gordon, tics on delinquent and prosocial activities.
to adolescent delinquency: A fresh look at W., & Kim, D. Y. (2004). Reciprocal influence American Journal of Community Psychology,
the shared environment. Journal of Abnormal among relational self-views, social disen- 37, 111–127.
Child Psychology, 35, 787–800. gagement, and peer stress during early ado- Capaldi, D., & Clark, S. (1998). Prospective fam-
Burton, L. (2007). Childhood adultification lescence. Child Development, 75, 1140–1154. ily predictors of aggression toward female
in economically disadvantaged families: Calkins, S. D., & Keane, S. P. (2009). Develop- partners for at-risk young men. Developmen-
A conceptual model. Family Relations, 56, mental origins of early antisocial behavior. tal Psychology, 34, 1175–1188.
329–345. Development and Psychopathology, 21, Capaldi, D., Dishion, T., Stoolmiller, M., & Yoerger,
Busseri, M. A., Willoughby, T., Chalmers, H., & 1095–1109. K. (2001). Aggression toward female partners
Bogaert, A. F. (2008). On the association be- Call, K. T., Riedel, A. A., Hein, K., McLoyd, V., by at-risk young men: The contribution of
tween sexual attraction and adolescent risk Petersen, A., & Kipke, M. (2002). Adolescent male adolescent friendships. Developmental
behavior involvement: Examining mediation health and well-being in the twenty-first Psychology, 37, 61–73.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-12 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-12 References

Capaldi, D., Stoolmiller, M., Clark, S., & Owen, L. Carlson, W., & Rose, A. (2012). Activities in het- Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man:
(2002). Heterosexual risk behaviors in at- erosexual romantic relationships: Grade Personality continuities from childhood to
risk young men from early adolescence to differences and associations with relation- adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social
young adulthood: Prevalence, prediction, ship satisfaction. Journal of Adolescence, 35, Psychology, 78, 158–172.
and association with STD contraction. De- 219–224 Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E.,
velopmental Psychology, 38, 394–406. Carroll, A., Hemingway, F., Bower, J., Ashman, & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental origins
Caplan, N., Choy, M., & Whitmore, J. (1992, A., Houghton, S., & Durkin, K. (2006). of child and adolescent behavior problems:
February). Indochinese refugee families and Impulsivity in juvenile delinquency: Differ- From age 3 to age 15. Child Development, 66,
academic achievement. Scientific American, ences among early-onset, late-onset, and 55–68.
pp. 36–42. non-offenders. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Caspi, A., Lynam, D., Moffitt, T., & Silva, P. (1993).
Cappelleri, J., Eckenrode, J., & Powers, J. (1993). cence, 35, 519–529. Unraveling girls’ delinquency: Biological, dis-
The epidemiology of child abuse: Find- Carroll, J. S., Badger, S., Willoughby, B. J., Nelson, positional, and contextual contributions to
ings from the Second National Incidence L. J., Madsen, S. D., & McNamara Barry, C. adolescent misbehavior. Developmental Psy-
and Prevalence Study of Child Abuse and (2009). Ready or not?: Criteria for marriage chology, 29, 19–30.
Neglect. American Journal of Public Health, readiness among emerging adults. Journal of Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T., Taylor, A.,
83,1622–1624. Adolescent Research, 24, 349–375. Craig, I., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influ-
Carbonaro, W. (2005). Tracking, students’ effort, Carroll, J. S., Willoughby, B., Badger, S., Nelson, ence of life stress on depression: Moderation
and academic achievement. Sociology of Edu- L. J., Barry, C. M., & Madsen, S. D. (2007). So by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Sci-
cation, 78, 27–49. close, yet so far away: The impact of varying ence, 301, 386–389.
Carbonaro, W., & Covay, E. (2010). School sector marital horizons on emerging adulthood.
Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Mehta, T. G., & Feeney,
and student achievement in the era of stan- Journal of Adolescent Research, 22, 219–247.
B. C. (2003). Feedback seeking in children
dards-based reforms. Sociology of Education, Carskadon, M. (2011). Sleep in adolescents: The and adolescents: Associations with self-
83, 160–182. perfect storm. Pediatric Clinics of North perceptions, attachment representations,
Card, N. A., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2008). Peer vic- America, 58, 637–647 and depression. Child Development, 74,
timization among schoolchildren: Correla- Carskadon, M., & Acebo, C. (2002). Regulation 612–628.
tions, causes, consequences, and consideration of sleepiness in adolescence: Update, in- Casteel, M. (1993). Effects of inference necessity
in assessment and intervention. School Psy- sights, and speculation. Sleep, 25, 606–616. and reading goal on children’s inferential
chology Quarterly, 23, 451–461. Carson, D., Chowdhury, A., Perry, C., & Pati, C. generation. Developmental Psychology, 29,
Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani, G. M., & Little, (1999). Family characteristics and adolescent 346–357.
T. D. (2008). Direct and indirect aggression competence in India: Investigation of youth
Catsambis, S. (1992, March). The many faces of
during childhood and adolescence: A meta- in southern Orissa. Journal of Youth and Ado-
tracking in middle school grades: Between-
analytic review of gender differences, inter- lescence, 28, 211–233.
and within-school differentiation of students
correlations, and relations to maladjustment. Carter, R., Caldwell, C. H., Matusko, N., Anto- and resources. Paper presented at the biennial
Child Development, 79, 1185–1229. nucci, T., & Jackson, J. S. (2011). Ethnicity, meetings of the Society for Research on Ado-
Carlo, G., Crockett, L. J., Randall, B. A., & Roesch, perceived pubertal timing, externalizing be- lescence, Washington, DC.
S. C. (2007). A latent growth curve analysis haviors, and depressive symptoms among
Catterall, J. (1998). Risk and resilience in student
of prosocial behavior among rural adoles- Black adolescent girls. Journal of Youth and
transitions to high school. American Journal
cents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, Adolescence, 40, 1394–1406.
of Education, 106, 302–333.
301–324. Casella, R. (2003). Zero tolerance policy in
schools: Rationale, consequences, and alter- Cauffman, E. (2008). Understanding the female
Carlo, G., Knight, G., McGinley, M., & Hayes,
natives. Teachers College Review, 105, 872– offender. Future of Children, 18, 119–142.
R. (2011a). The roles of parental induc-
tions, moral emotions, and moral cognitions in 892. Cauffman, E., Farruggia, S. P., & Goldweber, A.
prosocial tendencies among Mexican Ameri- Casey, B. J., Duhoux, S., & Cohen, M. (2010). Ad- (2008). Bad boys or poor parents: Relations
can and European American early adolescents. olescence: what do transmission, transition, to female juvenile delinquency. Journal of Re-
Journal of Early Adolescence, 31, 757–781. and translation have to do with it? Neuron, 67, search on Adolescence, 18, 699–712.
Carlo, G., Koller, S., Eisenberg, N., Da Silva, M., 749–760. Cauffman, E., Piquero, A., Kimonis, E., Steinberg,
& Frohlich, C. (1996). A cross-national study Casey, B. J., Jones, R., & Somerville, L. (2011). L., & Chassin, L. (2007). Legal, individual,
of the relations among prosocial moral rea- Braking and accelerating of the adolescent and contextual predictors of court disposi-
soning, gender role orientations, and proso- brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, tion. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 519–535.
cial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 21–33. Cauffman, E., Shulman, E., Steinberg, L., Claus,
231–240. Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J.
Carlo, G., Padilla-Walker, L. P., & Day, R. (2011b). S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: What (2010). Age differences in affective decision
A test of the economic strain model on ado- have we learned about cognitive develop- making as indexed by performance on the
lescents’ prosocial behaviors. Journal of Re- ment? Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 104–110. Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychol-
search on Adolescence, 21, 842–848. Cash, T. F., Morrow, J., Hrabosky, J. I., & Perry, A. ogy, 46, 193–207.
Carlson Jones, D. (2004). Body image among ad- A. (2004). How has body image changed? A Cauffman, E., & Steinberg, L. (1996). Effects
olescent girls and boys: A longitudinal study. cross-sectional investigation of college of menarche, dating, and heterosocial
Developmental Psychology, 40, 823–835. women and men from 1983 to 2001. Journal involvement on dieting behavior in early
Carlson, E., Sroufe, L. A., Collins, W. A., Jimerson, of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 32,
S., Weinfield, N., Henninghausen, K., . . . 1081–1089. 631–635.
Meyer, S. (1999). Early environmental sup- Casper, D., & Card, N. (2010). “We were best Cauffman, E., Steinberg, L., & Piquero, A. R.
port and elementary school adjustment as friends, but . . .”: Two studies of antipathetic (2005). Psychological, neuropsychological
predictors of school adjustment in middle relationships emerging from broken friend- and physiological correlates of serious anti-
adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Research, ships. Journal of Adolescent Research, 25, social behavior in adolescence: The role of
14, 72–94. 499–526. self-control. Criminology, 43, 133–175.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-13 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-13

Cavanagh, S. E., Crissey, S. R., & Raley, R. K. Nonparental adults as social resources in the Chen, C. S., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1996).
(2008). Family structure history and adoles- transition to adulthood. Journal of Research Academic achievement and motivation of
cent romance. Journal of Marriage and Fam- on Adolescence, 20, 1065–1082. Chinese students: A cross-national perspec-
ily, 70, 698–714. Chao, R., & Otsuki-Clutter, M. (2011). Racial tive. In S. Lau (Ed.), Growing up the Chinese
Cavanagh, S. E., & Fomby, P. (2012). Family in- and ethnic differences: Sociocultural and way: Chinese child and adolescent develop-
stability, school context, and the academic contextual explanations. Journal of Research ment. Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.
careers of adolescents. Sociology of Educa- on Adolescence, 21, 47–60. Chen, C. Y., Dormitzer, C. M., Bejarno, J., &
tion, 85, 81–97. Charbonneau, A. M., Mezulis, A. H., & Hyde, J. S. Anthony, J. C. (2004). Religiosity and the ear-
Cavazos-Rehg, P. A., Spitznagel, E. L., Bucholz, (2009). Stress and emotional reactivity as ex- liest stages of adolescent drug involvement in
K. K., Nurnberger, J., Edenberg, H. J., Kramer, planations for gender differences in adoles- seven countries of Latin America. American
J. R., . . . Beirut, L. (2010). Predictors of sexual cents’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth Journal of Epidemiology, 159, 1180–1188.
debut at age 16 or younger. Archives of Sexual and Adolescence, 38, 1050–1058. Chen, E., Langer, D. A., Raphaelson, Y. E., &
Behavior, 39, 664–673. Chase-Landsdale, P. L., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Matthews, K. (2004). Socioeconomic sta-
Ceballo, R., McLoyd, V. C., & Toyokawa, T. Zamsky, E. (1994). Young African-American tus and health in adolescents: The role of
(2004). The influence of neighborhood multigenerational families in poverty: Qual- stress interpretations. Child Development,
quality on adolescents’ educational values ity of mothering and grandmothering. Child 75, 1039–1052.
and school effort. Journal of Adolescent Re- Development, 65, 373–393. Chen, K., & Kandel, D. (1996). The natural his-
search, 19, 716–739. Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Moffit, R. A., Lohman, B. tory of drug use from adolescence to the
Ceci, S., & Williams, W. (1999). Schooling, intel- J., Cherlin, A. J., Coley, R. L., Pittman, L. D., mid-thirties in a general population sample.
ligence, and income. American Psychologist, Roff, J., & Votruba-Drazl, E. (2003). Mothers’ American Journal of Public Health, 85, 41–47.
52, 1051–1058. transitions from welfare to work and the Chen, M.-J., Gruenewald, P. J., & Remer, L. G.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. well-being of preschooler and adolescents. (2009a). Does alcohol outlet density affect
(2006). Youth behavior surveillance—United Science, 299, 15–28. youth access to alcohol? Journal of Adolescent
States, 2005. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Chassin, L., Hussong, A., & Beltran, I. (2009). Health, 44, 582–589.
Report, 55, SS-5. Adolescent substance use. In R. Lerner & Chen, P., & Jacobson, K. C. (2012). Developmen-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent tal trajectories of substance use from early
(2012). Youth risk behavior surveillance— psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 723–764). adolescence to young adulthood: Gender
United States, 2011. Morbidity and Mortality New York: Wiley. and racial/ethnic differences. Journal of Ado-
Weekly Report, 61(4). Chassin, L., Presson, C., Todd, M., Rose, J., & lescent Health, 50, 154–163.
Cepeda, A., & Valdez, A. (2003). Risk behav- Sherman, S. (1998). Maternal socialization of Chen, P., & Vazsonyi, A. T. (2011). Future orien-
iors among young Mexican American gang- adolescent smoking: The intergenerational tation, impulsivity, and problem behaviors:
associated females: Sexual relations, partying, transmission of parenting and smoking. De- A longitudinal moderation model. Develop-
substance abuse, and crime. Journal of Ado- velopmental Psychology, 34, 1189–1201. mental Psychology, 47, 1633–1645.
lescent Research, 18, 90–106. Chauhan, P., Reppucci, N. D., Burnette, M., & Chen, R., & Simons-Morton, B. (2009). Concur-
Cervantes, R., & Cordova, D. (2011). Life experi- Reiner, S. (2010). Race, neighborhood disad- rent changes in conduct problems and de-
ences of Hispanic adolescents: developmental vantage, and antisocial behavior among fe- pressive symptoms in early adolescents: A
and language considerations in acculturation male juvenile offenders. Journal of Community developmental person-centered approach.
stress. Journal of Community Psychology, 39, Psychology, 38, 532–540. Development and Psychopathology, 21,
336–352. Chauhan, P., Reppucci, N., & Turkheimer, E. 285–307.
Chaloupka, F. (2004). The effects of price on al- (2009). Racial differences in the associations Chen, W-I. (2010). Exposure to community vio-
cohol use, abuse, and their consequences. In of neighborhood disadvantage, exposure to lence and adolescents’ internalizing behav-
R. Bonnie & M. O’Connell (Eds.,) Reducing violence, and criminal recidivism among fe- iors among African American and Asian
underage drinking: a collective responsibility. male juvenile offenders. Behavioral Sciences American adolescents. Journal of Youth and
Washington: The National Academies Press. & the Law, 27, 531–552. Adolescence, 39, 403–413.
Chan, D. (1997). Depressive symptoms and per- Chavous, T., Bernat, D., Schmeelk-Cone, K., Chen, X., & Adams, M. (2010). Are teen delin-
ceived competence among Chinese second- Caldwell, C., Kohn-Wood, L., & Zimmerman, quency abstainers social introverts?: A test of
ary school students in Hong Kong. Journal of M. (2003). Racial identity and academic at- Moffitt’s theory. Journal of Research in Crime
Youth and Adolescence, 26, 303–319. tainment among African American adoles- and Delinquency, 47, 439–468.
Chandler, M. (1987). The Othello effect: Essay cents. Child Development, 74, 1076–1090. Chen, X., Rubin, K., & Li, D. (1997). Relation be-
on the emergence and eclipse of skeptical Chavous, T., Rivas-Drake, D., Smalls, C., Griffin, tween academic achievement and social ad-
doubt. Human Development, 30, 137–159. T., & Cogburn, C. (2008). Gender matters, too: justment: Evidence from Chinese children.
Chandra, A., Martino, S., Collins, R., Elliott, M., The influences of school racial discrimination Developmental Psychology, 33, 518–525.
Berry, S., Kanouse, D., & Miu, A. (2008). and racial identity on academic engagement Chen, Z., Guo, F., Yang, X., Li, X., Duan, Q.,
Does watching sex on television predict teen outcomes among African American adoles- Zhang, J., & Ge, X. (2009b). Emotional and
pregnancy? Findings from a national longi- cents. Developmental Psychology, 44, 637–654. behavioral effects of romantic relationships
tudinal survey of youth. Pediatrics, 122, Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., & in Chinese adolescents. Journal of Youth and
1047–1054. Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adoles- Adolescence, 38, 1282–1293.
Chang, E. S., Chen, C., Greenberger, E., Dooley, cent risk taking by enhancing activity in the Cherlin, A., Chase-Lansdale, P., & McRae, C.
D., & Heckhausen, J. (2006). What do they brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Psy- (1998). Effects of parental divorce on mental
want in life?: The life goals of a multi-ethnic, chology, 14, F1–F10. health throughout the life course. American
multi-generational sample of high school se- Chen, C. S., Greenberger, E., Lester, J., Dong, Q., Sociological Review, 63, 239–249.
niors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, & Guo, M. (1998). A cross-cultural study of Cherney, I. D., & Shing, Y. L. (2008). Children’s
321–332. family and peer correlates of adolescent nurturance and self-determination rights: A
Chang, E. S., Greenberger, E., Chen, C., misconduct. Developmental Psychology, 34, cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Social
Heckhausen, J., & Farruggia, S. P. (2010). 770–781. Issues, 64, 835–856.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-14 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-14 References

Child Trends (2010). High school students carry- Church, R. (1976). Education in the United intercourse within adolescent relationships.
ing weapons. Washington: Author. States. New York: Free Press. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13,
Chiodo, D., Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C., Hughes, R., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Sturge-Apple, M. 81–112.
& Jaffe, P. (2009). Impact of sexual harass- (2007). Interactions of child maltreatment Cleveland, H., & Wiebe, R. P. (2003). The modera-
ment victimization by peers on subsequent and serotonin transporter and monoamine tion of adolescent-to-peer similarity in tobacco
adolescent victimization and adjustment: A oxidase A polymorphisms: Depressive symp- and alcohol use by school levels of substance
longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescent tomatology among adolescents from low so- use. Child Development, 74, 279–291.
Health, 45, 246–252. cioeconomic status backgrounds. Development Cleveland, M. J., Feinberg, M. E., Bontempo,
Chisholm, L., & Hurrelmann, K. (1995). Adoles- and Psychopathology, 19, 1161–1180. D. E., & Greenberg, M. T. (2008). The role of
cence in modern Europe: Pluralized transi- Cillessen, A. H. N., & Rose, A. J. (2005). Under- risk and protective factors in substance use
tion patterns and their implications for standing popularity in the peer system. Cur- across adolescence. Journal of Adolescent
personal and social risks. Journal of Adoles- rent Directions in Psychological Science, 14, Health, 43, 157–164.
cence, 18, 129–158. 102–105. Cleveland, M. J., Feinberg, M. E., & Greenberg,
Choi, Y., Harachi, T. W., Gillmore, M. R., & Cinamon, R. G., & Rich, Y. (2002). Gender differ- M. T. (2010). Protective families in high- and
Catalano, R. F. (2005). Applicability of the ence in the importance of work and family low-risk environments: Implications for ad-
social development model to urban ethnic roles: Implications for work–family conflict. olescent substance use. Journal of Youth and
minority youth: Examining the relationship Sex Roles, 47, 531–541. Adolescence, 39, 114–126.
between external constraints, family social- Claes, M. (1998). Adolescents’ closeness with Cleveland, M. J., Gibbons, F., Gerrard, M., Pomery,
ization, and problem behaviors. Journal of parents, siblings, and friends in three coun- E., & Brody, G. (2005). The impact of parent-
Research on Adolescence, 15, 505–534. tries: Canada, Belgium, and Italy. Journal of ing on risk cognitions and risk behavior: A
Choi, Y., He, M., & Harachi, T. W. (2008). Inter- Youth and Adolescence, 27, 165–184. study of mediation and moderation in a panel
generational cultural dissonance, parent–child Clampet-Lundquist, S., Edin, K., Kling, J., & of African American adolescents. Child Devel-
conflict and bonding, and youth problem be- Duncan, G. (2011). Moving teenagers out of opment, 76, 900–916.
haviors among Vietnamese and Cambodian high-risk neighborhoods: How girls fare bet- Clinkinbeard, S. S., Simi, P., Evans, M. K., &
immigrant families. Journal of Youth and Ado- ter than boys. American Journal of Sociology, Anderson, A. L. (2011). Sleep and delinquency:
lescence, 37, 85–96. 116, 1154–1189. Does the amount of sleep matter? Journal of
Choudhury, S., Blakemore, S., & Charman, T. Youth and Adolescence, 40, 916–930.
Clancy, S., & Dollinger, S. (1993). Identity, self,
(2006). Social cognitive development during and personality, I: Identity status and the Coatsworth, J. D., & Conroy, D. E. (2009). The
adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective five-factor model of personality. Journal of effects of autonomy-supportive coaching,
Neuroscience, 3, 165–174. Research on Adolescence, 3, 227–246. need satisfaction, and self-perceptions on
Christakou, A. Brammer, M., & Rubia, K. (2011). initiative and identity in youth swimmers.
Clardy, C., & King, P. (2011). Religious involve-
Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activa- Developmental Psychology, 45, 320–328.
ment. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.),
tion and connectivity associated with devel- Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 279– Coatsworth, J. D., Sharp, E. H., Palen, L., Darling,
opmental changes in temporal discounting. 288.). New York: Academic Press. N., Cumsille, P., & Marta, E. (2005). Explor-
Neuroimage, 54, 1344–1354. ing adolescent self-defining leisure activities
Clark, J., & Barber, B. (1994). Adolescents in
and identity experiences across three coun-
Chua, A. (2011). Battle hymn of the Tiger Mother. postdivorce and always-married families:
tries. International Journal of Behavioral De-
New York: Penguin. Self-esteem and perceptions of fathers’ inter-
velopment, 29, 361–370.
Chumlea, W., Schubert, C., Roche, A., Kulin, H., est. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56,
Cochran, B. N., Stewart, A. J., Ginzler, J. A., &
Lee, P., . . . Himes, J. (2003). Age at menarche 608–614.
Cauce, A. (2002). Challenges faced by home-
and racial comparisons in U.S. girls. Pediat- Clark, R., Dogan, R. R., & Akbar, N. J. (2003). less sexual minorities: Comparison of gay,
rics, 111, 110–113. Youth and parental correlates of externaliz- lesbian, bisexual, and transgender homeless
Chun, H., & Dickson, G. (2011). A psychoeco- ing symptoms, adaptive functioning, and adolescents with their heterosexual counter-
logical model of academic performance academic performance: An exploratory parts. American Journal of Public Health, 92,
among Hispanic adolescents. Journal of study in preadolescent Blacks. Journal of 773–777.
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1581–1594. Black Psychology, 29, 210–229.
Cohen, D., Farley, T., Taylor, S., Martin, D., &
Chun, Y.-J., & MacDermid, S. M. (1997). Percep- Clark, R., Novak, J. D., & Dupree, D. (2002). Re- Schuster, M. (2002). When and where do
tions of family differentiation, individuation, lationship of perceived parenting practices youths have sex? The potential role of adult
and self-esteem among Korean adolescents. to anger regulation and coping strategies in supervision. Pediatrics, 110, E66.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, African American adolescents. Journal of
Cohen, G. L., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Peer con-
451–462. Adolescence, 25, 373–384.
tagion of aggression and health risk behavior
Chung, G. H., Flook, L., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). Clarke, A. T. (2006). Coping with interpersonal among adolescent males: An experimental
Daily family conflict and emotional distress stress and psychosocial health among chil- investigation of effects on public conduct
among adolescents from Latin American, dren and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Jour- and private attitudes. Child Development, 77,
Asian, and European backgrounds. Develop- nal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 11–24. 967–983.
mental Psychology, 45, 1406–1415. Clasen, D., & Brown, B. (1985). The multidimen- Cohen, P. K., Chen, H., Hartmark, C., & Gordon,
Chung, G. H., Flook, L., & Fuligni, A. (2011). Re- sionality of peer pressure in adolescence. K. (2003). Variations in patterns of develop-
ciprocal associations between family and Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 451–468. mental transmissions in the emerging adult-
peer conflict in adolescents’ daily lives. Child Clements, M., Aber, J. L., & Seidman, E. (2008). hood period. Developmental Psychology, 39,
Development, 82, 1390–1396. The dynamics of life stressors and depressive 657–669.
Chung, H. L., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Relations symptoms in early adolescence: A test of six Coie, J., Terry, R., Lenox, K., Lochman, J., &
between neighborhood factors, parenting theoretical models. Child Development, 79, Hyman, C. (1995). Childhood peer rejection
behaviors, peer deviance, and delinquency 1168–1182. and aggression as predictors of stable pat-
among serious juvenile offenders. Develop- Cleveland, H. (2003). The influence of female terns of adolescent disorder. Development
mental Psychology, 42, 319–331. and male risk on the occurrence of sexual and Psychopathology, 7, 697–713.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-15 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-15

Coiro, M., & Emery, R. (1996, March). Adoles- Coley, R. L., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (1998). Ado- Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality de-
cents’ adjustment as a function of their involve- lescent pregnancy and parenthood: Recent velopment (6th ed., pp. 1003–1067). New
ment in post-divorce conflict. Paper presented evidence and future directions. American York: Wiley.
at the biennial meetings of the Society for Psychologist, 53, 152–166. Comings, D. E., Muhleman, D., Johnson, J. P.,
Research on Adolescence, Boston. Coley, R. L., & Medeiros, B. L. (2007). Reciprocal & MacMurray, J. P. (2002). Parent–daughter
Coker, T., Elliott, M., Kanouse, D., & Grunbaum, longitudinal relations between nonresident transmission of the androgen receptor gene
J. (2009). Perceived racial/ethnic discrimina- father involvement and adolescent delin- as an explanation of the effect of father ab-
tion among fifth-grade students and its as- quency. Child Development, 78, 132–147. sence on age of menarche. Child Develop-
sociation with mental health. American Coley, R. L., Medeiros, B. L., & Schindler, H. S. ment, 73, 1046–1051.
Journal of Public Health, 99, 878–884. (2008). Using sibling differences to estimate Compas, B., & Reeslund, K. (2009). Processes of
Cole, A., & Kerns, K. A. (2001). Perceptions of effects of parenting on adolescent sexual risk risk and resilience during adolescence. In R.
sibling qualities and activities of early ado- behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43, Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of
lescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 133–140. adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp.
204–226. Coley, R. L., Morris, J. E., & Hernandez, D. (2004). 561–588). New York: Wiley.
Cole, D. A., Jacquez, F. M., LaGrange, B., Pineda, A. Out-of-school care and problem behavior Compian, L., Gowen, L., & Hayward, C. (2004).
Q., Truss, A. E., Weitlauf, A. S., . . . Dufton, L. trajectories among low-income adolescents: Peripubertal girls’ romantic and platonic in-
(2011). A longitudinal study of cognitive Individual, family, and neighborhood char- volvement with boys: Associations with body
risks for depressive symptoms in children acteristics as added risks. Child Development, image and depression symptoms. Journal of
and young adolescents. Journal of Early Ado- 75, 948–965. Research on Adolescence, 14, 23–47.
lescence, 31, 782–816. Coley, R. L., Votruba-Drzal, E., & Schindler, H. S. Complete College. (2012). Remediation: Higher
Cole, D., & Jordan, A. (1995). Competence and (2008). Trajectories of parenting processes education’s bridge to nowhere. Washington:
memory: Integrating psychosocial and cog- and adolescent substance use: Reciprocal Author.
nitive correlates of child depression. Child effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Compton, K., Snyder, J., & Schrepferman, L.
Development, 66, 459–473. 36, 613–625.
(2003). The contribution of parents and sib-
Cole, D., Martin, J., & Powers, B. (1997). A com- Coley, R., Votruba-Drzal, E., & Schindler, H. S. lings to antisocial and depressive behavior
petency-based model of child depression: A (2009). Fathers’ and mothers’ parenting pre- in adolescents: A double jeopardy coercion
longitudinal study of peer, parent, teacher, dicting and responding to adolescent sexual model. Development and Psychopathology,
and self-evaluations. Journal of Child Psy- risk behaviors. Child Development, 80, 808–827. 15, 163–182.
chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Collaer, M., & Hines, M. (1995). Human behav-
Conant, J. (1959). The American high school today.
38, 505–514. ioral sex differences: A role for gonadal hor-
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cole, D., Maxwell, S., Martin, J., Peeke, L., mones during early development? Psychological
Seroczynski, A., Tram, J., . . . Maschman, T. Bulletin, 118, 55–107. Conchas, G. Q. (2001). Structuring failure and
(2001). The development of multiple do- success: Understanding the variability in
College Board. (2012). College-bound seniors,
mains of child and adolescent self-concept: Latino school engagement. Harvard Educa-
2011. New York: Author.
A cohort sequential longitudinal design. tional Review, 71, 475–504.
Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working
Child Development, 72, 1723–1746. models of attachment shape perceptions of Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group
Cole, D., Peeke, L., Dolezal, S., Murray, N., & social support: Evidence from experimental (1999). Initial impact of the Fast Track Pre-
Canzoniero, A. (1999). A longitudinal study and observational studies. Journal of Person- vention Trial for Conduct Problems, II: Class-
of negative affect and self-perceived compe- ality and Social Psychology, 87, 363–383. room effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
tence in young adolescents. Journal of Per- Psychology, 67, 648–657.
Collins, R. L., Martino, S. C., Elliott, M. N., & Miu,
sonality and Social Psychology, 77, 851–862. A. (2011). Relationships between adolescent Conger, K., Conger, R., & Elder, G., Jr. (1994).
Coleman, J. (1961). The adolescent society. Glencoe, sexual outcomes and exposure to sex in me- Sibling relationships during hard times. In R.
IL: Free Press. dia: Robustness to propensity-based analysis. Conger & G. Elder, Jr. (Eds.), Families in
Coleman, J., & Hoffer, T. (1987). Public and pri- Developmental Psychology, 47, 585–591. troubled times: Adapting to change in rural
vate high schools: The impact of communities. America (pp. 235–252). New York: Aldine.
Collins, W. A. (1990). Parent–child relation-
New York: Basic Books. ships in the transition to adolescence: Con- Conger, K., Conger, R., & Scaramella, L. (1997).
Coleman, J., Hoffer, T., & Kilgore, S. (1982). High tinuity and change in interaction, affect, and Parents, siblings, psychological control, and
school achievement: Public, Catholic and other cognition. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, & adolescent adjustment. Journal of Adolescent
private schools compared. New York: Basic T. Gullotta (Eds.), Advances in adolescent de- Research, 12, 113–138.
Books. velopment: Vol. 2. The transition from child- Conger, R., Cui, M., Bryant, C. M., & Elder, G. H.,
Coleman, L. M., & Cater, S. M. (2005). A qualita- hood to adolescence (pp. 85–106). Beverly Jr. (2000). Competence in early adult roman-
tive study of the relationship between alco- Hills, CA: Sage. tic relationships: A developmental perspective
hol consumption and risky sex in adolescents. Collins, W. A. (2003). More than a myth: The on family influences. Journal of Personality
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 649–661. developmental significance of romantic rela- and Social Psychology, 79, 224–237.
Coleman, P. K. (2006). Resolution of unwanted tionships during adolescence. Journal of Re- Conger, R., Ge, X., Elder, G., Jr., Lorenz, F., &
pregnancy during adolescence through search on Adolescents, 13, 1–24. Simons, R. (1994). Economic stress, coercive
abortion versus childbirth: Individual and Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E., Steinberg, L., family process, and developmental prob-
family predictors and psychological conse- Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. (2000). lems of adolescents. Child Development, 65,
quences. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Contemporary research on parenting: The 541–561.
35, 903–911. case for nature and nurture. American Conklin, H. M., Luciana, M., Hooper, C. J., &
Coles, M. S., Makino, K. K., Stanwood, N.L., Psychologist, 55, 218–232. Yarger, R. S. (2007). Working memory per-
Dozier, A. & Klein, J. D. (2010). How are re- Collins, W. A., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Adoles- formance in typically developing children
strictive abortion statutes associated with cent development in interpersonal context. In and adolescents: Behavioral evidence of pro-
unintended teen birth? Journal of Adolescent W. Damon & R. Lerner (Ser. Eds.) & N. Eisen- tracted frontal lobe development. Develop-
Health, 47, 160–167. berg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: mental Neuropsychology, 31, 103–128.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-16 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-16 References

Conley, C. S., & Rudolph, K. D. (2009). The among inner-city boys. Child Development, Côté, J. (2009). Identity formation and self de-
emerging sex difference in adolescent 67, 3368–3385. velopment in adolescence. In R. Lerner & L.
depression: Interacting contributions of Cook, T., Deng, Y., & Morgano, E. (2007). Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psy-
puberty and peer stress. Development and Friendship influences during early adoles- chology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 266–304). New
Psychopathology, 21, 593–620. cence: The special role of friends’ grade point York: Wiley.
Connell, A. M., & Dishion, T. J. (2008). Reducing average. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Côté, J., & Bynner, J. (2008). Changes in the tran-
depression among at-risk early adolescents: 17, 325–356. sition to adulthood in the UK and Canada:
Three-year effects of a family-centered inter- Cook, T., Herman, M., Phillips, M., & Settersten, The role of structure and agency in emerging
vention embedded within schools. Journal of R., Jr. (2002). Some ways in which neighbor- adulthood. Journal of Youth Studies, 11,
Family Psychology, 22, 574–585. hoods, nuclear families, friendship groups, 251–268.
Connell, C. M., Gilreath, T. D., Aklin, W. M., & and schools jointly affect changes in early Couperus, J. W., & Nelson, C. A. (2006). Early brain
Brex, R. A. (2010). Social-ecological influ- adolescent development. Child Development, development and plasticity. In K. McCartney
ences on patterns of substance use among 73, 1283–1309. & D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of
non-metropolitan high school students. Cook, W. L. (2001). Interpersonal influence in early childhood development (pp. 85–105).
American Journal of Community Psychology, family systems: A social relations model Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
45(1–2), 36–48. analysis. Child Development, 72, 1179–1197. Courtney, M., & Cohen, R. (1996). Behavior
Connell, J., Spencer, M., & Aber, J. (1994). Educa- Cooksey, E. C., Mott, F. L., & Neubauer, S. A. segmentation by boys as a function of ag-
tional risk and resilience in African-American (2002). Friendships and early relationships: gressiveness and prior information. Child
youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in Links to sexual initiation among American Development, 67, 1034–1047.
school. Child Development, 65, 493–506. adolescents born to young mothers. Perspec- Cox, M., Wang, F., & Gustafsson, H. (2011).
Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. tives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 34, Family organization and adolescent develop-
(1999). Conceptions of cross-sex friendships 118–126. ment. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.),
and romantic relationships in early adoles- Cooley-Quille, M. & Lorion, R. (1999). Adoles- Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 75–83).
cence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, cents’ exposure to community violence: Sleep New York: Academic Press.
481–494. and psychophysiological functioning. Journal Cox, S. J., Mezulis, A. H., & Hyde, J. S. (2010).
Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, of Community Psychology, 27, 367–375. The influence of child gender role and
D. (2004). Mixed-gender groups, dating, Cooper, C. (1994). Cultural perspectives on con- maternal feedback to child stress on the
and romantic relationships in early adoles- tinuity and change in adolescents’ relation- emergence of the gender difference in
cence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14, ships. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, & T. depressive rumination in adolescence.
185–207. Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during Developmental Psychology, 46, 842–852.
Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). adolescence. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Coyne, S., Padilla-Walker, L., Stockdale, L., &
The role of peers in the emergence of hetero- Cooper, H., Charlton, K., Valentine, J., & Muhlen- Day, R. (2011). Game on . . . girls: Associa-
sexual romantic relationships in adolescence. bruck, L. (2000). Making the most of sum- tions between co-playing video games and
Child Development, 71, 1395–1408. mer school: A meta-analytic and narrative adolescent behavioral and family outcomes.
Connolly, J., & Goldberg, A. (1999). Romantic review. Monographs of the Society for Research Journal of Adolescent Health, 49, 160–165.
relationships in adolescence: The role of in Child Development, 65, Serial No. 260. Crane, D. R., So Wa, N., Larson, J. H., & Hafen,
friends and peers in their emergence and Cooper, M. L., Shaver, P. R., & Collins, N. L. M., Jr. (2005). The influence of family func-
development. In W. Furman, B. Brown, & C. (1998). Attachment styles, emotion regula- tioning and parent–adolescent acculturation
Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on tion, and adjustment in adolescence. Journal on North American Chinese adolescent out-
adolescent romantic relationships (pp. 266– of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, comes. Family Relations, 54, 400–410.
290). New York: Cambridge University Press. 1380–1397. Crapanzano, A., Frick, P., Childs, K., & Terranova,
Connolly, J., & Johnson, A. (1993, March). The Cooper, M. L., Wood, P. K., Orcutt, H. K., & A. (2011). Gender differences in the assess-
psychosocial context of romantic relationships Albino, A. (2003). Personality and the pre- ment, stability, and correlates to bullying
in adolescence. Paper presented at the bien- disposition to engage in risky or problem roles in middle school children. Behavioral
nial meetings of the Society for Research in behaviors during adolescence. Journal of Per- Sciences & the Law, 29, 677–694.
Child Development, New Orleans. sonality and Social Psychology, 84, 390–410. Crawford, T. N., Cohen, P., Midlarsky, E., &
Connolly, J., & Konarski, R. (1994). Peer self- Copeland-Linder, N., Lambert, S. F., Chen, Y.-F., Brook, J. S. (2001). Internalizing symptoms
concept in adolescence: Analysis of factor & Ialongo, N. S. (2010). Contextual stress in adolescents: Gender differences in vulner-
structure and of associations with peer expe- and health risk behaviors among African ability to parental distress and discord. Jour-
rience. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4, American adolescents. Journal of Youth and nal of Research on Adolescence, 11, 95–118.
385–403. Adolescence, 40, 158–173. Creasey, G., & Kaliher, G. (1994). Age differences
Connolly, J., & McIsaac, C. (2009). Romantic re- Cortina, K., & Arel, S. (2011). Schools and in grandchildren’s perceptions of relations
lationships in adolescence. In R. Lerner & L. schooling. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), with grandparents. Journal of Adolescence,
Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psy- Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 299– 17, 411–426.
chology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 104–151). New 305). New York: Academic Press. Crick, N. (1996). The role of overt aggression, re-
York: Wiley. Cosgrove, V. E., Rhee, S. H., Gelhorn, H. L., lational aggression, and prosocial behavior in
Conseur, A., Rivara, F., Barnoski, R., & Emanuel, Boeldt, D., Corley, R. C., Ehringer, M. A., the prediction of children’s future social ad-
I. (1997). Maternal and perinatal risk factors . . . Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Structure and eti- justment. Child Development, 67, 2317–2327.
for later delinquency. Pediatrics, 99, 785–790. ology of co-occurring internalizing and Crick, N. (1997). Engagement in gender nor-
Cook, P. J., & Ludwig, J. (2004). Does gun preva- externalizing disorders in adolescents. mative versus nonnormative forms of
lence affect teen gun carrying after all? Crim- Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, aggression: Links to social-psychological
inology, 42, 27–54. 109–123. adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 33,
Cook, T., Church, M., Ajanaku, S., Shadish, W., Jr., Côté, J. (2000). Arrested adulthood: The changing 610–617.
Kim, J., & Cohen, R. (1996). The development nature of maturity and identity. New York: Crick, N., & Dodge, K. (1994). A review and re-
of occupational aspirations and expectations New York University Press. formulation of social information-processing
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-17 02/04/13 6:35 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-17

mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. friends. Journal of Adolescent Research, 17, of Adolescent Health. Perspectives on Sexual
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101. 143–167. and Reproductive Health, 37, 125–134.
Criss, M. M., & Shaw, D. (2005). Sibling relation- Crosnoe, R. (2006). The connection between ac- Çuhadaroğlu Çetin, F., Tüzün, Z., Pehlivantürk,
ships as contexts for delinquency training in ademic failure and adolescent drinking in B., Ünal, F., & Gökler, B. (2010). Attach-
low-income families. Journal of Family Psy- secondary school. Sociology of Education, 79, ment styles and self-image in Turkish ado-
chology, 19, 592–600. 44–60. lescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
Crocetti, E., Klimstra, T., Keijsers, L., Hale, W. W., Crosnoe, R. (2007). Gender, obesity, and educa- 20, 840–848.
& Meeus, W. (2009). Anxiety trajectories and tion. Sociology of Education, 80, 241–260. Cui, M., & Conger, R. (2008). Parenting behavior
identity development in adolescence: A five- Crosnoe, R. (2009). Low-income students and as mediator and moderator of the associa-
wave longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and the socioeconomic composition of public tion between marital problems and adoles-
Adolescence, 38, 839–849. high schools. American Sociological Review, cent maladjustment. Journal of Research on
Crockett, L., Bingham, C., Chopak, J., & Vicary, J. 74, 709–730. Adolescence, 18, 261–284.
(1996). Timing of first sexual intercourse: Crosnoe, R., & Cavanagh, S. (2010). Families Cui, M., Conger, R., & Lorenz, F. O. (2005). Pre-
The role of social control, social learning and with children and adolescents: A review, cri- dicting change in adolescent adjustment
problem behavior. Journal of Youth and Ado- tique, and future agenda. Journal of Marriage from change in marital problems. Develop-
lescence, 25, 89–111. and Family, 72, 594–611. mental Psychology, 41, 812–823.
Crockett, L., Brown, J., Russell, S. T., & Shen, Y.
Crosnoe, R., Erickson, K., & Dornbusch, S. M. Cui, M., Donnellan, M. B., & Conger, R. (2007).
(2007). The meaning of good parent–child
(2002). Protective functions of family rela- Reciprocal influences between parents’ mari-
relationships for Mexican American adoles-
tionships and school factors on the deviant tal problems and adolescent internalizing
cents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17,
behavior of adolescent boys and girls: Re- and externalizing behavior. Developmental
639–668.
ducing the impact of risky friendships. Youth Psychology, 43, 1544–1552.
Crockett, L., & Hayes, R. (2011). Parenting prac- and Society, 33, 515–544. Cummings, E., Ballard, M., El-Sheikh, M., &
tices and styles. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein
Crosnoe, R., & Huston, A. C. (2007). Socioeco- Lake, M. (1991). Resolution and children’s
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2,
nomic status, schooling, and the developmen- responses to interadult anger. Developmental
pp. 241–248). New York: Academic Press.
tal trajectories of adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 27, 462–470.
Crockett, L., Raffaelli, M., & Shen, Y. (2006). Psychology, 43, 1097–1110. Cummings, J. R., Wen, H., & Druss, B. G. (2011).
Linking self-regulation and risk proneness to
Crosnoe, R., Mistry, R. S., & Elder, G. H., Jr. Racial/ethnic differences in treatment for
risky sexual behavior: Pathways through peer
(2002). Economic disadvantage, family dy- substance use disorders among U.S. adoles-
pressure and early substance use. Journal of
namics, and adolescent enrollment in higher cents. Journal of the American Academy of
Research on Adolescence, 16, 503–525.
education. Journal of Marriage and the Fam- Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 1265–1274.
Croll, J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & ily, 64, 690–702.
Ireland, M. (2002). Prevalence and risk and Cumsille, P., Darling, N., Flaherty, B., & Martinez,
protective factors related to disordered eating Crosnoe, R., & Needham, B. (2004). Holism, con- M. (2009). Heterogeneity and change in the
behaviors among adolescents: Relationship textual variability, and the study of friendships patterning of adolescents’ perceptions of the
to gender and ethnicity. Journal of Adolescent in adolescent development. Child Develop- legitimacy of parental authority: A latent
Health, 31, 166–175. ment, 75, 264–279. transition model. Child Development, 80,
Crouter, A., Bumpus, M., Davis, K., & McHale, S. 418–432.
Crone, E. (2009). Executive functions in adoles-
cence: Inferences from brain and behavior. (2005). How do parents learn about adoles- Cunningham, M. (1999). African-American ad-
Developmental Science, 12, 825–830. cents’ experiences? Implications for parental olescent males’ perceptions of their commu-
knowledge and adolescent risky behavior. nity resources and constraints: A longitudinal
Crone, E., Somsen, R., Zanolie, K., & Van der
Child Development, 76, 869–882. analysis. Journal of Community Psychology,
Molen, M. (2006). A heart rate analysis of
Crouter, A., Whiteman, S., McHale, S., & Osgood, 27, 569–588.
developmental change in feedback process-
ing and rule shifting from childhood to early D. W. (2007). Development of gender attitude Curran, P., Stice, E., & Chassin, L. (1997). The
adulthood. Journal of Experimental Child traditionality across middle childhood and relation between adolescent alcohol use and
Psychology, 95, 99–116. adolescence. Child Development, 78, 911–926. peer alcohol use: A longitudinal random co-
Crow, S., Eisenberg, M. E., Story, M., & Neumark- efficients model. Journal of Consulting and
Crone, E. A., & van der Molen, M. W. (2007). De-
Sztainer, D. (2006). Psychosocial and behav- Clinical Psychology, 65, 130–140.
velopment of decision making in school-
aged children and adolescents: Evidence ioral correlates of dieting among overweight Curtner-Smith, M., & MacKinnon-Lewis, C.
from heart rate and skin conductance analy- and non-overweight adolescents. Journal of (1994). Family process effects on adolescent
sis. Child Development, 78, 1288–1301. Adolescent Health, 38, 569–574. males’ susceptibility to antisocial peer pres-
Croninger, R. G., & Lee, V. E. (2001). Social capital Crystal, D., Chen, C., Fuligni, A., Stevenson, H., sure. Family Relations, 43, 462–468.
and dropping out of school: Benefits to at- Hsu, C., Ko, H., . . . Kimura, S. (1994). Psycho- Cusick, P. A. (1973). Inside high school. New York:
risk students of teachers’ support and guid- logical maladjustment and academic achieve- Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
ance. Teachers College Record, 103, 548–581. ment: A cross-cultural study of Japanese, Cvijanovich, N. Z., Cook, L. J., Mann, N. C., &
Crosby, R. A., DiClemente, R. J., Wingood, G. M., Chinese, and American high school students. Dean, J. M. (2001). A population-based
Lang, D., & Harrington, K. F. (2003). Value of Child Development, 65, 738–753. study of crashes involving 16- and 17-year-
consistent condom use: A study of sexually Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychol- old drivers: The potential benefit of gradu-
transmitted disease prevention among African ogy of optimal experience. New York: Harper ated driver licensing restrictions. Pediatrics,
American adolescent females. American Jour- and Row. 107, 632–637.
nal of Public Health, 93, 901–902. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being Cyranowski, J., & Frank, E. (2000). Adolescent
Crosnoe, R. (2001). Academic orientation and pa- adolescent. New York: Basic Books. onset of the gender difference in lifetime
rental involvement in education during high Cubbin, C., Santelli, J., Brindis, C. D., & Brave- rates of major depression. Archives of General
school. Sociology of Education, 74, 210–230. man, P. (2005). Neighborhood context and Psychiatry, 57, 21–27.
Crosnoe, R. (2002). High school curriculum track sexual behaviors among adolescents: Find- D’Onofrio, B. M., Turkheimer, E., Emery, R. E.,
and adolescent association with delinquent ings from the National Longitudinal Study Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., . . . Madden, P. A.,
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-18 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-18 References

(2006). A genetically informed study of the Darling, N., Cumsille, P., Peña-Alampay, L., & Coat- of supportive and problematic interactions.
processes underlying the association between sworth, D. (2009). Individual and issue-specific Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 12–19.
parental marital instability and offspring differences in parental knowledge and adoles- Davis, A., Rhodes, J., & Hamilton-Leaks, J. (1997).
adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 42, cent disclosure in Chile, the Philippines, and When both parents may be a source of sup-
486–499. the United States. Journal of Research on Adoles- port and problems: An analysis of pregnant
Daddis, C. (2011). Desire for increased auton- cence, 19, 715–740. and parenting female African American ado-
omy and adolescents’ perceptions of peer Darling, N., Dowdy, B. B., Van Horn, M. L., & lescents’ relationships with their mothers and
autonomy: “Everyone else can; Why can’t I?” Caldwell, L. L. (1999). Mixed-sex settings fathers. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 7,
Child Development, 82, 1310–1326. and the perception of competence. Journal of 331–348.
Daddis, C., & Smetana, J. (2005). Middle-class Youth and Adolescence, 28, 461–480. Davis, B., & Carpenter, C. (2009). Proximity of
African American families’ expectations for Darling, N., Hamilton, S., & Matsuda, S. fast-food restaurants to schools and adoles-
adolescents’ behavioral autonomy. Interna- (1990, March). Functional roles and social cent obesity. American Journal of Public
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, roles: Adolescents’ significant others in the Health, 99, 505–510.
371–381. United States and Japan. Paper presented at Davis, E., & Friel, L. V. (2001). Adolescent sexual-
Daha, M. (2011). Contextual factors contributing the biennial meetings of the Society for Re- ity: Disentangling the effects of family struc-
to ethnic identity development of second- search on Adolescence, Atlanta. ture and family context. Journal of Marriage
generation Iranian American adolescents. Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting and the Family, 63, 669–681.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 26, 543–569. style as context: An integrative model. Psy- Davis, K., & Kirkpatrick, L. (1994). Attachment
Dahl, R. (2008). Biological, developmental, and chological Bulletin, 113, 487–496. style, gender, and relationship stability: A
neurobehavioral factors relevant to adoles- Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1997). Community longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality
cent driving risks. American Journal of Pre- influences on adolescent achievement and and Social Psychology, 66, 502–512.
ventive Medicine, 35, S278–S284. deviance. In J. Brooks-Gunn, G. Duncan, & Davis, K. A., & Epkins, C. (2009). Do private re-
Dahl, R. (2011). Understanding the risky busi- L. Aber (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty: Context ligious practices moderate the relation be-
ness of adolescence. Neuron, 69, 837–839. and consequences for children: Vol. 2. Concep- tween family conflict and preadolescents’
tual, methodological, and policy approaches to depression and anxiety symptoms? Journal of
Dahl, R., & Hariri, A. (2005). Lessons from G.
studying neighborhoods (pp. 120–131). New Early Adolescence, 29, 693–717.
Stanley Hall: Connecting new research in
York: Russell Sage Foundation. Davis, L. E., Ajzen, I., Saunders, J., & Williams,
biological science to the study of adolescent
development. Journal of Research on Adoles- Darling-Hammond, L. (1997). The right to learn. T. (2002). The decision of African Ameri-
cence 15, 367–382. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. can students to complete high school: An
Dallago, L., Perkins, D., Santinello, M., Boyce, W., Darling-Hammond, L. (2006). No child left be- application of the theory of planned behav-
Molcho, M., & Morgan, A. (2009). Adolescent hind and high school reform. Harvard Edu- ior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94,
place attachment, social capital, and per- cational Review, 76, 642–667. 810–819.
ceived safety: A comparison of 13 countries. Darroch, J. E., Singh, S., & Frost, J. J. (2001). Dif- Davis-Kean, P., Huesmann, L. R., Jager, J., Collins,
American Journal of Community Psychology, ferences in teenage pregnancy rates among W. A., Bates, J. E., & Lansford, J. E. (2008).
44, 148–160. five developed countries: The roles of sexual Changes in the relation of self-efficacy beliefs
Dandreaux, D. M., & Frick, P. J. (2009). Develop- activity and contraceptive use. Family Plan- and behaviors across development. Child De-
mental pathways to conduct problems: A ning Perspectives, 33, 244–250. velopment, 79, 1257–1269.
further test of the childhood and adolescent- Dauber, S., Alexander, K., & Entwisle, D. (1996). Davison, T., & McCabe, M. (2011). Physical
onset distinction. Journal of Abnormal Child Tracking and transitions through the middle attractiveness. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein
Psychology, 37, 375–385. grades: Channeling educational trajectories. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp.
Daniel, D. B., & Klaczynski, P. A. (2006). Develop- Sociology of Education, 69, 290–307. 269–274). New York: Academic Press.
mental and individual differences in condi- Davey, C. G., Yücel, M., & Allen, N. B. (2008). Dawes, N. P., & Larson, R. (2011). How youth get
tional reasoning: Effects of logic instructions The emergence of depression in adolescence: engaged: Grounded-theory research on mo-
and alternative antecedents. Child Develop- Development of the prefrontal cortex and tivational development in organized youth
ment, 77, 339–354. the representation of reward. Neuroscience & programs. Developmental Psychology, 47,
Daniels, D., Dunn, J., Furstenberg, F., Jr., & Plomin, Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1–19. 259–269.
R. (1985). Environmental differences within Davies, P., & Lindsay, L. (2004). Interparental de Boer, H., Bosker, R., & van der Werf, M.
the family and adjustment differences within conflict and adolescent adjustment: Why (2010). Sustainability of teacher expectation
pairs of adolescent siblings. Child Develop- does gender moderate early adolescent vul- bias effects on long-term student perfor-
ment, 56, 764–774. nerability? Journal of Family Psychology, 18, mance. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Daniels, E., & Leaper, C. (2011). Gender issues. 160–170. 102, 168–179.
In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclo- Davies, P., & Windle, M. (2000). Middle adoles- de Bruyn, E., & Cillessen, A. (2006). Heterogene-
pedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 151–159). cents’ dating pathways and psychosocial ad- ity of girls’ consensual popularity: Academic
New York: Academic Press. justment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 90–118. and interpersonal behavioral profiles. Jour-
Darling, N., Cumsille, P., & Martínez, M. L. Davies, P., & Windle, M. (2001). Interparental nal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 435–445.
(2007). Adolescents as active agents in the discord and adolescent adjustment trajecto- de Bruyn, E., Cillessen, A., & Wissink, I. (2010).
socialization process: Legitimacy of parental ries: The potentiating and protective role of Associations of peer acceptance and per-
authority and obligation to obey as predic- intrapersonal attributes. Child Development, ceived popularity with bullying and victim-
tors of obedience. Journal of Adolescence, 30, 72, 1163–1178. ization in early adolescence. Journal of Early
297–311. Davila, J. (2008). Depressive symptoms and ado- Adolescence, 30, 543–566.
Darling, N., Cumsille, P., & Martínez, M. (2008). lescent romance: Theory, research, and im- De Goede, I. H. A., Branje, S. J. T., Delsing, M. J.
Individual differences in adolescents’ beliefs plications. Child Development Perspectives, 2, M. H., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2009). Linkages
about the legitimacy of parental authority 26–31. over time between adolescents’ relationships
and obligation to obey: A longitudinal inves- Davis, A., & Rhodes, J. (1994). African-American with parents and friends. Journal of Youth
tigation. Child Development, 79, 1103–1118. teenage mothers and their mothers: An analysis and Adolescence, 38, 1304–1315.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-19 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-19

De Goede, I., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2009). De- Structure and associations with problem be- your friend matter? Journal of Youth and Ad-
velopmental changes in adolescents’ percep- haviors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, olescence, 31, 155–166.
tions of relationships with their parents. 467–480. Diamond, L., & Lucas, S. (2004). Sexual-minority
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 75–88. Delva, J., Wallace, J. M., Jr., O’Malley, P. M., and heterosexual youths’ peer relationships:
de Graaf, H., Vanwesenbeeck, I., Meijer, S., Bachman, J. G., Johnston, L. D., & Schulen- Experiences, expectations, and implications
Woertman, L., & Meeus, W. (2009). Sexual berg, J. E. (2005). The epidemiology of alcohol, for well-being. Journal of Research of Adoles-
trajectories during adolescence: Relation to marijuana, and cocaine use among Mexican cence, 14, 313–340.
demographic characteristics and sexual risk. American, Puerto Rican, Cuban American, Diamond, L., & Savin-Williams, R. (2009). Ado-
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 276–282. and other Latin American eighth-grade stu- lescent sexuality. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg
Deardorff, J., Gonzales, N. A., Christopher, F. S., dents in the United States: 1991–2002. Ameri- (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd
Roosa, M. W., & Millsap, R. E. (2005). Early can Journal of Public Health, 95, 696–702. ed., Vol. 1, pp. 479–523). New York: Wiley.
puberty and adolescent pregnancy: The influ- Demetriou, A., Christou, C., Spanoudis, G., & Diamond, L., & Savin-Williams, R. (2011). Sexu-
ence of alcohol use. Pediatrics 116, 1451–1456. Platsidou, M. (2002). The development of ality. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Ency-
Deater-Deckard, K., & Plomin, R. (1999). An mental processing: Efficiency, working mem- clopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 314–321).
adoption study of etiology of teacher and ory, and thinking. Monographs of the Society New York: Academic Press.
parent reports of externalizing behavior for Research in Child Development, 67, Serial Diamond, L., Savin-Williams, R., & Dubé, E.
problems in middle childhood. Child Devel- No. 268. (1999). Sex, dating, passionate friendships,
opment, 70, 144–154. Demo, D., & Acock, A. (1996). Family structure, and romance: Intimate peer relations among
DeBerry, K., Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. (1996). family process, and adolescent well-being. lesbian, gay, and bisexual adolescents. In W.
Family racial socialization and ecological Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 457–488. Furman, B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), Con-
competence: Longitudinal assessments of Deng, S., Kim, S. Y., Vaughan, P. W., & Li, J. temporary perspectives on adolescent roman-
African-American transracial adoptees. (2010). Cultural orientation as a moderator tic relationships (pp. 175–210). New York:
Child Development, 67, 2375–2399. of the relationship between Chinese Ameri- Cambridge University Press.
Decety, J., & Michalska, K. (2010). Neurodevelop- can adolescents’ discrimination experiences Dick, D. (2011). Developmental changes in ge-
mental changes in the circuits underlying em- and delinquent behaviors. Journal of Youth netic influences on alcohol use and depen-
pathy and sympathy from childhood to and Adolescence, 39, 1027–1040. dence. Child Development Perspectives, 5,
adulthood. Developmental Science, 13, 886–899. Denissen, J. J. A., van Aken, M. A. G., & Dubas, 223–230.
Dee, T., & Evans, W. (2001). Teens and traffic J. S. (2009). It takes two to tango: How par- Dick, D., Rose, R., Pulkkinen, L., & Kaprio, J.
safety. In J. Gruber (Ed.), Risky behavior among ents’ and adolescents’ personalities link to (2001). Measuring puberty and understand-
youths: An economic analysis (pp. 121–165). the quality of their mutual relationship. De- ing its impact: A longitudinal study of adoles-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. velopmental Psychology, 45, 928–941. cent twins. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Denton, K., & Zarbatany, L. (1996). Age differ- 30, 385–400.
Değirmencioğilu, S., Tolson, J., & Urberg, K.
ences in support processes in conversations Dick, D., Rose, R. J., Viken, R. J., & Kaprio, J.
(1993, March). Stability of adolescent social
between friends. Child Development, 67, (2000). Pubertal timing and substance use:
networks over the school year. Paper presented
1360–1373. Associations between and within families
at the biennial meetings of the Society for Re-
DeRose, L. M., Shiyko, M. P., Foster, H., & across late adolescence. Developmental Psy-
search in Child Development, New Orleans.
Brooks-Gunn, J. (2011). Associations be- chology, 36, 180–189.
Değirmencioğilu, S., & Urberg, K. (1994, tween menarcheal timing and behavioral Dick, D., Viken, R., Purcell, S., Kaprio, J.,
February). Cross-gender friendships in adoles- developmental trajectories for girls from age Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2007). Parental
cence: Who chooses the “other”? Paper 6 to age 15. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, monitoring moderates the importance of ge-
presented at the biennial meetings of the 40, 1329–1342. netic and environmental influences on ado-
Society for Research on Adolescence, San Di- lescent smoking. Journal of Abnormal
Desjardins, T. L., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2011). Rela-
ego. Psychology, 116, 213–218.
tional victimization and depressive symp-
Değirmencioğilu, S., Urberg, K., Tolson, J., & toms in adolescence: Moderating effects of DiClemente, R., Durbin, M., Siegel, D., Krasnovsky,
Richard, P. (1998). Adolescent friendship net- mother, father, and peer emotional support. F., Lazarus, N., & Comacho, T. (1992). Deter-
works: Continuity and change over the school Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 531–544. minants of condom use among junior high
year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 313–337. Dhariwal, A., Connolly, J., Paciello, M., & Caprara, school students in a minority, inner-city school
DeGarmo, D. S., & Martinez, C. R. J. (2006). A G. V. (2009). Adolescent peer relationships and district. Pediatrics, 89, 197–202.
culturally informed model of academic well- emerging adult romantic styles: A longitudinal DiClemente, R., Wingood, G., Harrington, K.,
being for Latino youth: The importance of study of youth in an Italian community. Jour- Lang, D., Davies, S., . . . Hook, E., (2004). Ef-
discriminatory experiences and social sup- nal of Adolescent Research, 24, 579–600. ficacy of an HIV prevention intervention for
port. Family Relations, 55, 267–278. Diamond, L. (1998). Development of sexual African American adolescent girls: A
Del Giudice, M., Angeleri, R., & Manera, V. orientation among adolescent and young randomized controlled trial. Journal of the
(2009). The juvenile transition: A develop- adult women. Developmental Psychology, 34, American Medical Association, 292, 171–179.
mental switch point in human life history. 1085–1095. Diehl, L., Vicary, J., & Deike, R. (1997). Longitu-
Developmental Review, 29, 1–31. Diamond, L. (2000). Passionate friendships among dinal trajectories of self-esteem from early to
Delgado, M. Y., Updegraff, K. A., Roosa, M. W., & adolescent sexual-minority women. Journal of middle adolescence and related psychosocial
Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2011). Discrimination Research on Adolescence, 10, 191–209. variables among rural adolescents. Journal of
and Mexican-Origin adolescents’ adjuroles Diamond, L. (2008). Female bisexuality from Research on Adolescence, 7, 393–411.
of adolescents’, mothers’, and fathers’ cultural adolescence to adulthood: Results from a 10- Dielman, T. (1994). School-based research on the
orientations and values. Journal of Youth and year longitudinal study. Developmental Psy- prevention of adolescent alcohol use and
Adolescence, 40, 125–139. chology, 44, 5–14. misuse: Methodological issues and advances.
Delsing, M. J. M. H., ter Bogt, Tom F. M., Engels, R. Diamond, L., & Dubé, E. M. (2002). Friendship Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4, 271–293.
C. M. E., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2007). Adolescents’ and attachment among heterosexual and DiIorio, C., Dudley, W. N., Kelly, M., Soet, J. E.,
peer crowd identification in the Netherlands: sexual-minority youths: Does the gender of Mbwara, J., & Sharpe Potter, J. (2001). Social
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-20 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-20 References

cognitive correlates of sexual experience and and course of male antisocial behavior. Journal and number of sexual partners from adoles-
condom use among 13- through 15-year-old of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 651–663. cence through adulthood. Developmental Psy-
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 29, Dishion, T., Patterson, G., Stoolmiller, M., & chology, 46, 1747–1759.
208–216. Skinner, M. (1991). Family, school, and behav- Dollahite, D., Layton, E., Bahr, H., Walker, A., &
DiIorio, C., Kelley, M., & Hockenberry-Eaton, ioral antecedents to early adolescent involve- Thatcher, J. (2009). Giving up something
M. (1999). Communication about sexual ment with antisocial peers. Developmental good for something better: Sacred sacrifices
issues: Mothers, fathers, and friends. Journal Psychology, 27, 172–180. made by religious youth. Journal of Adoles-
of Adolescent Health, 24, 181–189. Dishion, T. J., Véronneau, M.-H., & Myers, M. W. cent Research, 24, 691–725.
Dijkstra, J., Cillessen, A., Lindenberg, S., & Veen- (2010). Cascading peer dynamics underlying Dolliver, M. (2010, November 15). Teen girls as
stra, R. (2010a). Same-gender and cross-gen- the progression from problem behavior to vio- avid shoppers. Adweek (accessed online at
der likeability: Associations with popularity lence in early to late adolescence. Development http://www.adweek.com/news/advertising-
and status enhancement: The TRAILS study. and Psychopathology, 22, 603–619. branding/teen-girls-avid-shoppers-103813).
Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 773–802. Dittus, P. J., & Jaccard, J. (2000). Adolescents’ Domina, T., Conley, A., & Farkas, G. (2011). The
Dijkstra, J., Cillessen, A., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, perceptions of maternal disapproval of sex: link between educational expectations and
R. (2010b). Basking in reflected glory and its Relationship to sexual outcomes. Journal of effort in the college-for-all era. Sociology of
limits: Why adolescents hang out with popular Adolescent Health, 26, 268–278. Education, 84, 93–112.
peers. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, Dmitrieva, J., Chen, C., Greenberger, E., & Gil- Donahue, K. L., D’Onofrio, B. M., Bates, J. E.,
942–958. Rivas, V. (2004). Family relationships and ado- Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S.
Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, R. lescent psychosocial outcomes: Converging (2010). Early exposure to parents’ relation-
(2008). Beyond the class norm: Bullying be- findings from Eastern and Western cultures. ship instability: Implications for sexual
havior of popular adolescents and its rela- Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14, 425–447. behavior and depression in adolescence.
tion to peer acceptance and rejection. Journal Dobkin, P., Tremblay, R., Mâsse, L., & Vitaro, F. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, 547–554.
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1289–1299. (1995). Individual and peer characteristics Donenberg, G. R., Emerson, E., & Mackesy-Amiti,
Dijkstra, J., Lindenberg, S., Veenstra, R., Steglich, in predicting boys’ early onset of substance M. E. (2011). Sexual risk among African
C., Isaacs, J., Card, N., & Hodges, E. (2010). abuse: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child American girls: Psychopathology and mother–
Influence and selection processes in weapon Development, 66, 1198–1214. daughter relationships. Journal of Consulting
carrying during adolescence: The roles of Dobkin, P., Tremblay, R., & Sacchitelle, C. (1997). and Clinical Psychology, 79, 153–158.
status, aggression, and vulnerability. Crimi- Predicting boys’ early-onset substance abuse Donnellan, M., Larsen-Rife, D., & Conger, R.
nology, 48, 187–220. from father’s alcoholism, son’s disruptive- (2005). Personality, family history, and
Dijkstra, J., Lindenberg, S., Verhulst, F., Ormel, J., ness, and mother’s parenting behavior. Jour- competence in early adult romantic relation-
& Veenstra, R. (2009). The relation between nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, ships. Journal of Personality and Social
popularity and aggressive, destructive, and 86–92. Psychology, 88, 562–576.
norm-breaking behaviors: Moderating ef- Dodge, K. (1986). A social information-processing Dorn, L., & Biro, F. (2011). Puberty and its mea-
fects of athletic abilities, physical attractive- model of social competence in children. In M. surement: A decade in review. Journal of Re-
ness, and prosociality. Journal of Research on Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on search on Adolescence, 21, 180–195.
Adolescence, 19, 401–413. Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77–125). Dorn, L., Dahl, R., Woodward, H., & Biro, F.
Dijkstra, J., & Veenstra, R. (2011). Peer relations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (2006). Defining the boundaries of early ad-
In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclo- Dodge, K., & Coie, J. (1987). Social information- olescence: A user’s guide to assessing puber-
pedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 255–259). processing factors in reactive and proactive tal status and pubertal timing in research
New York: Academic Press. aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal with adolescents. Applied Developmental Sci-
Dishion, T., & Owen, L. (2002). A longitudinal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, ence, 1, 30–56.
analysis of friendships and substance use: Bidi- 1146–1158. Dorn, L., Nottelmann, E. D., Susman, E. J., Inoff-
rectional influence from adolescence to adult- Dodge, K., Coie, J., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggres- Germain, G., Cutler, G. B., Jr., & Chrousos,
hood. Developmental Psychology, 38, 480–491. sion and antisocial behavior in youth. In W. G. P. (1999). Variability in hormone concen-
Dishion, T., Andrews, D., & Crosby, L. (1995). Damon & R. Lerner (Ser. Eds.) & N. Eisenberg trations and self-reported menstrual histo-
Antisocial boys and their friends in early (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. ries in young adolescents: Menarche as an
adolescence: Relationship characteristics, 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop- integral part of a developmental process.
quality, and interactional process. Child De- ment (6th ed., pp. 719–788). New York: Wiley. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 283–
velopment, 66, 139–151. Dodge, K., Greenberg, M., Malone, P., & Conduct 304.
Dishion, T., Capaldi, D., & Yoerger, K. (1999). Problems Prevention Research Group. (2008). Dornbusch, S., Carlsmith, J., Bushwall, S., Ritter,
Middle childhood antecedents to progres- Testing an idealized dynamic cascade model P., Leiderman, P., Hastorf, A., & Gross, T.
sions in male adolescent substance use: An of the development of serious violence in ad- (1985). Single parents, extended households,
ecological analysis of risk and protection. olescence. Child Development, 79, 1907–1927. and the control of adolescents. Child Devel-
Journal of Adolescent Research, 14, 175–205. Dodge, K., & Pettit, G. (2003). A biopsychosocial opment, 56, 326–341.
Dishion, T., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When model of the development of chronic con- Dornbusch, S., Erickson, K., Laird, J., & Wong, C.
interventions harm: Peer groups and problem duct problems in adolescence. Developmen- (2001). The relation of family and school at-
behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 755–764. tal Psychology, 39, 349–371. tachment to adolescent deviance in diverse
Dishion, T., Nelson, S., & Bullock, B. (2004). Pre- Dogan, S., Conger, R., Kim, K., & Masyn, K. groups and communities. Journal of Adoles-
mature adolescent autonomy: Parent disen- (2007). Cognitive and parenting pathways in cent Research, 16, 396–422.
gagement and deviant peer process in the the transmission of antisocial behavior from Dosenbach, N., Nardos, B., Cohen, A., Fair, D.,
amplification of problem behavior. Journal parents to adolescents. Child Development, Power, J., Church, J. A., . . . Schlaggar, B.
of Adolescence, 27, 515–530. 78, 335–349. (2010). Prediction of individual brain matu-
Dishion, T., Nelson, S., Winter, C., & Bullock, B. Dogan, S. J., Stockdale, G. D., Widaman, K. F., & rity using fMRI. Science, 329, 1358–1361.
(2004). Adolescent friendship as a dynamic Conger, R. D. (2010). Developmental relations Dotterer, A. M., & Lowe, K. (2011). Classroom
system: Entropy and deviance in the etiology and patterns of change between alcohol use context, school engagement, and academic
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-21 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-21

achievement in early adolescence. Journal of sleep disturbances—A cohort study. Journal epiphenomenon. Applied and Preventive Psy-
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1649–1660. of Adolescent Health, 46, 482–487. chology, 8, 103–117.
Dotterer, A., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (2007). Drumm, P., & Jackson, D. (1996). Developmen- DuBois, D., Tevendale, H., Burk-Braxton, C.,
Implications of out-of-school activities for tal changes in questioning strategies during Swenson, L., & Hardesty, J. (2000). Self-
school engagement in African American ad- adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Research, system influences during early adolescence:
olescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 11, 285–305. Investigation of an integrative model. Jour-
36, 391–401. Dryfoos, J. (1993). Schools as places for health, nal of Early Adolescence, 20, 12–43.
Dotterer, A., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. C. (2009). mental health, and social services. Teachers Dubow, E. F., Huesmann, L. R., Boxer, P., Pulkkinen,
The development and correlates of academic College Record, 94, 540–567. L., & Kokko, K. (2006). Middle childhood and
interests from childhood through adoles- Dubas, J., Graber, J., & Petersen, A. (1991). A adolescent contextual and personal predictors
cence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, longitudinal investigation of adolescents’ of adult educational and occupational out-
509–519. changing perceptions of pubertal timing. comes: A mediational model in two countries.
Dowdy, B. B., & Kliewer, W. (1998). Dating, Developmental Psychology, 27, 580–586. Developmental Psychology, 42, 937–949.
parent–adolescent conflict, and behavioral Dubé, E., & Savin-Williams, R. (1999). Sexual Duchesne, S., & Ratelle, C. (2010). Parental be-
autonomy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, identity development among ethnic sexual- haviors and adolescents’ achievement goals at
27, 473–492. minority male youths. Developmental Psy- the beginning of middle school: Emotional
Downey, D., Ainsworth, J. W., & Qian, Z. (2009). chology, 35, 1389–1398. problems as potential mediators. Journal of
Rethinking the attitude-achievement para- Educational Psychology, 102, 497–507.
Dube, S. R., Felitti, V. J., Dong, M., Chapman, D.
dox among Blacks. Sociology of Education, Duckworth, A., Peterson, C., Matthews, M., &
P., Giles, W. H., & Anda, R. F. (2003). Child-
82, 1–19. Kelly, D. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and pas-
hood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunc-
Downey, D., & Ainsworth-Darnell, J. W. (2002). sion for long-term goals. Journal of Personal-
tion and the risk of illicit drug use: The
The search for oppositional culture among ity and Social Psychology, 92, 1087–1101.
adverse childhood experiences study. Pediat-
Black students. American Sociological Review, rics, 111, 564–572. Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005).
67, 156–164. Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting
DuBois, D., Bull, C., Sherman, M., & Roberts, M.
academic performance of adolescents.
Downey, D., Ainsworth-Darnell, J., & Dufur, M. (1998). Self-esteem and adjustment in early
Psychological Science, 16, 939–944.
(1998). Sex of parent and children’s well- adolescence: A social-contextual perspective.
being in single-parent households. Journal of Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 27, 557– Due, P., Merlo, J., Harel-Fisch, Y., Damsgaard,
Marriage and the Family, 60, 878–893. 583. M., Holstein, B., Hetland, J., Currie, C., et al.
(2009). Socioeconomic inequality in expo-
Downey, D., & Pribesh, S. (2004). When race DuBois, D., Burk-Braxton, C., Swenson, L. P.,
sure to bullying during adolescence: a com-
matters: Teachers’ evaluations of students’ Tevendale, H. D., & Hardesty, J. L. (2002). Race
parative, cross-sectional, multilevel study in
classroom behavior. Sociology of Education, and gender influences on adjustment in early
35 countries. American Journal of Public
77, 267–282. adolescence: Investigation of an integrative
Health, 99, 907–915.
Downey, G., Bonica, C., & Rincón, C. (1999). model. Child Development, 73, 1573–1592.
Dukes, R., Martinez, R., & Stein, J. (1997).
Rejection sensitivity and adolescent roman- DuBois, D., Felner, R., Brand, S., Adan, A., & Precursors and consequences of member-
tic relationships. In W. Furman, B. Brown, & Evans, E. (1992). A prospective study of life ship in youth gangs. Youth and Society, 29,
C. Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives stress, social support, and adaptation in 139–165.
on adolescent romantic relationships (pp. early adolescence. Child Development, 63,
Dumas, T. M., Lawford, H., Tieu, T.-T., & Pratt,
148–174). New York: Cambridge University 542–557.
M. W. (2009). Positive parenting in adoles-
Press. DuBois, D., Felner, R., Brand, S., & George, cence and its relation to low-point narration
Downey, G., Lebolt, A., Rincón, C., & Freitas, A. G. (1999). Profiles of self-esteem in early and identity status in emerging adulthood:
(1998). Rejection sensitivity and children’s adolescence: Identification and investigation A longitudinal analysis. Developmental Psy-
interpersonal difficulties. Child Develop- of adaptive correlates. American Journal of chology, 45, 1531–1544.
ment, 69, 1074–1091. Community Psychology, 27, 899–932.
Dumont, M., & Provost, M. (1999). Resilience in
Doyle, A., Lawford, H., & Markiewicz, D. (2009). DuBois, D., Felner, R., Meares, H., & Krier, M. adolescents: Protective role of social sup-
Attachment style with mother, father, (1994). Prospective investigation of the ef- port, coping strategies, self-esteem, and so-
bestfriend, and romantic partner during fects of socioeconomic disadvantage, life cial activities on experience of stress and
adolescence. Journal of Research on Adoles- stress, and social support on early adolescent depression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
cence, 19, 690–714. adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 28, 343–363.
Doyle, A., & Markiewicz, D. (2005). Parenting, 103, 511–522. Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I., & Blakemore, S.-J.
marital conflict, and adjustment from early DuBois, D., & Hirsch, B. (1990). School and (2010). Online usage of theory of mind con-
to mid-adolescence: Mediated by adolescent neighborhood friendship patterns of Blacks tinues to develop in late adolescence. Devel-
attachment style? Journal of Youth and Ado- and Whites in early adolescence. Child De- opmental Science, 13, 331–338.
lescence, 34, 97–110. velopment, 61, 524–536. Duncan, G. (1994). Families and neighbors as
Drabick, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Develop- DuBois, D., Holloway, B., Valentine, J., & Cooper, sources of disadvantage in the schooling de-
mental psychopathology. In B. Brown & H. (2002). Effectiveness of mentoring pro- cisions of White and Black adolescents.
M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of grams for youth: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Education, 103, 20–53.
adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 135–142). New York: American Journal of Community Psychology, Dunlop, S. M., Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent and
Academic Press. 30, 157–197. young adult crash risk: Sensation seeking,
Drapela, L. A., Gebelt, J. L., & McRee, N. (2006). DuBois, D., & Silverthorn, N. (2005). Natural substance use propensity, and substance use
Pubertal development, choice of friends, and mentoring relationships and adolescent behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46,
smoking initiation among adolescent males. health: Evidence from a national study. Amer- 90–92.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 717–727. ican Journal of Public Health, 95, 518–524. Dunn, J., Slomkowski, C., & Beardsall, L. (1994).
Dregan, A., & Armstrong, D. (2010). Adolescence DuBois, D., & Tevendale, H. (1999). Self-esteem Sibling relationships from the preschool pe-
sleep disturbances as predictors of adulthood in childhood and adolescence: Vaccine or riod through middle childhood and early
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-22 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-22 References

adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, behavior: Effects on early adolescent girls’ young adolescents’ experiences in schools and
315–324. sexual outcomes. Journal of Marriage and the families. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101.
Dunn, M., & Goldman, M. (1998). Age and Family, 55, 953–963. Eccles, J., & Roeser, R. (2009). Schools, aca-
drinking-related differences in the memory East, P., & Jacobson, L. (2003). Mothers’ differen- demic motivation, and stage-environment
organization of alcohol expectances in 3rd-, tial treatment of their adolescent childbear- fit. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Hand-
6th-, 9th-, and 12th-grade children. Journal of ing and nonchildbearing children: Contrasts book of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1,
Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 66, 579–585. between and within families. Journal of Fam- pp. 404–434). New York: Wiley.
Dunn, S., Putallaz, M., Sheppard, B., & Lindstrom, ily Psychology, 19, 384–396. Eccles, J., & Roeser, R. (2011). Schools as devel-
R. (1987). Social support and adjustment in East, P., & Khoo, S. (2005). Longitudinal path- opmental contexts during adolescence. Jour-
gifted adolescents. Journal of Educational Psy- ways linking family factors and sibling rela- nal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 225–241.
chology, 79, 467–473. tionship qualities to adolescent substance Edens, J. F., Marcus, D. K., & Vaughn, M. G.
Dupéré, V., Lacourse, E., Willms, D., Leventhal, use and sexual risk behaviors. Journal of (2011). Exploring the taxometric status of
T., & Tremblay, R. (2008). Neighborhood Family Psychology, 19, 571–580. psychopathy among youthful offenders: Is
poverty and early transition to sexual activity East, P., Reyes, B. T., & Horn, E. J. (2007). Associa- there a juvenile psychopath taxon? Law and
in young adolescents: A developmental eco- tion between adolescent pregnancy and a fam- Human Behavior, 35, 13–24.
logical approach. Child Development, 79, ily history of teenage births. Perspectives on Edens, J., Skeem, J., Cruise, K., & Cauffman,
1463–1476. Sexual and Reproductive Health, 39, 108–115. E. (2000). The assessment of juvenile psy-
Dupéré, V., Lacourse, É., Willms, J. D., Vitaro, F., East, P., & Rook, K. (1992). Compensatory pat- chopathy and its association with violence: A
& Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Affiliation to youth terns of support among children’s peer critical review. Behavioral Sciences and the
gangs during adolescence: The interaction relationships: A test using school friends, Law, 18, 53–80.
between childhood psychopathic tendencies nonschool friends, and siblings. Develop- Eder, D. (1985). The cycle of popularity: Inter-
and neighborhood disadvantage. Journal of mental Psychology, 28, 163–172. personal relations among female adolescence.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 1035–1045. Eaton, D., McKnight-Eily, L., Lowry, R., Perry, Sociology of Education, 58, 154–165.
DuRant, R., Rome, E., Rich, M., Allred, E., G., Presley-Cantrell, L., & Croft, J. (2010). Edwards, A. C., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Dick, D.
Emans, S., & Woods, E. (1997). Tobacco and Prevalence of insufficient, borderline, and M. (2011). Pubertal development moderates
alcohol use behaviors portrayed in music optimal hours of sleep among high school the importance of environmental influences
videos: A content analysis. American Journal students–United States, 2007. Journal of Ado- on depressive symptoms in adolescent girls
of Public Health, 87, 1131–1135. lescent Health, 46, 399–401. and boys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. (2006). Task Eaton, M. J., & Dembo, M. H. (1997). Differences 40, 1383–1393.
values and ability beliefs as predictors of in the motivational beliefs of Asian Ameri-
Egan, S., & Perry, D. (1998). Does low self-regard
high school literacy choices: A developmen- can and non-Asian students. Journal of Edu-
invite victimization? Developmental Psychol-
tal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychol- cational Psychology, 89, 433–440.
ogy, 34, 299–309.
ogy, 98, 382–393. Eberhart, N. K., Shih, J. H., Hammen, C. L., &
Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability Egan, S., & Perry, D. (2001). Gender identity: A
Brennan, P. A. (2006). Understanding the
conceptions. In A. Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.), multidimensional analysis with implications
sex difference in vulnerability to adolescent
The development of achievement motivation for psychosocial adjustment. Developmental
depression: An examination of child and
(pp. 57–88). New York: Academic Press. Psychology, 37, 451–463.
parent characteristics. Journal of Abnormal
Eamon, M. K. (2005). Social-demographic, Child Psychology, 34, 495–508. Eichen, D. M., Conner, B. T., Daly, B. P., & Fauber,
school, neighborhood, and parenting influ- R. L. (2012). Weight perception, substance
Ebin, V. J., Sneed, C. D., Morisky, D. E., Rotherman-
ences on the academic achievement of La- use, and disordered eating behaviors: Com-
Borus, M., Magnusson, A. M., & Malotte, C.
tino young adolescents. Journal of Youth and paring normal weight and overweight high-
(2001). Acculturation and interrelationships
Adolescence, 34, 163–174. school students. Journal of Youth and
between problem and health-promoting be-
Adolescence, 41, 1–13.
Earls, F., Cairns, R., & Mercy, J. (1993). The con- haviors among Latino adolescents. Journal of
trol of violence and the promotion of Adolescent Health, 28, 62–72. Eisenberg, M., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2010).
nonviolence in adolescents. In S. Millstein, Friends’ dieting and disordered eating be-
Eccles, J. (2004). Schools, academic motivation,
A. Petersen, & E. Nightingale (Eds.), Promot- haviors among adolescents five years later:
and stage-environment fit. In R. Lerner &
ing the health of adolescents: New directions Findings from Project EAT. Journal of Adoles-
L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent
for the twenty-first century (pp. 285–304). cent Health, 47, 67–73.
psychology. New York: Wiley.
New York: Oxford University Press. Eccles, J., Early, D., Frasier, K., Belansky, E., & Eisenberg, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Haines, J.,
East, P. (2009). Adolescents’ relationships with McCarthy, K. (1997). The relation of connec- & Wall, M. (2006). Weight-teasing and emo-
siblings. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), tion, regulation, and support for autonomy tional well-being in adolescents: Longitudi-
Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., to adolescents’ functioning. Journal of Ado- nal findings from project EAT. Journal of
Vol. 2, pp. 43–73). New York: Wiley. lescent Research, 12, 263–286. Adolescent Health, 38, 675–683.
East, P., & Blaustein, E. (1995, March). Perceived Eccles, J., Lord, S. E., Roeser, R. (1996). Round Eisenberg, N., Carlo, G., Murphy, B., & Van
timing of life-course transitions: Race differ- holes, square pegs, rocky roads, and sore Court, P. (1995). Prosocial development in
ences in early adolescent girls’ sexual, mar- feet: The impact of stage/environment fit on late adolescence: A longitudinal study. Child
riage, and childbearing expectations. Paper young adolescents’ experiences in schools Development, 66, 1179–1197.
presented at the biennial meetings of the and families. In S. L. Toth D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Guthrie, I. K.,
Society for Research in Child Development, Adolescence: Opportunities and challenges Murphy, B. C., & Shepard, S. A (2005). Age
Indianapolis. (Vol. 7, pp. 49–93). Rochester, NY: University changes in prosocial responding and moral
East, P., & Felice, M. (1996). Adolescent pregnancy of Rochester Press. reasoning in adolescence and early adult-
and parenting: Findings from a racially diverse Eccles, J., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C., hood. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15,
sample. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., & Mac Iver, D. 235–260.
East, P., Felice, M., & Morgan, M. (1993). Sisters’ (1993). Development during adolescence: Eisenberg, N., Morris, A., McDaniel, B., & Spinrad,
and girlfriends’ sexual and childbearing The impact of stage-environment fit on T. (2009). Moral cognitions and prosocial
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-23 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-23

responding in adolescence. In R. Lerner & adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. factors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent 193–227). New York: Wiley. ogy, 35, 405–416.
psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 229–265). New Elo, I., King, R., & Furstenberg, F., Jr. (1999). Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness
York: Wiley. Adolescent females: Their sexual partners of marital relations and parent–child rela-
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., & Koller, S. (2001). and the fathers of their children. Journal of tions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological
Brazilian adolescents’ prosocial moral judg- Marriage and the Family, 61, 74–84. Bulletin, 118, 108–132.
ment and behavior: Relations to sympathy, Emery, R., Beam, C., & Rowen, J. (2011). Adoles- Erickson, K., Crosnoe, R., & Dornbusch, S. M.
perspective taking, gender-role orientation, cents’ experience of parental divorce. In (2000). A social process model of adolescent
and demographic characteristics. Child De- B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia deviance: Combining social control and dif-
velopment, 72, 518–534. of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 10–17). New York: ferential association perspectives. Journal of
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1980). Adolescence in historical Academic Press. Youth and Adolescence, 29, 395–425.
perspective. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of Ennett, S., & Bauman, K. (1994). The contribu- Erikson, E. (1959). Identity and the life cycle.
adolescent psychology. New York: Wiley. tion of influence and selection to adolescent Psychological Issues, 1, 1–171.
Elders, M., Perry, C., Eriksen, M., & Giovino, G. peer group homogeneity: The case of adoles- Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New
(1994). The report of the Surgeon General: cent cigarette smoking. Journal of Personality York: Norton.
Preventing tobacco use among young peo- and Social Psychology, 67, 653–663. Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New
ple. American Journal of Public Health, 84, Ennett, S., & Bauman, K. (1996). Adolescent York: Norton.
543–547. social networks: School, demographic, and Erkut, S., Szalacha, L. A., Garcia Coll, C., &
Eley, T., Lichenstein, P., & Stevenson, J. (1999). longitudinal considerations. Journal of Ado- Alarcon, O. (2000). Puerto Rican early ado-
Sex differences in the etiology of aggressive lescent Research, 11, 194–215. lescents’ self-esteem patterns. Journal of Re-
and nonaggressive antisocial behavior: Re- Ennett, S., Bauman, K., Hussong, A., Faris, R., search on Adolescence, 10, 339–364.
sults from two twin studies. Child Develop- Foshee, V., Cai, L., & DuRant, R. (2006). The Ernst, M., Nelson, E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E., Monk,
ment, 70, 155–168. peer context of adolescent substance use: C., Leibenluft, E., . . . Pine, D. (2005). Amyg-
Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Findings from social network analysis. Jour- dala and nucleus accumbens in response to
Child Development, 38, 1025–1034. nal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 159–186. receipt and omission of gains in adults and
Elkington, K. S., Bauermeister, J. A., & Zimmerman, Ennett, S., Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Hussong, adolescents. Neuroimage, 25, 1279–1291.
M. A. (2010). Psychological distress, substance A., Faris, R., . . . Durant, R. (2008). The social Erol, R. Y., & Orth, U. (2011). Self-esteem devel-
use, and HIV/STI risk behaviors among ecology of adolescent alcohol misuse. Child opment from age 14 to 30 years: A longitudi-
youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, Development, 79, 1777–1791. nal study. Journal of Personality and Social
514–527. Ennett, S., Tobler, N., Ringwalt, C., & Flewelling, Psychology, 101, 607–619.
Ellickson, P., Bell, R., & McGuigan, K. (1993). R. (1994). How effective is drug abuse resis- Esbensen, F., Deschenes, E., & Winfree, L., Jr.
Preventing adolescent drug use: Long-term tance education? A meta-analysis of Project (1999). Differences between gang girls and
results of a junior high program. American DARE outcome evaluations. American Jour- gang boys: Results from a multisite survey.
Journal of Public Health, 83, 856–861. nal of Public Health, 84, 1394–1401. Youth and Society, 31, 27–53.
Elliott, D., Huizinga, D., & Menard, S. (1989). Enright, R., Levy, V., Harris, D., & Lapsley, D. Espelage, D. L., Basile, K. C., & Hamburger, M. E.
Multiple problem youth: Delinquency, sub- (1987). Do economic conditions influence (2012). Bullying perpetration and subsequent
stance abuse, and mental health problems. how theorists view adolescents? Journal of sexual violence perpetration among middle
New York: Springer-Verlag. Youth and Adolescence, 16, 541–560. school students. Journal of Adolescent Health,
Ellis, B. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation Enriquez, L. E. (2011). “Because we feel the pres- 50, 60–65.
in girls: An integrated life history approach. sure and we also feel the support”: Examin- Espelage, D. L., Holt, M. K., & Henkel, R. R.
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920–958. ing the educational success of undocumented (2003). Examination of peer-group contex-
Ellis, B., Bates, J., Dodge, K., Fergusson, D., immigrant Latina/o students. Harvard Edu- tual effects on aggression during early ado-
Horwood, L., . . . Pettit, G. (2003). Does cational Review, 81, 476–499. lescence. Child Development, 74, 205–220.
father absence place daughters at special risk Ensminger, M., Lamkin, R., & Jacobson, N. Espenshade, T. J., Hale, L. E., & Chung, C. Y.
for early sexual activity and teenage preg- (1996). School leaving: A longitudinal per- (2005). The frog pond revisited: High school
nancy? Child Development, 74, 801–821. spective including neighborhood effects. academic context, class rank, and elite col-
Child Development, 67, 2400–2416. lege admission. Sociology of Education, 78,
Ellis, B., Del Guidice, M., Dishion, T., Figueredo,
A., Gray, G., Griskevicius, G., . . . Wilson, D. Entwisle, D. (1990). Schools and the adolescent. 269–293.
(2012). The evolutionary basis of risky In S. Feldman & G. Elliott (Eds.), At the Eurostat. (2010). Young adults in the EU27 in
adolescent behavior: Implications for sci- threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 2008. Luxembourg: Author.
ence, policy, and practice. Developmental 197–224). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- Evans, A. D., & Lee, K. (2011). Verbal deception
Psychology, 48, 598–623. sity Press. from late childhood to middle adolescence
Ellis, B., Shirtcliff, E., Boyce, W., Deardoff, J., & Entwisle, D., Alexander, K., & Olson, L. (2004). and its relation to executive functioning skills.
Essex, M. (2011). Quality of early family rela- Temporary as compared to permanent high Developmental Psychology, 47, 1108–1116.
tionships and the timing and tempo of pu- school dropout. Social Forces, 82, 1181–1205. Evans, E., Schweingruber, H., & Stevenson, H. W.
berty: Effects depend on biological sensitivity Entwisle, D., Alexander, K., & Olson, L. (2005). (2002). Gender differences in interest and
to context. Development and Psychopathol- Urban teenagers work and dropout. Youth knowledge acquisition: The United States,
ogy, 23, 85–99. and Society, 37, 3–32. Taiwan, and Japan. Sex Roles, 47, 153–167.
Elmen, J. (1991). Achievement orientation in Epstein, R. (2007). The case against adolescence: Eveleth, P., & Tanner, J. (1990). Worldwide varia-
early adolescence: Developmental patterns Rediscovering the adult in every teen. Sanger, tion in human growth (2nd ed.). New York:
and social correlates. Journal of Early Adoles- CA: Quill Driver Books. Cambridge University Press.
cence, 11, 125–151. Erath, S. A., Flanagan, K. S., & Bierman, K. L. Everett, S. A., Warren, C. W., Santelli, J. S., Kann,
Elmore, R. (2009). Schooling adolescents. In (2007). Social anxiety and peer relations in L., Collins, J. L., & Kolbe, L. J. (2000). Use of
R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of early adolescence: Behavioral and cognitive birth control pills, condoms, and withdrawal
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-24 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-24 References

among U.S. high school students. Journal of Environmental influences on fighting versus Feinberg, I., & Campbell, I. (2010). Sleep EEG
Adolescent Health, 27, 112–118. nonviolent behavior in peer situations: A changes during adolescence: An index of a
Eyre, S., & Millstein, S. (1999). What leads to sex? qualitative study with urban African Ameri- fundamental brain reorganization. Brain and
Adolescent preferred partners and reasons can adolescents. American Journal of Com- Cognition, 72, 56–65.
for sex. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 9, munity Psychology, 46, 19–35. Feinberg, M. E., & Hetherington, E. (2000). Sib-
277–307. Farrell, A. D., & Sullivan, T. (2004). Impact of ling differentiation in adolescence: Implica-
Fagot, B., Pears, K., Capaldi, D., Crosby, L., & witnessing violence on growth curves for tions for behavioral genetic theory. Child
Leve, C. (1998). Becoming an adolescent fa- problem behaviors among early adolescents Development, 71, 1512–1524.
ther: Precursors and parenting. Developmen- in urban and rural settings. Journal of Com- Feinberg, M. E., Howe, G. W., Reiss, D., &
tal Psychology, 34, 1209–1219. munity Psychology, 32, 505–525. Hetherington, E. (2000). Relationship be-
Faircloth, B. (2009). Making the most of adoles- Farrell, A. D., Sullivan, T. N., Esposito, L. E., tween perceptual differences of parenting
cence: Harnessing the search for identity to Meyer, A. L., & Valois, R. F. (2005). A latent and adolescent antisocial behavior and de-
understand classroom belonging. Journal of growth curve analysis of the structure of ag- pressive symptoms. Journal of Family Psy-
Adolescent Research, 24, 321–348. gression, drug use, and delinquent behaviors chology, 14, 531–555.
and their interrelations over time in urban Feinberg, M., McHale, S., Crouter, A., & Cumsille,
Falk, R., & Wilkening, F. (1998). Children’s con-
and rural adolescents. Journal of Research on P. (2003). Sibling differentation: Sibling and
struction of fair chances: Adjusting probabili-
Adolescence, 15, 179–203. parent relationship trajectories in adoles-
ties. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1340–1357.
Farrell, A. D., & White, K. S. (1998). Peer influ- cence. Child Development, 74, 1261–1274.
Fallon, B. J., & Bowles, T. V. (1997). The effect of
ences and drug use among urban adolescents: Feinberg, M., Neiderhiser, J., Simmens, S., Reiss,
family structure and family functioning
Family structure and parent–adolescent rela- D., & Hetherington, E. (2000). Sibling com-
on adolescents’ perceptions of intimate time
tionship as protective factors. Journal of Con- parison of differential parental treatment in
spent with parents, siblings, and peers. Journal
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 248–258. adolescence: Gender, self-esteem, and emo-
of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 25–43.
Farrelly, M. C., Davis, K. C., Haviland, M. L., Heal- tionality as mediators of the parenting-
Fang, X., Stanton, B., Li, X., Feigelman, S., & adjustment association. Child Development,
ton, C. G., & Messeri, P. (2005). Evidence of a
Baldwin, R. (1998). Similarities in sexual ac- 71, 1611–1628.
dose-response relationship between “truth”
tivity and condom use among friends within
antismoking ads and youth smoking preva- Feiring, C. (1993, March). Developing concepts
groups before and after a risk-reduction
lence. American Journal of Public Health, 95, of romance from 15 to 18 years. Paper pre-
intervention. Youth and Society, 29, 431–450.
425–431. sented at the biennial meetings of the Soci-
Fanti, K., Brookmeyer, K., Henrich, C., & Kuper- Farrington, D. (2009). Conduct disorder, aggres- ety for Research in Child Development,
minc, G. (2009). Aggressive behavior and sion, and delinquency. In R. Lerner & L. New Orleans.
quality of friendships. Journal of Early Ado- Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psy- Feiring, C. (1999). Gender identity and the de-
lescence, 29, 826–838. chology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 683–722). New velopment of romantic relationships in
Farb, A., & Matjasko, J. (2012). Recent advances York: Wiley. adolescence. In W. Furman, B. Brown, & C.
in research on school-based extracurricular Farrington, D., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on
activities and adolescent development. De- M. (2003). How can the relationship between adolescent romantic relationships (pp. 211–
velopmental Review, 32, 1–48. race and violence be explained? In D. 232). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Farkas, G., Grobe, R., & Shuan, Y. (1990). Cultural Hawkins (Ed.), Violent crimes: Assessing race Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1991). The transition
resources and school success: Gender, ethnic- and ethnic differences (pp. 213–237). New from middle childhood to early adolescence:
ity, and poverty groups within an urban York: Cambridge University Press. Sex differences in the social network and per-
school district. American Sociological Review, Fasick, F. (1994). On the “invention” of adoles- ceived self-competence. Sex Roles, 24, 489–510.
55, 127–142. cence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 6–23. Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1993). Do mothers
Farkas, G., Lleras, C., & Maczuga, S. (2002). Does Fasula, A. M., & Miller, K. S. (2006). African- know their teenagers’ friends? Implications
oppositional culture exist in minority and American and Hispanic adolescents’ inten- for individuation in early adolescence. Jour-
poverty peer groups? American Sociological tions to delay first intercourse: Parental nal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 337–354.
Review, 67, 148–155. communication as a buffer for sexually ac- Feldman, A. F., & Matjasko, J. L. (2007). Profiles
Farmer, T. W., Estell, D. B., Bishop, J. L., O’Neal, tive peers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, and portfolios of adolescent school-based
K. K., & Cairns, B. D. (2003). Rejected bullies 193–200. extracurricular activity participation. Jour-
or popular leaders? The social relations of Fauth, R. C., Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. nal of Adolescence, 30, 313–332.
aggressive subtypes of rural African Ameri- (2007). Welcome to the neighborhood? Feldman, S., & Fisher, L. (1997). The effect of
can early adolescents. Developmental Psy- Long-term impacts of moving to low- parents’ marital satisfaction on young adults’
chology, 39, 992–1004. poverty neighborhoods on poor children’s adaptation: A longitudinal study. Journal of
Farmer, T. W., Hamm, J. V., Leung, M.-C., Lambert, and adolescents’ outcomes. Journal of Re- Research on Adolescence, 7, 55-80.
K., & Gravelle, M. (2011). Early adolescent search on Adolescence, 17, 249–284. Feldman, S., & Gehring, T. (1988). Changing
peer ecologies in rural communities: Bullying Fauth, R. C., Roth, J. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. perceptions of family cohesion and power
in schools that do and do not have a transition (2007). Does the neighborhood context alter across adolescence. Child Development, 59,
during the middle grades. Journal of Youth and the link between youth’s after-school time 1034–1045.
Adolescence, 40, 1106–1117. activities and developmental outcomes? A Feldman, S., & Quatman, T. (1988). Factors influ-
Farrell, A. D., Erwin, E. H., Allison, K. W., Meyer, multilevel analysis. Developmental Psychol- encing age expectations for adolescent auton-
A., Sullivan, T., Camou, S., . . . Esposito, L. ogy, 43, 760–777. omy: A study of early adolescents and parents.
(2007). Problematic situations in the lives of Federal Bureau of Investigation. (2009). Crime Journal of Early Adolescence, 8, 325–343.
urban African American middle school stu- in the United States, 2008. Washington, DC: Feldman, S., & Rosenthal, D. (2000). The effect
dents: A qualitative study. Journal of Research Author. of communication characteristics on family
on Adolescence, 17, 413–454. Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Fam- members’ perceptions of parents as sex edu-
Farrell, A.D., Mays, S., Bettencourt, A., Erwin, E., ily Statistics. (2005). America’s children. cators. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10,
Vullin-Reynolds, M., & Allision, K. (2010). Washington: Author. 119–150.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-25 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-25

Felix, E., & You, S. (2011). Peer victimization within Figner, B., & Weber, E. (2011). Who takes risks Fitzpatrick, K. M., Dulin, A., & Piko, B. (2010).
the ethnic context of high school. Journal of when and why?: Determinants of risk tak- Bullying and depressive symptomatology
Community Psychology, 39, 860–875. ing. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- among low-income, African–American youth.
Felner, R., Brand, S., DuBois, D., Adan, A., Mulhall, ence, 20, 211–216. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 634–645.
P., & Evans, E. (1995). Socioeconomic disad- Filardo, E. (1996). Gender patterns in African Flammer, A., Alasker, F., & Noack, P. (1999).
vantage, proximal environmental experiences, American and White adolescents’ social Time use by adolescents in an interna-
and socioemotional and academic adjustment interactions in same-race, mixed-gender tional perspective, I: The case of leisure
in early adolescence: Investigation of a medi- groups. Journal of Personality and Social activities. In F. Alasker & A. Flammer
ated effects model. Child Development, 66, Psychology, 71, 71–82. (Eds.), The adolescent experience: European
774–792. Fincham, F. (1994). Understanding the associa- and American adolescents in the 1990s (pp.
Felson, R. B., Savolainen, J., Bjarnason, T., tion between marital conflict and child ad- 33–60). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Anderson, A. L., & Zohra, I. T. (2011). The justment: Overview. Journal of Family Flanagan, C. (2004). Volunteerism, leadership,
cultural context of adolescent drinking and Psychology, 8, 123–127. political socialization, and civic engagement.
violence in 30 European countries. Criminol- Finer, L. B. (2010). Unintended pregnancy among In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook
ogy, 49, 699–728. U.S. adolescents: Accounting for sexual activ- of adolescent psychology. New York: Wiley.
Fenzel, L. (2001). Prospective study of changes in ity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, 312–314. Flanagan, C., & Galay, L. (1995). Reframing the
global self-worth and strain during the tran- Fingerson, L. (2005). Do mother’s opinions mat- meaning of “political” in research with
sition to middle school. Journal of Early Ado- ter in teens’ sexual activity? Journal of Family adolescents. Perspectives on Political Sci-
lescence, 20, 93–116. Influence, 26, 947–974. ence, 24, 34–41.
Fergus, S., Zimmerman, M. A., & Caldwell, C. H. Finken, L., & Jacobs, J. (1996). Consultant choice Flanagan, C., & Stout, M. (2010). Developmen-
(2007). Growth trajectories of sexual risk across decision contexts: Are abortion deci- tal patterns of social trust between early
behavior in adolescence and young adult- sions different? Journal of Adolescent Re- and late adolescence: Age and school cli-
hood. American Journal of Public Health, 97, search, 11, 235–260. mate effects. Journal of Research on Adoles-
1096–1101. cence, 20, 748–773.
Finkenauer, C., Engles, R., & Meeus, W. (2002).
Ferguson, C. (2011). Video games and youth vio- Keeping secrets from parents: Advantages Flanagan, C., & Tucker, C. (1999). Adolescents’
lence: A prospective analysis in adolescents. and disadvantages of secrecy in adolescence. explanations for political issues: Concor-
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 377–391. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 123–136. dance with their views of self and society.
Ferguson, C., & Kilburn, J. (2009). The public Developmental Psychology, 35, 1198–1209.
Finn, A., Sheridan, M., Kam, C., Hinshaw, S., &
health risks of media violence: A meta-analytic D’Esposito, M. (2010). Longitudinal evi- Flanagan, C., & Wray-Lake, L. (2011). Civic and po-
review. Journal of Pediatrics, 154, 759–763. dence for functional specialization of the litical engagement. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein
Ferguson, Y., Kasser, T., & Jahng, S. (2011). neural circuit supporting working memory (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2,
Differences in life satisfaction and school in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, pp. 35–43). New York: Academic Press.
satisfaction among adolescents from three 30(33), 11062–11067. Flannery, D., Torquati, J., & Lindemeier, L. (1994).
nations: The role of perceived autonomy Finn, J., Gerber, S. B., & Boyd-Zaharias, J. (2005). The method and meaning of emotional ex-
support. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Small classes in the early grades, academic pression and experience during adolescence.
21, 649–661. achievement, and graduating from high school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 9, 8–27.
Fergusson, D., & Horwood, L. (2002). Male and Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 214–223. Flannery, D., Vazsonyi, A., & Rowe, D. (1996).
female offending trajectories. Development Fischhoff, B., de Bruin, W. B., Parker, A., Millstein, Caucasian and Hispanic early adolescent
and Psychopathology, 14, 159–177. S., & Halpern-Felsher, B. (2010). Adolescents’ substance use: Parenting, personality, and
Fergusson, D., & Lynskey, M. (1996). Alcohol perceived risk of dying. Journal of Adolescent school adjustment. Journal of Early Adoles-
misuse and adolescent sexual behaviors and Health, 46, 265–269. cence, 16, 71–89.
risk taking. Pediatrics, 98, 91–96. Fischhoff, B., & Quadrel, M. (1995). Adolescent Flannery, D., Weseter, K., & Singer, M. (2004).
Fergusson, D., Lynskey, M., & Horwood, L. alcohol decisions. In G. Boyd, J. Howard, & Impact of exposure to violence in school on
(1996). Factors associated with continuity R. Zucker (Eds.), Alcohol problems among child and adolescent mental health and
and changes in disruptive behavior patterns adolescents: Current directions in prevention behavior. Journal of Community Psychology,
between childhood and adolescence. Journal research (pp. 59–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 32, 559–573.
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 533–553. Fisher, C., Wallace, S. A., & Fenton, R. E. (2000). Flashman, J. (2012). Academic achievement and
Fergusson, D., & Woodward, L. (2000). Teenage Discrimination distress during adolescence. its impact on friend dynamics. Sociology of
pregnancy and female educational under- Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 679–695. Education, 85, 61–80.
achievement: A prospective study of a New Fisher, M., Golden, N., Katzman, D., Kriepe, Flavell, J., Green, F., & Flavell, E. (1998). The
Zealand birth cohort. Journal of Marriage R., Rees, J., Schebendach, J., . . . Hoberman, mind has a mind of its own: Developing
and the Family, 62, 147–161. H. (1995). Eating disorders in adolescents: knowledge about mental uncontrollability.
Fichtenberg, C. M., & Glantz, S. A. (2002). Youth A background paper. Journal of Adolescent Cognitive Development, 13, 127–138.
access interventions do not affect youth Health, 16, 420–437. Fletcher, A., Bonell, C., & Hargreaves, J. (2008).
smoking. Pediatrics, 109, 1088–1092. Fisher, P., Stoolmiller, M., Mannering, A., School effects on young people’s drug use: A
Field, T., Greenwald, P., Morrow, C., Healy, B., Takahashi, A., & Chamberlain, P. (2011). systematic review of intervention and obser-
Foster, T., . . . Guthertz, M., (1992). Behavior Foster placement disruptions associated with vational studies. Journal of Adolescent Health,
state matching during interactions of preado- problem behavior: Mitigating a threshold 42, 209–220.
lescent friends versus acquaintances. Develop- effect. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- Fletcher, A., Darling, N., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch,
mental Psychology, 28, 242–250. chology, 79, 481–487. S. (1995). The company they keep: Relation of
Fields, J. (2003). Children’s living arrangements Fite, P. J., Wynn, P., & Pardini, D. A. (2009). adolescents’ adjustment and behavior to their
and characteristics: March 2002. Current Explaining discrepancies in arrest rates between friends’ perceptions of authoritative parenting
Population Reports P20–547. Washington, Black and White male juveniles. Journal of Con- in the social network. Developmental Psychol-
DC: U.S. Census Bureau. sulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 916–927. ogy, 31, 300–310.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-26 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-26 References

Fletcher, A., Elder, G., Jr., & Mekos, D. (2000). (2010). Healthy adolescents’ neural response Moderating effects of race and religiosity in
Parental influences on adolescent involvement to reward: Associations with puberty, posi- emerging adulthood. Journal of Community
in community activities. Journal of Research on tive affect, and depressive symptoms. Journal Psychology, 36, 835–850.
Adolescence, 10, 29–48. of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- Fowler, P., Tompsett, C., Braciszewski, J., Jacques-
Fletcher, A., Steinberg, L., & Williams-Wheeler, cent Psychiatry, 49, 162–172. Tiura, A., & Baltes, B. (2010). Community
M. (2004). Parental influences on adolescent Ford, D., & Harris, J. I. (1996). Perceptions and violence: a meta-analysis on the effect of
problem-behavior: Revisiting Stattin and attitudes of Black students toward school, exposure and mental health outcomes of
Kerr. Child Development, 75, 781–796. achievement, and other educational vari- children and adolescents. Development and
Flieller, A. (1999). Comparison of the develop- ables. Child Development, 67, 1141–1152. Psychopathology, 21, 227–259.
ment of formal thought in adolescent co- Ford, J. A. (2009). Nonmedical prescription drug Fowler, P., Toro, P., & Miles, B. (2009). Pathways
horts aged 10 to 15 years (1967–1996 and use among adolescents: The influence of to and from homelessness and associated
1972–1993). Developmental Psychology, 35, bonds to family and school. Youth and Soci- psychosocial outcomes among adolescents
1048–1058. ety, 40, 336–352. leaving the foster care system. American Jour-
Flisher, A. J., Kramer, R. A., Hoven, C. W., King, Fordham, C., & Ogbu, J. (1986). Black students’ nal of Public Health, 99, 1453–1458.
R. A., Bird, H. R., Davies, M., . . . Shaffer, D. school success: Coping with the burden of Frabutt, J. M., Walker, A. M., & MacKinnon-
(2000). Risk behavior in a community sam- “acting White.” Urban Review, 18, 176–206. Lewis, C. (2002). Racial socialization mes-
ple of children and adolescents. Journal of the Forehand, R., Biggar, H., & Kotchick, B. (1998). sages and the quality of mother/child
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Cumulative risk across family stressors: interactions in African American families.
Psychiatry, 39, 881–887. Short- and long-term effects for adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 200–217.
Flook, L. (2011). Gender differences in adolescents’ Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, Frank, K. A., Muller, C., Schiller, K. S., Riegle-
daily interpersonal events and well-being. 119–128. Crumb, C., Mueller, A. S., Crosnoe, R., &
Child Development, 82, 454–461 Forehand, R., Neighbors, B., Devine, D., & Pearson, J. (2008). The social dynamics of
Flook, L., & Fuligni, A. J. (2008). Family and Armistead, L. (1994). Interparental conflict mathematics coursetaking in high school.
school spillover in adolescents’ daily lives. and parental divorce: The individual, relative, American Journal of Sociology, 113,
Child Development, 79, 776–787. and interactive effects on adolescents across 1645–1696.
Flory, K., Lynam, D., & Milich, R. (2004). Early ado- four years. Family Relations, 43, 387–393. Frank, S., & Jackson, S. (1996). Family experi-
lescent through young adult alcohol and mari- Forgatch, M., DeGarmo, D., & Knutson, N. ences as moderators of the relationship
juana use trajectories: Early predictors, young (1994, February). Transitions within tran- between eating symptoms and personality
adult outcomes, and predictive utility. Develop- sitions: The impact of adolescence and fam- disturbance. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
ment and Psychopathology, 16, 193–213. ily structure on boys’ antisocial behavior. cence, 25, 55–72.
Flynn, B., Worden, J., Secker-Walker, R., Pirie, P., Paper presented at the biennial meetings Frank, S., Pirsch, L., & Wright, V. (1990). Late
Badger, G., Carpenter, J., & Geller, B. (1994). of the Society for Research on Adoles- adolescents’ perceptions of their relation-
Mass media and school interventions for cence, San Diego. ships with their parents: Relationships
cigarette smoking prevention: Effects 2 years Forgatch, M., Patterson, G. R., Degarmo, D. S., & among de-idealization, autonomy, related-
after completion. American Journal of Public Beldavs, Z. G. (2009). Testing the Oregon de- ness, and insecurity and implications for ad-
Health, 84, 1148–1150. linquency model with 9-year follow-up of olescent adjustment and ego identity status.
Flynn, M., & Rudolph, K. (2011). Depression the Oregon Divorce Study. Development and Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 571–588.
and depressive disorders. In B. Brown & Psychopathology, 21, 637–660. Frankenberger, K. D. (2000). Adolescent egocen-
M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- Forhan, S., Gottlieb, S., Sternberg, M., Xu, F., Datta, trism: A comparison among adolescents and
cence (Vol. 3, pp. 127–135). New York: Aca- S., McQuillan, G., . . . Markowitz, L. (2009). adults. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 343–354.
demic Press. Prevalence of sexually transmitted infections Franzoi, S., Davis, M., & Vasquez-Suson, K.
Fontaine, R. G., Yang, C., Burks, V. S., Dodge, K. among female adolescents aged 14 to 19 in the (1994). Two social worlds: Social correlates
A., Price, J. M., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. United States. Pediatrics, 124, 1505–1512. and stability of adolescent status groups.
(2009). Loneliness as a partial mediator of Fortner, M., Crouter, A., & McHale, S. (2004). Is par- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
the relation between low social preference in ents’ work involvement responsive to the qual- 67, 462–473.
childhood and anxious/depressed symptoms ity of relationships with adolescent offspring? Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. (2002). Children’s
in adolescence. Development and Psychopa- Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 530–538. competence and value beliefs from childhood
thology, 21, 479. Fosco, G., & Grych, J. (2010). Adolescent triangu- through adolescence: Growth trajectories in
Fontaine, R., Yang, C., Dodge, K., Bates, J., & lation into parental conflicts: Longitudinal two male-sex-typed domains. Developmental
Pettit, G. (2008). Testing an individual systems implications for appraisals and adolescent- Psychology, 38, 519–533.
model of response evaluation and decision parent relations. Journal of Marriage and Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. (2008). Participation
(RED) and antisocial behavior across adoles- Family, 72, 254–266. in extracurricular activities in the middle
cence. Child Development, 79, 462–475. Foshee, V., Benefield, T., Suchindran, C., Ennett, school years: Are there developmental bene-
Forbes, E., & Dahl, R. (2010). Pubertal develop- S. T., Bauman, K. E., Karriker-Jaffe, K., . . . fits for African American and European
ment and behavior: Hormonal activation of Mathias, J. (2009). The development of four American youth? Journal of Youth and Ado-
social and motivational tendencies. Brain types of adolescent dating abuse and selected lescence, 37, 1029–1043.
and Cognition, 72, 66-72. demographic correlates. Journal of Research Fredricks, J., & Eccles, J. S. (2010). Breadth of ex-
Forbes, E. E., Phillips, M. L., Silk, J. S., Ryan, N. on Adolescence, 19, 380–400. tracurricular participation and adolescent
D., & Dahl, R. E. (2011). Neural systems of Foshee, V., Bauman, K., Greene, W., Koch, G., adjustment among African American and
threat processing in adolescents: Role of pu- Linder, G., & MacDougall, J. (2000). The safe European American youth. Journal of Re-
bertal maturation and relation to measures dates program: 1-year follow-up results. Amer- search on Adolescence, 20, 307–333.
of negative affect. Developmental Neuropsy- ican Journal of Public Health, 90, 1619–1622. Fredriksen, K., Rhodes, J., Reddy, R., & Way, N.
chology, 36, 429–452. Fowler, P., Ahmed, S., Tompsett, C., Jozefowicz- (2004). Sleepless in Chicago: Tracking the ef-
Forbes, E., Ryan, N., Phillips, M., Manuck, S., Simbeni, D., & Toro, P. (2008). Community fects of adolescent sleep loss during the middle
Worthman, C. M., Moyles, D. L., . . . Dahl, R. violence and externalizing problems: school years. Child Development, 75, 84–95.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-27 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-27

Fredstrom, B. K., Adams, R. E., & Gilman, R. timing of menarche on female adolescent study of adolescents from Mexican, Chinese,
(2011). Electronic and school-based victim- sleep phase preference. PLoS ONE, 4, e5217. Filipino, and European backgrounds. Devel-
ization: Unique contexts for adjustment dif- Frey, W. (2012, April 25). Analysis of U.S. Cen- opmental Psychology, 34, 782–792.
ficulties during adolescence. Journal of Youth sus data. The Washington Post. Accessed on Fuligni, A., & Eccles, J. (1993). Perceived parent–
and Adolescence, 40, 405–415. June 8, 2012 at http://www.washington- child relationships and early adolescents’
Freedner, N., Freed, L. H., Yang, Y., & Austin, S. post.com/local/an-increasing-number-of- orientation toward peers. Developmental
(2002). Dating violence among gay, lesbian, interracial-babies-nationwide/2012/04/25/ Psychology, 29, 622–632.
and bisexual adolescents: Results from a gIQAzKFohT_graphic.html Fuligni, A., Eccles, J., &, Barber, B. (1995). The
community survey. Journal of Adolescent Frick, P., Kotov, R., Loney, B., & Vasey, M. (2005). long-term effects of seventh grade ability
Health, 31, 469–474. The latent structure of psychopathy in youth: grouping in mathematics. Journal of Early
Freeman, H., & Brown, B. (2001). Primary A taxometric investigation. Journal of Abnor- Adolescence, 15, 58–89.
attachment to parents and peers during mal Child Psychology, 33. Fuligni, A., Eccles, J., Barber, B., & Clements, P.
adolescence: Differences by attachment style. Frick, P., & White, S. F. (2008). The importance of (2001). Early adolescent peer orientation
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 653–674. callous-unemotional traits for the develop- and adjustment during high school. Develop-
French, D., & Conrad, J. (2001). School dropout ment of aggressive and antisocial behavior. mental Psychology, 37, 28–36.
as predicted by peer rejection and antisocial Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, Fuligni, A., & Hardway, C. (2006). Daily varia-
behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 359–375. tion in adolescents’ sleep, activities, and psy-
11, 225–244. Fried, M., & Fried, M. (1980). Transitions: Four chological well-being. Journal of Research on
French, D., Conrad, J., & Turner, T. (1995). rituals in eight cultures. New York: Norton. Adolescence, 16, 353–378.
Adjustment of antisocial and nonantisocial Friedenberg, E. (1959). The vanishing adolescent. Fuligni, A., Hughes, D., & Way, N. (2009). Ethnicity
rejected adolescents. Development and Psy- Boston: Beacon Press. and immigration. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg
chopathology, 7, 857–874. Friedenberg, E. (1967). Coming of age in America. (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd
French, D., & Dishion, T. (2003). Predictors of New York: Vintage Books. ed., Vol. 2, pp. 527–569). New York: Wiley.
early initiation of sexual intercourse among Friedlander, L. J., Connolly, J. A., Pepler, D. J., & Fuligni, A., Kiang, L., Witkow, M., & Baldelomar, O.
high-risk adolescents. Journal of Early Ado- Craig, W. M. (2007). Biological, familial, (2008). Stability and change in ethnic labeling
lescence, 23, 295–315. and peer influences on dating in early ado- among adolescents from Asian and Latin
French, D. C., Eisenberg, N., Vaughan, J., Purwono, lescence. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, American immigrant families. Child Develop-
U., & Suryanti, T. A. (2008). Religious involve- 821–830. ment, 79, 944–956.
ment and the social competence and adjust- Frijns, T., Finkenauer, C., Vermulst, A. A., & Fuligni, A., & Pedersen, S. (2002). Family obliga-
ment of Indonesian Muslim adolescents. Engels, R. (2005). Keeping secrets from par- tion and the transition to young adulthood.
Developmental Psychology, 44, 597–611. ents: Longitudinal associations of secrecy in Developmental Psychology, 38, 856–868.
French, D. C., Jansen, E., & Pidada, S. (2002). United adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Fuligni, A., & Stevenson, H. (1995). Time-use
States and Indonesian children’s and adoles- cence, 34, 137–148. and mathematics achievement among Amer-
cents’ reports of relational aggression by dis- Frisch, R. (1983). Fatness, puberty, and fertility: ican, Chinese, and Japanese high school stu-
liked peers. Child Development, 73, 1143–1150. The effects of nutrition and physical training dents. Child Development, 66, 830–842.
French, D. C., Purwono, U., & Triwahyuni, A. on menarche and ovulation. In J. Brooks- Fuligni, A., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudes
(2011). Friendship and the religiosity of In- Gunn & A. Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty. toward family obligations among American
donesian Muslim adolescents. Journal of New York: Plenum. adolescents from Asian, Latin American, and
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1623–1633. Frisco, M. L. (2005). Parental involvement and European backgrounds. Child Development,
French, D. C., Rianasari, M., Pidada, S., Nelwan, young women’s contraceptive use. Journal of 70, 1030–1044.
P., & Buhrmester, D. (2001). Social support Marriage and the Family, 67, 110. Fuligni, A., & Witkow, M. (2004). The postsec-
of Indonesian and U.S. children and adoles- Frisco, M. L. (2008). Adolescents’ sexual behav- ondary educational progress of youth from
cents by family members and friends. ior and academic attainment. Sociology of immigrant families. Journal of Research on
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 377–394. Education, 81, 284–311. Adolescence, 14, 159–183.
French, S. E., Seidman, E., Allen, L., & Aber, J. L. Frisco, M. L., Muller, C., & Frank, K. (2007). Fuligni, A., Witkow, M., & Garcia, C. (2005). Ethnic
(2006). The development of ethnic identity Parents’ union dissolution and adolescents’ identity and the academic adjustment of ado-
during adolescence. Developmental Psychol- school performance: Comparing method- lescents from Mexican, Chinese, and European
ogy, 42, 1–10. ological approaches. Journal of Marriage and backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 41,
French, S., Story, M., Downes, B., Resnick, M., & Family, 69, 721–741. 799–811.
Blum, R. (1995). Frequent dieting among Fromme, K., Corbin, W. R., & Kruse, M. I. Fuligni, A., Yip, T., & Tseng, V. (2002). The im-
adolescents: Psychosocial and health behav- (2008). Behavioral risks during the transi- pact of family obligation on the daily activi-
ior correlates. American Journal of Public tion from high school to college. Develop- ties and psychological well-being of Chinese
Health, 85, 695–701. mental Psychology, 44, 1497–1504. American adolescents. Child Development,
Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., & Watt, H. Fuhrman, T., & Holmbeck, G. (1995). A contex- 73, 302–314.
M. G. (2010). Development of mathematics tual-moderator analysis of emotional au- Fulkerson, J. A., & French, S. A. (2003). Cigarette
interest in adolescence: Influences of gender, tonomy and adjustment in adolescence. smoking for weight loss or control among
family, and school context. Journal of Re- Child Development, 66, 793–811. adolescents: Gender and racial/ethnic differ-
search on Adolescence, 20, 507–537. ences. Journal of Adolescent Health, 32,
Fuligni, A. (1994, February). Academic achievement
Freud, A. (1958). Adolescence. Psychoanalytic and motivation among Asian-American and 306–313.
Study of the Child, 13, 255–278. European-American early adolescents. Paper Furbey, M., & Beyth-Marom, R. (1992). Risk-
Freud, S. (1938). An outline of psychoanalysis. presented at the biennial meetings of the Soci- taking in adolescence: A decision-making
London: Hogarth Press. ety for Research on Adolescence, San Diego. perspective. Developmental Review, 12, 1–44.
Frey, S., Balu, S., Greusing, S., Rothen, N., & Fuligni, A. (1998). Authority, autonomy, and Furman, W., Brown, B., & Feiring, C. (Eds.).
Cajochen, C. (2009). Consequences of the parent–adolescent conflict and cohesion: A (1999). Contemporary perspectives on adolescent
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-28 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-28 References

romantic relationships. New York: Cambridge Furstenberg, F., Jr., Morgan, S. P., Moore, K. A., and urbanicity. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
University Press. & Peterson, J. L. (1987a). Race differences in cence, 33, 235–245.
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s the timing of adolescent intercourse. Ameri- Gallup, G., & Bezilla, R. (1992). The religious life
perceptions of the personal relationships can Sociological Review, 52, 511–518. of young Americans. Princeton, NJ: Gallup
in their social networks. Developmental Psy- Furstenberg, F., Jr., Morgan, S. P., & Allison, P. Institute.
chology, 21, 1016–1024. (1987b). Paternal participation and chil- Galvan, A. (2010). Adolescent development of
Furman, W., & Shomaker, L. B. (2008). Patterns dren’s well-being after marital dissolution. the reward system. Frontiers in Neuroscience,
of interaction in adolescent romantic rela- American Sociological Review, 52, 695–701. 4, 1–9.
tionships: Distinct features and links to other Fussell, M., & Greene, M. (2002). Demographic Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss,
close relationships. Journal of Adolescence, trends affecting youth around the world. In K., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J. (2006). Earlier
31, 771–788. B. Brown, R. Larson, & T. Saraswathi (Eds.), development of the accumbens relative to
Furman, W., & Simon, V. (1999). Cognitive The world’s youth: Adolescence in eight regions orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-
representations of adolescent romantic of the globe. New York: Cambridge University taking behavior in adolescents. Journal of
relationships. In W. Furman, B. Brown, & Press. Neuroscience, 26, 6885–6892.
C. Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives Gage, J. C., Overpeck, M. D., Nansel, T. R., & Kogan, Galvan, A., Hare, T., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey,
on adolescent romantic relationships (pp. M. D. (2005). Peer activity in the evenings and B. J. (2007). Risk-taking and the adolescent
75–98). New York: Cambridge University participation in aggressive and problem behav- brain: Who is at risk? Developmental Science,
Press. iors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 37, 517.e7– 10, f8–f14.
Furman, W., & Simon, V. (2006). Actor and part- 517.e14.
Gamoran, A. (1993). Alternative uses of ability
ner effects of adolescents’ romantic working Galambos, N., Barker, E., & Almeida, D. (2003). grouping in secondary schools: Can we
models and styles on interactions with roman- Parents do matter: Trajectories of change bring high-quality instruction to low-ability
tic partners. Child Development, 77, 588–604. in externalizing and internalizing problems classes? American Journal of Education, 102,
Furman, W., Simon, V., Shaffer, L., & Bouchey, H. in adolescence. Child Development, 74, 1–22.
A. (2002). Adolescents’ working models and 578–594.
Gamoran, A. (1996). Curriculum standardiza-
styles for relationships with parents, friends, Galambos, N., Barker, E., & Krahn, H. (2006). De- tion and equality of opportunity in Scottish
and romantic partners. Child Development, pression, self-esteem, and anger in emerging secondary education: 1984–90. Sociology of
73, 241–255. adulthood: Seven-year trajectories. Develop- Education, 69, 1–21.
Furman, W., & Wehner, E. (1994). Romantic mental Psychology, 42, 350–365.
Gano-Overway, L., Newton, M., Magyar, T. M.,
views: Toward a theory of adolescent roman- Galambos, N., Barker, E., & Tilton-Weaver, L.
Fry, M. D., Kim, M., & Guivernau, M. R.
tic relationships. In R. Montemayor (Ed.), (2003). Who gets caught at maturity gap? A
(2009). Influence of caring youth sport con-
Advances in adolescent development: Vol. 3. study of pseudomature, immature, and ma-
texts on efficacy-related beliefs and social
Relationships in adolescence (pp. 168–195). ture adolescents. International Journal of Be-
behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 45,
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. havioral Development, 27, 253–263.
329–340.
Furr-Holden, C., Milam, A., Reynolds, E., Galambos, N., Berenbaum, S., & McHale, S.
Gans, J. (1990). America’s adolescents: How
MacPherson, L., & Lejuez, C. (2012). Disor- (2009). Gender development in adolescence.
healthy are they? Chicago: American Medical
dered neighborhood environments and risk- In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook
Association.
taking propensity in late childhood through of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp.
adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 305–357). New York: Wiley. Garber, J., & Cole, D. A. (2010). Intergenera-
100–102. tional transmission of depression: A launch
Galambos, N., Dalton, A., & Maggs, J. (2009).
and grow model of change across adoles-
Furstenberg, F., Jr. (1990). Coming of age in a Losing sleep over it: Daily variation in sleep
cence. Development and Psychopathology, 22,
changing family system. In S. Feldman & G. quantity and quality in Canadian students’
819–830.
Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing first semester of university. Journal of Re-
adolescent (pp. 147–170). Cambridge, MA: search on Adolescence, 19, 741–761. Garber, J., Robinson, N., & Valentiner, D. (1997).
Harvard University Press. The relation between parenting and adoles-
Galambos, N., Kolaric, G., Sears, H., & Maggs, J.
cent depression: Self-worth as a mediator.
Furstenberg, F., Jr. (1996). Family management of (1999). Adolescents’ subjective age: An indi-
Journal of Adolescent Research, 12, 12–33.
adolescent success in inner-city Philadelphia. cator of perceived maturity. Journal of Re-
Paper presented at the biennial meetings of search on Adolescence, 9, 309–337. Garcia Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo,
the Society for Research on Adolescence, Galambos, N., & Maggs, J. (1991). Out-of-school H., Crnic, K., Wasik, B., . . . Vasquez Garcia,
Boston. care of young adolescents and self-reported H. (1996). An integrative model for the study
Furstenberg, F., Jr. (2000). The sociology of ado- behavior. Developmental Psychology, 27, of developmental competencies in minority
lescence and youth in the 1990s: A critical 644–655. children. Child Development, 67, 1891–1914.
commentary. Journal of Marriage and Family, Galambos, N., Turner, P., & Tilton-Weaver, L. Garcia-Reid, P. (2007). Examining social capital
62, 896–910. (2005). Chronological and subjective age in as a mechanism for improving school en-
Furstenberg, F., Jr. (2006, March). Diverging devel- emerging adulthood: The crossover effect. gagement among low income Hispanic girls.
opment: The not-so-invisible hand of social class Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 538–556. Youth and Society, 39, 164–181.
in the United States. Invited address, Society Galliher, R. V., Jones, M. D., & Dahl, A. (2011). Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York:
for Research on Adolescence, San Francisco. Concurrent and longitudinal effects of eth- Basic Books.
Furstenberg, F., Jr., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Chase- nic identity and experiences of discrimina- Gardner, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). Adoles-
Lansdale, P. L. (1989). Teenaged pregnancy tion on psychosocial adjustment of Navajo cents’ exposure to community violence: Are
and childbearing. American Psychologist, 44, adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 47, neighborhood youth organizations protec-
313–320. 509–526. tive? Journal of Community Psychology, 37,
Furstenberg, F., Jr., Cook, T., Eccles, J., Elder, G., Galliher, R. V., Rostosky, S. S., & Hughes, H. K. 505–525.
Jr., & Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to make (2004). School belonging, self-esteem, and Gardner, M., Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2008).
it: Urban families and adolescent success. Chi- depressive symptoms in adolescents: An ex- Adolescents’ participation in organized
cago: University of Chicago Press. amination of sex, sexual attraction status, activities and developmental success 2 and
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-29 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-29

8 years after high school: Do sponsorship, during early adolescence. Child Develop- outcomes: A synthesis of evidence from ex-
duration, and intensity matter? Developmen- ment, 67, 2201–2216. perimental studies. Journal of Research on Ad-
tal Psychology, 44, 814–830. Gavin, L., & Furman, W. (1996). Adolescent girls’ olescence, 14, 399–423.
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influ- relationships with mothers and best friends. Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological video game
ence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky Child Development, 67, 375–386. use among youth 8 to 18: A national study.
decision making in adolescence and adult- Gayle, H., Keeling, R., Garcia-Tunon, M., Psychological Science, 20, 594–602.
hood: An experimental study. Developmental Kilbourne, B., Narkunas, J., Ingram, F., & Gentile, D., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D.,
Psychology, 41, 625–635. Curran, J. (1990). Prevalence of the human Fung, D., & Khoo, A. (2011). Pathological
Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Connell, A. M. immunodeficiency virus among university video game use among youths: a two-year
(2008). Adolescent self-regulation as resil- students. New England Journal of Medicine, longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127, e319–e329.
ience: Resistance to antisocial behavior 323, 1538–1541. Gentile, D., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D.
within the deviant peer context. Journal of Gaylord-Harden, N., Ragsdale, B. L., Mandara, J., A. (2004). The effects of violent video game
Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 273–284. Richards, M. H., & Petersen, A. C. (2007). habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive be-
Garg, R., Melanson, S., & Levin, E. (2007). Edu- Perceived support and internalizing symp- haviors, and school performance. Journal of
cational aspirations of male and female toms in African American adolescents: Self- Adolescence, 27, 5–22.
adolescents from single-parent and two bio- esteem and ethnic identity as mediators. Gentry, M., Gable, R. K., & Rizza, M. G. (2002).
logical parent families: A comparison of in- Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 77–88. Students’ perceptions of classroom activities:
fluential factors. Journal of Youth and Ge, X., Best, K., Conger, R., & Simons, R. (1996). Are there grade-level and gender differences?
Adolescence, 36, 1010–1023. Parenting behaviors and the occurrence Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 539–544.
Gargiulo, J., Attie, I., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, and co-occurrence of adolescent depressive Georgiades, K., Boyle, M. H., Duku, E., &
M. (1987). Girls’ dating behavior as a func- symptoms and conduct problems. Develop- Racine, Y. (2006). Tobacco use among immi-
tion of social context and maturation. Devel- mental Psychology, 32, 717–731. grant and nonimmigrant adolescents: Indi-
opmental Psychology, 23, 730–737. Ge, X., Brody, G., Conger, R., Simons, R., & vidual and family level influences. Journal of
Garnefski, N. (2000). Age differences in depres- Murry, V. (2002). Contextual amplification Adolescent Health, 38, 443.e1–443.e7.
sive symptoms, antisocial behavior, and neg- of pubertal transition effects on deviant peer Gerard, J. M., & Buehler, C. (2004). Cumulative
ative perceptions of family, school, and peers affiliation and externalizing behavior among environmental risk and youth maladjust-
among adolescents. Journal of the American African American children. Developmental ment: The role of youth attributes. Child De-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychology, 38, 42–54. velopment, 75, 1832–1849.
39, 1175–1181. Ge, X., Conger, R., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2001). The Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorelli, N.
Garner, R., Bootcheck, J., Lorr, M., & Rauch, K. relation between puberty and psychological (1994). Growing up with television: The culti-
(2006). The adolescent society revisited: Cul- distress in adolescent boys. Journal of Re- vation perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman
tures, crowds, climates, and status structures search on Adolescence, 11, 49–70. (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and
in seven secondary schools. Journal of Youth Ge, X., Kim, I. J., Brody, G., Conger, R., Simons, research (pp. 17–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and Adolescence, 35, 1023–1035. R., Gibbons, F., & Cutrona, C. (2003). It’s Germán, M., Gonzales, N., & Dumka, L. (2009).
Garnier, H., & Stein, J. (2002). An 18-year model about timing and change: Pubertal transi- Familism values as a protective factor for
of family and peer effects on adolescent drug tion effects on symptoms of major depres- Mexican-origin adolescents exposed to devi-
use and delinquency. Journal of Youth and sion among African American youths. ant peers. Journal of Early Adolescence, 29,
Adolescence, 31, 45–56. Developmental Psychology, 39, 430–439. 16–42.
Gartrell, N., Bos, H., Peyser, H., Deck, A., & Ge, X., Natsuaki, M., & Conger, R. (2006). Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F., & Bushman, B. (1996).
Rodas, C. (2011). Family characteristics, cus- Trajectories of depressive symptoms and Relation between perceived vulnerability to
tody arrangements, and adolescent stressful life events among male and female HIV and precautionary sexual behavior. Psy-
psychological well-being after lesbian moth- adolescents in divorced and nondivorced chological Bulletin, 119, 390–409.
ers break up. Family Relations, 60, 572–585. families. Development and Psychopathology, Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., Ware, A., & Kotler, J. A.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & 18, 253–273. (2010). Exposure to 9/11 among youth and
Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of work- Ge, X., Natsuaki, M., Neiderhiser, J., & Reiss, D. their mothers in New York City: Enduring
ing memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Devel- (2007). Genetic and environmental influ- associations with mental health and socio-
opmental Psychology, 40, 177–190. ences on pubertal timing: Results from two political attitudes. Child Development, 81,
Gau, S. S., Soong, W., & Merikangas, K. R. (2004). national sibling studies. Journal of Research 1142–1160.
Correlates of sleep-wake patterns among on Adolescence, 17, 767–788. Gest, S. (1997). Behavioral inhibition: Stability
children and young adolescents in Taiwan. Ge, X., Natsuaki, M. N., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Reiss, and associations with adaptation from child-
Sleep, 27, 512–519. D. (2009). The longitudinal effects of stressful hood to early adulthood. Journal of Personal-
Gaudreau, P., Amiot, C. E., & Vallerand, R. J. life events on adolescent depression are buff- ity and Social Psychology, 72, 467–475.
(2009). Trajectories of affective states in ered by parent–child closeness. Development Gestsdóttir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2007). Intentional
adolescent hockey players: Turning point and Psychopathology, 21, 621–635. self-regulation and positive youth develop-
and motivational antecedents. Developmen- Gecas, V., & Seff, M. (1990). Families and adoles- ment in early adolescence: Findings from the
tal Psychology, 45, 307–319. cents: A review of the 1980s. Journal of Mar- 4-H study of positive youth development.
Gault-Sherman, M. (2012). It’s a two-way street: riage and the Family, 52, 941–958. Developmental Psychology, 43, 508–521.
The bidirectional relationship between par- Geier, C., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., & Luna, B. Gfroerer, J. C., & Tan, L. L. (2003). Substance
enting and delinquency. Journal of Youth and (2010). Immaturities in reward processing use among foreign-born youths in the
Adolescence, 41, 121–145. and its influence on inhibitory control in United States: Does the length of residence
Gauze, C., Bukowski, W., Aquan-Assee, J., & adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1613–1629. matter? American Journal of Public Health,
Sippola, L. (1996). Interactions between Gennetian, L. A., Duncan, G., Knox, V., Vargas, W., 93, 1892–1895.
family environment and friendship and Clark-Kauffman, E., & London, A. S. (2004). Ghetti, S., DeMaster, D., Yonelinas, A., & Bunge,
associations with self-perceived well-being How welfare policies affect adolescents’ school S. (2010). Developmental differences in
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-30 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-30 References

medial temporal lobe function during Longitudinal changes in grey and white matter Gonzales, R. (2011). Learning to be illegal un-
memory encoding. Journal of Neuroscience, during adolescence. Neuroimage, 49, 94–103. documented youth and shifting legal con-
30(28), 9548–9556. Glaberson, W. (2007, June 7). A legal debate in texts in the transition to adulthood. American
Gibbons, F., Benbow, C., & Gerrard, M. (1994). Guantánamo on boy fighters. The New York Sociological Review, 76, 602–619.
From top dog to bottom half: Social com- Times, p. A1. Good, M., & Willoughby, T. (2006). The role of
parison strategies in response to poor per- Glaser, B, Shelton, K. H., & van den Bree, M. B. spirituality versus religiosity in adolescent
formance. Journal of Personality and Social M. (2010). The moderating role of close psychosocial adjustment. Journal of Youth
Psychology, 67, 638–652. friends in the relationship between conduct and Adolescence, 35, 41–55.
Gibbs, J., Basinger, K. S., Grime, R., & Snarney, J. problems and adolescent substance use. Good, M., & Willoughby, T. (2007). The identity
(2007). Moral judgment development across Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, 35–42. formation experiences of church-attending
cultures: Revisiting Kohlberg’s universality Glasgow, K., Dornbusch, S., Ritter, P., Troyer, L., rural adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Re-
claims. Developmental Review, 27, 443–500. & Steinberg, L. (1997). Parenting styles, dys- search, 22, 387–412.
Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from functional attributions, and adolescent out- Good, M., & Willoughby, T. (2011). Evaluating
neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, comes in diverse groups. Child Development, the direction of effects in the relationship
42, 335–343. 67, 507–529. between religious versus non-religious activi-
Giletta, M., Scholte, R. H. J., Burk, W. J., Engels, Glenwright, M., & Pexman, P. (2010). Development ties, academic success, and substance use.
R. C. M. E., Larsen, J. K., Prinstein, M. J., & of children’s ability to distinguish sarcasm and Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 680–693.
Ciairano, S. (2011). Similarity in depressive verbal irony. Journal of Child Language, 37, Good, M., Willoughby, T., & Busseri, M. A. (2011).
symptoms in adolescents’ friendship dyads: 429–451. Stability and change in adolescent spiritual-
Selection or socialization? Developmental Glick, G. C., & Rose, A. J. (2011). Prospective as- ity/religiosity: A person-centered approach.
Psychology, 47, 1804–1814. sociations between friendship adjustment Developmental Psychology, 47, 538–550.
Gilliam, F., & Bales, S. (2001). Strategic frame and social strategies: Friendship as a context Good, M., Willoughby, T., & Fritjers, J. (2009). Just
analysis: Reframing America’s youth. SRCD for building social skills. Developmental Psy- another club? The distinctiveness of the rela-
Social Policy Report, 15. chology, 47, 1117–1132. tion between religious service attendance and
Gillmore, M., Archibald, M. E., Morrison, D. Goldfield, G., Kenny, G., Hadjiyannakis, S., Phillips, adolescent psychosocial adjustment. Journal
M., Wilsdon, A., Wells, E. A., Hoppe, M. J., . . . P., Alberga, A., Saunders, T., . . . Sigal, R. (2011). of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 1153–1171.
Murowchick, E. (2002). Teen sexual behav- Video game playing is independently associ- Goodnight, J. A., Bates, J. E., Newman, J. P.,
ior: Applicability of the theory of reasoned ated with blood pressure and lipids in over- Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2006). The in-
action. Journal of Marriage and the Family, weight and obese adolescents. PLoS ONE, 6. teractive influences of friend deviance and
64, 885–897. reward dominance on the development of
Goldsmith, P. A. (2004). Schools’ racial mix, stu-
Gillmore, M., Lewis, S., Lohr, M., Spencer, M., & dents’ optimism, and the Black–White and externalizing behavior during middle ado-
White, R. (1997). Repeat pregnancies among Latino–White achievement gaps. Sociology of lescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
adolescent mothers. Journal of Marriage and Education, 77, 121–147. ogy, 34, 573–583.
the Family, 59, 536–550. Goodson, P., Buhi, E. R., & Dunsmore, S. C.
Goldstein, B. (1976). Introduction to human sex-
Gillmore, M., Morrison, D., Lowery, C., & Baker, (2006). Self-esteem and adolescent sexual
uality. Belmont, CA: Star.
S. (1994). Beliefs about condoms and their behaviors, attitudes, and intentions: A sys-
association with intentions to use condoms Goldstein, S., Davis-Kean, P., & Eccles, J. (2005). tematic review. Journal of Adolescent Health,
among youths in detention. Journal of Ado- Parents, peers, and problem behavior: A longi- 38, 310–319.
lescent Health, 15, 228–237. tudinal investigation of the impact of relation-
Goosby, B. J. (2007). Poverty duration, maternal
ship perceptions and characteristics on the
Gillock, K., & Reyes, O. (1996). High school psychological resources, and adolescent so-
development of adolescent problem behavior.
transition-related changes in urban minority cioemotional outcomes. Journal of Family
Developmental Psychology, 41, 401–413.
students’ academic performance and percep- Issues, 28, 1113–1134.
tions of self and school environment. Journal Goldston, D., Molock, S., Whitbeck, L., Murakami, Goossens, L., Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Marcoen, A.
of Community Psychology, 24, 245–261. J., Zayas, L., & Hall, G. (2008). Cultural con- (1992, March). The many faces of adolescent
Gillock, K., & Reyes, O. (1999). Stress, support, and siderations in adolescent suicide prevention egocentrism: Two European replications.
academic performance of urban, low-income, and psychosocial treatment. American Psy- Paper presented at the biennial meetings of
Mexican-American adolescents. Journal of chologist, 63, 14–31. the Society for Research on Adolescence,
Youth and Adolescence, 28, 259–282. Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New Washington, DC.
Gilman, S. E., Rende, R., Boergers, J., Abrams, D. B., York: Bantam Books. Goran, M., et al. (1998). Developmental changes
Buka, S. L., Clark, M. A., . . . Niaura, R. (2009). Golub, A., & Johnson, B. D. (2001). Variation in in energy expenditure and physical activity
Parental smoking and adolescent smoking ini- youthful risks of progression from alcohol in children: Evidence for a decline in physical
tiation: An intergeneration perspective on to- and tobacco to marijuana and to hard drugs activity in girls before puberty. Pediatrics,
bacco control. Pediatrics, 123, 274–281. across generations. American Journal of Pub- 101, 887–891.
Giordano, P. C., Longmore, M. A., & Manning, lic Health, 91, 225–232. Gordis, E., Margolin, G., & St. John, R. (1997).
W. D. (2006). Gender and the meanings of Gonzales, N., Cauce, A., Friedman, R., & Mason, Marital aggression, observed parental hostility,
adolescent romantic relationships: A focus C. (1996). Family, peer, and neighborhood and child behavior during triadic family inter-
on boys. American Sociological Review, 71, influences on academic achievement among action. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 76–89.
260–287. African-American adolescents: One-year Gordon, K. (1995). Self-concept and motiva-
Giordano, P. C., Manning, W. D., & Longmore, prospective effects. American Journal of tional patterns of resilient African American
M. A. (2010). Affairs of the heart: Qualities Community Psychology, 24, 365–387. high school students. Journal of Black Psy-
of adolescent romantic relationships and Gonzales, N. A., Deardorff, J., Formoso, D., Barr, chology, 21, 239–255.
sexual behavior. Journal of Research on Ado- A., & Barrera, M. Jr. (2006). Family media- Gordon, R. A., Lahey, B. B., Kawai, E., Loeber, R.,
lescence, 20, 983–1013. tors of the relation between acculturation Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Farrington, D. P.
Giorgio, A., Watkins, K., Chadwick, M., James, S., and adolescent mental health. Family Rela- (2004). Antisocial behavior and youth gang
Winmill, L., Douaud, G., . . . James, A. (2010). tions, 55, 318–330. membership. Criminology, 42, 55–87.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-31 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-31

Gore, S., & Aseltine, R., Jr. (1995). Protective pro- of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- students. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
cesses in adolescence: Matching stressors cent Psychiatry, 43, 718–726. 7, 109–131.
with social resources. American Journal of Graber, J., & Sontag, L. (2009). Internalizing Gray-Little, B., & Hafdahl, A. (2000). Factors in-
Community Psychology, 23, 301–327. problems during adolescence. In R. Lerner & fluencing racial comparisons of self-esteem:
Gottfredson, D., & DiPietro, S. (2011). School L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin,
size, social capital, and student victimization. psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 642–682). 126, 26–54.
Sociology of Education, 84, 69–89. New York: Wiley. Green, K. M., & Ensminger, M. E. (2006). Adult
Gottfredson, M., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general Graham, C. (1991). Menstrual synchrony: An up- social behavioral effects of heavy adolescent
theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford date and review. Human Nature, 2, 293–311. marijuana use among African Americans.
University Press. Graham, S. (1993, March). Peer-directed aggres- Developmental Psychology, 42, 1168–1178.
Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. sion in African-American youth from an at- Greenberg, M., & Kusche, C. (1998). Promot-
W., & Oliver, P. H. (2009). A latent curve tributional perspective. Paper presented at the ing alternative thinking strategies. Boulder:
model of parental motivational practices and biennial meetings of the Society for Research Institute of Behavioral Sciences, University
developmental decline in math and science in Child Development, New Orleans. of Colorado.
academic intrinsic motivation. Journal of Graham, S., Bellmore, A., Nishina, A., & Juvonen, Greenberg, M., Siegel, J., & Leitch, C. (1983).
Educational Psychology, 101, 729–739. J. (2009). “It must be ‘me’”: Ethnic diversity The nature and importance of attachment
Gottfried, A., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. and attributions for peer victimization in relationships to parents and peers during
(2001). Continuity of academic intrinsic middle school. Journal of Youth and Adoles- adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
motivation from childhood through late ad- cence, 38, 487–499. cence, 12, 373–386.
olescence: A longitudinal study. Journal of Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (1994). Attributions Greenberger, E., & Chen, C. (1996). Perceived fam-
Educational Psychology, 93, 3–13. of aggressive and nonaggressive African- ily relationships and depressed mood in early
Gottlieb, B. H., Still, E., & Newby-Clark, I. American male early adolescents: A study of and late adolescence: A comparison of Euro-
(2007). Types and precipitants of growth construct accessibility. Developmental Psy- pean and Asian Americans. Developmental
and decline in emerging adulthood. Journal chology, 30, 365–373. Psychology, 32, 707–716.
of Adolescent Research, 22, 132–155. Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (1998). Self-blame and Greenberger, E., Chen, C., & Beam, M. R. (1998).
Gould, M., Munfakh, J., Lubell, K., Kleinman, peer victimization in middle school: An The role of “very important” nonparental
M., & Parker, S. (2002). Seeking help from attributional analysis. Developmental Psy- adults in adolescent development. Journal of
the Internet during adolescence. Journal of chology, 34, 587–599. Youth and Adolescence, 27, 321–343.
the American Academy of Child and Adoles- Graham, S., & Lowery, B. (2004). Priming un- Greenberger, E., Chen, C., Beam, M., Whang, S., &
cent Psychiatry, 41, 1182–1189. conscious racial stereotypes about adoles- Dong, Q. (2000a). The perceived social con-
Gowen, L. K., Feldman, S. S., Diaz, R., & Yisrael, cent offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 28, texts of adolescents’ misconduct: A compara-
D. S. (2004). A comparison of the sexual be- 483–504. tive study of youths in three cultures. Journal
haviors and attitudes of adolescent girls with Granberg, E., Simons, L., & Simons, R. (2009). of Research on Adolescence, 10, 365–388.
older vs. similar-aged boyfriends. Journal of Body Size and Social Self-Image Among Ad- Greenberger, E., Chen, C., Tally, S., & Dong, Q.
Youth and Adolescence, 33, 167–175. olescent African American Girls The Moder- (2000b). Family, peer, and individual corre-
Gowen, L., Hayward, C., Killen, J., Robinson, T., ating Influence of Family Racial Socialization. lates of depressive symptomatology among
& Taylor, C. (1999). Acculturation and eating Youth & Society, 41, 256–277. U.S. and Chinese adolescents. Journal of Con-
disorder symptoms in adolescent girls. Jour- Granic, I., Hollenstein, T., Dishion, T. K., & sulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 209–219.
nal of Research on Adolescence, 9, 67–83. Patterson, G. R. (2003). Longitudinal analy- Greenberger, E., & Steinberg, L. (1986). When
sis of flexibility and reorganization in early teenagers work: The psychological and social
Goyette, K., & Xie, Y. (1999). Educational expec-
adolescence: A dynamic systems study of costs of adolescent employment. New York:
tations of Asian American youths: Determi-
family interactions. Developmental Psychol- Basic Books.
nants and ethnic differences. Sociology of
Education, 72, 22–36. ogy, 39, 606–617. Greene, M. L., & Way, N. (2005). Self-esteem tra-
Granillo, M., Grogan-Kaylor, A., Delva, J., & Castillo, jectories among ethnic minority adolescents:
Goza, F., & Ryabov, I. (2009). Adolescents’ educa-
M. (2011). Eating disorders among a commu- A growth curve analysis of the patterns and
tional outcomes: Racial and ethnic variations
nity-based sample of Chilean female adoles- predictors of change. Journal of Research on
in peer network importance. Journal of Youth
cents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, Adolescence, 15, 151–177.
and Adolescence, 38, 1264–1279.
762–768. Greene, M. L., Way, N., & Pahl, K. (2006). Trajec-
Graber, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Paikoff, R., & Warren,
Grant, K., Lyons, A., Finkelstein, J., Conway, K., tories of perceived adult and peer discrimi-
M. (1994). Prediction of eating problems: An
Reynolds, L., O’Koon, J., . . . Hicks, K. (2004). nation among Black, Latino, and Asian
8-year study of adolescent girls. Developmen-
Gender differences in rates of depressive American adolescents: Patterns and psycho-
tal Psychology, 30, 823–834.
symptoms among low-income, urban, Afri- logical correlates. Developmental Psychology,
Graber, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. can American youth: A test of two media- 42, 218–236.
(2006). Pubertal effects on adjustment in tional hypotheses. Journal of Youth and Gregory, A., Cornell, D., & Fan, X. (2011). The
girls: Moving from demonstrating effects to Adolescence, 33, 523–533. relationship of school structure and support
identifying pathways. Journal of Youth and to suspension rates for Black and White high
Gray, M., & Steinberg, L. (1999). Adolescent ro-
Adolescence, 35, 391–401. school students. American Educational Re-
mance and the parent–child relationship: A
Graber, J., Lewinsohn, P., Seeley, J., & Brooks- contextual perspective. In W. Furman, B. search Journal, 48, 904–934.
Gunn, J. (1997). Is psychopathology associ- Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary Gregory, A., Cornell, D., Fan, X., Sheras, P., Shih,
ated with the timing of pubertal development? perspectives on adolescent romantic relation- T., & Huang, F. (2010). Authoritative school
Journal of the American Academy of Child and ships (pp. 235–265). New York: Cambridge discipline: High school practices associated
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1768–1776. University Press. with lower bullying and victimization. Jour-
Graber, J., Seeley, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewinsohn, Gray-Little, B., & Carels, R. (1997). The effect of nal of Educational Psychology, 102, 483–496.
P. (2004). Is pubertal timing associated with racial dissonance on academic achievement Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2004). Connec-
psychopathology in young adulthood? Journal in elementary, junior high, and high school tion and regulation at home and in school:
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-32 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-32 References

Predicting growth in achievement for ado- Gruber, J., & Zinman, J. (2001). Youth smoking Gummerum, M., Keller, M., Takezawa, M., &
lescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, in the United States. In J. Gruber (Ed.), Risky Mata, J. (2008). To give or not to give: Chil-
405–427. behavior among youths: An economic analysis dren’s and adolescents’ sharing and moral
Gregory, E. (2007). Ready: Why women are em- (pp. 69–120). Chicago: University of Chicago negotiations in economic decision situa-
bracing the new later motherhood. New York: Press. tions. Child Development, 79, 562–576.
Basic Books. Grucza, R. A., Norberg, K. E., & Bierut, L. J. Gunlicks-Stoessel, M., & Powers, S. I. (2008).
Greven, C. U., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2011). (2009). Binge drinking among youths and Adolescents’ emotional experiences of
A twin study of ADHD symptoms in early young adults in the United States: 1979–2006. mother–adolescent conflict predict internal-
adolescence: Hyperactivity-impulsivity and Journal of the American Academy of Child and izing and externalizing symptoms. Journal of
inattentiveness show substantial genetic Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 692–702. Research on Adolescence, 18, 621–642.
overlap but also genetic specificity. Journal of Grumbach, M., Roth, J., Kaplan, S., & Kelch, R. Gunnar, M., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J., &
Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 265–275. (1974). Hypothalamic-pituitary regulation Griggs, C. (2009). Developmental changes
Griffin, K. W., Epstein, J. A., Botvin, G. J., & Spoth, of puberty in man: Evidence and concepts in hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal activity
R. L. (2001). Social competence and sub- derived from clinical research. In over the transition to adolescence: Norma-
stance use among rural youth: Mediating role M. Grumbach, G. Grave, & F. Mayer (Eds.), tive changes and associations with puberty.
of social benefit expectancies of use. Journal Control of the onset of puberty. Philadelphia: Development and Psychopathology, 21, 69–85.
of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 485–498. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Gunnoe, M. (1994, February). Noncustodial
Griffith Fontaine, R., Salzer Burks, V., & Dodge, K. Grunbaum, J., Lowry, R., Kann, L., & Pateman, B. mothers’ and fathers’ contributions to the ad-
(2002). Response decision processes and exter- (2000). Prevalence of health risk behaviors justment of adolescents in stepfamilies. Paper
nalizing behavior problems in adolescents. De- among Asian American/Pacific Islander high presented at the biennial meetings of the
velopment and Psychopathology, 14, 107–122. school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, Society for Research on Adolescence, San
27, 322–330. Diego.
Grisso, T., Steinberg, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E.,
Scott, E., Graham, S., et al. (2003). Juveniles’ Grundy, A., Gondoli, D., & Salafia, E. B. (2010). Gunnoe, M., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D.
competence to stand trial: A comparison of Hierarchical linear modeling analysis of (1999). Parental religiosity, parenting style,
adolescents’ and adults’ capacities as trial defen- change in maternal knowledge over the transi- and adolescent social responsibility. Journal
dants. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 333–363. tion to adolescence. Journal of Early Adoles- of Early Adolescence, 19, 199–225.
cence, 30, 707–732. Gunter, W., & Bakken, N. (2010). Transitioning to
Grolnick, W., Kurowski, C. O., Dunlap, K. G., &
Hevey, C. (2000). Parental resources and the Grych, J. H., Raynor, S. R., & Fosco, G. M. (2004). middle school in the sixth grade: A hierarchi-
transition to junior high. Journal of Research Family processes that shape the impact of cal linear modeling (HLM) analysis of sub-
on Adolescence, 10, 465–488. interparental conflict on adolescents. Devel- stance use, violence, and suicidal thoughts.
opment and Psychopathology, 16, 649–665. Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 895–915.
Grolnick, W., & Slowiaczek, M. (1994). Parents’
Guberman, S. (1996). The development of ev- Guo, G., Roettger, M. E., & Cai, T. (2008). The
involvement in children’s schooling: A
eryday mathematics in Brazilian children integration of genetic propensities into
multi-dimensional conceptualization and
with limited formal education. Child Devel- social-control models of delinquency and
motivational model. Child Development, 65,
opment, 67, 1609–1623. violence among male youths. American Soci-
237–252.
Gudiño, O., Nadeem, E., Kataoka, S., & Lau, A. ological Review, 73, 543–568.
Grosbras, M., Jansen, M., Leonard, G., McIntosh,
(2011). Relative impact of violence exposure Guo, G., Tong, Y., & Cai, T. (2008). Gene by social
A., Osswald, K., Poulsen, C., . . . Paus, T.
and immigrant stressors on Latino youth context interactions for number of sexual
(2007). Neural mechanisms of resistance to
psychopathology. Journal of Community Psy- partners among White male youths: Genetics
peer influence in early adolescence. Journal
chology, 39, 316–335. informed sociology. American Journal of So-
of Neuroscience, 27, 8040–8045.
Guerra, N. G., Williams, K. R., & Sadek, S. ciology, 114, 36–66.
Grossman, J. M., & Charmaraman, L. (2009).
(2011). Understanding bullying and victim- Güroğlu, B., Haselager, G. J. T., van Lieshout,
Race, context, and privilege: White adoles-
ization during childhood and adolescence: A Cornelis F. M., & Scholte, R. H. J. (2009).
cents’ explanations of racial-ethnic centrality.
mixed methods study. Child Development, Antagonists in mutual antipathies: A person-
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 139–152.
82, 295–310. oriented approach. Journal of Research on
Grossman, M., Chaloupka, F., Saffer, H., & Adolescence, 19, 35–46.
Guest, A., & Schneider, B. (2003). Adolescents’
Laixuthai, A. (1994). Effects of alcohol price
extracurricular participation in context: Gutierrez, R. (2000). Advancing African Ameri-
policy on youth: A summary of economic
The mediating effects of schools, communi- can, urban youth in mathematics: Unpack-
research. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
ties, and identity. Sociology of Education, 76, ing the success of one math department.
4, 347–364.
89–109. American Journal of Education, 109, 63–111.
Grotevant, H. (1997). Adolescent development Guillen, E., & Barr, S. (1994). Nutrition, dieting, Gutman, L., & Eccles, J. (2007). Stage-environ-
in family contexts. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), and fitness messages in a magazine for ado- ment fit during adolescence: Trajectories of
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, lescent women, 1970–1990. Journal of Ado- family relations and adolescent outcomes.
emotional, and personality development (5th lescent Health, 15, 464–472. Developmental Psychology, 43, 522–537.
ed., pp. 1097–1149). New York: Wiley.
Guion, K., Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2009). Pre- Gutman, L., McLoyd, V., & Tokoyawa, T. (2005).
Grotpeter, J., & Crick, N. (1996). Relational ag- dictive value of informant discrepancies in Financial strain, neighborhood stress, par-
gression, overt aggression, and friendship. reports of parenting: Relations to early ado- enting behaviors, and adolescent adjustment
Child Development, 67, 2328–2338. lescents’ adjustment. Journal of Abnormal in urban African American families. Journal
Grover, R. L., & Nangle, D. W. (2003). Adolescent Child Psychology, 37, 17–30. of Research on Adolescence, 15, 425–449.
perceptions of problematic heterosocial situ- Guldi, M., Page, M., & Stevens, A. (2007). Family Gutman, L., & Midgley, C. (2000). The role of
ations: A focus group study. Journal of Youth background and children’s transition to protective factors in supporting the aca-
and Adolescence, 32, 129–139. adulthood over time. In S. Danziger & demic achievement of poor African Ameri-
Gruber, J. (Ed.). (2001). Risky behavior among C. Rouse (Eds.), The price of independence: can students during the middle school
youths: An economic analysis. Chicago: Uni- The economics of early adulthood. New York: transition. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
versity of Chicago Press. Russell Sage Foundation. 29, 223–248.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-33 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-33

Guyer, A., McClure-Tone, E., Shiffrin, N., Pine, D., Gender-specific implications for stress and Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the
& Nelson, E. (2009). Probing the neural cor- health in college students. Anxiety, Stress & Society for Research on Adolescence, Boston.
relates of anticipated peer evaluation in ado- Coping: An International Journal, 19, 189–210. Halpern-Felsher, B. (2011). Adolescent decision-
lescence. Child Development, 80, 1000–1015. Haller, M., Handley, E., Chassin, L., & Bountress, K. making. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.),
Ha, T., Overbeek, G., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2010). Developmental cascades: Linking ado- Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 30–37).
(2010). Effects of attractiveness and social lescent substance use, affiliation with substance New York: Academic Press.
status on dating desire in heterosexual ado- use promoting peers, and academic achieve- Halpern-Felsher, B., & Cauffman, E. (2001).
lescents: An experimental study. Archives of ment to adult substance use disorders. Devel- Costs and benefits of a decision: Decision-
Sexual Behavior, 39, 1063–1071. opment and Psychopathology, 22, 899–916. making competence in adolescents and
Habermas, T., & de Silveira, C. (2008). The de- Halligan, S. L., & Philips, K. J. (2010). Are you adults. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
velopment of global coherence in life narra- thinking what I’m thinking? Peer group chology, 22, 257–273.
tives across adolescence: Temporal, causal, similarities in adolescent hostile attribution Halpern-Meekin, S., & Tach, L. (2008). Hetero-
and thematic aspects. Developmental Psy- tendencies. Developmental Psychology, 46, geneity in two-parent families and adoles-
chology, 44, 707–721. 1385–1388. cent well-being. Journal of Marriage and
Haddad, E., Chen, C., & Greenberger, E. (2011). Hallinan, M. (1996). Track mobility in second- Family, 70, 435–451.
The pole of important nonparental adults ary school. Social Forces, 74, 983–1002. Haltigan, J. D., Roisman, G. I., Susman, E. J.,
(VIPs) in the lives of older adolescents: A Hallinan, M. (2008). Teacher influences on stu- Barnett-Walker, K., & Monahan, K. C. (2011).
comparison of three ethnic groups. Journal dents’ attachment to school. Sociology of Ed- Elevated trajectories of externalizing prob-
of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 310–319. ucation, 81, 271–283. lems are associated with lower awakening cor-
Hagan, J., & Foster, H. (2001). Youth violence Hallinan, M., & Kubitschek, W. (2012). A com- tisol levels in midadolescence. Developmental
and the end of adolescence. American Socio- parison of academic achievement and adher- Psychology, 47, 472–478.
logical Review, 66, 874–899. ence to the common school ideal in public Hamill, S. (1994). Parent–adolescent communi-
Hahm, H., Lahiff, M., & Barreto, R. M. (2006). and Catholic schools. Sociology of Education, cation in sandwich generation families. Jour-
Asian American adolescents’ first sexual in- 85, 1–22. nal of Adolescent Research, 9, 458–482.
tercourse: Gender and acculturation differ- Hallinan, M., & Williams, R. (1989). Interracial Hamilton, B., Martin, J., & Ventura, S. (2007).
ences. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive friendship choices in secondary schools. Births: Preliminary data for 2006. National
Health, 38, 28–36. American Sociological Review, 54, 67–78. Vital Statistics Reports, 56, Number 7.
Hahm, H., Lahiff, M., & Guterman, N. (2003). Hallquist, S., Cuthbertson, C., Killeya-Jones, L., Hamilton, C. (2000). Continuity and discontinu-
Acculturation and parental attachment in Halpern, C., & Harris, K. (November, 2011). ity of attachment from infancy through ado-
Asian-American adolescents’ alcohol use. Living arrangements in young adulthood: Re- lescence. Child Development, 71, 690–694.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 33, 119–129. sults from wave iv of the national longitudinal Hamilton, S., & Hamilton, M. (2009). The
Hahm, H., Lahiff, M., & Guterman, N. (2004). study of adolescent health. Add Health Re- transition to adulthood: Challenges of pov-
Asian American adolescents’ acculturation, search Brief, Carolina Population Center, The erty and structural lag. In R. Lerner &
binge drinking, and alcohol- and tobacco- University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent
using peers. Journal of Community Psychol- Halpern, C. T. (2003). Biological influences on psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 492–526).
ogy, 32, 295–308. adolescent romantic and sexual behavior. In New York: Wiley.
Haines, J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Eisenberg, M., P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romantic rela- Hamm, J. (2000). Do birds of a feather flock to-
& Hannan, P. (2006). Weight teasing and dis- tions and sexual behavior: Theory, research, gether? The variable bases for African Ameri-
ordered eating behaviors in adolescents: Lon- and practical implications (pp. 57–84). can, Asian American, and European American
gitudinal findings from Project EAT (Eating Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. adolescents’ selection of similar friends.
Among Teens). Pediatrics, 117, e209–15. Halpern, C., King, R., Oslak, S., & Udry, J. (2005). Developmental Psychology, 36, 209–219.
Body mass index, dieting, romance, and sex- Hamm, J., Brown, B., & Heck, D. (2005). Bridg-
Hair, E. C., Moore, K. A., Garrett, S. B., Ling, T.,
ual activity in adolescent girls: Relationships ing the ethnic divide: Student and school
& Cleveland, K. (2008). The continued im-
over time. Journal of Research on Adolescence, characteristics in African American, Asian-
portance of quality parent–adolescent rela-
15, 535–559. descent, Latino, and White adolescents’
tionships during late adolescence. Journal of
Halpern, C., Oslak, S., Young, M., Martin, S., & cross-ethnic friend nominations. Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 18, 187–200.
Kupper, L. (2001). Partner violence among Research on Adolescence, 15, 21–46.
Haj-Yahia, M. M., Musleh, K., & Haj-Yahia, Y.
adolescents in opposite-sex romantic rela- Hammons, A., & Fiese, B. (2011). Is frequency of
(2002). The incidence of adolescent mal-
tionships: Findings from the National Longi- shared family meals related to the nutritional
treatment in Arab society and some of its
tudinal Study of Adolescent Health. American health of children and adolescents? Pediat-
psychological effects. Journal of Family Issues,
Journal of Public Health, 91, 1679–1685. rics, 127, 1565–1574.
23, 1032–1064.
Halpern, C. T., Spriggs, A. L., Martin, S. L., & Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (2006). A first
Hale, S., Bronik, M., & Fry, A. (1997). Verbal large cohort study of personality trait stabil-
Kupper, L. L. (2009). Patterns of intimate
and spatial working memory in school-age ity over the 40 years between elementary
partner violence victimization from adoles-
children: Developmental differences in sus- school and midlife. Journal of Personality and
cence to young adulthood in a nationally
ceptibility to interference. Developmental Social Psychology, 91, 763–779.
representative sample. Journal of Adolescent
Psychology, 33, 364–371.
Health, 45, 508–516. Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early
Halgunseth, L., Ispa, J., & Rudy, D. (2006). Paren- teacher–child relationships and the trajectory
Halpern, C., Udry, J., Campbell, B., & Suchindran,
tal control in Latino families: An integrated of children’s school outcomes through eighth
C. (1999). Effects of body fat on weight con-
review of the literature. Child Development, grade. Child Development, 72, 625–638.
cerns, dating, and sexual activity: A longitudi-
77, 1282–1297. Hamza, C. A., & Willoughby, T. (2011). Perceived
nal analysis of Black and White adolescent
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence. New York: Appleton. girls. Developmental Psychology, 35, 721–736. parental monitoring, adolescent disclosure,
Hall, N. C., Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., Ruthig, Halpern, C., Udry, J., & Suchindran, C. (1996, and adolescent depressive symptoms: A lon-
J. C., & Goetz, T. (2006). Primary and second- March). Monthly measures of salivary testoster- gitudinal examination. Journal of Youth and
ary control in academic development: one predict sexual activity in adolescent males. Adolescence, 40, 902–915.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-34 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-34 References

Han, W., Miller, D., & Waldfogel, J. (2010). Hardy, D., Astone, N., Brooks-Gunn, J., Shapiro, Hart, D., Donnelly, T. M., Youniss, J., & Atkins, R.
Parental work schedules and adolescent risky X., & Miller, X. (1998). Like mother, like (2007). High school community service as a
behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 46, child: Intergenerational patterns of age at predictor of adult voting and volunteering.
1245–1267. first birth and associations with childhood American Educational Research Journal, 44,
Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., & Roesch, L. and adolescent characteristics and adult out- 197–219.
(2007). Sex differences in adolescent depres- comes in the second generation. Develop- Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1995). Prosocial behavior
sion: Stress exposure and reactivity models. mental Psychology, 34, 1220–1232. and caring in adolescence: Relations to self-
Child Development, 78, 279–295. Hardy, S. A., Carlo, G., & Roesch, S. C. (2010). understanding and social judgment. Child
Hankin, B. L., Stone, L., & Wright, P. A. (2010). Links between adolescents’ expected parental Development, 66, 1346–1359.
Corumination, interpersonal stress genera- reactions and prosocial behavioral tenden- Hart, D., Hofmann, V., Edelstein, W., & Keller, M.
tion, and internalizing symptoms: Accumu- cies: The mediating role of prosocial values. (1997). The relation of childhood personality
lating effects and transactional influences in a Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 84–95. types to adolescent behavior and develop-
multiwave study of adolescents. Development Hardy, S. A., & Raffaelli, M. (2003). Adolescent ment: A longitudinal study of Icelandic chil-
and Psychopathology, 22, 217–235. religiosity and sexuality: An investigation of dren. Developmental Psychology, 33, 195–205.
Hansen, D. M., Janssen, I., Schiff, A., Zee, P. C., & reciprocal influences. Journal of Adolescence, Harter, S. (1990). Identity and self development.
Dubocovich, M. L. (2005). The impact of 26, 731–739. In S. Feldman & G. Elliott (Eds.), At the thresh-
school daily schedule on adolescent sleep. Hare, A. L., Marston, E. G., & Allen, J. P. (2011). old: The developing adolescent (pp. 352–387).
Pediatrics, 115, 1555–1561. Maternal acceptance and adolescents’ emo- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hanson, S. (1994). Lost talent: Unrealized educa- tional communication: A longitudinal study. Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self.
tional aspirations and expectations among U.S. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 744–751. New York: Guilford Press.
youths. Sociology of Education, 67, 159–183. Hargreaves, D., & Tiggemann, M. (2003). The ef- Harter, S. (2011). Self-development during ado-
Hanson, T., McLanahan, S., & Thomson, E. fect of thin ideal television commercials on lescence. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.),
(1996). Double jeopardy: Parental conflict body dissatisfaction and schema activation Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 307–
and stepfamily outcomes for children. Jour- during early adolescence. Journal of Youth 315). New York: Academic Press.
nal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 141–154. and Adolescence, 32, 367–373.
Harter, S., Marold, D., Whitesell, N., & Cobbs, G.
Harden, K. P., & Mendle, J. (2011a). Adolescent Harker, K. (2001). Immigrant generation, as- (1996). A model of the effects of parent and
sexual activity and the development of delin- similation, and adolescent psychological peer support on adolescent false-self behav-
quent behavior: The role of relationship con- well-being. Social Forces, 79, 969–1004. ior. Child Development, 67, 360–374.
text. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, Harold, G., & Conger, R. (1997). Marital conflict Harter, S., & Monsour, A. (1992). Developmental
825–838. and adolescent distress: The role of adolescent analysis of conflict caused by opposing attri-
Harden, K. P., & Mendle, J. (2011b). Gene- awareness. Child Development, 68, 333–350. butes in the adolescent self-portrait. Devel-
environment interplay in the association be- Harper, G., & Robinson, W. L. (1999). Pathways opmental Psychology, 28, 251–260.
tween pubertal timing and delinquency in to risk among inner-city African-American Harter, S., Stocker, C., & Robinson, N. (1996).
adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- adolescent females: The influence of gang The perceived directionality of the link be-
ogy, 120, 73–87. membership. American Journal of Commu- tween approval and self-worth: The liabili-
Harden, K. P., Mendle, J., Hill, J. E., Turkheimer, nity Psychology, 27, 383–404. ties of a looking glass self orientation among
E., & Emery, R. E. (2008). Rethinking timing Harré, N. (2000). Risk evaluation, driving, and young adolescents. Journal of Research on
of first sex and delinquency. Journal of Youth adolescents: A typology. Developmental Re- Adolescence, 6, 285–308.
and Adolescence, 37, 373–385. view, 20, 206–226. Harter, S., Waters, P., & Whitesell, N. (1998). Re-
Harden, K. P., Quinn, T., & Tucker-Drob, E. Harris, J. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? lational self-worth: Differences in perceived
(2012). Genetically influenced change in A group socialization theory of development. worth as a person across interpersonal con-
sensation seeking drives the rise of delin- Psychological Bulletin, 102, 458–489. texts among adolescents. Child Development,
quent behavior during adolescence. Develop- Harris, J. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why 69, 756–766.
mental Science, 15, 150–163. children turn out the way they do. New York: Hartney, C. (2006). Youth under 18 in the crimi-
Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2011). Free Press. nal justice system. Oakland: National Council
Individual differences in the development Harris, K. (1999). The health status and risk be- on Crime and Delinquency.
of sensation seeking and impulsivity during havior of adolescents in immigrant families. Hartung, C., Lefner, E., & Fedele, D. (2011).
adolescence: Further evidence for a dual sys- In D. Hernandez (Ed.), Children of immi- Disruptive behaviors and aggression. In
tems model. Developmental Psychology, 47, grants: Health, adjustment, and public B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia
739–746. assistance (pp. 286–347). Washington, DC: of adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 143–150). New
Hardin, M., & Ernst, M. (2009). Functional brain National Academy Press. York: Academic Press.
imaging of development-related risk and vul- Harris, M. (1994). Cholas, Mexican-American Hartup, W. (1977). Adolescent peer relations:
nerability for substance use in adolescents. girls, and gangs. Sex Roles, 30, 289–301. A look to the future. In J. Hill & F. Monks
Journal of Addiction Medicine, 3, 47–54. Harris-Britt, A., Valrie, C. R., Kurtz-Costes, B., & (Eds.), Adolescence and youth in prospect.
Harding, D. J. (2003). Counterfactual models of Rowley, S. J. (2007). Perceived racial discrim- Guildford, England: IPC Press.
neighborhood effects: The effect of neigh- ination and self-esteem in African American Hartup, W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendships
borhood poverty on dropping out and teen- youth: Racial socialization as a protective and adaptation in the life course. Psychologi-
age pregnancy. American Journal of Sociology, factor. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, cal Bulletin, 121, 335–370.
109, 676–719. 669–682.
Hasebe, Y., Nucci, L., & Nucci, M. S. (2004).
Hardway, C., & Fuligni, A. (2006). Dimensions Harrison, K., & Heffner, V. (2008). Media, body Parental control of the personal domain
of family connectedness among adolescents image, and eating disorders. In S. L. Calvert and adolescent symptoms of psychopa-
with Mexican, Chinese, and European back- & B. J. Wilson (Eds.), The handbook of chil- thology: A cross-national study in the
grounds. Developmental Psychology, 42, dren, media, and development (pp. 381–406). United States and Japan. Child Develop-
1246–1258. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. ment, 75, 815–828.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-35 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-35

Hastings, J. S., & Weinstein, J. M. (2008). Informa- Haynie, D. L., Giordano, P. C., Manning, W. D., & group jurisdiction. Child Development, 82,
tion, school choices, and academic achieve- Longmore, M. A. (2005). Adolescent romantic 701–716.
ment: Evidence from two experiments. relationships and delinquency involvement. Henderson, A., Brown, S. D., Pancer, S. M., &
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 123, 1373–1414. Criminology, 43, 177–210. Ellis-Hale, K. (2007). Mandated community
Hatzichristou, C., & Hopf, D. (1996). A multi- Haynie, D. L., & McHugh, S. (2003). Sibling devi- service in high school and subsequent civic
perspective comparison of peer sociometric ance in the shadows of mutual and unique engagement: The case of the “double cohort”
status groups in childhood and adolescence. friendship effects? Criminology, 41, 355–391. in Ontario, Canada. Journal of Youth and
Child Development, 67, 1085–1102. Haynie, D. L., Nansel, T., Eitel, P., Crump, A., Saylor, Adolescence, 36, 849–860.
Hauser, S., & Bowlds, M. (1990). Stress, coping, K., Yu, K., & Simons-Morton, B. (2001). Bul- Henderson, C., Hayslip, B., Sanders, L., & Louden,
and adaptation. In S. Feldman & G. Elliott lies, victims, and bully/victims: Distinct groups L. (2009). Grandmother—grandchild rela-
(Eds.), At the threshold: The developing ado- of at-risk youth. Journal of Early Adolescence, tionship quality predicts psychological adjust-
lescent (pp. 388–413). Cambridge, MA: 21, 29–49. ment among youth from divorced families.
Harvard University Press. Haynie, D. L., & Osgood, D. W. (2005). Reconsid- Journal of Family Issues, 30, 1245–1264.
Hausser, D., & Michaud, P. (1994). Does a condom- ering peers and delinquency: How do peers Hendrick, S., & Hendrick, C. (1994, February).
promoting strategy (the Swiss STOP-AIDS matter? Social Forces, 84, 1109–1130. Gender, sexuality, and close relationships. Paper
campaign) modify sexual behavior among Hayward, C., Gotlib, I., Schraedley, P., & Litt, I. presented at the biennial meetings of the Soci-
adolescents? Pediatrics, 93, 580–585. (1999). Ethnic differences in the association ety for Research on Adolescence, San Diego.
Havighurst, R. (1952). Developmental tasks and between pubertal status and symptoms of Henry, B., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., Harrington, H.,
education. New York: McKay. depression in adolescent girls. Journal of Ad- & Silva, P. (1999). Staying in school protects
Hawkins, D. N., Amato, P. R., & King, V. (2006). olescent Health, 25, 143–149. boys with poor self-regulation in childhood
Parent–adolescent involvement: The relative Hazan, C. (1994, February). The role of sexuality from later crime: A longitudinal study. Inter-
influence of parent gender and residence. in peer attachment formation. Paper pre- national Journal of Behavioral Development,
Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 125–136. sented at the biennial meetings of the Society 23, 1049–1073.
Hawkins, D. N., Amato, P. R., & King, V. (2007). for Research on Adolescence, San Diego. Henry, B., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., & Silva, P. (1996).
Nonresident father involvement and adoles- Heard, H. E. (2007). The family structure trajec- Temperamental and familial predictors of
cent well-being: Father effects or child ef- tory and adolescent school performance: violent and nonviolent criminal convictions:
fects? American Sociological Review, 72, Differential effects by race and ethnicity. Age 3 to age 18. Developmental Psychology,
990–1010. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 319–354. 32, 614–623.
Hawkins, J., Brown, E. C., Oesterle, S., Arthur, M. Henry, D. B., Schoeny, M. E., Deptula, D. P., &
Heatherton, T. F., & Sargent, J. D. (2009). Does
W., Abbott, R. D., & Catalano, R. F. (2008). Slavick, J. T. (2007). Peer selection and social-
watching smoking in movies promote teen-
Early effects of communities that care on ization effects on adolescent intercourse
age smoking? Current Directions in Psycho-
targeted risks and initiation of delinquent without a condom and attitudes about the
logical Science, 18, 63–67.
behavior and substance use. Journal of Ado- costs of sex. Child Development, 78, 825–838.
Hechinger, F. (1993). Schools for teenagers: A
lescent Health, 43, 15–22. Henry, K. L., Cavanagh, T. M., & Oetting, E. R.
historic dilemma. Teachers College Record,
Hay, C., Fortson, E. N., Hollist, D. R., Altheimer, (2011). Perceived parental investment in
94, 522–539.
I., & Schaible, L. M. (2007). Compounded school as a mediator of the relationship be-
risk: The implications for delinquency of Heck, R. H., Price, C. L., & Thomas, S. L. (2004). tween socio-economic indicators and educa-
coming from a poor family that lives in a Tracks as emergent structures: A network tional outcomes in rural America. Journal of
poor community. Journal of Youth and Ado- analysis of student differentiation in a high Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1164–1177.
lescence, 36, 593–605. school. American Journal of Education, 110,
Henry, K. L., Knight, K. E., & Thornberry, T. P.
321–353.
Hay, C., & Meldrum, R. (2010). Bullying victim- (2012). School disengagement as a predictor
ization and adolescent self-harm: Testing Hein, K., Dell, R., Futterman, D., Rotheram- of dropout, delinquency, and problem sub-
hypotheses from general strain theory. Jour- Borus, M., & Shaffer, N. (1995). Comparison stance use during adolescence and early
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 446–459. of HIV1 and HIV2 adolescents: Risk factors adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
and psychosocial determinants. Pediatrics, 41, 156–166.
Haydon, A. A., & Halpern, C. T. (2010). Older
95, 96–104. Hensel, D., Fortenberry, J., & Orr, D. (2008).
romantic partners and depressive symptoms
during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Helsen, M., Vollebergh, W., & Meeus, W. (2000). Variations in coital and noncoital sexual rep-
Adolescence, 39, 1240–1251. Social support from parents and friends and ertoire among adolescent women. Journal of
emotional problems in adolescence. Journal Adolescent Health, 42, 170–176.
Haydon, A., McRee, A-L., & Halpern, C. (2011).
of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 319–335. Henshaw, S. (1995). The impact of requirements
Risk-taking behavior. In B. Brown & M.
Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence Helwig, C. (1995). Adolescents’ and young adults’ for parental consent on minors’ abortions in
(Vol. 3, pp. 255–263). New York: Academic conceptions of civil liberties: Freedom of Mississippi. Family Planning Perspectives, 27,
Press. speech and religion. Child Development, 66, 120–122.
Hayes, D., Wolfer, L., & Wolfe, M. (1996). School- 152–166. Herdt, G., & McClintock, M. (2000). The magi-
book simplification and its relation to the Helwig, C. (1997). The role of agent and social cal age of 10. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29,
decline in SAT-Verbal scores. American Edu- context in judgments of freedom of speech 587–606.
cational Research Journal, 33, 489–508. and religion. Child Development, 68, 484–495. Herman, M. (2004). Forced to choose: Some de-
Hayford, S. R., & Furstenberg, F. F. J. (2008). De- Helwig, C., Arnold, M. L., Tan, D., & Boyd, D. terminants of racial identification in multi-
layed adulthood, delayed desistance? Trends (2007). Mainland Chinese and Canadian ado- racial adolescents. Child Development, 75,
in the age distribution of problem behaviors. lescents’ judgments and reasoning about the 730–748.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 285–304. fairness of democratic and other forms of gov- Herman, M. (2009). The Black-White-other
Haynie, D. L. (2003). Contexts of risk? Explaining ernment. Cognitive Development, 22, 96–109. achievement gap: Testing theories of aca-
the link between girls’ pubertal development Helwig, C., Yang, S., Tan, D., Liu, C., & Shao, T. demic performance among multiracial and
and their delinquency involvement. Social (2011). Urban and rural Chinese adolescents’ monoracial adolescents. Sociology of Educa-
Forces, 82, 355–397. judgments and reasoning about personal and tion, 82, 20–46.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-36 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-36 References

Herman-Giddens, M., Slora, E., Wasserman, Hill, J., Emery, R. E., Harden, K. P., Mendle, J., & Hirschfield, P. (2009). Another way out: The im-
R., Bourdony, C., Bhapkar, M., Koch, G., . . . Turkheimer, E. (2008). Alcohol use in adoles- pact of juvenile arrests on high school dropout.
Hasemeier, C. (1997). Secondary sexual cent twins and affiliation with substance Sociology of Education, 82, 368–393.
characteristics and menses in young girls using peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- Hitlin, S., Brown, J. S., & Elder, G. H. J. (2006).
seen in office practice: A study from the Pe- chology, 36, 81–94. Racial self-categorization in adolescence:
diatric Research in Office Settings Network. Hill, J. P. (1983). Early adolescence: A frame- Multiracial development and social pathways.
Pediatrics, 88, 505–512. work. Journal of Early Adolescence, 3, 1–21. Child Development, 77, 1298–1308.
Herman-Giddens, M., Steffes, J., Harris, D., Hill, K. G., Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., Abbott, Hochberg, Z., Gawlik, A., & Walker, R. S. (2011).
Slora, E., Hussey, M., Dowshen, S., . . . R. D., & Guo, J. (2005). Family influences on Evolutionary fitness as a function of puber-
Reiter, E. (2012, October 20). Secondary the risk of daily smoking initiation. Journal tal age in 22 subsistence-based traditional
sexual characteristics in boys: Data from of Adolescent Health, 37, 202–210. societies. International Journal of Pediatric
the Pediatric Research in Office Settings Endocrinology, 2, 1–7.
Hill, N. E., Castellino, D. R., Lansford, J. E., Nowlin,
Network. Pediatrics, published online (doi: Hock, E., Eberly, M., Bartle-Haring, S., Ellwanger,
P., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S.
10.1542/peds.2011-3291). P., & Widaman, K. F. (2001). Separation anxi-
(2004). Parent academic involvement as related
Herman-Stahl, M., Stemmler, M., & Petersen, A. to school behavior, achievement, and aspira- ety in parents of adolescents: Theoretical
(1995). Approach and avoidant coping: Impli- tions: Demographic variations across adoles- significance and scale development. Child
cations for adolescent mental health. Journal cence. Child Development, 75, 1491–1509. Development, 72, 284–298.
of Youth and Adolescence, 24, 649–665. Hill, N. E., Ramirez, C., & Dumka, L. E. (2003). Hodges, E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W.
Herrenkohl, T. I., Hill, K. G., Hawkins, J. D., Early adolescents’ career aspirations: A quali- (1999). The power of friendship: Protection
Chung, I., & Nagin, D. (2006). Developmental tative study of perceived barriers and family against an escalating cycle of peer victimiza-
trajectories of family management and risk support among low-income, ethnically di- tion. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101.
for violent behavior in adolescence. Journal of verse adolescents. Journal of Family Issues, 24, Hodges, E., & Perry, D. (1999). Personal and in-
Adolescent Health, 39, 206–213. 934–959. terpersonal antecedents and consequences of
Herrenkohl, T. I., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Hill, N. E., & Tyson, D. (2009). Parental involve- victimization by peers. Journal of Personality
& Mason, W. A. (2009). Effects of growth in ment in middle school: A meta-analytic as- and Social Psychology, 76, 677–685.
family conflict in adolescence on adult de- sessment of the strategies that promote Hoeve, M., Blokland, A., Dubas, J. S., Loeber, R.,
pressive symptoms: Mediating and modera- achievement. Developmental Psychology, 45, Gerris, J., & van der Laan, P. (2008). Trajecto-
tion effects of stress and school bonding. 740–763. ries of delinquency and parenting styles. Jour-
Journal of Adolescent Health, 44, 146–152. Hilsman, R., & Garber, J. (1995). A test of the cog- nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 223–235.
Herrenkohl, T. I., Maguin, E., Hill, K. G., Hawkins, nitive diathesis–stress model of depression in Hofferth, S. L., & Reid, L. (2002). Early child-
J., Abbott, R. D., & Catalano, R. F. (2000). De- children: Academic stressors, attributional bearing and children’s achievement and be-
velopmental risk factors for youth violence. style, perceived competence, and control. havior over time. Perspectives on Sexual and
Journal of Adolescent Health, 26, 176–186. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Reproductive Health, 34, 41–49.
69, 370–380.
Herrera, V. M., Wiersma, J. D., & Cleveland, H. Hofferth, S. L., Reid, L., & Mott, F. L. (2001). The
H. (2011). Romantic partners’ contribution Hilt, L. M., McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, effects of early childbearing on schooling
to the continuity of male and female delin- S. (2010). Examination of the Response Styles over time. Family Planning Perspective, 33,
quent and violent behavior. Journal of Re- Theory in a community sample of young ado- 259–267.
search on Adolescence, 21, 608–618. lescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Hoffman, J. P. (2002). The community context of
38, 545–556.
Hetherington, E. M. (1991). The role of individ- family structure and adolescent drug use. Jour-
Hindin, M. J., & Fatusi, A. O. (2009). Adolescent
ual differences and family relationships in nal of Marriage and the Family, 64, 314–330.
sexual and reproductive health in develop-
children’s coping with divorce and remar- Hoffman, J. P. (2003). A contextual analysis of
ing countries: An overview of trends and
riage. In P. Cowan & E. M. Hetherington differential association, social control, and
interventions. International Perspectives on
(Eds.), Advances in family research: Vol. 2. strain theories of delinquency. Social Forces,
Sexual and Reproductive Health, 35, 58–62.
Family transitions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 81, 753–785.
Hine, T. (1999). The rise and fall of the American
Hetherington, E. M. (1993). An overview of the Hoffman, K. L., Kiecolt, J., & Edwards, J. N.
teenager. New York: Bard Books.
Virginia longitudinal study of divorce and (2005). Physical violence between siblings: A
remarriage with a focus on early adolescence. Hingson, R., Heeren, T., & Winter, M. (2006).
theoretical and empirical analysis. Journal of
Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 39–56. Age at drinking onset and alcohol depen-
Family Issues, 26, 1103–1130.
dence: Age at onset, duration, and severity.
Hetherington, E. M., Bridges, M., & Insabella, G. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, Hogan, D., Sun, R., & Cornwell, G. (2000). Sexual
(1998). What matters? What does not? Five 160, 739–746. and fertility behaviors of American females
perspectives on the association between aged 15–19 years: 1985, 1990, and 1995. Amer-
Hingson, R., & Zha, W. (2009). Age of drinking
marital transitions and children’s adjust- ican Journal of Public Health, 90, 1421–1425.
onset, alcohol use disorders, frequent heavy
ment. American Psychologist, 53, 167–184. Hogue, A., & Steinberg, L. (1995). Homophily of
drinking, and unintentionally injuring one-
Hetherington, E. M., Henderson, S., & Reiss, D. self and others after drinking. Pediatrics, 123, internalized distress in adolescent peer groups.
(1999). Adolescent siblings in stepfamilies: 1477–1484. Developmental Psychology, 31, 897–906.
Family functioning and adolescent adjust- Hirsch, B., & DuBois, D. (1991). Self-esteem in Holder, D. W., DuRant, R. H., Harris, T. L., Daniel,
ment. Monographs of the Society for Research early adolescence: The identification and pre- J. H., Obeidallah, D., & Goodman, E. (2000).
in Child Development, 64, Serial No. 259. diction of contrasting longitudinal trajectories. The association between adolescent spiritual-
Hightower, E. (1990). Adolescent interpersonal Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 53–72. ity and voluntary sexual activity. Journal of
and familial predictors of positive mental Hirsch, B., Mickus, M., & Boerger, R. (2002). Ties Adolescent Health, 26, 295–302.
health at midlife. Journal of Youth and Ado- to influential adults among Black and White Hollander, D. (2006). Many teenagers who say
lescence, 19, 257–276. adolescents: Culture, social class, and family they have taken a virginity pledge retract that
Hill, C., Corbett, C., & Rose, A. (2010). Why so networks. American Journal of Community statement after having intercourse. Perspectives
few? Washington: AAUW. Psychology, 30, 289–303. on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 38, 168.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-37 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-37

Hollingshead, A. (1975). Elmtown’s youth and Hornik, R., Jacobsohn, L., Orwin, R., Piesse, A., & remission. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Elmtown revisited. New York: Wiley. (Origi- Kalton, G. (2008). Effects of the national youth 41, 76–85.
nal work published 1949) anti-drug media campaign on youths. Ameri- Huh, D., Tristan, J., Wade, E., & Stice, E. (2006).
Holmbeck, G. (1996). A model of family rela- can Journal of Public Health, 98, 2229–2236. Does problem behavior elicit poor parent-
tional transformations during the transition Horvat, E. M., & Lewis, K. S. (2003). Reassessing ing?: A prospective study of adolescent girls.
to adolescence: Parent–adolescent conflict the “burden of ‘acting White’”: The impor- Journal of Adolescent Research, 21, 185–204.
and adaptation. In J. Graber, J. Brooks-Gunn, tance of peer group in managing academic Huizenga, H., Crone, E., & Jansen, B. (2007). De-
& A. Petersen (Eds.), Transitions through ad- success. Sociology of Education, 76, 265–280. cision-making in healthy children,
olescence: Interpersonal domains and context Howard, A., & Galambos, N. (2011). Transitions adolescents, and adults explained by the use
(pp. 167–199). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. to adulthood. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein of increasingly complex proportional reason-
Holmbeck, G., Durbin, D., & Kung, E. (1995, (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. ing rules. Developmental Science, 10, 814–825.
March). Attachment, autonomy, and adjust- 376–383). New York: Academic Press. Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009).
ment before and after leaving home: Sullivan Howard, K., Budge, S., & McKay, K. (2010). Promoting interest and performance in high
and Sullivan revisited. Paper presented at the Youth exposed to violence: the role of pro- school science classes. Science, 326(5958),
Society for Research in Child Development, tective factors. Journal of Community Psy- 1410–1412.
Indianapolis. chology, 38, 63–79.
Hunter, S., Barber, B., Olsen, J., McNeely, C., &
Holmbeck, G., & Hill, J. P. (1991). Conflictive en- Howell, W., & Peterson, P. (2002). The education Bose, K. (2011). Adolescents’ self-disclosure
gagement, positive affect, and menarche in gap: Vouchers and urban schools. Washington, to parents across cultures: Who discloses
families with seventh-grade girls. Child De- DC: Brookings Institution. and why. Journal of Adolescent Research, 26,
velopment, 62, 1030–1048. Hoyt, L., Chase-Lansdale, P., McDade, T., & 447–478.
Holmbeck, G., Shapera, W., Westhoven, V., Johnson, Adam, E. K. (2011). Positive youth, healthy Huntsinger, C. S., & Jose, P. E. (2006). A longitu-
S., Millstein, R., & Hommeyer, J. (2000, March). adults: Does positive well-being in adoles- dinal investigation of personality and social
A longitudinal study of observed and perceived cence predict better perceived health and adjustment among Chinese American and
parenting behaviors and autonomy development fewer risky health behaviors in young adult- European American adolescents. Child De-
in families of young adolescents with spina bifida. hood? Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 66–73. velopment, 77, 1309–1324.
Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the
Hu, M., Davies, M., & Kandel, D. (2006). Epide- Hurley, D. (2005, April 19). Divorce rate: It’s not
Society for Research on Adolescence, Chicago.
miology and correlates of daily smoking and as high as you think. The New York Times.
Holmen, T., Barrett-Connor, E., Holmen, J., & nicotine dependence among young adults in
Bjermer, L. (2000). Health problems in teenage Hussong, A. M. (2000). Perceived peer context
the United States. American Journal of Public
daily smokers versus nonsmokers, Norway, and adolescent adjustment. Journal of Re-
Health, 96, 299–308.
1995–1997. American Journal of Epidemiology, search on Adolescence, 10, 391–415.
Hudley, C. (1995). Assessing the impact of separate
151, 148–155. Hussong, A. M., Curran, P. J., & Moffitt, T. E.
schooling for African American male adoles-
Holsen, I., Kraft, P., & Vitterso, J. (2000). Stability (2004). Substance abuse hinders desistance
cents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 15, 38–57.
in depressed mood in adolescence: Results in young adults’ antisocial behavior. Devel-
Hudley, C. (1997). Supporting achievement opment and Psychopathology, 16, 1029–1046.
from a 6-year longitudinal panel study. Journal
beliefs among ethnic minority adolescents:
of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 61–78. Hussong, A. M., & Hicks, R. E. (2003). Affect and
Two case examples. Journal of Research on
Hooper, C. J., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., & peer context interactively impact adolescent
Adolescence, 7, 133–152.
Yarger, R. S. (2004). Adolescents’ perfor- substance use. Journal of Abnormal Child
mance on the Iowa gambling task: Implica- Huebner, A. J., & Howell, L. W. (2003). Examining Psychology, 31, 413–426.
tions for the development of decision the relationship between adolescent sexual
Hutchinson, D., Rapee, R., & Taylor, A. (2010).
making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. risk-taking and perceptions of monitoring,
Body dissatisfaction and eating disturbances in
Developmental Psychology, 40, 1148–1158. communication, and parenting styles. Journal
early adolescence: A structural modeling inves-
of Adolescent Health, 33, 71–78.
Horan, P., & Hargis, P. (1991). Children’s work tigation examining negative affect and peer fac-
and schooling in the late nineteenth-century Huebner, A. J., & Mancini, J. A. (2003). Shaping tors. Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 489–517.
family economy. American Sociological Re- structured out-of-school time use among
Hutchinson, M. K., Jemmott, J. B., III, Jemmott,
view, 56, 583–596. youth: The effects of self, family, and friend
L. S., Braverman, P., & Fong, G. T. (2003).
systems. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32,
Horn, S. S. (2003). Adolescents’ reasoning about The role of mother–daughter sexual risk
453–463.
exclusion from social groups. Developmental communication in reducing sexual risk
Psychology, 39, 71–84. Huesmann, L. R. (2007). The impact of electronic behaviors among urban adolescent females:
Horn, S. S. (2007). Adolescents’ acceptance of media violence: Scientific theory and research. A prospective study. Journal of Adolescent
same-sex peers based on sexual orientation Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, S6–S13. Health, 33, 98–107.
and gender expression. Journal of Youth and Hughes, D., Way, N., & Rivas-Drake, D. (2011). Huynh, V., & Fuligni, A. (2008). Ethnic socializa-
Adolescence, 36, 363–371. Stability and change in private and public tion and the academic adjustment of adoles-
Horner, J., Jamieson, P., & Romer, D. (2008). The ethnic regard among African American, cents from Mexican, Chinese, and European
changing portrayal of alcohol use in televi- Puerto Rican, Dominican, and Chinese backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 44,
sion. In P. Jamieson & D. Romer (Eds.), The American early adolescents. Journal of Re- 1202–1208.
changing portrayal of adolescents in the media search on Adolescence, 21, 861–870. Huynh, V., & Fuligni, A. (2010). Discrimination
since 1950 (pp. 284–312). New York: Oxford Hughes, S., Power, T., & Francis, D. (1992). De- hurts: The academic, psychological, and
University Press. fining patterns of drinking in adolescence: A physical well-being of adolescents. Journal of
Horner, J., Salazar, L. F., Romer, D., Vanable, P. A., cluster analytic approach. Journal of Studies Research on Adolescence, 20, 916–941.
DiClemente, R., Carey, M. P., . . . Brown, L. on Alcohol, 53, 40–47. Hwang, K., Velanova, K., & Luna, B. (2010).
(2009). Withdrawal (coitus interruptus) as a Huh, D., Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Boutelle, K. (2012). Strengthening of top-down frontal cognitive
sexual risk reduction strategy: perspectives Female overweight and obesity in adoles- control networks underlying the develop-
from African-American adolescents. Archives cence: Developmental trends and ethnic ment of inhibitory control: a functional
of Sexual Behavior, 38, 779–787. differences in prevalence, incidence, and magnetic resonance imaging effective
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-38 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-38 References

connectivity study. Journal of Neuroscience, Ivory, J. (2008). The games, they are a-changin’: Jacobson, K., & Rowe, D. (1999). Genetic and
30(46), 15535–15545. Technological advancements in video games environmental influences on the relation-
Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., & Moilanen, K. L. and implications for effects on youth. In ships between family connectedness, school
(2010). Developmental precursors of moral P. Jamieson & D. Romer (Eds.), The changing connectedness, and adolescent depressed
disengagement and the role of moral disen- portrayal of adolescents in the media since mood: Sex differences. Developmental
gagement in the development of antisocial 1950 (pp. 347–376). New York: Oxford Psychology, 35, 926–939.
behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, University Press. Jaffari-Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H.,
38, 197–209. Jaccard, J., Blanton, H., & Dodge, T. (2005). Peer Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., & Mooijaart, A.
Ibañez, G. E., Kuperminc, G. P., Jurkovic, G., & influences on risk behavior: An analysis of (2006). Social development from infancy to
Perilla, J. (2004). Cultural attributes and ad- the effects of a close friend. Developmental adolescence: Longitudinal and concurrent fac-
aptations linked to achievement motivation Psychology, 41, 135–147. tors in an adoption sample. Developmental Psy-
among Latino adolescents. Journal of Youth Jaccard, J., Dittus, P., & Gordon, V. (1998). Parent– chology, 42, 1143–1153.
and Adolescence, 33, 559–568. adolescent congruency in reports of adoles- Jaffee, S., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Belsky, J., &
Iglowstein, I., Jenni, O., Molinari, L., & Largo, R. cent sexual behavior and in communications Silva, P. (2001). Why are children born to
(2003). Sleep duration from infancy to ado- about sexual behavior. Child Development, 69, teen mothers at risk for adverse outcomes in
lescence: Reference values and generational 247–261. young adulthood? Results from a 20-year
trends. Pediatrics, 111, 302–307. Jaccard, J., Dodge, T., & Dittus, P. (2003a). Do longitudinal study. Development and Psycho-
adolescents want to avoid pregnancy? Attti- pathology, 13, 377–397.
Impett, E. A., Sorsoli, L., Schooler, D., Henson,
J. M., & Tolman, D. L. (2008). Girls’ relation- tudes toward pregnancy as predictors of preg- James-Burdumy, S., Goesling, B., Deke, J., &
ship authenticity and self-esteem across nancy. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33, 79–83. Einspruch, E. (2012). The effectiveness of
adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 44, Jaccard, J., Dodge, T., & Dittus, P. (2003b). Maternal mandatory-random student drug testing: A
722–733. discussion about pregnancy and adolescents’ cluster randomized trial. Journal of Adoles-
attitudes towards pregnancy. Journal of Adoles- cent Health, 50, 172–178.
Ingoldsby, E. M., Kohl, G. O., McMahon, R. J.,
& Lengua, L. (2006). Conduct problems, cent Health, 33, 84–87. Jang, S., & Thornberry, T. (1998). Self-esteem,
depressive symptomatology, and their co- Jackson, C. (2002). Perceived legitimacy of pa- delinquent peers, and delinquency: A test of
occurring presentation in childhood as pre- rental authority and tobacco and alcohol use the self-enhancement thesis. American Socio-
dictors of adjustment in early adolescence. during early adolescence. Journal of Adoles- logical Review, 63, 586–598.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, cent Health, 31, 425–432. Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Pagani, L. S., Pascal,
603–621. Jackson, L. (2008). Adolescents and the Internet. S., Morin, A. J. S., & Bowen, F. (2008). Are
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of In P. Jamieson & D. Romer (Eds.), The there detrimental effects of witnessing
logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. changing portrayal of adolescents in the media school violence in early adolescence? Journal
New York: Basic Books. since 1950 (pp. 377–411). New York: Oxford of Adolescent Health, 43, 600–608.
University Press. Janosz, M., LeBlanc, M., Boulerice, B., & Tremblay,
IOM (Institute of Medicine) and NRC (National
Research Council) (2006). Food marketing to Jackson, T., & Chen, H. (2011). Risk factors for R. E. (2000). Predicting different types of
children and youth: Threat or opportunity? disordered eating during early and middle school dropouts: A typological approach with
Washington, DC: National Academies Press. adolescence: Prospective evidence from two longitudinal samples. Journal of Educa-
mainland Chinese boys and girls. Journal of tional Psychology, 92, 171–190.
IOM (Institute of Medicine) and NRC (National
Abnormal Psychology, 120, 454–464.
Research Council). (2011a). The science of Jarrett, R. (1995). Growing up poor: The family
Jackson-Newsom, J., Buchanan, C. M., & experiences of socially mobile youth in
adolescent risk-taking: Workshop report. Com-
McDonald, R. M. (2008). Parenting and low-income African American neighbor-
mittee on the Science of Adolescence. Wash-
perceived maternal warmth in European hoods. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10,
ington, DC: The National Academies Press.
American and African American adolescents. 111–135.
IOM (Institute of Medicine) and NRC (National Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 62–75.
Research Council). (2011b). The health of Jarvinen, M., & Østergaard, J. (2009). Governing
Jacobi, C., Hayward, C., de Zwaan, M., Kraemer,
lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender people: adolescent drinking. Youth & Society, 40,
H. C., & Agras, W. S. (2004). Coming to
Building a foundation for better understand- 377–402.
terms with risk factors for eating disorders:
ing. Washington, DC: The National Acade- Jaser, S. S., Champion, J. E., Reeslund, K. L.,
Application of risk terminology and sugges-
mies Press. Keller, G., Merchant, M. J., Benson, M., &
tions for a general taxonomy. Psychological
Irwin, K. (2004). The violence of adolescent life Bulletin, 130, 19–65. Compas, B. (2007). Cross-situational coping
experiencing and managing everyday threats. with peer and family stressors in adolescent
Jacobs, J. E., Chin, C. S., & Shaver, K. (2005).
Youth and Society, 35, 452–479. offspring of depressed parents. Journal of
Longitudinal links between perceptions of
Isakson, K., & Jarvis, P. (1999). The adjustment Adolescence, 30, 917–932.
adolescence and the social beliefs of adoles-
of adolescents during the transition into cents: Are parents’ stereotypes related to be- Jemmott, J., III, Jemmott, L., Fong, G., & McCaffree,
high school: A short-term longitudinal study. liefs held about and by their children? Journal K. (1999). Reducing HIV risk-associated sexual
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 1–26. of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 61–72. behavior among African American adolescents:
Ivanova, K., Mills, M., & Veenstra, R. (2011). The Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J., & Testing the generality of intervention effects.
initiation of dating in adolescence: The effect Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s American Journal of Community Psychology, 27,
of parental divorce. The TRAILS study. Journal self-competence and values: Gender and do- 161–187.
of Research on Adolescence, 21, 769–775. main differences across grades one through Jensen, L. A., Arnett, J. J., Feldman, S. S., &
Ivers, R., Senserrick, T., Boufous, S., Stevenson, twelve. Child Development, 73, 509–527. Cauffman, E. (2004). The right to do wrong:
M., Chen, H., Woodward, M., & Norton, R. Jacobson, C., & Gould, M. (2007). The epidemi- Lying to parents among adolescents and
(2009). Novice drivers’ risky driving behav- ology and phenomenology of non-suicidal emerging adults. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
ior, risk perception, and crash risk: Findings self-injurious behavior among adolescents: cence, 33, 101–112.
from the DRIVE study. American Journal of A critical review of the literature. Archives of Jenssen, B., Klein, J., Salazar, L., Daluga, N., &
Public Health, 99, 1638–1644. Suicide Research, 11, 129-147. DiClemente, R. (2009). Exposure to tobacco
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-39 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-39

on the internet: Content analysis of adoles- Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kotler, L., Kasen, S., & Jones, D., Costin, S., & Ricard, R. (1994, February).
cents’ internet use. Pediatrics, 124, e180–e186. Brook, J. S. (2002). Psychiatric disorders Ethnic and sex differences in best friendship
Jeong, Y.-J., & Chun, Y.-J. (2010). The pathways associated with risk for the development of characteristics among African-American, Mexi-
from parents’ marital quality to adolescents’ eating disorders during adolescence and can-American, and European-American ado-
school adjustment in South Korea. Journal of early adulthood. Journal of Consulting and lescents. Paper presented at the biennial
Family Issues, 31, 1604–1621. Clinical Psychology, 70, 1119–1128. meetings of the Society for Research on Ado-
Johnson, M. (2002). Social origins, adolescent lescence, San Diego.
Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. (1977). Problem behavior
and psychosocial development: A longitudi- experiences, and work value trajectories dur- Jones, D., & Smolak, L. (2011). Body image dur-
nal study of youth. New York: Academic Press. ing the transition to adulthood. Social Forces, ing adolescence: A developmental perspec-
80, 1307–1341. tive. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.),
Jessor, R., Turbin, M., & Costa, F. (2010). Predicting
Johnson, R., & Gerstein, D. (1998). Initiation of Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 77–
developmental change in healthy eating and
use of alcohol, cigarettes, marijuana, cocaine, 86). New York: Academic Press.
regular exercise among adolescents in China
and the United States: The role of psychosocial and other substances in U.S. birth cohorts Jones, J., Vanfossen, B., & Ensminger, M. (1995).
and behavioral protection and risk. Journal of since 1919. American Journal of Public Individual and organizational predictors of
Research on Adolescence, 20, 707–725. Health, 88, 27–33. track placement. Sociology of Education, 68,
Johnson, R., Johnson, D., Wang, M., Smiciklas- 287–300.
Jessor, R., Turbin, M. S., Costa, F. M., Dong, Q.,
Zhang, H., & Wang, C. (2003). Adolescent Wright, H., & Guthrie, H. (1994). Character- Jones, L., Mitchell, K., & Finkelhor, D. (2011).
problem behavior in China and the United izing nutrient intakes of adolescents by Trends in youth internet victimization: Find-
States: A cross-national study of psychosocial sociodemographic factors. Journal of Adoles- ings from three youth internet safety surveys
protective factors. Journal of Research on Ado- cent Health, 15, 149–154. 2000–2010. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50,
lescence, 13, 329–360. Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2006). 179–186.
Jessor, R., Van Den Bos, J., Vanderryn, J., Costa, F., Genetic and environmental influences on Jones, M. C., & Bayley, N. (1950). Physical ma-
& Turbin, M. (1995). Protective factors in ad- academic achievement trajectories during turing among boys as related to behavior.
olescent problem behavior: Moderator effects adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 42, Journal of Educational Psychology, 41, 129–148.
and developmental change. Developmental 514–532. Jones, M., Luce, K. H., Osborne, M. I., Taylor, K.,
Psychology, 31, 923–933. Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2009). Cunning, D., Doyle, A. C., & Taylor, C.
Jeynes, W. (1999). Effects of remarriage follow- School performance and genetic and (2008). Randomized, controlled trial of an
ing divorce on the academic achievement of environmental variance in antisocial behav- Internet-facilitated intervention for reducing
children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, ior at the transition from adolescence to binge eating and overweight in adolescents.
28, 385–393. adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 45, Pediatrics, 121, 453–462.
Jeynes, W. (2001). The effects of recent parental 973–987. Jones, M. D., & Galliher, R. V. (2007). Ethnic
divorce on their children’s consumption of Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P., Bachman, J., & identity and psychosocial functioning in
alcohol. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, Schulenberg, J. (2012a). Monitoring the Fu- Navajo adolescents. Journal of Research on
305–319. ture national results on adolescent drug use: Adolescence, 17, 683–696.
Jeynes, W. (2002). A meta-analysis of the Overview of key findings, 2011. Ann Arbor: Jones, N., Pieper, C., & Robertson, L. (1992). The
effects of attending religious schools and Institute for Social Research, The University effect of legal drinking age on fatal injuries
religiosity on Black and Hispanic academic of Michigan. of adolescents and young adults. American
achievement. Education and Urban Society, Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P., Bachman, J., & Schul- Journal of Public Health, 82, 112–115.
35, 27–49. enberg, J. (2012b). Demographic subgroup Jones, R. K., Biddlecom, A. E., Hebert, L., & Mellor,
Jia, Y., Way, N., Ling, G., Yoskikawa, H., Chen, X., trends for various licit and illicit drugs, 1975– R. (2011b). Teens reflect on their sources of
Hughes, D., . . . Lu, Z. (2009). The influence 2011 (Monitoring the Future Occasional Paper contraceptive information. Journal of Adoles-
of student perceptions of school climate on No. 77). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social cent Research, 26, 423–446.
socioemotional and academic adjustment: A Research. Jones, R. K., Darroch, J. E., & Singh, S. (2005).
comparison of Chinese and American ado- Johnston, M., Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. H., Jr. Religious differentials in the sexual and
lescents. Child Development, 80, 1514–1530. (2001). Students’ attachment and academic reproductive behaviors of young women in
Jodl, K. M., Michael, A., Malanchuk, O., Eccles, J., engagement: The role of race and ethnicity. the United States. Journal of Adolescent
& Sameroff, A. (2001). Parents’ roles in shap- Sociology of Education, 74, 318–340. Health, 36, 279–288.
ing early adolescents’ occupational aspira- Jolles, D., Kleibeuker, S., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Jones, R. K., Purcell, A., Singh, S., & Finer, L. B.
tions. Child Development, 72, 1247–1265. Crone, E. A. (2011). Developmental differ- (2005). Adolescents’ reports of parental
John, O., Caspi, A., Robins, R., Moffitt, T., & ences in prefrontal activation during work- knowledge of adolescents’ use of sexual
Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). The “Little ing memory maintenance and manipulation health services and their reactions to man-
Five”: Exploring the nomological network of for different memory loads. Developmental dated parental notification for prescription
the five-factor model of personality in ado- Science, 14, 713–724. contraception. Journal of the American Medi-
lescent boys. Child Development, 65, 160–178. Jones, D. C., Vigfusdottir, T. H., & Lee, Y. (2004). cal Association, 293, 340–348.
Johnson, C. A., Xiao, L., Palmer, P., Sun, P., Wang, Body image and the appearance culture Jonkman, K., Trautwein, U., & Lüdtke, O. (2009).
Q., Wei, Y., . . . Bechara, A. (2008). Affective among adolescent girls and boys: An Social dominance in adolescence: The mod-
decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunc- examination of friend conversations, peer erating role of the classroom context and
tional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed criticism, appearance magazines, and the in- behavioral heterogeneity. Child Develop-
in 10th grade Chinese adolescent binge drink- ternalization of appearance ideals. Journal of ment, 80, 338–355.
ers. Neuropsychologia, 46, 714–726. Adolescent Research, 19, 323–339. Jordan, A., & Cole, D. (1996). Relation of depres-
Johnson, E., Roth, T., Schultz, L., & Breslau, N. Jones, D. J., Forehand, R., Brody, G., & Armistead, sive symptoms to the structure of self-
(2006). Epidemiology of DSM-IV insomnia L. (2002). Psychosocial adjustment of knowledge in childhood. Journal of Abnormal
in adolescence: Lifetime prevalence, chronic- African American children in single-mother Psychology, 105, 530–540.
ity, and emergent gender difference. Pediat- families: A test of three risk models. Journal Jordan, L. C., & Lewis, M. L. (2005). Paternal
rics, 117, 247–256. of Marriage and the Family, 64, 105–115. relationship quality as a protective factor:
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-40 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-40 References

Preventing alcohol use among African cyberspace. Journal of School Health, 78, children and adolescents: Psychological and
American adolescents. Journal of Black Psy- 496–505. neurological perspectives and their implications
chology, 31, 152–171. Juvonen, J., & Murdock, T. (1995). Grade-level for juvenile defendants. Behavioral Sciences &
Jose, P. E., & Brown, I. (2008). When does the differences in the social value of effort: the Law, 27, 173–190.
gender difference in rumination begin? Gen- Implications for self-presentation tactics of Kamenetz, A. (2006, May 30). Take this intern-
der and age differences in the use of rumina- early adolescents. Child Development, 66, ship and shove it. The New York Times, p. A19.
tion by adolescents. Journal of Youth and 1694–1705. Kaminski, J. W., Puddy, R. W., Hall, D. M., Cashman,
Adolescence, 37, 180–192. Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2006). S. Y., Crosby, A. E., & Ortega, L. A. G. (2010).
Joyce T., Kaestner, R., & Colman, S. (2006). Ethnic diversity and perceptions of safety in The relative influence of different domains of
Changes in abortions and births and the urban middle schools. Psychological Science, social connectedness on self-directed violence
Texas parental notification law. New England 17, 393–400. in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Journal of Medicine, 354, 1031–1038. Juvonen, J., Wang, Y., & Espinoza, G. (2011). 39, 460–473.
Joyce, T., & Mocan, N. (1990). The impact of Bullying experiences and compromised Kan, M. L., Cheng, Y.-H. A., Landale, N. S., &
legalized abortion on adolescent childbear- academic performance across middle McHale, S. M. (2010). Longitudinal predic-
ing in New York City. American Journal of school grades. Journal of Early Adolescence, tors of change in number of sexual partners
Public Health, 80, 273–278. 31, 152–173. across adolescence and early adulthood.
Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2000). You don’t bring Kaestle, C. E., Halpern, C. T., Miller, W. C., & Ford, Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 25–31.
me anything but down: Adolescent romance C. A. (2005). Young age at first sexual inter- Kandel, D., Johnson, J., Bird, H., & Canino, G.
and depression. Journal of Health and Social course and sexually transmitted infections (1997). Psychiatric disorders associated with
Behavior, 41, 369–391. in adolescents and young adults. American substance use among children and adoles-
Juang, L., & Alvarez, A. (2010). Discrimination and Journal of Epidemiology, 161, 774–780. cents: Findings from the Methods for the
adjustment among Chinese American adoles- Kaestle, C. E., Morisky, D. E., & Wiley, D. J. Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Men-
cents: Family conflict and family cohesion as (2002). Sexual intercourse and the age differ- tal Disorders (MECA) Study. Journal of Ab-
vulnerability and protective factors. American ence between adolescent females and their normal Child Psychology, 25, 121–132.
Journal of Public Health, 100, 2403–2409. romantic partners. Perspectives on Sexual and Kang, H., Okazaki, S., Abelmann, N., Kim-
Reproductive Health, 34, 304–305. Prieto, C., & Lan, S. (2010). Redeeming im-
Juang, L., Lerner, J., McKinney, J., & von Eye, A.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. migrant parents: How Korean American
(1999). The goodness of fit in autonomy time-
New York: Farrar, Strauss, and Giroux. emerging adults reinterpret their childhood.
table expectations between Asian-American
Journal of Adolescent Research, 25, 441–464.
late adolescents and their parents. Interna- Kail, R. V., & Ferrer, E. (2007). Processing speed
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, in childhood and adolescence: Longitudinal Kantor, H., & Brenzel, B. (1992). Urban educa-
1023–1048. models for examining developmental change. tion and the “truly disadvantaged”: The his-
Child Development, 78, 1760–1770. torical roots of the contemporary crisis,
Juang, L., Nguyen, H. H., & Lin, Y. (2006). The
1945–1990. Teachers College Record, 94,
ethnic identity, other-group attitudes, and Kakihara, F., Tilton-Weaver, L., Kerr, M., &
278–314.
psychosocial functioning of Asian American Stattin, H. (2010). The relationship of paren-
emerging adults from two contexts. Journal Kao, G. (1999). Psychological well-being and
tal control to youth adjustment: Do youths’
of Adolescent Research, 21, 542–568. educational achievement among immigrant
feelings about their parents play a role? Jour-
youth. In D. Hernandez (Ed.), Children of
Juang, L., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2002). The rela- nal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 1442–1456.
immigrants: Health, adjustment, and public
tionship between adolescent academic capa- Kalakoski, V., & Nurmi, J.-E. (1998). Identity and assistance (pp. 410–477). Washington, DC:
bility beliefs, parenting and school grades. educational transitions: Age differences in National Academy Press.
Journal of Adolescence, 25, 3–18. adolescent exploration and commitment re-
Kao, G., & Tienda, M. (1998). Educational aspi-
Judge, B., & Billick, S. B. (2004). Suicidality in lated to education, occupation, and family.
rations of minority youth. American Journal
adolescence: Review and legal considerations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 8, 29–47.
of Education, 106, 349–384.
Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 22, 681–695. Kalil, A., & Eccles, J. (1998). Does welfare affect
Kaplan, C., Erickson, P., & Juarez-Reyes, M.
Jumping-Eagle, S., Sheeder, J., Kelly, L. S., & Stevens- family processes and adolescent adjustment?
(2002). Acculturation, gender role orienta-
Simon, C. (2008). Association of conventional Child Development, 69, 1597–1613.
tion, and reproductive risk-taking behavior
goals and perceptions of pregnancy with female Kalil, A., & Kunz, J. (2002). Teenage childbearing, among Latina adolescent family planning.
teenagers’ pregnancy avoidance behavior and marital status, and depressive symptoms in Journal of Adolescent Research, 17, 103–121.
attitudes. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproduc- later life. Child Development, 73, 1748–1760.
Karevold, E., Røysamb, E., Ystrom, E., & Mathiesen,
tive Health, 40, 74–80. Kalil, A., & Ziol-Guest, K. (2008). Teacher sup- K. S. (2009). Predictors and pathways from in-
Juonala, M., Magnussen, C., & Berenson, G. port, school goal structures, and teenage fancy to symptoms of anxiety and depression
(2011). Childhood adiposity, adult adiposity, mothers’ school engagement. Youth and Soci- in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology,
and cardiovascular risk factors. New England ety, 39, 524–548. 45, 1051–1060.
Journal of Medicine, 365, 1876–1885. Kalmuss, D., & Namerow, P. (1994). Subsequent Karriker-Jaffe, K., Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T., &
Jussim, L., Eccles, J., & Madon, S. (1996). Social childbearing among teenage mothers: The Suchindran, C. (2008). The development of
perception, social stereotypes, and teacher determinants of a closely spaced second aggression during adolescence: Sex differ-
expectations: Accuracy and the quest for the birth. Family Planning Perspectives, 26, 149– ences in trajectories of physical and social ag-
powerful self-fulfilling prophecy. Advances in 153, 159. gression among youth in rural areas. Journal
Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 281–388. Kaltiala-Heino, R., Koivisto, A.-M., Marttunen, of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1227–1236.
Juvonen, J., Graham, S., & Schuster, M. A. (2003). M., & Fröjd, S. (2011). Pubertal timing and Karriker-Jaffe, K., Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T., &
Bullying among young adolescents: The substance use in middle adolescence: A Suchindran, C. (2009). Sex differences in the
strong, the weak, and the troubled. Pediat- 2-Year follow-up study. Journal of Youth and effects of neighborhood socioeconomic disad-
rics, 112, 1231–1237. Adolescence, 40, 1288–1301. vantage and social organization on rural ado-
Juvonen, J., & Gross, E. F. (2008). Extending Kambam, P., & Thompson, C. (2009). The devel- lescents’ aggression trajectories. American
the school grounds? Bullying experiences in opment of decision-making capacities in Journal of Community Psychology, 43, 189–203.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-41 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-41

Kaslow, N. J., Adamson, L. B., & Collins, M. H. Keijsers, L., Branje, S., Hawk, S., Schwartz, S., Kerr, D., Capaldi, D., Pears, K., & Owen, L.
(2000). A developmental psychopathology Frijns, T., Koot, H., . . . Meuss, W. (2012). (2009). A prospective three-generational
perspective on the cognitive components of Forbidden friends as forbidden fruit: Paren- study of fathers’ constructive parenting:
child and adolescent depression. In A. J. tal supervision of friendships, contact with Influences from family of origin, adolescent
Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller (Eds.), deviant peers, and adolescent delinquency. adjustment, and offspring temperament. De-
Handbook of developmental psychopathology Child Development, 83, 651–656. velopmental Psychology, 45, 1257–1275.
(2nd ed., pp. 491–510). New York: Plenum. Keijsers, L., Branje, S., VanderValk, I., & Meeus, Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Burk, W. (2010). A rein-
Kassin, S. (2008). The psychology of confessions. W. (2010b). Reciprocal effects between pa- terpretation of parental monitoring in longi-
Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 4, rental solicitation, parental control, adoles- tudinal perspective. Journal of Research on
193–217. cent disclosure, and adolescent delinquency. Adolescence, 20, 39–64.
Katainen, S., Raeikkoenen, K., & Keltikangas- Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 88–113. Kessler, R., Avenevoli, S., Costello, E., Georgia-
Jaervinen, L. (1998). Development of tem- Keijsers, L., Frijns, T., Branje, S. J. T., & Meeus, W. des, K., Green, J., . . . Gruber, M. (2012).
perament: Childhood temperament and the (2009). Developmental links of adolescent Prevalence, persistence, and sociodemo-
mother’s childrearing attitudes as predic- disclosure, parental solicitation, and control graphic correlates of DSM-IV disorders in
tors of adolescent temperament in a 9-year with delinquency: Moderation by parental the National Comorbidity Survey Replica-
follow-up study. Journal of Research on Ado- support. Developmental Psychology, 45, tion Adolescent Supplement. Archives of
lescence, 8, 485–509. 1314–1327. General Psychiatry, 69, 372–380.
Katz, E., Blumler, J., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Uses Kellam, S., Ling, X., Merisca, R., Brown, C., & Kett, J. (1977). Rites of passage: Adolescence in
and gratifications research. Public Opinion Ialongo, N. (1998). The effect of the level of America, 1790 to the present. New York: Basic
Quarterly, 37, 509–523. aggression in the first grade classroom on Books.
Katz, M. (1975). The people of Hamilton, Canada the course and malleability of aggressive be- Khoury-Kassibri, M., Benbensihty, R., Astor, R. A.,
West: Family and class in a mid-nineteenth- havior into middle school. Development and & Zeira, A. (2004). The contributions of com-
century city. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- Psychopathology, 10, 165–185. munity, family, and school variables to stu-
versity Press. Keller, J. (2002). Blatant stereotype threat and dent victimization. American Journal of
Kaufman, K., Gregory, W., & Stephan, W. (1990). women’s math performances: Self-handicap- Community Psychology, 34, 187–204.
Maladjustment in statistical minorities ping as a strategic means to cope with obtru- Kiang, L., & Fuligni, A. (2009). Ethnic identity
within ethnically unbalanced classrooms. sive negative performance expectations. Sex and family processes among adolescents
American Journal of Community Psychology, Roles, 47, 193–198. from Latin American, Asian, and European
18, 757–762. Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Schmid, C., Fang, F., & backgrounds. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Kawabata, Y., Alink, L., Tseng, W., van IJzendoorn, Fang, G. (1998). Reasoning about responsibili- cence, 38, 228–241.
M., & Crick, N. (2011). Maternal and paternal ties and obligations in close relationships: A Kiang, L., & Fuligni, A. (2010). Meaning in life as
parenting styles associated with relational ag- comparison across two cultures. Developmental a mediator of ethnic identity and adjustment
gression in children and adolescents: A con- Psychology, 34, 731–741. among adolescents from Latin, Asian, and
ceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Kelly, A., Wall, M., Eisenberg, M., Story, M., & European American backgrounds. Journal of
Developmental Review, 31, 240–278. Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2005). Adolescent Youth and Adolescence, 39, 1253–1264.
Kaye, K. (2009). Changes in the teen birth rate girls with high body satisfaction: Who are Kiang, L., Peterson, J. L., & Thompson, T. L.
from 1991 to 2005 and 2005 to 2006: Assessing they and what can they teach us? Journal of (2011). Ethnic peer preferences among Asian
the role of changes in the teen population. Adolescent Health, 37, 391–396. American adolescents in emerging immi-
Washington, DC: National Campaign to Pre- Kelly, S. (2009). The Black–White gap in mathe- grant communities. Journal of Research on
vent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy. matics course taking. Sociology of Education, Adolescence, 21, 754–761.
Kazis, R. (1993). Improving the transition 82, 47–69. Kiang, L., Witkow, M., Baldelomar, O., & Fuligni,
from school to work in the United States. Keniston, K. (1970). Youth: A “new” stage of life. A. (2010). Change in ethnic identity across
Washington, DC: American Youth Policy Fo- American Scholar, 39, 631–641. the high school years among adolescents
rum, Competitiveness Policy Council, and Kennard, B. D., Silva, S. G., Tonev, S., Rohde, P., with Latin American, Asian, and European
Jobs for the Future. Hughes, J. L., Vitiello, B., . . . March, J. (2009). backgrounds. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Keating, D. (2004). Cognitive and brain develop- Remission and recovery in the treatment for cence, 39, 683–693.
ment. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), adolescents with depression study (TADS): Kiang, L., Yip, T., Gonzales-Backen, M., Witkow,
Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed.). Acute and long-term outcomes. Journal of M., & Fuligni, A. (2006). Ethnic identity and
New York: Wiley. the American Academy of Child and Adoles- the daily psychological well-being of adoles-
Keating, D. (2011). Cognitive development. In B. cent Psychiatry, 48, 186–195. cents from Mexican and Chinese backgrounds.
Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Kenyon, D. B., Rankin, L. A., Koerner, S. S., & Child Development, 77, 1338–1350.
adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 106–114). New York: Dennison, R. P. (2007). What makes an Kidd, S. A. (2004). “The walls were closing in,
Academic Press. adult? Examining descriptions from adoles- and we were trapped”: A qualitative analysis
Keating, D., & Hertzman, C. (Eds.). (2000). cents of divorce. Journal of Youth and Adoles- of street youth suicide. Youth and Society, 36,
Developmental health and the wealth of cence, 36, 813–823. 30–55.
nations: Social, biological, and educational Kercher, A., & Rapee, R. M. (2009). A test of a Kiene, S. M., Barta, W. D., Tennen, H., & Armeli,
dynamics. New York: Guilford Press. cognitive diathesis–stress generation path- S. (2009). Alcohol, helping young adults to
Keel, P. K., & Klump, K. L. (2003). Are eating dis- way in early adolescent depression. Journal of have unprotected sex with casual partners:
orders culture-bound syndromes? Implica- Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 845–855. Findings from a daily diary study of alcohol
tions for conceptualizing their etiology. Kerns, K. (1996). Individual differences in friend- use and sexual behavior. Journal of Adolescent
Psychological Bulletin, 129, 747–769. ship quality: Links to child–mother attachment. Health, 44, 73–80.
Keijsers, L., Branje, S., Frijns, T., Finkenauer, C., In W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup, Kiesner, J. (2002). Depressive symptoms in early
& Meeus, W. (2010a). Gender differences in (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in adolescence: Their relations with classroom
keeping secrets from parents in adolescence. childhood and adolescence (pp. 137–157). New problem behavior and peer status. Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 46, 293–298. York: Cambridge University Press. Research on Adolescence, 12, 463–478.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-42 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-42 References

Kiesner, J., Cadinu, M., Poulin, F., & Bucci, M. Kim, K., Conger, R. D., Lorenz, F. O., & Elder, G. perspective. Journal of Research on Adoles-
(2002). Group identification in early adoles- H., Jr. (2001). Parent–adolescent reciprocity cence, 7, 431–456.
cence: Its relation with peer adjustment and in negative affect and its relation to early Kingston, B., Huizinga, D., & Elliot, D. (2009).
its moderator effect on peer influence. Child adult social development. Developmental A test of social disorganization theory in high-
Development, 73, 196–208. Psychology, 37, 775–790. risk urban neighborhoods. Youth & Society,
Kiesner, J., & Pastore, M. (2005). Differences in Kim, S., & Brody, G. H. (2005). Longitudinal 41, 53–79.
the relations between antisocial behavior pathways to psychological adjustment Kinney, D. (1993). From nerds to normals: The
and peer acceptance across contexts and among Black youth living in single-parent recovery of identity among adolescents from
across adolescence. Child Development, 76, households. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, middle school to high school. Sociology of
1278–1293. 305–313. Education, 66, 21–40.
Kiesner, J., Poulin, F., & Nicotra, E. (2003). Peer Kim, S., & Ge, X. (2000). Parenting practices Kinsfogel, K. M., & Grych, J. H. (2004). Interpa-
relations across contexts: Individual–network and adolescent depressive symptoms in rental conflict and adolescent dating rela-
homophily and network inclusion in and af- Chinese American families. Journal of Family tionships: Integrating cognitive, emotional,
ter school. Child Development, 74, 1328–1343. Psychology, 14, 420–435. and peer influences. Journal of Family Psy-
Killen, M., Henning, A., Kelly, M. C., Crystal, D., Kim, S., Wang, Y., Deng, S., Alvarez, R., & Li, J. chology, 18, 505–515.
& Ruck, M. (2007). Evaluations of interra- (2011). Accent, perpetual foreigner stereo- Kirby, D. (2007). Emerging answers 2007: Re-
cial peer encounters by majority and type, and perceived discrimination as indi- search findings on programs to reduce teen
minority U.S. children and adolescents. In- rect links between English proficiency and pregnancy and sexually transmitted diseases.
ternational Journal of Behavioral Develop- depressive symptoms in Chinese American Washington, DC: National Campaign to Pre-
ment, 31, 491–500. adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 47, vent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy.
Kilpatrick, D., Acierno, R., Saunders, B., Resnick, 289–301.
Kirby, D. (2011). Risky sexual behavior. In
H., Best, C., & Schnurr, P. (2000). Risk factors King, C., Akiyama, M., & Elling, K. (1996). Self- B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclope-
for adolescent substance abuse and depen- perceived competencies and depression dia of adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 264–275). New
dence: Data from a national sample. Journal of among middle school students in Japan and York: Academic Press.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 19–30. the United States. Journal of Early Adoles-
Kirby, D., Coyle, K., & Gould, J. B. (2001). Mani-
Kim, H., & Capaldi, D. (2004). The association of cence, 16, 192–210.
festations of poverty and birthrates among
antisocial behavior and depressive symp- King, J. (2006). Gender equity in higher educa- young teenagers in California zip code areas.
toms between partners and risk for aggres- tion, 2006. Washington: American Council Family Planning Perspectives, 33, 63–69.
sion in romantic relationships. Journal of on Education.
Kirby, D., & Laris, B. A. (2009). Effective curricu-
Family Psychology 18, 82–96. King, K. M., Molina, B. S. G., & Chassin, L. lum-based sex and STD/HIV education pro-
Kim, H., Capaldi, D., & Stoolmiller, M. (2003). (2009). Prospective relations between growth grams for adolescents. Child Development
Depressive symptoms across adolescence and in drinking and familial stressors across ado- Perspectives, 3, 21–29.
young adulthood in men: Predictions from lescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Kirby, J. B. (2006). From single-parent families to
parental and contextual risk factors. Develop- 118, 610–622.
stepfamilies: Is the transition associated with
ment and Psychopathology, 15, 469–495. King, P., Carr, A. & Boiter, C. (2011). Spiritual- adolescent alcohol initiation? Journal of
Kim, I. J., Zane, N., & Hong, S. (2002). Protective ity, religiosity, and youth thriving. In Family Issues, 27, 685–711.
factors against substance use among Asian R. M. Lerner, J. V. Lerner, and J. B. Benson
American youth: A test of the peer cluster (Eds.). Advances in Child Development and Kirk, D. (2009). Unraveling the contextual effects
theory. Journal of Community Psychology, 30, Behavior, Vol (41). Positive Youth Develop- on student suspension and juvenile arrest:
565–584. ment: Research and Applications for Promot- The independent and interdependent influ-
ing Thriving in Adolescence. Amsterdam: ences of school, neighborhood, and family
Kim, J., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Osgood,
Elsevier Press. social controls. Criminology, 47, 479–520.
D. W. (2007). Longitudinal linkages between
sibling relationships and adjustment from King, P., & Furrow, J. (2004). Religion as a Kiselica, M., & Sturmer, P. (1993). Is society giv-
middle childhood through adolescence. De- resource for positive youth development: ing teenage fathers a mixed message? Youth
velopmental Psychology, 43, 960–973. Religion, social capital, and moral outcomes. and Society, 24, 487–501.
Kim, J., McHale, S. M., Osgood, D. W., & Crouter, Developmental Psychology, 40, 703–713. Kistler, M., Rodgers, K., Power, T., Austin, E., &
A. C. (2006). Longitudinal course and family King, P., Ramos, J. & Clardy, C. (2013). Searching Hill, L. (2010). Adolescents and music media:
correlates of sibling relationships from child- for the sacred: Religious and spiritual Toward an involvement-mediational model
hood through adolescence. Child Develop- development among adolescents. In K. I. of consumption and self-concept. Journal of
ment, 77, 1746–1761. Pargament, J. Exline, & J. Jones (Eds.), APA Research on Adolescence, 20, 616–630.
Kim, J. L., & Ward, L. M. (2007). Silence speaks Handbook of Psychology, Religion and Kiuhara, S. A., Graham, S., & Hawken, L. S.
volumes: Parental sexual communication Spirituality, Washington D.C.: American (2009). Teaching writing to high school stu-
among Asian American emerging adults. Psychological Association. dents: A national survey. Journal of Educa-
Journal of Adolescent Research, 22, 3–31. King, P., & Roeser, R. (2009). Religion and tional Psychology, 101, 136–160.
Kim, J.-I., Schallert, D. L., & Kim, M. (2010). An spirituality in adolescent development. In Klaczynski, P. (2000). Motivated scientific rea-
integrative cultural view of achievement mo- R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of soning biases, epistemological beliefs, and
tivation: Parental and classroom predictors adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. theory polarization: A two-process approach
of children’s goal orientations when learning 435–478). New York: Wiley. to adolescent cognition. Child Development,
mathematics in Korea. Journal of Educational King, V. (2006). The antecedents and conse- 71, 1347–1366.
Psychology, 102, 418–437. quences of adolescents’ relationships with Klaczynski, P. (2001). Analytic and heuristic pro-
Kim, K., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Lorenz, stepfathers and nonresident fathers. Journal cessing influences on adolescent reasoning
F. O. (2003). Reciprocal influences between of Marriage and Family, 68, 910–928. and decision making. Child Development, 72,
stressful life events and adolescent internal- King, V., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Whitbeck, L. B. 844–861.
izing and externalizing problems. Child De- (1997). Religious involvement among ru- Klaczynski, P., & Narasimham, G. (1998). De-
velopment, 74, 127–143. ral youth: An ecological and life-course velopment of scientific reasoning biases:
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-43 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-43

Cognitive versus ego-protective explanations. Knecht, A., Burk, W., Weesie, J., & Steglich, C. adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
Developmental Psychology, 34, 175–187. (2011). Friendship and alcohol use in early cal Psychology, 79, 458–469.
Klahr, A., McGue, M., Iacono, W., & Burt, S. adolescence: A multilevel social network Kogan, S. M., Brody, G. H., Gibbons, F. X.,
(2011a). The association between parent– approach. Journal of Research on Adoles- Chen, Y.-F., Grange, C. M., Simons, R. L., . . .
child conflict and adolescent conduct prob- cence, 21, 475–487. Cutrona, C. (2011). Mechanisms of family
lems over time: Results from a longitudinal Knoester, C., & Haynie, D. L. (2005). Commu- impact on African American adolescents’
adoption study. Journal of Abnormal Psy- nity context, social integration into family, HIV-related behavior. Journal of Research on
chology, 120, 46–56. and youth violence. Journal of Marriage and Adolescence, 21, 361–375.
Klahr, A., Rueter, M., McGue, M., Iacono, W., & the Family, 67, 767. Kohler, J. K., Grotevant, H. D., & McRoy, R. G.
Burt, S. (2011b). The relationship between Knoester, C., Haynie, D. L., & Stephens, C. M. (2002). Adopted adolescents’ preoccupation
parent–child conflict and adolescent antiso- (2006). Parenting practices and adolescents’ with adoption: The impact on adoptive fam-
cial behavior: Confirming shared environ- friendship networks. Journal of Marriage and ily relationships. Journal of Marriage and
mental mediation. Journal of Abnormal Child Family, 68, 1247–1260. Family, 64, 93–104.
Psychology, 39, 683–694. Ko, L., & Perreira, K. (2010). “It turned my world Kohler, P. K., Manhart, L. E., & Lafferty, W. E.
Klassen, R. M., Ang, R. P., Chong, W. H., upside down”: Latino youths’ perspectives (2008). Abstinence-only and comprehensive
Krawchuk, L. L., Huan, V. S., Wong, I., & on immigration. Journal of Adolescent Re- sex education and the initiation of sexual ac-
Yeo, L. S. (2009). A cross-cultural study of search, 25, 465–493. tivity and teen pregnancy. Journal of Adoles-
adolescent procrastination. Journal of Re- Kobak, R., Cole, H., Ferenz-Gillies, R., Fleming, cent Health, 42, 344–351.
search on Adolescence, 19, 799–811. W., & Gamble, W. (1993). Attachment and Kohls, G., Peltzer, J., Herpertz-Dahlman, B., &
Klein, J., & Cornell, D. (2010). Is the link between emotion regulation during mother–teen Konrad, K. (2009). Differential effects of social
large high schools and student victimization problem-solving: A control theory analysis. and non-social reward on response inhibition
an illusion? Journal of Educational Psychol- Child Development, 64, 231–245. in children and adolescents. Developmental
ogy, 102, 933–946. Kobak, R., & Madsen, S. (2011). Attachment. In Science, 12, 614–625.
Klepinger, D., Lundberg, S., & Plotnick, R. (1995). B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia
Kohn, M. (1977). Class and conformity (2nd ed.).
Adolescent fertility and the educational at- of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 18–24). New York:
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
tainment of young women. Family Planning Academic Press.
Perspectives, 27, 23–28. Kokko, K., Pulkkinen, L., Huesmann, L. R.,
Kobak, R., Zajac, K., & Smith, C. (2009). Adoles-
Dubow, E. F., & Boxer, P. (2009). Intensity of
Klerman, L. (1993). The influence of economic cent attachment and trajectories of hostile–
aggression in childhood as a predictor of dif-
factors on health-related behaviors in impulsive behavior: Implications for the
ferent forms of adult aggression: A two-
adolescents. In S. Millstein, A. Petersen, & development of personality disorders. Devel-
country (Finland and the United States)
E. Nightingale (Eds.), Promoting the health opment and Psychopathology, 21, 839.
analysis. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
of adolescents: New directions for the twenty- Kochel, K., Ladd, G., & Rudolph, K. (2012). 19, 9–34.
first century (pp. 38–57). New York: Oxford Longitudinal associations among youth de-
University Press. Kolburn Kowal, A., & Blinn-Pike, L. (2004). Sib-
pressive symptoms, peer victimization, and
ling influences on adolescents’ attitudes to-
Kliewer, W., Murrelle, L., Prom, E., Ramirez, M., low peer acceptance: An interpersonal pro-
ward safe sex practices. Family Relations, 53,
Obando, P., . . . Sandi, L., (2006). Violence cess perspective. Child Development, 83,
377–384.
exposure and drug use in Central American 637–650.
youth: Family cohesion and parental moni- Koenig, L., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2008). Konijn, E. A., Nije Bijvank, M., & Bushman, B.
toring as protective factors. Journal of Re- Stability and change in religiousness during J. (2007). I wish I were a warrior: The role
search on Adolescence, 16, 455–478. emerging adulthood. Developmental Psychol- of wishful identification in the effects of
Klimstra, T., Hale, W., Raaijmakers, Q., Branje, ogy, 44, 532–543. violent video games on aggression in ado-
S., & Meeus, W. (2009). Maturation of per- lescent boys. Developmental Psychology, 43,
Koerner, S., Jacobs, S., & Raymond, M. (2000).
sonality in adolescence. Journal of Personality 1038–1044.
When mothers turn to their adolescent
and Social Psychology, 96, 898–912. daughters: Predicting daughters’ vulnerabil- Kort-Butler, L. A., & Hagewen, K. J. (2011).
Klimstra, T., Hale, W., Raaijmakers, Q., Branje, ity to negative adjustment outcomes. Family School-based extracurricular activity in-
S., & Meeus, W. (2010). Identity formation in Relations, 49, 301–309. volvement and adolescent self-esteem: A
adolescence: Change or stability? Journal of growth-curve analysis. Journal of Youth and
Koerner, S., Korn, M., Dennison, R., & Witthoft,
Youth and Adolescence, 39, 150–162. Adolescence, 40, 568–581.
S. (2011). Future money-related worries
Kling, J. R., Ludwig, J., & Katz, L. F. (2005). among adolescents after divorce. Journal of Kosciw, J. G., Greytak, E. A., & Diaz, E. M. (2009).
Neighborhood effects on crime for female Adolescent Research, 26, 299–317. Who, what, where, when, and why: Demo-
and male youth: Evidence from a random- Koerner, S., Wallace, S. R., Lehman, S. J., Lee, S., graphic and ecological factors contributing
ized housing voucher experiment. Quarterly & Escalante, K. A. (2004). Sensitive mother- to hostile school climate for lesbian, gay,
Journal of Economics, 120, 87–130. to-adolescent disclosures after divorce: Is the bisexual, and transgender youth. Journal of
experience of sons different from that of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 976–988.
Klingberg, T. (2006). Development of a supe-
rior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo- daughters? Journal of Family Psychology, 18, Kowal, A., & Kramer, L. (1997). Children’s un-
spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia, 46–57. derstanding of parental differential treat-
44, 2171–2177. Koff, E., & Rierdan, J. (1996). Premenarcheal ex- ment. Child Development, 68, 113–126.
Klomek, A. B., Marrocco, F., Kleinman, M., pectations and postmenarcheal experiences Kowaleski-Jones, L. (2000). Staying out of trou-
Schonfeld, I. S., & Gould, M. S. (2007). Bully- of positive and negative menstrual related ble: Community resources and problem be-
ing, depression, and suicidality in adoles- changes. Journal of Adolescent Health, 18, havior among high-risk adolescents. Journal
cents. Journal of the American Academy of 286–291. of Marriage and the Family, 62, 449–464.
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 40–49. Kofler, M. J., McCart, M. R., Zajac, K., Ruggiero, Kowaleski-Jones, L., & Dunifon, R. (2006). Fam-
Knafo, A., & Schwartz, S. H. (2003). Parenting and K. J., Saunders, B. E., & Kilpatrick, D. G. ily structure and community context: Evalu-
adolescents’ accuracy in perceiving parental (2011). Depression and delinquency covaria- ating influences on adolescent outcomes.
values. Child Development, 74, 595–611. tion in an accelerated longitudinal sample of Youth and Society, 38, 110–130.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-44 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-44 References

Kracke, B. (2002). The role of personality, parents, Kuperminc, G., Blatt, S., Shahar, B., Henrich, C., & disasters and terrorism: What is the evidence?
and peers in adolescents career exploration. Leadbeater, B. (2004). Cultural equivalence Child Development Perspectives, 3, 4–10.
Journal of Adolescence, 25, 19–30. and cultural variance in longitudinal associa- la Haye, de, K., Robins, G., Mohr, P., & Wilson, C.
Kramer, L., & Conger, K. (2011). Adolescent sib- tions of young adolescents’ self-definition (2011). Homophily and contagion as expla-
ling relations. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein and interpersonal relatedness to psychological nations for weight similarities among ado-
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2. and school adjustment. Journal of Youth and lescent friends. Journal of Adolescent Health,
pp. 1–9). New York: Academic Press. Adolescence, 33, 13–30. 49, 421–427.
Kramer, L., & Kowal, A. K. (2005). Sibling Kuperminc, G., Darnell, A., & Alvarez-Jimenez, Lachman, M. (2004). Development in midlife.
relationship quality from birth to adoles- A. (2008). Parent involvement in the aca- Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 305–331.
cence: The enduring contributions of demic adjustment of Latino middle and Lacourse, R., Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2003).
friends. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, high school youth: Teacher expectations and Developmental trajectories of boys’ delin-
503–511. school belonging as mediators. Journal of quent group membership and facilitation of
Adolescence, 31, 469–483. violent behaviors during adolescence. Devel-
Kreager, D. A. (2004). Strangers in the halls: Iso-
lation and delinquency in social networks. Kupersmidt, J., Burchinal, M., & Patterson, C. opment and Psychopathology, 15, 183–197.
Social Forces, 83, 351–390. (1995). Developmental patterns of child- Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., & Carter, C. S.
hood peer relations as predictors of external- (2007). Development of action monitoring
Kreager, D. A. (2007a). Unnecessary roughness?
izing behavior problems. Development and through adolescence into adulthood: ERP
School sports, peer networks, and male
Psychopathology, 7, 825–843. and source localization. Developmental Sci-
adolescent violence. American Sociological
Review, 72, 705–724. Kurdek, L., & Fine, M. (1993). The relation be- ence, 10, 874–891.
tween family structure and young adoles- Lahey, B., Van Hulle, C. A., D’Onofrio, B. M.,
Kreager, D. A. (2007b). When it’s good to be
cents’ appraisals of family climate and Rodgers, J. L., & Waldman, I. D. (2008). Is
“bad”: Violence and adolescent peer accep-
parenting behavior. Journal of Family Issues, parental knowledge of their adolescent off-
tance. Criminology: An Interdisciplinary Jour-
14, 279–290. spring’s whereabouts and peer associations
nal, 45, 893–923.
Kurdek, L., Fine, M., & Sinclair, R. (1995). School spuriously associated with offspring delin-
Kreager, D., & Haynie, D. (2011). Dangerous liai-
adjustment in sixth graders: Parenting tran- quency? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
sons? Dating and drinking diffusion in ado-
sitions, family climate, and peer norm ef- ogy, 36, 807–823.
lescent peer networks. American Sociological
fects. Child Development, 66, 430–445. Laible, D., Carlo, G., & Raffaelli, M. (2000). The
Review, 76, 737–763.
Kurlychek, M., & Johnson, B. (2010). Juvenility differential relations of parent and peer at-
Krei, M., & Rosenbaum, J. E. (2001). Career and and punishment: Sentencing juveniles in adult
college advice to the forgotten half: What do tachment to adolescent adjustment. Journal
criminal court. Criminology, 48, 725–758. of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 45–59.
counselors and vocational teachers advise?
Kuttler, A. F., & La Greca, A. M. (2004). Linkages Laird, R., Criss, M., Pettit, G., Dodge, K., & Bates,
Teachers College Record, 103, 823–842.
among adolescent girls’ romantic relation- J. (2008). Parents’ monitoring knowledge at-
Kroger, J. (1993). The role of historical context in ships, best friendships, and peer networks. tenuates the link between antisocial friends
the identity formation process of late adoles- Journal of Adolescence, 27, 395–414. and adolescent delinquent behavior. Journal
cence. Youth and Society, 24, 363–376.
Kuttler, A. F., La Greca, A. M., & Prinstein, M. of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 299–310.
Kroger, J. (2003). Identity development during ado- J. (1999). Friendship qualities and social– Laird, R., Marrero, M., & Sentse, M. (2010). Re-
lescence. In G. R. Adams, & M. D. Berzonsky emotional functioning of adolescents visiting parental monitoring: Evidence that
(Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Adolescence (pp. with close, cross-sex friendships. Journal parental solicitation can be effective when
205–226). Malden, MA: Blackwell. of Research on Adolescence, 9 , 339–366. needed most. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Kroger, J., & Green, K. (1996). Events associated Kwak, K. (2003). Adolescents and their parents: cence, 39, 1431–1441.
with identity status change. Journal of Ado- A review of intergenerational family rela- Laird, R., & Marrero, M. (2011). Mothers’ knowl-
lescence, 19, 477–490. tions for immigrant and non-immigrant edge of early adolescents’ activities following
Kuhn, D. (2009). Adolescent thinking. In R. Lerner families. Human Development, 46, 115–136. the middle school transition and pubertal
& L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adoles- Kypri, K., Voas, R. B., Langley, J. D., Stephenson, maturation. Journal of Early Adolescence, 31,
cent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 152–186). S. C. R., Begg, D. J., Tippetts, A. S., & Davie, G. 209–233.
New York: Wiley. (2006). Minimum purchasing age for alcohol Laird, R., Pettit, G., Bates, J., & Dodge, K. (2003).
Kuhn, E. S., & Laird, R. (2011). Individual differ- and traffic crash injuries among 15- to Parents’ monitoring relevant knowledge and
ences in early adolescents’ beliefs in the le- 19-year-olds in New Zealand. American Jour- adolescents’ delinquent behavior: Evidence of
gitimacy of parental authority. Developmental nal of Public Health, 96, 126–131. correlated developmental changes and recipro-
Psychology, 47, 1353–1365. L’Engle, K. L., Brown, J. D., & Kenneavy, K. cal influences. Child Development, 74, 752–768.
Kulis, S., Marsiglia, F., & Hurdle, D. (2003). (2006). The mass media are an important Laird, R., Pettit, G., Dodge, K., & Bates, J. (2005).
Gender identity, ethnicity, acculturation, context for adolescents’ sexual behavior. Peer relationship antecedents of delinquent
and drug use: Exploring differences among Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, 186–192. behavior in late adolescence: Is there evi-
adolescents in the Southwest. Journal of L’Engle, K. L., & Jackson, C. (2008). Socialization dence of demographic group differences in
Community Psychology, 31, 167–188. influences on early adolescents’ cognitive developmental processes? Development and
Kumpfer, K. L., & Alvarado, R. (2003). Family- susceptibility and transition to sexual inter- Psychopathology, 17, 127–144.
strengthening approaches for the prevention course. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Lam, L. T., & Yang, L. (2007). Overweight/obesity
of youth problem behaviors. American Psy- 18, 353–378. and attention deficit and hyperactivity disor-
chologist, 58, 457–465. L’Engle, K. L., Jackson, C., & Brown, J. D. (2006). der tendency among adolescents in China.
Kunkel, D., Eyal, E., Finnerty, K., Biely, E., & Early adolescents’ cognitive susceptibility to International Journal of Obesity, 31, 584–590.
Donnerstein, E. (2005). Sex on TV. Menlo initiating sexual intercourse. Perspectives on Lam, T., Shi, H., Ho, L., Stewart, S. M., & Fan, S.
Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation. Sexual and Reproductive Health, 38, 97–105. (2002). Timing of pubertal maturation and
Kupchik, A., & Ellis, N. (2008). School discipline La Greca, A., & Silverman, W. K. (2009). Treatment heterosexual behavior among Hong Kong
and security: Fair for all students? Youth and and prevention of posttraumatic stress reac- Chinese adolescents. Archives of Sexual Be-
Society, 39, 549–574. tions in children and adolescents exposed to havior, 31, 359–366.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-45 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-45

Lambert, S. F., Herman, K. C., Bynum, M. S., & 22. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, autonomy with connection. Journal of
Ialongo, N. S. (2009). Perceptions of rac- 651–677. Youth and Adolescence, 36, 31–45.
ism and depressive symptoms in African Lansu, T. A. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2011). Peer Larson, R., & Richards, M. (1991). Daily com-
American adolescents: The role of per- status in emerging adulthood: Associations panionship in late childhood and early
ceived academic and social control. Journal of popularity and preference with social adolescence: Changing developmental
of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 519–531. roles and behavior. Journal of Adolescent Re- contexts. Child Development, 62, 284–300.
Lamborn, S., & Nguyen, D. T. (2004). African search, 27, 132–150. Larson, R., & Richards, M. (1994). Family emo-
American adolescents’ perceptions of family Lansu, T. A. M., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Karremans, tions: Do young adolescents and their par-
interactions: Kinship support, parent–child J. C. (2012). Implicit associations with popu- ents experience the same states? Journal of
relationships, and teen adjustment. Journal larity in early adolescence: An approach- Research on Adolescence, 4, 567–583.
of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 547–558. avoidance analysis. Developmental Psychology, Larson, R., & Richards, M. (1998). Waiting for
Lamborn, S., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Emotional 48, 65–75. the weekend: Friday and Saturday night as
autonomy redux: Revisiting Ryan and Lynch. Lanz, M., Scabini, E., Vermulst, A. A., & Gerris, J. the emotional climax of the week. New Di-
Child Development, 64, 483–499. M. (2001). Congruence on child rearing in rections for Child and Adolescent Develop-
Lammers, C., Ireland, M., Resnick, M., & Blum, families with early adolescent and middle ment, Winter, 37–51.
R. (2000). Influences on adolescents’ deci- adolescent children. International Journal of Larson, R., Richards, M., Moneta, G., Holmbeck,
sion to postpone onset of sexual intercourse: Behavioral Development, 25, 133–139. G., & Duckett, E. (1996). Changes in adoles-
A survival analysis of virginity among youths Lapsley, D., Enright, R., & Serlin, R. (1985). cents’ daily interactions with their families from
aged 13 to 18 years. Journal of Adolescent Toward a theoretical perspective on the ages 10 to 18: Disengagement and transforma-
Health, 26, 42–48. legislation of adolescence. Journal of Early tion. Developmental Psychology, 32, 744–754.
Lang, F. & Carstensen, L. (2002). Time counts: Adolescence, 5, 441–466. Larson, R., Richards, M., Sims, B., & Dworkin, J.
Future time perspective, goals, and social Larson, R. (1983). Adolescents’ daily experience (2001). How urban African American young
relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17, with family and friends: Contrasting oppor- adolescents spend their time: Time budgets
125–139. tunity systems. Journal of Marriage and the for locations, activities, and companionship.
Lang, S., Waller, P., & Shope, J. (1996). Adoles- Family, 11, 739–750. American Journal of Community Psychology,
cent driving: Characteristics associated with 29, 565–597.
Larson, R. (1995). Secrets in the bedroom: Ado-
single-vehicle and injury crashes. Journal of Larson, R., & Seepersad, S. (2003). Adolescents’
lescents’ private use of media. Journal of
Safety Research, 27, 241–257. leisure time in the United States: Partying,
Youth and Adolescence, 24, 535–550.
Langer, J. A. (2001). Beating the odds: Teaching sports, and the American experiment. New
Larson, R. (1997). The emergence of solitude as a
middle and high school students to read and Directions for Child and Adolescent Develop-
constructive domain of experience in early
write well. American Educational Research ment, 99, 53–64.
adolescence. Child Development, 68, 80–93.
Journal, 38, 837–880. Larson, R., & Verma, S. (1999). How children
Larson, R. (2000). Toward a psychology of posi-
Langer, L., Zimmerman, R., & Katz, J. (1995). Vir- and adolescents spend time across the world:
tive youth development. American Psycholo-
gins’ expectations and nonvirgins’ reports: Work, play, and developmental opportuni-
gist, 55, 170–183.
How adolescents feel about themselves. Jour- ties. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 701–736.
nal of Adolescent Research, 10, 291–306. Larson, R., & Angus, R. (2011). Adolescents’ de-
Larson, R., Wilson, S., & Rickman, A. (2009). Glo-
velopment of skills for agency in youth pro-
Langhout, R. D., Rhodes, J. E., & Osborne, L. N. balization, societal change, and adolescence
grams: Learning to think strategically. Child
(2004). An exploratory study of youth men- across the world. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg
Development, 82, 277–294.
toring in an urban context: Adolescents’ per- (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd
ceptions of relational styles. Journal of Youth Larson, R., & Brown, J. (2007). Emotional devel- ed., Vol. 2, pp. 590–622). New York: Wiley.
and Adolescence, 33, 293–306. opment in adolescence: What can be learned
Latendresse, S. J., Bates, J. E., Goodnight, J. A.,
from a high school theater program? Child
Lansford, J. (2011). Immigrant issues. In B. Brown Lansford, J. E., Budde, J. P., Goate, A., . . .
Development, 78, 1083–1099.
& M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of ado- Dick, D. (2011). Differential susceptibility to
lescence (Vol. 2, pp. 143–151). New York: Larson, R., Clore, G., & Wood, G. (1999). The adolescent externalizing trajectories: Exam-
Academic Press. emotions of romantic relationships: Do they ining the interplay between CHRM2 and
Lansford, J., Criss, M., Laird, R., Shaw, D., Pettit, wreak havoc on adolescents? In W. Furman, peer group antisocial behavior. Child Devel-
G., Bates, J., & Dodge, K. (2011). Reciprocal B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), Contemporary opment, 82, 1797–1814.
relations between parents’ physical discipline perspectives on adolescent romantic relation-
Lau, J., & Eley, T. (2008). Attributional style as a
and children’s externalizing behavior during ships (pp. 19–49). New York: Cambridge
risk marker of genetic effects for adolescent
middle childhood and adolescence. Develop- University Press.
depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal
ment and Psychopathology, 23, 225–238. Larson, R., & Gillman, S. (1996, March). Daily Psychology, 117, 849–859.
Lansford, J., Killeya-Jones, L. A., Miller, S., & processes in single parent families. Paper pre- Lau, M., Markham, C., Lin, H., Flores, G., &
Costanzo, P. R. (2009). Early adolescents’ so- sented at the biennial meetings of the Society Chacko, M. R. (2009). Dating and sexual at-
cial standing in peer groups: Behavioral cor- for Research on Adolescence, Boston. titudes in Asian-American adolescents. Jour-
relates of stability and change. Journal of Larson, R., Hansen, D. M., & Moneta, G. (2006). nal of Adolescent Research, 24, 91–113.
Youth and Adolescence, 38, 1084–1095. Differing profiles of developmental experi- Laucht, M., Becker, K., Frank, J., Schmidt, M. H.,
Lansford, J., Malone, P., Dodge, K., Crozier, J., ences across types of organized youth activi- Esser, G., Treutlein, J., . . . Schumann, G.
Pettit, G., & Bates, J. (2006). A 12-year pro- ties. Developmental Psychology, 42, 849–863. (2008). Genetic variation in dopamine path-
spective study of patterns of social informa- Larson, R., Moneta, G., Richards, M. H., & Wilson, ways differentially associated with smoking
tion processing problems and externalizing S. (2002). Continuity, stability, and change in progression in adolescence. Journal of the
behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- daily emotional experience across adoles- American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psy-
chology, 34, 715–724. cence. Child Development, 73, 1151–1165. chiatry, 47, 673–681.
Lansford, J., Yu, T., Erath, S., Pettit, G., Bates, J., & Larson, R., Pearce, N., Sullivan, P., & Jarrett, R. Laursen, B. (1993). The perceived impact of
Dodge, K. (2010). Developmental precursors (2007). Participation in youth programs conflict on adolescent relationships. Merrill-
of number of sexual partners from ages 16 to as a catalyst for negotiation of family Palmer Quarterly, 39, 535–550.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-46 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-46 References

Laursen, B. (1995). Conflict and social interac- and internalization of appearance ideals. adolescents transitioning to young adulthood.
tion in adolescent relationships. Journal of Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 59–71. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 414–427.
Research on Adolescence, 5, 55–70. Leadbeater, B. (1996). School outcomes for Lee, J. C., & Staff, J. (2007). When work matters:
Laursen, B. (1996). Closeness and conflict in minority-group adolescent mothers at 28 The varying impact of work intensity on
adolescent peer relationships: Interdepen- to 36 months postpartum: A longitudinal high school dropout. Sociology of Education,
dence with friends and romantic partners. In follow-up. Journal of Research on Adoles- 80, 158–178.
W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup cence, 6, 629–648. Lee, M., & Larson, R. (2000). The Korean “ex-
(Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in Leadbeater, B., Kuperminc, G., Blatt, S., & Hertzog, amination hell”: Long hours of studying,
childhood and adolescence (pp. 186–210). C. (1999). A multivariate model of gender distress, and depression. Journal of Youth and
New York: Cambridge University Press. differences in adolescents’ internalizing and Adolescence, 29, 249–271.
Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (1994). Interper- externalizing problems. Developmental Psy- Lee, S. (2001). More than “model minorities” or
sonal conflict during adolescence. Psycholog- chology, 35, 1268–1282. “delinquents”: A look at Hmong American
ical Bulletin, 115, 197–209. Leahy, E. (2001). Gender differences in mathe- high school students. Harvard Educational
Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (2009). Parent– matical trajectories. Social Forces, 80, 713–732. Review, 71, 505–528.
child relationships during adolescence. In Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2008). Perceived ex- Lee, S. (2011). Deviant peer affiliation and anti-
R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of periences with sexism among adolescent social behavior: Interaction with Mono-
adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. girls. Child Development, 79, 685–704. amine Oxidase A (MAOA) genotype. Journal
3–42). New York: Wiley. of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 321–332.
Leatherdale, S. T., & Papadakis, S. (2011). A
Laursen, B., Coy, K., & Collins, W. A. (1998). Re- multi-level examination of the association Lee, V., & Burkam, D. (1992). Transferring high
considering changes in parent–child conflict between older social models in the school schools: An alternative to dropping out?
across adolescence: A meta-analysis. Child environment and overweight and obesity American Journal of Education, 100, 420–453.
Development, 69, 817–832. among younger students. Journal of Youth Lee, V., & Burkam, D. (2003). Dropping out of
Laursen, B., & DeLay, D. (2011). Parent-child re- and Adolescence, 40, 361–372. high school: The role of school organization
lationship. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein and structure. American Educational Re-
Lebel, C., & Beaulieu, C. (2011). Longitudinal
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. search Journal, 40, 353–393.
development of human brain wiring contin-
233–240). New York: Academic Press.
ues from childhood into adulthood. Journal Lee, V., Burkam, D., Zimiles, H., & Ladewski, B.
Laursen, B., DeLay, D., & Adams, R. E. (2010). of Neuroscience, 31(30), 10937–10947. (1994). Family structure and its effect on be-
Trajectories of perceived support in mother– havioral and emotional problems in young
LeBlanc, A. (2003). Random family: Love, drugs,
adolescent relationships: The poor (quality) adolescents. Journal of Research on Adoles-
trouble, and coming of age in the Bronx. New
get poorer. Developmental Psychology, 46, cence, 4, 405–437.
York: Scribner.
1792–1798.
LeBlanc, L., Swisher, R., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, Lee, V., & Croninger, R. (1994). The relative impor-
Laursen, B., Finkelstein, B. D., & Townsend Betts, tance of home and school in the development
R. E. (2008). High school social climate and
N. (2001). A developmental meta-analysis of of literacy skills for middle-grade students.
antisocial behavior: A 10 year longitudinal
peer conflict resolution. Developmental Re- American Journal of Education, 102, 286–329.
and multilevel study. Journal of Research on
view, 21, 423–449.
Adolescence, 18, 395–419. Lee, V., Croninger, R., Linn, E., & Chen, X.
Laursen, B., Hafen, C., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H.
LeBlanc, M., Self-Brown, S., Shepard, D., & Kelley, (1996). The culture of sexual harassment in
(2012). Friend influence over adolescent
M. (2011). Buffering the effects of violence: secondary schools. American Educational Re-
problem behaviors as a function of relative
communication and problem-solving skills as search Journal, 33, 383–417.
peer acceptance: To be liked is to be emulated.
protective factors for adolescents exposed to Lee, V., & Smith, J. (1993). Effects of school re-
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 88–94.
violence. Journal of Community Psychology, structuring on the achievement and engage-
Laursen, B., & Jensen-Campbell, L. (1999). The 39, 353–367. ment of middle-grade students. Sociology of
nature and functions of social exchange
Lee, B., & Thompson, R. (2009). Examining Education, 66, 164–187.
in adolescent romantic relationships. In
externalizing behavior trajectories of Lee, V., & Smith, J. (1995). Effects of high school
W. Furman, B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.),
youth in group homes: Is there evidence restructuring and size in early gains in
Contemporary perspectives on adolescent ro-
for peer contagion? Journal of Abnormal achievement and engagement. Sociology of
mantic relationships (pp. 50–74). New York:
Child Psychology, 37, 31–44. Education, 68, 241–270.
Cambridge University Press.
Lavoie, F., Hebert, M., Tremblay, R., Vitaro, F., Lee, H., Lee, D., Guo, G., & Harris, K. (2011). Lee, V., & Smith, J. (1996). Collective responsibility
Vezina, L., & McDuff, P. (2002). History of Trends in body mass index in adolescence for learning and its effects on gains in achieve-
family dysfunction and perpetration of dat- and young adulthood in the United States: ment for early secondary school students.
ing violence by adolescent boys: A longitudi- 1959–2002. Journal of Adolescent Health, 49, American Journal of Education, 104, 103–147.
nal study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 601–608. Lee, V., & Smith, J. (1997). High school size: Which
375–383. Lee, J. (2008). “A Kotex and a Smile”: Mothers works best, and for whom? Educational Evalu-
LaVoie, J. (1994). Identity in adolescence: Issues and daughters at menarche. Journal of Family ation and Policy Analysis, 19, 205–227.
of theory, structure, and transition. Journal Issues, 29, 1325–1347. Lee, V., Smith, J., & Croninger, R. (1997). How
of Adolescence, 17, 17–28. Lee, J. (2010). Tripartite growth trajectories of high school organization influences the eq-
Lawford, H., Pratt, M. W., Hunsberger, B., & reading and math achievement: Tracking na- uitable distribution of learning in mathe-
Pancer, S. M. (2005). Adolescent generativ- tional academic progress at primary, middle, matics and science. Sociology of Education,
ity: A longitudinal study of two possible and high school levels. American Educational 70, 128–150.
contexts for learning concern for future gen- Research Journal, 47, 800–832. Leech, T. G. J., & Dias, J. J. (2012). Risky sexual
erations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Lee, J., Edwards, K., Menson, R., & Rawls, A. behavior: A race-specific social consequence
15, 261–273. (2011). The college completion agenda: 2011 of obesity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Lawler, M., & Nixon, E. (2011). Body dissatisfac- progress report. New York: The College Board. 41, 41–52.
tion among adolescent boys and girls: The Lee, J., & Hahm, H. C. (2010). Acculturation Leets, L., & Sunwolf. (2005). Adolescent rules for
effects of body mass, peer appearance culture and sexual risk behaviors among Latina social exclusion: When is it fair to exclude
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-47 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-47

someone else? Journal of Moral Education, Lerner, J., Phelps, E., Forman, Y., & Bowers, E. Levine, J. A., Emery, C. R., & Pollack, H. (2007).
34, 343–362. (2009). Positive youth development. In The well-being of children born to teen
Lefkowitz, E. (2005). “Things have gotten bet- R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69,
ter”: Developmental changes among emerg- adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 105–122.
ing adults after the transition to university. 524–558). New York: Wiley. Levine, M., & Harrison, K. (2004). Media’s role
Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 40–63. Lerner, R., Lerner, J. V., Almerigi, J. B., Theokas, in the perpetuation and prevention of neg-
Lefkowitz, E., Boone, T. L., Sigman, M., & Kit-fong C., Phelps, E., Gestsdottir, S., . . . von Eye, A. ative body image and disordered eating. In
Au, T. (2002). He said, she said: Gender differ- (2005). Positive youth development, partici- J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of eating
ences in mother–adolescent conversations pation in community youth development disorders and obesity (pp. 695–717). New
about sexuality. Journal of Research on Adoles- programs, and community contributions of York: John Wiley.
cence, 12(2), 217–242. fifth-grade adolescents findings from the Levine, M., Smolak, L., & Hayden, H. (1994).
Lefkowitz, E., Romo, L., Corona, R., Au, T., & first wave of the 4-H study of positive youth The relation of sociocultural factors to eat-
Sigman, M. (2000). How Latino American development. Journal of Early Adolescence, ing attitudes and behaviors among middle
and European American adolescents dis- 25, 17–71. school girls. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14,
cuss conflicts, sexuality, and AIDS with Lerner, R., & Steinberg, L. (Eds.) (2009). Hand- 471–490.
their mothers. Developmental Psychology, book of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.). New Levine, P. (2001). The sexual activity and birth
36, 315–325. York: Wiley. control use of American teenagers. In J. Gruber
Lefkowitz, E., Sigman, M., & Au, T. (2000). Lerner, R., von Eye, A., Lerner, J. V., Lewin-Bizan, (Ed.), Risky behavior among youths: An
Helping mothers discuss sexuality and AIDS S., & Bowers, E. P. (2010). Special issue intro- economic analysis (pp. 167–218). Chicago:
with adolescents. Child Development, 71, duction: The meaning and measurement of University of Chicago Press.
1383–1394. thriving: A view of the issues. Journal of
Levinson, R., Jaccard, J., & Beamer, L. (1995).
Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., & Pelletier, L. Youth and Adolescence, 39, 707–719.
Older adolescents’ engagement in casual sex:
(2006). Why do high school students lack Lesane-Brown, C. (2006). A review of race so- Impact of risk perception and psychosocial
motivation in the classroom? Toward an un- cialization within Black families. Develop- motivations. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
derstanding of academic amotivation and mental Review, 26, 400–426. cence, 24, 349–364.
the role of social support. Journal of Educa- Lesko, N. (1996). Denaturalizing adolescence: Levitt, M., Guacci-Franci, N., & Levitt, J. (1993).
tional Psychology, 98, 567–582. The politics of contemporary representa- Convoys of social support in childhood and
Leitenberg, H., & Saltzman, H. (2000). A state- tions. Youth and Society, 28, 139–161. early adolescence: Structure and function.
wide survey of age at first intercourse for Lesthaeghe, R., & Neidert, L. (2006). The “Second Developmental Psychology, 29, 811–818.
adolescent females and age of their male Demographic Transition” in the U.S.: Spatial Levitt, S., & Dubner, S. (2005). Freakonomics.
partners: Relation to other risk behaviors patterns and correlates. PSC Research Report New York: William Morrow.
and statutory rape implications. Archives of No. 06-592. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory and social science.
Sexual Behavior, 29, 203–215. Research, University of Michigan.
New York: Harper & Row.
Lekes, N., Gingras, I., Philippe, F. L., Koestner, Leventhal, H., & Keeshan, P. (1993). Promoting
R., & Fang, J. (2010). Parental autonomy- healthy alternatives to substance abuse. In Lewin, T. (July 9, 2006). At colleges, women are
support, intrinsic life goals, and well-being S. Millstein, A. Petersen, & E. Nightingale leaving men in the dust. The New York Times,
among adolescents in China and North (Eds.), Promoting the health of adolescents: pp. 1ff.
America. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, New directions for the twenty-first century (pp. Lewin-Bizan, S., Bowers, E., & Lerner, R. (2010).
39, 858–869. 260–284). New York: Oxford University Press. One good thing leads to another: Cascades of
Lengua, L. J. (2006). Growth in temperament Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). Diver- positive youth development among Ameri-
and parenting as predictors of adjustment sity in developmental trajectories across can adolescents. Development and Psychopa-
during children’s transition to adolescence. adolescence: Neighborhood influences. In thology, 22, 759–770.
Developmental Psychology, 42, 819–832. R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of Lewinsohn, P., Gotlib, I., & Seeley, J. (1997).
Lenhart, A. (2012). Teens, smartphones, and adolescent psychology. New York: Wiley. Depression-related psychosocial variables:
texting. Washington: Pew Research Center. Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2011). Changes Are they specific to depression in adolescents?
in neighborhood poverty from 1990 to 2000 Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 365–375.
Lenhart, A., Madden, M., Smith, A., Purcell, K.,
Zickuhr, K., & Rainie, L. (2011). Teens, kind- and youth’s problem behaviors. Developmen- Lewinsohn, P., Joiner, T. E., Jr., & Rohde, P.
ness and cruelty on social network sites. tal Psychology, 47, 1680–1698. (2001). Evaluation of cognitive diathesis–
Washington: Pew Research Center. Leventhal, T., Dupéré, V., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). stress models in predicting major depressive
Lenhart, L., & Rabiner, D. (1995). An integrative Neighborhood influences on adolescent devel- disorder in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal
approach to the study of social competence opment. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Psychology, 110, 203–215.
in adolescence. Development and Psychopa- Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. Lewinsohn, P., Pettit, J., Joiner, T., Jr., & Seeley, J.
thology, 7, 543–561. 2, pp. 411–443). New York: Wiley. (2003). The symptomatic expression of ma-
Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). The chang- Leventhal, T., Fauth, R. C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. jor depressive disorder in adolescents and
ing impact of genes and environment on (2005). Neighborhood poverty and public young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
brain development during childhood and policy: A 5-year follow-up of children’s edu- ogy, 112, 244–252.
adolescence: Initial findings from a neuroim- cational outcomes in the New York City Lewinsohn, P., Roberts, R., Seeley, J., Rohde, P.,
aging study of pediatric twins. Development Moving to Opportunity demonstration. Gotlib, I., & Hops, H. (1994). Adolescent
and Psychopathology, 20, 1161–1175. Developmental Psychology, 41, 933–952. psychopathology, II: Psychosocial risk fac-
Leon, G., Fulkerson, J. A., Perry, C. L., Keel, P. K., Levesque, R. (1993). The romantic experience of tors for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
& Klump, K. L. (1999). Three to four year adolescents in satisfying love relationships. chology, 103, 302–325.
prospective evaluation of personality and Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 219–251. Lewinsohn, P., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J. (1994). Psy-
behavioral risk factors for later disordered Levin, M., Xu, X., & Bartkowski, J. (2002). Sea- chosocial risk factors for future adolescent
eating in adolescent girls and boys. Journal of sonality of sexual debut. Journal of Marriage suicide attempts. Journal of Consulting and
Youth and Adolescence, 28, 181–196. and the Family, 64, 871–884. Clinical Psychology, 62, 297–305.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-48 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-48 References

Lewinsohn, P., Rohde, P., Seeley, J., & Fischer, S. of attachment to mother and father in late and adolescent depression. Journal of Youth
(1993). Age-cohort changes in the lifetime childhood and early adolescence: Associa- and Adolescence, 35, 479–489.
occurrence of depression and other mental tions with peer relations. Child Development, Liu, X., Kaplan, H., & Risser, W. (1992). Decom-
disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 202–213. posing the reciprocal relationships between
102, 110–120. Lien, L., Sagatun, A., Heyerdahl, S., Søgaard, A. J., academic achievement and general self-
Lewinsohn, P., Rohde, P., Seeley, J., Klein, D., & & Bjertness, E. (2009). Is the relationship be- esteem. Youth and Society, 24, 123–148.
Gotlib, I. (2003). Psychosocial functioning tween smoking and mental health influenced Lobel, T. E., Nov-Krispin, N., Schiller, D., Lobel,
of young adults who have experienced and by other unhealthy lifestyle factors? Results O., & Feldman, A. (2004). Gender discrimi-
recovered from major depressive disorder from a 3-year follow-up study among ado- natory behavior during adolescence and
during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psy- lescents in Oslo, Norway. Journal of Adoles- young adulthood: A developmental analysis.
chology, 112, 353–363. cent Health, 45, 609–617. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 535–
Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Lightwood, J., Bibbins-Domingo, K., Coxson, P., 546.
Attachment over time. Child Development, & Wang, Y. (2009). Forecasting the future Lochman, J., Bierman, K. L., Coie, J. D., Dodge,
71, 707–720. economic burden of current adolescent K. A., Greenberg, M. T., McMahon, R. J., &
Li, D., London, S. J., Liu, J., Lee, W., Jiang, X., Van overweight: An estimate of the coronary Pinderhughes, E. E. (2010). The difficulty of
Den Berg, D., . . . Conti, D. (2011). Associa- heart disease policy model. American Journal maintaining positive intervention effects: A
tion of the calcyon neuron-specific vesicular of Public Health, 99, 2230–2237. look at disruptive behavior, deviant peer re-
protein gene (CALY) with adolescent smok- Lin, W., Cheong, P., Kim, Y., & Jung, J. (2010). lations, and social skills during the middle
ing initiation in China and California. Becoming citizens: Youths’ civic uses of new school years. Journal of Early Adolescence, 30,
American Journal of Epidemiology, 173, media in five digital cities in East Asia. Jour- 593–624.
1039–1048. nal of Adolescent Research, 25, 839–857. Loeber, R., & Burke, J. D. (2011). Developmental
Li, J. (2006). Self in learning: Chinese adoles- Lindberg, L., Jones, R., Santelli, J. (2008). Nonco- pathways in juvenile externalizing and inter-
cents’ goals and sense of agency. Child Devel- ital sexual activities among adolescents. Jour- nalizing problems. Journal of Research on
opment, 77, 482–501. nal of Adolescent Health, 43, 231–238. Adolescence, 21, 34–46.
Li, J. (2009). Forging the future between two Lindberg, S. M., Grabe, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2007). Loeber, R., & Farrington, D. P. (2000). Young
different worlds: Recent Chinese immigrant Gender, pubertal development, and peer children who commit crime: Epidemiology,
adolescents tell their cross-cultural experi- sexual harassment predict objectified body developmental origins, risk factors, early in-
ences. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24, consciousness in early adolescence. Journal terventions, and policy implications. Devel-
477–504. of Research on Adolescence, 17, 723–742. opment and Psychopathology, 12, 737–762.
Li, S. T., Nussbaum, K. M., & Richards, M. H. Linder, J. R., & Collins, W. A. (2005). Parent and Loehlin, J. C., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Reiss, D.
(2007). Risk and protective factors for urban peer predictors of physical aggression and (2005). Genetic and environmental compo-
African American youth. American Journal of conflict management in romantic relation- nents of adolescent adjustment and parental
Community Psychology, 39, 21–35. ships in early adulthood. Journal of Family behavior: A multivariate analysis. Child De-
Li, X., Stanton, B., & Feigelman, S. (2000). Im- Psychology, 19, 252–262. velopment, 76, 1104–1115.
pact of perceived parental monitoring on Linver, M. R., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Kohen, D. E. Loewenson, P. R., Ireland, M., & Resnick, M. D.
adolescent risk behavior over 4 years. Journal (2002). Family processes as pathways from (2004). Primary and secondary sexual absti-
of Adolescent Health, 27, 49–56. income to young children’s development. nence in high school students. Journal of
Li, X., Stanton, B., Pack, R., Harris, C., Cottrell, Developmental Psychology, 38, 719–734. Adolescent Health, 34, 209–215.
L., & Burns, J. (2002). Risk and protective Linver, M. R., Roth, J. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. Loftus, J., & Kelly, B. C. (2012). Short-term sex-
factors associated with gang involvement (2009). Patterns of adolescents’ participa- ual health effects of relationships with sig-
among urban African American adolescents. tion in organized activities: Are sports best nificantly older females on adolescent boys.
Youth and Society, 34, 172–194. when combined with other activities? Devel- Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 195–197.
Li, Y., & Lerner, R. M. (2011). Trajectories opmental Psychology, 45, 354–367. Loftus, J., Kelly, B. C., & Mustillo, S. A. (2011).
of school engagement during adolescence: Lipsey, M. (2009). The primary factors that char- Depressive symptoms among adolescent
Implications for grades, depression, delin- acterize effective interventions with juvenile girls in relationships with older partners:
quency, and substance use. Developmental offenders: A meta-analytic overview. Victims Causes and lasting effects? Journal of Youth
Psychology, 47, 233–247 and Offenders, 4, 124–147. and Adolescence, 40, 800–813.
Li, Y., Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (2010). Per- Little, S. A., & Garber, J. (2004). Interpersonal Lohman, B. J., & Billings, A. (2008). Protective
sonal and ecological assets and academic and achievement orientations and specific and risk factors associated with adolescent
competence in early adolescence: The medi- stressors predict depressive and aggressive boys’ early sexual debut and risky sexual be-
ating role of school engagement. Journal of symptoms. Journal of Adolescent Research, haviors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37,
Youth and Adolescence, 39, 801–815. 19, 63–84. 723–735.
Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A., Kunnen, S. E., & van Little, T. D., Brauner, J., Jones, S. M., Nock, M. K., Lonardo, R. A., Giordano, P. C., Longmore, M.
Geert, P. L. C. (2009). Here we go again: A & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Rethinking aggres- A., & Manning, W. D. (2009). Parents,
dynamic systems perspective on emotional sion: A typological examination of the func- friends, and romantic partners: Enmesh-
rigidity across parent–adolescent conflicts. tions of aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, ment in deviant networks and adolescent
Developmental Psychology, 45, 1364–1375. 49, 343–369. delinquency involvement. Journal of Youth
Liddle, H. A., Rowe, C. L., Dakof, G. A., Hender- Liu, J., Raine, A., Wuerker, A., Venables, P. H., & and Adolescence, 38, 367–383.
son, C. E., & Greenbaum, P. E. (2009). Multidi- Mednick, S. (2009). The association of birth Longest, K. C., & Shanahan, M. J. (2007). Adoles-
mensional family therapy for young adolescent complications and externalizing behavior cent work intensity and substance use: The
substance abuse: Twelve-month outcomes of a in early adolescents: Direct and mediating mediational and moderational roles of par-
randomized controlled trial. Journal of Con- effects. Journal of Research on Adolescence, enting. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69,
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 12–25. 19, 93–111. 703–720.
Lieberman, M., Doyle, A., & Markiewicz, D. Liu, R. X. (2006). Vulnerability to friends’ suicide Longmore, M. A., Eng, A. L., Giordano, P. C.,
(1999). Developmental patterns in security influence: The moderating effects of gender & Manning, W. D. (2009). Parenting and
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-49 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-49

adolescents’ sexual initiation. Journal of and HIV-related sexual behaviors among US Luster, T., & Small, S. (1997). Sexual abuse his-
Marriage and Family, 71, 969–982. high school students: Are they related? Amer- tory and problems in adolescence: Exploring
Longmore, M. A., Manning, W. D., & Giordano, ican Journal of Public Health, 84, 1116–1120. the effects of moderating variables. Journal of
P. C. (2001). Preadolescent parenting strate- Lubienski, S. T., & Lubienski, C. (2006). School Marriage and the Family, 59, 131–142.
gies and teens’ dating and sexual initiation: sector and academic achievement: A multi- Luthar, S. (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A
A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Marriage level analysis of NAEP mathematics data. study of high-risk adolescents. Child Devel-
and the Family, 63, 322–335. American Education Research Journal, 43, opment, 62, 600–616.
Lopez, A. B., Huynh, V. W., & Fuligni, A. J. (2011). 651–698. Luthar, S. (1994, February). Social competence of
A longitudinal study of religious identity and Lucas, S. (1996). Selective attrition in a newly inner-city adolescents: A six-month prospec-
participation during adolescence. Child De- hostile regime: The case of 1980 sopho- tive study. Paper presented at the biennial
velopment, 82, 1297–1309. mores. Social Forces, 75, 511–533. meetings of the Society for Research on Ado-
Lopez, E. M., Wishard, A., Gallimore, R., & Lucas, S., & Berends, M. (2002). Sociodemo- lescence, San Diego.
Rivera, W. (2006). Latino high school stu- graphic diversity, correlated achievement, Luthar, S., & Becker, B. (2002). Privileged but
dents? Perceptions of gangs and crews. Jour- and de facto tracking. Sociology of Education, pressured? A study of affluent youth. Child
nal of Adolescent Research, 21, 299–318. 75, 328–348. Development, 73, 1593–1610.
Lopez, S. (2009). The rigors of life unplugged. Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., Hooper, C. J., & Luthar, S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The
Los Angeles Times, May 6. Yarger, R. S. (2005). The development of construct of resilience: A critical evaluation
nonverbal working memory and executive and guidelines for future work. Child Devel-
Lopez-Larson, M., Anderson, J., Ferguson, M., &
control processes in adolescents. Child De- opment, 71, 543–562.
Yurkelun-Todd, D. A. (2011). Local brain
velopment, 76, 697. Luthar, S., & Goldstein, A. S. (2008). Substance
connectivity and associations with gender
and age. Developmental Cognitive Neurosci- Ludden, A. B. (2011). Engagement in school and use and related behaviors among suburban
ence, 1, 187–197. community civic activities among rural ado- late adolescents: The importance of per-
lescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, ceived parent containment. Development and
Lord, S., Eccles, J., & McCarthy, K. (1994). Sur-
1254–1270. Psychopathology, 20, 591–614.
viving the junior high transition: Family
processes and self-perceptions as protective Ludden, A. B., & Eccles, J. (2007). Psychosocial, Luthar, S., & Latendresse, S. (2005). Comparable
and risk factors. Journal of Early Adolescence, motivational, and contextual profiles of “risks” at the socioeconomic status extremes:
14, 162–199. youth reporting different patterns of sub- Preadolescents’ perceptions of parenting. De-
stance use during adolescence. Journal of Re- velopment and Psychopathology, 17, 207–230.
Loughran, T. A., Mulvey, E. P., Schubert, C. A.,
search on Adolescence, 17, 51–88. Luthar, S., & McMahon, T. (1996). Peer reputa-
Fagan, J., Piquero, A. R., & Losoya, S. H.
Luder, M. T., Pittet, I., Berchtold, A., Akré, C., tion among inner-city adolescents: Structure
(2009). Estimating a dose-response rela-
Michaud, P. A., & Suris, J. C. (2011). Associa- and correlates. Journal of Research on Adoles-
tionship between length of stay and future
tions between online pornography and sex- cence, 6, 581–603.
recidivism in serious juvenile offenders.
ual behavior among adolescents: Myth or Luthar, S., Shoum, K. A., & Brown, P. J. (2006).
Criminology, 47, 699–740.
reality? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, Extracurricular involvement among affluent
Louis, K., & Smith, B. (1992). Breaking the iron 1027–1035. youth: A scapegoat for “ubiquitous achieve-
law of social class: The renewal of teachers’
Ludwig, J., Duncan, G., & Hirschfield, P. (2001). ment pressures”? Developmental Psychology,
professional status and engagement. In
Urban poverty and juvenile crime: Evidence 42, 583–597.
F. Newmann (Ed.), Student engagement and
from a randomized housing-mobility exper- Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., & Soenens, B. (2006). A
achievement in American high schools. New
iment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, developmental contextual perspective on
York: Teachers College Press.
665–679. identity construction in emerging adult-
Loukas, A. (2009). Examining temporal associa-
Luna B., Garver K., Urban, T., Lazar, N., & hood: Change dynamics in commitment
tions between perceived maternal psycho-
Sweeney, J. (2004). Maturation of cognitive formation and commitment evaluation. De-
logical control and early adolescent
processes from late childhood to adulthood. velopmental Psychology, 42, 366–380.
internalizing problems. Journal of Abnormal
Child Development, 75, 1357–1372. Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Wik-
Child Psychology, 37, 1113–1122.
Luo, Q., Urberg, K., & Rao, P. (1995, March). stroem, P. O., Loeber, R., & Novak, S. (2000).
Loukas, A., Ripperger-Suhler, K. G., & Horton,
Selection of best friends among Chinese The interaction between impulsivity and
K. D. (2009). Examining temporal associa-
adolescents. Paper presented at the biennial neighborhood context on offending: The ef-
tions between school connectedness and
meetings of the Society for Research in Child fects of impulsivity are stronger in poorer
early adolescent adjustment. Journal of Youth
Development, Indianapolis. neighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
and Adolescence, 38, 804–812.
Lussier, P., Farrington, D. P., & Moffitt, T. E. ogy, 109, 563–574.
Loukas, A., Roalson, L., & Herrera, D. (2010). (2009). Is the antisocial child father of the Lynam, D. R., Charnigo, R., Moffitt, T. E., Raine,
School connectedness buffers the effects of abusive man? A 40-year prospective longitu- A., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M.
negative family relations and poor effortful dinal study on the developmental anteced- (2009). The stability of psychopathy across
control on early adolescent conduct prob- ents of intimate partner violence. Criminology, adolescence. Development and Psychopathol-
lems. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 47, 741–779. ogy, 21, 1133–1153.
20, 13–22.
Luster, T., Bates, L., Fitzgerald, H., Vandenbelt, M., Lynne-Landsman, S. D., Bradshaw, C. P., &
Lounds Taylor, J. (2009). Midlife impacts of ado- & Key, J. (2000). Factors related to successful Ialongo, N. S. (2010a). Testing a develop-
lescent parenthood. Journal of Family Issues, outcomes among preschool children born to mental cascade model of adolescent sub-
30, 484–510. low-income adolescent mothers. Journal of stance use trajectories and young adult
Loveless, T. (2002). How well are American stu- Marriage and the Family, 62, 133–146. adjustment. Development and Psychopathol-
dents learning? The 2002 Brown Center Re- Luster, T., & McAdoo, H. (1995). Factors related ogy, 22, 933–948.
port on American Education. Washington, to self-esteem among African American Lynne-Landsman, S. D., Graber, J. A., & Andrews,
DC: Brookings Institution. youths: A secondary analysis of the High/ J. A. (2010b). Do trajectories of household
Lowry, R., Holtzman, D., Truman, B., Kann, L., Scope Perry Preschool data. Journal of Re- risk in childhood moderate pubertal
Collins, J., & Kolbe, L. (1994). Substance use search on Adolescence, 5, 451–467. timing effects on substance initiation in
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-50 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-50 References

middle school? Developmental Psychology, alcohol use: Accumulation, dissipation, and Mahoney, J., Vandell, D., Simpkins, S., & Zarrett,
46, 853–868. stability over time. Journal of Personality and N. (2009). Adolescent out-of-school activi-
Ma, H.-M., Du, M.-L., Luo, X.-P., Chen, S.-K., Social Psychology, 90, 911–926. ties. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.),
Liu, L., Chen, R.-M., . . . Chinese Medical Madsen, S. D., & Collins, W. A. (2011). The sa- Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.,
Association (2009). Onset of breast and pu- lience of adolescent romantic experiences for Vol. 2, pp. 228–269). New York: Wiley.
bic hair development and menses in urban romantic relationship qualities in young Maimon, D., & Browning, C. (2010). Unstruc-
Chinese girls. Pediatrics, 124, 269–277. adulthood. Journal of Research on Adoles- tured socializing, collective efficacy, and
Ma, J., Flanders, W., Ward, E., & Jemal, A. (2011). cence, 21, 789–801. violent behavior among urban youth. Crimi-
Body mass index in young adulthood Mager, W., Milich, R., Harris, M. J., & Howard, A. nology, 48, 443–474.
and premature death: analyses of the US Na- (2005). Intervention groups for adolescents Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security
tional Health Interview Survey linked mor- with conduct problems: Is aggregation harmful in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move
tality files. American Journal of Epidemiology, or helpful? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton
174, 934–944. ogy, 33, 349–362. and E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attach-
Määttä, S., Nurmi, J., & Stattin, H. (2007). Magnusson, D., Stattin, H., & Allen, V. (1986). ment theory and research, Monographs of the
Achievement orientations, school adjust- Differential maturation among girls and its Society for Research on Child Development,
ment, and well-being: A longitudinal study. relation to social adjustment in a longitudi- 50(1–2), Serial No. 209, pp. 66–106.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, nal perspective. In P. Baltes, D. Featherman, Malanchuk, O., & Eccles, J. (April, 1999). Deter-
789–812. & R. Lerner (Eds.), Life span development and minants of self-esteem in African-American
Määttä, S., Stattin, H., & Nurmi, J. E. (2002). behavior (Vol. 7). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and White adolescent girls. Paper presented at
Achievement strategies at school: Types and Maharaj, S., & Connolly, J. (1994). Peer network the biennial meetings of the Society for Re-
correlates. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 31–46. composition of acculturated and ethnocul- search on Child Development, Albuquerque.
Maccoby, E. (1990). Gender and relationships: turally affiliated adolescents in a multicul- Maldonado-Molina, M. M., Piquero, A. R.,
A developmental account. American Psychol- tural setting. Journal of Adolescent Research, Jennings, W. G., Bird, H., & Canino, G.
ogist, 45, 513–520. 9, 218–240. (2009). Trajectories of delinquency among
Maccoby, E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization Mahoney, A., Donelly, W. O., Boxer, P., & Lewis, Puerto Rican children and adolescents at two
in the context of the family: Parent–child T. (2003). Marital and severe parent-to- sites. Journal of Research in Crime and Delin-
interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), adolescent physical aggression in clinic- quency, 46, 144–181.
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Social- referred families: Mother and adolescent Males, M., & Chew, K. (1996). The ages of fathers
ization, personality, and social development reports on co-occurrence and links to child in California adolescent births. American
(pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley. behavior problems. Journal of Family Psy- Journal of Public Health, 86, 565–568.
MacDonald, W., & DeMaris, A. (1995). Remar- chology, 17, 3–19. Malone, P. S., Van Eck, K., Flory, K., & Lamis, D.
riage, stepchildren, and marital conflict: Mahoney, J. (2000). School extracurricular A. (2010). A mixture-model approach to
Challenges to the incomplete institutional- activity participation as a moderator in the linking ADHD to adolescent onset of illicit
ization hypothesis. Journal of Marriage and development of antisocial patterns. Child drug use. Developmental Psychology, 46,
the Family, 57, 387–398. Development, 71, 502–516. 1543–1555.
Mackey, E. R., & La Greca, A. M. (2008). Does Mahoney, J., & Cairns, R. (1997). Do extracur- Malone, S. M., Taylor, J., & Marmorstein, N. R.
this make me look fat? Peer crowd and peer ricular activities protect against early (2004). Genetic and environmental influ-
contributions to adolescent girls’ weight school dropout? Developmental Psychology, ences on antisocial behavior and alcohol
control behaviors. Journal of Youth and Ado- 33, 241–253. dependence from adolescence to early adult-
lescence, 37, 1097–1110. Mahoney, J., Larson, R., Eccles, J., & Lord, H. hood. Development and Psychopathology, 16,
Mackey, K., Arnold, M. L., & Pratt, M. W. (2001). (2005). Organized activities as developmen- 943–966.
Adolescents’ stories of decision making in tal contexts for children and adolescents. Malti, T., Killen, M., & Gasser, L. (2012). Social
more and less authoritative families: Repre- In J. Mahoney, R. Larson, & J. Eccles (Eds.), judgments and emotion attributions about
senting the voices of parents in narrative. Organized activities as contexts of develop- exclusion in Switzerland. Child Development,
Journal of Adolescent Research, 16, 243–268. ment (pp. 3–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 83, 697–711.
Macmillan, R., & Hagan, J. (2004). Violence in Mahoney, J., & Parente, M. (2009). Should we Manchikanti Gomez, A. (2011). Testing the cycle
the transition to adulthood: Adolescent vic- care about adolescents who care for them- of violence hypothesis: Child abuse and ado-
timization, education, and socioeconomic selves? What we have learned and what we lescent dating violence as predictors of inti-
attainment in later life. Journal of Research on need to know about youth in self-care. Child mate partner violence in young adulthood.
Adolescence, 14, 127–158. Development Perspectives, 3, 189–195. Youth & Society, 43, 171–192.
Madkour, A. S., Farhat, T., Halpern, C. T., Mahoney, J., Schweder, A. E., & Stattin, H. (2002). Mandara, J., Gaylord-Harden, N., Richards, M.
Godeau, E., & Gabhainn, S. (2010a). Early Structured after-school activities as a mod- H., & Ragsdale, B. L. (2009a). The effects of
adolescent sexual initiation as a problem erator of depressed mood for adolescents changes in racial identity and self-esteem
behavior: A comparative study of five na- with detached relations to their parents. Jour- on changes in African American adoles-
tions. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, nal of Community Psychology, 30, 69–86. cents’ mental health. Child Development, 80,
389–398. Mahoney, J., & Stattin, H. (2000). Leisure activi- 1660–1675.
Madkour, A. S., Farhat, T., Halpern, C. T., ties and adolescent antisocial behavior: The Mandara, J., & Murray, C. B. (2000). Effects of
Godeau, E., & Gabhainn, S. (2010b). Early role of structure and social context. Journal parental marital status, income, and family
adolescent sexual initiation and physical/ of Adolescence, 23, 113–127. functioning on African American adolescent
psychological symptoms: A comparative Mahoney, J., Stattin, H., & Lord, H. (2004). Un- self-esteem. Journal of Family Psychology, 14,
analysis of five nations. Journal of Youth structured youth recreation centre participa- 475–490.
and Adolescence, 39, 1211–1225 tion and antisocial behaviour development: Mandara, J., Varner, F., Greene, N., & Richman, S.
Madon, S., Willard, J., Guyll, M., Trudeau, L., & Selective influences and the moderating role (2009b). Intergenerational family predictors
Spoth, R. (2006). Self-fulfilling prophecy of antisocial peers. International Journal of of the Black–White achievement gap. Journal
effects of mothers’ beliefs on children’s Behavioral Development, 28, 553–560. of Educational Psychology, 101, 867–878.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-51 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-51

Manlove, J. (1998). The influence of high adolescents’ implicitly and explicitly mea- Marsh, P., McFarland, F. C., & Allen, J. P. (2003).
school dropout and school disengagement sured multiethnic identities. Developmental Attachment, autonomy, and multifinality in
on the risk of school-age pregnancy. Journal Psychology, 47, 270–288. adolescent internalizing and risky behavioral
of Research on Adolescence, 8, 187–220. Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in in- symptoms. Development and Psychopathol-
Manlove, J., Logan, C., Moore, K. A., & Ikramul- structional activity: Patterns in the elemen- ogy, 15, 451–467.
lah, E. (2008). Pathways from family religiosity tary, middle, and high school years. American Marshall, S. (1995). Ethnic socialization of
to adolescent sexual activity and contraceptive Educational Research Journal, 37, 153–184. African American children: Implications
use. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Markstrom, C. (2011a). Identity formation of for parenting, identity development, and
Health, 40, 105–117. American Indian adolescents: Local, na- academic achievement. Journal of Youth and
Manlove, J., Mariner, C., & Papillo, A. (2000). Sub- tional, and global considerations. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 377–396.
sequent fertility among teen mothers: Longitu- Research on Adolescence, 21, 519–535. Marshall, W. (1978). Puberty. In F. Faulkner &
dinal analyses of recent national data. Journal Markstrom, C. (2011b). Initiation ceremonies J. Tanner (Eds.), Human growth (Vol. 2, pp.
of Marriage and the Family, 62, 430–448. and rites of passage. In B. Brown & 141–181). New York: Plenum.
Manlove, J., Terry-Humen, E., & Ikramullah, E. M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- Marshall, W., & Tanner, J. (1969). Variations in
(2006). Young teenagers and older sexual cence (Vol. 2, pp. 152–159). New York: the pattern of pubertal change in girls. Ar-
partners: Correlates and consequences for Academic Press. chive of Diseases of Childhood, 44, 130.
males and females. Perspectives on Sexual and Markstrom-Adams, C., & Adams, G. (1995). Marsiglia, F., Kulis, S., & Hecht, M. L. (2001).
Reproductive Health, 38, 197–207. Gender, ethnic group, and grade differences Ethnic labels and ethnic identity as predic-
Mannheim, K. (1952). The problem of genera- in psychosocial functioning during middle tors of drug use among middle school stu-
tions. In K. Mannheim (Ed.), Essays on the adolescence? Journal of Youth and Adoles- dents in the Southwest. Journal of Research
sociology of knowledge. London: Routledge & cence, 24, 397–417. on Adolescence, 11, 21–48.
Kegan Paul. Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. Marston, E. G., Hare, A., & Allen, J. P. (2010).
Manning, W., & Lamb, K. (2003). Adolescent American Psychologist, 41, 954–969. Rejection sensitivity in late adolescence:
well-being in cohabiting, married, and Marsh, H., Chessor, D., Craven, R., & Roche, L. Social and emotional sequelae. Journal of
single-parent families. Journal of Marriage (1995). The effects of gifted and talented Research on Adolescence, 20, 959–982.
and Family, 65, 876–893. programs on academic self-concept: The big Martel, M. M., Pierce, L., Nigg, J. T., Jester, J. M.,
Manning, W., & Landale, N. (1996). Racial and fish strikes again. American Educational Re- Adams, K., Puttler, L. I., . . . Zucker, R. (2009).
ethnic differences in the role of cohabitation search Journal, 32, 285–319. Temperament pathways to childhood dis-
in premarital childbearing. Journal of Mar- Marsh, H., & Hau, K. (2003). Big-fish–little- ruptive behavior and adolescent substance
riage and the Family, 58, 63–77. pond effect on academic self-concept: A abuse: Testing a cascade model. Journal of
Manning, W. D., Giordano, P. C., & Longmore, cross-cultural (26-country) test of the nega- Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 363–373.
M. A. (2006). Hooking up: The relationship tive effects of academically selective schools. Martin, M., Bascoe, S., & Davies, P. (2011). Family
contexts of ‘nonrelationship’ sex. Journal of American Psychologist, 58, 364–376. relationships. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein
Adolescent Research, 21, 459–483. Marsh, H., & Hau, K. (2004). Explaining para- (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2,
Manning, W. D., Longmore, M., & Giordano, P. doxical relationship between academic self- pp. 84–94). New York: Academic Press.
(2005). Adolescents’ involvement in non- concepts and achievements: Cross-cultural Martin, M., McCarthy, B., Conger, R., Gibbons,
romantic sexual activity. Social Science Re- generalizability of the internal/external F., Simons, R., Cutrona, C., & Brody, G.
search, 34, 384–407. frame of reference predictions across 26 (2011). The enduring significance of racism:
Manning, W. D., Longmore, M. A., & Giordano, countries. Journal of Educational Psychology, Discrimination and delinquency among
P. (2007). The changing institution of mar- 96, 56–67. Black American youth. Journal of Research
riage: Adolescents’ expectations to cohabit Marsh, H., & Kleitman, S. (2002). Extracurricu- on Adolescence, 21, 662–676.
and to marry. Journal of Marriage and Fam- lar school activities: The good, the bad, and Martinez, C. R., Jr. (2006). Effects of differential
ily, 69, 559–575. the nonlinear. Harvard Educational Review, family acculturation on Latino adolescent
Manuck, S., Craig, A., Flory, J., Halder, I., & 72, 464–514. substance use. Family Relations, 55, 306–317.
Ferrell, R. (2011). Reported early family en- Marsh, H., & Kleitman, S. (2005). Consequences Martinez, R., & Dukes, R. (1997). The effects of
vironment covaries with menarcheal age as a of employment during high school: Charac- ethnic identity, ethnicity, and gender on ado-
function of polymorphic variation in estro- ter building, subversion of academic goals, lescent well-being. Journal of Youth and Ado-
gen receptor-a. Development and Psychopa- or a threshold? American Educational Re- lescence, 26, 503–516.
thology, 23, 69–83. search Journal, 42, 331–369. Martino, S. C., Elliott, M. N., Corona, R.,
Marcia, J. (1966). Development and validation of Marsh, H., Kong, C., & Hau, K. (2000). Longitudinal Kanouse, D. E., & Schuster, M. A. (2008).
ego identity status. Journal of Personality and multilevel models of the big-fish–little-pond Beyond the “big talk”: The roles of breadth
Social Psychology, 3, 551–558. effect on academic self-concept: Counterbal- and repetition in parent–adolescent com-
Marin, B. V., Kirby, D. B., Hudes, E. S., Coyle, K. ancing contrast and reflected-glory effects in munication about sexual topics. Pediatrics,
K., & Gomez, C. A. (2006). Boyfriends, girl- Hong Kong schools. Journal of Personality and 121, 612–618.
friends, and teenagers’ risk of sexual involve- Social Psychology, 78, 337–349. Marx, D., Ko, S., & Friedman, R. (2009). The
ment. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Marsh, H., Trautwein, U., Ludtke, O., Baumert, “Obama Effect”: How a salient role model
Health, 38, 76–83. J., & Koller, O. (2007). The big-fish–little- reduces race-based performance differences.
Markiewicz, D., Lawford, H., Doyle, A. B., & pond effect: Persistent negative effects of se- Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45,
Haggart, N. (2006). Developmental differ- lective high schools on self-concept after 953–956.
ences in adolescents’ and young adults’ use of graduation. American Education Research Masi, G., Millepiedi, S., Mucci, M., Poll, P.,
mothers, fathers, best friends, and romantic Journal, 44, 631–669. Bertini, N., & Milantoni, L. (2004). Gener-
partners to fulfill attachment needs. Journal Marsh, H., & Yeung, A. (1997). Coursework se- alized anxiety disorder in referred children
of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 127–140. lection: Relations to academic self-concept and adolescents. Journal of the American
Marks, A. K., Patton, F., & Coll, C. G. (2011). Be- and achievement. American Educational Re- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
ing bicultural: A mixed-methods study of search Journal, 34, 691–720. 43, 752–760.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-52 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-52 References

Mason, C., Cauce, A., Gonzales, N., & Hiraga, Y. Matricciani, L., Olds, T., Blunden, S., Rigney, G., McCabe, K., Hough, R., Wood, P. A., & Yeh, M.
(1996). Neither too sweet nor too sour: & Williams, M. (2012). Never enough sleep: (2001). Childhood and adolescent onset
Problem peers, maternal control, and prob- A brief history of sleep recommendations for conduct disorder: A test of the developmen-
lem behavior in African American adoles- children. Pediatrics, 129, 548–556. tal taxonomy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
cents. Child Development, 67, 2115–2130. Matsuba, M. K., & Walker, L. J. (2005). Young chology, 29, 305–316.
Mason, W. (2001). Self-esteem and delinquency adult moral exemplars: The making of self McCarthy, K., Lord, S., Eccles, J., Kalil, A., &
revisited (again): A test of Kaplan’s self- through stories. Journal of Research on Ado- Furstenberg, F., Jr. (1992, March). The impact
derogation theory of delinquency using la- lescence, 15, 275–297. of family management strategies on adoles-
tent growth curve modeling. Journal of Matsueda, R. L., Kreager, D. A., & Huizinga, D. cents in high risk environments. Paper pre-
Youth and Adolescence, 30, 83–102. (2006). Deterring delinquents: A rational sented at the biennial meetings of the Society
Mason, W., & Windle, M. (2002). Reciprocal re- choice model of theft and violence. American for Research on Adolescence, Washington.
lations between adolescent substance use Sociological Review, 71, 95–122. McCarthy, W. J., Mistry, R., Lu, Y., Patel, M.,
and delinquency: A longitudinal latent vari- Matthews, D., & Keating, D. (1995). Domain Zheng, H., & Dietsch, B. (2009). Density of
able analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, specificity and habits of mind: An investiga- tobacco retailers near schools: Effects of to-
111, 63–76. tion of patterns of high-level development. bacco use among students. American Journal
Massoglia, M., & Uggen, C. (2010). Settling Journal of Early Adolescence, 15, 319–343. of Public Health, 99, 2006–2013.
down and aging out: Toward an interaction- Matthews, L. S., & Conger, R. D. (2004). “He did it McClintock, M. (1980). Major gaps in men-
ist theory of desistance and the transition to on purpose!” Family correlates of negative at- strual cycle research: Behavioral and physi-
adulthood. The American Journal of Sociol- tributions about an adolescent sibling. Jour- ological controls in a biological context. In
ogy, 116, 543–582. nal of Research on Adolescence, 14, 257–284. P. Komenich, M. McSweeney, J. Noack, &
Masten, A. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience Mattison, E., & Aber, M. S. (2007). Closing the N. Elder (Eds.), The menstrual cycle (Vol. 2,
processes in development. American Psychol- achievement gap: The association of racial pp. 7–23). New York: Springer.
ogist, 56, 227–238. climate with achievement and behavioral McClintock, M., & Herdt, G. (1996). Rethinking
Masten, A., Burt, K. B., & Roisman, G. I. (2004). outcomes. American Journal of Community puberty: The development of sexual attrac-
Resources and resilience in the transition to Psychology, 40, 1–12. tion. Psychological Sciences, 5, 178–183.
adulthood: Continuity and change. Develop- Matza, L. S., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Glenn, D. McCoy, J. (1996, March). Parents’ involvement in
ment and Psychopathology, 16, 1071–1094. (2001). Adolescents’ perceptions and youths’ peer relationships as a predictor of
Masten, A., Hubbard, J., Gest, S., Tellegen, A., standards of their parents as a function of youths’ later psychological well-being. Paper
Garmezy, N., & Ramirez, M. (1999). Compe- sociometric status. Journal of Research on presented at the biennial meetings of the So-
tence in the context of adversity: Pathways to Adolescence, 11, 245–272. ciety for Research on Adolescence, Boston.
resilience and maladaptation from child- Mayberry, M. L., Espelage, D. L., & Koenig, B. McCrae, R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Terracciano, A.,
hood to late adolescence. Development and (2009). Multilevel modeling of direct effects Parker, W. D., Mills, C. J., De Fruyt, F., &
Psychopathology, 11, 143–169. and interactions of peers, parents, school, Mervielde, I. (2002). Personality trait de-
Masten, A., Miliotis, D., Graham-Bermann, S., and community influences on adolescent velopment from age 12 to age 18: Longitu-
Ramirez, M., & Neemann, J. (1993). Children substance use. Journal of Youth and Adoles- dinal, cross-sectional, and cross-cultural
in homeless families: Risks to mental health cence, 38, 1038–1049. analyses. Journal of Personality and Social
and development. Journal of Consulting and Mayeux, L., Sandstrom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. Psychology, 83, 1456–1468.
Clinical Psychology, 61, 335–343. N. (2008). Is being popular a risky proposi- McCrae, R., & John, O. (1992). An introduction
Masten, A., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., Burt, K. B., tion? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, to the five-factor model and its applications.
Obradovic, J., Riley, J. R., Boelcke-Stennes, 49–74. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215.
K., et al. (2005). Developmental cascades: Mays, D., & Thompson, N. (2009). Alcohol- McCree, D. J., Wingood, G. M., DiClemente, R.,
Linking academic achievement and exter- related risk behaviors and sports participa- Davies, S., & Harrington, K. F. (2003). Religi-
nalizing and internalizing symptoms over tion among adolescents: An analysis of 2005 osity and risky sexual behavior in African-
20 years. Developmental Psychology, 41, youth risk behavior survey data. Journal of American adolescent females. Journal of
733–746. Adolescent Health, 44, 87–89. Adolescent Health, 33, 2–8.
Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Mayseless, O., & Scharf, M. (2007). Adolescents’ McDonald, J. A., Manlove, J., & Ikramullah, E. N.
McNealy, K., Pfeifer, J. H., & Dapretto, M. attachment representations and their capac- (2009). Immigration measures and repro-
(2011). Subgenual anterior cingulate re- ity for intimacy in close relationships. Jour- ductive health among Hispanic youth: Find-
sponses to peer rejection: A marker of ado- nal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 23–50. ings from the National Longitudinal Survey
lescents’ risk for depression. Development Mayseless, O., Scharf, M., & Sholt, M. (2003). of Youth, 1997–2003. Journal of Adolescent
and Psychopathology, 23, 283–292. From authoritative parenting practices to an Health, 44, 14–24.
Masten, C., Eisenberger, N., Pfeifer, J., & Dapre- authoritarian context: Exploring the person– McElhaney, K., Allen, J., Stephenson, J., & Hare, A.
tto, M. (2010). Witnessing peer rejection environment fit. Journal of Research on Ado- (2009). Attachment and autonomy during
during early adolescence: Neural correlates lescence, 13, 427–457. adolescence. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg
of empathy for experiences of social exclu- McAloney, K., McCrystal, P., Percy, A., & (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd
sion. Social Neuroscience, 5, 496–507. McCartan, C. (2009). Damaged youth: prev- ed., Vol. 1, pp. 358–403). New York: Wiley.
Masten, S. V., Foss, R. D., & Marshall, S. W. alence of community violence exposure and McElhaney, K., Antonishak, J., & Allen, J. P.
(2011). Graduated driver licensing and fatal implications for adolescent well-being in (2008). “They like me, they like me not”:
crashes involving 16- to 19-year-old drivers. post-conflict Northern Ireland. Journal of Popularity and adolescents’ perceptions of
JAMA, 306, 1098–1103. Community Psychology, 37, 635–648. acceptance predicting social functioning
Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. (1978). Conti- McBride, C. K., Paikoff, R. L., & Holmbeck, G. N. over time. Child Development, 79, 720–731.
nuity in adaptation in the second year: The (2003). Individual and familial influences on McGloin, J. (2009). Delinquency balance: Revisit-
relationship between quality of attachment the onset of sexual intercourse among urban ing peer influence. Criminology, 47, 439–477.
and later competence. Child Development, African American adolescents. Journal of Con- McGue, M., Elkins, I., Walden, B., & Iacono, W. G.
49, 547–556. sulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 159–167. (2005). Perceptions of the parent–adolescent
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-53 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-53

relationship: A longitudinal investigation. the American Academy of Child & Adoles- violence among early adolescents. Paper pre-
Developmental Psychology, 41, 971–984. cent Psychiatry, 49, 990–1000. sented at the biennial meetings of the Soci-
McGue, M., Sharma, A., & Benson, P. (1996). McLaughlin, K., Hatzenbuehler, M., & Hilt, L. ety for Research in Child Development,
The effects of common rearing on adoles- (2009). Emotion dysregulation as a mecha- Washington, DC.
cent adjustment: Evidence from a U.S. adop- nism linking peer victimization to inter- McMaster, L., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W.
tion cohort. Developmental Psychology, 32, nalizing symptoms in adolescents. Journal (2002). Peer to peer sexual harassment in
604–613. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, early adolescence: A developmental perspec-
McGuire, S., Manke, B., Saudino, K., Reiss, D., 894–904. tive. Development and Psychopathology, 14,
Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin R. (1999). Per- McLean, K. (2005). Late adolescent identity 91–105.
ceived competence and self-worth during ad- development: Narrative meaning making McNaughton Reyes, H. L., Foshee, V. A., Bauer,
olescence: A longitudinal behavioral genetic and memory telling. Developmental Psychol- D. J., & Ennett, S. T. (2011). The role of heavy
study. Child Development, 70, 1283–1296. ogy, 41, 683–691. alcohol use in the developmental process of
McHale, S. M., Corneal, D., Crouter, A., & McLean, K., & Breen, A. V. (2009). Processes and desistance in dating aggression during ado-
Birch, L. (2001). Gender and weight con- content of narrative identity development in lescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
cerns in early and middle adolescence: adolescence: Gender and well-being. Devel- ogy, 39, 239–250.
Links with well-being and family character- opmental Psychology, 45, 702–710. McNeely, C., & Barber, B. (2010). How do parents
istics. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, McLean, K., Breen, A., & Fournier, M. (2010). make adolescents feel loved? Perspectives on
30, 338–348. Constructing the self in early, middle, supportive parenting from adolescents in 12
McHale, S. M., Crouter, A., Kim, J., Burton, L., and late adolescent boys: Narrative identity, cultures. Journal of Adolescent Research, 25,
Davis, K., . . . Dotterer, A. (2006). Mothers’ individuation, and well-being. Journal of 601–631.
and fathers’ racial socialization in African Research on Adolescence, 20, 166–187. McNeely, C., Shew, M. L., Beurhing, T., Sieving,
American families: Implications for youth. McLean, K., & Pratt, M. (2006). Life’s little (and R., Miller, B. C., & Blum, R. (2002). Mothers’
Child Development, 77, 1387–1402. big) lessons: Identity statuses and meaning- influence on the timing of first sex among
McHale, S. M., Crouter, A., & Tucker, C. (2001). making in the turning point narratives of 14- and 15-year olds. Journal of Adolescent
Free time activities in middle childhood: emerging adults. Developmental Psychology, Health, 31, 256–265.
Links with adjustment in early adolescence. 42, 714–722. McNelles, L., & Connolly, J. (1999). Intimacy be-
Child Development, 72, 1764–1778. McLean, K., & Thorne, A. (2003). Late adoles- tween adolescent friends: Age and gender
McHale, S. M., Kim, J., Dotterer, A., Crouter, A., cents’ self-defining memories about rela- differences in intimate affect and intimate
& Booth, A. (2009). The development of tionships. Developmental Psychology, 39, behaviors. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
gendered interests and personality qualities 635–645. 9, 143–159.
from middle childhood through adoles- McLellan, J. A., & Youniss, J. (2003). Two systems McNulty, T., & Bellair, P. (2003). Explaining
cence: A biosocial analysis. Child Develop- of youth service: Determinants of voluntary racial and ethnic differences in serious
ment, 80, 482–495. and required youth community service. adolescent violent behavior. Criminology, 41,
McHale, S. M., Kim, J., Whiteman, S., & Crouter, Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 47–58. 709–748.
A. C. (2004). Links between sex-typed time McLoyd, V., Kaplan, R., Purtell, K., Bagley, E., McQueen, A., Getz, J., & Bray, J. (2003). Accul-
use in middle childhood and gender devel- Hardaway, C., & Smalls, C. (2009). Poverty turation, substance use, and deviant
opment in early adolescence. Developmental and socioeconomic disadvantage in adoles- behavior: Examining separation and family
Psychology, 40, 868–881. cence. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), conflict as mediators. Child Development, 74,
McHale, S., Shanahan, L., Updegraff, K. A., Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., 1737–1750.
Crouter, A., & Booth, A. (2004). Develop- Vol. 2, pp. 444–491). New York: Wiley. Mead, M. (1978). Coming of age in Samoa.
mental and individual differences in girls’ McLoyd, V., Kaplan, R., Purtell, K. M., & New York: Morrow. (Original work pub-
sex-typed activities in middle childhood Huston, A. C. (2011). Assessing the effects lished 1928)
and adolescence. Child Development, 75, of a work-based antipoverty program for Meadows, S. O., Brown, J. S., & Elder, G. H. J.
1575–1593. parents on youth’s future orientation and (2006). Depressive symptoms, stress, and
McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., Kim, J.-Y., & employment Experiences. Child Develop- support: Gendered trajectories from adoles-
Cansler, E. (2009). Cultural orientations, ment, 82, 113–132. cence to young adulthood. Journal of Youth
daily activities, and adjustment in Mexican McMahon, R. J., Witkiewitz, K., Kotler, J. S., & and Adolescence, 35, 93–103.
American youth. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Conduct Problems Prevention Research Measelle, J. R., Stice, E., & Hogansen, J. M.
cence, 38, 627–641. Group. (2010). Predictive validity of callous- (2006). Developmental trajectories of co-
McIsaac, C., Connolly, J., McKenney, K., Pepler, unemotional traits measured in early adoles- occurring depressive, eating, antisocial, and
D., & Craig, W. (2008). Conflict negotiation cence with respect to multiple antisocial substance abuse problems in female adoles-
and autonomy processes in adolescent outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, cents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115,
romantic relationships: An observational 119, 752–763. 524–538.
study of interdependency in boyfriend and McMahon, S., Felix, E., Halpert, J., & Petropoulos, L. Mednick, S., Gabrielli, W., & Hitchings, B. (1987).
girlfriend effects. Journal of Adolescence, 31, (2009). Community violence exposure and Genetic factors in the etiology of criminal
691–707. aggression among urban adolescents: Testing behavior. In S. Mednick, T. Moffitt, & S. Stack
McKinney, C., & Renk, K. (2008). Differential a cognitive mediator model. Journal of Com- (Eds.), The causes of crime: New biological ap-
parenting between mothers and fathers: Im- munity Psychology, 37, 895–910. proaches (pp. 74–91). Cambridge: Cambridge
plications for late adolescents. Journal of McMahon, S., & Watts, R. (2002). Ethnic identity University Press.
Family Issues, 29, 806–827. in urban African American youth: Exploring Meeus, W. (2011). The study of adolescent iden-
McLaughlin, K., Fairbank, J., Gruber, M., links with self-worth, aggression, and other tity formation 2000–2010: A review of longi-
Jones, R., Osofsky, J., Pfefferbaum, B., & psychosocial variables. Journal of Commu- tudinal research. Journal of Research on
Kessler, R. (2010). Trends in serious nity Psychology, 30, 411–431. Adolescence, 21, 75–94.
emotional disturbance among youths McMaster, L., Connolly, J., & Craig, W. (1997, Meeus, W., Iedema, J., & Vollebergh, W. (1999).
exposed to Hurricane Katrina. Journal of March). Sexual harassment and dating Rejoinder: Identity formation re-revisited:
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-54 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-54 References

A rejoinder to Waterman in developmental Mendle, J., Harden, K. P., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Midgley, C., Arunkumar, R., & Urdan T. (1996).
and cross-cultural issues. Developmental Graber, J. A. (2010). Development’s tortoise If I don’t do well tomorrow, there’s a reason:
Review, 19, 480–496. and hare: Pubertal timing, pubertal tempo, Predictors of adolescents’ use of academic
Meeus, W., Van de Schoot, R., Keijsers, L., and depressive symptoms in boys and girls. self-handicapping strategies. Journal of Edu-
Schwartz, S., & Branje, S. (2010). On the pro- Developmental Psychology, 46, 1341–1353. cational Psychology, 88, 423–434.
gression and stability of adolescent identity Mendle, J., Harden, K., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Graber, Midgley, C., Berman, E., & Hicks, L. (1995). Dif-
formation: A five-wave longitudinal study in J. (2012). Peer relationships and depressive ferences between elementary and middle
early-to-middle and middle-to-late adoles- symptomatology in boys at puberty. Develop- school teachers and students: A goal theory
cence. Child Development, 81, 1565–1581. mental Psychology, 48, 429–435. approach. Journal of Early Adolescence, 15,
Meeus, W., Van de Schoot, R., Klimstra, T., & Mendle, J., Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., Van 90–113.
Branje, S. (2011). Personality types in adoles- Hulle, C. A., D’Onofrio, B. M., Brooks- Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. (1988).
cence: Change and stability and links with Gunn, J., . . . Lahey, B. (2009). Associations The transition to junior high school: Beliefs
adjustment and relationships: A five-wave between father absence and age of first sex- of pre- and posttransition teachers. Journal
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychol- ual intercourse. Child Development, 80, of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 543–562.
ogy, 47, 1181–1195. 1463–1480. Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (1995). Predictors of
Mehta, C., & Strough, J. (2009). Sex segregation in Mendle, J., Leve, L., Van Ryzin, M., Natsuaki, M., middle school students’ use of self-handi-
friendships and normative contexts across the & Ge, X. (2011). Associations between early capping strategies. Journal of Early Adoles-
life span. Developmental Review, 29, 201–220. life stress, child maltreatment, and pubertal cence, 15, 389–411.
Meier, A. M. (2003). Adolescents’ transition to development among girls in foster care. Jour- Miech, R. A., Kumanyika, S. K., Stettler, N.,
first intercourse, religiosity, and attitudes nal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 871–880. Link, B. G., Phelan, J. C., & Chang, V. W.
about sex. Social Forces, 81, 1031–1052. Mendle, J., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2007). (2006). Trends in the association of poverty
Meier, A. M. (2007). Adolescent first sex and sub- Detrimental psychological outcomes associ- with overweight among US adolescents,
sequent mental health. American Journal of ated with early pubertal timing in adolescent 1971–2004. JAMA: Journal of the American
Sociology, 112, 1811–1847. girls. Developmental Review, 27, 151–171. Medical Association, 295, 2385–2393.
Meier, M., Slutske, W., Arndt, S., & Cadoret, R. Menning, C. L. (2002). Absent parents are more Mihalic, S., & Elliot, D. (1997). Short- and long-
(2008). Impulsive and callous traits are more than money: The joint effect of activities and term consequences of adolescent work.
strongly associated with delinquent behavior financial support on youths’ educational at- Youth and Society, 28, 464–498.
in higher risk neighborhoods among boys tainment. Journal of Family Issues, 23, 648–671. Mikami, A. Y., Szwedo, D. E., Allen, J. P., Evans,
and girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Menon, M. (2011). Does felt gender compati- M. A., & Hare, A. L. (2010). Adolescent peer
117, 377–385. bility mediate influences of self-perceived relationships and behavior problems predict
Mekos, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D. gender nonconformity on early adolescents’ young adults’ communication on social net-
(1996). Sibling differences in problem behav- psychosocial adjustment? Child Develop- working websites. Developmental Psychology,
ior and parental treatment in nondivorced ment, 82, 1152–1162. 46, 46–56.
and remarried families. Child Development, Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, Miklikowska, M., Duriez, B., & Soenens, B.
67, 2148–2165. S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., . . . Swendsen, J. (2011). Family roots of empathy-related
Melby, J. (1995, March). Early family and peer (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disor- characteristics: The role of perceived
predictors of later adolescent tobacco use. ders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the maternal and paternal need support in
Paper presented at the biennial meetings of National Comorbidity Survey Replication– adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 47,
the Society for Research in Child Develop- Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of 1342–1352.
ment, Indianapolis. the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Milan, S., Ethier, K., Lewis, J., Kershaw, T.,
Melby, J., & Conger, R. (1996). Parental behav- Psychiatry, 49, 980–989. Niccolai, L., & Ickovics, J. (2006). Reproduc-
iors and adolescent academic performance: Merskin, D. (1999). Adolescence, advertising, tive health of urban adolescents: Differences
A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Research and the ideology of menstruation. Sex Roles, in the behaviors, cognitions, and social con-
on Adolescence, 6, 113–137. 40, 941–957. text of African-American and Puerto Rican
Melby, J., Conger, R., Fang, S., Wickrama, K. A. Merten, D. (1997). The meaning of meanness: females. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35,
S., & Conger, K. J. (2008). Adolescent family Popularity, competition and conflict among 959–967.
experiences and educational attainment dur- junior high school girls. Sociology of Educa- Milbrath, C., Ohlson, B., & Eyre, S. L. (2009).
ing early adulthood. Developmental Psychol- tion, 70, 175–191. Analyzing cultural models in adolescent ac-
ogy, 44, 1519–1536. Merten, M., Wickrama, K., & Williams, A. counts of romantic relationships. Journal of
Melde, C., & Esbensen, F. (2011). Gang member- (2008). Adolescent obesity and young adult Research on Adolescence, 19, 313–351.
ship as the turning point in the life course. psychosocial outcomes: Gender and racial Miller, B., & Moore, K. (1990). Adolescent sexual
Criminology, 49, 513–552. differences. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, behavior, pregnancy, and parenting: Re-
Melde, C., Taylor, T., & Esbensen, F. (2009). “I got 37, 1111–1122. search through the 1980s. Journal of Mar-
your back”: An examination of the protective Mesch, G. S., & Talmud, I. (2007). Similarity and riage and the Family, 52, 1025–1044.
function of gang membership in adoles- the quality of online and offline social rela- Miller, B., Benson, B., & Galbraith, K. A. (2001).
cence. Criminology, 47, 565–594. tionships among adolescents in Israel. Jour- Family relationships and adolescent preg-
Mello, Z. R. (2008). Gender variation in develop- nal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 455–466. nancy risk: A research synthesis. Develop-
mental trajectories of educational and occu- Meschke, L. L., Bartholomae, S., & Zentall, S. R. mental Review, 21, 1–38.
pational expectations and attainment from (2000). Adolescent sexuality and parent– Miller, B., Fan, X., Christensen, M., Grotevant,
adolescence to adulthood. Developmental adolescent processes: Promoting healthy teen H. D., & van Dulmen, M. (2000). Compari-
Psychology, 44, 1069–1080. choices. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary sons of adopted and nonadopted adolescents
Mendle, J., & Ferrero, J. (2012). Detrimental Journal of Applied Family Studies, 49, 143–154. in a large, nationally represented sample.
psychological outcomes associated with Mickelson, R. (1990). The attitude–achievement Child Development, 71, 1458–1473.
pubertal timing in adolescent boys. Devel- paradox among Black adolescents. Sociology Miller, B., Norton, M., Curtis, T., Hill, E.,
opmental Review, 32, 49–66. of Education, 63, 44–61. Schvaneveldt, P., & Young, M. (1997). The
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-55 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-55

timing of sexual intercourse among adoles- perception and risk taking. Psychological Sci- adolescent (pp. 93–122). Cambridge, MA:
cents: Family, peer, and other antecedents. ence, 19, 429–433. Harvard University Press.
Youth and Society, 29, 54–83. Millstein, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2002). Modell, J., Furstenberg, F., Jr., & Hershberg, T.
Miller, B., Norton, M., Fan, X., & Christopherson, C. Judgments about risk and perceived invulner- (1976). Social change and transitions to
(1998). Pubertal development, parental ability in adolescents and young adults. Jour- adulthood in historical perspective. Journal
communication, and sexual values in rela- nal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 399–422. of Family History, 1, 7–32.
tion to adolescent sexual behaviors. Journal Milnitsky-Sapiro, C., Turiel, E., & Nucci, L. Moffitt, T. (2006). Life-course persistent versus
of Early Adolescence, 18, 27–52. (2006). Brazilian adolescents’ conceptions of adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In
Miller, D., & Byrnes, J. (1997). The role of contex- autonomy and parental authority. Cognitive D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental
tual and personal factors in children’s risk tak- Development, 21, 317–331. psychopathology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
ing. Developmental Psychology, 33, 814–823. Milot, A., & Ludden, A. B. (2009). The effects of Moffitt, T., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood pre-
Miller, J., & White, N. (2003). Gender and ado- religion and gender on well-being, substance dictors differentiate life-course-persistent
lescent relationship violence: A contextual use, and academic engagement among rural and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways
examination. Criminology, 41, 1207–1248. adolescents. Youth & Society, 40, 403–425. among males and females. Development and
Miller, K., Kotchick, B., Dorsey, S., Forehand, R., Mischel, W. (in press). The marshmallow effect. Psychopathology, 13, 355–375.
& Ham, A. (1998). Family communication New York: Little, Brown. Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., Harkness, A., & Silva, P.
about sex: What are parents saying and are (1993). The natural history of change in in-
Mitchell, C., O’Nell, T., Beals, J., Dick, R., Keane,
their adolescents listening? Family Planning tellectual performance: Who changes? How
E., & Manson, S. (1996). Dimensionality of
Perspectives, 30, 218–222, 235. much? Is it meaningful? Journal of Child Psy-
alcohol use among American Indian adoles-
chology and Psychiatry, 34, 455–506.
Miller, K., Levin, M., Whitaker, D., & Xu, X. cents: Latent structure, construct validity,
(1998). Patterns of condom use among ado- and implications for developmental research. Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., & Milne, B. J.
lescents: The impact of mother–adolescent Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 151–180. (2002). Males on the life-course-persistent
communication. American Journal of Public and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways:
Mitchell, C. M., Whitesell, N. R., Spicer, P., Beals, J.,
Health, 88, 1542–1544. Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and
& Kaufman, C. E. (2007). Cumulative risk for
Psychopathology, 14, 179–207.
Miller, K., & Whitaker, D. (2001). Predictors of early sexual initiation among American Indian
mother–adolescent discussions about con- youth: A discrete-time survival analysis. Jour- Moilanen, K., Crockett, L. J., Raffaelli, M., &
doms: Implications for providers who serve nal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 387–412. Jones, B. L. (2010a). Trajectories of sexual
youth. Pediatrics, 108, E28. risk from middle adolescence to early
Mitchell, E. (Ed.). (1985). Anorexia nervosa and
adulthood. Journal of Research on Adoles-
Miller, K. E., Melnick, M. J., Barnes, G. M., Sabo, bulimia: Diagnosis and treatment. Minneap-
cence, 20, 114–139.
D., & Farrell, M. P. (2007). Athletic involve- olis: University of Minnesota Press.
ment and adolescent delinquency. Journal of Moilanen, K., Shaw, D., & Maxwell K. (2010b).
Mitchell, J., Pate, R., Beets, M., & Nader, P.
Youth and Adolescence, 36, 711–723. Developmental cascades: Externalizing, in-
(2013). Time spent in sedentary behavior
ternalizing, and academic competence from
Miller, L., & Gur, M. (2002). Religiousness and and changes in childhood BMI: A longitudi-
middle childhood to early adolescence. De-
sexual responsibility in adolescent girls. Jour- nal study from ages 9 to 15 years. Interna-
velopment and Psychopathology, 22, 635–653.
nal of Adolescent Health, 31, 401–406. tional Journal of Obesity, 37, 54–60.
Molina, B., & Chassin, L. (1996). The parent–
Miller, L., Davies, M., & Greenwald, S. (2000). Re- Mitchell, K., Booth, A., & King, V. (2009). Ado- adolescent relationship at puberty: Hispanic
ligiosity and substance use and abuse among lescents with nonresident fathers: Are ethnicity and parent alcoholism as modera-
adolescents in the National Comorbidity Sur- daughters more disadvantaged than sons? tors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 675–686.
vey. Journal of the American Academy of Child Journal of Marriage and Family, 71, 650–662.
Mollen, C. J., Barg, F. K., Hayes, K. L., Gotcsik, M.,
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1190–1197. Mitchell, K., Finkelhor, D., Jones, L., & Wolak, J. Blades, N. M., & Schwarz, D. F. (2008). Assess-
Miller, N. (1928). The child in primitive society. (2012). Prevalence and characteristics of ing attitudes about emergency contraception
New York: Brentano’s. youth sexting: A national study. Pediatrics, among urban, minority adolescent girls: An in-
Miller, S., Malone, P. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2010). 129, 13–20. depth interview study. Pediatrics, 122, 395–401.
Developmental trajectories of boys’ and girls’ Mitchell, K., Finkelhor, D., Wolak, J., Ybarra, M., Molloy, L. E., Ram, N., & Gest, S. D. (2011). The
delinquency: Sex differences and links to & Turner, H. (2011). Youth internet victim- storm and stress (or calm) of early adolescent
later adolescent outcomes. Journal of Abnor- ization in a broader victimization context. self-concepts: Within- and between-subjects
mal Child Psychology, 38, 1021–1032. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 128–134. variability. Developmental Psychology, 47,
Miller, S. R., Tserakhava, V., & Miller, C. J. (2011). Mitru, G., Millrood, D. L., & Mateika, J. H. 1589–1607.
“My child is shy and has no friends: What (2002). The impact of sleep on learning and Molnar, B. E., Cerda, M., Roberts, A. L., & Buka,
does parenting have to do with it?” Journal of behavior in adolescents. Teachers College Re- S. L. (2008). Effects of neighborhood re-
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 442–452. cord, 104, 704–726. sources on aggressive and delinquent behav-
Miller-Johnson, S., Winn, D. C., Coie, J. D., Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Fukuda, S., Sasabe, T., iors among urban youths. American Journal
Malone, P. S., & Lochman, J. (2004). Risk fac- Imai-Matsumura, K., & Watanabe, Y. (2011). of Public Health, 98, 1086–1093.
tors for adolescent pregnancy reports among Changes in cognitive functions of students in Monahan, K., Egan, E., Van Horn, M., Arthur, M.,
African American males. Journal of Research the transitional period from elementary & Hawkins, D. (2011). Community-level ef-
on Adolescence 14, 471–495. school to junior high school. Brain Develop- fects of individual and peer risk and protective
Miller-Johnson, S., Winn, D., Coie, J., Maumary- ment, 33, 412–420. factors on adolescent substance use. Journal of
Gremaud, A., Hyman, C., Retty, R., et al. Modecki, K. L. (2008). Addressing gaps in the Community Psychology, 39, 478–498.
(1999). Motherhood during the teen years: maturity of judgment literature: Age differ- Monahan, K., & Lee, J. (2008). Adolescent sexual
A developmental perspective on risk factors ences and delinquency. Law and Human Be- activity: Links between relational context
for childbearing. Development and Psychopa- havior, 32, 78–91. and depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth
thology, 11, 85–100. Modell, J., & Goodman, M. (1990). Historical and Adolescence, 37, 917–927.
Mills, B., Reyna, V., & Estrada, S. (2008). Explain- perspectives. In S. Feldman & G. Elliott Monahan, K., Lee, J., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Re-
ing contradictory relations between risk (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing visiting the negative impact of part-time
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-56 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-56 References

work on adolescent adjustment: Distin- use among adolescents. Journal of Adolescent of Hispanic adolescents varying in migrant
guishing between selection and socialization Health, 36, 486–493. status. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26,
using propensity score matching. Child De- Moore, M. R., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (2001). 233–252.
velopment, 82, 96–112. Sexual intercourse and pregnancy among Af- Mortimer, J. (2003). Working and growing up in
Monahan, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Accentu- rican American girls in high-poverty neigh- America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
ation of individual differences in social borhoods: The role of family and perceived sity Press.
competence during the transition to ado- community environment. Journal of Mar- Mortimer, J., & Johnson, M. (1998). New per-
lescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, riage and the Family, 63, 1146–1157. spectives on adolescent work and the transi-
21, 576–585. Moore, S. (1995). Girls’ understanding and so- tion to adulthood. In R. Jessor & M. Chase
Monahan, K., Steinberg, L., & Cauffman, E. cial construction of menarche. Journal of (Eds.), New perspectives on adolescent risk
(2009). Affiliation with antisocial peers, sus- Adolescence, 18, 87–104. behavior. New York: Cambridge University
ceptibility to peer influence, and desistance Moore, S., & Gullone, E. (1996). Predicting ado- Press.
from antisocial behavior during the transi- lescent risk behavior using a personalized Mortimer, J., & Larson, R. (2002). Adolescence
tion to adulthood. Developmental Psychol- cost-benefit analysis. Journal of Youth and in the 21st century: A worldwide perspec-
ogy, 45, 1520–1530. Adolescence, 25, 343–359. tive. Introduction: Macro societal trends
Monahan, K., Steinberg, L., & Cauffman, E. Moore, W., Pfeifer, J., Masten, C., Iacoboni, M., and the changing experiences of adoles-
(2013). Age differences in the impact of Mazziotta, & Dapretto, M. (2012). Facing cence. In J. Mortimer & R. Larson (Eds.),
employment on antisocial behavior. Child puberty: Associations between pubertal de- The future of adolescent experience: Societal
Development. velopment and neural responses to affective trends and the transition to adulthood.
Monahan, K., Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., & facial displays. Social Cognitive and Affective New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mulvey, E. (2009). Trajectories of antisocial Neuroscience, 7, 35–43. Mortimer, J., Pimentel, E., Ryu, S., Nash, K., &
behavior and psychosocial maturity from Moos, R. (1978). A typology of junior high and Lee, C. (1996). Part-time work and occupa-
adolescence to young adulthood. Develop- high school classrooms. American Educa- tional value formation in adolescence. Social
mental Psychology, 45, 1654–1668. tional Research Journal, 15, 53–66. Forces, 74, 1405–1418.
Monck, E. (1991). Patterns of confiding rela- Moses, A. (1999). Exposure to violence, depression,
Moran, P., Rowe, R., Flach, C., Briskman, J., Ford,
tionships among adolescent girls. Journal and hostility in a sample of inner city high
T., Maughan, B., . . . Goodman, R. (2009).
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, school youth. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 21–32.
Predictive value of callous-unemotional
333–345. traits in a large community sample. Journal Mosher, W., & Bachrach, C. (1996). Understand-
Monk, C., McClure, E., Nelson, E., Zarahn, E., of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- ing U.S. fertility: Continuity and change in
Bilder, R., Leibenluft, E., . . . Pine, D. (2003). cent Psychiatry, 48, 1079–1084. the National Survey of Family Growth, 1988–
Adolescent immaturity in attention-related 1995. Family Planning Perspectives, 28, 4–12.
Morisi, T. (2008). Youth enrollment and employ-
brain engagement to emotional facial ment during the school year. Monthly Labor Moshman, D. (1993). Adolescent reasoning and
expressions. Neuroimage, 20, 420–428. Review, 131, 51–63. adolescent rights. Human Development, 36,
Monroe, S. M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., & 27–40.
Morris, A., Eisenberg, N., & Houltberg, B.
Lewinsohn, P. M. (1999). Life events and Mosteller, F., Light, R., & Sachs, J. (1996). Sus-
(2011). Adolescent moral development. In
depression in adolescence: Relationship loss tained inquiry in education: Lessons from
B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclope-
as a prospective risk factor for first onset of skill grouping and class size. Harvard Educa-
dia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 48–55). New
major depressive disorder. Journal of tional Review, 66, 797–842.
York: Academic Press.
Abnormal Psychology, 108, 606–614. Motl, R. W., McAuley, E., Birnbaum, A. S., &
Morris, A. K., & Sloutsky, V. (1998). Understand-
Montgomery, M. (1996). “The fruit that hangs Lytle, L. A. (2006). Naturally occurring
ing of logical necessity: Developmental ante-
highest”: Courtship and chaperonage in New changes in time spent watching television
cedents and cognitive consequences. Child
York high society, 1880–1920. Journal of are inversely related to frequency of physical
Development, 69, 721–741.
Family History, 21, 172–191. activity during early adolescence. Journal of
Morris, A. K., & Sloutsky, V. (2001). Children’s Adolescence, 29, 19–32.
Montgomery, M. J. (2005). Psychosocial inti- solutions of logical versus empirical prob-
macy and identity: From early adolescence to Mott, F., Fondell, M., Hu, P., Kowaleski-Jones, P.,
lems: What’s missing and what develops? & Menaghan, E. (1996). The determinants of
emerging adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Cognitive Development, 16, 907–928.
Research, 20, 346–374. first sex by age 14 in a high-risk adolescent
Morris, M. C., Ciesla, J. A., & Garber, J. (2010). A population. Family Planning Perspectives, 28,
Montgomery, M. J., & Sorell, G. T. (1998). Love prospective study of stress autonomy versus 13–18.
and dating experience in early and middle stress sensitization in adolescents at varied
adolescence: Grade and gender comparisons. Moua, M., & Lamborn, S. (2010). Hmong
risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psy- American adolescents’ perceptions of ethnic
Journal of Adolescence, 21, 677–689. chology, 119, 341–354. socialization practices. Journal of Adolescent
Moody, J. (2001). Race, school integration, and Morris, N., & Udry, J. (1980). Validation of a self- Research, 25, 416–440.
friendship segregation in America. American administered instrument to assess stage of Mounts, N. (2002). Parental management of ad-
Journal of Sociology, 107, 679–716. adolescent development. Journal of Youth olescent peer relationships in context: The
Moore, K., Myers, D., Morrison, D., Nord, C., and Adolescence, 9, 271–280. role of parenting style. Journal of Family Psy-
Brown, B., & Edmonston, B. (1993). Age at Morrison Gutman, L., Sameroff, A. J., & Eccles, J. chology, 16, 58–69.
first childbirth and later poverty. Journal of (2002). The academic achievement of African Mounts, N. (2004). Adolescents’ perceptions of
Research on Adolescence, 3, 393–422. American students during early adolescence: parental management of peer relationships
Moore, M., Petrie, C., Braga, A., & McLaughlin, B. An examination of multiple risk, promotive, in an ethnically diverse sample. Journal of
(2003). Deadly lessons: Understanding lethal and protective factors. American Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 446–467.
school violence. Washington, DC: National Community Psychology, 30, 367–399. Mounts, N. (2007). Adolescents’ and their moth-
Academies Press. Morrison, G. M., Laughlin, J., Miguel, S. S., ers’ perceptions of parental management of
Moore, M. J., & Werch, C. E. (2005). Sport and Smith, D. C., & Widaman, K. (1997). Sources peer relationships. Journal of Research on
physical activity participation and substance of support for school-related issues: Choices Adolescence, 17, 169–178.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-57 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-57

Mounts, N. (2011). Parental management of Murdock, T., Anderman, L. H., & Hodge, S. A. Nasrallah, N. A., Clark, J. J., Collins, A. L., Akers,
peer relationships and early adolescents’ so- (2000). Middle-grade predictors of students’ C. A., Phillips, P. E., & Bernstein, I. L. (2011).
cial skills. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, motivation and behavior in high school. Risk preference following adolescent alcohol
40, 416–427. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 327–351. use is associated with corrupted encoding of
Mounts, N., & Kim, H. S. (2009). Expectations Murray, C. (2009). Parent and teacher relation- costs but not rewards by mesolimbic dopa-
for parental management of dating in an eth- ships as predictors of school engagement and mine. Proceedings from the National Academy
nically diverse sample of early adolescents. functioning among low-income urban youth. of Sciences, 108, 5466–5471.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 24, 531–560. Journal of Early Adolescence, 29, 376–404. Nasrallah, N. A., Yang, T. W. H., & Bernstein, I. L.
Mounts, N., & Steinberg, L. (1995). An ecological Musick, J. (1994). Grandmothers and grand- (2009). Long-term risk preference and sub-
analysis of peer influence on adolescent mothers-to-be: Effects on adolescent moth- optimal decision making following adoles-
grade point average and drug use. Develop- ers and adolescent mothering. Infancy and cent alcohol use. Proceedings from the
mental Psychology, 31, 915–922. Young Children, 6, 1–9. National Academy of Sciences, 106(41),
Mouw, T., & Entwisle, B. (2006). Residential seg- 17600–17604.
Mustanski, B. S., Viken, R. J., Kaprio, J.,
regation and interracial friendship in schools. Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2004). Genetic Nation, M., Crusto, C., Wandersman, A., Kumpfer,
American Journal of Sociology, 112, 394–441. and environmental influences on pubertal K. L., Seybolt, D., Morrissey-Kane, E., &
Mrug, S., Borch, C., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2011). development: Longitudinal data from Davino, K. (2003). What works in prevention:
Other-sex friendships in late adolescence: Finnish twins at ages 11 and 14. Developmen- Principles of effective prevention programs.
Risky associations for substance use and tal Psychology, 40, 1188–1198. American Psychologist, 58, 449–456.
sexual debut? Journal of Youth and Adoles- Myers, J., Lindentthal, J., & Pepper, M. (1975). National Center for Children in Poverty. (2010).
cence, 40, 875–888. Life events, social integration, and psychiat- Basic facts about low-income children, 2009.
Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2009). Bidirectional in- ric symptomatology. Journal of Health and New York: Author.
fluences of violence exposure and adjust- Social Behavior, 16, 421–429. National Center for Education Statistics. (2002).
ment in early adolescence: Externalizing Mylod, D., Whitman, T., & Borkowski, J. (1997). Digest of education statistics, 2001. Washington,
behaviors and school connectedness. Journal Predicting adolescent mothers’ transition to DC: Author.
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 611–623. adulthood. Journal of Research on Adoles- National Center for Education Statistics. (2005).
Muise, A. M., Stein, D. G., & Arbess, G. (2003). cence, 7, 457–478. Youth indicators, 2005. Washington, DC:
Eating disorders in adolescent boys: A review Author.
Nadeem, E., & Graham, S. (2005). Early puberty,
of the adolescent and young adult literature. peer victimization, and internalizing symp- National Center for Education Statistics (2006).
Journal of Adolescent Health, 33, 427–435. toms in ethnic minority adolescents. Journal The nation’s report card: Science 2005 Trial
Muller, P. A., Stage, F. K., & Kinzie, J. (2001). Sci- of Early Adolescence, 25, 197–222. Urban District Assessment. Washington:
ence achievement growth trajectories: Un- Author.
Nader, P. R., Bradley, R. H., Houts, R. M.,
derstanding factors related to gender and National Center for Education Statistics. (2009).
McRitchie, S. L., & O’Brien, M. (2008).
racial-ethnic differences in precollege science The condition of education 2009. Washington:
Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity from
achievement. American Educational Re- Author.
ages 9 to 15 years. Journal of the American
search, 3, 981–1012. National Center for Education Statistics (2011a).
Medical Association, 300, 295–305.
Mulvey, E., & Cauffman, E. (2001). The inher- NAEP Assessment. Washington: Author.
Nagin, D., Farrington, D., & Moffitt, T. (1995).
ent limits of predicting school violence. National Center for Education Statistics.
Life-course trajectories of different types of
American Psychologist, 56, 797–802. (2011b). The condition of education 2011.
offenders. Criminology, 33, 111–139.
Mulvey, E., Steinberg, L., Fagan, J., Cauffman, E., Washington:
Piquero, A., Chassin, L., . . . Losoya, S. (2004). Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. (1999). Trajectories of
boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and National Center for Education Statistics. (2012).
Theory and research on desistance from an-
hyperactivity on the path to physically vio- The condition of education 2012. Washington:
tisocial activity among serious adolescent of-
lent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Author.
fenders. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 2,
213–236. Child Development, 70, 1181–1196. National Center for Health Statistics (2008).
Nagoshi, J. L., Marsiglia, F. F., Parsai, M., & Leading causes of death, 2005. Retrieved from
Mun, E. Y., Windle, M., & Schainker, L. M.
Castro, F. G. (2011). The moderating effects http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/dvs/LCWK4
(2008). A model-based cluster analysis ap-
of ethnic identification on the relationship _2005.pdf
proach to adolescent problem behaviors and
young adult outcomes. Development and between parental monitoring and substance National Coalition for the Homeless (2007).
Psychopathology, 20, 291–318. use in Mexican heritage adolescents in the NCH fact sheet #13. Washington: Author.
Southwest United States. Journal of Commu- National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute
Munsch, J., Liang, S., & DeSecottier, L. (1996,
nity Psychology, 39, 520–533. Growth and Health Study Research Group.
March). Natural mentors: Who they are and
the roles they fill. A gender and ethnic com- Nagy, G., Watt, H. M. G., Eccles, J. S., Trautwein, (1992). Obesity and cardiovascular disease
parison. Paper presented at the biennial U., Lüdtke, O., & Baumert, J. (2010). The de- risk factors in Black and White girls: The
meetings of the Society for Research on Ado- velopment of students’ mathematics self- NHLBI Growth and Health Study. American
lescence, Boston. concept in relation to gender: Different Journal of Public Health, 82, 1613–1620.
Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint countries, different trajectories? Journal of National Research Council and Institute of Med-
influence of personal achievement goals and Research on Adolescence, 20, 482–506. icine. (2009). Adolescent health services: Miss-
classroom goal structures on achievement- Nagy, S., DiClimente, R., & Adcock, A. (1995). ing opportunities. Washington: The National
relevant outcomes. Journal of Educational Adverse factors associated with forced sex Academies Press.
Psychology, 101, 432–447. among Southern adolescent girls. Pediatrics, National Research Council. (1998). Protecting
Murdock, T. (1994, February). Who are you and 96, 944–946. youth at work. Washington, DC: National
how do you treat me? Student withdrawal as Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W., Academy Press.
motivated alienation. Paper presented at the Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bully- National Research Council. (2004). Reducing
biennial meetings of the Society for Research ing behaviors among U.S. youth. Journal of the underage drinking: A collective responsibility.
on Adolescence, San Diego. American Medical Association, 285, 2094–2100. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-58 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-58 References

National Research Council. (2005). Growing up Nelson, C., Bloom, F., Cameron, J., Amaral, D., Newman, B., & Newman, P. (2001a). Group iden-
global. Washington, DC: National Academies Dahl, R., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, tity and alienation: Giving the we its due.
Press. multidisciplinary approach to the study of Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 515–538.
Natsuaki, M. N., Biehl, M. C., & Ge, X. (2009). brain–behavior relations in the context of Newman, B., & Newman, P. (2011b). Adolescence,
Trajectories of depressed mood from early typical and atypical development. Develop- theories of. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.),
adolescence to young adulthood: The effects ment and Psychopathology, 14, 499–520. Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 20–
of pubertal timing and adolescent dating. Nelson, E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E., & Pine, D. 29). New York: Academic Press.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 47–74. (2005). The social re-orientation of adoles- Newman, D. L. (2005). Ego development and
Neal, J. (2010). Social aggression and social posi- cence: A neuroscience perspective on the ethnic identity formation in rural American
tion in middle childhood and early adoles- process and its relation to psychopathology. Indian adolescents. Child Development, 76,
cence: Burning bridges or building them? Psychological Medicine, 35, 163–174. 734–746.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 122–137. Nelson, E., McClure, E., Parrish, J., Leibenluft, E., Newman, K. (1999). No shame in my game.
Neblett, E., Jr., Gaskin, A., Lee, D., & Carter, S. Ernst, M., Fox, N., . . . Pine, D. (2007). Brain New York: Knopf.
(2011). Discrimination, racial and ethnic. systems underlying peer social acceptance in
Newman, R., & Schwager, M. (1995). Students’
In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclo- adolescents. Unpublished paper, Development
help seeking during problem solving: Effects of
pedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 53–58). New and Anxiety Neuroscience Section, Mood and
grade, goal, and prior achievement. American
York: Academic Press. Activity Disorders Program, National Insti-
Educational Research Journal, 32, 352–376.
Neblett, E., Jr., Smalls, C., Ford, K., Nguyên, H., & tute of Mental Health, Washington, DC.
Newmann, F. (1992). Higher order thinking and
Sellers, R. (2009). Racial socialization and ra- Nelson, I. A., & Gastic, B. (2009). Street ball,
prospects for classroom thoughtfulness. In
cial identity: African American parents’ mes- swim team and the sour cream machine: A
F. Newmann (Ed.), Student engagement and
sages about race as precursors to identity. cluster analysis of out of school time partici-
achievement in American high schools.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 189–203. pation portfolios. Journal of Youth and Ado-
New York: Teachers College Press.
Neblett, E., Jr., White, R., Ford, K., Philip, C., lescence, 38, 1172–1186.
Nelson, L. J., Badger, S., & Bo, W. (2004). The Newmann, F., Marks, H., & Gamoran, A. (1996).
Nguyên, H., & Sellers, R. M. (2008). Patterns of
influence of culture in emerging adulthood: Authentic pedagogy and student performance.
racial socialization and psychological adjust-
Perspectives of Chinese college students. In- American Journal of Education, 104, 280–312.
ment: Can parental communications about
race reduce the impact of racial discrimination? ternational Journal of Behavioral Develop- Nguyen, J., & Brown, B. (2010). Making mean-
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 477–515. ment, 28, 26–36. ings, meaning identity: Hmong adolescent
Nelson, L. J., & Barry, C. M. (2005). Distinguish- perceptions and use of language and style as
Neckerman, H., Cairns, B., & Cairns, R. (1993,
ing features of emerging adulthood. Journal identity symbols. Journal of Research on Ado-
March). Peers and families: Developmental
of Adolescent Research, 20, 242–262. lescence, 20, 849–868.
changes, constraints, and continuities. Paper
presented at the biennial meetings of the So- Neuenschwander, M., & Kracke, B. (2011). Career Nichols, S., & Good, T. (1998). Students’ per-
ciety for Research on Child Development, development. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein ceptions of fairness in school settings: A
New Orleans. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, gender analysis. Teachers College Record,
pp. 97–105). New York: Academic Press. 100, 369–401.
Needham, B. L. (2012). Sexual attraction and
trajectories of mental health and substance Neumark-Sztainer, D., Bauer, K., Friend, S., Nieto, M., Lambert, S., Briggs, E., McCoy, J.,
use during the transition from adolescence Hannan, P. J., Story, M., & Berge, J. (2010). Brunson, L., & Aber, M. (1996, March).
to adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Family weight talk and dieting: How much do Untangling the relationship between ethnic
cence, 41, 179–190. they matter for body dissatisfaction and dis- composition of neighborhood and school
ordered eating behaviors in adolescent girls? adjustment. Paper presented at the biennial
Neemann, J., Hubbard, J., & Masten, A. (1995).
Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, 270–276. meetings of the Society for Research on Ado-
The changing importance of romantic rela-
lescence, Boston.
tionship involvement to competence from Neumark-Sztainer, D., Paxton, S. J., Hannan, P.
late childhood to late adolescence. Develop- J., Haines, J., & Story, M. (2006). Does body Nishina, A. (2012). Microcontextual characteris-
ment and Psychopathology, 7, 727–750. satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal tics of peer victimization experiences and
associations between body satisfaction and adolescents’ daily well-being. Journal of
Negriff, S., Ji, J., & Trickett, P. (2011b). Exposure
health behaviors in adolescent females and Youth and Adolescence, 41, 191–201.
to peer delinquency as a mediator between
self-report pubertal timing and delinquency: males. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, Nishina, A., Ammon, N. Y., Bellmore, A. D., &
A longitudinal study of mediation. Develop- 244–251. Graham, S. (2006). Body dissatisfaction and
ment and Psychopathology, 23, 293–304. Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., Dixon, L., & physical development among ethnic minor-
Negriff, S., & Susman, E. J. (2011). Pubertal tim- Murray, D. (1998). Adolescents engaging in ity adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
ing, depression, and externalizing problems: unhealthy weight control behaviors: Are they cence, 35, 189–201.
A framework, review, and examination of at risk for other health-compromising be- Nishina, A., Bellmore, A., Witkow, M. R., &
gender differences. Journal of Research on haviors? American Journal of Public Health, Nylund-Gibson, K. (2010). Longitudinal
Adolescence, 21, 717–746. 88, 952–955. consistency of adolescent ethnic identifica-
Negriff, S., Susman, E. J., & Trickett, P. K. (2011b). Neumark-Sztainer, D., Wall, M., Story, M., & tion across varying school ethnic contexts.
The developmental pathway from pubertal Standish, A. R. (2012). Dieting and un- Developmental Psychology, 46, 1389–1401.
timing to delinquency and sexual activity healthy weight control behaviors during ad- Nishina, A., & Juvonen, J. (2005). Daily reports
from early to late adolescence. Journal of olescence: Associations with 10-year changes of witnessing and experiencing peer harass-
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1343–1356. in body mass index. Journal of Adolescent ment in middle school. Child Development,
Neiderhiser, J. M., Reiss, D., Pedersen, N. L., Health, 50, 80–86. 76, 435–450.
Lictenstein, P., Spotts, E. L., Hansson, K., . . . Newcomb, M., & Bentler, P. (1988). Impact of Noack, P., Kracke, B., & Hofer, M. (1994, February).
Ellhammer, O. (2004). Genetic and environ- adolescent drug use and social support on The family context of rightist attitudes among
mental influences on mothering of adoles- problems of young adults: A longitudinal adolescents in East and West Germany. Paper
cents: A comparison of two samples. study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, presented at the biennial meetings of the Soci-
Developmental Psychology, 40, 335–351. 64–75. ety for Research on Adolescence, San Diego.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-59 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-59

Noakes, M. A., & Rinaldi, C. M. (2006). Age and developmental tasks and standards. Interna- puff and daily cigarette smoking in adoles-
gender differences in peer conflict. Journal of tional Journal of Behavioural Development, cents. American Journal of Epidemiology, 170,
Youth and Adolescence, 35, 881–891. 16, 169–189. 585–597.
Nock, M. K., Prinstein, M. J., & Sterba, S. K. Nurmi, J. (2004). Socialization and self-develop- O’Malley, P., & Johnston, L. (1999). Drinking
(2009). Revealing the form and function of ment: Channeling, selection, adjustment, and driving among U.S. high school seniors,
self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: A and reflection. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg 1984–1997. American Journal of Public
real-time ecological assessment study among (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. Health, 89, 678–684.
adolescents and young adults. Journal of Ab- New York: Wiley. O’Sullivan, L. F., Cheng, M. M., Harris, K. M., &
normal Psychology, 118, 816–827. Nurmi, J., Onatsu, T., & Haavisto, T. (1995). Brooks-Gunn, J. (2007). I wanna hold your
Nock, S. (1998). The consequences of premarital Underachievers’ cognitive and behavioral hand: The progression of social, romantic,
fatherhood. American Sociological Review, strategies: Self-handicapping at school. and sexual events in adolescent relationships.
63, 250–263. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive
Nofziger, S., & Lee, H. (2006). Differential asso- 188–200. Health, 39, 100–107.
ciations and daily smoking of adolescents: Nylund, K., Bellmore, A., Nishina, A., & Graham, Oakes, J. (1995). Two cities’ tracking and within-
The importance of same-sex models. Youth S. (2007). Subtypes, severity, and structural school segregation. Teachers College Record,
and Society, 37, 453–478. stability of peer victimization: What does la- 96, 681–690.
Noguera, P. (1995). Preventing and producing tent class analysis say? Child Development, Oakes, J. (2005) Keeping track: How schools struc-
violence: A critical analysis of responses to 78, 1706–1722. ture inequality. New Haven: Yale University
school violence. Harvard Educational Re- O’Brien, L., Albert, D., Chein, J., & Steinberg, Press.
view, 65, 189–212. L. (2011). Adolescents prefer more imme- Oakley, D., & Bogue, E. (1995). Quality of con-
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. (1994). The diate rewards when in the presence of their dom use as reported by female clients of a
emergence of gender differences in depres- peers. Journal of Research on Adolescence, family planning clinic. American Journal of
sion during adolescence. Psychological Bulle- 21, 747–753. Public Health, 85, 1526–1530.
tin, 115, 424–443. O’Callaghan, M., Borkowski, J., Whitman, T.,
Obeidallah, D., Brennan, R. T., Brooks-Gunn, J.,
Nolle, K., Guerino, P., & Dinkes, R. (2007). Maxwell, S., & Keogh, D. (1999). A model of
& Earls, F. (2004). Links between pubertal
Crime, violence, discipline, and safety in U.S. adolescent parenting: The role of cognitive
timing and neighborhood contexts: Implica-
public schools: Findings from the School Sur- readiness to parent. Journal of Research on
tions for girls’ violent behavior. Journal of the
vey on Crime and Safety: 2005–06. National Adolescence, 9, 203–225.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Center for Education Statistics, Institute of O’Connor, T., Caspi, A., DeFries, J., & Plomin, R. Psychiatry, 43, 1460–1468.
Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Ed- (2000). Are associations between parental
Oelsner, J., Lippold, M., & Greenberg, M. (2011).
ucation. Washington, DC. divorce and children’s adjustment genetically
Factors influencing the development of school
Noller, P., & Callan, V. (1990). Adolescents’ per- mediated? An adoption study. Developmen-
bonding among middle school students. Jour-
ceptions of the nature of their communica- tal Psychology, 36, 429–437.
nal of Early Adolescence, 31, 463–487.
tion with parents. Journal of Youth and O’Connor, T., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D.,
Oesterle, S., David Hawkins, J., Hill, K. G., &
Adolescence, 19, 349–362. Rutter, M., & Plomin, R. (1998). Genotype-
Bailey, J. A. (2010). Men’s and women’s path-
Nosko, A., Tieu, T.-T., Lawford, H., & Pratt, M. W. environment correlations in late childhood
ways to adulthood and their adolescent pre-
(2011). How do I love thee? Let me count the and early adolescence: Antisocial behavioral
cursors. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72,
ways: Parenting during adolescence, attach- problems and coercive parenting. Develop-
1436–1453.
ment styles, and romantic narratives in emerg- mental Psychology, 34, 970–981.
Oettinger, G. (1999). Does high school employ-
ing adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 47, O’Donnell, D., Schwab-Stone, M., & Ruchkin, V.
ment affect high school academic perfor-
645–657. (2006). The mediating role of alienation in
mance? Industrial and Labor Relations
Novaira, H., Yates, M., Diaczok, D., Kim, H., the development of maladjustment in youth
Review, 53, 136–151.
Wolfe, A., & Radovick, S. (2011). The gonad- exposed to community violence. Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 18, 215–232. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Pre-
otropin-releasing hormone cell-specific ele-
O’Donnell, L., Stueve, A., Myint-U. A., Duran, R., vention. (2011, December). Juvenile arrests,
ment is required for normal puberty and
Agronick, G., & Wilson-Simmons, R. (2006). 2009. Washington: Author.
estrous cyclicity. Journal of Neuroscience, 31,
3336–3343. Middle school aggression and subsequent in- Ogden, C., Carroll, M., Curtin, L., Lamb, M., &
Novak, S. P., Reardon, S. F., Raudenbush, S. W., & timate partner physical violence. Journal of Flegal, K. (2010). Prevalence of high body
Buka, S. L. (2006). Retail tobacco outlet density Youth and Adolescence, 35, 693–703. mass index in US children and adolescents,
and youth cigarette smoking: A propensity- O’Hare, E. D., Lu, L. H., Houston, S. M., 2007–2008. JAMA, 303, 242–249.
modeling approach. American Journal of Pub- Bookheimer, S. Y., & Sowell, E. R. (2008). Ogden, C., Carroll, M. D., & Flegal, K. M. (2008).
lic Health, 96, 670–676. Neurodevelopmental changes in verbal work- High body mass index for age among US
Ntoumanis, N., Taylor, I. M., & Thøgersen- ing memory load-dependency: An fMRI in- children and adolescents, 2003–2006. JAMA:
Ntoumanis, C. (2012). A longitudinal exami- vestigation. Neuroimage, 42, 1678–1685. Journal of the American Medical Association,
nation of coach and peer motivational climates O’Keefe, G., Clarke-Pearson, K., & Council on 299, 2401–2405.
in youth sport: Implications for moral atti- Communications and Media. (2011). The Ogle, J. P., & Damhorst, M. L. (2003). Mothers’
tudes, well-being, and behavioral investment. impact of social media on children, adoles- and daughters’ interpersonal approaches to
Developmental Psychology, 48, 213–223. cents, and families. Pediatrics, 127, 800–804. body and dieting. Journal of Family Influence,
Nuñez, A. (2009). Latino students’ transitions to O’Leary, K. D., Slep, A. M. S., Avery-Leaf, S., & 24, 448–487.
college: A social and intercultural capital Cascardi, M. (2008). Gender differences in Ogletree, M. D., Jones, R. M., & Coyl, D. D. (2002).
perspective. Harvard Educational Review, dating aggression among multiethnic high Fathers and their adolescent sons: Pubertal
79, 22–49. school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, development and paternal involvement. Jour-
Nurmi, J. (1993). Adolescent development in an 42, 473–479. nal of Adolescent Research, 17, 418–424.
age-graded context: The role of personal be- O’Loughlin, J., Karp, I., Koulis, T., Paradis, G., & Ohannessian, C., Lerner, R., Lerner, J., & von Eye,
liefs, goals, and strategies in the tackling of DiFranza, J. (2009). Determinants of first A. (1994). A longitudinal study of perceived
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-60 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-60 References

family adjustment and emotional adjust- Osgood, D. W., & Anderson, A. (2004). Unstruc- educational attainment. Sociology of Educa-
ment in early adolescence. Journal of Early tured socializing and rates of delinquency. tion, 83, 287–311.
Adolescence, 14, 371–390. Criminology, 42, 519–549. Owens, T., Mortimer, J., & Finch, M. (1996). Self-
Ohida, T., Osaki, Y., Doi, Y., Tanihata, T., Osgood, D. W., Anderson, A., & Shaffer, J. (2005). determination as a source of self-esteem in
Minowa, M., Suzuki, K., . . . Kaneita, Y. Unstructured leisure in the after-school hours. adolescence. Social Forces, 74, 1377–1404.
(2004). An epidemiologic study of self-re- In J. Mahoney, R. Larson, & J. Eccles (Eds.), Oxford, M. L., Gilchrist, L. D., Gillmore, M. R., &
ported sleep problems among Japanese ado- Organized activities as contexts of development Lohr, M. J. (2006). Predicting variation in the
lescents. Sleep, 27, 978–985. (pp. 45–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. life course of adolescent mothers as they en-
Ojanen, T., & Perry, D. G. (2007). Relational Osgood, D. W., Ruth, G., Eccles, J., Jacobs, J., & ter adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health,
schemas and the developing self: Perceptions Barber, B. (2005). Six paths to adulthood. In 39, 20–26.
of mother and of self as joint predictors of R. Settersten, R., Furstenberg, F., Jr., & R. Oxford, M. L., Gilchrist, L. D., Lohr, M. J.,
early adolescents’ self-esteem. Developmental Rumbaut (Eds.), On the frontier of adult- Gillmore, M. R., Morrison, D. M., & Spieker,
Psychology, 43, 1474–1483. hood (pp. 340–355). Chicago: University of S. J. (2005). Life course heterogeneity in the
Olatunji, A. N. (2005). Dropping out of high Chicago Press. transition from adolescence to adulthood
school among Mexican-origin youths: Is Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J., O’Malley, P., Bachman, among adolescent mothers. Journal of Re-
early work experience a factor? Harvard Edu- J., & Johnston, L. (1996). Routine activities search on Adolescence, 15, 479–504.
cational Review, 75, 286–305. and individual deviant behavior. American Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. (1990). Possible
Olds, T., Ferrar, K., Schranz, N., & Maher, C. A. Sociological Review, 61, 635–655. selves and delinquency. Journal of Personality
(2011). Obese adolescents are less active than Ostaszewski, K., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2006). and Social Psychology, 59, 112–125.
their normal-weight peers, but wherein lies The effects of cumulative risks and promo- Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006).
the difference? Journal of Adolescent Health, tive factors on urban adolescent alcohol and Possible selves and academic outcomes:
48, 189–195. other drug use: A longitudinal study of resil- How and when possible selves impel action.
Olweus, D. (1993). Victimization by peers: Ante- iency. American Journal of Community Psy- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
cedents and long-term outcomes. In K. Rubin chology, 38, 237–249. 91, 188–204.
& J. Asendorf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhi- Oswald, H., Bahne, J., & Feder, M. (1994, February). Oyserman, D., Radin, N., & Benn, R. (1993). Dy-
bition, and shyness in childhood. Hillsdale, Love and sexuality in adolescence: Gender- namics in a three-generational family: Teens,
NJ: Erlbaum. specific differences in East and West Berlin. Paper grandparents, and babies. Developmental
Op de Macks, Z., Moor, B., Overgaauw, S., Guroglu, presented at the biennial meetings of the Soci- Psychology, 29, 564–572.
B., Dahl, R. E., & Crone, E. A. (2011). ety for Research on Adolescence, San Diego. Ozer, E., & Irwin, C. (2009). Adolescent and
Testosterone levels correspond with in- Ott, M. A., Millstein, S. G., Ofner, S., & Halpern- young adult health: From basic health sta-
creased ventral striatum activation in re- Felsher, B. L. (2006). Greater expectations: tus to clinical interventions. In R. Lerner &
sponse to monetary rewards in adolescents. Adolescents’ positive motivations for sex. L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 618–641).
506–516. Health, 38, 84–89. New York: Wiley.
Oppedal, B., Røysamb, E., & Sam, D. L. (2004). Ott, M. Q., Corliss, H. L., Wypij, D., Rosario, M., Pabon, E., Rodriguez, O., & Gurin, G. (1992).
The effect of acculturation and social sup- & Austin, S. B. (2011). Stability and change in Clarifying peer relations and delinquency.
port on change in mental health among self-reported sexual orientation identity in Youth and Society, 24, 149–165.
young immigrants. International Journal of young people: Application of mobility met- Paciello, M., Fida, R., Tramontano, C., Lupinetti,
Behavioral Development, 28, 481–494. rics. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 519–532. C., & Caprara, G. V. (2008). Stability and
Oransky, M., & Marecek, J. (2009). “I’m not go- Otterblad Olausson, P., Haglund, B., Ringback change of moral disengagement and its im-
ing to be a girl”: Masculinity and Emotions Weitoft, G., & Cnattingius, S. (2001). Teenage pact on aggression and violence in late ado-
in Boys’ Friendships and peer groups. Jour- childbearing and long-term socioeconomic lescence. Child Development, 79, 1288–1309.
nal of Adolescent Research, 24, 218–241. consequences: A case study in Sweden. Fam- Packaged Facts. (2007). The teens market in the
Organization for Economic Cooperation and ily Planning Perspectives, 33, 70–74. U.S. New York: Market Research Group.
Development (OECD). (2010). PISA 2009 Overbaugh, K., & Allen, J. (1994). The adolescent Padilla, A. M., & Gonzalez, R. (2001). Academic
results. Paris: Author. athlete, II: Injury patterns and prevention. performance of immigrant and U.S.-born
Orlando, M., Tucker, J. S., Ellickson, P., & Klein, D. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 8, 203–211. Mexican heritage students: Effects of school-
(2004). Developmental trajectories of cigarette Overbeek, G., Stattin, H., Vermulst, A., Ha, T., & ing in Mexico and bilingual/English lan-
smoking and their correlates from early ado- Engels, R. (2007). Parent–child relationships, guage instruction. American Educational
lescence to young adulthood. Journal of Con- partner relationships, and emotional adjust- Research Journal, 38, 727–742.
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 400–410. ment: A birth-to-maturity prospective study. Padilla-Walker, L. (2008). Domain-appropriateness
Orr, D., & Langefeld, C. (1993). Factors associ- Developmental Psychology, 43, 429–437. of maternal discipline as a predictor of ado-
ated with condom use by sexually active Overton, W. (1990). Competence and procedures: lescents’ positive and negative outcomes.
male adolescents at risk for sexually trans- Constraints on the development of logical Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 456–464.
mitted disease. Pediatrics, 91, 873–879. reasoning. In W. Overton (Ed.), Reasoning, Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Christensen, K. J.
Ortiz, J., & Raine, A. (2004). Heart rate level and necessity, and logic: Developmental perspectives (2011). Empathy and self-regulation as me-
antisocial behavior in children and adoles- (pp. 1–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. diators between parenting and adolescents’
cents. Journal of the American Academy of Owen, J. (1995). Why our kids don’t study. Balti- prosocial behaviors toward strangers,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 154–162. more: Johns Hopkins University Press. friends, and family. Journal of Research on
Osgerby, B. (2008). Understanding the “Jackpot Owen-Kostelnik, J., Reppucci, N. D., & Meyer, J. R. Adolescence, 21, 545–551.
Market”: Media, marketing, and the rise (2006). Testimony and interrogation of mi- Padmanabhan, A., Geier, C., Ordaz, S., Teslovich, T.,
of the American teenager. In P. Jamieson & nors: Assumptions about maturity and mo- & Luna, B. (2011). Developmental changes in
D. Romer (Eds.), The changing portrayal of rality. American Psychologist, 61, 286–304. brain function underlying the influence of re-
adolescents in the media since 1950 (pp. 27–58). Owens, A. (2010). Neighborhoods and schools ward processing on inhibitory control. Devel-
New York: Oxford University Press. as competing and reinforcing contexts for opmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 517–529.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-61 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-61

Pahl, K., & Way, N. (2006). Longitudinal trajec- Parker, K. (2012). The boomerang generation. Patton, G. C., Bond, L., Carlin, J. B., Thomas, L.,
tories of ethnic identity among urban Black Washington: Pew Research Center. Butler, H., Glover, S., . . . Bowes, G. (2006).
and Latino adolescents. Child Development, Parker, K. F., & Reckdenwald, A. (2008). Concen- Promoting social inclusion in schools: A
77, 1403–1415. trated disadvantage, traditional male role group-randomized trial of effects on student
Paikoff, R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1991). Do parent– models, and African-American juvenile vio- health risk behavior and well-being. Ameri-
child relationships change during puberty? lence. Criminology, 46, 711–735. can Journal of Public Health, 96, 1582–1587.
Psychological Bulletin, 110, 47–66. Parsai, M., Voisine, S., Marsiglia, F. F., Kulis, S., & Patton, G. C., Olsson, C., Bond, L., Toumbourou,
Paikoff, R., Parfenoff, S., Williams, S., McCormick, Nieri, T. (2009). The protective and risk ef- J. W., Carlin, J. B., . . . Hemphill, S. A. (2008).
A., Greenwood, G., & Holmbeck, G. (1997). fects of parents and peers on substance use, Predicting female depression across puberty:
Parenting, parent–child relationships, and attitudes, and behaviors of Mexican and A two-nation longitudinal study. Journal of
sexual possibility situations among urban Af- Mexican American female and male adoles- the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
rican American preadolescents: Preliminary cents. Youth and Society, 40, 353–376. Psychiatry, 47, 1424–1432.
findings and implications for HIV prevention. Parvanta, S., Brown, J., Du, S., Zimmer, C., Zhao, Patton, W., & Mannison, M. (1995). Sexual coer-
Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 11–22. X., & Zhai, F. (2010). Television use and cion in high school dating. Sex Roles, 33,
Pajer, K., Chung, J., Leininger, L., Wang, W., snacking behaviors among children and ado- 447–457.
Gardner, W., & Yeates, K. (2008). Neuropsy- lescents in China. Journal of Adolescent Paus, T. (2009). Brain development. In R. Lerner
chological function in adolescent girls with Health, 46, 339–345. & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent
conduct disorder. Journal of the American psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 95–115). New
Pascarella, E., & Terenzini, P. (2005). How college
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, York: Wiley.
affects students: Vol. 2. A third decade of re-
416–425. Paus, T., Keshavan, B., & Giedd, J. (2008). Why
search. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Paley, B., Conger, R., & Harold, G. (2000). Par- do so many psychiatric disorders emerge
Paschall, M. J., Flewelling, R. L., & Russell, T.
ents’ affect, adolescent cognitive represen- during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuro-
(2004). Why is work intensity associated
tations, and adolescent social development. science, 9, 947–957.
with heavy alcohol use among adolescents?
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, Paus, T., Toro, R., Leonard, G., Lerner, J., Lerner,
Journal of Adolescent Health, 34, 79–87.
761–776. R., Perron, M., . . . Pausova, Z. (2008). Mor-
Paschall, M. J., Freisthler, B., & Lipton, R. I.
Palladino, G. (1996). Teenagers: An American his- phological properties of the action-observa-
(2005). Moderate alcohol use and depression
tory. New York: Basic Books. tion cortical network in adolescents with low
in young adults: Findings from a national
Pallock, L., & Lamborn, S. (2006). Beyond par- and high resistance to peer influence. Social
longitudinal study. American Journal of Pub-
enting practices: Extended kinship support Neuroscience, 3, 303–316.
lic Health, 95, 453–457.
and the academic adjustment of African- Paxton, S., Wertheim, E., Gibbons, K., Szmukler,
Pasley, K., & Gecas, V. (1984). Stresses and satis-
American and European-American teens. G., Hillier, L., & Petrovich, J. (1991). Body im-
factions of the parental role. Personnel and
Journal of Adolescence, 29, 813–828. age satisfaction, dieting beliefs, and weight
Guidance Journal, 2, 400–404.
Pampel, F. C., & Aguilar, J. (2008). Changes in loss behaviors in adolescent girls and boys.
youth smoking, 1976–2002: A time-series Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Wynn, S. R. (2010). Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 361–380.
analysis. Youth and Society, 39, 453–479. The effectiveness and relative importance of
Payne, A. (2009). Girls, boys, and schools: Gen-
choice in the classroom. Journal of Educa-
Panchaud, C., Singh, S., Feivelson, D., & Darroch, der differences in the relationships between
tional Psychology, 102, 896–915.
J. E. (2000). Sexually transmitted diseases school-related factors and student deviance.
among adolescents in developed countries. Patrick, M. E., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2011). How Criminology, 47, 1167–1200.
Family Planning Perspectives, 32, 24–32, 45. trajectories of reasons for alcohol use relate to
Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar,
trajectories of binge drinking: National panel
Papadakis, A. A., Prince, R. P., Jones, N. P., & A., Bamford, H., . . . Zhou, M. (2012). Media
data spanning late adolescence to early adult-
Strauman, T. J. (2006). Self-regulation, rumi- use, face-to-face communication, media
hood. Developmental Psychology, 47, 311–317.
nation, and vulnerability to depression in multitasking, and social well-being among
adolescent girls. Development and Psychopa- Patrikakou, E. (1996). Investigating the aca- 8-to 12-year-old girls. Developmental Psy-
thology, 18, 815–829. demic achievement of adolescents with chology, 48, 327–336.
Pardini, D. A., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, learning disabilities: A structural modeling Pearce, M. J., Jones, S. M., Schwab-Stone, M. E.,
M. (2005). Developmental shifts in parent approach. Journal of Education Psychology, & Ruchkin, V. (2003). The protective effects
and peer influences on boys’ beliefs about 88, 435–450. of religiousness and parent involvement
delinquent behavior. Journal of Research on Patten, C. A., Choi, W. S., Gillin, C. J., & Pierce, J. P. on the development of conduct problems
Adolescence 15, 299–323. (2000). Depressive symptoms and cigarette among youth exposed to violence. Child De-
Park, A., Sher, K. J., Todorov, A. A., & Heath, A. C. smoking predict development and persistence velopment, 74, 1682–1696.
(2011). Interaction between the DRD4 of sleep problems in U.S. adolescents. Pediat- Pechmann, C., Levine, L., Loughlin, S., & Leslie,
VNTR polymorphism and proximal and dis- rics, 106, Article e23. F. (2005). Impulsive and self-conscious: Ad-
tal environments in alcohol dependence dur- Patterson, C. J. (2009). Children of lesbian and olescents’ vulnerability to advertising and
ing emerging and young adulthood. Journal gay parents: Psychology, law, and policy. promotion. Journal of Public Policy and Mar-
of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 585–595. American Psychologist, 64, 727–736. keting, 24, 202–221.
Parker, J., Low, C., Walker, A. R., & Gamm, B. K. Patterson, G., DeGarmo, D., & Knutson, M. Pedersen, S., Seidman, E., Yoshikawa, H., Rivera,
(2005). Friendship jealousy in young adoles- (2000). Hyperactive and antisocial behav- A. C., Allen, L., & Aber, J. L. (2005). Contex-
cents: Individual differences and links to sex, iors: Comorbid or two points in the same tual competence: Multiple manifestations
self-esteem, aggression, and social adjust- process? Development and Psychopathology, among urban adolescents. American Journal
ment. Developmental Psychology, 41, 235–250. 12, 91–106. of Community Psychology, 35, 65–82.
Parker, J., & Seal, J. (1996). Forming, losing, re- Patterson, G., Forgatch, M., Yoerger, K., & Stool- Pedersen, S., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., & Borge,
newing, and replacing friendships: Apply- miller, M. (1998). Variables that initiate and A. I. H. (2007). The timing of middle-
ing temporal parameters to the assessment maintain an early-onset trajectory for juve- childhood peer rejection and friendship:
of children’s friendship experiences. Child nile offending. Development and Psychopa- Linking early behavior to early-adolescent ad-
Development, 67, 2248–2268. thology, 10, 531–547. justment. Child Development, 78, 1037–1051.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-62 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-62 References

Peeters, M., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Scholte, R. H. J. drinking, family management, and parental Phinney, J., & Ong, A. D. (2002). Adolescent–
(2010). Clueless or powerful? Identifying alcohol norms on current drinking by Black parent disagreements and life satisfaction in
subtypes of bullies in adolescence. Journal of and White adolescents. Journal of Research on families from Vietnamese- and European-
Youth and Adolescence, 39, 1041–1052. Adolescence, 4, 203–227. American backgrounds. Journal of Behav-
Pellegrini, A. D. (2003). Perceptions and func- Petraitis, J., Flay, B., & Miller, T. (1995). Review- ioral Development, 26, 556–561.
tions of play and real fighting in early adoles- ing theories of adolescent substance use: Phinney, J., Romero, I., Nava, M., & Huang, D.
cence. Child Development, 74, 1522–1533. Organizing pieces in the puzzle. Psychologi- (2001). The role of language, parents, and
Pellegrini, A. D., & Long, J. D. (2007). An observa- cal Bulletin, 117, 67–86. peers in ethnic identity among adolescents in
tional study of early heterosexual interaction Pettifor, A., O’Brien, K., MacPhail, C., Miller, W. immigrant families. Journal of Youth and
at middle-school dances. Journal of Research C., & Rees, H. (2010). Early coital debut and Adolescence, 30, 135–153.
on Adolescence, 17, 613–638. associated HIV risk factors among young Piehler, T. (2011). Peer influence. In B. Brown &
Pellerin, L. A. (2005). Student disengagement women and men in South Africa. Interna- M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles-
and the socialization styles of high schools. tional Perspectives on Sexual and Reproduc- cence (Vol. 2, pp. 249–254). New York: Aca-
Social Forces, 84, 1159–1179. tive Health, 35, 74–82. demic Press.
Pepler, D., Jiang, D., Craig, W., & Connolly, J. Pettit, G., Bates, J., Dodge, K., & Meece, D. Piehler, T., & Dishion, T. (2007). Interpersonal
(2008). Developmental trajectories of bully- (1999). The impact of after-school peer con- dynamics within adolescent friendships:
ing and associated factors. Child Develop- tact on early adolescent externalizing prob- Dyadic mutuality, deviant talk, and patterns
ment, 79, 325–338. lems is moderated by parental monitoring, of antisocial behavior. Child Development,
perceived neighborhood safety, and prior 78, 1611–1624.
Perez-Febles, A., Allison, K., & Burton, L. (April,
adjustment. Child Development, 70, 768–778. Pierce, J., & Gilpin, E. (1996). How long will
1999). Sociocultural context and the construc-
tion of research questions: The case of adoles- Pfeifer, J., & Blakemore, S-J. (2012). Adolescent today’s new adolescent smoker be addicted
cent childbearing. Paper presented at the social cognitive and affective neuroscience: to cigarettes? American Journal of Public
biennial meetings of the Society for Research past, present, and future. Social Cognitive and Health, 86, 253–256.
on Child Development, Albuquerque. Affective Neuroscience, 7, 1–10. Pietiläinen, K. H., Kaprio, J., Rasanen, M.,
Perkins, D., Jacobs, J. E., Barber, B. L., & Eccles, J. Pfeifer, J., Masten, C. L., Borofsky, L. A., Dapretto, Winter, T., Rissanen, A., & Rose, R. (2001).
(2003). Childhood and adolescent sports M., Fuligni, A. J., & Lieberman, M. D. Tracking of body size from birth to late
participation as predictors of participation (2009). Neural correlates of direct and re- adolescence: Contributions of birth length,
in sports and physical fitness activities dur- flected self-appraisals in adolescents and birth weight, duration of gestation, par-
ing young adulthood. Youth and Society, 35, adults: When social perspective-taking in- ents’ body size, and twinship. American
295–520. forms self-perception. Child Development, Journal of Epidemiology, 154, 21–29.
80, 1016–1038. Pike, A., McGuire, S., Hetherington, E. M., Reiss,
Perkins, D., Luster, T., & Jank, W. (2002). Protec-
tive factors, physical abuse, and purging Pfeifer, J., Masten, C., Moore, W., III, & Oswald, D., et al. (1996). Family environment and
from community-wide surveys of female T. (2011). Entering adolescence: Resistance adolescent depressive symptoms and antiso-
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, to peer influence, risky behavior, and neural cial behavior: A multivariate genetic analysis.
17, 377–400. changes in emotion reactivity. Neuron, 69, Developmental Psychology, 32, 590–604.
1029–1036. Pilgrim, C., Luo, Q., Urberg, K. A., & Fang, X.
Perkins, D., Luster, T., Villarruel, F., & Small, S.
Pfeifer, J., Masten, C., Moore, W., Oswald, T., (1999). Influence of peers, parents, and indi-
(1998). An ecological, risk-factor examina-
Mazziotta, J., Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. vidual characteristics on adolescent drug use
tion of adolescents’ sexual activity in three
(2012). Entering adolescence: Resistance to in two cultures. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45,
ethnic groups. Journal of Marriage and the
peer influence, risky behavior, and neural 85–107.
Family, 60, 660–673.
changes in emotion reactivity Neuron, 69, Pillay, Y. (2005). Racial identity as a predictor of
Perkins, S. A., & Turiel, E. (2007). To lie or not to 1029–1036 the psychological health of African American
lie: To whom and under what circumstances.
Phares, V., Steinberg, A. R., & Thompson, J. K. students at a predominantly White university.
Child Development, 78, 609–621.
(2004). Gender differences in peer and pa- Journal of Black Psychology, 31, 46–66.
Perry, C., Williams, C., Veblen-Mortenson, S., rental influences: Body image disturbance, Pinderhughes, E., Jones Harden, B., & Guyer, A.
Toomey, T., Komro, K., Anstine, P., McGovern, self-worth, and psychological functioning in (2007). Children in foster care. In D. Phillips,
P., . . . Wolfson, M. (1996). Project North- preadolescent children. Journal of Youth and L. Aber, L. Allen, & S. Jones (Eds.), Child de-
land: Outcomes of a communitywide alco- Adolescence, 33, 421–429. velopment and social policy: Knowledge for ac-
hol use prevention program during early
Phelan, P., Yu, H., & Davidson, A. (1994). Navi- tion. Washington: American Psychological
adolescence. American Journal of Public
gating the psychosocial pressures of adoles- Association.
Health, 86, 956–965.
cence: The voices and experiences of high Pine, D., Wasserman, G., Coplan, J., Fried, J.,
Perry, D., & Pauletti, R. (2011). Gender and ado- school youth. American Educational Research Huang, Y., Kassir, S., . . . Parsons, B. (1996).
lescent development. Journal of Research on Journal, 31, 415–447. Platelet serotonin 2A (5HT2A) receptor char-
Adolescence, 21, 61–74.
Phillipsen, L. C. (1999). Associations between acteristics and parenting factors for boys at
Persson, A., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2007). Staying age, gender, and group acceptance and three risk for delinquency: A preliminary report.
in or moving away from structured activities: components of friendship quality. Journal of American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 538–544.
Explanations involving parents and peers. Early Adolescence, 19, 438–464. Pinquart, M., & Silbereisen, R. (2002). Changes
Developmental Psychology, 43, 197–207.
Phinney, J., & Chavira, V. (1995). Parental ethnic in adolescents’ and mothers’ autonomy and
Peskin, H. (1967). Pubertal onset and ego func- socialization and adolescent coping with connectedness in conflict discussions: An
tioning: A psychoanalytic approach. Journal problems related to ethnicity. Journal of Re- observation study. Journal of Adolescence, 25,
of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 1–15. search on Adolescence, 5, 31–53. 509–522.
Petersen, A. (1988). Adolescent development. Phinney, J., Ferguson, D., & Tate, J. (1997). Inter- Pintrich, P. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple
Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 583–607. group attitudes among ethnic minority ado- pathways: The role of goal orientation in
Peterson, P., Hawkins, J., Abbott, R., & Catalano, R. lescents: A causal model. Child Development, learning and achievement. Journal of Educa-
(1994). Disentangling the effects of parental 68, 955–969. tional Psychology, 92, 544–555.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-63 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-63

Piquero, A., & Chung, H. L. (2001). On the rela- Pong, S. (1998). The school compositional effect health: Gender intensification revisited. Child
tionship between gender, early onset, and the of single parenthood on 10th-grade achieve- Development, 80, 1531–1544
seriousness of offending. Journal of Criminal ment. Sociology of Education, 71, 23–42. Prinstein, M. J., & Aikins, J. W. (2004). Cognitive
Justice, 29, 189–206. Pong, S., & Ju, D. (2000). The effects of change in moderators of the longitudinal association
Piquero, A., Farrington, D., & Blumstein, A. family structure and income on dropping between peer rejection and adolescent de-
(2003). The criminal career paradigm: Back- out in middle and high school. Journal of pressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal
ground and recent developments. Crime and Family Issues, 21, 147–169. Child Psychology, 32, 147–158.
Justice: A Review of Research, 30, 359–506. Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the Prinstein, M. J., Brechwald, W. A., & Cohen, G.
Pitner, R. O., Astor, R. A., Benbenishty, R., five-factor model of personality and aca- L. (2011). Susceptibility to peer influence:
Haj-Yahia, M. M., & Zeira, A. (2003). The ef- demic performance. Psychological Bulletin, Using a performance-based measure to
fects of group stereotypes on adolescents’ rea- 135, 322–338. identify adolescent males at heightened risk
soning about peer retribution. Child Poteat, P. V., Espelage, D. L., & Koenig, B. W. for deviant peer socialization. Developmen-
Development, 74, 413–425. (2009). Willingness to remain friends and tal Psychology, 47, 1167–1172.
Pittard, W., Laditka, J., & Laditka S. (2008). As- attend school with lesbian and gay peers: Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. (2003). Forms
sociations between maternal age and infant relational expressions of prejudice among and functions of adolescent peer aggression
health outcomes among Medicaid-insured heterosexual youth. Journal of Youth and associated with high levels of peer status.
infants in South Carolina: Mediating effects Adolescence, 38, 952–962. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 310–342.
of socioeconomic factors, Pediatrics, 122, Prinstein, M., Heilbron, N., Guerry, J., Franklin,
Poulin, F., & Chan, A. (2010). Friendship stabil-
e100–e106. J., Rancourt, D., Simon, V., & Spirito, A.
ity and change in childhood and adoles-
Pittman, L. D., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (2001). cence. Developmental Review, 30, 257–272. (2010). Peer influence and nonsuicidal self
African American adolescent girls in impov- injury: longitudinal results in community
Poulin, F., Denault, A.-S., & Pedersen, S. (2011).
erished communities: Parenting style and and clinically referred adolescent samples.
Longitudinal associations between other-sex
adolescent outcomes. Journal of Research on Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38,
friendships and substance use in adoles-
Adolescence, 11, 199–224. 669–682.
cence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21,
Planty, M., Bozick, R., & Regnier, M. (2006). 776–788. Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Peer
Helping because you have to or helping be- crowd affiliation and internalizing distress in
Poulin, F., Dishion, T., & Haas, E. (1999). The
cause you want to?: Sustaining participation childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal
peer influence paradox: Friendship quality
in service work from adolescence through follow-back study. Journal of Research on
and deviancy training within male adoles-
young adulthood. Youth and Society, 38, Adolescence, 12, 325–351.
cent friendships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
177–202. Prinstein, M. J., Meade, C., & Cohen, G. (2003).
45, 42–61.
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are chil- Adolescent oral sex, peer popularity, and
Poulin, F., & Pedersen, S. (2007). Developmental
dren in the same family so different from perceptions of best friends’ sexual behavior.
changes in gender composition of friendship
one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28, 243–249.
networks in adolescent girls and boys. Devel-
10, 1–60. Proctor, L., Skriner, L., Roesch, S., & Litrownik,
opmental Psychology, 43, 1484–1496.
Plumert, J. (1994). Flexibility in children’s use of A. (2010). Trajectories of behavioral ad-
spatial and categorical organizational strate- Power, J., Fair, D., Schlaggar, B., & Petersen, S.
justment following early placement in fos-
gies in recall. Developmental Psychology, 30, (2010). The development of human func-
ter care: Predicting stability and change
738–747. tional brain networks. Neuron, 67, 735–748.
over 8 years. Journal of the American Acad-
Pogarsky, G., Lizotte, A. J., & Thornberry, T. P. Prado, G., Huang, S., Schwartz, S., Maldonado- emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49,
(2003). The delinquency of children born to Molina, M., Bandiera, F., la Rosa, de, M., & 464–473.
young mothers: Results from the Rochester Pantin, H. (2009). What accounts for dif-
Pronk, R., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. (2010). It’s
Youth Development Study. Criminology, 41, ferences in substance use among US-born
‘mean,’ but what does it mean to adolescents?
1249–1286. and immigrant Hispanic adolescents?: Re-
Relational aggression described by victims,
sults from a longitudinal prospective co-
Pollack, C., & Bright, D. (2003). Caffeine con- aggressors, and their peers. Journal of Adoles-
hort study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45,
sumption and weekly sleep patterns in U.S. cent Research, 25, 175–204.
118–125.
seventh-, eighth-, and ninth-graders. Pediat- Prosser, E., & Carlson, C. (April, 1999). Ethnic
rics, 111, 42–46. Prelow, H., Danoff-Burg, S., Swenson, R., & Pul-
differences in fluctuations in female self-
giano, D. (2004). The impact of ecological
Pomerantz, E. (2001). Parent–child socialization: esteem during early adolescence. Paper pre-
risk and perceived discrimination on the psy-
Implications for the development of depres- sented at the biennial meetings of the
chological adjustment of African American
sive symptoms. Journal of Family Psychology, Society for Research on Child Development,
and European American youth. Journal of
15, 510–525. Albuquerque.
Community Psychology, 32, 375–389.
Pomerantz, E., Altermatt, E., & Saxon, J. L. Public Agenda. (1997). Getting by: What Ameri-
(2002). Making the grade but feeling dis- President’s Science Advisory Committee. (1974). can teenagers really think about their schools.
tressed: Gender differences in academic per- Youth: Transition to adulthood. Chicago: New York: Author.
formance and internal distress. Journal of University of Chicago Press.
Puelo, C., Settipani, C., Crawley, S., Beidas, R., &
Educational Psychology, 94, 396–404. Price, M. N., & Hyde, J. S. (2011). Perceived and Kendall, P. (2011). Anxiety disorders. In B.
Pomerantz, E., Qin, L., Wang, Q., & Chen, H. observed maternal relationship quality pre- Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of
(2009). American and Chinese early adoles- dict sexual debut by age 15. Journal of Youth adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 48–55). New York:
cents’ inclusion of their relationships with and Adolescence, 40, 1595–1606. Academic Press.
their parents in their self-construals. Child Priess, H., & Hyde, J. (2011). Gender roles. In B. Punamäki, R., Qouta, S., & Sarraj, E. (1997).
Development, 80, 792–807. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Models of traumatic experiences and chil-
Pong, S. (1997). Family structure, school context, adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 99–108). New York: dren’s psychological adjustment: The roles of
and eighth-grade math and reading achieve- Academic Press. perceived parenting and the children’s own
ment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, Priess, H., Lindberg, S., & Hyde, J. (2009). Ado- resources and activity. Child Development,
734–746. lescent gender-role identity and mental 68, 718–728.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-64 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-64 References

Punamäki, R., Wallenius, M., Nygård, C., Saarni, Raffaelli, M. (1997). Young adolescents’ conflicts longitudinal neuroimaging study of matura-
L., & Rimpelä, A. (2007). Use of information with siblings and friends. Journal of Youth tional coupling. Neuron, 72, 873–884.
and communication technology (ICT) and and Adolescence, 26, 539–558. Ready, D. D., Lee, V., & Welner, K. G. (2004).
perceived health in adolescence: The role of Raffaelli, M., & Green, S. (2003). Parent–adolescent Educational equity and school structure:
sleeping habits and waking-time tiredness. communication about sex: Retrospective re- School size, overcrowding, and schools-
Journal of Adolescence, 30, 569–585. ports by Latino college students. Journal of within-schools. Teachers College Review,
Pungello, E., Kupersmidt, J., Burchinal, M., & Marriage and the Family, 65, 474. 106, 1989–2014.
Patterson, C. (1996). Environmental risk Raine, A., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Ream, G., & Savin-Williams, R. (2005). Recip-
factors and children’s achievement from Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Lynam, D. (2005). rocal associations between adolescent sex-
middle childhood to early adolescence. De- Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the ual activity and quality of youth–parent
velopmental Psychology, 32, 755–767. life-course-persistent antisocial path. Journal interactions. Journal of Family Psychology,
Purdie, N., Hattie, J., & Douglas, G. (1996). Stu- of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 38–49. 19, 171–179.
dent conceptions of learning and their use of Ramchand, R., Ialongo, N. S., & Chilcoat, H. D. Ream, R., & Rumberger, R. (2008). Student en-
self-regulated learning strategies: A cross- (2007). The effect of working for pay on gagement, peer social capital, and school
cultural comparison. Journal of Educational adolescent tobacco use. American Journal of dropout among Mexican American and
Psychology, 88, 87–100. Public Health, 97, 2056–2062. non-Latino White students. Sociology of
Qin, D. (2008). Doing well vs. feeling well: Un- Education, 81, 109–139.
Ramirez-Valles, J., Zimmerman, M. A., & Juarez,
derstanding family dynamics and the psy- Rebellon, C. J. (2006). Do adolescents engage in
L. (2002). Gender differences of neighbor-
chological adjustment of Chinese immigrant delinquency to attract the social attention of
hood and social control processes: A study
adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, peers?: An extension and longitudinal test of
of the timing of first intercourse among low-
37, 22–35. the social reinforcement hypothesis. Journal
achieving, urban, African American youth.
Qin, D. (2009). Being “good” or being “popular”: Youth and Society, 33, 418–441. of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 43,
Gender and ethnic identity negotiations of 387–411.
Rampey, B. D., Dion, G. S., & Donahue, P. L.
Chinese immigrant adolescents. Journal of Reddy, R., Rhodes, J. E., & Mulhall, P. (2003).
(2009). NAEP 2008 Trends in Academic
Adolescent Research, 24, 37–66. The influence of teacher support on student
Progress. Washington, DC: National Center
Qin, D., Way, N., & Mukherjee, P. (2008). The for Education Statistics. adjustment in the middle school years: A
other side of the model minority story: The latent growth curve study. Development and
Ramsden, S., Richardson, F., Josse, G., Thomas,
familial and peer challenges faced by Chinese Psychopathology, 15, 119–138.
M., & Ellis, C. (2011). Verbal and non-verbal
American adolescents. Youth and Society, 39, Redlich, A., & Goodman, G. (2003). Taking re-
intelligence changes in the teenage brain.
480–506. sponsibility for an act not committed: The
Nature, 479, 113–116.
Qin, L., Pomerantz, E. M., & Wang, Q. (2009). influence of age and suggestibility. Law and
Randall, E. T., & Bohnert, A. M. (2009). Orga- Human Behavior, 27, 141–156.
Are gains in decision-making autonomy
nized activity involvement, depressive symp-
during early adolescence beneficial for emo- Redlich, A., Silverman, A., & Steiner, H. (2003).
toms, and social adjustment in adolescents:
tional functioning? The case of the United Pre-adjudicative competence in juveniles
Ethnicity and socioeconomic status as mod-
States and China. Child Development, 80, and young adults. Behavioral Sciences and the
erators. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38,
1705–1721. Law, 21, 393–410.
1187–1198.
Quadrel, M., Fischhoff, B., & Davis, W. (1993). Reese, E., Jack, F., & White, N. (2010). Origins
Adolescent (in)vulnerability. American Psy- Randel, B., Stevenson, H. W., & Witruk, E. (2000).
of adolescents’ autobiographical memories.
chologist, 48, 102–116. Attitudes, beliefs, and mathematics achieve-
Cognitive Development, 25, 352–367.
ment of German and Japanese high school
Quatman, T., Sokolik, E., & Smith, K. (2000). Ado- Reese-Weber, M. (2000). Middle and late adoles-
students. International Journal of Behavioral
lescent perception of peer success: A gendered cents’ conflict resolution skills and siblings:
Development, 24, 190–198.
perspective over time. Sex Roles, 43, 61–84. Associations with interparental and parent–
Rangel, M. C., Gavin, L., Reed, C., Fowler, M. G., adolescent conflict resolution. Journal of
Quillian, L., & Campbell, M. E. (2003). Beyond
& Lee, L. M. (2006). Epidemiology of HIV Youth and Adolescence, 29, 697–711.
Black and White: The present and future of
and AIDS among adolescents and young
multiracial friendship segregation. American Reich, K., Oser, F., & Valentin, P. (1994). Knowing
adults in the United States. Journal of Adoles-
Sociological Review, 68, 540–566. why I now know better: Children’s and youth’s
cent Health, 39, 156–163.
Quintana, S., Castaneda-English, P., & Ybarra, V. explanations of their worldview changes. Jour-
Rankin, J., Lane, D., Gibbons, F., & Gerrard, M. nal of Research on Adolescence, 4, 151–173.
C. (1999). Role of perspective-taking abilities
(2004). Adolescent self-consciousness: Lon-
and ethnic socialization in development of Reich, S. (2010). Adolescents’ sense of commu-
gitudinal age changes and gender differences
adolescent ethnic identity. Journal of Re- nity on Myspace and Facebook: A mixed-
in two cohorts. Journal of Research on Adoles-
search on Adolescence, 9, 161–184. methods approach. Journal of Community
cence, 14, 1–21.
Quittner, A., Romero, S., Kimberg, C., Blackwell, Psychology, 38, 688–705.
L., & Cruz, I. (2011). Chronic illness. In B. Raskauskas, J., & Stoltz, A. D. (2007). Involvement Reich, S., Subrahmanyam, K., & Espinosa, G.
Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of in traditional and electronic bullying among (2012). Friending, IMing, and hanging out
adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 91–99). New York: adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 43, face-to-face: Overlap in adolescents’ online
Academic Press. 564–575. and offline social networks. Developmental
Raabe, T., & Beelman, A. (2011). Development of Ravitch, D. (2000). Left back: A century of failed Psychology, 48, 356–368.
ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in school reforms. New York: Simon & Schuster. Reichhardt, T. (2003). Playing with fire. Nature,
childhood and adolescence: A multinational Ravitch, D. (Ed.). (2001). Brookings papers on 424, 367–368.
meta-analysis of age differences. Child Devel- education policy. Washington, DC: Brookings Reijneveld, S., Veenstra, R., de Winter, A., Ver-
opment, 82, 1715–1737. Institution. hulst, F., Ormel, J., & de Meer, G. (2010).
Radmacher, K., & Azmitia, M. (2006). Are there Raznahan, A., Lerch, J., Lee, N., Greenstein, D., Area deprivation affects behavioral problems
gendered pathways to intimacy in early ado- Wallace, G. L., Stockman, M., . . . Giedd, J. of young adolescents in mixed urban and
lescents’ and emerging adults’ friendships? (2011). Patterns of coordinated anatomical rural areas: The TRAILS study. Journal of
Journal of Adolescent Research, 21, 415–448. change in human cortical development: A Adolescent Health, 46, 189–196.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-65 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-65

Reinders, H., & Youniss, J. (2006). School-based Reynolds, A., Temple, J., Robertson, D. L., & Richardson, J. (2009). Men do matter: Ethno-
required community service and civic devel- Mann, E. A. (2001). Long-term effects of an graphic insights on the socially supportive
opment in adolescents. Applied Developmental early childhood intervention on educational role of the African American uncle in the
Science, 10, 2–12. achievement and juvenile arrest: A 15-year lives of inner-city African American youth.
Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., Plomin, R., Howe, G., follow-up of low-income children in public Journal of Family Issues, 30, 1041–1069.
Simmens, S., Henderson S., . . . Law, T. (1995). schools. Journal of the American Medical As- Richardson, R. (1996, March). Competence in the
Genetic questions for environmental studies. sociation, 285, 2339–2346. transition to adulthood: Exploring the influ-
Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 925–936. Reynolds, B. M., & Juvonen, J. (2011). The role of ence of adolescent motherhood for low-income,
Repetti, R. (1996). The effects of perceived daily early maturation, perceived popularity, and urban, African-American women. Paper pre-
social and academic failure experiences on rumors in the emergence of internalizing sented at the biennial meetings of the Society
school-age children’s subsequent interac- symptoms among adolescent girls. Journal of for Research on Adolescence, Boston.
tions with parents. Child Development, 67, Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1407–1422. Ridenour, T., Cottier, L., Robins, L., Campton, W.,
1467–1482. Reynolds, J. R., & Baird, C. L. (2010). Is there a Spitznagel, E., & Cunningham-Williams, R.
Repetto, P. B., Zimmerman, M. A., & Caldwell, C. downside to shooting for the stars?: Unreal- (2002). Test of the plausibility of adolescent
H. (2008). A longitudinal study of depressive ized educational expectations and symptoms substance use playing a causal role in devel-
symptoms and marijuana use in a sample of of depression. American Sociological Review, oping adulthood antisocial behavior. Journal
inner-city African Americans. Journal of Re- 75, 151–172. of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 144–155.
search on Adolescence, 18, 421–447. Rhodes, J. (2004). Stand by me. Cambridge, MA: Rideout, V., Foeher, U., & Roberts, D. (2010). Gen-
Repinski, D., & Leffert, N. (1994, February). Ado- Harvard University Press. eration M2: Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-
lescents’ relations with friends: The effects of a Rhodes, J., Haight, W., & Briggs, E. (1999). The olds. Palo Alto: Kaiser Family Foundation.
psychoeducational intervention. Paper pre- influence of mentoring on the peer relation- Riegle-Crumb, C., Farkas, G., & Muller, C. (2006).
sented at the biennial meetings of the Society ships of foster youth in relative and nonrela- The role of gender and friendship in ad-
for Research on Adolescence, San Diego. tive care. Journal of Research on Adolescence, vanced course taking. Sociology of Education,
Reppucci, N., Scott, E., & Antonishak, J. (2009). 9, 185–201. 79, 206–228.
Political orientation and perceptions of ado- Rhodes, J., & Lowe, S. (2009). Mentoring in ado- Riggs, L., Holmbeck, G., Paikoff, R., & Bryant, F.
lescent autonomy and judicial culpability. lescence. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), (2004). Teen mothers parenting their own
Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 27, 29–34. Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., teen offspring: The moderating role of par-
Resko, S. M., Walton, M. A., Bingham, C. R., Vol. 2, pp. 152–190). New York: Wiley. enting support. Journal of Early Adolescence,
Shope, J. T., Zimmerman, M., Chermack, S. Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2001). Self- 24, 200–230.
T., . . . Cunningham, R. (2010). Alcohol avail- esteem and negative affect as moderators of Rigon, F., Bianchin, L., Bernasconi, S., Bona, G.,
ability and violence among inner-city ado- sociocultural influences on body dissatisfac- Bozzola, M., Buzi, F., . . . Perissinotto, E.
lescents: A multi-level analysis of the role of tion, strategies to decrease weight, and strat- (2010). Update on age at menarche in Italy:
alcohol outlet density. American Journal of egies to increase muscles among adolescent toward the leveling off of the secular trend.
Community Psychology, 46(3–4), 253–262. boys and girls. Sex Roles, 44(3–4), 189–207. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 238–244.
Resnick, M., Bearman, P., Blum, R., Bauman, K., Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2004). A Rigsby, L., & McDill, E. (1975). Value orienta-
Harris, K., Jones, J., . . . Udry, J. (1997). Pro- biopsychosocial model of disorder eating tions of high school students. In H. R. Stub
tecting adolescents from harm: Findings and the pursuit of muscularity in adolescent (Ed.), The sociology of education: A source-
from the National Longitudinal Study of boys. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 179–205. book (3rd ed., pp. 53–74). Homewood, IL:
Adolescent Health. Journal of the American Rice, F., Harold, G., Shelton, K., & Thapar, A. Dorsey.
Medical Association, 278, 823–832. (2006). Family conflict interacts with genetic Risch, S., Jodl, K., & Eccles, J. (2004). Role of the
Rest, J., Narvaez, D., Bebeau, M., & Thoma, S. liability in predicting childhood and adoles- father–adolescent relationship in shaping
(1999). Postconventional moral thinking: A neo- cent depression. Journal of the American adolescents’ attitudes toward divorce. Journal
Kohlbergian approach. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, of Marriage and the Family, 66, 46.
Reuman, D. (1989). How social comparison me- 45, 841–848. Ritakallio, M., Koivisto, A., von der Pahlen, B.,
diates the relation between ability-grouping Rice, K., & Mulkeen, P. (1995). Relationships Pelkonen, M., Marttunen, M., & Kaltiala-
practices and students’ achievement expec- with parents and peers: A longitudinal study Heino, R. (2008). Continuity, comorbidity,
tancies in mathematics. Journal of Educa- of adolescent intimacy. Journal of Adolescent and longitudinal associations between de-
tional Psychology, 81, 178–189. Research, 10, 338–357. pression and antisocial behaviour in middle
Reyes, O., & Jason, L. (1993). Pilot study examin- Rich, L. M., & Kim, S.-B. (2002). Employment adolescence: A 2-year prospective follow-up
ing factors associated with academic success and the sexual and reproductive behavior of study. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 355–370.
for Hispanic high school students. Journal of female adolescents. Perspectives on Sexual Rivas-Drake, D., Hughes, D., & Way, N. (2009).
Youth and Adolescence, 22, 57–71. and Reproductive Health, 34, 127–134. A preliminary analysis of associations
Reyna, V., & Brainerd, C. (2011). Dual processes Richards, M., Boxer, A., Petersen, A., & Albrecht, among ethnic racial socialization, ethnic
in decision making and developmental neu- R. (1990). Relation of weight to body image in discrimination, and ethnic identity among
roscience: A fuzzy-trace model. Developmen- pubertal girls and boys from two communi- urban sixth graders. Journal of Research on
tal Review, 31, 180–206. ties. Developmental Psychology, 26, 313–321. Adolescence, 19, 558–584.
Reyna, V., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality Richards, M., Crowe, P., Larson, R., & Swarr, A. Rivers, S., Reyna, V., & Mills, B. (2008). Risk tak-
in adolescent decision making: Implications (1998). Developmental patterns and gender ing under the influence: A fuzzy-trace theory
for theory, practice, and public policy. Psycho- differences in the experience of peer com- of emotion in adolescence. Developmental
logical Science in the Public Interest, 7, 1–44. panionship during adolescence. Child Devel- Review, 28, 107–144.
Reynolds, A., & Temple, J. (1998). Extended opment, 69, 154–163. Roberts, A., Seidman, E., Pederson, S., Chesir-
early childhood intervention and school Richards, M., & Larson, R. (1993). Pubertal de- Teran, D., Allen, L., Aber, J., . . . Hseuh, J. (2000).
achievement: Age thirteen findings from the velopment and the daily subjective states of Perceived family and peer transactions and
Chicago Longitudinal Study. Child Develop- young adolescents. Journal of Research on self-esteem among urban early adolescents.
ment, 69, 231–246. Adolescence, 3, 145–169. Journal of Early Adolescence, 20, 68–92.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-66 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-66 References

Roberts, B., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2001). The perceived self-worth in adolescents. Journal adolescence: Motivation, mental health, and
kids are alright: Growth and stability in of Research on Adolescence, 5, 253–280. family experience. Paper presented at the bi-
personality development from adolescence Roche, K. M., Ensminger, M. E., & Cherlin, A. J. ennial meetings of the Society for Research
to adulthood. Journal of Personality and (2007). Variations in parenting and adoles- on Adolescence, San Diego.
Social Psychology, 81, 670–683. cent outcomes among African American and Roeser, R., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (1996). Per-
Roberts, B., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The Latino families living in low-income, urban ceptions of the school psychological environ-
rank-order consistency of personality traits areas. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 882–909. ment and early adolescents’ psychological and
from childhood to old age: A quantitative Roche, K. M., Ensminger, M. E., Chilcoat, H., & behavioral functioning in school: The medi-
review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Storr, C. (2003). Establishing independence ating role of goals and belonging. Journal of
Bulletin, 126, 3–25. in low-income urban areas: The relationship Educational Psychology, 88, 408–422.
Roberts, B., O’Donnell, M., & Robins, R. (2004). to adolescent aggressive behavior. Journal of Rogers, I., Northstone, K., Dunger, D., Cooper,
Goal and personality trait development in Marriage and the Family, 65, 668. A., Ness, A., & Emmett, P. (2010). Diet
emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality Roche, K., Ghazarian, S., Little, T., & Leventhal, T. throughout childhood and age at menarche
and Social Psychology, 87, 541–550. (2011). Understanding links between puni- in a contemporary cohort of British girls.
Roberts, B., Walton, K., & Viechtbauer, W. tive parenting and adolescent adjustment: Public Health Nutrition, 13, 2052–2063.
(2006). Patterns of mean-level change in The relevance of context and reciprocal as- Rogers, J., Boruch, R., Stoms, G., & DeMoya, D.
personality traits across the life course: A sociations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, (1991). Impact of the Minnesota parental no-
meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psy- 21, 448–460. tification law on abortion and birth. American
chological Bulletin, 132, 1–25. Roderick, M., & Camburn, E. (1999). Risk and Journal of Public Health, 81, 294–298.
Roberts, D. (1993). Adolescents and the mass recovery from course failure in the early Rogers, M., & Holmbeck, G. (1997). Effects of
media: From “Leave It to Beaver” to “Beverly years of high school. American Educational brief interparental aggression on children’s
Hills 90210.” Teachers College Record, 94, Research Journal, 36, 303–343. adjustment: The moderating role of cogni-
629–644. Roderick, M., Coca, V., & Nagaoka, J. (2011). tive appraisal and coping. Journal of Family
Roberts, D., Foehr, U., & Rideout, V. (2005). Potholes on the road to college: High school Psychology, 11, 125–130.
Generation M: Media in the lives of 8–18- effects in shaping urban students’ participa- Rogosch, F. A., Oshri, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2010).
year-olds. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family tion in college application, four-year college From child maltreatment to adolescent
Foundation. enrollment, and college match. Sociology of cannabis abuse and dependence: A devel-
Roberts, D., & Henriksen, L., & Foehr, U. Education, 84, 178–211. opmental cascade model. Development and
(2009). Adolescence, adolescents, and me- Rodgers, J., & Rowe, D. (1993). Social contagion Psychopathology, 22, 883–897.
dia. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), and adolescent sexual behavior: A develop- Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Kahler, C. W., Seeley,
Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., mental EMOSA model. Psychological Review, J. R., & Brown, R. A. (2001). Natural course
Vol. 2, pp. 314–344). New York: Wiley. 100, 479–510. of alcohol use disorders from adolescence to
Roberts, M., Gibbons, F., Gerard, M., Weng, Rodgers, K., & Rose, H. A. (2002). Risk and resil- young adulthood. Journal of the American
C., Murry, V., Simons, L., . . . Lorenz, F. iency factors among adolescents who experi- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
(2012). From racial discrimination to risky ence marital transitions. Journal of Marriage 40, 83–90.
sex: Prospective relations involving peers and the Family, 64, 1024–1037. Roisman, G. I. (2002). Beyond main effect mod-
and parents. Developmental Psychology, 48, Rodkin, P., Farmer, T., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. els of adolescent work intensity, family
89–102. (2000). Heterogeneity of popular boys: Anti- closeness and school disengagement: Media-
Roberts, R., Phinney, J., Mâsse, L., Chen, Y., social and prosocial configurations. Develop- tional and conditional hypotheses. Journal of
Roberts, C., & Romero, A. (1999). The struc- mental Psychology, 36, 14–24. Adolescent Research, 17, 331–345.
ture of ethnic identity of young adolescents Rodriguez, N. (2010). The cumulative effect of Roisman, G. I., Aguilar, B., & Egeland, B. (2004).
from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal race and ethnicity in juvenile court outcomes Antisocial behavior in the transition to
of Early Adolescence, 19, 301–322. and why preadjudication detention matters. adulthood: The independent and interactive
Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C., & Chen, Y. (1997). Journal of Research in Crime & Delinquency, roles of developmental history and emerg-
Ethno cultural differences in prevalence of 47, 391–413. ing developmental tasks. Development and
adolescent depression. American Journal of Roe, K. (1995). Adolescents’ use of socially dis- Psychopathology, 16, 857–871.
Community Psychology, 25, 95–110. valued media: Towards a theory of media Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Cauffman, E.,
Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R., & Duong, H. T. delinquency. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Spieker, S., & The NICHD Early Child Care
(2008). Chronic insomnia and its negative cence, 24, 617–631. Research Network. (2009). The developmen-
consequences for health and functioning of Roenkae, A., & Pulkkinen, L. (1998). Work in- tal significance of adolescent romantic rela-
adolescents: A 12-month prospective study. volvement and timing of motherhood in the tionships: parent and peer predictors of
Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 294–302. accumulation of problems in social func- engagement and quality at age 15. Journal of
Robertson, A., Stein, J., & Schaefer-Rohleder, L. tioning in young women. Journal of Research Youth and Adolescence, 38, 1294–1303.
(2010). Effects of Hurricane Katrina and on Adolescence, 8, 221–239. Roisman, G. I., Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D.,
other adverse life events on adolescent female Roenneberg, T., Kuehnle, T., Pramstaller, P., & Tellegen, A. (2004). Salient and emerging
offenders: A test of General Strain Theory. Ricken, J., Havel, M., Guth, A., & Merrow, M. developmental tasks in the transition to
Journal of Research in Crime & Delinquency, (2004). A marker for the end of adolescence. adulthood. Child Development, 75, 123–133.
47, 469–495. Current Biology, 14, 1038–1039. Roisman, G., Monahan, K., Campbell, S.,
Robins, R., John, O., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., & Roeser, R., Eccles, J., & Freedman-Doan, C. Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., & The National
Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Resilient, (1999). Academic functioning and mental Institute of Child Health and Human
overcontrolled, and undercontrolled boys: health in adolescence: Patterns, progressions, Development Early Child Care Research
Three replicable personality types. Journal of and routes from childhood. Journal of Ado- Network. (2010). Is adolescence-onset anti-
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 157–171. lescent Research, 14, 135–174. social behavior developmentally norma-
Robinson, N. (1995). Evaluating the nature of Roeser, R., Lord, S., & Eccles, J. (1994, February). tive? Development and Psychopathology, 22,
perceived support and its relation to A portrait of academic alienation in 295–311.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-67 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-67

Romer, D., Betancourt, L., Brodsky, N., Giannetta, with friendship and emotional adjustment: Rosenthal, D., & Smith, A. (1997). Adolescent sex-
J., Yang, W., & Hurt, H. (2011). Does adolescent Considering the socioemotional trade-offs ual timetable. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
risk taking imply weak executive function? of co-rumination. Developmental Psychology, 26, 619–636.
A prospective study of relations between work- 43, 1019–1031. Rosenthal, D., Smith, A., & de Visser, R. (1999).
ing memory performance, impulsivity, and Rose, A. J., & Swenson, L. P. (2009). Do perceived Personal and social factors influencing age at
risk taking in early adolesence. Developmental popular adolescents who aggress against first sexual intercourse. Archives of Sexual
Science, 14, 1119–1133. others experience emotional adjustment Behavior, 28, 319–333.
Romer, D., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J., Brodsky, N., problems themselves? Developmental Psy- Rosenthal, N. L., & Kobak, R. (2010). Assessing
Farah, M., & Hurt, H. (2009a). Executive cogni- chology, 45, 868–872. adolescents’ attachment hierarchies: Dif-
tive functions and impulsivity as correlates of Rose, A. J., Schwartz-Mette, R., Smith, R., Asher, ferences across developmental periods and
risk taking and problem behavior in preadoles- S., Swenson, L., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. associations with individual adaptation.
cents. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2916–2926. (2012). How girls and boys expect that talk- Journal of Research on Adolescence , 20,
Romer, D., Black, M., Ricardo, I., Feigelman, S., ing about problems will make them feel: 678–706.
Kaljee, L., Galbraith, J., . . . Stanton, B. (1994). Associations with disclosure to friends in Roseth, C. J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T.
Social influences on the sexual behavior of childhood and adolescence. Child Develop- (2008). Promoting early adolescents’
youth at risk for HIV exposure. American ment, 83, 844–863. achievement and peer relationships: The ef-
Journal of Public Health, 84, 977–985. Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. fects of cooperative, competitive, and indi-
Romer, D., Sznitman, S., DiClemente, R., Salazar, (2004). Overt and relational aggression vidualistic goal structures. Psychological
L. F., Vanable, P. A., Carey, M. P., . . . Juzang, I. and perceived popularity: Developmental Bulletin, 134, 223–246.
(2009b). Mass media as an HIV-prevention differences in concurrent and prospective Rote, W., & Smetana, J. (2011). Social cognition.
strategy: Using culturally sensitive messages relations. Developmental Psychology, 40, In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclo-
to reduce HIV-associated sexual behavior of 378–387. pedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 333–341).
at-risk African American youth. American New York: Academic Press.
Rose, S., & Feldman, J. (1995). Prediction of IQ
Journal of Public Health, 99, 2150–2159. Roth, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Murray, L., & Foster,
and specific cognitive abilities at 11 years
Romero, A. J., Carvajal, S. C., Valle, F., & Orduña, from infancy measures. Developmental Psy- W. (1998). Promoting healthy adolescents:
M. (2007). Adolescent bicultural stress and its chology, 31, 685–696. Synthesis of youth development program
impact on mental well-being among Latinos, evaluations. Journal of Research on Adoles-
Rosen, L., Underwood, M., Beron, K., Gentsch, J.,
Asian Americans, and European Americans. cence, 8, 423–459.
Wharton, M., & Rahdar, A. (2009). Persistent
Journal of Community Psychology, 35, 519–534. Rotheram-Borus, M., & Koopman, C. (1991). Sex-
versus periodic experiences of social victim-
Romich, J., Lundberg, S., & Tsang, K. (2009). Inde- ization: Predictors of adjustment. Journal of ual risk behaviors, AIDS knowledge, and be-
pendence giving or autonomy taking? Child- Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 693–704. liefs about AIDS among runaways. American
hood predictors of decision-sharing patterns Journal of Public Health, 81, 206–208.
Rosenbaum, J. E. (2009). Patient teenagers? A
between young adolescents and parents. Jour- Rotheram-Borus, M., Marelich, W., & Srinivasan, S.
comparison of the sexual behavior of virginity
nal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 587–600. (1999). HIV risk among homosexual, bi-
pledgers and matched nonpledgers. Pediatrics,
Romo, L. F., Lefkowitz, E. S., Sigman, M., & Au, T. 123, e110–120. sexual, and heterosexual male and female
K. (2002). A longitudinal study of maternal youths. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28,
messages about dating and sexuality and Rosenbaum, J. E. (2011). The complexities of
159–177.
their influence on Latino adolescents. Journal college for all: Beyond fairy-tale dreams.
Sociology of Education, 84, 113–117. Rothman, E. F., Miller, E., Terpeluk, A., Glauber,
of Adolescent Health, 31, 59–69. A., & Randel, J. (2011). The proportion of
Rong, X., & Brown, F. (2001). The effects of im- Rosenberg, M. (1975). The dissonant context and U.S. parents who talk with their adolescent
migrant generation and ethnicity on educa- the adolescent self-concept. In S. Dragastin & children about dating abuse. Journal of
tional attainment among young African G. Elder, Jr. (Eds.), Adolescence in the life cycle. Adolescent Health, 49, 216–218.
and Caribbean Blacks in the United States. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Rouse, C. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., & McLanahan, S.
Harvard Educational Review, 71, 536–565. Rosenberg, M. (1986). Self-concept from middle (Eds.) (2005). The future of children: School
Roosa, M. W., Zeiders, K. H., Knight, G. P., Gonzales, childhood through adolescence. In J. Suls & A. Readiness: Closing racial and ethnic gaps.
N. A., Tein, J.-Y., Saenz, D., . . . Berkel, C. (2011). Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives Brooking Institution and Woodrow Wilson
A test of the social development model during on the self (Vol. 3). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. School of Public and International Affairs,
the transition to junior high with Mexican Rosenbloom, S. R., & Way, N. (2004). Experiences Princeton University.
American adolescents. Developmental Psychol- of discrimination among African American, Rousseau, J. (1911). Emile (B. Foxley, trans.).
ogy, 47, 527–537. Asian American, and Latino adolescents in an London: Dent. (Original work published
Rosario, M., Salzinger, S., Feldman R., & Ng-Mak, urban high school. Youth and Society, 35, 1762.)
D. (2008). Intervening processes between 420–451. Rovner, A., Nansel, T., Wang, J., & Iannotti, R.
youths’ exposure to community violence and Rosenblum, G., & Lewis, M. (1999). The rela- (2011). Food sold in school vending ma-
internalizing symptoms over time: The roles of tions among body image, physical attractive- chines is associated with overall dietary in-
social support and coping. American Journal of ness, and body mass in adolescence. Child take. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 13–19.
Community Psychology, 41, 43–62. Development, 70, 50–64. Rowe, C., La Greca, A., & Alexandersson, A.
Roscigno, V., & Ainsworth-Darnell, J. (1999). Race, Rosenthal, D. (1994, February). Gendered con- (2010). Family and individual factors as-
cultural capital, and educational resources: structions of adolescent sexuality. Paper pre- sociated with substance involvement and
Persistent inequalities and achievement re- sented at the biennial meetings of the Society PTS symptoms among adolescents in
turns. Sociology of Education, 72, 158–178. for Research on Adolescence, San Diego. greater New Orleans after Hurricane
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friend- Rosenthal, D., & Feldman, S. (1990). The accul- Katrina. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
ships of girls and boys. Child Development, turation of Chinese immigrants: The effects Psychology, 78, 806–817.
73, 1830–1843. on family functioning of length of residence Rowe, D., Rodgers, J., Meseck-Bushey S., & St.
Rose, A. J., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. M. (2007). in two cultural contexts. Journal of Genetic John, C. (1989). Sexual behavior and non-
Prospective associations of co-rumination Psychology, 151, 493–514. sexual deviance: A sibling study of their
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-68 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-68 References

relationship. Developmental Psychology, 25, Rumbaut, R. (1997). Assimilation and its discon- the school tobacco context: Are they linked
61–69. tents: Between rhetoric and reality. Interna- to students’ smoking status? Journal of
Rowe, D., Vazsonyi, A., & Flannery, D. (1994). No tional Migration Review, 31, 923–960. Youth and Adolescence, 38, 1374–1387.
more than skin deep: Ethnic and racial similar- Rumberger, R. (2012). Dropping out. Cambridge, Sackett, P. R., Kuncel, N. R., Arneson, J. J., Cooper,
ity in developmental processes. Psychological MA: Harvard University Press. S. R., & Waters, S. D. (2009). Does socioeco-
Review, 101, 396–413. Rumberger, R., Ghatak, R., Poulos, G., Ritter, P., nomic status explain the relationship be-
Roye, C., & Balk, S. (1996). The relationship of & Dornbusch, S. (1990). Family influences tween admissions tests and post-secondary
partner support to outcomes for teenage on dropout behavior in one California high academic performance? Psychological Bulle-
mothers and their children: A review. Journal school. Sociology of Education, 63, 283–299. tin, 135, 1–22.
of Adolescent Health, 19, 86–93. Rumberger, R., & Palardy, G. (2005). Test Saewyc, E. M. (2011). Research on adolescent
Rozin, P., Bauer, R., & Catanese, D. (2003). Food scores, dropout rates, and transfer rates as sexual orientation: Development, health dis-
and life, pleasure and worry, among American alternative indicators of high school per- parities, stigma, and resilience. Journal of Re-
college students: Gender differences and re- formance. American Education Research search on Adolescence, 21, 256–272.
gional similarities. Journal of Personality and Journal, 42, 3–42. Safron, J., Sy, S., & Schulenberg, J. (2003). Wishing
Social Psychology, 85, 132–141. Rusby, J. C., Forrester, K. K., Biglan A., & Metzler, to work: New perspectives on how adolescents’
Rubin, K., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. (2006). Peer C. W. (2005). Relationships between peer ha- part-time work intensity is linked to educa-
interactions, relationships, and groups. In rassment and adolescent problem behaviors. tional disengagement, substance use, and
W. Damon & R. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Journal of Early Adolescence, 25, 453–477. other problem behaviours. International Jour-
Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child Russell, S. (1994). Life course antecedents of pre- nal of Behavioral Development, 27, 301–315.
psychology: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and per- marital conception in Great Britain. Journal Sagar, H., Schofield, J., & Snyder, H. (1983). Race
sonality development (6th ed., pp 571–645). of Marriage and the Family, 56, 480–492. and gender barriers: Preadolescent peer be-
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Russell, S. (2002). Childhood development risk havior in academic classrooms. Child Devel-
Rubin, K., LeMare, L., & Lollis, S. (1990). Social for teen childbearing in Britain. Journal of opment, 54, 1032–1040.
withdrawal in childhood: Developmental Research on Adolescence, 12, 305–324. Sagrestano, L., McCormick, S., Paikoff, R., &
pathways to peer rejection. In S. Asher & Russell, S., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Conger, R. (1997, Holmbeck, G. (1999). Pubertal development
J. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood (pp. April). School transitions and academic and parent–child conflict in low-income, ur-
217–249). New York: Cambridge University achievement. Paper presented at the biennial ban, African American adolescents. Journal
Press. meetings of the Society for Research in Child of Research on Adolescence, 9, 85–107.
Ruchkin, V., Henrich, C. C., Jones, S. M., Vermeiren, Development, Washington, DC. Salafia, E., & Gondoli, D. (2011). A 4-year longi-
R., & Schwab-Stone, M. (2007). Violence Russell, S., Thompson, E., & Harris, R. (2011). tudinal investigation of the processes by
exposure and psychopathology in urban Sexual orientation. In B. Brown & M. which parents and peers influence the devel-
youth: The mediating role of posttraumatic Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- opment of early adolescent girls’ bulimic
stress. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, cence (Vol. 1, pp. 325–332). New York: Ac- symptoms. Journal of Early Adolescence, 31,
35, 578–593. ademic Press. 390–414.
Ruck, M., Abramovitch, R., & Keating, D. Ryan, A. M. (2001). The peer group as a context Sale, E., Sambrano, S., Springer, J. F., Pena, C.,
(1998). Children and adolescents’ under- for the development of young adolescent Pan, W., & Kasim, R. (2005). Family protec-
standing of rights: Balancing nurturance motivation and achievement. Child Develop- tion and prevention of alcohol use among
and self-determination. Child Development, ment, 72, 1135–1150. Hispanic youth at high risk. American Journal
64, 404–417. Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The class- of Community Psychology, 36, 195–205.
Ruck, M., Peterson-Badali, M., & Day, D. M. room social environment and changes in Salekin, R. T. (2008). Psychopathy and recidivism
(2002). Adolescents’ and mothers’ under- adolescents’ motivation and engagement from mid-adolescence to young adulthood:
standing of children’s rights in the home. during middle school. American Educational Cumulating legal problems and limiting life
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12 , Research Journal, 38, 437–460. opportunities. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
373–398. Ryan, J., Marshall, J., Herz, D., & Hernandez, P. 117, 386–395.
Rudolph, K., & Hammen, C. (1999). Age and (2008). Juvenile delinquency in child welfare: Salem, D., Zimmerman, M., & Notaro, P. (1998).
gender as determinants of stress exposure, Investigating group home effects. Children Effects of family structure, family process,
generation, and reactions in youngsters: A and Youth Services Review, 30, 1088–1099. and father involvement on psychosocial out-
transactional perspective. Child Develop- Ryan, R., & Lynch, J. (1989). Emotional auton- comes among African American adolescents.
ment, 70, 660–677. omy versus detachment: Revisiting the vicis- Family Relations, 47, 331–341.
Rudolph, K., & Klein, D. (2009). Exploring situdes of adolescence and young adulthood. Salinger, J. D. (1964). The catcher in the rye. New
depressive personality traits in youth: Ori- Child Development, 60, 340–356. York: Bantam Books. (Original work pub-
gins, correlates, and developmental conse- Ryan, S., Franzetta, K., & Manlove, J. (2007). lished 1951)
quences. Development and Psychopathology, Knowledge, perceptions, and motivations for Sallquist, J., Eisenberg, N., French, D. C., Purwono,
21, 1155–1180. contraception: Influence on teens’ contra- U., & Suryanti, T. A. (2010). Indonesian ado-
Rueter, M., & Kwon, H. (2005). Developmental ceptive consistency. Youth and Society, 39, lescents’ spiritual and religious experiences
trends in adolescent suicidal ideation. Journal 182–208. and their longitudinal relations with socio-
of Research on Adolescence, 15, 205–222. Ryan, S., Franzetta, K., Manlove, J., & Schelar, E. emotional functioning. Developmental Psy-
Ruggles, S. (1994). The origins of African- (2008). Older sexual partners during adoles- chology, 46, 699–716.
American family structure. American Socio- cence: Links to reproductive health out- Salmivalli, C. (1998). Intelligent, attractive, well-
logical Review, 59, 136–151. comes in young adulthood. Perspectives on behaving, unhappy: The structure of ado-
Ruiz, S., Roosa, M., & Gonzales, N. (2002). Pre- Sexual and Reproductive Health, 40, 17–26. lescents’ self-concept and its relations to
dictors of self-esteem for Mexican American Sabiston, C. M., Lovato, C. Y., Ahmed, R., their social behavior. Journal of Research on
and European American youths: A reexami- Pullman, A. W., Hadd, V., Campbell, H. S., . . . Adolescence, 8, 333–354.
nation of the influence of parenting. Journal Brown, K. (2009). School smoking policy Salsberry, P., Reagan, P., & Pajer, K. (2009).
of Family Psychology, 16, 70–80. characteristics and individual perceptions of Growth differences by age of menarche in
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-69 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-69

African American and White girls. Nursing behavior: Estimates and trends from four processes. Journal of Community Psychol-
Research, 58, 382–390. nationally representative surveys. Family ogy, 34, 401–413.
Salzinger, S., Feldman, R., Rosario, M., & Planning Perspectives, 32, 156–165. Scales, P., Benson, P., Roehlkepartain, E., Sesma,
Ng-Mak, D. (2011). Role of parent and Santelli, J., Morrow, B., Anderson, J., & Lindberg, L. Jr., A., & van Dulmen, M. (2006). The role of
peer relationships and individual charac- (2006). Contraceptive use and pregnancy risk developmental assets in predicting academic
teristics in middle school children’s behav- among U.S. high school students, 1991–2003. achievement: A longitudinal study. Journal of
ioral outcomes in the face of community Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, Adolescence, 29, 691–708.
violence. Journal of Research on Adoles- 38, 106–111. Scanlan, T., Bakes, M., & Scanlan, L. (2005). Par-
cence, 21, 395–407. Santelli, J., Orr, M., Lindberg, L., & Diaz, D. (2009). ticipation in sport: A developmental glimpse
Samela-Aro, K. (2011). Stages of adolescence. In B. Changing behavioral risk for pregnancy at emotion. In J. Mahoney, R. Larson, & J.
Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of among high school students in the United Eccles (Eds.), Organized activities as contexts
adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 360–368). New York: States, 1991–2007. Journal of Adolescent of development (pp. 275–309). Hillsdale, NJ:
Academic Press. Health, 45, 25–32. Erlbaum.
Sampson, R. (1992). Family management and Santelli, J., Warren, C., Lowry, R., Sogolow, E., Scaramella, L., Conger, R. D., Spoth, R., & Simons,
child development: Insights from social dis- Collins, J., Kann, L., . . . Celentano, D. (1997). R. L. (2002). Evaluation of a social contex-
organization theory. In J. McCord (Ed.), The use of condoms with other contracep- tual model of delinquency: A cross-study
Advances in criminological theory (Vol. 3, pp. tive methods among young men and women. replication. Child Development, 73, 175–195.
63–93). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Family Planning Perspectives, 29, 261–267. Schaefer, D. R., Simpkins, S. D., Vest, A. E., &
Sampson, R. (1997). Collective regulation of Santesso, D. L., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2008). Devel- Price, C. D. (2011). The contribution of ex-
adolescent misbehavior: Validation results opmental differences in error-related ERPs tracurricular activities to adolescent friend-
from eighty Chicago neighborhoods. Journal in middle- to late-adolescent males. Develop- ships: New insights through social network
of Adolescent Research, 12, 227–244. mental Psychology, 44, 205–217. analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47,
Sampson, R., & Laub, J. (2003). Life-course desis- 1141–1152.
Saporito, S., & Sohoni, D. (2006). Coloring out-
tors? Trajectories of crime among delinquent side the lines: Racial segregation in public Scharf, M., Shulman, S., & Avigad-Spitz, L.
boys followed to age 70. Criminology, 41, schools and their attendance boundaries. (2005). Sibling relationships in emerging
555–592. Sociology of Education, 79, 81–105. adulthood and in adolescence. Journal of
Sampson, R., Raudenbusch, S., & Earls, F. (1997). Adolescent Research, 20, 64–90.
Sargent, J., & Dalton, M. (2001). Does parental
Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multi- disapproval of smoking prevent adolescents Scheier, L., & Botvin, G. (1998). Relations of
level study of collective efficacy for children. from becoming established smokers? Pediat- social skills, personal competence, and ado-
Science, 277, 918–924. rics, 108, 1256–1262. lescent alcohol use: A developmental explor-
Sánchez, B., Esparza, P., & Colón, Y. (2008). atory study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18,
Sarwer, D. B., & Dilks, R. J. (2012). Invited com-
Natural mentoring under the microscope: 77–114.
mentary: Childhood and adolescent obesity:
An investigation of mentoring relationships Scheier, L., Botvin, G. J., Griffin, K. W., & Diaz,
Psychological and behavioral issues in
and Latino adolescents’ academic perfor- T. (2000). Dynamic growth models of self-
weight loss treatment. Journal of Youth and
mance. Journal of Community Psychology, esteem and adolescent alcohol use. Journal
Adolescence, 41, 98–104.
36, 468–482. of Early Adolescence, 20, 178–209.
Saunders, J., Davis, L., Williams, T., & Williams, J. H.
Sanchez, Z. M., Opaleye, E. S., Chaves, T. V., Schellenbach, C., Whitman, T., & Borkowski, J.
(2004). Gender differences in self-perceptions
Noto, A. R., & Nappo, S. A. (2011). God for- (1992). Toward an integrative model of ado-
and academic outcomes: A study of African
bids or mom disapproves? Religious beliefs lescent parenting. Human Development, 35,
American high school students. Journal of
that prevent drug use among youth. Journal 81–99.
Youth and Adolescence, 33, 81–90.
of Adolescent Research, 26, 591–616. Schepis, T. S., & Krishnan-Sarin, S. (2009).
Savage, M., & Scott, L. (1998). Physical activity
Sandfort, J., & Hill, M. (1996). Assisting young, Sources of prescriptions for misuse by ado-
and rural middle school adolescents. Journal
unmarried mothers to become self-sufficient: lescents: Differences in sex, ethnicity, and
of Youth and Adolescence, 27, 245–253.
The effects of different types of early eco- severity of misuse in a population-based
nomic support. Journal of Marriage and the Savin-Williams, R. (2006). Who’s gay? Does it study. Journal of the American Academy of
Family, 58, 311–326. matter? Current Directions in Psychological Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 828–836.
Science, 15, 40–44.
Sandfort, T. G., Bos, H., Collier, K. L., & Metselaar, Scherf, K., Behrmann, M., & Dahl, R. (2012).
M. (2010). School environment and the Savin-Williams, R., & Berndt, T. (1990). Friend- Facing changes and changing faces in ado-
mental health of sexual minority youths: A ship and peer relations. In S. Feldman & G. lescence: A new model for investigating
study among Dutch young adolescents. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent-specific interactions between
American Journal of Public Health, 100, adolescent (pp. 277–307). Cambridge, MA: pubertal, brain and behavioral develop-
1696–1700. Harvard University Press. ment. Developmental Cognitive Neurosci-
Santelli, J., Abma, J., Ventura, S., Lindberg, L., Savin-Williams, R., & Demo, D. (1983). Situa- ence, 2, 199–219.
Morrow, B., Anderson, J. E., . . . Hamilton, B. tional and transituational determinants of Schiller, K. (1999). Effects of feeder patterns on
(2004). Can changes in sexual behaviors adolescent self-feelings. Journal of Personality students’ transition to high school. Sociology
among high school students explain the de- and Social Psychology, 44, 824–833. of Education, 72, 216–233.
cline in teen pregnancy rates in the 1990s? Savin-Williams, R., & Ream, G. (2007). Preva- Schilling, E. A., Aseltine, R. H. J., Glanovsky, J. L.,
Journal of Adolescent Health, 35, 80–90. lence and stability of sexual orientation com- James, A., & Jacobs, D. (2009). Adolescent
Santelli, J., Carter, M., Orr, M., & Dittus, P. ponents during adolescence and young alcohol use, suicidal ideation, and suicide at-
(2009). Trends in sexual risk behaviors, by adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, tempts. Journal of Adolescent Health, 44,
nonsexual risk behavior involvement, U.S. 385–394. 335–341.
high school students, 1991–2007. Journal of Scales, P., Benson, P., & Mannes, M. (2006). Schleepen, T., & Jonkman, L. (2009). The devel-
Adolescent Health, 44, 372–379. The contribution to adolescent well-being opment of non-spatial working memory
Santelli, J., Lindberg, L., Abma, J., McNeely, C., made by nonfamily adults: An examination capacity during childhood and adolescence
& Resnick, M. (2000). Adolescent sexual of developmental assets as contexts and and the role of interference control: An
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-70 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-70 References

n-back task study. Developmental Neuropsy- students: A longitudinal study. Journal of with perceived racial discrimination among
chology, 35, 37–56. Educational Psychology, 89, 37–40. Black youth. Journal of Black Psychology, 31,
Schlegel, A. (2009). Cross-cultural issues in Schooler, D., Sorsoli, C., Kim, J., & Tolman, D. 254–272.
the study of adolescent development. In R. (2009). Beyond exposure: A person-oriented Seaton, E., Caldwell, C., Sellers, R., & Jackson, J.
Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of approach to adolescent media diets. Journal (2008). The prevalence of perceived dis-
adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. of Research on Adolescence, 19, 484–508. crimination among African American and
570–589). New York: Wiley. Schoon, I., Bynner, J., Joshi, H., Parsons, S., Caribbean Black youth. Developmental Psy-
Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. (1991). Adolescence: An Wiggins, R. D., & Sacker, A. (2002). The in- chology, 44, 1288–1297.
anthropological inquiry. New York: Free Press. fluence of context, timing, and duration of Seaton, E., Caldwell, C., Sellers, R., & Jackson, J.
Schmidt, J. (2003). Correlates of reduced mis- risk experiences for the passage from child- (2010a). An intersectional approach for un-
conduct among adolescents facing adver- hood to midadulthood. Child Development, derstanding perceived discrimination and
sity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 73, 1486–1504. psychological well-being among African
439–452. Schoon, I., Parsons, S., & Sacker, A. (2004). Socio- American and Caribbean Black youth. De-
Schmidt, J., & Padilla, B. (2003). Self-esteem and economic adversity, educational resilience, velopmental Psychology, 46, 1372–1379.
family challenge: An investigation of their and subsequent levels of adult adaptation. Seaton, E., & Gilbert, A. (2011). Ethnic/racial
effects on achievement. Journal of Youth and Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 383–404. identity among minority youth. In B. Brown
Adolescence, 32, 37–46. Schulenberg, J., Bryant, A. L., & O’Malley, P. & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of ado-
Schmidt, J., Shumow, L., & Kackar, H. (2007). (2004). Taking hold of some kind of life: How lescence (Vol. 2, pp. 68–74). New York: Aca-
Adolescents’ participation in service activi- developmental tasks relate to trajectories of demic Press.
ties and its impact on academic, behavioral, well-being during the transition to adult- Seaton, E., Neblett, E., Upton, R., Hammond, W.,
and civic outcomes. Journal of Youth and hood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, & Sellers, R. (2011). The moderating capac-
Adolescence, 36, 127–140. 1119–1140. ity of racial identity between perceived dis-
Schmidt, M. G., Reppucci, N. D., & Woolard, J. Schulenberg, J., Maggs, J., Dielman, T., Leech, S., crimination and psychological well-being
(2003). Effectiveness of participation as a Kloska, D., Shope, J., & Laetz, V. (1999). On over time among African American youth.
defendant: The attorney–juvenile client rela- peer influences to get drunk: A panel study of Child Development, 82, 1850–1867.
tionships. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, young adolescents. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Seaton, E., Scottham, K., & Sellers, R. (2006). The
21, 175–198. 45, 108–142. status model of racial identity development
Schneider, B., & Stevenson, D. (1999). The ambi- Schulenberg, J., Wadsworth, K., O’Malley, P., in African American adolescents: Evidence
tious generations: America’s teenagers, moti- Bachman, J., & Johnston, L. (1996). Adoles- of structure, trajectories, and well-being.
vated but directionless. New Haven, CT: Yale cent risk factors for binge drinking during Child Development, 77, 1416–1426.
University Press. the transition to young adulthood: Variable- Seaton, E., & Yip, T. (2009). School and neighbor-
Schneiders, J., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, J., Feron, and pattern-centered approaches to change. hood contexts, perceptions of racial discrimi-
F. J., van Os, J., & deVries, M. W. (2006). Developmental Psychology, 32, 659–674. nation, and psychological well-being among
Mood reactivity to daily negative events in Schwartz, S. E. O., Rhodes, J. E., Chan, C. S., & African American adolescents. Journal of
early adolescence: Relationship to risk for Herrera, C. (2011). The impact of school- Youth and Adolescence, 38, 153–163.
psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, based mentoring on youths with different Seaton, E., Yip, T., & Sellers, R. (2009). A longi-
42, 543–554. relational profiles. Developmental Psychology, tudinal examination of racial identity
Schochet, T., Kelley, A., & Landry, C. (2004). 47, 450–462. and racial discrimination among African
Differential behavioral effects of nicotine Schwartz, S. J., Côté, J. E., & Arnett, J. J. (2005). American adolescents. Child Development,
exposure in adolescent and adult rats. Psy- Identity and agency in emerging adult- 80, 406–417.
chopharmacology, 175, 265–273. hood: Two developmental routes in the in- Sebastian, C., Burnett, S., & Blakemore, S.
Schochet, T., Kelley, A., & Landry, C. (2005). Dif- dividualization process. Youth and Society, (2008). Development of the self-concept
ferential expression of arc mRNA and other 37, 201–229. during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive
plasticity-related genes induced by nicotine Schwartz, S. J., Pantin, H., Prado, G., Sullivan, S., Science, 12, 441–446.
in adolescent rat forebrain. Neuroscience, & Szapocznik, J. (2005). Family functioning, Sebastian, C., Tan, G., Roiser, J., Viding, E.,
135, 285–297. identity and problem behavior in Hispanic Dumontheil, I., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2011).
Schofield, H. T., Bierman, K. L., Heinrichs, B., immigrant early adolescents. Journal of Early Developmental influences on the neural
& Nix, R. L. (2008). Predicting early sexual Adolescence, 25, 392–420. bases of responses to social rejection: Impli-
activity with behavior problems exhibited Schwartz-Mette, R., & Rose, A. (2012). Co-rumi- cations of social neuroscience for education.
at school entry and in early adolescence. nation mediates contagion of internalizing Neuroimage, 57, 686–694.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, symptoms within youths’ friendships. Devel- Seegers, V., Petit, D., Falissard, B., Vitaro, F.,
1175–1188. opmental Psychology, 48, 1355–1365. Tremblay, R., Montplaisir, J., & Touchette, E.
Schofield, T. J., Conger, R. D., Martin, M. J., Schweder, R. (Ed.). (1998). Welcome to middle (2011). Short sleep duration and body mass
Stockdale, G. D., Conger, K. J., & Widaman, age! And other cultural fictions. Chicago: Uni- index: A prospective longitudinal study in
K. F. (2009). Reciprocity in parenting of ado- versity of Chicago Press. preadolescence. American Journal of Epide-
lescents within the context of marital negativ- Schweingruber, H. A., & Kalil, A. (2000). Deci- miology, 173, 621–629.
ity. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1708–1722. sion making and depressive symptoms in Segalowitz, S. J., & Davies, P. L. (2004). Chart-
Scholte, R., van Lieshout, C., & van Aken, C. Black and White multigenerational teen- ing the maturation of the frontal lobe: An
(2001). Perceived relational support in ado- parent families. Journal of Family Psychology, electrophysiological strategy. Brain and
lescence: Dimensions, configurations, and 14, 556–569. Cognition, 55, 116–133.
adolescent adjustment. Journal of Research Scott, E., & Steinberg, L. (2008). Rethinking juve- Seguin, J., Arseneault, L., & Tremblay, R. (2007).
on Adolescence, 11, 71–94. nile justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- The contribution of “cool” and “hot” com-
Schommer, M., Calvert, C., Gariglietti, G., & versity Press. ponents of decision-making in adolescence:
Bajaj, A. (1997). The development of episte- Scott, L. D., & House, L. E. (2005). Relationship Implications for developmental psychopa-
mological beliefs among secondary school of distress and perceived control to coping thology. Cognitive Development, 22, 530–543.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-71 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-71

Seidman, E., Aber, J., Allen, L., & French, S. (1996). among incarcerated adolescents. Journal of D. T. (2002). Association of early childbear-
The impact of the transition to high school Abnormal Psychology, 110, 136–141. ing and low cognitive ability. Perspectives on
on the self-system and perceived social con- Shanahan, L., McHale, S., Crouter, A., & Osgood, Sexual and Reproductive Health, 34, 236.
text of poor urban youth. American Journal of D. W. (2008). Linkages between parents’ dif- Shedler, J., & Block, J. (1990). Adolescent drug
Community Psychology, 24, 489–515. ferential treatment, youth depressive symp- use and psychological health: A longitudinal
Seidman, E., & French, S. E. (2004). Developmen- toms, and sibling relationships. Journal of inquiry. American Psychologist, 45, 612–630.
tal trajectories and ecological transitions: A Marriage and Family, 70, 480–494. Sheeran, P., Abraham, C., & Orbell, S. (1999).
two-step procedure to aid in the choice of pre- Shanahan, L., McHale, S., Osgood, D. W., & Psychosocial correlates of heterosexual con-
vention and promotion interventions. Devel- Crouter, A. (2007). Conflict frequency with dom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bul-
opment and Psychopathology, 16, 1141–1159. mothers and fathers from middle childhood letin, 125, 90–132.
Seidman, E., Lambert, L. E., Allen, L., & Aber, J. to late adolescence: Within- and between- Sheidow, A. J., Gorman-Smith, D., Tolan, P. H.,
L. (2003). Urban adolescents’ transition to families comparisons. Developmental Psy- & Henry, D. B. (2001). Family and commu-
junior high school and protective family chology, 43, 539–550. nity characteristics: Risk factors for violence
transactions. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, Shanahan, M. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2004). Develop- exposure in inner-city youth. Journal of
166–193. mental properties of transactional models: Community Psychology, 29, 345–360.
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2003). Testing theories of The case of life events and mastery from Sheidow, A. J., Strachan, M. K., Minden, J. A.,
romantic development from adolescence to adolescence to young adulthood. Develop- Henry, D. B., Tolan, P. H., & Gorman-
young adulthood: Evidence of a develop- ment and Psychopathology, 16, 1095–1117. Smith, D. (2008). The relation of antisocial
mental sequence. International Journal of Shanahan, M. J., & Flaherty, B. P. (2001). Dy- behavior patterns and changes in internal-
Behavioral Development, 27, 519–531. namic patterns of time use in adolescence. izing symptoms for a sample of inner-city
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2006). Leaving home or still Child Development, 72, 385–401. youth: Comorbidity within a developmen-
in the nest? Parent–child relationships and Shanahan, M. J., Porfeli, E., Mortimer, J. T., & tal framework. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
psychological health as predictors of differ- Erickson, L. (2005). Subjective age identity cence, 37, 821–829.
ent leaving home patterns. Developmental and the transition to adulthood: When do Shek, D. (2007). A longitudinal study of per-
Psychology, 42, 864–876. adolescents become adults? In R. Settersten, ceived differences in parental control and
Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Beyers, W. (2005). Coping F. Furstenberg, Jr., & R. Rumbaut (Eds.), On parent–child relational qualities in Chinese
trajectories from adolescence to young the frontier of adulthood (pp. 225–255). adolescents in Hong Kong. Journal of Adoles-
adulthood: Links to attachment state of Chicago: University of Chicago Press. cent Research, 22, 156–188.
mind. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15, Shannon, B. J., Raichle, M. E., Snyder, A. Z., Fair, Sher, K. J., Gotham, H. J., & Watson, A. L. (2004).
561–582. D. A., Mills, K. L., Zhang, D., . . . Kiehl, K. Trajectories of dynamic predictors of disor-
Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Klessinger, N. (2000). Long- (2011). Premotor functional connectivity der: Their meanings and implications. De-
term effects of avoidant coping on adoles- predicts impulsivity in juvenile offenders. velopment and Psychopathology, 16, 825–856.
cents’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth Proceedings of the National Academy of Sher-Censor, E., Parke, R. D., & Coltrane, S.
and Adolescence, 29, 617–630. Sciences, 108(27), 11241–11245. (2011). Parents’ promotion of psychologi-
Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Stemmler, M. (2002). Sharabany, R., Gershoni, R., & Hofman, J. (1981). cal autonomy, psychological control, and
Factors contributing to gender differences Girlfriend, boyfriend: Age and sex differ- Mexican–American adolescents’ adjust-
in depressive symptoms: A test of three de- ences in intimate friendship. Developmental ment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40,
velopmental models. Journal of Youth and Psychology, 17, 800–808. 620–632.
Adolescence, 31, 405–417. Sharkey, P., & Sampson, R. (2010). Destination ef- Shernoff, D. J., & Schmidt, J. A. (2008). Further
Selfhout, M., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2008). The fects: Residential mobility and trajectories of evidence of an engagement–achievement
development of delinquency and perceived adolescent violence in a stratified metropolis. paradox among U.S. high school students.
friendship quality in adolescent best friend- Criminology, 48, 639–681. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 564–580.
ship dyads. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- Sharp, C., HA, C., & Fonagy, P. (2011). Get them Shernoff, D. J., & Vandell, D. L. (2007). Engage-
chology, 36, 471–485. before they get you: Trust, trustworthiness, ment in after-school program activities:
Sellers, R., Copeland-Linder, N., Martin, P., & and social cognition in boys with and with- Quality of experience from the perspective
Lewis, R. (2006). Racial identity matters: The out externalizing behavior problems. Devel- of participants. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
relationship between racial discrimination opment and Psychopathology, 23, 647–658. cence, 36, 891–903.
and psychological functioning in African Shaw, P., Gilliam, M., Liverpool, M., Weddle, C., Sherrod, L., & Lauckhardt, J. (2009). The devel-
American adolescents. Journal of Research on Malek, M., Sharp, W., . . . Giedd, J. (2011). opment of citizenship. In R. Lerner & L.
Adolescence, 16, 187–216. Cortical development in typically develop- Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psy-
Sells, C., & Blum, R. (1996). Morbidity and mor- ing children with symptoms of hyperactiv- chology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 372–408). New
tality among U.S. adolescents: An overview ity and impulsivity: Support for a York: Wiley.
of data and trends. American Journal of Pub- dimensional view of Attention Deficit Hy- Shih, T. (1998). Finding the niche: Friendship
lic Health, 86, 513–519. peractivity Disorder. American Journal of formation of immigrant adolescents. Youth
Serovich, J., & Greene, K. (1997). Predictors of Psychiatry, 168, 143–151. and Society, 30, 209–240.
adolescent sexual risk taking behaviors Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Klasen, L., Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A.
which put them at risk for contracting Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., . . . Giedd, J. (2006). In- (2002). A developmental perspective on
HIV. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, tellectual ability and cortical development in personality in emerging adulthood: Child-
429–444. children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 676–679. hood antecedents and concurrent adapta-
Settersten, R., Furstenberg, F., Jr., & Rumbaut, R. Shaywitz, S., Gruen, J., & Shaywitz, B. (2007). tion. Journal of Personality and Social
(Eds.). (2005). On the frontier of adulthood. Management of dyslexia, its rationale and Psychology, 83, 1165–1177.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. underlying neurobiology. Pediatric Clinics of Shirtcliff, E. A., Vitacco, M. J., Graf, A. R.,
Shahinfar, A., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Matza, L. S. North America, 54, 609–623. Gostisha, A. J., Merz, J. L., & Zahn-Waxler, C.
(2001). The relation between exposure to Shearer, D. L., Mulvihill, B. A., Klerman, L. V., (2009). Neurobiology of empathy and cal-
violence and social information processing Wallander, J. L., Hovinga, M. E., & Redden, lousness: Implications for the development
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-72 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-72 References

of antisocial behavior. Behavioral Sciences & Siegel, J., Yancey, A., Aneshensel, C., & Schuler, R. unhealthy weight control, sexual risk be-
the Law, 27, 137–171. (1999). Body image, perceived pubertal tim- havior, pregnancy, and suicidality. Journal
Shoop, D., & Davidson, P. (1994). AIDS and ado- ing, and adolescent mental health. Journal of of the American Medical Association, 286,
lescents: The relation of parent and partner Adolescent Health, 25, 155–165. 572–579.
communication to adolescent condom use. Siegel, M., & Biener, L. (2000). The impact of an Sim, T. (2000). Adolescent psychosocial compe-
Journal of Adolescence, 17, 137–148. antismoking media campaign on progres- tence: The importance and role of regard for
Shtarkshall, R. A., Carmel, S., Jaffe-Hirschfield, sion to established smoking: Results of a lon- parents. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
D., & Woloski-Wruble, A. (2009). Sexual gitudinal study. American Journal of Public 10, 49–64.
milestones and factors associated with coitus Health, 90, 380–386. Sim, T., & Koh, S. (2003). A domain conceptual-
initiation among Israeli high school students. Siegel, R. S., La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. ization of adolescent susceptibility to peer
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 591–604. (2009). Peer victimization and social anxiety pressure. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
Shulman, E. P., Cauffman, E., Piquero, A. R., & in adolescents: Prospective and reciprocal 13, 57–80.
Fagan, J. (2011). Moral disengagement among relationships. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Simmons, R. (2003). Odd girl out. New York:
serious juvenile offenders: A longitudinal cence, 38, 1096–1109. Harvest Books.
study of the relations between morally disen- Siegler, R. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learn- Simmons, R., Blyth, D., & McKinney, K. (1983).
gaged attitudes and offending. Developmental ing. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & The social and psychological effects of pu-
Psychology, 47, 1619–1632. D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child berty on White females. In J. Brooks-Gunn
Shulman, S., Connolly, J., & McIssac, C. (2011). psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and & A. Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty (pp.
Romantic relationships. In B. Brown & M. language (6th ed., pp. 464–510). Hoboken, NJ: 229–272). New York: Plenum.
Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence Wiley. Simmons, R., Rosenberg, F., & Rosenberg, M.
(Vol. 2, pp. 289–298). New York: Academic Sieving, R. E., Eisenberg, M. E., Pettingell, S., & (1973). Disturbance in the self-image at ado-
Press. Skay, C. (2006). Friends’ influence on adoles- lescence. American Sociological Review, 38,
Shulman, S., & Laursen, B. (2002). Adolescent cents’ first sexual intercourse. Perspectives on 553–568.
perceptions of conflict in interdependent Sexual and Reproductive Health, 38, 13–19. Simon, T. R., Miller, S., Gorman-Smith, D., Orpinas,
and disengaged friendships. Journal of Re- Sigler-Rushton, W. (2005). Young fatherhood P., & Sullivan, T. (2010). Physical dating vio-
search on Adolescence, 12, 353–372. and subsequent disadvantage in the United lence norms and behavior among sixth-
Shulman, S., Laursen, B., Kalman, Z., & Karpovsky, Kingdom. Journal of Marriage and the Family, grade students from four U.S. sites. Journal of
S. (1997). Adolescent intimacy revisited. Jour- 67, 735. Early Adolescence, 30, 395–409.
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 597–617. Silbereisen, R., Petersen, A., Albrecht, H., & Simon, V. A., Aikins, J. W., & Prinstein, M. J.
Shulman, S., & Scharf, M. (2000). Adolescent ro- Kracke, B. (1989). Maturational timing and (2008). Romantic partner selection and so-
mantic behaviors and perceptions: Age- and the development of problem behavior: cialization during early adolescence. Child
gender-related differences, and links with fam- Longitudinal studies in adolescence. Jour- Development, 79, 1676–1692.
ily and peer relationships. Journal of Research nal of Early Adolescence, 9, 247–268. Simon, V. A., & Furman, W. (2010). Interparen-
on Adolescence, 10, 99–118. Silk, J., Morris, A., Kanaya, T., & Steinberg, L. tal conflict and adolescents’ romantic rela-
Shulman, S., Tuval-Mashiach, R., Levran, E., & (2003). Psychological control and autonomy tionship conflict. Journal of Research on
Anbar, S. (2006). Conflict resolution pat- granting: Opposite ends of a continuum or Adolescence, 20, 188–209.
terns and longevity of adolescent romantic distinct constructs? Journal of Research on Simons, L., & Conger, R. (2007). Linking mother–
couples: A 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 113–128. father differences in parenting to a typology
Adolescence, 29, 575–588. Silk, J., Siegle, G., Whalen, D., Ostapenko, L., of family parenting styles and adolescent out-
Shumow, L., & Miller, J. D. (2001). Parents’ at- Ladouceur, D., & Dahl, R. (2009). Pubertal comes. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 212–241.
home and at-school academic involvement changes in emotional information process- Simons, R., & Burt, C. (2011). Learning to be
with youth adolescents. Journal of Early ing: Pupillary, behavioral, and subjective bad: Adverse social conditions, social sche-
Adolescence, 21, 68–91. evidence during emotional word identifi- mas, and crime. Criminology, 49, 553–598.
Shumow, L., Smith, T., & Smith, M. (2009). Aca- cation. Development and Psychopathology, Simons, R., Chao, W., Conger, R., & Elder, G. H.,
demic and behavioral characteristics of 21, 7–26. Jr. (2001). Quality of parenting as mediator
young adolescents in self-care. Journal of Silk, J., Vanderbilt-Adriance, E., Shaw, D. S., of the effect of childhood defiance on adoles-
Early Adolescence, 29, 233–257. Forbes, E. E., Whalen, D. J., Ryan, N. D., . . . cent friendship choice and delinquency: A
Sibley, M. H., Pelham, W. E., Molina, B. S. G., Dahl, R. (2007). Resilience among children growth curve analysis. Journal of Marriage
Gnagy, E. M., Waschbusch, D. A., Biswas, A., and adolescents at risk for depression: Media- and the Family, 63, 63–79.
. . . Karch, K. (2011). The delinquency out- tion and moderation across social and neuro- Simons, R., Johnson, C., Beaman, J., Conger, R.,
comes of boys with ADHD with and without biological context. Development and & Whitbeck, L. (1996). Parents and peer
comorbidity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- Psychopathology, 19, 841–865. group as mediators of the effect of commu-
chology, 39, 21–32. Silverberg, S., Marczak, M., & Gondoli, D. nity structure on adolescent problem
Siebenbruner, J., Englund, M. M., Egeland, B., (1996). Maternal depressive symptoms and behavior. American Journal of Community
& Hudson, K. (2006). Developmental ante- achievement-related outcomes among ad- Psychology, 24, 145–171.
cedents of late adolescence substance use olescent daughters: Variations by family Simons, R., Lin, K., & Gordon, L. (1998). Social-
patterns. Development and Psychopathology, structure. Journal of Early Adolescence, 16, ization in the family of origin and male dat-
18, 551–571. 90–109. ing violence: A prospective study. Journal of
Siegel, A., & Scovill, L. C. (2000). Problem behav- Silverberg, S., & Steinberg, L. (1990). Psycho- Marriage and the Family, 60, 467–478.
ior: The double symptom of adolescence. De- logical well-being of parents at midlife: The Simons, R., Simons, L., Burt, C., Brody, G., &
velopment and Psychopathology, 12, 763–793. impact of early adolescent children. Devel- Cutrona, C. (2005). Collective efficacy, au-
Siegel, J., Aneshensel, C., Taub, B., Cantwell, D., opmental Psychology, 26, 658–666. thoritative parenting and delinquency: A
& Driscoll, A. (1998). Adolescent depressed Silverman, J., Raj, A., Mucci, L. A., & Hathaway, longitudinal test of a model integrating
mood in a multiethnic sample. Journal of J. E. (2001). Dating violence against adoles- community- and family-level processes.
Youth and Adolescence, 27, 413–427. cent girls and associated substance abuse, Criminology, 43, 989–1029.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-73 02/04/13 6:36 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-73

Simons, R., Whitbeck, L., Conger, R., & Chyi-In, Sionéan, C., DiClemente, R. J., Wingood, G. M., Smetana, J. (1995b). Parenting styles and con-
W. (1991). Intergenerational transmission of Crosby, R., Cobb, B. K., Harrington, K., . . . ceptions of parental authority during adoles-
harsh parenting. Developmental Psychology, Oh, M. (2002). Psychosocial and behavioral cence. Child Development, 66, 299–316.
27, 159–171. correlates of refusing unwanted sex among Smetana, J. (2005). Adolescent–parent conflict:
Simons-Morton, B. (2011). Adolescent driving African-American adolescent females. Journal Resistance and subversion as developmental
behavior: A developmental challenge. In B. of Adolescent Health, 30, 55–63. process. In L. Nucci (Ed.), Conflict, contra-
Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Sipsma, H., Biello, K. B., Cole-Lewis, H., & diction, and contrarian elements in moral
adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 38–47). New York: Kershaw, T. (2010). Like father, like son: The development and education (pp. 69–91).
Academic Press. intergenerational cycle of adolescent father- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Simons-Morton, B., & Chen, R. (2009). Peer and hood. American Journal of Public Health, Smetana, J., & Asquith, P. (1994). Adolescents’ and
parent influences on school engagement 100, 517–524. parents’ conceptions of parental authority
among early adolescents. Youth & Society, 41, Sirin, S. R., & Rogers-Sirin, L. (2004). Exploring and personal autonomy. Child Development,
3–25. school engagement of middle-class African 65, 1147–1162.
Simons-Morton, B., Hartos, J., Leaf, W., & Preusser, American adolescents. Youth and Society, 35, Smetana, J., & Bitz, B. (1996). Adolescents’ con-
D. (2005). Persistence of effects of the Check- 323–340. ceptions of teachers’ authority and their rela-
points Program on parental restrictions on Sisk, C. L., & Foster, D. L. (2004). The neural tions to rule violations in school. Child
teen driving privileges. American Journal of basis of puberty and adolescence. Nature Development, 67, 1153–1172.
Public Health, 95, 447–452. Neuroscience, 7, 1040–1047. Smetana, J., Campione-Barr, N., & Daddis, C.
Simons-Morton, B., Ouimet, M., Zhang, Z., Skeem, J., & Cauffman. E. (2003). Views of the (2004). Longitudinal development of family
Klauer, S., Lee, S., Wang, J., . . . Dingus, T. downward extension: Comparing the youth decision making: Defining healthy behav-
(2011). The effect of passengers and risk- version of the Psychopathy Checklist with the ioral autonomy for middle-class African
taking friends on risky driving and crashes/ Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory. Behav- American adolescents. Child Development,
near crashes among novice teenagers. Journal ioral Sciences and the Law, 21, 737–770. 75, 1418–1434.
of Adolescent Health, 49, 587–593. Smetana, J., & Chuang, S. (2001). Middle-class Af-
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.
Simpkins, S., Bouffard, S., Dearing, E., Kreider, New York: Free Press. rican American parents’ conceptions of par-
H., Wilmer, C., Caronongan, P., & Weiss, H. enting in the transition to adolescence. Journal
Skitch, S. A., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2008). Rumination
(2009). Adolescent adjustment and patterns of Research on Adolescence, 11, 177–198.
in response to stress as a common vulnera-
of parents’ behaviors in early and middle ad- Smetana, J., Crean, H. F., & Daddis, C. (2002). Fam-
bility factor to depression and substance
olescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, ily processes and problem behaviors in middle-
misuse in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal
19, 530–557. class African American adolescents. Journal of
Child Psychology, 36, 1029–1045.
Simpkins, S., Davis-Kean, P., & Eccles, J. (2006). Research on Adolescence, 12, 275–304.
Skoog, T., Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2009). The role
Math and science motivation: A longitudinal Smetana, J., & Daddis, C. (2002). Domain-
of pubertal timing in what adolescent boys
examination of the links between choices and specific antecedents of parental psychologi-
do online. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42, 70–83. cal control and monitoring: The role of
19, 1–7.
Simpkins, S., Eccles, J., & Becnel, J. N. (2008). parenting beliefs and practices. Child Devel-
The mediational role of adolescents’ friends Skorikov, V. B., & Vondracek, F. W. (2007). Voca- opment, 73, 563–580.
in relations between activity breadth and tional identity. In V. B. Skorikov & W. Patton
Smetana, J., Daddis, C., & Chuang, S. (2003).
adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 44, (Eds.), Career development in childhood and
“Clean your room!” Journal of Adolescent
1081–1094. adolescence (pp. 143–168). Rotterdam, The
Research, 18, 631–650.
Netherlands: Sense.
Simpson, J. A., Collins, W. A., Tran, S., & Haydon, Smetana, J., & Gaines, C. (2000). Adolescent–
K. C. (2007). Attachment and the experience Slap, G., Goodman, E., & Huang, B. (2001). parent conflict in middle-class African
and expression of emotions in romantic rela- Adoption as a risk factor for attempted sui- American families. Child Development, 70,
tionships: A developmental perspective. cide during adolescence. Pediatrics, 108, 1447–1463.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Article E30.
Smetana, J., & Gettman, D. C. (2006). Autonomy
92, 355–367. Smahel, D., Brown, B., & Blinka, L. (2012). As- and relatedness with parents and romantic de-
Singh, P., & Bussey, K. (2011). Peer victimization sociations between online friendship and velopment in African American adolescents.
and psychological maladjustment: The me- internet addiction among adolescents and Developmental Psychology, 42, 1347–1351.
diating role of coping self-efficacy. Journal of emerging adults. Developmental Psychology,
Smetana, J., Metzger, A., Gettman, D., & Campione-
Research on Adolescence, 21, 420–433. 48, 381–388.
Barr, N. (2006). Disclosure and secrecy in
Singh, S., & Darroch, J. (1999). Trends in sexual Small, S., & Luster, T. (1994). Adolescent sexual adolescent–parent relationships. Child De-
activity among adolescent American women: activity: An ecological, risk-factor ap- velopment, 77, 201–217.
1982–1995. Family Planning Perspectives, 31, proach. Journal of Marriage and the Family, Smetana, J., & Villalobos, M. (2009). Social cogni-
212–219. 56, 181–192. tive development in adolescence. In R. Lerner
Singh, S., & Darroch, J. E. (2000). Adolescent Small, S., & Memmo, M. (2004). Contemporary & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent
pregnancy and childbearing: Levels and models of youth development and problem psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 187–228). New
trends in developed countries. Family Plan- prevention: Toward an integration of terms, York: Wiley.
ning Perspectives, 32(1), 14–23. concepts and models. Family Relations, 53, Smetana, J., Yau, J., Restrepo, A., & Braeges, J.
Sinha, J. W., Cnaan, R. A., & Gelles, R. J. (2007). 3–11. (1991). Adolescent–parent conflict in mar-
Adolescent risk behaviors and religion: Smetana, J. (1989). Adolescents’ and parents’ rea- ried and divorced families. Developmental
Findings from a national study. Journal of soning about actual family conflict. Child Psychology, 27, 1000–1010.
Adolescence, 30, 231–249. Development, 59, 1052–1067. Smith, A., & Lalonde, R. N. (2003). “Racelessness”
Sinopoli, K. J., Schachar, R., & Dennis, M. (2011). Smetana, J. (1995a). Conflict and coordination in in a Canadian context? Exploring the link be-
Reward improves cancellation and restraint adolescent–parent relationships. In S. Shulman tween Black students’ identity, achievement,
inhibition across childhood and adolescence. (Ed.), Close relationships and socioemotional de- and mental health. Journal of Black Psychology,
Developmental Psychology, 47, 1479–1489. velopment (pp. 155–184). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 29, 142–164.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-74 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-74 References

Smith, C., & Denton, M. (2005). Soul searching: Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Luyckx, K., & Spear, L. (2010). The behavioral neuroscience of
The religious and spiritual lives of American Goossens, L. (2006). Parenting and adoles- adolescence. New York: Norton.
teenagers. New York: Oxford University Press. cent problem behavior: An integrated model Spear, L. (2011a). Brain development. In B.
Smith, E., & Zabin, L. (1993). Marital and birth with adolescent self-disclosure and perceived Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of
expectations of urban adolescents. Youth and parental knowledge as intervening variables. adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 87–95). New York:
Society, 25, 62–74. Developmental Psychology, 42, 305–318. Academic Press.
Smith, G., Goldman, M., Greenbaum, P., & Soli, A. R., McHale, S. M., & Feinberg, M. E. Spear, L. P. (2011b). Adolescent neurobehavioral
Christiansen, B. (1995). Expectancy for social (2009). Risk and protective effects of sib- characteristics, alcohol sensitivities, and in-
facilitation from drinking: The divergent ling relationships among African American take: Setting the stage for alcohol use disor-
paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Family Relations, 58, 578–592. ders? Child Development Perspectives, 5,
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Solomon, R., & Liefeld, C. (1998). Effectiveness of 231–238.
104, 32–40. a family support center approach to adoles- Spencer, G. A., & Bryant, S. A. (2000). Dating
Smith, J. L., Skinner, S. R., & Fenwick, J. (2011). cent mothers: Repeat pregnancy and school violence: A comparison of rural, suburban,
How Australian female adolescents prioritize drop-out rates. Family Relations, 47, 139–144. and urban teens. Journal of Adolescent
pregnancy protection: A grounded theory Sommer, K., Whitman, T., Borkowski, J., Health, 27, 302–305.
study of contraceptive histories. Journal of Schellenbach, C., Maxwell, S., & Keogh, D. Spencer, M. B. (2005). Crafting identities and ac-
Adolescent Research, 26, 617–644. (1993). Cognitive readiness and adolescent par- cessing opportunities post-Brown. American
enting. Developmental Psychology, 29, 389–398. Psychologist, 60, 821–830.
Smith, P. H., White, J. W., & Holland, L. J. (2003).
A longitudinal perspective on dating vio- Sommers, C. (2000). The war against boys. New
Spieker, S., & Bensley, L. (1994). Roles of living
lence among adolescent and college-age York: Simon & Schuster.
arrangements and grandmother social sup-
women. American Journal of Public Health, Sontag, L. M., Clemans, K. H., Graber, J. A., & port in adolescent mothering and infant at-
93, 1104–1109. Lyndon, S. T. (2011). Traditional and cyber tachment. Developmental Psychology, 30,
Smith, R., & Rose, A. (2011). The “cost of car- aggressors and victims: A comparison of 102–111.
ing” in youths’ friendships: Considering as- psychosocial characteristics. Journal of Youth
Spies, L. A., Margolin, G., Susman, E. J., & Gordis,
sociations among social perspective-taking, and Adolescence, 40, 392–404
E. B. (2011). Adolescents’ cortisol reactivity
co-rumination, and empathetic distress. Sontag, L. M., & Graber, J. A. (2010). Coping and subjective distress in response to family
Developmental Psychology, 47, 1792–1803. with perceived peer stress: Gender-specific conflict: The moderating role of internaliz-
and common pathways to symptoms of psy- ing symptoms. Journal of Adolescent Health,
Smith, T. E., & Leaper, C. (2006). Self-perceived
chopathology. Developmental Psychology, 49, 386–392.
gender typicality and the peer context during
46, 1605–1620.
adolescence. Journal of Research on Adoles- Spotts, E. L., Neiderheiser, J. M., Hetherington,
cence, 16, 91–103. Sontag, L. M., Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & E., & Reiss, D. (2001). The relation between
Warren, M. P. (2008). Coping with social observational measures of social problem
Smokowski, P., Bacallao, M., & Buchanan, R.
stress: Implications for psychopathology in solving and familial antisocial behavior: Ge-
(2009). Interpersonal mediators linking ac-
young adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal netic and environmental influences. Journal
culturation stressors to subsequent inter-
Child Psychology, 36, 1159–1174. of Research on Adolescence, 11, 351–374.
nalizing symptoms and self-esteem in
Latino adolescents. Journal of Community Sontag, L. M., Graber, J. A., & Clemans, K. H.
Spriggs, A. L., & Halpern, C. T. (2008). Sexual
Psychology, 37, 1024–1045. (2011). The role of peer stress and pubertal
debut timing and depressive symptoms in
timing on symptoms of psychopathology
Smolak, L., Levine, M., & Gralen, S. (1993). The emerging adulthood. Journal of Youth and
during early adolescence. Journal of Youth
impact of puberty and dating on eating Adolescence, 37, 1085–1096.
and Adolescence, 40, 1371–1382.
problems among middle school girls. Journal Sprondel, V., Kipp, K. H., & Mecklinger, A.
Sorensen, S., Richardson, B., & Peterson, J. (1993).
of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 355–368. (2011). Developmental changes in item and
Race/ethnicity patterns in the homicide of
Smoll, F., & Schutz, R. (1990). Quantifying gen- source memory: Evidence from an ERP rec-
children in Los Angeles, 1980 through 1989.
der differences in physical performance: A ognition memory study with children, ado-
American Journal of Public Health, 83, 725–727.
developmental perspective. Developmental lescents, and adults. Child Development, 82,
Sourander, A., Elonheimo, H., Niemelä, S., Nuutila, 1638–1953.
Psychology, 26, 360–369.
A., Helenius, H., Sillanmäki, L., . . . Almqvist, F.
Smollar, J., & Youniss, J. (1985). Transformation in Spruijt-Metz, D. S. (2011). Etiology, treatment,
(2006). Childhood predictors of male crimi-
adolescents’ perceptions of parents. Paper pre- and prevention of obesity in childhood and
nality: A prospective population-based follow-
sented at the biennial meetings of the Society adolescence: A decade in review. Journal of
up study from age 8 to late adolescence. Journal
for Research in Child Development, Baltimore. Research on Adolescence, 129, 129–152.
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Snell, E. K., Adam, E. K., & Duncan, G. J. (2007). Psychiatry, 45, 578–586. Squeglia, L. M., Jacobus, J., & Tapert, S. F. (2009).
Sleep and the body mass index and over- South, S. J., & Baumer, E. P. (2000). Deciphering The influence of substance use on adoles-
weight status of children and adolescents. community and race effects on adolescent cent brain development. Clinical EEG and
Child Development, 78, 309–323. premarital childbearing. Social Forces, 78, Neuroscience, 40, 32–39.
Snyder, H., & Sickmund, M. (2006). Juvenile of- 1379–1407. St. George, I., Williams, S., & Silva, P. (1994).
fenders and victims: 2006 national report. South, S. J., & Baumer, E. P. (2001). Community Body size and menarche: The Dunedin study.
Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice effects on the resolution of adolescent pre- Journal of Adolescent Health, 15, 573–576.
and Delinquency Prevention. marital pregnancy. Journal of Family Issues, St. Lawrence, J., Brasfield, T., Jefferson, K., Allyene,
Snyder, J., Bank, L., & Burraston, B. (2005). The 22, 1025–1043. E., & Shirley, A. (1994). Social support as a
consequences of antisocial behavior in older South, S. J., Baumer, E. P., & Lutz, A. (2003). Inter- factor in African-American adolescents’ sex-
male siblings for younger brothers and sisters. preting community effects on youth educa- ual risk behavior. Journal of Adolescent Re-
Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 643–653. tional attainment. Youth and Society, 35, 3–36. search, 9, 292–310.
Sobesky, W. (1983). The effects of situational South, S. J., & Haynie, D. L. (2004). Friendship Staff, J., Messersmith, E., & Schulenberg, J.
factors on moral judgments. Child Develop- networks of mobile adolescents. Social (2009). Adolescents and the world of work.
ment, 54, 575–584. Forces, 83, 315–350. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-75 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-75

of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. Steele, J., & Brown, J. (1995). Adolescent room speculative propositions. Child Development,
270–313). New York: Wiley. culture: Studying media in the context of 71, 66–74.
Staff, J., & Mortimer, J. T. (2007). Educational everyday life. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Steinberg, L., & Belsky, J. (1996). A sociobio-
and work strategies from adolescence to early cence, 24, 551–576. logical perspective on psychopathology in
adulthood: Consequences for educational at- Steffensmeier, D., Schwartz, J., Zhong, H., & adolescence. In D. Cicchetti and S. Toth
tainment. Social Forces, 85, 1169–1194. Ackerman, J. (2005). An assessment of recent (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Develop-
Staff, J., Osgood, D. W., Schulenberg, J., Bachman, trends in girls’ violence using diverse longi- mental Psychopathology (Vol. 7, pp. 93–124).
J., & Messersmith, E. (2010). Explaining the tudinal sources: Is the gender gap closing? Rochester, NY: University of Rochester
relationship between employment and juve- Criminology, 43, 355–406. Press.
nile delinquency. Criminology, 48, 1101–1131. Stein, J., & Reiser, L. (1994). A study of White Steinberg, L., Blatt-Eisengart, I., & Cauffman, E.
Staff, J., Schulenberg, J. E., Maslowsky, J., middle-class adolescent boys’ responses to (2006). Patterns of competence and adjust-
Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., Maggs, J. L., “semenarche” (the first ejaculation). Journal ment among adolescents from authoritative,
& Johnston, L. D. (2010). Substance use of Youth and Adolescence, 23, 373–384. authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful
changes and social role transitions: Proximal Steinberg, L. (1987). Single parents, steppar- homes: Replication in a sample of serious
developmental effects on ongoing trajecto- ents, and the susceptibility of adolescents to juvenile offenders. Journal of Research on
ries from late adolescence through early antisocial peer pressure. Child Development, Adolescence, 16, 47–58.
adulthood. Development and Psychopathol- 58, 269–275. Steinberg, L., & Cauffman, E. (1996). Maturity of
ogy, 22, 917–932. Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict, and judgment in adolescence: Psychosocial fac-
Stams, G. J., Brugman, D., Dekovic, M., van harmony in the family relationship. In S. tors in adolescent decisionmaking. Law and
Rosmalen, L., van der Laan, P., & Gibbs, J. C. Feldman & G. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: Human Behavior, 20, 249–272.
(2006). The moral judgment of juvenile delin- The developing adolescent (pp. 255–276). Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., Woolard, J., Graham,
quents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. S., & Banich, M. (2009). Are adolescents less
Child Psychology, 34, 697–713. Steinberg, L. (1996). Beyond the classroom: Why mature than adults? Minors’ access to abor-
Stanger-Hall, K. F., & Hall, D. W. (2011). Absti- school reform has failed and what parents need tion, the juvenile death penalty, and the al-
nence-only education and teen pregnancy to do. New York: Simon & Schuster. leged APA “flip-flop.” American Psychologist,
rates: Why we need comprehensive sex edu- 64, 583–594.
Steinberg, L. (2000). Youth violence: Do parents
cation in the U.S. PLoS ONE, 6, e24658. Steinberg, L., Dahl, R., Keating, D., Kupfer, D.,
and families make a difference? National In-
Stanley, L. R., Henry, K. L., & Swaim, R. C. (2011). stitute of Justice Journal, April, 30–38. Masten, A., & Pine, D. (2006). Psychopa-
Physical, social, and perceived availabilities thology in adolescence: Integrating affective
Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things:
of alcohol and last month alcohol use in rural neuroscience with the study of context. In
Adolescent–parent relationships in retro-
and small urban communities. Journal of D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Develop-
spect and prospect. Journal of Research on
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1203–1214. mental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Developmen-
Adolescence, 11, 1–19.
Stanton-Salazar, R., & Spina, S. (2005). Adoles- tal neuroscience (pp. 710–741). New York:
Steinberg, L. (2003). The state of adolescence. Wiley.
cent peer networks as a context for social
In D. Ravitch (Ed.), Brookings papers on ed-
and emotional support. Youth and Society, Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. (1991). Negative
ucation policy. Washington, DC: Brookings
36, 379–417. correlates of part-time work in adolescence:
Institution.
Starr, L. R., & Davila, J. (2009). Clarifying co- Replication and elaboration. Developmental
Steinberg, L. (2005). The 10 basic principles of Psychology, 17, 304–313.
rumination: Associations with internalizing
good parenting. New York: Simon & Schuster.
symptoms and romantic involvement Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S., & Brown, B. (1992).
among adolescent girls. Journal of Adoles- Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk-taking in adolescence: Ethnic differences in adolescent achieve-
cence, 32, 19–37. New perspectives from brain and behavioral ment: An ecological perspective. American
science. Current Directions in Psychological Psychologist, 47, 723–729.
Stattin, H., Kerr, M., & Skoog, T. (2011). Early
Science, 16, 55–59.
pubertal timing and girls’ problem behavior: Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard,
Integrating two hypotheses. Journal of Youth Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience per- J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age
and Adolescence, 40, 1271–1287. spective on adolescent risk-taking. Develop- differences in future orientation and delay
Stearns, E. (2004). Interracial friendliness and mental Review, 28, 78–106. discounting. Child Development, 80, 28–44.
the social organization of schools. Youth and Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S., Darling, N., Mounts,
Society 35, 395–419. adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psy- N., & Dornbusch, S. (1994). Over-time
Stearns, E., Buchmann, C., & Bonneau, K. chobiology, 52, 216–224. changes in adjustment and competence
(2009). Interracial friendships in the transi- Steinberg, L. (2011). You and your adolescent: among adolescents from authoritative, au-
tion to college: Do birds of a feather flock The essential guide for ages 10–25. New York: thoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful fami-
together once they leave the nest? Sociology of Simon & Schuster. lies. Child Development, 65, 754–770.
Education, 82, 173–195. Steinberg, L. (2012). Should the science of ado- Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S., Dornbusch, S., &
Stearns, E., & Glennie, E. J. (2006). When and lescent brain development inform public Darling, N. (1992). Impact of parenting
why dropouts leave high school. Youth and policy? Issues in Science and Technology, practices on adolescent achievement: Au-
Society, 38, 29–57. Spring, 67–78. thoritative parenting, school involvement,
Stedman, L. (1998). An assessment of the con- Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, and encouragement to succeed. Child Devel-
temporary debate over U.S. achievement. In M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age opment, 63, 1266–1281.
D. Ravitch (Ed.). Brookings papers on educa- differences in sensation seeking and im- Steinberg, L., & Monahan, K. (2007). Age differ-
tion policy: 1998, pp. 53–121. Washington: pulsivity as indexed by behavior and self- ences in resistance to peer influence. Devel-
Brookings Institution. report: Evidence for a dual systems model. opmental Psychology, 43, 1531–1543.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764–1778. Steinberg, L., & Monahan, K. (2011). Adoles-
stereotypes shape intellectural identity and Steinberg, L., & Avenevoli, S. (2000). The role cents’ exposure to sexy media does not has-
performance. American Psychologist, 52, of context in the development of psychopa- ten the initiation of sexual intercourse.
613–619. thology: A conceptual framework and some Developmental Psychology, 47, 562–576.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-76 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-76 References

Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. (2001). Adolescent increases in depressive symptoms in adoles- Stone, J. (2011). Employment. In B. Brown &
development. Annual Review of Psychology, cent girls: A growth curve analysis. Develop- M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles-
52, 83–110. mental Psychology, 37, 597–607. cence (Vol. 2, pp. 59–67). New York: Aca-
Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. (2002). Parenting ado- Stice, E., Burton, E., & Shaw, H. (2004). Prospec- demic Press.
lescents. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook tive relations between bulimic pathology, de- Stone, L. B., Hankin, B. L., Gibb, B. E., & Abela,
of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parent- pression, and substance abuse: Unpacking J. R. Z. (2011). Co-rumination predicts the
ing (2nd ed., pp. 103–133). Mahwah, NJ: comorbidity in adolescent girls. Journal of onset of depressive disorders during adoles-
Erlbaum. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 62–71. cence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120,
Steinberg, L., & Silverberg, S. (1986). The vicis- Stice, E., & Gonzales, N. (1998). Adolescent tem- 752–757.
situdes of autonomy in early adolescence. perament moderates the relation of parent- Stoolmiller, M., Kim, H. K., & Capaldi, D. M.
Child Development, 57, 841–851. ing to antisocial behavior and substance use. (2005). The course of depressive symptoms
Steinberg, L., & Steinberg, W. (1994). Crossing Journal of Adolescent Research, 13, 5–31. in men from early adolescence to young
paths: How your child’s adolescence triggers Stice, E., Hayward, C., Cameron, R. P., Killen, J., adulthood: Identifying latent trajectories
your own crisis. New York: Simon & Schuster. & Taylor, C. (2000). Body-image and eating and early predictors. Journal of Abnormal
disturbances predict onset of depression Psychology, 114, 331–345.
Steinberg, S. J., Davila, J., & Fincham, F. (2006).
Adolescent marital expectations and roman- among female adolescents: A longitudinal Storvoll, E. E., & Wichstrøm, L. (2002). Do the
tic experiences: Associations with percep- study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, risk factors associated with conduct prob-
tions about parental conflict and adolescent 438–444. lems vary according to gender? Journal of
attachment security. Journal of Youth and Stice, E., Presnell, K., Shaw, H., & Rohde, P. Adolescence, 25, 183–202.
Adolescence, 35, 333–348. (2005). Psychological and behavioral risk Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Wei, E., & Loeber, R.
Steinman, K. J., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2004). factors for obesity onset in adolescent girls: A (2004). Desistance from persistent serious
Religious activity and risk behavior among prospective study. Journal of Consulting and delinquency in the transition to adult-
African American adolescents: Concurrent Clinical Psychology, 73, 195–202. hood. Development and Psychopathology,
and developmental effects. American Journal Stice, E., & Shaw, H. (2003). Prospective relations 16, 897–918.
of Community Psychology, 33, 151–161. of body image, eating, and affective distur- Strachman, A., Impett, E. A., Henson, J. M., &
bances to smoking onset in adolescent girls: Pentz, M. A. (2009). Early adolescent alco-
Stephens, L. (1996). Will Johnny see Daddy this
How Virginia Slims. Journal of Consulting hol use and sexual experience by emerging
week? An empirical test of three theoretical
and Clinical Psychology, 71, 129–135. adulthood: A 10-year longitudinal investi-
perspectives of postdivorce contact. Journal
Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Marti, C. N. (2006). A gation. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45,
of Family Issues, 17, 466–494.
meta-analytic review of obesity prevention 478–482.
Sternberg, R. (1988). The triarchic mind. New
programs for children and adolescents: The Strang, N., Pruessner, J., & Pollack, S. (2011). De-
York: Viking Penguin.
skinny on interventions that work. Psycho- velopmental changes in adolescents’ neural
Stevens, E. A., & Prinstein, M. J. (2005). Peer con- logical Bulletin, 132, 667–691. response to challenge. Developmental Cogni-
tagion of depressogenic attributional styles tive Neuroscience,1, 560–569.
Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Ochner, C. (2011). Eating
among adolescents: A longitudinal study.
disorders. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Strasburger, V., & Donnerstein, E. (1999). Chil-
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33,
Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 151– dren, adolescents, and the media: Issues and
25–38.
159). New York: Academic Press. solutions. Pediatrics, 103, 129–139.
Stevens, J. (1988). Social support, locus of con-
Stice, E., & Whitenton, K. (2002). Risk factors for Strasburger, V., Jordan, A., & Donnerstein, E.
trol, and parenting in three low-income
body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls: A (2010). Health effects of media on children
groups of mothers: Black teenagers, Black
longitudinal investigation. Developmental and adolescents. Pediatrics, 125, 756–767.
adults, and White adults. Child Development,
Psychology, 38, 669–678. Straub, D. M., Hills, N. K., Thompson, P. J., &
59, 635–642.
Stice, E., Yokum, S., Burger, K., Epstein, L., & Moscicki, A. (2003). Effects of pro- and anti-
Stevenson, D., Schiller, K., & Schneider, B. Small, D. (2011). Youth at risk for obesity tobacco advertising on nonsmoking adoles-
(1994). Sequences of opportunities for show greater activation of striatal and so- cents’ intentions to smoke. Journal of
learning. Sociology of Education, 67, 184–198. matosensory regions to food. Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, 36–43.
Stevenson, H., & Stigler, J. (1992). The learning Neuroscience, 31, 4360–4366. Straus, M., & Yodanis, C. (1996). Corporal pun-
gap: Why our schools are failing and what we Stickle, T. R., Kirkpatrick, N. M., & Brush, L. ishment in adolescence and physical assaults
can learn from Japanese and Chinese educa- N. (2009). Callous-unemotional traits and on spouses in later life: What accounts for
tion. New York: Simon & Schuster. social information processing: Multiple the link? Journal of Marriage and the Family,
Stevenson, H., Jr., Reed, J., Bodison, P., & Bishop, risk-factor models for understanding ag- 58, 825–841.
A. (1997). Racism stress management: Racial gressive behavior in antisocial youth. Law Strenziok, M., Krueger, F., Pulaski, S., Openshaw,
social beliefs and the experience of depres- and Human Behavior, 33, 515–529. A., Zamboni, G., van de Meer, E., & Grafman,
sion and anger in African American youth. Stipek, D., & Gralinski, J. (1996). Children’s be- J. (2010). Lower lateral orbitofrontal cortex
Youth and Society, 29, 197–222. liefs about intelligence and school perfor- density associated with more frequent expo-
Stewart, E. B., Stewart, E. A., & Simons, R. L. mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, sure to television and movie violence in male
(2007). The effect of neighborhood context 397–407. adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46,
on the college aspirations of African Ameri- Stocker, C. M., Burwell, R. A., & Briggs, M. L. 607–609.
can adolescents. American Education Re- (2002). Sibling conflict in middle childhood Strong American Schools. (2008). Diploma to
search Journal, 44, 896–919. predicts children’s adjustment in early ado- nowhere. Washington, DC: Author.
Stewart, S. D. (2003). Nonresident parenting and lescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, Stroud, L., Foster, E., Papandonatos, G., Hand-
adolescent adjustment: The quality of non- 50–57. werger, K., Granger, D., Kivlighan, K., . . .
resident father–child interaction. Journal of Stocker, C. M., Lanthier, R. P., & Furman, W. Niaura, R. (2009). Stress response and the
Family Issues, 24, 217–244. (1997). Sibling relationships in early adult- adolescent transition: Performance versus
Stice, E., & Bearman, S. (2001). Body-image and hood. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, peer rejection stressors. Development and
eating disturbances prospectively predict 210–221. Psychopathology, 21, 47–68.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-77 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-77

Strough, J., & Berg, C. (2000). Goals as a media- performance: A growth-curve model. Journal Syvertsen, A., Flanagan, C., & Stout, M. (2009).
tor of gender differences in high-affiliation of Family Issues, 30, 1527–1555. Code of silence: Students’ perceptions of
dyadic conversations. Developmental Psy- Supple, A. J., Ghazarian, S. R., Frabutt, J. M., school climate and willingness to intervene
chology, 36, 117–125. Plunkett, S. W., & Sands, T. (2006). Contex- in a peer’s dangerous plan. Journal of Educa-
Stuart, E. A., & Green, K. M. (2008). Using full tual influences on Latino adolescent ethnic tional Psychology, 101, 219–232.
matching to estimate causal effects in nonex- identity and academic outcomes. Child Syvertsen, A., Wray-Lake, L., Flanagan, C., Osgood,
perimental studies: Examining the relation- Development, 77, 1427–1433. D. W., & Briddell, L. (2011). Thirty-year trends
ship between adolescent marijuana use and Supple, A. J., Peterson, G. W., & Bush, K. R. in U.S. adolescents’ civic engagement: A story
adult outcomes. Developmental Psychology, (2004). Assessing the validity of parenting of changing participation and educational dif-
44, 395–406. measures in a sample of Chinese adolescents. ferences. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21,
Sturman, D., & Moghaddam, B. (2012). Striatum Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 539–544. 586–594.
processes reward differently in adolescents Susman, E. (1997). Modeling developmental Szeszulski, P., Martinez, A., & Reyes, B. (1994,
versus adults. Proceedings of the National complexity in adolescence: Hormones and February). Patterns and predictors of self-
Academy of Sciences, 109, 1719–1724. behavior in context. Journal of Research on satisfaction among culturally diverse high
Sturmhöfel, S., & Swartzwelder, H. (2004). Alco- Adolescence, 7, 283–306. school students. Paper presented at the bien-
hol’s effects on the adolescent brain: What Susman, E. (2006). Psychobiology of persistent nial meetings of the Society for Research on
can be learned from animal models? Alcohol antisocial behavior: Stress, early vulnerabili- Adolescence, San Diego.
Research and Health, 28, 213–221. ties, and the attenuation hypothesis. Neuro- Szwedo, D., Mikami, A., & Allen, J. (2011).
Su, X., Simons, R. L., & Simons, L. G. (2011). In- science Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 376–389. Qualities of peer relations on social net-
terparental aggression and antisocial behavior Susman, E. J., Dockray, S., Schiefelbein, V. L., working websites: Predictions from negative
among African American youth: A simultane- Herwehe, S., Heaton, J. A., & Dorn, L. D. mother–teen interactions. Journal of Re-
ous test of competing explanations. Journal of (2007). Morningness/eveningness, morning- search on Adolescence, 21, 595–607.
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1489–1502. to-afternoon cortisol ratio, and antisocial Tamnes, C., Østby, Y., Fjell, A., Westlye, L., Due-
Suárez-Orozco, C., Rhodes, J., & Milburn, M. behavior problems during puberty. Develop- Tonnessen, P., & Walhovd, K. (2010). Brain
(2009). Unraveling the immigrant paradox: mental Psychology, 43, 811–822. maturation in adolescence and young
Academic engagement and disengagement Susman, E., & Dorn, L. (2009). Puberty: Its role in adulthood: regional age-related changes in
among recently arrived immigrant youth. development. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg cortical thickness and white matter volume
Youth & Society, 41, 151–185. (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd and microstructure. Cerebral Cortex, 20,
ed., Vol. 1, pp. 116–151). New York: Wiley. 534–548.
Sue, S., & Okazaki, S. (1990). Asian-American
educational achievements: A phenomenon Susman, E., Dorn, L., Inoff-Germain, G., Tang, C. S., Yeung, D. Y., & Lee, A. M. (2003).
in search of an explanation. American Psy- Nottelmann, E., & Chrousos, G. (1997). Cor- Psychosocial correlates of emotional re-
chologist, 45, 913–920. tisol reactivity, distress behavior, and behav- sponses to menarche among Chinese ado-
ioral and psychological problems in young lescent girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33,
Sui-Chu, E., & Willms, J. (1996). Effects of paren-
adolescents: A longitudinal perspective. Jour- 193–201.
tal involvement on eighth-grade achievement.
Sociology of Education, 69, 126–141. nal of Research on Adolescence, 7, 81–105. Tang, C. S., Yeung, D. Y., & Lee, A. M. (2004). A
Swahn, M. H., Bossarte, R. M., & Sullivent, E. E. comparison of premenarcheal expectations
Sullivan, C. J., Childs, K. K., & O’Connell, D. and postmenarcheal experiences in Chinese
I., II. (2008). Age of alcohol use initiation,
(2010). Adolescent risk behavior subgroups: early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence,
suicidal behavior, and peer and dating vio-
An empirical assessment. Journal of Youth 24, 180–195.
lence victimization and perpetration among
and Adolescence, 39, 541–562.
high-risk, seventh-grade adolescents. Pediat- Tanner, D. (1972). Secondary education. New
Sullivan, H. S. (1953a). The interpersonal theory rics, 121, 297–305. York: Macmillan.
of psychiatry. New York: Norton.
Swanson, C. (2009). Cities in crisis 2009. Bethesda, Tanner, J. (1972). Sequence, tempo, and individ-
Sullivan, H. S. (1953b). Conceptions of modern MD: EPE Research Center. ual variation in growth and development of
psychiatry. New York: Norton. boys and girls aged twelve to sixteen. In J.
Swarr, A., & Richards, M. (1996). Longitudinal
Sullivan, T. N., Farrell, A. D., & Kliewer, W. (2006). effects of adolescent girls’ pubertal develop- Kagan & R. Coles (Eds.), Twelve to sixteen:
Peer victimization in early adolescence: As- ment, perceptions of pubertal timing, and Early adolescence. New York: Norton.
sociation between physical and relational vic- parental relations on eating problems. Devel- Tanner-Smith, E. E. (2010). Negotiating the
timization and drug use, aggression, and opmental Psychology, 32, 636–646. early developing body: Pubertal timing,
delinquent behaviors among urban middle Sweeten, G., Bushway, S. D., & Paternoster, R. body weight, and adolescent girls’ substance
school students. Development and Psychopa- (2009). Does dropping out of school mean use. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39,
thology, 18, 119–137. dropping into delinquency? Criminology: An 1402–1416.
Sun, Y. (2001). Family environment and ado- Interdisciplinary Journal, 47, 47–91. Tapert, S. F., Baratta, M. V., Abrantes, A. M., &
lescents’ well-being before and after par- Swenson, L. P., & Rose, A. J. (2003). Friends as Brown, S. A. (2002). Attention dysfunction
ents’ marital disruption: A longitudinal reporters of children’s and adolescents’ de- predicts involvement in community youths.
analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Fam- pressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Journal of the American Academy of Child and
ily, 63, 697–713. Child Psychology, 31, 619–631. Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 680–686.
Sun, Y. (2003). The well-being of adolescents in Swinton, A. D., Kurtz-Costes, B., Rowley, S. J., & Taradash, A., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., &
households with no biological parents. Jour- Okeke-Adeyanju, N. (2011). A longitudinal Costa, M. (2001). The interpersonal context
nal of Marriage and Family, 65, 894. examination of African American adoles- of romantic autonomy in adolescence. Journal
Sun, Y., & Li, Y. (2002). Children’s well-being cents’ attributions about achievement out- of Adolescence, 24, 365–377.
during the parents’ marital disruption pro- comes. Child Development, 82, 1486–1500. Tartakovsky, E. (2009). Cultural identities of ad-
cess: A pooled time-series analysis. Journal of Syed, M., & Azmitia, M. (2008). A narrative ap- olescent immigrants: A three-year longitudi-
Marriage and the Family, 64, 472–488. proach to ethnic identity in emerging adult- nal study including the pre-migration
Sun, Y., & Li, Y. (2009). Postdivorce family stabil- hood: Bringing life to the identity status model. period. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38,
ity and changes in adolescents’ academic Developmental Psychology, 44, 1012–1027. 654–671.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-78 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-78 References

Taylor, D., Jenni, O., Acebo, C., & Carskadon, M. Teti, D., & Lamb, M. (1989). Socioeconomic lives: The intergenerational transmission of
(2005). Sleep tendency during extended and marital outcomes of adolescent mar- antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal
wakefulness: Insights into adolescent sleep riage, adolescent childbirth, and their co- Child Psychology, 31, 171–184.
regulation and behavior. Journal of Sleep Re- occurrence. Journal of Marriage and the Thornberry, T., Smith, C., & Howard, G. (1997).
search, 14, 239–244. Family, 51, 203–212. Risk factors for teenage fatherhood. Journal
Taylor, N. (2011). “Guys, she’s humongous!”: Tevendale, H., DuBois, D., Lopez, C., & Prindiville, of Marriage and the Family, 59, 505–522.
Gender and weight-based teasing in adoles- S. (1997). Self-esteem stability and early ado- Thorne, A. (2000). Personal memory telling and
cence. Journal of Adolescent Research, 26, lescent adjustment: An exploratory study. personality development. Personality and So-
178–199. Journal of Early Adolescence, 17, 216–237. cial Psychology Review, 4, 45–56.
Taylor, R., Casten, R., Flickinger, S., Roberts, D., & Theokas, C. (2009). Youth sport participation— Thornton, A., Orbuch, T. L., & Axinn, W. G.
Fulmore, C. (1994). Explaining the school per- A view of the issues: Introduction to the spe- (1995). Parent–child relationships during
formance of African-American adolescents. cial section. Developmental Psychology, 45, the transition to adulthood. Journal of Fam-
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4, 21–44. 303–306. ily Issues, 16, 538–564.
Taylor, R., & Roberts, D. (1995). Kinship support Theokas, C., & Lerner, R. (2006). Promoting Thurber, C. (1995). The experience and expres-
and maternal and adolescent well-being in ec- positive development in adolescence: The sion of homesickness in preadolescent and
onomically disadvantaged African-American role of ecological assets in families, schools, adolescent boys. Child Development, 66,
families. Child Development, 66, 1585–1597. and neighborhoods. Applied Developmental 1162–1178.
Taylor, R., Rodriguez, A. U., Seaton, E. K., & Science, 10, 61–74. Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Galambos, N. L. (2003).
Dominguez, A. (2004). Association of finan- Thijs, J., Verkuyten, M., & Helmond, P. (2010). A Adolescents’ characteristics and parents’ be-
cial resources with parenting and adolescent further examination of the big-fish–little- liefs as predictors of parents’ peer manage-
adjustment in African American families. pond effect: Perceived position in class, class ment behaviors. Journal of Research on
Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 267–283. size, and gender comparisons. Sociology of Adolescence, 13, 269–300.
Taylor, T. J., Peterson, D., Esbensen, F., & Freng, Education, 83, 333–345. Timmerman, G. (2002). A comparison between
A. (2007). Gang membership as a risk factor Thomaes, S., Bushman, B. J., de Castro, B. O., & unwanted sexual behavior by teachers and by
for adolescent violent victimization. Journal Reijntjes, A. (2012). Arousing “gentle pas- peers in secondary schools. Journal of Youth
of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 44, sions” in young adolescents: Sustained ex- and Adolescence, 31, 397–404.
351–380. perimental effects of value affirmations on Tobian, A., Serwadda, D., Quinn, T., Kigozi, G.,
Teachman, J. (1996). Intellectual skill and aca- prosocial feelings and behaviors. Develop- Gravitt, P., Laeyendecker, O., . . . Gray, R.
demic performance: Do families bias the re- mental Psychology, 48, 103–110. (2009). Male circumcision for the preven-
lationship? Sociology of Education, 69, 35–48. Thomaes, S., Poorthuis, A., & Nelemans, S. (2011). tion of HSV-2 and HPV infections and
Teachman, J. (1997). Gender of siblings, cogni- Self-esteem. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein syphilis. New England Journal of Medicine,
tive achievement, and academic perfor- (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 360, 1298–1309.
mance: Familial and nonfamilial influences 316–324). New York: Academic Press. Tobler, A. L., & Komro, K. A. (2010). Trajectories
on children. Journal of Marriage and the Thomas, J., & Daubman, K. A. (2001). The rela- or parental monitoring and communication
Family, 59, 363–374. tionship between friendship quality and self- and effects on drug use among urban young
Teachman, J., & Paasch, K. (1998). The family esteem in adolescent girls and boys. Sex Roles, adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46,
and educational aspirations. Journal of Mar- 45, 53–65. 560–568.
riage and the Family, 60, 704–714. Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., & Tolan, P., & Gorman-Smith, D. (2002). What vio-
Teachman, J., Paasch, K., & Carver, K. (1996). LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emo- lence prevention research can tell us about
Social capital and dropping out of school tional facial recognition in late childhood developmental psychopathology. Develop-
early. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, and adolescence. Developmental Science, 10, ment and Psychopathology, 14, 713–729.
773–783. 547–558. Tolan, P., Gorman-Smith, D., & Henry, D. (2003).
Teachman, J., Paasch, K., & Carver, K. (1997). So- Thomas, L., Hall, J., Skup, M., Jenkins, S., Pine, D. The developmental ecology of urban males’
cial capital and the generation of human S., & Leibenluft, E. (2011). A developmental youth violence. Developmental Psychology,
capital. Social Forces, 75, 1343–1359. neuroimaging investigation of the change par- 39, 274–291.
Teitler, J. O., & Weiss, C. C. (2000). Effects of adigm. Developmental Science, 14, 148–161. Tolan, P., Gorman-Smith, D., Henry, D., Chung,
neighborhood and school environments on Thomas, M., & Johnson, M. (2008). New ad- K., & Hunt, M. (2002). The relation of pat-
transitions to first sexual intercourse. Sociol- vances in understanding sensitive periods in terns of coping inner-city youth to psycho-
ogy of Education, 73, 112–132. brain development. Current Directions in pathology symptoms. Journal of Research on
Psychological Science, 17, 1–5. Adolescence, 12, 423–449.
Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. (2009). A longitudinal
daily diary study of family assistance and ac- Thomas, O., Caldwell, C., Faison, N., & Jackson, Tolman, D. (1993, March). “When my body says
ademic achievement among adolescents J. (2009). Promoting academic achievement: yes”: Adolescent girls’ experiences of sexual de-
from Mexican, Chinese, and European back- The role of racial identity in buffering per- sire. Paper presented at the biennial meetings
grounds. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, ceptions of teacher discrimination on aca- of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
38, 560–571. demic achievement among African American ment, New Orleans.
Terry, M., Ferris, J., Tehranifar, P., Wei, Y., & Flom, and Carribean Black adolescents. Journal of Tolman, D. L., & McClelland, S. I. (2011). Nor-
J. (2009). Birth weight, postnatal growth, and Educational Psychology, 101, 420–431. mative sexuality development in adolescence:
age at menarche. American journal of Epide- Thompson, R., & Zuroff, D. C. (1999). Depen- A decade in review, 2000–2009. Journal of
miology, 170, 72–79. dency, self-criticism, and mothers’ responses Research on Adolescence, 21, 242–255.
Terry, R., & Winston, C. (2010). Personality to adolescent sons’ autonomy and compe- Tomal, A. (2001). The effect of religious mem-
characteristic adaptations: Multiracial ado- tence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, bership on teen abortion rates. Journal of
lescents’ patterns of racial self-identification 365–384. Youth and Adolescence, 30, 103–116.
change. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Thornberry, T., Freeman-Gallant, A., Lizotte, A. Tomasik, M., & Silbereisen, R. (2011). Global-
20, 432–455. J., Krohn, M. D., & Smith, C. (2003). Linked ization and adolescence. In B. Brown &
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-79 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-79

M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- Lessons from a multigenerational, longitudi- sex-typed personal qualities. Family Rela-
cence (Vol. 2, pp. 109–117). New York: Aca- nal research study. Development and Psycho- tions: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied
demic Press. pathology, 23, 453–476. Family Studies, 52, 82–89.
Toomey, R. B., Ryan, C., Diaz, R. M., Card, N. A., Truong, K. D., & Sturm, R. (2009). Alcohol envi- Tucker, C., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (2003b).
& Russell, S. T. (2010). Gender-nonconforming ronments and disparities in exposure associ- Conflict resolution links with adolescents’
lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth: ated with adolescent drinking in California. family relationships and individual well-being.
School victimization and young adult psy- American Journal of Public Health, 99, 264–270. Journal of Family Influence, 24, 715–736.
chosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychol- Trusty, J., Plata, M., & Salazar, C. F. (2003). Mod- Tucker, C., Updegraff, K. A., & Baril, M. E.
ogy, 46, 1580–1589. eling Mexican Americans’ educational ex- (2010). Who’s the boss? Patterns of control
Topolski, T. D., Patrick, D. L., Edwards, T. C., pectations: Longitudinal effects of variables in adolescents’ sibling relationships. Family
Huebner, C. E., Connell, F. A., & Mount, K. K. across adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Re- Relations, 59, 520–532.
(2001). Quality of life and health-risk behav- search, 18, 131–153. Tucker, J. S., Ellickson, P. L., Collins, R. L., & Klein,
iors among adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Trzesniewski, K., & Donnellan, M. (2009). Re- D. J. (2006). Are drug experimenters better
Health, 29, 426–435. evaluating the evidence for increasingly pos- adjusted than abstainers and users?: A longi-
Torney-Purta, J. (1990). Youth in relation to so- itive self-views among high school students: tudinal study of adolescent marijuana use.
cial institutions. In S. Feldman & G. Elliott More evidence for consistency across genera- Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 488–494.
(Eds.), At the threshold: The developing ado- tions (1976–2006). Psychological Science, 20, Tucker-Drob, E., & Harden, K. (2012). Intellectual
lescent, (pp. 457–478). Cambridge, MA: Har- 920–922 interest mediates gene–socioeconomic status
vard University Press. Trzesniewski, K., Donnellan, M., Moffitt, T., interaction on adolescent academic achieve-
Tough, P. (2008). Whatever it takes: Geoffrey Can- Robins, R., Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2006). ment. Child Development, 83, 743–757.
ada’s quest to change Harlem and America. Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. (2000). Non-
New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. poor health, criminal behavior, and limited shared environment: A theoretical, method-
Tram, J. M., & Cole, D. A. (2000). Self-perceived economic prospects during adulthood. De- ological, and quantitative review. Psychological
competence and the relation between life velopmental Psychology, 42, 381–390. Bulletin, 126, 78–108.
events and depressive symptoms in adoles- Trzesniewski, K., Donnellan, M., & Robins, R. Turner, H. A., Finkelhor, D., Hamby, S. L., Shattuck,
cence: Mediator or moderator? Journal of (2003). Stability of self-esteem across the life A., & Ormrod, R. K. (2011). Specifying type
Abnormal Psychology, 109, 753–760. span. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- and location of peer victimization in a national
Treaster, J. (1994, February 1). Survey finds mar- chology, 84, 205–220. sample of children and youth. Journal of Youth
ijuana use is up in high schools. The New Tsai, K. M., & Fuligni, A. J. (2012). Change in and Adolescence, 40, 1052–1067.
York Times, pp. A1ff. ethnic identity across the college transition. Turner, R., Sorenson, A. M., & Turner, J. (2000).
Trejos-Castillo, E. & Vazsonyi, A. (2011). Tran- Developmental Psychology, 48, 56–64. Social contingencies in mental health: A seven-
sitions into adolescence. In B. Brown & Tschann, J., & Adler, N. (1997). Sexual self- year follow-up study of teenage mothers. Jour-
M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- acceptance, communication with partner, nal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 777–791.
cence (Vol. 1, pp. 369–375). New York: Aca- and contraceptive use among adolescent fe- Twenge, J. (2000). The age of anxiety? The birth
demic Press. males: A longitudinal study. Journal of Re- cohort change in anxiety and neuroticism,
Tremblay, R., Pagani-Kurtz, L., Mâsse, L., Vitaro, search on Adolescence, 7, 413–430. 1952–1993. Journal of Personality and Social
F., & Pihl, R. (1995). A bimodal preventive Tschann, J., Flores, E., VanOss Marin, B., Pasch, Psychology, 79, 1007–1021.
intervention for disruptive kindergarten L. A., Baisch, E., & Wibbelsman, C. J. (2002). Twenge, J., & Crocker, J. (2002). Race and self-
boys: Its impact through mid-adolescence. Interparental conflict and risk behaviors esteem: Meta-analyses comparing Whites,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, among Mexican American adolescents: A Blacks, Hispanics, Asians, and American In-
63, 560–568. cognitive-emotional model. Journal of Ab- dians and comment on Gray-Little and
Trent, K., & Crowder, K. (1997). Adolescent birth normal Psychology, 30, 373–385. Hafdahl. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 371–408.
intentions, social disadvantage, and behav- Tschann, J., Pasch, L., Flores, E., Marin, B., Twenge, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2002). Age,
ioral outcomes. Journal of Marriage and the Baisch, E., & Wibbelsman, C. (2009). Nonvi- gender, race, socioeconomic status, and
Family, 59, 523–535. olent aspects of interparental conflict and birth cohort difference on the children’s de-
Trent, K., & Harlan, S. (1994). Teenage mothers dating violence among adolescents. Journal pression inventory: A meta-analysis. Journal
in nuclear and extended households: Differ- of Family Issues, 30, 295–319. of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 578–588.
ences by marital status and race/ethnicity. Tseng, V., & Fuligni, A. (2000). Parent–adolescent Tynes, B. (2007). Role taking in online “class-
Journal of Family Issues, 15, 309–337. language use and relationship among immi- rooms”: What adolescents are learning about
Trentacosta, C. J., Criss, M. M., Shaw, D. S., grant families with East Asian, Filipino and race and ethnicity. Developmental Psychology,
Lacourse, E., Hyde, L. W., & Dishion, T. J. Latin American backgrounds. Journal of Mar- 43, 1312–1320.
(2011). Antecedents and outcomes of joint riage and the Family, 62, 465–476. Tynes, B., Umaña-Taylor, A., Rose, C., Lin, J., &
trajectories of mother-son conflict and Tubman, J. G., Montgomery, M. J., Gil, A. G., & Anderson, C. (2012). Online racial discrimi-
warmth during middle childhood and ado- Wagner, E. F. (2004). Abuse experiences in a nation and the protective function of ethnic
lescence. Child Development, 82, 1676–1690. community sample of young adults: Rela- identity and self-esteem for African Ameri-
Trickett, P., McBride-Chang, C., & Putnam, F. tions with psychiatric disorders, sexual risk can adolescents. Developmental Psychology,
(1994). The classroom performance and be- behaviors, and sexually transmitted diseases. 48, 343–355.
havior of sexually abused females. Develop- American Journal of Community Psychology, Tyrka, A. R., Graber, J. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J.
ment and Psychopathology, 6, 183–194. 34, 147–162. (2000). The development of disordered eat-
Trickett, P., Negriff, S., Ji, J., & Peckins, M. (2011). Tucker, C., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (2001). ing: Correlate and predictors of eating prob-
Child maltreatment and adolescent develop- Conditions of sibling support in adolescence. lems in the context of adolescence. In A. J.
ment. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 254–271. Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller (Eds.),
3–20. Tucker, C., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (2003a). Di- Handbook of developmental psychopathology
Trickett, P., Noll, J., & Putnam, F. (2011). The im- mensions of mothers’ and fathers’ differential (2nd ed., pp. 607–624). New York: Kluwer
pact of sexual abuse on female development: treatment of siblings: Links with adolescents’ Academic/Plenum.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-80 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-80 References

Tyson, K., Darity, W., & Castellino, D. R. (2005). Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Guimond, A. B. (2010). A Updegraff, K., Kim, J., Killoren, S., & Thayer, S.
It’s not “a Black thing.” Understanding the longitudinal examination of parenting be- (2010). Mexican American parents’ involve-
burden of acting White and other dilemmas haviors and perceived discrimination pre- ment in adolescents’ peer relationships: Ex-
of high achievement. American Sociological dicting Latino adolescents’ ethnic identity. ploring the role of culture and adolescents’
Review, 70, 582–605. Developmental Psychology, 46, 636–650. peer experiences. Journal of Research on Ado-
U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). Projections of the to- Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Updegraff, K. A. (2007). lescence, 20, 65–87.
tal resident population by 5-year age groups Latino adolescents’ mental health: Exploring Updegraff, K., Madden-Derdich, D., Estrada, A.,
and sex with special age categories: Middle se- the interrelations among discrimination, Sales, L., & Leonard, S. (2002). Young adoles-
ries, 1999 to 2100. Washington, DC: Author. ethnic identity, cultural orientation, self- cents’ experiences with parents and friends:
U.S. Census Bureau. (2009a). Educational attain- esteem, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Exploring the connections. Family Relations,
ment in the United States: 2007. Washington, Adolescence, 30, 549–567. 51, 72–80.
DC: Author. Underwood, M., Rosen, L., More, D. Ehrenreich, Updegraff, K., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (2000).
U.S. Census Bureau. (2009b). Statistical abstract S., & Gentsch, J. (2012). The BlackBerry Adolescents’ sex-typed friendship experi-
of the United States. Washington, DC: Author. project: Capturing the content of adoles- ences: Does having a sister versus a brother
cents’ text messaging. Developmental Psy- matter? Child Development, 71, 1597–1610.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2011). Current population
survey. Washington, DC: Author. chology, 48, 295–302. Updegraff, K., McHale, S., Crouter, A., & Kupanoff,
Unger, J., Kipke, M., Simon, T., Montgomery, S., K. (2001). Parents’ involvement in adolescents’
U.S. Census Bureau. (2012). Current population
& Johnson, C. (1997). Homeless youths and peer relationships: A comparison of mothers’
survey. Washington, DC: Author.
young adults in Los Angeles: Prevalence of and fathers’ roles. Journal of Marriage and the
U.S. Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Family, 63, 655–668.
mental health problems and the relationship
Census. (1940). Characteristics of the popula-
between mental health and substance abuse Updegraff, K., McHale, S., Whiteman, S., Thayer,
tion. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
disorders. American Journal of Community S., & Crouter, A. (2006). The nature and cor-
Printing Office.
Psychology, 25, 371–394. relates of Mexican-American adolescents’
U.S. Department of Education. (2006). No Child time with parents and peers. Child Develop-
Unger J., Molina G., & Teran, L. (2002). Per-
Left Behind. Available at www.ed.gov. ment, 77, 1470–1486.
ceived consequences of teenage childbearing
U.S. Department of Education. (2009). Creating among adolescent girls in an urban sample. Urberg, K., Değirmencioğlu, S., & Pilgrim, C.
common standards, turning around schools. Journal of Adolescent Health, 26, 205–212. (1997). Close friend and group influence on
Retrieved from http://www.ed.gov/index. adolescent cigarette smoking and alcohol
jhtml Unger, J., & Rohrbach, L. (2002). Why do adoles-
cents overestimate their peers’ smoking prev- use. Developmental Psychology, 33, 834–844.
U.S. Senate Committee on the Judiciary (1955, Urberg, K., Değirmencioğilu, S., Tolson, J., &
alence? Correlates of prevalence estimates
March). Comic books and juvenile delin- Halliday-Scher, K. (1995). The structure of
among California 8th-grade students. Jour-
quency, Interim report. Washington: Author. adolescent peer networks. Developmental
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 147–153.
Udry, J., Billy, J., Morris, N., Gruff, T., & Raj, M. Psychology, 31, 540–547.
UNICEF (2001, July). A league table of teenage
(1985). Serum androgenic hormones moti- Urdan, T. (2004). Predictors of academic self-
births in rich nations. Innocenti Report Card
vate sexual behavior in boys. Fertility and handicapping and achievement: Examining
No. 3. UNICEF Innocenti Research Centre,
Sterility, 43, 90–94. achievement goals, classroom goal struc-
Florence.
Uhls, Y., Espinoza, G., Greenfield, P., Subrah- tures, and culture. Journal of Educational
United Nations. (2010). Population by age, sex
manyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011). Internet Psychology, 96, 251–264.
and urban/rural residence: Latest available
and other interactive media. In B. Brown & Usher, E. L. (2009). Sources of middle school
year, 2000–2009. Demographic yearbook.
M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adoles- students’ self-efficacy in mathematics: A
New York: Author.
cence (Vol. 2, pp. 160–168). New York: Aca- qualitative investigation. American Educa-
demic Press. United Nations. (2012). Table 4c: Secondary ed- tional Research Journal, 46, 275–314.
ucation. Social indicators. New York: Author.
Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2004). Ethnic identity and Usher-Seriki, K., Bynum, M. S., & Callands, T. A.
self-esteem: Examining the role of social Upchurch, D., & McCarthy, J. (1990). The timing (2008). Mother-daughter communication
context. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 139–146. of a first birth and high school completion. about sex and sexual intercourse among
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Alfaro, E. C., Bámaca, M. Y., American Sociological Review, 55, 224–234. middle- to upper-class African American
& Guimond, A. B. (2009). The central role of Upchurch, D., Aneshensel, C. S., Mudgal, J., & girls. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 901–917.
familial ethnic socialization in Latino adoles- McNeely, C. (2001). Sociocultural contexts Valente, T., Fujimoto, K., Chou, C., & Spruijt-
cents’ cultural orientation. Journal of Mar- of time to first sex among Hispanic adoles- Metz, D. (2009). Adolescent affiliations and
riage and the Family, 71, 46–90. cents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, adiposity: a social network analysis of friend-
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Bámaca-Gómez, M. Y. 1158–1169. ships and obesity. Journal of Adolescent Health,
(2003). Generational differences in resis- Upchurch, D., Aneshensel, C., Sucoff, C., & Levy- 45, 202–204.
tance to peer pressure among Mexican- Storms, L. (1999). Neighborhood and family Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Preadoles-
origin adolescents. Youth and Society, 35, contexts of adolescent sexual activity. Journal cents’ and adolescents’ online communica-
183–203. of Marriage and the Family, 61, 920–933. tion and their closeness to friends.
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Bhanot, R., & Shin, N. Upchurch, D., Levy-Storms, L., Sucoff, C., & Developmental Psychology, 43, 267–277.
(2006). Ethnic identity formation during Aneshensel, C. (1998). Gender and ethnic Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2009). Social con-
adolescence: The critical role of families. differences in the timing of first sexual inter- sequences of the Internet for adolescents: A
Journal of Family Issues, 27, 390–414. course. Family Planning Perspectives, 30, decade of research. Current Directions in Psy-
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Gonzales-Backen, M. A., & 121–127. chological Science, 18, 1–5.
Guimond, A. B. (2009). Latino adolescents’ Updegraff, K., Helms, H., McHale, S., Crouter, Valkenburg, P., & Peter, J. (2011). Online com-
ethnic identity: Is there a developmental A., Thayer, S., & Sales, L. (2004). Who’s the munication among adolescents: An inte-
progression and does growth in ethnic iden- boss? Patterns of perceived control in adoles- grated model of its attraction, opportunities,
tity predict growth in self-esteem? Child De- cents’ friendships. Journal of Youth and Ado- and risks. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48,
velopment, 80, 391–405. lescence, 33, 403–420. 121–127.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-81 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-81

Valois, R., Oeltmann, J., Waller, J., & Hussey, J. to 32. Journal of Research on Crime and De- Van Lier, P., Wanner, B., & Vitaro, F. (2007). Onset
(1999). Relationship between number of linquency, 46, 111–143. of antisocial behavior, affiliation with devi-
sexual intercourse partners and selected Van Doorn, M. D., Branje, S. J. T., & Meeus, W. H. J. ant friends, and childhood maladjustment: A
health risk behaviors among public high (2011). Developmental changes in con- test of the childhood- and adolescent-onset
school adolescents. Journal of Adolescent flict resolution styles in parent–adolescent models. Development and Psychopathology,
Health, 25, 328–335. relationships: A four-wave longitudinal 19, 167–185.
van Aken, M., Hutteman, R., & Denissen, J. study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, Van Petegem, S., Beyers, W., Vansteenkiste, M.,
(2011). Personality traits in adolescence. In 97–107. & Soenens, B. (2012). On the association
B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia van Duijvenvoorde, A., Jansen, B. R. J., Bredman, between adolescent autonomy and psychoso-
of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 261–268). New J. C., & Huizenga, H. M. (2012). Age-related cial functioning: Examining decisional inde-
York: Academic Press. changes in decision making: Comparing in- pendence from a self-determination theory
van den Akker, A., Deković, M., & Prinzie, P. formed and noninformed situations. Devel- perspective. Developmental Psychology, 48,
(2010). Transitioning to adolescence: How opmental Psychology, 48, 192–203. 76–88.
changes in child personality and overreactive van Duijvenvoorde, A., Jansen, B., Visser, I., & van Rooij, A., Schoenmakers, T., van de Eijnden,
parenting predict adolescent adjustment Huizenga, H. M. (2010). Affective and cogni- R., & van de Mheen, D. (2010). Compulsive
problems. Development and Psychopathology, tive decision-making in adolescents. Devel- Internet use: the role of online gaming and
22, 151–163. opmental Neuropsychology, 35, 539–554. other internet applications. Journal of Ado-
van den Berg, P., Mond, J., Eisenberg, M., Ackard, lescent Health, 47, 51–57.
van Geel, M., & Vedder, P. (2011). The role of
D., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2010). The link family obligations and school adjustment in Van Ryzin, M., Johnson, A. B., Leve, L., & Kim,
between body dissatisfaction and self-esteem explaining the Immigrant paradox. Journal H. K. (2011). The number of sexual partners
in adolescents: similarities across gender, age, of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 187–196. and health-risking sexual behavior: Predic-
weight status, race/ethnicity, and socioeco- tion from high school entry to high school
van Holst, R., Lemmens, J., Valkenburg, Peter, J.,
nomic status. Journal of Adolescent Health, exit. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 939–949.
Veltman, D., & Goudriaan, A. (2012). Atten-
47, 290–296. tional bias and disinhibition toward gaming van Wel, F. (1994). “I count my parents among
van den Berg, P., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Hannan, cues are related to problem gaming in male my best friends”: Youths’ bonds with parents
P. J., & Haines, J. (2007). Is dieting advice from adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, and friends in the Netherlands. Journal of
magazines helpful or harmful? Five-year as- 541–546. Marriage and the Family, 56, 835–843.
sociations with weight-control behaviors and van Leeuwen, A. P., Verhulst, F. C., Reijneveld, Van Zalk, M. H. W., Kerr, M., Branje, S. J. T., Stat-
psychological outcomes in adolescents. Pedi- S. A., Vollebergh, W. A. M., Ormel, J., & Huizink, tin, H., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2010). It takes
atrics, 119, 30–37. A. C. (2011). Can the gateway hypothesis, the three: Selection, influence, and de-selection
van den Bos W., Crone, E., & Güroğlu, B. (2012). common liability model and/or, the route of processes of depression in adolescent friend-
Brain function during probabilistic learning administration model predict initiation of ship networks. Developmental Psychology, 46,
in relation to IQ and level of education. Devel- cannabis use during adolescence? A survival 927–938.
opmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, S78–89. analysis—The TRAILS study. Journal of Ado- Vandell, D., & Ramanan, J. (1991). Children of
van den Bos, W., Westenberg, M., van Dijk, E., & lescent Health, 48, 73–78. the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth:
Crone, E. A. (2010). Development of trust Van Leeuwen, K. G., Mervielda, I., Braet, C., & Choices in after-school care and child develop-
and reciprocity in adolescence. Cognitive De- Bosmans, G. (2004). Child personality and ment. Developmental Psychology, 27, 637–643.
velopment, 25, 90–102. parental behavior as moderators of problem Vaughan, C. A., Foshee, V. A., & Ennett, S. T.
Van den Bulck, J. (2004). Television viewing, behavior: Variable- and person-centered (2010). Protective effects of maternal and
computer game playing, and Internet use approaches. Developmental Psychology, 40, peer support on depressive symptoms dur-
and self-reported time to bed and time out 1028–1046. ing adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child
of bed in secondary-school children. Sleep, Van Lieijenhorst, L., Moor, B., Op de Macks, Z. O., Psychology, 38, 261–272.
27, 101–104. Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Westenberg, P. M., & Vaughan, C. A., & Halpern, C. T. (2010). Gender
van den Eijnden, R., Meerkerk, G., Vermulst, A. Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision- differences in depressive symptoms during
A., Spijkerman, R., & Engels, R. (2008). On- making: Neurocognitive development of re- adolescence: The contributions of weight-
line communication, compulsive Internet ward and control regions. Neuroimage, 51, related concerns and behaviors. Journal of
use, and psychosocial well-being among ad- 345–355. Research on Adolescence, 20, 389–419.
olescents: A longitudinal study. Developmen- Van Leijenhorst, L., Westenberg, P. M., & Crone, Vaughan, R., McCarthy, J., Armstrong, B., Walter,
tal Psychology, 44, 655–665. E. A. (2008). A developmental study of risky H., Waterman, P., & Tiezzi, L. (1996). Carry-
van den Eijnden, R., Spijkerman, R., Vermulst, A., decisions on the cake gambling task: Age and ing and using weapons: A survey of minority
Van Rooij, T., & Engels, R. (2010). Compul- gender analyses of probability estimation junior high school students in New York
sive Internet use among adolescents: Bidirec- and reward evaluation. Developmental Neu- City. American Journal of Public Health, 86,
tional parent–child relationships. Journal of ropsychology, 33, 179–196. 568–572.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 77–89. Van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C., Vazsonyi, A. T., & Belliston, L. M. (2006). The
van der Aa, N., Overbeek, G., Engels, R. C. M. E., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & cultural and developmental significance of
Scholte, R. H. J., Meerkerk, G.-J., & Eijnden, Crone, E. A. (2010). What motivates the ado- parenting processes in adolescent anxiety
R. J. J. M. (2009). Daily and compulsive inter- lescent? Brain regions mediating reward sen- and depression symptoms. Journal of Youth
net use and well-being in adolescence: A dia- sitivity across adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, and Adolescence, 35, 491–505.
thesis-stress model based on big five 20, 61–69. Vazsonyi, A. T., Hibbert, J. R., & Black, S. J.
personality traits. Journal of Youth and Ado- Van Lier, P. A. C., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Koot, (2003). Exotic enterprise no more? Adoles-
lescence, 38, 765–776. H. M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2009). Develop- cent reports of family and parenting pro-
van der Geest, V., Blokland, A., & Bijleveld, C. mental links between trajectories of physical cesses from youth in four countries. Journal
(2009). Delinquent development in a sample violence, vandalism, theft, and alcohol-drug of Research on Adolescence, 13, 129–160.
of high-risk youth: Shape, content, and pre- use from childhood to adolescence. Journal Vazsonyi, A. T., Trejos-Castillo, E., & Huang, L.
dictors of delinquent trajectories from age 12 of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 481–492. (2006). Are developmental processes affected
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-82 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-82 References

by immigration? Family processes, internal- defendants: Predictors of confessions, pleas, during adolescence: National panel results
izing behaviors, and externalizing behaviors. communication with attorneys, and appeals. from three countries. Journal of the American
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 799–813. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 253–277. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Verhulst, F. C., & Viljoen, J., McLachlan, K., Wingrove, T., & 41, 190–198.
Ormel, J. (2009). Childhood-limited versus Penner, E. (2010). Defense attorneys’ con- Wadley, J. (2012, June 1). American teens are less
persistent antisocial behavior: Why do some cerns about the competence of adolescent likely than European teens to use cigarettes
recover and others do not? The TRAILS study. defendants. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, and alcohol, but more likely to use illicit
Journal of Early Adolescence, 29, 718–742. 28, 630–646. drugs. University of Michigan News Service,
Ventura, S., Abma, J., Mosher, W., & Henshaw, S. Vincent, G. M., Vitacco, M. J., Grisso, T., & Ann Arbor, MI.
(2008). Estimated pregnancy rates by out- Corrado, R. R. (2003). Subtypes of adoles- Wadsworth, M. E., & Compas, B. E. (2002). Cop-
come for the United States, 1990–2004. Na- cent offenders: Affective traits and antisocial ing with family conflict and economic strain:
tional Vital Statistics Reports, 56, Number 15. behavior patterns. Behavioral Sciences and The adolescent perspective. Journal of Re-
Vera Institute of Justice. (1990). The male role in the Law, 21, 695–712. search on Adolescence, 12, 243–274.
teenage pregnancy and parenting. New York: Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. Waite, E. B., Shanahan, L., Calkins, S. D., Keane,
Vera Institute of Justice. (2000). Influence of deviant friends on de- S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2011). Life events, sib-
Verkuyten, M., & Slooter, L. (2008). Muslim and linquency: Searching for moderator vari- ling warmth, and youths’ adjustment. Jour-
non-Muslim adolescents’ reasoning about ables. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, nal of Marriage and Family, 73, 902–912.
freedom of speech and minority rights. Child 28, 313–325. Waite, L., Goldscheider, F., & Witsberger, C.
Development, 79, 514–528. Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R., Kerr, M., Pagani, L., (1986). Nonfamily living and the erosion of
Verma, S., & Larson, R. (Eds.). (2003). Examin- & Bukowski, W. (1997). Disruptiveness, traditional family orientations among young
ing adolescent leisure time across cultures. friends’ characteristics, and delinquency in adults. American Sociological Review, 51,
New Directions for Child and Adolescent De- early adolescence: A test of two competing 541–554.
velopment, 99. models. Child Development, 68, 676–689. Waizenhofer, R., Buchanan, C. M., & Jackson-
Vermeer, H. J., Boekaerts, M., & Seegers, G. Vloet, T. D., Konrad, K., Huebner, T., Herpertz, S., Newsom, J. (2004). Mothers’ and fathers’
(2000). Motivational and gender differences: & Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2008). Structural knowledge of adolescents’ daily activities:
Sixth-grade students’ mathematical problem- and functional MRI-findings in children and Its sources and its links with adolescent ad-
solving behavior. Journal of Educational Psy- adolescents with antisocial behavior. Behav- justment. Journal of Family Psychology, 18,
chology, 92, 308–315. ioral Sciences & the Law, 26, 99–111. 348–360.
Verona, E., & Javdani, S. (2011). Dimensions of Voelkl, K. (1997). Identification with school. Wakefield, M., Terry-McElrath, Y., Emery, S.,
adolescent psychopathology and relation- American Journal of Education, 105, 294–318. Saffer, H., Chaloupka, F. J., Szczypka, G., . . .
ships to suicide risk indicators. Journal of Vogt Yuan, A. S. (2010). Body perceptions, Johnston, L. (2006). Effect of televised, to-
Youth and Adolescence, 40, 958–971. weight control behavior, and changes in ado- bacco company–funded smoking prevention
Véronneau, M., & Dishion, T. (2011). Middle lescents’ psychological well-being over time: advertising on youth smoking-related be-
school friendships and academic achievement A Longitudinal examination of gender. Jour- liefs, intentions, and behavior. American
in early adolescence: A longitudinal analysis. nal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 927–939. Journal of Public Health, 96, 2154–2160.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 31, 99–124. Vo-Jutabha, E. D., Dinh, K. T., McHale, J. P., & Wakschlag, L. S., Gordon, R. A., Lahey, B. B.,
Véronneau, M.-H., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Dish- Valsiner, J. (2009). A qualitative analysis of Loeber, R., Green, S. M., & Leventhal, B. L.
ion, T. J., & Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Transac- Vietnamese adolescent identity exploration (2000). Maternal age at first birth and boys’
tional analysis of the reciprocal links between within and outside an ethnic enclave. Journal risk for conduct disorder. Journal of Research
peer experiences and academic achievement of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 672–690. on Adolescence, 10, 417–441.
from middle childhood to early adolescence. Volkow, N., & Ting-Kai, L. (2005). The neurosci- Walberg, H. (1998). Spending more while learn-
Developmental Psychology, 46, 773–790. ence of addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 8, ing less. New York: Thomas B. Fordham
Videon, T. M. (2002). The effects of parent- 1429–1430. Foundation.
relationships and parental separation on ad- Vrangalova, Z., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2011). Wald, M. (2005). Foreword. In D. W. Osgood, M.
olescent well-being. Journal of Marriage and Adolescent sexuality and positive well-being: Foster, C. Flanagan, & G. Ruth (Eds.), On
the Family, 64, 489–503. A group-norms approach. Journal of Youth your own without a net: The transition to
Vieno, A., Nation, M., Pastore, M., & Santinello, and Adolescence, 40, 931–944. adulthood for vulnerable populations, (pp.
M. (2009). Parenting and antisocial behav- Vuchinich, S., Angeletti, J., & Gatherum, A. vii–xi). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
ior: A model of the relationship between ad- (1996). Context and development in family Waldman, I. (1996). Aggressive boys’ hostile per-
olescent self-disclosure, parental closeness, problem solving with preadolescent chil- ceptual and response biases: The role of at-
parental control, and adolescent antisocial dren. Child Development, 67, 1276–1288. tention and impulsivity. Child Development,
behavior. Developmental Psychology, 45, Vuchinich, S., Hetherington, E. M., Vuchinich, 67, 1015–1033.
1509–1519. R., & Clingempeel, W. (1991). Parent–child Walker, E., Sabuwalla, Z., & Huot, R. (2004). Pu-
Vieno, A., Nation, M., Perkins, D., Pastore, M., & interaction and gender differences in early bertal neuromaturation, stress sensitivity,
Santinello, M. (2010). Social capital, safety adolescents’ adaptation to stepfamilies. De- and psychopathology. Development and Psy-
concerns, parenting, and early adolescents’ velopmental Psychology, 27, 618–626. chopathology, 16, 807–824.
antisocial behavior. Journal of Community Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, Walker, L., Gustafson, P., & Hennig, K. H. (2001).
Psychology, 38, 314–328. MA: Harvard University Press. (Original The consolidation/transition model in moral
Vieno, A., Perkins, D. D., Smith, T. M., & Santinello, work published 1930) reasoning development. Developmental Psy-
M. (2005). Democratic school climate and Waasdorp, T., & Bradshaw, C. (2011). Examining chology, 37, 187–197.
sense of community in the school: A multilevel student responses to frequent bullying: A Walker, M. (2009). The role of sleep in cognition
analysis. American Journal of Community Psy- latent class approach. Journal of Educational and emotion. Annals of the New York Acad-
chology, 36, 327–341. Psychology, 103, 336–352. emy of Sciences, 1156, 168–197.
Viljoen, J. L., Klaver, J., & Roesch, R. (2005). Le- Wade, T. J., Cairney, J., & Pevalin, D. J. (2002). Walker-Barnes, C. J., & Mason, C. A. (2001).
gal decisions of preadolescent and adolescent Emergence of gender differences in depression Ethnic difference in the effect of parenting
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-83 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-83

on gang involvement and gang delin- Wang, Q., & Pomerantz, E. (2009). The motiva- in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-
quency: A longitudinal, hierarchical linear tional landscape of early adolescence in the year longitudinal study. Child Development,
modeling perspective. Child Development, United States and China: A longitudinal inves- 71, 684–689.
72, 1814–1831. tigation. Child Development, 80, 1272–1287. Watt, H. M. (2004). Development of adolescents’
Walker-Barnes, C. J., & Mason, C. A. (2004). De- Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E., & Chen, H. (2007). The self-perceptions, values, and task percep-
linquency and substance use among gang- role of parents’ control in early adolescents’ tions according to gender and domain in
involved youth: The moderating role of psychological functioning: A longitudinal 7th- through 11th-grade Australian students.
parenting practices. American Journal of investigation in the United States and China. Child Development, 75, 1556–1574.
Community Psychology, 34, 235–250. Child Development, 78, 1592–1610. Watt, T. T. (2003). Are small schools and private
Walkup, J. T., Albano, A. M., Piacentini, J., Ward, L. M. (1995). Talking about sex: Common schools better for adolescents’ emotional
Birmaher, B., Compton, S. N., . . . Sherrill, themes about sexuality in the prime-time adjustment? Sociology of Education, 76,
J. T. (2008). Cognitive behavioral therapy, television programs children and adoles- 344–367.
sertraline, or a combination in childhood cents view most. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Way, N., & Chen, L. (2000). Close and general
anxiety. New England Journal of Medicine, cence, 24, 595–615. friendships among African American,
359, 2753–2766. Ward, L. M. (2003). Understanding the role of Latino, and Asian American adolescents
Wall, J., Power, T., & Arbona, C. (1993). Suscepti- entertainment media in the sexual socializa- from low-income families. Journal of Adoles-
bility to antisocial peer pressure and its rela- tion of American youth: A review of empiri- cent Research, 15, 274–301.
tion to acculturation in Mexican-American cal research. Developmental Review, 23, Way, N., & Greene, M. L. (2006). Trajectories of
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 8, 347–388. perceived friendship quality during adoles-
403–418. cence: The patterns and contextual predic-
Ward, L. M., & Friedman, K. (2006). Using TV as
Wallace, J. M., Forman, T. A., Caldwell, C. H., & a guide: Associations between television tors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16,
Willis, D. S. (2003). Religion and U.S. sec- viewing and adolescents’ sexual attitudes and 293–320.
ondary school students: Current patterns, behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Way, N., & Pahl, K. (2001). Individual and con-
recent trends, and sociodemographic corre- 16, 133–156. textual predictors of perceived friendship
lates. Youth and Society, 35, 98–125. quality among ethnic minority, low-income
Ward, L. M., Hansbrough, E., & Walker, E.
Walls, N. E., Kane, S. B., & Wisneski, H. (2010). (2005). Contributions of music video expo- adolescents. Journal of Research on Adoles-
Gay–straight alliances and school experi- sure to Black adolescents’ gender and sexual cence, 11, 325–349.
ences of sexual minority youth. Youth & So- schemas. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, Way, N., Reddy, R., & Rhodes, J. (2007). Students’
ciety, 41, 307–332. 143–166. perceptions of school climate during the
Walsh, B., Kaplan, A., Attia, E., Olmsted, M., Warneke, C., & Cooper, S. (1994). Child and ad- middle school years: Associations with tra-
Parides, M., . . . Carter, J. (2006). Fluoxetine olescent drownings in Harris County, Texas, jectories of psychological and behavioral ad-
after weight restoration in anorexia 1983 through 1990. American Journal of Pub- justment. American Journal of Community
nervosa: A randomized controlled trial. lic Health, 84, 593–598. Psychology, 40, 194–213.
Journal of the American Medical Association, Way, N., & Robinson, M. G. (2003). A longitudi-
Warner, S., & Moore, S. (2004). Excuses, excuses:
i2605–2612. nal study of the effects of family, friends, and
Self-handicapping in an Australian adoles-
Walsh, S., Shulman, S., Bar-On, Z. & Tsur, A. cent sample. Journal of Youth and Adoles- school experiences on the psychological ad-
(2006). The role of parentification and fam- cence, 33, 271–281. justment of ethnic minority, low-SES adoles-
ily climate in adaptation among immigrant cents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18,
adolescents in Israel. Journal of Research on Warr, M. (2007). The tangled web: Delinquency,
324–346.
Adolescence, 16, 321–350. deception, and parental attachment. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 607–622. Weaver, S. R., & Kim, S. Y. (2008). A person-
Wang, L., Huettel, S., & De Bellis, M. D. (2008). centered approach to studying the linkages
Neural substrates for processing task- Warren, J. R. (2002). Reconsidering the relation- among parent–child differences in cultural
irrelevant sad images in adolescents. Devel- ship between student employment and aca- orientation, supportive parenting, and ado-
opmental Science, 11, 23–32. demic outcomes: A new theory and better lescent depressive symptoms in Chinese
data. Youth and Society, 33, 366–393. American families. Journal of Youth and Ado-
Wang, L., McCarthy, G., Song, A., & LaBar, K.
(2005). Amygdala activation to sad pictures Warren, J. R., & Jenkins, K. N. (2005). High lescence, 37, 36–49.
during high-field (4 Tesla) functional school exit examinations and high school Weems, C., Taylor, L., Cannon, M., Marino, R.,
magnetic resonance imaging. Emotion, 5, dropout in Texas and Florida, 1971–2000. Scott, B., . . . Triplett, V. (2010). Post trau-
12–22. Sociology of Education, 78, 122–143. matic stress, context, and the lingering
Wang, M., Dishion, T. J., Stormshak, E., & Warren, J. R., Grodsky, E., & Lee, J. C. (2008). effects of the Hurricane Katrina disaster
Willett, J. B. (2011). Trajectories of family State high school exit examinations and among ethnic minority youth. Journal of Ab-
management practices and early adolescent postsecondary labor market outcomes. Soci- normal Child Psychology, 38, 49–56.
behavioral outcomes. Developmental Psy- ology of Education, 81, 77–107. Weerman, F. (2011). Delinquent peers in con-
chology, 47, 1324–1341. Washburn-Ormachea, J. M., Hillman, S. B., & text: a longitudinal network analysis of selec-
Wang, M., & Holcombe, R. (2010). Adolescents’ Sawilowsky, S. S. (2004). Gender and gender- tion and influence effects. Criminology, 49,
perceptions of school environment, engage- role orientation differences on adolescents’ 253–286.
ment, and academic achievement in middle coping with peer stressors. Journal of Youth Weinfield, N., Ogawa, J. R., & Sroufe, L. A.
school. American Educational Research Jour- and Adolescent, 33, 31–40. (1997). Early attachment as a pathway to
nal, 47, 633–662. Wasserman, G., Rauh, V., Brunelli, S., Garcia- adolescent peer competence. Journal of Re-
Wang, M., Selman, R., Dishion, T., & Stormshak, Castro, M., & Necos, B. (1990). Psychosocial search on Adolescence, 7(3), 241–265.
E. (2010). A tobit regression analysis of the attributes and life experiences of disadvan- Weinfield, N., Sroufe, A., & Egeland, B. (2000).
covariation between middle school students’ taged minority mothers: Age and ethnic Attachment from infancy to early adulthood
perceived school climate and behavioral variations. Child Development, 61, 566–580. in a high-risk sample: Continuity, disconti-
problems. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & nuity, and their correlates. Child Develop-
20, 274–286. Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment security ment, 71, 695–702.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-84 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-84 References

Weinstein, S. M., Mermelstein, R. J., Hankin, B. L., adjustment in early adolescence. Child De- White, A. M., & Gager, C. T. (2007). Idle hands
Hedeker, D., & Flay, B. R. (2007). Longitudi- velopment, 73, 287–301. and empty pockets? Youth involvement in ex-
nal patterns of daily affect and global mood Wentzel, K. (2003). Sociometric status and ad- tracurricular activities, social capital, and eco-
during adolescence. Journal of Research on justment in middle school: A longitudinal nomic status. Youth and Society, 39, 75–111.
Adolescence, 17, 587–600. study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 5–28. White, H. R., Fleming, C. B., Kim, M. J., Catalano,
Weiser, J., & Reynolds, B. (2011). Impulsivity and Wentzel, K., & Asher, S. (1995). The academic R. F., & McMorris, B. J. (2008). Identifying
adolescence. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein lives of neglected, rejected, popular, and con- two potential mechanisms for changes in
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, troversial children. Child Development, 66, alcohol use among college-attending and
pp. 187–192). New York: Academic Press. 754–763. non-college-attending emerging adults. De-
Weisgram, E. S., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. Wentzel, K., Barry, C., & Caldwell, K. (2004). velopmental Psychology, 44, 1625–1639.
(2010). Gender, values, and occupational in- Friendships in middle school: Influences on Whitehead, K. A., Ainsworth, A. T., Wittig, M. A.,
terests among children, adolescents, and motivation and school adjustment. Journal & Gadino, B. (2009). Implications of ethnic
adults. Child Development, 81, 778–796. of Educational Psychology, 96, 195–203. identity exploration and ethnic identity
Weiss, C., & Baker-Smith, E. (2010). Eighth- Werner, N., & Nixon, C. L. (2005). Normative affirmation and belonging for intergroup at-
grade school form and resilience in the tran- beliefs about relational aggression: An inves- titudes among adolescents. Journal of Re-
sition to high school: A comparison of tigation of the cognitive bases of adolescent search on Adolescence, 19, 123–135.
middle schools and K–8 schools. Journal of aggressive behavior. Journal of Youth and Whiteman, S., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C.
Research on Adolescence, 20, 825–839. Adolescence, 34, 229–243. (2011). Family relationships from adoles-
Weiss, C., Carolan, B. V., & Baker-Smith, E. C. Werner, N., & Silbereisen, R. (2003). Family rela- cence to early adulthood: Changes in the
(2010). Big school, small school: (Re)Testing tionship quality and contact with deviant family system following firstborns’ leaving
assumptions about high school size, school peers as predictors of adolescent problem home. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21,
engagement and mathematics achievement. behaviors: The moderating role of gender. 461–474.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 163–176. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 454–480. Whitesell, N., Mitchell, C., Kaufman, C., &
Weissberg, R., Caplan, M., & Harwood, R. Wetter, E. K., & Hankin, B. L. (2009). Media- Spicer, P. (2006). Developmental trajectories
(1991). Promoting competent young people tional pathways through which positive and of personal and collective self-concept
in competence-enhancing environments: A negative emotionality contribute to anhe- among American Indian adolescents. Child
systems-based perspective on primary pre- donic symptoms of depression: A prospec- Development, 77, 1487–1503.
vention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical tive study of adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Whitlock, J. L., Powers, J. L., & Eckenrode, J.
Psychology, 59, 830–841. Child Psychology, 37, 507–520. (2006). The virtual cutting edge: The Inter-
Weisskirch, R. S. (2009). Parenting by cell phone: Whitaker, D., & Miller, K. (2000). Parent–adolescent net and adolescent self-injury. Developmen-
Parental monitoring of adolescents and fam- discussions about sex and condoms: Impact on tal Psychology, 42, 407–417.
ily relations. Journal of Youth and Adoles- peer influences of sexual risk behavior. Journal Wichstrøm, L. (1999). The emergence of gender
cence, 38, 1123–1139. of Adolescent Research, 15, 251–273. difference in depressed mood during adoles-
Weist, M., Freedman, A., Paskewitz, D., Proescher, Whitbeck, L., Conger, R., Simons, R., & Kao, M. cence: The role of intensified gender socializa-
E., & Flaherty, L. (1995). Urban youth under (1993). Minor deviant behaviors and adoles- tion. Developmental Psychology, 35, 232–245.
stress: Empirical identification of protective cent sexuality. Youth and Society, 25, 24–37. Wichstrøm, L. (2001). The impact of pubertal
factors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24, Whitbeck, L., Hoyt, D., & Ackley, K. (1997). Abu- timing on adolescents’ alcohol use. Journal of
705–729. sive family backgrounds and later victimiza- Research on Adolescence, 11, 131–150.
Weisz, J. R., Francis, S. E., & Bearman, S. K. tion among runaway and homeless adolescents. Wickrama, K. A. T., Wickrama, K. A. S., & Bryant,
(2010). Assessing secondary control and its Journal of Research on Adolescence, 7, 375–392. C. M. (2006). Community influence on ado-
association with youth depression symp- Whitbeck, L., Hoyt, D., & Bao, W. (2000). De- lescent obesity: Race/ethnic differences. Jour-
toms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, pressive symptoms and co-occurring depres- nal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 647–657.
38, 883–893. sive symptoms, substance abuse, and conduct Widman, L., Welsh, D. P., McNulty, J. K., & Little,
Weisz, J. R., McCarty, C. A., & Valeri, S. M. problems among runaway and homeless K. C. (2006). Sexual communication and con-
(2006). Effects of psychotherapy for depres- adolescents. Child Development, 71, 721–732. traceptive use in adolescent dating couples.
sion in children and adolescents: A meta- Whitbeck, L., Hoyt, D., Miller, M., & Kao, M. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 893–899.
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 132–149. (1992). Parental support, depressed affect, Wiesner, M., Capaldi, D. M., & Kim, H. K.
Weisz, J. R., Southam-Gerow, M. A., Gordis, E. B., and sexual experience among adolescents. (2012). General versus specific predictors of
Connor-Smith, J. K., Chu, B. C., Langer, D. Youth and Society, 24, 166–177. male arrest trajectories: A test of the Moffitt
A., . . . Weiss, B. (2009). Cognitive-behavioral Whitbeck, L., Simons, R., & Kao, M. (1994). The and Patterson theories. Journal of Youth and
therapy versus usual clinical care for youth effects of divorced mothers’ dating behaviors Adolescence, 41, 217–228.
depression: An initial test of transportability and sexual attitudes on the sexual attitudes and Wiesner, M., & Kim, H. K. (2006). Co-occurring
to community clinics and clinicians. Journal behaviors of their adolescent children. Journal delinquency and depressive symptoms of
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, of Marriage and the Family, 56, 615–621. adolescent boys and girls: A dual trajectory
383–396. Whitbeck, L., Yoder, K., Hoyt, D., & Conger, R. modeling approach. Developmental Psychol-
Wells, A., & Serna, I. (1996). The politics of cul- (1999). Early adolescent sexual activity: A ogy, 42, 1220–1235.
ture: Understanding local political resistance developmental study. Journal of Marriage Wiesner, M., Kim, H. K., & Capaldi, D. M.
to detracking in racially mixed schools. Har- and the Family, 61, 934–946. (2005). Developmental trajectories of of-
vard Educational Review, 66, 93–118. Whitbeck, L., Yu, M., Johnson, K., Hoyt, D., & fending: Validation and prediction to young
Wentzel, K. (1998). Social relationships and mo- Walls, M. (2008). Diagnostic prevalence rates adult alcohol use, drug use, and depressive
tivation in middle school: The role of parents, from early to mid-adolescence among indig- symptoms. Development and Psychopathol-
teachers, and peers. Journal of Educational enous adolescents: First results from a longi- ogy, 17, 251–270.
Psychology, 90, 202–209. tudinal study. Journal of the American Wiesner, M., & Windle, M. (2006). Young adult
Wentzel, K. (2002). Are effective teachers like Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, substance use and depression as a conse-
good parents? Teaching styles and student 890–900. quence of delinquency trajectories during
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-85 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-85

middle adolescence. Journal of Research on Williams, P., Holmbeck, G. N., & Greenley, R. N. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitive-
Adolescence, 16, 239–264. (2002). Adolescent health psychology. Jour- ness, risk taking, and violence: The young
Wigfield, A., Ho, A., & Mason-Singh, A. (2011). nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology,
Achievement motivation. In B. Brown & M. 828–842. 6, 59–73.
Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence Williams, S. K., & Kelly, F. D. (2005). Relation- Wilson, R. (February 1, 2005). The six simple
(Vol. 1, pp. 10–19). New York: Academic ships among involvement, attachment, and principles of viral marketing. Web Marketing
Press. behavioral problems in adolescence: Exam- Today. Retrieved from www.wilsonweb.com/
Wight, R. G., Sepúlveda, J. E., & Aneshensel, C. S. ining fathers’ influence. Journal of Early Ado- wmt5/viral-principles.htm
(2004). Depressive symptoms: How do ado- lescence, 25, 168–196. Windle, M., Miller-Tutzauer, C., & Barnes, G.
lescents compare with adults? Journal of Ado- Williams, S. T., Conger, K. J., & Blozis, S. A. (1991). Adolescent perceptions of help-
lescent Health, 34, 314–323. (2007). The development of interpersonal ag- seeking resources for substance abuse. Child
Wilcox, W. (1998). Conservative Protestant child gression during adolescence: The importance Development, 62, 179–189.
rearing: Authoritarian or authoritative? of parents, siblings, and family economics. Windle, M., & Wiesner, M. (2004). Trajectories
American Sociological Review, 63, 796–809. Child Development, 78, 1526–1542. of marijuana use from adolescence to young
Wilens, T. E., Martelon, M. K., Joshi, G., Bateman, Williams, T., Connolly, J., & Cribbie, R. (2008). adulthood: Predictors and outcomes. Devel-
C., Fried, R., Petty, C., & Biederman, J. (2011). Light and heavy heterosexual activities of opment and Psychopathology, 16, 1007–1027.
Does ADHD predict substance-use disor- young Canadian adolescents: Normative Winfree, J., L., Bäckström, T., & Mays, G. (1994).
ders? A 10-year follow-up study of young patterns and differential predictors. Journal Social learning theory, self-reported delin-
adults with ADHD. Journal of the American of Research on Adolescence, 18, 145–172. quency, and youth gangs: A new twist on a
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, Williams, T., & Williams, K. (2010). Self-efficacy general theory of crime and delinquency.
543–553. and performance in mathematics: Recipro- Youth and Society, 26, 147–177.
Wilkinson, D., & Carr, P. J. (2008). Violent cal determinism in 33 nations. Journal of Wingo, P. A., Smith, R. A., Tevendale, H. D., &
youths’ responses to high levels of exposure Educational Psychology, 102, 453–466. Ferré, C. (2011). Recent changes in the trends
to community violence: What violent events Williams, W., Blythe, T., White, N., Li, J., Gardner, of teen birth rates, 1981–2006. Journal of
reveal about youth violence. Journal of Com- H., & Sternberg, R. J. (2002). Practical intel- Adolescent Health, 48, 281–288.
munity Psychology, 36, 1026–1051. ligence for school: Developing metacogni- Wiseman, R. (2003). Queen bees and wannabes.
Wilkinson, D., & Fagan, J. (1996). The role of tive sources of achievement in adolescence. New York: Three Rivers Press.
firearms in violence “scripts”: The dynamics Developmental Review, 22, 162–210.
Wissink, I. B., Deković, M., & Meijer, A. M.
of gun events among adolescent males. Law Williford, A. P., Brisson, D., Bender, K. A., Jenson,
(2009). Adolescent friendship relations and
and Contemporary Problems, 59, 55–89. J. M., & Forrest-Bank, S. (2011). Patterns of
developmental outcomes: Ethnic and gender
Wilkinson, R. B. (2004). The role of parental and aggressive behavior and peer victimization
differences. Journal of Early Adolescence, 29,
peer attachment in the psychological health from childhood to early adolescence: A latent
405–425.
and self-esteem of adolescents. Journal of class analysis. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
cence, 40, 644–655. Witkow, M. (2009). Academic achievement and
Youth and Adolescence, 33, 479–493.
adolescents’ daily time use in the social and
Wilkinson-Lee, A. M., Zhang, Q., Nuno, V. L., & Willoughby, T., Chalmers, H., & Busseri, M. A.
academic domains. Journal of Research on
Wilhelm, M. S. (2011). Adolescent emotional (2004). Where is the syndrome? Examining
Adolescence, 19, 151–172.
distress: The role of family obligations and co-occurrence among multiple problem be-
haviors in adolescence. Journal of Consulting Witkow, M., & Fuligni, A. (2010). In-school ver-
school connectedness. Journal of Youth and
and Clinical Psychology, 72, 1022–1037. sus out-of-school friendships and academic
Adolescence, 40, 221–230.
achievement among an ethnically diverse
William T. Grant Foundation Commission on Willoughby, T., & Hamza, C. A. (2011). A longitu-
sample of adolescents. Journal of Research on
Work, Family, and Citizenship. (1988). The dinal examination of the bidirectional associ-
Adolescence, 20, 631–650.
forgotten half: Non-college youth in America. ations among perceived parenting behaviors,
Washington, DC: Author. adolescent disclosure and problem behavior Witvliet, M., Brendgen, M., van Lier, P., Koot, H.,
across the high school years. Journal of Youth & Vitaro, F. (2010). Early adolescent depressive
Williams, A., & Merten, M. (2009). Adolescents’
and Adolescence, 40, 463–478. symptoms: Prediction from clique isolation,
online social networking following the death
loneliness, and perceived social acceptance.
of a peer. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24, Wills, T., McNamara, G., Vaccaro, D., & Hirky, A.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38,
67–90. (1996). Escalated substance use: A longitudi-
1045–1056.
Williams, K., & Guerra, N. (2007). Prevalence nal grouping analysis from early to middle
and predictors of Internet bullying. Journal adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Wodtke, G., Harding, D., & Elwert, F. (2011).
of Adolescent Health, 31, S14–S21. 105, 166–180. Neighborhood effects in temporal perspective:
Wills, T., Sandy, J. M., Yaeger, A., & Shinar, O. The impact of long-term exposure to concen-
Williams, K., Yeager, D., Cheung, C., & Choi, W.
(2001). Family risk factors and adolescent trated disadvantage on High School gradua-
(2012). Cyberball (version 4.0) [Software].
substance use: Moderation effects for tem- tion. American Sociological Review, 76, 713–736.
Available from https://cyberball.wikispaces
.com. perament dimensions. Developmental Psy- Wolak, J., Mitchell, K. J., & Finkelhor, D. (2003).
Williams, L. R., Degnan, K., Perez-Edgar, K., chology, 37, 283–297. Escaping or connecting? Characteristics of
Henderson, H., Rubin, K., Pine, D. S., . . . Fox, N. Wilson, H. W., & Widom, C. S. (2010). The role youth who form close online relationships.
(2009). Impact of behavioral inhibition and of youth problem behaviors in the path from Journal of Adolescence, 26, 105–119.
parenting style on internalizing and exter- child abuse and neglect to prostitution: A Wolf, A., Gortmaker, S., Cheung, L., Gray, H.,
nalizing problems from early childhood prospective examination. Journal of Research Herzog, D., & Colditz, G. (1993). Activity, in-
through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal on Adolescence, 20, 210–236. activity, and obesity: Racial, ethnic, and age
Child Psychology, 37, 1063–1075. Wilson, J. L., Peebles, R., Hardy, K. K., & Litt, I. F. differences among schoolgirls. American
Williams, L. R., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Recipro- (2006). Surfing for thinness: A pilot study of Journal of Public Health, 83, 1625–1627.
cal relations between parenting and adjust- pro-eating disorder web site usage in adoles- Wolfe, S., & Truxillo, C. (1996, March). The rela-
ment in a sample of juvenile offenders. Child cents with eating disorders. Pediatrics, 118, tionship between decisional control, responsi-
Development, 82, 633–645. 1635–1643. bility, and positive and negative outcomes
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-86 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-86 References

during early adolescence. Paper presented at Wray-Lake, L., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. students: A national cross-sectional survey.
the biennial meetings of the Society for Re- (2010). Developmental patterns in decision- Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 592–599.
search on Adolescence, Boston. making autonomy across middle childhood Xu, J. (2004). Family help and homework manage-
Wolff, J. M., & Crockett, L. J. (2011). The role of and adolescence: European American par- ment in urban and rural secondary schools.
deliberative decision making, parenting, and ents’ perspectives. Child Development, 81, Teachers College Review, 106, 1786–1803.
friends in adolescent risk behaviors. Journal 636–651.
Xue, Y., Zimmerman, M., & Cunningham, R. M.
of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1607–1622. Wray-Lake, L., Syvertsen, A., Briddell, L., Osgood, (2009). Relationship between alcohol use
Wolfson, A., & Carskadon, M. (1998). Sleep D. W., & Flanagan, C. (2009). Exploring the and violent behavior among urban African
schedules and daytime functioning in ado- changing meaning of work for American high American youths from adolescence to emerg-
lescents. Child Development, 69, 875–887. school seniors from 1976 to 2005. Unpublished ing adulthood: A longitudinal study. Ameri-
paper, Pennsylvania State University, Univer- can Journal of Public Health, 99, 2041–2048.
Wong, C., Crosnoe, R., Laird, J., & Dornbusch, S.
sity Park, PA.
(2003). Relations with parents and teachers, Yamaguchi, R., Johnston, L., & O’Malley, P.
susceptibility to friends’ negative influences, Wray-Lake, L., Syvertsen, A., & Briddell, L, (2003). Relationship between student illicit
and adolescent deviance. Unpublished manu- Osgood, D. W., & Flanagan, C. (2011). Ex- drug use and school drug-testing policies.
script, Department of Sociology, University ploring the changing meaning of work for Journal of School Health, 73, 159–164.
of Texas at Austin. American high school seniors from 1976 to
Yang, Z., & Gaydos, L. M. (2010). Reasons for and
2005. Youth & Society, 43, 1110–1135.
Wong, M., Nigg, J., Zucker, R., Puttler, L., Fitzgerald, challenges of recent increases in teen birth
H., Jester, J., . . . Adams, K. (2006). Behavioral Wright, B. R. E., & Younts, C. W. (2009). Recon- rates: A study of family planning service poli-
control and resiliency in the onset of alcohol sidering the relationship between race and cies and demographic changes at the state
and illicit drug use: A prospective study from crime: Positive and negative predictors of level. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 517–524.
preschool to adolescence. Child Development, crime among African American youth. Jour-
Yap, M., Schwartz, O., Byrne, M., Simmons, J., &
77, 1016–1033. nal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 46,
Allen, N. (2010). Maternal positive and nega-
327–352.
Wood, D., Kurtz-Costes, B., Rowley, S., & Okeke- tive interaction behaviors and early adoles-
Adeyanju, N. (2010). Mothers’ academic gen- Wright, J., Cullen, F., & Williams, N. (1997). cents’ depressive symptoms: Adolescent
der stereotypes and education-related beliefs Working while in school and delinquent in- emotion regulation as a mediator. Journal of
about sons and daughters in African Ameri- volvement: Implications for social policy. Research on Adolescence, 20, 1014–1043.
can families. Journal of Educational Psychol- Crime and Delinquency, 43, 203–221.
Yasui, M., Dorham, C. L., & Dishion, T. (2004).
ogy, 102, 521–530. Wu, C., & Chao, R. K. (2011). Intergenerational Ethnic identity and psychological adjust-
Wood, D., Larson, R. W., & Brown, J. R. (2009). cultural dissonance in parent–adolescent re- ment: A validity analysis for European Amer-
How adolescents come to see themselves as lationships among Chinese and European ican and African American adolescents.
more responsible through participation in Americans. Developmental Psychology, 47, Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 807–825.
youth programs. Child Development, 80, 493–508. Yates, A., Edman, J., & Aruguete, M. (2004).
295–309. Wu, L., & Anthony, J. (1999). Tobacco smoking Ethnic differences in BMI and body/self-
Wood, P., & Clay, W. (1996). Perceived structural and depressed mood in late childhood and dissatisfaction among Whites, Asian sub-
barriers and academic performance among early adolescence. American Journal of Public groups, Pacifi c Islanders, and African
American Indian high school students. Youth Health, 89, 1837–1840. Americans. Journal of Adolescent Health, 34,
and Society, 28, 40–61. Wu, L., Pilowsky, D. J., & Schlenger, W. E. (2004). 300–307.
Woods, L. N., & Jagers, R. J. (2003). Are cultural Inhalant abuse and dependence among ado- Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1996). Community service
values predictors of moral reasoning in Afri- lescents in the United States. Journal of the and political–moral identity in adolescents.
can American adolescents? Journal of Black American Academy of Child and Adolescent Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 271–284.
Psychology, 29, 102–118. Psychiatry, 43, 1206–1214. Yau, J., & Smetana, J. (1996). Adolescent–parent
Woodward, L., & Fergusson, D. (1999). Child- Wu, L., Ringwalt, C. L., Mannelli, P., & Patkar, conflict among Chinese adolescents in Hong
hood peer relationship problems and psy- A. A. (2008). Prescription pain reliever abuse Kong. Child Development, 67, 1262–1275.
chosocial adjustment in late adolescence. and dependence among adolescents: A na- Yau, J., & Smetana, J. (2003). Adolescent–parent
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, tionally representative study. Journal of the conflict in Hong Kong and Shenzhen: A
87–104. American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psy- comparison of youth in two cultural con-
Woodward, L., Fergusson, D., & Belsky, J. (2000). chiatry, 47, 1020–1029. texts. International Journal of Behavioral De-
Timing of parental separation and attach- Wu, L., Schlenger, W. E., & Galvin, D. M. (2003). velopment, 27, 201–211.
ment to parents in adolescence: Results of a The relationship between employment and Yau, J., Tasopoulos-Chan, M., & Smetana, J. G.
prospective study from birth to age 16. Jour- substance use among students aged 12 to 17. (2009). Disclosure to parents about everyday
nal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 162–174. Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, 5–15. activities among American adolescents from
Woolard, J., & Scott, E. (2009). The legal regulation Wu, L., & Thomson, E. (2001). Race differences Mexican, Chinese, and European back-
of adolescence. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg in family experience and early sexual initia- grounds. Child Development, 80, 1481–1498.
(Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd tion: Dynamic models of family structure Ybarra, M. L., Alexander, C., & Mitchell, K. J.
ed., Vol. 2, pp. 345–371). New York: Wiley. and family change. Journal of Marriage and (2005). Depressive symptomatology, youth
Woolley, M. E., Kol, K. L., & Bowen, G. L. (2009). the Family, 63, 682–696. Internet use, and online interactions:
The social context of school success for Latino Xia, G., & Qian, M. (2001). The relationship of A national survey. Journal of Adolescent
middle school students: Direct and indirect parenting style to self-reported mental Health, 36, 9–18.
influences of teachers, family, and friends. health among two subcultures of Chinese. Yeh, H., & Lempers, J. D. (2004). Perceived
Journal of Early Adolescence, 29, 43–70. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 251–260. sibling relationships and adolescent develop-
Worthman, C. (2011). Hormones and behavior. Xing, X. Y., Tao, F. B., Wan, Y. H., Xing, C., Qi, ment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33,
In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclo- X. Y., Hao, J. H., . . . Huang, L. (2010). 133–147.
pedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 117–186). Family factors associated with suicide Yeung, R., & Leadbeater, B. (2010). Adults make
New York: Academic Press. attempts among Chinese adolescent a difference: The protective effects of parent
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-87 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e References R-87

and teacher emotional support on emotional Young, S. E., Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Willcutt, Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 221–
and behavioral problems of peer-victimized E. G., Corley, R. P., Haberstick, B. C., & 231). New York: Academic Press.
adolescents. Journal of Community Psychol- Hewitt, J. (2009). Behavioral disinhibition: Zavodny, M. (2004). Fertility and parental consent
ogy, 38, 80–98. Liability for externalizing spectrum disorders for minors to receive contraceptives. American
Yip, T., Seaton, E., & Sellers, R. (2006). African and its genetic and environmental relation to Journal of Public Health, 94, 1347–1351.
American racial identity across the lifespan: response inhibition across adolescence. Jour- Zeldin, S., & Topitzes, D. (2002). Neighborhood
Identity status, identity content, and depres- nal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 117–130. experiences, community connection, and
sive symptoms. Child Development, 77, Youngstrom, E., Weist, M. D., & Albus, K. E. positive beliefs about adolescents among ur-
1504–1517. (2003). Exploring violence exposure, stress, ban adults and youth. Journal of Community
Yip, T., Seaton, E., & Sellers, R. (2010). Interracial protective factors and behavioral problems Psychology, 30, 647–669.
and intraracial contact, school-level diver- among inner-city youth. American Journal of Zeman, J., & Shipman, K. (1997). Social-contextual
sity, and change in racial identity status Community Psychology, 32, 115–129. influences on expectancies for managing anger
among African American adolescents. Child Youniss, J., & Smollar, J. (1985). Adolescent and sadness: The transition from middle child-
Development, 81, 1431–1444. relations with mothers, fathers, and friends. hood to adolescence. Developmental Psychol-
YMCA. (2000). Telephone survey conducted for Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ogy, 33, 917–924.
the White House Conference on Teenagers. Yu, J. J. (2011). Reciprocal associations between Zentner, M., & Renaud, O. (2007). Origins of
Chicago: Author. connectedness and autonomy among Ko- adolescents’ ideal self: An intergenerational
YMCA. (2006). Information on the history of the rean adolescents: Compatible or antithetical? perspective. Journal of Personality and Social
organization is available at www.ymca.net. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73, 692–703. Psychology, 92, 557–574.
Yohalem, N., & Wilson-Ahlstrom, A. (2010). In- Yuan, A. S. V., & Hamilton, H. A. (2006). Stepfa- Zhang, J., Seo, D., Kolbe, L., Lee, A., Middlestadt,
side the black box: Assessing and improving ther involvement and adolescent well-being: S., Zhao, W., & Huang, S. (2011a). Compari-
quality in youth programs. American Journal Do mothers and nonresidential fathers mat- son of overweight, weight perception, and
of Community Psychology, 45, 350–357. ter? Journal of Family Issues, 27, 1191–1213. weight-related practices among high school
Yoon, J. S., Barton, E., & Taiarol, J. (2004). Rela- Zabin, L., Astone, N., & Emerson, M. (1993). Do students in three large Chinese cities and two
tional aggression in middle school: Educa- adolescents want babies? The relationship large US cities. Journal of Adolescent Health,
tional implications of developmental between attitudes and behavior. Journal of 48, 366–372.
research. Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, Research on Adolescence, 3, 67–86. Zhang, W., & Fuligni, A. (2006). Authority, au-
303–318. Zabin, L., Hirsch, M., & Emerson, M. (1989). tonomy, and family relationships among ad-
Yoon, K., Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., & Barber, B. When urban adolescents choose abortion: olescents in urban and rural China. Journal
(1996, March). Developmental trajectories of Effects on education, psychological status, of Research on Adolescence, 16, 527–537.
early to middle adolescents’ academic achieve- and subsequent pregnancy. Family Planning Zhang, Y., Haddad, E., Torres, B., & Chen, C.
ment and motivation. Paper presented at the Perspectives, 21, 248–255. (2011b). The reciprocal relationships
biennial meetings of the Society for Research Zaff, J. F., Moore, K. A., Papillo, A. R., & Williams, among parents’ expectations, adolescents’
on Adolescence, Boston. S. (2003). Implications of extracurricular ac- expectations, and adolescents’ achievement:
Yorgason, J. B., Padilla-Walker, L., & Jackson, J. tivity participation during adolescence on A two-wave longitudinal analysis of the
(2011). Nonresidential grandparents’ emo- positive outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Re- NELS data. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
tional and financial involvement in relation search, 18, 599–630. cence, 40, 479–489.
to early adolescent grandchild outcomes. Zaider, T., Johnson, J. G., & Cockell, S. J. (2002). Zhao, Y., Montoro, R., Igartua, K., & Thombs,
Journal of Research on Adolescence , 21, Psychiatric disorders associated with the B. D. (2010). Suicidal ideation and attempt
552–558. onset and persistence of bulimia nervosa among adolescents reporting “unsure” sex-
Yoshikawa, H. (1994). Prevention as cumulative and binge eating disorder during adoles- ual identity or heterosexual identity plus
protection: Effects of early family support cence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, same-sex attraction or behavior: Forgotten
and education on chronic delinquency and 319–329. groups? Journal of the American Academy of
its risks. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 28–54. Zametkin, A. J., Alter, M. R., & Yemini, T. (2001). Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 104–113.
Young, A. M., & D’Arcy, H. (2005). Older boy- Suicide in teenagers. Journal of the American Zhong, H., & Schwartz, J. (2010). Exploring gen-
friends of adolescent girls: The cause or a Medical Association, 286, 3120–3125. der-specific trends in underage drinking
sign of the problem? Journal of Adolescent Zametkin, A. J., Zoon, C. K., Klein, H. W., & across adolescent age groups and measures
Health, 36, 410–419. Munson, S. (2004). Psychiatric aspects of of drinking: Is girls’ drinking catching up
child and adolescent obesity: A review of the with boys’? Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Young, A. M., Grey, M., & Boyd, C. J. (2009).
past 10 years. Journal of the American Acad- 39, 911–926.
Adolescents’ experiences of sexual assault
by peers: Prevalence and nature of victim- emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, Zick, C. (2010). The shifting balance of adoles-
ization occurring within and outside of 134–150. cent time use. Youth & Society, 41, 569–596.
school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, Zapert, K., Snow, D. L., & Tebes, J. K. (2002). Pat- Zickler, P. (2004). Early nicotine initiation in-
1072–1083. terns of substance use in early through late creases severity of addiction, vulnerability to
Young, J. F., Berenson, K., Cohen, P., & Garcia, J. adolescence. American Journal of Community some effects of cocaine. NIDA Notes, 19, 2.
(2005). The role of parent and peer support Psychology, 30, 835–852. Zillman, D. (2000). Influence of unrestrained ac-
in predicting adolescent depression: A longi- Zarrett, N., Fay, K., Li, Y., Carrano, J., Phelps, E., cess to erotica on adolescents’ and young
tudinal community study. Journal of Re- & Lerner, R. (2009). More than child’s play: adults’ dispositions toward sexuality. Journal
search on Adolescence 15, 407–423. Variable- and pattern-centered approaches of Adolescent Health, 27, 41–44.
Young, M. E. D., Deardorff, J., Ozer, E., & Lahiff, for examining effects of sports participation Zima, B., Wells, K., & Freeman, H. (1994). Emo-
M. (2011). Sexual abuse in childhood and on youth development. Developmental tional and behavioral problems and severe
adolescence and the risk of early pregnancy Psychology, 45, 368–382. academic delays among sheltered homeless
among women ages 18–22. Journal of Adoles- Zarrett, N., & Mahoney, J. (2011). Out-of-school children in Los Angeles County. American
cent Health, 49, 287–293. activities. In B. Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Journal of Public Health, 84, 260–264.
ste32118_ref_R1-R88.indd Page R-88 02/04/13 6:37 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

R-88 References

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. (1999). Stability, change Associations with psychosocial functioning during adolescence: The infl uence of
and individual differences in involvement from early to middle adolescence. Journal of ego-identity status. Journal of Adolescence,
with friends and romantic partners among Adolescence, 24, 313–336. 25, 107–124.
adolescent females. Journal of Youth and Zimmer-Gembeck, M., Siebenbruner, J., & Collins, Zimmerman, R., Sprecher, S., Langer, L., &
Adolescence, 28, 419–438. W. A. (2004). A prospective study of intraindi- Holloway, C. (1995). Adolescents’ perceived
Zimmer-Gembeck, M., Ducat, W., & Boislard- vidual and peer influences on adolescents’ ability to say “No” to unwanted sex. Journal
Pepin, M.-A. (2011). A prospective study of heterosexual romantic and sexual behavior. of Adolescent Research, 10, 383–399.
young females’ sexual subjectivity: Associa- Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 381–394. Zimring, F. (1982). The changing legal world of
tions with age, sexual behavior, and dating. Zimmerman, G., & Messner, S. (2010). Neighbor- adolescence. New York: Free Press.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 927–938. hood context and the gender gap in adolescent Zimring, F. (1998). American youth violence.
Zimmer-Gembeck, M., Ducat, W., & Collins, W. A. violent crime. American Sociological Review, New York: Oxford University Press.
(2011). Autonomy, development of. In B. 75, 958–980.
Brown & M. Prinstein (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Zito, J. M., Safer, D. J., DosReis, S., Gardner, J. F.,
Zimmerman, M., Bingenheimer, J. B., & Notaro,
adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 66–76). New York: Soeken, K., Boles, M., & Lynch, F. (2002).
P. C. (2002). Natural mentors and adolescent
Academic Press. Rising prevalence of antidepressants among
resiliency: A study with urban youth.
U.S. youths. Pediatrics, 109, 721–727.
Zimmer-Gembeck, M., & Helfand, M. (2008). American Journal of Community Psychology,
Ten years of longitudinal research on U.S. 30, 221–243. Zollo, P. (2004). Getting wiser to teens: More
adolescent sexual behavior: Developmental Zimmerman, M., Copeland, L., Shope, J., & insights into marketing to teenagers. Ithaca,
correlates of sexual intercourse, and the Dielman, T. (1997). A longitudinal study of NY: New Strategist Publications.
importance of age, gender, and ethnic self-esteem: Implications for adolescent Zweig, J. M., Lindberg, L., & McGinley, K.
background. Developmental Review, 28, development. Journal of Youth and Adoles- (2001). Adolescent health risk profiles: The
153–224. cence, 26, 117–141. co-occurrence of health risks among females
Zimmer-Gembeck, M., Siebenbruner, J., & Collins, Zimmermann, P., & Becker-Stoll, F. (2002). and males. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
W. A. (2001). Diverse aspects of dating: Stability of attachment representations 30, 707–728.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 2 21/03/13 9:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 3 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

INTRODUCTION

The Study of Adolescent


Development

A MULTIDISCIPLINARY APPROACH TO ADOLESCENCE


THE BOUNDARIES OF ADOLESCENCE
Early, Middle, and Late Adolescence
A FRAMEWORK FOR STUDYING ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence
The Contexts of Adolescence
Psychosocial Development of Adolescence
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON ADOLESCENCE
Biosocial Theories
Organismic Theories
Learning Theories
Sociological Theories
Historical and Anthropological Theories
STEREOTYPES VERSUS SCIENTIFIC STUDY

3
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 4 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

4 Introduction

THE ODDEST QUESTION I HAVE EVER BEEN ASKED and viewing his responses during the interrogation as
during the 35 years that I have been studying adoles- no different from those an adult would have provided.
cent development is this: “Do you believe that some- The Omar Khadr case is certainly the most unusual
one would need to be capable of formal operational one in which I have been involved, but the central issue
thinking in order to build an IED?” As you will learn in for discussion is one that my collaborators and I have
Chapter 2 of this book, formal operational thinking is, grappled with for the past 15 years: Where, on the basis
according to one theory of adolescence, the highest of scientific study, should we draw the line between
level of cognitive development, which is not attained adolescence and adulthood? This is not just an abstract,
until early adolescence (Kuhn, 2009). And, as you prob- academic exercise. How we answer this question has far-
ably know from the news, an IED is an improvised ex- reaching ramifications for society and, of course, for
plosive device, a homemade bomb often planted along teenagers. How old does someone have to be in order to
the roadside in an effort to attack the enemy. be tried and sentenced as an adult for a crime he has
This unusual question was posed to me during a pretrial committed? At what age should a pregnant adolescent
investigation held at Guantánamo Bay, where I was serving be able to obtain an abortion without parental permis-
as an expert witness in a case involving a detainee who sion? How old should individuals have to be to obtain
had been accused of building and setting IEDs in eastern contraception, see a psychologist, or have cosmetic sur-
Afghanistan, as an assistant to Al Qaeda and Taliban gery without their parents knowing? Have we picked the
operatives, and throwing a hand grenade that killed right ages in deciding where to draw the line as to who
an American soldier. Omar Khadr, the detainee, was 15 can drive, see R-rated movies, or buy cigarettes? And how
when he was captured by American soldiers (Glaberson, should we respond to young offenders? “Do the adult
2007). Omar had been interrogated for many months, first crime, do the adult time” may sound fair from the per-
in Afghanistan and later at Guantánamo Bay, where he was spective of crime victims, but does it make sense in light
still being held, nearly seven years after his capture. of what we know about adolescent development? While
Omar’s defense team, which had retained me, was I was working on this edition of Adolescence, the U.S.
planning to argue in court that a 15-year-old should not Supreme Court ruled that laws mandating that juveniles
have been interrogated using the same methods used convicted of homicide be sentenced to life in prison vio-
for adults and should not have been held to the same lated the Constitution’s protection against “cruel and un-
level of criminal responsibility that applied to someone usual punishment,” because such laws did not allow
who was older. The prosecutor questioning me was judges and juries to take into account research showing
looking for evidence that Omar, by virtue of his bomb- that young adolescents are still relatively immature, and
building ability, demonstrated adultlike cognitive matu- less responsible for their behavior than adults. A lot of
rity, which argued in favor of treating him as an adult that research (and much more) is covered in this book.

MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION


As you will read in Chapter 2, recent studies of
A Multidisciplinary
adolescent brain development have revealed that the Approach to Adolescence
brain continues to mature well into the mid-20s. This
What is the nature of adolescents’ identity development
research was used in several recent U.S. Supreme Court
in a changing world? How should society deal with
cases, where it was ruled that adolescents should not
problems of youth unemployment, underage drinking,
be as punished as severely as adults, even when they
teenage pregnancy, and juvenile crime? What is the best
have been convicted of the same crimes. Some advo-
way to prepare young people for the work, family, and
cates for youth have worried that this same research can
citizenship roles of adulthood?
be used to limit what teen-
Answering these questions requires a thorough un-
agers are allowed to do,
derstanding of adolescents’ psychological development;
such as drive. How would
and in this book, we will examine how—and why—
you respond to someone
people’s hopes and plans, fears and anxieties, and ques-
who, on the basis of this
tions and concerns change as they grow into adulthood.
research, believes we should
Answering these difficult questions requires more than
set the driving age at 18?
an understanding of the ways in which individuals
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 5 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 5

One significant aspect of transition from childhood into adolescence is the young person’s entry into the labor force.

change psychologically as they move through adoles- childhood into the maturity of adulthood, of prepara-
cence, though. It also requires knowledge of how they tion for the future (Larson, Wilson, & Rickman, 2009;
develop physically, how their brain matures, how their Schlegel, 2009). Adolescence is a period of transitions:
relationships with others change, how as a group they biological, psychological, social, economic. During ad-
are viewed and treated by society, how adolescence in olescence, individuals become interested in sex and
our society differs from adolescence in other cultures, biologically capable of having children. They become
and how the nature of adolescence itself has changed wiser, more sophisticated, and better able to make their
over the years. In other words, a complete understand- own decisions. They become more self-aware, more in-
ing of adolescence in contemporary society depends on dependent, and more concerned about what the future
being familiar with biological, social, sociological, cul- holds. They are permitted to work, to get married, to
tural, and historical perspectives on adolescence (Dahl & drive, and to vote. Think for a moment about how
Hariri, 2005). much you changed between when you finished elemen-
In this book, we will look at adolescence from a tary school and when you graduated from high school.
multidisciplinary perspective—a perspective that draws I’m sure you’ll agree that the changes you went through
on a variety of disciplines. Each provides a view of ado- were remarkable.
lescence that, in its own way, helps further our under- For the purposes of this book, adolescence is de-
standing of this period of the life cycle. We will look at fined, roughly speaking, as the second decade of the life
contributions to the study of adolescence made by biol- span. Although at one time “adolescence” may have
ogists, psychologists, educators, sociologists, historians, been synonymous with the teenage years (from 13 to
and anthropologists. The challenge here is not to deter- 19), the adolescent period lengthened considerably in
mine which perspective on adolescence is best, but to the 20th century, both because physical maturation oc-
integrate contributions from different disciplines into a curs earlier (you’ll read about this in the next chapter)
coherent and comprehensive viewpoint on the nature of and because so many individuals delay entering into
adolescent development in contemporary society. work and marriage until their mid-20s (Settersten,
Furstenberg, & Rumbaut, 2005). It therefore makes
more sense to think of adolescence as beginning around
The Boundaries age 10 and ending in the early 20s.
As you can see in Table I.1, there are a variety of
of Adolescence boundaries we might draw between childhood and ado-
The word adolescence is derived from the Latin adoles- lescence, and between adolescence and adulthood.
cere, which means “to grow into adulthood” (R. Lerner & Whereas a biologist would place a great deal of emphasis
Steinberg, 2009). In all societies, adolescence is a time
of growing up, of moving from the immaturity of adolescence The second decade of life.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 6 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

6 Introduction

TABLE I.1 The boundaries of adolescence. One problem that students of adolescence encounter early is a fundamental
one: deciding when adolescence begins and ends, or what the boundaries of the period are. Different theorists have
proposed various markers, but there is little agreement on the issue. Here are some examples of the ways in which
adolescence has been distinguished from childhood and adulthood that we examine in this book. Which boundaries
make the most sense to you?

Perspective When Adolescence Begins When Adolescence Ends

Biological Onset of puberty Becoming capable of sexual reproduction


Emotional Beginning of detachment from parents Attainment of separate sense of identity
Cognitive Emergence of more advanced reasoning Consolidation of advanced reasoning
abilities abilities
Interpersonal Beginning of shift in interest from parental to Development of capacity for intimacy
peer relations with peers
Social Beginning of training for adult work, family, Full attainment of adult status and
and citizen roles privileges
Educational Entrance into junior high school Completion of formal schooling
Legal Attainment of juvenile status Attainment of majority status
Chronological Attainment of designated age of adolescence Attainment of designated age of
(e.g., 10 years) adulthood (e.g., 21 years)
Cultural Entrance into period of training for ceremonial Completion of ceremonial rite of
rite of passage passage

on the attainment and completion of puberty, an attor- different endings for every individual. An individual
ney would look instead at important age breaks desig- can be a child in some ways, an adolescent in other
nated by law, and an educator might draw attention to ways, and an adult in still others.
differences between students enrolled in different grades
in school. Is a biologically mature fifth-grader an adoles- EARLY, MIDDLE, AND LATE ADOLESCENCE
cent or a child? Is a 20-year-old college student who lives
at home an adolescent or an adult? There are no right or Although adolescence may span a 10-year period, be-
wrong answers to these questions. It all depends on the cause so much psychological and social growth takes
boundaries we use to define the period. Determining place during this decade, most social scientists and
the beginning and ending of adolescence is a matter practitioners argue that it makes more sense to view the
more of opinion than of absolute fact. adolescent years as composed of a series of stages than
Rather than argue about which boundaries are the as one single stage (Samela-Aro, 2011). The 11-year-old
correct ones, it makes more sense to think of devel- whose time and energy is wrapped up in hip-hop, Face-
opment during adolescence as involving a series of book, and baseball, for example, has little in common
transitions from immaturity into maturity (Howard & with the 21-year-old who is involved in a serious ro-
Galambos, 2011; Settersten et al., 2005; Trejos-Castillo & mance, worried about pressures at work, and looking
Vazsonyi, 2011). Some of these passages are long and for an affordable apartment.
some are short; some are smooth and others are Social scientists who study adolescence usually dif-
rough. And not all of them occur at the same time. ferentiate among early adolescence (about ages 10–13),
Consequently, it is quite possible—and perhaps even middle adolescence (about ages 14–17), and late
likely—that an individual will mature in some re- adolescence (about ages 18–21). These divisions, as
spects before he or she matures in others. The various you may have noticed, roughly correspond to the way
aspects of adolescence have different beginnings and our society groups young people in educational institu-
tions; they are the approximate ages that customarily
mark attendance at middle or junior high school, high
early adolescence The period spanning roughly ages 10–13, school, and college. In discussing development during
corresponding roughly to the junior high or middle school years. adolescence, then, we will need to be sensitive not only
to differences between adolescence and childhood, or
middle adolescence The period spanning roughly ages 14–17,
corresponding to the high school years. between adolescence and adulthood, but also to differ-
ences among the various phases of adolescence itself.
late adolescence The period spanning roughly ages 18–21, Some writers also have suggested that a new phase of
corresponding approximately to the college years. life, called emerging adulthood (Arnett, 2004) or
emerging adulthood The period spanning roughly ages 18–25, during “youthhood” (Côté, 2000), now characterizes the early
which individuals make the transition from adolescence to adulthood.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 7 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 7

and mid-20s. However, despite the popularity of this John Hill (1983). The framework is organized around
idea in the mass media, there is little evidence that this three basic components: (1) the fundamental changes of
is a universal stage of life or that the majority of young adolescence, (2) the contexts of adolescence, and (3) the
people in their mid-20s are in some sort of psychologi- psychosocial developments of adolescence.
cal or social limbo (Côté & Bynner, 2008; M. J. Shanahan,
Porfeli, Mortimer, & Erikson, 2005). Indeed, what is THE FUNDAMENTAL CHANGES
most striking about the transition from adolescence to OF ADOLESCENCE
adulthood today is just how many different pathways
there are. Some individuals—those we might label What, if anything, is distinctive about adolescence as a
“emerging adults”—spend their 20s single, dependent period in the life cycle? According to Hill, three fea-
on their parents, and bouncing from job to job, while tures of adolescent development give the period its
others leave adolescence and go straight into marriage, special flavor and significance: (1) the onset of puberty,
full-time employment, and economic independence (2) the emergence of more advanced thinking abilities,
(Osgood, Ruth, Eccles, Jacobs, & Barber, 2005). As you’ll and (3) the transition into new roles in society. We re-
see in Chapter 3, the pathways through adolescence fer to these three sets of changes—biological, cognitive,
itself are equally varied. and social—as the fundamental changes of adolescence.
And they are changes that occur universally; virtually
without exception, all adolescents in every society go
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION through them.
Thinking about your own life, when would you say
❚ Biological Transitions The chief elements of the
that you became an adolescent? Would you say that
biological changes of adolescence—which collectively
you are still an adolescent now, or have
are referred to as puberty—involve changes in the
you become an adult? Or would you say
young person’s physical appearance (including breast
that you are an adolescent in some ways
development in girls, the growth of facial hair in boys,
and an adult in others?
and a dramatic increase in height for both sexes) and
the development of the ability to conceive children
(Bogin, 2011).
RECAP Chapter 1 not only describes the biological changes
that occur in early adolescence but also examines how
• The study of adolescence draws on different
puberty affects the adolescent’s psychological develop-
disciplines, including psychology, biology,
ment and social relationships. The adolescent’s self-
sociology, anthropology, and education.
image, for example, may be temporarily threatened by
• Adolescence comes from the Greek word meaning
marked changes in physical appearance. When your
“to grow into adulthood.”
body and face are changing, not surprisingly, the way
• There is no single accepted way to define the
you look at yourself changes as well. In addition, rela-
beginning or end of adolescence—it has different
tionships inside the family are transformed by adoles-
boundaries depending on how one chooses to
cents’ greater need for privacy and their interest in
define the period.
forming intimate relationships with peers. Girls may
• Most social scientists distinguish among three
suddenly feel uncomfortable about being physically af-
stages of adolescence: early (10–13), middle (14–17),
fectionate with their father, and boys with their mother.
and late (18–21).
And, of course, adolescents’ friendships are altered by
• Some writers have suggested that a new
newly emerging sexual impulses and concerns. In short,
phase of life, called “emerging adult-
puberty is not just a biological event.
hood,” now characterizes
the early and ❚ Cognitive Transitions The word cognitive is
mid-20s. used to refer to the processes that underlie how people
think about things. Memory and problem solving are
both examples of cognitive processes. Changes in think-
ing abilities, which are dealt with in Chapter 2, make up
the second of the three fundamental changes of the
A Framework for Studying adolescent period. The emergence of more sophisti-
Adolescent Development cated thinking abilities is one of the most striking
changes to take place during adolescence. Compared
In order to organize information from a variety of differ- with children, for example, adolescents are much better
ent perspectives, this book uses a framework that is based
largely on a model suggested by the late psychologist
puberty The biological changes of adolescence.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 8 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

8 Introduction

“underage,” or minors, and people who have reached the


age of majority. Not until adolescence are individuals
permitted to drive, marry, and vote. Such changes in
rights, privileges, and responsibilities—which we’ll ex-
amine in Chapter 3—constitute the third set of funda-
mental changes that occur at adolescence: social changes.
In some cultures, the social changes of adolescence
are marked by a formal ceremony—a rite of passage.
In most contemporary industrialized societies, though,
the transition is less clearly marked. But, everywhere, a
change in social status is a universal feature of adoles-
cence (Markstrom, 2011b).

MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION


In contemporary industrialized society, we do not
The implications of the cognitive changes of adolescence are have formal ceremonies that designate when a person
far-reaching. has become an “adult.” Do we have more
informal ways to let individuals know
when they have made the transition?
able to think about hypothetical situations (that is, What were the most important
things that have not yet happened but might, or things events in your life that signaled
that may not happen but could) and about abstract your entrance into adulthood?
concepts, such as friendship, democracy, or morality
(Keating, 2011). As you’ll read, groundbreaking research
on brain development is beginning to shed light on the
ways in which these and other changes in thinking dur-
ing adolescence result from the maturation of particu- As the young person’s treatment by society changes,
lar brain regions and systems (Casey, Jones, & Somerville, so do relationships around the home, at school, and in
2011; Spear, 2011a). the peer group. Changes in social status also permit
The implications of these cognitive changes are young people to enter new roles and engage in new ac-
also far-reaching. The ability to think more capably in tivities, such as marriage and work, that dramatically
hypothetical and abstract terms affects the way ado- alter their self-image and relationships with others. The
lescents think about themselves, their relationships, adolescent, on the verge of becoming an adult, has im-
and the world around them. We will see, for example, portant decisions to make and new options to choose
that teenagers’ abilities to plan ahead, argue with among. Even something as simple as being able to drive
their parents, solve algebraic equations, and resolve has implications for the adolescent’s relationships with
moral dilemmas are all linked to changes in the way parents and friends.
they think. Even the way day-to-day decisions are
made is affected. For the first time, individuals be-
come able to think in logical ways about what their RECAP
life will be like in the future, about their relationships • The three features of adolescence that make the
with friends and family, and about politics, religion, period unique are called the “fundamental changes
and philosophy. of adolescence.”
• One fundamental change is biological and involves
❚ Social Transitions All societies distinguish be- the physical changes of puberty.
tween individuals who are thought of as children and • A second fundamental change is cognitive and in-
those who are seen as ready to become adults. Our soci- volves changes in thinking abilities and in the brain
ety, for example, distinguishes between people who are processes that underlie these changes.
• The third fundamental change is social and
involves changes in the way
that society defines
rite of passage A ceremony or ritual marking an individual’s the individual.
transition from one social status to another, especially marking the
young person’s transition to adulthood.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 9 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 9

THE CONTEXTS
OF ADOLESCENCE
Although all adolescents experience the bi-
ological, cognitive, and social transitions of
the period, the effects of these changes are
not uniform for all young people. Puberty
makes some adolescents feel attractive and
self-assured, but it makes others feel ugly
and self-conscious. Being able to think in
hypothetical terms makes some teenagers
thankful that they grew up with the parents
they have, but it prompts others to run
away in search of a better life. Reaching 18
prompts some teenagers to enlist in the mil-
itary or apply for a marriage license, but for
others, becoming an adult is frightening
and unsettling.
If the fundamental changes of adoles-
cence are universal, why are their effects so
varied? Why aren’t all individuals affected in
the same ways by puberty, by cognitive
development, and by changes in social and
legal status? The answer lies in the fact that
the psychological impact of the biological, One of the most important contexts for adolescent development is the peer group.
cognitive, and social changes of adolescence
is shaped by the environment in which the
changes take place. In other words, psychological de- would begin dating soon, because in her community,
velopment during adolescence is a product of the in- the junior and senior high schools had been combined
terplay between a set of three very basic, universal into one large school, and the older boys frequently
changes and the context in which these changes are asked the younger girls out.
experienced. According to the ecological perspective Two teenage girls. Each goes through puberty, each
on human development, whose main proponent was grows intellectually, and each moves closer in age to
Urie Bronfenbrenner (1979), we cannot understand de- adulthood. Yet each grows up under very different cir-
velopment without examining the settings, or context, cumstances: in different families, in different schools,
in which it occurs. I always laugh to myself when par- with different groups of peers, and in different commu-
ents ask me how their children are going to be affected nities. Both are adolescents, but their adolescent experi-
by puberty, because the only honest answer I can give ences are markedly different. And, as a result, each girl’s
them is “It depends.” psychological development will follow a different course.
Consider, for example, two 14-year-old girls grow- Diane’s and Maria’s worlds may seem quite different
ing up in neighboring communities. When Diane went from each other. Yet the two girls share many things in
through puberty, around age 13, her parents responded common, at least in comparison to two girls growing up
by restricting her social life because they were afraid she in different parts of the world or in different historical
would become too involved with boys and neglect her eras. Imagine how different your adolescence would
schoolwork. Diane felt her parents were being unfair have been if you had grown up a century ago and, in-
and foolish. She rarely had a chance to meet any boys stead of going to high school, had been expected to work
she wanted to date, because all the older boys went to full-time from the age of 12. Imagine how different it
the high school across town. Even though she was in the might be to grow up 100 years from today. And imagine
eighth grade, she was still going to school with fifth- how different adolescence is for the teenager whose fam-
graders. ily is very poor than for one whose family is wealthy. It is
Maria’s adolescence was very different. When she impossible to generalize about the nature of adolescence
had her first period, her parents took her aside and dis- without taking into account the surroundings and
cussed sex and pregnancy with her. They explained how circumstances in which young people grow up.
different contraceptives worked and made an appoint-
ment for Maria to see a gynecologist in case she ever
ecological perspective on human development A perspective
needed to discuss something with a doctor. Although on development that emphasizes the broader context in which
she was still only 14 years old, Maria knew that she development occurs.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 10 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

10 Introduction

For this reason, the second component of our frame- which adolescents live (such as the family and the
work is the context of adolescence. In modern societies, school), which he calls micro-systems; (2) the system of
there are four main contexts in which young people relations between these immediate settings (such as the
spend time: families, peer groups, schools, and work family–peer group link or the home–school link), which
and leisure settings. But it is not enough to consider he calls the meso-system; (3) the settings that do not
these settings in isolation, because they themselves are contain the adolescent but that affect him or her indi-
located within a neighborhood or community, which rectly (such as the parent’s workplace), which he calls the
influences how they are structured and what takes place exo-system; and (4) the broader context of culture and
in them. It would be naïve, for example, to discuss the historical time (such as the country and era in which an
impact that “school” has on adolescent development adolescent lives), which he calls the macro-system.
without recognizing that a school in an affluent suburb
is likely very different from one in the inner city or in a
remote rural area. Moreover, the community in which
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
these settings are located is itself embedded in a broader
context that is shaped by culture, geography, and his- Think about the context in which you went through
torical forces (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). To the extent adolescence, and compare it to that in which your par-
that one adolescent’s world differs from another’s, the ents went through the same period of de-
two young people will have very different experiences velopment. How do these contexts differ
during the adolescent years. from each other? What difference might it
Most social scientists interested in adolescent devel- have made for psychological development?
opment study young people in one setting at a time.
Researchers traditionally have looked at family rela-
tions or at peer relations, at school or at work, at home
Although young people growing up in modern
or in the community. Increasingly, however, researchers
America share some experiences with young people all
have begun to acknowledge the linkages among the dif-
over the world, their development is different in many
ferent contexts of adolescence, and they are beginning
ways from that of young people in other societies, espe-
to ask questions about the nature of these connections.
cially those in less affluent and less industrialized na-
Instead of asking how adolescents are affected in the
tions, because their families, peer groups, schools, work
home environment, for example, studies might ask how
settings, and social institutions are different (Larson
events in the home environment affect events in the
et al., 2009). In other words, the contexts of adolescence
peer group.
are themselves shaped and defined by the larger society
This change in orientation reflects, in part, the in-
in which young people live. In this book, we’ll be espe-
creased importance of the ecological perspective on de-
cially interested in how the contexts of adolescence have
velopment. From this perspective, we focus not only on
changed in contemporary industrialized society and
the developing individual but also on the interrelations
what the implications of these changes are for adoles-
between the individual and his or her contexts, and on
cent development. Key contexts include the following:
the interconnections among the contexts themselves.
According to Bronfenbrenner (1979), the ecology of ad-
olescent development can be thought of as having four ❚ Families Adolescence is a time of dramatic
distinct levels: (1) each of the immediate settings in change in family relationships (Cox, Wang, & Gustafs-
son, 2011; Martin, Bascoe, & Davies, 2011). In addition,
frequent moves, high rates of divorce, and increasing
numbers of single-parent households have altered
micro-systems In the ecological perspective on human aspects of family life in contemporary America. In
development, the immediate settings in which adolescents develop, Chapter 4, we examine how changes in the family sys-
such as the family or the peer group. tem, and in the context of family life, affect young
people’s psychological development.
meso-system In the ecological perspective on human
development, the layer of the environment formed by two or more
immediate settings, as in the home–school linkage. ❚ Peer Groups Over the past 100 years, the peer
group has come to play an increasingly important role
exo-system In the ecological perspective on human development, in the socialization and development of teenagers
the layer of the environment that does not directly contain the
(Dijkstra & Veenstra, 2011). But has the rise of peer
developing person but that affects the setting in which the person
lives. groups in contemporary society been a positive or neg-
ative influence on young people’s development? In
macro-system In the ecological perspective on human Chapter 5, we discuss how peer groups have changed
development, the outermost layer of the environment, containing adolescence, for better and for worse.
forces such as history and culture.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 11 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 11

❚ Schools Chapter 6 examines schools as a context sexuality, and achievement arise, and psychological or
for adolescent development. Contemporary society de- social problems can and do occur during all periods of
pends on schools to occupy, socialize, and educate ado- the life cycle. These five sets of issues are present
lescents. But how good a job are schools doing? What throughout the life span, from infancy through late
should schools do to help prepare adolescents for adult- adulthood. They represent basic developmental chal-
hood? And how should schools for adolescents be lenges that all people face as they grow and change:
structured (Cortina & Arel, 2011)? These are three of (1) discovering and understanding who they are as
the many difficult questions we will be examining. individuals—identity; (2) establishing a healthy sense
of independence—autonomy; (3) forming close and
❚ Work, Leisure, and the Mass Media In Chap- caring relationships with other people—intimacy;
ter 7, we look at the most important after-school (4) expressing sexual feelings and enjoying physical
influences on adolescent development: part-time jobs contact with others—sexuality; and (5) being success-
(Stone, 2011), extracurricular activities (Zarrett & ful and competent members of society—achievement.
Mahoney, 2011), the mass media (Brown & Bobkowski, Although these issues are not unique to adolescence,
2011a), and, of course, the Internet (Uhls, Espinoza, development in each of these areas takes a special turn
Greenfield, Subrahmanyam, & Šmahel, 2011). during this stage. Understanding how and why such
psychosocial developments take place during adoles-
cence is a special concern of social scientists interested
RECAP in this age period. We know that individuals form close
• According to Urie Bronfenbrenner’s ecological per- relationships before adolescence, for example, but why
spective on human development, we cannot under- is it that intimate romantic relationships first develop
stand development without examining the settings, during adolescence? We know that infants struggle with
or context, in which it occurs. learning how to be independent, but why during
• Although the fundamental biological, cognitive, adolescence do individuals need to be more on their
and social changes of adolescence are universal, own and make some decisions apart from their par-
their effects depend on the context in which they ents? We know that children fantasize about what they
take place. will be when they grow up, but why is it that these fan-
• The immediate, or proximal, contexts of adoles- tasies are not transformed into serious concerns until
cent development are the family, the peer group, adolescence?
school, and work and leisure settings, including Part Three of this book discusses changes in each of
mass media and the Internet. the five psychosocial areas and examines several com-
• What takes place in these immediate settings is in- mon psychosocial problems.
fluenced by the broader context in which they are
contained, including the community, ❚ Identity Chapter 8 deals with changes in identity,
the culture, and the historical self-esteem, and self-conceptions. In adolescence, a vari-
era in which people ety of important changes in the realm of identity occur
grow up. (Harter, 2011; Thomaes, Poorthuis, & Nelemans, 2011).
The adolescent may wonder who he or she really is and
where he or she is headed. Coming to terms with these

PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT
OF ADOLESCENCE psychosocial Referring to aspects of development that are both
The third, and final, component of our framework con- psychological and social in nature, such as developing a sense of
cerns the major psychosocial developments of adoles- identity or sexuality.
cence—identity, autonomy, intimacy, sexuality, and identity The domain of psychosocial development involving
achievement—as well as certain psychosocial problems self-conceptions, self-esteem, and the sense of who one is.
that may arise in adolescence. Social scientists use the
autonomy The psychosocial domain concerning the development
word psychosocial to describe aspects of development
and expression of independence.
that are both psychological and social in nature. Sexual-
ity, for instance, is a psychosocial issue because it in- intimacy The psychosocial domain concerning the formation,
volves both psychological change (that is, changes in the maintenance, and termination of close relationships.
individual’s emotions, motivations, and behavior) and
sexuality The psychosocial domain concerning the development
changes in the individual’s social relations with others. and expression of sexual feelings.
Of course, it is not only during the adolescent years
that concerns about identity, autonomy, intimacy, achievement The psychosocial domain concerning behaviors and
feelings in evaluative situations.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 12 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

12 Introduction

2011). Becoming sexual is an important aspect of de-


velopment during adolescence—not only because it
transforms the nature of relationships between adoles-
cents and their peers but also because it raises a range of
difficult questions for the young person. Chapter 11
discusses these concerns, including efforts to incorpo-
rate sexuality into a still-developing sense of self, the
need to resolve questions about sexual values and mor-
als, and the need to come to terms with the sorts of re-
lationships into which the adolescent is prepared—or
not prepared—to enter. We also look at sex education,
contraceptive use, sexually transmitted diseases, and
adolescent childbearing.

❚ Achievement Chapter 12 examines changes in


individuals’ educational and vocational behavior and
plans. Important decisions—many with long-term
consequences—about schooling and careers are made
during adolescence. Many of these decisions depend on
Sexuality is a central psychosocial issue of adolescence.
adolescents’ achievement in school, on their evaluations
of their own competencies and capabilities, on their as-
questions may involve a period of experimentation—a pirations and expectations for the future, and on the
time of trying on different personalities in an attempt to direction and advice they receive from parents, teach-
discover one’s true self. As you will read, the adolescent’s ers, and friends (Wigfield, Ho, & Mason-Singh, 2011).
quest for identity is a quest not only for a personal sense
of self but also for recognition from others and from ❚ Psychosocial Problems Historically, the scien-
society that he or she is a special, unique individual. tific study of adolescence has focused much attention
on the problems that some young people have during
❚ Autonomy Adolescents’ struggle to establish this time, despite the fact that most adolescents move
themselves as independent, self-governing individuals— through the period without getting into serious trouble
in their own eyes and in the eyes of others—is a long and (Steinberg & Morris, 2001). In Chapter 13, we’ll look
occasionally difficult process, not only for young people at three sets of problems typically associated with ado-
but also for those around them, especially their parents lescence: drug and alcohol use and abuse (Chassin,
(Zimmer-Gembeck, Ducat, & Collins, 2011). Chapter 9 Hussong, & Beltran, 2009), delinquency and other “ex-
focuses on three sorts of concerns about autonomy that ternalizing problems” (Farrington, 2009), and depression
are of special importance during adolescence: becoming and other “internalizing problems” (Graber & Sontag,
less emotionally dependent on parents (McElhaney, 2009). In each case, we examine the prevalence of the
Allen, Stephenson, & Hare, 2009), learning to make problem, the factors believed to contribute to its develop-
independent decisions (W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006), ment, and approaches to prevention and intervention.
and establishing a personal code of values and morals
(Morris, Eisenberg, & Houltberg, 2011).
MAKING THE PERSONAL
❚ Intimacy During adolescence, important changes CONNECTION
take place in the individual’s capacity to be intimate Of the five psychosocial issues just de-
with others, especially with peers. As we’ll see in Chapter scribed, which posed the most challenges
10, during adolescence, for the first time, friendships for you as a teenager?
emerge that involve openness, honesty, loyalty, and
exchange of confidences, rather than simply a sharing of
activities and interests (B. Brown & Larson, 2009;
W. Collins & Steinberg, 2006). Dating takes on increased RECAP
importance, and as a consequence, so does the capacity • Five main psychosocial issues are especially impor-
to form romantic relationships that are trusting and lov- tant in adolescence.
ing (Shulman, Connolly, & McIssac, 2011). • Identity involves changes in the ways individuals
see, evaluate, and think about themselves.
❚ Sexuality Sexual activity generally begins during • Autonomy involves the development of emotional,
the adolescent years (Diamond & Savin-Williams, behavioral, and moral independence.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 13 21/03/13 9:15 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 13

• Intimacy involves the development of the ability to when puberty is such an obvious part of adolescence?
form close and satisfying relationships with others. But how important this biological change is in defining
• Sexuality involves the development of the ability the psychosocial issues of the period is something that
to express sexual feelings and enjoy physical con- students of adolescence disagree about. Theorists who
tact with others. have taken a biological or, more accurately, “biosocial”
• Achievement involves the development of beliefs view of adolescence stress the hormonal and physical
and abilities that permit one to be a competent changes of puberty as driving forces. The most impor-
and successful member of society. tant biosocial theorist was G. Stanley Hall (1904), con-
• Some individuals experience psychological problems sidered the “father” of the scientific study of adolescence.
during adolescence—most commonly depression,
delinquency, and substance abuse—although ❚ Hall’s Theory of Recapitulation G. Stanley
the majority of individuals move Hall, who was very much influenced by the work of
through the period with- Charles Darwin, the author of the theory of evolution,
out serious believed that the development of the individual paral-
difficulties. leled the development of the human species, a notion
referred to as his theory of recapitulation. Infancy, in
his view, was equivalent to the time during our evolu-
tion when we were primitive, like animals. Adolescence,
Theoretical Perspectives in contrast, was seen as a time that paralleled the evolu-
tion of our species into civilization. For Hall, the devel-
on Adolescence opment of the individual through these stages was
The study of adolescence is based not just on empirical determined primarily by instinct—by biological and
research but also on theories of development (Newman & genetic forces within the person—and hardly influ-
Newman, 2011b). You will read more about different enced by the environment.
theories of adolescence throughout this book, but an The most important legacy of Hall’s view of adoles-
overview of the major ones may be helpful. cence is the notion that it is inevitably a period of
I find it useful to organize theoretical perspectives “storm and stress.” He believed that the hormonal
on adolescence around a question that has long domi- changes of puberty cause upheaval, both for the indi-
nated discussions of human development: How much vidual and for those around him or her. Because this
is due to “nature,” or biology, and how much is due to turbulence was biologically determined, it was un-
“nurture,” or the environment? Some theories of ado- avoidable. The best that society could do was to find
lescence emphasize biology, others emphasize the envi- ways of managing the young person whose “raging hor-
ronment, and still others fall somewhere between the mones” would invariably lead to difficulties.
two extremes (see Figure I.1). The purpose of this brief Although scientists no longer believe that adolescence
overview is not to argue for one approach over another, is an inherently stressful period or that the hormonal
but rather to demonstrate how each of these views has changes of puberty cause emotional problems, much
helped us gain a better understanding of the nature of contemporary work continues to emphasize the role that
adolescence. We’ll begin with a look at the most ex- biological factors play in shaping the adolescent experi-
treme biological perspectives and work our way across ence. More than 100 years ago, in fact, Hall speculated
a continuum toward the other extreme—perspectives about brain maturation, hormonal influences on behav-
that give primacy to the role of the environment. ior, and changes in patterns of sleep during adolescence—
all very hot topics in the study of adolescence today
(Dahl & Hariri, 2005). Current work in the biosocial tra-
BIOSOCIAL THEORIES
dition, also influenced by Hall and his followers, explores
The fact that biological change during adolescence is the genetic bases of individual differences in adolescence
noteworthy is not a matter of dispute—how could it be, and the evolutionary bases of adolescent behavior.

Theoretical Perspectives on Adolescence

Extremely Extremely
biological environmental

Biosocial Organismic Learning Sociological Historical/Anthropological


(e.g., Hall) (e.g., Piaget, Erikson) (e.g., Bandura) (e.g., Mannheim, Lewin) (e.g., Benedict)
FIGURE I.1 Theories of adolescence range from the extremely biological, like that of G. Stanley Hall, to the extremely
environmental, like that of Ruth Benedict.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 14 21/03/13 9:16 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

14 Introduction

ORGANISMIC THEORIES the development of the ego—that part of the psyche


believed to regulate thought, emotion, and behavior.
Our next stop on the continuum is in the domain of
Erikson proposed eight stages in psychosocial devel-
what are called “organismic” theorists. Like biosocial
opment, each characterized by a specific “crisis” that
theorists, organismic theorists stress the importance of
arises at that point in development because of the inter-
the biological changes of adolescence. But unlike their
play between the internal forces of biology and the
biosocial counterparts, organismic theories also take
unique demands of society. In Erikson’s theory, which
into account the ways in which contextual forces inter-
we look at in detail in Chapter 8, adolescence is seen as
act with and modify these biological forces.
a period that revolves around the identity crisis, a crisis
If you have had previous course work in develop-
that is shaped both by the changes of puberty and by
mental psychology, you have undoubtedly encountered
the specific demands that society places on young peo-
the major organismic theorists, for they have long dom-
ple to make decisions about the future. According to
inated the study of human development more generally.
Erikson, the challenge of adolescence is to resolve the
Three of these theorists, in particular, have had a great
identity crisis and to emerge with a coherent sense of
influence over the study of adolescence: Sigmund Freud
who one is and where one is headed.
(1938), Erik Erikson (1968), and Jean Piaget (Inhelder &
Piaget, 1958). Although these theorists share in com-
❚ Piagetian Theory Freud and Erikson both em-
mon an organismic orientation, the theories they devel-
phasized emotional and social development. For Jean
oped emphasized different aspects of individual growth
Piaget, in contrast, development could best be under-
and development.
stood by examining changes in the nature of thinking,
❚ Freudian Theory For Freud, development was or cognition. Piaget believed that, as children mature,
best understood in terms of the psychosexual conflicts they pass through stages of cognitive development, and
that arise at different points in development. Like Hall, in each stage, from birth to adolescence, their ways of
Freud saw adolescence as a time of upheaval. Accord- thinking are qualitatively distinct. In his view, under-
ing to Freud, the hormonal changes of puberty upset standing the distinctive features of thought and reason-
the psychic balance that had been achieved during the ing at each stage can give us insight into the overall
prior psychosexual stage, called “latency.” Because the development of the individual at that point in time.
hormonal changes of puberty were responsible for In Piaget’s theory, which is discussed in Chapter 2,
marked increases in sex drive, the adolescent was tem- adolescence marks the transition from concrete to ab-
porarily thrown into a period of intrapsychic crisis; stract thought. According to this model, adolescence is
and old psychosexual conflicts, long buried in the un- the period in which individuals become capable of
conscious, were revived. Freud and his followers be- thinking in hypothetical terms, a development that per-
lieved that the main challenge of adolescence was to mits a broad expansion of logical capabilities. As you
resolve these conflicts and restore a psychic balance. will see, many of the familiar changes in behavior that
Although the process was driven by the hormonal we associate with adolescence have been attributed to
changes of puberty, the specific conflicts faced by the these changes in cognitive abilities. Piaget’s views have
young person were seen as dependent on his or her been applied to the study of moral development, social
early experiences in the family. development, and education.
Sigmund Freud himself actually had very little to say As is the case with the other organismic theories,
specifically about adolescence. But his daughter, Anna Piaget’s theory of cognitive development emphasizes
Freud (1958), extended much of her father’s thinking to the interplay between biological and contextual forces.
the study of development during the second decade of The development of abstract thinking in adolescence,
life. This work was carried on by neo-Freudians such as for example, is influenced both by the internal biologi-
Peter Blos (1979), whose theories of adolescent auton- cal changes of the developmental period and by changes
omy we explore in detail in Chapter 9. in the intellectual environment encountered by the
individual.
❚ Eriksonian Theory In Erikson’s theory, the em-
phasis was on the psychosocial crises characteristic of
LEARNING THEORIES
each period of growth. Like Freud, Erikson believed that
internal, biological developments moved the individual As we move across the theoretical continuum from
from one developmental stage to the next. But unlike extreme biological views to extreme environmental
Freud, Erikson stressed the psychosocial, rather than the ones, we encounter a group of theories that shift the
psychosexual, conflicts faced by the individual at each emphasis from biological forces to environmental ones.
point in time. Whereas Freud emphasized the develop- Whereas organismic theorists tend to emphasize the
ment of the id—that part of the psyche believed to be interaction between biological change and environ-
dominated by instinctual urges—Erikson emphasized mental demands, learning theorists stress the context in
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 15 21/03/13 9:16 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 15

which behavior takes place. The capacity of the indi- SOCIOLOGICAL THEORIES
vidual to learn from experience is assumed to be a bio-
The emphasis of the biosocial, organismic, and learning
logical given that is in place long before adolescence.
theories is mainly on forces within the individual, or
What is of interest to learning theorists is the content of
within the individual’s specific environment, that shape
what is learned.
development and behavior. In contrast, sociological
Learning theorists are not especially developmental
theories of adolescence attempt to understand how ad-
in their approach and, as a consequence, have little to
olescents, as a group, come of age in society. Instead of
say specifically about adolescence as a developmental
emphasizing differences among individuals in their bi-
period. Indeed, for learning theorists, the basic pro-
ological makeups or their experiences in the world, so-
cesses of human behavior are the same during adoles-
ciological theorists focus on the factors that all
cence as during other periods of the life span. But
adolescents or groups of adolescents have in common
learning theorists have been extremely influential in the
by virtue of their age, gender, ethnicity, social class, or
study of adolescent development because they have
some other demographic factor.
helped us understand how the specific environment in
which an adolescent lives can shape his or her behavior. ❚ Adolescent Marginality Sociological theories of
adolescence often have focused on relations between the
❚ Behaviorism There are two general categories of generations and have tended to emphasize the difficul-
learning theorists. One group, known as behaviorists, ties young people have in making the transition from
emphasizes the processes of reinforcement and punish- adolescence to adulthood, especially in industrialized
ment as the main influences on adolescent behavior. society. Two themes have dominated these discussions.
The main proponent of this view was B. F. Skinner One theme, concerning the marginality of young people,
(1953), whose theory of operant conditioning has had a emphasizes the difference in power that exists between
tremendous impact on the entire field of psychology. the adult and the adolescent generations. Two important
Within an operant conditioning framework, reinforce- thinkers in this vein are Kurt Lewin (1951) and Edgar
ment is defined as the process through which a behavior Friedenberg (1959). Although the view that adolescents
is made more likely to occur again, whereas punishment are “second-class citizens” was more influential 50 years
is defined as the process through which a behavior is ago than today, contemporary applications of this view-
made less likely to occur again. From this vantage point, point stress the fact that many adolescents are prohibited
adolescent behavior is nothing more or less than the from occupying meaningful roles in society and there-
product of the various reinforcements and punishments fore experience frustration and restlessness. Some writ-
to which the individual has been exposed. An adolescent ers have noted that many of the problems we associate
who strives to do well in school, for example, does so with adolescence have been created, in part, by the way
because in the past she or he has been reinforced for this in which we have structured the adolescent experience
behavior or has been punished for not behaving this and isolated young people from adults (Epstein, 2007).
way. Similarly, a teenager who continues to experiment A modification of this view focuses on differences
with risky behavior is being reinforced for this sort of within the adolescent population. According to this
activity or punished for being especially cautious. viewpoint, the adolescent’s social class, or socioeco-
nomic status, as it is formally known, structures his or
❚ Social Learning Theory A related approach is her experience of growing up. Theorists such as August
taken by social learning theorists such as Albert Bandura Hollingshead (1949/1975) and Robert Havighurst (1952)
(Bandura & Walters, 1959). Social learning theorists also have emphasized the fact that the experience of adoles-
emphasize the ways in which adolescents learn how to cence differs markedly as a function of the young per-
behave, but in contrast to behaviorists, they place more son’s family background. They argue that it is impossible
weight on the processes of modeling and observational to generalize about the “nature” of adolescence because
learning. According to these theorists, adolescents learn it varies so much depending on the resources of the
how to behave not simply by being reinforced and pun- adolescent’s family.
ished by forces in the environment but also by watching
and imitating those around them. As is clear throughout ❚ Intergenerational Conflict The other theme in
the text, social learning approaches to adolescence have sociological theories of adolescence concerns intergener-
been very influential in explaining how adolescents are ational conflict, or as it is more commonly known, the
affected by the child-rearing methods employed by their generation gap. Theorists such as Karl Mannheim (1952)
parents and by the influence, or pressure, of their peers. and James Coleman (1961) stressed the fact that adoles-
From this vantage point, an adolescent who strives to do cents and adults grow up under different social circum-
well in school or who takes a lot of risks is probably stances and therefore develop different sets of attitudes,
imitating peers, family members, or actors portrayed in values, and beliefs. As a consequence, there is inevitable
the mass media. tension between the adolescent and the adult generations.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 16 21/03/13 9:16 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

16 Introduction

people come of age, but they take a much


more relativistic stance. Historical perspec-
tives, such as those offered by Glen Elder
(1980), Joseph Kett (1977), and Thomas Hine
(1999), stress the fact that adolescence as a
developmental period has varied consider-
ably from one historical era to another. As
a consequence, it is impossible to general-
ize about such issues as the degree to which
adolescence is stressful, the developmental
tasks of the period, or the nature of inter-
generational relations. Historians would
say that these issues all depend on the so-
cial, political, and economic forces present
at a given time. Even something as basic
to our view of adolescence as the “identity
crisis,” they say, is a social invention that
arose because of industrialization and the
prolongation of schooling. Prior to the In-
dustrial Revolution, when most adolescents
followed in their parents’ occupation, peo-
ple didn’t have “crises” over who they were
or what they were going to do in life.
Dramatic historical events, such as a major economic recession, can transform the
nature of adolescence. ❚ Adolescence as an Invention One
group of theorists has taken this viewpoint
to its extreme, arguing that adolescence is
Some writers, like Coleman, have gone so far as to argue entirely a social invention (Bakan, 1972).
that adolescents develop a different cultural viewpoint— They believe that the way in which we divide the life
a counterculture—that may be hostile to the values or cycle into stages—drawing a boundary between child-
beliefs of adult society. We’ll look more closely at this idea hood and adolescence, for example—is nothing more
in Chapter 5. than a reflection of the political, economic, and social
Although sociological theories of adolescence clearly circumstances in which we live. They point out that, al-
place the emphasis on the broader context in which though puberty has always been a feature of human de-
adolescents come of age, a theme of inevitability still velopment, it was not until the rise of compulsory
runs through their approach. Mannheim, for example, education that we began treating adolescents as a special
believed that because modern society changes so rap- and distinct group. This suggests that social conditions,
idly, there will always be problems between generations, not biological givens, define the nature of adolescent de-
because each generation grows up with different expe- velopment. We noted earlier that contemporary writers
riences and beliefs. American adults who were teenag- debate whether a new phase of life, “emerging adult-
ers in the late 1960s, for instance, when “sex, drugs, and hood,” actually exists. Writers who believe that different
rock ’n’ roll” was the mantra, may find themselves argu- stages of life are social inventions would say that if
ing with their children, who may have come of age dur- emerging adulthood does exist, it does so because soci-
ing a time of extreme political conservatism, such as the ety has made it so, not because people have really
1980s. Similarly, Lewin believed that marginality is an changed in any fundamental way.
inherent feature of adolescence because adults always
control more resources and have more power than ❚ Anthropological Perspectives A similar theme
young people. is echoed by anthropologists who study adolescence,
the most important of whom were Ruth Benedict
(1934) and Margaret Mead (1928/1978). Benedict and
HISTORICAL AND ANTHROPOLOGICAL
Mead, whose work is examined in Chapter 3, pointed
THEORIES
out that societies vary considerably in the ways in which
Our final stop on the continuum takes us to the extreme they view and structure adolescence. As a consequence,
environmental position. Historians and anthropolo- these thinkers viewed adolescence as a culturally de-
gists who study adolescence share with sociologists fined experience—stressful and difficult in societies
an interest in the broader context in which young that saw it this way, but calm and peaceful in societies
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 17 21/03/13 9:16 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e INDRODUCTION The Study of Adolescent Development 17

that had an alternative vision. Benedict, in particular, • Anthropological theorists emphasize the diversity
drew a distinction between continuous and discon- of adolescent experiences across different cultures.
tinuous societies. In continuous societies (typically, Like historical theorists, they believe that the na-
nonindustrialized societies with little social change), ture of adolescence is determined
the transition from adolescence to adulthood is grad- by the conditions under
ual and peaceful. In discontinuous societies (typi- which people
cally, industrialized societies characterized by rapid grow up.
social change), the transition to adulthood is abrupt
and difficult. In Chapter 3, we’ll look at whether the
nature of development differs for individuals in
continuous versus discontinuous societies.
Stereotypes Versus
Scientific Study
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION One of the oldest debates in the study of adolescence is
Some writers have argued that the stage whether adolescence is an inherently stressful time for
of life we call adolescence is a social in- individuals. As we noted earlier, G. Stanley Hall, who is
vention. What do they mean by this? generally acknowledged as the father of the modern
Could you say this about other periods study of adolescence, likened adolescence to the turbu-
of development? Is infancy a social in- lent, transitional period in the evolution of the human
vention? Is middle age? What about species from savagery into civilization. Long before
“emerging adulthood”? Hall, in the 18th century, the French philosopher Jean-
Jacques Rousseau had described adolescence by draw-
ing an analogy to a violent storm. “As the roaring of the
waves precedes the tempest, so the murmur of rising
passions announces the tumultuous change. . . . Keep
RECAP your hand upon the helm,” he warned parents, “or all is
• Theoretical perspectives on adolescence range lost” (Rousseau, 1762/1911, pp. 172–173).
from those that emphasize the role of biology to Although neither Hall nor Rousseau had any scien-
those that emphasize the role of the environment. tific evidence that adolescence was any more stormy
• Biosocial theorists view development in adoles- than childhood or adulthood, their portrayal of teenag-
cence as the inevitable outcome of the physical ers as passionate, troubled, and unpredictable persists
changes of puberty. Hall believed that adolescence today. One 12-year-old girl I was counseling told me
was necessarily a period of storm and stress. that her mother had been telling her that she would go
• Organismic theorists like Freud, Erikson, and Piaget through a difficult time when she turned 14—as if
view development as the product of an interaction some magical, internal alarm clock was set to trigger
between biology and the environment. Freud and storm and stress on schedule.
his followers emphasized the internal psychological The girl’s mother wasn’t alone in her view of adoles-
conflicts brought on by puberty. Erikson viewed cence, of course. Sometime this week, turn on the television
adolescence as a time of “identity crisis.” Piaget or go to a movie and note how teenagers are depicted.
emphasized the development of abstract thinking. If they are not portrayed as troublemakers—the usual
• Learning theorists like Skinner and Bandura view role in which they are cast—adolescents are depicted as
development in adolescence as the result of expo- sex-crazed idiots (if they are male), giggling fools (if they
sure to forces that reinforce, punish, or model are female), or tormented lost souls, searching for their
particular behaviors. place in a strange, cruel world (if they aren’t delinquent,
• Sociological theorists focus more on the ways in sex-crazed, or giggling). It’s not only fictionalized por-
which adolescents as a group, rather than as indi- trayals of teenagers that are stereotyped—studies of local
viduals, are treated by society. They emphasize newscasts find that the majority of stories on youth are
differences between the adolescent and adult about violence (Gilliam & Bales, 2001). Scholars, too,
generations in power, attitudes, and values. have been influenced by this viewpoint—a dispropor-
• Historical theorists focus on the ways in which ad- tionate number of scientific studies of adolescents have
olescence differs during different periods. They focused on young people’s problems rather than their
emphasize the fact that adolescence is at least in normative development (Steinberg & Morris, 2001).
part a social “invention,” whose characteristics de- These stereotypes of adolescents as troubling, and
pend on the way in which young people are viewed troubled, have important implications for how teenagers
at that point in time. are treated—by teachers, by salespersons, and by parents.
ste32118_ch00_002-018.indd Page 18 21/03/13 9:16 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

18 Introduction

One study, for example, measured mothers’ general be- jobs to go around, and adults may “need” to see adoles-
liefs about adolescence to see how well these preconcep- cents as incapable of working, whereas the reverse is
tions predicted how their teenager behaved (Buchanan & true during wartime, when adolescents are needed to
Hughes, 2009). The more likely a mother was to believe take on jobs and serve in the military.
that teenagers are risk taking and rebellious, the more The truth of the matter is that most characteristics
likely it was that her teenager actually behaved this way of adolescence as a developmental stage have both pos-
one year later, perhaps because the mother’s expectations itive and negative elements (Siegel & Scovill, 2000).
led her to behave in a way that brought out the worst in Young people’s willingness to challenge authority, for
her adolescent. This finding is important because, as instance, is both refreshing (when we agree with them)
you’ll read in Chapter 4, parent–teenager relations are and annoying (when we do not). Their propensity to
influenced by the expectations they have about each take risks is both admirable and frightening. Their en-
other. For example, one study found that mothers’ beliefs ergy and exuberance is both exciting and unsettling.
about their teenagers’ likely alcohol use may actually lead One of the goals of this book is to provide you with
to increases in their child’s drinking (Madon, Willard, a more realistic understanding of adolescent develop-
Guyll, Trudeau, & Spoth, 2006). ment in contemporary society—an understanding
The tremendous growth of the scientific literature on that reflects the best and most up-to-date scientific
adolescence over the past two decades has, fortunately, knowledge. As you read the material, you should think
led to more accurate views of normal adolescence among about your personal experiences as an adolescent, but
practitioners who work with young people, although a you should also try to look beyond them and be will-
trip to the “Parenting” section of your local bookstore ing to question the “truths” about teenagers that you
will quickly reveal that the storm-and-stress stereotype is have grown accustomed to over the years. This does
still alive and well (Steinberg, 2001). (I once saw a book not mean that your experiences were not valid, or your
titled Surviving Your Dog’s Adolescence!) Today, most ex- recollections inaccurate. Rather, your experiences as a
perts do not dismiss the storm-and-stress viewpoint as teenager were the product of a unique set of forces that
entirely incorrect but do see the difficulties that some have made you the individual you are today. The per-
adolescents have as due largely to the cultural context son who sits next to you in class—or the person who
within which they grow up. Studies show that adoles- right now, in some distant region of the world, is
cents are more stressed in contemporary industrialized thinking back to his or her adolescence—was probably
societies, for example, than they are in nonindustrialized, exposed to different forces than you were and probably
traditional ones, although this may change as globaliza- had a different set of adolescent experiences as a
tion brings some of the more stressful aspects of adoles- consequence.
cence to less industrialized parts of the world (Larson et
al., 2009). Indeed, one of the most striking trends of the
past several decades is that globalization has made ado- MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
lescence in different parts of the world increasingly simi- If someone were to make generalizations
lar (National Research Council, 2005). about the nature of adolescence by analyz-
You have no doubt come into this course with many ing your experiences as a teenager, how
convictions of your own about adolescence. These be- would the period be portrayed?
liefs are based in part on your own experiences as a
teenager and in part on the images of adolescents to
which you have been exposed over the years—in books,
on film, and on television. As several writers have
pointed out, even social scientists’ portrayal of teenag- RECAP
ers is influenced by the broader social and historical • Adolescents are one of the most stereotyped
context in which they work. To the extent that we want groups in society.
to see adolescents as different from adults, we exagger- • Many stereotypes about adolescence are exces-
ate the differences between teenagers and their elders sively negative, portraying teenagers as sex-crazed,
and portray young people as “out of control due to hor- foolish, irresponsible, difficult, or troubled.
monal storms” (Lesko, 1996, p. 157). During periods of • The scientific study of adolescence has not pro-
economic downturn, for instance, when jobs are scarce, vided evidence in support of these stereotypes.
scholars depict adolescents as immature, unstable, and • One goal of this book is to foster an objective
undereducated, whereas during periods of war, the understanding of adolescence that
same age group is portrayed as mature, responsible, and is grounded in solid
competent (Enright, Levy, Harris, & Lapsley, 1987). research and
Presumably, these characterizations serve a broader, if theory.
hidden, agenda—during depressions, there are fewer
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 19 21/03/13 9:21 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

PART 1

1 ❘ Biological Transitions 2 ❘ Cognitive Transitions 3 ❘ Social Transitions

The Fundamental Changes


of Adolescence
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 20 21/03/13 9:21 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 21 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
1
Biological Transitions
PUBERTY: AN OVERVIEW
The Endocrine System
What Triggers Puberty?
How Hormones Influence Adolescent Development
SOMATIC DEVELOPMENT
Changes in Stature and the Dimensions of the Body
Sexual Maturation
THE TIMING AND TEMPO OF PUBERTY
Variations in the Timing and Tempo of Puberty
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Pubertal Timing
THE PSYCHOLOGICAL AND SOCIAL IMPACT OF PUBERTY
The Immediate Impact of Puberty
The Impact of Specific Pubertal Events
The Impact of Early or Late Maturation
OBESITY AND EATING DISORDERS
Obesity
Anorexia Nervosa, Bulimia, and Binge Eating Disorder
PHYSICAL HEALTH AND HEALTH CARE IN ADOLESCENCE
The Paradox of Adolescent Health
Causes of Mortality in Adolescence
Promoting Adolescent Health

21
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 22 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

22 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

ACCORDING TO AN OLD JOKE, there are only two viewed and treated by others. Yet the social environ-
things in life that one can be sure of—death and taxes. ment exerts a tremendous impact on puberty and its
To this brief list, we might add puberty—the physical psychological and social consequences; indeed, as you
changes of adolescence—for, of all the developments will read in this chapter, the social environment even
that take place during the second decade of life, the affects the timing of puberty (that is, whether a person
only truly inevitable one is physical maturation. Not all matures early or late). In some societies, pubertal matu-
adolescents experience identity crises, rebel against ration brings with it a series of complex and public ini-
their parents, or fall madly in love, but virtually all un- tiation rites that mark the passage of the young person
dergo the biological transitions associated with matu- into adulthood, socially as well as physically. In other
ration into adult reproductive capability. societies, recognition of the physical transformation
Puberty, however, is considerably affected by the from child into adult takes more subtle forms. Parents
context in which it occurs. Physical development is in- may merely remark, “Our little boy has become a man,”
fluenced by a host of environmental factors, and the when they discover that he needs to shave, or “Our little
timing and rate of pubertal growth vary across regions girl has grown up,” when they learn that she has gotten
of the world, socioeconomic classes, ethnic groups, her first period. Early or late maturation may be cause
and historical eras. Today, in contemporary America, for celebration or cause for concern, depending on what
the average girl reaches menarche—the time of first is admired or derogated in a given peer group at a given
menstruation—at about age 12. But among the Lumi point in time. The fifth-grader who is developing breasts
people of New Guinea, the typical girl does not reach might be embarrassed, but the ninth-grader who has not
menarche until after 18 years of age (Eveleth & Tanner, developed breasts might be equally self-conscious.
1990). Imagine how that five-year difference transforms In sum, even the most universal aspect of adoles-
the nature of adolescence. Picture how different high cence—puberty—is hardly universal in its impact on
school would be if sexual maturation did not occur un- the young person. In this chapter, we examine just how
til after graduation! and why the environment in which adolescents de-
Physical and sexual maturation profoundly affects velop exerts its influence even on something as funda-
the ways in which adolescents view themselves and are mental as puberty.

Puberty: An Overview (sex glands), which results in hormonal changes


that ultimately enable reproduction.
Puberty derives from the Latin word pubertas, which 3. The development of secondary sex characteristics,
means “adult.” Technically, the term refers to the period including changes in the genitals and breasts, and
during which an individual becomes capable of sexual the growth of pubic, facial, and body hair.
reproduction. More broadly, however, puberty encom-
passes all the physical changes that occur in the growing Each of these sets of changes is the result of develop-
girl or boy as the individual passes from childhood into ments in the endocrine and central nervous systems,
adulthood (Dorn & Biro, 2011). many of which begin years before the external signs of
Puberty has three chief physical manifestations: puberty are evident—some occur even before birth.
Puberty may appear to come on suddenly, judging
1. A rapid acceleration in growth, resulting in dra- from its external signs, but in fact it is part of a gradual
matic increases in both height and weight. process that begins at conception (Susman & Dorn,
2. The development of primary sex characteristics, 2009). You may be surprised to learn that no new hor-
including the further development of the gonads mones are produced and no new bodily systems de-
velop at puberty. Rather, some hormones that have
been present since before birth increase, and others
menarche The time of first menstruation, one of the most decrease.
important changes to occur among females during puberty.
endocrine system The system of the body that produces, THE ENDOCRINE SYSTEM
circulates, and regulates hormones.
The endocrine system produces, circulates, and regu-
hormones Highly specialized substances secreted by one or more lates levels of hormones in the body. Hormones are
endocrine glands.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 23 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 23

falls below this level. Similarly, when a particular hor-


Hypothalamus monal level in your body dips below the endocrine
system’s set point for that hormone, secretion of the
LH-RF (luteinizing hormone- hormone increases; when the level reaches the set point,
Hypothalamus releasing factor) and FSH-RF secretion temporarily stops. And, as is the case with a
monitors levels (follicle-stimulating hormone-
releasing factor) stimulate thermostat, the setting level, or set point, for a particu-
of androgens lar hormone can be adjusted up or down, depending on
and estrogens. pituitary gland.
environmental or internal bodily conditions.
Such a feedback loop becomes increasingly impor-
Pituitary tant at the onset of puberty. Long before early adoles-
gland cence—in fact, before birth—a feedback loop develops
involving the pituitary gland (which controls hormone
LH (luteinizing hormone) and FSH
(follicle-stimulating hormone) levels in general), the hypothalamus (the part of the
stimulate gonads. brain that controls the pituitary gland, and where there
is a concentration of GnRH neurons), and the gonads
(in males, the testes; in females, the ovaries), which
(Testes in males, release the “sex” hormones—androgens and estro-
Gonads
ovaries in females) gens. This feedback loop is known as the HPG axis (for
Hypothalamus, Pituitary, Gonads) (see Figure 1.1). Al-
though you may think of androgens as “male” hormones
and estrogens as “female” hormones, both types of hor-
mones are produced by each sex, and both are present
Androgens
Estrogens

FIGURE 1.1 Levels of sex hormones are regulated by a


feedback system (the HPG axis) composed of the hypothal- glands Organs that stimulate particular parts of the body to
amus, pituitary gland, and gonads. (Grumbach, Roth, Kaplan, & respond in specific ways to particular hormones.
Kelch, 1974)
gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) neurons Specialized
neurons that are activated by certain pubertal hormones.

highly specialized substances that are secreted by one or set point A physiological level or setting (e.g., of a specific
more endocrine glands and then enter the bloodstream hormone) that the body attempts to maintain through a self-
and travel throughout the body. Glands are organs that regulating system.
stimulate particular parts of the body to respond in feedback loop A cycle through which two or more bodily
specific ways. Just as specialized hormones carry “mes- functions respond to and regulate each other, such as that formed
sages” to particular cells in the body, so are the body’s by the hypothalamus, the pituitary gland, and the gonads.
cells designed to receive hormonal messages selectively.
pituitary gland One of the chief glands responsible for regulating
Many of the hormones that play important roles at pu-
levels of hormones in the body.
berty carry their instructions by activating very specific
types of neurons in the brain, called gonadotropin- hypothalamus A part of the brain that controls the functioning of
releasing hormone (GnRH) neurons (Bogin, 2011; the pituitary gland.
Novaira et al., 2011). gonads The glands that secrete sex hormones: in males, the testes;
in females, the ovaries.
❚ The Hormonal Feedback Loop The endo- testes The male gonads.
crine system receives its instructions to increase or de-
crease circulating levels of particular hormones from ovaries The female gonads.
the central nervous system, mainly through the firing androgens A class of sex hormones secreted by the gonads, found
of GnRH neurons in the brain. The system works like a in both sexes, but in higher levels among males than females
thermostat. Hormonal levels are “set” at a certain point, following puberty.
which may differ depending on the stage of develop-
ment, just as you might set a thermostat at a certain estrogens A class of sex hormones secreted by the gonads, found
in both sexes, but in higher levels among females than males
temperature (and use different settings during different
following puberty.
seasons or different times of the day). By setting your
room’s thermostat at 608F, you are instructing your HPG (hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal) axis The
heating system to go into action when the temperature neurophysiological pathway that involves the hypothalamus, the
pituitary gland, and the gonads.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 24 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

24 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

The thyroid and adrenal cortex, in turn, secrete hor-


mones that cause various bodily changes to take place at
puberty. Research also indicates that early feelings of
sexual attraction to others—most individuals, in Amer-
ica and around the world, report that their first sexual
attraction took place at the “magical age of 10,” before
they went through puberty—may be stimulated by
maturation of the adrenal glands, called adrenarche
(Herdt & McClintock, 2000), which contributes to the
development of body odor, signaling the beginning of
sexual maturation to others (Campbell, 2011). Changes
at puberty in the brain system that regulates the adrenal
gland are also important because this is the brain sys-
tem that controls how we respond to stress (Del Giudice,
Angeleri, & Manera, 2009). One reason adolescence is a
period of great vulnerability for the onset of many seri-
ous mental disorders is that the hormonal changes of
puberty make us more responsive to stress (Sontag,
Early feelings of sexual attraction to others are stimulated by Graber, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, 2008; Steinberg et al.,
adrenarche, the maturation of the adrenal glands, which takes
place before the outward signs of puberty are evident.
2006; Stroud et al., 2009; E. Walker, Sabuwalla, & Huot,
2004; Worthman, 2011). This leads to excessive secre-
tion of the stress hormone cortisol, a substance that at
in males and females at birth. During adolescence, high and chronic levels can cause brain cells to die
however, the average male produces more androgens (Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long, & Griggs, 2009). Keep
than estrogens, and the average female produces more in mind that there is a difference between saying that
estrogens than androgens (Susman & Dorn, 2009). adolescence is an inherently stressful time (which it is
The hypothalamus responds to the levels of sex hor- not) and saying that adolescence is a time of heightened
mones circulating in the body. Your HPG axis is set to vulnerability to stress (which it is).
maintain certain levels of androgens and estrogens.
When these levels fall below the set points, the hypo-
thalamus no longer inhibits the pituitary, thus permit- MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
ting it to stimulate the release of sex hormones by the
Do you remember your first feelings of sex-
gonads, and other puberty-related hormones by the
ual attraction for someone? How old were
adrenal gland. When sex hormone levels reach the set
you?
point, the hypothalamus responds by inhibiting its
stimulation of the pituitary gland. As you will see,
puberty occurs when it does because several different
signals—genetic as well as environmental—instruct the
brain to change the set point (Sisk & Foster, 2004). Just WHAT TRIGGERS PUBERTY?
as you might change the setting on your heating ther-
mostat automatically every November 1, or when your Although the HPG axis is active before birth, it is rela-
utility bill has become too expensive, your brain is con- tively quiet during much of childhood. Something hap-
stantly monitoring a variety of signals and adjusting pens during middle childhood, though, that reawakens
your hormonal set points in response. the HPG axis and signals it that the body is ready for
puberty. Some of this is due to a clock whose “puberty
❚ Adrenarche During and just before puberty, the alarm” is set very early in life by information coded in
pituitary also secretes hormones that act on the thyroid the genes (as you will read, the age at which someone
and on the adrenal cortex as well as hormones that goes through puberty is largely inherited). But some of
stimulate overall bodily growth. The release of these the reawakening of the HPG axis at puberty is due to
substances is also under the control of the hypothalamus. multiple signals that tell the brain it is time to “get the
childbearing show on the road.” These signals indicate
whether there are sexually mature mating partners in
the environment, whether there are sufficient nutri-
adrenarche The maturation of the adrenal glands that takes place tional resources to support a pregnancy, and whether the
during adolescence. individual is physically mature and healthy enough to
begin reproducing.
cortisol A hormone produced when a person is exposed to stress.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 25 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 25

Adolescence FIGURE 1.2 The biological


changes we associate with ado-
lescence actually unfold over a
Puberty long period of time, beginning
with the maturation of the adre-
nal axis during childhood and
Tanner 1
ending with the maturation of
Tanner 2 the gonadal axis in the early 20s.
Tanner 3 Tanner 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 refer to
Tanner 4 different stages of pubertal
maturation (see page 29).
Tanner 5
n (Adapted from Dorn et al., 2006)
t ion tio
at ura at ura
is m is m
l ax l ax
d rena d a
A na
Go

Adrenarche Gonadarche
6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20+
Age

For example, some evidence suggests that rising predetermines certain patterns of behavior, many
levels of a protein produced by fat cells, leptin, may be of which may not actually appear until much later
the most important signal, at least in females (Susman & (Collaer & Hines, 1995). Studies of sex differences in
Dorn, 2009). This idea is consistent with observations aggression, for example, show that even though some
that individuals may not go through puberty until they of these differences may not appear until adolescence,
have accumulated a certain amount of body fat. Re- they likely result from the impact of prenatal hormones,
search also shows that stress, illness, nutritional defi- rather than from hormonal changes at puberty.
ciencies, excessive exercise, and excessive thinness can In other words, the presence or absence of certain
all delay the onset of puberty (Susman & Dorn, 2009). hormones early in life may “program” the brain and the
The signal carried by rising levels of leptin instructs the central nervous system to develop in certain ways and
hypothalamus to stop doing things that have been in- according to a certain timetable later on (Sisk & Foster,
hibiting puberty (Sisk & Foster, 2004). As a result of 2004). Because we may not see the resulting changes in
these genetic and signaling processes, the GnRH neu- behavior until adolescence, it is easy to conclude, mis-
rons are excited, and the hypothalamus initiates a cas- takenly, that the behaviors result from hormonal
cade of hormonal events that ultimately results in changes that take place at the time of puberty. In reality,
sexual maturation (see Figure 1.2). however, exposure to certain hormones before birth
may set a sort of alarm clock that does not go off until
adolescence. Just because the alarm clock rings at the
HOW HORMONES INFLUENCE
same time that puberty begins does not mean that pu-
ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
berty caused the alarm to go off.
Hormones play two very different roles in adolescent Many changes in behavior at adolescence occur,
development: an organizational role and an activa- however, because of changes in hormone levels at pu-
tional role (Sisk & Foster, 2004). Most people under- berty; these hormonal changes are said to activate the
standably think that changes in behavior at puberty changes in behavior. For instance, the increase in cer-
result from changes in hormones that occur at that tain hormones at puberty is thought to stimulate the
time. But this is only partially correct. Long before
adolescence—in fact, before birth—hormones shape,
or organize, the brain in ways that may not be mani- leptin A protein produced by the fat cells that may play a role in
fested in behavior until childhood or even adolescence. the onset of puberty.
Generally, until about eight weeks after conception, the
human brain is “feminine” unless and until it is exposed organizational role (of hormones) The process through which
early exposure to hormones, especially prenatally, organizes the
to certain “masculinizing” hormones, like testosterone.
brain or other organs in anticipation of later changes in behavior or
Because levels of testosterone are higher among males
patterns of growth.
than females while the brain is developing, males
usually end up with a more “masculinized” brain than activational role (of hormones) The process through which
females. This sex difference in brain organization changes in hormone levels, especially at puberty, stimulate changes
in the adolescent’s behavior, appearance, or growth.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 26 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

26 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

development of secondary sex characteristics, such as


the growth of pubic hair. Other hormonal changes at
Somatic Development
adolescence, controlled by the adrenal gland, may stim- The effects of the hormonal changes of puberty on the
ulate an increase in individuals’ sex drive (McClintock & adolescent’s body are remarkable. Consider the dra-
Herdt, 1996). There is also growing evidence that the matic changes in physical appearance that occur dur-
hormonal changes of puberty affect the brain in ways ing early adolescence. The individual enters puberty
that increase adolescents’ desire for highly rewarding, looking like a child but within four years or so has the
sensation-seeking activities, which may lead some teen- physical appearance of a young adult. During this rela-
agers to engage in risky behavior (Forbes & Dahl, 2010; tively brief period, the average individual grows about
Op de Macks et al., 2011). 10 inches taller, matures sexually, and develops an
Still other changes during puberty are likely to be adult-proportioned body. Along with many other or-
results of an interaction between organizational and ac- gans, the brain changes in size, structure, and function
tivational effects of hormones (Collaer & Hines, 1995). at puberty, a series of developments we’ll discuss in
Hormones that are present during the development of Chapter 2.
the fetus may organize a certain set of behaviors (for
example, our brains may be set up to have us later en-
CHANGES IN STATURE AND
gage in sexual behavior), but certain changes in those
THE DIMENSIONS OF THE BODY
hormones at puberty may be needed to activate the pat-
tern; that is, individuals may not become motivated to ❚ The Adolescent Growth Spurt The simulta-
engage in sex until puberty. neous release of growth hormones, thyroid hormones,
and androgens stimulates rapid acceleration in height
and weight. This dramatic increase in stature is referred
RECAP to as the adolescent growth spurt. What is most in-
• Puberty has three main physical manifestations: a credible about the adolescent growth spurt is not so
rapid acceleration in growth, the development of much the absolute gain of height and weight that typi-
primary sex characteristics, and the development cally occurs but the speed with which the increases take
of secondary sex characteristics. place. Think for a moment of how quickly very young
• Puberty is regulated by a hormonal feedback loop children grow. At the time of peak height velocity—
that includes the hypothalamus, the pituitary gland, the time at which the adolescent is growing most rap-
and the gonads. idly—he or she is growing at the same rate as a toddler.
• Many important changes at puberty involve a For boys, peak height velocity averages about 4 inches
process called adrenarche, which involves the reg- (10.3 centimeters) per year; for girls, it’s about 3.5
ulation of the adrenal gland rather than the inches (9.0 centimeters). One marker of the conclusion
gonads. of puberty is the closing of the ends of the long bones
• The onset of puberty is triggered by several factors, in the body, a process called epiphysis, which termi-
including a biological “alarm” that is coded in the nates growth in height. Puberty is also a time of sig-
individual’s genes and various environmental sig- nificant increase in weight—nearly half of one’s adult
nals, which let the body know that it is ready to body weight is gained during adolescence (Susman &
begin reproduction. Dorn, 2009).
• Physical and behavioral changes at puberty result Figure 1.3 shows just how remarkable the growth
from two different hormonal processes: organiza- spurt is in terms of height. The graph on the left shows
tional, which take place long before adoles- changes in absolute height and indicates, as you would
cence, and activational, which result expect, that the average individual grows throughout
from changes in hormones infancy, childhood, and adolescence. As you can see,
when puberty there is little gain in height after age 18. But look now at
takes place. the right-hand graph, which shows the average increase
in height per year (i.e., the rate of change) over the same
age span. Here you can see the acceleration in height at
the time of peak height velocity.
Figure 1.3 also indicates that the growth spurt oc-
adolescent growth spurt The dramatic increase in height and curs, on average, about two years earlier among girls
weight that occurs during puberty. than boys. In general, as you can see by comparing the
two graphs, boys tend to be somewhat taller than girls
peak height velocity The point at which the adolescent is
growing most rapidly. before age 11; then girls tend to be taller than boys be-
tween ages 11 and 13; and finally, boys tend to be taller
epiphysis The closing of the ends of the bones, which terminates than girls from about age 14 on. You may remember
growth after the adolescent growth spurt has been completed.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 27 21/03/13 9:23 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 27

(a) (b)
24
22
190
180 Boy 20
170 18
Girl
160

Height gain (cm/year)


150 16
140 14
Height (cm)

130
12
120
110 10 Boy
100 8 Girl
90
6
80
70 4
60
2
50
1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19
Age Age
FIGURE 1.3 (a) Height (in centimeters) at different ages for the average male and female youngster. (b) Gain in height
per year (in centimeters) for the average male and female youngster. Note the adolescent growth spurt. (Adapted from
Marshall, 1978)

what this was like during the fifth and sixth grades. Sex young adolescent, who may be embarrassed by the
differences in height can be a concern for many young disproportionate growth of different body parts. It is
adolescents when they begin socializing with members probably little consolation for the young adolescent
of the opposite sex, especially if they are tall, early- going through the awkward phase to be told that an at-
maturing girls or short, late-maturing boys. tractive balance probably will be restored within a few
years, but fortunately, this is what usually happens.

❚ Sex Differences in Muscle and Fat The spurt


MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
in height during adolescence is accompanied by an in-
Think back to a time when the girls in your crease in weight that results from an increase in both
elementary school class tended to be muscle and fat, but there are important sex differences
taller than the boys. What impact, if any, in body composition. Before puberty, there are rela-
did this have on relationships between the tively few sex differences in muscle development and
sexes? only slight sex differences in body fat. In both sexes,
muscular development is rapid during puberty, but
muscle tissue grows faster in boys than girls (Bogin,
2011) (see Figure 1.4). In contrast, body fat increases
Much of the height gain during puberty results from for both sexes during puberty, but more so for females
an increase in torso length rather than in leg length. The than for males, especially during the years just before
sequence in which various parts of the body grow is puberty. For boys, there is actually a slight decline in
fairly regular. Extremities—the head, hands, and feet— body fat just before puberty. The end result of these
are the first to accelerate in growth. Then accelerated sex differences is that boys finish adolescence with a
growth occurs in the arms and legs, followed by torso muscle-to-fat ratio of about 3 to 1, but the comparable
and shoulder growth. ratio for girls is approximately 5 to 4. This has impor-
Young adolescents often appear to be out of propor- tant implications for understanding why sex differences
tion physically—as though their nose or legs were in strength and athletic ability often appear for the first
growing faster than the rest of them. This is because time during adolescence. According to one estimate,
different parts of the body do not all grow at the same about half of the sex difference in athletic performance
rate or at the same time during puberty. This can lead during early adolescence results simply from the differ-
to an appearance of awkwardness or gawkiness in the ence in body fat (Smoll & Schutz, 1990).
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 28 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

28 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

180 ❚ Body Dissatisfaction Among Adolescent


Boys
Girls The rapid increase in body fat that occurs
Girls
170 among females in early adolescence frequently
Stature
prompts young girls to become overly concerned
160 about their weight—even when their weight is
Stature (cm)

25 within the normal range for their height and age


150 24 (Smolak, Levine, & Gralen, 1993). As you will read

Lean arm circumference (cm)


23 later in this chapter, many studies point to adoles-
140 Lean arm 22 cence as the period of greatest risk for the devel-
circum- 21 opment of eating disorders such as anorexia and
ference bulimia.
130 20
Although the majority of girls diet unneces-
19
sarily during this time in response to the increase
120 18
in body fat, the girls who are most susceptible to
17
feelings of dissatisfaction with their bodies dur-
30 16
ing this phase of development are those who ma-
28 15
ture early, begin dating early, and come from
26 14
Skinfold (mm)

relatively more affluent families (Smolak et al.,


24 Sum of 3
skinfolds 1993). Girls who spend a lot of time talking
22 about their looks with their friends are especially
20 vulnerable to feelings of body dissatisfaction. In
18 fact, for girls, it is comparing themselves with
16 their friends, and not just being exposed to me-
14 dia portrayals of thinness, that leads to dissatis-
6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
faction (Carlson Jones, 2004). In contrast, boys’
Age (years)
feelings about how they look revolve around
FIGURE 1.4 Sex differences in body composition after puberty, on how muscular they are and do not seem to be
average leave males taller, leaner, and more muscular than females.
affected by comparisons with peers. There are
(Bogin, 2011)
also important ethnic and cross-cultural differ-
ences in the ways in which adolescent girls feel about
It is tempting to attribute sex differences in athletic their changing bodies (Jones & Smolak, 2011; Yates,
performance purely to changes in body fat and to Edman, & Aruguete, 2004). In many parts of the
hormonal factors, because androgens, which increase world, including North and South America, Europe,
during puberty in males at a much faster rate than in and Asia, there is strong pressure on girls to be thin
females, are closely linked to growth in aspects of the (Jones & Smolak, 2011). Black adolescents seem less
body that influence athletic ability. But with age, envi- vulnerable to these feelings of body dissatisfaction
ronmental factors like diet and exercise become in- than other girls (Nishina, Ammon, Bellmore, &
creasingly important influences on sex differences in Graham, 2006), and consequently they are less likely
physical performance (Smoll & Schutz, 1990). There to diet, in part because of ethnic differences in con-
are strong social pressures on girls to curtail “mascu- ceptions of the ideal body type (Granberg, Simons, &
line” activities—including some forms of exercise—at Simons, 2009).
adolescence, and studies show that girls are more likely
than boys to markedly reduce their physical activity in
preadolescence, with a very large proportion of adoles-
cent girls failing to meet national guidelines for exercise
(Goran et al., 1998; Savage & Scott, 1998). Moreover,
adolescent girls’ diets, especially those of Black girls, are MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
generally less adequate nutritionally than the diets of Given the nature of sex differences in the physical
boys, particularly in important minerals like iron changes of puberty, is body dis-
(Johnson, Johnson, Wang, Smiciklas-Wright, & Guthrie, satisfaction inevitable among
1994). Both factors could result in sex differences in adolescent girls? Can we
exercise tolerance. In other words, sex differences in learn anything from studies
physical ability are influenced by a variety of factors, of of ethnic or cross-cul-
which hormonal differences are but one part of a com- tural differences in body
plex picture. dissatisfaction that might
be helpful?
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 29 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 29

TABLE 1.1 The sequence of physical changes at puberty

Boys Girls
Age of First Age of First
Characteristic Appearance (Years) Characteristic Appearance (Years)

1. Growth of testes, 10–13 ½ 1. Growth of breasts 7–13


scrotal sac 2. Growth of pubic hair 7–14
2. Growth of pubic hair 10–15 3. Body growth 9 ½–14 ½
3. Body growth 10 ½–16 4. Menarche 10–16 ½
4. Growth of penis 11–14 ½ 5. Underarm hair About two years
5. Change in voice About the same time after pubic hair
(growth of larynx) as penis growth 6. Oil- and sweat- About same time
6. Facial and About two years after producing glands as underarm
underarm hair pubic hair appears hair
7. Oil- and sweat- About same time
producing glands, as underarm hair
acne

Source: B. Goldstein, 1976.

SEXUAL MATURATION In the male adolescent, the areola (the area around the
nipple) increases in size, and the nipple becomes more
Puberty brings with it a series of developments associ-
prominent. Some boys show a slight enlargement of
ated with sexual maturation. In both boys and girls,
the breast, although in the majority of cases this is
the development of the secondary sex characteristics
temporary.
is typically divided into five stages, often called Tanner
Other, internal changes occur that are important el-
stages, after the British pediatrician who devised the
ements of sexual maturation. At the time that the penis
categorization system.
develops, the seminal vesicles, the prostate, and the bul-
bourethral glands also enlarge and develop. The first
❚ Sexual Maturation in Boys The sequence of
ejaculation of seminal fluid generally occurs about one
developments in secondary sex characteristics among
year after the beginning of accelerated penis growth,
boys is fairly orderly (see Table 1.1). Generally, the first
although this is often determined culturally rather than
stages of puberty involve growth of the testes and scro-
biologically, since for many boys first ejaculation oc-
tum, accompanied by the first appearance of pubic
curs as a result of masturbation (J. Tanner, 1972). One
hair. Approximately one year later, the growth spurt in
interesting observation about the timing and sequence
height begins, accompanied by growth of the penis
of pubertal changes in boys is that boys are generally
and further development of pubic hair—now of a
fertile (i.e., capable of fathering a child) before they
coarser texture and darker color. The five Tanner stages
have developed an adultlike appearance (Bogin, 2011).
of penis and pubic hair growth in boys are shown in
As you will read in the next section, the opposite is true
Figure 1.5.
for girls.
The emergence of facial and body hair are relatively
late developments. The same is true for the deepening
❚ Sexual Maturation in Girls The sequence of
of the voice, which is gradual and generally does not
development of secondary sex characteristics among
occur until very late adolescence. During puberty, there
girls (shown in Table 1.1) is less regular than it is among
are changes in the skin as well. The skin becomes
boys. Generally, the first sign of sexual maturation in
rougher, especially around the upper arms and thighs,
girls is the elevation of the breast—the emergence of
and there is increased development of the sweat glands,
which often gives rise to acne, pimples, and increased
oiliness of the skin.
During puberty, there are slight changes in the
male breast—to the embarrassment of many boys.
Breast development is largely influenced by the estro- secondary sex characteristics The manifestations of sexual
gen hormones. As noted earlier, both estrogens and maturity at puberty, including the development of breasts, the
androgens are present in both sexes and increase in growth of facial and body hair, and changes in the voice.
both sexes at puberty, although in differing amounts. Tanner stages A widely used system that describes the five stages
of pubertal development.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 30 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

30 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Penis and Scrotum


Penis
Scrotum
Glans (Head)

Testes
1 2 3 4 5
Stage 1: The infantile state that persists from birth until puberty begins. During this time
the genitalia increase slightly in overall size but there is little change in general appearance.
Stage 2: The scrotum has begun to enlarge, and there is some reddening and change in
texture of the scrotal skin.
Stage 3: The penis has increased in length and there is a smaller increase in breadth. There
has been further growth of the scrotum.
Stage 4: The length and breadth of the penis have increased further and the glans has
developed. The scrotum is further enlarged and the scrotal skin has become darker.
Stage 5: The genitalia are adult in size and shape. The appearance of the genitalia may
satisfy the criteria for one of these stages for a considerable time before the penis and
scrotum are sufficiently developed to be classified as belonging to the next stage.

Pubic Hair

1 2 3 4 5
Stage 1: There is no true pubic hair, although there may be a fine velus over the pubes
similar to that over other parts of the abdomen.
Stage 2: Sparse growth of lightly pigmented hair, which is usually straight or only slightly
curled. This usually begins at either side of the base of the penis.
Stage 3: The hair spreads over the pubic symphysis and is considerably darker and coarser
and usually more curled.
Stage 4: The hair is now adult in character but covers an area considerably smaller than in
most adults. There is no spread to the medial surface of the thighs.
Stage 5: The hair is distributed in an inverse triangle as in the female. It has spread to the
medial surface of the thighs but not elsewhere above the base of the triangle.

FIGURE 1.5 The five pubertal stages for penile and pubic hair growth. (From Morris & Udry, 1980)

the so-called breast bud. In about one-third of all ado- reason, changes in the shape and definition of the are-
lescent girls, however, the appearance of pubic hair pre- ola and nipple are far better indicators of sexual matu-
cedes breast development. The development of pubic ration among adolescent girls than is breast growth
hair in females follows a sequence similar to that in alone. The five Tanner stages of breast and pubic hair
males—generally, from sparse, downy, light-colored growth in girls are shown in Figure 1.6.
hair to denser, curlier, coarser, darker hair. Breast devel- As is the case for boys, puberty brings important in-
opment often occurs concurrently and generally pro- ternal changes for adolescent girls that are associated
ceeds through several stages. In the bud stage, the areola with the development of reproductive capacity. In girls,
widens, and the breast and nipple are elevated as a small these changes involve development and growth of the
mound. In the middle stages, the areola and nipple be- uterus, vagina, and other aspects of the reproductive
come distinct from the breast and project beyond the system. In addition, there is enlargement of the labia
breast contour. In the final stages, the areola is recessed and clitoris.
to the contour of the breast, and only the nipple is ele- As is apparent in Table 1.1, the growth spurt is
vated. The female breast undergoes these changes at likely to occur during the early and middle stages of
puberty regardless of changes in breast size. For this breast and pubic hair development. Menarche, the
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 31 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 31

Breasts
1
Stage 1: No breast development.

Stage 2: The first sign of breast development


has appeared. This stage is sometimes
2 referred to as the breast budding
stage. Some palpable breast tissue
under the nipple; the flat area of the
nipple (areola) may be somewhat
enlarged.

3 Stage 3: The breast is more distinct although


there is no separation between
contours of the two breasts.

Stage 4: The breast is further enlarged and


4 there is greater contour distinction.
The nipple, including the areola, forms
a secondary mound on the breast.

Stage 5: Size may vary in the mature stage. The


5 breast is fully developed. The contours
are distinct and the areola has receded
into the general contour of the breast.

Pubic Hair
1 2 3 Stage 1: No pubic hair.
Stage 2: There is a small amount of long pubic
hair chiefly along the vaginal lips.
Stage 3: The hair is darker, coarser, and curlier
and spreads sparsely over the skin
around the vaginal lips.
4 5
Stage 4: The hair is now adult in type, but the
area covered is smaller than in most
adults. There is no pubic hair on the
inside of the thighs.
Stage 5: The hair is adult in type, distributed as
an inverse triangle. There may be hair
on the inside of the thighs.

FIGURE 1.6 The five pubertal stages for breast and pubic hair growth. (From Marshall & Tanner, 1969)

beginning of menstruation, is a relatively late devel-


MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
opment that reflects the culmination of a long series
Think about the changes in secondary
of hormonal changes (Dorn et al., 1999). Generally,
sex characteristics that take place at
full reproductive function does not occur until several
puberty in humans. Why might we have
years after menarche, and regular ovulation follows
evolved so that puberty occurs outside
menarche by about two years (Bogin, 2011; Hoch-
as well as inside the body?
berg, Gawlik, & Walker, 2011). Unlike boys, therefore,
girls generally appear physically mature before they
are fertile.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 32 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

32 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

RECAP far more strongly correlated with height and weight


• The adolescent growth spurt, which takes place during childhood than with the timing of puberty,
about two years earlier among girls than boys, however (Pietiläinen et al., 2001).
involves a rapid increase in height and weight. Within the United States, there are ethnic differences
• Puberty brings with it changes in ratio of muscle to in the timing and rate of pubertal maturation. Several
fat, with marked gender differences in the way in large-scale studies of U.S. youngsters indicate that
which body composition changes. The increase in Black females mature significantly earlier than Mexican
fat, which is greater among females than males, is a American girls, who, in turn, mature earlier than White
source of concern to many girls. girls (Chumlea et al., 2003; Herman-Giddens et al.,
• Sexual maturation in both sexes is measured 1997). Although the reasons for this ethnic difference
according to Tanner stages, which are used are not known, it does not appear to be due to ethnic
to chart the development of differences in income, weight, or area of residence
secondary sex (S. E. Anderson, Dallal, & Must, 2003). One possible
characteristics. explanation for the earlier maturation of non-White
girls is that they may be more frequently exposed to
chemicals in the environment that stimulate earlier pu-
berty, such as animal hormones contained in certain
hair care products, although this has not been studied
systematically (Susman & Dorn, 2009).
The Timing and Tempo
of Puberty GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL
INFLUENCES ON PUBERTAL TIMING
You may have noted that, thus far, no mention has been
made of the “normal” ages at which various pubertal Why do some individuals mature relatively early and
changes are likely to take place. This is because varia- others relatively late? Researchers who study variability
tions in the timing of puberty (the age at which puberty in the onset and timing of puberty approach the issue in
begins) and in the tempo of puberty (the rate at which two ways. One strategy involves the study of differences
maturation occurs) are so great that it is misleading to among individuals (i.e., studying why one individual
talk even about average ages. matures earlier or faster than another). The other in-
volves the study of differences among groups of adoles-
cents (i.e., studying why puberty occurs earlier or more
VARIATIONS IN THE TIMING
rapidly in certain populations than in others). Both sets
AND TEMPO OF PUBERTY
of studies point to both genetic and environmental fac-
The onset of puberty can occur as early as age 7 in girls tors (Ge, Natsuaki, Neiderhiser, & Reiss, 2007).
and 9½ in boys, or as late as age 13 in girls and 13½ in
boys. In girls, the interval between the first sign of pu- ❚ Individual Differences in Pubertal Maturation
berty and complete physical maturation can be as short Differences in the timing and rate of puberty among
as a year and a half or as long as six years. In boys, the individuals growing up in the same general environ-
comparable interval ranges from about two to five years ment result chiefly, but not exclusively, from genetic
(J. Tanner, 1972). Think about it: Within a totally normal factors. Comparisons between individuals who are ge-
population of young adolescents, some individuals will netically identical (identical twins) and individuals
have completed the entire sequence of pubertal changes who are not indicate that the timing and tempo of an
before others have even begun. In more concrete terms, individual’s pubertal maturation are largely inherited
it is possible for an early-maturing, fast-maturing (Mustanski, Viken, Kaprio, Pulkkinen, & Rose, 2004). A
youngster to complete pubertal maturation by age 10 or specific region on chromosome 6 has been identified as
11—two years before a late-maturing youngster has even one of the markers for pubertal timing in both boys
begun puberty, and seven years before a late-maturing, and girls (Bogin, 2011).
slow-maturing youngster has matured completely! Despite this powerful influence of genetic factors,
There is no relation between the age at which pu- the environment plays an important role. In all likeli-
berty begins and the rate at which pubertal develop- hood, every individual inherits a predisposition to de-
ment proceeds. The timing of puberty may have a small velop at a certain rate and to begin pubertal maturation
effect on one’s ultimate height or weight, however, with at a certain time. But this predisposition is best thought
late maturers, on average, being taller than early matur- of as an upper and lower age limit, not a fixed absolute.
ers as adults, and early maturers, on average, being Whether the genetic predisposition that each person
somewhat heavier—at least among females (St. George, has to mature around a given age is actually realized,
Williams, & Silva, 1994). Adult height and weight are and when within the predisposed age boundaries she or
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 33 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 33

he actually goes through puberty, is subject to the influ- Although it may seem surprising that something as
ence of many external factors. In this respect, the tim- biological as puberty can be influenced by factors in the
ing and rate of pubertal maturation are the product of social environment, scientists have long known that our
an interaction between nature and nurture, between social relationships can indeed affect our biological
one’s genetic makeup and the environmental condi- functioning. One of the best-known examples of this is
tions under which one has developed. that women who live together—such as dormitory
By far the two most important environmental influ- roommates—find that their menstrual periods begin to
ences on pubertal maturation are nutrition and health. synchronize over time (C. Graham, 1991; McClintock,
Puberty occurs earlier among individuals who are bet- 1980).
ter nourished and grow more throughout their prena-
tal, infant, and childhood years (Terry, Ferris, Tehranifar, ❚ Group Differences in Pubertal Maturation
Wei, & Flom, 2009). Not surprisingly, then, whereas Researchers typically study group differences in puberty
girls who are taller or heavier than their peers mature by comparing average ages of menarche in different re-
earlier (St. George et al., 1994), delayed puberty is more gions. Most of these studies have indicated that genetic
likely to occur among individuals with a history of pro- factors play an extremely small role in determining
tein and/or caloric deficiency. Chronic illness during group differences in pubertal maturation (Eveleth &
childhood and adolescence is also associated with de- Tanner, 1990). Differences among countries in the aver-
layed puberty, as is excessive exercise. For example, girls age rate and timing of puberty are more likely to reflect
in ballet companies or in other rigorous training pro- differences in their environments than differences in
grams often mature later than their peers (Frisch, 1983). their populations’ gene pools (Bogin, 2011).
Generally, then, after genetic factors, the most impor- The influence of the broader environment on the
tant determinant of the timing of puberty is the overall timing and tempo of puberty can be seen in more con-
physical well-being of the individual from conception crete terms by looking at two sets of findings: (1) com-
through preadolescence (Susman & Dorn, 2009). parisons of the average age of menarche across countries
and (2) changes in the average age of menarche over
❚ Familial Influences on Pubertal Timing In- time. (Although menarche does not signal the onset of
terestingly, a number of studies suggest that social fac- puberty, researchers often use the average age of men-
tors in the home environment may influence the onset arche when comparing the timing of puberty across
of maturation, especially in girls; in contrast, boys’ pu- different groups or regions, because it can be measured
bertal maturation has not been consistently linked to more reliably than other indicators.)
family functioning. Puberty occurs somewhat earlier First, consider variations in the age of menarche
among girls who grew up in father-absent families, in across different regions of the world. Menarche gener-
less cohesive or more conflict-ridden households, or ally is earlier in countries where individuals are less
with a stepfather (Ellis, 2004); early puberty is also likely to be malnourished or to suffer from chronic dis-
more common among girls who were physically or ease (Bogin, 2011). For example, in western Europe and
sexually abused during childhood (Mendle, Leve, Van in the United States, the median age of menarche ranges
Ryzin, Natsuaki, & Ge, 2011; Trickett, Noll, & Putnam, from about 12½ to 13½ years. In Africa, however, the
2011). One explanation for the finding that family median age ranges from about 14 to 17 years. The range
conflict may accelerate pubertal maturation is that is much wider across Africa because of the greater varia-
tension in the family may induce stress, which, in turn, tion in environmental conditions there.
may affect hormonal secretions in the adolescent
(Arim, Tramonte, Shapka, Dahinten, & Willms, 2011; MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
Belsky, Steinberg, Houts, & Halpern-Felsher, 2010; Puberty occurs much earlier in industri-
Belsky et al., 2007), especially among girls who are ge- alized countries than in developing
netically susceptible to this influence (Ellis, Shirtcliff, ones. How might this make adoles-
Boyce, Deardorff, & Essex, 2011; Manuck, Craig, Flory, cence vary around the globe?
Halder, & Ferrell, 2011). In addition, the presence of a
stepfather may expose the adolescent girl to phero-
mones (a class of chemicals secreted by animals that
stimulate certain behaviors in other members of the
❚ The Secular Trend We can also examine envi-
species) that stimulate pubertal maturation. In gen-
ronmental influences on the timing of puberty by look-
eral, among humans and other mammals, living in
ing at changes in the average age of menarche over the
proximity to one’s close biological relatives appears to
past two centuries. Because nutritional conditions have
slow the process of pubertal maturation, whereas
exposure to unrelated members of the other sex may
accelerate it. pheromones A class of chemicals secreted by animals that
stimulate certain behaviors in other members of the species.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 34 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

34 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

FIGURE 1.7 The age at menarche


has declined considerably over the Germany
past 150 years. This decline is known 17.0 Norway
as the secular trend. (Adapted from Sweden
Eveleth & Tanner, 1990) Denmark
United States
16.0 Finland

Age at menarche (years)


15.0

14.0
14.0

13.5

13.0
13.0

1950 1960 1970


12.0
1840 1860 1880 1900 1920 1940 1960 1980
Year of menarche

improved during the past 150 years, we would expect to The average age of menarche among American girls
find a decline in the average age at menarche over time. dropped by about two and a half months between the
This is indeed the case, as can be seen in Figure 1.7. This 1960s and 1990s, most probably because of increased
pattern, known as the secular trend, is attributable not rates of obesity (Salsberry, Regan, and Pajer, 2009); ex-
only to improved nutrition but also to better sanitation posure to certain man-made chemicals in cosmetics,
and better control of infectious diseases. In most Euro- food, and the environment that affect hormonal devel-
pean countries, maturation has become earlier by about opment (Hochberg et al., 2011); and changes in diet
three to four months per decade. For example, in Norway (Rogers et al., 2010). The decline has been especially
150 years ago, the average age of menarche may have marked among Black girls (Salsberry et al., 2009). Sci-
been about 17 years. Today, it is between 12 and 13 years. entists disagree about whether this trend toward ear-
Similar declines have been observed over the same pe- lier female puberty has continued in the United States
riod in other industrialized nations and, more recently, in recent years (Susman & Dorn, 2009). There is evi-
in developing countries as well (Ma et al., 2009). The dence of a continued decline in the age of puberty
secular trend is less well documented among boys, in among girls as indexed by breast development and
part because there is no easily measured marker of pu- menarche, but not as indexed by genital maturation or
berty, like menarche. One unusual factoid that is consis- the appearance of pubic hair (Aksglaede, Sørensen,
tent with the decline in the age of puberty among boys Petersen, Skakkebæk, & Juul, 2009; Biro et al., 2010).
over many centuries, though, is the observation that the One reason for these discrepancies is that puberty can
average age at which boys experience their voice breaking be measured in many ways (Dorn, Dahl, Woodward, &
(a sign of male pubertal development), based on reports Biro, 2006), and what we conclude about the average
from European children’s choirs, dropped from about 18 age of puberty depends on what indicator is used. Be-
in the mid-1700s to around 10½ today (Mendle & Fer- cause the development of pubic hair (which is influ-
rero, 2012). The drop in the age of male puberty appears enced by androgens) is affected by different factors
to be continuing, and has fallen during the past three than is menarche (which is influenced by estrogen and
decades (Herman-Giddens et al., 2012). progesterone), patterns of change over time in each
would not necessarily be identical, especially if expo-
sure to environmental chemicals was driving the
change, since the same chemicals can have different
secular trend The tendency, over the past two centuries, for effects on different hormones (Biro, Huang et al.,
individuals to be larger in stature and to reach puberty earlier, 2006). In general, though, most scientists agree that
primarily because of improvements in health and nutrition.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 35 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 35

any changes in the average age of puberty have been


much less dramatic in recent decades than they were in
The Psychological and
the early 20th century, when the average age of men- Social Impact of Puberty
arche dropped by about four months every decade
Puberty can affect the adolescent’s behavior and psy-
(Rigon et al., 2010).
chological functioning in a number of ways (Brooks-
Gunn, Graber, & Paikoff, 1994) (see Figure 1.8). First,
the biological changes of puberty can have a direct ef-
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION fect on behavior. Increases in testosterone at puberty
Some studies indicate that the secular are directly linked, for example, to an increase in sex
trend has been more dramatic among drive and sexual activity among adolescent boys
females than males. Why might this (Halpern, Udry, & Suchindran, 1996). (The impact of
be the case? hormonal change on girls’ sex drive and sexual activity
is more complicated, as you’ll see in Chapter 11.)
Second, the biological changes of puberty cause
changes in the adolescent’s self-image, which, in turn,
may affect how he or she behaves. For example, a boy
who has recently gone through puberty may feel more
RECAP grown up as a result of his more adultlike appearance.
• The timing of puberty, as well as the rate at which This, in turn, may make him seek more independence
it occurs, is highly variable. from his parents. He may ask for a later curfew, a larger
• The timing of puberty is influenced by both genetic allowance, or the right to make decisions about things
and environmental factors. Among the most that previously were decided by his parents. As we will
important environmental influences are health and see later in this chapter, the physical changes of puberty
nutrition. Generally, individuals who are healthier often spark conflict between teenagers and their par-
and better nourished begin puberty at a relatively ents, in part because of the ways in which puberty af-
younger age than their less healthy or more poorly fects the adolescent’s desire for autonomy.
nourished peers. Finally, biological change at puberty transforms the
• Some evidence suggests that girls growing up adolescent’s appearance, which, in turn, may elicit
in single-parent families, stepfamilies, or families changes in how others react to the teenager. These
in which there is a high level of conflict go changes in reactions may provoke changes in the ado-
through puberty somewhat earlier than their lescent’s behavior. An adolescent girl who has recently
peers. matured physically may find herself suddenly receiving
• The age at which puberty occurs has declined the attention of older boys who had not previously paid
over time, a phenomenon known as the “secular her much heed. She may feel nervous about all the extra
trend.” This trend continues today in attention and confused about how she should respond
the United States and other to it. Moreover, she must now make decisions about
industrialized how much time she wishes to devote to dating and how
countries. she should behave when out on a date.
Young people’s reactions to the changes brought on
by puberty, and others’ reactions to them, are influenced

FIGURE 1.8 The biological


Changes in
changes of puberty can Hormonal
adolescent’s
affect the adolescent’s changes
behavior
behavior in at least three
ways.
Changes in Changes in Changes in
Hormonal
adolescent’s adolescent’s adolescent’s
changes
appearance self-image behavior

Changes in Changes in Changes in


Hormonal
adolescent’s reactions of adolescent’s
changes
appearance others behavior
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 36 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

36 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

by the broader social environment, in which messages you will see, however, the short-term consequences of
about physical attractiveness, sexuality, and sexual matu- puberty may be more taxing on the adolescent’s family
ration change, often markedly, from era to era. Although than on the adolescent.
it is difficult to imagine an era in which adolescents, es-
pecially girls, did not obsess about their shape, size, and ❚ Puberty and Self-Esteem Although research
sexual allure, adolescent females’ preoccupation with suggests that puberty can be a potential stressor with
their body is a relatively recent phenomenon, created temporary adverse psychological consequences, this is
largely by marketers of clothing, underwear, cosmetics, true only when it is coupled with other changes that
weight loss programs, and “feminine” products (Brum- necessitate adjustment. In this respect, the impact of
berg, 1997). Contemporary society’s views of puberty puberty on adolescents’ psychological functioning is to
and physical maturation are expressed through televi- a great extent shaped by the social context in which pu-
sion commercials, newspaper and magazine advertise- berty takes place (Susman & Dorn, 2009). The impact
ments, and depictions of young adolescents in films and of puberty on mental health varies by gender and across
other media. People cannot help but be influenced by ethnic groups, with girls more adversely affected than
these images, and the expectations they associate with boys, and with White girls, in particular, at greatest risk
puberty, as well as the meaning they give it, determine for developing a poor body image (J. Siegel, Yancey,
the reactions puberty brings out in them. Aneshensel, & Schuler, 1999). Given the premium in
Researchers have generally taken two approaches to contemporary American society placed on thinness,
studying the psychological and social consequences of the increase in body dissatisfaction among White girls
puberty. One approach is to look at individuals who are that takes place at puberty is, not surprisingly, linked
at various stages of puberty, either in a cross-sectional to specific concerns that girls have about their hips,
study (in which groups of individuals are compared at thighs, waist, and weight (Rosenblum & Lewis, 1999).
different stages of puberty) or in a longitudinal study Interestingly, the way adolescents feel about their phys-
(in which the same individuals are tracked over time as ical appearance when they begin adolescence remains
they move through the different stages of puberty). remarkably stable over time, regardless of whether their
Studies of this sort examine the impact of puberty on actual attractiveness changes (Rosenblum & Lewis,
young people’s psychological development and social 1999).
relations. Researchers might ask, for example, whether
youngsters’ self-esteem is higher or lower during pu- ❚ Puberty and Adolescent Moodiness Although
berty than before or after. an adolescent’s self-image can be expected to change
A second approach compares the psychological de- during a time of dramatic physical development, as you
velopment of early and late maturers. As noted previ- will read in Chapter 8, self-esteem or self-image is rea-
ously, there is large variation in pubertal timing, and sonably stable over time, with long and sturdy roots
individuals of the same chronological age, who are in reaching back to childhood. For this reason, some re-
the same grade in school, may be at very different stages searchers have turned their attention to the impact of
of puberty. Researchers have been tremendously inter- puberty on more transient states, such as mood. One
ested in whether the timing of puberty affects the ado- reason for this focus is that adolescents are thought to
lescent’s psychological development. The focus of these be moodier, on average, than either children or adults.
studies is not so much on the absolute impact of pu- One study, in which adolescents’ moods were moni-
berty as on the effects of differential timing of the tored repeatedly by electronic pagers, for example,
changes. Here, a typical question might be whether showed that their moods fluctuate during the course of
early maturers are more popular in the peer group than the day more than the moods of adults do (Csikszent-
late maturers are. mihalyi & Larson, 1984). Many adults assume that
adolescent moodiness is directly related to the hor-
monal changes of puberty (C. Buchanan, Eccles, &
THE IMMEDIATE IMPACT OF PUBERTY
Becker, 1992). Is there any scientific evidence that the
Studies of the psychological and social impacts of pu- hormonal changes of puberty cause adolescents to be
berty indicate that physical maturation, regardless of moody or, for that matter, that these hormonal changes
whether it occurs early or late, affects the adolescent’s affect the adolescent’s psychological functioning or
self-image, mood, and relationships with parents. As behavior at all?
According to several comprehensive reviews of re-
search on hormones and adolescent mood and behav-
ior, the direct connection between hormones and mood
cross-sectional study A study that compares two or more groups
of individuals at one point in time. is not very strong (C. Buchanan et al., 1992; Flannery,
Torquati, & Lindemeier, 1994). When studies do find a
longitudinal study A study that follows the same group of connection between hormonal changes at puberty and
individuals over time.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 37 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 37

Johnson, Granger, Crouter, & McHale, 2003; Susman,


1997). For example, although rapid increases in hor-
mones early in puberty are associated with depressed
mood in girls, it turns out that stressful life events, such
as problems in the family, in school, or with friends,
play a far greater role in the development of depression
than do hormonal changes (Brooks-Gunn, Graber, &
Paikoff, 1994). Similarly, while high levels of testoster-
one have been associated with impulsivity and aggres-
sion and low levels with depression, these associations
are weaker among adolescents who have positive family
relationships (A. Booth et al., 2003).
In other words, there is there little evidence that
adolescents’ moodiness results exclusively from the
“storm and stress” of raging hormones. Over the course
of a day, a teenager may shift from elation to boredom,
back to happiness, and then to anger. But these shifts
in mood appear to have more to do with shifts in
activities—elated when seeing a girlfriend, bored in so-
cial studies class, happy when having lunch with friends,
and angry when assigned extra work at the fast-food
restaurant—than with internal, biological changes
(Schneiders et al., 2006).

❚ Puberty and Changes in Patterns of Sleep


One fascinating finding on hormones and behavior
in adolescence concerns adolescents’ sleep preferences.
Many parents complain that their teenage children go
Contrary to widespread belief, there is little evidence that the to bed too late in the evening and sleep in too late in
hormonal changes of puberty contribute in a dramatic way to the morning, a pattern that begins to emerge in early
adolescent moodiness. adolescence (see Figure 1.9). It now appears that the
emergence of this pattern—called a delayed phase
adolescent mood or behavior, the effects are strongest preference—is driven by the biological changes of pu-
early in puberty, when the process is being “turned on” berty, and it is seen not only in humans, but in other
and when hormonal levels are highly variable. As we mammals as well (Carskadon, 2011).
will see in Chapter 2, there is increasing evidence that Falling asleep is caused by a combination of biologi-
pubertal hormones affect brain systems responsible for cal and environmental factors. One of the most impor-
emotional arousal in ways that make adolescents more tant is the secretion of a hormone in the brain called
responsive to what is going on around them (Forbes, melatonin. Melatonin levels change naturally over the
Phillips, Silk, Ryan, & Dahl, 2011; Masten et al., 2011). course of the 24-hour day, mainly in response to the
For example, studies indicate that rapid increases in amount of light in the environment. Feelings of sleepi-
many of the hormones associated with puberty—such ness increase and decrease with melatonin levels—as
as testosterone, estrogen, and various adrenal andro- melatonin rises, we feel sleepier, and as it falls, we feel
gens—may be associated with increased irritability, im- more awake. Over the course of the day, we follow a
pulsivity, aggression (in boys) and depression (in girls), sleep–wake cycle that is calibrated to changes in light
especially when the increases take place very early in and regulated by melatonin secretion.
adolescence. One interpretation of these findings is During puberty, the time of night at which melatonin
that it is not so much the absolute increases in these levels begin to rise changes, becoming later and later as
hormones during puberty but their rapid fluctuation individuals mature physically. In fact, the nighttime in-
early in puberty that may affect adolescents’ moods. crease in melatonin starts about two hours later among
Once the hormone levels stabilize at higher levels,
later in puberty, their negative effects appear to wane
(C. Buchanan et al., 1992).
delayed phase preference A pattern of sleep characterized by
Most researchers agree that the impact of hormo- later sleep and wake times, which usually emerges during puberty.
nal change on mood and behavior in adolescence is
greatly influenced by environmental factors (A. Booth, melatonin A hormone secreted by the brain that contributes to
sleepiness.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 38 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

38 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

12:30 Falling asleep is affected by the environment as


Weekday bedtime well—as you know, it is much easier to fall asleep when
Weekend bedtime a room is dark than when it is bright. But when preado-
Bedtime (P.M.) and wake time (A.M.)

11:30
Weekday wake time lescents get into bed at night, they tend to fall asleep
Weekend wake time very quickly—even if there is something that they want
10:30
to stay up for—because their melatonin levels are al-
ready high. After going through puberty, though, be-
9:30 cause of the delayed timing of the increase in melatonin,
it is easier for individuals to stay up later, so that if there
8:30 is something more exciting to do—surf the Internet,
stream a YouTube, talk on the phone—it is not difficult
7:30 to remain awake (Taylor, Jenni, Acebo, & Carskadon,
2005), and some scientists believe that many adoles-
cents’ sleep deprivation stems directly from overuse
6:30
3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 of computers and cell phones (Punamäki, Wallenius,
Child’s age in years Nygård, Saarni, & Rimpelä, 2007), although the notion
that adolescents’ lack of sleep is due to overstimulation
FIGURE 1.9 Children’s weekday and weekend bedtimes
and wake times, by age. (Adapted from Snell et al., 2007)
at bedtime is at least 100 years old (Matricciani, Olds,
Blunden, Rigney, & Williams, 2012). Thus, the tendency
for adolescents to stay up late is due to the interaction
adolescents who have completed puberty than among of biology (which delays the onset of sleepiness) and
those who have not yet entered puberty (Carskadon & the environment (which provides an impetus to stay
Acebo, 2002). As a result of this shift, individuals become up). This shift in sleep preferences, to a later bedtime
able to stay up later before feeling sleepy. In fact, when and a later wake time, begins to reverse around age 20,
allowed to regulate their own sleep schedules (as on at a slightly earlier age among females than males (Frey,
weekends), most teenagers will stay up until around Balu, Greusing, Rothen, & Cajochen, 2009; Roenneberg
1:00 A.M. and sleep until about 10:00 A.M. Because the et al., 2004).
whole cycle of melatonin secretion is shifted later at pu- If getting up early the next day were not an issue,
berty, this also means that once adolescents have gone staying up late would not be a problem. Unfortu-
through puberty, they are more sleepy early in the morn- nately, most teenagers need to get up early on school
ing than they were when they were prepubertal. days, and the combination of staying up late and get-
ting up early leads to sleep deprivation and daytime
sleepiness. The shift in the timing of the
melatonin cycle contributes to this, be-
cause the decrease in melatonin that takes
place in the middle of the night happens
later after puberty than before. This
means that when teenagers get out of bed
early in the morning, their melatonin
levels are relatively higher than they are
at the same time of day for preadoles-
cents. Indeed, one study found that ado-
lescents were least alert between the hours
of 8:00 and 9:00 A.M. (when most schools
start) and were most alert after 3.00 P.M.,
when the school day is over (R. Allen &
Mirabell, 1990). Sleep researchers esti-
mate that, because of early school start
times, adolescents get two fewer hours
of sleep per night when the school year
begins than they did during the preced-
ing summer months (Hansen, Janssen,
Schiff, Zee, & Dubocovich, 2005). This
Important changes in the sleep cycle take place after puberty. This “delayed phase
has prompted some communities to de-
preference” causes adolescents to want to stay up later at night and makes them feel lay their school starting time for teenagers
more tired in the early morning hours. (Mitru, Millrood, & Mateika, 2002).
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 39 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 39

Interestingly, the tendency for individuals to go to Walker, 2009), more delinquency (Clinkenbeard, Simi,
bed later as they become teenagers has become stronger Evans, & Anderson, 2011), more frequent injuries
over the past 30 years (Iglowstein, Jenni, Molinari, & (Lam & Yang, 2007), being overweight (Seegers et al.,
Largo, 2003), perhaps because the availability of televi- 2011), and poorer school performance (Fredriksen et
sion, the Internet, and other mass media during late- al., 2004). In one study, students who were earning
night and early-morning hours has increased (Van den grades of C or lower were going to bed 40 minutes
Bulck, 2004). This suggests that the late-night hours later and sleeping about 25 minutes less each night
kept by many adolescents are at least somewhat volun- than their classmates whose grade point averages were
tary, made easier by the changes in the sleep centers of B or better (Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998). The stu-
the brain. (There is also evidence that exposure to light dents with poorer grades also reported staying up a lot
depresses melatonin secretion, so that staying up late later on weekends than they did on school nights;
with the lights on or even staring at computer or TV other studies find higher rates of problem behavior
screens will delay the rise in melatonin even more.) among adolescents who prefer to stay up later (Sus-
Whatever the explanation, because teenagers’ wake man et al., 2007).
time has not changed, but their bedtime has gotten Despite many adolescents’ belief that catching up
later, today’s teenagers get significantly less sleep than on their sleep on weekends will make up for sleep
their counterparts did several decades ago. Excessive deprivation during the week, research indicates that
daytime sleepiness is rampant among American teen- having markedly different bedtimes on weekends
agers, a finding that has been replicated in several coun- versus weekdays contributes to further sleep-related
tries, including Japan and Taiwan (Gau, Soong, & problems (Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998). (Actually,
Merikangas, 2004; Ohida et al., 2004). the best thing teenagers can do to avoid problems
Although individuals’ preferred bedtime gets later waking up on school days is to force themselves to
as they move from childhood into adolescence, the get up at the same time on the weekend as on school
amount of sleep they need each night remains con- days, regardless of how late they have stayed up.) Not
stant, at around nine hours. Yet, one study of more surprisingly, sleep-related difficulties among teenag-
than 14,000 American high school students found that ers are also linked to the consumption of caffeine
fewer than 10% of the students got at least nine hours (Pollack & Bright, 2003) and tobacco (Patten, Choi,
of sleep on an average school night and that more than Gillin, & Pierce, 2000), both of which are stimulants.
two-thirds got seven hours or less (Eaton et al., 2010) About 10% of teenagers report chronic insomnia
(see Figure 1.10). There is now a clear consensus (E. Johnson, Roth, Schultz, & Breslau, 2006), which
among scientists that most teenagers are not getting increases the risk of other psychological problems
enough sleep and that inadequate sleep is associated in (R. Roberts, Roberts, & Duong, 2008) and for con-
adolescence with poorer mental health (Fuligni & tinued sleep difficulties in adulthood (Dregan &
Hardway, 2006; Galambos, Dalton, & Maggs, 2009; Armstrong, 2010).

MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION


Some policymakers have called for delaying the time at
12th which the high school day begins, in view of new studies
on changing patterns of
sleep during adolescence.
11th Do you think this is a good
idea? Or should parents
just insist that their teenag-
10th ers go to sleep earlier?

9th
❚ Puberty and Family Relationships Research
0 20 40 60 80 100 into the impact of puberty on family relationships has
Percent pointed to a fairly consistent pattern, namely, that pu-
Optimal ($9 h) Borderline (8 h) berty appears to increase conflict and distance between
Insufficient (#7 h) parents and children. You should be aware, however,
FIGURE 1.10 Very few U.S. high school students get that the “distancing” effect of puberty on adolescent–
enough sleep. (Eaton et al., 2010) parent relationships is not as consistently observed in
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 40 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

40 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

ethnic minority families (Molina & Chassin, 1996; discussions. But as the children go through puberty,
Sagrestano, McCormick, Paikoff, & Holmbeck, 1999). they may want to be treated more like adults and have a
Among White families, however, several studies show greater say in family decisions. Consequently, families
that as youngsters mature from childhood toward the may experience a temporary period of conflict or ten-
middle of puberty, emotional distance between them sion when sons and daughters enter early adolescence.
and their parents increases, and conflict intensifies, es- It may take some time for the individual and the family
pecially between the adolescent and his or her mother to achieve a new equilibrium that takes into account the
(Laursen, Coy, & Collins, 1998; Ogletree, Jones, & Coyl, changes brought on by puberty.
2002). The change that takes place is reflected in an in-
crease in “negatives” (e.g., conflict, complaining, anger) ❚ Pubertal Maturation and Peer Relation-
and, to a lesser extent, a decrease in “positives” (e.g., ships Puberty may have an effect on relationships
support, smiling, laughter) (e.g., Flannery et al., 1994; in the peer group, too. Boys and girls who are physi-
Holmbeck & Hill, 1991). Although negative inter- cally mature are more likely than their less mature
changes may diminish after the adolescent growth peers to be involved in cross-sex romantic activities
spurt, adolescents and their parents do not immediately such as having a boyfriend or girlfriend or going out
become as close as they were before the adolescents en- on dates (Compian, Gowen, & Hayward, 2004), al-
tered puberty. Interestingly, puberty increases distance though this depends on the social norms of the ado-
between children and their parents in most species of lescent’s peer group and the prevailing expectations
monkeys and apes, and some writers have suggested about the age at which teenagers should begin dating
that the pattern seen in human adolescents may have (Gargiulo, Attie, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, 1987). Pu-
some evolutionary basis—it helps ensure that once they bertal maturation is not associated with having pla-
mature sexually, adolescents will leave home and mate tonic relationships with other-sex peers, however
outside the family (Steinberg & Belsky, 1996). (Compian et al., 2004).
Because this connection between pubertal matura-
tion and parent–child distance is not affected by the
THE IMPACT OF SPECIFIC
age at which the adolescent goes through puberty—in
PUBERTAL EVENTS
other words, the pattern is seen among early as well as
late maturers—it suggests that something about pu- Several studies have focused specifically on adoles-
berty in particular may transform the parent–child cents’ attitudes toward and reactions to particular
bond. To date, we do not know whether this effect re- events at puberty, such as girls’ reactions to menarche
sults from the hormonal changes of puberty (which or breast development and boys’ reactions to their first
may make young adolescents more testy), from changes ejaculation.
in the adolescent’s physical appearance (which may In general, most adolescents react positively to the
change the way parents treat their adolescent), or from biological changes associated with puberty, especially
changes in adolescent psychological functioning that, those associated with the development of secondary sex
in turn, affect family relationships. Moreover, because characteristics. One study of adolescent girls’ attitudes
few studies of family relationships at puberty have ex- toward breast development, for example, found that the
amined multiple aspects of adolescent development majority of girls greeted this change positively (Brooks-
simultaneously, it is difficult to say whether the pat- Gunn, Newman, Holderness, & Warren, 1994).
terns of change in family relationships that many stud- Adolescent girls’ attitudes toward menarche are less
ies have found do, in fact, result from puberty and not negative today than they appear to have been in the past
from some other change taking place at the same time (J. Lee, 2008), a change that may be attributable to the
in the adolescent (starting junior high school) or in the increase in information about menstruation provided
parent (going through a midlife crisis) (Paikoff & in schools and in the media (Merskin, 1999). In general,
Brooks-Gunn, 1991). among today’s adolescent girls, menarche is typically
Whatever underlying mechanism is involved, one accompanied by gains in social maturity, peer pres-
interpretation of these studies is that developments oc- tige, and self-esteem—as well as by heightened self-
curring around the time of puberty can upset interper- consciousness (Brooks-Gunn & Reiter, 1990). Never-
sonal balances that are established during childhood, theless, many young women have developed a negative
causing temporary periods of disruption in the family image of menstruation before reaching adolescence,
system. During a son’s or daughter’s childhood, fami- and they enter puberty with ambivalent attitudes about
lies develop patterns of relationships that are comfort- menarche—a mixture of excitement and fear (S. Moore,
able and workable, but they may find that puberty 1995). Generally speaking, girls whose mothers are
disrupts the patterns to which they have grown accus- helpful and matter-of-fact in their response to men-
tomed. They have developed a certain way of discussing arche report the most positive memories of the experi-
things and a certain way of including the children in ence (J. Lee, 2008).
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 41 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 41

Interestingly, one set of studies indicates that a


strong negative bias toward menstruation before men-
arche may actually be associated with greater menstrual
discomfort. Menstrual symptoms are reported to be
more severe among women who expect menstruation
to be uncomfortable, among girls whose mothers lead
them to believe that menstruation will be an unpleas-
ant or uncomfortable experience, and in cultures that
label menstruation as an important event. In Mexico
and in China, for example, where attitudes toward
menarche may be especially ambivalent, menarche may
have an adverse effect on girls’ mental health, an effect
not generally observed in the United States (Benjet &
Hernandez-Guzman, 2002; Tang, Yeung, & Lee, 2003).
In addition, girls who experience menarche early, rela-
tive to their peers, who are otherwise unprepared for
puberty report more negative reactions to the event
(Koff & Rierdan, 1996; Tang, Yeung, & Lee, 2004).
Because of wide variability in the timing and tempo of puberty,
Far less is known about boys’ reactions to their first
individuals of the same chronological age can have very different
ejaculation, an experience that we might consider anal- physical appearances.
ogous to menarche in girls. Although most boys are not
very well prepared for this event by their parents or
other adults, first ejaculation does not appear to cause Indeed, adolescents’ perceptions of whether they are an
undue anxiety, embarrassment, or fear. Interestingly, early or a late maturer are more strongly related to their
however, in contrast to girls, who generally tell their feelings about their physical maturation than whether
mothers shortly after they have begun menstruating they actually are early or late (Dubas, Graber, & Petersen,
and tell their girlfriends soon thereafter, boys, at least in 1991). Further, adolescents’ behavior is related to how
the United States, do not discuss their first ejaculation old they feel, not simply to how physically mature they
with either parents or friends (J. Stein & Reiser, 1994). are (Galambos, Kolaric, Sears, & Maggs, 1999). Never-
In other cultures, the event may be experienced some- theless, early and late maturers are often treated differ-
what differently. For example, one study of first ejacula- ently by others and view themselves differently, and
tion among adolescent boys in Nigeria found not only they may, as a result, behave differently. As we shall
that boys were not upset by the event but also that they see, however, early and late maturation have different
told their friends about the experience very soon after it consequences in the immediate present and in the
occurred (Adegoke, 1993). Cultural differences in boys’ long run, different consequences in different contexts,
responses to their first ejaculation are likely related to and, most important, different consequences for boys
differences in how cultures view masturbation. As is the and girls.
case with girls and menarche, boys’ reactions to their
first ejaculation are more positive when they have been ❚ Early Versus Late Maturation Among Boys
prepared for the event (J. Stein & Reiser, 1994). Over the past 60 years, research on boys’ pubertal
timing has usually found that early-maturing boys
feel better about themselves and are more popular
THE IMPACT OF EARLY
than their late-maturing peers (Graber, Lewinsohn,
OR LATE MATURATION
Seeley, & Brooks-Gunn, 1997; M. C. Jones & Bayley,
Adolescents who mature relatively early or relatively late 1950; Richards & Larson, 1993), although a few studies
stand apart from their peers physically and may, as a have found elevated rates of depression and anxiety
consequence, elicit different sorts of reactions and ex- among early-maturing boys relative to their on-time
pectations from those around them. Moreover, individ- peers (Mendle & Ferrero, 2012; Negriff & Susman,
ual adolescents may be all too aware of whether they are 2011) and among boys who go through puberty es-
early or late relative to their age-mates, and their feel- pecially rapidly (Mendle, Harden, Brooks-Gunn, &
ings about themselves are likely to be influenced by their Graber, 2010). Interestingly, however, although all ado-
comparisons. One study found that early-maturing ad- lescents are adversely affected by being bullied by their
olescents were more likely to be “pseudomature”— peers, the impact of victimization is greater for early
wishing they were older, hanging around with older maturers, perhaps because being picked on when one is
peers, less involved in school, and more oriented toward larger than average is all the more embarrassing
their peers (Galambos, Barker, & Tilton-Weaver, 2003). (Nadeem & Graham, 2005).
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 42 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

42 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

the midst of puberty, early maturers experience more


frequent and more intense temper tantrums and de-
pression (Ge, Brody, Conger, Simons, & Murry, 2002;
Ge et al., 2003).

❚ Early Versus Late Maturation in Girls In


contrast to the mixed impact that early maturation
has on the psychological well-being of boys, early-
maturing girls have more emotional difficulties than
their peers, including lowered self-image and higher
rates of depression, anxiety, eating disorders, and
panic attacks (Negriff & Susman, 2011). These diffi-
culties seem to have less to do with the direct effects of
hormones and more to do with the ways in which
standing apart from their peers physically affects girls’
feelings about their appearance and social relation-
ships with other adolescents (Mendle et al., 2007).
One recent study found, for example, that the impact
Although they are often more popular than their peers, early of early maturation was worse on girls who were
maturing girls are at greater risk for wide range of emotional
and behavioral problems.
heavier than on their thinner peers (Tanner-Smith,
2010). There is also evidence that early maturation
in girls is associated with higher emotional arousal
(Graber, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, 2006).
Although results of research on the psychological Given the role of social factors in linking early matu-
effects of early maturation are mixed, it is quite clear ration and girls’ psychological distress, it is no surprise
that early-maturing boys are more likely than their that the ultimate impact of early maturation on the
peers to get involved in antisocial or deviant activities, young girl’s feelings about herself appears to depend on
including truancy, minor delinquency, and problems the broader context in which maturation takes place.
at school (Negriff & Susman, 2011). They are also For example, studies of American girls generally find
more likely to use drugs and alcohol and engage in that early-maturing girls have lower self-esteem and a
other risky activities (Dick, Rose, Pulkkinen, & Kaprio, poorer self-image, because of our cultural preference
2001; Drapela, Gebelt, & McRee, 2006; Kaltiala-Heino, for thinness and our ambivalence about adolescent sex-
Koivisto, Martutunen, & Fröjd, 2011; Wichstrøm, uality (Brooks-Gunn & Reiter, 1990). The negative
2001), even as young adults (Biehl, Natsuaki, & Ge, effects of early maturation on girls’ mental health vary
2007). One explanation for this is that boys who are across ethnic groups, however, with more adverse con-
more physically mature develop friendships with older sequences seen among White girls than their Black or
peers and that these friendships lead them into activi- Hispanic peers, presumably because puberty is more
ties that are problematic for the younger boys (Negriff, likely to lead to body dissatisfaction among White girls
Ji, & Trickett, 2011a). Once involved with these older (Negriff & Susman, 2011).
peer groups, the early maturers’ higher rate of delin- The impact of physical maturation also depends on
quency and substance use increases over time through the social context in which teenagers live (Negriff &
their social contacts (Silbereisen et al., 1989). Susman, 2011). One study of suburban Chicago
Clearly, early-maturing boys enjoy some psycholog- youngsters, for example, found that girls’ body image
ical advantages over late maturers with respect to was significantly higher in one community than in
self-esteem and admiration from peers during early another—despite comparable levels of physical matu-
adolescence, when some boys have matured physically ration between the two groups. One factor that differ-
but others have not. But what about later during ado- entiated the two communities was “cliquishness”: In
lescence, when the late maturers have caught up? At the more cliquish high school, girls were less satisfied
least one study points to some interesting advantages with the way they looked, perhaps because cliquish
for late-maturing boys, despite their initially lower girls place more emphasis on physical appearance in
popularity. Although early and late maturers exhibit determining popularity (Richards, Boxer, Petersen, &
similar psychological profiles before they enter adoles- Albrecht, 1990). One recent study of both boys and girls
cence, during the time of pubertal onset, and one year found that the adverse consequences of early puberty
later as well, late maturers show significantly higher were limited to adolescents who came from high-risk
ratings on measures of intellectual curiosity, explor- households (Lynne-Landsman, Graber, & Andrews,
atory behavior, and social initiative. While they are in 2010b).
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 43 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 43

Although some early-maturing girls may have self- boys (for whom the negative effects of early puberty
image difficulties, their popularity with peers is not occur during puberty itself, but then fade), but not for
jeopardized. Indeed, some studies indicate that early girls (for whom the negative effects of early puberty
maturers are more popular than other girls, especially, persist) (Ge et al., 2003).
as you would expect, when the index of popularity A final explanation for the relatively greater disad-
includes popularity with boys (Simmons, Blyth, & vantage of early maturation for girls concerns the cul-
McKinney, 1983). However, early-maturing girls not tural desirability of different body types (Petersen,
only enjoy more popularity with boys, but are often the 1988). Early maturation for girls means leaving behind
victims of rumors and gossip (Reynolds & Juvonen, the culturally admired state of thinness. As noted previ-
2011; Sontag, Graber, & Clemans, 2011) and are more ously, among girls, the ratio of fat to muscle increases
likely to suffer from social anxiety (Blumenthal et al., dramatically at puberty, and many girls are distressed
2011). Ironically, then, it may be in part because the when they mature because they gain weight. Early ma-
early maturer is more popular with boys that she re- turers experience this weight gain at a time when most
ports more emotional upset: Early pressure to date and, of their peers are still girlishly thin. One interesting
perhaps, to be involved in a sexual relationship may take study showed that in ballet companies—where thin-
its toll on the adolescent girl’s mental health. Consistent ness is even more important than in the culture at
with this, research indicates that early-maturing girls large—late maturers, who can retain the “ideal” shape
are more vulnerable to emotional distress when they much longer than earlier maturers, have fewer psy-
have relatively more other-sex friendships (Ge, Best, chological problems than even on-time girls (Brooks-
Conger, & Simons, 1996) and when they are in schools Gunn & Warren, 1985). In contrast, at puberty, boys
with older peers (for example, sixth-graders who are in move from a culturally undesirable state for males
a school that has seventh- and eighth-graders, too) (short and scrawny) to a culturally admired one (tall
(Blyth, Simmons, & Zakin, 1985). and muscular). Early maturers enjoy the special advan-
Psychologists have offered several explanations for tage of being tall and muscular before their peers—a
the fact that early maturation appears to be harder on special benefit in a society that values males’ athletic
girls than boys (Negriff & Susman, 2011). One explana- prowess—and therefore are more likely to react well
tion is the “maturational deviance” hypothesis. Simply to puberty. The fact that the effects of early maturation
put, youngsters who stand far apart from their peers— on girls’ self-esteem vary across cultures suggests that
in physical appearance, for instance—may experience contextual factors need to be taken into account in
more psychological distress than adolescents who blend explaining this pattern of sex differences.
in more easily. Because girls mature earlier than boys, Whatever the explanation, it’s important for par-
on average, early-maturing girls mature earlier than ents and school counselors to bear in mind that early-
both their male and female peers. This makes them maturing girls are at heightened risk for psychological
stand out at a time when they would rather fit in and, as problems, at least in the United States. Unfortunately,
a result, may make them more vulnerable to emotional as long as our culture overvalues thinness and encour-
distress. This explanation would also account for the ages the view that females should be judged on the basis
lower self-esteem of late-maturing boys, who deviate of their physical appearance rather than their abilities,
toward the other extreme. values, or personality, the risks of early puberty will
A second explanation for the sex difference in the probably endure. Adults can help by being supportive,
impact of early maturation focuses on “developmental by helping the early-maturing girl recognize her
readiness.” If puberty is a challenge that requires strengths and positive features—physical and non-
psychological adaptation by the adolescent, perhaps physical alike—and by preparing her for puberty be-
younger adolescents are less ready to cope with the fore it takes place.
challenge than older ones. Because puberty occurs Like their male counterparts, early-maturing girls
quite early among early-maturing girls, it may tax their are also more likely to become involved in problem be-
psychological resources. Early maturation among boys, havior, including delinquency and drugs and alcohol
because it occurs at a later age, would pose less of a use; are more likely to have school problems; and are more
problem. This also helps to explain why late-maturing likely to experience early sexual intercourse (Boden,
boys seem better able than early maturers to control Fergusson, & Horwood, 2011; Dick, Rose, Viken, &
their temper and their impulses when they are going Kaprio, 2000; Negriff & Susman, 2011) (see Figure 1.11).
through puberty: They are relatively older and psycho- This is true in Europe and the United States (Silbereisen
logically more mature (Peskin, 1967). If the develop- et al., 1989) and across ethnic groups within the United
mental readiness hypothesis is true, both girls and boys States (Deardorff, Gonzales, Christopher, Roosa, &
should experience more difficulty if they are early ma- Millsap, 2005). These problems appear to arise because
turers than if they are on time or late, but the difficulty early-maturing girls are more likely to spend time with
should be temporary. This appears to be the case among older adolescents, especially older adolescent boys, who
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 44 21/03/13 9:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

44 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

FIGURE 1.11 Early-maturing 70


girls are more likely to use Weekly
tobacco, alcohol, and other Never
60

Percent of girls reporting smoking


drugs than girls who mature
on time or late. (From Dick et
al., 2000)
50

40

30

20

10

0
ⱕ10 11 12 13 14 ⱖ15
Age at menarche

initiate them into activities that might otherwise be de- nities (Negriff & Susman, 2011), because studies that
layed (Haynie, 2003; Negriff et al., 2011b). One recent take community context into account sometimes find
study suggests that the link between early maturation that early maturation has no adverse effects on Black
and girls’ problem behavior may be partly due to com- girls (Carter, Caldwell, Matusko, Antoinucci, & Jackson,
mon genetic influences (i.e., genes that influence both 2011; DeRose, Shiyko, Foster, & Brooks-Gunn, 2011).
the timing of puberty and involvement in delinquency) The earlier involvement of early-maturing girls in
(Harden & Mendle, 2011c). Another found that early problem behavior may adversely affect their long-term
maturation leads to early sexual activity, which in turn educational achievement and mental health. In one
leads to delinquency (Negriff, Susman, & Trickett, study of Swedish girls, the researchers found that the
2011b). school problems of early-maturing girls persisted over
Again, however, it is important to consider the role time, leading to the development of negative attitudes
of context in interaction with pubertal change. Al- toward school and lower educational aspirations. In
though early-maturing girls generally are more likely to young adulthood, there were marked differences be-
engage in delinquent behavior than their late-maturing tween the early- and late-maturing girls’ levels of edu-
peers, a study of New Zealand youngsters indicates that cation; for example, the late-maturing girls were twice
this may hold true only for girls who attend coeduca- as likely as early-maturing girls to continue beyond
tional high schools (Caspi, Lynam, Moffitt, & Silva, the compulsory minimum number of years of high
1993). Early-maturing girls in all-female schools are no school (Magnusson et al., 1986). In a different study of
more likely than late maturers to be involved in delin- American girls, researchers found that women who had
quent activities, presumably because there are far fewer been early maturers reported higher levels of psycho-
opportunities for delinquency in same-sex schools. logical distress and were more likely than others to have
Thus, while early puberty may predispose girls toward experienced a serious mental disorder at some point
more frequent and earlier deviance, this predisposition in adolescence or young adulthood (Graber, Seeley,
may be realized only in an environment that permits Brooks-Gunn, & Lewinsohn, 2004).
the behavior—such as a school or out-of-school setting
that places early-maturing girls in close contact with
older boys (Stattin, Kerr, & Skoog, 2011). Similarly,
among both boys and girls, the impact of early matura- MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
tion on problem behavior or depression is accentuated
Consider the research on the
when adolescents have many stressful life events, have
psychological consequences of early
harsh and inconsistent parents, or live in disadvantaged
versus late maturation in males and
urban neighborhoods (Ge, Brody, Conger, Simons, &
females. Most of this research has
Murry, 2002; Ge, Conger, & Elder, 2001; Obeidallah,
been done in the United States. Are
Brennan, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2004). This helps
the effects of being early, on time,
explain why the impact of early maturation on prob-
or late likely to be similar in different
lem behavior is relatively greater among minority
parts of the world?
adolescents, who are more likely to live in poor commu-
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 45 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 45

RECAP whether someone is overweight is to calculate his or


• Although going through puberty in and of itself is her body mass index (BMI), which is done by dividing
not ordinarily stressful, some evidence suggests the person’s weight, measured in kilograms, by the
that adolescents may be moodier during periods square of the person’s height, measured in meters. In-
of rapid changes in pubertal hormones. In general, dividuals are considered obese if their BMI is at or
though, stress in the environment is a much above the 95th percentile for people of the same age
stronger influence on adolescent mental health and gender, at great risk for obesity if their BMI is at or
than is puberty. above the 90th percentile, and overweight if their BMI
• Important changes in patterns of sleep take place is at or above the 85th percentile (Zametkin, Zoon,
at puberty. As the result of the emergence of a Klein, & Munson, 2004). (Charts showing the BMI cut-
“delayed phase preference,” after puberty, adoles- offs for males and females of different ages can be
cents tend to stay up later at night and sleep later found at www.cdc.gov/growthcharts.) Using this defi-
in the morning. nition, more than one-sixth of adolescents in the
• Family relationships frequently become temporarily United States are obese (Ogden, Carroll, & Flegal,
more strained and distant during the early stages of 2008), and another 15% are at great risk for obesity, a
puberty. rate that has increased substantially over the past 30
• Among boys, early maturation is associated with years (Spruijt-Metz, 2011) (see Figure 1.12). Com-
popularity, higher self-esteem, and more self- pared to their peers in the mid-1960s, the average
confidence, but also with higher rates of problem 15-year-old boy today is 15 pounds heavier, and the
behavior. average 15-year-old girl is 10 pounds heavier—in-
• Among girls, early maturation is associated with creases that are far greater than could possibly be due
higher rates of problem behavior, with psychological to changes in height. Obesity is now considered the
distress, and with emotional difficulties. single most serious public health problem afflicting
• In both sexes, the impact of early or late American teenagers, and, unfortunately, there is no
maturation depends on the evidence that the situation is getting any better (thank-
social context in fully, it isn’t worsening, either) (Ogden, Carroll, Cur-
which it takes tin, Lamb, & Flegal, 2010; Spruijt-Metz, 2011). By some
place. estimates, the adolescent obesity epidemic will cost the
United States more than $250 billion as a result of
obese individuals’ lost productivity and direct medical
costs in adulthood (Lightwood, Bibbins-Domingo,
Coxson, Wang et al., 2009). The increase in adolescent
Obesity and Eating obesity has been especially dramatic among Black
females (H. Lee, Lee, Guo, & Harris, 2011). The adoles-
Disorders cent obesity epidemic is by no means limited to the
Although a variety of nutritional and behavioral factors United States, however, and has been documented in
can lead to weight gain during adolescence, weight gain many other industrialized and developing nations
sometimes results directly from the physical changes of (Jessor, Turbin, & Costa, 2010).
puberty. Not only does the ratio of body fat to muscle
increase markedly during puberty, but the body’s basal ❚ Correlates and Consequences of Obesity
metabolism rate—the minimal amount of energy one Current research indicates that obesity is a result of
uses when resting—also drops about 15%. A person’s the interplay of genetic and environmental factors
weight is partly dependent on this rate. (Zametkin et al., 2004). Recent neuroimaging studies
Because adolescence is a time of dramatic change in find that individuals at risk for obesity show relatively
physical appearance, teenagers’ overall self-image is very greater activation of the brain’s reward centers in gen-
much tied to their body image. In light of the tremen- eral, heightened responses to images of food, and
dous emphasis that contemporary society places on be- poorer impulse control (Batterink, Yokum, & Stice,
ing thin, particularly for females, the normal weight 2010; Stice, Yokum, Burger, Epstein, & Small, 2011).
gain and change in body composition that accompany
puberty leads many adolescents, especially girls, to be-
come extremely concerned about their weight.

OBESITY basal metabolism rate The minimal amount of energy used by


the body during a resting state.
Many adolescents, of course, have legitimate concerns
about being overweight. The easiest way to determine body mass index (BMI) A measure of an individual’s body fat, the
ratio of weight to height; used to gauge overweight and obesity.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 46 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

46 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

FIGURE 1.12 The rate of obesity 45


among American adolescents has African American Hispanic/Latino White
risen steadily over the past sev- 40
eral decades. (Spruijt-Metz, 2011) 35

30

25

Percent
20

15

10

0
1986 1990 1996 1999–00 2001–02 2003–06 2007–08

Obesity is especially prevalent among poor youth and obesity disappear among individuals who are not obese
among Black, Latino, and Native American adolescents as adults (Juonala, Magnussen, & Berenson, 2011).
(Burdette & Needham, 2012; Huh, Stice, Shaw, & Although genetic factors are important contribu-
Boutelle, 2012; Miech et al., 2006; K. A. T. Wickrama, tors, the dramatically increased prevalence of adoles-
Wickrama, & Bryant, 2006). There is also worrisome cent obesity over such a short time period indicates
evidence that, with each successive generation born in that the problem has strong environmental causes.
the United States, Latino youth show increasingly Indeed, it is not at all difficult to understand why so
poorer nutrition (M. L. Allen et al., 2007). many American adolescents today are overweight.
Research on the psychological consequences of obe- Studies show that too many adolescents eat far too
sity has not led to consistent conclusions, in part much high-calorie, low-fiber food (drinking far too
because the psychological correlates of being over- many sugary soft drinks and consuming far too much
weight vary across ethnic groups, with more adverse junk food), and too few are physically active, spending
correlates seen among White and Hispanic than among far too much time with television, video games, or the
Black adolescents (BeLue, Francis, & Colaco, 2009; computer (Barnett et al., 2010) and not enough time
Leech & Dias, 2012). And while some studies show playing sports (Olds, Ferrar, Schranz, & Maher, 2011).
higher levels of psychological distress (such as depres- One recent study of American youth (Nader, Bradley,
sion and low self-esteem) among obese individuals, Houts, McRitchie, & O’Brien, 2008) found that the
many studies show no such effect, and some research amount of time individuals spend in moderate-to-
indicates that depression leads to obesity, rather than vigorous physical activity falls from 3 hours per day
vice versa (Zametkin et al., 2004). In addition, the long- at age 9 to only 49 minutes on weekdays and only
term psychological consequences of obesity in adoles- 35 minutes on weekend days at age 15 (see Figure
cence appear to be greater for females than males, 1.13). In addition, as noted earlier, inadequate sleep
perhaps because overweight girls are teased more than also contributed to weight gain, and greater night-
overweight boys (M. Merten, Wickrama, & Williams, time technology use, which we will look at in Chapter
2008; Taylor, 2011; Vogt Yuan, 2010). This harassment, 7, has led to increases in the numbers of adolescents
like other forms of bullying, can disrupt adolescents’ who are sleep-deprived. The combination of poor nu-
academic performance; one study found, for example, trition, insufficient exercise, and inadequate sleep is a
that obese girls were less likely to enter college than recipe for obesity (Spruijt-Metz, 2011). And because
other girls, but that no such pattern existed among boys adolescents tend to affiliate with peers who share their
(Crosnoe, 2007). Nevertheless, because nearly 80% of tastes and interests, obese teenagers are relatively more
obese adolescents will be obese adults, obesity during likely to have obese friends, which may reinforce bad
adolescence places the individual at much higher risk behavioral habits (Valente, Fujimoto, Chou, & Spruijt-
for other health problems, including hypertension Metz, 2009). Indeed, one recent study found that at-
(high blood pressure), high cholesterol levels, diabetes, tending a high school where a large proportion of
and premature death (Institute of Medicine, 2006; juniors and seniors were obese significantly increased
Ma, Flanders, Ward, & Jemal, 2011). The good news is the likelihood that the freshmen and sophomores
that the long-term health consequences of adolescent would be obese, too (Leatherdale & Papadakis, 2011).
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 47 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 47

200
190 Weekdays
180 Boys
170 Girls
160 Weekend days
150 Boys
Mean minutes of activity per day

140 Girls
Daily recommended
130
MVPA for children
120
110
100
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
9 10 11
12 13 14 15
Age (years)
FIGURE 1.13 Between ages 9 and 15, there is a dramatic decline in the amount of time individuals spend in moderate-to-
vigorous physical activity (MVPA). (Nader et al., 2008)

❚ Preventing and Treating Obesity Much recent obesity may differ from country to country, the factors
attention has focused on the availability of unhealthy that contribute to obesity are similar in different parts
foods and beverages in and near American schools of the world (Jessor et al., 2010).
(Rovner, Nansel, Wang, & Iannotti, 2011). One study of Research has also evaluated a variety of approaches
500,000 California adolescents found that students to individual weight loss, including behavioral therapy
attending schools that were in close proximity to fast- designed to gradually alter patterns of diet and exer-
food restaurants were significantly more likely to be cise and medications designed to promote weight loss.
overweight (B. Davis & Carpenter, 2009). Manufactur-
ers of high-calorie and high-fat foods also have been
criticized for marketing these products to younger chil-
dren, because food preferences are known to develop
largely during early childhood (Institute of Medicine,
2006). Of course, although schools and advertisers un-
doubtedly influence what children and adolescents eat
and drink, the bulk of what children and adolescents
put into their mouths comes from their own homes,
and studies suggest that obesity is less likely to develop
among adolescents who have good relationships with
their parents, probably because they are more likely to
share family meals where healthy food is served (Berge,
Wall, Loth, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2010; Hammons &
Fiese, 2011). In addition, the availability of parks and
recreational facilities is linked to lower rates of obesity,
as is parental encouragement of exercise (Spruijt-Metz,
2011). Taken together, these studies indicate that
preventing obesity will require multifaceted efforts
involving parents, the mass media, food and beverage
manufacturers, restaurants, schools, and communities. Obesity is by far the most common eating disorder among
Cross-cultural studies show that, although rates of adolescents.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 48 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

48 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Several evaluations indicate that the combination of to full-blown clinical eating disorders, such as anorexia
behavior modification and weight loss medication is and bulimia. Research shows that disordered eating is
more effective than either component by itself associated with a range of stress-related psychological
(Berkowitz, Wadden, Tershakovec, & Cronquist, 2003; problems, including poor body image, depression, alco-
Chanoine, Hampl, Jensen, Boldrin, & Hauptman, hol and tobacco use, and poor interpersonal relation-
2005). Although there is evidence suggesting that ships (Eichen, Conner, Daly, & Fauber, 2012; Graber,
some weight loss programs work, there is wide vari- Brooks-Gunn, Paikoff, & Warren, 1994; Neumark-
ability in success rates depending on the nature of the Sztainer, Story, Dixon, & Murray, 1998). It is not clear,
program (Sarwer & Dilks, 2012; Stice, Shaw, & Marti, however, whether these problems precede or follow
2006). Radical approaches to weight control—fad from disordered eating (Leon, Fulkerson, Perry, Keel, &
diets and the like—actually increase, rather than de- Klump, 1999).
crease, obesity (Neumark-Sztainer, Wall, Story, & Studies of magazines aimed at women and adoles-
Standish, 2012; Stice, Presnell, Shaw, & Rohde, 2005; cent girls reveal clear and consistent messages implying
Zhang et al., 2011a). that women cannot be beautiful without being slim and
promoting a range of weight loss products (Davison &
McCabe, 2011). Between 1970 and 1990, moreover, im-
ANOREXIA NERVOSA, BULIMIA,
ages presented in these magazines’ advertisements
AND BINGE EATING DISORDER
changed, with the “ideal” body shape becoming slim-
Health care professionals are concerned not only about mer and less curvaceous (Guillen & Barr, 1994). Expo-
adolescents who are obese but also about adolescents sure to commercials containing images of females with
who have unhealthy attitudes toward eating and idealized thin bodies increases girls’ dissatisfaction with
toward their body image (S. French, Story, Downes, their own bodies (Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2003). In-
Resnick, & Blum, 1995). Only about one-fourth of terestingly, among Hispanic girls, those who are more
American adolescents are highly satisfied with their acculturated (“Americanized”) are significantly more
body (A. Kelly, Wall, Eisenberg, Story, & Neumark- likely to develop disordered eating than those who are
Sztainer, 2005). Egged on by advertisers, who promote less acculturated (Gowen, Hayward, Killen, Robinson, &
the idea that “thin is beautiful,” many adolescents re- Taylor, 1999). Girls whose mothers have body image
spond to normal bodily changes at puberty by dieting, problems are especially likely to engage in extreme
often unnecessarily (Brumberg, 1997). More than half weight loss behaviors (Ogle & Damhorst, 2003), as are
of all adolescent girls consider themselves overweight those who report more negative relationships with
and have attempted to diet (M. Fisher et al., 1995). One their parents (Archibald, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn,
study found that 14% of female college undergraduates 1999).
were so concerned about eating that they were embar- Some young women become so concerned about
rassed at buying a chocolate bar in a store (Rozin, gaining weight that they take drastic—and danger-
Bauer, & Catanese, 2003)! Gender differences in con- ous—measures to remain thin. In the more severe
cerns about weight emerge long before adolescence cases, young women who suffer from an eating disorder
(Phares, Steinberg, & Thompson, 2004). called anorexia nervosa actually starve themselves in
an effort to keep their weight down. Others go on eat-
❚ Disordered Eating Psychologists use the term ing binges and then force themselves to vomit or take
disordered eating to refer to unhealthy eating attitudes laxatives to avoid gaining weight, a pattern associated
and behaviors. Disordered eating can range from un- with an eating disorder called bulimia. Adolescents
necessary preoccupation with weight and body image with these sorts of eating disorders have an extremely
disturbed body image: They see themselves as over-
weight when they are actually underweight. Some an-
orexic youngsters may lose between 25% and 50% of
disordered eating Mild, moderate, or severe disturbance in eating their body weight. As you might expect, bulimia and
habits and attitudes. anorexia, if untreated, lead to a variety of serious physi-
anorexia nervosa An eating disorder found chiefly among cal problems; in fact, nearly 20% of anorexic teenagers
young women, characterized by dramatic and severe self-induced inadvertently starve themselves to death.
weight loss. A newly identified disorder, called binge eating
disorder, has recently been defined (Stice, Shaw, &
bulimia An eating disorder found primarily among young women,
Ochner, 2011). Individuals with this disorder binge eat
characterized by a pattern of binge eating and extreme weight loss
and feel distressed about doing so, but do not try to
measures, including self-induced vomiting.
compensate for their binges through extreme weight
binge eating disorder An eating disorder characterized by a loss measures. As a consequence, individuals with binge
pattern of binge eating that is not accompanied by drastic attempts eating disorder are at high risk for developing obesity.
to lose weight.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 49 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 49

Because this disorder has only been defined recently, Unfortunately, many girls gain weight during pu-
there is very little research on its causes, correlates, or berty, and for early adolescent girls, being overweight
treatment. is highly correlated with being seen as unattractive by
Anorexia and bulimia each began to receive a great others (Rosenblum & Lewis, 1999). Despite adults’
deal of popular attention during the 1980s, because of wishes that girls not place so much emphasis on being
their dramatic nature and their frequent association in thin, research indicates that the widespread belief
the mass media with celebrities. Perhaps because of this among adolescent girls that being slim will increase
attention, initial reports characterized these eating dis- their popularity, especially with boys, is in fact based in
orders as being of epidemic proportion. Although un- reality (Halpern, Udry, Campbell, & Suchindran, 1999).
healthy eating and unnecessary dieting are prevalent That is, the pressure girls feel to be thin in order to
among teenagers, careful studies indicate that the inci- attract boys does not just come from television, movies,
dence of clinically defined anorexia and bulimia is and magazines—it comes from their actual experience.
small (Stice et al., 2011). Fewer than 1% of adolescents Indeed, one analysis found that, regardless of a girl’s
are anorexic, and only about 3% are bulimic (Stice ethnicity, each 1-point increase in a young woman’s
et al., 2011). Rates among females are substantially BMI (an index of the degree to which she is overweight)
higher than among males—anorexia and bulimia are was associated with a 6% decrease in the probability of
10 times more prevalent among adolescent girls than her being in a romantic relationship (Halpern, King,
boys (Stice et al., 2011), although the female-to-male Oslak, & Udry, 2005). In that study, a 5-foot 3-inch girl
ratio is substantially smaller when less severe forms of who weighed 110 pounds was twice as likely to date as a
these disorders are considered (Muise, Stein, & Arbess, girl of the same height and level of pubertal maturity
2003; Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2004). Unlike anorexia who weighed 126 pounds.
and bulimia, binge eating disorder is only slightly more Fewer studies have examined body dissatisfaction
prevalent among females than males, which suggests among adolescent boys, although it is clear that there is
that it may have an entirely different etiology. an idealized, muscular, male body type that many boys
aspire to. As is the case with girls, boys who do not fit
❚ Body Dissatisfaction Although the incidence this image report more body dissatisfaction, but the re-
of anorexia and bulimia is small, the proportion of ado- lation between appearance and body dissatisfaction
lescents who are unhappy with their body shape or among males is somewhat more complicated than it is
weight is not. In one study, for example, more than one- among females. Whereas being heavy is the main source
third of girls whose weight was considered normal by of dissatisfaction among girls, being heavy or being thin
medical and health standards believed that they were are both sources of dissatisfaction among boys (see
overweight—including 5% who actually were under- Figure 1.14). In fact, boys are more likely to be teased for
weight by medical criteria. (In contrast, fewer than 7% being underweight (and underdeveloped) than for be-
of normal-weight boys and no underweight boys de- ing overweight. As is the case among girls, being teased
scribed themselves as being overweight.) In this study, by peers about one’s body is a significant source of dis-
more than 70% of the girls reported that they would tress for boys as well (Lawler & Nixon, 2011).
like to be thinner than they are (as opposed to one-third
of the boys), and more than 80% said that being thinner ❚ Prevalence and Causes of Anorexia and Bulimia
would make them happier, more successful, and more Although it is widely believed that eating disorders are
popular (Paxton et al., 1991). Dissatisfaction with body especially common among affluent, suburban, White,
shape and weight is likely to lead to the development of and Asian American girls, systematic studies do not
eating problems (Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989), depres- support this contention. Disordered eating and body
sion (Stice & Bearman, 2001; Stice, Hayward, Cameron, dissatisfaction have been reported among poor as well
Killen, & Taylor, 2000), and the initiation of smoking as affluent teenagers and among Black and Hispanic as
(Austin & Gortmaker, 2001; Fulkerson & French, 2003; well as Asian and White youngsters (Jacobi et al., 2004).
Stice & Shaw, 2003). More than half of high school girls Although less is known about the causes and conse-
have engaged in some form of unhealthy behavior (e.g., quences of body dissatisfaction among adolescent
fasting, smoking, vomiting after eating, using diet pills) males, evidence suggests that many contemporary ado-
in order to lose weight (Croll, Neumark-Sztainer, Story, & lescent boys feel pressure to be especially muscular and
Ireland, 2002). Taken together, these findings have led engage in unhealthy behaviors, such as anabolic steroid
some writers to suggest that we think about disordered use, in order to develop an appearance that is more
eating on a continuum, ranging from dieting that may similar to the idealized male body type (Ricciardelli &
be perfectly sensible and healthy, to disordered eating McCabe, 2004). Moreover, body dissatisfaction is
that is unhealthy but not psychopathological (i.e., at a predictive of dieting, unhealthy weight control behav-
level requiring treatment), to full-blown anorexia or iors, and binge eating among males as well as females,
bulimia (Tyrka, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). regardless of whether they are actually overweight
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 50 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

50 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

100
90
80

Percent of adolescents 70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Underweight Underweight Average weight Average weight Overweight Overweight
girls boys girls boys girls boys
Want to be bigger Body satisfied Want to be smaller
FIGURE 1.14 Body dissatisfaction by BMI classification for girls and boys. (Lawler & Nixon, 2011)

(Crow, Eisenberg, Story, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2006; Instead, experts now view eating disorders as part of
Neumark-Sztainer, Paxton, Hannan, Haines, & Story, a more general syndrome of psychological distress.
2006). Recent studies of adolescents in China have Many studies have pointed to links between eating dis-
found similar patterns (Jackson & Chen, 2011). orders and other serious mental health problems, such
Historical and cross-cultural trends in the preva- as depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder, or sub-
lence of anorexia and bulimia points to important dif- stance abuse; many anorexic and bulimic women dis-
ferences between the two disorders (Keel & Klump, play such psychological problems along with their
2003). Whereas anorexia has been observed all over eating disorder (Granillo, Grogan-Kaylor, Delva, &
the world, bulimia has been reported almost exclu- Castillo, 2011; Jacobi et al., 2004; Stice, Burton, & Shaw,
sively in Western cultures or in cultures exposed to 2004). These studies suggest that anorexia and bulimia
strong Western influences. And whereas anorexia has may be best understood not as independent or unique
increased in prevalence steadily over time, the preva- disorders, but as particular manifestations of a more
lence of bulimia increased significantly between 1970 general underlying psychological problem—called
and 1990 but has declined somewhat since then— “internalized distress”—that can be displayed in a vari-
interestingly, paralleling trends in females’ reported ety of ways. (As you’ll see in Chapter 13, many different
body dissatisfaction, which also peaked in the early psychological disorders in adolescence occur together,
1990s, despite the fact that individuals’ BMI continued or are “comorbid.”) In support of this view, some evi-
to increase (Cash, Morrow, Hrabosky, & Perry, 2004). dence suggests that the same medications that are suc-
This suggests that bulimia is a much more culturally cessful in treating depression and obsessive-compulsive
determined disorder than is anorexia. Consistent with disorder are useful in treating bulimia (but, interest-
this, the degree to which anorexia is an inherited dis- ingly, not anorexia) (B. Walsh et al., 2006).
order is far more comparable from one culture to an- In addition, because anorexia and bulimia are 10
other than is the case for bulimia. times more common in females than males, broader so-
Several theories have been proposed to account for cial forces are probably a main factor in the develop-
the onset of anorexia and bulimia during adolescence ment of these eating disorders (Keel & Klump, 2003).
(Stice et al., 2011). Although it had once been theorized Research indicates, for example, that girls who are early
that eating disorders were caused by dysfunctional maturers and early daters are likely to report greater
family relationships, evidence in support of these theo- dissatisfaction with their body and to be at greater risk
ries is sparse, and the same familial factors that have for disordered eating (Cauffman & Steinberg, 1996;
been proposed to cause eating disorders have been Smolak, Levine, & Gralen, 1993; Swarr & Richards,
shown to influence a wide variety of psychological disor- 1996); that girls who perceive that they are under pressure
ders (Jacobi et al., 2004). In addition, longitudinal stud- to be thin or who have accepted thinness as an ideal
ies have shown that negative family relationships often toward which to strive are more susceptible to eating
follow from, rather than precede, the onset of disordered disorders (Stice & Whitenton, 2002); and that girls who
eating, raising questions about which causes which turn to popular magazines, such as Seventeen, Twist, and
(Archibald, Linver, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002). Glamour, for information about dieting and appearance
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 51 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 51

are more likely to have a high drive for thinness, low RECAP
body satisfaction, and disturbed patterns of eating (D. C. • About one in six American adolescents are obese,
Jones, Vigfusdottir, & Lee, 2004; M. Levine, Smolack, & and another one in six are at great risk for obesity.
Hayden, 1994). As noted earlier, cultural influences on Obesity is associated with a wide array of serious
the development of bulimia are especially strong. health problems.
Adolescents’ beliefs about ideal body types are also • Anorexia and bulimia are eating disorders that do
shaped by the people they spend time with. Girls’ atti- not appear until adolescence and that are 10 times
tudes toward eating and dieting are influenced by the more prevalent among females than males. A newly
attitudes of their parents (especially their mothers) and identified disorder, called binge eating disorder, also
friends (Eisenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2010; Hutchin- emerges during adolescence, but its female-to-male
son, Rapee, & Taylor, 2010; E. R. Mackey & La Greca, ratio is much smaller.
2008; Neumark-Sztainer, Bauer, Friend, Hannan, Story, • Many girls with anorexia or bulimia develop the
& Berge, 2010; Salafia & Gondoli, 2011). Being teased disorder as a result of dissatisfaction with the way
about one’s weight is especially likely to lead to disor- in which their body has changed following puberty,
dered eating, as well as symptoms of depression (M. especially within a culture that idealizes thinness
Eisenberg, Neumark-Sztainer, Haines, & Wall, 2006; among women.
Haines, Neumark-Sztainer, Eisenberg, & Hannan, 2006). • Anorexia and bulimia are often accompanied by
Just because cultural conditions contribute to the other emotional problems, such
development of disordered eating doesn’t mean that in- as depression, anxiety, or
dividual characteristics do not play a role as well. Cul- obsessive-compulsive
tural conditions may predispose females more than disorder.
males toward anorexia and bulimia, and girls and
young women who have certain genetic vulnerabilities
(eating disorders are partly heritable), psychological
traits (such as proneness to depression or low self-
esteem), physical characteristics (such as early pubertal
maturation), familial characteristics (such as strained
relations with parents), or social concerns (such as a
Physical Health and Health
strong interest in dating) may be more likely to develop Care in Adolescence
problems (e.g., Cauffman & Steinberg, 1996; S. Frank & Although puberty is undoubtedly the most important
Jackson, 1996; Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2001). The onset biological development of adolescence, concerns about
of eating disorders, like so many aspects of adolescent the physical health and well-being of young people are
development, is likely the product of a complex interac- far broader than those involving reproductive matura-
tion between individual and contextual factors. tion. In the past two decades, the field of adolescent
A variety of therapeutic approaches have been health care has grown rapidly, as health educators and
employed successfully in the treatment of anorexia and health care practitioners have come to better under-
bulimia, including individual psychotherapy and cog- stand that the health care needs of adolescents differ
nitive behavioral therapy, group therapy, family ther- from those of children and adults in important respects
apy, and, more recently, antidepressant medications (P. Williams, Holmbeck, & Greenley, 2002).
(Stice et al., 2011). The treatment of anorexia often re-
quires hospitalization initially in order to ensure that
starvation does not progress to fatal or near-fatal levels THE PARADOX OF ADOLESCENT HEALTH
(E. Mitchell, 1985). The treatment of bulimia, espe-
cially with cognitive behavioral therapy, has proven far Adolescence is a paradox as far as physical health is con-
more successful than the treatment of anorexia. cerned. On the one hand, adolescence is one of the
healthiest periods in the life span, characterized by a
relatively low incidence of disabling or chronic illnesses
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION (such as asthma or cancer), fewer short-term hospital
What might be done to counter the impact of cultural stays, and fewer days in which individuals stay home
pressures that encourage the sick. Nonetheless, in the United States, nearly 1 in
development of eating dis- 15 adolescents has at least one disabling chronic ill-
orders among young ness, with the main causes of disability being mental
women? Is the spread of
Western media around the
world likely to contribute adolescent health care A field of study and health care devoted
to rising rates of eating disor- to understanding the health care needs of individuals during the
ders in other countries? second decade of life.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 52 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

52 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

disorders such as depression, respiratory illnesses such while driving (Cvijanovich, Cook, Mann, & Dean,
as asthma, and muscular and skeletal disorders such as 2001; Dee & Evans, 2001; Harré, 2000). Two of the most
arthritis (Ozer & Irwin, 2009). Fortunately, in the past important contributors to serious car accidents involv-
50 years, rates of death and disability resulting from ill- ing teenage drivers are driving at night and driving with
ness and disease during adolescence have decreased other teenagers in the car (Simons-Morton, Hartos,
substantially, and new medical technologies and better Leaf, & Preusser, 2005). This finding has led many states
health care delivery have improved the physical well- to implement graduated driver licensing programs,
being of children, especially those with chronic illnesses which place restrictions on when and with whom teen-
and disabling medical conditions (Quittner, Romero, agers can drive until they have gained sufficient experi-
Kimberg, Blackwell, & Cruz, 2011). Adolescents are far ence; these programs have reduced automobile fatalities
less likely than individuals of any other age to seek and (Simons-Morton, 2011).
receive medical care through traditional office visits to The consensus among health care experts, then, is
practitioners, however; and there are large socioeco- that the most significant threats to the health of today’s
nomic and ethnic disparities in adolescents’ access to youths arise from psychosocial rather than from natu-
health care, with poor and ethnic minority youths far ral causes (Ozer & Irwin, 2009). Unlike some other pe-
less likely to have adequate health insurance and health riods of the life span (such as infancy or old age), when
care access than affluent or White youths (Ozer & Irwin, we are more vulnerable to disease and illness, in ado-
2009). lescence most health problems are preventable. More-
The most virulent threat to adolescent health comes over, patterns of diet, drug use, and exercise established
from unhealthy behaviors (such as drug use), violence during adolescence persist into adulthood (P. Williams
(both self-inflicted and inflicted by others), and risky et al., 2002). As a result of this recognition, the focus in
activity (such as unprotected sexual intercourse or the field of adolescent health has shifted away from tra-
reckless driving) (P. Williams et al., 2002). In some ditional medical models (in which the emphasis is on
senses, then, many of the improvements in preventing the assessment, diagnosis, and treatment of disease)
and treating the traditional medical problems of the and toward more community-oriented, educational
period—those having to do with chronic illnesses— approaches (in which the emphasis is on the preven-
have been offset by what some scientists call the “new tion of illness and injury and the promotion of good
morbidity and mortality” of adolescence. Contributors health) (National Research Council and Institute of
to this new morbidity and mortality include accidents Medicine, 2009).
(especially automobile accidents), suicide, homicide,
substance abuse (including tobacco and alcohol use),
and sexually transmitted diseases (including AIDS). MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
Although most adolescent health problems are
caused by teenagers’ own behavior, getting
CAUSES OF MORTALITY IN ADOLESCENCE
adolescents to change
The contrast between the old and new mortalities of how they live has proven
adolescence is readily apparent. Fifty years ago, illness remarkably difficult. Why
and disease accounted for more than twice as many do you think this is the
deaths among teenagers as violence or injury, but the case? What strategies
reverse is true today. Unintentional injuries are the would you suggest to
leading cause of death worldwide, followed by AIDS, discourage underage
infectious disease, homicide, and suicide, in that order drinking or smoking?
(Blum & Nelson-Mmari, 2004). Approximately 45% of
all teenage deaths in the United States result from car
accidents and other unintentional injuries, and another In other words, instead of asking how we can best
27% are a result of homicide or suicide (Ozer & Irwin, treat sick adolescents, experts in adolescent health care
2009). Adolescents are involved in more driving acci- are now asking how we can encourage adolescents to
dents than adults, primarily because they are less expe- take the steps necessary to prevent illness and disability.
rienced behind the wheel (at any age, new drivers are Current efforts include providing teenagers with edu-
more likely to have accidents than seasoned drivers), cation about alcohol and drug use, accident prevention,
but also because they are more likely to take chances safe sex, and proper nutrition, as well as encouraging
health care professionals to do more direct screening
for risky health practices among their adolescent pa-
tients. As one group of experts put it, “Don’t ask, they
graduated driver licensing A licensing system in which full adult won’t tell” (Blum, Beuhring, Wunderlich, & Resnick,
driving privileges are not granted all at once to teen drivers, but 1996, p. 1767).
phased in over time.
ste32118_ch01_019-053.indd Page 53 21/03/13 9:25 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 1 Biological Transitions 53

PROMOTING ADOLESCENT HEALTH among Black youth. American Indian/Alaska Native


males have a suicide rate higher than that of any other
As many experts point out, health behavior is influ-
racial/ethnic group. Yet, despite their generally poorer
enced by a number of factors, of which knowledge is
health, minority youngsters are less likely to have access
only one component (e.g., H. Leventhal & Keeshan,
to sources of medical care, less likely to visit the doctor
1993). Changes in the context in which adolescents
when ill, and less likely to have health insurance (Ozer &
live (such as the accessibility of handguns or the avail-
Irwin, 2009).
ability of tobacco, alcohol, and illicit drugs) must
The combination of poor health and limited access
accompany changes in adolescents’ knowledge and
to health care is even more concentrated among the
understanding if lasting health promotion is to be
sizable proportion of adolescents who live in poverty,
accomplished (Nation et al., 2003). For example, inves-
a disproportionate number of whom are from ethnic
tigations of the impact of changing one element of the
minority backgrounds (Klerman, 1993). There is now
broader context of adolescent health—the legal drink-
convincing evidence that the links between health and
ing age—have found that raising the age leads to a sig-
socioeconomic status are strong and pervasive across
nificant decline in accidental death rates among young
different sorts of health problems, with physical and
automobile drivers and pedestrians, as well as in the
mental health problems increasing linearly as one
rates of unintentional injuries not involving cars and
moves down the socioeconomic ladder (Adler et al.,
homicides (N. Jones, Pieper, & Robertson, 1992; Kypri
1994; Keating & Hertzman, 2000). Because increases
et al., 2006). Similarly, the single most effective policy
in the size of the adolescent population over the next
for reducing teen smoking has been raising the price of
several decades worldwide will be concentrated
cigarettes (Gruber & Zinman, 2001), and the prevalence
among poor and minority youth (Fussell & Greene,
of adolescent smoking is a direct function of the num-
2002), the most daunting challenge facing health care
ber of retail outlets selling tobacco in their immediate
providers and policymakers will be finding ways of
neighborhood (Novak, Reardon, Raudenbush, & Buka,
minimizing or even eliminating the socioeconomic
2006). New research also indicates that positive psycho-
and ethnic disparities in health and health care that
logical functioning has a beneficial impact on physical
currently exist around the world (Call et al., 2002;
health among adolescents, as is the case among adults
Ozer & Irwin, 2009).
(Hoyt, Chase-Lansdale, McDade, & Adam, 2012).
Improving the health of young people is an espe-
cially important concern among those working with
adolescents who are poor or from ethnic minority
RECAP
groups, because these youngsters are at greater risk for • The most important physical health problems af-
many of the old and new morbidities and mortalities of flicting adolescents have behavioral rather than
adolescence (Ozer & Irwin, 2009). Non-White young- natural causes.
sters, for example, are relatively more likely than White • The leading cause of mortality among adoles-
youngsters to suffer from a chronic illness (Ozer & cents around the world is unintentional
Irwin, 2009), to be obese or to have high blood pressure accidents.
or high cholesterol levels (National Heart, Lung, and • Experts in adolescent health care argue that it is
Blood Institute Growth and Health Study Research important to focus on changing adolescents’
Group, 1992), to be physically inactive (Wolf et al., behavior in order to prevent health-
1993), to be victims of violent crimes (Earls, Cairns, & compromising activities and
Mercy, 1993), to contract AIDS (Sells & Blum, 1996), to encourage health-
die from drowning (Warneke & Cooper, 1994), and to promoting ones.
be murdered (Sorenson, Richardson, & Peterson, 1993).
Homicide is the leading cause of death for Black
adolescents, accounting for almost half of all deaths
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 54 21/03/13 9:28 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 55 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
2
Cognitive Transitions
CHANGES IN COGNITION
Thinking About Possibilities
Thinking About Abstract Concepts
Thinking About Thinking
Thinking in Multiple Dimensions
Adolescent Relativism
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON ADOLESCENT THINKING
The Piagetian View of Adolescent Thinking
The Information-Processing View of Adolescent Thinking
THE ADOLESCENT BRAIN
What Changes in Adolescence?
Implications for Adolescent Behavior
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN INTELLIGENCE IN ADOLESCENCE
The Measurement of IQ
Types of Intelligence
Intelligence Test Performance in Adolescence
Culture and Intelligence
ADOLESCENT THINKING IN CONTEXT
Social Cognition in Adolescence
Adolescent Risk Taking

55
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 56 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

56 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

CHANGES IN COGNITION, or thinking, represent the During the last two decades, scientists have made
second of three fundamental changes that occur during tremendous gains in understanding brain maturation
adolescence—the others being the biological changes during adolescence through the use of imaging tech-
of puberty and the transition into new social roles. Like niques that permit us to look inside the adolescent
developments in the other two domains, the cognitive brain, just as an X-ray permits physicians to look di-
transitions of adolescence have far-reaching implica- rectly at bones. We now have a good idea of how the
tions for the young person’s psychological develop- brain changes during the adolescent years and the
ment and social relations. Indeed, the expansion of implications of these changes for behavioral, emo-
thought during adolescence represents as significant an tional, and, of course, cognitive development. Later
event and as important an influence on the adolescent’s in this chapter, we’ll look at brain maturation in ado-
development and behavior as puberty. In the view of lescence in detail. But let's begin by simply describ-
some writers, adolescence is a critical period for the ing how adolescents think and, more importantly,
development of more complex and sophisticated ways how their thinking differs from that of children and
of reasoning about the world (Kuhn, 2009). adults.

Changes in Cognition adolescents often do, about how their personalities


might change in the future, or how their lives might be
Most people would agree that adolescents are “smarter” affected by different career choices, or how they might
than children. Not only do teenagers know more than have been different had they grown up under different
children—after all, the longer we live, the more oppor- circumstances. As a young child, you simply are who you
tunities we have to acquire new information—but they are. But as an adolescent, who you are is just one possi-
actually think in ways that are more advanced, more bility of who you could be.
efficient, and generally more effective. This can be seen This does not mean that children are incapable of
in five chief ways (Keating, 2011): imagination or fantasy. Nor does it mean that children
are unable to conceive of things being different from the
1. Adolescents are better able than children to think
way they observe them to be. Rather, the advantage that
about what is possible, instead of limiting their
adolescents enjoy over children when it comes to think-
thought to what is real.
ing about possibilities is that adolescents are able to
2. Adolescents are better able than children to think move easily between the specific and the abstract, to
about abstract things. generate alternative possibilities and explanations sys-
3. Adolescents think more often than children about tematically, and to compare what they actually observe
the process of thinking itself. with what they believe is possible.
4. Adolescents’ thinking, compared to children’s, is The adolescent’s ability to reason systematically in
more often multidimensional, rather than limited terms of what is possible comes in handy in a variety of
to a single issue. mathematical, scientific, and other problem-solving
5. Adolescents are more likely than children to see contexts. For instance, the study of mathematics in ju-
things as relative, rather than as absolute. nior and senior high school (algebra, geometry, and trig-
onometry) often requires that you begin with an abstract
Let’s look at each of these advantages—and some of or theoretical formulation—for example, “the square of
their implications for adolescents’ behavior—in greater a right triangle’s hypotenuse is equal to the sum of the
detail. squares of the other two sides” (the Pythagorean
theorem). This theorem, after all, is a proposition about
the possible rather than the real. It is a statement about
THINKING ABOUT POSSIBILITIES
all possible right triangles, not just triangles that you
Adolescents’ thinking is less bound to concrete events might actually observe. In mathematics, you learn how
than is that of children. Children’s thinking is oriented to to apply these theorems to concrete examples (that is,
the here and now—that is, to things and events that they real triangles). Scientific experimentation also involves
can observe directly. But adolescents are able to consider the ability to generate possibilities systematically. In a
what they observe against a backdrop of what is possible. chemistry experiment in which you are trying to identify
Put another way, for the child, what is possible is what is an unknown substance by performing various tests, you
real; for the adolescent, what is real is just a subset of must first be able to imagine alternative possibilities for
what is possible. Consider how individuals think about the substance’s identity in order to know what tests to
themselves. Children generally do not wonder, the way conduct.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 57 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 57

The adolescent’s use of this sort of thinking is not If you answer this type of question quickly, without
limited to scientific problem solving. We see it in the thinking it through, you might say that Kim is indeed a
types of arguments adolescents employ, in which they hockey player. But, in fact, this is not necessarily the case.
are better able than younger children to envision and Whereas children are easily fooled by such problems,
therefore anticipate the possible responses of an op- adolescents are more likely to say that there is no way of
ponent and to have handy a counterargument or se- knowing whether Kim plays hockey, because we are not
ries of counterarguments. Many parents believe that told that the only people who wear mouth guards are
their children become more argumentative during hockey players. One reason for their superior perfor-
adolescence. What probably happens, though, is that mance on these sorts of problems is that adolescents are
their children become better arguers (Steinberg, 2011). better able to catch themselves before they incorrectly
An adolescent does not accept other people’s points of answer the question and pause a moment before re-
view unquestioningly—including his or her parents’ sponding (Daniel & Klaczynski, 2006). Deductive rea-
viewpoints—but instead evaluates them against other soning is seldom used before adolescence, and its
theoretically possible beliefs. As you’ll see in Chapter development is seen by many researchers as the major
4, this improvement in the adolescent’s intellectual intellectual accomplishment of adolescence (Klaczynski &
ability likely contributes to the bickering and squab- Narasimham, 1998; Morris & Sloutsky, 2001). As you
bling that often occur between teenagers and their will read later in this chapter, the ability to stop yourself
parents (Smetana, 1989). before acting automatically (and perhaps incorrectly) is
controlled by a region of the brain that has been shown
to mature during adolescence (Luna, Garver, Urban,
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION Lazar, & Sweeney, 2004).
In what ways did your high school classes take advan-
tage of the advanced think- ❚ Hypothetical Thinking Related to the develop-
ing abilities that develop ment of deductive reasoning is the emergence of
in adolescence? In what hypothetical, or “if-then,” thinking. In order to think hy-
ways were opportunities pothetically, you need to see beyond what is directly ob-
to do this missed? What servable and apply logical reasoning to anticipate what
might teachers do to stimu- might be possible. Being able to plan ahead, to see the
late more advanced thinking? future consequences of an action, and to provide alterna-
tive explanations of events are all dependent on being
able to hypothesize effectively.
Thinking in hypothetical terms also permits us to
❚ Deductive Reasoning One manifestation of the suspend our beliefs about something in order to argue
adolescent’s increased facility with thinking about pos- in the abstract. Being capable of assuming a hypotheti-
sibilities is the development of deductive reasoning. cal stance is important when it comes to debating an
Deductive reasoning is a type of logical reasoning in issue, since doing so permits us to understand the logic
which you draw logically necessary conclusions from a behind the other person’s argument without necessarily
general set of premises, or givens. Consider the follow- agreeing with its conclusion. Playing devil’s advocate,
ing problem: for example—as when you formulate a position con-
All hockey players wear mouth guards. trary to what you really believe in order to challenge
Kim is a hockey player. someone else’s reasoning—requires the ability to think
in hypothetical terms.
Does Kim wear a mouth guard?
Hypothetical thinking also has implications for the
Individuals who reason deductively understand that adolescent’s social behavior. It helps the young person to
the correct conclusion (that Kim wears a mouth guard) take the perspective of others by enabling him or her to
necessarily follows from the first two statements. No think through what someone else might be thinking or
additional knowledge about hockey or about Kim is feeling, given that person’s point of view (“If I were in
necessary to reach the correct answer. Adolescents are her situation, I would feel pretty angry”). Hypothetical
also better able than children to recognize when a logi- thinking helps in formulating and arguing a viewpoint,
cal problem does not provide sufficient information because it allows adolescents to think a step ahead of the
and to respond by saying that the question can’t be an- opposition—a cognitive tool that comes in handy when
swered with any certainty. Suppose we were to change dealing with parents (“If they come back with ‘You have
the problem to read like this:
All hockey players wear mouth guards.
deductive reasoning A type of logical reasoning in which one
Kim is wearing a mouth guard. draws logically necessary conclusions from a general set of
Is Kim a hockey player? premises, or givens.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 58 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

58 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

to stay home and clean up the garage,’ then I’ll remind processes they are using. Adolescence is an important
them about the time they let my sister go out when she time for changes in our understanding of what knowl-
had chores to do”). And hypothetical thinking plays an edge is and how it is acquired (Schommer, Calvert,
important role in decision making, because it permits Gariglietti, & Bajaj, 1997). When asked, adolescents
the young person to plan ahead and to foresee the con- can explain not only what they know but why know-
sequences of choosing one alternative over another (“If ing what they know enables them to think differently
I go out for the soccer team, then I am going to have to and solve problems more effectively (Reich, Oser, &
give up my part-time job”). Valentin, 1994). In addition, adolescents are much bet-
ter able than children to understand that people do not
have complete control over their mental activity. One
THINKING ABOUT ABSTRACT CONCEPTS study found, for example, that adolescents and adults
were much more likely than children to understand
The appearance of more systematic, abstract thinking is
that it is impossible to go for a long period of time
the second notable aspect of cognitive development
without thinking about anything, that we often have
during adolescence. We noted earlier that children’s
thoughts that we do not want to have, and that un-
thinking is more concrete and more bound to observ-
wanted thoughts we try to get rid of often return
able events and objects than is that of adolescents. This
(Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1998).
difference is clearly evident when we consider the abil-
Another interesting way in which thinking about
ity to deal with abstract concepts—things that cannot
thinking becomes more apparent during adolescence is
be experienced directly through the senses.
in increased introspection and self-consciousness. When
The adolescent’s greater facility with abstract thinking
we are introspective, we are thinking about our own
is clearly seen in the adolescent’s increased facility and
emotions. When we are self-conscious, we are thinking
interest in thinking about interpersonal relationships,
about how others think about us. As we’ll see in Chapter
politics, philosophy, religion, and morality—topics that
8, these processes permit the sorts of self-examination
involve such abstract concepts as friendship, faith, de-
and exploration that are important components of the
mocracy, fairness, and honesty. The growth of social
young person’s attempt to establish a coherent sense of
thinking—generally referred to as “social cognition”—
identity.
during adolescence is directly related to the young per-
son’s improving ability to think abstractly. Later in this
chapter, we will examine the ways in which social think-
ing improves in adolescence. MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
What metacognitive tools do you use to aid
in your own studying? When did you start
THINKING ABOUT THINKING using them?
A third noteworthy gain in cognitive ability during
adolescence involves thinking about thinking itself, a
process sometimes referred to as metacognition. Meta-
cognition often involves monitoring your own cogni- ❚ Adolescent Egocentrism These intellectual ad-
tive activity during the thinking process—for example, vances may occasionally result in problems for the
when you consciously use a strategy for remembering young adolescent, particularly before she or he adjusts
something (such as Every Good Boy Deserves Fun, for to having such powerful cognitive tools. Being able to
the notes of the treble clef in music notation) or when introspect, for instance, may lead to periods of extreme
you appraise your own comprehension of something self-absorption—referred to as “adolescent egocen-
you are reading before going on to the next paragraph. trism” (Elkind, 1967). Adolescent egocentrism results
Interventions designed to improve adolescents’ meta- in two distinct problems in thinking that help to ex-
cognitive skills have been shown to enhance reading, plain some of the seemingly odd beliefs and behaviors
writing, test taking, and performance on homework of teenagers (Goossens, Seiffge-Krenke, & Marcoen,
(W. Williams et al., 2002). Not only do adolescents 1992).
“manage” their thinking more than children do, but The first, the imaginary audience, involves having
they are also better able to explain to others the such a heightened sense of self-consciousness that the
teenager imagines that his or her behavior is the focus
of everyone else’s attention. For example, a teenager
metacognition The process of thinking about thinking itself.
who is going to a concert with 10,000 other people may
worry about dressing the right way because “everybody
imaginary audience The belief, often brought on by the will notice.” Given the cognitive limitations of adoles-
heightened self-consciousness of early adolescence, that everyone cent egocentrism, it is difficult indeed to persuade the
is watching and evaluating one’s behavior.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 59 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 59

young person that the “audience” is not all that con-


cerned with his or her behavior or appearance. Accord-
ing to one study (Rankin, Lane, Gibbons, & Gerrard,
2004), feelings of self-consciousness, which on average
are more intense among girls than boys, increase dur-
ing early adolescence, peak around age 15, and then de-
cline, presumably as the adolescent gains more social
confidence. And evidence from recent studies of brain
maturation suggests that the parts of the brain that
process social information—such as perceptions of
what others are thinking—undergo significant change
during early adolescence, just when self-consciousness
is increasing (Burnett, Sebastian, Kadosh, & Blakemore,
2011; E. Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005;
Pfeifer & Blakemore, 2012). In fact, brain imaging stud-
ies indicate that adolescents’ self-perceptions rely more
on their sense of what others think of them than is the
case among adults (Pfeifer, Masten, Borofsky, Dapretto,
Adolescent egocentrism brought on by the cognitive changes of the
Fuligni et al., 2009).
period can contribute to a heightened sense of self-consciousness.
A second problem resulting from adolescent egocen-
trism is called the personal fable. The personal fable
revolves around the adolescent’s egocentric (and erro- easy to imagine that the same adolescent who is worried
neous) belief that his or her experiences are unique. For about being seen by “everyone” at a rock concert might
instance, an adolescent whose relationship with a girl- not endorse a questionnaire item that reads, “When I am
friend has just ended might tell his sympathetic mother at a concert, I think everyone in the audience is looking
that she could not possibly understand what it feels like at me.” One explanation for this discrepancy is that ado-
to break up with someone—even though breaking up is lescent egocentrism is an emotional and social phenom-
something that most people have experienced plenty of enon, not a cognitive one. Another is that adolescents
times during their adolescent and young adult years. are overly concerned about what others think of them
Maintaining a personal fable of uniqueness has some not because they are intellectually compromised, but
benefits, in that it enhances adolescents’ self-esteem and because, in the highly social world in which they live,
feelings of self-importance. But holding on to a personal other peoples’ opinions actually have genuine and im-
fable also can be dangerous, as in the case of a sexually portant consequences (J. Bell & Bromnick, 2000). It’s
active adolescent who believes that pregnancy simply like the old joke: “Just because you’re paranoid doesn’t
won’t happen to her or a reckless driver who believes mean that people aren’t following you.”
that he will defy the laws of nature by taking hairpin
turns at breakneck speed.
Although it had once been thought that much of MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
the risk-taking behavior engaged in by adolescents was
Think back to your own adolescence. Can
due in part to teenagers’ heightened susceptibility to the
you recall times when you experienced an
personal fable, researchers have found it difficult to con-
imaginary audience? How about more re-
firm that egocentrism actually peaks in early adolescence.
cently? Do you think this happened more
In fact, certain aspects of adolescent egocentrism, such as
when you were younger than it does now?
the personal fable, may persist through the adult years
(Frankenberger, 2000; Quadrel, Fischhoff, & Davis,
1993). Ask any adult cigarette smoker if she or he is aware
THINKING IN MULTIPLE DIMENSIONS
of the scientific evidence linking cigarette smoking with
heart and lung disease, and you’ll see that the personal A fourth way in which thinking changes during adoles-
fable is quite common among many individuals who cence involves the ability to think about things in a mul-
have long since left adolescence. tidimensional fashion (Huizenga, Crone, & Jansen,
Why have studies failed to find that egocentrism is 2007; Kuhn, 2009). Whereas children tend to think
greater in adolescence than at other ages? One problem
with many studies of adolescent egocentrism is that they
rely on fairly simple questionnaires to assess rather
complicated belief systems invoked in real-life situations personal fable An adolescent’s belief that he or she is unique and
(Alberts, Elkind, & Ginsberg, 2007). For example, it is therefore not subject to the rules that govern other people’s
behavior.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 60 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

60 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

about things one aspect at a time, adolescents can see and the context in which it is said. If I turned to you
things through more complicated lenses. For instance, during a boring lecture, rolled my eyes, and said, in an
when a certain hitter comes up to the plate in a baseball exaggeratedly earnest tone, “This is the most interesting
game, a preadolescent who knows that the hitter has a lecture I’ve ever heard,” you would know that I actually
good home-run record might exclaim that the batter meant just the opposite. But you would know this only
will hit the ball out of the park. An adolescent, however, if you paid attention to my inflection and to the context,
would consider the hitter’s record in relation to the spe- as well as the content, of my statement. Only by attend-
cific pitcher on the mound and would weigh both fac- ing simultaneously to multiple dimensions of speech
tors, or dimensions, before making a prediction (perhaps can we distinguish between the sincere and the sarcastic.
this player often hits homers against left-handed pitch- Not surprisingly, our ability to use and detect sarcasm
ers but strikes out against righties). and irony improves during preadolescence and adoles-
The ability to think in multidimensional terms is cence (Glenwright & Pexman, 2010).
evident in a variety of situations. Obviously, adoles- Why do young adolescents laugh hysterically when
cents can give much more complicated answers than characters in movies aimed at their age group say things
children to questions such as “Why did the Civil War like “He said ‘erector set’”? Adolescents’ increased facility
begin?” or “How did Jane Austen’s novels reflect the in thinking along multiple dimensions also permits them
changing position of women in European society?” to appreciate satire, metaphor, and the ways in which
Thorough answers to these sorts of questions require language can be used to convey multiple messages, as in
thinking about several dimensions simultaneously. double entendres—expressions that have two meanings,
The development of a more sophisticated under- one of them typically rude or crude. Teenagers’ new-
standing of probability is also made possible by an im- found ability to use and appreciate sarcasm, irony, and
proved ability to think in multidimensional terms. satire helps to explain why shows like The Simpsons,
Suppose I give you a set of blue and yellow beads. Now South Park, and Family Guy have always had such strong
I ask you to divide them into two containers so that the appeal in this age group. (Not to mention that they are
containers have different numbers of beads overall but often pretty funny to adults, too. Our son’s school once
the probability of reaching into a container and picking summoned his class’s parents to watch an “offensive”
a blue bead is the same for each. In order to do so, you episode of South Park to show us how our children were
would have to vary the number of blue beads and the being harmed by television; the demonstration ended
number of yellow beads between the two containers, prematurely, though, because we parents were laughing
because the probability of drawing a blue bead is a func- too hard.)
tion of both the number of blue beads and the number
of yellow beads. It is not until early adolescence that
ADOLESCENT RELATIVISM
individuals can solve this sort of problem successfully
(Falk & Wilkening, 1998). A final aspect of cognition that changes during adoles-
As is the case with other gains in cognitive ability, the cence concerns a shift from seeing things in absolute
increasing capability of individuals to think in multiple terms—in black and white—to seeing things as relative.
dimensions also has consequences for their behavior Compared to children, adolescents are more likely to
and thinking outside of academic settings. Adolescents question others’ assertions and less likely to accept
describe themselves and others in more differentiated “facts” as absolute truths.
and complicated terms (“I’m both shy and extro- This increase in relativism can be particularly exas-
verted”) and find it easier to look at problems from perating to parents, who may feel as though their ado-
multiple perspectives (“I know that’s the way you see it, lescent children question everything just for the
but try to look at it from her point of view”). Under- sake of argument. Difficulties often arise, for example,
standing that people’s personalities are not one-sided or when adolescents begin seeing parents’ values that
that social situations can have different interpretations, they had previously considered absolutely correct
depending on one’s point of view, permits the adoles- (“Moral people do not have sex before they are mar-
cent to have far more sophisticated—and far more ried”) as completely relative (“Welcome to the twenty-
complicated—self-conceptions as well as relationships first century, Dad”).
with other people. Adolescents’ belief that everything is relative can be-
come so overwhelming that they may become extremely
❚ Sarcasm and South Park One interesting mani- skeptical about many things (Chandler, 1987). In fact,
festation of adolescents’ ability to look at things in mul- once adolescents begin to doubt the certainty of things
tiple dimensions concerns the development of children’s that they had previously believed, they may come to
understanding of sarcasm. As an adult, you understand believe that everything is uncertain, that no knowledge
that the meaning of a speaker’s statement is communi- is completely reliable. Some theorists have suggested
cated by a combination of what is said, how it is said, that adolescents pass through such a period of extreme
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 61 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 61

skepticism on the way toward reaching a more sophisti- THE PIAGETIAN VIEW OF
cated understanding of the complexity of knowledge. ADOLESCENT THINKING
❚ Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development
Generally, theorists who adopt a Piagetian perspective
RECAP
and take a cognitive-developmental view of intellectual
• One of the most important changes in thinking development argue that cognitive development pro-
during adolescence is the increased ease with ceeds through a fixed sequence of qualitatively distinct
which individuals think about what is possible or stages, that adolescent thinking is fundamentally differ-
hypothetical. ent from the type of thinking employed by children, and
• Being able to think about what is possible, and not that during adolescence, individuals develop a special
just what is real, is reflected in improvements in de- type of thinking that they use in a variety of situations.
ductive reasoning—reasoning that is based on prin- According to Piaget, cognitive development proceeds
ciples of logic. through four stages: (1) the sensorimotor period (from
• Adolescence is a time of improvements in thinking birth until about age 2), (2) the preoperational period
about abstract concepts. (from about age 2 until about age 5), (3) the period of
• During adolescence, individuals become better concrete operations (from about age 6 until early ado-
able to engage in “thinking about thinking,” or lescence), and (4) the period of formal operations
metacognition. (from adolescence through adulthood). Each stage is
• Compared with children, adolescents are better characterized by a particular type of thinking, with ear-
able to think in multiple dimensions. lier stages of thinking being incorporated into new,
• Adolescents often go through a stage of relativistic more advanced, and more adaptive forms of reasoning.
thinking, during which they question According to Piaget, transitions into higher stages of
the validity of absolutes. reasoning are most likely to occur when the child’s bio-
logical readiness and the increasing complexity of envi-
ronmental demands interact to stimulate more advanced
thinking.
Piagetian theorists believe that the use of abstract logi-
cal reasoning—the foundation of formal-operational
thinking—is the chief feature of adolescent thinking that
Theoretical Perspectives differentiates it from the type of thinking employed by
on Adolescent Thinking children (Keating, 2011). This is similar to the claim, dis-
cussed earlier, that the ability to use deductive reasoning
Although there is general agreement that adolescents’
is the defining feature of adolescent cognition. We noted
thinking is different and more advanced than children’s
that adolescents’ thinking can be distinguished from the
in the ways we have just described, there is far less con-
thinking of children in several respects—among them, in
sensus about the processes underlying the cognitive dif-
thinking about possibilities, in thinking multidimension-
ferences between children and adolescents. Part of the
ally, and in thinking about abstract concepts. The connec-
lack of agreement stems from the fact that no one single
tion between these types of thinking and the development
factor distinguishes thinking during adolescence from
thinking during childhood (Keating, 2011). And part of
the lack of agreement stems from the different points of
view that theorists have taken toward cognitive devel-
cognitive-developmental view A perspective on development,
opment in general. Because researchers working from based on the work of Piaget, that takes a qualitative, stage-theory
different theoretical perspectives have posed different approach.
research questions, used different tasks to measure cog-
nitive growth, and emphasized different aspects of sensorimotor period The first stage of cognitive development,
cognitive activity, their studies provide different, but according to Piaget, spanning the period roughly between birth and
nevertheless compatible, pictures of mental develop- age 2.
ment during adolescence. preoperational period The second stage of cognitive
The two theoretical viewpoints that have been espe- development, according to Piaget, spanning roughly ages 2–5.
cially important are the Piagetian perspective and the
concrete operations The third stage of cognitive development,
information-processing perspective. Although these
according to Piaget, spanning the period roughly between age 6 and
two views of adolescent thinking begin from different early adolescence.
assumptions about the nature of cognitive development
in general, they each provide valuable insight into why formal operations The fourth stage of cognitive development,
thinking changes during adolescence (Kuhn, 2009). according to Piaget, spanning the period from early adolescence
through adulthood.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 62 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

62 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Improvements in selective attention and divided attention enable adolescents to tune out interference and focus on the task at hand.

of formal operations is clear: In order to think about al- try to pull a lucky lottery ticket from an envelope of 10
ternatives to what really exists, in order to think in multi- tickets, of which only 1 is lucky, versus an envelope of
dimensional terms, and in order to systematically think 100 tickets, of which 10 are lucky, most people select the
about concepts that are not directly observable, you must second option—even though, if quizzed, they know that
have a system of reasoning that works just as well in ab- the mathematical odds of pulling a lucky ticket are iden-
stract, imagined, and complicated situations as it does in tical in the two scenarios.
concrete ones. Although its influence has waned considerably over
Not all adolescents (or, for that matter, all adults) the past three decades, the Piagetian perspective on cog-
develop formal-operational thinking or employ it regu- nitive development during adolescence has stimulated a
larly and in a variety of situations. At least some re- great deal of research on how young people think
search suggests that adolescents who have received (Keating, 2011). Where the perspective falls short is in its
explicit instruction in deductive reasoning are more claim that cognitive development proceeds in a stage-like
likely to display formal thinking, which suggests that fashion and that the stage of formal operations is the
the development of advanced reasoning abilities can be stage of cognitive development characteristic of adoles-
facilitated by training (Morris & Sloutsky, 1998). Con- cence (Keating, 2011; Kuhn, 2009). In fact, very little re-
sistent with this, one analysis of French students’ rea- search supports this view. Rather, research suggests that
soning abilities found that adolescents in the 1990s advanced reasoning capabilities develop gradually and
performed significantly better than their counterparts continuously from childhood through adolescence and
who were tested 20 or 30 years earlier, a change that beyond, in more of a steady fashion than was proposed
maybe related to changes in schooling (Flieller, 1999). by Piaget (that is, more like a ramp than like a staircase).
The extent to which formal-operational thinking is Rather than talking about a stage of cognitive activity
displayed consistently by an individual depends a great characteristic of adolescence, then, it is more accurate to
deal on the conditions under which his or her reasoning depict these advanced reasoning capabilities as skills that
is assessed (Overton, 1990). It is important, therefore, to are employed by older children more often than by
differentiate between competence (that is, what the ado- younger ones, by some adolescents more often than by
lescent is capable of doing) and performance (that is, others, and by individuals when they are in certain situa-
what the adolescent actually does in the assessment situ- tions (especially familiar ones) more often than when
ation). Much research, on adults as well as adolescents, they are in other ones (Kuhn, 2009).
indicates that gaps between individuals’ logical reason-
ing abilities and their actual use of logical reasoning in
THE INFORMATION-PROCESSING VIEW
everyday situations are very large, with everyday deci-
OF ADOLESCENT THINKING
sion making fraught with logical errors that cannot be
explained by cognitive incompetence (Klaczynski, 2000, Whereas Piaget attempted to describe adolescent thinking
2001). For example, if asked whether they would rather as a whole, and to use one overarching concept—formal
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 63 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 63

operations—to characterize the period, others have tried were about two and a half years old, but not much earlier
to identify the specific abilities that improve as individu- than this (Reese, Jack, & White, 2010).
als move from childhood into adolescence and beyond. When we think of the importance of memory in
Just what is it about the ways adolescents think about problem solving, we typically think of having to retrieve
things that makes them better problem solvers than chil- facts that we deliberately have memorized—one aspect
dren? This question has been the focus of researchers of long-term memory. But working memory may be
working from a second theoretical vantage point: the even more important for the sort of problem solving
information-processing perspective. likely encountered in adolescence. For example, in order
Studies of changes in specific components of infor- to answer multiple-choice questions, you need to be able
mation processing have focused on five areas in which to remember each option long enough to compare it
improvement occurs during adolescence: attention, with the other choices as you read them. Think for a mo-
memory, processing speed, organization, and metacog- ment of how frustrating it would be to try to solve a
nition. Studies show that improvements in all of these multiple-choice problem if, by the time you had read the
areas take place as individuals move from childhood final potential answer, you had forgotten the first one!
through adolescence, mainly during the first half of the Studies show that working memory skills for both
adolescent decade (Demetriou, Christou, Spanoudis, & verbal and visual information increase between child-
Platsidou, 2002; Keating, 2004). Taken together, these hood and adolescence and over the course of adoles-
gains help to explain why adolescents are better than cence (Hale, Bronik, & Fry, 1997; Sprondel, Kipp, &
children at abstract, multidimensional, and hypotheti- Mecklinger, 2011). Improvements in working memory
cal thinking. coincide with the continued maturation of brain re-
gions during adolescence that are responsible for this
❚ Attention During adolescence, there are advances aspect of cognition (Conklin, Luciana, Hooper, & Yarger,
in individuals’ ability to pay attention. Improvements 2007; Jolles, Kleibeuker, Rombouts, & Crone, 2011;
are seen both in selective attention, in which adoles- Klingberg, 2006; O’Hare, Lu, Houston, Bookheimer, &
cents must focus on one stimulus (such as a reading Sowell, 2008). More specifically, improvements in work-
assignment) and tune out another (such as the elec- ing memory during adolescence are linked to the ways
tronic beeping of a younger brother’s computer game), in which these areas of the brain are organized and con-
and in divided attention, in which adolescents must nected, which permits more efficient and powerful in-
pay attention to two sets of stimuli at the same time formation processing (Finn, Sheridan, Kam, Hinshaw, &
(such as studying while texting back and forth with D’Esposito, 2010; Ghetti, DeMaster, Yonelinas, & Bunge,
a friend) (Mizuno et al., 2011; Monk et al., 2003). 2010; Schleepen & Jonkman, 2009).
Improvements in attention mean that adolescents are
better able than children to concentrate and stay focused
on complicated tasks, such as reading and comprehend- MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
ing difficult material (Casteel, 1993). There also is con- What is your earliest memory? How old
siderable evidence that the ability to inhibit an unwanted were you at the time this event took place?
response (for instance, stopping yourself from looking
up at a commercial that suddenly appears on the televi-
sion in the corner of the room while you are reading)
improves during early and middle adolescence (Kuhn,
2009). This improvement is likely linked to maturation
of brain systems that govern impulse control (Luna, information-processing perspective A perspective on cognition
Garver, Urban, Lazar, & Sweeney, 2004). that derives from the study of artificial intelligence and attempts to
explain cognitive development in terms of the growth of specific
❚ Memory Second, during adolescence, memory components of the thinking process (such as memory).
abilities improve. This is reflected both in working selective attention The process by which we focus on one
memory, which involves the ability to remember some- stimulus while tuning out another.
thing for a brief period of time, such as 30 seconds, and
divided attention The process of paying attention to two or more
in long-term memory, which involves being able to re-
stimuli at the same time.
call something from a long time ago (Keating, 2004).
Studies of adolescents’ ability to remember personally working memory That aspect of memory in which information is
meaningful events from earlier in life, an aspect of long- held for a short time while a problem is being solved.
term memory called autobiographical memory, find long-term memory The ability to recall something from a long
that our earliest memories, some of which we lose during time ago.
childhood, stabilize sometime during early adolescence,
when most people can remember back to when they autobiographical memory The recall of personally meaningful
past events.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 64 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

64 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

textbook chapter. After years of studying, you are proba-


bly well aware of particular strategies that work well for
50 you (underlining, highlighting, taking notes, writing in
Speed of performance

the margins), and you begin a reading assignment with


40 these strategies in mind. Because children are not as
“planful” as adolescents, their learning is not as efficient.
30 Developmental differences in levels of planning during
childhood and adolescence can be seen quite readily by
comparing individuals’ approaches to the guessing game
20
20 Questions. With age, individuals’ strategies become
increasingly more efficient—when guessing the name of
5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 a person, an adolescent might begin by asking whether
Age (years) the person is dead or alive, whereas a young child might
FIGURE 2.1 Speed of information processing increases
just start throwing out the names of specific people
markedly between ages 5 and 15 and then begins to level (Drumm & Jackson, 1996).
off. (Adapted from Kail & Ferrer, 2007)
❚ Metacognition Finally, individuals’ knowledge
about their own thinking processes improves during
adolescence. We noted earlier that one of the most
❚ Speed A third component of information process- important gains in adolescence is in the realm of
ing that may be closely related to the observed improve- metacognition—thinking about thinking. Adolescents
ments in thinking is an increase in the sheer speed of are more likely than children to think about their own
information processing (Kail & Ferrer, 2007). Regardless thoughts—a tendency, as we saw, that helps to explain
of the type of cognitive task employed, researchers find their greater self-consciousness. One explanation for
that older adolescents process the information necessary this emphasizes adolescents’ greater sensitivity to social
to solve the problem faster than early adolescents, who, information. But from an information-processing per-
in turn, process information faster than preadolescents. spective, adolescents’ heightened self-consciousness re-
This fact would certainly help explain age differences sults from advances in basic cognitive abilities. For the
in performance on timed tests, such as standardized first time, the adolescent is capable of “thinking about
achievement tests. Generally, the increase in the speed of thinking about thinking,” a process that places demands
information processing that occurs with age becomes on working memory. Once you begin thinking about
smaller over the course of adolescence. Therefore, the what other people might think you are thinking, it is
difference in speed between a 9-year-old and a 12-year- hard to avoid becoming self-conscious.
old is greater than that between a 12-year-old and a Of course, advances in metacognition are more a
15-year-old, which, in turn, is greater than that between blessing than a curse. Because adolescents are better
a 15-year-old and an 18-year-old (see Figure 2.1) (Kail & able to think about their own thoughts, they are much
Ferrer, 2007). In fact, speed of processing does not ap- better at monitoring their own learning processes
pear to change very much between middle adolescence (Crone, Somsen, Zanolie, & Van der Molen, 2006;
and young adulthood (Brahmbhatt, McAuley, & Barch, Kuhn, 2009). For example, during the course of study-
2008). ing, adolescents are more able than children to step
back and assess how well they are learning the material.
❚ Organization A fourth type of information- Doing this enables them to pace their studying
processing gain seen in adolescence involves improve- accordingly—to speed up and skim the material if they
ments in individuals’ organizational strategies (Siegler, feel that they are learning it easily or to slow down and
2006). Adolescents are more “planful” than children— repeat a section if they feel that they are having a hard
they are more likely to approach a problem with an ap- time. Recent studies of brain maturation indicate that
propriate information-processing strategy in mind and brain systems that are active when individuals are mon-
are more flexible in their ability to use different strategies itoring their own performance continue to mature
in different situations (Plumert, 1994). The use of mne- throughout adolescence and early adulthood, which
monic devices (such as using HOMES to remember the may play a role in the development of metacognition
names of the Great Lakes—Huron, Ontario, Michigan, (Ladouceur, Dahl, & Carter, 2007).
Erie, and Superior) and other organizational strategies Most basic information-processing skills, like work-
helps to account for differences in the performance of ing memory or attention, as well as logical reasoning
older and younger children on tasks requiring memory abilities, increase throughout childhood and early ado-
(Siegler, 2006). For instance, think for a moment about lescence and then level off around age 15 (Gathercole,
how you approach learning the information in a new Pickering, Ambridge, & Wearing, 2004; Keating, 2004;
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 65 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 65

Luciana, Conklin, Hooper, & Yarger, 2005). In other All this changed about 15 years ago. Since 2000,
words, by the time they have turned 15, adolescents are there has been an explosion in research on adolescent
just as proficient as adults in these basic skills. Where brain development, and the speed with which our un-
adolescents of this age are lacking, relative to adults, derstanding of adolescent brain development has
and where cognitive development continues beyond grown has been absolutely breathtaking (Blakemore,
age 15 and into young adulthood, is in more sophisti- 2011).
cated cognitive skills, such as planning ahead or judging Improvements in the methods used to study brain
the relative costs and benefits of a risky decision, and in maturation—including studies of brain growth and
the coordination of cognition and emotion, when feel- development in other animals (because all mammals
ings might interfere with logical reasoning (for exam- go through puberty, it is possible to study “adolescent”
ple, when you have to make a decision when you are brain development in other species), studies of changes
angry or when faced with peer pressure) (Albert & in brain chemistry in humans and other species, and
Steinberg, 2011). In fact, much of what we have learned postmortem studies of brain anatomy—have advanced
about brain maturation in adolescence—the subject of the field in important ways (Giedd, 2008; Paus, 2009;
the next section—helps explain why the development Spear, 2010). But the major contribution to our under-
of these advanced abilities may not be complete until standing of what takes place in the brain during adoles-
individuals reach their mid-20s. cence has come from studies using various imaging
techniques, especially functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI).
RECAP These techniques allow researchers to take pictures of
• The two dominant theoretical perspectives in the individuals’ brains and compare their anatomy and ac-
study of adolescent cognition are the Piagetian tivity. Some aspects of brain development in adoles-
perspective and the information-processing cence are reflected in changes in brain structure (for
perspective. instance, certain parts of the brain are relatively smaller
• According to Piaget, adolescence is a period char- in childhood than adolescence, while others are rela-
acterized by the stage of formal operations. tively larger), whereas others are reflected not so much
• Although Piaget theorized that thinking during ado- in the brain’s structure but in changes in brain function
lescence is qualitatively different from thinking (for instance, adolescents may use different parts of the
during childhood, research shows that changes in brain than children when performing the same task)
cognition during this time are more gradual. (e.g., Dosenbach et al., 2010; O’Hare, Lu, Houston,
• The information-processing perspective focuses on Bookheimer, & Sowell, 2008; Wang, Huettel, & De
the specific components of thinking, such as mem- Bellis, 2008).
ory and attention, and asks whether and to what Using DTI, scientists are able to see the ways in
extent these improve in adolescence. which various regions of the brain are connected and
• In general, there are major improvements in basic compare patterns of interconnections among people at
cognitive processes in early adolescence, but by different ages (e.g., Klingberg, 2006). This allows us to
the time individuals have reached age 15 or better understand how “communication” patterns link-
so, their basic thinking and rea- ing different regions of the brain change with develop-
soning abilities are com- ment. By using fMRI, researchers can examine patterns
parable to those of activity in various regions of the brain while indi-
of adults. viduals are performing a variety of tasks (for example,
recalling a list of words, viewing photos of friends, or
listening to music). Participants in an fMRI study are
asked to perform tasks on a computer while they are
lying inside a brain scanner. With this setup, it is possi-
ble to study both how patterns of brain activity differ
The Adolescent Brain
Although it had once been believed that improved in-
tellectual functioning in adolescence would be reflected
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) A technique used
in larger brain size, the brain reaches its adult size by to produce images of the brain, often while the subject is
age 10, making it impossible that changes in thinking performing some sort of mental task.
during adolescence are due to sheer increases in the size
or volume of the brain (Paus, 2009). For many years, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) A technique used to produce
scientists could not find links between physical changes images of the brain that shows connections among different regions.
in the brain and improvements in cognitive function- brain structure The physical form or composition of the brain.
ing during adolescence.
brain function Patterns of brain activity.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 66 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

66 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

during different tasks (for example, when we are ac-


tively reading versus being read to) and whether people
of different ages show different patterns of brain activ-
ity while performing the very same task. In our lab, for
instance, my collaborators and I are studying how pat-
terns of brain activity vary when individuals perform
tasks either alone or with their friends watching them,
and whether the ways in which the presence of friends
affects brain activity differ between teenagers and
adults.
Scientists have also studied age differences in pat-
terns of brain activity using electroencephalography
(EEG), which measures electrical activity at different
locations on the scalp. EEG can be used to examine
changes in electrical activity—called event-related po-
tentials (ERP)—in response to different stimuli or
events (Segalowitz & Davies, 2004). Scientists often
compare ERPs between people of different ages to de-
termine when, if at all, patterns of brain activity un-
dergo developmental change (e.g., Bishop, Hardiman,
Uwer, & von Suchodoletz, 2007; Feinberg & Campbell, Advances in brain-imaging technology have contributed to our un-
2010). derstanding of how the brain changes at adolescence. These images
are created through a process known as functional magnetic reso-
nance imaging, or fMRI.
❚ Are Male and Female Brains Different? Many
popular books claim that there are important differences
between the brains of adolescent boys and girls (and, for
that matter, adult men and women). Research indicates, Anderson, Ferguson, & Yurgelun-Todd, 2011; Raznahan
however, that differences between the genders in brain et al., 2011), although the importance of these changes
structure and function are very small and unlikely to ex- for understanding sex differences in behavior is not
plain differences between males and females in the way known. By and large, however, the similarities between
they behave or think (Paus, 2009; Spear, 2010). In gen- males and females in brain structure and function—
eral, male brains are about 10% larger than female brains before, during, and after adolescence—are far more im-
(even accounting for the fact that male bodies, on aver- pressive than the differences.
age, are bigger than females’), but as noted above, there is
no relation between sheer brain size and intellectual WHAT CHANGES IN ADOLESCENCE?
functioning, so it is unlikely that this small difference in
size has any practical significance. In addition, there are In order to understand how the brain changes in ado-
few consistent sex differences in the size of specific brain lescence, it is necessary to have a little background on
regions or structures—some parts of the brain are how the brain works more generally.
slightly larger among females, and some are slightly
larger among males (Blakemore, 2011). Some studies ❚ How Your Brain Works Your brain contains
also show different patterns of connections between approximately 100 billion neurons (often referred to as
brain regions in males and females (Lopez-Larson, gray matter), which are cells that carry information by
transmitting electrical charges across the brain by
means of chemicals called neurotransmitters. The cell
bodies of neurons have projections, like tentacles, but
electroencephalography (EEG) A technique for measuring these projections do not actually touch; there is a mi-
electrical activity at different locations on the scalp. nuscule gap between them, called a synapse. When the
electrical charge travels through a neuron and its pro-
event-related potentials (ERP) Changes in electrical activity in
areas of the brain in response to specific stimuli or events.
jections, it stimulates the release of neurotransmitters,
chemicals that carry the signal across the synapse from
neurons Nerve cells. one neuron to the next. Any time we perceive some-
neurotransmitters Specialized chemicals that carry electrical
thing (e.g., feel an itch), move something (e.g., scratch
impulses between neurons. the itch), or process information (e.g., wonder where
the itch came from), this process of electrical transmis-
synapse The gap in space between neurons, across which sion is involved.
neurotransmitters carry electrical impulses.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 67 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 67

A key process in early brain development is the de- that stage, because as a particular pathway of neural
velopment of connections—synapses—between neu- transmission becomes more efficient, the specific cog-
rons. By age 2, a single neuron may have 10,000 nitive process it supports improves. For example, syn-
connections to other neurons. The formation of some aptic pruning in the brain’s visual system is most
synapses is genetically programmed, but others are dramatic early in infancy, when our visual abilities are
formed through experience. The rate of synapse forma- improving the most.
tion peaks at about age 1 and slows down in early child- Myelination is another key process in brain devel-
hood, but the development of new synapses continues opment. Initially, neurons are “nude,” but over develop-
throughout life as we learn new skills, build memories, ment, white fatty tissue, called myelin (and often
acquire knowledge, and adapt to changing circum- referred to as white matter), encases the projections of
stances. The more a synapse is used, the stronger its neurons that interconnect them. Myelin, which acts a
electrical pathway becomes. little like plastic insulation around an electrical wire,
Initially the brain produces many more connections increases the speed of neural impulses and so improves
among cells than it will use. At 1 year of age, the num- information transmission. Myelination occurs in waves,
ber of synapses in the infant brain is about twice the beginning in the prenatal period and continuing into
number in the adult brain (Couperus & Nelson, 2006). young adulthood (Paus, 2009). Unlike synapses, with
However, soon after birth unused and unnecessary syn- their ∩-shaped pattern of growth, white matter in-
apses start to be eliminated, a process called synaptic creases throughout childhood and adolescence, well
pruning. As a general rule, we tend to assume that into adulthood, although at different rates in different
“more is better,” but that’s not the case here. Imagine a regions of the brain at different points in development
meadow between two patches of forest. Hundreds of (Lebel & Beaulieu, 2011). As with synaptic pruning, ex-
lightly trodden paths connect one side to the other (the amining where myelination is occurring most dramati-
unpruned brain). Over time people discover that one cally at a particular point in development provides
path is more direct than others. More people begin clues about the aspects of cognitive functioning that are
using this path more often, so it becomes wider and changing most at that stage.
deeper. Because the other paths are not used any more,
the grass grows back and those paths disappear. That’s ❚ Changes in Brain Structure During Adoles-
what synaptic pruning is like—the “paths” we use re- cence During adolescence, the brain is “remodeled”
peatedly become more and more ingrained, whereas through synaptic pruning and myelination in particular
those we do not use disappear. Synaptic pruning results brain regions (Spear, 2010; Tamnes et al., 2010) (see
in a decrease in the amount of gray matter in the brain, Figure 2.2). One part of the brain that is pruned dra-
which is often manifested in a “thinning” of the areas matically in adolescence is the prefrontal cortex, the re-
that have been pruned (Tamnes et al., 2010). gion of the brain most important for sophisticated
The elimination of synapses continues through ado- thinking abilities, such as planning, thinking ahead,
lescence and is normal and necessary to development weighing risks and rewards, and controlling impulses
and functioning. Just as pruning a rose bush—cutting (Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston, 2005). Pruning
off weak and misshapen branches—produces a health- also takes place in other parts of the cortex in adoles-
ier plant with larger flowers, so synaptic pruning en- cence, most notably, in the parietal cortex (an important
hances the brain’s functioning. Synaptic pruning makes region for working memory) and in the temporal cortex
the brain more efficient by transforming an unwieldy (an important region for memory and also for thinking
network of small pathways into a better organized sys-
tem of “superhighways.”
Generally, the development of synapses is character-
ized by a period of growth (when more and more syn- synaptic pruning The process through which unnecessary
apses are created) followed by a period of decline (when connections between neurons are eliminated, improving the
more and more synapses are eliminated). When we plot efficiency of information processing.
the density of synapses over time, we see a ∩-shaped myelination The process through which brain circuits are
curve—or, more accurately, a series of ∩-shaped curves insulated with myelin, which improves the efficiency of information
that peak at different ages, depending on the specific processing.
region of the brain. That is, although synaptic pruning
prefrontal cortex The region of the brain most important for
takes place throughout infancy, childhood, and adoles- sophisticated thinking abilities, such as planning, thinking ahead,
cence, different regions of the brain are pruned at dif- weighing risks and rewards, and controlling impulses.
ferent points in development. As a rule, the brain
parietal cortex A region of the brain that is especially important
regions in which pruning is taking place at a particular
for working memory.
point in development are the regions associated with
the greatest changes in cognitive functioning during temporal cortex A region of the brain that is especially important
for social cognition.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 68 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

68 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

FIGURE 2.2 Synaptic pruning (reflected in Cortical thickness White matter


the thinking of the cortex) and myelination (mm) volume
(reflected in increases in white matter) take 3.2
place in many brain regions during adoles- 3.0 4.0
cence, including the parietal, temporal, and 2.8

Parietal
frontal lobes. (Tamnes et al., 2010) 2.6 2.0
2.4 0.0
2.2
2.0 –2.0
1.8
10 15 20 25 30 10 15 20 25 30
Age Age

3.6 4.0
3.4
2.0

Temporal
3.2
3.0 0.0
2.8 –2.0
2.6
10 15 20 25 30 10 15 20 25 30
Age Age

3.2 4.0
3.0
Orbitofrontal

2.0
2.8
2.6 0.0
2.4
2.2 –2.0
2.0
10 15 20 25 30 10 15 20 25 30
Age Age

about the thoughts of other people) (Blakemore, 2011). relatively earlier among females than males (since girls
There is a relation between intelligence and patterns of tend to go through puberty earlier than boys) (Asato,
synaptic growth and pruning in the cortex, with relatively Terwilliger, Woo, & Luna, 2010). Although scientists
more intelligent adolescents showing a more dramatic initially focused on the thinning of gray matter as the
and longer period of production of synapses before main feature of structural change in the brain at adoles-
adolescence and a more dramatic pruning of them after cence, there has been increasing interest in the impor-
(P. Shaw, Greenstein, Lerch, Klasen, Lenroot et al., 2006). tance of the increase in white matter, which improves
There is also continued myelination of the cortex the efficiency of connections within and across brain
throughout adolescence, which also leads to many regions (Giorgio et al., 2010; Paus, 2009; Spear, 2010).
cognitive advances (Paus, 2009; Spear, 2010). There is Better connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and
evidence that myelination is affected by the impact the limbic system, an area of the brain involved in the
of pubertal hormones on the brain, which may ex- processing of emotions, social information, and reward
plain why the maturation of white matter takes place and punishment, lead to improvements in our ability to
regulate our emotions and coordinate our thoughts
and feelings (Asato et al., 2010; Steinberg, 2008). We
limbic system An area of the brain that plays an important role in now know that full structural maturation of the pre-
the processing of emotional experience, social information, and frontal cortex is not complete until the mid-20s (Casey
reward and punishment. et al., 2005; Hooper, Luciana, Conklin, & Yarger, 2004;
Paus, 2009; Santesso & Segalowitz, 2008). Of special
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex The outer and upper areas of the
front of the brain, important for skills such as planning ahead and
importance are developments in the dorsolateral pre-
controlling impulses. frontal cortex, the outer and upper areas of the front of
the brain, which is important for skills such as planning
ventromedial prefrontal cortex The lower and central area at the ahead and controlling impulses (Casey et al., 2005); the
front of the brain, important for gut-level decision making.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 69 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 69

et al., 2011). This is especially important on difficult


tasks, where the task demands may overtax the prefron-
tal cortex working alone, and especially on tasks that
require self-control, where it is necessary to coordinate
thinking and feeling (Albert & Steinberg, 2011). In fact,
when adolescents who are tested for self-control are told
that they will be rewarded for controlling themselves,
they perform better than when no such rewards are of-
fered (Kohls, Peltzer, Herpertz-Dahlmann, & Konrad,
2009; Sinopoli, Schachar, & Dennis, 2011).
This simultaneous recruitment of multiple brain re-
gions working as a “team,” referred to as functional
connectivity, is made possible by the increase in physi-
cal connections between brain regions that we dis-
cussed earlier (Dosenbach et al., 2010). Generally
speaking, children’s brains are characterized by a large
number of relatively “local” connections (i.e., connec-
tions between nearby brain regions), and as individuals
Myelination increases the speed of neural impulses and so improves mature through adolescence and into adulthood, more
information transmission. The amount of myelin in the brain in-
creases throughout childhood and adolescence, well into adulthood.
distant regions become increasingly interconnected
(Hwang, Velanova, & Luna, 2010). This is seen even
when individuals are lying still, just resting (Power, Fair,
Schlaggar, & Petersen, 2010). The maturation of func-
ventromedial prefrontal cortex, the lower and central tional connectivity is more or less complete by age 22
area of the front of the brain, which is important (see Figure 2.3).
for more gut-level, intuitive decision making, and
which has strong connections with the limbic system, ❚ Risk and Reward A different type of functional
where emotions and social information are processed change results from changes, especially in the limbic sys-
(Bechara, 2005); and the orbitofrontal cortex, the area tem, in the ways in which the brain is affected by certain
of the brain directly behind the eyes, which is impor- neurotransmitters, including dopamine (which plays an
tant for evaluating risks and rewards, and which also important role in our experience of reward) and sero-
has strong connections to the limbic system (Galvan et tonin (which plays an important role in the experience
al., 2006). of different moods). These changes, which are linked to
the hormonal changes of puberty, make adolescents
❚ Changes in Brain Function During Adoles- more emotional, more responsive to stress, and more
cence The two most important changes in brain likely to engage in reward seeking and sensation seeking
function involving the prefrontal cortex in adolescence than either children or adults (Ernst et al., 2005; Galvan,
both lead to greater efficiency in information processing Hare, Voss, Glover, & Casey, 2007; Silk et al., 2009; Spear,
(Spear, 2010). First, patterns of activation within the 2010; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010). They are also thought
prefrontal cortex generally become more focused. For to increase individuals’ vulnerability to substance abuse,
instance, in experiments in which participants are pre- because they seek higher levels of reward; depression, be-
sented with a rapid succession of images and asked to cause of their increased vulnerability to stress; and other
push a button when a certain image appears, but refrain
from pushing it when a different image appears (a pro-
cess known as response inhibition), adolescents are less orbitofrontal cortex The region of the brain located directly
likely than children to activate prefrontal regions that behind the eyes, important for the evaluation of risk and reward.
are not relevant to performing the task well. As adoles-
cents grow into adulthood and these brain systems fur- response inhibition The suppression of a behavior that is
inappropriate or no longer required.
ther mature, self-control improves (Andrews-Hanna,
Seghete, Claus, Burgess, Ruzic, & Banich, 2011). Second, functional connectivity The extent to which multiple brain
over the course of adolescence, individuals become regions function at the same time, which improves during
more likely to use multiple parts of the brain simultane- adolescence.
ously and coordinate activity between prefrontal regions dopamine A neurotransmitter especially important in the brain
and other areas, including other portions of the cortex circuits that regulate the experience of reward.
and areas of the limbic system (Christakou, Brammer, &
Rubia, 2011; Strang, Pruessner, & Pollak, 2011; Thomas serotonin A neurotransmitter that is especially important for the
experience of different moods.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 70 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

70 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

FIGURE 2.3 The maturation of func- 1.4


tional connectivity is more or less com-
plete by age 22. (Dosenbach et al., 2010)
1.2

Functional connectivity maturation index


1.0

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2

0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30
Age (years)

mental health problems, because of their easily aroused the subject of much discussion among those interested
emotions, including anger and sadness (see Figure 2.4) in risk taking and behavioral problems in adolescence,
(Forbes et al., 2010; Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenna, Long, & because this gap in timing may help explain the
Griggs, 2009; Paus, Keshavan, & Giedd, 2008; Stroud et dramatic increase in risky behavior that takes place be-
al., 2009; Sturman & Moghaddam, 2012). As adolescents tween childhood and adolescence, as well as the de-
mature toward adulthood, these trends begin to reverse, cline in risk taking that occurs as individuals mature
and individuals become less easily aroused by positive or into adulthood (Casey et al., 2011; Geier, Terwilliger,
negative stimuli. Teslovich, Velanova, & Luna, 2010; Spear, 2010;
Importantly, these changes in the functioning of the Steinberg, 2008). In essence, the brain changes in ways
limbic system occur relatively early in adolescence, in that may provoke individuals to seek novelty, reward,
contrast to developments in the prefrontal cortex, and stimulation several years before the complete mat-
which, as you now know, are still ongoing in early uration of the brain systems that regulate judgment,
adulthood. This relatively late maturation of the pre- decision making, and impulse control (Galvan, 2010;
frontal cortex, particularly compared to the changes Padmanabhan, Geier, Ordaz, Teslovich, & Luna, 2011;
that take place in the limbic system at puberty, has been Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010). In the words of one

FIGURE 2.4 The age of onset of most


Impulse-control
common psychiatric disorders is some- disorders
where between the ages of 10 and 20. New Substance-use
research on adolescent brain development disorders
helps explain why this is the case. (Paus et al.,
2008)
Anxiety disorders

Mood disorders

Schizophrenia
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40
Age of onset (years)
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 71 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 71

team of writers, it’s like “starting the engines with an Behrmann, & Dahl, 2012), which makes sense, given
unskilled driver” (C. Nelson et al., 2002, p. 515). As the link between puberty and adolescents’ interest in
the “braking system” improves, in part because of mat- sex. (If you are interested in finding a willing sex part-
uration of the prefrontal cortex and its connections to ner, it probably helps to pay attention to other peo-
other brain regions, and as reward seeking declines, ples’ facial expressions.)
individuals become less likely to engage in risky Although this increased attentiveness to other peo-
behavior. ple’s mental states likely has a number of benefits (it
may make teenagers more socially skilled, for example),
❚ The Social Brain Changes in the limbic system it also appears that adolescents are more easily dis-
in early adolescence may also help explain why adoles- tracted than adults by others’ emotional expressions. In
cents’ concerns about what their peers think increase one experiment, researchers presented adolescents
during this time, because the limbic system is impor- whose brains were being scanned with a mixed se-
tant for the processing of social information (Dahl, quence of one of four types of images (red circles,
2011; E. Nelson et al., 2005). Conceivably, this could scrambled images, neutral faces, and sad faces); the ad-
make adolescents more susceptible to peer pressure, a olescents were asked to indicate when pictures of the
topic we’ll look at in Chapter 9. In one very clever study, red circle appeared (L. Wang, Huettel, & De Bellis,
researchers imaged the brains of adolescents who 2008). Unlike adults, who had been studied using the
thought they were participating in a Facebook-style same paradigm (L. Wang, McCarthy, Song, & LaBar,
task, networking with other teenagers who were being 2005), adolescents showed heightened activation when
imaged at the same time in different locations (Guyer, viewing the sad faces, which interfered with their ability
McClure-Tone, Shiffrin, Pine, & Nelson, 2009). (Inside to pay attention to the appearance of the red circles. An
the fMRI equipment was a computer screen, on which important implication of this research for parents is
the researchers could show any images of their choos- that yelling at a teenager in an angry voice may not be
ing.) While being imaged, the adolescents were shown an especially effective means of getting the adolescent
pictures of the other teenagers and asked to rate how to listen, because the teenager may end up paying more
interested they were in chatting with them online. The attention to the angry emotion than to the content of
adolescents, who were told that their own photograph what the parent is saying.
was “posted” online, received what they thought was
feedback from the other teenagers. In reality, though,
no other teenagers were connected to the network, and MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
the feedback the adolescents received was rigged to be New research shows that brain systems governing
positive (interested in chatting) half the time and nega- things like impulse control, planning ahead, and
tive (not interested in chatting) half the time. When the balancing risk and reward are still maturing during late
adolescents were told that other teenagers were inter- adolescence. Yet, rates of adolescents’ risky behavior,
ested in them, areas of their brain known to be sensitive such as experimentation with drugs or unprotected
to rewards like food and money were activated, suggest- sex, vary considerably around the
ing that social rewards may be processed during adoles- world. If these sorts of behaviors
cence in ways similar to the ways in which we process are more common in adolescence
other types of rewards. Other studies have identified because of the way the brain is
regions of the brain are activated when adolescents are changing, shouldn’t they be more
made to feel excluded, and have shown that teenagers universal?
who show stronger activation of these regions when
made to feel rejected are at greater risk for depression
(Masten et al., 2011).
IMPLICATIONS FOR ADOLESCENT
Other research indicates that sensitivity to others’
BEHAVIOR
mental states increases during adolescence, a change
that is reflected in changes in patterns of brain activ- ❚ Correlation Is Not Causation It is important
ity when individuals witness others (Burnett et al., to keep in mind that evidence of a correlation between
2011; Decety & Michalska, 2010; Masten, Eisenberger, changes in brain structure or function and changes in
Pfeifer, & Dapretto, 2010; Pfeifer & Blakemore, 2012; adolescent behavior does not necessarily mean that the
Pfeifer et al., 2012). For instance, individuals’ ability first is necessarily causing the second (Kuhn, 2009; Paus,
to recognize subtle changes in others’ facial expres- 2009). One important hallmark of brain development
sions improves during adolescence (L. A. Thomas, De in adolescence is that the brain is very malleable, or
Bellis, Graham, & LaBar, 2007). Puberty appears to “plastic,” and its development is affected by experience
play a role in influencing this increase in sensitivity to as well as biology and, most probably, by the interaction
others’ facial expressions (Moore et al., 2012; Scherf, between the two (M. Thomas & Johnson, 2008). For
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 72 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

72 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

instance, individuals may inherit genes that direct the are relevant to the behavior in question (Steinberg,
brain to develop in certain ways during adolescence, but Cauffman, Woolard, Graham, & Banich, 2009).
whether these genes are actually expressed (that is,
whether they actually result in observable behavior) RECAP
may depend on the environment. Studies have found, • New imaging techniques, especially functional
for example, that individuals who have a genetic predis- magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), have enabled
position to develop depression become depressed only scientists to chart changes in the structure and
if they are exposed to a certain degree of life stress. Peo- function of the brain in adolescence.
ple with the same genetic profile, but without the stress • Among the most important structural changes to
exposure, have no higher rates of depression than those take place in adolescence is the elimination of un-
without the genetic vulnerability (Caspi et al., 2003). necessary synapses, a process called synaptic prun-
It is also the case that adolescents’ behavior affects ing, particularly in the prefrontal cortex.
their brain development. An obvious illustration of this • A second important structural change involves in-
relationship, which we will look at in Chapter 13, in- creased myelination of the brain, which permits
volves the impact of alcohol and other drugs on the more efficient connections within and between
brain, but there are other, more subtle examples as well. brain regions. Along with synaptic pruning, myelina-
As mentioned earlier, the process of synaptic pruning, tion is thought to lead to improvements in many
for example, is influenced by experience: Repeated acti- advanced thinking abilities.
vation of a specific collection of neurons as a result of • Adolescence is also a time of changes in brain func-
engaging in a particular behavior will actually result in tion. Two specific changes involving the prefrontal
structural changes that strengthen the connections
cortex are noteworthy: First, activation within the
among those neurons, which in turn will make them
prefrontal cortex becomes more focused during
function more efficiently. For example, practicing the
adolescence, and the activation of irrelevant brain
same task over and over again makes it easier and easier
areas declines. Second, activity in the prefrontal
to perform the task each time. Scientists have grown
cortex becomes increasingly coordinated with ac-
increasingly interested in seeing whether different sorts
tivity in other parts of the brain.
of training programs or interventions can improve ado-
• Another important functional change in the brain
lescents’ self-control (Crone, 2009) or reduce their ten-
at adolescence involves changes in the activity of
dencies toward sensation seeking (Romer et al., 2011),
several neurotransmitters in the limbic system. This
both of which may reduce risky behavior.
is thought to increase sensation seeking and emo-
Although scientists agree about the ways in which
tional arousal and to make adolescents more sensi-
the structure and function of the brain change during
tive to emotional stimuli and social information.
adolescence, the implications of these changes for ado- • The combination of heightened sensation seeking and
lescent development are still the subject of a great deal a still-maturing prefrontal cortex may make adoles-
of ongoing research and considerable speculation, be- cence a period of experimentation with risky activity.
cause few studies have directly linked changes in brain • It is important to remember that the relation be-
images with changes in thought, emotion, or behavior. tween brain development and psychological devel-
One question that is often asked is when adolescents opment is a two-way street. The adolescent brain is
begin to think like adults. As you now know, brain sci- very malleable, and experiences shape its structure
ence alone cannot answer this question, because it de- and function.
pends on which aspects of thinking one is concerned
about. When it comes to relatively more sophisticated
cognitive abilities, such as thinking ahead, envisioning
the future consequences of a decision, balancing risks
and rewards, or controlling impulses—all of which are
governed mainly by the prefrontal cortex—research on
brain maturation certainly suggests that these capabili-
ties are still developing well after individuals enter their
20s. But when it comes to more basic abilities, such as
those involving memory, attention, and logical reason- Individual Differences in
ing, especially under optimal conditions, brain and
behavioral studies indicate that the average 15-year-old
Intelligence in Adolescence
performs no worse than the average adult. Where For the most part, theorists who have studied adolescent
we draw the boundary between adolescence and cognitive development from either a Piagetian or an
adulthood—at least as far as cognitive development is information-processing framework, or through brain
concerned—should probably depend on why the research, have focused on the universals in adolescent
boundary is being drawn and on what specific abilities intellectual growth. These theorists ask, How does
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 73 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 73

thinking change as individuals move into adolescence? Although an individual’s score on an intelligence test
What processes drive cognitive development as children is often reported in terms of her or his overall IQ,
become teenagers? What cognitive competencies do all intelligence tests actually comprise a series of tests, and
adolescents share? it is usually possible to look at performance in different
In contrast, other theorists have been more inter- areas independently. The WISC-IV and the WAIS-III,
ested in studying individual differences in intellectual for example, each contain two groups of tests: verbal
abilities. They ask, How can we account for different tests, which include measures of vocabulary, general in-
patterns of intellectual growth within the adolescent formation, comprehension, and arithmetic abilities,
population? How large are individual differences in in- and performance tests, which include measures of
telligence in adolescence? Are some adolescents brighter memory, perceptual reasoning, and picture completion.
than others? If so, why, and in what ways? Changes in specific aspects of IQ performance during
adolescence are correlated with synaptic pruning in
THE MEASUREMENT OF IQ brain regions known to play a role in those specific
types of learning (Ramsden, Richardson, Josse, Thomas,
To answer questions about the relative intelligence of
& Ellis, 2011; van den Bos, Crone, & Güroğlu, 2012).
individuals, psychologists have had to devise ways of
The IQ test represents only one of many ways of
assessing intelligence—no easy feat given the consider-
assessing intelligence in adolescence. Indeed, many the-
able disagreement over what “intelligence” really is. To-
orists have argued that its exclusive focus on “school
day, the most widely used measures are intelligence
smarts”—the sorts of abilities that are related to scholas-
tests, or IQ (for “intelligence quotient”) tests. Among
tic success—yields a one-sided picture of what it means
these tests are the Stanford-Binet, the Wechsler Intelli-
to be an intelligent person. Two of the better-known at-
gence Scale for Children (WISC-IV), and the Wechsler
tempts to expand on this narrow definition come from
Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III). An individual’s IQ
the work of Robert Sternberg (1988) and Howard
is computed by dividing his or her mental age by his or
Gardner (1983). Many of Sternberg and Gardner’s ideas
her chronological age and then multiplying the result
formed the basis for the best-selling book Emotional In-
by 100. A score of 100 is used to designate the midway
telligence, by journalist Daniel Goleman (1995).
point. An IQ score below 100 indicates a poorer test
performance than the average person of the same age; a
TYPES OF INTELLIGENCE
score above 100 indicates a better performance than av-
erage. The higher an individual’s IQ, the smaller the ❚ Sternberg’s “Triarchic” Theory Sternberg
number of age-mates who perform equally or better on proposed a triarchic, or three-part, theory of intelli-
the same test. gence. He argued that a thorough assessment of an

Critics of standardized testing argue that these tests measure just one type of intelligence—“school smarts”—and
neglect other, equally important skills, such as emotional intelligence, creativity, and “street smarts.”
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 74 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

74 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

individual’s intellectual capabilities requires that we INTELLIGENCE TEST PERFORMANCE


look at three distinct but interrelated types of intelli- IN ADOLESCENCE
gence: (1) componential intelligence, which involves our
Although the existence of different forms of intelli-
abilities to acquire, store, and process information (as
gence is a popular idea, most research on individual
described in the previous section about information
differences in intelligence during adolescence has em-
processing); (2) experiential intelligence, which in-
ployed traditional IQ tests. Assessments based on IQ
volves our abilities to use insight and creativity; and (3)
tests have been used to examine two seemingly similar
contextual intelligence, which involves our ability to
but actually very different questions: (1) How stable are
think practically. Componential intelligence is closest
IQ scores during adolescence? and (2) Do the sorts of
to the type of intelligence measured on traditional in-
mental abilities that are assessed via intelligence tests
telligence tests. Experiential intelligence is closest to
improve during adolescence?
what we call “creativity.” And contextual intelligence is
It is easy to confuse these questions, because at first
closest to what we might call “street smarts.” All indi-
glance, they seem to be asking the same thing. But con-
viduals have all three types of intelligence, but some
sider this: Individuals’ IQ scores remain remarkably
individuals are stronger in one respect than in others.
stable during the adolescent years; yet, during the same
You probably can think of individuals who are good
period, their mental abilities improve dramatically. Al-
test takers but who are not particularly creative or sen-
though this might seem contradictory, it is not. Studies
sible. According to Sternberg’s model, these individuals
of stability examine changes in individuals’ relative
would be high in componential intelligence but low in
standing over time, whereas studies of change examine
experiential and contextual intelligence.
changes in individuals’ absolute scores.
More importantly, Sternberg’s view forces us to look
Take height, for example. Children who are taller
at individuals who are not good test takers but who are
than their peers during middle childhood are likely to
creative or street smart as being just as intelligent as
be taller than their peers during adulthood as well; chil-
individuals who score high on IQ tests—but intelligent
dren who are average in height remain so throughout
in a different way. Sternberg argued that society needs
childhood and adulthood; and children shorter than
individuals with all types of intelligence and that it
their peers at one time are likely to be shorter than their
is time we started assessing—and encouraging—
peers later on. Height, therefore, is a very stable trait.
experiential and contextual intelligence as much as we
But this does not mean that individuals don’t grow be-
do componential intelligence (D. Matthews & Keating,
tween childhood and adolescence.
1995).
Like height, intelligence is characterized by high sta-
bility and a good deal of change during childhood and
❚ Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelli-
adolescence. To the first question posed above—about
gences Howard Gardner’s theory of multiple intelli-
how stable IQ scores are during adolescence—the
gences also stresses that there is more to being smart
answer, then, is very stable. Youngsters who score higher
than being “book smart.” Gardner proposed that there
than their peers on intelligence tests during early
are seven types of intelligence: verbal, mathematical,
adolescence are likely to score higher throughout the
spatial, kinesthetic (having to do with movement), self-
adolescent years. In fact, certain measures of intellec-
reflective, interpersonal, and musical. According to his
tual performance taken during the first year of life—
view, for example, outstanding athletes such as basket-
not IQ tests, but measures of speed of information
ball great LeBron James or soccer legend Mia Hamm
processing—are significantly predictive of IQ test per-
have a well-developed kinesthetic intelligence, which
formance in early adolescence (S. Rose & Feldman,
allows them to control their bodies and process the
1995). This does not mean, however, that individuals’
movements of others in extraordinary ways. Although
intellectual performance remains fixed or is not suscep-
conventional tests of intelligence emphasize verbal and
tible to change—as you will see in a moment.
mathematical abilities, these are not the only types of
A 10-year-old boy whose score is average for his age
intelligence that we possess—nor are they the only
would have an IQ of about 100. If his score were
types that we should value.
stable—that is, if he remained average in comparison
with his peers—his IQ would remain at about 100.
Even if he became more intelligent over time, as any
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION normal child would, his IQ score probably would not
Think about how you might perform on change very much, because the score would always re-
tests that measure different types of in- flect his performance relative to his peers. If his abilities
telligence. What particular intellectual increased at the same rate as his age-mates’, his relative
strengths do you have that might not be standing (and therefore his IQ) would not change. For
picked up by conventional IQ tests? most individuals, this is indeed what happens. Al-
though some individuals show fluctuation in IQ scores
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 75 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 75

between childhood and adolescence, ultimately, most

Mean standard score


adolescents end up with IQ scores that are not very dif-
ferent from their scores as children. In one study, for
example, the average amount of change over a 7-year
period (from age 7 to age 13) was only 5 IQ points
among all of the individuals whose scores fluctuated to
any degree, which is not a change of much practical
significance (Moffitt, Caspi, Harkness, & Silva, 1993).
In absolute terms, however, mental abilities assessed
by conventional IQ tests increase dramatically through 0 4 8 12
16 20 24 28 32 36
Age
childhood and adolescence, reaching a plateau some-
time in mid-to-late adolescence (see Figure 2.5). (It is FIGURE 2.5 The growth of mental abilities from birth to
age 36, as measured by standardized IQ tests. (Adapted from
no coincidence that this plateau occurs at around the
Bayley, 1949)
same age as that for information processing, because IQ
test performance depends a lot on information-pro-
cessing abilities.) Thus, despite the fact that IQ scores
remain stable over time, individuals do become smarter with an equally challenging test of competence in the
as they get older—a fact that argues strongly in favor of real world—such as figuring out the most efficient
educational interventions, especially in early child- route between school and home through a dangerous
hood, because they have been shown to improve intel- neighborhood.
lectual performance during adolescence (B. C. Campbell, Vygotsky argued that children and adolescents learn
Pungello, Miller-Johnson, Burchinal, & Ramey, 2001). best in everyday situations when they encounter tasks
In addition, research shows that extended schooling that are neither too simple nor too advanced, but just
during adolescence itself enhances individuals’ perfor- slightly more challenging than their abilities permit
mance on standardized tests of intelligence (Ceci & them to solve on their own. Within this so-called zone
Williams, 1999). Whereas individuals who had dropped of proximal development, young people, through close
out of school early showed unchanging—and relatively collaboration with a more experienced instructor
lower—scores on intelligence tests during adolescence, (whether an adult or another child), are stimulated to
students who remained in school, especially those in “reach” for the more advanced level of performance.
the more advanced tracks, showed impressive gains The role of the instructor is to help structure the learn-
in verbal ability over time. A recent study conducted in ing situation so that it is within the reach of the
Norway showed that the country’s decision some time student—a structuring process called scaffolding. If
ago to increase the number of years of compulsory you watch good parents, teachers, or coaches at work,
schooling (from 7 years to 9 years) resulted in modest you will probably observe a great deal of scaffolding.
improvements in IQ scores (Brinch & Galloway, 2012).

RECAP
CULTURE AND INTELLIGENCE • The IQ test measures the specific type of intelli-
❚ Vygotsky’s Perspective Much of our current gence that is predictive of success in school.
thinking about the nature of intelligence has been in- • Although there is considerable growth in mental
fluenced by the work of the late Russian psychologist abilities during adolescence, IQ scores, which mea-
Lev Vygotsky (1930/1978), who emphasized the broader sure individuals’ performance relative to people
context in which intellectual development occurs. Ac- their age, are remarkably stable during this time.
cording to this view, it is essential that we understand • There is some evidence that schooling can increase
the nature of the environment in which an adolescent individuals’ IQ scores.
develops in terms of its demands for intelligent behav- • According to theorists like Sternberg and Gardner,
ior and its opportunities for learning. Individuals de- there are other types of intelligence important to
velop and use intellectual skills not simply as a function success in life that are not captured on conven-
of their cognitive maturation but also in response to the tional IQ tests.
everyday problems they are expected to solve. Young
street merchants in Brazil, for example, who might not
perform well on standardized tests of math knowledge, zone of proximal development In Vygotsky’s theory, the level of
nevertheless use sophisticated mathematical skills dur- challenge that is still within the individual’s reach but that forces an
ing transactions with customers (Guberman, 1996). individual to develop more advanced skills.
The very same children who perform poorly on scaffolding Structuring a learning situation so that it is just within
school-based tests of knowledge may excel when faced the reach of the student.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 76 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

76 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

• Experts agree that it is essential to view and assess advanced, their ideas about social institutions and or-
intelligence in a way that takes into account the ganizations are more complex, and their ability to fig-
cultural context in which the adolescent has ure out what other people are thinking is far more
grown up. An important influ- developed. As we shall see in subsequent chapters, gains
ence on this perspective in the area of social cognition help account for many of
has been the psychosocial advances typically associated with
Vygotsky. adolescence—advances in the realms of identity, au-
tonomy, intimacy, sexuality, and achievement. Individ-
ual differences in social cognitive abilities also help
explain why some adolescents have more social prob-
Adolescent Thinking lems than others (Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, 2006; Fon-
taine, Yang, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 2008).
in Context Research on social cognition during adolescence in-
Just as it is important to ask how the broader context cludes many topics, but four of the most often studied
influences adolescents’ cognitive development, it is also concern (1) theory of mind; (2) individuals’ thinking
important to ask how their cognitive development in- about social relationships; (3) individuals’ conceptions
fluences their interactions with their environment. Af- of laws, civil liberties, and rights; and (4) individuals’
ter all, most of the thinking adolescents do occurs in understanding of social conventions (Rote & Smetana,
everyday situations, not just when they are taking tests 2011; Smetana & Villalobos, 2009).
designed to see how smart they are.
As our understanding of adolescent thinking has ex- ❚ Theory of Mind During preadolescence and
panded, researchers have begun to look beyond labora- adolescence, individuals develop a more nuanced un-
tory experiments and standardized tests to examine derstanding of other people’s personalities and psycho-
how the cognitive changes of adolescence actually affect logical states, enabled in part by brain maturation in
teenagers’ day-to-day thoughts and actions. Do ad- systems that support what is called mentalizing—
vances in deductive reasoning or information-process- the ability to understand someone else’s mental state
ing abilities make a difference in the real world? How (Burnett et al., 2011). As they develop a more sophisti-
do the brain changes that take place in adolescence play cated theory of mind, the ability to understand that
out in everyday experiences? To answer these questions, others have beliefs, intentions, and knowledge that may
psychologists and educators have studied the practical be different from one’s own, adolescents are better able
side of adolescent thinking in three domains: in social to interpret the feelings of others and to infer their
situations, in risk taking, and in the classroom. motives and feelings, even when specific information
of this sort is not directly observable (Choudhury,
Blakemore, & Charman, 2006; Dumontheil, Apperly, &
SOCIAL COGNITION IN ADOLESCENCE Blakemore, 2010). Adolescents also become better at
It is not difficult to imagine that adolescents’ advanced lying as a result of these improvements in social cogni-
abilities in thinking about possibilities, thinking in tion (Evans & Lee, 2011).
multiple dimensions, and thinking about abstract con- As they grow older, children become better able to
cepts make them more sophisticated when it comes to recognize that others may view an event from a differ-
reasoning about social matters. Social cognition in- ent, but equally valid, perspective. Not only are adoles-
volves such cognitive activities as thinking about cents more capable of discerning another person’s
people, social relationships, and social institutions perspective on some issue or event, but they are also
(Smetana & Villalobos, 2009). better able to understand that person’s perspective on
Compared with those of children, adolescents’ con- their own point of view. Ultimately, adolescents’ im-
ceptions of interpersonal relationships are more ma- provements in their ability to figure out what others are
ture, their understanding of human behavior is more thinking lead to improvements in communication, be-
cause they become more capable of formulating argu-
ments in terms that are more likely to be understood by
someone whose opinion is different. As you will read in
social cognition The aspect of cognition that concerns thinking Chapter 4, this gain in perspective taking may change
about other people, about interpersonal relations, and about social the dynamics of adolescents’ relationships with their
institutions. parents—for better (because adolescents are able to see
more things from their parents’ point of view) and for
mentalizing The ability to understand someone else’s mental
state worse (because adolescents may use these advanced so-
cial cognitive abilities to challenge their parents’ au-
theory of mind The ability to understand that others have beliefs, thority) (Smetana & Villalobos, 2009).
intentions, and knowledge that may be different from one’s own.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 77 21/03/13 9:29 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 77

❚ Thinking About Relationships These improve-


ments in mentalizing, in turn, lead to changes in the
way that adolescents think about relationships with
peers and parents. One topic that researchers have been
especially interested in concerns adolescents’ beliefs
about peer exclusion (Leets & Sunwolf, 2005). All other
things equal, children believe that it is wrong to exclude
peers from social activities (i.e., whether to invite the
whole class or just one’s close friends to a birthday
party). With age, however, as adolescents’ understand-
ing of group dynamics becomes more sophisticated,
they begin to take into account other considerations,
like loyalty, social status, the activity context, and the
reason for excluding some individuals but not others.
For example, one study found that adolescents who
were members of high-status peer crowds (e.g., jocks)
judged excluding other teenagers from peer groups as
less wrong than did individuals who were members of
Improvements in social cognition during adolescence lead adoles-
low-status peer groups (e.g., druggies) (Horn, 2003).
cents to think in more sophisticated ways about themselves and
On the other hand, adolescents become more likely, their relationships.
with age, to believe that social exclusion on the basis of
gender orientation, nationality, or ethnicity is wrong
(Killen, Henning, Kelly, Crystal, & Ruck, 2007; Malti, instance, adolescents understand that teachers have the
Killen, & Gasser, 2012). right to demand that students show up for class on time
Changes in adolescents’ understanding of social re- and sit quietly if asked, but believe that students should
lationships also transform their beliefs about authority, be able to decide where they sit in class or during lunch.
which has important implications for their relation-
ships with parents and other adults (Smetana & ❚ Social Conventions The realization that indi-
Villalobos, 2009). With age, adolescents increasingly viduals’ perspectives vary, and that their opinions may
distinguish between moral issues (such as whether it is differ as a result, leads to changes in the ways that ado-
acceptable to steal from someone else) and conven- lescents approach issues regarding social conventions
tional ones (such as whether one eats dessert before or (Smetana & Villalobos, 2009). During middle child-
after the main course). Although the stereotype of ado- hood, social conventions—the social norms that guide
lescents is that they invariably come to reject the au- day-to-day behavior, such as waiting in line to buy
thority of adults, research shows that what happens movie tickets—are seen as arbitrary and changeable,
instead is that adolescents increasingly distinguish be- but adherence to them is not; compliance with such
tween issues that authority figures have the right to conventions is based on rules and on the dictates of
regulate and issues that are their own personal choices. authority. When you were 7 years old, you might not
As adolescents begin to make these distinctions, they have understood why people had to wait in line to buy
may come to question their parents’ authority in vari- movie tickets, but when your parents told you to wait in
ous contexts. Issues that had been viewed as matters of line, you waited. By early adolescence, however, conven-
right and wrong start to seem like matters of personal tions are seen as arbitrary and changeable in terms of
choice and, as such, beyond the bounds of parental au- both origins and enforcement; conventions are merely
thority (Cumsille, Darling, Flaherty, & Martínez, 2009). social expectations. As an adolescent, you begin to real-
For example, parents’ rules about things like the cleanli- ize that people wait in line because they are expected to,
ness of the adolescent’s bedroom or how late she or he not because they are forced to. Indeed, young adoles-
can stay up on school nights, which had been accepted cents often see social conventions as nothing but social
as matters of right and wrong, start to seem like arbi- expectations and, consequently, as insufficient reasons
trary conventions that are open to debate. As we’ll see in for compliance. You can probably imagine youngsters
Chapter 4, one main source of conflict between adoles- in their midteens saying something like this: “Why wait
cents and their parents involves which issues parents in a ticket line simply because other people are lined
have legitimate authority over and which they do not. up? There isn’t a law that forces you to wait in line, is
Similar changes occur in adolescents’ beliefs about their there?”
teachers’ authority (Smetana & Bitz, 1996) and the
authority of groups to dictate how individuals should
behave (Helwig, Yang, Tan, Liu, & Shao, 2011). For social conventions The norms that govern everyday behavior in
social situations.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 78 21/03/13 9:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

78 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Gradually, however, adolescents begin to see social (called “nurturance rights”) and their rights to make
conventions as the means by which society regulates their own decisions (called “self-determination rights”)
people’s behavior. Conventions may be arbitrary, but (Ruck, Abramovich, & Keating, 1998). In general, there
we follow them because we share an understanding of are few changes in individuals’ endorsement of nurtur-
how people are expected to behave in various situa- ance rights as they get older—that is, adolescents are
tions. In fact, high schoolers see conventions as so just as likely as children to believe that parents have an
ingrained in the social system that individuals follow obligation to provide food, clothing, and other types of
them partly out of habit. We wait in line for theater support—but that support for self-determination
tickets not because we want to comply with any rule, rights (e.g., the right to keep a private diary) increases
but because it is something we are accustomed to markedly over the course of adolescence. As is the case
doing. with support for basic individual rights, like the right to
Ultimately, individuals come to see that social con- free speech or religion, the growth in support for self-
ventions serve a function in coordinating interactions determination rights is seen among adolescents from a
among people. Social norms and expectations are de- diverse array of Western and non-Western cultures
rived from and maintained by individuals’ having a (Cherney & Shing, 2008).
common perspective and agreeing that, in given situa- Several themes cut across the research findings from
tions, certain behaviors are more desirable than others, studies of different aspects of social cognition—the way
because such behaviors help society and its institutions we think about people, relationships, conventions, and
function more smoothly. Without the convention of rights. First, as individuals move into and through ado-
waiting in line to buy movie tickets, the pushiest people lescence they become better able to step outside them-
would always get tickets first. Older adolescents can see selves and see things from other vantage points. Second,
that waiting in line not only benefits the theater by adolescents are better able to see that the social “rules”
keeping order but also preserves everyone’s right to a we follow (in the family, at school, and in broader soci-
fair chance to buy tickets. In other words, we wait in ety) are not absolute and are therefore subject to debate
line patiently because we all agree that it is better if tick- and questioning. Third, with age, adolescents develop a
ets are distributed fairly. more differentiated, more nuanced understanding of
social norms; yes, individuals are entitled to certain
❚ Laws, Civil Liberties, and Rights As is the case rights, but there are some situations under which it
with individuals’ developing understanding of relation- might be appropriate to curtail them. Yes, it is generally
ships between people, over the course of adolescence wrong to exclude others, but sometimes social exclu-
individuals also become more nuanced in the way they sion is justifiable.
think about the relationship between the individual These gains in social cognition help to account for
and society. Most research on adolescents’ beliefs about gains in social competence during adolescence. As you
rights and civil liberties comes from studies of Western, will see in Chapter 10, adolescents who have more so-
middle-class youth, and it is important to be cautious phisticated social cognitive abilities (for example, more
about generalizing the findings of these studies to advanced perspective-taking abilities) actually behave in
young people from other cultures. Nevertheless, even in more socially competent ways (N. Eisenberg, Morris,
collectivist cultures that place less emphasis on the McDaniel, & Spinrad, 2009; L. Lenhart & Rabiner, 1995).
rights of the individual than is the case in America, ado- Although there is more to social competence than social
lescents become increasingly likely to believe that there cognition, being able to understand social relationships
are some freedoms—like freedom of speech and is an important component of social maturity.
freedom of religion—that should not be restricted
(Smetana & Villalobos, 2009). That said, research also
ADOLESCENT RISK TAKING
finds that, with age, teenagers come to believe that there
are situations in which it may be legitimate to restrict A second practical application of research into adoles-
individual rights to serve the benefit of the community. cent thinking involves the study of adolescent risk tak-
For instance, one study of Muslim and non-Muslim ing. In Chapter 1, you read that many of the health
Dutch youth found that, although both groups favored problems of adolescence are the result of behaviors that
freedom of speech in general, Muslim youth were rela- can be prevented—behaviors such as substance abuse,
tively more likely to reject freedom of speech that might reckless driving, and unprotected sex. In the real world
be offensive to God or their religion, whereas non- (IOM and NRC, 2011a), and on laboratory tasks of
Muslim youth were more likely to reject the rights of risky decision-making (Burnett, Bault, Coricelli, &
minority groups to burn the Dutch flag during political Blakemore, 2010; Steinberg et al., 2009), adolescents
demonstrations (Verkuyten & Slooter, 2008). take more risks than either children or adults.
Researchers have also looked at changes in adoles- The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, a
cents’ beliefs about their rights to be taken care of federal agency that monitors the health of Americans,
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 79 21/03/13 9:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 79

now surveys American teenagers annually and asks processing—in attention, memory, metacognition, or
whether they had engaged in various behaviors during organization, for example—perhaps it would make
the previous 30 days (Centers for Disease Control and sense to train adolescents in these basic cognitive abili-
Prevention, 2012). Risk taking is common among ado- ties as a means of lessening their risk taking.
lescents. Recent surveys have revealed, for example, As we have seen, however, adolescents, at least by the
that although most high school students regularly use time they are 15 or so, make decisions using the same
a seat belt, of those teenagers who ride motorcycles, 33 basic cognitive processes that adults use (Beyth-Marom
percent report rarely or never wearing a helmet, and of et al., 1993; Furbey & Beyth-Marom, 1992). This is true
those who ride bicycles, 85 percent report rarely or even for issues as complicated as deciding whether to
never wearing a helmet. More than one quarter of both abort a pregnancy (Steinberg, Cauffman et al., 2009).
sexes have been passengers in cars driven by intoxi- The major gains in the cognitive skills that affect deci-
cated drivers, and one-tenth have driven while drink- sion making appear to occur between childhood and
ing (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, adolescence, rather than between adolescence and
2012). adulthood (Steinberg & Cauffman, 1996). Thus, edu-
cating adolescents in how to make “better” decisions is
❚ Behavioral Decision Theory A number of not likely to reduce risk taking (Steinberg, 2007).
writers have looked at adolescent risk taking from
a perspective called behavioral decision theory
(Fischhoff & Quadrel, 1995; Kahneman, 2011; Reyna & MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
Farley, 2006). In this perspective, which draws heavily
on economics, decision making is a rational process in What risky behavior did you engage in during
which individuals calculate the costs and benefits of al- adolescence that you would be reluctant to
ternative courses of action and behave in ways that engage in today? If you did, why do you
maximize the benefits and minimize the costs (Gruber, think you took those risks? If you no longer
2001). According to this theory, all behaviors, including behave as recklessly as you did as a
risky ones, can be analyzed as the outcome of a process teenager, what would have led to this?
involving five steps: (1) identifying alternative choices,
(2) identifying the consequences that might follow
from each choice, (3) evaluating the costs and benefits ❚ Do Adolescents Really Feel Invulnerable? A
of each possible consequence, (4) assessing the likeli- second possibility that is often suggested is that adoles-
hood of each possible consequence, and (5) combining cents are more likely to feel invulnerable—more likely,
all this information according to some decision rule that is, to subscribe to the personal fable that they will
(Beyth-Marom, Austin, Fischhoff, Palmgren, & Jacobs- not be harmed by potentially harmful activities. How-
Quadrel, 1993). ever, as you read earlier, there is no evidence for the
So, for example, an adolescent girl who is trying to widely held belief that adolescents are more likely to
decide whether to accept a ride home from a party with subscribe to personal fables than are adults (Quadrel,
friends who have been drinking will (1) identify the Fischhoff, & Davis, 1993). More importantly, studies
choices (to accept the ride or not), (2) identify the con- indicate that young adolescents are less likely than
sequences (“If I accept the ride, and we get into an ac- young adults to see themselves as invulnerable—if any-
cident, I could be seriously hurt, but if I don’t accept the thing, young adolescents overestimate the risks involved
ride, my friends will make fun of me for being a ‘loser’”), in potentially harmful behavior (Fischhoff, de Bruin,
(3) evaluate the desirability of each consequence (“Ap- Perker, Millstein, & Halpern-Felsher, 2010). There is no
pearing like a ‘loser’ to my friends is bad, but being in evidence, for example, that adolescents are worse at
an accident would be terrible”), (4) assess the likeli- perceiving risks than adults are (Ivers et al., 2009; Van
hood of each consequence (“My friends probably won’t Leijenhorst, Westenberg, & Crone, 2008). However, re-
really change their opinion of me just because I turn search indicates that adolescents vary far more than
down the ride, and my friend who is driving is so drunk adults in how they interpret words and phrases used
that he really might get into an accident”), and (5) com- to describe risk—words like “probably,” “likely,” or “a
bine all the information according to some decision very low chance”—suggesting that health educators
rule (“All things considered, I think I won’t take the and practitioners should not take for granted that an
ride”). adolescent’s understanding of a message about risk is
From the perspective of behavioral decision theory,
then, it is important to ask whether adolescents use dif-
ferent processes than adults in identifying, estimating,
and evaluating behavioral options and consequences. If behavioral decision theory An approach to understanding
risky decisions are the result of faulty information adolescent risk taking, in which behaviors are seen as the outcome
of systematic decision-making processes.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 80 21/03/13 9:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

80 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

on the health risks of trying the drug, while the adoles-


cent may place relatively more weight on the social con-
sequences of not trying it. Although an adult may see
an adolescent’s decision to value peer acceptance more
than health as irrational, an adolescent may see the
adult’s decision as equally incomprehensible. Behav-
ioral decision theory reminds us that all decisions—
even risky ones—can be seen as rational once we
understand how an individual estimates and evaluates
the consequences of various courses of action.
One very important difference between adolescents
and adults, for instance, is that, when weighing the
costs and benefits of engaging in a risky behavior, ado-
lescents are more attuned to the potential rewards than
adults are (Ben-Zur & Reshef-Kfir, 2003; Cauffman et
al., 2010; Millstein & Halpern-Felsher, 2002). This dif-
ference is consistent with changes that are taking place
in the limbic system around the time of puberty, which
we discussed earlier in this chapter. One study of delin-
quents found, for instance, that adolescents’ criminal
activity was more strongly related to their beliefs about
the potential rewards of the activity (for example, being
seen as “cool”) than to their perceptions of the activi-
ty’s riskiness (for example, the chances of being ar-
rested) (Matsueda, Kreager, & Huizinga, 2006). As
several writers have pointed out, this has important
implications for the prevention of risky behavior
among adolescents. It may be more important to con-
Recent research on cognitive development in adolescence has been vince adolescents that the rewards of a risky activity are
aimed at understanding the thinking behind risk-taking behavior. small (for example, that few people will actually look
up to someone for being violent) than to persuade
them that the costs are large (for example, that being
incarcerated will be terrible).
what he or she thinks it is (Biehl & Halpern-Felsher, In all likelihood, of course, neither adolescents’ nor
2001; Mills, Reyna, & Estrada, 2008). Similarly, just adults’ decisions are always made in as straightforward
because an adolescent says that she knows that having or rational a way as suggested by behavioral decision
“safe sex” can protect her against sexually transmitted theory. Nevertheless, this approach has opened up a
diseases doesn’t necessarily mean that she knows the new way of thinking about adolescent risk taking. In-
specific behaviors that constitute safe sex (Reyna & stead of viewing risky activities as the result of irratio-
Farley, 2006). nal or faulty judgment, experts are now trying to
understand where and how adolescents obtain the in-
❚ Age Differences in Values and Priorities If formation they use in reaching their conclusions, and
adolescents use the same decision-making processes as how accurate the information is. If, for example, ado-
adults, and if adolescents are no more likely than adults lescents underestimate the likelihood of getting preg-
to think of themselves as invulnerable, why, then, are nant following unprotected sex, sex education efforts
adolescents more likely to engage in risky behavior? might focus on teaching teenagers the actual probabil-
One answer may involve the different values and pri- ity. (Of course, this presumes that adolescents’ deci-
orities that adolescents and adults have (S. Moore & sions about whether to have sex are made rationally,
Gullone, 1996). For example, an individual’s decision to which may not be the case [P. Levine, 2001].)
try cocaine at a party may involve evaluating a number
of different consequences, including the legal and ❚ Emotional and Contextual Influences on Risk
health risks, the pleasure the drug may induce, and the Taking We should also keep in mind that emotional
way in which he or she will be judged (both positively and contextual factors, as well as cognitive ones, con-
and negatively) by the other people present. An adult tribute to adolescent risk taking (Dahl, 2008; Rivers,
and an adolescent may both consider all these conse- Reyna, & Mills, 2008; Seguin, Arseneault, & Tremblay,
quences, but the adult may place relatively more weight 2007; Steinberg, 2010). Several researchers have noted
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 81 21/03/13 9:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 81

FIGURE2.6 Self-reported 0.75


sensation seeking increases
between childhood and 0.70
adolescence and declines
0.65

Score
between adolescence and
adulthood. (Steinberg et al., 0.60
2008)
0.55

0.50
10–11 12–13 14–15 16–17 18–21 22–25 26–30
Age (years)

that adolescents may differ from adults in important they spend so much time in the peer group (Steinberg
ways that are not captured by measures of logical rea- & Monahan, 2007). Most adolescent risk taking, in-
soning, such as susceptibility to peer pressure, impul- cluding delinquency, drinking, and reckless behavior,
sivity, orientation to the present rather than the future, occurs when other teenagers are present, and adoles-
or reward seeking (Cauffman et al., 2010; Steinberg et cents are more likely to take risks when their friends are
al., 2008; Steinberg et al., 2009). A number of studies around (Steinberg, 2008). Although adolescent drivers,
have shown that adolescents’ decision making is as on average, take more chances than adults, how adoles-
good as adults’ when individuals are tested under calm cents drive depends on who is in the car; adolescents
conditions, but that the quality of adolescents’ decision drive much more safely when their parents are passen-
making declines more than adults’ when they are gers than when they are driving alone or with their
emotionally aroused (Figner & Weber, 2011; van friends (Simons-Morton et al., 2011).
Duijvenvoorde, Jansen, Visser, & Huizenga, 2010). The effect of peers on adolescent risk taking is clearly
With respect to emotional factors, for example, evident in studies of driving accidents. As Figure 2.7
studies show that individuals who are high in reward shows, having multiple passengers in the car increases
seeking and sensation seeking—that is, who seek out the risk of crashes dramatically among 16- and 17-year-
novel and intense experiences—are more likely to en- old drivers, significantly among 18- and 19-year-old
gage in various types of risky behaviors than their peers drivers, and not at all among adults. Consistent with
(C. A. Johnson et al., 2008; D. Miller & Byrnes, 1997), this, in one experiment, in which adolescents, college
and that both reward seeking and sensation seeking are undergraduates, and adults who were either alone or in
higher during adolescence than childhood or adult- a room with their friends played a video driving game
hood (Cauffman et al., 2010; Steinberg et al., 2008). that permitted risky driving—for instance, driving
Similarly, adolescents who are especially impulsive are through an intersection after a traffic light had turned
also more likely to engage in risky behavior (Romer et yellow—found that the mere fact of having friends
al., 2009a). One reason that middle adolescence is a watching their performance increased risk taking
period of heightened risk taking is that it is a period among adolescents and undergraduates, but not adults
characterized by a combination of high sensation seek- (M. Gardner & Steinberg, 2005). In that study, the pres-
ing and high impulsivity (Harden & Tucker-Drob, ence of peers also increased adolescents’ stated willing-
2011) (see Figure 2.6). ness to take all sorts of risks, including shoplifting,
The context in which individuals spend time mat- stealing, and allowing friends to bring drugs into their
ters, too (Boyer, 2006). Keep in mind that a good deal of house. In a subsequent study, in which the researchers
adolescents’ risk taking takes place in contexts in which imaged the teens’ brains while they played a similar
they are emotionally aroused (either very positively or video driving game, the results indicated that the brain
very negatively), unsupervised by adults, and encour- regions associated with the experience of reward were
aged by peers to engage in risky behavior (Albert & much more likely to be activated when the teenagers
Steinberg, 2001). For example, one recent study found were observed by their friends than when they were
that adolescents with mothers who worked nights were alone, and that risky driving was correlated with height-
more likely to take risks, in part because the parents ened activity in the brain’s reward areas. Thus, in the
were less likely to know their teen’s whereabouts (Han, presence of their peers, adolescents may pay more at-
Miller, & Waldfogel, 2010). As noted earlier, individuals’ tention to the potential rewards of a risky decision than
susceptibility to peer pressure is higher during early
and middle adolescence than later, suggesting that one
reason for teenagers’ greater risk taking is the fact that sensation seeking The pursuit of experiences that are novel or
exciting.
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 82 21/03/13 9:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

82 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

FIGURE 2.7 Although adolescents 8.00


on average are riskier drivers than
7.00
adults, who is in the car makes a big

Reckless driving index


difference. Teens drive more recklessly 6.00
when they are alone or with peers
5.00
than when they have adult passengers.
(Simons-Mortin et al., 2011) 4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1 2 3 4 5 6
Time since licensure (quarter)

they do when they are alone (O’Brien, Albert, Chein, & probabilities of various outcomes, but because they get
Steinberg, 2011). a gut-level feeling that keeps them away from the risky
act. If I am standing on a cliff overlooking a dark body
❚ Logic and Intuition More recently, several theo- of water that I can’t see into, the reason I don’t jump is
rists have proposed models of adolescent risk taking not that I’ve logically assessed my chances of getting
that consider the ways in which two different thinking hurt and decided that the odds are not in my favor. I
systems—one that is deliberative and logical, and one don’t jump because something inside me immediately
that is intuitive and gut-level—interact to influence be- tells me that it is a dumb thing to do. I don’t even have
havior (Reyna & Brainerd, 2011). According to these to think about it. In one study in which adolescents and
dual systems models, the heightened risk taking seen adults were asked whether some obviously risky things
during adolescence, and the drop in risk taking in (e.g., “setting your hair on fire,” “swimming with
adulthood, can’t be entirely due to deficiencies in logi- sharks,”) were bad things to do while their brain was
cal reasoning, because adults themselves do not always scanned, the researchers found that the adolescents
act logically. As the Nobel Prize–winning psychologist took longer to respond and were more likely than adults
Daniel Kahneman has pointed out (2011), we all often to activate brain regions that are involved in delibera-
behave in ways that defy logic. For instance, suppose I tive thinking; adults, in contrast, were more likely than
describe someone to you as quiet, introspective, me- adolescents to activate regions that reflect gut-level re-
thodical, and nerdy, and ask you whether that person is sponding (Baird, Fugelsang, & Bennett, 2005). One
more likely to be a mathematician or a waiter. Most other study has found that improvements in delibera-
people say “mathematician.” But this is certainly not tive decision-making abilities are associated with more,
correct—since in the United States there are over 7 mil- not less, risk taking (Wolff & Crockett, 2011).
lion people who are waiters or waitresses and only 3,000
people employed as mathematicians! If you answered ❚ Reducing Adolescent Risk Taking Generally
“mathematician,” you were probably using your intu- speaking, the most common approach to reducing ado-
ition, rather than logic. In this example, the gut-level lescent risk taking is through classroom-based educa-
choice happens to be wrong, but in many situations in tion programs designed to teach adolescents about the
life, our intuitions are correct. Being able to make some dangers of various activities (e.g., smoking, unpro-
decisions intuitively takes advantage of experience and tected sex, drinking and driving), about making better
permits us to make decisions much more quickly decisions, and about resisting peer pressure to engage
than we would be able to do if we had to reason every- in risky activity. As you will read in later chapters, how-
thing out. ever, the evidence that these programs work is very
Although the development of logical thinking may shaky. If, as we have seen, adolescents do not seem to be
differentiate adolescents from children, the main ignorant about the risks of these activities or deficient
change to take place between adolescence and adult- in the ways in which they make decisions, it does not
hood is not the further development of logical decision seem likely that efforts to change their knowledge or
making (as you read earlier, this is pretty much decision making will result in very much risk reduc-
completed by age 15 or so), but the continued develop- tion. And if recent research on brain maturation is
ment of intuitive decision making that is based on pointing to reasons for the inherent vulnerability of
experience. What stops adults from taking a lot of risks adolescence—the combination of heightened sensation
is not that they are good at systematically analyzing the seeking and immature impulse control—perhaps it
ste32118_ch02_054-083.indd Page 83 21/03/13 9:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 2 Cognitive Transitions 83

makes sense to rethink our approach to the problem after all. The challenge for parents, educators, and poli-
(Steinberg, 2007). cymakers is to find ways to permit adolescents to take
One alternative approach might focus on limiting risks without putting themselves in situations in which
adolescents’ opportunities to put themselves in risky they can hurt themselves.
situations. For example, because we know that adoles-
cents are more likely to have automobile accidents
when they have teenage passengers in the car or drive at RECAP
night, limiting the situations under which teenagers are • Contemporary researchers have been interested in
permitted to drive, especially after they are newly li- the ways in which the cognitive changes of adoles-
censed, would give adolescents that coveted driver’s li- cence play out in everyday activities.
cense but limit risky driving. Many states have found • One domain in which improvements in thinking are
that graduated driver licensing, which phases adoles- evident is the realm of social cognition. Adoles-
cent drivers into full unrestricted driving privileges cence is a time of increased understanding of other
over time, lowers the incidence of fatal crashes involv- people, social relationships, social conventions, and
ing 16-year-old drivers although it appears to increase personal rights.
the incidence of crashes among 18-year-olds (Masten, • As individuals move into and through adolescence
Foss, & Marshall, 2011b). they become better able to step outside them-
It is also possible to reduce adolescent risk taking selves and see things from other vantage points;
through economic policies. For example, although better able to see that the social “rules” we follow
anti-tobacco education has met with only limited suc- are not absolute; and more likely to develop a dif-
cess, increases in the cost of cigarettes have dramatically ferentiated, more nuanced understanding of social
reduced the rate of teen smoking. Thus, raising the norms.
price of tobacco or alcohol would likely diminish ado- • Scientists who have examined the underlying rea-
lescents’ use of these products, because adolescents sons for the higher rate of risk taking among ado-
generally do not have a great deal of money and would lescents have noted that this is not due to
therefore be sensitive to increases in the cost of smok- deficiencies in basic cognitive abilities. More likely
ing or drinking (Chaloupka, 2004; Gruber, 2001). An- causes are age differences in priorities, in psychoso-
other possibility would be to make risky substances cial factors, and in the contexts in which adoles-
harder for adolescents to obtain, by more strictly en- cents and adults spend time.
forcing policies that prohibit stores from selling to- • Efforts to educate adolescents about the health
bacco and alcohol to minors, limiting the supply of risks of various activities have had limited success,
illegal drugs, or enforcing laws that limit adolescents’ in part because adolescents are well aware of these
access to firearms. Yet a third possibility would be to risks but engage in these behaviors anyway. One al-
make the penalties for engaging in certain risky behav- ternative approach to reducing adolescent risk tak-
iors, like reckless or drunk driving, more severe, and in ing is focus on limiting adolescents'
so doing increase the salience of the potential costs of opportunities to put them-
engaging in the risky behavior. Finally, to the extent selves in risky
that sensation seeking may be a normal part of adoles- situations.
cence, perhaps we can figure out how to provide safe
outlets for this motivation. Not all risk taking is bad,
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 84 21/03/13 9:35 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 85 21/03/13 9:36 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
3
Social Transitions
SOCIAL REDEFINITION AND PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT
THE ELONGATION OF ADOLESCENCE
ADOLESCENCE AS A SOCIAL INVENTION
The “Invention” of Adolescence
Emerging Adulthood: A New Stage of Life or a Luxury of the Middle Class?
CHANGES IN STATUS DURING ADOLESCENCE
Drawing a Legal Boundary
Inconsistencies in Adolescents’ Legal Status
THE PROCESS OF SOCIAL REDEFINITION
Common Practices in the Process of Social Redefinition
VARIATIONS IN SOCIAL TRANSITIONS
Variations in Clarity
Variations in Continuity
THE TRANSITION INTO ADULTHOOD IN CONTEMPORARY SOCIETY
Special Transitional Problems of Poor and Minority Youth
The Effects of Poverty on the Transition Into Adulthood
What Can Be Done to Ease the Transition?
THE INFLUENCE OF NEIGHBORHOOD CONDITIONS ON ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
Processes of Neighborhood Influences

85
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 86 21/03/13 9:36 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

86 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

WHAT EVENTS IN YOUR LIFE told you—and others In this chapter, we will look at a third fundamental
around you—that you were no longer a child and had feature of adolescence—changes in the way in which
finally become an adolescent? Was it when you turned society defines who that person is and determines
13? Finished elementary school? Went to your first what rights and responsibilities she or he has as a result.
boy–girl party? Were allowed to be out at the mall Along with the biological changes of puberty and
without an adult? changes in thinking abilities, changes in social roles and
And what signaled, or will signal, that you are an social status constitute yet another universal feature of
adult? Turning 18? Turning 21? Getting your first full-time development during adolescence. As you will read,
job? Getting your driver’s license? Graduating from col- some theorists have argued that the nature of adoles-
lege? Getting your first apartment? Getting married? cent development is far more influenced by the way in
Each of these social transitions is not just an event. which society defines the economic and social roles of
Each is also a source of information—to the individual young people than by the biological or cognitive
and those in his or her life—about the individual’s stage changes of the period.
of development. Parents may treat their children differ- The study of social transitions in adolescence pro-
ently once they start high school, even if their appear- vides an interesting vehicle through which to compare
ance and behavior haven’t changed. Neighbors might adolescence across different cultures and historical ep-
look at a child who has grown up next door differently ochs. Puberty, after all, is pretty much the same every-
once they see him or her driving. Adolescents may feel where (although its timing and meaning may vary across
differently about themselves once they have started contexts). Abstract thinking and logical reasoning don’t
working at a “real” job. differ from one society to the next (although what
In all societies, adolescence is a period of social tran- people think and reason about certainly does). The so-
sition for the individual. Over the course of the adoles- cial transitions of adolescence are not the same, how-
cent years, the individual ceases to be viewed by society ever. Although the existence of a social passage from
as a child and comes to be recognized as an adult. This childhood into adulthood is universal, huge differences
chapter is about the ways in which individuals are rede- exist between the processes of social redefinition in
fined during adolescence and the implications of this industrialized society and those in the developing
process for psychological development. Although the world. In examining some of these differences, you will
specific elements of this social passage from childhood come to understand better how the way in which soci-
into adulthood vary across time and space, the recogni- ety structures the transition of adolescents into adult
tion that the individual’s status has changed—a social roles influences the nature of psychosocial develop-
redefinition of the individual—is universal. ment during the period.

Social Redefinition and Becoming an adult member of society, accompanied


as it is by increases in responsibilities and freedom, also
Psychosocial Development has an impact on the development of autonomy, or
independence. In contrast to the child, the adolescent-
Like the biological and cognitive transitions of adoles-
turned-adult faces a wider range of decisions that may
cence, the social transitions have important consequences
have serious long-term consequences (Woolard & Scott,
for the young person’s psychosocial development. In the
2009). An individual who has reached the drinking age,
realm of identity, for example, attainment of adult status
for example, must decide how to handle this new privi-
may transform a young woman’s self-concept, causing
lege. Should he go along with the crowd and drink every
her to feel more mature and to think more seriously about
weekend night, follow his parents’ example and abstain
future work and family roles. Similarly, an individual may
from drinking, or chart a middle ground? And in return
feel older and more mature the first time he or she reports
for the privileges that come with adult status, the
to work, goes into a bar, drives without an adult in the car,
adolescent-turned-adult is expected to behave in a more
or votes. In turn, these new activities and opportunities
responsible fashion. For example, receiving a driver’s li-
may prompt self-evaluation and introspection.
cense carries with it the obligation of driving safely.
Thus, the attainment of adult status provides chances
for the young person to exercise autonomy and to de-
social redefinition The process through which an individual’s velop a greater sense of independence.
position or status is redefined by society.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 87 21/03/13 9:36 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 87

Changes in social definition often bring with them


changes in relationships with others. Social redefinition
at adolescence is therefore likely to raise new questions
and concerns for the young person about intimacy—
including such matters as dating and marriage. Many
parents prohibit their children from dating until they
have reached an “appropriate” age, and not until the age
of majority (the legal age for adult status) are individu-
als allowed to marry without first gaining their parents’
permission. In certain societies, young people may even
be required to marry when they reach adulthood, enter-
ing into a marriage that may have been arranged while
they were children (Schlegel, 2009).
Changes in status at adolescence also may affect de-
velopment in the domain of sexuality. In contemporary
society, for example, laws governing sexual behavior
(such as the definition of statutory rape) typically dif-
ferentiate between individuals who have and have not
attained adult status. By becoming an adult in a legal
sense, the young person may be confronted with the Research on adolescent development has been discussed in court
cases involving whether teenagers should be permitted to make
need for new decisions about sexual activity. One prob-
medical decisions without their parents’ involvement.
lem continuing to face contemporary society is whether
sexually active individuals who have not yet attained
adult legal status should be able to make independent
• Although societies vary in how the transition from
decisions about such adult matters as abortion and
childhood to adulthood is signified, all cultures
contraception (Woolard & Scott, 2009).
have some way of recognizing that the individual’s
Finally, reaching adulthood often has important im-
rights and responsibilities have changed once he or
plications in the realm of achievement. For instance, in
she becomes an adult.
contemporary society, individuals can hold informal
• Changes in social definition may have profound
jobs, like babysitting, when they are still young, but it is
effects on development in the realms of
not until adult work status is attained (typically at age
identity, autonomy, intimacy,
15 or 16 in the United States) that young people can
sexuality, and
enter the labor force as full-time employees. And not
achievement.
until young people have reached a designated age are
they permitted to leave school of their own volition. In
less industrialized societies, becoming an adult typi-
cally entails entrance into the productive activities of
the community. Together, these shifts are likely to
prompt changes in the young person’s skills, aspira-
The Elongation of
tions, and expectations. Adolescence
One of the most striking aspects of the social transi-
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION tion experienced by adolescents today is the sheer
Which fundamental change of adoles- length of the time it takes to travel from childhood to
cence—biological, cognitive, or social—do adulthood. By virtually any indicator, adolescence lasts
you think has the most powerful effect on longer today than ever before, because young people
psychological development? Why? go through puberty (one marker of the beginning of
adolescence) earlier and enter into adult roles of work
and family (one way to define the end of adolescence)

age of majority The designated age at which an individual is


RECAP recognized as an adult.
• Changes in social definition—in how society views statutory rape Sex between two individuals, even when it is
the individual—constitute a third set of fundamen- consensual, when at least one of the persons is below the legal age
tal changes that define adolescence. of consent; in the United States, the specific age of consent varies
from state to state.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 88 21/03/13 9:36 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

88 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

later. Although this is truer in industrialized societies ability to behave responsibly). What implications
than in developing ones, there is evidence that adoles- does this have? How does it feel to be an adult psy-
cence lasts longer in developing countries as well chologically but a child financially?
(Larson, Wilson, & Rickman, 2009). One of the main reasons that adolescence has been
In previous eras, when puberty occurred around age lengthened is that much more formal education is
16 and individuals left school and went to work just a now necessary in order to make a successful transi-
few years later, adolescence, at least by these markers, tion into adult work roles. School, however, is not
lasted only a few years. Today, in contrast, young people something that all individuals enjoy equally. How
are caught between the worlds of childhood and adult- might forcing all adolescents to follow the same
hood for an extremely long time, often longer than a pathway into adulthood benefit some but not
decade, and frequently with only a vague sense of others?
when—and how—they become adults. Indeed, in the
minds of many social scientists who study adolescence
in modern society, the social passage of young people
into adult roles is too long, too vague, and too rocky MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
(S. Hamilton & Hamilton, 2009). And, as we shall see, The time between the age when individuals become
the passage from adolescence into adulthood is espe- sexually mature and when they marry was much
cially difficult for young people growing up in poverty, shorter in previous generations than it is today—
in part because social institutions that once enabled because sexual maturity takes place so much earlier
poor youth to make a successful transition into adult- now and marriage takes place so much later.
hood no longer provide sufficient support (Settersten, As a result, the admonition to “wait until
Furstenberg, & Rumbaut, 2005). In today’s economy, you’re married” to have sex was probably a
making a smooth and successful transition into adult lot easier to do 150 years ago than it is
work and family roles is challenging enough for indi- today. Do you agree?
viduals who graduate from college, but it has become
incredibly difficult for those who only complete high
school and almost impossible for those without a high
school diploma. RECAP
The lengthening of adolescence as a developmental
• The length of adolescence as a developmental pe-
period has had important implications for how young
riod has increased dramatically due to the earlier
people see themselves, relate to others, and develop
onset of puberty and the prolongation of formal
psychologically. Consider just a few examples:
education, which delays many of the role transi-
tions that mark the beginning of adulthood.
Parents used to “launch” their children from home
• Many observers of adolescence in contemporary
very close to the age of puberty. As a consequence,
society believe that the transition into adulthood is
individuals spent very little time living under the
too long, too vague, and too disorderly and that
same roof with their parents once they had become
this has had harmful effects on adolescents’ devel-
sexual beings. In contemporary society, however,
opment and well-being, especially those
individuals live with their parents long after they
for whom formal schooling is
have become sexually mature. What impact might
not a fulfilling
this have on parent–child relationships?
experience.
Adolescents today probably aren’t any less emotion-
ally mature than they were 100 years ago—if any-
thing, growing up under many of the pressures to
which individuals are exposed in today’s world may
have made them more mature at a younger age. But
because the financial cost of living independently
has increased so rapidly, adolescents’ economic Adolescence as a
“maturity” (that is, their ability to support them-
selves without help from their parents) lags far be-
Social Invention
hind their psychological maturity (that is, their Many writers have argued that adolescence, as a period
in the life cycle, is mainly a social invention (e.g., Fasick,
1994; Lapsley, Enright, & Serlin, 1985). These writers,
often referred to as inventionists, point out that, al-
though the biological and cognitive changes character-
inventionists Theorists who argue that the period of adolescence istic of the period are important in their own right,
is mainly a social invention.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 89 21/03/13 9:36 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 89

adolescence is defined primarily by the ways in which increasingly uncertain. Although a man may have been
society recognizes (or does not recognize) the period as a farmer, his son would not necessarily follow in his
distinct from childhood or adulthood. footsteps. One response to this uncertainty was that
Many of our images of adolescence are influenced by parents, especially in middle-class families, encouraged
the fact that society draws lines between adolescence and adolescents to spend time in school, preparing for
childhood (for instance, the boundary between elemen- adulthood. Instead of working side by side with their
tary and secondary school) and between adolescence and parents and other adults at home, as was the case before
adulthood (for instance, the age at which someone can industrialization, late–19th-century adolescents be-
vote). Inventionists stress that it is only because we see came increasingly more likely to spend their days with
adolescence as distinct that it exists as such. They point their peers, being educated or trained for the future. As
to other cultures and other historical periods in which you will read in later chapters, this change was one fac-
adolescence has been viewed very differently. Many of tor that led to the increased importance of peer groups
these theorists view the behaviors and problems charac- and youth culture, defining characteristics of modern
teristic of adolescence in contemporary society, such as adolescence we take for granted today that actually were
delinquency, as a consequence of the way that adoles- not prominent until the early 20th century.
cence is defined and young people are treated, rather Inventionists point out that the redefinition of ado-
than the result of the biological or cognitive givens of the lescence as a time of preparation rather than participa-
period. As you know, this is an entirely different view tion suited society’s changing economic needs as well
from that espoused by writers such as G. Stanley Hall, (Fasick, 1994). One initial outcome of industrialization
who saw the psychological changes of adolescence as was a shortage of job opportunities, because machines
driven by puberty and, as a result, by biological destiny. were replacing workers. Although adolescents provided
inexpensive labor, they were now competing with
adults for a limited number of jobs. One way of pro-
THE “INVENTION” OF ADOLESCENCE
tecting adults’ jobs was to remove adolescents from the
Have there always been adolescents? Although this may labor force, by turning them into full-time students. To
seem like a simple question with an obvious answer, it is accomplish this, society needed to begin discriminating
actually a very complicated issue. Naturally, there have between individuals who were “ready” for work and
always been individuals between the ages of 10 and 20, those who were not. And although there was little fac-
or who just passed through puberty, or whose frontal tual basis for the distinction, society began to view ado-
lobes were still maturing. But according to the inven- lescents as less capable and more in need of guidance
tionist view, adolescence as we know it in contemporary and training—as a way of legitimizing what was little
society did not really exist until the Industrial Revolu- more than age discrimination. Individuals who earlier
tion of the mid–19th century (Fasick, 1994). Prior to in the century would have been working next to adults
that time, in the agricultural world of the 16th or 17th were now seen as too immature or too unskilled to
century, children were treated primarily as miniature carry out similar tasks—even though the individuals
adults, and people did not make precise distinctions themselves hadn’t changed in any meaningful way.
among children of different ages (“child” referred to any- A less cynical view of the events of the late 19th
one under the age of 18 or even 21). Children provided century emphasizes the genuine motivation of some
important labor to their families, and they learned early adults to protect adolescents from the dangers of the
in their development the roles they were expected to fill new workplace, rather than the selfish desire to protect
later in life. The main distinction between children and adults’ jobs from teenagers. Industrialization brought
adults was based not on their age or their abilities but on with it worrisome changes in community life, espe-
whether they owned property (Modell & Goodman, 1990). cially in the cities. Many factories were dangerous
As a consequence, there was little reason to label some working environments, filled with new and unfamiliar
youngsters as “children” and others as “adolescents”—in machinery. The disruption of small farming commu-
fact, the term adolescent was not widely used prior to the nities and the growth of large urban areas was accom-
19th century. panied by increases in crime and in “moral degeneracy.”
Child protectionists argued that young people needed
❚ The Impact of Industrialization With indus- to be kept away from the labor force for their own
trialization came new patterns of work, education, and good. In addition to the rise of schools during this
family life. Adolescents were among those most dramat- time, the early 20th century saw the growth of many
ically affected by these changes. First, because the econ- organizations aimed at protecting young people, such
omy was changing so rapidly, away from the simple and
predictable life known in agrarian society, the connec-
tion between what individuals learned in childhood and child protectionists Individuals who argued, early in the 20th
what they would need to know in adulthood became century, that adolescents needed to be kept out of the labor force
in order to protect them from the hazards of the workplace.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 90 21/03/13 9:36 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

90 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

as the Boy Scouts and other adult-supervised youth


clubs (Modell & Goodman, 1990).

❚ The Origins of Adolescence as We Know It


Today For whatever the reason, it was not until the
late 19th century—little more than 100 years ago—that
adolescence came to be viewed as it is today: a lengthy
period of preparation for adulthood, in which young
people, in need of guidance and supervision, remain
economically dependent on their elders. This view
started within the middle class—where parents had
more to gain by keeping their children out of the labor
force and educating them for a better adulthood—but it
spread quickly throughout society. Because the work-
place has continued to change in ways that make the
future uncertain, the idea of adolescence as a distinctive
period of preparation for adulthood has remained in-
tact. Adolescence, as a transitional stage between child-
hood and adulthood, now exists in virtually all societies
around the world (Larson et al., 2009).

MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION


How has the rise of new technologies, Although adolescence was invented during the late 19th century,
such as the Internet, changed the nature it was not until the middle of the 20th century that our present-
of adolescence as a stage in life? day image of the teenager was created. Making an important
contribution to this image were the mass media—magazines such
as Seventeen cultivated the picture of the happy-go-lucky teenager
as a way of targeting advertisements toward an increasingly
lucrative adolescent market.

Interestingly, although the image of the American


Two other modifications of the definition of adoles- teenager—fun-loving, irresponsible, and independent—
cence also gave rise to new terminology and ideas. The now appears all over the world, in some societies it is
first of these is the introduction of the term teenager, viewed favorably (because it is evidence that the society
which was not employed until about 60 years ago. In con- has reached a level of affluence to be able to afford it),
trast to “adolescent,” “teenager” suggests a less serious age, while in others it is held up as an example of what adults
during which individuals concern themselves with such do not want their children to become (Larson et al., 2009).
weighty matters as cars, clothes, and cosmetics. An im- A second term whose acceptance grew as a result of
portant social change that led to the development of the social change is youth, which was used long before
concept of teenager was the increased affluence and eco- “adolescent.” But, prior to industrialization, youth had a
nomic freedom enjoyed by American adolescents during vague, imprecise meaning and could refer to someone as
the late 1940s and early 1950s (Fasick, 1994; Hine, 1999). young as 12 or as old as 24 (Modell & Goodman, 1990).
Advertisers recognized that these young people repre- Gradually, and during the 1960s in particular, the growth
sented an important consumer group and, with the help of the college population and the rise in student activism
of new publications such as Seventeen magazine, began focused attention on individuals who were somewhere
cultivating the image of the happy-go-lucky teenager as a between adolescence and young adulthood—those from
means of targeting ad campaigns toward the lucrative 18 to 22. Many adults referred to the changes they saw in
adolescent market (Osgerby, 2008; Palladino, 1996). attitudes and values among college students as the “youth
movement.” One theorist went so far as to argue that
youth is a separate stage in the life cycle, psychologically
as well as chronologically distinct from adolescence and
teenager A term popularized about 50 years ago to refer to young
adulthood (Keniston, 1970), an idea that is similar to the
people; it connoted a more frivolous and lighthearted image than
did “adolescent.” concept of “emerging adulthood” that we will examine
later in this chapter. Indeed, many college students today
youth Today, a term used to refer to individuals ages 18–22; it are unsure about whether they are adolescents or adults,
once referred to individuals ages 12–24.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 91 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 91

since they may feel mature in some respects (for exam- ❚ Is Emerging Adulthood Universal? This pro-
ple, keeping up an apartment or being involved in a file certainly describes many young people in contempo-
serious relationship) but immature in others (for exam- rary society, particularly those whose parents can foot the
ple, having to depend on parents for economic support bill while their “emerging adults” are figuring out what
or having to have an advisor approve class schedules). they want to do with their lives. As Arnett himself points
Although it may strike you as odd to think of 22-year- out (2000), however, emerging adulthood does not exist
olds as adolescents, the lengthening of formal schooling in all cultures—in fact, it exists in very few (the United
in contemporary society has altered the way we define States, Canada, Australia, New Zealand, Japan, and the
adolescence, because the majority of young people con- more affluent nations of Western Europe). A study of
tinue their education past high school and are forced to Chinese college students, for instance, found that the ma-
delay their transition into many adult work and family jority feel they have reached adult status in their early 20s
roles (Furstenberg, 2000). By this definition, many (L. J. Nelson, Badger, & Bo, 2004). And even within coun-
22-year-olds (and many individuals who are even older) tries in which there are significant numbers of emerging
are still not yet adults—a situation that often perplexes adults, the majority of individuals cannot afford to delay
parents as much as their adult children. In fact, when I the transition from adolescence into full-fledged adult-
now speak to groups of parents, I often receive more hood for a half decade. Several recent analyses indicate
questions about their young adult children than their that there is a great deal of variability among people in
teenagers—so much so that I expanded my book for par- their mid-20s with respect to the dimensions of emerg-
ents of adolescents, which had stopped at age 20, to cover ing adulthood (Howard & Galambos, 2011). As one team
development through age 25 (Steinberg, 2011). of researchers put it, “It is a mistake to think that a sample
of youth in their midtwenties are all emerging adults”
(e.g., M. J. Shanahan, Porfeli, Mortimer, & Erickson,
2005, p. 251). According to one study of Michigan
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION working- and middle-class youths in their mid-20s
Think for a moment about how your (Osgood, Ruth, Eccles, Jacobs, & Butler, 2005), for ex-
parents’ adolescence differed from yours? ample, six distinct lifestyle patterns, each characterized
What changes have taken place in society by different degrees of commitment to adultlike work,
since then that might have contributed to romantic, and residential arrangements, were evident:
this? fast starters (12%), parents without careers (10%), edu-
cated partners (19%), educated singles (37%), working
singles (7%), and slow starters (14%) (see Figure 3.1).
Note that the group that most resembled emerging
EMERGING ADULTHOOD: A NEW STAGE adults, the “educated singles,” made up less than 40% of
OF LIFE OR A LUXURY OF THE MIDDLE the sample.
CLASS? It is also important to note that the existence of
emerging adulthood is not entirely an economic phe-
The transition to adulthood has become so delayed in nomenon. Many emerging adults live the way they do
many industrialized societies that some have suggested because the economy forces them to, but many simply
that there is a new stage in life—emerging adulthood— do it by choice—that is, they want to take some time
that may last for some individuals until their mid-20s before assuming full adult responsibilities. Expectations
(Arnett, 2004). The main proponent of this idea has about the age at which one will likely marry appear to be
been psychologist Jeffrey Arnett, who contends that the especially important to this decision (J. S. Carroll et al.,
period from ages 18 to 25 is neither adolescence nor 2007). And, as one analysis of patterns of marriage and
adulthood, but a unique developmental period in its residential arrangements indicates, the lifestyle associ-
own right, characterized by five main features: ated with emerging adulthood—delaying marriage and
1. the exploration of possible identities before making parenthood, living alone or cohabiting, changing jobs
enduring choices; frequently, and “finding” oneself—is more prevalent in
some communities than in others. The researchers, us-
2. instability in work, romantic relationships, and liv-
ing U.S. census data, looked at differences between states
ing arrangements;
in patterns of marriage, household formation, and so
3. a focus on oneself and, in particular, on functioning forth (Lesthaeghe & Neidert, 2006). They found that in
as an independent person; some states a substantial number of individuals ap-
4. the subjective feeling of being between adolescence peared to be living the life of the quintessential emerg-
and adulthood; and ing adult, but in other states this was not very common.
5. the subjective sense that life holds many In particular, young adults in states that are generally
possibilities. more politically and socially conservative were far less
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 92 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

92 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Romantic Relationships Single Residence With parents


Steady dating Renting
80% Cohabiting 80% Home owner
Married

60% 60%

40% 40%

20% 20%

0% 0%
Fast Parents Educated Educated Working Slow Fast Parents Educated Educated Working Slow
starters without partners singles singles starters starters without partners singles singles starters
careers careers

Parenthood No Employment Not employed


Yes Short-term
100% 80% Career step
Long-term
80%
60%
60%
40%
40%
20%
20%

0% 0%
Fast Parents Educated Educated Working Slow Fast Parents Educated Educated Working Slow
starters without partners singles singles starters starters without partners singles singles starters
careers careers
FIGURE 3.1 It is difficult to generalize about lifestyle patterns followed in early adulthood. One study found six distinct
clusters that varied in their relationships, living arrangements, employment, and transition to parenthood. (Osgood et al., 2005)

likely to follow the emerging-adulthood pattern than suggests both a potentially difficult time, characterized
those in more liberal states. This suggests that the exis- by floundering and financial instability, and a time of
tence of emerging adulthood may have a lot to do with carefree optimism and independence. By all accounts,
values and priorities, and not just the economy. though, the second picture seems to be more accurate
than the first (e.g., Arias & Hernández, 2007; Arnett,
2007; Gottlieb, Still, & Newby-Clark, 2007). Several
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION studies show that the period is generally one of positive
and improving mental health (see Figure 3.2), although,
Emerging adulthood as a unique
naturally, not all individuals show this pattern
stage of life is seen more in some
(Galambos, Barker, & Krahn, 2006; H. Kim, Capaldi, &
cultures than others. What factors
Stoolmiller, 2003).
do you think make it more or less
One study of mental health during this age period
likely to exist?
followed a national sample of American youths from age
18 to their mid-20s. The researchers compared four
groups: (1) those who reported positive well-being across
the entire interval, (2) those who reported negative well-
❚ Psychological Well-Being in Emerging Adult- being across the entire interval, (3) those whose well-
hood Very little research has examined psychological being started low but increased, and (4) those whose
development and functioning during emerging well-being started high but decreased (Schulenberg,
adulthood. The profile initially described by Arnett Bryant, & O’Malley, 2004). (There also was a large group
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 93 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 93

2.87 4.05
Female
Depressive symptoms

2.76 3.98
Male

Self-esteem
2.66 3.90
Male

2.55 3.82
Female
2.44 3.75
18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25
Age Age
FIGRUE 3.2 Early adulthood is a time of improved mental health, as indicated by decreases in depression and increases in
self-esteem. (Galambos et al., 2006)

whose well-being was average to begin with and stayed distinct and unique set of psychological transitions dur-
that way.) They then looked to see whether these patterns ing middle age has become widely accepted (if perhaps
of well-being over time were related to indicators of indi- less supported by hard evidence than journalists and
vidual functioning, by rating whether individuals had other writers in the popular media suggest).
been succeeding, maintaining, or stalling as they moved Clearly, in some parts of the world, and in some seg-
into adulthood (see Table 3.1). Three main findings ments of society, the transition between adolescence
emerged. First, over 80% of the sample showed great sta- and adulthood has been lengthened—that much is in-
bility in their well-being over the period. This is consis- disputable. More individuals attend college than in pre-
tent with findings from many other studies indicating vious eras, which delays their entrance into the world of
that psychological functioning in childhood and adoles- work, and more postpone getting married, which de-
cence is highly predictive of success later in life (e.g., lays their settling into a more stable lifestyle. But
M. J. Shanahan & Bauer, 2004). This is because success in whether this means that the psychological functioning
one stage (doing well in high school) usually leads to suc- of 23-year-olds who follow this pattern of a delayed and
cess in the next stage (getting into a good college), and prolonged transition is significantly different from that
some basic “resources” predict success throughout the of 23-year-olds who transition from college directly
life span—as the old joke goes, if you want to succeed in into full-time employment and marriage is anyone’s
life, make sure you have a high IQ, money, and good guess, because it hasn’t been studied. In other words,
parents (e.g., K. Burt, Obradović, Long, & Masten, 2008; whether a psychological stage of emerging adulthood
Masten, Burt, & Roisman, 2004). Second, for more than really exists has yet to be established (Côté, & Bynner,
a sixth of the sample, this period was one of substantial 2008). For example, even though the transition into
change in mental health—about 7% of the sample were adult work and family roles has been delayed in many
well-functioning adolescents whose mental health de- segments of society, there is no evidence that this has
clined, and another 10% were troubled adolescents who been accompanied by a comparable prolongation into
became “exemplary” young adults. Finally, experiences in adulthood of problem behaviors typically associated
the domains of work, romance, and citizenship—but not with late adolescence, such as crime or substance abuse
in the domains of school or finances—were especially (Hayford & Furstenberg, 2008). In other words, it is
linked to changes in well-being. Other studies have also entirely possible that economic forces have delayed the
found that success in the worlds of work and romance transition into social adulthood (i.e., the roles that peo-
are related to well-being during this time (Galambos ple occupy) but have not affected the transition into
et al., 2006). psychological adulthood.
Has a new stage of life actually arisen? How wide- Note that when we say that adolescence (or the teen-
spread does a pattern of behavior have to be for us to ager, or youth, or emerging adulthood) is in part a so-
conclude that a new stage of development has emerged? cial invention, we do not mean that its significance is in
Does emerging adulthood exist if it is common in Boston any way diminished or that it is somehow less real than
or Berkeley, but not in Biloxi, Baghdad, or Beijing? It is if it were an entirely biological phenomenon. Democ-
hard to say. “Midlife” is a stage that we take for granted in racy, after all, is a social invention, too, but its creation
contemporary America but that doesn’t exist every- and development have had profound effects on the way
where, or at least, not as we think of it (Schweder, 1998). that we live. As with other social inventions, the notion
Nevertheless, the notion that individuals go through a that there should be a distinct period of adolescence has
94

TABLE 3.1 Descriptions of developmental task domains


PART 1

Domain Succeeding Maintaining Stalling

Education • Graduated from 4-year college by age 26, or • Expected to graduate from 4-year college • Expected 4-year college degree and did
• Expected to graduate from 2-year college and and received 2-year degree, or not receive either 2- or 4-year degree by
received 2-year degree • Did not expect 2- or 4-year degree and did age 26
not receive either degree, or
• Expected 2-year degree and did not receive
2-year degree by age 26
Work • No unemployment at age 22 or 26, and • Neither succeeding nor stalling (e.g., • Some unemployment at age 22 and/or
• Working 101 months/year at full-time job homemaker or otherwise not working and 26, and
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 94 21/03/13 9:37 AM user

by age 26, and not looking for work outside home) • Low job confidence at age 26
• High job confidence at age 26
The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Financial autonomy • Self and/or spouse providing all resources • Some resources come from other than self • At age 26, less than half of support is
at age 26 or spouse (and not stalling) from self and/or spouse, and/or
• Live with parents and receive some
financial help (.20%) from them
Romantic involvement • At age 26, married or engaged (with or without • At age 26, cohabiting, or • At age 26, not married, not engaged not
cohabitation), and • Dating more than once a month, and/or cohabiting, and
• No divorce history • Divorced, but remarried • Dating once a month or less
Peer involvement • Goes out two or more times a week for fun and • Neither succeeding nor stalling • Goes out one or fewer times a week for
recreation at age 22 and age 26 fun and recreation at age 22 and age 26
Substance abuse avoidance • No substance use at any age (18, 22, 26) • Some substance use, but less than stalling • Use of two or more substances at all
(healthy coping/lifestyle) [Four indicators of substance use: three ages (18, 22, 26), and/or
cigarettes (current use), binge drinking • Use of three or more substances at age
(in past 2 weeks), marijuana (current use), 26
and other illicit drugs (in past 12 months)]
Citizenship • Three indicators reported at age 22 and age 26: • Measures indicate some strong • Measures indicate no strong
social conscience, charity, and awareness connections but not as frequent as at connections at age 26
of social/political events succeeding level
• Measures indicate at least one strong connection
at age 22 and at least two strong connections
at age 26

Source: Schulenberg et al., 2004.


/204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 95 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 95

endured over time and has had important and concrete adult status (Markstrom, 2011a). In contemporary
repercussions. But as with other social constructs, the America, attaining the age of majority brings the right
nature of adolescence changes over time, and it will to vote. But along with this increased power usually
continue to change as we revise our notions of what it come new obligations. In most societies, young adults
means to grow from childhood into adulthood. are expected to serve their communities in cases of
emergency or need, and in many cultures, training for
warfare is often demanded of young people once they
RECAP attain adult status (Benedict, 1934). On the other hand,
• Inventionists argue that adolescence is more a so- in most societies, it is not until adult status is attained
cial invention than a biological or cognitive that individuals are permitted to participate in certain
phenomenon. activities that are reserved for adults. Gambling, pur-
• Our conception of adolescence—whether it exists chasing alcoholic beverages, and seeing X-rated films
as a separate period and what its nature is—is are but three of the many privileges we reserve in
determined largely by forces in the broader social America for individuals who have reached the legal age
environment. Changes in the broader environment, of adulthood.
therefore, can change the very nature of Once an adolescent is designated as an adult, she or
adolescence. he is also subject to a new set of laws and will be treated
• Adolescence as we know it today is largely a prod- differently by the society’s legal institutions than would
uct of the Industrial Revolution of the late 19th be a child. In some instances, attaining adult status
century. brings with it more lenient treatment under the law,
• Some writers have suggested that a new stage of whereas in others, it may be associated with harsher
life, called emerging adulthood, has arisen in recent treatment. In the United States, for example, certain ac-
years. Evidence for the existence of emerging adult- tivities that are permissible among adults, such as not
hood as a widespread phenomenon is mixed. showing up for school (truancy) or leaving one’s home
• Although there is not a great deal of research on without informing others (running away), are consid-
psychological development during the years imme- ered offenses when they are committed by young people.
diately following adolescence, several studies indi- (We use the term status offense to refer to a behavior
cate that for most people, especially those who that is problematic because of the young person’s status
successfully move into adult work and ro- as a juvenile.) As a college student, you cannot be legally
mantic roles, this is a time of in- punished for not showing up for class, as would have
creasing well-being and been the case if you didn’t show up for high school. If
positive mental you decide you don’t want to return home when you are
health. on a break, you don’t have to, at least not as far as the law
is concerned; in contrast, running away from home dur-
ing adolescence is against the law in many jurisdictions.
Certain crimes, when committed by a minor, are adjudi-
cated in a separate juvenile justice system, which oper-
Changes in Status ates under different rules and principles than the
criminal justice system that applies to adults. Although
During Adolescence being tried in the juvenile justice system usually results
Changes in social definition at adolescence typically in a less severe sanction than being found guilty of the
involve a two-sided modification in the individual’s same crime in adult court, this is not always the case
status. On the one hand, the adolescent is given certain (Kurlychek & Johnson, 2010).
privileges and rights that are typically reserved for the The legal regulation of adolescent behavior in the
society’s adult members. On the other hand, this in- United States has been quite controversial in recent
creased power and freedom generally are accompanied years. Part of the problem is that development during
by increased expectations for self-management, per-
sonal responsibility, and social participation.
initiation ceremony The formal induction of a young person into
adulthood.
DRAWING A LEGAL BOUNDARY
status offense A violation of the law that pertains to minors but
With the attainment of adult status, the young person is not adults.
often permitted more extensive participation in the
juvenile justice system A separate system of courts and related
community’s decision making. In many Native American institutions developed to handle juvenile crime and delinquency.
groups, such as the Navajo or Apache, it is only after a
formal initiation ceremony that adolescents are granted criminal justice system The system of courts and related
institutions developed to handle adult crime.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 96 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

96 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

❚ Adolescents as Criminal Defendants There is


especially great disagreement about how we should view
and treat young people who commit serious violent of-
fenses (E. Scott & Steinberg, 2008). Should juveniles who
commit crimes be viewed as less blameworthy than
adults because they are less able to foresee the conse-
quences of their actions or resist the pressure of others to
engage in antisocial activity? Or should we hold adoles-
cents and adults to the same standards of criminal re-
sponsibility? If a youngster has committed a violent
crime, should he or she be treated as a child (and
processed as a delinquent) or tried as an adult (and
processed as a criminal)? Should young teenagers and
adults who are convicted of the same crime receive the
same penalties? In 2005, the U.S. Supreme Court raised
the age at which individuals can be exposed to the death
penalty from 16 to 18 (Roper v. Simmons, 2005), and, as
noted in the Introduction to this book, several more re-
cent cases have asked whether it is constitutional to
sentence juveniles to life without parole (Steinberg, 2012).
One issue that arises in cases in which a juvenile
might be tried as an adult is whether the adolescent is
competent to stand trial. In the United States, it is not
permissible to try someone in a criminal proceeding if
the individual does not understand the charges against
him or her, does not understand the nature of the trial,
or is unable to make reasoned decisions about the case
During the mid-1990s, many states changed their laws to permit
adolescents accused of violent crimes to be tried as adults. (for example, whether to take the stand in his or her
own defense). Historically, questions about a defen-
dant’s competence to stand trial have been raised only
adolescence is so rapid and so variable between indi- in cases in which the individual is mentally ill or men-
viduals that it is difficult to know at what chronological tally retarded. Now that more and more juveniles are
age a line should be drawn between legally viewing being tried as adults at younger and younger ages, how-
someone as an adult and viewing him or her as a child ever, experts have asked whether some young defen-
(E. Scott & Steinberg, 2008; Woolard & Scott, 2009). dants may be incompetent to stand trial simply because
This problem is compounded by the fact that we draw of cognitive or emotional immaturity (Viljoen,
the boundary at different places for different purposes McLachlan, Wingrove, & Penner, 2010). One study of
(for example, driving at 16, voting at 18, buying alcohol this issue examined the competence to stand trial of a
at 21, and so on). This inconsistency makes it hard to large sample of individuals between the ages of 11 and
point to any specific age and say with certainty that 24 (Grisso et al., 2003). The researchers found that
there is consensus about where the legal boundary about one-third of those aged 13 and younger, and one-
should be drawn. One current controversy is whether fifth of 14- and 15-year-olds, were as impaired in their
new findings from studies of adolescent brain develop- abilities to serve as a competent defendant as were men-
ment should influence where we draw these legal lines tally ill adults who had been found not competent to
(Steinberg, 2012). stand trial (see Figure 3.3). This study suggests that
courts should not assume that younger adolescents,
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION even those who are not mentally ill or retarded, are nec-
essarily competent to be tried as adults and that juve-
At what age do you think
niles’ competence to stand trial should be evaluated
we should draw the line
before their cases can be heard in adult court. Research
between legal adolescence
also indicates that juveniles are less likely than adults to
and legal adulthood?
understand their rights when being questioned by the
Should this age be the
police, more likely to confess to a crime than remain
same for all activities, or
silent, and less likely to discuss disagreements with their
should different activities
attorneys (Redlich, Silverman, & Steiner, 2003; Viljoen,
have different age boundaries?
Klaver, & Roesch, 2005). Ironically, when people are
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 97 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 97

35 although vulgar, are not considered so obscene that


Percent significantly impaired they are outlawed among adults (Zimring, 1982). As
30
you know, the age at which adolescents are permitted to
25 engage in various adult behaviors—driving, voting,
drinking, viewing R-rated movies, smoking—varies
20
considerably from one domain to the next. Is there a
15 pattern to this inconsistency? In general, legal decisions
tend to set the age boundary high, in order to restrict
10
the behavior of adolescents, when the behavior in ques-
5 tion is viewed as potentially damaging to the young
person (for example, buying cigarettes), but have sup-
0
11–13 14–15 16–17 18+ ported the concept of adolescent autonomy and set the
Age group age boundary low when the behavior is viewed as hav-
FIGURE 3.3 Significant numbers of juveniles under the age ing potential benefit (for example, having access to con-
of 16 may not be competent to stand trial as adults. Shown traceptives) (E. Scott & Steinberg, 2008).
are the proportions of individuals at different ages whose
trial competence is as impaired as mentally ill adults who
have been found incompetent. (Grisso et al., 2003) RECAP
• Changes in the individual’s social definition at ado-
polled about the ages at which adolescents should be lescence typically involve changes in status. When
treated as adults, they favor younger ages for criminal they become adults, individuals are given greater
prosecution than for granting adolescents various types privileges but are expected to take increased re-
of independence (Reppucci, Scott, & Antonishak, sponsibility for self-management and participation
2009). in adult society.
• An especially controversial issue concerns whether
INCONSISTENCIES IN ADOLESCENTS’ juveniles who commit serious crimes should be
LEGAL STATUS treated as children or tried as adults.
• In contemporary America, there is great inconsis-
Many other issues surrounding the legal status of adoles- tency in where we draw age boundaries between
cents in the United States remain vague and confusing childhood and adulthood. Different
(Steinberg, 2012). Two U.S. Supreme Court cases indi- ages are used for different
cate just how inconsistent our views of adolescents’ sta- purposes.
tus are (Moshman, 1993). In one case, Hazelwood v.
Kuhlmeier, the Court ruled that a public high school can
censor articles written by students for their school news-
paper, on the grounds that adolescents are so “immature”
that they need the protection of “wiser” adults. Yet the
same Court also ruled, in Board of Education v. Mergens, The Process of Social
that students who wanted to form a Bible study group
had the right to meet on campus because high school
Redefinition
students are “mature” enough to understand that a Social redefinition during adolescence is not a single
school can permit the expression of ideas that it does not event but, like puberty or cognitive maturation, a series
necessarily endorse. Similarly, in Hodgson v. Minnesota, of events that often occur over a relatively long time. In
the Court ruled that, because of their maturity, adoles- contemporary America, the process of redefinition typ-
cents do not need to obtain parental consent to get an ically begins at age 15 or 16, when the young person is
abortion. Yet the Court also ruled, in Roper v. Simmons, first permitted to drive, to work, and to leave school.
that adolescents should not be subject to the death pen- But in most states, the social redefinition of the adoles-
alty, because their immaturity makes them less respon- cent continues well into young adulthood. Some privi-
sible for their criminal behavior (and therefore less leges of adulthood, such as voting, are not conferred
“punishable”) (Steinberg, Cauffman, et al., 2009). until the age of 18, and others, such as purchasing alco-
There are many other examples of this sort of holic beverages, are not conferred until the age of 21,
inconsistency. For example, courts have ruled that teen- five or six years after the redefinition process begins.
agers have the right to obtain contraceptives or pur- Even in societies that mark the social redefinition of the
chase violent video games without their parents’ young person with a dramatic and elaborate initiation
approval. But they also have upheld laws forbidding ceremony, the social transformation of the individual
adolescents access to cigarettes or to magazines that, from child into adult may span many years, and the
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 98 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

98 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

initiation ceremony may represent just one element of


the process (Markstrom, 2011b). In fact, the initiation
ceremony usually marks the beginning of a long period
of training and preparation for adulthood and not the
adolescent’s final passage into adult status.
In many cultures, the social redefinition of young
people occurs in groups. That is, the young people of a
community are grouped with peers of approximately
the same age—a cohort—and move through the series
of status transitions together. One of the results of such
age-grouped social transitions is that very strong bonds
are formed among youngsters who have shared certain
rituals. In many American high schools, for example,
attempts are made to create class spirit or class unity by
fostering bonds among students who will graduate to-
gether. In many Latino communities, adolescent girls
participate together in an elaborate sort of “coming-
out” celebration, called the quinceañera. On college
campuses, fraternities and sororities may conduct Formal rites of passage from adolescence to adulthood are rare in
contemporary society. Certain cultural ceremonies—like the
group initiations that involve difficult or unpleasant quinceañera, a coming-of-age celebration for young women in
tasks, and special ties may be forged between “brothers” some Latino communities—are about as close as we come in
or “sisters” who have pledged together. today’s society.

COMMON PRACTICES IN THE PROCESS


❚ An Emphasis on Differences Between the
OF SOCIAL REDEFINITION
Sexes A second aspect of social redefinition during
Although the specific ceremonies, signs, and timetables adolescence entails the accentuation of physical and
of social redefinition during adolescence vary from one social differences between males and females (Schlegel
culture to another, several general themes characterize & Barry, 1991). This accentuation of differences occurs
the process in all societies. partly because of the physical changes of puberty and
partly because adult work and family roles are often
❚ Real or Symbolic Separation From Parents highly sex-differentiated. Many societies separate
First, social redefinition usually entails the real or sym- males and females during religious ceremonies, have
bolic separation of young persons from their parents individuals begin wearing sex-specific articles of cloth-
(Markstrom, 2011b). During late childhood in some soci- ing (rather than clothing permissible for either gen-
eties, children are expected to begin sleeping in house- der), and keep males and females apart during
holds other than their own. Youngsters may spend the day initiation ceremonies.
with their parents but spend the night with friends of the In many non-Western societies today, the privileges
family, with relatives, or in a separate residence reserved extended to males and females once they have reached
for preadolescents. In America, during earlier times, it puberty are so different that adolescence often is an en-
was customary for adolescents to leave home temporarily tirely different phenomenon for boys and girls (Larson
and live with other families in the community, either to et al., 2009). Examples of the differential treatment of
learn specific occupational skills as apprentices or to work adolescent boys and girls in non-Western cultures
as domestic servants (Kett, 1977). Interestingly, the “plac- abound, but in general, girls’ behavior is more subject
ing out” of adolescents from their parents’ home often to the control of adults, whereas boys are given more
coincided with puberty (which, as you now know, freedom and autonomy (Markstrom, 2011b). Girls are
occurred at a much later age at this time in history) expected to remain virgins until marriage, for example,
(M. Katz, 1975). In contemporary societies, the separa- whereas boys’ premarital sexual activity is tolerated.
tion of adolescents from their parents takes somewhat Girls are expected to spend time preparing for domestic
different forms. They are sent to summer camps, to roles, whereas boys are expected to acquire vocational
boarding schools, or, as is more common, to college. skills for employment outside the home. And formal
schooling is far less available to girls than to boys, espe-
cohort A group of individuals born during the same general
cially in rural societies.
historical era. The separation of males and females in adolescence
is not limited to non-Western societies. In earlier times
quinceañera An elaborate sort of “coming-out” celebration for in America (and to a certain extent in many other
adolescent girls that is practiced in many Latino communities.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 99 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 99

industrialized societies today), during adolescence, the body, often done as part of an initiation ceremony—
males and females were separated in educational insti- may seem alien to us. But if we look beneath the sur-
tutions, either by excluding adolescent girls from sec- face, at the meaning and significance of each culture’s
ondary and higher education, grouping males and practices, we find many common threads. In contem-
females in different schools or different classrooms, or porary society, for example, although we do not prac-
having males and females follow different curricula. In tice anything as “alien” as scarification, we do have our
present-day America, many of these practices have been share of body rituals, many of which are not seen until
discontinued because of legal rulings prohibiting sex adolescence and which might seem equally alien to
discrimination, but some elements of accentuated sex someone unfamiliar with our society: the punching of
differentiation and sex segregation during adolescence holes in earlobes or other parts of the body (ear or body
still exist—for example, in residential arrangements, piercing), the scraping of hair from faces or legs (shav-
styles of dress, athletic activities, and household chores. ing), the permanent decoration of skin (tattoos), and
And many contemporary ceremonies designed to rec- the application of brightly colored paints to lips, eyes,
ognize the young person’s passage into adulthood are and cheeks (putting on makeup).
either limited to one sex or the other (for example, deb-
utante balls or the quinceañera, each of which is for
young women) or somehow differentiate between MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
males and females (for example, the Bar Mitzvah and Social redefinition in contemporary society
the Bas Mitzvah ceremonies for Jewish males and fe- is often so familiar that we overlook it. Can
males, respectively). you think of examples of social definition
practices within your personal experience
❚ The Passing on of Information From the Older that parallel practices in more traditional
Generation A third aspect of social redefinition cultures?
during adolescence typically entails the passing on of
cultural, historical, and practical information from the
adult generation to the newly inducted cohort of
young people. This information may concern (1) mat- RECAP
ters thought to be important to adults but of limited • Certain themes are common to the process of so-
utility to children (for example, information about the cial redefinition across many societies. These in-
performance of certain adult work tasks), (2) matters clude the real or symbolic separation of young
thought to be necessary for adults but unfit for chil- people from their parents, the accentuation of dif-
dren (for example, information regarding sex), or (3) ferences between males and females, and the pass-
matters concerning the history or rituals of the family ing on of cultural, historical, or practical
or community (for example, how to perform certain information deemed important for adulthood.
ceremonies). In traditional societies, initiates are often • Although these themes are more explicit in the for-
sent to some sort of “school” in which they are in- mal initiation ceremonies practiced in traditional
structed in the productive activities of the community societies, they are evident in the process through
(hunting, fishing, or farming). Following puberty, boys which individuals are redefined in
and girls receive instruction about sexual relations, contemporary industrial-
moral behavior, and societal lore (M. Fried & Fried, ized society as
1980; N. Miller, 1928). well.
In contemporary society, too, adolescence is a time of
instruction in preparation for adulthood. Elementary
school students, for example, are generally not taught a
great deal about sexuality, work, or financial matters;
such course work is typically reserved for high school Variations in
students. We also restrict entrance into certain “adult”
activities (such as sexually explicit movies) until adoles- Social Transitions
cents are believed old enough to be exposed to them. Although the presence of social redefinition in a general
Because formal initiation ceremonies are neither sense is a universal feature of adolescent development,
very common nor very meaningful in modern society,
students sometimes overlook important similarities
between the processes of social redefinition in tradi- Bar (Bas) Mitzvah In Judaism, the religious ceremony marking the
tional and contemporary societies. Practices like sepa- young person’s transition to adulthood.
rating children from their parents or scarification—the
intentional creation of scars on some part or parts of scarification The intentional creation of scars on some part or
parts of the body, often done as part of an initiation ceremony.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 100 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

100 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

there is considerable diversity in the nature of the


transition. Examining social redefinition from cross-
cultural and historical perspectives provides a valuable
means of contrasting the nature of adolescence in dif-
ferent social contexts. Two very important dimensions
along which societies differ in the process of social
redefinition are in the explicitness, or clarity, of the
transition and in the smoothness, or continuity, of the
passage.

VARIATIONS IN CLARITY
Because initiation ceremonies are in many ways
religious ceremonies, they are most often found in
societies in which a shared religious belief unites the
community and structures individuals’ daily experi-
ences. Universal, formal initiation ceremonies there-
fore have never been prevalent in U.S. society, largely
because of the cultural diversity of the population and In many nonindustrial societies, a formal initiation ceremony is
used to mark the beginning and end of adolescence.
the general separation of religious experience from
everyday affairs.
There are, however, factors other than the presence
of formal rites of passage that determine how clear the and social groups, the young American adolescent
transition into adult status is to young persons and to may undergo an initiation ceremony of sorts—the
society. One such factor concerns the extent to which confirmation, the Bar or Bas Mitzvah, and the quincea-
various aspects of the status change occur at about the ñera are some examples—rarely does such a rite have
same time for individuals and during the same general much significance outside the youngster’s family, circle
period for adolescents growing up together (Elder, of friends, or religious community. School graduation
1980). When transitions into adult work, family, and ceremonies perhaps come the closest to universal rites
citizenship roles occur close in time, and when most of passage in contemporary society, but school gradu-
members of a cohort experience these transitions at ation does not bring with it many meaningful or uni-
about the same age, the passage into adulthood takes on versal changes in social status, responsibilities, or
greater clarity. If all young people were to graduate privileges. As a result, social redefinition in contempo-
from high school, enter the labor force, and marry at rary society does not give adolescents any clear indica-
the age of 18, this age would be an implicit boundary tion of when their responsibilities and privileges as an
between adolescence and adulthood, even without a adult begin. As we noted earlier, laws governing the age
formal ceremony. But when different aspects of the pas- at which individuals can and cannot do “adult” activi-
sage occur at different times, and when adolescents ties are inconsistent (Steinberg, 2012). In many states,
growing up in a similar environment experience these for example, the age for starting employment is 15; for
transitions in different order and along different sched- driving, 16; for attending restricted (R-rated) movies
ules, the boundary between adolescence and adulthood without parents, 17; for voting, 18; and for drinking,
becomes cloudier. 21. In some states, the age at which someone can be
tried as an adult for a serious violent crime is as low as
❚ The Clarity of Social Redefinition in Contem- 10 (Hartney, 2006).
porary Society When did you become an adoles- In short, we have few universal markers of
cent? When did you (or when will you) become an adulthood—adolescents are treated as adults at differ-
adult? If you are like most individuals in contemporary ent times by different people in different contexts. A
society, your answers to these questions will not be young person may be legally old enough to drive, but
clear-cut. One study, for example, found that, when his parents may feel that 16 is too early and may refuse
asked, “Do you feel that you have reached adulthood?” to let him use the family car. Another may be treated
nearly half of all 12- to 17-year-olds and nearly three- like an adult at work, where she works side by side with
fifths of 18- to 25-year-olds answered, “Yes and no” people 3 times her age, but be treated like a child at
(Arnett, 2000). We have no formal ceremonies marking home. A third may be viewed as an adult by her mother
the transition from childhood into adolescence, nor do but as a child by her father. The same young person
we have any way to mark the passage from adolescence whom we send into combat is not permitted to buy
into adulthood. Although in many religious, cultural, beer, even though combat is far more dangerous than
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 101 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 101

drinking. It is little wonder, in light of the mixed and Second, over time, there has been a striking decline in
sometimes contradictory expectations facing young the importance of family roles—marriage and parent-
people, that for many adolescents the transition into hood—as defining features of the transition from ado-
adult roles is sometimes a difficult passage to navigate. lescence to adulthood. In early American society, for
example, the role of head of household was an especially
❚ Adolescents’ Views of Themselves Because important indicator of adult status for males, and taking
contemporary society does not send clear or consistent on the roles of wife and mother defined adulthood for
messages to young people about when adolescence ends females. In contrast, in surveys of contemporary youth,
and adulthood begins, young people living within the in which they were asked whether certain accomplish-
same society can have widely varying views of their own ments were necessary for an individual to be considered
social status and beliefs about age-appropriate behavior an adult, only 17% of the respondents indicated that be-
(Arnett, 1994; Nurmi, 1993). For this reason, it is instruc- ing married was necessary, and only 14% indicated that
tive to ask people what they think defines the transition it was necessary to become a parent (Arnett, 1998).
to adulthood, as a way of gauging the way in which adult Finally, the defining criteria of adulthood have be-
status is conceptualized by the broader society. Studies of come more or less the same for males and females in
how people define adulthood in contemporary society contemporary industrialized society, unlike the case in
indicate three interesting trends (Arnett, 1998). traditional societies or during previous eras. In nonin-
First, in modern industrialized society, at least in the dustrialized cultures, the requirements for male adult-
United States and Canada, adolescents place relatively hood were to be able to “provide, protect, and procreate,”
less of an emphasis than is the case in traditional societ- whereas for females, the requirements for adulthood
ies on the attainment of specific roles (for example, were to care for children and run a household (Mark-
worker, spouse, parent) as defining characteristics strom, 2011b). Contemporary youth, in contrast, view
of adulthood and relatively more emphasis on the de- the various indicators of adult status as equally impor-
velopment of various character traits indicative of tant (or equally unimportant) for males and females
self-reliance (for example, being responsible, indepen- (Arnett, 1998).
dent, or self-controlled) (Kenyon, Rankin, Koerner, & Given the absence of clear criteria that define adult
Dennison, 2007). Parents of young people similarly status in contemporary industrialized societies, it is not
emphasize psychosocial maturity as the defining fea- surprising that, among people of the same age, some
ture of reaching adulthood (E. Nelson et al., 2007). may feel older than their peers, while others may feel
Consistent with this, in one recent study of individu- younger. How old an adolescent feels affects his or her
als ages 17–29, the best predictor of subjective age—that behavior, with adolescents who feel older spending more
is, what age they felt they were, regardless of how old they time with other-sex peers, feeling more autonomous,
actually were—was their level of psychosocial maturity spending more time with antisocial peers, and engaging
(Galambos, Turner, & Tilton-Weaver, 2005). Among in more problem behavior (Galambos, Kolaric, Sears, &
contemporary American youths, for instance, “accepting Maggs, 1999). Psychologist Nancy Galambos has been
responsibility for one’s self” is the most frequently men- interested in changes in individuals’ subjective age and,
tioned criterion for being an adult; among Inuit adoles- more specifically, in when individuals make the transi-
cents in the Canadian Arctic, the most important is the tion from feeling older than they really are (as most
establishment of a marriage-like relationship by moving teenagers do), to feeling younger than they really are (as
into a separate household with a prospective mate. Of most adults do) (Galambos et al., 2005). In her research
the role-related transitions viewed as important among on Canadian youth, Galambos found that this shift takes
contemporary youth, being able to support oneself fi- place around age 25, among both males and females.
nancially was the most important defining criterion of
adulthood (Arnett, 1998). Perhaps for this reason, less ❚ The Clarity of Social Redefinition in Tradi-
than one-third of college undergraduates see themselves tional Cultures Unlike the case in contemporary so-
unambiguously as adults. Interestingly, indicators of en- ciety, social redefinition during adolescence is very clear
tering into adultlike roles of romantic partner and in most traditional cultures. Typically, the passage from
worker at age 20—basically, successfully holding down a childhood into adolescence is marked by a formal initia-
job and being involved in an emotionally close romantic tion ceremony, which publicly proclaims the young per-
relationship—are not especially good predictors of later son’s assumption of a new position in the community
adult competence, according to a longitudinal study that (Markstrom, 2011b). For boys, such ceremonies may take
tracked individuals from age 20 to age 30 (Roisman, place at puberty, at a designated chronological age, or at a
Masten, Coatsworth, & Tellegen, 2004). Far better pre- time when the community decides that the individual is
dictors of later adult success were more “adolescent” in- ready for the status change. For girls, initiation is more
dicators of competence—doing well in school, having an often linked to puberty and, in particular, to the onset of
active social life, and staying out of trouble. menstruation. In both cases, the initiation ceremony
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 102 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

102 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

serves to ritualize the passing of the young person out of associated health benefits and carries many risks, in-
childhood and, if not directly into adulthood, into a pe- cluding infection and chronic pain during urination,
riod of training for it. menstruation, and sexual intercourse. After circumci-
In many initiation ceremonies, the adolescent’s sion, it is virtually impossible for a woman to achieve
physical appearance is changed, so that other members an orgasm during sex (Althaus, 1997). Many interna-
of the community can distinguish between initiated tional groups, citing female circumcision as a human
and uninitiated young people. For example, new types rights violation, have called for a worldwide prohibi-
of clothing may be worn following initiation, or some tion against the practice.
sort of surgical operation or scarification may be per-
formed to create a permanent means of marking the ❚ The Clarity of Social Redefinition in Previous
individual’s adult status. Unlike the case in contempo- Eras What is the transition to adulthood like today?
rary society, where we often can’t tell who is a juvenile Well, compared to what? We often use the baby boom
and who is an adult by physical appearance alone (and generation—individuals who were adolescents in the
where adults are often upset by images of preadoles- late 1950s and 1960s—as an implicit point of com-
cents that are too adultlike), in most traditional societ- parison when characterizing today’s young people,
ies, there is no mistaking which individuals are adults perhaps because the baby boom generation has pro-
and which are still children. In most modern industri- vided the basis for so many of the images of modern
alized societies, we have grown accustomed to seeing family life that are deeply embedded in our cultural
teenagers who try to dress like adults, and adults who psyche. (How often have you heard someone use the
try to dress like teenagers, but such a state of affairs television show Leave It to Beaver, which ran from
would be highly uncommon in traditional cultures. 1957 to 1963, as a comparison point in discussions of
family life?) But the baby boomers’ transition to adult-
❚ The Circumcision Controversy One practice hood was highly unusual in many respects. Let’s com-
involving the physical transformation of the adoles- pare life today with life in 1960, during the middle of
cent that has generated a great deal of controversy is Leave It to Beaver’s run on television, as an example:
circumcision. Circumcision is a procedure in which
In 1960, the average age of marriage was 20 for
some part of the genitals is cut and permanently
women and 22 for men; today, it is 27 and 29,
altered. There are important differences between male
respectively (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012).
and female circumcision. In the United States, male
circumcision, in which the foreskin around the penis In 1960, fewer than 15% of young adults between
is removed during infancy, is very common and is per- the ages of 25 and 34 lived with their parents; in
formed both for religious reasons (mainly among 2010, more than 20% were (Parker, 2012). Within
Jews) and for health reasons, because male circumci- this age group, living with one’s parents is more
sion is associated with decreased risk of urinary tract common among younger, less educated, and
infections and sexually transmitted diseases, includ- non-White individuals (Hallquist, Cuthbertson,
ing HIV infection. There is no evidence that men are Killeya-Jones, Halpern, & Harris, 2011).
harmed emotionally by being circumcised, and com- In 1960, a very high proportion of adolescents went
plications from the procedure are minimal and far directly from high school into full-time employ-
fewer than the health risks associated with not being ment or the military, and only one-third of Ameri-
circumcised (Tobian et al., 2009). can high school graduates went directly to college;
Female circumcision, or female genital mutilation, today, about 70% of high school graduates go
which involves the cutting or removal of the clitoris directly to college (National Center for Education
and, often, the labia, is rarely practiced outside of North Statistics, 2011b).
Africa (where, in some countries, such as Mali, Soma- In other words, in 1960, three key elements of the tran-
lia, and Egypt, virtually all women have been circum- sition to adulthood—getting married, moving out of
cised, usually during childhood or preadolescence). the parents’ home, and completing one’s education—
Unlike male circumcision, female circumcision has no all occurred relatively early compared to today, and all
took place within a fairly constricted time frame. By
that standard, today’s transition to adulthood looks ex-
circumcision A procedure in which some part of the genitals is cessively long and vaguely defined. Indeed, one recent
cut and permanently altered. study of patterns of schooling, work, romance, and
female genital mutilation The cutting or removal of the clitoris, residence during emerging adulthood found that indi-
performed in some cultures as part of the initiation of female viduals frequently move back and forth between peri-
adolescents ods of independence and dependence. This suggests
that the progression from adolescence to adulthood
baby boom The period following World War II, during which the
number of infants born was extremely large.
today not only is long but also occurs in fits and starts
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 103 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 103

(P. K. Cohen, Chen, Hartmark, & Gordon, 2003). It’s people then shared a great deal in common with today’s
little wonder that 60% of people in their mid-20s today “emerging adults.”
don’t know whether they are adolescents or adults.
Compared to the situation 50 or 60 years ago, then, VARIATIONS IN CONTINUITY
today’s transition to adulthood is long and rocky. But
the transition into adulthood was just as disorderly and The well-known anthropologist Ruth Benedict
prolonged during the 19th century as it is today. Ac- (1934), after surveying many different societies, noted
cording to historian Joseph Kett (1977), many young that the process of social redefinition also varies
people at that time moved back and forth between across cultural and historical contexts along the di-
school, where they were viewed as children, and work, mension of continuity—the extent to which the ado-
where they were viewed as adults. Moreover, timetables lescent’s transition into adulthood is gradual or
for the assumption of adult roles varied considerably abrupt. Gradual transitions, in which the adolescent
from one individual to the next, because they were assumes the roles and status of adulthood bit by bit,
highly dependent on family and household needs rather are referred to as continuous transitions.
than on generally accepted age patterns of school, fam- Transitions that are not so smooth, in which the
ily, and work transitions. An adolescent might have young person’s entrance into adulthood is more sud-
been working and living away from home, but if his den, are referred to as discontinuous transitions. For
family needed him—because, let’s say, someone became example, children who grow up working on the fam-
ill—he would leave his job and move back in with his ily farm and continue this work as adults have a con-
parents. During the middle of the 19th century, in fact, tinuous transition into adult work roles. In contrast,
many young people were neither enrolled in school nor children who do not have any work experience while
working, occupying a halfway stage that was not quite they are growing up and who enter the labor force for
childhood but not quite adulthood (M. Katz, 1975). the first time when they graduate from college have a
Even as recently as the beginning of the 20th cen- discontinuous transition into adult work roles.
tury, the transition to adulthood was very drawn out. ❚ The Continuity of the Adolescent Passage in
Age at first marriage was just about the same among Contemporary Society In contemporary society,
males at the turn of the 20th century (26) as it is today we tend to exclude young people from the world
(29); age at first marriage among females actually is of adults; we give them little direct training for adult
much older today (27) than it was a century ago (22) life and then thrust them abruptly into total adult
(U.S. Census Bureau, 2012). And the proportion of 25- independence. Transitions into adulthood in contem-
to 34-year-olds living at home was much higher early in porary industrialized society are therefore more dis-
the 20th century than it was during the 1950s. Even as continuous than in other cultural or historical contexts.
recently as 1940, nearly 30% of people this age lived Consider, for example, three of the most important
with their parents or grandparents (Parker, 2012) (see roles of adulthood that individuals are expected to
Figure 3.4). At least in industrialized societies, the brief carry out successfully—worker, parent, and citizen.
and clear transition into adulthood experienced by Adolescents in contemporary society receive little prior
many baby boomers in the mid–20th century was the preparation for any of these positions.
exception, not the rule. It’s important not to lose sight For instance, young people are segregated from the
of that. Although the notion of “emerging adulthood” workplace throughout most of their childhood and
may ring true today, it is by no means a new phenome- early adolescent years, and they receive little direct
non. The label didn’t exist 150 years ago, but young training in school relevant to the work roles they will
likely find themselves in as adults. As you will read in
Chapter 7, the sorts of jobs available to teenagers today,
such as working the counter of a fast-food restaurant,
a multi-generational household
% of adults ages 25–34 living in

30 bear little resemblance to the jobs most of them will


27.7 hold as adults. The transition into adult work roles,
21.6 therefore, is fairly discontinuous for most young people
20
in industrialized society, and, according to many em-
ployers, a high proportion of young people leave school
10 without adequate preparation for the workplace.
11.0

0
1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
continuous transitions Passages into adulthood in which adult
FIGURE 3.4 In general, a higher proportion of American roles and statuses are entered into gradually.
young adults live with their parents today than has been
discontinuous transitions Passages into adulthood in which adult
the case in recent history. (Parker, 2012)
roles and statuses are entered into abruptly.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 104 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

104 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

The transition of young people into adult family and preparation of food. Their entrance into adult
roles is even more abrupt than is their transition into work roles is gradual and continuous, with work tasks
work roles. Before actually becoming parents, most being graded to their skills and intelligence. They are
young people have little training in child rearing or charged with the socialization of their infant brothers
other related matters. Families are relatively small to- and sisters, particularly during middle childhood,
day, and youngsters are likely to be close in age to their when they are not yet strong enough to make a sub-
siblings; as a result, few opportunities exist for partici- stantial contribution to the community’s fishing and
pating in child care activities at home. Schools generally farming activities. Gradually, they are taught the fun-
offer little, if any, instruction in family relationships damentals of weaving, boating, fishing, building, and
and domestic activities. And with childbirth generally farming. By the time they reach late adolescence,
taking place in hospitals rather than at home, few young Samoan youngsters are well trained in the tasks they
people today have the opportunity of observing a will need to perform as adults.
younger sibling’s birth. Such continuity is generally the case in societies in
Passage into adult citizenship and decision-making which hunting, fishing, and farming are the chief work
roles is also highly discontinuous in contemporary activities. As Mead observed, the emphasis in these so-
Western society. Adolescents are permitted few oppor- cieties is on informal education in context rather than
tunities for independence and autonomy in school and on formal education in schools. Children are typically
are segregated from most of society’s political institu- not isolated in separate educational institutions, and
tions until they complete their formal education. Young they accompany the adult members of their commu-
people are permitted to vote once they turn 18, but they nity in daily activities. Adolescents’ preparation for
receive little preparation for participation in govern- adulthood, therefore, comes largely from observation
ment and community roles prior to this time. In the and hands-on experience in the same tasks that they
United States, for example, we require foreigners who will carry out as adults. Typically, boys learn the tasks
wish to become citizens to understand the workings of performed by adult men, and girls learn those per-
the American government, but no such familiarity is formed by adult women. When work activities take
required of individuals in order to graduate from high adults out of the community, it is not uncommon for
school. children to accompany their parents on these expedi-
Instead of being gradually socialized into work, tions (N. Miller, 1928).
family, and citizenship roles, adolescents in modern As several writers have pointed out, moderniza-
society typically are segregated from activities in these tion and globalization have made the transition from
arenas during most of their childhood and youth. Yet adolescence to adulthood longer and increasingly
young people are expected to perform these roles ca- more discontinuous all over the world (Larson et al.,
pably on reaching the age of majority. With little ex- 2009; Tomasik & Silbereisen, 2011). As successful
perience in meaningful work, adolescents are expected participation in the workforce increasingly has come
to find, get, and keep a job immediately after complet- to require formal education, parents have become
ing their schooling. With essentially no training for less able to provide their children with advice on how
marriage or parenting, they are expected to form their best to prepare for adulthood. Increasingly, school,
own families, manage their own households, and raise rather than hands-on experience in the workplace, is
their own children soon after they reach adulthood. how individuals all over the world are expected to
And without any previous involvement in community prepare for adult work (National Research Council,
activities, they are expected on reaching the age of 2005). How these changes are affecting the psycho-
majority to vote, file their taxes, and behave as respon- logical development of young people in developing
sible citizens. countries is a question that researchers are only now
beginning to examine. It is very likely, however, that
❚ The Continuity of the Adolescent Passage in many of the familiar psychological struggles that
Traditional Cultures The high level of discontinu- have up until now been characteristic of adolescence
ity found in contemporary America is not characteris- in modern, industrialized societies—developing a
tic of adolescence in traditional societies. Consider the sense of identity, choosing among occupational al-
socialization of young people in Samoa, described in ternatives, stressing out over getting into a good col-
detail by the late anthropologist Margaret Mead in her lege, and renegotiating relationships with one’s
classic book Coming of Age in Samoa (1928/1978). parents, to name just three—are becoming more
From early childhood on, Samoan youngsters are in- common among young people all over the world.
volved in work tasks that have a meaningful connec- Ironically, then, at the same time that cross-cultural
tion to the work they will perform as adults. They research on adolescents has expanded dramatically,
participate in the care of younger children, in the the nature of adolescence has become more and
planting and harvesting of crops, and in the gathering more similar around the world. While reviewing
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 105 21/03/13 9:37 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 105

recent research in order to update this book, I was


struck by how many authors who studied multiple
cultural or ethnic groups noted the similarity of re-
sults across their samples of adolescents.

MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION


Globalization is changing the face of adolescence all
over the world, but many experts believe that it has
been a mixed blessing for many de-
veloping countries. With respect to
young people, what are some of
the pros and cons of the increasing
integration of various countries and
cultures into a more global society?

❚ The Continuity of the Adolescent Passage in


Previous Eras During earlier periods in American
history, the transition into adult roles and responsi-
bilities began at a younger age and proceeded along a
more continuous path than generally is the case today.
This is especially true with regard to work. During the
18th century and the early 19th century, when many
families were engaged in farming, many adolescents
were expected to work on the family farm and learn
the skills necessary to carry on the enterprise. Boys
often accompanied their fathers on business trips, Changes in the economy have led many young adults to move back
in with their parents.
learning the nuances of salesmanship and commerce
(Kett, 1977)—a pattern reminiscent of that found in
many traditional societies. many young people spanned the decade from about
Many other young people left home relatively ages 12 to 22, and often beyond—may have increased
early—some as early as age 12—to work for nonfamil- the degree of continuity of the passage into adulthood
ial adults in the community or in nearby villages by providing a time during which young people could
(M. Katz, 1975; Kett, 1977). Even as recently as the mid- assume certain adult responsibilities gradually (M. Katz,
19th century, young adolescents commonly worked as 1975). The semi-independence characteristic of adoles-
apprentices, learning skills and trades in preparation cence in the 19th century had largely disappeared by
for the work roles of adulthood; others left home 1900, however (Modell & Goodman, 1990). Despite the
temporarily to work as servants or to learn domestic complaints of many social commentators (and many
skills. The average 19th-century youngster in Europe college graduates) about the increasing prevalence of
or America left school well before the age of 15 unpaid internships as a bridge between college and full-
(Chisholm & Hurrelmann, 1995; Modell, Furstenberg, time paid employment (e.g., Kamenetz, 2006), this
& Hershberg, 1976). transitional pathway into the world of adult work to-
Census data and historical documents—such as let- day is actually pretty similar to what existed in the
ters, diaries, and community histories—indicate that, 19th century.
although adolescents of 100 years ago took on full-time Socialization for family and citizenship roles may
employment earlier in life than they typically do today, also have been more continuous in previous eras. Liv-
they were likely to live under adult supervision for a ing at home during the late-adolescent and early-adult
longer period than is usual in contemporary society. years, particularly in the larger families characteristic
That is, although the transition into work roles may of households 100 years ago, contributed to the prepa-
have occurred at a younger age in the 19th century than ration of young people for future family life. It was
in the 20th, this transition was made in the context of common for the children in a family to span a wide age
semi-independence rather than complete emancipa- range, and remaining at home undoubtedly placed the
tion (M. Katz, 1975; Kett, 1977; Modell & Goodman, older adolescent from time to time in child-rearing
1990). This semi-independent period—which for roles. As opposed to today’s adolescents, who typically
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 106 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

106 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

have little experience with infants, adolescents 100 parents (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012). Most experts
years ago were more likely to have fed, dressed, and attribute this trend to the increased costs of housing
cared for their younger siblings. They were also ex- and transportation, which make it difficult for indi-
pected to assist their parents in maintaining the house- viduals to move out of their parents’ home (or give up
hold (Modell et al., 1976), which no doubt benefited their parents’ financial support) and establish a sepa-
young people when they eventually established a home rate residence and the increase in the proportion of
separate from their parents. This pattern also likely high school graduates who attend college (and rely on
strengthened sex-role stereotypes, though. Studies their parents’ support for this). A similar trend is evi-
show that leaving their parents’ home earlier and liv- dent in Europe, although the proportion of young
ing independently before marriage encourages young adults who live with their parents varies considerably
women to develop less traditional attitudes, values, from country to country (Eurostat, 2010) (see Fig-
and plans than those who live with their parents as ure 3.5). One potentially positive consequence of the
young adults (Waite, Goldscheider, & Witsberger, increase in young adults living at home is that the
1986). usual rise in alcohol and drug use seen when adoles-
cents go off to college (Fromme, Corbin, & Kruse,
❚ Current Trends in Home Leaving Recent re- 2008) is not nearly as great when individuals continue
ports of trends in home leaving suggest that this as- to live with their parents (White, Fleming, Kim, Catalano,
pect of the transition into adulthood may be changing & McMorris, 2008) (see Figure 3.6). The presence of
in many industrialized countries. On average, indi- parents tends to put a damper on partying.
viduals are living with their parents longer today than How living with their parents in late adolescence
in recent years. More than 55% of all Americans ages affects psychological development and mental health
18 to 24 (about 60% of males, and about 50% of likely depends on the extent to which this experience
females) either live with or are supported by their is seen as normative within the cultural group. But

100
90
80
70
Aged 18–24

60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Bulgaria Denmark Germany Greece France Italy Hungary Romania Sweden United
Kingdom

100
90
80
70
Aged 25–34

60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Bulgaria Denmark Germany Greece France Italy Hungary Romania Sweden United
Kingdom
Women Men
FIGURE 3.5 The proportion of young adults who live with their parents varies considerably across Europe. (Eurostat, 2010)
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 107 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 107

1 Taylor, Cannon, Marino et al., 2010). It will be interest-


Frequency of binge drinking per month ing to see how the dramatic economic downturn that
College, not living
0.9 at home took place in 2009 altered the nature of the transition
College, living into adulthood and, if so, whether this affected adoles-
0.8
at home cents’ psychological development. It is not yet clear
Non-college, not whether this crisis forced young people to grow up
0.7
living at home
faster (because their families needed their help) or,
0.6 Non-college, living
at home alternatively, actually slowed their development
(because economic conditions limited opportunities
0.5
to enter the labor force and establish independent
0.4 residences).

0.3
Spring of Two years later MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
12th grade
Was your experience as an adolescent or
FIGURE 3.6 The frequency of binge drinking more than young adult affected in important ways by
doubles when high school seniors go off to college and the Great Recession of this century’s first
move out of their parents’ home. In contrast, there is very
decade? If so, how?
little change in binge drinking after high school graduation
among non-college youth or among college students who
continue to live with their parents. (White et al., 2008)
RECAP
even in the United States, where a premium is placed
• The process of social definition varies from society
to society in terms of its clarity.
on becoming independent from one’s parents, about
half of all young adults living at home report that it
• In most traditional societies, adolescence is marked
by a formal initiation ceremony and specific rites of
has not affected their relationships with their parents
passage. In contemporary industrialized society,
one way or the other, and one-quarter actually say that
however, the transition from childhood into adult-
their relationship has improved (Parker, 2012). Among
hood is vague, inconsistent across different aspects
Asian and Hispanic young adults in particular, who
of life, and poorly defined.
are more likely to have been raised in a culture that
places special importance on family obligations, living
• Contemporary adolescents are more likely to de-
fine adulthood with respect to psychological
with one’s parents in late adolescence and early adult-
achievements than to role transitions.
hood may be characterized by especially positive
feelings and close family relationships (Fuligni &
• Societies vary in the extent to which the passage
into adulthood is continuous or discontinuous. In a
Pedersen, 2002).
continuous passage, the adolescent assumes the
The current impact of the economy on adolescents’
roles and status of adulthood bit by bit, with a
home leaving reaffirms the importance of looking at
good deal of preparation and training along the
the broader context in defining what “normal” adoles-
way. In a discontinuous passage, the ad-
cence is. In 1960, because it was the exception for ado-
olescent is thrust into adult-
lescents to live at home past high school, we tended to
hood abruptly, with
view individuals who did so as being less independent
little prior
or less mature than their peers. But now that living at
preparation.
home has become the norm, we no longer view it as a
sign of immaturity. Above all, we need to keep in mind
that, because adolescence is in part defined by society,
its nature changes along with society.
Historic events, such as the Great Recession in the
first decade of this century, the 2003 war in Iraq, and
The Transition Into
Hurricane Katrina, may temporarily alter the nature of Adulthood in Contemporary
the adolescent passage. For instance, studies show that
adolescents who were exposed to Hurricane Katrina
Society
showed symptoms of exposure to stress that persisted We do not know for certain whether today’s prolonged
significantly longer than is usually the case (McLaugh- and discontinuous passage into adulthood impedes or
lin et al., 2010; Robertson, Stein, & Schaefer-Rohleder, enhances adolescents’ psychosocial development. Much
2010; Rowe, La Greca, & Alexandersson, 2010; Weems, probably depends on whether the adolescent has access
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 108 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

108 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

to the resources necessary for such a protracted transi- One extremely important international trend con-
tion, especially when jobs are scarce and the cost of hous- cerns different birthrates in different parts of the world:
ing is high. Indeed, many commentators have noted that Because the birthrate in poor and developing countries
there is not one transition into adulthood in contempo- is so much higher than it is in wealthy nations, the distri-
rary America, but three very different transitions: one for bution of the world’s adolescents is changing dramati-
the “haves,” one for the “have-nots,” and one for those cally. As we move further into the 21st century, relatively
who are somewhere in between (Furstenberg, 2006). fewer and fewer of the world’s teenagers will come from
Growing income inequality, not only in the United affluent parts of the world, and relatively more and more
States, but around the world, is furthering this trend. will live in impoverished countries (Larson et al., 2009).
Some observers of adolescence in America have sug-
gested that the discontinuity in the passage into adult-
SPECIAL TRANSITIONAL PROBLEMS
hood has become so great that many youngsters,
OF POOR AND MINORITY YOUTH
especially those not bound for college, are having tre-
mendous problems negotiating the passage into adult No discussion of the transitional problems of young
roles (Osgood, Anderson, & Shafer, 2005). These ob- people in America today would be complete without
servers point to problems that the most vulnerable noting that youngsters from some minority groups—
young people in society face in developing a coherent Black, Hispanic, and American Indian youth, in
sense of identity, establishing a healthy sense of particular—have more trouble negotiating the transi-
autonomy, and making informed decisions about com- tion into adulthood than do their White and Asian coun-
mitments to family and work. They note that the lack of terparts. This is due to many factors, including poverty,
clarity and continuity in the transition into adulthood discrimination, segregation, and disproportionate in-
may contribute to some of the problems faced by ado- volvement with the justice system (Neblett, Gaskin, Lee,
lescents in contemporary society and may also contrib- & Carter, 2011; McLoyd et al., 2009; Osgood et al., 2005).
ute to some of the problems faced by contemporary Youngsters from minority backgrounds make up a
society in dealing with young people. substantial and growing portion of the adolescent pop-
As we look to the future, we can point to two specific ulation in America. At the beginning of this century,
societal trends that are reshaping the nature of the tran- about two-thirds of American adolescents were White.
sition from adolescence to adulthood (Mortimer & Today, about 22% of American adolescents are His-
Larson, 2002). First, as I have noted throughout this panic, 14% Black, 6% Asian or Pacific Islanders, 3% are
chapter, the length of the transitional period is increas- biethnic youth, and 1% American Indian and Native
ing. As the labor force continues to shift toward jobs that Alaskan youth. In other words, about 45% of American
demand more and more formal education, the amount adolescents are from ethnic minority groups. By the
of time individuals need to spend as economically de- end of this century, the U.S. Census Bureau estimates
pendent students will increase, which will delay their that nearly two-thirds of American adolescents will be
assumption of all sorts of adult roles, including family non-White and that Hispanic adolescents will be the
roles. (One of the reasons that individuals are marrying largest ethnic group in the country (see Figure 3.7).
at a later age today is that it takes longer to accumulate Many American adolescents were not born in the
enough wealth to establish a separate residence or start a United States, of course. One curiosity within studies of
family.) Today, the transition between childhood and ethnic minority youth and the transition to adulthood
adulthood takes longer than it did in the past century. concerns the better-than-expected mental health and
Tomorrow, it will probably take even longer. school performance of immigrant adolescents in the
Second, as success in the labor force comes to be United States. For reasons not entirely understood, for-
more and more dependent on formal education, the eign-born adolescent immigrants generally have better
division between the “haves”—those who have access to mental health, exhibit less problem behavior, and per-
money, schools, and information technology—and form better in school than do adolescents from the same
“have-nots”—those who are poor, less well educated, ethnic group who are native-born Americans (Fuligni,
and cut off from important resources, will grow. This Hughes, & Way, 2009; Lansford, 2011). Indeed, one of
division will be seen not only between wealthy and the most interesting findings to emerge from research
poor countries but, increasingly, between the affluent on immigrant adolescents is that their “Americaniza-
and the poor within countries, because globalization, tion” appears to be associated with worse, not better,
for all its potentially positive features, contributes to outcomes. For example, the longer a foreign-born ado-
income inequality (Tomasik & Silbereisen, 2011). As lescent has lived in the United States, the more likely he
the economies of developing countries improve, the or she is to smoke cigarettes or binge drink (Gfroerer &
importance of formal education increases, which then Tan, 2003). Similarly, a study of Latino adolescents in
further separates the life conditions of the educated and Los Angeles found that those who spoke English exclu-
the uneducated, a pattern seen vividly today in China. sively (an index of Americanization) were more likely to
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 109 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 109

2000 2010 2015 2020 2100

White Black Hispanic Asian Other


FIGURE 3.7 The ethnic composition of the United States will continue to change dramatically over the 21st century. (Federal
Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2005)

drink alcohol, smoke cigarettes, and engage in sexual pregnancy, all of which contribute to transitional dif-
intercourse than were those who spoke a mixture of ficulties (Guldi, Page, & Stevens, 2007; McLoyd et al.,
English and Spanish, who, in turn, were more likely to 2009; Schoon et al., 2002). Because minority young-
engage in these behaviors than those who spoke Spanish sters are more likely than other teenagers to grow up
exclusively (Ebin et al., 2001) (see Figure 3.8). in poverty, they are also more likely to encounter tran-
One possible reason for this is that when adoles- sitional problems during middle and late adolescence.
cents from immigrant families become more Ameri- Experiencing poverty during adolescence has an espe-
canized, family conflict and increased distance cially negative effect on adolescents’ school achievement
between adolescent and parents may result, leading, in (McLoyd et al., 2009). As you will see in later chapters,
turn, to problem behavior (McQueen, Getz, & Bray, school dropout rates are much higher among Hispanic
2003). For example, highly Americanized Asian ado- and American Indian teenagers than among other groups,
lescents who have strong attachments to their parents and college enrollment is lower among Black, Hispanic,
are not at heightened risk for problems (Hahm, Lahiff, and American Indian youth. In addition, unemployment
& Guterman, 2003). is much higher among Black, Hispanic, and American
Indian teenagers; Black and Hispanic youth are more
likely to be victimized by crime and exposed to violence;
THE EFFECTS OF POVERTY ON THE
TRANSITION INTO ADULTHOOD
and rates of out-of-wedlock births are higher among
Black and Hispanic teenagers than among White teenag-
Of all the factors that may impair youngsters’ ability to ers. All these factors disrupt the transition into adulthood
move easily between adolescence and adulthood, pov- by limiting individuals’ economic and occupational suc-
erty is at the top of the list. Poverty is associated with cess. Poverty impedes the transition to adulthood among
failure in school, unemployment, and out-of-wedlock all teenagers, regardless of race, of course; but because
minority youth are more likely to grow up poor, they are
also more likely to have transition problems.
60

55
WHAT CAN BE DONE TO EASE
Alcohol THE TRANSITION?
Cigarettes
Percent reporting activity

50 Sexual intercourse A variety of suggestions have been offered for making


45 the transition into adulthood smoother for all young
people, especially those who are not college-bound, in-
40 cluding restructuring secondary education, expanding
work and volunteer opportunities, and improving the
35
quality of community life for adolescents and their par-
30 ents. Some groups have called for expanded opportuni-
ties in the workplace as a way of making the high school
25 years more of a “bridge” between adolescence and
adulthood (Kazis, 1993). Others have suggested that
20
English English/Spanish Spanish only adolescents be encouraged to spend time in voluntary,
FIGURE 3.8 Latino adolescents who are relatively more nonmilitary service activities—such as staffing day care
acculturated, as indexed by their primary language, are centers, working with the elderly, or cleaning up the
more likely to engage in problem behavior than those who environment—for a few years after high school gradu-
are less acculturated. (Ebin et al., 2001) ation so that they can learn responsibility and adult
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 110 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

110 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

roles (McLellan & Youniss, 2003; Sherrod & Lauckhardt, 2005). One potentially important influence, to which
2009). Still others have pointed out that adolescents we now turn, is the neighborhood in which the adoles-
cannot come of age successfully without the help of cent lives.
adults and that programs are needed to strengthen
families and communities and to bring adolescents into
contact with adult mentors (Sánchez, Esparza, & Colón, RECAP
2008). Overall, most experts agree that a comprehen- • There are three very different “transitions” to adult-
sive approach to the problem is needed and that such hood in contemporary America, one for the haves,
an approach must simultaneously address the educa- one for the have-nots, and one for those who are
tional, employment, interpersonal, and health needs of somewhere in between the extremes.
adolescents from all walks of life (Balsano, Theokas, & • The discontinuous and prolonged nature of the ad-
Bobek, 2009). olescent passage in contemporary society has
caused difficulties for many of today’s youth, espe-
❚ Mentoring In recent years, there has been cially poor minority youth living in pockets of con-
growing interest in mentoring programs for at-risk centrated poverty.
adolescents, many of whom have few relationships • Mentoring programs have a small, but significant
with positive adult role models (DuBois, Holloway, positive effect on the development of at-risk ado-
Valentine, & Cooper, 2002; Rhodes, 2004). Adolescents lescents, but mentoring alone is not sufficient to
who lack positive adult role models are more likely to help poor adolescents make a successful transition
have psychological and behavioral problems (Bryant to adulthood.
& Zimmerman, 2003; K. F. Parker & Reckdenwald, • Most experts agree that a comprehensive approach
2008; M. Zimmerman, Bingenheimer, & Notaro, to the problem is needed and that such an ap-
2002). proach must simultaneously address adoles-
Mentoring programs seek to pair adults with young cents’ educational, employment,
people through community- or school-based efforts interpersonal, and
designed to facilitate positive youth development, im- health needs.
prove academic achievement, and deter antisocial be-
havior. Evaluations of mentoring programs indicate
that they have a small, but significant positive effect on
youth development. On average, adolescents who have
been mentored are less likely to have problems in
school and at home, less likely to use drugs and alco-
hol, and less likely to get into trouble with the law
The Influence of
(Rhodes & Lowe, 2009). Not surprisingly, the impact Neighborhood Conditions
of mentoring varies as a function of characteristics of
the mentor, the young person, and their relationship
on Adolescent Development
(DuBois, Holloway, et al., 2002; Rhodes & Lowe, 2009). One factor contributing to the especially worrisome
Mentoring appears to have the most beneficial effects situation of poor and minority youth in the United
on adolescents whose other relationships were good, States is that poverty has become much more concen-
but not great (perhaps because the ones with great trated over the past 40 years, with greater and greater
other relationships didn’t need the mentoring as much clustering of poor families into economically and ra-
and because the ones with poor ones did not have the cially segregated communities. In response to this, a
social skills to profit from the mentoring) (Schwartz, number of researchers have turned their attention to
Rhodes, Chan, & Herrera, 2011). In general, mentoring the study of the ways in which neighborhoods influ-
tends to be more successful when the mentor main- ence adolescent development (T. Leventhal, Dupéré, &
tains a steady presence in the youth’s life over an ex- Brooks-Gunn, 2009). Although other characteristics of
tended period (at least two years), has frequent contact neighborhoods in addition to poverty potentially can
with the youngster, and involves the adolescent in a affect adolescents’ development (for example, the eth-
wide range of recreational, social, and practical activi- nic composition, crime rate, or availability of social ser-
ties (Langhout, Rhodes, & Osborne, 2004; Roth, vice programs), far more is known about the effects of
Brooks-Gunn, Murray, & Foster, 1998). It is important neighborhood poverty than about any other neighbor-
to note, however, that although mentoring may benefit hood factor. Exposure to neighborhood poverty is an
adolescents, other influences in their lives are also im- especially prevalent problem among non-White ado-
portant, and mentoring alone is not sufficient to meet lescents (Chauhan, Reppucci, & Turkheimer, 2009;
the needs of at-risk youth (DuBois & Silverthorn, Gudiño, Nadeem, Kataoka, & Lau, 2011).
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 111 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 111

Studying neighborhood influences on adolescent


development is tricky business. We know that growing
up in a very poor household increases adolescents’ risks
for all sorts of problems. But because poor families tend
to live in poor neighborhoods, it is not always easy to
separate the effects of neighborhood disadvantage from
the effects of family disadvantage. To do this, research-
ers have had to compare adolescents whose family situ-
ations are similar, but who live in very different types of
neighborhoods. This is not always easy to do—as you
can imagine, few affluent families live in poor neigh-
borhoods, and few poor families live in affluent ones.
There is also the problem of cause and effect. If fam-
ilies in a good neighborhood seem to be functioning
better than families in a poor one, it might simply re-
flect the fact that better-functioning families choose to
live in better neighborhoods (rather than indicate that
the neighborhood actually influenced family function-
A variety of psychological and social problems are more common
ing) (Boyle, Georgiades, Racine, & Mustard, 2007). among adolescents who grow up amidst poverty.
There have been a few experiments in which the re-
searchers took this into account, by randomly assigning
families from poor neighborhoods to either remain
where they were living or be relocated into more ad- further, some of these experiments have found that
vantaged neighborhoods, and then tracking the psy- moving to a more affluent neighborhood tends to have
chological development and behavior of adolescents in a more positive effect on girls than boys (Clampet-
the two groups. These studies have found mixed effects Lundquist, Edin, Kling, & Duncan, 2011). Other studies
of relocation, with some studies showing positive ef- have found similarly puzzling results. For example, one
fects, others showing no effect, and some actually analysis of changes in the level of neighborhood pov-
showing negative effects (J. R. Kling, Ludwig, & Katz, erty found that adolescents in high poverty neighbor-
2005; T. Leventhal, Fauth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2005; hoods that changed for the better over time were more
Ludwig, Duncan, & Hirschfield, 2001; Sharkey & likely to show increases in problems than those whose
Sampson, 2010) (see Figure 3.9). To complicate things neighborhoods did not, but that the opposite pattern
held for adolescents in moderate-poverty neighbor-
hoods, where change for the better led to improvements
in boys’ well-being (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2011).
0.6
Movers How can we account for the finding that relocating
Stayers poor families to more affluent neighborhoods actually
0.5 may have negative effects on adolescents’ behavior?
According to one team of researchers, there are several
Substance-use index

0.4 possible explanations (Fauth, Leventhal, & Brooks-


Gunn, 2007). First, adolescents from poor families that
0.3 moved may have encountered more discrimination in
the new neighborhoods than in their old ones. Second,
0.2 although it is generally true that advantaged communi-
ties have more resources than disadvantaged ones, it is
possible that poor families who moved to more affluent
0.1
neighborhoods actually may have had less access to
community resources than they did in their old neigh-
0 borhoods. Third, adolescents who moved to more ad-
8–11
12–14 15–18
Child age at follow-up (years) vantaged neighborhoods may have ended up feeling
FIGURE 3.9 Contrary to expectation, in an experiment more disadvantaged than their peers who remained in
designed to study neighborhood effects, poor adolescents poor communities because the adolescents who moved
whose families were moved to more affluent neighbor- compared their life circumstances to those of their
hoods increased their use of alcohol and illicit drugs. (Fauth more affluent peers. Finally, there is some evidence
et al., 2007) that parents in poor neighborhoods may monitor their
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 112 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

112 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

Community norms FIGURE 3.10 Neighborhood


and collective conditions influence adolescents’
efficacy development by shaping the
norms to which adolescents are
exposed; by influencing the
quality of the relationships they
Neighborhood Interpersonal Adolescent
have with others, including their
conditions relationships development
parents; and by facilitating or
limiting adolescents’ and families’
access to economic and
Economic and institutional resources.
institutional
resources

children relatively more vigilantly because they worry ❚ Collective Efficacy First, neighborhood con-
about crime and other dangers; as you’ll read in Chap- ditions shape the norms that guide individuals’ values
ter 4, adolescents who are more closely monitored tend and behaviors. Poverty in neighborhoods breeds social
to have fewer problems. isolation and social disorganization, undermining a
Despite these cautions, evidence suggests that grow- neighborhood’s sense of collective efficacy—the ex-
ing up in a poor neighborhood has negative effects on tent to which neighbors trust each other, share com-
adolescent behavior, achievement, and mental health, mon values, and count on each other to monitor the
and that these effects are above and beyond those attrib- activities of youth in the community (Sampson,
utable to growing up in a poor family or attending a Raudenbusch, & Earls, 1997). As a consequence, it is
poor school (Harding, 2003; T. Leventhal et al., 2009; easier for deviant peer groups to form and to influence
McBride, Berkel, Gaylord-Harden, Copeland-Linder, & the behavior of adolescents in these communities
Nation, 2011). Adolescents growing up in impoverished (Maimon & Browning, 2010). Rates of teen pregnancy,
urban communities are more likely than their peers school failure, and antisocial behavior are all higher in
from equally poor households but better neighborhoods neighborhoods that have low levels of collective effi-
to be sexually active at an earlier age, to bear children as cacy (Ainsworth, 2002; Bowen & Bowen, 2002;
teenagers, to become involved in criminal activity, and Herrenkohl et al., 2000; Karriker-Jaffe, Foshee, Ennett,
to achieve less in, or even drop out of, high school— & Suchindran, 2008; Sampson, 1997; Tolan, Gorman-
factors that, as you will read in later chapters, seriously Smith, & Henry, 2003). Living in a neighborhood high
interfere with the successful transition into adulthood in collective efficacy—where adults monitor the be-
(Dupéré, Lacourse, Willms, Leventhal, & Tremblay, havior of all adolescents, not just their own—is espe-
2008; T. Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2004; Wodtke, cially important for adolescents whose parents are
Harding, & Elwert, 2011). Interestingly, it is the absence themselves not very vigilant (Browning, Leventhal, &
of affluent neighbors, rather than the presence of poor Brooks-Gunn, 2005; Kirk, 2009). In addition, it appears
ones, that seems to place adolescents in impoverished that collective efficacy encourages adolescents to form
communities at greatest risk (Duncan, 1994; Ensminger, a deeper emotional bond with their community, which
Lamkin, & Jacobson, 1996; T. Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, in turn makes them feel safer, a finding that has been
2004). Although virtually all neighborhood research has replicated in many countries (Dallago et al., 2009;
focused on urban adolescents, studies find that growing Vieno, Nation, Perkins, Pastore, & Santinello, 2010).
up in poor rural communities also places adolescents at Some writers (e.g., R. Simons, Johnson, Beaman,
risk (Farmer et al., 2003; Reijneveld et al., 2010). Conger, & Whitbeck, 1996) suggest that, under condi-
tions of low collective efficacy, social problems are
PROCESSES OF NEIGHBORHOOD contagious—they spread from one adolescent to
INFLUENCES another in a pattern not unlike a disease epidemic. To
the extent that poverty increases behavior problems, for
How might neighborhood conditions affect the behav- example, adolescents living in poor neighborhoods will
ior and development of adolescents? Three different come into contact with deviant peers more often
mechanisms have been suggested (T. Leventhal et al., (Monahan, Egan, Van Horn, Arthur, & Hawkins, 2011),
2009) (see Figure 3.10). and adolescents who associate with delinquent peers
are more likely to be drawn into criminal and delin-
quent activity (Simons & Burt, 2011). Similarly, adoles-
collective efficacy A community’s social capital, derived from its cents who live in neighborhoods characterized by high
members’ common values and goals.
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 113 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 113

rates of teenage childbearing grow up exposed to large Black youth, has shown in numerous studies that both
numbers of peers who are relatively more tolerant of the financial strain and the neighborhood stress
this behavior, which affects their own attitudes toward associated with poverty undermines the quality of par-
premarital childbearing (Baumer & South, 2001; South enting in a family, which, in turn, leads to adolescent
& Baumer, 2000). Adolescents who see nothing but maladjustment (McLoyd et al., 2009). Indeed, across all
poverty and unemployment in their communities have ethnic groups, poverty is associated with harsh, incon-
little reason to be hopeful about their own future, and sistent, and punitive parenting, and these factors, in
they may feel that they have little to lose by having a turn, are linked to adolescent misbehavior (Bradley,
baby, dropping out of school, or becoming involved in Corwyn, Pipes McAdoo, & Garcia-Coll, 2001; Gutman,
criminal activity (D. Kirby, Coyle, & Gould, 2001; McLoyd, & Tokoyawa, 2005; Ramirez-Valles, Zimmer-
Kingston, Huizinga, & Elliott, 2009; LeBlanc, 2003). man, & Juarez, 2002). When parents are not effective in
One recent evaluation of an anti-poverty program supervising and monitoring their teenagers, for exam-
named New Hope, which provided financial assistance, ple, and when teenagers have little social support from
child care, and health care to working, low-income par- parents or other adults, the teenagers are more likely to
ents, found that the program had positive effects on associate with antisocial peers and get into trouble (see
adolescent boys’ hopes for the future and career prepa- Figure 3.11) (Chung & Steinberg, 2006; J. P. Hoffman,
ration (McLoyd, Kaplan, Purtell, & Huston, 2011). 2003; Lynam et al., 2000; Tolan, Gorman-Smith, &
Neighborhood poverty also has an impact on adoles- Henry, 2003). As a consequence, the link between fam-
cents’ sexual behavior and decisions about whether to ily poverty and delinquency is even stronger among
abort a pregnancy—with those living in poor neigh- poor families living in poor neighborhoods (Hay,
borhoods more likely to be sexually active at an early Fortson, Hollist, Altheimer, & Schaible, 2007).
age and to have the baby—but not on their likelihood Consistent with this, the harmful effects of exposure
of getting pregnant or using contraception (Cubbin, to negligent or harsh parenting are even more pronounced
Santelli, Brindis, & Braveman, 2005; South & Baumer, in disadvantaged neighborhoods (J. M. Beyers, Bates,
2001). The impact of living amidst low collective effi- Pettit, & Dodge, 2003; Brody et al., 2003; Browning,
cacy is especially bad among individuals who are im- Leventhal, & Brooks-Gunn, 2005; H. Cleveland, 2003;
pulsive or insensitive to the feelings of others (Dupéré, Knoester & Haynie, 2005; Roche, Ensminger, & Cherlin,
Lacourse, Willms, Vitaro, & Tremblay, 2007; M. Meier, 2007). Studies of whether the impact of good parenting is
Slutske, Arndt, & Cadoret, 2008). affected by neighborhood conditions have not yielded
consistent results. Some studies show that positive family
❚ The Impact of Stress Second, the stresses asso- relationships are more effective in good neighborhoods
ciated with poverty undermine the quality of people’s (e.g., Cleveland, Feinberg, & Greenberg, 2010; R. Simons,
relationships with each other. Poverty, we know, inter- Simons, Burt, Brody, & Cutrona, 2005), while others
feres with parents’ ability to be effective parents show the reverse (M. J. Cleveland, Gibbons, Gerrard,
(Goosby, 2007). Psychologist Vonnie McLoyd, who has Pomery, & Brody, 2005; Salzinger, Feldman, Rosario, &
done extensive research on the impact of poverty on Ng-Mak, 2011).

Neighborhood
disorder

Concentrated
poverty

Parenting Peer Individual


behavior deviance offending

Residential
instability

Social
cohesion

FIGURE 3.11 Neighborhood disadvantage diminishes parental effectiveness, which leads adolescents to affiliate with devi-
ant peers and get involved in crime and delinquency. (Chung & Steinberg, 2006)
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 114 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

114 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence

The impact of poverty on levels of neighborhood vio- to think about killing themselves, and to report symp-
lence is especially devastating. Adolescents who grow up toms of depression, posttraumatic stress disorder,
in poor neighborhoods are far more likely than other hopelessness, and substance abuse (Allwood & Bell,
youth to be exposed to chronic community violence, and 2008; Bolland, Lian, & Formichella, 2005; McMahon,
repeated exposure to violence and other types of stress Felix, Halpert, & Petropoulos, 2009; Ruchkin, Henrich,
increases the risk of behavioral, emotional, and even Jones, Vermeiren, & Schwab-Stone, 2007; Youngstrom,
physical health problems (Copeland-Linder, Lambert, Weist, & Albus, 2003). One study found that witnessing
Chen, & Ialongo, 2011; Farrell & Sullivan, 2004; Mrug & gun violence doubles an adolescent’s risk for commit-
Windle, 2009; D. O’Donnell, Schwab-Stone, & Ruchkin, ting violence in the future (Bingenheimer, 2005). The
2006; Shahinfar, Kupersmidt, & Matza, 2001; Zimmer- adverse effects of exposure to violence have been docu-
man & Messner, 2010). Perhaps not surprisingly, adoles- mented in hundreds of studies, not only of urban youth
cents who grow up in poor neighborhoods are more in the United States (where most of these studies have
likely to interpret ambiguous situations as threatening been conducted), but in other parts of the world as well,
and to show increases in blood pressure and heart rate as such as Northern Ireland (McAloney, McCrystal, Percy,
a consequence, placing them at risk for heart disease & McCartan, 2009). Generally speaking, being the vic-
(E. Chen, Langer, Raphaelson, & Matthews, 2004). When tim of violence has more consistent harmful effects
tested in laboratory experiments, adolescents who have than witnessing it, which, in turn, is more harmful than
grown up in disordered neighborhoods show a relatively simply hearing about it (Fowler, Tompsett, Bracisze-
greater tendency to take risks (Furr-Holden, Milam, wski, Jacques-Tiura, & Baltes, 2009).
Reynolds, MacPherson, & Lejuez, 2012). Not all adolescents who are exposed to violence are
Exposure to violence is pervasive in poor neighbor- equally affected, of course; among the factors that help
hoods. In one study of a high-risk sample of New York protect against the harmful effects of exposure to vio-
City adolescents, the researchers found that 93% had seen lence are having positive family relationships and, espe-
someone get beaten badly, 75% had seen someone get cially among Black youth, strong religious beliefs
knifed, 92% had seen someone get shot, and 77% had (Aceves & Cookston, 2007; Benhorin & McMahon,
seen someone get killed. In addition, 78% reported that 2008; Brookmeyer, Henrich, & Schwab-Stone, 2005;
they had lost a close friend due to violence, and 85% re- Fowler, Ahmed, Tompsett, Jozefowicz-Simbeni, & Toro,
ported losing someone they knew from the neighbor- 2008; Howard, Budge, & McKay, 2010; Kliewer et al.,
hood to violence (D. Wilkinson & Carr, 2008). Here is one 2006; LeBlanc, Self-Brown, Shepard, & Kelley, 2011;
16-year-old’s account of an experience that has become S. T. Li, Nussbaum, & Richards, 2007; Pearce, Jones,
all too common in many inner-city neighborhoods: Schwab-Stone, & Ruchkin, 2003; Rosario, Salzinger,
Feldman, & Ng-Mak, 2008). Exposure to violence also
Rashard: We was in the projects. He had beef
seems to have a less adverse effect on Asian American
with some kids around the corner,
adolescents than on Black adolescents (Chen, 2010).
another rival, over a drug spot and as
we sat on the gates . . . them kids
from around the corner came from
both ways. . . . you see, it’s hard for MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
me to talk about it [respondent is
Inner-city neighborhoods typically come
crying]. And . . . they came from both
to mind when we think of adolescents
ways and they pulled out guns from
growing up in poverty, but many poor
every angle and they just let off and
adolescents live in rural, not urban, ar-
my friend got shot in the head with
eas. In what ways is poverty different for
a .44, magnum.
urban versus rural youth? How might
Interviewer: And what happened, how did you this affect the nature of adolescence in
escape that situation? each type of community?
Rashard: I was on my side [and I] jumped off
the gate . . . as they were shooting, I
ducked, I ran for dear life, that’s the
only thing I could tell you. It hap- ❚ Limited Access to Resources Third, adoles-
pened so fast, it ain’t like I could tell cents who grow up in poor neighborhoods have access
you no whole war story. (D. Wilkinson to fewer resources than do those who grow up in more
& Carr, 2008, p. 1035) advantaged communities. In poor neighborhoods, for
example, the quality of schools, health care, transporta-
Adolescents who themselves have been exposed to tion, employment opportunities, and recreational ser-
violence are more likely to engage in violent behavior, vices are all lower than they are in affluent neighborhoods
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 115 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 3 Social Transitions 115

(M. Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). As a result, adoles- matters most. The greater the number of positive
cents in poor communities have fewer chances to en- features an adolescent’s environment contains, the
gage in activities that facilitate positive development better off he or she will be. By the same token, the
and fewer chances to receive services when they are hav- more sources of contextual risk an adolescent is exposed
ing difficulties (T. Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2004). to, the greater his or her chances are of developing
Adolescents who live in communities with relatively problems (D. J. Jones et al., 2002; Morrison, Gutman,
greater resources, such as higher-quality schools, are Sameroff, & Eccles, 2002; Prelow, Danoff-Burg, Swenson,
less likely to become involved in antisocial behavior & Pulgiano, 2004; Prelow & Loukas, 2003).
(Kowaleski-Jones, 2000; Molnar, Cerda, Roberts, & In the next set of chapters, we’ll look in detail at the
Buka, 2008). Interestingly, in neighborhoods with immediate contexts in which adolescents spend time—
higher levels of resources and greater feelings of cohe- the family, the peer group, the school, and work and
sion, adults’ beliefs about teenagers tend to be more leisure settings—and at the features of each that help or
favorable, probably because the casual interactions that hinder healthy adolescent development.
take place between adults and adolescents in these set-
tings are more positive (Zeldin & Topitzes, 2002). The
presence of institutional resources, then, often goes RECAP
hand in hand with the presence of positive social rela- • Neighborhoods affect adolescents through three
tionships. interrelated processes: by influencing the sorts of
Most of the effects of neighborhoods on adolescent norms to which adolescents are exposed, by alter-
development are indirect, transmitted through the im- ing the nature of interpersonal relationships inside
pact of the neighborhood on the more immediate set- and outside the family, and by facilitating or limit-
tings in which adolescents spend time (Cantillon, 2006; ing adolescents’ access to economic and institu-
Chung & Steinberg, 2006; D. J. Jones, Forehand, Brody, tional resources.
& Armistead, 2002; Teitler & Weiss, 2000). For example, • To date, most research on neighborhood influences
neighborhood disorder affects the way that parents be- has focused on the impact of growing up in a poor
have, and this, in turn, affects adolescents’ development neighborhood, which has been shown to have ad-
and mental health. Consistent with the ecological per- verse effects on adolescents’ development, above
spective on human development discussed in the Intro- and beyond the adverse effects of growing up in a
duction, then, neighborhoods influence individuals by poor family.
transforming what takes place within the more imme- • Exposure to violence is an especially severe
diate contexts that are embedded in them. problem for adolescents in poor neighborhoods.
Is one setting—the family, the peer group, the • The impact of the neighborhood on adolescent de-
school—a more influential transmitter of neighbor- velopment occurs through the neighborhood’s in-
hood effects than others? A study by Thomas Cook and fluence on the more immediate settings in which
his colleagues shows that no single context alone pro- adolescents spend time.
vides a complete explanation of why some adolescents • The more positive features an adoles-
have an easier time making a successful transition into cent’s context contains, the
adulthood than do others (Cook, Herman, Phillips, & more likely he or she
Settersten, 2002). Each setting contributes modestly and is to succeed.
independently to the course of adolescent development,
and it is the cumulative impact of multiple contexts that
ste32118_ch03_084-116.indd Page 116 21/03/13 9:38 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

116 PART 1 The Fundamental Changes of Adolescence


ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 117 21/03/13 9:43 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

PART 2

4 ❘ Families 5 ❘ Peer Groups 6 ❘ Schools 7 ❘ Work, Leisure, and Media

The Contexts of Adolescence


ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 118 21/03/13 9:46 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 119 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
4
Families
IS CONFLICT BETWEEN PARENTS AND TEENAGERS INEVITABLE?
The Generation Gap: Fact and Fiction
What Do Adolescents and Parents Usually Fight About?
FAMILY RELATIONSHIPS AT ADOLESCENCE
A Time of Reorganization and Change
The Adolescent’s Parents at Midlife
Changes in Family Needs and Functions
Transformations in Family Relations
Sex Differences in Family Relationships
FAMILY RELATIONSHIPS AND ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
Parenting Styles and Their Effects
Ethnic Differences in Parenting Practices
Autonomy and Attachment in the Adolescent’s Family
Adolescents’ Relationships With Siblings
BEHAVIORAL GENETICS AND ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Development
Why Are Siblings Often So Different?
THE ADOLESCENT’S FAMILY IN A CHANGING SOCIETY
The Changed and Changing Nature of Family Life
Adolescents and Divorce
The Specific Impact of Marital Conflict
The Longer-Term Effects of Divorce
Custody, Contact, and Conflict Following Divorce
Remarriage
Economic Stress and Poverty
Special Family Forms
THE IMPORTANCE OF THE FAMILY IN ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT

119
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 120 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

120 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

THE NEXT TIME you are in a bookstore, take a look at as individuals (Steinberg, 2001). In fact, one-fifth of
the books in the section on parent–adolescent rela- American teenagers say that their top concern is that
tionships. Judging from the titles—ones like Why Do they don’t spend enough time with their parents; ironi-
They Act That Way?; Get Out of My Life, But First, Could cally, less than one-tenth of parents say that their top
You Drive Me and Cheryl to the Mall?; How to Survive concern is that they don’t spend enough time with
the Coming Years of Change; Yes, Your Teen Is Crazy; their kids (YMCA, 2000).
and even Yes, Your Parents Are Crazy—you would think To be sure, there are times when adolescents and par-
that stress and strain between teenagers and their par- ents have their problems. But there are times when
ents is commonplace, even normal. In contrast to ad- younger children and their parents have problems, and
vice books on infancy, which emphasize normative when adults and their parents do, too. No systematic
development, books for parents of teenagers tend to studies demonstrate that family problems are any more
focus on problems (Steinberg, 2011). This is unfortunate, likely to occur during adolescence than at other times in
for two reasons. First, as you’ll read, the stereotype pre- the life span. Most research indicates that among those
sented in these writings isn’t true. And second, the more teenagers and parents who report having problems, the
parents believe in the stereotype of adolescents as dif- great majority had problematic relations during child-
ficult, the more they expect their own child to conform hood (Laursen & Collins, 2009), and declines in the quality
to it, and the worse their relationship with their teen- of family relationships are greatest in families where rela-
ager becomes (Jacobs, Chin, & Shaver, 2005). In other tionships were less close to begin with (Laursen, DeLay, &
words, parents’ beliefs that they are going to have a Adams, 2010). Only a very small percentage of families
difficult time with their child once he or she enters ad- who enjoy positive relations during childhood can expect
olescence can become what psychologists call a self- to develop serious problems during adolescence.
fulfilling prophecy—an expectation that is realized In this chapter, we’ll look at the family as a context
because parents act in ways that make it happen. for adolescent development, with three broad ques-
In truth, scientific studies indicate that, on average, tions in mind. First, how do relationships in the family
there is very little emotional distance between young change during adolescence—that is, what is the effect
people and their parents (Laursen & Collins, 2009). Al- of adolescence on the family? Second, how are ado-
though some adolescents and their parents have seri- lescents affected by their experiences in the family—in
ous interpersonal problems, the overwhelming majority other words, what is the effect of the family on ado-
of adolescents feel close to their parents, respect their lescents? And third, how have changes in family life
parents’ judgment, feel that their parents love and care during the past half century affected the adolescent
about them, and have a lot of respect for their parents experience?

MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION Is Conflict Between


Portrayals of adolescent–parent relationships in the
popular press suggest that
Teenagers and
the period is much more Parents Inevitable?
difficult and unpleasant
It is impossible to discuss adolescents’ relationships with
than research indicates.
their parents without talking about parent–adolescent
Why might there be such a
conflict, a topic that not only dominates popular writings
discrepancy between what
on this stage of life but also has been the focus of decades
research finds and what
of research by scholars of adolescence (Laursen & Collins,
parents are led to believe?
2009).

THE GENERATION GAP:


FACT AND FICTION
Most people believe that adolescents and adults hold
self-fulfilling prophecy The idea that individuals’ behavior is different values and attitudes, but this is not the case.
influenced by others’ expectations for them.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 121 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 121

Systematic studies of teenagers and their parents find Why do parents and teenagers argue over such mun-
that they usually have surprisingly similar beliefs about dane matters? According to several studies, a major
such things as the importance of hard work, educa- contributor to adolescent–parent bickering is the fact
tional and occupational ambitions, and the personal that teenagers and their parents define the issues of
characteristics and attributes that they feel are impor- contention very differently—a finding that has been
tant and desirable (Knafo & Schwartz, 2003). Indeed, replicated across many cultural and ethnic groups
when it comes to basic, core values—concerning reli- (Smetana & Villalobos, 2009). Parents view many issues
gion, work, education, and so on—diversity within the as matters of right and wrong—not necessarily in a
adolescent population is much more striking than are moral sense, but as matters of custom or convention.
differences between the generations. Why is this so? Be- Adolescents, in contrast, are likely to define these same
cause adolescents and their parents share a common issues as matters of personal choice (Martin, Bascoe, &
social, regional, and cultural background, and these are Davies, 2011). A mother who disapproves of her
the factors that shape our central beliefs. daughter’s clothing says, “People just don’t dress that
Although there is not much of a gap between the way to go to school.” The daughter responds, “Maybe
generations when it comes to core values, there is of- you wouldn’t dress this way, but I do.”
ten a gap between teenagers and adults in matters of
personal taste, most clearly evident in styles of dress, ❚ Rebels With a Cause Contrary to stereotype,
preferences in music, and patterns of leisure activity though, adolescents rarely rebel against their parents
(Laursen & Collins, 2009). The explanation for a gap just for the sake of rebelling (Darling, Cumsille, &
of this sort is not surprising: Unlike basic values, Martínez, 2007). In fact, they are willing to accept their
which develop gradually over time and are shaped parents’ rules as legitimate when they agree that the is-
from an early age, preferences and tastes for things like sue is a moral one (whether it is permissible to cheat on
clothing, music, and hairstyles are far more transitory a school test) or one involving safety (whether it is per-
and subject to current fads and fashions. Adolescents missible to drink and drive), but they are less inclined
are more likely to be influenced by their friends than to accept their parents’ authority when they view the
by their parents in these matters, and as a conse- issue as personal (what clothes to wear to a party)
quence, disagreements and differences in opinion be- (Jackson, 2002; Smetana & Daddis, 2002). In other
tween old and young often result. Because adolescents words, rather than resisting all of their parents’ at-
spend a great deal of time with their friends (and be- tempts to make and enforce rules (the stereotype that
cause a good deal of that time is spent in social activi- many people have of teenagers), adolescents draw dis-
ties in which taste in clothes, music, and so on is tinctions between parental rules that they think their
especially important), teenagers’ preferences are likely parents have a right to make (for instance, having to let
to be shaped to a large measure by forces outside the their parents know what time they’ll be home after go-
family. ing out) and rules that they think are out of bounds (for
example, having to keep their bedroom orderly), a dis-
tinction that in many ways is quite understandable
WHAT DO ADOLESCENTS AND PARENTS
(Smetana & Villalobos, 2009). Of course, there are dif-
USUALLY FIGHT ABOUT?
ferences among adolescents in the extent to which they
If parents and teenagers typically don’t argue over “big” believe their parents have the authority to regulate vari-
issues, what do they fight about? Studies find that they ous sorts of decisions; adolescents who see parents as
squabble about things like curfews, leisure time activi- having more legitimate authority have fewer behavior
ties, clothing, and the cleanliness of their rooms. These problems (Cumsille et al., 2009; Darling, Cumsille, &
have been the major sources of disagreement in fami- Martínez, 2008; Kuhn & Laird, 2011).
lies with teenagers for at least as long as scientists have As you’ll read, conflict between parents and chil-
been studying the issue (Laursen & DeLay, 2011; Martin, dren increases during early adolescence. One reason
Bascoe, & Davies, 2011). And, although conflict between for this is that, with time, adolescents see more and
adolescents and parents over these mundane matters is more issues that they previously saw as legitimate for
generally less frequent in ethnic minority than in White their parents to regulate (for example, how late they
families, the topics of disagreement are similar across can stay up on school nights) as matters of personal
ethnic groups and cultures (Smetana, Daddis, & choice—a finding that has been replicated in numer-
Chuang, 2003). A study of adolescents in the People’s ous parts of the world, including North America,
Republic of China and Hong Kong, for example, found South America, and Asia, and among both White and
that the most common sources of conflict between ado- Black adolescents in the United States (e.g., Hasebe,
lescents and parents were everyday issues, such as time Nucci, & Nucci, 2004; Milnitsky-Sapiro, Turiel, & Nucci,
spent on schoolwork, household chores, and choice of 2006; Smetana, 2005). Adolescents whose parents
friends (Yau & Smetana, 2003). attempt to regulate what they believe are personal
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 122 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

122 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

with hypothetical situations and asked whether it was


acceptable to lie to one’s parents about a personal
matter (e.g., dating someone one’s parents didn’t
like), about half of the younger adolescents (12- to
14-year-olds) said it was acceptable to lie, whereas
more than 80% of the older adolescents (15- to
17-year-olds) did. Partial disclosures—“lies of omis-
sion”—are especially common. As one Black 14-year-
old explained:

I just tell [my mother] where we be going and stuff. And


if we’re going to stay out late and stuff like that, I don’t
tell her none of that stuff. . . . Like if we’re going to the
mall and stuff, I just don’t tell her that we’re going to go
to parties. (Bakken & Brown, 2010, p. 374)

One source of conflict between parents and teenagers involves differ- RECAP
ences in the way they define issues. Making sure that the adolescent’s
bedroom is tidy is seen by parents as an area over which parents
• Although popular books for parents of teenagers
should have jurisdiction. Teenagers, however, tend to see their bed- present adolescence as a problematic time for the
room as their own private space and decisions about neatness as family, the notion that a wide gap exists between
matters of personal choice. the generations in fundamental values is a myth.
• When parents and adolescents disagree, it tends to
be over mundane, day-to-day issues, not over major
issues are more likely to describe their parents as being values or priorities.
overly controlling. Perhaps because of this, the effects • Many disagreements between parents and teenag-
of feeling psychologically controlled by their parents, ers stem from the different perspectives that they
which has a negative impact on adolescents’ mental bring to the discussion: The same issues that par-
health, are very different from the effects of feeling that ents see as matters of right or wrong are often seen
their parents simply want to know where they go and by adolescents as matters of personal choice.
what they do, which has a positive impact (Hasebe • Conflict between parents and children may in-
et al., 2004; Loukas, 2009; Padilla-Walker, 2008; crease in early adolescence because adolescents
Silk, Morris, Kanaya, & Steinberg, 2003; Smetana, change their perspective on many issues that they
Campione-Barr, & Daddis, 2004; Smetana & Daddis, had previously viewed as ones their parents had
2002). legitimate authority to regulate.
In other words, teenagers and their parents often
clash more over the definition of the issue (e.g.,
whether something is a safety issue versus a matter of
personal choice) than over the specific details. The
struggle, then, is over who has the authority—and
whose jurisdiction the issue falls into. Because early
adolescence is a time when adolescents’ reasoning
abilities are changing, the ways that individuals un- Family Relationships
derstand family rules and regulations change as well.
As a consequence of normal cognitive development, a
at Adolescence
9-year-old child who is willing to accept his parents’ Although it is incorrect to characterize adolescence as a
views—who doesn’t question his mother when she time of high conflict in most families, it is important to
says, “We do not leave clothes on the floor”—grows keep in mind that adolescence is nevertheless a period
into an adolescent who understands that some issues of change and reorganization in family relationships
are matters of personal choice, rather than social con- and daily interactions (Laursen & DeLay, 2011; Martin
vention (“It’s my room, so why should it bother Bascoe, & Davies, 2011). As they develop, adolescents
you?”). One manifestation of this change in perspec- spend increasingly less time in family activities, espe-
tive is an increase, with age, in adolescents’ willingness cially in activities with the family as a group (see
to lie to their parents. In one study (S. A. Perkins & Figure 4.1) (Larson, Richards, Moneta, Holmbeck, &
Turiel, 2007), in which adolescents were presented Duckett, 1996).
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 123 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 123

35 for many adults, whether they have children or not. In-


Extended family
Family group deed, some theorists have gone so far as to describe it as
30 Siblings only a time of midlife crisis (Lachman, 2004).
Percent of waking hours

Father only
25 Mother only
❚ Midlife Meets Adolescence If we look at the
20 nature of the midlife crises in some detail, we find that
the developmental concerns of parents and adolescents
15
are complementary (Steinberg & Steinberg, 1994).
10 First, consider the issue of biological change. At the
same time that adolescents are entering into a period
5 of rapid physical growth, sexual maturation, and, ulti-
mately, the period of the life span that society has la-
0
5th 6th 7th 8th 9th 10th 11th 12th beled one of the most physically attractive, their parents
Grade are beginning to feel increased concern about their own
FIGURE 4.1 Age differences in the amount of time adoles- bodies, about their physical attractiveness, and about
cents spend with family members. (Larson et al., 1996) their sexual appeal (Banister, 1999). One mother of an
early-adolescent girl once remarked, in an interview
with my research staff, that it was jarring to realize that
A TIME OF REORGANIZATION when she and her daughter walked down the street,
AND CHANGE men now looked at her daughter, and not at her.
A second overlap of crises concerns perceptions of
According to family systems theory, relationships in fam-
time and the future. At the same time that adolescents
ilies change most dramatically during those times when
are beginning to develop the capability to think system-
individual family members or the family’s circumstances
atically about the future and do, in fact, begin to look
are changing, since it is during these times that the family’s
ahead, their parents are beginning to feel that the pos-
previously established equilibrium, or balance, will be up-
sibilities for changes in their own lives are limited. Be-
set. Not surprisingly, one period in which family relation-
fore midlife, individuals tend to measure time in terms
ships usually change a great deal is adolescence. One study
of how long they have been alive; after midlife, they are
of interactions between adolescent boys and their parents
more likely to see things in terms of how much longer
found that the peak time for this equilibrium to be upset
they have to live (Lang & Carstensen, 2002). One reason
was around age 13 or 14; the researchers speculate that,
for this shift may be that at midlife adults are reminded
because some of this transformation may be driven by
of their mortality because they see their own parents
puberty, in families with girls, this “disequilibrium” is
aging. Whatever the reason, it is not hard to imagine
more likely to occur earlier, around age 11 or 12 (Granic,
that the naïve optimism of adolescence may clash with
Hollenstein, Dishion, & Patterson, 2003). Adaptation to
the hardened pragmatism of middle age.
adolescence may be especially difficult when the teenager
Finally, consider the issue of power, status, and en-
is emotionally rigid (Lichtwarck-Aschoff, Kunnen, & van
trance into the roles of adulthood. Adolescence is the
Geert, 2009) or temperamentally difficult (Trentacosta
time when individuals are on the threshold of gaining a
et al., 2011).
great deal of status. Their careers and marriages lie
The specific concerns and issues characteristic of
ahead of them, and choices may seem limitless. For their
families at adolescence arise not just because of the
parents, in contrast, many choices have already been
changing needs and concerns of the young person but
made—some successfully, others perhaps less so. Most
also because of changes in the adolescent’s parents and
adults reach their “occupational plateau”—the point at
in the needs and functions of the family. You already
which they can tell how successful they are likely to be—
have some understanding of the biological, cognitive,
during midlife, and many must deal with whatever gap
and social changes adolescents go through and how
exists between their early aspirations and their actual
these may affect the family system. But to fully under-
achievements (Lachman, 2004). In sum, for adolescents,
stand family relationships during the adolescent years,
this phase in the family life cycle is a time of boundless
we must also take into account characteristics of the
adolescent’s parents and of families at this stage as well.

THE ADOLESCENT’S PARENTS AT MIDLIFE family systems theory A perspective on family functioning that
emphasizes interconnections among different family relationships
Because people typically have their first child around
(such as marital, parent–child, sibling).
age 30, most parents are in their early 40s when the first
child enters early adolescence. Some research suggests midlife crisis A psychological crisis over identity believed to
that this time of life can be a potentially difficult time occur between the ages of 35 and 45, the age range of most
adolescents’ parents.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 124 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

124 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

has increased, and proportionately more couples today


are delaying childbearing until they have become estab-
lished in their careers (Gregory, 2007). As a conse-
quence, adults tend to be older today when their
children reach adolescence than was the case two de-
cades ago. We do not know how being an older parent
affects relationships during adolescence.

❚ The Mental Health of Parents What we do


know is that in families with middle-aged adults, ad-
justing to adolescence may take more of a toll on the
mental health of parents than on the mental health of
adolescents (Steinberg & Steinberg, 1994). One study
found that nearly two-thirds of mothers and fathers de-
scribed adolescence as the most difficult stage of par-
enting (Pasley & Gecas, 1984), and several studies have
found this period in the family life cycle to be a low
point in parents’ marital and life satisfaction (Gecas &
Seff, 1990). Parents who are deeply involved in work
outside the home or who have an especially happy mar-
riage may be buffered against some of these negative
consequences, however, whereas single mothers may be
especially vulnerable to them (Silverberg, Marczak, &
Gondoli, 1996; Silverberg & Steinberg, 1990; Steinberg
& Steinberg, 1994). In fact, a strained relationship be-
tween a midlife parent and his or her adolescent child
may drive the parent to devote relatively more time to
work (Fortner, Crouter, & McHale, 2004). At the same
For many adults, their child’s adolescence coincides with their own time, studies show that parents’ mental health affects
passage through midlife. Not all adults experience a “midlife crisis,” the way they interact with their adolescent, which in
but for many, this is a time of heightened introspection and self-doubt. turn adversely affects the teenagers (Yap, Schwartz,
The collision of adolescence and midlife may make the period an
especially challenging one in some families.
Byrne, Simmons, & Allen, 2010).
By the way, the notion that parents’ mental health
declines when they enter the “empty nest” stage is a
horizons; for their parents, it is a time of coming to myth, especially among mothers. Parents’ mental health
terms with choices made when they were younger. is worse when their teenage children are living at home
This overlap of crises is likely to have an impact on than it is once they have moved out, and when children
family relationships (Hamill, 1994; Steinberg & Stein- leave home, it is fathers, not mothers, who typically feel
berg, 1994). A father who is worried about his own the greatest sense of loss (Steinberg & Steinberg, 1994).
physical health may suddenly feel uncomfortable about
playing tennis each weekend with his growing son, as
they did for years when the boy was younger. An adoles- MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
cent girl with big plans for the future may find it diffi- Many studies have found that marital
cult to understand why her father seems so cautious satisfaction is lower when parents’
and narrow-minded when she asks him for advice. An first-born child is a teenager than at any
adolescent boy may find his mother’s constant atten- other point in the marriage. Why do you
tion annoying; he doesn’t see that, to her, his interest in think this might be?
independence signifies the end of an important stage in
her career as a parent. The adolescent’s desire for inde-
pendence appears to be especially stressful for parents
(Steinberg & Steinberg, 1994).
This generalization about the collision of adoles-
CHANGES IN FAMILY NEEDS
cence and midlife must be tempered by the fact that
AND FUNCTIONS
recent decades have seen important changes in both the
age at which individuals marry and the age at which It is not only individual family members who undergo
they have their first child. The average age at marriage change during the family’s adolescent years. The family as
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 125 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 125

a unit changes as well in its economic circumstances, their teenagers, as this Chinese mother of a 15-year-old
its relationship to other social institutions, and its girl explains:
functions.
Sometimes some of her male friends called. After she
One of the most important changes is financial, with hung up the phone I often asked her, “Who is he?” She
family finances likely to be strained during adolescence. then answered, “My male classmates.” I asked her, “Why is
Children grow rapidly during puberty, and clothing for he calling you every night?” then she said I was overpow-
adolescents is expensive. Keeping up with the “must- ering her and even intruding into her privacy. I said “I’m
haves” of the peer culture—designer clothes, smart- just asking. Is there any problem?” I asked her, “Are you
phones, computers—may push a family budget to the dating? He does not have to call and ask you about home-
limit. Many families also begin saving money for large work every night, does he?” She then said, “We are just
anticipated expenditures, such as the adolescent’s col- chatting.” I said, “If you guys are having casual conversa-
lege education. And in some families, parents may find tions, does he have to call her every night?” Since then,
themselves having to help support their own parents at her male friends dared not call her. (Qin, 2008, p. 27)
a time when their children are still economically depen- Finally, important changes in family functions also
dent. The financial demands placed on parents in the take place during adolescence. During infancy and
“sandwich generation” (that is, sandwiched between childhood, the functions and responsibilities of the
their adolescent children and their aging parents) re- family are fairly clear: nurturance, protection, and so-
quire considerable adjustment. cialization. While all these roles are still important dur-
In addition to these financial pressures, the adoles- ing adolescence, adolescents are in need of support
cent’s family must cope with the increasing importance more than nurturance, of guidance more than protec-
of the peer group (Laursen & DeLay, 2011). During the tion, and of direction more than socialization. Making
early stages of the life cycle, the child’s social world is the transition from the family functions of childhood to
fairly narrow, and the family is the central setting. Dur- the family functions of adolescence is not necessarily
ing late childhood and early adolescence, however, the easy, especially in contemporary society, where prepara-
peer group becomes a setting in which extremely close tion for adulthood—one of the chief tasks of adoles-
ties are forged, and parents and adolescents may argue cence that was once carried out primarily by the
about the teenager’s reluctance to give up time with her family—is increasingly performed by other institutions,
or his friends for family activities. How adolescents and such as the school. Many families may feel at a loss to
parents adjust to this shift in orientation is likely to vary figure out just what their role during adolescence is.
across ethnic groups, since certain cultures are more
likely to stress family obligations—like helping with
household chores—than others (Fuligni, Yip, & Tseng, TRANSFORMATIONS
2002; Hardway & Fuligni, 2006; S. Lee, 2001). Indeed, IN FAMILY RELATIONS
many immigrant families place an especially high value Together, the biological, cognitive, and social transi-
on familism, an orientation toward life in which the tions of adolescence; the changes experienced by adults
needs of one’s family take precedence over the needs of at midlife; and the changes undergone by the family
the individual (Germán, Gonzales, & Dumka, 2009), a during this stage in the family life cycle set in motion a
view that may clash with the more individualistic orien- series of transformations in family relationships. In
tation characteristics of many mainstream American most families, there is a movement away from patterns
families. Younger adolescents who value familism are of influence and interaction that are asymmetrical and
less likely to get involved with antisocial peer groups imbalanced toward ones in which parents and adoles-
(Roosa et al., 2011). cents are on a more equal footing. And some evidence
Different expectations between immigrant parents suggests that early adolescence—when this shift toward
and teenagers are a significant source of stress for ado- more egalitarian relationships first begins—may be a
lescents and parents alike, especially when the adolescent time of temporary disruption in the family system.
is more Americanized and the parents are less so, a phe-
nomenon known as generational dissonance (Choi, ❚ Changes in the Balance of Power Studies of
He, & Harachi, 2008; D. R. Crane, So Wa, Larson, & family interaction suggest that during early adolescence
Hafen, 2005; Phinney & Ong, 2002; Weaver & Kim, 2008; young people begin to try to play a more forceful role
Wu & Chao, 2011). Studies of Mexican American fami-
lies, for example, have found that stress and family con-
flict are higher in Latino families with relatively more
familism An orientation toward life in which the needs of one’s
acculturated adolescents (Gonzales, Deardoff, Formoso,
family take precedence over the needs of the individual.
Barr, & Barrera, Jr., 2006; C. R. Martinez, Jr., 2006). Dif-
ferent expectations about the adolescent’s social life can generational dissonance Divergence of views between
cause problems between newly arrived immigrants and adolescents and parents that is common in families of immigrant
parents and American-born adolescents.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 126 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

126 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

in the family, but parents may not yet acknowledge teenagers and parents, rather than any serious loss of
adolescents’ input. As a result, young adolescents may love or respect between parents and children (Keijsers,
interrupt their parents more often but have little im- Branje, Frijns, Finkenauer, & Meeus, 2010; Laursen &
pact. By middle adolescence, however, teenagers act and Collins, 2009). Research suggests that the distancing
are treated much more like adults. They have more in- that takes place between parents and teenagers in early
fluence over family decisions, but they do not need to and middle adolescence is temporary, however, and that
assert their opinions through interruptions and simi- parent–child relationships may become less conflicted
larly immature behavior (Grotevant, 1997). and more intimate during late adolescence (Thornton,
Increases in the assertiveness and influence of adoles- Orbuch, & Axinn, 1995). Nevertheless, studies indicate
cents as they get older are consistent with their changing that the more frequent bickering characteristic of early
needs and capabilities. To adapt to the changes triggered adolescence may take a modest toll on parents’ mental
by the child’s entrance into adolescence, family members health (Steinberg, 2001) and some studies have found
must have some shared sense of what they are experienc- that individuals who reported more conflict with their
ing and how they are changing. Yet studies show that in parents during adolescence had more problems both
many families parents and children live in “separate reali- later in adolescence and in young adulthood (Herren-
ties,” perceiving their day-to-day experiences in very dif- kohl, Kosterman, Hawkins, & Mason, 2009; Klahr, Mc-
ferent ways (Larson & Richards, 1994; Laursen & DeLay, Gue, Iacono, & Burt, 2011a). Although it is certainly
2011). A mother and son, for example, may have a conver- possible that adolescents who have problems are more
sation about schoolwork, and while she may experience likely to provoke conflict with their parents, studies that
the conversation as a serious discussion, he may perceive have been able to separate cause and effect have found
it as an argument. One study of Black families, in which that conflict actually leads to the development of men-
mothers, teenagers, and researchers all rated a videotape tal health problems (Klahr, Rueter, McGue, Iacono, &
of the mother and teenager having a discussion, found Burt, 2011b) and emotional distress, regardless of eth-
that the teenagers rated their mother’s behavior far more nicity (Chung, Flook, & Fuligni, 2009). Conflict at
negatively than did either the mother or the researcher home spills over into the adolescents’ relationships with
(Campione-Barr & Smetana, 2004). One interesting find- friends, and viceversa (Chung, Flook, & Fuligni, 2011).
ing to emerge from recent research on brain maturation Patterns of conflict and closeness in the family at ado-
in adolescence is that young adolescents may be especially lescence may vary across ethnic groups. One study found
sensitive—perhaps even overreactive—to the emotional that in Asian households, there is an increase in conflict,
signals given off by others (Pfeifer & Blakemore, 2012). A as in non-Asian families, but not until later in adolescence
parent may speak to an adolescent in a serious voice, but (Greenberger & Chen, 1996). Another study, comparing
the adolescent may experience it as anger (E. Nelson, Black families with White families, found that White par-
Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005). ents tended to give adolescents relatively more autonomy
over decisions, although opportunities for adolescent
❚ The Role of Puberty The adolescent’s biological decision making increased in both ethnic groups over
and cognitive maturation likely plays a role in unbalanc- time (see Figure 4.2) (Gutman & Eccles, 2007).
ing the family system during early adolescence. Several
researchers have demonstrated that family relationships
3.0
change during puberty, with bickering between adoles- White females
cents and their parents increasing, and closeness be- White males
Decision-making opportunities

tween adolescents and their parents diminishing (Baer, Black females


2002; De Goede, Branje, & Meeus, 2009; McGue, Elkins, Black males
Walden, & Iacono, 2005).
Although puberty seems to distance adolescents
2.8
from their parents, it is not associated with familial
“storm and stress,” and rates of outright conflict be-
tween parents and children are not dramatically higher
during adolescence than before or after (Laursen &
DeLay, 2011). Rather, disputes between parents and
teenagers are typical of the sorts of arguments people
2.5
have when a more powerful person (in this case, the 13 15 17 19
parent) is trying to get a less powerful one (in this case, Age (years)
the adolescent) to do something (Adams & Laursen, FIGURE 4.2 Parents generally give adolescents greater
2001). Similarly, the diminished closeness is more likely opportunity to make decisions as they get older, although
to be manifested in increased privacy on the part of the the pattern of change varies by race and gender. (Gutman &
adolescent and diminished physical affection between Eccles, 2007)
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 127 21/03/13 9:52 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 127

members’ expectations of each other (Lanz, Scabini,


MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
Vermulst, & Gerris, 2001).
What aspects of the changing parent- Psychologist W. Andrew Collins (1990) has examined
adolescent relationship would you changes in children’s and parents’ expectations for each
expect to be common across other during adolescence and how “violations” of these
different ethnic and cultural groups? expectations can cause family conflict. A child may enter
Which ones are likely to differ? adolescence expecting that it will be a time of great free-
dom, for example, whereas the parents may view the
same period as one in which tighter reins are necessary.
Alternatively, another child, perhaps influenced by tele-
vision sitcoms portraying happy families, may imagine
Because the disagreements parents and adolescents that adolescence will be a time of increased closeness and
have typically revolve around issues of parental control, shared activities in the family, only to find that his or her
patterns of squabbling and bickering will likely vary parents have been looking forward to having time to
across cultural groups whose timetables for adolescent themselves. It is easy to see how differences in expecta-
independence differ. Patterns of conflict and closeness tions about what adolescence is going to be like can
in immigrant families may therefore differ as a function escalate into arguments and misunderstandings. When
of the family’s degree of acculturation. One study of questioned about whether adolescents were expected to
Mexican American families found higher rates of disclose secrets to their parents, for example, adolescents’
parent–adolescent cohesion during mid-adolescence expectations for secrecy were much greater than parents’
among more highly acculturated families (Baer & (Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, & Campione-Barr, 2006).
Schmitz, 2007). Conversely, conflict is less frequent, and
cohesion higher, in households where teenagers and
SEX DIFFERENCES IN FAMILY
their parents communicate in their native language
RELATIONSHIPS
(Tseng & Fuligni, 2000). Parental monitoring appears to
be more effective in ethnic minority families in which In general, differences between the family relations of
the adolescent has a weaker ethnic identity, consistent sons and daughters are minimal. Although there are oc-
with the idea that adolescents who are more American- casional exceptions to the rule, sons and daughters re-
ized may need more vigilant parents to keep them out of port similar degrees of closeness to their parents, similar
trouble (Nagoshi, Marsiglia, Parsai, & Castro, 2011). amounts of conflict, similar types of rules (and dis-
In any event, it is probably fair to say that the first half agreements about those rules), and similar patterns of
of adolescence may be a somewhat more strained and
distant time for the family than earlier or later, although
this may be more true in the case of firstborns than in the
case of later-borns, perhaps because parents may learn
from experience (L. Shanahan, McHale, Osgood, &
Crouter, 2007). Part of the problem may be that conflicts
between teenagers and parents tend to be resolved not
through compromise but through one party giving in or
walking away, neither of which enhances the quality of
the relationship or contributes to the adolescent’s or the
parent’s well-being (Laursen & Collins, 1994; C. Tucker,
McHale, & Crouter, 2003b). As relationships between
parents and adolescents become more egalitarian, they
get better at resolving conflicts (Van Doorn, Branje, &
Meuss, 2011).

❚ Violations of Expectations Several researchers


have studied changes in adolescents’ cognitive abilities
and how these changes may reverberate throughout the
family. We noted earlier in this chapter that changes in
the ways adolescents view family rules and regulations
may contribute to increased conflict between them and
their parents (Laursen & DeLay, 2011). Research also
indicates that early adolescence is a time of changes in Adolescents fare better when their parents are warm and firm,
youngsters’ views of family relationships and in family a style known as authoritative parenting.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 128 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

128 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

activity. And observational studies of interactions be-


tween parents and adolescents indicate that sons and Family Relationships and
daughters interact with their parents in remarkably
similar ways (Steinberg & Silk, 2002).
Adolescent Development
On the other hand, teenagers relate very differently to Thus far, we have looked at the sorts of issues and con-
mothers and to fathers. Across many ethnic groups and cerns faced by most families during the adolescent years.
cultures, adolescents tend to be closer to their mother, In our focus on those experiences that all families share,
to spend more time alone with their mother, and to feel however, we have not addressed the very important ques-
more comfortable talking to their mother about prob- tions of how relationships differ from family to family
lems and other emotional matters; as a consequence, and whether these differences have important conse-
mothers tend to be more involved than fathers in their quences for the developing adolescent. Some parents are
adolescents’ lives (Updegraff, McHale, Crouter, & stricter than others. Some adolescents are given a great
Kupanoff, 2001; S. K. Williams & Kelly, 2005). Not sur- deal of affection, while others are treated more distantly.
prisingly, fathers rely on mothers for information about In some households, decisions are made through open
their adolescent’s activities, but mothers rarely rely on discussion and verbal give-and-take; in others, parents
fathers for this (Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, & McHale, lay down the rules, and children are expected to follow
2005; Waizenhofer, Buchanan, & Jackson-Newsom, them. To what extent are different patterns of family rela-
2004). Fathers are more likely to be perceived as rela- tionships associated with different patterns of adolescent
tively distant authority figures who may be consulted for development? Are some styles of parenting more likely to
objective information (such as help with homework) be associated with healthy development than others?
but who are rarely sought for support or guidance (such Before we try to answer these questions, several cau-
as help with problems with a boyfriend or girlfriend) tions are in order. Although our tendency is to see chil-
(Crockett, Brown, Russell, & Shen, 2007). Interestingly, dren’s behavior as the result of their parents’ behavior,
adolescents also fight more often with their mothers socialization is a two-way, not a one-way, street (W. A.
than with their fathers and perceive mothers as more Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein,
controlling, but this does not appear to jeopardize the 2000). Just as parents affect their adolescents’ behavior,
closeness of the mother–adolescent relationship (Shek, adolescents affect their parents’ behavior (Coley, Votruba-
2007). In other words, relationships between adoles- Drzal, & Schindler, 2009; Wang, Dishion, Stoirmshak, &
cents and their mothers are more emotionally intense in Willet, 2011; Willoughby & Hamza, 2011). One nine-year
general, and this intensity has both positive and negative longitudinal study found, for example, that adolescents’
manifestations (Larson & Richards, 1994). and parents’ negative feelings toward each other had a
reciprocal relationship over time—the more negative ado-
lescents felt, the more this led to negative feelings on the
part of their parents, and vice versa (K. Kim, Conger,
RECAP Lorenz, & Elder, 2001). Similarly, although harsh and
• Transformations in family relations are sparked by physical discipline leads to increases in adolescent behav-
the biological, cognitive, and social maturation of ior problems, when adolescents engage in problematic
the adolescent; by the changes parents face at behavior, their parents respond by becoming more puni-
midlife; and by changes in the needs and functions tive or detached (Gault-Sherman, 2012; Lansford et al.,
of the family. 2011; Roche, Ghazarian, Little, & Leventhal, 2011; Williams
• Adolescence is a time of reorganization in family & Steinberg, 2011). This reciprocal interplay between par-
relationships, with adolescents gaining increasingly enting and adolescent development is so strong that it
more power and becoming increasingly more
assertive.
• Many of the changes in family relations that occur
at adolescence affect the psychological well-being MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
of the adolescent’s middle-aged parents as well as Cultures vary in the extent to which physical
the teenager; indeed, in some regards, adolescence punishment is used. In some countries, it actually is
may be a more difficult time for the parents than illegal for a parent to hit his or her child. In others,
the teenager. however, physical punishment is common. Do you
• Although the family relations of adolescent males think that the effect of physical punishment on
and females are very similar, studies show that adolescent development would be
mothers and fathers have very different different in countries where it was
relationships with their acceptable than in countries where
teenagers. it was outlawed?
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 129 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 129

High demandingness
Authoritarian Authoritative

• Adult-centered, autocratic, rigid • Child-centered, democratic, flexible


• Strict rules and expectations • Establish firm behavioral guidelines
• Unilateral decision making • Engage adolescent in decision making
• Little warmth or communication • Warm, accepting, involved, trusting
• Expects obedience, distrusting • Monitoring
• Punitive punishment • Supports assertiveness,
• Discourages open communication responsiveness, and self-regulation
• Encourages psychological autonomy

Low responsiveness High responsiveness

Indifferent Indulgent

• Adult-centered, passive, dismissing • Child-centered, indulgent, appeasing


• Pose few demands on the adolescent • No guidelines, nondirective
• Poor or little communication • Avoid confrontation, rarely discipline
• Do not monitor or supervise behavior adolescent
• Detached, distant, withdrawn, absent • Warm, accepting, nurturing
• Overinvolved, blurred roles
• Few rules or expectations

Low demandingness
FIGURE 4.3 A typological conceptualization of parenting styles based on the dimensions
of responsiveness and demandingness. (adapted from Martin, Bascoe, & Davies, 2011)

even contributes to the transmission of parenting styles which the parent responds to the child’s needs in an ac-
across generations (Kerr, Capaldi, Pears, & Owen, 2009). cepting, supportive manner. Parental demandingness
In addition, various types of parenting affect different refers to the extent to which the parent expects and de-
adolescents differently. For example, although adoles- mands mature, responsible behavior from the child.
cents whose parents are hostile or aloof are more likely Parents vary on each of these dimensions. Some are
to exhibit antisocial behavior (e.g., Dobkin, Tremblay, & warm and accepting, while others are unresponsive and
Sacchitelle, 1997), the link between negative parenting rejecting; some are demanding and expect a great deal
and adolescent problem behavior is stronger among of their child, while others are permissive and demand
teenagers who are temperamentally more impulsive; in very little.
contrast, among adolescents who are temperamentally
more introverted, the same sort of parenting leads to ❚ Four Styles of Parenting Because parental re-
anxiety and depression (Stice & Gonzales, 1998; Van sponsiveness and demandingness are more or less inde-
Leeuwen, Mervielde, Braet, & Bosmans, 2004; Williams pendent of each other—that is, it is possible for a parent
et al., 2009). Furthermore, adolescents who have a to be demanding without being responsive, and vice
greater genetic risk for developing problems (by vir- versa—it is possible to look at various combinations of
tue of their family history) are more likely to evoke these two dimensions (see Figure 4.3). Many studies of
from their parents the sort of behavior that has been parents and children indicate that the fourfold classifi-
shown to lead to the development of behavior prob- cation scheme presented in Figure 4.3 is very important
lems (O’Connor, Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & in understanding the impact of parents’ behavior on
Plomin, 1998). the child, and psychologists have given labels to the four
different prototypes presented in the figure (Crockett &
Hayes, 2011; Martin, Bascoe, & Davies, 2011).
PARENTING STYLES AND THEIR EFFECTS
There are a variety of ways to characterize parents’ be-
havior toward their children. One of the most useful parental responsiveness One of the two important dimensions
of parenting; responsiveness refers to the degree to which the
approaches derives from the work of psychologist Diana
parent responds to the child’s needs in an accepting, supportive
Baumrind (1978). According to her work and that of
manner.
others in this vein, two aspects of the parent’s behavior
toward the adolescent are critical: parental responsive- parental demandingness One of two important dimensions of
ness and parental “demandingness” (Maccoby & Martin, parenting; demandingness refers to the degree to which the parent
1983). Parental responsiveness refers to the degree to expects and insists on mature, responsible behavior from the child.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 130 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

130 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Parents who are both responsive and demanding are about their child’s activities and whereabouts, show
authoritative. Authoritative parents are warm but firm. little interest in their child’s experiences at school or
They set standards for the child’s conduct but form ex- with friends, rarely converse with their child, and rarely
pectations that are consistent with the child’s develop- consider their child’s opinion when making decisions.
ing needs and capabilities. They place a high value on Rather than raising their child according to a set of be-
the development of autonomy and self-direction but liefs about what is good for the child’s development (as
assume the ultimate responsibility for their child’s be- do the other three parent types), indifferent parents
havior. Authoritative parents deal with their child in a structure their home life primarily around their own
rational, issue-oriented manner, frequently engaging in needs and interests.
discussion and explanation over matters of discipline.
Authoritative parents strive to raise a child who is self-
reliant and who has a strong sense of initiative.
Parents who are very demanding but not responsive MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
are authoritarian. Authoritarian parents place a high Where would you place your parents in the
value on obedience and conformity. They tend to favor four-way model of parenting styles? Would
more punitive, absolute, and forceful disciplinary mea- your parents agree?
sures. Verbal give-and-take is not common in authori-
tarian households, because the underlying belief of
authoritarian parents is that the child should accept
without question the rules and standards established by
the parents. They tend not to encourage independent ❚ The Power of Authoritative Parenting Few
behavior and, instead, place a good deal of importance areas of research in the field of adolescent development
on restricting the child’s autonomy. Authoritarian par- have received as much attention as the link between
ents place a premium on obedience. what parents do and how adolescents turn out, and the
A parent who is very responsive but not at all de- findings of this body of work are amazingly consistent
manding is indulgent. Indulgent parents behave in an (Steinberg, 2001). Generally, young people who have
accepting, benign, and somewhat more passive way in been raised in authoritative households are more psy-
matters of discipline. They place relatively few demands chosocially mature than peers who have been raised in
on the child’s behavior, giving the child a high degree of authoritarian, indulgent, or indifferent homes. Adoles-
freedom to act as he or she wishes. Indulgent parents cents raised in authoritative homes are more responsi-
are more likely to believe that control is an infringe- ble, self-assured, creative, intellectually curious, socially
ment on the child’s freedom that may interfere with skilled, and academically successful. Adolescents raised
healthy development. Instead of actively shaping their in authoritarian homes, in contrast, are more depen-
child’s behavior, indulgent parents are more likely to dent, more passive, less socially adept, less self-assured,
view themselves as resources for the child. Indulgent and less intellectually curious. Adolescents raised in in-
parents tend to be especially concerned with raising a dulgent households are less mature, less responsible,
happy child. and more conforming to their peers. Adolescents raised
Parents who are neither demanding nor responsive in indifferent homes are often impulsive and more likely
are indifferent. Indifferent parents try to do whatever to be involved in delinquent behavior and in precocious
is necessary to minimize the time and energy they must experiments with sex, drugs, and alcohol (W. A. Collins &
devote to interacting with their child. In extreme cases, Steinberg, 2006). Although it generally is not a good
indifferent parents may be neglectful. They know little thing for parents to disagree about how they raise their
teenager, studies show that it is better to have at least
one authoritative parent than two nonauthoritative
ones who happen to be consistent (McKinney & Renk,
authoritative parents Parents who use warmth, firm control, and 2008; L. Simons & Conger, 2007).
rational, issue-oriented discipline, in which emphasis is placed on The evidence linking authoritative parenting and
the development of self-direction. healthy adolescent development is remarkably strong,
authoritarian parents Parents who use punitive, absolute, and and it has been found in studies of a wide range of
forceful discipline, and who place a premium on obedience and ethnicities, social classes, and family structures, not
conformity. only within the United States (e.g., Bean, Barber, &
Crane, 2006; R. Clark, Novak, & Dupree, 2002;
indulgent parents Parents who are characterized by
Cleveland, Gibbons, Gerrard, Pomery, & Brody,
responsiveness but low demandingness, and who are mainly
concerned with the child’s happiness. 2005; S. Kim & Ge, 2000; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005;
Matza, Kupersmidt, & Glenn, 2001; Simpkins et al.,
indifferent parents Parents who are characterized by low levels 2009) but also in parts of the world as diverse as
of both responsiveness and demandingness.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 131 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 131

TABLE 4.1 The 10 Basic Principles of Good Parenting parents provide an appropriate balance between restric-
tiveness and autonomy, giving the adolescent opportu-
Several years ago, after reviewing decades of research nities to develop self-reliance while providing the sorts
on parenting and child development, I came to the of standards, limits, and guidelines that developing
conclusion that we really did know what sort of parenting
individuals need (Martin, Bascoe, & Davies, 2011).
is most likely to help children and adolescents grow up in
healthy ways. I summarized this evidence in a book titled
Authoritative parents, for instance, are more likely to
The 10 Basic Principles of Good Parenting (Steinberg, give children more independence gradually as they get
2005b). Here’s what all parents, regardless of their child’s older, which helps children develop self-reliance and
age, should keep in mind: self-assurance. Because of this, authoritative parenting
1. What You Do Matters promotes the development of adolescents’ competence
2. You Can Not Be Too Loving (Steinberg, 2001) and enhances their ability to withstand
3. Be Involved in Your Child’s Life
a variety of potentially negative influences, including life
stress (Pearce, Jones, Schwab-Stone, & Ruchkin, 2003)
4. Adapt Your Parenting to Fit Your Child
and exposure to antisocial peers (Galambos, Barker, &
5. Establish Rules and Set Limits
Almeida, 2003; Walker-Barnes & Mason, 2004).
6. Help Foster Your Child’s Independence Second, because authoritative parents are more
7. Be Consistent likely to engage their children in verbal give-and-take,
8. Avoid Harsh Discipline they are likely to promote the sort of intellectual devel-
9. Explain Your Rules and Decisions opment that provides an important foundation for
10. Treat Your Child With Respect the development of psychosocial maturity (Smetana,
Crean, & Daddis, 2002). Authoritative parents, for ex-
Source: Steinberg, 2005b.
ample, are less likely than other parents to assert their
authority by turning adolescents’ personal decisions
Iceland (Adalbjarnardottir & Hafsteinsson, 2001),
the Czech Republic (Dmitrieva, Chen, Greenberger,
& Gil-Rivas, 2004), India (Carson, Chowdhury, Perry,
& Pati, 1999), China (Pilgrim, Luo, Urberg, & Fang,
1999), Israel (Mayseless, Scharf, & Sholt, 2003),
Switzerland (Vazsonyi, Hibbert, & Black, 2003), and
Palestine (Punamäki, Qouta, & Sarraj, 1997). Indeed,
the evidence favoring authoritative parenting is so
strong that some experts have suggested that the
question of which type of parenting benefits teenag-
ers the most need not be studied anymore (see
Table 4.1) (Steinberg, 2001). Furthermore, research
shows that educational programs designed to teach
parents how to be more responsive and more de-
manding are effective in fostering healthy adolescent
development and behavior (e.g., Brody et al., 2006;
Connell & Dishion, 2008; Herrenkohl, Hill, Hawkins,
Chung, & Nagin, 2006).
At the other extreme, parenting that is indifferent,
neglectful, hostile, or abusive has been shown consis-
tently to have harmful effects on adolescents’ mental
health and development, leading to depression and a
variety of behavior problems (Buehler, 2006; Coley,
Medeiros, & Schindler, 2008; Hoeve et al., 2008;
Pittman & Chase-Lansdale, 2001). Severe psychologi-
cal abuse (excessive criticism, rejection, or emotional
harshness) appears to have the most deleterious
effects (Dube et al., 2003; Haj-Yahia, M., Musleh, &
Haj-Yahia, Y., 2002).

❚ How Authoritative Parenting Works Why is Although parenting practices vary across cultures, the ways in
authoritative parenting so consistently associated with which adolescents are affected by different types of parenting
healthy adolescent development? First, authoritative generally do not.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 132 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

132 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

(such as over what type of music they listen to) into Leads to
“moral” issues (Smetana, 1995b; Smetana & Asquith,
1994). Family discussions in which decisions, rules, and
expectations are explained help the child understand Low levels of Adolescent
social systems and social relationships. This under- parental knowledge misbehavior
standing plays an important role in the development
of reasoning abilities, theory of mind, moral judgment,
Leads to
and empathy (N. Eisenberg, Morris, McDaniel, &
Spinrad, 2009; Miklikowska, Duriez, & Soenens, 2011). FIGURE 4.4 The relation between parenting and adoles-
Third, because authoritative parenting is based on a cent behavior is often reciprocal. (Adapted from Laird et al.,
2003)
warm parent–child relationship, adolescents are more
likely to identify with, admire, and form strong attach-
ments to their parents, which leaves them more open to actually be the end result of a warm parent–adolescent
their parents’ influence (Darling & Steinberg, 1993; relationship in which the adolescent willingly discloses
K. Mackey, Arnold, & Pratt, 2001; Mounts, 2002; T. Sim, information to the parent (Blodgett Salafia, Gondoli, &
2000). Adolescents who are raised by nonauthoritative Grundy, 2009; Darling, Cumsille, Peña-Alampay, &
parents, in contrast, often end up having friends their Coatsworth, 2009; Hare, Marston, & Allen, 2011; Hunter,
parents disapprove of, including those involved in Barber, Olsen, McNeely, & Bose, 2011; Keijsers, Branje,
problem behavior (Adamczyk-Robinette, Fletcher, & VanderValk, & Meeus, 2010b; Kerr, Stattin, & Burk, 2010;
Wright, 2002; Knoester, Haynie, & Stephens, 2006). Soenens, Vansteenkiste, Luyckx, & Goossens, 2006;
Vieno, Nation, Pastore, & Santinello, 2009; Warr, 2007;
Yau, Tasopoulos-Chan, & Smetana, 2009).
In contrast, children who are continually acting up
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
make their parents short-tempered, impatient, or dis-
Our discussion has focused mainly on the tant. In one study, for instance, the researchers found
consequences of growing up with parents that when parents had little knowledge of their adoles-
who use different styles of parenting. But cent’s behavior, this led to an increase in delinquency,
what makes parents choose the sort of but that increases in delinquency, in turn, led to de-
parenting they use? Why are some creases in parental knowledge (Laird, Pettit, Bates, &
parents authoritative but others not? Dodge, 2003) (see Figure 4.4). In other words, the rela-
tionship between adolescent competence and authorita-
tive parenting may be the result of a reciprocal cycle in
which the child’s psychosocial maturity leads to authori-
Finally, the child’s own behavior, temperament, and tative parenting, which, in turn, leads to the further de-
personality may play a role in shaping parenting prac- velopment of maturity (Repetti, 1996). In contrast,
tices (Albrecht, Galambos, & Jansson, 2007; Beaver & nonauthoritative parenting may lead to the development
Wright, 2007; W. L. Cook, 2001; Denissen, van Aken, & of emotional and behavioral problems, which may lead
Dubas, 2009). Children who are responsible, self- parents to disengage even more (Burke, Pardini, &
directed, curious, and self-assured elicit from their Loeber, 2008; Dishion, Nelson, & Bullock, 2004).
parents warmth, flexible guidance, and verbal give-and-
take. In contrast, children who are irritable, aggressive,
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN PARENTING
dependent, or less psychosocially mature may provoke
PRACTICES
parents’ behavior that is excessively harsh, passive, or dis-
tant (Huh, Tristan, Wade, & Stice, 2006; Lansford et al., A number of researchers have asked whether parents
2011; Lengua, 2006; Roche et al., 2011). Parents may en- from different ethnic groups vary in their child rearing
joy being around children who are responsible, indepen- and whether the relation between parenting styles and
dent, and willing to tell them about their activities and adolescent outcomes is the same across different ethnic
whereabouts, and they may treat them more warmly as a groups. These, of course, are two different questions:
result. Thus, although evidence suggests that active pa- The first concerns average differences between groups
rental monitoring does deter adolescent problem behav- in their approaches to parenting (for example, whether
ior (Coley, Votruba-Drzal, & Schindler, 2008; Fletcher, ethnic minority parents are stricter than White par-
Steinberg, & Williams-Wheeler, 2004; Hair, Moore, ents), whereas the second concerns the correlation be-
Garrett, Ling, & Cleveland, 2008; Lahey, Van Hulle, tween parenting practices and adolescent adjustment in
D’Onofrio, Rodgers, & Waldman, 2008; Laird, Marrero, different groups (for example, whether the effect of
& Sentse, 2010; Waizenhofer et al., 2004), some of what strictness is the same in ethnic minority families as it is
often appears to be “effective parental monitoring” may in White families).
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 133 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 133

In general, researchers find that authoritative par-


enting, as defined earlier in this chapter, is less prevalent
among Black, Asian, or Hispanic families than among
White families, no doubt reflecting the fact that parent-
ing practices are often linked to cultural values and be-
liefs (Smetana & Chuang, 2001; Yau & Smetana, 1996).
Nevertheless, even though authoritative parenting is
less common in ethnic minority families, its effects on
adolescent adjustment are beneficial in all ethnic groups
(Amato & Fowler, 2002; Vazsonyi & Belliston, 2006;
Vazsonyi, Trejos-Castillo, & Huang, 2006; Walker-
Barnes & Mason, 2001). In other words, ethnic minor-
ity youngsters benefit from parenting that is responsive
and demanding, just as their nonminority peers do.
Research also indicates that ethnic minority parents
are, in general, relatively more demanding than White
parents, even after taking ethnic differences in socio-
economic status into account (Chao & Otsuki-Clutter,
2011) an approach exemplified (some might say, cari- Family relationships often differ across ethnic groups.
catured) by a type of parent described as a “Tiger
Mother” (Chua, 2011). As opposed to research on au-
thoritative parenting, however, which suggests similar for ethnic minority adolescents, but rather that it is not
effects across ethnic groups, research on parental con- as harmful in these groups as it has been shown to be
trol indicates that the adverse effects of this style of par- among White adolescents (W. A. Collins & Steinberg,
enting may be greater among White youngsters than 2006).
among their ethnic minority counterparts (Morrison
Gutman, Sameroff, & Eccles, 2002; Ruiz, Roosa, &
AUTONOMY AND ATTACHMENT
Gonzales, 2002; Schweingruber & Kalil, 2000). Several
IN THE ADOLESCENT’S FAMILY
explanations have been offered for this finding.
First, because ethnic minority families are more Several studies of conversations between adolescents and
likely to live in dangerous communities, authoritarian their parents have examined factors in the nature of
parenting, with its emphasis on control, may not be as parent–adolescent communication that contribute to
harmful and may even offer some benefits (Steinberg, healthy adolescent development. In these studies, fami-
Blatt-Eisengart, & Cauffman, 2006). Second, as several lies were asked to discuss a problem together, and their
researchers have pointed out, definitions of parental interaction was videotaped and later analyzed. Generally,
control may not always make sense when applied to families with psychologically competent teenagers inter-
parents from other cultures (Chao & Otsuki-Clutter, act in ways that permit family members to express their
2011). For example, non-White parents frequently autonomy and individuality while remaining attached,
combine a very high degree of strictness (like White or connected, to other family members (McElhaney,
authoritarian parents) with warmth (like White au- Allen, Stephenson, & Hare, 2009). In these families, ver-
thoritative parents) (Jackson-Newsom, Buchanan, & bal give-and-take is the norm, and adolescents (as well as
McDonald, 2008; Smetana & Gaines, 2000; Supple, parents) are encouraged to express their own opinions,
Peterson, & Bush, 2004). Because most of the research even if this sometimes leads to disagreements. At the
on parenting and adolescent development has been same time, however, the importance of maintaining
conducted by White researchers, other ethnic groups’ close relationships in the family is emphasized, and
approaches to child rearing (which appear very con- individuals are encouraged to consider how their
trolling, but which are neither aloof nor hostile) may be actions may affect other family members. Indeed, ado-
mislabeled as authoritarian when they are actually bet- lescents who are permitted to assert their own opinions
ter understood as protective (Halgunseth, Ispa, & Rudy, within a family context that is secure and loving develop
2006). Similar arguments have been made about higher self-esteem and more mature coping abilities.
the misinterpretation of strictness among American Adolescents whose autonomy is squelched are at risk
parents who are conservative Protestants (Gunnoe, for developing feelings of depression and low self-
Hetherington, & Reiss, 1999; Wilcox, 1998). Neverthe- esteem, whereas those who do not feel connected are
less, it is important to keep in mind that the conclusion more likely than their peers to develop behavior prob-
to be drawn from these studies is not that overly con- lems (Aquilino & Supple, 2001; Bender et al., 2007;
trolling parenting is better than authoritative parenting McElhaney et al., 2009).
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 134 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

134 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

In other words, adolescents appear to do best when 27


they grow up in a family atmosphere that permits the Same-sex dyads
26
development of individuality against a backdrop of Mixed-sex dyads

Sibling intimacy
close family ties (Grolnick, Kurowski, Dunlap, & Hevey, 25
2000; Walsh, Shulman, Bar-On, & Tsur, 2006). In these
24
families, conflict between parents and adolescents can
play a very important and positive role in the adoles- 23
cent’s social and cognitive development, because indi-
22
viduals are encouraged to express their opinions in an
atmosphere that does not risk severing the emotional 21
7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
attachment (McElhaney et al., 2009). Perhaps for this
Age (years)
reason, adolescents whose perceptions of their family
differ from those of their parents a little bit are better FIGURE 4.5 Patterns of change in intimacy between
siblings differ between same-sex and mixed-sex dyads.
adjusted than those whose views are either identical to
(Kim et al., 2006)
their parents’ or extremely divergent (Feinberg, Howe,
Reiss, & Hetherington, 2000). Too much discrepancy
may not be advantageous, though: One recent study with their parents (C. Tucker, McHale, & Crouter, 2003a).
found, however, that adolescents are more likely to have Over the course of adolescence, adolescents’ relation-
emotional problems when there are large disagree- ships with siblings, and especially with younger sib-
ments between how they and their parents describe lings, become more egalitarian but also more distant
their relationship (Guion, Mrug, & Windle, 2009). and less emotionally intense (A. Cole & Kerns, 2001;
Tucker, Updegraff, & Baril, 2010), although patterns of
change in sibling relationships differ between same-sex
ADOLESCENTS’ RELATIONSHIPS
and mixed-sex dyads (see Figure 4.5). As Figure 4.5 in-
WITH SIBLINGS
dicates, in same-sex dyads, intimacy increases between
❚ The Nature of Sibling Relationships in Ado- preadolescence and middle adolescence, and then de-
lescence Sibling relationships during adolescence clines somewhat. In mixed-sex dyads, the pattern is the
have characteristics that set them apart both from rela- opposite: intimacy drops between preadolescence and
tionships with parents and relationships with friends mid-adolescence, and then increases. In fact, by late
(East, 2009; Kramer & Conger, 2011). Adolescents rate adolescence, mixed-sex siblings are closer than are
their sibling relationships similarly to those with their same-sex siblings, although both types of relationships
parents with regard to measures of companionship and become closer as individuals leave home and move into
importance, but sibling relationships are rated more young adulthood (Scharf, Shulman, & Avigad-Spitz,
like friendships with respect to power, assistance, and 2005; Whiteman, McHale, & Crouter, 2011). Despite
their satisfaction with the relationship (Furman & these changes over time, there is considerable stability
Buhrmester, 1985). in the quality of sibling relationships between child-
hood and adolescence, and siblings who are relatively
closer during middle childhood are relatively closer as
adolescents (J. Dunn, Slomkowski, & Beardsall, 1994).
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
Did you grow up with siblings? How did ❚ A Network of Relationships It is helpful to
your relationship with them change as you think of the adolescent’s interpersonal world as consist-
moved through adolescence? ing of a web of interconnected relationships. The qual-
ity of the parent–adolescent relationship influences the
quality of relations among brothers and sisters (e.g.,
East & Khoo, 2005; K. L. Hoffman, Kiecolt, & Edwards,
2005; L. S. Matthews & Conger, 2004; Paley, Conger, &
Young adolescents often have emotionally charged re- Harold, 2000; Reese-Weber, 2000). Harmony and cohe-
lationships with siblings that are marked by conflict and siveness in the parent–adolescent relationship are as-
rivalry, but also by nurturance and support (Campione- sociated with less sibling conflict and a more positive
Barr & Smetana, 2010). As children mature from child- sibling relationship (East, 2009). In contrast, adoles-
hood to early adolescence, sibling conflict increases cents who experience maternal rejection and negativity
(Brody, Stoneman, & McCoy, 1994), with adolescents re- are more likely to display aggression with siblings.
porting more negativity in their sibling relationships By the same token, children and adolescents learn
than in their relationships with peers (Buhrmester & much about social relationships from sibling interac-
Furman 1990) and less effective conflict resolution than tions, and they bring this knowledge and experience to
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 135 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 135

siblings can influence the development of problems as


well (Bank, Burraston, & Snyder, 2004; K. Conger, Con-
ger, & Scaramella, 1997). For example, younger sisters
of adolescent mothers are relatively more likely to
engage in early sexual activity and to become pregnant
during adolescence (East, Reyes, & Horn, 2007). Sib-
lings also influence each other’s drug use and antisocial
behavior (e.g., Ardelt & Day, 2002; Bullock & Dishion,
2002; Haynie & McHugh, 2003).

RECAP
• Psychologists have identified four basic styles
of parenting during adolescence: authoritative
(responsive and demanding), authoritarian (demanding
but not responsive), indulgent (responsive but not de-
manding), and indifferent (neither demanding nor
responsive).
• In general, adolescents from authoritative homes
fare best on measures of psychological adjustment,
whereas adolescents from indifferent homes fare
worst.
• Studies of parent–adolescent interaction show that
the healthiest families are those that permit the
adolescent to develop a sense of autonomy while
staying emotionally connected to the family.
• Over the course of adolescence, adolescents’ rela-
Because siblings live in close proximity to each other, they have tionships with siblings become more equal but
added opportunities for both positive and negative interactions. more distant and less emotionally intense.
• The quality of the adolescent’s sibling relationships
are affected by the quality of the parent–child rela-
friendships outside the family (East, 2009; Updegraff, tionship and, as well, the quality of adolescent–
McHale, & Crouter, 2000). In poorly functioning families, sibling relationships affects adoles-
aggressive interchanges between unsupervised siblings cents’ mental health and
may provide a training ground within which adolescents peer relationships.
learn, practice, and perfect antisocial and aggressive be-
havior (Criss & Shaw, 2005; J. Snyder, Bank, & Burraston,
2005). The reverse is true as well—the quality of adoles-
cents’ relationships with their friends influences how
they interact with their siblings (Kramer & Kowal, 2005).
The quality of the sibling relationship affects not Behavioral Genetics and
only adolescents’ peer relations but also their adjust-
ment in general (Stocker, Burwell, & Briggs, 2002), es-
Adolescent Development
pecially in households that place a high value on the One topic of interest to researchers who study adoles-
importance of the family, or familism (Soli, McHale, & cents and their siblings concerns how closely siblings
Feinberg, 2009). Positive sibling relationships contrib- resemble each other in various characteristics, such as
ute to adolescents’ academic competence, sociability, intelligence, personality, and interests. Recent advances
autonomy, and self-worth (Kim, McHale, Crouter, & in the study of behavioral genetics have provided new
Osgood, 2007; Yeh & Lempers, 2004). Having a close insights into this issue, as well as a host of others con-
sibling relationship can partially ameliorate the nega- cerning the joint impact of genes and environment on
tive effects of family stress (Waite, Shanahan, Calkins, development.
Keane, & O’Brien, 2011) and of not having friends in
school (East, 2009), and siblings can serve as sources of
advice and guidance (Kolburn Kowal & Blinn-Pike,
2004; C. Tucker, McHale, & Crouter, 2001). Of course, behavioral genetics The scientific study of genetic influences on
behavior.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 136 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

136 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Researchers examine this issue by studying adolescents Studies have revealed that genetic factors strongly
who are twins, to see whether identical twins are more influence many qualities that previously had been as-
similar than fraternal twins (e.g., McGue, Elkins, Walden, sumed to be shaped mainly by the environment.
& Iacono, 2005); by studying adolescents who have been Aggressive behavior is especially driven by genetic
adopted, to see whether adopted adolescents are more like influences, although shared and nonshared environ-
their biological parents than like their adoptive parents mental influences on adolescents’ antisocial behavior,
(e.g., Abrahamson, Baker, & Caspi, 2002); and by studying including aggression, also have been found (S. A. Burt,
adolescents and their siblings in stepfamilies, to see McGue, Kreuger, & Iacono, 2007; Deater-Deckard &
whether similarity between siblings varies with their bio- Plomin 1999; Spotts, Neiderhiser, Hetherington, &
logical relatedness (e.g., Neiderhiser et al., 2004). In addi- Reiss, 2001). Genetic factors also have been linked to
tion to examining whether and how much given traits are various emotional and behavioral problems in adoles-
genetically versus environmentally determined, research- cence, such as risk for suicide and depression (Jacobson
ers also ask how these two sets of factors interact (for & Rowe, 1999) and alcohol dependence (Dick, 2011).
example, whether the same environment affects people Research also has found strong genetic influences on
with different genetic makeups in different ways, or adolescent competence, self-image, and self-conceptions
whether people with different genetic makeups evoke dif- (S. McGuire et al., 1999).
ferent reactions from their environment) (W. A. Collins, Intelligence in adolescence (as indexed by IQ) is also
Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000). under strong genetic control, with genetic influences
compounding over time and ultimately becoming more
influential than the family environment. Interestingly,
GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL
the maturation of brain regions associated with complex
INFLUENCES ON ADOLESCENT
reasoning becomes increasingly under genetic control
DEVELOPMENT
over time (Lenroot & Giedd, 2008). Genetic influences
In studies of genetic and environmental influences on on school performance, in contrast to intelligence, are
adolescent development, researchers distinguish be- more modest (Loehlin, Neiderhiser, & Reiss, 2005).
tween two types of environmental influences. Shared It is important to emphasize, however, that the same
environmental influences are factors in the environ- genes may have different effects on adolescent develop-
ment that individuals, such as siblings, have in common ment in different contexts (W. A. Collins et al., 2000).
and that make the individuals similar in personality and Consider the following findings from several studies:
behavior. Nonshared environmental influences are
factors in the environments of individuals that are not Genetic influences on sexual activity are stronger
similar and that, as a consequence, make the individuals when adolescents attend schools in which a higher
different from one another (Plomin & Daniels, 1987). In proportion of students are having sex at an early
studies of siblings, nonshared environmental influences age (Guo, Tong, & Cai, 2008).
can include factors within the family as well as outside Genetic influences on antisocial behavior are stronger
of it. For example, if two siblings are treated very differ- among adolescents who have delinquent peers (Beaver,
ently by their parents, this would be considered a non- DeLisi, Wright, & Vaughn, 2009; Latendresse et al.,
shared environmental influence. Indeed, some evidence 2011) and weaker among those who do well in school
suggests that this sort of nonshared environment—that (Johnson, McGue, & Iacono, 2009).
is, the nonshared environment that results from people Genetic influences on depression are stronger
having different experiences within what would appear among adolescents exposed to family conflict
to be the same context—may be the most important (F. Rice, Harold, Shelton, & Thapar, 2006).
(Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). Studies indicate that Genetic influences on intelligence are stronger in
both genetic and nonshared environmental influences, families with highly educated parents, because the
such as differential parental treatment, peer relations, influence of genes on intelligence is stronger in
and school experiences, are particularly strong in ado- environments that provide more learning opportu-
lescence. In contrast, shared environmental factors, such nities, allowing children to benefit from their
as family socioeconomic status or the neighborhood in genetic advantages (Tucker-Drob & Harden, 2012).
which two siblings live, are less influential (McGue, Genetic influences on smoking are stronger among
Sharma, & Benson, 1996; Pike et al., 1996). adolescents whose parents do not monitor them
closely (Dick, Viken, Purcell, Kaprio, Pulkkinen
shared environmental influences Nongenetic influences that et al., 2007) (see Figure 4.6)
make individuals living in the same family similar to each other.
In other words, genes may shape tendencies, but whether
nonshared environmental influences The nongenetic these tendencies are actualized often depends on the
influences in individuals’ lives that make them different from environment.
people they live with.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 137 21/03/13 9:53 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 137

1 adolescents, especially as they get older, generally ap-


Additive genetic influence
0.9 preciate the reasons for parents treating siblings differ-
Common environmental influence
Proportion of variance
0.8 Unique environmental influence ently and that sibling relationships are strained only
0.7 when this differential treatment is perceived as unfair
0.6 (Feinberg, Neiderhiser, Simmens, Reiss, & Hethering-
0.5
ton, 2000; Kowal & Kramer, 1997).
0.4
Oddly, treating siblings differently may actually be a
0.3
0.2
good thing for parents to do, so long as each sibling is
0.1 treated well. There is some evidence that when siblings
0 are treated differently by their parents, they get along
⫺3 ⫺2.5 ⫺2 ⫺1.5 ⫺1 ⫺0.5 0 0.5 1 better—presumably because this differential treatment
Parental monitoring makes them feel unique and lessens sibling rivalry
Low High
(Feinberg, McHale, Crouter, & Cumsille, 2003). Per-
FIGURE 4.6 Genetic influences on smoking are much haps you have a brother or sister whom you resemble
stronger among adolescents whose parents do not monitor more than you’d like—so much, in fact, that you’ve had
their behavior closely. (Dick et al., 2007)
a hard time establishing your own personality. When
siblings feel this way, they often deliberately try to be
different from each other—a phenomenon known as
WHY ARE SIBLINGS OFTEN
sibling deidentification. An adolescent whose brother
SO DIFFERENT?
or sister is a star athlete, for instance, may shun sports
Given the fact that the family context is an important and focus on cultivating other types of talents, perhaps
influence on development, how can we explain the fact in academics or in the arts, a strategy that will diminish
that siblings who grow up in the same family often turn feelings of competition. Similarly, although having an
out to be very different from one another? One answer is older sibling who is academically successful seems to
that siblings actually may have very different family expe- promote younger adolescents’ achievement, too much
riences, both because they are treated differently by their academic support from an older sibling may actually
parents and because they often perceive similar experi- undermine a younger adolescent’s success in school
ences in different ways (Feinberg & Hetherington, 2000; (Bouchey, Shoulberg, Jodl, & Eccles, 2010).
C. Tucker, McHale, & Crouter, 2003b). For example, one In addition to having different experiences inside the
brother may describe his family as very close-knit, while family, siblings also may have very different experiences
another may have experienced it as very distant. One girl outside the family—at school, with friends, in the neigh-
may describe her family life as plagued with argument borhood. These contexts provide yet another source of
and conflict, while her sister describes it as peaceful and nonshared environmental influence. Because factors
agreeable. In other words, even though we may assume other than the family environment shape adolescent de-
that children growing up in the same family have shared velopment and behavior, siblings may turn out very dif-
the same environment, this is not necessarily the case. ferent if they have divergent experiences outside the home.
As you might expect, unequal treatment from moth-
ers or fathers often creates conflict among siblings and is RECAP
linked to a variety of problems, such as depression, anti- • Researchers have examined genetic and environ-
social behavior, and early pregnancy (East & Jacobson, mental influences by studying identical and frater-
2003; Reiss et al., 1995). Studies also show that differ- nal twins, adolescents who have been adopted, and
ences in siblings’ real and perceived family experiences adolescents and their siblings in stepfamilies.
are related to different patterns of development (Barrett- • Researchers distinguish between two types of
Singer & Weinstein, 2000; Mekos, Hetherington, & Reiss, environmental influences: shared environmental
1996). In general, better-adjusted adolescents are more influences, which make individuals in the same
likely than their siblings to report that they had close environment similar, and nonshared environmental
relationships with their parents, that their relations with influences, which make them different.
brothers or sisters were friendly, that they were involved • Although genetic influences on a wide range of
in family decision making, and that they were given a characteristics have been documented, adolescent
high level of responsibility around the house (Daniels, development is the product of the interaction
Dunn, Furstenberg, & Plomin, 1985; L. Shanahan, between genetics and environment.
McHale, Crouter, & Osgood, 2008).
Despite this evidence for differential treatment and
sibling rivalry Competition between siblings, often for parental
its potentially adverse influence on adolescent develop- attention.
ment, adolescents report that 75% of parental treat-
ment is not differential. Interestingly, studies show that sibling deidentification The process through which siblings
deliberately try to be different from each other.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 138 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

138 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

• Siblings often differ from each other because living in single-parent households actually live with more
they have different experiences both inside and than one adult, often with the unmarried partner of the
outside the family and because they child’s parent (Fields, 2003). When youngsters live with
sometimes deliberately only one of their biological parents, either in single-
strive to be parent or in two-parent households, it is usually with the
different. mother; only about 15% of children who live with one
parent live with their father (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012).
There are important racial and ethnic differences in
these patterns of family life. Black youngsters are far
The Adolescent’s Family more likely than other youngsters to experience paren-
tal divorce and to be born outside of marriage, but they
in a Changing Society are far less likely to experience their parents’ remar-
riage. As a consequence, Black adolescents spend longer
THE CHANGED AND CHANGING periods of time in single-parent households than do
NATURE OF FAMILY LIFE other adolescents. Whereas close to 85% of all Asian
In America and in many other industrialized countries, children, 75% of all White children, and 60% of all His-
the family has undergone a series of profound changes panic children live with two parents, only 33% of Black
during the past half century that have diversified its children do (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012).
form and, as a result, adolescents’ daily experiences. ❚ Remarriage Because more than three-fourths
Increased rates of divorce and childbearing outside of of divorced men and two-thirds of divorced women
marriage, as well as a changing international economy remarry, the majority of youngsters whose parents
and accelerating geographic mobility, have dramati- separate also experience living in a stepfamily at some
cally altered the world in which children and adoles- time. And, because the divorce rate is higher for sec-
cents grow up. Although some of the most striking ond marriages than first marriages, the majority of
trends in family life slowed during the early 1990s, they youth whose parents remarry will experience a second
did not reverse by any means. The divorce rate and pro- divorce. Moreover, because divorces generally occur
portion of single-parent families, which skyrocketed faster in remarriages—one-fourth happen within five
during the 1970s and 1980s, stabilized at their histori- years—many children confront a second divorce be-
cally high levels at the beginning of the 1990s and have fore they have finished adapting to having a stepparent
changed relatively little since then (U.S. Census Bureau, (Hetherington, Henderson, & Reiss, 1999).
2012). Diversity in family forms is also reflected in the
sizable numbers of adolescents who are raised by adop- ❚ Poverty Approximately 17% of all adolescents in
tive parents, lesbian and gay parents, and foster parents. the United States grow up in abject poverty, and an ad-
ditional 21% grow up in low-income families (National
❚ Divorce The U.S. divorce rate increased mark- Center for Children in Poverty, 2010). Although the
edly beginning in the 1960s, rising steadily, and at times proportion of children living in poverty declined
rapidly, until 1980. Although this rate declined during during the 1990s, it began to rise again in 2000 (see
the 1990s, it has leveled off, and is now estimated that Figure 4.7). Perhaps more importantly, the gap between
about 40% of all first marriages end in divorce. About the very poor and the very wealthy has widened sub-
40% of all American children will experience their par- stantially in the past 30 years (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012).
ents’ divorce (Emery, Beam, & Rowen, 2011). Because
most divorces occur early in a marriage, adolescents are
more likely than children to grow up in a divorced fam- Adolescents Living in Low-Income
and Poor Families, 2000–2009
ily than to experience their parents’ divorce. It is impor-
tant to keep in mind, however, that the divorce rate 45
varies considerably among people with different levels 40
35 38%
of education—divorce is much less common among
30 33%
Percent (%)

college graduates than nongraduates (Hurley, 2005). 25 Near Poor


100–200% Federal Poverty Level
20
❚ Single Parenthood In addition to adolescents
15 17%
who live in a single-parent household as a consequence of
10 14% Poor
their parents’ divorce, a sizable percentage of youngsters 5 Less than 100% Federal Poverty Level
will spend time in a single-parent household from birth; 0
indeed, today, 40 percent of all children are born outside ’00 ’01 ’02 ’03 ’04 ’05 ’06 ’07 ’08 ’09
of marriage, only half of whom are born to a cohabiting FIGURE 4.7 The proportion of children growing up in pov-
couple (Emery et al., 2011). Keep in mind, though, that a erty has increased during the last decades. (National Center for
substantial number of adolescents who are classified as Children in Poverty, 2010)
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 139 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 139

Poverty, as we noted in Chapter 3, is much more likely in the early 1980s, despite the continued “demise” of the
to touch the lives of non-White adolescents; approxi- family (see Furstenberg, 1990). Moreover, the one group
mately 30% of Black and Hispanic children grow up of young people whose psychological and behavioral
in poverty. One reason for the large disparity in poverty profile has improved most markedly in the past 20
rates between White and non-White children, in fact, years—poor, minority youngsters—experienced the most
is the racial disparity in rates of single parenthood: dramatic “decline” in family life during this time.
Because non-White children are more likely to be Second, because the conditions under which divorce,
raised in single-parent homes, they are more likely to be single parenthood, and remarriage take place vary tre-
poor. mendously from family to family, it is hard to generalize
To what extent has the changed nature of the American about their effects on adolescents. (In contrast, it is rela-
family changed the nature of adolescent development? tively easy to generalize about the effects of poverty on
How do divorce, single parenthood, remarriage, poverty, adolescents, which are almost always negative.) For some
and new family forms affect adolescents’ development? young people, divorce may bring a welcome end to family
Many individuals are certain that the answer to these conflict and tension; for others, it may be extremely dis-
questions is, “For the worse.” But before we jump to this ruptive psychologically. Some young people living with
conclusion, it is important to raise two considerations. only their mother actually see their father more often than
First, although increases in adolescents’ problems between do their peers who live in homes where the father ostensi-
1950 and 1980—as indexed by such indicators as drug bly is present (D. N. Hawkins, Amato, & King, 2006).
use, suicide, and poor school achievement, for example— It is also the case that broad categories of family
occurred alongside many of these shifts in family life, it is structures (e.g., “intact,” “single-parent,” etc.) often
difficult to say that the family changes caused the changes combine types of households that are very different in
in adolescent behavior. In particular, adolescents’ psycho- other respects. Take a look at Figure 4.8, which shows
logical and behavioral well-being took a turn for the better rates of antisocial and delinquent behavior across

4.0
Nonintact family
3.5
Intact family
Mean variety of antisocial behavior

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

0.0
parents, blended
parents, nuclear

parents, cohabiting

Biological mother and

Biological mother and

Biological father and

Biological father and

Biological father only

Grandparent(s)

Other older

Adoptive or foster

Other living situation


Two biological

Biological mother
Two biological

relative(s)
cohabiting partner
cohabiting partner
Two biological

stepmother

parent(s)
stepfather

only

Two biological parents One biological parent No biological parent

Notes: N ⫽ 8,330. Estimates are weighted. The dashed lines represent the means for a traditional binary measure of family structure, in
which an “intact” family is headed by two biological parents, irrespective of whether it is a nuclear, blended, or cohabiting family. A
“nonintact” family includes all other family living situations.
FIGURE 4.8 Adolescents’ antisocial behavior varies as a function of their family structure. (Apel & Kaukinen, 2008)
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 140 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

140 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

divorced homes have more difficulties than those from


nondivorced homes, the explanation for this effect is
far more complicated than the conventional wisdom
that “Two parents are better than one” or “All children
need a mother and a father.” Five sets of findings have
questioned these simple assertions.

❚ The Effect of Divorce Is Small in Magni-


tude First, although divorce clearly diminishes
youngsters’ well-being, the impact of divorce itself is
small (Amato & Keith, 1991), and it is possible in a
study with a large sample for a small effect to be statisti-
cally significant. Thus, whereas there are statistically
significant differences between children from divorced
versus nondivorced homes in school achievement,
behavior problems, psychosocial adjustment, and
family relations—all favoring individuals from nondi-
Poverty often diminishes the quality of parenting in a household, vorced homes—the absolute difference in the groups’
which adversely affects adolescent development. scores is seldom substantial. In general, the effects of
divorce tend to be stronger among school-aged indi-
viduals than preschoolers or college students. One es-
pecially intriguing finding is that the effects of divorce
different types of households (Apel & Kaukinen, are smaller among youngsters from the United States
2008). The first three columns all show “intact” fami- than among those from other countries. The explana-
lies, where adolescents live with both biological par- tion: Divorce is more common in the United States
ents. But as you can see, adolescents whose biological than abroad, and as a consequence, American children
parents are cohabiting have a rate of antisocial behav- from divorced homes are less likely to be stigmatized
ior that is 40% higher than those whose biological par- and more likely to have access to psychological services,
ents are married and a rate of antisocial behavior that such as counseling, that may attenuate the negative im-
is about the same as adolescents who live in house- pact of family disruption.
holds with a biological mother and no other adult.
Similarly, adolescents from “intact” families who live ❚ Quality Matters Second, the quality of the re-
with both biological parents in families where one or lationships the young person has with the important
both of the parents has a child from a prior marriage adults in her or his life matters more than the num-
generally have a fairly high rate of problems, despite ber of parents present in the home (C. Buchanan,
their living with both biological parents (Halpern- Maccoby, & Dornbusch, 1996; Mandara & Murray,
Meekin & Tach, 2008). In other words, variations 2000). Adolescents from stepfamilies, for example,
within different family structures are likely to be more have as many, if not more, problems than those from
important than the differences among them. This is single-parent homes, even though adolescents in step-
readily apparent when we look at how adolescents are families have two parents in the home (Hetherington
affected by parental divorce. et al., 1999; J. P. Hoffman, 2002; Jeynes, 1999). In addi-
tion, youngsters from single-parent families that have
not experienced divorce (for example, youngsters who
ADOLESCENTS AND DIVORCE
have lost a parent through death, or youngsters with a
At one time, any discussion of adolescents and divorce single mother who never married) have fewer difficul-
would likely have started with the assumption that ties than their counterparts from divorced or remar-
living with one parent was not as good as living with ried homes (Demo & Acock, 1996). Finally, adolescents
two and, consequently, that children whose parents in two-parent homes do not always have warm and
divorced would be at a distinct disadvantage relative close relationships with their parents. Indeed, ado-
to those whose parents remained married. Over time, lescents living in father-absent homes have higher
however, researchers’ ideas about divorce and its self-esteem than adolescents who live in two-parent
impact on adolescents have changed dramatically, homes but who feel that their father has little interest
as new and better studies have challenged, clarified, in them (J. Clark & Barber, 1994). In fact, adolescents
and tempered the conclusions of past research. While in divorced, single-parent families describe their par-
most social scientists still agree that adolescents from ents as friendlier than do adolescents whose parents
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 141 21/03/13 9:06 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 141

are married (Asmussen & Larson, 1991) and are in a


relatively more positive mood when with their family
than when with friends (Larson & Gillman, 1996). Ad-
olescents and their parents argue less often in single-
parent households, perhaps because single parents
tend to be more permissive, which may make for less
parent–adolescent conflict (Smetana, Yau, Restrepo, &
Braeges, 1991).

❚ Adaptation to Divorce Third, it is the process


of going through a divorce, not the resulting family
structure, that matters most for adolescents’ mental
health (C. Buchanan et al., 1996; Hetherington,
Bridges, & Insabella, 1998). In general, most studies
show that the period of greatest difficulty for most
adolescents is right around the time of the divorce it-
self (Jeynes, 2001). Although many young people
show signs of difficulty immediately after their par- One reason adolescents with divorced parents are more likely to
ents divorce—problems in school, behavior problems, have problems than their peers is that divorce often exposes chil-
and increased anxiety—by the time two years have dren to marital conflict, which adversely affects their mental
passed, the majority of these children have adjusted to health.
the change and behave comparably to their peers
whose biological parents have remained married
exposure to such stressors as marital conflict or disor-
(Hetherington et al., 1998). Although adolescents
ganized parenting (S. A. Burt, Barnes, McGue, & Iacono,
whose parents have divorced have, on average, more
2008; Frisco, Muller, & Frank, 2007), genetic differences
problems than those whose parents remain married,
between adolescents whose parents have divorced and
the vast majority of individuals with divorced parents
those whose parents have not may account for part of
do not have significant problems (Hetherington et al.,
this. Adults who divorce are different from those who
1998).
do not with respect to many traits that have strong ge-
netic origins—such as predispositions to different sorts
❚ Conflict and Stress Fourth, research has linked of emotional and behavioral problems, like depression
the adverse consequences of divorce to a number of or substance abuse—and these traits are passed on
factors not specifically due to having a single parent from parents to children (D’Onofrio et al., 2006). In
(Crosnoe & Cavanaugh, 2010). These include the expo- other words, one reason that adolescents from divorced
sure of the children to marital conflict (Amato & Chea- homes have more problems than their peers is that they
dle, 2008), disorganized or disrupted parenting (Linver, have inherited from their divorced parents some of the
Brooks-Gunn, & Kohen, 2002), and increased stress in same traits that may have influenced their parents’ deci-
the household, often due to loss of income (Pong & Ju, sion to get divorced in the first place (O’Connor, Caspi,
2000; Sun & Li, 2002). As you will read, adolescents liv- DeFries, & Plomin, 2000).
ing in two-parent families in which no divorce has oc-
curred are also harmed by marital conflict, suboptimal ❚ Individual Differences in the Effects of
parenting (especially parenting that is too lenient, too Divorce There also are differences among children
harsh, or inconsistent), and loss of income. In other in how vulnerable they are to the short-term effects of
words, the adverse temporary effects of divorce or re- divorce. In general, immediate problems are relatively
marriage on youngsters’ well-being appear to reflect the more common among boys, younger children, children
heightened conflict, disorganization, and stress sur- with a difficult temperament, children who do not have
rounding the event, not the divorce or remarriage per supportive relationships with adults outside the imme-
se. The most important pathway through which divorce diate family, and youngsters whose parents divorce dur-
may adversely affect adolescent adjustment is via its di- ing the time of the transition into adolescence as
rect and indirect impact on the quality of parenting to opposed to before or after (Davies & Windle, 2001;
which the child is exposed (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001; K. Rodgers & Rose, 2002). Because early adolescence is
Jeong & Chun, 2010). a time during which individuals seem to be especially
sensitive to stress, parental divorce at this time may
❚ Genetic Influences Finally, although some of have a relatively stronger impact (Ivanova, Mills, &
the apparent effects of parental divorce are the result of Veenstra, 2011).
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 142 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

142 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Studies of Black youth indicate that social support


from friends and relatives outside the family may be an
especially important resource for inner-city children Marital Ineffective Adolescent
growing up in single-parent homes (Lamborn & conflict parenting problems
Nguyen, 2004; Pallock & Lamborn, 2006; Salem,
Zimmerman, & Notaro, 1998; R. Taylor & Roberts, FIGURE 4.9 The effects of exposure to marital conflict
are both direct and indirect through its negative impact on
1995). Support from kin, in particular, appears to in-
parenting.
crease single parents’ effectiveness in child rearing, and
this, in turn, tends to limit adolescents’ misbehavior.
This support can come from relatives living apart 1997). Several conclusions have emerged from this
from, or within, the adolescent’s household. For ex- research. First, children are more adversely affected by
ample, several studies of Black youngsters have found marital conflict when they are aware of it than when it
that children growing up in home environments that is more covert (Harold & Conger, 1997). For this rea-
include a grandparent as well as a parent fare signifi- son, marital conflict is particularly harmful when it is
cantly better than those growing up in single-parent especially hostile, physically violent, or frightening
homes or in stepfamilies. These studies, as well as oth- (Buehler et al., 1998; Gordis, Margolin, & St. John,
ers, remind us that relatives other than parents may 1997; Harold & Conger, 1997). Exposure to overt mari-
play an extremely important role in adolescents’ lives tal conflict and domestic violence has been linked to a
(Henderson, Hayslip, Sanders, & Louden, 2009; Rich- wide range of adolescent problems, including depres-
ardson, 2009), especially within ethnic groups that his- sion (especially in girls) and aggression and delin-
torically have placed a great deal of importance on quency (especially in boys) (Davies & Lindsay, 2004; Su,
maintaining close ties to extended family members. Simons, & Simons, 2011).
Single parenthood and extensive contact with the ex- Second, children are more negatively affected when
tended family have been salient features of Black fam- the marital conflict leads to feelings of insecurity or
ily life for more than 100 years (Ruggles, 1994), which self-blame (Fosco & Grych, 2010). Adolescents who
may explain why, generally speaking, the relations be- blame themselves for their parents’ conflict, whose feel-
tween adjustment and divorce are generally weaker ings of security are challenged, or who are drawn into
among Black adolescents than among adolescents their parents’ arguments are more likely to feel anxious,
from other backgrounds (Heard, 2007; Kowaleski- depressed, and distressed (Buehler, Franck, & Cook,
Jones & Dunifon, 2006). 2009; Buehler & Welsh, 2009; Grych, Raynor, & Fosco,
2004; Tschann et al., 2002).
Finally, marital conflict more adversely affects the
THE SPECIFIC IMPACT OF MARITAL adolescent when the conflict disrupts the quality of the
CONFLICT parent–child relationship (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001).
Adolescents are directly affected by exposure to their par-
Although divorce is generally associated with short-
ents’ conflict, to be sure, but several studies have found as
term difficulties for the adolescent, several studies show
well that tension between spouses spills over into the par-
that at least some of the differences between adolescents
ent–child relationship, making mothers and fathers more
from divorced versus nondivorced homes were present
hostile, more irritable, and less effective as parents (Brad-
before the parents divorced (Sun, 2001). One explana-
ford, Vaughn, & Barber, 2008; Buehler, Benson, & Gerard,
tion for this is that children in the households that later
2006; Cui & Conger, 2008; Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005;
divorced were exposed to higher levels of marital
A. Mahoney, Donelly, Boxer, & Lewis, 2003; T. Schofield
unhappiness and conflict and strained parent–child re-
et al., 2009). As you will read later in this chapter, adoles-
lationships, both of which are known to increase chil-
cents who perceive their parents as hostile or uncaring
dren’s difficulties (Amato & Booth, 1996; Forehand,
are more likely to report a wide range of emotional and
Neighbors, Devine, & Armistead, 1994) (see Figure 4.9).
behavioral problems than are their peers.
Children’s maladjustment, in turn, adversely affects the
quality of their parents’ marriage, creating a vicious
cycle (Cui, Donnellan, & Conger, 2007).
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
The recognition that exposure to marital conflict,
apart from and in addition to divorce itself, has harmful Based on the available
effects on children’s development has prompted many research, what advice
researchers to study why and how the quality of the would you give to par-
adolescent’s parents’ marriage affects teenagers’ mental ents of teenagers who
health and behavior (Cui, Conger, & Lorenz, 2005; Erel are considering divorce?
& Burman, 1995; S. Feldman & Fisher, 1997; Fincham,
1994; Harold & Conger, 1997; M. Rogers & Holmbeck,
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 143 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 143

THE LONGER-TERM EFFECTS CUSTODY, CONTACT, AND CONFLICT


OF DIVORCE FOLLOWING DIVORCE
Social scientists have also looked at the longer-term After a divorce, do adolescents fare better or worse in
(more than two or three years) consequences of di- different kinds of living arrangements? Does contact
vorce. These studies, for example, might look at ele- with the nonresidential parent contribute to the adoles-
mentary school children whose parents divorced cent’s well-being?
when their child was in preschool, or at adolescents Studies indicate that it is the nature of the relation-
whose parents split up during the youngster’s elemen- ship between the adolescent’s divorced parents, and not
tary school years. Presumably, if the adverse effects of which one he or she lives with, that makes a difference
divorce are solely attributable to the immediate prob- (Booth, Scott, & King, 2010; D. Downey, Ainsworth-
lems of adjusting to a new household structure, or Darnell, & Dufur, 1998; Hetherington et al., 1998). In
due to exposure to intense marital conflict before and the years immediately following a divorce, children may
during the divorce, these effects will dissipate within a fare a bit better in the custody of the parent of the same
few years. sex, but these effects are not long-lasting; over time, both
Research on the longer-term consequences of paren- male and female adolescents fare equally well either in
tal divorce has yielded very interesting findings. Some dual custody or in sole custody (C. Buchanan et al.,
studies show that individuals whose parents divorce dur- 1996), a finding that was replicated recently in a study of
ing preadolescence and adolescence often demonstrate lesbian couples who have separated (Gartrell, Bos,
adjustment difficulties later, even after two or three years Peyser, Deck, & Rodas, 2011). More important, espe-
(e.g., Hetherington, 1993). Some research indicates that cially for adolescents who have dual residences, are two
individuals whose parents divorced during childhood or factors: whether the ex-spouses continue to fight and
adolescence continue to have adjustment problems place the child between them, and whether the adoles-
well into their 30s (Cherlin, Chase-Lansdale, & McRae, cent’s discipline is consistent across the two households.
1998). These effects do not appear to be ameliorated by Adolescents whose parents have a congenial, cooperative
parental remarriage; adolescents from stepfamilies score relationship and who receive consistent and appropriate
similarly, or worse, on measures of longer-term adjust- discipline from both homes report less emotional
ment, as do adolescents from single-parent, divorced difficulty and fewer behavioral problems than those
homes (Hetherington et al., 1998). whose parents fight or are inconsistent with each other
(C. Buchanan et al., 1996; Coiro & Emery, 1996).
❚ Sleeper Effects To what can we attribute these Adolescents whose parents have divorced also vary
“sleeper” effects—effects of divorce that may not be ap- in the extent to which they have contact with the parent
parent until much later in the child’s development? Two they no longer live with, typically their father. Contact
possible explanations come to mind. The first is that the between adolescents and their father following a di-
ways in which adjustment difficulties might be ex- vorce usually diminishes very quickly after the father
pressed may not surface until adolescence. For example, moves out and continues to decline over time, especially
social scientists believe that increased drug use and among men who remarry or enter into a new romantic
higher rates of early pregnancy are consequences of the relationship (Stephens, 1996). Generally speaking, sons
lower level of parental monitoring in divorced homes and daughters who have regular post-divorce contact
(M. R. Moore & Chase-Lansdale, 2001). But because with their father have fewer problems (e.g., Booth et al.,
younger children—even poorly monitored ones—are 2010; Coley & Medeiros, 2007; Mitchell, Booth, & King,
unlikely to use drugs or be sexually active, no matter 2009), but not all studies have reached this conclusion,
what their family background, the effect of the poor and some have concluded that it is healthy adolescent
monitoring is not seen until adolescence, when indi- functioning that is influencing fathers’ involvement,
viduals might begin using drugs and having sex. rather than the reverse (D. N. Hawkins, Amato, & King,
A second explanation concerns the particular devel- 2007). More important than the father’s involvement in
opmental challenges of adolescence (S. J. Steinberg, Da- and of itself may be the level of conflict between the di-
vila, & Fincham, 2006). Adolescence is a time when vorced parents and the nature of the adolescent’s rela-
individuals first begin experimenting with intimate tionship with the father before the divorce (S. D. Stewart,
sexual relationships. If having one’s parents divorce or 2003; Videon, 2002). Adolescents benefit from contact
being exposed to marital conflict affects one’s concep- with their nonresidential parent when conflict between
tions of relationships or views of romantic commit- their parents is minimal, but suffer from such contact
ment (Woodward, Fergusson, & Belsky, 2000), it makes when parental conflict is intense. Similarly, adolescents
sense that some of the effects of early parental divorce benefit when they have frequent contact with a non-
will not be manifested until the adolescent begins dat- residential parent with whom they had a close relation-
ing and gets seriously involved in romantic relation- ship when their parents were married, but suffer from
ships (Donahue et al., 2010). contact with one with whom they didn’t get along prior
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 144 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

144 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

to the divorce. One consistent finding, though, is that calling my dad and yelling at him because I think he
financial support from fathers is associated with less doesn’t care about it. (11-year-old daughter, quoted in
problem behavior and higher academic achievement Koerner et al., 2004, p. 52)
(Menning, 2002). My mom constantly rags on and on about how my dad is
such a jerk and won’t pay her as much as she thinks he
❚ What Divorced Parents Tell Their Teenagers should. It disturbs me incredibly because my dad is very
Psychologist Susan Koerner and colleagues have exam- kind when I visit him and I don’t think my mom is fair.
ined the sorts of disclosure that take place between re- (15-year-old son, quoted in Koerner et al., 2004, p. 52)
cently divorced mothers and their children (Koerner, My mom talks to me about how we don’t have extra
Jacobs, & Raymond, 2000; Koerner, Wallace, Lehman, money. I don’t really like to hear how little money we
Lee, & Escalante, 2004). To the researchers’ surprise, have. . . . I feel sad and kinda angry when we talk about
mothers were equally likely to talk to sons and daugh- money cause before the divorce this topic never, really
ters, and their conversations were similar, consistent came up to talk about. . . . (13-year-old daughter, quoted
with a point made earlier—namely, that for the most in Koerner et al., 2004, p. 52)
part adolescent males and females are treated similarly
by their parents. Koerner has been especially interested REMARRIAGE
in two topics of conversation: the mother’s complaints
about and anger toward her ex-husband and the moth- Adolescents growing up in stepfamilies—especially if
er’s concerns about finances (both common concerns the remarriage occurred during early adolescence rather
among recently divorced women). than childhood—often have more problems than their
Among mothers who disclosed these sorts of feelings, peers, a finding that holds regardless of whether the
an important motive for doing so turned out not to be stepparents are legally married or cohabiting (S. Brown
the mother’s need for a confidante, but rather her desire & Rinelli, 2010; Manning & Lamb, 2003). For example,
to shape her daughter’s impression of her and the cir- youngsters growing up in single-parent homes are more
cumstances surrounding the divorce. Here’s one example: likely than those in intact homes to be involved in delin-
quent activity, but adolescents in stepfamilies are even
I talk to her about anything. Complaints I have about her more at risk for this sort of problem behavior than are
father. [Alison] thinks the divorce is all my fault. Her Dad
adolescents in single-parent families (Dornbusch et al.,
has been absent for two years. He called on occasion and
1985; Steinberg, 1987). This results, in part but not en-
sent presents on Christmas and B-Days. While he was
gone Alison turned him into some sort of God in her tirely, because they are exposed to a “double dose” of
mind. Now that he has returned to the area he wants little marital conflict—normal, everyday conflict between
or nothing to do with his kids. When Alison complains, the parent and stepparent and additional conflict be-
I make statements like, “Alison, you’re starting to see the tween ex-spouses (T. Hanson, McLanahan, & Thom-
side of your Dad that I was married to” or “He never gave son, 1996; MacDonald & DeMaris, 1995)—and because
me any time either.” (Koerner et al., 2000, p. 305) they are exposed to a new set of potentially difficult is-
sues that arise in the blending of children from two dif-
Not all mothers shared their feelings about these topics
ferent marriages (Hetherington et al., 1999).
with their child. As one put it:
Like the short-term effects of divorce, the short-term
I try really hard not to say anything negative about my effects of remarriage vary among children, although
ex-husband to my children. We agreed when we got di- not necessarily in the same ways. In general, girls have
vorced to always do what was best for our children. So more difficulty in adjusting to remarriage than do boys,
far, so good. (Koerner et al., 2000, p. 305) and older children have more difficulty than younger
Consistent with other research indicating that ado- ones (Hetherington, 1993; V. Lee, Burkham, Zimiles, &
lescents fare worse when they are drawn into their di- Ladewski, 1994). One explanation for this is that both
vorced parents’ conflict, adolescents whose mothers boys and younger children have more to gain from their
complain to them about their ex-husbands or discuss mother’s remarriage than do girls or older children,
their financial concerns report more psychological dis- who may have become accustomed to having a single
tress, in the form of anxiety, depression, tension, and mother (Hetherington, 1991). Over time, however, gen-
psychosomatic complaints. Adolescents are especially der differences in adjustment to remarriage disappear,
likely to worry about family finances and the impact of and in remarriages that last more than five years, the
the divorce on their future (Koerner, Korn, Dennison, adjustment of male and female children is similar
& Witthoft, 2011). As several adolescents explained: (Hetherington et al., 1999). One interesting finding, es-
pecially in light of the growing number of young adults
I was thinking, my gosh, my dad doesn’t care about me— who depend on their parents’ financial support, is that
he’s not paying it [child support]! I felt like going and remarried parents and stepparents are less inclined than
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 145 21/03/13 9:07 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 145

other parents to provide money to their children over


the transition to adulthood (Aquilino, 2005).

❚ Difficulties Adjusting to Parental Remar-


riage Remarriage during the adolescent years may be
extremely stressful when families are unable to accom-
modate the new stepparent relationship. Given what we
know about family reorganization and change during
adolescence, having to integrate a new type of relation-
ship into a family system that is already undergoing a
great deal of change may be more than some families
can cope with. Many adolescents find it difficult to ad-
just to a new authority figure moving into the house-
hold, especially if that person has different ideas about
rules and discipline, and particularly if the new author-
ity figure is not legally married to the child’s biological
parent (Hetherington et al., 1999). This appears to be
especially true when the adolescent in question is al-
Studies show that each time a family goes through a marital transi-
ready somewhat vulnerable, either because of previous tion, such as divorce or remarriage, the adolescent is placed at some
psychological problems or because of a recent divorce risk for minor adjustment difficulties.
or other stressful event.
By the same token, many stepparents find it diffi-
cult to join a family and not be accepted immediately marital transitions should help parents learn and
by the children as the new parent. Stepparents may adopt this parenting style (Hetherington et al., 1999).
wonder why love is not forthcoming from their step- One factor that seems to make a very big difference
children, who often act critical, resistant, and sulky in the adjustment of children in stepfamilies is the na-
(Vuchinich, Hetherington, Vuchinich, & Clingempeel, ture of the relationship they have with their noncusto-
1991). Although many stepfathers and their adolescent dial parent—that is, the biological parent with whom
stepchildren establish positive relationships, the lack they do not live. Children in stepfamilies fare better
of a biological connection between stepparent and when there is consistency in discipline between their
stepchild—coupled with the stresses associated with custodial and noncustodial parents and when they have
divorce and remarriage—may make this relationship a good relationship with the noncustodial parent, espe-
especially vulnerable to problems (Hetherington et al., cially in the years immediately following the remarriage
1999). Adolescents in remarried households fare better (Gunnoe, 1994). Having a close relationship with the
when their stepparent can establish a consistent, sup- noncustodial parent does not appear to undermine the
portive, authoritative style of discipline (Hetherington relationship with the custodial parent (C. Buchanan &
et al., 1999). Maccoby, 1993), nor does it undermine the relationship
This research underscores the need—particularly as between the adolescent and the stepparent (Yuan &
remarriage becomes a more common part of Ameri- Hamilton, 2006). Indeed, studies find that adolescents
can family life—to understand the special problems who feel close to both their father and stepfather have
that may arise in the course of family reorganization better outcomes than those who feel close to one but
(Crosnoe & Cavanaugh, 2010). Several studies indicate not the other (V. King, 2006; Risch, Jodl, & Eccles,
that children’s adjustment declines somewhat each 2004).
time they must cope with a change in their family’s
household composition (e.g., Adam & Chase-Lansdale,
ECONOMIC STRESS AND POVERTY
2002; Albrecht & Teachman, 2003; Barnett, Rowley,
Zimmerman, Vansadia, & Howard Caldwell, 2011; In recent years, and in light of the economic downturn
K. Kim, Conger, Elder, & Lorenz, 2003; J. B. Kirby, of the last decade, there has been an upsurge in interest
2006; Sun & Li, 2009), in part because parenting may in the relationship between parents’ unemployment
become less effective during each family transition and adolescents’ well-being—in particular, in the ways
(Forgatch, DeGarmo, & Knutson, 1994; Kurdek & Fine, in which adolescents’ mental health is affected by
1993). Given the fact that the benefits of authoritative changes in their family’s financial situation. To date, the
parenting are just as strong in divorced and remarried studies of family income loss and adolescent adjust-
families as they are in other homes, experts believe that ment suggest a number of parallels with the research on
clinicians who work with families that have undergone divorce and remarriage.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 146 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

146 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

❚ The Effects of Financial Strain Like divorce, ❚ The Impact of Chronic Poverty Researchers
income loss tends to be associated with disruptions in have also studied the impact on adolescents of grow-
parenting, which, in turn, lead to increases in adolescent ing up amidst chronic economic disadvantage
difficulties, including a diminished sense of mastery, in- (McLoyd et al., 2009). In general, persistent poverty,
creased emotional distress, academic and interpersonal like temporary economic strain, undermines parental
problems, and delinquency. According to the Family effectiveness, making mothers and fathers harsher,
Stress Model (Conger, Ge, Elder, Lorenz, & Simons, more depressed, less vigilant, less consistent, and more
1994), financial strain increases mothers’ and fathers’ embroiled in conflict. These consequences all have
feelings of depression, worsens parents’ marriages, and negative effects on adolescent adjustment, which are
causes conflicts between parents and adolescents over manifested in increases in anxiety and depression,
money. These consequences, in turn, make parents more frequent conduct problems, diminished school
more irritable, which adversely affects the quality of performance, and less prosocial behavior (Benner &
their parenting (see Figure 4.10). Studies show that par- Kim, 2010; Burrell & Roosa, 2009; Carlo, Padilla-
ents under economic strain are less involved, less nur- Walker, & Day, 2011b; S. Kim & Brody, 2005; R. Taylor,
turing, harsher, and less consistent in their discipline Rodriguez, Seaton, & Dominguez, 2004; Wadsworth &
(McLoyd et al., 2009). Compas, 2002).
The family climate created by economic strain puts Interestingly, there appear to be few differences in
adolescents at risk for a variety of problems. As you the behavior, values, or family relationships between
now know, adolescents who are exposed to harsh, un- adolescents in families that are on welfare and those
involved, and inconsistent parenting are at greater risk that are not, calling into question the notion that wel-
for a wide range of psychological and behavioral fare undermines parental effectiveness, contributes to
problems. When adolescents are repeatedly exposed adolescents’ development of undesirable beliefs and
to marital conflict—especially when it is not re- values about work, or encourages problem behavior
solved—they are more likely to become aggressive and among teenagers (Kalil & Eccles, 1998). At the same
depressed (Cummings, Ballard, El-Sheikh, & Lake, time, studies of the impact of requiring women who
1991). And when adolescents themselves are the re- have been on welfare to enter the labor force find very
cipients of aggressive parenting, they are likely to few effects—positive or negative—on adolescent devel-
imitate this behavior in their relationships with sib- opment or behavior (Chase-Lansdale et al., 2003).
lings (K. Conger, Conger, & Elder, 1994) and peers Some evidence suggests that adolescents whose moth-
(S. T. Williams, Conger, & Blozis, 2007), and, later, ers have been required to go off public assistance and go
in their own marriage (Straus & Yodanis, 1996) and to work show somewhat worse school performance as a
with their children (R. Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & result, but this effect is most pronounced when the ado-
Chyi-In, 1991). lescent has a younger sibling, presumably because of

Family
income

Father’s
depressed
mood
Unstable
work
Family Marital Poor Adolescent
economic conflict parenting difficulties
pressure
Debts to
assets
Mother’s
depressed
mood

Income
loss
FIGURE 4.10 Economic strain affects adolescent adjustment via several pathways. (Conger et al. 1994)
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 147 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 147

the added household responsibilities the mother’s em- overly) restrictive strategies may be especially beneficial
ployment created for the teenager (Gennetian et al., to adolescents living in impoverished communities. Al-
2004). though adolescents in poor neighborhoods benefit
Growing up poor affects adolescents’ mental health from consistent parental monitoring—perhaps even
in a variety of ways. As we discussed in Chapter 3, poor from monitoring that is more vigilant than that used by
adolescents are more likely to be exposed to violence families in more advantaged communities—they do
(Sheidow, Gorman-Smith, Tolan, & Henry, 2001), to not thrive when their parents exercise excessive control
feel more alienated from school (Felner et al., 1995), (McCarthy, Lord, Eccles, Kalil, & Furstenberg, 1992).
and to be exposed to high levels of stress (Masten,
Miliotis, Graham-Bermann, Ramirez, & Neeman,
1993). Although few scientific studies have been con- SPECIAL FAMILY FORMS
ducted on homeless adolescents, research suggests that ❚ Adolescents and Adoption Studies of the psy-
these youngsters share many of the same problems chological development of adolescents who have been
with other youth who experience chronic poverty, in- adopted have yielded mixed and often contradictory re-
cluding higher rates of depression and suicidal sults. On average, adopted individuals show relatively
thoughts, academic difficulties, and behavior problems higher rates of delinquency, substance use, psychological
(Unger, Kipke, Simon, Montgomery, & Johnson, 1997; difficulties, and poorer school performance, but the
Whitbeck, Hoyt, & Bao, 2000; Zima, Wells, & Freeman, magnitude of the difference between adopted and non-
1994). There are nearly 2 million homeless children adopted adolescents is small, especially when other
and adolescents in the United States, the majority of factors, like family resources, are taken into account
whom are adolescents (National Coalition for the (B. Miller, Fan, Christensen, Grotevant, & van Dulmen,
Homeless, 2007). 2000; Slap, Goodman, & Huang, 2001; Sun, 2003). One
reason for the mixed results and relatively modest effects
is that there is a good deal of variability among adopted
adolescents in their adjustment and in their feelings
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION about being adopted. For instance, adopted adolescents
Most research on economic who are preoccupied with having been adopted are rela-
conditions, family functioning, and tively more alienated from and mistrustful of their adop-
adolescent development has focused tive parents (J. K. Kohler, Grotevant, & McRoy, 2002).
on poverty. Do you think there are
negative effects of growing up under ❚ Adolescents With Lesbian Or Gay Parents
extreme affluence? Several well-designed studies have looked at the psy-
chological consequences of growing up with lesbian or
gay parents. Many states still do not recognize the right
of same-sex couples to legally marry, and in some states,
a parent’s sexual orientation may be a consideration in
Studies of families living in poverty also tell us what adoption, custody, and parental visitation decisions, in
parents living in poor neighborhoods can do to help part because of concerns about the impact of living
protect their children from the adverse consequences of with a lesbian or gay parent. Based on the research that
growing up in poor inner-city or rural neighborhoods has been conducted, though, these concerns are unwar-
(Brody, Stoneman, & Flor, 1996; Furstenberg, 1996; ranted. There is no evidence whatsoever that children
Jarrett, 1995). In general, families fare better when they or adolescents with lesbian or gay parents are psycho-
have adequate sources of social support (R. Taylor & logically different from those with straight parents, a
Roberts, 1995) and when they have strong ties to reli- finding that has now been replicated numerous times,
gious institutions (Brody et al., 1996). In addition, two across many different domains of psychological devel-
specific sets of family management strategies employed opment, including gender identity and sexual orienta-
by parents in poor neighborhoods seem to work: pro- tion. In the words of one expert:
motive strategies, which attempt to strengthen the ado-
lescent’s competence through effective child rearing More than 25 years of research on the offspring of non-
heterosexual parents has yielded results of remarkable
within the home environment or through involving the
clarity. Regardless of whether researchers have studied the
child in positive activities outside the home, and re-
offspring of divorced lesbian and gay parents or those
strictive strategies, which attempt to minimize the born to lesbian and gay parents, their findings have been
child’s exposure to dangers in the neighborhood (Cros- similar. Regardless of whether researchers have studied
noe, Mistry, & Elder, 2002; Furstenberg, Cook, Eccles, children or adolescents, they have reported similar re-
Elder, & Sameroff, 1999). Studies indicate that a combi- sults. Regardless of whether investigators have examined
nation of promotive strategies and moderately (but not sexual identity, self-esteem, adjustment, or qualities of
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 148 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

148 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

social relationships, the results have been remarkably in a group home increases the adolescent’s risk for delin-
consistent. In study after study, the offspring of lesbian quency; Ryan, Marshall, Herz, & Hernandez, 2008).
and gay parents have been found to be at least as well Many adolescents move in and out of different place-
adjusted overall as those of other parents. (Patterson, ments, back and forth between their parents’ home and
2009, p. 732)
a foster care placement, or between different foster care
arrangements, and frequent disruptions in living ar-
rangements can lead to behavioral problems (Fisher,
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION Stoolmiller, Mannering, Takahashi, & Chamberlain,
Are you surprised that research has failed to find any 2011; Proctor, Skriner, Roesch, & Litrownik, 2010). After
relation between parents’ sexual orientation and their a period of time in foster care, adolescents are either
adolescent’s psychological development? Should reunified with their biological parents or adopted by
research on the psychological someone other than their parents, or, if they are old
development of adolescents enough, declared independent. As you can imagine,
raised by lesbian or gay par- making the transition to independent adulthood—
ents be taken into account already a challenge for many youth who grew up in sta-
in debates about the legal ble and supportive family environments—is even more
rights of same-sex couples? difficult for adolescents whose lives have been so dis-
rupted and who do not have parents on whom they can
rely for support. Adolescents who have been in foster
care are at higher risk for homelessness than other youth
(Fowler, Toro, & Miles, 2009).
❚ Adolescents in Foster Care According to re-
cent estimates, about 175,000 teenagers are in foster
care. Foster care is a broad term that refers to a place- RECAP
ment in a temporary living arrangement when the ado- • Research on divorce indicates that the period of
lescent’s parents are not able to provide care, nurturance, greatest difficulty for adolescents whose parents
or safety (Pinderhughes, Jones Harden, & Guyer, 2007). divorce is the time immediately after the event and
Such placements can be with members of the extended that this is largely attributable to the exposure of
family, nonrelatives, or group homes. Although we tend the child to marital conflict, disrupted parenting,
to think of foster homes as living arrangements for and family stress.
younger children, nearly one-third of young people in • It is the nature of the relationship between the ad-
foster care enter into their foster home as adolescents. olescent’s divorced parents, and not which one he
Moreover, because adolescents are less likely to be ad- or she lives with, that makes a difference.
opted than younger children, teenagers tend to remain • Because adolescents’ mental health suffers some-
in foster care longer. what each time their family situation changes,
Adolescents generally enter the foster care system for young people growing up in stepfamilies are at
one of two reasons: parental maltreatment (when the even greater risk for problems than their peers in
adolescent’s well-being or safety is endangered) or delin- single-parent, divorced homes.
quency (when an adolescent’s parents are unable to pro- • Research on economic strain and its impact on
vide the supervision necessary to keep their teenager adolescents indicates that the main effects of
from violating the law). Adolescents who have spent financial stress are transmitted to the adolescent
time in foster care are at relatively greater risk for emo- through the negative impact they have on parents’
tional and behavioral problems, some of which are the mental health and marital relations.
product of the abuse or neglect that necessitated their • Adolescents who have been adopted are a very
removal from their biological parents’ home (maltreat- diverse group, and it is hard to generalize about the
ment frequently causes psychological problems), some impact of adoption on adolescent development.
of which may have made it too difficult for their parents In contrast, one can generalize about adolescents
to adequately care for them (in which case the adoles- who have been raised by lesbian or gay parents,
cent may be placed in a therapeutic foster care environ- who are no different than those who have
ment designed for teenagers with psychological been raised by straight parents, and about
problems), and some of which may actually result from those who have spent time in foster care,
the foster care placement itself (for example, placement who tend to have more
problems than other
adolescents.
foster care A placement in a temporary living arrangement when
a child’s parents are not able to provide care, nurturance, or safety.
ste32118_ch04_117-149.indd Page 149 21/03/13 9:54 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 4 Families 149

The Importance of the 2005b). The challenge facing us is to figure out how best
to disseminate this information to the people who need
Family in Adolescent it most—parents.
Despite the tremendous growth and psychological de-
Development velopment that take place as individuals leave childhood
As you have seen, there is considerable diversity among on the road toward adulthood, despite society’s pressures
families with adolescents—diversity in background, in on young people to grow up fast, despite all the techno-
income, in parenting style, and in household composi- logical and social innovations that have transformed fam-
tion. Yet no factor seems to influence adolescent adjust- ily life, and contrary to claims that parents don’t really
ment more than the quality of relationships at home make a difference—that by adolescence, parents’ influ-
(Garnefski, 2000; Kaminski, Puddy, Hall, Cashman, ence is overshadowed by the peer group or the mass me-
Crosby et al., 2010). As one team of experts concluded on dia (e.g., J. Harris, 1998)—adolescents continue to need
the basis of a comprehensive study of the lives, behavior, the love, support, and guidance of adults who genuinely
and health of 90,000 American teenagers: care about their development and well-being. Being raised
in the presence of caring and committed adults is one of
Across all of the health outcomes examined, the results the most important advantages a young person can have
point to the importance of family and the home environ-
in life. Although parental love may be expressed in differ-
ment for protecting adolescents from harm. What
emerges most consistently as protective is the teenager’s
ent ways in different parts of the world, its importance for
feeling of connectedness with parents and family. Feeling healthy adolescent development is unquestionable, re-
loved and cared for by parents matters in a big way. gardless of cultural context (McNeely & Barber, 2010).
(Blum & Rinehart, 2000, p. 31)

Study after study finds that adolescents who believe


their parents or guardians are there for them—caring, RECAP
involved, and accepting—are healthier, happier, and • No factor seems to influence adolescent adjust-
more competent than their peers, however health, hap- ment more than the quality of his or her relation-
piness, or competence is assessed. This conclusion holds ships at home.
true regardless of the adolescent’s sex, ethnicity, social • Even after reaching adolescence, young people
class, or age, and across all types of families, whether continue to need the love, support, and guidance
married or divorced, single-parent or two-parent, rich of adults who genuinely care about their develop-
or poor (Dornbusch, Erickson, Laird, & Wong, 2001). ment and well-being.
This has led many psychologists, including myself, to • We know what the basic principles of good parent-
call for widespread efforts to increase the quality of par- ing are. The challenge facing us is to figure
enting that children and adolescents receive as a way of out how best to disseminate
preventing emotional and behavioral problems and this information to
promoting healthy development (Kumpfer & Alvarado, parents.
2003). As I have written elsewhere, we know what the
basic principles of good parenting are (Steinberg,
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 150 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 151 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
5
Peer Groups
THE ORIGINS OF ADOLESCENT PEER GROUPS IN CONTEMPORARY SOCIETY
Age Grading and Compulsory Education
Changes in the Size of the Youth Population
THE ADOLESCENT PEER GROUP: A PROBLEM OR A NECESSITY?
Is There a Separate Youth Culture?
THE NATURE OF ADOLESCENT PEER GROUPS
Changes in Peer Groups During Adolescence
Cliques and Crowds
Changes in Clique and Crowd Structure Over Time
ADOLESCENTS AND THEIR CROWDS
The Social Map of Adolescence
Crowds as Reference Groups
ADOLESCENTS AND THEIR CLIQUES
Similarity Among Clique Members
Common Interests Among Friends
Similarity Between Friends: Selection or Socialization?
POPULARITY AND REJECTION IN ADOLESCENT PEER GROUPS
Determinants of Popularity and Rejection
Relational Aggression
Victimization and Harassment
THE PEER GROUP AND PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT

151
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 152 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

152 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

IT IS ABOUT 8:00 A.M. A group of teenagers congre- American society is highly age segregated. From the
gates in the hallway in front of their first-period class- time youngsters stop spending their full day at home—
room, discussing their plans for the weekend. As the certainly by age 5, but for many, as early as the first year
first-period bell sounds, they enter the classroom and of life if they go to a day care center—until they gradu-
take their seats. For the next 4 hours (until there is a ate from high school at age 18 or so, they are grouped
break in their schedule for lunch), they will attend class with children their own age. Other than relatives, they
in groups of about 25 adolescents to 1 adult. have little extended contact with people who are older
At lunch, the group meets again to talk about the or younger. Because schools play such an important
weekend. They have about 45 minutes until the first role in determining children’s friendships, age grouping
afternoon period begins. After lunch, they spend an- carries over into after-school, weekend, and vacation
other 2 hours in class—again, in groups of about 25 ad- activities. In contemporary society, peer groups—
olescents and 1 adult. The school day ends, the group groups of people who are roughly the same age—have
convenes yet again, and they go to someone’s house to become an increasingly important context in which
hang out for the rest of the day. Everyone’s parents are adolescents spend time. Indeed, the significance of
working; they are on their own. At about 6:00 P.M., they peer groups gives adolescence in contemporary soci-
disperse and head home for dinner. Several will talk on ety some of its most distinctive features.
the phone that night. Others will communicate by text- Understanding how adolescent peer groups form
ing or on Facebook. And they will see one another first and what takes place within their boundaries is critical
thing the next morning. to understanding adolescent development. No discus-
Adolescents in modern society spend a remarkable sion of adolescent identity development is complete
amount of time with their peers. High school students without an examination of how and why teenagers de-
in the United States and Europe spend twice as much rive part of their identity from the group they spend
of their time each week with peers as with parents or time with. No discussion of the development of au-
other adults—even discounting time in class (B. Brown & tonomy is complete without consideration of how ad-
Larson, 2009; Dijkstra & Veenstra, 2011). Virtually all ado- olescents learn to be independent decision makers
lescents spend most of each weekday with their peers when they are with their friends. No discussion of inti-
while at school, and the vast majority also see or talk to macy is complete without an understanding of teenag-
their friends in the late afternoon, in the evening, and ers’ friendship groups and how they are formed. No
over the weekend (Larson & Verma, 1999). And studies discussion of adolescent sexuality is complete without
show that adolescents’ moods are most positive when an examination of how, when, and why peer groups
they are with their friends, that time spent with friends change from same-sex groups to mixed-sex groups.
becomes more rewarding over the course of adoles- And no discussion of the development of achievement
cence, and, as Figure 5.1 illustrates, that teenagers’ moods in adolescence can ignore the role that friends play in
become more positive over the course of the week, as influencing each other’s attitudes toward school.
the weekend approaches (Larson & Richards, 1998).

FIGURE 5.1 As the weekend Positive 0.15


approaches and adolescents
anticipate spending more time 0.10
with their friends, their mood
takes a marked turn for the 0.05
Mood

better. (Larson & Richards, 1998) 5th–8th grade


0
9th–12th grade
–0.05

Negative –0.10
Monday
  Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday Saturday Sunday

peer groups Groups of individuals of approximately the same age.


ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 153 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 153

The Origins of Adolescent


Peer Groups in
Contemporary Society
Contact between adolescents and their peers is found
in all cultures, of course. But not all societies have peer
groups that are as narrowly defined and age segre-
gated as those in contemporary industrialized societies
(B. Brown, 2004; Larson & Verma, 1999).

AGE GRADING AND COMPULSORY


EDUCATION
Educators first developed the idea of free public educa-
tion, with students grouped by age—a practice known as
age grading—in the middle of the 19th century. Thus,
they established an arrangement that would encourage
the development and maintenance of age-segregated Contemporary society is very age-graded. Even when they are in
peer groups. It was not until the second quarter of the activities outside school, adolescents tend to be with same-aged peers.
20th century, however, that most adolescents were di-
rectly affected by educational age grouping. Attending
elementary school may have been common before 1900,
but until 1930 or so, high school was a luxury available This trend turned downward in 1975, and the rela-
only to the affluent. In other words, adolescent peer tive size of the adolescent population decreased until
groups based on friendships formed in school were not 1995. But during the last decade of the 20th century—
prevalent until well into the 20th century. when the products of the baby boom began raising
adolescents of their own—the size of the teenage popu-
lation began increasing once again. In the year 2000, the
population of 15- to 19-year-olds in the United States
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
numbered about 20 million. An additional 20 million
Do you think that the increase in individuals were between ages 10 and 14 at that time,
the number of adolescents in meaning that as we entered the 21st century, approxi-
single-parent households has had any mately 1 in 7 individuals in the United States were
impact on the rise of peer groups in adolescents. The proportion of the U.S. population that
modern society? is adolescent is estimated to remain at about this level
through the next half century (U.S. Census Bureau,
2000). Keep in mind, though, that patterns of change in
the size of the adolescent population vary considerably
around the world, mainly because of different birthrates
CHANGES IN THE SIZE OF
(see Figure 5.2).
THE YOUTH POPULATION
Social scientists track the size of the adolescent popu-
Perhaps the most important factor influencing the rise lation for several reasons. First, changes in the number
of adolescent peer groups in contemporary society was of adolescents may warrant changes in the allocation
the rapid growth of the teenage population between of funds for social services, educational programs, and
1955 and 1975. Following the end of World War II, many health care, since adolescents’ needs are not the same
parents wanted to have children as soon as possible, as those of children or adults. Second, changes in the
creating what has been called the postwar baby boom. size of the adolescent population have implications for
The products of this baby boom became adolescents understanding the behavior of cohorts. As we noted in
during the 1960s and early 1970s, creating an “adoles- Chapter 3, a cohort is a group of individuals born during
cent boom” for about 15 years. The size of the popula-
tion ages 15–19 nearly doubled between 1955 and 1975
and, more importantly, rose from less than 7% of the
age grading The process of grouping individuals within social
total population to well over 10%. During the mid- institutions on the basis of age.
1970s, in fact, more than 1 out of every 6 Americans was
a teenager. baby boom The period following World War II, during which the
number of infants born was extremely large.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 154 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

154 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

30
25
20
15
10
5
0
pt

an

ria

ico

il

nd

Ge s

ly

an
e
di

in

an
az
ng

Ita
at
y

ist

Jap
ge

ila
Ch
In

ex

Br
Eg

rm
Co

St
k

Ni

a
M
Pa

Th

d
ite
Un
FIGURE 5.2 The proportion of the population that is adolescent varies around the world—it is highest in developing
countries, especially in the Arab world, and lowest in highly industrialized countries, like Japan. (United Nations, 2010)

a particular period, such as the baby boomers (born in


the late 1940s and early to mid-1950s), “Gen X” (born in
The Adolescent Peer
the early 1970s), the “Millennial generation” (the adoles- Group: A Problem
cent children of the baby boomers, born in the 1980s and
early 1990s, also known as “Generation Y”), or “Genera- or a Necessity?
tion Z” (born during the 1990s, and sometimes referred Is the rise of peer groups in modern society a problem
to as iGeneration, because of the growth of the Internet that needs to be remedied, or is it an inevitable—
during their time) (see Table 5.1). Baby boomers, for ex- perhaps even necessary—part of life in contemporary
ample, were members of a very crowded cohort, which America? This question has sparked some of the hottest
meant that during their adolescence, in the late 1960s and debates in the study of adolescence over the past 25
early 1970s, they encountered a relatively high degree of years (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). On one side are those
competition for places in college, jobs, and so on. The size who claim that age segregation has led to the develop-
of this cohort also meant that it could attract a great deal ment of a separate youth culture, in which young people
of public attention, from politicians to advertisers. In maintain attitudes and values that are different from—
contrast, members of Gen X, who were adolescents in the even contrary to—those of adults. On the other side are
late 1980s and early 1990s, were members of a much those who argue that industrialization and moderniza-
smaller cohort, with less competition among individuals tion have made peer groups more important, that adults
but also with far less clout. Because Gen Z, today’s teenag- alone can no longer prepare young people for the future,
ers, is considerably larger than Gen X, it will likely be and that peer groups play a vital role in the socialization
much more influential. We can already see how impor- of adolescents for adulthood (J. Harris, 1995). Let’s look
tant teenagers have been to the growth of Facebook and at both sides of the debate.
other social networking media.

RECAP
• In contemporary society, adolescents spend a great
deal of time with age-mates, or peers.
• Peer groups became a far more important influence
on adolescent development in the 20th century
than they had been in previous eras, when age seg-
regation was not as pervasive.
• Many factors contributed to the rise of peer groups
during the 20th century, including the spread of
compulsory secondary education and
changes in the relative size of
various cohorts of
adolescents.
Many adults have worried whether there is a distinct youth culture
that is counter to mainstream values.
TABLE 5.1 Cohorts, periods, and ages

in Census Year
Became Young Key Events
Cohort Name Adults (16–30) at That Time 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010

Generation Z 2002–2005 Internet explosion 16–24


Millennial 1992–2015 Information era:
generation economic growth
and global politics 16–24 25–30
Gen X 1982–2005 Reagan era:
economic
polarization,
political
conservatism 16–24 25–30
Late baby 1972–95 Watergate era:
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 155 21/03/13 10:02 AM user

boomers economic recession,


employment
restructuring 16–24 25–30
Early baby 1962–85 Hippies: social
boomers movements,
campus revolts 16–24 25–30
Happy days 1952–75 Family and
generation conformity:
baby boom and
Cold War/
McCarthy era 16–24 25–30
Happy days/greatest 1942–65 Family and
generation conformity:
baby boom and
Cold War/
McCarthy era 16–24 25–30
Greatest generation/ 1932–55 Hard times:
children of Great economic
Depression depression and
World War II 16–24 25–30
Children of Great 1922–45 Hard times:
Depression economic
depression and
World War II 16–24 25–30
Lost Generation 1912–35 World War I and
Roaring Twenties,
Prohibition 16–24 25–30
... 1902–25 Age of invention
and World War I 16–24 25–30
... 1892–1915 Age of invention,
urbanization 16–24 25–30

155
/204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 156 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

156 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

IS THERE A SEPARATE YOUTH CULTURE? society. An important lesson here is that the world of ad-
olescents usually reflects the broader context at that time.
The belief that age segregation has fueled the develop-
ment of a separate—and troublesome—youth culture
❚ Do Adolescents Inhabit a Separate World?
was first expressed about 50 years ago, in a book titled
According to some observers, age segregation has so
The Adolescent Society, which presented the findings of
strengthened the power of the peer group that American
an extensive study of the social worlds of 10 American
adolescents have become alienated from and unfamiliar
high schools (Coleman, 1961). The study found that ad-
with the values of adults. In this view, problems such as
olescents lived in a social world where academic success
youth unemployment, teenage suicide, juvenile delin-
was frowned on, where doing well in school did not earn
quency, drug and alcohol use, and premarital pregnancy
the admiration of peers, and where wealth, athletic abil-
can be attributed to the rise of peer groups and the isola-
ity (for boys), and good looks (for girls) mattered most.
tion of adolescents from adults. Many observers of the
Many of the concerns expressed in this book have
adolescent scene note that all these problems have in-
echoed repeatedly over the past 6 decades. When adults
creased dramatically since the 1940s, as peer groups
today complain about the questionable morals and
have become more prominent and age segregation has
poor character of today’s young people, they are saying
become more prevalent. Their argument is that the in-
nothing different from what adults were asserting in
crease in adolescents’ problems can be directly linked to
the middle of the 20th century (and what adults said
the rise in the power of adolescent peer groups.
about them, when they were teenagers). Indeed, there
As B. Bradford Brown, an expert on adolescent peer
probably hasn’t been a generation of adults that didn’t
groups, has noted, however, the nature and strength
complain about young people.
of adolescent peer influence varies a great deal from one
One team of researchers wanted to see just how much
historical period to the next and from one context to
had changed since The Adolescent Society was published
another (Brown & Larson, 2009). One study of Canadian
(Garner, Bootcheck, Lorr, & Rauch, 2006). They assessed
adolescents, for example, found that the majority of
the social climate of seven schools that varied with re-
students preferred to be remembered as outstanding
spect to their size; the ethnic and socioeconomic compo-
students (rather than as great athletes or popular adoles-
sition of the student body; and whether they were inner
cents), and other studies have found that students’ pref-
city, suburban, or semirural. The researchers concluded
erences for how they will be remembered vary a great
that there were both similarities and differences between
deal by sex, age, and their place in the social structure of
what they found and the adolescent society of the 1960s.
the school (B. Brown, 1990).
The combination of physical attractiveness, athletic
ability, and money was still associated with popularity.
❚ Has the Youth Culture Harmed Adolescents?
Academic success was valued, but an atmosphere of anti-
Has the rise of adolescent peer groups really caused as
intellectualism prevailed, and doing well in school clearly
many problems as adults often believe?
was not a pathway to popularity.
This question is very difficult to answer. While age
Other things were different, though, especially in
segregation certainly has increased over the past 60
communities that were not composed mainly of White,
years, society has changed in other ways during this
middle-class families. In these schools, the student body
same time—ways that may also have contributed to in-
was fragmented into crowds defined largely by ethnicity
creases in such problems as crime and drug use. In
and social class. There was often tension among crowds
many regards, the world is a far more stressful place to
as well as strong opposition to middle-class culture
grow up in now than it was in the past. Families move
among some crowds. In some senses, then, the diversifi-
every few years. Divorce is commonplace. Adolescents
cation of American society also diversified the adolescent
experience enormous pressures from parents, peers,
and the mass media. More important, as you will read
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION in later chapters, even though society has continued to
Do you think the values of the become increasingly age segregated, the rates of many
adolescent “society” differ in differ- adolescent problem behaviors—crime and drug use are
ent parts of the world, or is it more good examples—have fluctuated considerably over the
likely that athletic ability, good looks, past three decades. If age segregation were the “true”
and money contribute to popularity cause of adolescents’ problems, this would not be the
everywhere? If you have classmates case. There have been periods when adolescent prob-
who grew up somewhere other than lem behaviors have declined despite the fact that age
the United States, ask them what segregation has remained high.
made people popular in their high To be sure, contemporary adolescents spend more
school. time in peer groups than adolescents did in past eras.
But we do not know if today’s young people are any
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 157 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 157

more susceptible to the influence of their friends than peer groups exist well before adolescence, during the
their counterparts were previously, nor do we know if teenage years they change in significance and structure.
teenagers are any worse off because peer groups have Four specific developments stand out (B. Brown, 1990;
come to play a more prominent role. Moreover, adoles- B. Brown & Larson, 2009).
cents exert both positive and negative influences on
each other, and it is simply incorrect to describe the peer ❚ In What Ways Do Peer Groups Change?
group as a universally negative influence (Piehler, 2011). First, as noted earlier, there is a sharp increase during
As you will read, some peers influence each other to use adolescence in the sheer amount of time individuals
drugs and spend time partying rather than studying, spend with their peers and in the relative time they
but other peer groups discourage drug use and value spend in the company of peers versus adults. If we
academic achievement. In other words, although peers count school as a setting in which adolescents are
continue to remain highly influential, the directions in mainly with people their age, well over half of the typi-
which they influence each other are highly variable. cal American adolescent’s waking hours are spent with
peers, as opposed to only 15% with adults—including
their parents (a good deal of the remaining time is
spent alone or with a combination of adults and peers).
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION Indeed, during the transition into adolescence, there
The common stereotype of peer influence is a dramatic drop in the amount of time adolescents
is that it encourages adolescents to spend with parents; for boys, this is mainly replaced by
behave in ways that adults disapprove. time spent alone, whereas for girls, it is replaced by time
Can you think of examples from your own alone and time with friends (see Figure 5.3) (Dijkstra &
experience that demonstrate that peers Veenstra, 2011). There are important ethnic and gender
often can be positive influences differences in patterns of time use, however. For exam-
on development? ple, the decline in time spent with family members and
the dramatic increase in time spent with peers in early
adolescence are more striking among White girls than
among boys or Black youth (Larson, Richards, Sims, &
RECAP
Dworkin, 2001).
• Social scientists have long debated whether the Second, during adolescence, peer groups function
prominent role played by peer groups in the social- much more often without adult supervision than they
ization of young people is a cause for concern or do during childhood, partly because adolescents are
celebration. more mobile and partly because they seek, and are
• Some commentators have suggested that the rise granted, more independence (Dijkstra & Veenstra,
of peer groups has contributed to the development 2011). Groups of younger children typically play in the
of a separate youth culture that is hostile toward presence of adults or in activities organized or super-
adult values. Others have argued that the rise of vised by adults (for example, Little League or Brownies),
peer groups has contributed to increases in all sorts whereas adolescents are granted far more independence.
of problem behavior. A group of teenagers may go off to the mall or the mov-
• Research indicates that it is unwise to generalize ies on their own or hang out at the home of someone
about the nature of peer influence. While some whose parents are absent.
peer groups encourage antisocial Third, during adolescence, increasingly more contact
behavior, others actively with peers is with other-sex friends. During childhood,
discourage it. peer groups are highly sex segregated. This is especially
true of peer activities of children in school and other
settings organized by adults, although somewhat less so
of their more informal activities, such as neighborhood
play. During adolescence, however, an increasingly
The Nature of Adolescent larger proportion of an individual’s significant others
are other-sex peers, even in public settings (Djikstra &
Peer Groups Veenstra, 2011; Mehta & Strough, 2009).
Finally, whereas children’s peer relationships are lim-
CHANGES IN PEER GROUPS DURING
ited mainly to relatively small groups—at most, three
ADOLESCENCE
or four children at a time, for example—adolescence
On any typical elementary school playground, it would marks the emergence of larger collectives of peers, or
appear evident that peer groups are an important fea- crowds (Brown & Larson, 2009). In junior high school
ture of the social world of childhood. But even though cafeterias, for example, the “popular” crowd sits in one
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 158 21/03/13 10:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

158 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Boys stimulates adolescents’ interest in other-sex relation-


100 ships and serves to distance them from their parents,
which helps to explain why adolescents’ social networks
80 increasingly include more other-sex peers and fewer
adults. The cognitive changes of adolescence permit a
Mean percent of time

more sophisticated understanding of social relation-


60 ships, an understanding that may allow the sort of ab-
stract categorization that leads to grouping individuals
into crowds. And changes in social definition may stim-
40
ulate changes in peer relations as a sort of adaptive re-
sponse: The larger, more anonymous social setting of
20 the secondary school, for instance, may force adoles-
cents to seek out individuals whom they perceive as hav-
ing common interests and values, perhaps as a way of
0 re-creating the smaller, more intimate groups of child-
5th 6th 7th 8th 9th
Grade hood (B. Brown, 2004). Instead of floundering in a large,
impersonal high school cafeteria, the adolescent who
With friends Alone With family belongs to the “cheerleader” crowd, or even the “nerds,”
may head directly for her or his place at a familiar table.
Girls
100
CLIQUES AND CROWDS
To better understand the significance of peer relations
80 during adolescence, it is helpful to think of adolescents’
Mean percent of time

peer groups as organized around two related, but dif-


60 ferent, structures (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). Cliques
are small groups of between 2 and 12 individuals—the
average is about 5 or 6—generally of the same sex and,
40 of course, the same age. Cliques can be defined by com-
mon activities (for example, the football players or a
group of students who study together regularly) or sim-
20
ply by friendship (e.g., a group of girls who have lunch
together every day or a group of boys who have grown
0 up together). The importance of the clique, whatever its
5th 6th 7th 8th 9th basis, is that it provides the main social context in which
Grade adolescents interact with one another. The clique is the
FIGURE 5.3 There are significant changes in how social setting in which adolescents hang out, talk to
individuals spend leisure time during early adolescence. each other, and form close friendships. Some cliques
(Larson & Richards, 1991) are more open to outsiders than others (that is, the
members are less “cliquish”), but virtually all cliques are
section of the room, the “brains” in another, and the small enough that the members feel they know each
“jocks” in a third. These crowds typically develop their other well and appreciate each other more than people
own minicultures, characterized by particular styles of outside the clique do (B. Brown & Larson, 2009).
dressing, talking, and behaving. It is not until early ado-
lescence that individuals can confidently list the various ❚ How Cliques Structure Social Networks One
crowds in their schools and reliably describe the stereo- study of the structure, prevalence, and stability of cliques
types that distinguish the different crowds from one an- among 9th-graders in five different high schools within
other (B. Brown, Mory, & Kinney, 1994). a large American school district illustrated the ways in
which cliques structure adolescents’ friendship networks
❚ What Causes Peer Groups to Change? These (Ennett & Bauman, 1996). Based on interviews with
changes in peer relations have their origins in the bio- students over a 1-year period, the researchers categorized
logical, cognitive, and social transitions of adolescence adolescents as clique members (individuals who have most
(Djikstra & Veenstra, 2011). Puberty, as we have seen, of their interactions with the same small group of peo-
ple), liaisons (individuals who interact with two or more
adolescents who are members of cliques, but who them-
cliques Small, tightly knit groups of between 2 and 12 friends, selves are not part of a clique), and isolates (individuals
generally of the same sex and age.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 159 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 159

Adolescents usually belong to at least one clique, a close group of about a half dozen friends, usually of the same age and sex.

who have few or no links to others in the network). Three crowds. Crowds are based on “the identification of ado-
interesting patterns emerged. First, despite the popular lescents who share a similar image or reputation among
image of adolescents as “cliquish,” fewer than half the peers, or who have a common feature such as ethnicity
adolescents in any school were members of cliques. Sec- or neighborhood, even if they do not consider each
ond, girls were more likely than boys to be members of other friends or spend much time interacting with each
cliques, whereas boys were more likely than girls to be other” (B. Brown, 2004). In contemporary American
isolates. Finally, adolescents’ positions in their school’s high schools, typical crowds include “jocks,” “brains,”
social network were relatively stable over time: Adoles- “nerds,” “populars,” and “druggies.” The labels for these
cents who were members of cliques in the 9th grade were crowds may vary from school to school (“nerds” versus
clique members in 10th grade; 9th-grade isolates re- “geeks,” “populars” versus “preps”), but their generic
mained, for the most part, isolates 1 year later. This presence is commonplace, at least in the United States,
does not mean that the membership of specific cliques Canada, Australia, and, with the exception of the “jock”
is stable—typically, it is not—only that individuals who crowd, most western European countries (Arnett, 2002;
were members of some clique at one point of time were Delsing, ter Bogt, Engels, & Meeus, 2007). (The jock
members of some clique 1 year later. In other words, crowd is seldom found in European schools because
there is stability in adolescents’ tendency to join cliques, athletic events are typically organized around clubs lo-
but not in the makeup of particular groups (B. Brown, cated in the community rather than school teams.) If
2004). In one study, about 75% of 7th-graders were you grew up in one of these countries, you can probably
members of cliques and about 15% were isolates; very recognize these different types of crowds from your
few were liaisons or connected to just one other adoles- own school experience. In contrast to cliques, crowds
cent in a dyad (A. M. Ryan, 2001). Not surprisingly, the are not settings for adolescents’ intimate interactions or
more recently a student has arrived at a school, the less friendships but instead serve three broad purposes: to
“well connected” he or she is likely to be; developing a locate adolescents (to themselves and to others) within
social network is especially hard on students whose the social structure of the school, to channel adolescents
families move frequently (South & Haynie, 2004).

❚ How Adolescents Sort Into Crowds Cliques crowds Large, loosely organized groups of young people,
are quite different in structure and purpose from composed of several cliques and typically organized around a
common shared activity.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 160 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

160 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

into associations with some peers and away from oth- how to communicate effectively, how to be a leader,
ers, and to provide contexts that reward certain lifestyles how to enjoy someone else’s company, or even how to
and disparage others (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). break off a friendship that is no longer satisfying. In
contrast, because crowds are based more on reputation
and stereotype than on interaction, they probably con-
tribute more to the adolescent’s sense of identity and
self-conception—for better and for worse—than to his
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION or her actual social development.
What were the names of the major crowds
at your school? Were you a member of
CHANGES IN CLIQUE AND CROWD
any of them?
STRUCTURE OVER TIME
Studies of the structure of adolescents’ peer groups of-
ten make use of a research technique called participant
observation. In this approach, the researcher estab-
Membership in a crowd is based mainly on reputa- lishes rapport with a group of individuals in order to
tion and stereotype, rather than on actual friendship or infiltrate and eventually join the group. In Inside High
social interaction. This is very different from member- School (Cusick, 1973), the author pretended to be a
ship in a clique, which, by definition, hinges on shared newcomer to the community and attended high school
activity and friendship. In concrete terms, and perhaps for a year to learn more about the adolescents’ social
ironically, an adolescent does not have to actually have world (yes, people were doing this before 21 Jump
“brains” as friends, or hang around with “brainy” stu- Street). As an observer who is also a participant, the re-
dents, to be one of the “brains.” If he dresses like a searcher can observe the group’s behavior under condi-
“brain,” acts like a “brain,” and takes honors courses, tions that are more natural and more private than
then he is a “brain” as far as his crowd membership goes. would otherwise be the case. Overhearing a 10-minute
The fact that crowd membership is based on reputa- conversation in a high school locker room often can be
tion and stereotype can be very difficult for individual more informative than interviewing a student for
adolescents, who—if they do not change their reputation 3 hours.
early on in high school—may find themselves stuck, at
least in the eyes of their peers, in a crowd that they do not ❚ How Romance Changes the Peer Group
wish to belong to (or even see themselves as a part of) (B. Observational studies of young people indicate that
Brown, Von Bank, & Steinberg, 2008). This also means there are important changes in the structure of cliques
that some individuals can be members of more than one and crowds during the adolescent years, driven in large
crowd simultaneously, if their reputation is such that they measure by the increased importance of romantic rela-
fit into them (B. Brown, 2004). According to some esti- tionships (e.g., Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000;
mates, close to half of high school students are associated Kuttler & La Greca, 2004). During early adolescence,
with one crowd, about one-third are associated with two adolescents’ activities revolve around same-sex cliques.
or more crowds, and about one-sixth do not clearly fit Adolescents at this stage are not yet involved in party-
into any crowd (B. Brown, 2004). Although an adoles- ing and typically spend their leisure time with a small
cent’s closest friends are almost always members of the group of friends, playing sports, talking, or simply
same clique, some of them may belong to a different hanging out.
crowd, especially when one crowd is close in lifestyle to Somewhat later, as boys and girls become more in-
the other (Urberg, Değirmencioğlu, Tolson, & Halliday- terested in one another romantically—but before ro-
Scher, 1995). Thus, for example, a “brain” will have some mantic relationships actually begin—boys’ and girls’
friends who are also “brains” and some who are “nerds” cliques come together. This is clearly a transitional
but few, if any, who are “druggies” (B. Brown et al., 1994). stage. Boys and girls may go to parties or hang out to-
More importantly, crowds are not simply clusters of gether, but the time they spend there actually involves
cliques; the two different structures serve entirely differ- interaction with peers of the same sex. When young
ent purposes. Because the clique is based on activity and teenagers are still uncomfortable about dealing with
friendship, it is the important setting in which the ado- members of the other sex, this setting provides an op-
lescent learns social skills—how to be a good friend, portunity in which they can learn more about other-sex
peers without having to be intimate and without hav-
ing to risk losing face. It is not unusual, for example, at
young adolescents’ first mixed-sex parties, for groups of
participant observation A research technique in which the boys and girls to position themselves at other sides of a
researcher “infiltrates” a group of individuals in order to study their room, watching each other but seldom interacting.
behavior and relationships.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 161 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 161

The peer group then enters a stage of structural


transformation. As some adolescents become interested
in romance, part of the group begins to split off into
mixed-sex cliques, while other individuals remain in the
group but in same-sex cliques. This shift is usually led
by the clique leaders, with other clique members follow-
ing along. For instance, a clique of boys whose main
activity is playing basketball may discover that one of
the guys they look up to has become more interested in
going to mixed-sex parties Saturday nights than in
hanging out with the guys and playing video games.
Over time, they will begin to follow his lead, and their
all-male activities will become more infrequent. An ob-
servational study of middle school dances over the
course of the academic year found that the integration
of boys’ and girls’ peer groups increased over time, but
that this occurred mainly among physically attractive
adolescents (no surprise, because being good-looking
Early adolescence is often marked by the transition from same-sex
contributes to status in the peer group) (see Figure 5.4) peer groups to mixed-sex peer groups.
(Pellegrini & Long, 2007).
During middle adolescence, mixed-sex and mixed-
age cliques become more prevalent, and in time, the toward romantic partners (Kuttler & La Greca, 2004).
peer group becomes composed entirely of mixed-sex Groups of couples may go out together from time to
cliques (Cooksey, Mott, & Neubauer, 2002). One clique time, but the feeling of being in a crowd has disap-
might consist of the drama students—male and female peared. This pattern—in which the couple becomes the
students who know each other from acting together in focus of social activity—persists into adulthood.
school plays. Another might be composed of four girls When viewed from a structural point of view, the
and four boys who like to drink on weekends. The peer group’s role in the development of intimacy is
preppies—male and female—might make up a third. clear. Over time, the structure of the peer group
Interestingly, the transition from same-sex groups to changes, in keeping with adolescents’ changing needs
mixed-sex groups is associated with an increase in and interests. As we’ll see in Chapter 10, the adolescent’s
alcohol and drug use among females, and alcohol use capacity for close relationships develops first through
among males (Poulin, Denault, & Pedersen, 2011). friendships with peers of the same sex. Only later does
Finally, during late adolescence, peer crowds begin intimacy enter into other-sex relationships. Thus, the
to disintegrate. Pairs of adolescents who see themselves structure of the peer group changes during adolescence
as couples begin to split off from the activities of the in a way that parallels the adolescent’s development of
larger group. The larger peer group is replaced by intimacy: As the adolescent develops increasing facility
loosely associated sets of couples. Adolescents begin to in intimate relationships, the peer group moves from
shift some of their attention away from friends and the familiarity of same-sex activities to contact with
other-sex peers, but mainly in the safety of the larger
group. It is only after adolescent males and females have
2.5 been slowly socialized into dating roles—primarily by
Low attractiveness modeling their higher-status peers—that the safety of
2.0 High attractiveness numbers is no longer needed and adolescents begin
Mean count

1.5 pairing off.


1.0
❚ Changes in Crowds There also are changes in
0.5 peer crowds during this time. Many of these changes re-
flect the growing cognitive sophistication of the adoles-
0.0 cent. For example, as adolescents mature intellectually,
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Month they come to define crowds more in terms of abstract,
FIGURE 5.4 Intermingling of boys’ and girls’ peer groups global characteristics (“preppies,” “nerds,” “jocks”) than
increases during early adolescence, but it starts first among in terms of concrete, behavioral features (“the ballet
physically attractive adolescents, who often are clique crowd,” “the Mass Effect crowd,” “the kids who play bas-
leaders. Mean count refers to the mean number of interac- ketball on 114th Street”) (Djikstra & Veenstra, 2011). As
tions with other-sex peer groups. (Pellegrini & Long, 2007) you know, this shift from concrete to abstract is a general
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 162 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

162 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

feature of cognitive development in adolescence. In ad- you did because if you did one thing that
dition, as adolescents become more cognitively capable, was out of the ordinary, and you weren’t ex-
they become more consciously aware of the crowd struc- pected to do anything out of the ordinary,
ture of their school and their place in it (B. Brown, 2004). then you were laughed at and made fun of,
Over the course of adolescence, the crowd structure also and you wouldn’t fit the group at all, and
becomes more differentiated, more permeable, and less then, of course, you were excluded and then
hierarchical, which allows adolescents more freedom to you didn’t even exist.
change crowds and enhance their status (B. Brown, 2004; Ross: You got “nuked,” so to speak. (Kinney, 1993,
Horn, 2003). In early adolescence, a school may have p. 27)
only two broad crowds (e.g., “normals” and “losers”). By
high school, there may be several different ways to be Kinney discovered, however, that many individuals
“normal” (“populars,” “jocks,” “average”) and several dif- who had been “nuked” in middle school had managed
ferent ways to be a “loser” (“brains,” “nerds,” “burnouts”). to transform themselves from “nerds” into “normals”
sometime during high school. For some, this transition
❚ The Transformation of the Nerds In one fas- was accomplished because the high school peer struc-
cinating study of the day-to-day experiences of “nerds” ture was more differentiated and more permeable. As
and their interactions with other students, sociologist opposed to middle school, where there were only two
David Kinney conducted an ethnography of the social groups—the popular and the unpopular—in high
interaction and peer culture in a high school in a small school, there were more socially acceptable groups with
Midwestern city. In contrast to survey or experimental which to affiliate. For others, the transition to “normal”
research, which is typically quantitative in nature (that came about through gains in self-assurance that came
is, the data collected can be quantified), ethnographic with physical and social development. And for still oth-
research is qualitative. The researcher spends a consid- ers, the transformation was facilitated by the develop-
erable amount of time observing interactions within ment of a more cognitively sophisticated, confident
the setting, interviewing many adolescents, and writing view of the social hierarchy—one that permitted them
up field notes, much as an anthropologist would do in to reject the premise that whatever the “trendies” val-
studying a foreign culture. Ethnographic approaches ued was necessarily desirable.
can be extremely useful in studying social relationships, In essence, the transformation of “nerds” to “nor-
because they provide rich, descriptive data. mals” was enabled by a combination of factors both
Here, for example, is an excerpt of an interview within the context (for example, the increasing differen-
Kinney conducted with two students who had been tiation and permeability of the peer crowd system) and
“nerds” in middle school: within the adolescent (for example, the physical, cogni-
tive, and social maturation of the individual) (B. Brown,
Ross: And middle school— 1996). Kinney’s study, as well as other ethnographies of
Ted: We were just nerds. I mean— peer groups, reminds us of the potential for growth and
Ross: Yeah— change during the adolescent years, even for individuals
Ted: People hated us. who begin the period at a social disadvantage.
Ross: Well, they didn’t hate us, but we weren’t—
Ted: Popular. Which was either you were popular ❚ The Waxing and Waning of Crowds As
or you weren’t. crowds become more salient influences on adolescents’
view of their social world, they come to play an increas-
Ross: In middle school it’s very defined. There’s
ingly important role in structuring adolescent social be-
popular people and unpopular people.
havior (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). By 9th grade, there is
It’s just very—rigid. You were popular or
nearly universal agreement among students in a school
unpopular. That’s it.
about their school’s crowd structure, and students’ as-
Ted: And there wasn’t people that were in sessment of the strength of peer group influence is very
between. high. Between 9th and 12th grades, however, the signifi-
Ross: Oh no! cance of the crowd structure begins to decline, and the
Ted: You just had one route [to becoming popu- salience of peer pressure wanes.
lar], and then there was the other. And we In one study, students were presented with several
were the other, and—basically you were scenarios asking if it was all right to exclude someone
afraid of getting laughed at about anything from a school activity (cheerleading, basketball, student
council) because the person was a member of a certain
crowd (“jock,” “gothic,” “preppie”) (Horn, 2003). They
were also asked whether it was acceptable to deny indi-
ethnography A type of research in which individuals are observed viduals resources (for example, a scholarship) on the
in their natural settings.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 163 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 163

basis of their crowd membership. Consistent with the RECAP


decline in the salience of peer crowds between mid- and • As individuals move into adolescence, there is an
late adolescence, 9th-graders were more likely to say increase in the amount of time they spend in the
that excluding someone from an activity on the basis of exclusive company of their peers, their peer groups
his or her crowd was all right. Students of all ages agreed function increasingly outside adult supervision, and
that it was less acceptable to deny students resources they spend more time with other-sex peers.
because of crowd membership (which virtually all stu- • Social scientists distinguish between crowds and
dents viewed as immoral) than to exclude them from cliques. Cliques are small groups of friends; crowds,
an activity (which was less often seen as a moral issue). in contrast, are larger and more vaguely defined
As we’ll see in Chapter 9, this pattern of a decline in groups that are based on reputation.
the salience of peer crowds parallels developmental • Cliques, which begin as same-sex groups of individ-
changes in adolescents’ susceptibility to peer pressure uals, gradually merge to form larger, mixed-sex
(Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). In other words, as groups, as adolescents develop romantic relation-
crowds become less important, between middle and ships and start socializing with peers of the other
late adolescence, their influence over the individual’s sex. In late adolescence, these groups begin to
behavior weakens as well (B. Brown, 2004). Most prob- break down, as adolescents’ social lives start to
ably, the interplay between changes in the importance revolve more around couple-based activities.
of the crowd and changes in adolescent’s susceptibility • Crowds, which peak in importance during mid-
to peer influence is reciprocal. adolescence, become more differentiated and
Just as the changes in the structure of cliques play more permeable during high
a role in the development of intimacy during adoles- school, and their influence
cence, changes in the salience of crowds over the becomes less
adolescent years play an important role in adolescent salient.
identity development. In Chapter 8, we’ll see that
adolescence is frequently a time for experimentation
with different roles and identities. During the early
adolescent years, before adolescents have “found”
themselves, the crowd provides an important basis for
Adolescents and
self-definition (B. Newman & Newman, 2001a). By Their Crowds
locating themselves within the crowd structure of
THE SOCIAL MAP OF ADOLESCENCE
their school—through clothing, language, or choice
of hangouts—adolescents wear “badges” that say Although we often hear people talk about a universal
“This is who I am.” At a time when adolescents may “youth culture,” most ethnographic studies of high
not actually know just who they are, associating with schools indicate that the social world of adolescents is
a crowd provides them with a rudimentary sense of far more multifaceted than this. One helpful scheme for
identity. mapping the social world of adolescent crowds was
As adolescents become more secure in their identity suggested by Rigsby and McDill (1975) and later modi-
as individuals, the need for affiliation with a crowd di- fied by Brown (1990).
minishes. Indeed, by the time they have reached high According to this model, adolescents’ crowds can be
school, older adolescents are likely to feel that remain- placed along two dimensions: how involved they are in
ing a part of a crowd stifles their sense of identity and the institutions controlled by adults, such as school and
self-expression. The breakup of the larger peer group in extracurricular activities, and how involved they are in
late adolescence may both foreshadow and reflect the the informal, peer culture (see Figure 5.5). “Jocks” and
emergence of each adolescent’s unique and coherent “populars,” for example, are very involved in the peer
sense of self (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). culture, but they are also very involved in the institu-
tions valued by adults (sports and school organizations,
for example). “Brains” and “nerds,” in contrast, are also
involved in adult-controlled organizations (in their case,
academics), but they tend to be less involved in the peer
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION culture. “Partyers” are on the opposite side of the map
from “nerds”: These adolescents are very involved in the
Think back to your own high school
peer culture but are less involved in adult institutions.
experience. What were the major crowds
“Burnouts” and adolescents who are members of delin-
in your school? What common
quent gangs are not involved in either the peer culture
characteristics did you share with the
or adult institutions. Other crowds, such as “normals” or
people who were in your clique?
“druggies,” fall somewhere between these extremes.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 164 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

164 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

❚ Crowd Membership and Adolescent Identity


High Jocks Crowd membership is important not only because
Nerds Brains
Involvement in adult institutions crowds are used by adolescents when talking about one
Populars another but also because membership in a crowd is often
the basis for an adolescent’s own identity (B. Brown &
Normals
Larson, 2009). A girl who hangs out with the “preppies”
identifies herself as such by wearing their “uniform,” shop-
ping in their stores, and speaking their language. After a
while, preppiness becomes part of her own self-concept;
Toughs Druggies Partyers she wouldn’t think of dressing or talking any other way.
Or consider the boys whose clique is held together by a
dislike of school. Since this attitude toward school is con-
Low
Low High tinuously reinforced by the clique, each boy’s feelings
Involvement in peer culture about school become strengthened, and not liking school
becomes part of each boy’s identity. Even if something
FIGURE 5.5 A model for mapping the social world of very positive happens at school, it becomes difficult for
adolescent peer groups. (From Brown, 1990)
someone in the clique to admit that it makes him feel
good about himself. Doing well on a test or receiving a
CROWDS AS REFERENCE GROUPS compliment from a teacher is likely to be dismissed as
unimportant.
Knowing where an adolescent fits into the social system Because the adolescent’s peer group plays such an
of the school can often tell us a lot about the individu- important role as a reference group and a source of
al’s behavior and values. This is because crowds con- identity, the nature of the crowd with which an adoles-
tribute to the definition of norms and standards for cent affiliates is likely to have an important influence
such things as clothing, leisure, and tastes in music. Be- on his or her behavior, activities, and self-conceptions
ing a “jock” means more than simply being involved in (Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011; Djikstra & Veenstra,
athletics; it means wearing certain types of clothes, lis- 2011; Prinstein & La Greca, 2002). Although most ado-
tening to certain types of music, spending Saturday lescents feel pressure from their friends to behave in
nights in certain hangouts, and using a particular slang. ways that are consistent with their crowd’s values and
These adolescents accept many of the values of the goals, the specific nature of the pressure varies from one
adults around them but also value many elements of crowd to another. “Druggies” report much more peer
the contemporary peer culture. pressure to misbehave, for example, than “jocks” (Clasen
Another way of putting this is that adolescents’ & Brown, 1985). Crowd membership can also affect the
crowds serve as reference groups. They provide their way adolescents feel about themselves. Adolescents’ self-
members with an identity in the eyes of other adolescents. esteem is higher among students who are identified
Adolescents judge one another on the basis of the with peer groups that have relatively more status in
company they keep, and they become branded on the their school (B. Brown, Von Bank, & Steinberg, 2008).
basis of whom they hang out with. Such labels as “jocks,” One study similarly found that over the course of ado-
“brains,” “populars,” “druggies,” and “skaters” serve as lescence, symptoms of psychological distress declined
shorthand notations—accurate or inaccurate—to de- among the “populars” and “jocks” but increased among
scribe what someone is like as a person and what he or the “brains” (Prinstein & La Greca, 2002). Interestingly,
she holds as important. adolescents whose peers identify them as members of
low-status crowds fare better psychologically when they
don’t see themselves this way, but the opposite is true for
adolescents whose peers label them as members of high-
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
status crowds, where denying one’s affiliation with the
Think back on your own adolescent crowd is associated with worse mental health (B. Brown,
experience. Did you change crowds, or do Von Bank, & Steinberg, 2008). Of course, the longer-
you know someone who did? How was term consequences of crowd membership during ado-
this accomplished? Do you know anyone lescence are not necessarily the same as their immediate
who tried to change crowds but failed? impact. One study that examined the young-adult out-
Why did this happen? comes of high school crowd membership found that
both “brains” and “jocks” showed the most favorable
patterns of psychological adjustment over time (Barber,
Eccles, & Stone, 2001). Individuals who had been mem-
reference groups A group against which an individual compares bers of antisocial peer groups fared the worst.
him- or herself.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 165 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 165

Adolescents’ behavior is affected by


their crowd membership in several ways
(Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011). First, adoles-
cents often imitate the behavior of high-
status peers—the crowd leaders. The socially
popular girls, for example, may dress in a
certain way, and the less popular ones
(the “wannabes”), who want to be associ-
ated with them, follow suit. Second,
crowds establish social norms—values and
expectations—that members strive to fol-
low. That is, even lower-status members of a
crowd influence each other by behaving in
ways that identify them as members of the
crowd (e.g., using certain expressions when
they speak), and other adolescents who
want to be accepted by them conform to
these standards. Third, when crowd mem-
bers behave in ways that are consistent with
these norms, they are reinforced for doing
so. An adolescent who dresses in a way that
In multiethnic high schools, peer groups often divide along ethnic lines.
is consistent with a crowd’s expectation may
be complimented (“nice shoes, Susie”)
whereas one who does not may be ignored, or even made
fun of (“I can’t believe you’re wearing those!”). Finally, groups, although this varies considerably from school to
when adolescents are reinforced for following a crowd’s school (Tyson, Darity, & Castellino, 2005). Although one
norms, they feel better about themselves, and further in- widely cited study reported that high-achieving Black
corporate their crowd membership into their identity. students are ostracized for “acting White” (Fordham &
After being reinforced many times for her clothes by one Ogbu, 1986), many studies do not find this to be typical
of the popular crowd’s members, for example, a girl will (e.g., Horvat & Lewis, 2003; Tyson et al., 2005). In fact, in
start to think of herself as a member of that crowd and many schools, all students who are highly committed to
begin to derive her identity in part from it. school, regardless of their ethnicity, are teased or ex-
cluded for being “nerds” or “brains.” Similarly, in some
❚ Ethnicity and Crowd Membership Some re- schools it may be admirable to be a “jock,” while in oth-
search indicates that at least some of the basic distinc- ers it may be frowned upon. Thus, the values we associ-
tions among crowds that have been found in studies ate with being in one crowd as opposed to another may
of predominantly White high schools (for example, not be constant across all school contexts.
academically oriented crowds, partying crowds, deviant
crowds, trendy crowds) also exist among adolescents
from ethnic minority groups (B. Brown & Mounts,
1989). There is evidence, however, that in multiethnic RECAP
high schools adolescents may first divide across ethnic • Although the specific crowd names may differ from
lines and then form into the more familiar adolescent one school to another, and from one ethnic group
crowds within ethnic groups. Thus, in a large multieth- to the next, most high schools have relatively simi-
nic high school, there may be separate groups of Black lar crowd structures, with some version of “jocks,”
“jocks” and White “jocks,” of Hispanic “populars” and “populars,” “brains,” “nerds,” and “burnouts.”
Black “populars,” and so on (Steinberg, 1996). Interest- • Adolescents’ crowds can be mapped on two dis-
ingly, in multiethnic schools, adolescents from one eth- tinct dimensions: how involved they are in adult
nic group are less likely to see crowd distinctions within institutions, such as school, and how involved they
other ethnic groups than they are within their own are in the peer culture.
group. Thus, to White students, all Asian adolescents • Because they often serve as reference groups,
are part of the “Asian” crowd, whereas the Asian stu- crowds play an important role in
dents see themselves as divided into “brains,” “popu- the adolescent’s identity
lars,” and other groups (B. Brown & Mounts, 1989). development.
The meaning associated with belonging to different
crowds may differ across ethnic and socioeconomic
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 166 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

166 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Adolescents and 0.5

Proportion of network that is other-sex


Their Cliques 0.4
What draws adolescents into one clique and not an-
other? Because cliques serve as a basis for adolescents’
friendships and play an important role in their social 0.3
development, many researchers have studied the deter-
minants of clique composition.
0.2

SIMILARITY AMONG CLIQUE MEMBERS


0.1
The most important influence on the composition of
cliques is similarity. Adolescents’ cliques typically are com-
posed of people who are of the same age and the same 0.0
6 7 8 9 10
ethnicity, from the same socioeconomic background, Grade level of target child
and—at least during early and middle adolescence—
the same sex (Ennett & Bauman, 1996). Girls in school Boys in school
Girls out of school Boys out of school
❚ Age Segregation Although many adolescents FIGURE 5.6 Over time, there is a dramatic increase in the
have friends who are one school grade ahead or behind number of boys from other schools who are members of
(Bowker & Spencer, 2010), age grouping in junior and girls’ social networks. (Poulin & Pedersen, 2007)
senior high schools makes it unlikely that an individual
will have friends who are substantially older or younger.
A 10th-grader who is enrolled in 10th-grade English, friends more than doubles between 6th and 10th grades
10th-grade math, 10th-grade history, and 10th-grade (Poulin & Pedersen, 2007). And, in keeping with the no-
science simply does not have many opportunities to tion that this coincides with the onset of dating, the in-
meet adolescents who are in different grades. Age segre- crease is especially notable among early-maturing girls,
gation in adolescents’ cliques appears to result mostly whose networks increasingly include somewhat older
from the structure of schools. By way of comparison, boys that they know outside of school (see Figure 5.6).
adolescents’ online friends are less similar in age than the Even so, by 10th grade, most adolescents’ social net-
friends they make in school (Mesch & Talmud, 2007). works are still dominated by same-sex friends, who
make up about three-quarters of the average teenager’s
❚ Sex Segregation During the early and middle network.
adolescent years, cliques also tend to be composed of A second reason for sex segregation in adolescent peer
adolescents of the same sex (Ennett & Bauman, 1996). groups concerns young adolescents’ sensitivity about sex
This sex segregation begins in childhood and continues roles. Over the course of childhood, boys and girls be-
through most of adolescence, although it is stronger come increasingly concerned about behaving in ways
among White students than among Black students judged to be sex-appropriate. When boys show an interest
(Filardo, 1996; Sagar, Schofield, & Snyder, 1983), and it in dolls, they are often told either explicitly (by parents,
weakens later in adolescence (Mehta & Strough, 2009). friends, and teachers) or implicitly (by television, books,
The causes of sex segregation in adolescents’ cliques and other mass media), “Little boys don’t play with
are more interesting than the causes of age segregation, dolls—those are for girls.” And when girls start wrestling
because schools seldom separate boys and girls into dif- or roughhousing, they are often similarly reprimanded.
ferent classes. Why, then, do adolescent males and females As a consequence of these continual reminders
separate themselves into different cliques? Psychologist that there are boys’ activities and girls’ activities, early
Eleanor Maccoby, an expert on gender and development, adolescents—who are trying to establish a sense of
has suggested several reasons (Maccoby, 1990). identity—are very concerned about acting in sex-
First, cliques are formed largely on the basis of shared appropriate ways, although this is more true of boys
activities and interests. Generally, preadolescent and than girls (Galambos, Berenbaum, & McHale, 2009).
early adolescent boys and girls are interested in different This makes it very difficult for an adolescent boy to be a
things (Mehta & Strough, 2009). Not until adolescents part of a girls’ clique, in which activities are likely to
begin dating do boys’ cliques and girls’ cliques mix, pre- revolve around clothing, grooming, and talking about
sumably because dating provides a basis for common boys, or a girl to be part of a boys’ clique, in which ac-
activity. Consistent with this, one study of adolescents’ tivities are likely to be dominated by athletics and other
social networks found that the proportion of other-sex physical pursuits (Mehta & Strough, 2009). Adolescents
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 167 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 167

who go against prevailing sex-role norms by forming


friendships with members of the other sex may be
teased about being “fags” or may be ostracized by their
peers because they are “girly” (Oransky & Marecek,
2009). (Interestingly, gay male adolescents typically
have more cross-sex friendships than same-sex friend-
ships [Diamond & Dubé, 2002].) Ironically, once dat-
ing becomes the norm, adolescents who don’t have
relationships with peers of the other sex become the
objects of equally strong suspicion and social rejection.

❚ Ethnic Segregation Ethnicity is not a strong de-


terminant of clique composition during childhood, but
like social class, it becomes increasingly powerful as
youngsters get older, at least within the United States
(Raabe & Beelman, 2011). By middle and late adoles-
cence, adolescents’ peer groups typically are ethnically
segregated, with very few ethnically mixed cliques pres-
ent in most high schools (Ennett & Bauman, 1996).
This appears to be the case, although somewhat less so,
even within schools that have been desegregated. One
study of a multiethnic New York City public high school
found, for example, that nearly three-quarters of the
Latino students, two-thirds of the Black students, and
85% of the Asian students had friends who were pre-
dominantly from the same ethnic group (Way & Chen, Adolescents are more likely to have friends from the same ethnic
2000). In fact, cross-ethnic friendships are less common group than from the same socioeconomic background.
in ethnically diverse schools than in schools where one
ethnic group predominates (Moody, 2001; Quillian & (Quillian & Campbell, 2003). Parents appear to influ-
Campbell, 2003). ence this preference, as indicated by a study of Mexican
An analysis of data from a large, nationally represen- American adolescents, which found that adolescents
tative sample of adolescents found that ethnicity contin- were more likely to have non-Mexican friends when
ues to be an enormously powerful determinant of their parents were themselves more strongly oriented
friendship patterns—far more powerful than socioeco- toward Anglo culture (Updegraff, McHale, Whiteman,
nomic status (Quillian & Campbell, 2003). The rift be- Thayer, & Crouter, 2006). The social climate of the
tween Black students and students from all other ethnic school likely matters as well: Feelings of discrimination
groups, especially Whites and Asians, is especially strong, often drive ethnic minority students into peer crowds
and Black students’ reports of being discriminated that are defined by ethnicity (B. Brown, Herman,
against by other students increase over time (although Hamm, & Heck, 2008).
Asian students report the highest degree of discrimina- It is difficult to know why such strong ethnic segre-
tion by peers) (see Figure 5.7) (Greene, Way, & Pahl, gation persists in adolescents’ friendship selection. We
2006; Qin, Way, & Mukherjee, 2008). In fact, despite the know that ethnic segregation in adolescents’ cliques is
social class segregation discussed earlier, adolescents are only partly due to residential segregation (Mouw &
more likely to have friends of the same ethnicity who Entwisle, 2006). One strong possibility is that some
come from the opposite end of the socioeconomic spec- ethnic segregation in friendship patterns is due to dif-
trum than to have friends from the same social class but ferential levels of academic achievement of adoles-
a different ethnic group. Studies of whether cross-ethnic cents from different ethnic groups (Hamm et al.,
friendships are more common among Asian and His- 2005). As you’ll read in Chapter 12, on average, White
panic adolescents who are American born than among and Asian adolescents earn significantly higher grades
their peers who are immigrants have found that immi- in school than do Black or Hispanic adolescents.
grants are less likely to have cross-ethnic friendships, Adolescents who are friends usually have similar atti-
perhaps because of language barriers (Hamm, Brown, & tudes toward school, similar educational aspirations,
Heck, 2005; Kiang, Peterson, & Thompson, 2011). But and similar school achievement levels (B. Brown,
even among ethnic minority youth whose families have 2004). Ethnic differences in school achievement there-
been in the United States for generations, there still fore may lead to ethnic separation in adolescent peer
exists a strong preference for same-ethnicity friends groups (Hallinan & Williams, 1989). Cross-ethnic
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 168 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

168 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

FIGURE 5.7 Black students report increas- 2.4


ingly more discrimination by other students
over time, whereas it declines among Latino
2.2
students. Asian American students report

Perceived discrimination by peers


the highest level of peer discrimination. Asian American
(Greene et al., 2006) 2.0

1.8 Latino

1.6
Black
1.4
Puerto Rican
1.2

1.0
Time 0 Time 2 Time 3 Time 4 Time 5 Time 6

friendships are more rare, for instance, in schools that stereotypes, which tend to develop prior to adolescence
frequently separate students into different academic (Raabe & Beelman, 2011).
tracks (Stearns, 2004). Indeed, one study of Canadian adolescents indicated
A second reason for ethnically segregated peer that ethnic segregation in adolescent peer relationships
groups—according to one study of adolescents in a is far less common in Canada than in the United States
recently desegregated school—is attitudinal. In this (Maharaj & Connolly, 1994). According to the research-
school, the White adolescents perceived their Black ers, one reason for this is that the broader cultural
peers as aggressive, threatening, and hostile. The Black context in Canada is more supportive of multicultural
students, in turn, saw the White students as conceited, tolerance and more aggressive in its approach to multi-
prejudiced, and unwilling to be friends with them. cultural education and ethnic integration. Context
These perceptions, which fed on each other, made the continues to influence friendship choices as individuals
formation of interracial peer groups unlikely. The more move from high school to college: College students
the White students believed that the Black students who live in dorms where there is a lot of contact be-
were hostile, the more the White students acted distant tween ethnic groups are relatively more likely to de-
and kept to themselves. But the more the White stu- velop cross-ethnic friendships (Stearns, Buchmann, &
dents acted this way, the more likely the Black students Bonneau, 2009).
were to feel rejected, and the more hostile they became. Of course, not all adolescents have especially strong
In general, White students are less apt to initiate contact preferences for friends from the same ethnic background.
with Black students and to select them as friends than One comparison of two Chinese immigrant girls in a
vice versa (Quillian & Campbell, 2003). Similar find- U.S. high school illustrates this point nicely (Shih, 1998).
ings also have been reported in studies of Arab and One of the girls, Christine, had a strong preference for
Jewish students in Israel (Pitner, Astor, Benbenishty, friends who not only were Chinese but were Chinese im-
Haj-Yahia, & Zeira, 2003). One way out of this cycle of migrants as well. Although she initially was open-minded
misunderstanding is to bring youngsters from different about having friends who were not Chinese, she came to
backgrounds together from an early age, before they feel psychologically distant from them—to feel that she
have had time to build up prejudices and lock on to had little in common with them and that they made fun
of her speech and behavior. Denise, in contrast, was eager
to become Americanized and went out of her way to ac-
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION quire the slang, customs, and attitudes of mainstream
Thinking back to your adolescent culture. She saw having non-Chinese friends
experiences in high school, as a way of facilitating her Americanization and actively
what sorts of steps might sought them for this reason.
have been taken to foster
the development of more COMMON INTERESTS AMONG FRIENDS
cross-ethnic friendships? Thus far, we have seen that adolescents’ cliques are usu-
ally composed of individuals who are the same age, in
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 169 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 169

the same grade, from the same social class, and of the ❚ Orientation Toward the Teen Culture Ado-
same ethnicity. But what about factors beyond these? lescents and their friends generally listen to the same
Do adolescents who associate with one another also type of music, dress alike, spend their leisure time in
share certain interests and activities? Generally, they do. similar types of activities, and share patterns of drug use
Three factors appear to be especially important in de- (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). It would be very unlikely, for
termining adolescent clique membership and friend- example, for a “jock” and a “druggie” to be part of the
ship patterns: orientation toward school, orientation same clique, because their interests and attitudes are so
toward the teen culture, and involvement in antisocial different. In most high schools, it is fairly easy to see the
activity (Crosnoe & Needham, 2004). split between cliques—in how people dress, where they
eat lunch, how much they participate in the school’s ac-
❚ Orientation Toward School Adolescents and tivities, and how they spend their time outside of school.
their friends tend to be similar in their attitudes toward Indeed, similarity in patterns of substance use is such a
school, in their school achievement, and in their edu- strong influence that it often serves as the basis for
cational plans (Flashman, 2012), although this tends to forming cross-ethnic group friendships, which, as we
be more true among White and Asian adolescents than noted earlier, are not common (Hamm, 2000).
among Black adolescents (Hamm, 2000). Adolescents
who earn high grades, study a great deal, and plan to go ❚ Involvement in Antisocial Activity A number
on to college usually have friends who share these of studies, involving both boys and girls from different
characteristics and aspirations. One reason for this is ethnic groups, indicate that antisocial, aggressive adoles-
that how much time students devote to schoolwork af- cents often gravitate toward each other, forming deviant
fects their involvement in other activities. Another is peer groups (Dishion, Patterson, Stoolmiller, & Skinner,
that students’ friendships are often drawn from the 1991; Espelage, Holt, & Henkel, 2003; Kiesner, Cadinu,
peers with whom they have classes, and if schools track Poulin, & Bucci, 2002; Kiesner & Pastore, 2005; Kreager,
students on the basis of their academic achievement, 2004). That is, contrary to the popular belief that antiso-
their friends will be more likely to have similar records cial adolescents do not have friends or that they are in-
of school performance (Crosnoe, 2002). Someone who terpersonally inept, these youngsters do have friends,
is always studying will not have many friends who stay but their friends tend to be antisocial as well. Although
out late partying, because the two activities conflict. By adolescents with deviant friends show some of the same
the same token, someone who wants to spend after- emotional problems as adolescents without friends,
noons and evenings out having fun will find it difficult even those with deviant friends are less lonely than their
to remain friends with someone who prefers to stay friendless peers (Brendgen, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 2000).
home and study. One recent study found that when As you might expect, adolescents with more antisocial
adolescents’ academic performance changed (for bet- friends are more likely to engage in antisocial activity,
ter or for worse), they tended to change their friend- but some adolescents seem especially susceptible to the
ships in the same direction (Flashman, 2012). influence of antisocial peers (Laursen, Hafen, Kerr, &
Students also may influence each other’s academic Stattin, 2012; S. Lee, 2011; Monahan, Steinberg, &
performance. For instance, girls’ decisions about Cauffman, 2009). Even within facilities for juvenile de-
whether to take advanced math classes are significantly linquents, the relatively more antisocial adolescents tend
influenced by the course-taking decisions of their to gravitate together and influence each other toward
friends (K. A. Frank et al., 2008). Friends exert a similar more antisocial activity (Bayer, Hjalmarsson, & Pozen,
influence on GPA: Given two students with similar re- 2009; B. Lee & Thompson, 2009).
cords of past achievement, the student whose friends Although we would not necessarily want to call all of
do better in school is likely to achieve more than the these antisocial peer groups “delinquent,” since they are
one whose friends do worse (Véronneau & Dishion, not always involved in criminal activity, understanding
2011). Similarly, adolescents whose friends are disrup- the processes through which antisocial peer groups are
tive in school tend to become more disruptive over time formed provides some insight into the development of
(Berndt & Keefe, 1995). Indeed, of all the characteristics delinquent peer groups, or gangs (Melde & Esbensen,
of friends that influence adolescents’ behavior, their 2011). Gangs are antisocial peer groups that can be
friends’ school performance has the greatest impact, identified by name (often denoting a neighborhood or
not only on their own academic achievement, but also part of the city) and common symbols (“colors,” tat-
on their involvement in problem behavior and drug use toos, hand signs, jewelry, etc.) (Branch, 1995; M. Harris,
(T. Cook, Deng, & Morgano, 2007). Perhaps not sur- 1994; Winfree, Bäckström, & Mays, 1994). Adolescents
prisingly, students whose friends tend to come from who belong to gangs are at greater risk for many types
school have higher GPAs than those whose friends tend
to come from other contexts, such as the neighborhood
(Witkow & Fuligni, 2010).
gangs Organized peer groups of antisocial individuals.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 170 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

170 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

of problems in addition to antisocial behavior, includ- Robinson, 1999). In addition, a study of Puerto Rican
ing elevated levels of psychological distress, exposure to male adolescents from New York City suggests that the
violence, and violent victimization (X. Li et al., 2002; actual relationships that antisocial adolescents have with
Melde, Taylor, & Esbensen, 2009; T. J. Taylor, Peterson, their clique-mates may be less satisfying than are those
Esbensen, & Freng, 2007). This is also true for female between other adolescents and their friends (Pabon,
adolescents who hang around with male gangs, which Rodriguez, & Gurin, 1992). Although the antisocial
increases their involvement in high-risk sexual behav- peers in this study spent a lot of time together, they did
ior, drug use, and crime. Here is one girl’s description of not describe their relationships as emotionally close or
another girl’s rape, taken from a study of Mexican intimate. Rather, most of the boys felt estranged from
American females who associated with gang members each other. As the researchers point out, this finding has
in San Antonio: implications for the design of interventions aimed at
controlling delinquency by involving antisocial peer
She was already drunk when she came to the party. I
think she was 17, but I’m not too sure. She was a new girl. groups in positive activities; in the absence of their shared
She didn’t know nothing about the Brothers [male gang]. interest in antisocial activities, delinquent peers may have
She didn’t know nothing at all, nothing about gangs, little reason to maintain their friendship.
drugs. All she knew about was drinking, and drinking,
and drinking, and that’s it. ❚ The Role of Parents According to several studies
The Brother, he just took her to a bedroom. He was tell- (Dishion et al., 1991; Garnier & Stein, 2002; Scaramella,
ing her that he wanted to show her something. She went Conger, Spoth, & Simons, 2002; Tolan, Gorman-Smith,
up to the room after he told her that he had a diamond & Henry, 2003), the process of antisocial peer group for-
ring for her. So she went upstairs. He put alcohol on mation in adolescence begins in the home, during child-
some rag and covered her face. That’s all I heard. My hood. Problematic parent–child relationships—ones
homeboy said just go home, and I went. He told me that that are coercive and hostile—lead to the development
he didn’t want nothing to happen to me, and I just went of an antisocial disposition in the child, and this disposi-
home. (Cepeda & Valdez, 2003, p. 99)
tion contributes, in elementary school, to both school
Are adolescent gangs merely a peer group whose fo- failure and rejection by classmates (Dishion et al., 1991;
cus is on antisocial activity, or do they have unique char- Pardini, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2005). Rejected
acteristics? Research indicates that gangs both resemble by the bulk of his classmates, the aggressive boy “shops”
and differ from other sorts of peer groups. On the one for friends and finds that he is accepted only by other
hand, as one study of Mexican American female gang aggressive boys. Once these friendships are formed, the
members points out, adolescent gangs look much like boys, like any other clique, reward each other for par-
other types of adolescent cliques, in that they are groups ticipating in a shared activity—in this case, antisocial
of adolescents who are similar in background and ori- behavior. Interestingly, improvements in parenting
entation, share common interests and activities, and use during adolescence reduce teenagers’ association with
the group to derive a sense of identity (M. Harris, 1994). antisocial peers, which, in turn, reduces problematic be-
One study of Latino youth in Southern California found havior (R. Simons, Chao, Conger, & Elder, 2001).
that it was especially important to differentiate between The family and peer contexts are connected through
gangs, which were organized and had long histories of other processes as well (Brown & Bakken, 2011). As psy-
involvement in serious antisocial behavior, and “crews,” chologist Nina Mounts has pointed out (2004, 2007),
which also engaged in fighting, tagging, and partying, parents often “manage” their adolescent’s friendships by
but which did not engage in serious violence (Lopez, monitoring the individuals their child spends time with,
Wishard, Gallimore, & Rivera, 2006). This distinction guiding their child toward peers they like, prohibiting
has important legal ramifications, because anti-gang contact with peers they dislike, and supporting friend-
laws that mandate tougher penalties for crimes commit- ships they approve of (Tilton-Weaver & Galambos,
ted by gangs may be incorrectly applied to adolescents 2003; Updegraff, Kim, Killoren, & Thayer, 2010). Parents
who commit delinquent acts with their friends (or also act as “consultants,” helping their teenagers work
crew) but who are not members of gangs. out problems with their friends (Mounts, 2011). Adoles-
It is also the case that the processes that lead adoles- cents whose parents act as consultants in this way are
cents to join gangs are not the same as those that lead to less likely to be involved in drug use and delinquent ac-
membership in crews and other sorts of peer groups. tivity and report more positive relationships with their
Gang members tend to be more isolated from their fam- friends (Mounts, 2004). On the other hand, excessive
ily, to have more emotional and behavioral problems, and attempts to control an adolescent’s choice of friends
to have poorer self-conceptions than other adolescents, may backfire; when parents forbid adolescents from
including those who are involved in antisocial activity but associating with peers the parents disapprove of, they
who are not gang members (Dukes, Martinez, & Stein, may inadvertently drive adolescents to become closer to
1997; Esbensen, Deschenes, & Winfree, 1999; Harper & those peers, perhaps in defiance of these restrictions on
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 171 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 171

their independence (Keijsers et al., 2012). Rather than observational study of adolescent friends talking to
viewing the family and peer contexts as separate worlds, each other on camera (Piehler & Dishion, 2007) found
it is important to keep in mind that what takes place that individuals who had a history of involvement in
in one setting often has an impact on what occurs in antisocial behavior engaged in more spontaneous
others. conversation about antisocial activities and rewarded
The role of the family in friendship choice has each other in the way they responded (e.g., “We were so
also been described in studies of crowds (B. Brown, wasted last Friday.” “Oh, yeah, that was insane!”
Mounts, Lamborn, & Steinberg, 1993; Curtner-Smith & “Remember the time we stole that vodka?” “That was so
MacKinnon-Lewis, 1994; C. Mason, Cauce, Gonzales, & awesome!”). Several writers have described this process
Hiraga, 1996; Melby, 1995). One of the factors that influ- as “deviancy training” (e.g., Capaldi, Dishion, Stoolmiller,
ences the crowd an adolescent belongs to is her or his & Yoerger, 2001; Dishion, Nelson, Winter, & Bullock,
upbringing. Parents play a role in socializing certain 2004). Knowing that group treatments for antisocial
traits in their children, and these orientations, whether behavior have iatrogenic effects is obviously important
toward aggression or academic achievement, predispose for the design of programs for delinquent and aggres-
adolescents toward choosing certain friends or crowds sive youth.
with which to affiliate. Once in these cliques or crowds, Other approaches to violence reduction that make
adolescents are rewarded for the traits that led them sense intuitively have frequently proven ineffective, if
there in the first place, and these traits are strengthened. not exactly harmful. For instance, some researchers have
One problem with accounts of adolescent development found that trying to teach adolescents to use nonviolent
that posit the peer group as more important than the forms of conflict resolution are often unsuccessful be-
family (e.g., J. R. Harris, 1998) is that they fail to take cause such behaviors are perceived by adolescents as being
into account the fact that the family has a strong effect weak or even inviting retaliation (Farrell et al., 2010). Here
on adolescents’ choice of peers. are some typical responses from urban Black students
For example, a child who is raised to value academics when asked why using nonviolent means of settling argu-
will perform well in school and will likely select friends ments was unpopular (Farrell et al., 2010, p. 11):
who share this orientation. Over time, these friends will
They [other students] want to see a fight. They want to
reinforce the youngster’s academic orientation and see chaos. They want to see people go at each other, so it’s
strengthen his or her school performance. By the same like a big show or something. They like fighting. They’ll
token, antisocial adolescents, who are drawn toward try to egg them on.
other antisocial peers, become more antisocial over time If you don’t fight, people gonna say stuff about you. If you
as a result (Vitaro, Tremblay, Kerr, Pagani, & Bukowski, do fight, they still will, but you know that you won. . . . you
1997). Even when adolescents have relatively more anti- don’t want everybody to think you a punk or nothing.
social friends, having better relationships at home and a Other people think you’re scared or something, so you
stronger attachment to school will make them less sus- just go ahead and fight him, try to show off, try to prove
ceptible to their friends’ negative influence—even in the that you’re better or . . . that you’re not scared.
context of a gang (Crosnoe, Erickson, & Dornbusch,
2002; Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2000; Walker- SIMILARITY BETWEEN FRIENDS:
Barnes & Mason, 2004). SELECTION OR SOCIALIZATION?
The finding that adolescents become more antiso-
cial when they spend time with antisocial peers has Because antisocial activities are such a strong determi-
prompted some experts to question the wisdom of nant of clique composition, many adults have expressed
group-based interventions for adolescents with conduct concern over the influence of peers in the promotion of
problems (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999). Several delinquent activity and drug and alcohol use. Parents
studies of programs designed to diminish adolescents’ often feel that if their teenager runs with the wrong
delinquency or aggression, for example, have found that, crowd, he or she will acquire undesirable interests and
instead of having the desired effect, the programs actu- attitudes. They express concern, for instance, when their
ally increased participants’ problem behavior; that is, child starts spending time with peers who seem to be less
they had what scientists call iatrogenic effects (Mahoney, interested in school or more involved with drugs. But
Stattin, & Lord, 2004). which comes first—joining a clique or being interested
Iatrogenic effects are the undesirable consequences in a clique’s activities? Do adolescents develop interests
of well-intentioned treatments—for example, when the and attitudes because their friends influence them in
side effects of a medication are far worse than the prob- this direction, or is it more the case that people with
lem it is intended to treat. When antisocial adolescents similar interests and tastes are likely to become friends?
spend time with like-minded peers, they frequently
teach each other how to be “more effective” delin-
quents and reward each other for misbehavior. One iatrogenic effects Unintended adverse consequences
of a treatment or intervention.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 172 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

172 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

H. Cleveland & Wiebe, 2003; Ennett et al., 2006; Urberg,


Değirmencioğlu, & Pilgrim, 1997). The more substance-
using friends an adolescent has, and the closer he or she
feels to them, the more the adolescent is likely to use al-
cohol and drugs (Ennett et al., 2006; Hussong & Hicks,
2003). Even dating someone whose friends are substance
users has similar effects (Kreager & Haynie, 2011). Simi-
larly, adolescents who report more depressive symptoms
are likely to choose other depressed adolescents as
friends, which, in turn, negatively affects their own mood
and that of their friends, although in the case of de-
pression, adolescents are more likely to be affected
by hanging around with depressed friends than to ac-
tively select depressed individuals to spend time with
(Brendgen, Lamarche, Wanner, & Vitaro, 2010; Giletta et
al., 2011; Hogue & Steinberg, 1995; Prinstein et al., 2010;
E. A. Stevens & Prinstein, 2005). Adolescents who are
bullies are more likely to have friends who themselves
are bullies (Espelage, Holt, & Henkel, 2003). Conversely,
antisocial adolescents who have few friends, and few
aggressive friends in particular, are likely to become less
antisocial over time (Adams, Bukowski, & Bagwell,
2005; Botvin & Vitaro, 1995), whereas those with antiso-
cial friends who become even more antisocial them-
selves become more delinquent (Monahan, Steinberg, &
Adolescents and their friends usually share common interests, Cauffman, 2009; Weerman, 2011; Werner & Silbereisen,
in everything from academics to antisocial behavior. 2003). In general, adolescents’ level of antisocial behav-
ior tends to become more similar to that of their friends
This question has been examined in several large- over time, increasing if their friends are more antisocial
scale studies that have tracked adolescents and their than they are, but declining if their friends are less so;
friendships over time (e.g., Curran, Stice, & Chassin, interestingly, the larger the discrepancy between friends
1997; Dobkin, Tremblay, Mâsse, & Vitaro, 1995; Ennett in their levels of delinquency, the more likely they are to
& Bauman, 1994; Mounts & Steinberg, 1995; Poulin, become similar (McGloin, 2009).
Dishion, & Haas, 1999). By tracing patterns of attitu- How much of adolescents’ similarity to their friends
dinal and behavioral change, and comparing these is due to selection and how much is due to socialization?
shifts with patterns of friendship formation and The answer depends on what and who is being studied.
change, the researchers could determine whether ado- Peer influence (socialization) is far stronger over day-
lescents are attracted to one another because of their to-day preferences in things like clothing or music than
initial similarity (what social scientists refer to as selec- over many of the behaviors that adults worry about,
tion), whether they become similar because friends in- such as binge drinking or risky sex (Jaccard, Blanton, &
fluence each other (what is referred to as socialization), Dodge, 2005; Knecht, Burk, Weesie, & Steglich, 2011),
or a combination of the two (Brechwald & Prinstein, or even obesity (la Haye, Robins, Mohr, & Wilson, 2011).
2011). Selection may be a somewhat stronger factor as far as
In general, studies indicate that both selection and delinquency and gang membership are concerned
socialization are at work (see Figure 5.8) across a variety (Dobkin et al., 1995; R. A. Gordon et al., 2004; Haynie &
of attitudinal and behavioral domains, including school
achievement, drug use, mental health, and delinquency
(Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011; B. Brown & Larson, 2009),
and in romantic relationships as well as friendships
(Simon, Aikins, & Prinstein, 2008). Adolescents who use Adolescents’
Adolescents’ friends
alcohol or tobacco, for example, are more likely to choose characteristics
influence each
other alcohol or tobacco users as friends, especially when influence their
other’s characteristics
choice of friends
they attend schools with a large number of substance-
using students (an example of selection). By the same
token, spending time with friends who use these sub-
stances increases the adolescents’ own use as well (an FIGURE 5.8 Adolescents’ choice of friends both
example of socialization) (Bryant & Zimmerman, 2002; influences and is influenced by their traits and interests.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 173 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 173

Osgood, 2005; Selfhout, Branje, & Meeus, 2008), whereas Değirmencioğlu, Tolson, & Urberg, 1993; Poulin &
selection and socialization are about equally influential Chan, 2010). Although the actual composition of ado-
when it comes to drug use (Curran et al., 1997; Dishion lescents’ cliques may shift over time, the defining char-
& Owen, 2002; Ennett & Bauman, 1994). Peer influence acteristics of their cliques or their best friends do not
on antisocial behavior is generally stronger among White (Hogue & Steinberg, 1995; Luo, Urberg, & Rao, 1995;
adolescents than in other cultural groups (C. S. Chen, Neckerman, Cairns, & Cairns, 1993). That is, even
Greenberger, Lester, Dong, & Guo, 1998). though some members of an adolescent’s clique may
One analysis of a data from a large, nationally repre- leave and be replaced by others, the new members are
sentative sample of adolescents found that adolescents’ likely to have attitudes and values that are quite similar
friendship groups fall into one of four profiles: high to the former members’ (B. Brown, 2004). Even “best
functioning (a network of high-achieving friends who friendships” are likely to change during the school year.
were involved in school-based extracurricular activi- One study found that only about one-third of students
ties and who reported low use of alcohol and few who were best friends in the fall of a school year re-
symptoms of depression), maladjusted (friends showed named the same person as their best friend in the
the opposite pattern), disengaged (friends were not spring, although that person was typically listed as
engaged in much of anything, including drinking), and a friend (Değirmencioğlu, Urberg, Tolson, & Richard,
engaged (friends were engaged in school, achieved 1998). Only half of all reciprocated best friendships (in
decent grades, and neither abstained from nor abused which each person names the other as a best friend)
alcohol) (Crosnoe & Needham, 2004). More important, that exist at the beginning of a school year exist at the
an individual adolescent’s behavior could be predicted end; surprisingly, remaining friends over the course of
on the basis of her or his friendship group’s profile. a school year has nothing to do with the quality of the
Adolescents’ socioeconomic background and family friendship (Bowker, 2004). Same-sex friendships tend
situation were strongly related to characteristics of their to be more stable than opposite-sex friendships, and
friendship network, with teenagers from more educated, boys’ friendships trend to be more stable than girls’.
two-parent families more likely to fall into the high- Generally speaking, friendship stability is higher
functioning and engaged groups. among well-adjusted adolescents than their more
troubled peers, although it isn’t clear whether this is
❚ Stability of Adolescent Friendships How because stability contributes to adjustment, because
stable are adolescents’ friendships over time? In gen- better-adjusted adolescents are better at maintaining
eral, adolescents’ cliques show only moderate stability friendships, or, most likely, a combination of both
over the course of the school year—with some mem- (Poulin & Chan, 2010). The most common causes of
bers staying in the clique, others leaving, and new ones broken friendships are jealousy, incompatibility, viola-
joining—although cliques become more stable later in tions of intimacy, and aggression (Casper & Card,
high school (Cairns, Leung, Buchanan, & Cairns, 1995; 2010) (see Table 5.2).

TABLE 5.2 Summary of broad categories of features that led to dislike

Category Exemplar Quotes

Jealousy “She stole my boyfriend and my closest friends. Then got angry when she found out I slept
with him.”
“This person disliked my boyfriend/date I was going to bring to the prom, which led me to going
to the prom with my boyfriend only. I felt that she had ruined my senior life in high school.”
Incompatibility “An argument started when my friend would just make really loud noises for no reason, and
when I would ask her to stop she would just continue.”
“This person was loud, annoying, and really had no point of talking because her argument
made no sense.”
“We stopped talking and hanging out for no apparent reason. All of a sudden it just ended.”
Intimacy-rule Violations “We were best friends, but I couldn’t trust her because she lied to me too many times.”
“In the beginning she seemed like an awesome friend but then, after I started getting close
to her, I saw her true colors revealed. She had a very evil way of trying to hurt people and
put them down. She was also very untrustworthy.”
Aggression “She spread rumors about me because the guy she liked, liked me.”
“She was with a boy at our senior BBQ and was taking her time when we were in a rush. She
and the boy went home with someone else instead of me and didn’t tell me. The next day
I confronted her and we got into a fight and suspended from school. That’s when she
started spreading rumors.”
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 174 21/03/13 10:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

174 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

RECAP
• Cliques are small groups of adolescents who are
friends and who see each other regularly.
• Unlike crowds, cliques play an important role in the
development of social skills and intimacy.
• Generally, adolescents form cliques with peers who
are similar in family and ethnic background, in ori-
entation toward school and the peer culture, and in
their level of involvement in antisocial activities.
• Although clique members influence each other’s
behavior and values, research also shows that
adolescents initially select their friends on the
basis of similarity.
• Adolescents may change friends, but
they rarely change the type
of people they are
friends with.

Popularity and Rejection


in Adolescent Peer Groups
Thus far, our discussion has focused on how and why
crowds and cliques serve as the basis for adolescents’ Researchers distinguish between “sociometric” popularity—how
social activities and attitudes. But what about the inter- well-liked someone is—and “perceived” popularity—how much
nal structure of peer groups? Within a clique or a crowd, prestige someone has. They don’t always go hand in hand.
what determines which adolescents are popular and
which are not?
to be a really nice person with a good sense of humor
may be high in sociometric popularity but low in per-
DETERMINANTS OF POPULARITY
ceived popularity. If you think back to your own high
AND REJECTION
school days, you can probably remember people of
In recent years, psychologists have changed their think- each type.
ing about what it is that leads to popularity during ado- Whereas sociometric popularity is determined
lescence. Although it is widely agreed that popular mainly by social skills, friendliness, sense of humor, and
adolescents are generally more socially skilled than so forth, which are valued pretty much by people of all
their unpopular peers, there is surprising variability ages and backgrounds, the determinants of perceived
among popular teenagers with respect to other charac- popularity are highly variable. Having a boyfriend or
teristics. One reason for this is that there are two forms girlfriend, for example, may have little to do with per-
of popularity, and they don’t always go hand in hand ceived popularity in 5th grade, but may be highly cor-
(B. Brown & Larson, 2009). One form, sociometric related with it in 9th grade. Because the determinants
popularity, refers to how well-liked someone is. The of status can easily differ between schools, or even
other form, perceived popularity, refers to how much among groups within the same school, it is hard to pre-
status, or prestige, someone has (Cillessen & Rose, dict which adolescents will be popular without know-
2005). So, for example, a leader of the “preppie” crowd ing what is valued in that adolescent’s social context
who is snobby might be very high in perceived popular- (Jonkmann, Trautwein, & Lüdtke, 2009; Kreager,
ity but not in sociometric popularity. Conversely, a 2007b). Although there is one main pathway to socio-
member of a crowd that has less prestige who happens metric popularity (having good social skills), the deter-
minants of perceived popularity are variable and ever
changing. Within the very same school, some adoles-
sociometric popularity How well-liked an individual is. cents are highly regarded by their peers because they are
good-looking and athletic (the conventional image of
perceived popularity How much status or prestige an individual the popular teenager), whereas others are equally highly
has.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 175 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 175

regarded because they are rebellious, delinquent, and and young adolescent boys found two distinct types of
aggressive (B. E. Becker & Luthar, 2007; Jonkmann et al., popular boys (Rodkin, Farmer, Pearl, & Van Acker,
2009). Moreover, whereas many of the things that 2000). One group, whom the researchers described as
lead to popularity also make adolescents more likeable “model” boys, had the characteristics typically identi-
(e.g., athletic ability, physical attractiveness, social fied in studies of popular youth: They were physically
skills), some of the things that help to maintain popu- and academically competent, friendly, and neither shy
larity once it is established may actually make adoles- nor aggressive. A second group, however, whom the re-
cents less likeable (e.g., using gossip to control or searchers described as “tough,” were extremely aggres-
manipulate others) (Dijkstra, Cillessen, Lindenberg, & sive, physically competent, and average or below average
Veenstra, 2010b; Lansu & Cillessen, 2011; Neal, 2010). in friendliness, academic competence, and shyness.
Similarly, one study of girls found two distinctly differ-
ent groups of popular adolescents: girls who were pro-
social and good students, and girls who were antisocial
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
and antiacademic, some of whom actually were even
Which do you think are more bullies (de Bruyn & Cillessen, 2006). Other studies find
similar around the world, the that some highly aggressive boys are often very popular
determinants of sociometric popu- (Fanti, Brookmeyer, Henrich, & Kuperminc, 2009).
larity, or the determinants of How can we explain this? Wouldn’t we expect that
perceived popularity? Why? adolescents who are aggressive toward others would be
unpopular? Evidently, it is not aggression alone, but the
combination of aggression and difficulty controlling
emotions or a lack of social skills, that leads to problems
Predicting perceived popularity is further compli- with peers (Dijkstra, Lindenberg, Verhulst, Ormel, &
cated by the fact that peer norms change, and socially Veenstra, 2009; Farmer, Estell, Bishop, O’Neal, & Cairns,
competent adolescents are skilled at figuring them out, 2003; Peeters, Cillessen, & Scholte, 2010). Consistent
adjusting their behavior in response to them, and even with this, aggressive adolescents who use their aggres-
influencing them. If, for instance, smoking marijuana sion strategically—what is referred to as instrumental
becomes something that is valued by the peer group, aggression—are much more popular than aggressive
popular adolescents will start getting high more regu- adolescents whose aggression is unplanned—what is re-
larly (Allen, Porter, McFarland, Marsh, & McElhaney, ferred to as reactive aggression (T. D. Little, Brauner,
2005). And when popular adolescents start to engage in Jones, Nock, & Hawley, 2003; Prinstein & Cillessen,
a particular behavior, that behavior often becomes 2003).
more admired. Indeed, one of the reasons it is hard to
persuade adolescents to “just say no” to drinking, smok- ❚ The Dynamics of Popularity Two ethno-
ing, and sex is that these activities are often associated graphic studies of early adolescent girls provide insight
with being popular (Balsa, Homer, French, & Norton, into the dynamics of popularity. In a classic study, eth-
2011; Lansford, Killeya-Jones, Miller, & Costanzo, 2009; nographer Donna Eder (1985) spent two years in a
Mayeux, Sandstrom, & Cillessen, 2008). Even things middle school observing interactions among early ado-
like fighting, bullying or carrying a weapon, which lescent girls in various extracurricular and informal
most adolescents do not approve of, become more ac- settings (in the cafeteria, in the hallway, at school
ceptable when popular adolescents start to do these dances). Although the study is 30 years old, many of the
things (Bellmore, Villarreal, & Ho, 2011; Dijkstra, Lin- researcher’s observations still ring true today.
denberg, & Veenstra, 2008; Dijkstra et al., 2010). As you In this school, the cheerleaders were considered the
will read in a later chapter, in which we look at re- elite crowd, and girls who made the cheerleading squad
sponses to peer pressure, adolescents are easily swayed were immediately accorded social status. Other girls
by the opinions of high-status peers to endorse activi- then attempted to befriend the cheerleaders as a means
ties that they might otherwise reject and to run the of increasing their own status. This, in turn, increased
other way from activities endorsed by low-status peers, the cheerleaders’ popularity within the school, as they
even if they secretly enjoy them (G. L. Cohen & Prin- became the most sought-after friends. The girls who
stein, 2006). were successful in cultivating friendships with the

❚ Popularity and Aggression Although psycholo-


gists used to believe that aggressive adolescents are likely instrumental aggression Aggressive behavior that is deliberate
to be rejected by their classmates, it turns out that some and planned.
teenagers are both aggressive and popular (de Bruyn,
Cillessen, & Wissink, 2010). One study of preadolescent reactive aggression Aggressive behavior that is unplanned and
impulsive.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 176 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

176 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

cheerleaders then became a part of this high-status recognition (such as being selected as leaders of school
group and became more popular themselves. But be- organizations) (Franzoi et al., 1994). Part of the overlap
cause even popular adolescents can only maintain a fi- between popularity and friendship no doubt stems from
nite number of friendships, they end up snubbing other the fact that many of the characteristics that make ado-
classmates who want to be their friends. Ironically, this lescents popular are the same ones that make them
often leads to popular adolescents becoming disliked sought after as friends—chief among them, having good
(Mayeux et al., 2008). Thus, adolescents who hang out social skills.
with popular adolescents may themselves become per- Interestingly, adolescents who see themselves as
ceived as more popular over time but they may also well-liked and socially competent fare well over time,
become less well-liked, because they are seen as snobby regardless of whether they are genuinely popular
status-seekers, especially by their less popular peers among their classmates (McElhaney, Antonishak, &
(Dijkstra, Cillessen, Lindenberg, & Veenstra, 2010b; Allen, 2008). One possible reason for this is that some
Lansu, Cillessen, & Karremans, 2012). adolescents who are not especially popular in school
In another ethnography, the researcher spent time may have a well-developed network of friends outside
observing and interviewing a group described by teach- of school. Because most research on adolescents’ peer
ers as the “dirty dozen” (D. Merten, 1997). This group networks has been limited to school-based friendships,
of girls, “considered ‘cool,’ ‘popular,’ and ‘mean,’” were we know relatively little about the nature or effects of
“a combination of cute, talented, affluent, conceited, nonschool friendships. We do know that many adoles-
and powerful” (1997, p. 178). The researcher was inter- cents have a social life outside of school—at church, in
ested in understanding “why a clique of girls that was the neighborhood, in nonschool extracurricular activi-
popular and socially sophisticated was also renowned ties—that is quite different from their life in school.
for its meanness” (1997, p. 188). The answer, he discov- Studies that examine the impact of friends on adoles-
ered, was that meanness was one of the ways that the cent development may miss important information if
clique ensured that no one member became stuck-up as they do not include information about the number and
a result of her popularity in the eyes of her classmates. characteristics of the adolescent’s nonschool friends.
Thus, while it was important for clique members to One study of Italian middle school students found, for
maintain their popular image, if any clique member ap- example, that having friends outside school buffered
peared to become too popular, the other members the harmful consequences of having few friends in
would turn on her, undermining her standing with school (Kiesner, Poulin, & Nicotra, 2003).
other girls by gossiping, starting rumors, and deliber-
ately attempting to disrupt her friendships. The follow- ❚ Rejected Adolescents Just as there are different
ing quote, from a girl whose friends turned on her, will reasons for being popular, there are also different reasons
sound all too familiar: for being rejected. Social scientists have shown that it is
important to distinguish among three types of disliked
Gretchen was starting to get really mad at me. I talked to adolescents (Bierman & Wargo, 1995; Coie, Terry, Lenox,
her about it and I asked her what was wrong. She just Lochman, & Hyman, 1995; D. French, Conrad, & Turner,
said, “Oh, I heard something you said about me.” But I 1995; Hatzichristou and Hopf, 1996). As we have seen,
didn’t say anything about her. Sara was mad at me. I don’t
one set of unpopular adolescents comprises teenagers
know why. She started being mad at me and then she
started making things up that [she said] I said. Sara told
who have trouble controlling their aggression. With-
Brenda and Gretchen so that they would get mad at me, drawn adolescents make up a second unpopular set;
too. So now I guess Gretchen has made up something these adolescents are exceedingly shy, anxious, and in-
and told Wellesley. They are all mad at me and laughing hibited and are frequently victims of bullying, especially
and everything. (D. Merten, 1997, p. 182) when they are boys (Erath, Flanagan, & Bierman, 2007).
A third group is both aggressive and withdrawn. These
Ironically, then, one of the potential costs of being pop- adolescents have problems controlling their hostility, but
ular in adolescence is that if you become too popular, like other withdrawn children, they tend to be nervous
you face the very real possibility of being the object of about initiating friendships with other adolescents.
other classmates’ meanness. The origins of peer rejection in adolescence can often
Although popularity clearly has some costs, the advan- be traced to earlier periods of development. One study
tages of being popular far outweigh the disadvantages. Be- found that adolescents who were rejected by their peers
ing popular is not the same as having close and intimate also had experienced peer rejection during middle
friendships (Asher, Parker, & Walker, 1996), but the two childhood and that this rejection, in turn, was the con-
often go hand in hand (Franzoi, Davis, & Vasquez-Suson, sequence of behavioral and emotional difficulties in
1994). Compared with their less popular age-mates, pop- early elementary school (Pedersen, Vitaro, Barker, &
ular adolescents are more likely to have close and intimate Borge, 2007). Regardless of its causes, rejection by peers
friendships, participate in social activities with peers, take is a significant source of stress for adolescents, who show
part in extracurricular activities, and receive more social greater brain activation and a stronger biological stress
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 177 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 177

response to rejection than children do (Bolling et al.,


2011; Stroud et al., 2009). As we noted in Chapter 2,
during adolescence there are important changes in the
brain that lead individuals to become more sensitive to
the emotions, expressions, and opinions of others. One
fascinating study of adolescents’ ratings of music im-
aged participants’ brains while they were listening to
songs with or without the songs’ popularity revealed.
The study found that changing one’s evaluation of a
song after being told whether it was popular or not was
correlated with brain activity in regions known to re-
flect anxiety, suggesting that feeling anxious about
whether one’s tastes in music are “correct” may lead
teenagers to conform to others (Berns, Capra, Moore, &
Noussair, 2010).

RELATIONAL AGGRESSION
Most studies of the peer relations of aggressive children
have focused on children who are overtly aggressive (ei-
ther physically or verbally). This has led researchers to
focus relatively more attention on the social relation-
ships of aggressive boys than girls, because boys
exhibit more overt aggression than girls (Card, Stucky,
Sawalani, & Little, 2008). Girls also act aggressively to-
ward peers, but their aggression is often social, not
physical (Crick, 1996). Psychologists have been inter-
ested in the use of relational aggression—aggression
intended to harm other adolescents through deliberate Although boys are more physically aggressive than girls, girls often
manipulation of their social standing and relationships. engage in what has been called relational aggression—an attempt
to harm someone by ruining his or her reputation or disrupting his
Individuals who use relational aggression try to hurt
or her friendships.
others by excluding them from social activities, damag-
ing their reputations with others, or withdrawing atten-
tion and friendship. Physical and relational aggression
follow similar developmental trajectories during ado- ❚ “Mean Girls” Although relational aggression was
lescence, increasing during early adolescence and then first noticed in observations of girls, studies show that
declining from mid-adolescence on, and are correlated both genders employ it, but that girls are more aware of
(that is, individuals who are highly aggressive in one it, more distressed by it, and more often the victims of it
way are also aggressive in the other, and individuals (Card et al., 2008; D. French, Jansen, & Pidada, 2002;
who are frequent victims of physical aggression are also Pronk & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2010; Sullivan, Farrell, &
frequent victims of relational aggression) (Card et al., Kliewer, 2006). One study of adolescents’ reasoning
2008; Karriker-Jaffe, Foshee, Ennett, & Suchindran, about excluding people from group activities found, for
2009; Nylund, Bellmore, Nishina, & Graham, 2007). example, that girls are more likely than boys to say that it
Adolescents who use a lot of relational aggression often is morally wrong to exclude someone simply on the basis
have parents who are harsh or controlling (Kawabata, of the crowd to which he or she belongs (Horn, 2003).
Alink, Tseng, van IJzendoorn, & Crick, 2011). Interestingly, adolescents whose aggression is atypical for
their gender (that is, girls who are highly physically ag-
gressive and boys who are highly relationally aggressive)
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION show more maladjustment than their peers whose ag-
How might exposure to harsh and gression is more gender-stereotypic (Crick, 1997).
controlling parents lead adolescents Girls’ use of relational aggression has attracted a great
to use relational aggression when deal of popular attention, as reflected in the best-selling
dealing with peers?
relational aggression Acts intended to harm another through the
manipulation of his or her relationships with others, as in malicious
gossip.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 178 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

178 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

books Odd Girl Out (Simmons, 2003) and Queen Bees and popularity. Many programs designed to reduce relational
Wannabees (Wiseman, 2003), which served as the basis for aggression may be ineffective because adolescents are re-
the movie Mean Girls. Perhaps in response, educators have luctant to stop doing something that maintains their
expressed concerns about “meanness” in school environ- popularity, or even improves their friendships, even if it
ments, noting that teachers have devoted far more atten- is at the expense of someone else (Banny, Heilbron,
tion to preventing overt physical fighting than relational Ames, & Prinstein, 2011; A. J. Rose & Swenson, 2009).
aggression—despite the fact that victims of relational ag-
gression also suffer as a result (Desjardins & Leadbeater, ❚ Consequences of Rejection Not surprisingly,
2011; Siegel, La Greca, & Harrison, 2009). Some have being unpopular has negative consequences for adoles-
called for educational programs designed to help teachers cents’ mental health and psychological development—
understand, assess, prevent, and respond to the problem peer rejection and friendlessness are associated with
when it arises in their classroom, as well as schoolwide subsequent depression, behavior problems, and aca-
programs designed to teach tolerance and acceptance and demic difficulties (Bellmore, 2011; D. French & Conrad,
encourage students to disapprove of relational aggression 2001; Kupersmidt, Burchinal, & Patterson, 1995;
when they see it. Changing students’ attitudes about rela- Wentzel, 2003). But studies show that the specific con-
tional aggression—which many adolescents see as fine, sequences of peer rejection may differ for rejected
even if they object to physical aggression—is important, youth who are aggressive versus those who are with-
because adolescents’ attitudes about the acceptability of drawn. Aggressive individuals who are rejected are at
relational aggression (for example, agreeing with the state- risk for conduct problems and involvement in antiso-
ment “In general, it is OK to not let someone sit with your cial activity as adolescents, not just as a direct result of
group of friends at the lunch table”) predict their use of it their rejection, but because the underlying causes of
(Werner & Nixon, 2005). In the opinion of most experts, their aggression (for instance, poor self-control) also
middle schools ought to be the focus of such interventions contribute to later conduct problems (Laird, Pettit,
(Yoon, Barton, & Taiarol, 2004). Dodge, & Bates, 2005). In contrast, withdrawn children
who are rejected are likely to feel exceedingly lonely and
are at risk for low self-esteem, depression, and dimin-
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION ished social competence—again, both as a result of be-
ing rejected and in part because the underlying causes
Is relational aggression something that
of their timidity (for instance, high anxiety) also con-
is more common in adolescence than
tribute to later emotional problems (Card & Hodges,
adulthood? If so, why might this be? If
2008; Pedersen et al., 2007). Rejection is especially likely
not, is it expressed differently among
to lead to depression in adolescents who place a lot of
adults?
importance on their standing in the peer group and
who believe that they, rather than the peers who reject
them, are at fault (Prinstein & Aikins, 2004). Adoles-
cents who are both aggressive and withdrawn are at the
As psychologist Amanda Rose has pointed out, how- greatest risk of all (Rubin, LeMare, & Lollis, 1990).
ever, preventing relational aggression is easier said than Many psychologists believe that unpopular young-
done (A. J. Rose, Swenson, & Waller, 2004). She has sters lack some of the social skills and social under-
shown that adolescents who use relational aggression are standing necessary to be popular with peers. According
more popular than their peers. In some ways, this is to findings from an extensive and ongoing program of
hardly surprising, because the whole point of using rela- research by psychologist Kenneth Dodge and his col-
tional aggression is to maintain one’s status and popular- leagues, unpopular aggressive children are more likely
ity, and because the same social skills that make one than their peers to think that other children’s behavior
popular (learning how to “read” other people, being able is deliberately hostile, even when it is not (Dodge, 1986;
to adjust one’s behavior to maintain one’s status, etc.) are Dodge & Coie, 1987). When accidentally pushed while
useful when one is spreading rumors, gossiping, or try- waiting in line, for instance, unpopular aggressive chil-
ing to undermine someone else’s reputation. In fact, dren are more likely than others to believe that the
Rose’s findings suggest that the reason some physically person who did the pushing did it on purpose and, con-
aggressive boys are often more popular than their peers sequently, to retaliate. Numerous studies have now con-
is that physical aggression and relational aggression may firmed that this so-called hostile attributional bias
go hand in hand, and it is their relational aggression, plays a central role in the aggressive behavior of rejected
not their physical aggression, that contributes to their adolescents (Astor, 1994; Courtney & Cohen, 1996;
Crick & Dodge, 1994; S. Graham, 1993; S. Graham &
Hudley, 1994; Waldman, 1996). Adolescents who are
hostile attributional bias The tendency to interpret ambiguous prone to make hostile attributions tend to have friends
interactions with others as deliberately hostile.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 179 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 179

who view the world through a similar lens (Halligan & Depressive Victimization Peer
Phillips, 2010). symptoms by peers rejection

FIGURE 5.9 Depressed adolescents are frequently


VICTIMIZATION AND HARASSMENT harassed and picked on by others, which leads them to
be rejected by their peers. (Kochel et al., 2012)
What about unpopular withdrawn children? What are
their social skills deficits? In general, research shows
that unpopular withdrawn children are excessively anx- Peer harassment is something that students can be
ious and uncertain around other children, often hover- exposed to both directly (when they are the victims) or
ing around the group without knowing how to break indirectly (when they witness harassment but aren’t
into a conversation or activity (Rubin et al., 1990). themselves victimized). According to several studies,
Their hesitancy, low self-esteem, and lack of confidence these two different types of experience can have both
make other children feel uncomfortable, and their similar and dissimilar effects (Janosz et al., 2008;
submissiveness makes them easy targets for bullying Nishina & Juvonen, 2005). Being victimized or witness-
(Olweus, 1993; Salmivalli, 1998). Many of these young- ing the harassment of others makes students anxious,
sters are especially sensitive to being rejected, a trait but witnessing the harassment of others appears to buf-
that may have its origins in early experiences with par- fer some of the harmful effects of being victimized.
ents (Downey, Lebolt, Rincón, & Freitas, 1998). Some Adolescents who were victims of harassment but who
are depressed, and their depression leads them to be- did not see anyone else being victimized were more
have in ways that make them targets of harassment likely to feel humiliated and angry than those who were
(generally speaking, people of all ages don’t like to hang both victims and witnesses on the same day. Presum-
around with depressed individuals) (Kochel, Ladd, & ably, being singled out for harassment feels worse than
Rudolph, 2012) (see Figure 5.9). Unfortunately, the being just one of many who are picked on. For this rea-
more these children are teased, rejected, and victimized, son, studies find that in ethnically diverse schools, vic-
the more anxious and hesitant they feel, and the more timized students whose ethnic group is in the minority
they blame themselves for their victimization, which are not as harmed psychologically as are victimized
only compounds their problem—creating a sort of cy- students whose ethnic group is in the majority, who are
cle of victimization (S. Graham & Juvonen, 1998; Siegel less able to attribute their victimization to their ethnic-
et al., 2009). Interestingly, children who are victimized ity and more likely to blame it on their own shortcom-
but who have a best friend are less likely to be caught in ings as individuals (Graham, Bellmore, Nishina, &
this vicious cycle than those who are friendless (Hodges, Juvonen, 2009).
Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999). Although relationships between adolescents who
dislike each other have not been studied extensively,
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION such mutual antipathies are not uncommon among
What can be done to reduce victimization in schools? teenagers (Abecassis, Hartup, Haselager, Scholte, & Van
If you were asked to design Lieshout, 2002). These relationships frequently involve
an intervention, what bullies and victims, often with an antisocial adolescent
would it entail? repeatedly harassing a withdrawn classmate (Güroğlu,
Haselager, van Lieshout, Cornelis, & Scholte, 2009).
Adolescents who are bullies are also likely to assist and
reinforce other bullies and, like the bullies they support,
are also more likely to have conduct problems and be
indifferent to the problems of others (Crapanzano,
Frick, Childs, & Terranova, 2011).
❚ Victims and Bullies Young adolescents who are Studies of American and European youth indicate that
victimized by their peers typically develop problems about one-third of students report having been bullied at
that lead to further peer rejection and victimization some time during the past year, although in some studies,
(Hodges & Perry, 1999). One of the most pernicious the percentage of students who report having been vic-
effects of victimization is that it undermines feelings of timized has been considerably higher (Haynie et al., 2001;
academic competence, academic performance, and Nansel et al., 2001; Williford, Brisson, Bender, Jenson, &
school engagement, which has cascading effects well Forrest-Bank, 2011). Rates of victimization vary consid-
beyond adolescence—even after taking into account erably from country to country (Analitis et al., 2009). One
background factors, victimization during adolescence recent survey of more than 160,000 adolescents from 35
is associated with lower educational attainment and, countries found that, around the world, adolescents who
as a consequence, diminished earnings in adulthood come from less affluent families are more likely to be bul-
(Macmillan & Hagan, 2004). lied, and, moreover, that the prevalence of bullying is
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 180 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

180 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

40

35 Lithuania

30

25
Bullying prevalence, %

Latvia
20
Estonia
Ukraine Russian Federation
Flemish-speaking Portugal
Belgium Austria
Canada
15
Switzerland
French-speaking France United States
Belgium England Germany
Denmark Norway
Poland
Netherlands Italy
10 Macedonia
Wales
Finland Croatia Spain Greece Scotland
Ireland
Israel
Hungary Slovenia
Czech Republic
5
Sweden

0
0.2 0.250.35 0.3
0.4 0.45 0.5
Income equality
FIGURE 5.10 Rates of bullying vary considerably from country to country. Bullying is more prevalent in countries with
greater income inequality. (Due et al., 2009)

higher in schools characterized by greater income in- Internet or via cell phones). Despite the attention it re-
equality and, interestingly, in countries characterized by ceives in the popular media, in part because of a few
greater income equality (Due et al., 2009). For example, very high-profile cases, online harassment is far less
bullying is far less prevalent in Sweden, where the gap common than most people think, and, more impor-
between rich and poor is very small, than in Russia, where tantly, less common than in-person harassment. A re-
income inequality is much greater (see Figure 5.10). One cent survey of a nationally representative sample of
interpretation of this is that it is more acceptable for the more than 2,000 adolescents found that 11% of those
strong to victimize the weak in countries where having a surveyed in 2010 reported ever having been harassed
wide gap between the economically “strong” and eco- online—an increase from 9% in 2005, but hardly the
nomically “weak” is also more widely tolerated. epidemic that media reports have made it out to be. In
Researchers are just now beginning to systematically contrast, for instance, one study found that only 10% of
study cyberbullying (e.g., bullying that occurs over the adolescents had engaged in electronic bullying, as com-
pared with 40% who had physically bullied someone
and 70% who had verbally bullied someone
cyberbullying Bullying that occurs over the Internet or via cell (K. Williams & Guerra, 2007). One common finding,
phones.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 181 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 181

school were still observed in high school (Rusby,


Forrester, Biglan, & Metzler, 2005).
One finding that has now been replicated in several
different countries is that many of the same adolescents
who report having been victimized also report bullying
others and that these adolescents have the greatest ad-
justment problems, consistent with the finding discussed
earlier, that children who are both aggressive and with-
drawn are the most disturbed (Juvonen, Graham, &
Schuster, 2003; Klomek, Marrocco, Kleinman, Schonfield,
& Gould, 2007). One reason that bullying and victimiza-
tion are often seen in the same children may be that cer-
tain elements of the broader context—the climate of the
school, for instance—may increase or decrease the likeli-
hood of aggression between classmates, which suggests
that teachers and principals may be able to make changes
in their school’s climate that will reduce aggression be-
tween students (Aceves, Hinshaw, Mendoza-Denton, &
Page-Gould, 2010; Barboza et al., 2009; Bradshaw,
Sawyer, & O’Brennan, 2009; Guerra, Williams, & Sadek,
2011). It is important to note, however, that a significant
amount of bullying occurs outside of school—according
to one national survey; in fact, more high school students
reported being victimized outside school than at school
Studies find that more than one-third of American and European
students report having been bullied during the past year.
(Turner, Finkelhor, Hamby, Shattuck, & Ormrod, 2011).
One longitudinal study by psychologist Debra
Pepler, an expert on bullying, tracked a large sample of
despite these disparities, is that adolescents who engage Toronto youth from age 10 to 17, in order to see how
in traditional bullying also frequently engage in cyber- patterns of bullying changed over time (Pepler, Jiang,
bullying; similarly, adolescents who are frequent vic- Craig, & Connolly, 2008). The researchers identified
tims of traditional bullying are also the victims of four different developmental trajectories (see Figure
electronic harassment (Mitchell, Finkelhor, Wolak, 5.11). About 40% of the sample never bullied anyone.
Ybarra, & Turner, 2011; Raskauskas & Stoltz, 2007). Another 35% bullied other children occasionally when
And contrary to popular belief, most Internet bullying they were younger and continued to do so through
is not anonymous (Juvonen & Gross, 2008). Not sur- adolescence. The remaining 25% fell into two groups—
prisingly, bullies who “specialize” in cyberbullying, both groups were relatively frequent bullies when they
which takes a bit of planning, tend to be less reactive in were younger, but whereas one group (slightly less than
their aggression and more instrumental (Sontag, 15% of the sample) curtailed this behavior over time,
Clemans, Graber, & Lyndon, 2011). about 10% of the sample were persistent bullies. Not
Students who are harassed by their classmates,
whether in person or electronically, report a range of
adjustment problems, including low self-esteem, de- 4
pression, suicidal ideation, and academic difficulties;
whereas those who report frequent bullying are more 3
Bullying scale

likely to show problems in social skills and in the con-


trol of aggression (Ando, Asakura, & Simons-Morton, 2
2005; Fitzpatrick, Dulin, & Piko, 2010; Fredstrom,
Adams, & Gilman, 2011; Hay & Meldrum, 2010; Haynie 1
et al., 2001; Juvonen, Wang, & Espinoza, 2011; McLaughlin,
Hatzenbuehler, & Hilt, 2009; Rosen, Underwood, 0
10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
Beron, Gentsch et al., 2009). Although being bullied has Age (years)
adverse consequences regardless of whether other stu-
High–9.9% Moderate–35.1%
dents witness it, public victimization, especially when
other students watch but don’t offer any assistance, is Desist–13.4% Never–41.6%
particularly humiliating (Nishina, 2012). One study FIGURE 5.11 Trajectories of bullying over time. (Pepler
found that the effects of being harassed in middle et al., 2008)
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 182 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

182 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

surprisingly, the persistent bullies had more troubled to get attention, and that when they are ignored, they
family relationships, peer relationships, and difficulties are likely to seek other targets (Steinberg, 2011).
controlling aggression. Interestingly, however, individ-
uals who bullied other children when they were younger ❚ Helping Unpopular Teens Can unpopular
but who desisted over time were not dramatically dif- adolescents be helped? In recent years, several teams
ferent from those who never bullied at all. The research- of psychologists have experimented with different sorts
ers speculate that this group of individuals may be of interventions designed to improve the social skills of
popular and socially skilled, using bullying strategically unpopular adolescents. These social competence train-
to establish their status in the peer group when they are ing programs have focused on three different strategies.
younger. With age, as aggression becomes a less impor- One type of program has been designed to teach social
tant contributor to status in the peer group, these ado- skills—self-expression, leadership, and how to converse
lescents are socially savvy enough to stop. (Repinski & Leffert, 1994). These programs have been
Adolescents’ responses to being bullied vary, of shown to improve adolescents’ abilities to get along
course. One recent study found that there were four with peers. A second approach has been to have un-
categories of victims: those who were mainly passive popular adolescents participate in group activities with
(e.g., ignoring the bully or walking away), those who popular ones under the supervision of psychologists.
were mainly aggressive (e.g., fighting back, either physi- Programs like this have been shown to improve adoles-
cally or verbally), those who were support-seeking (e.g., cents’ self-conceptions and their acceptance by others
telling a parent), and those who did a little of every- (Bierman & Furman, 1984). Finally, some social com-
thing. (Support-seeking was reported by middle school petence programs focus on a combination of behav-
students but was rarely seen in high school, perhaps ioral and cognitive abilities, including social problem
because at this age, asking an adult for help in respond- solving (e.g., Greenberg & Kusche, 1998; Weissberg,
ing to a bully is seen as immature and weak.) Interest- Caplan, & Harwood, 1991). Social-problem-solving
ingly, victims who used passive strategies reported programs, such as PATHS (Promoting Alternative
fewer emotional or behavioral problems than those Thinking Strategies), are designed to improve individu-
who fought back, sought help, or used a mixture of als’ abilities to judge social situations and figure out ac-
approaches (Waasdorp & Bradshaw, 2011) (see Figure ceptable ways of behaving. Adolescents are taught to
5.12), although feeling supported by parents or teach- calm down and think before they react, to decide what
ers (if not directly asking for their help) seems to have a the problem is, to figure out what their goal is, and to
protective effect against the adverse effects of victimiza- think of positive approaches toward reaching that goal.
tion (Yeung & Leadbeater, 2010). Other studies find Instead of lashing out at a classmate who grabbed the
that victims who avoid blaming themselves for having last basketball from a gym closet, for example, a hot-
been bullied and respond by behaving proactively tempered boy who had been through this sort of pro-
(avoiding the bully), rather than retaliating, fare better gram might calm himself down, tell himself that his goal
(Singh & Bussey, 2011). Although it is hard to persuade is to play basketball rather than get into a fight, and ap-
adolescents that these are the most effective responses, proach another student to ask if he can get into a game.
it helps to explain that bullies do what they do in order PATHS has been shown to effectively reduce behavioral

FIGURE 5.12 Middle school students’ re- Undifferentiated Aggressive Passive Support-seeking
sponses to bullies have different effects on 3.5
their mental health. Passive responding
(ignoring or walking away) is best. (Waasdorp & 3.0
Bradshw, 2011)
2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

0.0
Internalizing Externalizing Internalizing Externalizing
Boys Girls
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 183 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 5 Peer Groups 183

problems among elementary school children (Conduct Brendgen, van Lier, Koot, & Vitaro, 2010; Woodward &
Problems Prevention Research Group, 1999). Fergusson, 1999).
Peers also play a crucial role in promoting healthy
psychosocial development. In the realm of identity, for
RECAP example, peers provide the sorts of models and feed-
• It is important to distinguish between two types of back that adolescents cannot get from adults (B. Brown,
popularity—sociometric (how well-liked someone 2004). In the context of the peer group, young people
is) and perceived (how much status someone has). can try on different roles and personalities and can
Sociometric popularity is associated with being experiment with different identities with greater ease
socially skilled, whereas the determinants of per- than at home. And, as we saw earlier, the peer group
ceived popularity are different at different times may serve as a way station in the development of iden-
and in different places. tity as adolescents begin to establish a separate sense of
• Unpopular adolescents tend to fall into three cate- self, one that is differentiated from the family (B. Brown
gories: aggressive adolescents who have difficulty & Larson, 2009). Experience in the peer group also can
controlling their emotions, withdrawn adolescents, be an important influence on adolescents’ self-image.
and adolescents who are both aggressive and Spending time with peers also is vital for the devel-
withdrawn. opment and expression of autonomy. The process of
• In general, adolescents who are rejected by their developing more mature and more independent rela-
peers are at risk for a wide variety of psychological tionships with parents is accompanied by the establish-
and behavioral problems, including academic fail- ment of more mature relationships with peers. In
ure, conduct problems, and depression. addition, the peer group provides a context for adoles-
• Victimization by peers, whether through physical cents to test out decision-making skills in an arena
bullying, relational aggression, or electronic harass- where there are no adults present to monitor and con-
ment, has harmful and often enduring conse- trol their choices (W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006).
quences for individuals’ mental health. Intimacy and sexuality, of course, are much more
• Numerous interventions have been designed to common between peers than between adolescents and
improve adolescents’ social competence, including adults, for a variety of reasons. Perhaps most critical is
those that focus on improving unpopular that both intimacy and sexuality require interaction be-
adolescents’ social skills and tween two individuals who are relative equals. More-
their social over, sexual relationships and close intimacy within the
understanding. family context would be likely to disrupt important
functions of family relationships (Hartup, 1977). It is
therefore the adolescent’s peer group that generally
plays the central role in socializing youngsters in ap-
propriate sexual behavior and in developing the capac-
The Peer Group and ity for intimate friendship (H. S. Sullivan, 1953a).
Finally, peers are an important influence on adoles-
Psychosocial Development cent achievement, especially in countries—like the
Regardless of the structure or norms of a particular United States, Spain, or Korea—where high schools
peer group, peers play an extremely important role in tend not to be specialized or separated according to stu-
the psychological development of adolescents. Prob- dent ability. In countries like France, Germany, and
lematic peer relationships are associated with a range of Switzerland, in contrast, where students are assigned to
serious psychological and behavior problems during different schools depending on their ability and aspira-
adolescence and adulthood. Individuals who are un- tions (for example, vocational schools versus college
popular or who have poor peer relationships during prep schools), peers are a less important influence on
adolescence are more likely than their socially accepted achievement (Buchmann & Dalton, 2002). Although peers
peers to be low achievers in school, drop out of high play a less prominent role than parents or teachers in
school, show higher rates of delinquent behavior, and influencing adolescents’ long-term educational and oc-
suffer from an array of emotional and mental health cupational plans, adolescents’ classmates are a signifi-
problems as adults (Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990). cant influence on their day-to-day school behaviors
Although it is likely that poorly adjusted individuals and feelings, including how much they value school,
have difficulty making friends, good evidence also sug- how much effort they devote to their studies, and how
gests that psychological problems result from—as well well they perform in class (A. M. Ryan, 2001; Steinberg,
as cause—problems with peers (Bagwell, Newcomb, & 1996). Peers seem to be an especially important influence
Bukowski, 1998; Buhrmester & Yin, 1997; Coie et al., on the achievement of ethnic minority youth (Steinberg,
1995; McCoy, 1996; J. Parker & Seal, 1996; Witvliet, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992).
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 184 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

184 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Adolescents consider the time they spend with their RECAP


peers to be among the most enjoyable parts of the day • Problematic peer relationships are associated with
(Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1984). One reason is that a range of serious psychological and behavior
activities with friends are typically organized around problems during adolescence and adulthood. These
having a good time, in contrast to activities with par- problems both cause, and result from, poor peer
ents, which are more likely to be organized around relationships.
household chores or the enforcement of parental rules • Peers play a crucial role in the development of
(Larson, 1983). Rather than being competing institu- identity, autonomy, intimacy, sexuality, and
tions, the family and peer group mainly provide con- achievement in adolescence.
trasting opportunities for adolescent activities and • Rather than being competing institutions, the
behaviors. The family is organized around work and family and peer group mainly provide
other tasks, and it may be important in the socialization contrasting opportunities for
of responsibility and achievement. The peer group pro- adolescent activities
vides more frequent opportunities for interaction and and behaviors.
leisure, which contributes to the development of inti-
macy and enhances the adolescent’s mood and psycho-
logical well-being.
ste32118_ch05_150-185.indd Page 185 21/03/13 10:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 186 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 187 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
6
Schools
THE BROADER CONTEXT OF U.S. SECONDARY EDUCATION
The Origins of Compulsory Education
The Rise of the Comprehensive High School
School Reform: Past and Present
What Should Schools Teach?
Education in the Inner Cities
THE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF SCHOOLS
School Size and Class Size
Age Grouping and School Transitions
How Secondary Schools Differ from Elementary Schools
Tracking
Ethnic Composition
Alternatives to Public Schools
CLASSROOM CLIMATE
The Best Classroom Climate for Adolescents
Teacher Expectations and Student Performance
The Importance of Student Engagement
School Violence
BEYOND HIGH SCHOOL
The College-Bound
The Non-College-Bound
SCHOOLS AND ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
Characteristics of Good Schools
The Effects of School on Adolescent Development

187
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 188 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

188 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

BECAUSE OF THE IMPORTANT and multifaceted role each week in school or in school-related activities
it has come to play in modern society, the secondary (Larson & Verma, 1999). One recent review estimates
educational system—secondary education refers to that between ages 11 and 18, the typical American stu-
middle schools, junior high schools, and high schools— dent will spend about 7,000 hours in school—not even
has been the target of a remarkable amount of criti- counting time on homework and school-related activi-
cism, scrutiny, and social science research. ties outside of school (Elmore, 2009).
Secondary school touches the lives of all adoles- Not only are schools the chief educational arena for
cents in industrialized societies, as well as an increas- adolescents, but they also play an extremely important
ingly larger proportion of the population in the role in defining the young person’s social world and in
developing world. Virtually all American adolescents shaping psychosocial development. Naturally, the de-
under the age of 17 and nearly all 17- and 18-year-olds velopment of achievement—motivation, aspirations,
are enrolled in school. In most developing countries, and expectations—is profoundly affected by the ado-
attending high school is much more common among lescent’s experiences in school. (Just think about the
children of the wealthy, often because poor families differences between going to a good school and going
need their adolescents to work. But even in the poorest to a bad one.) But schools influence psychosocial de-
parts of the world—sub-Saharan Africa, for example— velopment far beyond the domain of achievement.
close to two-thirds of 10- to 14-year-olds and 40–50% How adolescents do in school influences their aca-
of 15- to 19-year-olds are enrolled in school (National demic self-conceptions and occupational choices—
Research Council, 2005), although rates around the shaping their identity. The way in which a school is
world vary considerably from country to country organized affects the adolescent’s sense of indepen-
(United Nations, 2012) (see Figure 6.1). With the excep- dence, and the way a classroom is run affects the extent
tion of a few countries, such as Afghanistan, rates of to which the adolescent learns to think independently.
enrollment in secondary school are comparable for Schools often define adolescents’ social networks,
males and females. thereby influencing the development of interpersonal
Schooling is as time-consuming as it is pervasive. relationships. And the majority of adolescents, at least
During most of the year, the typical American student in the United States, learn about sexuality in school and
spends more than one-third of his or her waking hours are influenced by their classmates’ norms for sexual

100
Girls
Boys
80

60

40

20

0
Iraq

Thailand
Pakistan
Japan
Afghanistan

Brazil

Philippines
France

Ukraine
Mexico

U.S.A.
Kenya

Saudi Arabia

Uganda
Ethiopia
Egypt

FIGURE 6.1 The proportion of male and female adolescents in selected countries who are enrolled in secondary
school. (United Nations, 2012)

secondary education The system of middle schools, junior high


schools, and high schools.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 189 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 189

behavior. You simply cannot understand adolescence influenced by the way in which the school is organized,
as a developmental period without understanding the and the way in which the school is organized, in turn, is
ways in which schools shape the adolescent experience. influenced by the needs and demands of the commu-
In this chapter, we will examine the organization and nity and of society. Unlike the family or peer group,
workings of secondary schools at multiple levels of whose structure is not under the direct or deliberate
analysis. Perhaps your first inclination is to think about control of society, schools are environments created to
what takes place in the classroom. And while this is serve specific purposes (Elmore, 2009). In many re-
important, a thorough understanding of schooling and spects, the schools we have today—for all their
its impact on adolescent development requires an ex- strengths and weaknesses—are the schools we de-
amination that goes beyond the classroom (Eccles & signed. This is abundantly clear when we look at the
Roeser, 2011). What takes place in the classroom is history of secondary education in America.

The Broader Context of Not only is the school year longer today than it was in
the past, but adolescents also remain in school for more
U.S. Secondary Education years now than they did in previous eras. In 1924, fewer
than 33% of all youngsters entering the fifth grade even-
As Figure 6.2 shows, today in the United States, virtually
tually graduated from high school; today, about 75%
all young people ages 14–17 are enrolled in school. In
of all fifth-graders will eventually graduate on time, and
1930, only about half of this age group were students,
a substantial number of those who do not graduate on
and at the turn of the 20th century, only 1 in 10 were
schedule eventually get their diploma, either by com-
(D. Tanner, 1972; William T. Grant Foundation, 1988).
pleting high school at a later date or through equiva-
Not only are there considerably more youngsters
lency programs or continuation schools (National
enrolled in school today than there were 50 years ago,
Center for Education Statistics, 2011b).
but today’s students also spend more days per year in
school. In 1920, for example, the average school term
was 162 days, and the average student attended for THE ORIGINS OF COMPULSORY
only 121 days, or 75% of the term. By 1968, however, EDUCATION
the school term had been lengthened to nearly 180
days, which remains the national average, and the typi- The rise of secondary education in America was the
cal student today attends more than 90% of the term result of several historical and social trends that con-
(National Center for Education Statistics, 2011b). verged at the turn of the 20th century. Most important
were industrialization, urbanization, and immigration.
Following widespread industrialization during the late
100 19th century, the role of children and young adolescents in
90 the workplace changed dramatically. As productivity
became more dependent on workers’ use of machines,
80
Percent of 14- to 17-year-olds

employers recognized that they needed employees who


70 were more skilled than youngsters ordinarily were. In
enrolled in school

60 addition, the few unskilled jobs that remained after indus-


trialization required strength beyond the capacity of many
50 youth (Church, 1976). Social reformers also expressed
40 concerns about the dangers children faced working in fac-
tories, and labor unions—an increasingly powerful force
30
in the early 1900s—sought to protect not only the welfare
20 of children but also their own job security. In response,
10 child labor laws narrowed and limited the employment of
0 minors (Bakan, 1972). Together, these changes in the
1890 1910 1930 1950 1970 workplace kept many youngsters out of the labor force.
FIGURE 6.2 The proportion of the 14- to 17-year-old popu- During this same period, the nature of life in
lation enrolled in school increased dramatically between American cities was changing markedly. Industrializa-
1910 and 1940, continued to increase until 1970, and then tion brought with it urbanization and, along with sev-
leveled off. Today, nearly 95 percent of individuals this age eral waves of immigrants, new problems for urban
are in school. (D. Tanner, 1972; William T. Grant Foundation, 1988) centers. The effects of a rapidly expanding economy
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 190 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

190 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

youth for life in modern society. It was argued that


education should be more practical and include prepa-
ration for the roles of work and citizenship.
The 1920s marked the birth in the United States of
what came to be known as the comprehensive high
school, an educational institution that promised to
meet the needs of a diverse and growing population of
young people. Classes in general education, college
preparation, and vocational education were all housed
under one roof. This was also a time of tremendous
change in the high school curriculum. During these
years, new courses were added in music, art, family life,
health, physical education, and other subjects designed
to prepare adolescents for family and leisure as well as
work roles.
The proportion of American adolescents enrolled in high school By the middle of the 20th century, the high school
grew dramatically between 1920 and 1940. had come a long way from its exclusive focus at the turn
of the century on the intellectual development of the
socioeconomic elite. Its concern had broadened to in-
were seen in the tenements and slums of America’s cit- clude the social and intellectual development of all
ies: poor housing, overcrowded neighborhoods, crime. young people. And today, despite continuing question-
Eager to improve living conditions for the urban ing and criticism, the comprehensive high school re-
masses, social reformers envisioned education as a mains the cornerstone of the American system of
means of improving the life circumstances of the poor secondary education. It is, however, not the exemplar to
and working classes. Many also saw compulsory sec- which all countries aspire—few other countries at-
ondary education as a means of social control. High tempt to educate such a diverse group of young people
schools would take thousands of idle young people off under one roof, choosing instead to separate the col-
the streets and place them in an environment where lege-bound from the non-college-bound into separate
they could be supervised and kept out of trouble. In schools. Indeed, the American “high school” is in many
addition, anxious to see that foreign-born immigrants regards a distinctively American institution.
were well socialized to the American way of life, re-
formers presented universal secondary education as a
necessary part of the process of Americanization MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
(Church, 1976; D. Tanner, 1972). By 1915, the idea of The comprehensive high school, in which all students
universal compulsory education for adolescents had are educated within the same institution, regardless of
gained widespread acceptance. their future plans, was invented
within a specific historical
context. Does it make
THE RISE OF THE COMPREHENSIVE sense within today’s
HIGH SCHOOL society? If not, what
Prior to the early 20th century, high schools were for alternative or alternatives
the elite. In curriculum, staff, and student composition, would you suggest?
they were similar to the colleges of the day, with the
emphasis mainly on classical liberal arts instruction
(Church, 1976; D. Tanner, 1972).
By 1920, however, educators saw a need for curricu- SCHOOL REFORM: PAST AND PRESENT
lar reform. Now that secondary education was aimed at Although we naturally think of schools as institutions
the masses, schooling was seen not merely as a means of whose primary goal is education, they are much more
intellectual training but also as a way of preparing than this. Schools are also potentially important tools
of social intervention, because it is through schools that
the greatest number of young people can most easily be
reached. For this reason, the study of schools is ex-
comprehensive high school An educational institution that tremely important to social scientists and policymakers
evolved during the first half of the 20th century, offering a varied who are interested in influencing adolescent develop-
curriculum and designed to meet the needs of a diverse population ment. In fact, one way to understand the ways in which
of adolescents.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 191 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 191

adults want adolescents to change is to look at the ways their students were faring would give parents and the
that schools have been reformed over the years. community the information they needed to put pres-
During the 1950s, for example, when politicians felt sure on schools to do better. No one could disagree with
that the United States had lost its scientific edge to the the basic idea that all students—regardless of their
former Soviet Union, schools were called upon to offer background—deserve a high-quality public education
more courses in math and science (Conant, 1959). (Borkowski & Sneed, 2006).
When social scientists felt that adolescents were Although NCLB sounded good in principle, it was
growing up unfamiliar with the world of work—as they very problematic in practice and met with tremendous
did in the 1970s—schools were asked to provide resistance from the start (Darling-Hammond, 2006).
opportunities for work–study programs and classes in States complained that they simply did not have the
career education (President’s Science Advisory Com- resources to conduct the mandated assessments or to
mittee, 1974). In the 1990s, as society grappled with a respond to failing students’ poor performance. Teach-
broad array of social problems affecting and involving ers and parents complained that the focus on stan-
youth—problems such as violence, AIDS, and drug dardized testing adversely affects what takes place in the
abuse—we once again looked to schools for assistance, classroom—if a school’s financial future depends only
asking schools to implement an array of preventive in- on its students’ reading and math test scores, why
terventions (Dryfoos, 1993). should teachers do anything other than teach to the
test? (Be honest—how much effort do you devote to
❚ No Child Left Behind Toward the end of the learning material in your classes that you know you
1990s, amidst growing concerns that our inner-city won’t be tested on?) Many questions about the policy
schools were not producing graduates who could were raised: What happens to subjects that will not ap-
compete for high-skills jobs, and in response to a public pear on the test, like current events, or to the teaching of
increasingly interested in alternatives to conventional skills that are impossible to assess through standardized
public education, such as charter schools or home exams, such as critical thinking? Who determines how
schooling, schools were called upon to raise standards tough the tests are or what level of achievement on
for all students (e.g., Ravitch, 2001). In January 2002, them is acceptable? And, with millions of dollars at stake,
President Bush signed into law the No Child Left Be- what was to stop schools from manipulating their
hind Act, a sweeping and controversial piece of legisla- scores, by encouraging poor-performing students to be
tion mandating that states ensure that all students, absent on testing days or by actually helping students
regardless of their economic circumstances, achieve ac- cheat on the tests, which some schools evidently did
ademic proficiency (U.S. Department of Education, (S. Levitt & Dubner, 2005)? Some critics of NCLB
2006). No Child Left Behind (NCLB) required that argued that it was having the opposite effect of what
schools create and enforce academic standards by annu- was intended, providing incentives for schools to push
ally testing all students and by reporting the results of low-achieving students out of school (Darling-
students’ performance to the public. Underperforming Hammond, 2006). Nevertheless, the movement toward
schools—schools where students’ test scores did not performance-based accountability—holding teachers,
improve—initially would be given an opportunity to do schools, school districts, and states accountable for the
a better job the following year, by providing additional achievement of their students—has been the most im-
instruction, tutoring, or special services for students portant change in the world of American education in
who needed them. But schools that continued to fail the past 20 years (Elmore, 2009).
eventually would have funding taken away and could No Child Left Behind remained in place after Presi-
even be forced to close. dent Obama took office in 2009, although the Obama
On the face of it, NCLB sounds reasonable enough. administration sought to fix many of the problems
As you’ll read in Chapter 12, a huge proportion of that had developed during the policy’s early years, in-
American students do not meet even minimal stan- cluding the fact that schools were “gaming the system,”
dards for academic performance, and poor performance by setting their standards especially low, so that they
is disproportionately seen among disadvantaged, Black, could report that a high proportion of their students
Hispanic, and American Indian students. Many com- were making passing grades; that teachers were teach-
mentators had criticized the practice of social promo- ing to the test, in order to avoid being punished if their
tion—moving students from one grade to the next students tested poorly; and that school districts were
regardless of their academic performance—arguing
that poor and ethnic minority youth especially were be-
social promotion The practice of promoting students from one
ing cheated out of a good education and graduated
grade to the next automatically, regardless of their school
without the skills necessary to succeed in college or in performance.
the labor force (Steinberg, 1996). Forcing schools to
regularly assess student progress and publicize how critical thinking Thinking that involves analyzing, evaluating, and
interpreting information, rather than simply memorizing it.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 192 21/03/13 10:09 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

192 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

reporting school-wide average scores without reveal-


ing that there were huge achievement gaps between the
low- and high-performing students. President Obama’s
education secretary stressed the need to have high
standards for all students and, just as important, a set
of common standards across all 50 states (U.S. Depart-
ment of Education, 2009). (One of the problems with
NCLB as originally implemented is that it permitted
individual states to set their own standards, which re-
sulted in markedly different estimates of “proficiency”
from one state to another.) The administration also
tried to build more flexibility into NCLB by encourag-
ing schools to experiment with different approaches to
raising student achievement, through a competition
called “Race to the Top.” In addition, as new research
began to demonstrate the importance of having high-
quality teachers, schools were encouraged to develop
better ways of evaluating their teachers, helping teach-
ers to improve their classroom skills, and replacing
poor teachers with better ones. Because changes in
policy sometimes take years before results can be seen,
it is not yet known whether these reforms have had a
large-scale impact on American student achievement.

MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION


Were students in your high school required to pass a
standardized test in order to graduate? If so, Critical thinking is stimulated when students are encouraged to
do you think this had a positive or negative interpret, analyze, and evaluate information, rather than simply
effect on student achievement? If not, what memorize it or use it in a routine way.
effect do you think this policy would have
had on students in your school?
required, and which should be left as electives? If you
were to discuss this with your classmates, you’d proba-
bly find plenty of disagreement.
WHAT SHOULD SCHOOLS TEACH?
Suppose you are asked to list the things you think ❚ Standards-Based Reform The past two decades
young people need to know in order to function as have been dominated by what is called standards-
competent, responsible, satisfied adults. Which items based reform, which focuses on policies designed to
on your list should be the responsibility of high improve achievement by holding schools and students
schools? Should high school curricula be limited to the to a predetermined set of standards measured by
traditional academic subjects, or should schools play a achievement tests. Implementing this change has been
broader role in preparing young people for adulthood more difficult than you might think, for several reasons.
by providing instruction more directly relevant to First, educators haven’t been able to agree on the body
work, family, leisure, and citizenship? Should students of knowledge and skills that comprises what high school
receive instruction only in English, mathematics, sci- graduates should know and be able to do. Second, as
ence, and social studies, or should they take courses as states soon discovered, large numbers of their students
well in “general education”—in subjects such as art, did not fully acquire the knowledge and capabilities as-
home economics, health, sex education, driver educa- sessed on standardized graduation examinations. It is
tion, and personal finance? Which courses should be all well and good to propose that all high school seniors
must pass a graduation test in order to earn a diploma,
but what happens when one-third or one-half of a
state’s high school seniors fail the test? The economic,
standards-based reform Policies designed to improve social, and political costs of holding back such large
achievement by holding schools and students to a predetermined numbers of students because they could not pass these
set of standards measured by achievement tests.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 193 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 193

“exit exams” was simply too great. This created a huge large urban school districts. Although there are occa-
incentive for states to develop exams with very low re- sional success stories, such as the Harlem Children’s
quirements for passing, which, of course, defeats the Zone (Tough, 2008), inner-city schools in America con-
whole purpose of standards-based reform. tinue to have tremendous problems. Just 12% of the
Amid widespread disappointment over the state of high schools in the United States produce half of the
public education in America, increasing numbers of country’s dropouts, and nearly half the nation’s Black
parents began to look at other options—among them, and Latino students attend one of these schools
charter schools (public schools that are given more free- (Balfanz, Legters, West, & Weber, 2007). And, although
dom to set their own curricula), schools that are run by there have been modest improvements in some subjects
private corporations rather than local school boards, at some grade levels, the gap in achievement between
and government-subsidized school vouchers (which Black and Hispanic students, on the one hand, and
can be used for private school tuition). Although all of White and Asian students, on the other, remains very
these alternatives gained popularity during the late wide. Among 8th-graders, for example, about three
1990s, research on the costs and benefits of charter times as many White and Asian students than Black or
schools, privatization, and vouchers has been inconclu- Hispanic students are proficient in reading. Huge gaps
sive (Loveless, 2002). There is considerable variability exist in mathematics as well, with the numbers of White
among charter, for-profit, and private schools, just as and Asian students who are proficient sometimes out-
there is among public schools. The bottom line is that numbering their Black and Hispanic counterparts by
what takes place within a school is probably more ratios of 4 to 1, depending on the specific comparison.
important than the nature of its funding and oversight. According to recent test results, in the nation’s large
This idea has led many experts to argue that we should inner-city public schools, which mainly serve minority
focus on the ways in which we train, certify, place, and youth, only one-sixth of students are judged proficient
compensate teachers (Darling-Hammond, 2006). in science, a fact that has enormous implications for
these adolescents’ chances at success in an increasingly
high-tech economy (National Center for Education
Statistics, 2011a).
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION Why has school reform failed in so many urban
The United States is one of the only industrialized schools? Experts point to several reasons. First, the con-
countries in the world that does not have national centration of poverty in many inner-city communities
graduation examinations that are administered to all has produced a population of students with an array of
students, regardless of where they go personal and situational problems—problems that few
to school. Do you think having na- schools are equipped or able to address (Farrell et al.,
tional exams is a good or bad idea? 2007). Recent surveys of American high school stu-
Why do you think this practice is dents indicate that so many are afraid of being victim-
not as popular in the United States ized that 18% of high school students across the country
as it is elsewhere? carry a gun, knife, or club to school, with even higher
percentages doing so in inner-city schools (Child
Trends, 2010). Second, many urban school districts are
burdened by huge administrative bureaucracies that of-
ten impede reform and hinder educational innovation.
EDUCATION IN THE INNER CITIES
Third, students in urban schools report less of a sense
Although concerns about the demise of American educa- of “belonging” to their school, which leads to disen-
tion have raged for the past two decades, some commen- gagement and poor achievement (Anderman, 2002).
tators argued that the problem of low student achievement Finally, the erosion of job opportunities in inner-city
was not an across-the-board problem, but one that was communities has left many students with little incen-
concentrated mainly among poor and minority young- tive to remain in school or to devote a great deal of ef-
sters living in inner cities (Berliner & Biddle, 1995). fort to academic pursuits (Kantor & Brenzel, 1992).
Although other critics (e.g., Stedman, 1998; Stein- Many reformers now believe that to fix the problems of
berg, 1996) have noted that poor achievement is a urban education, we must change the entire context in
problem in all segments of American society, virtually which inner-city children live, not merely what goes on
all social scientists concur that the education crisis, and in their schools (Garcia-Reid, 2007; Tough, 2008).
its implications for the future of the labor force, is espe-
cially distressingly urgent within inner-city public charter schools Public schools that have been given the
schools. Indeed, the achievement gap between White autonomy to establish their own curricula and teaching practices.
and non-White youngsters, which had been closing for
some time, grew wider during the 1990s, especially in school vouchers Government-subsidized vouchers that can be
used for private school tuition.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 194 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

194 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

RECAP
• Schools play an extremely important role in struc-
turing the nature of adolescence in modern society.
In most of the industrialized world, virtually all 14-
to 17-year-olds are enrolled in school, a pattern
that is becoming increasingly prevalent in the de-
veloping world as well. In addition, school occupies
an enormous amount of adolescents’ time.
• A number of social forces combined to lead to the
development of compulsory education for adoles-
cents in America, including industrialization, immi-
gration, and urbanization.
• During the 1920s, the high school as we know it in
the United States today—the comprehensive high
school—was born.
• At the turn of the 21st century in the United States,
calls for the implementation and enforcement of
rigorous academic standards became widespread,
as the public grew increasingly worried about the
competitiveness of American students in an inter-
national economy that is increasingly reliant on
high-tech, high-skill jobs.
• There is broad consensus that inner-city public Students and teachers are more satisfied in classes that combine a
moderate degree of structure with high student involvement and
schools, which mainly serve disadvantaged ethnic
high teacher support.
minority adolescents, are in especially dire straits.
Experts agree that to change this state of affairs,
we need to transform the entire
context in which poor a wider range of courses and services under a single
urban youth roof. As a consequence, schools became larger and
live. larger over the course of the 20th century. By the end of
the 1990s, in many metropolitan areas, students at-
tended enormous schools, with enrollments of several
thousand students.

❚ Is Bigger Better? One advantage enjoyed


The Social Organization by larger schools is that they can offer a more varied
of Schools curriculum—a large high school, for instance, may be
able to offer many specialized courses that a small
In addition to debating curricular issues, social scien- school would be unable to staff. But is bigger necessar-
tists interested in school reform have discussed the ways ily better? Do students who attend larger schools reap
in which secondary schools should be organized. Be- any educational or psychological advantages as a result?
cause the organization of a school affects students’ day- A fair amount of research conducted over the past
to-day experiences, variations in school organization 40 years says, “No.” Indeed, one of the most consistent
can have profound effects on adolescents’ development conclusions to emerge from recent evaluations of
and behavior. In this section, we examine the research school reform efforts is that student performance and
on five key aspects of school organization: (1) school interest in school improve when their schools are made
and classroom size, (2) different approaches to age less bureaucratic and more intimate. Numerous studies
grouping, (3) tracking, or the grouping of students in indicate that students achieve more when they attend
classes according to their academic abilities, (4) the eth- schools that create a cohesive sense of community
nic composition of schools, and (5) public versus pri- (V. Lee, Smith, & Croninger, 1997; Ready, Lee, & Welner,
vate schools. 2004; Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996). Student engage-
ment is weaker in larger schools, particularly when the
number of students in a grade exceeds 400 (Weiss,
SCHOOL SIZE AND CLASS SIZE
Carolan, & Baker-Smith, 2010).
As the idea of the comprehensive high school gained While school size may affect academic outcomes, it
widespread acceptance, educators attempted to deliver does not necessarily affect students’ emotional attachment
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 195 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 195

to the institution (Anderman, 2002) or their mental schools, however, these students feel a sense of involve-
health (T. T. Watt, 2003). Contrary to widespread opin- ment and obligation equal to that of more academically
ion, there is no evidence that rates of student victimiza- successful students.
tion are higher in larger schools, although victimization is In short, although large schools may be able to offer
more likely in schools where the student–teacher ratio is more diverse curricula and provide greater material re-
higher, perhaps because it is easier for schools to establish sources to their students, the toll that school size may
and enforce norms about how to behave (Gottfredson & take on student learning and engagement appears to
DiPietro, 2011; Klein & Cornell, 2010). In addition, many exceed the benefits of being bigger (V. Lee & Smith,
large schools are broken down into schools within 1995). Evidence also suggests that there is more in-
schools. Although few such transformations have been equality in students’ educational experiences in larger
studied systematically, the existing research indicates schools, where students may be sorted into tracks of
both advantages and disadvantages to this approach. On differing quality. In small schools, in contrast, it is more
the positive side, creating schools within schools does likely that all students are exposed to the same curricu-
lead to the development of a more positive social envi- lum, if only because the school cannot afford to offer
ronment; on the negative side, though, if not done care- more than one.
fully, schools may inadvertently create “schools” within
one school that vary considerably in their educational
quality.
Some of the most interesting findings of research on MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
school size concern participation in extracurricular Did you attend an especially large (more
activities, not achievement in the classroom. You might than 1,500 students) or especially small
expect that, in addition to providing a more varied cur- (fewer than 500 students) high school?
riculum, large schools would be able to offer more If so, how do you think your experience
diverse extracurricular activities to their students—and was affected by the size of your
indeed they do. Large schools can support more athletic school?
teams, after-school clubs, and student organizations.
But because large schools also contain so many more
students, actual rates of participation in different ac-
tivities are only half as high in large schools as in smaller ❚ Variations in Class Size Experts now agree
ones. As a result, in larger schools, students tend more that the ideal size of a high school is between 600 and
often to be observers than participants in school activi- 900 students (V. Lee & Smith, 1997). Unfortunately,
ties. For instance, during the fall, a small school and a policymakers do not always implement social science
large school might each field teams in football, soccer, findings in ways that accurately reflect the research
and cross-country running, together requiring a total evidence. Encouraged by the results of research on
of 100 students. An individual’s chances of being 1 of smaller schools, many politicians have called for
those 100 students are greater in a school that has only smaller classes. However, in contrast to studies of
500 students than in a school with an enrollment schools, studies of classrooms indicate that variations
of 4,000. within the typical range of classroom sizes—from 20
to 40 students—do not generally affect students’
❚ The Strengths of Small Schools Because stu- scholastic achievement once they have reached ado-
dents in small schools are more likely than students in lescence. Small classes do benefit young elementary
large schools to be active in a wider range of activities, school children (that is, up until third grade), who
they are more likely to report doing things that help may need more individualized instruction (Finn,
them develop their skills and abilities, allow them to Gerber, & Boyd-Zaharias, 2005), but adolescents in
work closely with others, and make them feel needed classes with 40 students learn just as much as students
and important. In a small school, chances are, sooner in classes with 20 students (Mosteller, Light, & Sachs,
or later, most students will find themselves on a team, 1996).
in the student government, or in an extracurricular or- An important exception to this finding involves situa-
ganization. Students in small schools also are more tions that call for highly individualized instruction or tu-
likely to be placed in positions of leadership and re- toring, in which case smaller classes do appear to be more
sponsibility, and they more often report having done effective. For example, in remedial education classes,
things that made them feel confident and diligent. where teachers must give a great deal of attention to each
School size especially affects the participation of stu- student, small classes are valuable. One implication of
dents whose grades are not very good. In large schools,
academically marginal students often feel like outsiders
and rarely get involved in school activities. In small schools within schools Subdivisions of the student body within
large schools created to foster feelings of belongingness.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 196 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

196 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

these findings is that it may be profitable for schools that once (after either 6th or 8th grade). However, many edu-
maintain regular class sizes of 25–30 students to increase cators felt that the two-school system was unable to meet
the sizes of these classes a bit and cut down on the num- the special needs of young adolescents, whose intellectual
ber of classrooms in order to free some instructors, and and emotional maturity was greater than that expected in
to trim the sizes of classes for students who need special- elementary school, but not yet at the level necessary for
ized, small-group instruction. high school. During the early years of compulsory sec-
ondary education, the establishment of separate schools
❚ The Problem of Overcrowding One certain im- for young adolescents began, and the junior high school
pediment to delivering high-quality education, especially (which contained the 7th, 8th, and sometimes 9th grades)
in large metropolitan school districts, is overcrowding was born (Hechinger, 1993). Toward the end of the 20th
(Ready et al., 2004). According to national surveys, nearly century, the middle school—a three- or four-year school
15% of U.S. secondary schools are overcrowded—that is, housing the 7th and 8th grades with one or more younger
the size of the student body is at least 6% larger than the grades—gained in popularity, replacing the junior high
school was designed to house—and an additional 8% are school in many districts (Elmore, 2009).
“severely overcrowded,” with the student body 25% above In more recent years, school districts have moved
capacity. Schools with more than 50% ethnic minority away from housing young adolescents separately and
students are especially likely to be overcrowded. Achieve- are returning to a two-school model (usually K–8 and
ment is lower in overcrowded schools because of stress 9–12), in light of many studies showing that students
on both students and teachers, the use of facilities for in- demonstrate higher achievement and fewer behavioral
struction that were not designed to serve as classrooms problems under this arrangement (Weiss & Baker-
(such as gyms), and inadequate resources. Smith, 2010). It is important to note, however, that the
Educators have attempted to reduce the adverse ef- particular grade configuration of a school is less impor-
fects of overcrowding through a variety of measures— tant than the school’s educational climate and quality
some successful, others not. Many school districts use of instruction (Elmore, 2009). In fact, in one recent
temporary structures, such as trailers, to provide addi- study, 6th-grade girls attending elementary school,
tional classroom space. Unfortunately, many such porta- where they were the oldest students, reported more
ble units, especially older ones, are constructed with fighting and more suicidal thoughts than their counter-
materials that are harmful to students’ physical health, parts who were in middle school (Gunter & Bakken,
and the tight quarters and poor ventilation common in 2010), and in another, which was conducted in small,
these structures can create toxic environments (Ready et rural communities, there was more bullying in K–8 and
al., 2004). Other districts—most famously, Los Angeles— K–12 schools than in districts that had separate ele-
have created multitrack programs, whereby schools are mentary and secondary schools (Farmer, Hamm,
used year-round, and students are organized into groups, Leung, Lambert, & Gravelle, 2011).
with one group on vacation at any given point in time.
Evaluations of this approach have been mixed.
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
In light of what you know about devel-
AGE GROUPING AND
opment during early adolescence, are
SCHOOL TRANSITIONS
you surprised by the results of the study
A second issue that social scientists have examined in the that found higher rates of problems
study of school organization concerns the ways in which among 6th-grade girls attending elemen-
schools group students of different ages and the frequency tary school than among those in middle
with which students are expected to change schools. school? Can you speculate on why this
Early in the 20th century, most school districts sepa- may have been the case?
rated youngsters into an elementary school (which had
either six or eight grades) and a secondary school (which
had either four or six grades). Students changed schools
❚ The Transition Into Secondary School One of
the most commonly reported findings is that students’
academic motivation and school grades drop as they
move from elementary into middle or junior high school
junior high school An educational institution designed during the
(Dotterer, McHale, & Crouter, 2009; Eccles, 2004;
early era of public secondary education, in which young adolescents
Gentry, Gable, & Rizza, 2002; Gutman & Midgley, 2000;
are schooled separately from older adolescents.
Murdock, Anderman, & Hodge, 2000). (Interestingly,
middle school An educational institution housing 7th- and scores on standardized achievement tests do not decline
8th-grade students along with adolescents who are 1 or 2 years during this same time, suggesting that the drop in grades
younger.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 197 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 197

may be more a reflection of changes in grading practices what adolescents need and what their schools provide,
and student motivation than in students’ knowledge which leads many young adolescents to disengage from
[Eccles, 2004].) school (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). This is particularly prob-
lematic, because studies show that disengaging from
school during this time increases the risk of students’ de-
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION veloping behavior problems, whereas remaining con-
nected to school protects against some of the harmful
When you were a young adolescent, did you go to
effects of poor family relationships (Loukas, Roalson, &
school with older adolescents, with preadolescent
Herrera, 2010; Oelsner, Lippold, & Greenberg, 2011).
children, or only with people your age?
How do you think your experience was
affected by this arrangement? HOW SECONDARY SCHOOLS DIFFER
FROM ELEMENTARY SCHOOLS
The classroom environment in the typical middle school or
junior high school is quite different from that in the typical
Researchers also have examined how changing schools elementary school (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles, Lord, &
affects student achievement and behavior. In many of these Roeser, 1996). Not only are junior high schools larger and
studies, researchers compared school arrangements in less personal, but middle and junior high school teachers
which students remain in elementary school until 8th also hold different beliefs about students than do elemen-
grade—that is, where they have one school change—with tary school teachers—even when they teach students of the
arrangements in which they move from elementary school, same chronological age (Midgley, Berman, & Hicks, 1995).
to middle or junior high school, and then to high school— For example, teachers in junior high schools are less likely
where they change schools twice. In general, this research to trust their students and more likely to emphasize disci-
suggests that school transitions, whenever they occur, can pline, which creates a mismatch between what students at
disrupt the academic performance, behavior, and self- this age desire (more independence) and what their teach-
image of adolescents, although this effect may be stronger ers provide (more control). Teachers in junior high schools
among White students than their ethnic minority peers also tend to be more likely to believe that students’ abilities
(Kuperminc, Blatt, Shahar, Henrich, & Leadbeater, 2004). are fixed and not easily modified through instruction—a
This disruption is generally temporary, however. Over time, belief that interferes with their students’ achievement. In
most youngsters adapt successfully to changing schools, addition, teachers who teach in junior high or middle
especially when other aspects of their life—family and peer schools are less likely than other teachers to feel confident
relations, for example—remain stable and supportive and about their teaching ability (Eccles & Roeser, 2009).
when the new school environment is well suited for adoles- It is hardly surprising that students experience a
cents (Anderman & Midgley, 1996; Gillock & Reyes, 1996; drop in achievement motivation when they enter mid-
Lord, Eccles, & McCarthy, 1994; Seidman, Lambert, Allen, dle or junior high school, given the change in environ-
& Aber, 2003; Teachman, Paasch, & Carver, 1996). ments they experience and the mismatch between what
Researchers do not agree about whether the drop in adolescents need developmentally and what the typical
academic motivation and achievement that occurs after school context provides. The issue is not that the ado-
elementary school is due to the school transition itself lescents must make a transition; it is the nature of the
(that is, students suffer whenever they have to change transition they must make. Indeed, although students’
schools) or to the nature of the difference between ele- self-esteem drops during the transition into middle or
mentary school, on the one hand, and middle or junior junior high school, it increases somewhat during the
high school, on the other. Specifically, some experts be- early high school years, indicating that changing schools
lieve that the poor performance of middle and junior in and of itself isn’t the problem. Consistent with this,
high schools is due primarily to their failure to meet the middle school students attending more personal, less
particular developmental needs of young adolescents departmentalized schools, or schools where they are
(Barber & Olsen, 2004; Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Between more involved, do better than their peers in more rigid
6th and 8th grade, for example, students report declines and more anonymous schools (V. Lee & Smith, 1993;
in how supportive their teachers and classmates are, how Wang & Holcombe, 2010). Not surprisingly, changing
much autonomy students have to make decisions, and schools is easier on students who move into small rather
how clear and fair school rules are (Way, Reddy, & Rhodes, than large institutions (Russell, Elder, & Conger, 1997).
2007). Because adolescence is a time during which rela- Why do junior high school teachers differ from
tionships with peers and nonfamilial adults become more those who teach elementary school? The answer is not
important, independence becomes more desirable, and clear-cut. The individuals who choose to become junior
rules and regulations are increasingly scrutinized, these high teachers do not seem to differ all that much from
changes in school climate create a mismatch between those who choose to teach younger grades. Rather, it
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 198 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

198 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

may be that the organization and anonymity of junior new school became even more competent over the course
high schools have a negative effect on the teachers who of the change. As these authors wrote, in the face of the
work in them, which, in turn, affects the way they inter- challenges posed by changing schools, “the psychoso-
act with students. This is consistent with a large body of cially rich become richer, while the psychologically poor
evidence that students are more engaged in school become poorer” (Monahan & Steinberg, 2011, p. 576).
when their teachers themselves are more engaged in Factors other than students’ prior record may also
their work (Louis & Smith, 1992). influence the nature of their transition to middle or high
Cultural stereotypes about adolescence also may have school. For example, adolescents who have close friends
a negative influence on junior high school teachers’ be- before and during the transition adapt more successfully to
liefs. As we saw in the Introduction, many adults believe the new school environment (Wentzel, Barry, & Caldwell,
that adolescence is an inevitably difficult time—not only 2004), although the benefits of staying with their friends
for teenagers themselves but also for those who work with may accrue only to students who had been doing well pre-
them. To the extent that teachers come into the junior viously. Students who had been doing poorly actually ad-
high classroom with negative images of adolescence as a just better if they enroll in a different school than their
stage in the life cycle—that teenagers are inherently un- friends, perhaps because their friends were contributing to
ruly, or unteachable, or perplexing—their preconcep- their poor performance (Schiller, 1999).
tions may interfere with their work as educators (Midgley, Evidently, then, the transition into secondary school is
Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1988). (Recall that this same sort of not the sort of stressor that has uniform effects on all stu-
process affects parents who hold negative stereotypes dents (Li & Lerner, 2011). More vulnerable adolescents,
about adolescence.) As we shall see in a later section, one adolescents with fewer sources of social support, and ado-
of the most important influences on the adolescent’s ex- lescents moving into more impersonal schools may be
perience in school is the climate of his or her classroom. more susceptible to the adverse consequences of this tran-
sition than their peers are. Not surprisingly, then, studies
❚ Individual Differences in the Extent of Transi- of poor, inner-city youngsters, who often are coping with
tional Problems Although some aspects of the transi- problems associated with economic stress and neighbor-
tion into secondary school may be difficult for students hood disadvantage, find especially significant negative ef-
to negotiate, not all students experience the same degree fects of the school transition on these students’ self-esteem,
of stress (Fenzel, 2001). Not surprisingly, students who achievement, perceptions of the school environment, re-
have more academic and psychosocial problems before ports of social support, and participation in extracurricu-
making a school transition cope less successfully with it lar activities (Eccles, 2004; Seidman, Aber, Allen, & French,
(Carlson et al., 1999; S. A. Little & Garber, 2004; Murdock 1996). Generally speaking, boys, ethnic minority students,
et al., 2000; Roeser, Eccles, & Freedman-Doan, 1999). and students from poor families are more likely to be-
One recent study found a pattern of cascading effects, come disengaged from school during early adolescence
with academic and behavioral problems in elementary (Li & Lerner, 2011). Among Black and Latino students,
school leading to an increase in problems during the transitioning to a school where the proportion of students
transition into middle school (Moilanen, Shaw, & Max- from the same ethnic background is lower than it had
well, 2010b). Cascades can work in the opposite direction been at their previous school is associated with greater
as well; another recent study found that students scoring disengagement from school, lower grades, and more fre-
high in social competence before the transition into a quent absences (see Figure 6.3) (Benner & Graham, 2009).

4 4
Black Latino
3.8 3.8
Belonging

3.6 3.6

3.4 3.4

3.2 3.2

3 3
Spring 8th Fall 9th Spring 8th Fall 9th
Incongruent Congruent Incongruent Congruent
FIGURE 6.3 Transitioning into secondary school may lead to feelings of disengagement when there are fewer students
of the same ethnic background. Congruent refers to whether the student’s ethnicity is the same as that of the majority of
students in the school. (Benner & Graham, 2009)
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 199 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 199

Low-income students’ adjustment to middle school abilities; tracking may be especially useful in high
may be enhanced as a result of an intervention targeted school, where students must master certain basic skills
at their parents, according to one study (Bronstein et before they can learn such specialized subjects as sci-
al., 1998). The researchers had parents participate in an ence, math, or foreign languages. Critics of tracking
11-week program designed to increase their under- point out, however, that students who are placed in the
standing of adolescent development and their effective- remedial track generally receive not just a different edu-
ness as parents. The children whose parents participated cation, but a poorer-quality education, than those in
in the program were functioning better psychologically the more advanced tracks (Darling-Hammond, 1997).
and behaviorally, both immediately after the program Moreover, the effects of tracking are not limited to aca-
and one year later, than were their counterparts whose demic outcomes. As you read in Chapter 5, schools play
parents did not participate. Moreover, the children an important role in influencing adolescents’ friend-
whose parents participated did not show the decline in ship choices. When students are tracked, they tend to
functioning that often is associated with the transition socialize only with peers from the same academic
into middle school. In general, parental support and group. Tracking can thereby contribute to the polariza-
involvement are associated with better adolescent ad- tion of the student body into different subcultures that
justment during school transitions (Isakson & Jarvis, are often hostile toward each other (Eccles & Roeser,
1999). One study of low-income Black students found 2009).
that students who fare best during the transition not
only have parents who are involved in their education,
but supportive teachers as well (Gutman & Midgley, MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
2000). Tracking seems to have different effects on students
in the more advanced and less advanced tracks. In
TRACKING view of this, do you think
schools should engage in
In some schools, students with different academic abil- ability grouping? Is there
ities and interests do not attend classes together. Some a way to track students
classes are designated as more challenging and more that might maximize its
rigorous, and are reserved for students identified as es- benefits but reduce its
pecially capable. Other classes in the same subject area costs?
are designated as average classes and are taken by most
students. Still others are designated as remedial classes
and are reserved for students having academic difficul-
ties. The process of separating students into different Some critics of tracking also point out that decisions
levels of classes within the same school is called ability about track placements often discriminate against poor
grouping, or tracking. Not all high schools use tracking and ethnic minority students and may hinder rather
systems. In some schools, students with different abili- than enhance their academic progress (Oakes, 1995;
ties take all their classes together. Wells & Serna, 1996). Some school counselors may as-
Even among schools that use tracking, there are im- sume that ethnic minority or poor youngsters are not
portant differences in how tracking is done (J. Jones, capable of handling the work in advanced classes and
Vanfossen, & Ensminger, 1995). Some schools are more may automatically assign them to average or remedial
inclusive in their tracking, permitting a relatively high classes, where less material is covered and the work is
proportion of students into the highest track (even, less challenging. One recent analysis of national data
perhaps, some students whose abilities do not warrant found, for instance, that Black students were especially
such placement). Other schools are more exclusive, more likely to be enrolled in lower-track math classes in
limiting the places in the highest track to a privileged schools in which Blacks are in the minority, even after
few (even if this means placing some high-ability stu- taking into account students’ qualifications (S. Kelly,
dents in the lower tracks). And still other schools are 2009).
“meritocratic,” placing students in tracks that accu- Not all research indicates that track placements are
rately match their abilities. biased, however. Other studies have found that students’
ability has a stronger influence than their background on
❚ Pros and Cons of Tracking Educators have de- initial track placement (Dauber, Alexander, & Entwisle,
bated the pros and cons of tracking for years, but re- 1996), but that middle-class and White students initially
search provides no definitive answers about its overall placed in lower tracks are more likely to be moved into
effects (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Proponents of tracking
note that ability grouping allows teachers to design
class lessons that are more finely tuned to students’ tracking The practice of separating students into ability groups, so
that they take classes with peers at the same skill level.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 200 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

200 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

higher ones, in part because their parents frequently suc- emphasize critical thinking rather than rote memoriza-
ceed in “lobbying” their child’s school for a changed tion (Darling-Hammond, 1997). Being placed in a
track placement (Hallinan, 1996; Oakes, 1995; Wells & more advanced track has a positive influence on school
Serna, 1996). It is also the case that adolescents from achievement (how much the student actually learns
more well-off families more frequently consult with over time), on subsequent course selection (what cur-
their parents about what courses to take than do less af- riculum the student is exposed to), and on ultimate
fluent adolescents, and this leads more affluent students educational attainment (how many years of schooling
to take more (and more advanced) math and science the student completes). To the extent that a student’s
classes (Crosnoe & Huston, 2007). family background influences his or her track place-
ment, tracking has the effect of maintaining the status
❚ On the Wrong Track Early track placements set quo (Oakes, 2005).
in motion an educational trajectory that is often diffi- In addition, because students are assigned to differ-
cult to change without the deliberate intervention of ent tracks initially on the basis of test scores and other
the student’s parents (Dauber, Alexander, & Entwisle, indicators of aptitude, and because students in the
1996; Hallinan, 1996; D. Stevenson, Schiller, & Schneider, lower tracks receive an inferior education, the net effect
1994). And the ways in which students’ schedules are of tracking over time is to increase preexisting differ-
arranged may lead students to be tracked in several dif- ences among students. The students who need the most
ferent subject areas simply because they are tracked in help are assigned to the tracks in which the quality of
one class, which makes the effects of tracking even instruction is the poorest; not surprisingly, studies find
more substantial (Heck, Price, & Thomas, 2004). If the that students in lower tracks exert less effort, which, in
only class period during which advanced math is of- turn, limits their learning (Callahan, 2005; Carbonaro,
fered is the same as the class period during which reme- 2005). Although students in the lower tracks usually get
dial English is taught, a student who is assigned to the short end of the educational stick, there are some
remedial English will not be able to take advanced math exceptions—for example, schools in which classes in
(Lucas & Berends, 2002). the lower tracks are taught by strong teachers who insist
Research on tracking in high schools indicates quite on maintaining high standards (Gamoran, 1993;
clearly that students in different tracks have markedly Hallinan, 1996).
different opportunities to learn (Gamoran, 1996). Stu-
dents in the more advanced tracks receive more chal- ❚ The Effects of Tracking on Student Achieve-
lenging instruction and better teaching, and they are ment Literally hundreds of studies have been done on
more likely to engage in classroom activities that the impact of tracking on student achievement
(Hallinan, 1996). Unfortunately, this
research suggests both positive and
negative effects and, more importantly,
different effects on students in differ-
ent tracks. Tracking has positive effects
on the achievement of high-track stu-
dents, negative effects on low-track
students, and negligible effects on stu-
dents in the middle (Fuligni, Eccles, &
Barber, 1995; Hallinan, 1996). Because
of this, decisions about whether to
implement tracking in nontracked
schools, or whether to “detrack” schools
that use ability grouping, are typically
quite controversial; parents of stu-
dents in the higher tracks favor the
practice, while parents of students in
the lower tracks oppose it (Wells &
Serna, 1996).
Even in schools that do not have for-
mal tracking, teachers may group stu-
dents within the same class into
ability groups. In such an arrange-
Research on tracking suggests that it has positive effects on the achievement of students in ment, students may have a wider
the more advanced tracks but negative effects on students in the lower tracks. range of peers with whom to compare
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 201 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 201

themselves than they would in separate tracks, since the specific needs of students and can target educa-
their classes are more diverse in composition. The im- tional and professional resources in a cost-effective
pact of this comparison process on both students and way. On the other hand, segregating students on the
teachers is quite interesting. For high-ability students, basis of academic ability may foster social isolation and
within-classroom ability grouping raises their expecta- stigmatization—either for being “stupid” or for being a
tions for achievement and raises their teachers’ evalua- “brainiac.”
tions of them; for low-ability students, the opposite is Generally, educators have tended to favor main-
true: They have lowered expectations and get worse streaming over separate classrooms for adolescents with
grades from their teachers (Reuman, 1989). Presum- special needs. (In the case of adolescents with disabili-
ably, both adolescents and teachers make their evalua- ties, mainstreaming, whenever possible, is required by
tions based on unstated comparisons. In classes with law in the United States.) Proponents of mainstreaming
mixed ability groups, the high-ability students look argue that the psychological costs of separating adoles-
better, and the low-ability students look worse, than cents with special academic needs from their peers out-
they would in a conventionally tracked school or in a weigh the potential academic benefits. Studies of gifted
school in which ability grouping is not used (H. Marsh, youngsters have found, for example, that those who
Chessor, Craven, & Roche, 1995). As is the case with were integrated into regular classrooms have more pos-
tracking, within-classroom ability grouping also ex- itive academic self-conceptions than did those assigned
poses students in different groups to different levels of to special classes (H. Marsh et al., 1995) and that these
educational quality, with students in the high-ability effects persist even after graduating (H. Marsh, Traut-
groups receiving more challenging instruction and wein, Lüdtke, Baumert, & Köller, 2007).
more engaging learning experiences (Catsambis, One downside to being placed with students of high
1992). academic ability is that when other students compare
themselves to their high-achieving classmates, they
❚ Students at the Extremes Related to the issue don’t feel as competent as they would if their point of
of tracking are questions concerning the placement of comparison were students who were not so smart; stu-
individuals who are considered gifted students and of dents seem especially likely to compare themselves to
those who have a learning disability. Adolescents who same-sex peers (Thijs, Verkuyten, & Helmond, 2010).
score 130 or higher on an intelligence test are consid- This phenomenon, called the big fish–little pond
ered gifted. Adolescents with a learning disability are effect, has been documented around the world, most
those whose actual performance is significantly poorer recently in an analysis of data from over 100,000 stu-
than their expected performance (based on intelli- dents attending schools in 26 different countries (H.
gence or aptitude tests, for example) and whose diffi- Marsh & Hau, 2003). Being a big fish in a little pond is
culty with academic tasks cannot be traced to an also helpful for admission to college. One study of some
emotional problem, such as coping with a parental di- 45,000 applications to three elite universities found that
vorce, or a sensory dysfunction, such as a visual or applicants’ chances of being accepted are greater when
hearing impairment. Most learning disabilities are they come from high schools with a relatively lower
presumed to be neurological in origin (Berninger & proportion of other high-achieving students than when
Miller, 2011; Shaywitz, Gruen, & Shaywitz, 2007). applicants with the same credentials come from high
Common types of specifi c learning disabilities
include dyslexia (impaired ability in reading or spell-
ing), dysgraphia (impaired ability in handwriting),
and dyscalculia (impaired ability in arithmetic). gifted students Students who are unusually talented in some
Specific learning disabilities are common—about one aspect of intellectual performance.
in five school-age children and youth is at risk for a
learning disability, with rates of learning disabilities learning disability A difficulty with academic tasks that cannot be
traced to an emotional problem or sensory dysfunction.
significantly more common among boys than girls
(Berninger & Miller, 2011). dyslexia Impaired ability in reading or spelling.
Educators have debated whether gifted students and
dysgraphia Impaired ability in handwriting.
those with learning disabilities are best served by in-
struction in separate classes (for example, in enriched dyscalculia Impaired ability in arithmetic.
classes for gifted students or in special education classes
mainstreaming The integration of adolescents who have
for students with a learning disability) or by main-
educational handicaps into regular classrooms.
streaming, the integration of all students with special
needs into regular classrooms. Pros and cons of each big fish–little pond effect The reason that individuals who
approach have been identified. On the one hand, sepa- attend high school with high-achieving peers feel worse about
rate special education programs can be tailored to meet themselves than comparably successful individuals with lower-
achieving peers.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 202 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

202 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

schools with many other high achievers (Espenshade, ADHD is defined by persistent and impairing symp-
Hale, & Chung, 2005). toms of inattention, impulsivity, and/or hyperactivity.
Whereas the big fish–little pond effect suggests that Adolescents with ADHD are classified into one of three
gifted students might not be better off psychologically subtypes: predominantly inattentive (about 30 to 40
in classes restricted to high-achieving students—and percent of all cases), predominantly hyperactive/impul-
argues in favor of mainstreaming them—it poses a sive (fewer than 5 percent of all cases, and rarely seen
dilemma for those who favor mainstreaming students during adolescence), or combined (between 50 and 60
with learning disabilities. Low-achieving students, percent of all cases). One reason the prevalence of
when mainstreamed, will end up comparing themselves ADHD declines with age is that some individuals de-
to students whose performance is better than their velop better attentional and impulse control as they
own, and they may end up feeling worse about them- mature from childhood into adolescence and adult-
selves than had they been separated into special classes hood. In addition to being at risk for academic difficul-
with comparably achieving peers (H. Marsh & Hau, ties, individuals with ADHD are also at risk for a wide
2003). Perhaps because of this, studies find that, even range of nonscholastic problems, including criminal
with mainstreaming, adolescents who have learning activity, substance abuse, anxiety, and depression. As
disabilities may suffer psychological consequences re- discussed in Chapter 13, ADHD is present in many
lated to their problems in school. Compared with aver- cases of serious juvenile delinquency.
age-achieving students, adolescents with learning ADHD is considered to be primarily a biological
disabilities report more social and behavioral difficul- disorder with a strong genetic component. In addition,
ties and, not surprisingly, more problems in coping damage to the brain either prenatally (sometimes
with school. They are also more likely than other ado- caused by maternal smoking or drinking during preg-
lescents to have poor peer relations, are less likely to nancy) or shortly after birth (as the result of birth
participate in school-based extracurricular activities, complications or low birth weight). Recent studies of
and are more likely to drop out of school (Berninger & brain development during adolescence point to delays
Miller, 2011). Given the tremendous importance soci- or deficiencies in the development of regions that are
ety places on school success, it is not difficult to see why known to be associated with self-regulation, such as
students who have difficulties learning would suffer the prefrontal cortex; synaptic pruning of this region
psychological as well as scholastic problems. occurs at a slower pace among individuals with ADHD
Experts recommend that adolescents with a spe- than among those without the disorder. Interestingly,
cific learning disability receive extra instruction in individuals who do not have ADHD, but who are more
study skills, time management, organization skills, hyperactive and impulsive than their peers, show pat-
note-taking, and proofreading. In addition, students terns of brain development that are somewhere be-
with a learning disability may need help in increasing tween those seen in adolescents with ADHD and
motivation, dealing with social and emotional diffi- adolescents who have very good impulse control,
culties resulting from problematic peer relationships, which suggests that ADHD may be an extreme point
overcoming their reluctance to participate in classes on a continuum rather than a qualitatively distinct cat-
or seek assistance from teachers, and coping with self- egory (Shaw et al., 2011).
esteem issues related to fears that they are not as intel- ADHD is frequently treated with some sort of stim-
ligent as other students (which is not the case) or that ulant medication, such as methylphenidate (Ritalin).
they will be failures as adults (Berninger & Miller, Stimulant medication is helpful in about 70 percent
2011). of cases. Certain types of antidepressants have also
been shown to be effective, especially with adolescents
❚ Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder who have both ADHD and some sort of mood disor-
Although it is not technically considered to be a learning der, such as depression. One concern about the wide
disability, adolescents who have attention deficit/hy- use of stimulant medication by adolescents with
peractivity disorder (ADHD) frequently have academic ADHD is that many individuals who receive such
difficulties that can be traced to this problem. ADHD is medication share it with their nonafflicted friends,
usually diagnosed during childhood, but the condition who may use the medication recreationally or for help
persists into adolescence in 50 to 70 percent of cases, with studying (stimulant medication improves atten-
and into adulthood in about half of all children with the tion in most individuals, regardless of whether they
diagnosis (Antshel & Barkley, 2011). have ADHD). Psychological therapies for ADHD are
also widely used, often in conjunction with medica-
tion, although such therapies are more commonly used
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with children than adolescents (Antshel & Barkley,
A biologically based psychological disorder characterized by 2011).
impulsivity, inattentiveness, and restlessness, often in school
situations.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 203 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 203

ETHNIC COMPOSITION groups are similar) than in schools that are less bal-
anced (Juvonen, Nishina, & Graham, 2006). In general,
Following the landmark U.S. Supreme Court rulings
though, research suggests that being in the minority in
in Brown v. Board of Education of Topeka (1954, 1955),
one’s school is hard on students.
in which the Court found that it was unconstitutional
to maintain separate schools for children on the basis
of race, many school districts adopted measures de- ❚ Being in the Minority The difficulties associ-
signed to make schools more diverse. They did this ated with attending school where one is in a distinct
either by assigning students to schools in a way that minority were tellingly illustrated in a study by Patricia
would create ethnic diversity or by encouraging vol- Phelan and colleagues (Phelan, Yu, & Davidson, 1994).
untary desegregation, through measures like having Consider these excerpts from the researchers’ inter-
“magnet” schools that would create diversity by draw- views with students:
ing students from different neighborhoods (for in- Ivonne, Mexican American female: Well, I kind of feel
stance, by having citywide schools specializing in the uncomfortable. Not many Mexicans and Hispanics are in
performing arts). Although the Supreme Court ruled [my] classes. They [other students] probably think of me
in 2007 that school districts may no longer use race as as weird, because they probably have this view that most
a factor in deciding how to assign students to schools Hispanics are dumb or something. They have that opin-
(R. Barnes, 2007), efforts to create ethnic and racial ion, you know, [Hispanics] get bad grades. So, I don’t
diversity through voluntary measures are still in use in know why I feel uncomfortable. I just . . . it means you’re
not really with any other . . . many people. Maybe by the
many cities.
end of the year they will realize that I belong. (Phelan
❚ Effects of Desegregation Does transforming et al., 1994, p. 425)
the ethnic composition of a school make a big differ- Trinh, Vietnamese American female: [Because I’m Viet-
ence in student achievement? Studies of the short-term namese] I notice the little things more often than other
effects of desegregation on high school students have people. Just like, I don’t really get noticed by all the popu-
been mixed. On the one hand, research indicates that lar people. OK, everyone in the class, I know their names
desegregation has surprisingly little impact on the and everything. . . . Like being Vietnamese . . . like they
achievement levels of either minority or White young- have a lot of Americans in here. That there are more of
sters (Entwisle, 1990). In addition, some evidence sug- them, and when you’re alone, you’re nervous over little
things. (Phelan et al., 1994, p. 425)
gests that minority youngsters’ self-esteem is higher
when they attend schools in which they are in the ma- Sonia, Mexican American female: Yeah, it’s weird,
jority; in general, students fare better psychologically ’cause most teachers, you know—White teachers—
when the cultural environment of their neighborhood some of them are kind of prejudiced. . . . It’s probably
is consonant with the cultural environment of their the way they look at you, the way they talk, you know
when they’re talking about something—about some-
school (Goldsmith, 2004; Gray-Little & Carels, 1997;
thing like when they talk about the people who are go-
Hudley, 1995; Nieto et al., 1996; Seaton & Yip, 2009). In ing to drop out, and they . . . look around, look around
schools that mix students from low- and high-income [at you].
neighborhoods, students from low-income neighbor- And then Mr. Kula, when he’s talking about teenage
hoods actually do worse than they do when they attend pregnancy or something like that. He turns around and
schools that are less socioeconomically diverse, espe- he looks at us. It’s like—he tries to look around the
cially if they are Black or Hispanic (Crosnoe, 2009; whole room, so we won’t notice but like he mostly like
Owens, 2010). tries to tell us, tries to get it through our heads, you
Consistent with this, students who have been bused know. Sometimes I think he’s prejudiced. And some-
to school out of their neighborhood report weaker feel- times I think he’s trying to help us. (Phelan et al., 1994,
ings of attachment to their school than do students p. 431)
whose schools draw directly from the local community
(Anderman, 2002). Students’ attachment to school also
ALTERNATIVES TO PUBLIC SCHOOLS
is higher when they attend schools where relatively
more of their classmates are from the same ethnic While the vast majority of students attending second-
group; interestingly, ethnic composition affects stu- ary school in America are enrolled in conventional
dents’ emotional attachment to their school, but not public schools, a substantial minority attend private
their degree of participation in classroom activities schools, either parochial (i.e., with a religious affiliation)
(M. Johnston, Crosnoe, & Elder, 2001). Within multi- or independent, or specially created public schools, like
ethnic schools, however, students feel safer, less lonely, charter schools or magnet schools. In the past, research-
and less harassed in relatively more diverse schools (i.e., ers cared little about studying differences between
where the proportions of students from different ethnic public schools and their alternatives. But, as we noted
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 204 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

204 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

(especially Catholic schools), this appears to be due


more to the characteristics of the students who attend
them than to the private schools themselves (Braun,
Jenkins, & Grigg, 2006; Hallinan & Kubitschek, 2012;
Lubienski & Lubienski, 2006; Rumberger & Palardy,
2005), although some studies have found genuine ad-
vantages for Catholic school especially among poor,
inner-city, minority youth (Jeynes, 2002). Students
who attend private school may also be encouraged (or
required) to take more advanced courses than stu-
dents in public schools, which contributes to their su-
perior performance on achievement tests (Carbonaro
& Covay, 2010). These studies, as well as a large body
of research on the topic that we’ll look at in Chapter
12, indicate that students’ family background is a far
more powerful influence on their achievement than is
the quality of the school they attend. Similarly, evalu-
ations of the impact of charter schools and voucher
programs on student achievement, once students’
background characteristics are taken into account,
have not produced consistent results; at the very least,
research indicates that these are not likely to be “silver
bullets” in the effort to raise American student
achievement (Howell & Peterson, 2002; Loveless,
Although social scientists disagree over their interpretation, studies 2002). This is also true in disadvantaged urban areas,
have shown that adolescents who attend parochial schools generally
achieve at a higher rate than those attending public schools.
where it had been hoped that charter schools might be
the solution to the many problems that plague educa-
tion in the inner city. One additional concern is that in
urban areas, the availability of private schools con-
earlier, during the late 1980s and 1990s, many educa- tributes to racial segregation, because many White
tion policymakers suggested that one way to improve students who would otherwise attend their neighbor-
schools would be to give parents more of a choice in hood public school attend private school instead
determining where their child was enrolled, in order to (Saporito & Sohoni, 2006).
force schools to compete for the best students. There is One thing we do know is that the climate of public
some evidence to support this: When information and private schools, especially Catholic schools, is often
about school test scores is provided to parents, parents very different. As many writers have pointed out, a
choose to send their children to higher-performing Catholic school is a community in which parents,
schools, which in turn increases the students’ achieve- teachers, and students all share similar values and atti-
ment (Hastings & Weinstein, 2008). tudes. Strong communities, whether based in neigh-
One concrete suggestion in this spirit was that borhoods or schools, generate what has been called
states provide parents with school vouchers that could social capital—interpersonal resources that, like finan-
be used to “purchase” education at a school of their cial capital, give “richer” students advantages over
choosing—private or public. Another suggestion was “poorer” ones. Students profit from the social capital
that states permit the development of charter schools— associated with attending a Catholic school, for exam-
independent public schools that are freer to operate as ple, because the lessons taught in school are reinforced
they wish, without some of the constraints imposed by at home, at church, and in the neighborhood, and be-
the state’s education bureaucracy. In light of these sug- cause the links between home and school are stronger
gestions, researchers became interested in whether some (Teachman et al., 1996). In addition, private schools
types of schools produced more high-achieving stu- typically assign more homework and are more orderly
dents than others. and disciplined (as you will read, an important element
Are alternatives to public schools better than pub- of the climate in good schools) (Coleman, Hoffer, &
lic schools? Although some studies have found that stu- Kilgore, 1982). Students who attend private schools
dents’ test scores are higher in private schools (Catholic or otherwise) are substantially less likely to
report feeling unsafe, being exposed to gangs, or wit-
nessing fighting between ethnic groups (National Cen-
social capital The interpersonal resources available to an ter for Education Statistics, 2002).
adolescent or family.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 205 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 205

RECAP Various aspects of the school climate have impor-


• Generally, smaller schools, although not necessarily tant effects on youngsters’ learning and achievement
smaller classes, are more effective than larger ones. (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). Specifically, how teachers in-
• The transition from elementary school into sec- teract with students, how classroom time is used, and
ondary school, or from one secondary school to what sorts of standards and expectations teachers hold
another, can be difficult for some students, espe- for their students are all more important than the size
cially those who have academic or behavioral prob- of the school, the way age groups are combined, or the
lems at the time of the transition. ethnic composition of the school.
• Many researchers believe there is a worrisome mis-
match between the impersonal and rigid environ- THE BEST CLASSROOM CLIMATE
ment of most schools and the developmental FOR ADOLESCENTS
needs of young adolescents, who are especially in-
terested in forming relationships and forging their What sort of climate brings out the best in students?
independence. Considered together, the results of several studies indi-
• Research on tracking reveals that the academically cate that the same factors that influence positive adoles-
rich get richer and the poor get poorer. Students cent adjustment at home are important at school.
who are placed in the more advanced tracks or in Specifically, students achieve and are engaged more in
high-ability groups in classrooms achieve more than school when they attend schools that are responsive
those in lower tracks, in part because the quality of and demanding. Moreover, academic functioning and
instruction in the higher tracks is superior. psychological adjustment affect each other, so that a
• A large number of students who have academic dif- positive school climate—where relationships between
ficulties have a specific learning disability that im- students and teachers are positive, and teachers are
pairs their ability to read, write, or do arithmetic. both supportive and demanding—enhances adoles-
Other students who struggle in school may have cents’ psychological well-being as well as their achieve-
ADHD, a biologically based disorder characterized ment (Eccles, 2004; Gutierrez, 2000; Kalil & Ziol-Guest,
by inattentiveness, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. 2008; Reddy, Rhodes, & Mulhall, 2003;), in part by
• In general, the effects of changing the ethnic com- strengthening their engagement in class (Dotterer &
position of a school on student achievement are Lowe, 2011).
very modest, although in general students are hap- Generally, both students and teachers are more sat-
pier when their ethnic group is not in the minority. isfied in classes that combine a moderate degree of
• There appear to be advantages for students who structure with high student involvement and high
attend Catholic schools, but these may be due teacher support, a finding that has emerged in studies
more to student background characteristics than to of students from various socioeconomic backgrounds,
the schools themselves. As a rule, what takes ethnic groups, and countries (Jia et al., 2009; Langer,
place within schools is more im- 2001; Vieno, Perkins, Smith, & Santinello, 2005; Way &
portant than how they Robinson, 2003; Wentzel, 2002). In these classes, teach-
are funded or ers encourage their students’ participation but do not
organized. let the class get out of control. Classes that are too task
oriented—particularly those that also emphasize
teacher control—tend to make students anxious, un-
interested, and unhappy (Moos, 1978). Students do
best when their teachers spend a high proportion of
time on lessons (rather than on setting up equipment
Classroom Climate or dealing with discipline problems), begin and end
Thus far, we have seen that certain elements of the lessons on time, provide clear feedback to students
school’s social organization—size, age grouping, track- about what is expected of them and about their perfor-
ing, and so forth—can affect students’ motivation, mance, and give ample praise to students when they
behavior, and achievement. But these factors have rela- perform well (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). Students also
tively modest effects on students, and they are impor- demonstrate higher achievement when the classroom
tant mainly because they influence what takes place in climate promotes cooperation, rather than competi-
classrooms and in other school settings. Indeed, most tion, between students (Roseth, Johnson, & Johnson,
social scientists and educators now agree that the most 2008).
important school-related influences on learning and Perhaps not surprisingly, one of the strongest influ-
psychosocial development during adolescence are those ences on how much students enjoy going to school is
seen in the more immediate environment of the school the extent to which they feel their teachers respect and
and classroom. care about them (Hallinan, 2008). Students in schools
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 206 21/03/13 10:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

206 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

in which teachers are supportive but firm and maintain 500


high, well-defined standards for behavior and academic Control Group
495 MyTeachingPartner
work have stronger bonds to their school and more
positive achievement motives; these beliefs and emo- 490

Achievement Test Scores


tions, in turn, lead to fewer problems, better attendance,
lower rates of delinquency, more supportive friend- 485
ships, and higher scores on tests of achievement (Eccles, 480
2004; Li & Lerner, 2011; Loukas, Ripperger-Suhler, &
Horton, 2009; A. M. Ryan & Patrick, 2001; Way & Pahl, 475
2001), although students with especially high levels of 470
family obligation may not benefit as much as their
peers from being engaged in school (Wilkinson-Lee, 465
Zhang, Nuno, & Wilhelm, 2011).
460
Actually, there are many similarities between good
teachers and good parents (Wentzel, 2002). If you 455
think back to what you read in Chapter 4, you’ll see Pre-Test Post-Test
that the pattern of classroom variables associated with FIGURE 6.4 A recent evaluation of a program designed to
positive student behavior and attitudes is reminiscent improve the ways in which teachers interact with students
of the authoritative family environment (Pellerin, found significant improvements in student achievement.
2005). Similarly, an overemphasis on control in the (Allen et al., 2011)
classroom in the absence of support is reminiscent of
the authoritarian family, whereas a lack of clarity and
organization is reminiscent of both the indulgent fam- Schools that provide both structure and support have
ily and the indifferent family—and these styles in the lower rates of suspension than other schools (Gregory,
classroom appear to affect adolescents detrimentally, Cornell, & Fan, 2011).
just as they do at home. A recent evaluation of a pro-
gram designed to improve the ways in which teachers
TEACHER EXPECTATIONS AND
interact with students found significant improve-
STUDENT PERFORMANCE
ments in student achievement (Allen, Pianta, Gregory,
Mikami, & Lun, 2011) (see Figure 6.4). The combina- In addition to research on school climate, several stud-
tion of positive student-teacher relationships in the ies also point to the importance of teachers’ expecta-
context of an orderly and well-managed classroom and tions. There is a strong correlation between teacher
school not only facilitates academic achievement, but expectations and student performance. This is both
reduces behavior problems as well (Gregory et al., because teachers’ expectations are often accurate reflec-
2010; Wang, Selman, Dishion, & Stormshak, 2010). tions of their students’ ability and because teacher
expectations actually create self-fulfilling prophecies
that ultimately influence how their students behave
(Jussim, Eccles & Madon, 1996). Many studies have
found that teacher expectations have a cumulative
long-term impact on student achievement, lowering
the performance of students whose teachers perceive
them as less capable than they actually are (de Boer,
Bosker, & van der Werf, 2010).
Which pathway is more powerful—the impact of
student performance on teacher expectations, or the
impact of teacher expectations on student perfor-
mance? It appears that about 80% of the connection
between teacher expectations and student achievement
results from teachers having accurate perceptions, and
about 20% is an effect of the self-fulfilling prophecy.
(The self-fulfilling prophecy appears to be somewhat
stronger for academically weaker students, whose per-
formance is more tied to teacher expectations.) Even
though the self-fulfilling prophecy effect is relatively
Student performance influences, and is influenced by, teacher small, it may be quite powerful when accumulated over
expectations. years of schooling.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 207 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 207

Because teachers’ expectations influence students’ Teacher


performance, it is important to understand where these expectations
expectations come from. Unfortunately, research sug-
gests that teachers are likely to base their expectations Academic
in part on students’ ethnic and socioeconomic back- competence
ground. In much the same way that these factors some-
times influence tracking decisions, as we saw earlier, Parent school
involvement
they may consciously and unconsciously shape teach-
Grade point
ers’ expectations, which, in turn, affect students’ learn- average
ing. Thus, for example, teachers may call on poor or
minority students less often than they call on affluent
School
or White students—conveying a not-so-subtle message belonging
about whose responses the teacher believes are more
worthy of class attention (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). FIGURE 6.5 Parent involvement in school influences their
Several studies report that Black and Latino students high school children’s achievement, both directly and indi-
perceive their teachers as having low expectations and rectly, by raising teachers’ expectations for their child’s per-
holding stereotypes about their likelihood of misbe- formance. (Kuperminc et al., 2008)
having (M. B. Spencer, 2005). Some evidence suggests
that White teachers rate the misbehavior of Black stu- much the same way that the relationship between par-
dents more harshly than do Black teachers (D. Downey ents and adolescents is reciprocal—parents influence
& Pribesh, 2004), and, as you’ll read later in this chapter, how their teenagers develop, but teenagers influence
this leads to Black students receiving harsher discipline what their parents do—so is the relationship between
and more severe punishments than their peers receive teachers and their students. Effective teachers can en-
for the same school infractions. It is not difficult to see gage and excite their students, and engaged and excited
how years of exposure to this sort of treatment can ad- students can motivate their teachers to be more effective.
versely affect students’ self-concepts and interest in According to national surveys, however, levels of
school. Indeed, teachers’ biases against lower-class or student engagement and excitement in American
minority adolescents may make it difficult for students schools are not high. About half of all high school
from these groups to attain a level of academic accom- sophomores say that disruptive students get in the
plishment that permits upward mobility. In addition, way of their learning and that students who misbe-
biased treatment by teachers—having low expectations have are permitted to get away with it. Studies also
for some ethnic groups and high expectations for show that levels of student engagement in American
others—can contribute to feelings of hostility between high schools are very low, that many students are
students from different ethnicities (Rosenbloom & mainly going through the motions when they are in
Way, 2004). school, and that high school teachers often confront a
Parents also play an important role in the links be- roomful of students who are physically present but
tween teacher expectations and student achievement. psychologically absent (Steinberg, 1996). Engaging
One recent study of Latino students found that how students in school is also good for their overall mental
involved a student’s parents were in school influenced health: Studies find that students who are disengaged
their high school children’s achievement directly (as in from school are more likely to misbehave and engage
other studies, adolescents whose parents are involved in in substance use (G. C. Patton et al., 2006; Schmidt,
school perform better than their peers) but also af- 2003). Commitment to school is associated with less
fected teachers’ expectations for their child’s achieve- delinquency and drug use among male and female
ment, which, in turn, led to better student performance students alike (Payne, 2009).
(see Figure 6.5) (Kuperminc, Darnell, & Alvarez-Jimi-
nez, 2008). In addition, other research has found that ❚ Boring Classes, Bored Students In view of
one factor that helps protect low-income students this, several writers have suggested that if we want to
against the impact of low teacher expectations is having understand the impact of classroom climate on student
high expectations for achievement from their parents achievement, we need to better understand how to en-
(Benner & Mistry, 2007). hance student engagement—the extent to which stu-
dents are psychologically committed to learning and
mastering the material rather than simply completing
THE IMPORTANCE OF STUDENT
ENGAGEMENT
It is important to keep in mind, of course, that students, student engagement The extent to which students are
as well as teachers, influence the classroom climate. In psychologically committed to learning and mastering the material
rather than simply completing the assigned work.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 208 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

208 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

FIGURE 6.6 Studies of fluctua- Excited 0.2


tions in adolescents’ mood over
the course of the day show that 0.1
students—especially high school 5th–8th grade
students—feel bored most of the 0

Mood
time they are in school. (Larson &
Richards, 1998) –0.1

–0.2 9th–12th grade

Bored –0.3
8–9 A.M. 10–11 12–1 P.M. 2–3 4–5 6–7 8–9
Hour

the assigned work (Newmann, Marks, & Gamoran, should try to facilitate students’ feelings of belonging to
1996; Steinberg, 1996). Students frequently say they are their school. Finally, and most importantly, teachers
bored while in school—especially high school students, should assign work that is “authentic”—work that is
who find school far more boring than do middle school interesting, fun, and relevant to the real world (Marks,
students. As you can see in Figure 6.6, students are 2000). There is nothing more alienating to students
bored for most of the time on weekdays between 8:00 than being asked to perform tasks that are boring, un-
A.M. and 3:00 P.M., and the improvement in their mood interesting, and irrelevant. One recent evaluation of an
seems to have more to do with the school day ending intervention designed to help students make connec-
than with any special activity that takes place in the eve- tions between their lives and what they were learning in
ning. Many experts believe that the make-work, routin- science classes improved engagement and achievement
ized, rigid structure of most classrooms, in which among students who had low expectations about how
teachers lecture at students rather than engage them in well they would do (Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009).
discussion, alienates most students from school and
undermines their desire to achieve.
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
The notion that many students feel disengaged from
school—unchallenged and bored—has been borne out Research has revealed the elements of school climate
in numerous studies of contemporary American stu- that are favorable to learning. Given the consistency
dents (e.g., Owen, 1995; Steinberg, 1996). According to with which these factors appear in research on good
one study, titled Getting By: What American Teenagers schools, why might classrooms in which students
Really Think About Their Schools: feel genuinely engaged
be so rare? What impedi-
Students routinely admitted—some with bravado and ments make it difficult
some with chagrin—that they calibrate their efforts, of- for teachers to create
ten meticulously, to do only as much as it takes to get the classroom environments
grade they can live with. For youngsters aiming for pri- in which adolescents are
vate colleges or elite public universities, this concept of engaged?
“getting by” implies a certain grade-point average. . . .
Other youngsters . . . seem satisfied with any passing
grade. . . . Students from across the country repeatedly
❚ Out-of-School Influences on Student Engage-
said that they could “earn” acceptable grades, pass their
ment Teachers and school personnel, of course, are
courses, and receive a diploma, all while investing mini-
mal effort in their school work. . . . Almost two-thirds not the only influences on adolescents’ behavior in
of teens across the country (65%) say they could do school. Several writers have noted that the peer group’s
better in school if they tried harder. (Public Agenda, values and norms also exert an important influence,
1997, p. 20) especially in high school (Bishop, 1999; Steinberg,
1996). High schools vary a great deal in the extent to
Think back to your own high school experience. which the prevailing peer culture emphasizes academic
What distinguished the good classes from the tedious success as a route toward status and popularity. In
ones? Newmann (1992) suggests a number of specific schools in which academic success is not valued by the
factors that contribute to student engagement. First, student body, students are less likely to achieve grades
teachers need to provide opportunities for students to that are consonant with their tested ability. In other
genuinely display their competencies. Second, schools words, a bright student who attends a school in which
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 209 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 209

jeopardize their school performance (Mahoney et al.,


2009; Staff, Messersmith, & Schulenberg, 2009).

SCHOOL VIOLENCE
A sad fact of life in contemporary America is that many
students attend schools in which serious disruption—
even violence—is an all-too-prevalent feature of the
school climate (Noguera, 1995; Vaughan et al., 1996).
According to a national survey of secondary school stu-
dents in American public schools, 1 out of every 4 stu-
dents has been a victim of violence in or around school,
and 1 out of 6 is worried about being physically at-
tacked or hurt while in school. These problems are es-
pecially common in middle schools (Nolle, Guerino, &
One reason so many teenagers complain of boredom in school is
that few school hours are spent in activities that engage them
Dinkes, 2007). One study found that nearly half of all
intellectually or encourage critical thinking. middle schoolers had been threatened at school (Flan-
nery, Weseter, & Singer, 2004). In another study, of 6th-
graders attending a multiethnic school in Los Angeles,
half the students surveyed reported having been ver-
getting good grades is frowned upon by other students bally harassed during the previous 2 weeks, and about
will actually get lower grades than he or she would in a one-fifth said they had been physically victimized
school in which scholastic success is generally admired. (Nishina & Juvonen, 2005). Generally speaking, victim-
Even within the same school, cliques and crowds differ ization is less common in more ethnically diverse
enormously in the extent to which they encourage or schools, but within such schools, students who are in
discourage academic success (Steinberg, 1996). Some the smallest minority are the most likely to be victim-
peer groups (for instance, the “brains”) may place a ized (Felix & You, 2011). Violence is more common in
great deal of pressure on their members to succeed in overcrowded schools located in poor urban neighbor-
school and may engage in behaviors (such as studying hoods (Khoury-Kassabri, Benbenishty, Astor, & Zeira,
together) that promote academic success. Other 2004).
groups, in contrast, may actively discourage scholastic Interviews with students who live in communities
efforts and success. where violence is common illuminate the ways in which
Other researchers have focused on adolescents’ ex- these youngsters manage their day-to-day activities to
periences outside of school—at home, at work, and in avoid exposing themselves to harm (Irwin, 2004). Some
extracurricular activities—and on the impact of those make sure that they steer clear of students who have
experiences on their school achievement and engage- reputations for violent behavior and go out of their way
ment (Dotterer, McHale, & Crouter, 2007; Mahoney, to act friendly if they can’t avoid them. Others learn
Vandell, Simpkins, & Zarrett, 2009). This research dem- which parts of town to avoid. Still others befriend peers
onstrates that the impact of school on adolescent who can serve as protectors, as this 16-year-old Latina
achievement cannot be understood in isolation. Studies did after someone at her school threatened to kill her:
show, for example, that students whose parents are in-
volved in school activities (such as parent–teacher con- I got so scared. I didn’t know what to do. I ran in the
ferences and “back-to-school” nights), who encourage house and called my friend Daryl and I was really crying
and [said] “I don’t know what to do.” And Daryl’s all,
and emphasize academic success, and who use authori-
“What’s his number? What’s his number?” And I gave it
tative parenting practices do better in secondary school to him. Since that day, that same guy will leave me alone
than do their peers (A. Gregory & Weinstein, 2004; N. because Daryl went up to him and told him he better
E. Hill & Tyson, 2009; Simons-Morton & Chen, 2009; leave me alone or else something is going to happen to
Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992). In him and his family. (Irwin, 2004, pp. 467–468)
Chapter 7, we’ll look more closely at the effects of em-
ployment and extracurricular participation on school One study of violence in a multiethnic, urban high
achievement. As you will read, these two types of ac- school found that Asian students were often the victims
tivities may have different consequences for school of violence and verbal harassment at the hands of their
achievement. Generally speaking, involvement in Black and Latino classmates, in part because of percep-
school-based extracurricular activities strengthens stu- tions that teachers favored Asians and discriminated
dents’ attachment to school. In contrast, students who against their non-Asian classmates (Rosenbloom &
overextend themselves on the job may inadvertently Way, 2004). According to the researchers:
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 210 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

210 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Students reported random “slappings” by male and female attention to the problem of lethal school violence
peers as they walked through the hallways. Slappings are (M. Moore, Petrie, Braga, & McLaughlin, 2003). As with
quick, pop shots, often to the head or body as students many topics that generate a great deal of attention in
passed one another in the hall or anywhere else. Asian stu- the media, much of what was asserted about school
dents described them as unnerving, randomly occurring,
shootings has turned out not to be the case. Although
and humiliating violations that are particularly harrowing
violence in schools is indeed a significant problem, le-
for the boys when girls slap them. . . . Along with the slap-
pings, Asian American students were observed and re- thal school shootings are extremely rare events, espe-
ported being pushed, punched, teased, and mocked by cially when you consider the number of schools and
their non-Asian American peers. The racial slur “chino” or students in the United States (there are about 50 million
“geek” was often heard as Asian American students passed schoolchildren in the United States, and fewer than 20
by. (Rosenbloom & Way, 2004, p. 433) students are killed in American schools each year)
(M. Anderson et al., 2001).
Experts disagree about how best to respond to vio-
Actually, far more children and adolescents are killed
lence in schools. Some educators have suggested that
at home or in the community than in or around school;
schools should refer aggressive students to law enforce-
indeed, schools are among the safest places for adoles-
ment, and many schools have police officers on duty to
cents to be (Mulvey & Cauffman, 2001). In fact, an
deter assaults and arrest students who cause trouble.
American adolescent is 4 times more likely to be struck
But some writers contend that the new, get-tough ap-
by lightning than to be shot in school (Steinberg, 2001)!
proach to violence prevention in schools—referred to
In addition, although the school shootings that gar-
as zero tolerance—has not helped and that school vio-
nered public attention generally involved White youth,
lence is more effectively reduced through programs that
a disproportionate number of homicides in schools
attempt to create a more humane climate (American
involve non-White youth, both as perpetrators and
Psychological Association [APA] Zero Tolerance Task
victims (M. Anderson et al., 2001). Perhaps most im-
Force, 2008). One unintended consequence of zero-
portantly, it is virtually impossible to predict which
tolerance policies is that many students end up with
students will commit acts of lethal violence (Mulvey &
arrest records and contact with the justice system for
Cauffman, 2001). Boys, students with mental health
acts that in the past would have been treated as disci-
problems, and adolescents who have easy access to guns
plinary infractions by school officials (Casella, 2003).
are more likely than others to be involved in school
This has a disproportionate impact on Black students,
shootings (M. Moore et al., 2003), but identifying the
who are more likely than other students to report that
specific students with these characteristics who will
school rules are unfair and inconsistently enforced, and
commit lethal crimes in school is a different matter al-
to be suspended or expelled, even though they are no
together. Most experts believe that, in the absence of a
more likely to commit the sorts of acts that would war-
proven means of identifying in advance adolescents
rant these responses (APA Zero Tolerance Task Force,
who will commit acts of lethal violence in school, the
2008; Kupchik & Ellis, 2008). Among the many recom-
most effective policy involves limiting adolescents’ ac-
mendations offered by a task force of the American
cess to guns and identifying and treating young people
Psychological Association, after a careful review of the
with mental health problems (M. Moore et al., 2003). It
research evidence, are that schools define infractions
is also essential to create a school climate in which stu-
carefully and train all staff in how to respond appropri-
dents feel responsible for one another and are willing
ately to them, reserve suspension or expulsion for only
to take action if they hear a peer talking about “doing
the most serious disruptive behavior, require school po-
something dangerous” (Flanagan & Stout, 2010;
lice officers to have training in adolescent development,
Syvertsen, Flanagan, & Stout, 2009).
and implement preventive measures to improve school
climate and increase students’ attachment to school. Of
course, one of the ways to do this would be to reduce RECAP
the number of disruptive students, by intervening to
• Researchers agree that the climate of the school
change their behavior at an earlier age (LeBlanc,
and its classrooms are more important influences
Swisher, Vitaro, & Tremblay, 2008).
on students’ achievement, behavior, and psycholog-
ical development than its organization or structure.
❚ Lethal School Violence A series of widely pub-
• Effective teachers are like authoritative parents—
licized school shootings in the United States—such as
they are warm, firm, and fair, and they have high ex-
the school shootings at Columbine High School or
pectations for student performance.
Sandy Hook Elementary School—has drawn national
• Several writers have suggested that if we want
to understand the impact of the classroom
environment on student achievement, we need
zero tolerance A get-tough approach to adolescent misbehavior to better understand how to enhance student
that responds seriously or excessively to the first infraction.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 211 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 211

engagement—the extent to which students are ❚ The Growth of College Enrollment Although
psychologically committed to learning and master- postsecondary educational institutions multiplied and
ing the material. Unfortunately, many studies of became more varied during the early 20th century, en-
American students indicate that they find school rollment in college was still a privilege enjoyed by very
boring and undemanding. few young people until the 1960s. In 1900, only 4% of
• Students perform better in school when their par- the 18- to 21-year-old population was enrolled in col-
ents are involved in their education, when their lege, and by 1930, the proportion had grown only to
peers value academic achievement, and when their 12%. Even as recently as 1950, fewer than 1 in 5 young
involvement in activities that compete with school people were enrolled in college (Church, 1976). During
is not excessively time-consuming. the first half of the 20th century, then, colleges and uni-
• Many students attend schools in which serious versities were not prominent in the lives of most Amer-
disruption—even violence—is an all-too-prevalent ican youth.
feature of the school climate. Zero-tolerance poli- How different things are today! Paralleling the rise
cies, which harshly punish students for any of secondary education between 1920 and 1940, post-
act of violence, have been shown secondary education grew dramatically between 1950
to have unintended and and 1970. By 1960, one-third of all young people were
often harmful entering college directly from high school. Today, more
effects. than two-thirds of high school graduates enroll in col-
lege immediately after graduation (National Center for
Education Statistics, 2012). The increase in enrollments
has been especially dramatic among women. In 1970,
close to 70% of undergraduates were male; by the end
of this decade, it is estimated that about 60% of all col-
Beyond High School lege students will be female (see Figure 6.7) (National
Center for Education Statistics, 2012). Although there
THE COLLEGE-BOUND
were large increases in the enrollment of minority
The early 20th century was an important time in the youth in higher education during the 1970s, the pro-
development not only of secondary schools but also of portion fell during the early 1980s, primarily because of
postsecondary educational institutions in the United reductions in the availability of financial aid (Baker &
States. Although colleges and, to a lesser extent, univer- Velez, 1996). Today, among high school graduates,
sities had existed for some time previously, not until the more than 90% of Asian American students, 70% of
latter part of the 19th century did diversity in institu- White high school students, and 60% of Black and
tions of higher education begin to develop. Early post- Hispanic students go directly into college (see Figure
secondary institutions were typically small, private, 6.8). Youth from immigrant families, despite the fact
liberal arts academies, often with a strong theological that their parents typically did not attend American
emphasis. But during a relatively brief period bridging colleges themselves, and despite often having to sup-
the 19th and 20th centuries, these colleges were joined port their family financially, are just as likely to enroll in
by a host of other types of institutions, including large and succeed in college as are American-born youth
private universities, technical colleges, professional (Fuligni & Witkow, 2004).
schools, publicly financed state universities, land grant
colleges, urban universities, and two-year community ❚ American Postsecondary Education If there
colleges (Brubacher & Rudy, 1976). are two dominant characteristics that distinguish the

80

70

60

50

40
Males FIGURE 6.7 College enrollments in the
30 United States continue to increase, espe-
Females cially among females. This figure shows
20
the percent of high school graduates
10 who enroll in college in the year after
graduating. (National Center for Education
0
1960 1967 1974 1981 1988 1995 2002 2009 Statistics, 2011b)
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 212 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

212 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

FIGURE 6.8 Rates of 95


college enrollment immedi-
Black
ately after high school
continue to rise. (Data were Hispanic
85
not separated out for Asian Asian
American students prior to White
2003.) (National Center for 75
Education Statistics, 2011b)

Percent
65

55

45

35
1975 1978 1981 1984 1987 1990 1993 1996 1999 2002 2005 2008 2011

development of postsecondary education in contempo- ❚ The Transition from High School to College In
rary America from that in other parts of the world, they some respects, the transition from high school to college
are diversity and accessibility (Brubacher & Rudy, 1976). parallels the transition from elementary to secondary
In countries other than the United States, postsecondary school. For many students, going to college means enter-
education is likely to be monopolized by monolithic ing an even larger, more formidable, and more imper-
public universities. Individuals are often separated into sonal environment. For some, the transition may
college- and non-college-bound tracks early in adoles- coincide with other life changes, such as leaving home,
cence, typically on the basis of standardized national ex- breaking off or beginning an important romantic rela-
aminations. In fact, rather than housing all high school tionship, or having to manage their own residence or
students in comprehensive high schools such as those finances for the first time. Many Latino students report
found in the United States, most other industrialized na- that family obligations and financial responsibilities, in
tions separate students during early or middle adoles- combination with commitments to school and work,
cence into schools for college-bound youngsters and can make the transition to college especially stressful,
schools designed to provide vocational and technical ed- particularly for second-generation students, who may
ucation. In the United States, the postsecondary educa- feel caught between the challenges of adapting to the
tion system is composed of a wide variety of public and university environment and the demands placed on
private two- and four-year institutions, some emphasiz- them by their parents (Nuñez, 2009).
ing a liberal arts education and others focusing more on As a consequence of all these factors, although many
technical, vocational, and preprofessional training. more American adolescents enroll in college today than
The goals of students attending college in the United in previous years, a very large number do not graduate.
States also vary greatly. The population of individuals Fewer than 60% of all students who enroll in a four-
enrolled in community college, for instance, which year college complete their degree within 6 years; at
tends to be older than that attending four-year institu- private, for-profit schools, this figure is less than 25%
tions, includes highly committed students who intend (National Center for Education Statistics, 2011b). Per-
to transfer to a four-year college or are working toward haps as a consequence of increasing accessibility, poor
a specific associate’s degree or certificate (together, matching, and a lack of “consumer” knowledge among
about half of all community college students). But it college applicants, rates of college attrition are ex-
also includes students who are less committed and not tremely high: Nearly one-third of students who enter a
sure why they are going to school (and whose atten- full-time, two-year college program drop out after just
dance is sporadic), as well as some who are just taking a 1 year, as do about one-fifth of students who enter a
course here or there out of interest in the subject mat- four-year college (J. Lee, Edwards, Menson, & Rawls,
ter. Similar variability in commitment and goals likely 2011). And while many of the students who leave after
characterizes the population of students enrolled in 1 year eventually finish their degree program, if not
four-year colleges and universities. necessarily at the same school they started in, one-third
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 213 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 213

of all students who enroll in college never finish. In expected—is that we have turned our backs on indi-
other words, although a great deal has been done to viduals who do not go directly to college, even though
make college entrance more likely, rates of college grad- they compose one-third of the adolescent population.
uation lag far behind rates of enrollment. As many writers have noted, our secondary schools
are geared almost exclusively toward college-bound
youngsters (Krei & Rosenbaum, 2001). In most con-
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION temporary American high schools, counseling is geared
A large proportion of stu- toward helping college-bound students continue their
dents who enter college education. Billions of dollars, in the form of financial
leave after 1 year. Based on aid and subsidized public college tuition, are given to
your own experience in these students. Some critics have suggested that we
college, what might be should spend just as much time and money helping
done to help more students the other third of the adolescent population make
stay enrolled? their transition into adulthood as smooth as possible
(Wald, 2005).
We noted earlier that opportunities for learning and
THE NON-COLLEGE-BOUND for critical thinking are much greater in college-prep
The problems associated with moving from high school classes than in the general or vocational tracks. In addi-
to college pale in comparison with the problems associ- tion, students who are not headed for college—some by
ated with not going to college at all. In general, college choice, others by unavoidable circumstance—find that
graduates earn substantially more income over their their high schools have not prepared them at all for the
lifetime than do individuals who attend college but do world of work. Even those who complete school and
not graduate, and these individuals, in turn, earn much earn a diploma—who have done what they were sup-
more than students who do not attend college at all posed to do as adolescents—may have a hard time find-
(Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2012) (see Figure 6.9). Indi- ing employment and a nearly impossible time finding a
viduals who drop out of high school before graduation satisfying, well-paying job. As a consequence, many in-
fare especially poorly economically and suffer a wide dividuals who do not go to college spend their early
range of problems, which we’ll examine in detail in adult years floundering between periods of part-time
Chapter 12. work, underemployment (for example, working at a job
One of the unfortunate by-products of our having that is less challenging than they would like), and un-
made postsecondary education so accessible—and so employment.

Education Pays
Unemployment rate in 2011 (in %) Median weekly earnings in 2011 (in $)

2.5 Doctoral degree 1,551

2.4 Professional degree 1,665

3.6 Master’s degree 1,263

4.9 Bachelor’s degree 1,053

6.8 Associate degree 768

8.7 Some college, no degree 719

9.4 High school diploma 638

14.1 Less than high school diploma 451

Average: 7.6% Average: $797


FIGURE 6.9 Individuals with more years of education earn more and are more likely to be employed. (Bureau of
Labor Statistics, 2012)
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 214 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

214 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

One important contributing factor to the continu- are not the same as those with low rates of dropping
ing difficulties faced by adolescents who do not go to out) (Rumberger & Palardy, 2005).
college has been the change in the world of work First and foremost, good schools emphasize intel-
mentioned earlier in this chapter: As manufacturing lectual activities (Ravitch, 2000). They create this atmo-
jobs began to be replaced by minimum-wage service sphere in different ways, depending on the nature and
jobs, the chances of making a decent living without size of the student body, but in these good schools, a
at least two years of college experience worsened common purpose—quality education—is valued and
appreciably. Today, young adults without college ex- shared by students, teachers, administrators, and par-
perience often must try to make ends meet on mini- ents (V. Lee, Smith, & Croninger, 1994). Learning is
mum-wage jobs—jobs that offer little in the way of more important to students than athletics or extracur-
promotion or advancement. The economic problems ricular activities, and seeing that students learn is more
faced by non-college-bound youth have been com- important to teachers and administrators than seeing
pounded by the escalating costs of such essentials as that they graduate. All students are expected to learn,
housing and health care. It’s not surprising that rates and all students are taught by teachers who use proven
of depression are significantly higher among young instructional methods.
adults who are not in school than among those who Second, good schools have teachers who are com-
are, and they are especially high among individuals mitted to their students and who are given a good deal
who are neither in school nor steadily employed of freedom and autonomy by administrators in the way
(Aseltine & Gore, 2005). that this commitment is expressed in the classroom
(V. Lee & Smith, 1996; V. Lee et al., 1997). In all schools,
of course, teachers must have curricular requirements
RECAP that they must fulfill. But in good schools, teachers are
• Today, more than two-thirds of high school gradu- given relatively more authority to decide how their les-
ates enroll in college immediately after graduation. sons are planned and how their classes are conducted.
The increase in enrollments in recent years has When teachers are given this sort of say in school gov-
been especially dramatic among women. ernance, they may find it easier to commit to the shared
• Although many more high school graduates are en- values of the institution.
rolling in college, only 60% of students who enter a Third, good schools are well integrated into the
four-year college program graduate within 6 years communities they serve (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Active
of enrolling. Many college students drop out after attempts are made to involve parents in their young-
their first year, probably because of a mismatch sters’ education, which is an important influence on
between their needs and the school environment. student achievement and a deterrent against students
• High schools as they are presently structured do dropping out (Rumberger & Palardy, 2005). Links are
not serve non-college-bound adolescents very well. forged between the high school and local colleges and
As a result, one-third of the country’s adolescents universities, so that advanced students may take more
complete their formal education without ade- challenging and more stimulating courses for high
quate preparation for school credit. Bridges are built between the high school
the world of work and local employers, so that students begin to see the
they hope to relevance of their high school education to their occu-
enter. pational futures.
Fourth, good schools are composed of good class-
rooms. In good classrooms, students are active partici-
pants in the process of education, not passive recipients
of lecture material. The atmosphere should be orderly
Schools and Adolescent but not oppressive. Innovative projects replace rote
memorization as a way of encouraging learning. Stu-
Development dents are challenged to think critically and to debate
important issues, rather than being asked simply to re-
CHARACTERISTICS OF GOOD SCHOOLS
gurgitate yesterday’s lessons (Eccles & Roeser, 2011).
Despite all the debate about how secondary schools Finally, good schools for adolescents are staffed by
ought to be organized and reformed, there is a fair de- teachers who are well-qualified and who have received
gree of consensus among experts about the characteris- specific training in teaching adolescents. Studies con-
tics of good schools for adolescents (Eccles & Roeser, ducted in many different countries find that students
2011), at least as far as student achievement is con- who attend schools with a high proportion of teachers
cerned (as you will read in Chapter 12, the characteris- who are certified, who majored in the subject they are
tics of schools with high rates of student achievement teaching, and who are experienced achieve more and
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 215 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 6 Schools 215

are more likely to graduate than their peers in schools socioeconomic status (SES), rates of academic progress
with less qualified teachers. Unfortunately, schools that during the school year and during the summer were
serve the most needy students—from poor families or comparable. Among disadvantaged students, however,
with limited language skills—are least likely to have the pattern was different. Although their rates of prog-
qualified teachers (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). ress during the school year were more or less equal to
those of higher-SES students, during the summer
months, disadvantaged students’ scores declined. In
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
other words, if it were not for the effects of school on
cognitive development, the discrepancy between afflu-
Based on the criteria of good schools
ent and poor youngsters’ achievement scores would be
discussed in this chapter, how would you
much greater than it currently is. One benefit of sum-
rate the high school that you attended?
mer school for disadvantaged students is that it dimin-
ishes the decline in achievement that would otherwise
occur between the spring and fall semesters (H. Cooper
et al., 2000).
Far less is known about the impact of schools on the
THE EFFECTS OF SCHOOL ON
psychosocial development of adolescents. Commenta-
ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT
tors have long noted that most schools are not
Whatever the shortcomings of schools, staying in structured to promote psychosocial development, given
school is preferable to dropping out, not only in terms their excessive focus on conformity and obedience and
of future earnings but in terms of cognitive develop- their lack of encouragement for creativity, indepen-
ment as well; a recent analysis of a change in the law in dence, and self-reliance (Friedenberg, 1967). This cer-
Norway some years ago found that when the country tainly comes through loud and clear when adolescents
increased the number of years of schooling it required, are asked about their classroom experiences. But there
the average IQ of the young adult population increased are many good schools in which students not only learn
significantly (Brinch & Galloway, 2012). Another study the academic material taught in classes but also learn
contrasted the performance of dropouts and graduates about themselves, their relationships with others, and
on a battery of standardized tests of achievement their society. Attending a school that has a positive cli-
administered during late adolescence (Alexander, mate can even help protect against some of the adverse
Natriello, & Pallas, 1985). The study took into account effects of exposure to the sort of family environment or
differences in achievement levels that existed before peer group that increases the risk of alcohol and drug
the dropouts had left school (2 years before the assess- use (Mayberry, Espelage, & Koenig, 2009).
ment was conducted), because dropouts are more It is also important to recognize that despite adults’
likely than graduates to show achievement problems intentions and objectives, students do not view school
early in their education. Compared with the dropouts, solely in terms of its academic agenda. Adults may eval-
adolescents who stayed in school gained far more in- uate schools in terms of their contribution to adoles-
tellectually over the 2-year interval in a variety of con- cents’ cognitive and career development, but for the
tent areas. More importantly, the results showed that typical adolescent, school is a primary setting for so-
the adverse effects of dropping out were most intense cializing. A recent study of British 11- and 12-year-olds
among socioeconomically disadvantaged youth. Para- found that students’ happiness in school was most in-
doxically, then, those students who are most likely to fluenced by their relationships with their peers (M. Z.
leave school prior to graduation may be the most Booth & Sheehan, 2008). When we ask about the conse-
harmed by doing so. quences of leaving school early, then, we must take into
One other way of assessing the contribution of account the impact this may have on the individual’s
schools to adolescents’ intellectual development is by social, as well as cognitive, development.
comparing early adolescents’ intellectual gains during Studies also show that students’ experiences within a
the school year with their gains during the summer. school can vary widely according to the track they are
Several studies have done just this (e.g., H. Cooper, on, the peer group they belong to, and the extracurricu-
Charlton, Valentine, & Muhlenbruck, 2000). Using in- lar activities they participate in. It seems safe to say that
formation about the academic progress of students academically talented and economically advantaged
measured at three points in time—the beginning of the students have a more positive experience in school than
school year, the end of the school year, and the begin- their less capable or less affluent counterparts do—
ning of the next year—researchers were able to see positive not only with respect to what they learn in class
how the academic progress of students during the but also with respect to the impact of school on their
summer compared with their academic progress dur- feelings about themselves as individuals. They receive
ing the school session. Among students with higher more attention from their teachers, are more likely to
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 216 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

216 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

hold positions of leadership in extracurricular organiza- • Evidence suggests that attending high school does,
tions, and are more likely to experience classes that are in fact, contribute to adolescents’ intellectual de-
engaging and challenging. In other words, the structure velopment. However, critics of contemporary sec-
of a school—its size, its tracking policy, its curricula— ondary schools have noted that most schools, with
provides different intellectual and psychosocial oppor- their focus on conformity and obedience and their
tunities for students who occupy different places within lack of encouragement for creativity, indepen-
that structure. The best answer to the question “How dence, and self-reliance, are not structured to
do schools affect adolescent development?” then, is an- promote adolescents’ psychosocial development.
other question: “Which schools, which adolescents, and • The ways in which adolescents are affected by
in what ways?” school depend a great deal on the characteristics
of the schools they attend. College-bound
students in the more advanced tracks have
RECAP markedly different experiences
• Good schools for adolescents have five characteris- than do non-college-bound
tics: They emphasize intellectual activities, they students in the lower
have committed teachers who are given autonomy, tracks.
they are well integrated into their community, they
have a high proportion of classrooms in which stu-
dents are active participants in their education, and
they have teachers who have received specialized
training in teaching adolescents.
ste32118_ch06_186-217.indd Page 217 21/03/13 10:11 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 218 21/03/13 11:10 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 219 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
7
Work, Leisure, and Media
ADOLESCENTS’ FREE TIME IN CONTEMPORARY SOCIETY
Patterns of Time Use in Contemporary America
Patterns of Time Use in Other Countries
ADOLESCENTS AND WORK
School and Work in the Early 20th Century
The Rise and Fall of the Student Worker
Teenage Employment in Other Nations
The Adolescent Workplace Today
Employment and Adolescent Development
Youth Unemployment
ADOLESCENTS AND LEISURE
Adolescents’ Free Time and Their Moods
Structured Leisure Activities
Unstructured Leisure Time
Promoting Positive Youth Development
ADOLESCENTS, MEDIA, AND THE INTERNET
Patterns of Media Use
Theories of Media Influence and Use
The Impact of Adolescents’ Exposure to Controversial Media Content
Electronic Media and Adolescent Development
Mass Media and Adolescent Girls’ Body Image
The Adolescent Consumer
FREE TIME AND ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT

219
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 220 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

220 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

AS YOU KNOW, one of the hallmarks of teenage life in teenagers spend more time in leisure activities than
industrialized society is that adolescents have consid- they do in the “productive” activities of school, more
erable amounts of time and money to devote to ac- time alone than with members of their family, about 4
tivities of their choosing. The scientific study of the times the number of hours each week on a part-time
ways in which work, leisure, and media influence ado- job as on homework, and considerably more time
lescent development is not as well established as “wired” to music, the Internet, or television than “tuned
the study of other contexts (J. Mahoney, Vandell, into” the classroom (D. Roberts et al., 2009; Staff et al.,
Simpkins, & Zarrett, 2009; D. Roberts, Henriksen, & 2009). In this chapter, we look at these other impor-
Foehr, 2009; Staff, Messersmith, & Schulenberg, 2009). tant contexts of adolescence—the contexts of work,
But you may be surprised to know that today’s leisure, and the media, including the Internet.

Adolescents’ Free Time occupied by work. At the turn of the 20th century,
adults were so worried about the free time available to
in Contemporary Society adolescents that they began to organize various youth
clubs and activities—such as the Boy Scouts and orga-
The abundance of free time in the lives of contemporary
nized sports—in order to occupy their “idle hands”
adolescents has several origins. Ironically, one of the
(Hine, 1999). Organized leisure became an institution-
most important contributors was the development of
alized part of adolescence as a supplement to school
compulsory schooling. Prior to this, adolescents were ex-
and a replacement for full-time employment.
pected to work full-time, and most maintained schedules
A second influence on the rise of free time for adoles-
comparable to those of adults, working long hours each
cents in contemporary society was the increased afflu-
week. With the spread of secondary schools during the
ence of Americans following World War II. As noted in
early decades of the 20th century, however, adolescents
Chapter 3, the invention of the “teenager”—and, more
were in effect barred from the labor force; the part-time
importantly, the discovery of the teenager by those in
jobs held by teenagers that are familiar to us today—
advertising and marketing—changed the nature of ado-
working behind fast-food counters, for instance—simply
lescence. As adolescents gained more autonomy, they
did not exist in large numbers, making after-school
became consumers with plenty of discretionary income
employment opportunities rare.
to spend on themselves (Osgerby, 2008). This week, no-
One indirect effect of compulsory high school, then,
tice the commercials and advertisements aimed at teen-
was to increase the amount of free time available to
agers on television, in magazines, or on the Internet.
young people—time that previously would have been
You’ll see that much of the advertising targeted toward
young people concerns leisure expenditures: music,
movies, restaurants, electronic gadgets, cosmetics, ath-
letic equipment, and so on.

MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION


Free time has not always played such an
important role in the lives of teenagers.
How do you think it has changed the
nature of adolescence?

PATTERNS OF TIME USE IN


CONTEMPORARY AMERICA
How do adolescents spend their free time? Figure 7.1
One important feature of adolescence in contemporary society is presents the results of two American time-use surveys of
the tremendous amount of time teenagers have for leisure activities. 15- to 17-year-olds, one conducted in the late 1970s, and
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 221 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 221

100%
90%
Leisure
80%
70% Organized activities
60% School work
50%
40% Paid work

30% Housework
20%
Personal care
10%
0%
Girls, 1970s Girls, 2000s Boys, 1970s Boys, 2000s
FIGURE 7.1 In recent decades, there has been a decline in the amount of time adolescents spend in paid jobs and an increase
in the amount of time they devote to leisure activities. (Zick, 2010)

the other conducted about 10 years ago (see Figure 7.1) ties thought to be mentally challenging, such as reading
(Zick, 2010). Among both girls and boys, there was a or playing a musical instrument or physically beneficial
significant decline in the proportion of time devoted (also about 30 minutes a day), such as sports or exer-
to paid jobs and a significant increase in time devoted to cise (Zick, 2010).
leisure; time devoted to housework also dropped among Group averages can be deceptive, however, because
boys, but not among girls. there are wide variations even among teenagers in how
Figure 7.2 further breaks down adolescents’ leisure they spend their free time. In one study, which tracked
time for the sample studied more recently. In that sur- adolescents’ time use over their high school years, the
vey, the average teenage girl reported spending more researchers identified several distinct groups of stu-
than 6 hours a day in leisure activity, and the average dents (M. J. Shanahan & Flaherty, 2001). One especially
boy reported spending more than 7 hours daily. But as busy group—about one-third of the sample in all
you can see, both girls and boys spend a huge amount grades—spent considerable time in a wide range of ac-
of free time in passive activities, like watching TV, talk- tivities, including extracurricular activities, paid work,
ing on the phone, and relaxing. In fact, more than one- schoolwork, time with friends, and household chores. A
third of all girls and nearly one-fourth of all boys second group, about one-fourth of the sample, was
reported spending all of their leisure time in passive similarly busy but did not hold a paying job. A third
activities—on average, more than 4 hours per day. In group, whose numbers increased from about 12% in
contrast, only about 30 minutes a day is spent in activi- the 9th grade to 20% in the 12th grade, devoted sub-
stantial time to a paying job but spent little time on
other activities. A fourth group spent no time in work
100% or extracurricular activities, but a substantial amount
of time hanging around with friends.
90% Passive (e.g., TV,
Although the study found that adolescents’ time use
80% socializing)
patterns changed a bit with age (as adolescents got older,
70% Computer (e.g., they were more likely to spend at least some of their free
60% Internet surfing, time working at a paid job), individuals who were busy
video games) 9th-graders were likely to be busy throughout high
50%
school. Overall, the results suggest that adolescents’ free
40% Physical (e.g.,
time is not best thought of as a “zero sum” phenome-
30% sports, exercising)
non, whereby involvement in one activity displaces in-
20% Intellectual (e.g., volvement in another. Rather, there appear to be
10% reading, hobbies) well-rounded adolescents who have substantial time
0%
commitments across many different activities, adoles-
Girls Boys cents who tend to focus on one type of activity (usually
FIGURE 7.2 Adolescents’ leisure time is dominated by sports), and adolescents who are nonparticipants (see
passive activities, such as watching TV or chatting with friends. Figure 7.3) (A. F. Feldman & Matjasko, 2007). Generally,
Far less time is spent in activities thought to be beneficial to most studies find that relatively busier adolescents are
intellectual or physical development. (Zick, 2010) better adjusted and more achievement oriented than
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 222 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

222 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

60
Boys
friends, caring for their physical appearance,
and working at after-school jobs (Alasker &
Girls
50 Flammer, 1999; Flammer, Alasker, & Noack,
1999; Larson & Seepersad, 2003; Larson &
40 Verma, 1999).
What is the effect of all this free time on
Score

30 adolescents’ behavior and development? Are


they learning about the real world from their
20 part-time jobs? Are extracurricular activities
beneficial? Are teenagers driven toward sex
10 and violence by the mass media? Are they re-
ally affected by the music they listen to, the
0 images on YouTube, or the brutality of video
Sports only Non-sports Multiple No games? Has the increase in time that they
only activities participation spend online adversely affected their ability to
Activity relate to others in person? Are concerns about
FIGURE 7.3 Studies of extracurricular participation generally find that adolescents’ exposure to pornography and
there are large numbers of students who participate in multiple activities, sexual predators on the Internet legitimate, or
large numbers who participate only in sports, and large numbers who are are they overstated? These are some of the
nonparticipants. (Feldman & Matjasko, 2007) questions we will address in this chapter. We
begin by examining how teenagers are affected
by after-school employment.
their classmates, but whether their better adjustment is
caused by or results from their busy schedules is not
clear (Bartko & Eccles, 2003; Nelson & Gastic, 2009). In RECAP
one study of low-income minority youth, some forms of • The rise of compulsory schooling and the increasing
engagement—athletics, for example—were associated affluence of American society in the second half of
with positive outcomes when combined with a second the 20th century contributed to the increased im-
type of engagement (for example, academics), but with portance of free time in the lives of contemporary
negative outcomes if it was the only activity an adoles- adolescents.
cent was involved in (Pedersen et al., 2005). Other stud- • Today’s American adolescents spend about half
ies of more representative populations also find that a their time in leisure activities—far more than is
“mixed” extracurricular portfolio may be better for ado- the case in most other industrialized countries
lescents’ development than one that only includes sports (where more time is devoted to school) and in
(Kort-Butler & Hagewen, 2011). developing nations (where more time is devoted
to work).
• Adolescents’ leisure time is dominated by passive
PATTERNS OF TIME USE activities that demand very little, either intellectu-
IN OTHER COUNTRIES ally or physically. About two-thirds of American
Patterns of time use differ considerably in different teenagers’ leisure time is spent watching TV or
parts of the world. Generally, American adolescents socializing with friends.
spend far more time on leisure, and far less time in • There is considerable variability in how individual
productive activities, than their counterparts in other adolescents spend their free time; some are
countries. American students’ use of their free time for very busy with structured extracurricular
school-related activities is especially low, and, as you activities, while others’ time is
would expect, it is particularly low among lower- far more unstructured.
achieving students (Witkow, 2009). For instance, the
average American high school student spends fewer
than 5 hours per week on homework; in Asian coun-
tries such as India, Taiwan, and Japan, the average is
between 4 and 5 hours per day (Larson & Verma,
1999). And European and Asian adolescents spend
almost 3 times as many hours each week reading for
Adolescents and Work
pleasure as do American adolescents (Larson & Verma, The majority of American high school students will
1999). In contrast, American teenagers spend rela- have worked in an after-school job before graduating
tively more time playing sports, socializing with (Staff et al., 2009). Although working while attending
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 223 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 223

high school is currently common in the United States,


part-time employment did not become widespread
among American adolescents until the 1980s, when
about half of all high school students—and nearly
three-quarters of all high school seniors—held jobs
during the school year. School-year employment be-
came less popular at the beginning of the 21st century,
and today, the proportion of high school students who
work during the school year is at its lowest level in
recent history (Morisi, 2008; Stone, 2011) (see Figure
7.4). As you will read, the pattern of rise and fall in stu-
dent employment tells an interesting story about the
nature of adolescence in modern society.

SCHOOL AND WORK IN THE


EARLY 20TH CENTURY
Prior to 1925, teenagers from all but the most affluent Many adolescents hold part-time jobs during the school year.
Research has questioned whether working is good or bad for
families left school between the ages of 12 and 15 and
adolescent development.
became full-time workers (Horan and Hargis, 1991;
Modell, Furstenberg, & Hershberg, 1976). Depending
on their social class, adolescents were either students or
workers, but not both.
As secondary education became more widespread (Kett, 1977). As a result of these social and legislative
among different socioeconomic segments of American changes, the employment of American teenagers de-
society, more youngsters remained in school well into clined steadily during the first 4 decades of the 20th
middle and late adolescence, and fewer elected to century. It’s hard to imagine, given the presence of
work. Compulsory education laws were passed in teenagers behind cash registers and fast-food counters
most states that required individuals to remain in today, but in 1940, only about 3% of high school stu-
school until at least turning 16, part-time jobs were dents worked during the school year (U.S. Depart-
not plentiful, and a variety of child labor laws ment of Commerce, 1940).
were enacted to restrict adolescents’ employment

THE RISE AND FALL OF THE


80 STUDENT WORKER
The situation began to change during the second half of
70 the 20th century, with the growth of the retail and ser-
vice sectors of the economy. Employers needed workers
who were willing to work part-time for relatively low
60 wages and for short work shifts. Many businesses
looked to teenagers to fill these jobs—and, in the mid-
50 1970s, as you read in Chapter 5, teenagers were plenti-
ful. The proportion of American high school students
holding part-time jobs rose dramatically during the
40 1970s, and working during the school year became a
way of life.
30 This trend began to reverse itself about 15 years
ago, for several reasons (Morisi, 2008). As we noted in
Chapter 6, during the past two decades, educational re-
20
1985 1988 1991 1994 1997 2000 2003 2006 2009 formers began calling for tougher standards in high
FIGURE 7.4 A smaller proportion of adolescents have schools. High schools began requiring more from their
after-school jobs today than any time in recent history. A bad students, and many implemented graduation require-
economy, more demands from schools, and an expanding ments. As more and more students began to seek col-
universe of leisure activities have all been factors. (Bureau of lege admission, a higher proportion of students began
Labor Statistics, 2012) taking Advanced Placement (AP) courses, which had
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 224 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

224 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

homework requirements that placed demands on stu- standards. In many parts of the developing world, work
dents out of school. obligations take precedence over schooling. In these
Second, just as adolescent workers became increas- countries, most adolescents enter into full-time em-
ingly in demand as the service economy expanded dur- ployment by age 15 or 16, in jobs similar to the ones
ing the last half of the 20th century, the retraction of the they will hold as adults. Very often, adolescents work
economy during the first decade of the 21st century in- for their families. The pattern in these countries closely
creased competition for the same jobs that teenagers resembles that found in America 100 years ago: school
could get just for the asking a couple of decades before. for adolescents of the extremely affluent, and work for
As the recession worsened and many adults lost their the rest (Larson & Verma, 1999). The trade-off between
jobs, restaurant and retail employers began hiring unem- school and work is changing in the developing world,
ployed adults instead of high school students. Immigra- however. As more and more adolescents from develop-
tion also brought to the United States many adults who ing nations have stayed in school, the number of ado-
were willing to take the part-time, minimum-wage jobs lescents in these countries’ labor force has declined at
that were the mainstays of the student employment. If about the same rate. In China, for example, where edu-
you had walked into a fast-food restaurant in the 1980s, cational opportunities have expanded rapidly in recent
you would have been struck by the high proportion of decades, especially outside urban centers, about half of
employees who were teenagers. Today, many fast-food all 16-year-olds were employed in the late 1980s, but
restaurants still have a lot of student employees, but the fewer than one-quarter were employed just 10 years
teenagers work side by side with people two, three, and later (National Research Council, 2005).
even four times their age. Student employment in other comparably industri-
Finally, the growth of new technologies during the alized countries, where social and economic conditions
first part of the 21st century expanded leisure opportu- are more similar to those in the United States, vary con-
nities for many teenagers, many of who simply preferred siderably from country to country (Stone, 2011). School
to spend their after-school hours on the computer or in year employment is common in Canada and Australia,
front of the television than behind a cash register at the where about half of all students hold jobs, but it is
mall. Over the past three decades there has been a sig- nearly unheard of in Japan or Korea, where schoolwork
nificant increase in the proportion of adolescents who, is more demanding of teenagers’ afternoons, evenings,
when asked what they want out of a job, report that hav- and weekends. The proportion of employed students in
ing time for leisure is important, and a decline in the Western Europe generally falls somewhere between
proportion who say they would work even if they had these two extremes, but it varies considerably from
enough money (Wray-Lake, Syvertsen, Briddell, Osgood, country to country—school-year employment is rare in
& Flanagan, 2011). As we noted earlier in this chapter, France, Italy, and Spain, but common in Great Britain,
the amount of time adolescents devoted to paid employ- the Netherlands, and Sweden. Generally speaking, the
ment shrank as the amount of time they spent on leisure scheduling of part-time jobs in most European coun-
increased. Studies of student workers conducted during tries is not well suited to the daily routines of students.
the 1980s and 1990s indicated that the vast majority of In Europe, for example, the school day lasts well into the
student workers were middle-class youth who were late afternoon. In the United States, many adolescents
working because they wanted to, not because they had leave school early in the afternoon and go straight to
to—to earn money for extras, rather than for necessities. their part-time jobs, where they work until 9:00 or
During the recession, teenagers’ hourly wages did not 10:00 P.M. In addition, in most other industrialized
keep pace with increases in the price of the things they countries, the employment of children is still associated
were interested in purchasing (Zick, 2010). As more and with being poor, and there is a strong stigma attached to
more attractive leisure options became available, and as having one’s children work. Many middle-class parents
the economic benefits of working declined, the goods do not feel that it is appropriate for their children to
that teenagers so desperately “needed” became items have jobs while attending school.
they could more easily do without.
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
TEENAGE EMPLOYMENT Student employment during the school year more
IN OTHER NATIONS common in the United States, Canada, and Australia
than elsewhere. Why might this be?
The extent and nature of teenage employment vary
How might this make the nature of
considerably from country to country (Stone, 2011;
adolescence different in these
Verma & Larson, 2003). In developing nations where
countries than in other parts of the
industrialization is still in a relatively early stage and a
world?
large percentage of the population is poor, adolescents
generally leave school early—at least by American
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 225 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 225

THE ADOLESCENT WORKPLACE TODAY some researchers have argued that adolescents in
better jobs—jobs in which they can learn genuinely
❚ Common Adolescent Jobs The vast majority
useful skills, for example—benefit from employment
of teenagers are employed in the retail and service in-
(Mortimer, Pimentel, Ryu, Nash, & Lee, 1996). While
dustries. In general, older students are more likely to
this may be true in theory, the fact is that the propor-
hold formal jobs (for example, retail or restaurant
tion of adolescents who hold jobs in which there is
work) than are younger students, who are more likely
ample opportunity to learn new or higher-level skills is
to hold informal jobs (such as babysitting or yard work)
quite small.
(see Figure 7.5) (Staff et al., 2009). As expected, work-
ing teenagers in rural areas are more likely to be em-
ployed in agricultural occupations than are their urban
or suburban counterparts.
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
As you also can see from Figure 7.5, a small number
of jobs accounts for a very large proportion of today’s Think back to your own work experiences as
student workers. About 60% of employed 8th-graders an adolescent, during either the summer or
work in just one of two jobs: babysitting or yard work. the school year. What were the best jobs
Job opportunities are nearly as restricted for older teen- you had? Which ones were the worst? Why?
agers: Restaurant work (such as a counter worker in a
fast-food restaurant) and retail sales work (such as a
cashier in a clothing store) account for more than half
of all working students’ jobs. Very few teenagers are This characterization of adolescents’ jobs as dreary
employed on farms or in factories anymore (Staff et al., is not something that teenagers themselves report,
2009). however. The majority of adolescent workers describe
their jobs in favorable terms, saying that they learned
❚ The Adolescent Work Environment Most things, liked the people with whom they worked, had
teenagers’ jobs are fairly dreary. Few jobs permit adoles- opportunities to exercise responsibility, and were satis-
cents to behave independently or make decisions; they fied with their pay.
receive little instruction from their supervisors, and How can we explain this difference between adults’
they are rarely required to use the skills they have been conceptions of adolescents’ work environments and
taught in school (Greenberger & Steinberg, 1986). With teenagers’ own descriptions of their experiences? Per-
occasional exceptions, most teenagers’ jobs are repeti- haps compared to school, which is a context in which
tive, monotonous, and intellectually unchallenging. adolescents are frequently bored and seldom chal-
Some are even highly stressful, requiring that young- lenged, even the jobs that adults consider menial are
sters work under intense time pressure without much comparatively satisfying to teenagers. And perhaps
letup and exposing them to potential injury and there is something about earning money and having
accidents (National Research Council, 1998). Not all some responsibility, regardless of how modest, that
jobs are this tedious or dangerous, of course, and makes adolescents feel better about themselves and

8th-Grade Workers 10th-Grade Workers 12th-Grade Workers


19% 21% 13% 3%
21% 13%
22% 7%
1% 7%
5% 3%
1%
1% 5% 6% 5%
2% 8%
2% 3% 7%
2% 1% 1%
5% 7% 5%
6% 7%
41% 27% 23%

Babysitting or child care Odd jobs Waiter or waitress


Farm Other Other restaurant
Store clerk or salesperson Lawn or yard work Newspaper route
Office or clerical Fast food
FIGURE 7.5 As adolescents age, they become less likely to work in informal jobs, like babysitting, and more likely to enter
the formal labor force, mainly in retail stores and restaurants. (Staff et al., 2009)
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 226 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

226 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

young people’s development and preparation for adult


work.
Studies of contemporary youth, for example, gener-
ally do not support the view that holding a job
makes adolescents become more personally responsible
(Monahan, Lee, & Steinberg, 2011; Mortimer & Johnson,
1998; Wright, Cullen, & Williams, 1997). Moreover,
some research has found high rates of misconduct on
the job among adolescent workers (for example, stealing
from employers, lying about the number of hours
worked), especially those whose work is not closely
supervised by adults (Staff et al., 2009).
One specific aspect of responsibility that working is
believed to affect is money management. Because a
high school senior who holds a minimum-wage job
for 20 hours a week—a common pattern—earns more
than $500 per month, holding a job may provide many
opportunities for learning how to budget, save, and
Few adolescents who work save a large percentage of their earnings
for their future education, and fewer still use their income to help
spend money wisely. Research indicates, however, that
their family with household expenses. Most working teenagers spend few teenagers exercise a great deal of responsibility
most of their earnings on clothing, cars, entertainment, and food. when it comes to managing their earnings. Most ado-
lescent workers spend most of their earnings on per-
sonal expenses, like clothes or cars. Fewer than 10% of
translates into a positive description of their work adolescents who work save most of their income for
experience. As you will see in a later section of this future education, and even fewer use their earnings to
chapter, when adolescents are placed in positions of help their families with household expenses (National
responsibility, they come to feel more responsible. Center for Education Statistics, 2005). Many working
teenagers may suffer from premature affluence—
which results from getting used to having a relatively
EMPLOYMENT AND ADOLESCENT luxurious standard of living before one has any serious
DEVELOPMENT financial responsibilities, like rent, food, or utility bills.
The impact of employment on the psychological devel- Perhaps as a consequence, adolescents who have earned
opment of adolescents has been the focus of numerous (and spent) a lot from their jobs while in high school
studies (Staff et al., 2009). Three broad questions have are less satisfied with their financial situations as young
been asked in this research: whether working helps ado- adults, because they had become accustomed to living
lescents develop a sense of responsibility and prepares in an unrealistic world, one in which they had a large
them for the transition to adulthood; whether working amount of discretionary income and few obligations
interferes with other activities, such as school; and (Bachman, 1983). Ironically, then, the very experience
whether working promotes the development of unde- that many adults believe builds character actually may
sirable behaviors, such as drug and alcohol use. As you teach adolescents undesirable lessons about the mean-
will read, the answers to these questions depend on ing of money.
many factors, including the nature of the job, the num- Findings from studies that ask adolescents directly
ber of hours worked each week, and the aspect of devel- whether working has helped them become more
opment studied. responsible or mature suggest that adolescents’ own
perceptions differ from what objective assessments in-
❚ The Development of Responsibility Most dicate, however (Mortimer, 2003). Many adults say that
people believe that working builds character, teaches their jobs as teenagers helped them learn things like
adolescents about the real world, and helps them pre- punctuality, ways to deal effectively with strangers, and
pare for adulthood, but these assumptions are not gen- even cope with work one didn’t necessarily want to do.
erally supported by research. Indeed, studies indicate Here’s how one adult put it, looking back on his job at a
that the benefits of working during adolescence have carwash:
probably been overstated and that intensive employ- It was a brainless job. . . . The hardest part was fighting
ment during the school year may even negatively affect off the boredom, and we would get creative about how
to do this, usually while getting high. But it was an im-
portant experience—I saw what it was like to work, got
premature affluence Having more income than one can manage more independent, and made some friends I still know.
maturely, especially during adolescence.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 227 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 227

The money helped me get my first car and first real girl- fewer than 20 hours per week does not appear to have
friend. (Staff et al., 2009, p. 270) these adverse effects (Staff et al., 2009). Indeed, some
Perhaps the most reasonable conclusion we can studies find that a large number of students are able to
draw about the impact of working on the development manage school and work effectively, as long as they
of maturity is that it depends largely on the nature of keep their work hours in check, and that many may
the job (Mortimer, 2003). In jobs in which adolescents benefit from learning how to manage their time effec-
are given genuine responsibility, make important deci- tively (M. J. Shanahan & Flaherty, 2001).
sions, and perform challenging tasks, they are more Although the impact of working on students’ actual
likely to come away feeling more mature, competent, grades and achievement test scores is small (Staff et al.,
and dependable. In jobs in which the work is repetitive, 2009), several studies indicate that extensive employ-
stressful, or unchallenging, however, they probably will ment during the school year may take its toll on school
gain very little from the experience. But given what we performance in ways that are not revealed by looking
know about employment opportunities for teenagers, it only at grade point averages. Students who work a great
seems reasonable to say that while it is possible for an deal, for example, report paying less attention in class
adolescent to benefit psychologically from working, it is and exerting less effort on their studies (Monahan et al.,
not probable, given the nature of most adolescents’ jobs. 2011). Additionally, when students work a great deal,
According to one recent analysis, the most common they often develop strategies for protecting their grades.
adolescent jobs—in fast-food restaurants or retail These strategies include taking easier courses, cutting
stores, for example—ranked highest in stress and in in- corners on homework assignments, copying homework
terference with other parts of life, and lowest in their from friends, and cheating (Steinberg & Dornbusch,
likelihood of providing skills or leading to a career 1991). Teachers express concern about the excessive in-
(Staff et al., 2009). volvement of students in after-school jobs (Bills, Helms,
& Ozcan, 1995), and some teachers may respond to an
❚ The Impact on Schooling A second question influx of students into the workplace by lowering class-
that has received a fair amount of research attention room expectations, assigning less homework, and using
concerns the impact of working on adolescents’ in- class time for students to complete assignments that
volvement in other activities, most notably, schooling. otherwise would be done outside of school (Bills et al.,
Here, studies indicate that the issue is not whether a 1995). As a consequence, when large numbers of stu-
teenager works, but how much (Stone, 2011). dents in a school are employed, even nonworkers’
Many experts now believe that working more than schooling may be affected.
20 hours a week may jeopardize adolescents’ school
performance and engagement (National Research ❚ The Promotion of Problem Behavior Some
Council, 1998). Youngsters who work long hours are studies have examined the belief that having a job
absent from school more often, are less likely to partici- deters youngsters from delinquent and criminal
pate in extracurricular activities, report enjoying school activity—that keeping teenagers busy with work will
less, spend less time on their homework, and earn keep them out of trouble. Contrary to popular belief,
slightly lower grades. These results occur both because however, employment during adolescence does not
teenagers who are less interested in school choose to deter delinquent activity (Monahan, Steinberg, &
work longer hours and because working long hours Cauffman, 2013). Indeed, several studies suggest that
leads to disengagement from school (Monahan et al., working long hours may actually be associated with
2011; Safron, Sy, & Schulenberg, 2003; Warren, 2002). increases in aggression, school misconduct, minor
There is no evidence that summer employment, even delinquency, and precocious sexual activity (Monahan
for long hours, affects school performance, however, et al., 2011; Rich & Kim, 2002). However, there is also
suggesting that the negative impact of working on evidence that the higher rate of problem behavior
school performance may be due to the time demands of among working adolescents is due to the fact that
having a job while going to school (Oettinger, 1999). delinquent youth are simply more likely than their
Intensive involvement in a part-time job may even peers to choose to work long hours (Apel et al., 2007;
increase the likelihood of dropping out of school Staff, Osgood, Schulenberg, Bachman, & Messersmith,
(Staff et al., 2009). Students who spend a lot of time on 2010).
the job have less ambitious plans for further education Many studies also have found that rates of smoking,
(H. Marsh & Kleitman, 2005), and they complete drinking, and drug use are higher among teenage
fewer years of college, perhaps because students with workers than nonworkers, especially among students
low aspirations for the future choose to work longer who work long hours (Bachman & Schulenberg, 1993;
hours than their peers (Bachman, Staff, O’Malley, Monahan et al., 2011; Mortimer & Johnson, 1998;
Schulenberg, & Freedman-Doan, 2011; Staff et al., Ramchand, Ialongo, & Chilcoat, 2007; L. Wu, Schlenger,
2009). It is important to stress, however, that working & Galvin, 2003). This is both because students who use
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 228 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

228 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

creased problem behavior and, in fact, may have special


benefits for inner-city adolescents from poor families,
with poor school records, or with prior involvement
in delinquency (Apel et al., 2007; Bauermeister,
Zimmerman, Barnett, & Caldwell, 2007; J. C. Lee &
Staff, 2007; Monahan et al., 2013; K. Newman, 1999;
Staff & Mortimer, 2007). However, working during ju-
nior high school or the early high school years (as op-
posed to later in high school) may increase the chances
that poor minority youth will drop out of school and
engage in problem behavior (Entwisle, Alexander, &
Olson, 2005; Olatunji, 2005; Roche, Ensminger, Chilcoat,
& Storr, 2003). Why might this be? One possibility is
that working in early adolescence may make school
seem less important, whereas working in later adoles-
cence, when making the transition to adult work roles is
more imminent, may make school seem more impor-
tant. One recent study of juvenile delinquents found
Participation in extracurricular activities, such as performing in a
school musical, is less common among students who work long that working during high school was only beneficial if
hours at a part-time job. the adolescent was also attending school regularly
(Monahan et al., 2013).
In sum, although teenagers generally enjoy working,
alcohol and other drugs are more likely to want to work there is little evidence that, with the exception of disad-
long hours and because increases in work hours tend to vantaged inner-city youth, doing so contributes in sig-
lead to increases in cigarette, drug, and alcohol use nificant ways to their psychosocial development.
(Bachman et al., 2011; Monahan et al., 2011; Paschall, Moreover, there is a fair amount of evidence that the
Flewelling, & Russell, 2004; Safron et al., 2003). psychological costs of working more than 20 hours a
A variety of explanations have been proposed for the week may outweigh the benefits. Studies of work and
connection between working and smoking, drinking, adolescent development point to a complicated pattern
and using drugs. The impact of extensive employment of cause and effect that unfolds over time. In general,
on adolescent drug and alcohol use probably reflects adolescents who are less attached and committed to
the fact that adolescents who work long hours have school, and who are more involved in problem behav-
more discretionary income and, hence, greater oppor- ior, are more likely to choose to work long hours. Work-
tunity to purchase cigarettes, alcohol, and other drugs. ing long hours, in turn, leads to more disengagement
In addition, drug and alcohol use are more common from school and increased problem behavior. In other
among adolescents who work under conditions of high words, intensive employment during the school year
job stress than among their peers who work for compa- most threatens the school performance and psycholog-
rable amounts of time and money but under less stress- ical well-being of those students who can least afford to
ful conditions—and many adolescents work in stressful suffer the consequences of overcommitment to a job.
work settings, like fast-food restaurants (Staff et al.,
2009). It may also be the case that when adolescents
work long hours, this disrupts their relationship with MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
their parents, which, in turn, is associated with problem How do you reconcile findings from
behavior (Longest & Shanahan, 2007; Roisman, 2002). research on the impact of adolescent
Whatever the reason, the impact of school-year em- employment, which do not indicate many
ployment on drug and alcohol use persists over time; positive effects on psychological develop-
individuals who worked long hours as teenagers drink ment, and the reports of teenagers, who
and use drugs more in their late 20s than their peers say they have benefited from their jobs?
who worked less or not at all (Mihalic & Elliott, 1997).

❚ Possible Benefits for Poor Youth One point of


debate among researchers who study adolescent em-
YOUTH UNEMPLOYMENT
ployment concerns the differential impact of working
on middle-class versus poor youth. Some researchers Although the employment of teenagers has become
have found that working, even in the sorts of minimum- commonplace in contemporary America, some young
wage jobs available to teenagers, may not lead to in- people who wish to work are unable to find jobs.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 229 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 229

In general, however, except for summer months, youth • Most employed U.S. adolescents work in retail
unemployment is not very widespread, once the pro- stores or restaurants. Although studies suggest that
portion of young people who are in school is taken into the work they do is highly routine, uninteresting,
account. For example, in 2012, 86% of all 16- to and occasionally stressful, adolescents’ own de-
19-year-olds were enrolled in school (either in high scriptions of their jobs are generally favorable.
school or college); of the remaining 14% who were not • The impact of work on adolescent development
in school, about 40% were employed, and about 20% depends on the nature of the job and the number
were looking for full- or part-time work (the other 40% of hours worked each week. Working generally has
were neither working nor looking for work) (Bureau of few effects on adolescents’ psychological develop-
Labor Statistics, 2012). Overall, then, only a small per- ment, but there is some evidence that working long
centage of 16- to 19-year-olds—about 3%—were out hours may increase rates of delinquency and drug
of school and looking for work. and alcohol use and take a toll on schooling. At the
Although the proportion of adolescents who are out same time, there is also evidence that adolescents
of school and out of work is small, their problems are who choose to work long hours may be more dis-
very large. It won’t shock you to learn that the majority engaged from school and inclined toward problem
of unemployed youth who are not in school are indi- behavior even before they enter the workplace.
viduals who have not gone on very far in school. You • Many social commentators have worried about the
may be surprised to find out, though, that the unem- high rate of youth unemployment, especially
ployment rate among recent high school graduates who among adolescents who have not pursued educa-
have not continued their education is not all that differ- tion beyond high school. Unemployment remains
ent from the rate among high school dropouts (in 2011, very high among high school dropouts
34% of recent high school graduates who were not in and youth who do not have
college were unemployed, compared with 38% of high at least some college
school dropouts) (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2012). In education.
contrast, just five years ago, the unemployment rate
among dropouts was twice as high as it was among re-
cent high school graduates who were not in college.
Unemployment has been a problem among high
school dropouts for decades, but the very high rate of
unemployment among high school graduates who do
Adolescents and Leisure
not go to college is a recent phenomenon. Unfortu- Adolescents in the United States and other Western
nately, many experts worry that this state of affairs may countries spend nearly half their waking hours in lei-
not reverse itself after the current recession lifts, be- sure activities, such as socializing with friends in per-
cause success in the labor force increasingly requires at son, by phone, or electronically; watching television
least some college education, if not a college degree. It and listening to music; browsing the Internet and play-
was once believed that we should focus our efforts on ing video games; playing sports, practicing a musical
finding ways to help high school dropouts and those instrument, or working on hobbies; and sometimes not
with just a high school diploma make the transition doing anything at all (J. Mahoney, Larson, Eccles, &
from high school into the world of work. In view of the Lord, 2005).
changed and changing labor market, with its demands One important difference between leisure and other
for well-educated workers, it may make more sense to activities is that adolescents choose their leisure activi-
reform schools so that all students graduate with ties, whereas their time at school and work is dictated
enough academic skill to enroll in college. by others (teachers, supervisors, etc.). Perhaps as a con-
sequence, and not surprisingly, studies show that ado-
lescents report being in a better mood during leisure
RECAP activities than during school or work. Leisure activities
• The student worker emerged in the United States that are both structured and voluntary—such as sports,
during the second half of the 20th century, as the hobbies, artistic activities, and clubs—seem to provide
need for cheap, part-time labor in the service and special psychological benefits (Larson, 2000).
retail industries grew. Although rates of adolescent
employment have declined over the past 15 years, a
ADOLESCENTS’ FREE TIME
large portion of American teenagers still hold part-
AND THEIR MOODS
time jobs during the school year. Having a job dur-
ing the school year is common in a few other Studying adolescents’ moods through the conventional
countries, but in most parts of the world, students methodologies of observational or questionnaire re-
do not combine school and work. search is difficult. Questions concerning adolescents’
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 230 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

230 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

FIGURE 7.6 Average affect 0.6


reported by girls and boys at
different ages when alone, with 0.4
friends, and with family. (Larson &
Richards, 1991) 0.2 Boys with friends

Average affect
Girls with friends
0.0 Boys with family
Girls with family
Boys alone
–0.2
Girls alone
–0.4

–0.6
5 6 7 8 9
Grade

emotional states, for instance, are especially tricky, be- In one early ESM study, before there were smart-
cause individuals’ emotions change during the day and phones, Larson had nearly 500 adolescents ages 9–15
may not be, at the time of a researcher’s assessment, carry pagers and booklets of self-report forms for one
reflective of their moods at other points in the day. Sup- week and asked them to fill out a form each time they
pose a researcher wanted to know how adolescents’ were signaled. The form contained a series of questions
moods were affected by various activities—such as at- about companionship (“Who were you with [or talking
tending school, watching television, or having dinner to on the phone]?”), location (“Where were you?”), ac-
with the family. Although it would be possible to inter- tivity (“What were you doing?”), and mood (the ado-
view respondents and ask them to recall their moods at lescents used a checklist to report their feelings). The
different points in the day, we cannot be sure whether adolescents were beeped seven times each day, once
their recollections would be entirely accurate. within every two-hour block between 7:30 A.M. and
9:30 P.M. By examining the adolescents’ many reports,
❚ The Experience Sampling Method One of the researchers were able to chart changes in activities,
the most interesting innovations in the study of adoles- companionship, and mood over the course of the week
cence was designed to overcome this and other sorts of and to relate variations in activity, companionship, and
methodological problems. Using a technique called the mood to one another. Thus, the method permitted the
experience sampling method (ESM), researchers can researchers to ask how activities, companionship, and
collect much more detailed information about adoles- mood varied as a function of age; how mood varied as
cents’ experiences over the course of the day and thereby a function of what the adolescent was doing; and how
illuminate many different aspects of the adolescent ex- both activity and mood varied as a function of whom
perience. The ESM has been used to chart adolescents’ the adolescent was with. One of the questions asked
moods, to monitor their social relationships, and to was how adolescents’ moods varied as a function of
catalog their activities in far greater detail than has been whom they were with (parents, peers, or no one) and
previously available. whether the connection between mood and compan-
The use of the ESM in the study of adolescent devel- ionship changed with age. The results are presented in
opment was pioneered by psychologist Reed Larson Figure 7.6.
and colleagues (Larson & Richards, 1991). In studies As you can see, adolescents’ moods are generally
using this method, adolescents carry electronic devices, most positive when they are with their friends and least
such as smartphones, and, when they are signaled, re- positive when they are alone; their moods when with
port on whom they are with, what they are doing, and their family fall somewhere in between. More interest-
how they are feeling. These reports can be filled out ing, perhaps, is that between grades 5 and 9, adoles-
on paper or entered directly into the smartphone or cents’ moods while with friends become more positive,
other device. whereas their moods while with their family follow a
curvilinear pattern (a curvilinear pattern can be either
U-shaped or can look like an inverted U). That is, their
experience sampling method (ESM) A method of collecting
moods while with their family become more negative
data about adolescents’ emotional states, in which individuals are
paged and asked to report on their mood and activity.
between elementary and middle school (between
grades 5 and 7) and then rise between middle school
curvilinear pattern In statistical analyses, a pattern of relations and high school (between grades 8 and 9). This dip par-
between two variables that resembles a U-shaped or an inverted allels findings from other research on family relations
U-shaped curve.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 231 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 231

(see Chapter 4) that point to early adolescence as a time


of heightened strain in the parent–child relationship.
Adolescents’ moods are strongly influenced by where
they are and whom they are with. When adolescents
are in school, they report moderate levels of concentra-
tion but very low levels of motivation or interest in
what they are doing. When they are with friends, teen-
agers report moderate levels of motivation and interest
but low levels of concentration. It is only when adoles-
cents are playing sports or involved in the arts, a hobby,
or an extracurricular organization that they report
high levels of both concentration and interest. This
combination produces what some psychologists have
referred to as a flow experience (Csikszentmihalyi,
1990). You’ve probably experienced this sensation at
one time or another, when you were deeply involved in
an activity that you found both enjoyable and
absorbing. For many adolescents around the world, school-sponsored
One ESM study of after-school programs found that extracurricular activities provide the context for much of their
the positive combination of high motivation, concen- leisure activity.
tration, and engagement was most commonly observed
when young adolescents were involved in sports or arts
enrichment (Shernoff & Vandell, 2007). While adoles- 2007; J. Mahoney et al., 2009; White & Gager, 2007). In
cents are in unstructured leisure activities, like watch- general, participation is somewhat more prevalent
ing TV, they tend to show the same pattern of moderate among adolescents from more affluent families, among
interest but low concentration as when they are social- students who earn better grades, and among students
izing with friends. Moreover, participation in struc- from smaller schools and smaller, more rural commu-
tured extracurricular activities, such as hobbies nities, where school activities often play a relatively
or sports, has been shown to be the most positive more central role in the lives of adults and adolescents
way for adolescents to spend free time, in terms of alike (for example, where an entire community may
their current and future psychological development turn out for a school’s Friday night football or basket-
(McHale, Crouter, & Tucker, 2001; Zaff, Moore, Papillo, ball game). Adolescents whose parents are themselves
& Williams, 2003). involved in the community or who reinforce their
children’s interests are also more likely to participate
(Fletcher, Elder, & Mekos, 2000; Huebner & Mancini,
STRUCTURED LEISURE ACTIVITIES
2003; Persson, Kerr, & Stattin, 2007). Generally speak-
For many adolescents around the world, school-spon- ing, extracurricular participation is stable over time—
sored extracurricular activities provide the context for students who are highly involved in these activities at
much of their leisure activity (Farb & Matjasko, 2012; the beginning of high school are likely to stay highly
J. Mahoney et al., 2009). According to recent surveys, involved throughout high school (Denault & Poulin,
about two-thirds of American high school students par- 2009).
ticipate in one or more extracurricular activities, al-
though the participation rate varies greatly from school ❚ The Impact of Extracurricular Participation
to school. The most popular extracurricular activity in on Development Although early research docu-
the United States is athletics, in which about half of all menting a positive correlation between extracurric-
adolescents participate; it is also the extracurricular ac- ular participation and adolescents’ well-being could
tivity that has been most extensively studied (Theokas, not clearly show that participation leads to improved
2009). The other two main activities in which many ad- functioning (rather than the opposite), newer lon-
olescents participate are those related to music (such as gitudinal studies indicate that participation in an ex-
band, chorus, orchestra, or glee club; about one-fifth of tracurricular activity actually improves students’
all adolescent students participate in these) and those performance in school and reduces the likelihood of
related to academic or occupational interests (such as dropping out; deters delinquency, drug use, and other
science clubs, language clubs, or clubs oriented toward
certain careers; about one-fifth participate in these).
Participation in extracurricular activities is influ-
enced by a number of factors (A. F. Feldman & Matjasko, flow experience The experience of high levels of both
concentration and interest at the same time.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 232 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

232 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Brooks-Gunn, 2008; Mays & Thompson, 2009; K. E.


Miller, Melnick, Barnes, Sabo, & Farrell, 2007; M. J.
Moore & Werch, 2005).
Extracurricular participation in high school seems
to be linked to extracurricular participation in college
and to community involvement in adulthood—just
as youngsters who are involved in extracurricular
activities as 9th-graders are likely to be similarly busy
throughout high school, “do-ers” as adolescents tend to
remain so in young and middle adulthood. Along sim-
ilar lines, individuals who participate in sports during
adolescence are likely to continue athletic activities in
adulthood (D. Perkins, Jacobs, Barber, & Eccles, 2003).
One recent study found benefits of participating in
structured extracurricular activities as many as 8 years
after high school graduation, especially among indi-
viduals whose extracurricular participation lasted at
least 2 years and participated for a relatively high num-
ber of hours each week, but only among individuals
whose activities were school-sponsored (M. Gardner,
Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2008). One reason for the
special benefits of school-sponsored extracurricular
participation is that it helps strengthen students’ at-
tachment to school, which in turn contributes to their
future educational success. This so-called “spillover
effect” may be especially strong among adolescents
Participation in school-sponsored extracurricular activities, like
athletics, leads to better school achievement. who are poorer students and in schools in poor com-
munities (Guest & Schneider, 2003; J. Mahoney &
Cairns, 1997).
types of risk taking; and enhances students’ psycho- A recent study of a high school theater production
logical well-being and social status, with positive ef- also found that the experience contributed in impor-
fects seen even after high school graduation (Bohnert, tant ways to adolescents’ emotional development.
Kane, & Garber, 2008; Boone & Leadbeater, 2006; Through the course of preparing for their perfor-
Brand et al., 2010; Farb & Matjasko, 2012; Fauth, mance, students learned how to better manage their
Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2007; Fredericks & Eccles, emotions, better understand others’ emotions, and
2008, 2010; Guest & Schneider, 2003; J. Mahoney learn how to deal more effectively with anger, frustra-
et al., 2009; Randall & Bohnert, 2009). Participation in tion, and stress. Although these opportunities arise in
organized community-based activities also appears to all sorts of situations, the atmosphere cultivated by the
protect adolescents in disadvantaged neighborhoods adults who advise and supervise adolescents appears to
from exposure to violence (Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, be especially important in making the experience a
2009). Despite concerns that the extracurricular over- positive influence on the participants’ development
scheduling of youth, and affluent youth in particular, (Gaudreau, Amiot, & Vallerand, 2009; Larson & Brown,
creates pressures that compromise adolescents’ men- 2007). This interpretation is consistent with the idea
tal health, several studies have found no such effect that the quality of relationships adolescents develop
(Farb & Matjasko, 2012; Luthar, Shoum, & Brown, with the adults they encounter in extracurricular
2006). activities may be especially important (Scales, Benson,
The one exception to this uniformly positive picture & Mannes, 2006).
is that some studies have found that involvement in Researchers speculate that extracurricular activities
team sports, which is associated with many psychologi- are generally positive in their impact because they in-
cal benefits, such as better mental health, better sleep, crease students’ contact with teachers and other school
and higher school achievement, is also associated with personnel who may reinforce the value of school (as
increased alcohol use and delinquency. This latter conse- when a coach or advisor counsels a student about plans
quence is seen especially among boys who have a strong for college) and because participation itself may im-
“jock” identity and who participate in school-sponsored, prove students’ self-confidence and self-esteem. Some
male-dominated sports, like football (Gardner, Roth, & educators believe that extracurricular participation also
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 233 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 233

helps bond students and parents to their school, espe- Moneta, 2006; Scanlan, Babkes, & Scanlan, 2005). This
cially in the case of adolescents who are not achieving is why having a coach who understands and looks out
academically; for many of them, their extracurricular for these potential problems is so important.
activity is what keeps them coming to school each day
(Blomfield & Barber, 2011; Broh, 2002; Dotterer,
McHale, & Crouter, 2007; J. Mahoney et al., 2009; MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
H. Marsh & Kleitman, 2002). Consistent with this,
Why are the apparent effects of
adolescents who combine participation in sports with
participation in extracurricular activities
other sorts of extracurricular activities, and who there-
different from those associated with
fore have multiple points of “attachment” to their
part-time employment? How might
school, fare better than those who are solely involved in
these two sets of experiences differ?
athletics (Linver, Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Zarrett
et al., 2009).
Some of the positive effects of extracurricular
participation may stem from the fact that these activities
bring adolescents into contact with peers who influence
them in beneficial ways; extracurricular activities UNSTRUCTURED LEISURE TIME
often lead to new friendships (Ntoumanis, Taylor, &
Thøgersen-Ntoumanis, 2012; Schaefer, Simpkins, Vest, One important distinction that is consistent with
& Price, 2011; Simpkins, Eccles, & Becnel, 2008). When adults’ preconceptions about leisure time and its im-
the activity strengthens membership in a peer group in- pact on adolescent development is that between struc-
volved in prosocial activities that revolve around the tured leisure activities, like school- or community-
school, participation can be beneficial to the adoles- sponsored extracurricular activities, which are run and
cent’s development and achievement (J. Mahoney, supervised by adults, and unstructured leisure activi-
2000). But not all activities revolve around prosocial en- ties, such as hanging out with friends without any or-
deavors, and as noted earlier, participation in certain ganized activity in mind. Whereas participation in
sports can increase adolescents’ involvement in problem structured leisure activities tends to have positive ef-
behavior. For example, one study of male football play- fects on adolescent development, time spent in un-
ers and wrestlers found that they were more likely than structured leisure activities does the reverse (Osgood,
nonathletic males to get into serious fights (similar ef- Anderson, & Shaffer, 2005).
fects were not found for boys who played other sports)
and that violence was most common among football ❚ Routine Activity Theory Sociologist Wayne
players whose friends also played football (Kreager, Osgood and colleagues (e.g., Osgood & Anderson,
2007a). Another found similar effects of sports partici- 2004) have argued that the combination of a lack of
pation on delinquency—boys who participated in orga- structure, socializing with peers, and the absence of
nized sports were more likely to spend their free time adult supervision encourages delinquency and other
hanging out with their friends, and this led them to be problem behaviors. Their view, called routine activity
involved in more antisocial (although not violent) be- theory, is that “the less structured an activity, the more
havior (Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). (As you will likely a person is to encounter opportunities for prob-
read in a moment, during adolescence, spending un- lem behavior in the simple sense that he or she is not
structured, unsupervised time with friends is a recipe occupied doing something else” (Osgood et al., 2005,
for trouble.) p. 51). Because adolescence is a time of heightened peer
Several cautions have been raised about adolescents’ pressure and heightened susceptibility to peer influence
participation in athletics, however. As extracurricular (as you will read in Chapter 9), and because one of the
sports have become more competitive, the number of strongest deterrents against problem behavior is the
young people injured during these activities has risen presence of an adult, it is hardly surprising that un-
substantially. According to some estimates, around 25% structured peer activity without adult supervision is
of all adolescent athletes are injured while playing associated with all sorts of problems—depression,
organized sports (Overbaugh & Allen, 1994). In addi- delinquency, drug and alcohol use, violence, and preco-
tion, many adolescents feel anxious and tense within cious sexual activity (A. Anderson & Hughes, 2009;
the competitive atmosphere that has come to dominate Bratt, 2008; Maimon & Browning, 2010; McHale,
after-school sports in many communities. Thus, while Updegraff, Kim, & Cansier, 2009).
competitive athletics are a source of considerable plea-
sure for some adolescents, they are a source of equally
considerable stress for others (Larson, Hansen, & routine activity theory A perspective on adolescence that views
unstructured, unsupervised time with peers as a main cause of
misbehavior.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 234 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

234 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Even something as positive sounding as attendance when it comes to psychological development, school
at a community recreation center can increase adoles- achievement, and self-conceptions (J. Mahoney et al.,
cents’ problem behavior if their time there is unstruc- 2009). These studies argue against the view that having
tured and minimally supervised (J. Mahoney & Stattin, to care for oneself contributes in positive ways to the de-
2000; J. Mahoney, Stattin, & Lord, 2004). Hanging out velopment of self-reliance or personal responsibility—
with friends in the absence of adult supervision at night findings reminiscent of those reported in studies of
increases the likelihood of adolescents engaging in part-time employment.
problem behavior substantially; one study found that More importantly, perhaps, several studies suggest
adolescents who spent 5 or more evenings out in an that, compared to young people who are supervised af-
average week, for instance, were at least 4 times more ter school by adults, those who are in self-care are more
likely than those who went out fewer than twice a week socially isolated, more depressed, and more likely to
to be involved in antisocial activity (Gage, Overpeck, have school problems, be involved in problem behavior,
Nansel, & Kogan, 2005). As one team of writers suc- be sexually active at earlier ages, and use more drugs
cinctly put it, “Whether you like or dislike your father, it and alcohol (D. Cohen, Farley, Taylor, Martin, &
will be more convenient to smoke marijuana when he Schuster, 2002; Mahoney & Parente, 2009; Shumow,
isn’t around” (Osgood, Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman, & Smith, & Smith, 2009). Together, these studies seem to
Johnston, 1996, p. 640). suggest that many self-care arrangements may have
more costs than benefits. As Figure 7.7 indicates, for
❚ Time After School In 2006, nearly 75% of instance, delinquency is more common on weekday af-
mothers with children between ages 6 and 17 were em- ternoons than at any other time (Osgood et al., 2005).
ployed (J. Mahoney et al., 2009). As a consequence, one One limitation of studies of youth in self-care is that
prime time for unstructured and unsupervised leisure they typically lump together all children who take care
is during the afternoon on school days—after school of themselves after school, even though there are im-
has let out but before parents have returned home from portant differences within the self-care population and
work. While some youngsters whose parents are at among after-school programs (J. Mahoney et al., 2009).
work during the afternoon are involved in school- or Several studies have shown, for example, that the set-
community-based programs that provide adult super- ting in which adolescents care for themselves makes a
vision, others spend their after-school hours away from difference: Adolescents who go straight home after
adults, in their homes, with friends, or simply hanging school are far less likely to engage in problem behavior
out in neighborhoods and shopping malls (Mahoney & than are their peers who go to a friend’s house or who
Parente, 2009). Affluent, suburban, and White children just hang out (Mahoney & Parente, 2009). In addition,
are most likely to be home unsupervised, and poor, youngsters in self-care who are raised by authoritative
minority, and urban and rural children are least likely parents and who are monitored by their parents from a
(J. Mahoney et al., 2009). distance—via telephone check-ins, for example—are
Psychologists have debated whether adolescents who no more susceptible to problem behavior than are chil-
take care of themselves after school profit from these op- dren whose parents are home with them after school
portunities for self-management or are at greater risk for (Galambos & Maggs, 1991; Vandell & Ramanan, 1991).
problems because they are not supervised by adults. As noted earlier, one of the most consistent findings in
With regard to the first, in general, most studies show studies of how adolescents spend their free time is that
that children in self-care do not differ from their peers spending free time with peers in unstructured activities

FIGURE 7.7 More arrests occur during 7


school afternoons than at any other
time, presumably because this is the 6
School days
time when adolescents are least likely
Percent of arrests for
aggravated assault

to be supervised. (Osgood et al., 2005) 5

1
Nonschool days
0
6 AM 8 AM 12 PM 3 PM 6 PM 9 PM 12 AM 3 AM 6 AM
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 235 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 235

without adult supervision is associated with increased or she gets home, and teaching the child how to handle
problem behavior (Mahoney, Schweder, & Stattin, emergencies, should they arise (Steinberg, 2011).
2002; McHale, Crouter, & Tucker, 2001; Osgood,
Anderson, & Shaffer, 2005).
Spending time after school with friends in unsuper- MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
vised settings is more problematic under some circum-
Most studies link unstructured leisure
stances than others, however. One study found, for
time with adolescent problem behavior.
example, that it was not simply spending unsupervised
But is there no value for adolescents in
time with peers that increased an adolescent’s likeli-
simply hanging out with friends, without
hood of alcohol and drug use; it was the combination
having adults organizing their activities for
of lacking supervision, having friends who liked to
them? Is all unstructured leisure time bad?
party and use drugs, and being especially susceptible to
peer pressure that was most damaging (L. Caldwell &
Darling, 1999). A second study found that the harmful
effects of low parental monitoring were especially bad
in neighborhoods low in collective efficacy (discussed in PROMOTING POSITIVE YOUTH
Chapter 3), where other adults were unlikely to provide DEVELOPMENT
supervision if parents were not around (Coley, Morris,
& Hernandez, 2004). In another study, the researchers In light of evidence that certain types of extracurricular
examined rates of problem behavior among 7th-graders activities appear to benefit adolescents, and in view of
as a function of three factors: whether they spent their the potential dangers of leaving adolescents unsuper-
after-school time in unsupervised settings, whether vised after school, experts have called for better and
they had parents who monitored their activities and more readily available after-school programming for
whereabouts, and whether they lived in a neighbor- adolescents. They argue that well-designed programs
hood that was considered safe (Pettit, Bates, Dodge, & not only will deter problem behavior by providing ado-
Meece, 1999). In all groups, being closely monitored by lescents with adult supervision but will also promote
parents was a deterrent against problem behavior. And, positive youth development (J. Lerner, Phelps, Forman,
not surprisingly, adolescents who reported spending a & Bowers, 2009; Small & Memmo, 2004; Yohalem &
lot of after-school time with peers in unsupervised set- Wilson-Ahlstrom, 2010). Although the label “positive
tings were more likely to show problem behavior one youth development” is new, the concept is not. Indeed,
year later than were other teenagers. But the negative the goals espoused by proponents of positive youth de-
effects of low parental monitoring and unsupervised velopment programs today bear a striking resemblance
peer group activity were especially pronounced among to the stated goals of youth programs that have been
teenagers living in unsafe neighborhoods. Indeed, in around for ages, like the YMCA (founded in London in
safe neighborhoods, adolescents who spent time in un- 1844 and transported to the United States in 1851), the
supervised peer activities were not at greater risk for Boys and Girls Clubs of America (with the first club
developing problems, even if they were not monitored founded in 1860), 4-H clubs (founded at the turn of the
closely by their parents. This study serves as a reminder 20th century), and scouting (founded in 1910). Indeed,
of the importance of looking at the broader context in in 1866, the YMCA of New York City announced that
which adolescents live in order to fully understand how its purpose was “the improvement of the spiritual,
they are affected by parents, peers, and other influences. mental, social and physical condition of young men”
Adolescents also differ in their susceptibility to the ad- (YMCA, 2006).
verse effects of self-care—not surprisingly, teenagers Experts’ interest in helping young people develop
with established behavior problems are more negatively strengths, rather than simply preventing them from
affected by being on their own than are their peers getting into trouble, has burgeoned in recent years (e.g.,
(Coley et al., 2004). Gano-Overway et al., 2009; Lerner, von Eye, Lerner,
Taken together, these studies suggest that self-care Lewin-Bizan, & Bowers, 2010; Lewin-Bizan, Bowers, &
after school probably does not hold great benefits for Lerner, 2010; Scales, Benson, Roehlkepartain, Sesma,
youngsters and, under some conditions, may cause Jr., & van Dulmen, 2006). There are many different
problems if adolescents’ parents do not promote the models of positive youth development, but they all
development of responsible behavior when they are are very similar (J. Lerner et al., 2009; J. Mahoney
with their child. What should parents who have no et al., 2009). One of the most widely cited is that of
choice but to leave their youngsters in self-care do? Ex- psychologist Richard Lerner, who describes the Five C’s
perts advise parents to provide clear instructions about
the child’s after-school activities and whereabouts, positive youth development The goal of programs designed to
asking the child to check in with an adult as soon as he facilitate healthy psychosocial development and not simply to deter
problematic development.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 236 21/03/13 11:12 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

236 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

TABLE 7.1 The Five C’s of positive youth development

Competence A positive view of one’s actions in domain-specific areas, including social, academic,
cognitive, and vocational. Social competence pertains to interpersonal skills (e.g., conflict
resolution). Cognitive competence pertains to cognitive abilities (e.g., decision making).
School grades, attendance, and test scores are part of academic competence. Vocational
competence involves work habits and career choice explorations.
Confidence An internal sense of overall positive self-worth and self-efficacy; one’s global self-regard, as
opposed to domain-specific beliefs.
Connection Positive bonds with people and institutions that are reflected in bidirectional exchanges
between the individual and peers, family, school, and community in which both parties
contribute to the relationship.
Character Respect for societal and cultural rules, possession of standards for correct behaviors, a sense
of right and wrong (morality), and integrity.
Caring/compassion A sense of sympathy and empathy for others.

Source: R. Lerner et al., 2005.

of positive youth development: competence, confi- adolescents as more self-reliant, which may increase
dence, connection, character, and caring/compassion parental autonomy-granting, further contributing to
(see Table 7.1) (Bowers et al., 2010; Lerner et al., 2005). the development of adolescent independence (Larson,
These characteristics, in one form or another, are Pearce, Sullivan, & Jarrett, 2007).
often the focus of contemporary community-based
programming for youth, including programs empha-
sizing community service, volunteer activity, mentoring, RECAP
and skill building. • In many industrialized countries, leisure occupies
What makes a positive youth development pro- more time in adolescence than do school and work
gram successful? One recent evaluation found that a combined.
key component to program success is the extent to • It is important to distinguish between participation
which participants were placed in demanding roles, in structured leisure activities, such as school-
encouraged to meet high expectations, expected to sponsored extracurricular activities, and unstruc-
take responsibility for their behavior, and helped to tured leisure, such as hanging out with friends in
understand the consequences of failing to fulfill their the absence of adult supervision.
obligations (Dawes & Larson, 2011). As one girl • Adolescents’ moods are better when they are
explained: engaged in structured leisure activities than at
any other time. In addition, participation in
You have to carry your own. Mr. Baker and Mr. Jensen school-sponsored extracurricular activities may
[the program advisors], they’ll push you, they’ll give you enhance adolescents’ well-being and strengthen
an encouragement, but they’re not going to do it for their attachment to school.
you: no way, shape, or form. (D. Wood, Larson, & Brown, • Having large amounts of unstructured leisure
2009, p. 300) time increases adolescents’ risk for engagement
in problem behavior. Because many adolescents’
This expectation, in turn, helped adolescents under- parents work, self-care during the after-school
stand how important their behavior was to other mem- hours can be problematic. The greatest risk for
bers of the group. In contrast, in programs that were problem behavior after school is found among
less successful in contributing to character develop- teenagers who live in unsafe neighborhoods,
ment, program leaders “had lower expectations and spend a lot of time in unsupervised activity with
more often let youth ‘off the hook.’ They filled in or peers, and are poorly monitored by their parents.
covered up for weaker work. In several cases, when • Many experts have called for better and more
members . . . did not complete or backed out on doing readily available after-school programming that
a task, the leader reported doing the work himself ” will not simply deter problem behavior
(Wood et al., 2009, p. 305). In other words, expecting but also promote positive
adolescents to behave responsibly may help them de- youth development.
velop a sense of responsibility and build competence
(Coatsworth & Conroy, 2009; Larson & Angus, 2011).
In some families, this may lead parents to see their
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 237 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 237

Adolescents, Media,
and the Internet
PATTERNS OF MEDIA USE
In 2009, a Los Angeles high school teacher asked her
10th-grade class to unplug themselves for a week—to
go entirely without electronic media for that whole
time (Lopez, 2009). Here’s one student’s report. It’s very
typical of what others wrote:

For me, the hardest media to break away from for 7 days
was the 46-inch TV in the living room, plus all of the
other TVs in the house. I watch TV almost every day.
When it is on, I cannot resist watching. I thought being
without media was hard at the beginning, but while it
went on, it got harder and harder, even in the same day. Studies of adolescent media use must now take into account the
The first day was the hardest. I was left in silence, with fact that teenagers often use multiple forms of mass media at the
everything gone and disconnected. I think that part was same time.
the hardest. Thinking about other things. Usually I like
hearing my thoughts, but that day, they kind of got an-
from a the report of a U.S. Senate committee hearing on
noying. I didn’t want to listen to myself anymore.
juvenile delinquency:
As this young woman’s account illustrates, mass me- The child today in the process of growing up is constantly
dia have become so much a part of the typical adoles- exposed to sights and sounds of a kind and quality un-
cent’s life that for many, staying “connected” starts to dreamed of in previous generations. As these sights and
take on qualities of an addiction. Another student in sounds can be a powerful force for good, so too can they
the study wrote, “Being without media was harder than be a powerful [counterforce] working evil. Their very
I ever realized. I thought it was going to be a cake walk, quantity makes them a factor to be reckoned with in
determining the total climate encountered by today’s
but it turned into a hell. I found myself thinking that I
children during their formative years. (U.S. Senate
should just give up, and I kept talking to myself.” Many Committee on the Judiciary, 1955, p.1)
students reported sleeping much more than usual as a
way of coping, in part because adolescents frequently The Senate hearing, held in 1955, was about the contri-
use media to distract themselves from negative thoughts bution of comic books to adolescent crime.
and emotions (Larson, 1995). At the end of the week, Research on new media and adolescent develop-
most said that the experience was interesting, in that it ment is similarly slanted. Among the questions asked
taught them how dependent on these media they were, are whether exposure to online sexual content encour-
but that they could not wait to return to their iPods, ages sexual activity, whether using social networking
smartphones, computers, and televisions. sites exposes adolescents to Internet predators, whether
Until the last 10 years or so, most research on the online gaming increases aggression, whether the grow-
impact of the media on adolescent development fo- ing use of electronic forms of communication is ham-
cused on television, movies, and recorded music. Dur- pering the development of social skills, and whether
ing the past two decades, though, there has been an some adolescents have actually become addicted to the
explosion in adolescents’ use of new media, in part be- Internet. We’ll take a look at these and other questions
cause access to electronic media has expanded so rap- about the media’s impact on adolescent development,
idly—adolescents not only access music and video but before we do, let’s look at how widespread media
content through the usual sources (iPods and televi- use is among today’s adolescents.
sions), but also access this material on computers and,
increasingly, on smartphones—and because the sheer ❚ Media Saturation By any measure, the availability
amount of media content that is created and distrib- of media in young people’s lives is remarkable—today’s
uted is so vast (D. Roberts et al., 2009). Many of the adolescents live in a world that is not simply “media-rich”
questions researchers have asked about new media fo- but absolutely “media-saturated” (J. Brown & Bobkowski,
cus on their assumed negative effects, just as with prior 2011a). Virtually all American households have at least
research on older media—for example, whether rock one television (and half subscribe to premium channels,
music promotes drug use, whether television viewing
“rots your brain,” whether video games encourage vio-
lence, and so on. Consider the following sentence, taken new media Digital media typically accessed via computers,
smartphones, or other Internet-based devices.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 238 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

238 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

FIGURE 7.8 The 25


average adolescent
sends more than

Proportion of Adolescents
20
100 text messages
a day. (Lenhart, 2012)
15

2009
10
2011

0
No texts 1–10 11–20 21–50 51–100 101–200 More than
texts texts texts texts texts 200 texts

like HBO), and 40% of adolescents live in a household close to 11 hours. The highest levels of media use are
that has a TV screen in the family car. Today, computers reported by early adolescents, and by Black and His-
and Internet access are present in virtually all homes, re- panic teenagers (Rideout et al., 2010). The only type
gardless of family income; as of 2009, nearly 85% of all of media use that has not increased in the past decade
American adolescents had Internet access in their home, is reading print materials. In 2009, adolescents spent
and more than one-third of adolescents had Internet ac- only an average of 20 minutes a day reading words on
cess in their bedroom. Well over two-thirds of all adoles- a printed page.
cents have their own cell phones, and they spend less time There has been an enormous increase in the last
on their phones talking to other people than they do on decade in the amount of time adolescents spend on
other activities (Rideout, Foeher & Roberts, 2010). In the Internet, in part because of easier access due to
2011, the average teenager sent 100 text messages each smartphones and in part because the Internet is now
day; nearly one-fifth sent more than 200 texts daily used to deliver content that had been delivered some
(Lenhart, 2012; Underwood, Rosen, More, Ehrenreich, & other way in the past (e.g., watching television pro-
Gentsch, 2012) (see Figure 7.8). Although a digital grams on Hulu rather than on a television, playing
divide—the fact that different groups have different video games online rather than with an Xbox or
degrees of access to the Internet—still remains, it has PlayStation (Rideout et al., 2010). There is some re-
become much smaller as public access to the Internet search indicating that close to 10% of preadolescents
has spread and as smartphones have become more popu- and adolescents devote so much energy to playing
lar and common, even in economically disadvantaged video games that their behavior is “pathological” (see
communities and countries. Table 7.2) (Gentile, 2009; Gentile et al., 2011; van den
Adolescents’ total media exposure—the amount of Eijnden, Spijkerman, Vermulst, van Rooij, & Engels,
time they spend each day using one of the mass 2010). On some experimental tests of attention and
media—is extremely high, and substantially higher self-control, individuals who report that their devo-
than it was 10 years ago. The average adolescent tion to online gaming is a problem appear similar to
spends nearly 8 hours each day using one or more me- individuals with other types of addiction (van Holst
dia, and this includes time spent using different media et al., 2012). Online gaming appears particularly likely
simultaneously (that is, where 1 hour watching TV to be associated with compulsive Internet use (van
while on the Internet and talking on a cell phone Rooij, Schoenmakers, van den Eijnden, & van de
would be recorded as only 1 hour of media use, even Mheen, 2010). Given that most of these games involve
though it technically is 3 hours of exposure). Although violence (Strasburger, Jordan, & Donnerstein, 2010), it
adolescents’ time spent viewing television programs at is not surprising that pathological video gaming is
their regularly scheduled times has declined, the avail- correlated with self-reported aggressive behavior (Ko,
ability of recorded programs and television content Yen, Liu, Huang, & Yen, 2009), although, as you will
on other devices (like computers) has led to an overall read, in many media studies, it is hard to determine
increase in time spent viewing video content. When cause and effect (i.e., aggressive teenagers may be more
media multitasking is taken into account, the amount drawn to aggressive games). Many parents have rules
of time teenagers are exposed to media each day is about what their children are permitted to watch or
listen to, but this is far more common in households
with preadolescents than teenagers (Rideout et al.,
digital divide The fact that adolescents from different economic 2010).
and cultural groups have differential access to digital technology.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 239 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 239

TABLE 7.2 Signs of pathological video game use

“Yes” Responses
Total Sample Only
Symptoms “Yes” “Sometimes” Boys Girls

Over time, have you been spending much more time thinking
about playing video games, learning about video-game playing,
or planning the next opportunity to play? 21% 19% 29% 11%b
Do you need to spend more and more time and/or money on video
games in order to feel the same amount of excitement? 8 9 12 3b
Have you tried to play video games less often or for shorter periods
of time, but are unsuccessful? 2 22 2 3
Do you become restless or irritable when attempting to cut down
or stop playing video games? 2 6 2 1
Have you played video games as a way of escaping from problems
or bad feelings? 25 20 29 19b
Have you ever lied to family or friends about how much you play
video games? 14 10 17 10a
Have you ever stolen a video game from a store or a friend, or have
you ever stolen money in order to buy a video game? 2 2 3 1b
Do you sometimes skip household chores in order to spend more
time playing video games? 33 21 40 24b
Do you sometimes skip doing homework in order to spend more
time playing video games? 23 19 29 15b
Have you ever done poorly on a school assignment or test because
you spent too much time playing video games? 20 12 26 11b
Have you ever needed friends or family to give you extra money
because you spent too much money on video-game equipment,
software, or game/Internet fees? 9 6 13 4b

Note: For each symptom, chi-square tests were used to compare prevalence among boys and girls. Overall, the number of symptoms reported
was significantly different (p < .001) between boys (M = 2.8) and girls (M = 1.3). Also, the prevalence of pathological gaming (i.e., displaying
at least 6 of the 11 symptoms) was significantly different (p < .001) between boys (12%) and girls (3%).
Source: Gentile, 2009.
a b
p , .05. p , .001.

THEORIES OF MEDIA INFLUENCE


aggressive adolescents are more prone to choose to
AND USE
watch violent images (Roe, 1995). Similarly, sexual
Given the considerable amount of time adolescents behavior may be correlated with listening to “sexy”
spend exposed to the mass media, not surprisingly, music or watching television programs with a lot of
the impact of the media on teenagers’ behavior and sexual content, but it is impossible to say which causes
development has been the subject of much debate which (Steinberg & Monahan, 2011). And, although
(J. Brown & Bobkowski, 2011b; Strasburger et al., several major studies of media use (D. Roberts, Foehr,
2010). Unfortunately, one tremendous problem in in- & Rideout, 2005; Rideout et al., 2010) have found that
terpreting studies of media use and adolescent devel- adolescents who report a lot of media use are signifi-
opment is that it is extremely difficult to disentangle cantly more troubled (bored, unhappy, in trouble at
cause and effect, because adolescents choose which home or school) than adolescents who used these
mass media they are exposed to and how much expo- media less often, it is not known whether large doses
sure they have (D. Roberts et al., 2009). (In Chapter 6 of mass media cause problems or, more plausibly,
we saw that this is also a problem in identifying the whether adolescents with more problems listen to
“effects” of peers on adolescent behavior, because ado- more music and spend more time online, perhaps as
lescents select their friends.) Although it has been a way of distracting themselves from their troubles
speculated that violent film images provoke aggres- or alleviating boredom (Larson, 1995; Pea et al.,
sion, for example, it is just as likely, if not more so, that 2012; Szwedo, Mikami, & Allen, 2011). It’s probably a
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 240 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

240 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

combination of both. It is important to keep in mind, influence of the media, but to the fact that individuals
too, that not all media exposure is the same: Some with particular inclinations choose media that are con-
adolescents use the Internet to stay up on the news (Lin, sistent with their interests. According to this view, ado-
Cheong, Kim, & Jung, 2010), and mass media have been lescents deliberately choose the media they use, either
used successfully to communicate information about for entertainment, information, bonding with others,
safe sex, dissuade teenagers from using tobacco and illicit or developing a sense of identity. Thus, aggressive ado-
drugs, and help chronically ill adolescents comply with lescents are more likely to purchase violent video
their medication regimens (J. Brown & Bobkowski, games because they enjoy being aggressive; teenagers
2011b; Strasburger et al., 2010). who are interested in sex are more likely to look for
There are three basic schools of thought concern- pornography on the Internet because they want to
ing the media’s impact (or lack thereof) on adolescent masturbate or feel sexually aroused; beer-drinking ad-
development. One school of thought argues that ado- olescents are more likely to watch football and to be
lescents are influenced by the content to which they exposed to beer commercials (which, after all, is why
are exposed, with respect to their knowledge about beer companies advertise during football games and
the world, their attitudes and values, and their behav- not on the Animal Planet network); and adolescents
ior. You’ve no doubt heard contentions like these who are involved with drugs like to listen to music that
thousands of times: Playing violent video games glorifies drug use. According to this view, adolescents’
makes adolescents aggressive, watching sexy movies preexisting interests and motives shape their media
makes adolescents sex-crazed, being exposed to Inter- choices.
net pornography affects the ways that adolescents According to the third school of thought, the links
think about gender roles, listening to rap lyrics between adolescents’ preferences, on the one hand, and
encourages adolescents to engage in violence and their media exposure, on the other, are reciprocal (that
crime, viewing beer commercials during the Super is, they affect each other); moreover, adolescents not
Bowl makes adolescents drink beer, and so on. Ac- only choose what they are exposed to but interpret the
cording to this view, the media shape adolescents’ in- media in ways that shape their impact. This view is re-
terests, motives, and beliefs about the world—a view ferred to as the media practice model (Steele & Brown,
known as cultivation theory (Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, 1995). Imagine two adolescents who accidentally stum-
& Signorelli, 1994). ble onto a sexually explicit website while on the Inter-
A second school of thought, called the uses and net. One, a sexually experienced teenager who is curious
gratifications approach (E. Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, about pornography, views the website with interest—
1974), emphasizes the fact that adolescents choose the perhaps it even makes him feel aroused. The other, who
media to which they are exposed. According to this isn’t interested in sex, sees the very same content and
view, any correlation between what adolescents are ex- feels repulsed. Not only is the experience not arousing,
posed to and what they do or think is due not to the but it also makes this adolescent even less interested in
having sex than he was before landing on the site. One
13-year-old sees a beer commercial and thinks, “That’s
cultivation theory A perspective on media use that emphasizes how I’m going to party when I’m old enough to drink.”
the impact media exposure has on individuals. Another sees the exact same images and thinks, “What
uses and gratification approach A perspective on media use that morons those people are—look how stupid beer makes
emphasizes the active role users play in selecting the media to you act.” Two adolescents are flipping through televi-
which they are exposed. sion channels—one, who collects rap music, sees a flash
of a Lil Wayne music video that piques her interest and
media practice model A perspective on media use that
stops flipping channels to watch; the other, who is into
emphasizes the fact that adolescents not only choose what media
they are exposed to but also interpret the media in ways that shape country music, doesn’t even notice the clip. According
their impact. to this third view, the ways in which media do (or do
not) affect adolescents depend on the ways in which the
correlation The extent to which two things vary systematically media are experienced and interpreted.
with each other. These problems in distinguishing among correla-
causation The correlation between two things attributable to the tion (when two things go hand in hand), causation
effect one thing has on the other. (when one thing actually causes another), reverse cau-
sation (when the correlation between two things is
reverse causation Relationship in which the correlation between
due not to the first thing causing the second, but to the
two things is due not to the first thing causing the second, but to
reverse), and spurious causation (when the correla-
the second causing the first.
tion between two things is due to the fact that each of
spurious causation Relationship in which the correlation between them is correlated with some third factor) make it
two things is due to the fact that each of them is correlated with almost impossible to say for sure whether media
some third factor.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 241 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 241

focused on television (as a consequence, we know very


Watching beer little about the impact of other media content on devel-
Drinking beer
commercials
opment), and the bulk of the research has focused on
Hypothesized Causation the three topics about which adults are most concerned
(some might say obsessed): sex, violence, and drugs.

❚ Sex Sexual themes are ubiquitous on television,


Watching beer with more than 70% of all shows popular among teen-
Drinking beer
commercials
agers containing sexual content, and with an average
Reverse Causation of nearly seven sexual scenes per hour (Kunkel, Eyal,
Finnerty, Biely, & Donnerstein, 2005). Sexual content is
found across virtually all sorts of television genres, so
teenagers are exposed to sex on television whether their
Drinking beer tastes run toward comedies, action shows, or dramas,
although the context in which sex is depicted varies
Liking football across different categories (Schooler, Sorsoli, Kim, &
Tolman, 2009). Actually, the prevalence of sexual con-
Watching beer tent in shows favored by adolescents declined a bit since
commercials
2000, most likely because of the increase in popularity
Spurious Causation of reality shows like American Idol, which have displaced
FIGURE 7.9 Two things can be correlated because the first shows with more sexual content (usually dramas) on
causes the second (the hypothesized correlation), or some the list of programs most watched by teenagers. Gener-
third factor causes both of them (spurious causation). Re- ally, sex on TV is portrayed as pleasurable and carefree
search on media “effects” on adolescent development has a (L. M. Ward & Friedman, 2006). Most sexual content
hard time separating the three. on prime-time TV is not composed of graphic images
of individuals having sex, but instead is made up of
humorous and suggestive comments. On daytime tele-
exposure genuinely affects adolescent development vision, however, a lot of sexual imagery is of individuals
(see Figure 7.9). engaging in passionate kissing and erotic touching.
The only sure way to demonstrate cause and effect Sexual content is also very common in music videos
where media influence is concerned is to conduct an (L. M. Ward, 2003).
experiment in which people are randomly assigned to The most common sexual messages concern men
be (or not be) exposed to the medium of interest and seeing women as sex objects, sex as a defining aspect of
see how it affects them. But experiments of this sort are masculinity, sex as a competition, and sex as fun and
rare, because they are difficult to do well. Even the most exciting (L. M. Ward, 1995). The message that women
ardent believers in the power of media influence gener- are sex objects is one that teenagers seem especially sus-
ally acknowledge that one exposure to a commercial, a ceptible to (L. M. Ward & Friedman, 2006). Similar
movie, a song, or an Internet site is unlikely to change messages are carried in most music videos, in which
someone’s behavior. But if the impact of media expo- men are shown as aggressive and dominant, and women
sure is incremental and cumulative—perhaps taking are seen as the subservient objects of men’s sexual
years of exposure to have an effect—it may be quite advances (D. Roberts et al., 2009). One issue that has
powerful but impossible to demonstrate in a brief ex- concerned many sex educators is the relative absence of
periment. All of this is to say that you should view any messages concerning the possible physical conse-
claims about the presence—or absence—of media in- quences of sex (for example, pregnancy and STDs), al-
fluence on adolescent development with tremendous though these messages have become more common
caution. now than in the past, in part because the entertainment
industry has tried to respond more responsibly to con-
cerns about the way sex has been portrayed (Kunkel
THE IMPACT OF ADOLESCENTS’
et al., 2005). There is plenty of sexual content in new
EXPOSURE TO CONTROVERSIAL
media as well, and the content is typically more explicit
MEDIA CONTENT
than that in older media. One study found that half of
These chicken-and-egg problems notwithstanding, a all adolescents had seen pornography on the Internet
few generalizations about media usage and adolescent (J. Brown and Bobkowski, 2011b).
development have enough supporting evidence, how- Whether and in what ways exposure to sexual media
ever indirect, to generate some consensus among ex- content affects adolescents’ sexual development is con-
perts in the area. Most of the relevant research has troversial. Some studies have concluded that exposure
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 242 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

242 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

Whether such exposure affects adolescents’ attitudes


and behaviors because it changes their beliefs about
what is normative, or whether it works by actually mak-
ing adolescents more sexually aroused is not known.

❚ Violence Adolescents are also exposed to a great


deal of violent imagery on television, in movies, in cer-
tain music genres, and in video games (J. Brown &
Bobkowski, 2011a; D. Roberts et al., 2009). More than
60% of TV programming contains violence; as a con-
sequence, young people see an estimated 10,000 acts
of media violence each year, and more than one-fourth
of all violent incidents on TV involve guns. By the
age of 18, the typical adolescent will have seen about
200,000 violent acts just on television alone (Strasburger
et al., 2010).
Precise estimates of the amount of violent imagery in
Research has not demonstrated that exposure to violent video
the most popular video games or other visual media are
games actually makes adolescents more violent. not available, but concerns also have been raised over
the impact of violent video games on young people’s
behavior and attitudes (Ivory, 2008; Konijn, Nije
to sex on television or over the Internet accelerates Bijvank, & Bushman, 2007; Strasburger et al., 2010).
adolescents’ sexual behavior, leading them to start hav- Studies have found that adolescents who spend a lot of
ing sex at an earlier age (J. Brown et al., 2006; Chandra time playing violent video games get into more fights
et al., 2008 R. L. Collins, Martino, Elliott & Miu, 2011), and arguments than do their peers (e.g., Gentile, Lynch,
others have found that adolescents who are interested Linder, & Walsh, 2004), but it is difficult to know
in sex choose to expose themselves to more sexual con- whether playing such games makes adolescents more
tent but are not affected by it (Steinberg & Monahan, hostile or whether adolescents who are more aggressive
2011), and others find evidence for both (Bleakley, to begin with are simply more likely to want to play
Hennessy, Fishbein, & Jordan, 2008). these sorts of games. Moreover, how adolescents re-
Although questions remain about whether exposure spond to video games may vary as a function of whom
to sexual media content alters adolescents’ sexual be- they play them with; one study found that when adoles-
havior, many studies demonstrate that repeated expo- cents played violent video games with their parents, they
sure affects adolescents’ attitudes, beliefs, and intentions reported declines in aggression (Coyne, Padilla-Walker,
(L’Engle, Brown, & Kenneavy, 2006; D. Roberts et al., Stockdale, & Day, 2011).
2009; L. M. Ward, 2003). For example, adolescents who Experimental research on the effects of video games
watch a lot of music videos have more tolerant attitudes on adolescent behavior are inconclusive, with some
toward sexual harassment and more sex-stereotyped at- studies finding small effects and others not (Ivory,
titudes about sexual relationships (L. M. Ward, Hansbrough, 2008). Although controlled experiments have shown
& Walker, 2005). Along similar lines, one study found that that exposure to the lyrics of violent songs increases
college students who frequently watched soap operas individuals’ aggressive thoughts (C. A. Anderson,
(which have a high sexual content) gave higher estimates Carnagey, & Eubanks, 2003), many experts doubt that
than nonviewers did of the number of real-life extra- playing violent video games or listening to music with
marital affairs, children born out of wedlock, and di- violent lyrics causes adolescents to engage in the sorts
vorces. Another study found that high school students of serious violent acts that alarmists have raised con-
who believed that TV characters have highly satisfying cerns about, such as lethal school shootings. As some
sex were less likely themselves to feel that sex is satisfy- have noted, given the millions of copies of violent
ing, presumably because the images they saw on TV games that have been sold, if playing video games had a
created false expectations (D. Roberts, 1993). Similarly, significant impact on real-world violence, we’d likely be
prolonged exposure to pornography leads to exagger- in the midst of a violence epidemic—yet juvenile vio-
ated beliefs about the extent of sexual activity in the real lence has declined substantially since peaking in the
world (Zillman, 2000). Other studies have found that early 1990s, despite the proliferation of violent video
exposure to explicit sex on the Internet is associated games (Reichhardt, 2003). Indeed, careful studies of the
with having more permissive attitudes about sex and impact of playing violent video games find that they do
greater willingness to engage in sexual harassment not make adolescents more aggressive (Ferguson, 2011;
(J. Brown & Bobkowski, 2011b; J. Brown & L’Engle, 2009). Ferguson & Kilburn, 2009), a point that was noted by a
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 243 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 243

prominent group of social scientists in a brief submit- television and movie violence and those who do not,
ted to the U.S. Supreme Court in Brown v. Entertain- it is not clear whether exposure to media violence
ment Merchants, a 2010 case in which the Court ruled causes brain changes or whether individuals with cer-
that a California law banning the sale of violent video tain patterns of brain structure or function are more
games to minors was unconstitutional. Nor is it clear drawn to certain types of stimulation (Strenziok
that putting labels on games to alert adolescents and et al., 2010).
their parents to violent content is an effective strategy,
anyway—one study found that this sort of labeling ❚ Drugs Finally, many analyses have shown that al-
made the games more, not less, attractive to teenagers cohol and tobacco use are ubiquitous in the mass media
(Bijvank, Konijn, Bushman, & Roelofsma, 2009). to which adolescents are exposed. Alcohol, tobacco, or
In contrast to studies of video games, numerous illicit drugs are present in nearly three-quarters of
studies have shown that repeated exposure to violent prime-time network dramatic programs, virtually all
imagery on television leads to aggressive behavior in top-grossing movies, and half of all music videos.
children and youth, especially among those who have Nearly 10% of the commercials that young people see
prior histories of aggression (J. G. Johnson, Cohen, on TV are for beer or wine—and for every public ser-
Kotler, Kasen, & Brook, 2002) (see Figure 7.10). It is vice announcement discouraging alcohol use, teenagers
important to note, however, that other factors, such will view 25–50 ads for alcoholic beverages. And alco-
as experiences in the family or community, are likely hol and tobacco companies have an increasing presence
far greater influences on adolescent violence than on the Internet (Jenssen, Klein, Salazar, Daluga, &
media exposure (Strasburger & Donnerstein, 1999). DiClemente, 2009; Strasburger et al., 2010). Adoles-
Nevertheless, it is now well established that exposure cents are also exposed to alcohol and tobacco through
to TV violence in childhood is linked to aggressive films, which frequently depict actors smoking and
behavior toward others in adolescence and adult- drinking, a concern because teenagers are more likely to
hood, a heightened tolerance of violence, and greater smoke if their favorite film star is a smoker (D. Roberts
desensitization to the effects of violence on others, et al., 2009). Importantly, the effects of seeing smoking
although some of this effect is likely due to the in movies are strongest among adolescents who are ini-
tendency for people with aggressive tendencies to tially less inclined to take up smoking, which argues
choose to watch violent programming (Huesmann, against the notion that adolescents’ desire to smoke
2007; D. Roberts et al., 2009). For instance, although leads to their exposure to smoking in movies and sug-
studies have found differences in brain anatomy be- gests instead that exposure to smoking in movies may
tween adolescents who report frequent exposure to actually influence adolescents to start smoking. As a
consequence of this research, movie distributors are
now putting antismoking ads on DVDs of movies that
Prevalence of subsequent aggressive acts

contain scenes with smoking (Heatherton & Sargent,


60
2009).
50 Studies of exposure to ads for alcohol and tobacco,
as well as antismoking commercial messages, have
against others (%)

40 shown that they are effective in changing teenagers’ at-


titudes about drinking and smoking, both positively
30 and negatively (Farrelly, Davis, Haviland, Healton, &
Messeri, 2005; D. Roberts et al., 2009; Straub, Hills,
20 Thompson, & Moscicki, 2003). Evaluations of media
campaigns designed to reduce adolescent smoking,
10 drinking, and drug use show that certain types of mes-
saging may be more effective than others, however. For
0 example, whereas emphasizing the questionable mo-
⬍1 1–3 ⱖ3
Hours of television viewing per day at mean age 14
tives of the tobacco industry (as in the “Truth” cam-
paign) has proven to be effective, repeated exposure to
Males: prior Males: no prior other kinds of antidrug messaging can lead adolescents
aggression aggression to believe that drug use is more common than it actu-
Females: prior Females: no prior ally is, inadvertently stimulating adolescents’ interest in
aggression aggression using drugs.
FIGURE 7.10 Exposure to violent television during adoles- It is very hard to know whether changing adoles-
cence is associated with increased aggression in young cents’ exposure to messages about drinking and smok-
adulthood, especially among individuals who had a prior ing actually changes their behavior. Adolescents are not
history of aggression. (Johnson et al., 2002) exposed to the mass media as blank slates; rather, they
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 244 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

244 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

bring preexisting values, beliefs, and expectations to the that draw many teenage visitors are YouTube (for enter-
experience of watching or listening, and these preexist- tainment) and Google (for information).
ing states influence what they perceive and remember The impact of Internet socializing on adolescents’
(D. Roberts et al., 2009). Although adolescents who re- behavior and development is interesting, poorly under-
port having seen ads for alcohol or tobacco are them- stood, and controversial. Among the main concerns
selves more likely to drink and smoke, this correlation that have been raised are whether communicating with
could be due to the fact that people who use and enjoy friends and acquaintances electronically rather than in
a product are simply more likely to attend to depictions person is adversely affecting adolescents’ development
of that product being used and are therefore more likely and whether strangers who might wish to harm young
to report having seen the ad or movie scene. (For in- people, such as sexual predators, could use publicly
stance, if you are shopping for a car, you are probably posted information to establish relationships and initi-
more likely to pay attention to automobile commer- ate sexual contact with them.
cials.) A few studies have tried to overcome this prob-
lem by looking at whether adolescents who live in ❚ Adolescent Development and the Inter-
media markets in which alcohol and tobacco advertis- net Many parents are concerned that the large
ing is more common drink and smoke more (they do), amount of time that teenagers spend on the Internet
but it is hard to determine cause and effect, since it is has had a negative effect on their social development
likely that companies that sell these products spend and relationships. Some worry that electronic interac-
more money advertising their brands in places where tions have replaced face-to-face interactions, to the det-
there are large numbers of people who buy cigarettes riment of the development of social skills. Still others
and alcohol. One problem in linking exposure to adver- worry that adolescents are forming relationships with
tising and the use of alcohol or tobacco is that it is un- strangers and that these may take time away from inti-
ethical to experimentally manipulate exposure in order macy with “real” friends. And, of course, many simply
to see whether increasing the number of ads adoles- worry that the sheer amount of time adolescents spend
cents see leads to an increase in their use of harmful online is unhealthy because it has displaced other, more
substances. valuable activities.
Has adolescents’ extensive use of the Internet af-
fected their development and mental health? Research
on this question suggests that the answer is compli-
cated, and many unanswered questions remain, but it
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
looks as if the effects of Internet use—whether positive
Politicians often argue that the mass media adversely or negative—are much smaller than its proponents or
affects adolescents’ development, and regularly detractors have claimed (Jackson, 2008).
propose legislation that would restrict adolescents’ We can safely assume that, to the extent that time
access to media content on the Internet is displacing time spent in physical
they believe is dangerous. activity, it is having a detrimental impact on adoles-
What do you think about cents’ physical health. The amount of time adolescents
this? What policies, if any, spend watching TV and playing video games is in-
do you support that would versely linked to the amount of time they spend in
affect adolescents’ access to physical activity (Motl, McAuley, Birnbaum, & Lytle,
mass media? 2006). On average, the typical American 15-year-old
spends at least 8 hours each day sitting in front of one
sort of screen or another, but less than an hour in
moderate physical activity (Nader, Bradley, Houts,
McRitchie, & O’Brien, 2008; Rideout et al., 2010).
ELECTRONIC MEDIA AND ADOLESCENT
Health experts are concerned about the amount of
DEVELOPMENT
time teenagers who are on the Internet are sedentary,
The increased availability of electronic communication which is associated with obesity and high blood pres-
has undoubtedly influenced the way that adolescents sure (Goldfield et al., 2011; Mitchell, Pates, Beets, &
socialize. According to recent estimates, 80% of teens Nader, 2013). Most experts believe that the high rate of
who are online (and virtually all adolescents are online) obesity among American teenagers is due, in part, to
use social networking websites, like Facebook. In keep- the large amount of time—estimated in 2009 to be
ing with the general notion that the main purposes of close to 60 hours a week, and rising steadily—young
adolescents’ media use, in addition to communication, people spend watching TV, playing video games, or
are entertainment and information seeking, other sites using the computer. Similar concerns have been raised
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 245 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 245

in China about rising levels of adolescent obesity there


(Parvanta et al., 2010). Keep in mind, though, that
some of the time adolescents spend on the Internet
displaces physical activity but that a fair amount sim-
ply replaces time that would have been spent watching
television (Jackson, 2008).
Researchers have also asked whether adolescents
use the Internet to acquire information that can
improve their health or understanding of the world.
To the extent that the Internet is used by adolescents
to acquire accurate information, it can be a positive
force. This may be especially true with respect to edu-
cating adolescents about healthy behavior, such as safe
sex, in developing countries, where it is often difficult
to reach a large audience of adolescents through class-
rooms (Borzekowski, Fobil, & Asante, 2006). But the
ultimate value of the Internet as an educational tool
depends on the quality and content of the informa-
tion conveyed, as two recent studies of eating disor- Although many adults worry that the increased use of electronic
ders illustrate. Whereas one study found that an communication has had an adverse impact on adolescents’ social
Internet-based intervention was effective in reducing development, this does not seem to be the case.
binge eating (M. Jones et al., 2008) another found
that many adolescents with eating disorders visit
websites that actually promote disordered eating told someone posting mean things to stop (Lenhart
(J. L. Wilson, Peebles, Hardy, & Litt, 2006). Similarly, et al., 2011).
whereas some message boards on the Internet devoted Although many adults worry that adolescents’ on-
to self-injurious behavior, like cutting, can provide line friends will displace the friendships they main-
valuable social support to adolescents who compul- tain in person, this fear appears to be unfounded
sively injure themselves, others may promote the be- (Valkenburg & Peter, 2011). Using a social network
havior and provide instructions on various techniques site does not seem to amount to all that much more
(Whitlock, Powers, & Eckenrode, 2006). One study of than finding an efficient way to stay in touch with
race-related exchanges among adolescents on the friends (Reich, 2010); individuals who, today, spend a
Internet found plenty of examples of both hostile, lot of time on social networking sites were just as so-
racist comments and civil discussions of racial toler- cial when they were teenagers, before such sites ex-
ance (Tynes, 2007). On the other hand, another study isted (Mikami, Szwedo, Allen, Evans, & Hare, 2010).
found that online social networking was used by Indeed, most adolescents use the Internet to commu-
adolescents to help cope with the death of a peer nicate with people they see in person, too. Even
(A. Williams & Merten, 2009). Obviously, it is hard among those adolescents with both online and face-
to generalize about the effects of spending time on- to-face friends, most maintain a balance of contact
line without examining the content of the informa- with each. But for some, things get out of hand, and
tion that is exchanged. they develop what has been termed Internet addic-
Social communication on the Internet, like social tion (Smahel, Brown, & Blinka, 2012). Internet addic-
communication in face-to-face interactions or over tion is defined by six symptoms: salience (being online
the phone, creates both positive and negative expe- is the most important thing in life), mood change
riences. Two-thirds of adolescents report that things (one’s mood fluctuates as a function of Internet expe-
have happened through social networking that have riences), tolerance (needing more and more Internet
made them feel better about themselves, and nearly time to feel satisfied), withdrawal (experiencing nega-
60% say that social networking has made them feel tive feelings when one is prevented from being on-
closer to someone. On the other hand, 25% of ado- line), conflict (the Internet has caused problems in
lescents report that something that happened online
led to a face-to-face argument with someone, and
nearly that percent reported that something online
led to the end of a friendship. Almost all adolescents
report having seen someone post something mean Internet addiction A disorder in which an individual’s use of the
about someone, but 85% have said that they had Internet is pathological, defined by six symptoms: salience, mood
change, tolerance, withdrawal, conflict, and relapse and
reinstatement
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 246 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

246 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

one’s relationships or some other aspect of life), and strangers, but we do not know whether having these
relapse and reinstatement (returning to addictive In- sorts of online relationships leads to or follows from
ternet behavior after one has gotten it under control). maladjustment (Szwedo, Mikami, & Allen, 2011; Wolak,
A certain proportion of teenagers become so addicted Mitchell, & Finkelhor, 2003; Ybarra, Alexander, &
to maintaining their online relationships that they Mitchell, 2005). That is, it is quite plausible that adoles-
may develop what has been nicknamed “Facebook de- cents who have problems simply are more likely to
pression,” which is thought to be the result of spend- seek out relationships with people over the Internet
ing too much time obsessing about one’s online (M. Gould, Munfakh, Lubell, Kleinman, & Parker,
relationships (O’Keefe, Clarke-Pearson, & Council on 2002). Despite the considerable media attention given
Communications and Media, 2011). Frequent instant to the topic, evidence linking psychological problems to
messaging, especially with acquaintances who are not excessive Internet use is inconsistent, with some studies
close friends, can become compulsive and lead to linking Internet addiction to insomnia, depression, so-
feelings of depression (van den Eijnden, Meerkerk, cial isolation, and missed school, but others finding no
Vermulst, Spijkerman, & Engels, 2008). Of course, it is such effects (Jackson, 2008).
not known whether these adolescents were especially
prone to depression or would have developed these ❚ The Internet and Cognitive Develop-
feelings as a result of ruminating about their offline ment Very few studies have looked at the impact of
friendships, and there is some evidence that compul- Internet use on cognitive development, despite hopes
sive Internet users are more introverted, less agree- that adolescents will benefit from having increased ac-
able, and less emotionally stable (van der Aa et al., cess to a world of information and fears that the Inter-
2009). net will distract teenagers from more stimulating
Studies find that more frequent online communi- pursuits, like reading (an odd fear, given the fact that
cation brings friends closer, perhaps because online even before computers were so widespread, reading
communication facilitates self-disclosure, and that outside of school had been pretty much displaced by
communicating over the Internet with friends may television). To date, research suggests that both the
be especially important for socially anxious adoles- hopes and the fears are probably exaggerated. There is
cents, who may find it easier to interact online than no evidence that adolescents’ school performance is ei-
in person (Blais, Craig, Pepler, & Connolly, 2008; ther helped or harmed by Internet use. A few studies
Valkenburg & Peter, 2007, 2009). However, these show that video-game playing may enhance visual
same studies find that spending a lot of time in other skills, reaction time, and hand-eye coordination, how-
online activities leads to lower quality relationships ever (Ivory, 2008; Jackson, 2008).
with one’s close friends, presumably because the ac-
tivities take time away from interacting with them. ❚ Sexual Predators Some parents are anxious
Another factor appears to be where individuals meet about the ways in which the anonymity of the Internet
the people they communicate with online; if their may expose their teenagers to individuals who wish to
online friends are people they know from school or harm them in some way, like sexual predators and cy-
from the neighborhood, their relationships tend to berbullies. As you read in Chapter 5, however, despite
be closer than those with people they met over the the attention devoted to it in the popular media, Inter-
Internet (Mesch & Talmud, 2007). It seems, then, net bullying is a far less prevalent problem than is face-
that the Internet can help strengthen adolescents’ to-face bullying, and most cyberbullying is very mild,
close relationships if it is used to communicate with taking the form of ignoring or disrespecting the victim
one’s existing friends (and in this sense, using the (Jackson, 2008). Is the same true for sexual predators
Internet to stay in touch is probably not all that dif- on the Internet? Has this problem been exaggerated by
ferent from spending time with one’s friends on the media coverage as well? Yes it has.
phone), but has the potential to weaken friendships if According to the most recent Youth Internet Safety
it occupies the adolescent in activities that aren’t Survey, conducted by the U.S. Department of Justice,
shared. about 9% of young people received an unwanted sexual
As with other research in media “effects,” studies of solicitation online in 2010, down from 19% in 2000 and
the Internet have difficulty separating cause and effect, 13% in 2005 (L. Jones, Mitchell, & Finkelhor, 2011a).
and these problems are compounded when the research The 50% decline during this 10-year period could be
is conducted over the Internet, since the samples in due to several factors, including successful efforts to
these studies are bound to be individuals who are on- teach adolescents Internet safety, publicity about the
line more frequently. Some evidence suggests that ado- criminal prosecution of predators, and, most likely, the
lescents with relatively more psychological problems shift in online socializing from public chat rooms to
and poorer family relationships are more likely than social networking sites, which permit adolescents to
their peers to form close online relationships with limit their online contact to people they choose.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 247 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 247

Although adults often express concerns about ado-


lescents using the Internet to view pornography, much
of the pornographic material on the Internet to which
adolescents are exposed is unwanted rather than
actively sought; in 2010, 15% of all adolescents re-
ported receiving unwanted sexual material, although
fewer than half said that the material they had received
material that upset them (L. Jones et al., 2011a). The
incidence of “sexting”—sending sexually explicit pic-
tures over the Internet, usually by smartphone—is
similarly exaggerated in the popular press. According
to systematic surveys, only 1% of adolescents had sent
or appeared in photos that showed naked breasts,
genitals, or buttocks (K. Mitchell, Finkelhor, Jones, &
Wolak, 2012). Evidently, those who are sending
the pictures are sending them to multiple recipients,
though—6% of adolescents reported having received
a sexually explicit photo. (Adolescents who received
such photos rarely distributed them.) Despite adults’
concern about the dangers the Internet poses to ado-
lescents, the fact of the matter is that the vast majority
of teenagers use the Internet in ways that are not only
benign, but similar to their parents: to stay in touch
with friends, to download and enjoy popular entertain-
ment, and to keep up with the world around them.

MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION Adolescent girls who frequently look at fashion magazines report
greater dissatisfaction with their body than girls without as much
Based on the current scientific literature on the exposure to similar images.
impact of Internet use on adolescent development,
what advice would you give parents
about the extent to which they should for women. Interestingly, adolescent girls who fre-
regulate their teen-agers’ Internet use? quently read fashion magazines are more dissatisfied
Would you place restrictions on the with their body than are girls who do not, and con-
number of hours they are allowed to trolled experiments have indicated that showing girls
spend online? On the sites they are images of thin models increases their body dissatisfac-
allowed to visit? tion (M. Levine & Harrison, 2004; D. Roberts et al.,
2009). Moreover, frequent reading of magazine articles
about dieting or weight loss leads to increases in un-
healthy weight control behaviors, such as intentional
vomiting and inappropriate use of laxatives (van den
MASS MEDIA AND ADOLESCENT
Berg, Neumark-Sztainer, Hannan, & Haines, 2007). Al-
GIRLS’ BODY IMAGE
though very few studies have examined the media’s im-
Several commentators have raised concerns about the pact on males’ body image, one study found that boys
messages to which young women are exposed in maga- and men were more dissatisfied with their body after
zines aimed at adolescent girls, especially in light of seeing advertising featuring muscular men (Agliata &
evidence (discussed in Chapter 1) of widespread body Tantleff-Dunn, 2004).
dissatisfaction among adolescent females. The majority Similar results are reported in studies of girls’ re-
of articles in these magazines focus on dating and het- sponses to appearance-related commercials on televi-
erosexual relationships, and most emphasize the im- sion, which, like ads in fashion magazines, typically
portance of physical attractiveness and thinness, in feature thin models (Harrison & Hefner, 2008). Ironi-
particular, for young women. According to researchers, cally, the same media that implicitly encourage adoles-
the articles and advertisements contained in such mag- cent girls to be thin and adolescent boys to be muscular
azines convey a clear message that attracting males by devote considerable time and resources to encouraging
being physically beautiful is the road to true happiness adolescents to eat, and to eat unhealthy food, at that!
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 248 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

248 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

More than one-quarter of the television advertisements potential for exponential growth in the message’s expo-
seen by American adolescents are for food, beverages, sure and influence”: “Like viruses, such strategies take
or restaurants, with ads for candy, snacks, cereals, and advantage of rapid multiplication to explode the mes-
fast food among the most frequent (J. Brown & sage to thousands, to millions” (R. Wilson, 2005). Be-
Bobkowski, 2011b). cause adolescents are likely to use technologies that can
easily and instantly connect them to thousands of other
teenagers, using young people to spread the word about
THE ADOLESCENT CONSUMER
new products is a highly effective marketing strategy
A final, and very important, aspect of adolescent media (Calvert, 2008).
use is economic. The size of the adolescent population, The influence of the adolescent market extends well
the prevalence of student employment, and the fact beyond the youth cohort, however—as evidenced by
that adolescents save less than any other age group the uncanny predictability with which adult tastes in
make young people an attractive target for a variety of clothing and music often follow those of teenagers, al-
businesses (Osgerby, 2008). Teenagers spend more beit in a toned-down fashion. Teenagers often have
than $90 billion per year, and the total amount of considerable influence over their parents’ purchases,
money spent by and for adolescents between 12 and 17 which gives added incentive for advertisers to market
exceeds $200 billion annually (Packaged Facts, 2007). products with young people’s tastes in mind (Zollo,
Although there has been a worldwide recession since 2004).
2008, 75% of teenagers report that they are receiving as
much or more spending money than they did the previ-
ous year (Dolliver, 2010). Not surprisingly, much of
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
adolescents’ money is spent on discretionary purchases
related to leisure activities. And, unfortunately, many Have you been the recipient of viral
adolescents spend money on alcohol and cigarettes— marketing? What product was marketed,
according to one estimate, underage drinkers account and how did you find out about it?
for about one-sixth of the $100 billion dollars spent
each year on alcohol (Horner, Jamieson, & Romer,
2008).
We can certainly debate the merits of cultivating
such strong consumer urges among the young and im- RECAP
pressionable. Some critics contend that advertising • Today’s adolescents live in a media-saturated envi-
aimed at teenagers takes advantage of the fact that they ronment, with the average adolescent exposed to
are more impulsive and self-conscious than adults (see the mass media about 8 hours each day. If media
Pechmann, Levine, Loughlin, & Leslie, 2005). On the multitasking is factored into the equation, daily
other hand, America’s economy is driven by consumer exposure is close to 11 hours.
spending, and the segments of our economy that are • One of the most significant changes in recent years
devoted to leisure and recreation depend on the adoles- has been in the amount of time adolescents spend
cent market. Among the most important industries are each day on the Internet. The most frequent Inter-
those connected with movies, music, sports, and televi- net activities reported by teenagers are playing
sion. Teenagers make up a large, and therefore influen- video games and communicating with friends, via
tial, segment of the consumers of these products—as a texting and social networking websites.
glance at the local movie listings or a few minutes of • Theories of media influence and use examine three
channel-surfing will readily attest. different processes: the influence of the media on
Supporters of adolescent consumerism also have adolescents’ attitudes, knowledge, and behavior;
been quick to point out the strong influence that teenag- the fact that adolescents choose the media that
ers have on each other when it comes to purchases—and they are exposed to; and the fact that adolescents
new ways of tapping into this influence that take advan- interpret the media that they are exposed to.
tage of the Internet and other new technologies, referred • Many observers of the adolescent scene are con-
to as viral marketing, have been exploited by businesses cerned about the high level of adolescents’ expo-
wishing to expand their share of the teen consumer sure to messages about sex, violence, and drug use
market. “Viral marketing” (sometimes called “stealth through the mass media. Some studies have shown
marketing”) refers to a strategy that encourages people that exposure to media violence does in fact lead
to pass on a marketing message to others, “creating the to more aggressive behavior (although not to seri-
ous violence) and that exposure to images of drug
and alcohol use and sex affects adolescents’ atti-
viral marketing A way of promoting products or services by tudes and beliefs about these matters. Other
encouraging individuals to pass information on to others.
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 249 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 7 Work, Leisure, and Media 249

research questions the direction of causality, and This mixed view of adolescents’ free time reflects an
suggests that adolescents determine their media interesting paradox about the nature of adolescence in
exposure on the basis of preexisting preferences modern society. Because industrialized society has
and inclinations. “given” adolescents a good deal of free time, adults ex-
• Adolescents’ use of the Internet has increased pect them to use it productively. But by definition, free
substantially in recent years, for communication, time is supposed to be time that can be used for pur-
entertainment, and information. Many claims in the poses other than being productive. As you will read in
popular press about the toll the Internet is taking the next chapter, some theorists of adolescence believe
on teenagers’ lives have not been supported by that the existence of large blocks of uncommitted time
scientific study. Rates of Internet harassment, is one feature of adolescence in modern society that has
sexual predation by strangers, and Internet addic- the potential to contribute in positive ways to young
tion are very low. Moreover, the notion that social- people’s development; one potential benefit of partici-
izing over the Internet is taking time away from pation in leisure activities is that this may help adoles-
“real” relationships does not seem to be true. cents explore different sides of themselves (Barber,
• Adolescents wield enormous power as consumers, Stone, Hunt, & Eccles, 2005).
both because they have a great deal of discretion- Nevertheless, many misconceptions about the pros
ary income of their own and because and cons of various uses of free time abound. Most
they influence how their adults view participation in structured extracurricular
families spend activities as a good thing, and this seems to be the case.
money. But most people are equally sure that working is good
for teenagers, even though studies show that the costs
of intensive involvement in part-time work during the
school year may outweigh the benefits. And, although
adults believe that the mass media have a negative effect
on adolescents’ behavior, studies show that it is more
likely that adolescents’ interests affect their media use
Free Time and Adolescent than the reverse.
Development The impact of the mass media on adolescent devel-
opment has become especially controversial as the role
Adults have mixed feelings about adolescents’ activities of technology in adolescents’ lives has expanded. Most
outside of school. On the positive side, adults take pride adults, especially parents, are absolutely certain that
in watching their children’s sports teams and creative nothing good comes from adolescents’ exposure to tele-
activities, and they believe that these productive uses of vision, movies, video games, popular music, or the In-
leisure time help build character and teach important ternet, and they often blame the mass media for a wide
skills, such as teamwork and perseverance. (Think array of adolescents’ problems—despite the fact that
about all the movies you have seen about the character- parents themselves exert a far greater influence on ado-
building benefits of team sports.) Similarly, most adults lescent development than do any of the media about
view holding a part-time job as a worthwhile activity which they are often so alarmed. Moreover, as you now
that provides opportunities for learning and for the de- know, because adolescents choose the media to which
velopment of a sense of responsibility. they are exposed, it is very difficult to demonstrate that
On the other hand, adults view many adolescent lei- adolescents are actually affected by what they see and
sure activities as wasted time or, worse, as preludes to hear. This is not to say that the media have no impact
trouble: They worry about groups of teenagers cruising on adolescents’ behavior and well-being, but it is to
the mall; they poke fun at groups of girls sequestered in suggest that we should be careful not to confuse cause
front of a mirror trying on an array of cosmetics; they and effect. It is also important to keep in mind that the
cringe at images of adolescents riveted to their smart- mass media can be used to promote positive behavior
phones; and they worry about adolescents’ exposure to and healthy development, to provide information
sex and violence on television, in film, in music, and on about a rapidly changing world, and to facilitate com-
the Internet. Although we might wish for the “good old munication with others.
days” before the advent of smartphones, Facebook, sat- By valuing adolescents’ free time only when it is
ellite television, and laptops, those good old days are used productively, adults may misunderstand the im-
long gone. And keep in mind that even during those portant functions that leisure time serves in the psy-
supposed good old days, adults worried about how chosocial development of young people. For example,
adolescents spent their idle time and about the corrupt- studies show that a moderate amount of solitude (dur-
ing influence of such evils as rock ’n’ roll, dime-store ing which daydreaming is a central activity) is posi-
romances, and comic books. tively related to high school students’ psychological
ste32118_ch07_218-250.indd Page 250 21/03/13 11:13 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

250 PART 2 The Contexts of Adolescence

well-being (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1984; Larson, • Many misconceptions about the pros and cons of
1997). Free time plays an important role in helping various uses of free time abound. Some of the
young people develop a sense of themselves, explore activities that adults believe are uniformly good for
their relationships with each other, and learn about the adolescents, like employment, have more costs
society around them (Coatsworth et al., 2005). And, for than benefits. Other activities that adults believe
better or for worse, the mass media are globalizing ado- can be a waste of time, or even bad for adoles-
lescence, contributing to the development of a com- cents’ development, like watching television,
mon culture that gives adolescents all over the world playing video games, and browsing the Internet, are
much to share. probably not as bad for teenagers as they are often
assumed to be.
RECAP • Leisure plays an important role in helping young
people develop a sense of themselves, explore
• Adults’ ambivalence about adolescents’ free time
their relationships with each other,
reflects an interesting paradox about the nature
and learn about the soci-
of adolescence in modern society. Because
ety around them.
industrialized society has “given” adolescents a
good deal of free time, adults expect them to use
it productively. But by definition, free time is sup-
posed to be time that can be used for purposes
other than being productive.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 251 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

PART 3

8 ❘ Identity 9 ❘ Autonomy 10 ❘ Intimacy


11 ❘ Sexuality 12 ❘ Achievement 13 ❘ Psychosocial Problems

Psychosocial Development During


Adolescence
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 252 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 253 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
8
Identity
IDENTITY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE
CHANGES IN SELF-CONCEPTIONS
Changes in the Content and Structure of Self-Conceptions
Dimensions of Personality in Adolescence
CHANGES IN SELF-ESTEEM
Stability and Changes in Self-Esteem
Group Differences in Self-Esteem
Antecedents and Consequences of High Self-Esteem
THE ADOLESCENT IDENTITY CRISIS
Erikson’s Theoretical Framework
Identity Versus Identity Diffusion
The Social Context of Identity Development
Resolving the Identity Crisis
Problems in Identity Development
RESEARCH ON IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
Determining an Adolescent’s Identity Status
Studying Identity Development Over Time
THE DEVELOPMENT OF ETHNIC IDENTITY
The Process of Ethnic Identity Development
Discrimination and Its Effects
Multiethnic Adolescents
GENDER-ROLE DEVELOPMENT
Gender-Role Socialization During Adolescence
Masculinity and Femininity

253
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 254 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

254 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

IN THIS CHAPTER, WE EXAMINE why adolescence is a and intolerant if I don’t like how they’re acting. I’d like to
time of major changes in identity, why individuals differ be friendly and tolerant all of the time, that’s the kind of
person I want to be, and I’m disappointed when I’m not.
in their patterns of identity development, and how the
At school, I’m serious, even studious every now and then,
course of adolescent identity development is shaped by
but on the other hand I’m a goof-off too, because if
the nature of life in contemporary society. Because you’re too studious, you won’t be popular. So I go back
changes take place during adolescence in the ways peo- and forth, which means I don’t do all that well in terms of
ple view and feel about themselves, the study of iden- my grades. But that causes problems at home, where I’m
tity development has been a major focus of research pretty anxious when I’m around my parents. They expect
me to get all A’s, and get pretty annoyed with me when
and theory on adolescents (Harter, 2011). For some young
report cards come out. I care what they think about me,
people, adolescence is a time of identity crisis. For other
and so then I get down on myself, but it’s not fair! I mean,
adolescents, though, this period is one of more gradual I worry about how I probably should get better grades,
and more subtle changes. Here’s how one girl responded but I’d be mortified in the eyes of my friends if I did too
when asked to describe herself: well. So I’m usually pretty stressed out at home, or
sarcastic, since my parents are always on my case. But I
What am I like as a person? You’re probably not going to really don’t understand how I can switch so fast. I mean,
understand. I’m complicated! With my really close how can I be cheerful with my friends, then coming
friends, I am very tolerant. I mean, I’m understanding and home and feeling anxious, and then getting frustrated
caring. With a group of friends, I’m rowdier. I’m also usu- and sarcastic with my parents. Which one is the real me?
ally friendly and cheerful but I can get pretty obnoxious (15-year-old girl, quoted in Harter, 2011, p. 311)

Identity as an asked to describe themselves, the older children provide


a far more complex self-portrait. Whereas young chil-
Adolescent Issue dren restrict their descriptions to lists of what they own
or what they like to do, older children are more likely to
Changes in the way we view and feel about ourselves
tell you about their personality as well.
occur throughout the life cycle. You have probably
If changes in identity occur throughout the life cycle,
heard and read about the so-called midlife crisis—an
why have researchers who are interested in identity de-
identity crisis thought to occur during middle age. And
velopment paid so much attention to adolescence? One
certainly, important changes in self-conceptions and in
reason is that the changes in identity that take place
self-image take place throughout childhood. When a
during adolescence involve the first substantial reorga-
group of 4-year-olds and a group of 10-year-olds are
nization and restructuring of the individual’s sense of
self at a time when he or she has the intellectual capa-
bility to appreciate fully just how significant the changes
are. Although important changes in identity certainly
occur during childhood, adolescents are far more self-
conscious about these changes and feel them much
more acutely.

❚ Puberty and Identity Development Another


reason for the attention that researchers and theorists
have given the study of identity development during
adolescence concerns the fundamental biological, cog-
nitive, and social changes characteristic of the period.
Puberty, as we saw in Chapter 1, brings with it dramatic
changes in physical appearance and alters the adoles-
cent’s self-conceptions and relationships with others. It
is not hard to see why puberty plays an important role
in provoking identity development during adolescence.
When you change the way you look—for example,
Adolescence is often a time when individuals ask questions about
when you have your hair colored or cut in a different
who they are and where they are headed. Concerns with physical way, lose a great deal of weight, or dramatically change
appearance often intensify. how you dress—you sometimes feel as though your
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 255 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 255

personality has changed, too. During puberty, when important to me?” “What kind of person would I really
adolescents are changing so dramatically on the out- like to be?” Questions about the future, which inevita-
side, they understandably have questions about changes bly arise as the adolescent prepares for adulthood, raise
that are taking place on the inside. For the adolescent, questions about identity (Côté, 2009).
undergoing the physical changes of puberty may Identity development is complex and multifaceted.
prompt fluctuations in self-image and a reevaluation of Actually, it is better understood as a series of interrelated
who he or she really is. developments—rather than one single development—
that all involve changes in the way we view ourselves in
❚ Cognitive Change and Identity Development relation to others and in relation to the broader society
Just as the broadening of intellectual capabilities during in which we live. Generally, researchers and theorists
early adolescence provides new ways of thinking about have taken three different approaches to the question of
problems, values, and interpersonal relationships, it how the individual’s sense of identity changes over the
also permits adolescents to think about themselves in course of adolescence.
new ways. We saw in Chapter 2 that it is not until ado- The first approach emphasizes changes in self-
lescence that people are able to think in systematic ways conceptions—the traits and attributes individuals see
about hypothetical and future events. This is mani- in themselves. A second approach focuses on adoles-
fested in two specific ways that have implications for cents’ self-esteem, or self-image—how positively or
identity development. First, adolescents become much negatively they feel about themselves. Finally, a third ap-
more able to imagine their possible selves—the various proach emphasizes changes in the sense of identity—
alternative identities that they may adopt (Markus & the sense of who one is, where one has come from, and
Nurius, 1986). This may be related to the heightened where one is going.
self-consciousness characteristic of early adolescence
discussed in Chapter 2. Indeed, brain-imaging studies
find that patterns of brain activity during tasks in which RECAP
individuals are asked to think about themselves differ • Although changes in the way we see and feel about
significantly between adolescents and adults (Burnett et ourselves occur throughout the life cycle, the study
al., 2011; Pfeifer & Blakemore, 2012; Sebastian, Burnett, of identity development has been a prominent
& Blakemore, 2008). issue in the field of adolescent development in
Second is an impressive increase in future orienta- particular.
tion—the ability and tendency to consider the long- • One reason for this attention concerns the impact
term consequences of one’s decisions and imagine what that the biological, cognitive, and social role
one’s life might be like in the years to come (Nurmi, changes of adolescence have on the young person’s
2004; Steinberg et al., 2009). For this reason, it is not ability to engage in self-examination and on his or
until adolescence that individuals typically begin to her interest in doing so.
wonder, “Who will I become?” or “What am I really • Researchers have traditionally distinguished among
like?” Because the preadolescent child’s thinking is con- three aspects of identity development in adoles-
crete, it is difficult for him or her to think seriously cence: changes in self-conceptions,
about being a different person. But the changes in changes in self-esteem, and
thinking that take place during adolescence open up a changes in the sense
whole new world of alternatives. of identity.

❚ Social Roles and Identity Development Finally,


as we saw in Chapter 3, the changes in social roles that
occur at adolescence open up a new array of choices
and decisions that were not concerns previously. In possible selves The various identities an adolescent might imagine
contemporary society, adolescence is a time of impor- for him- or herself.
tant decisions about school, work, relationships, and
the future. Facing these decisions about their place in future orientation The extent to which an individual is able and
society does more than provoke adolescents to ask inclined to think about the potential consequences of decisions and
choices.
questions about who they are and where they are
headed—it necessitates the questions. At this point in self-conceptions The collection of traits and attributes that
the life cycle, young people must make important individuals use to describe or characterize themselves.
choices about their education and their commitments
self-esteem The degree to which individuals feel positively or
to other people, and thinking about these questions negatively about themselves.
prompts them to ask more questions about themselves:
“What do I really want out of life?” “What things are sense of identity The extent to which individuals feel secure
about who they are and who they are becoming.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 256 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

256 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Changes in Self-Conceptions
As individuals mature intellectually and undergo the
sorts of cognitive changes described in Chapter 2,
they come to conceive of themselves in more sophisti-
cated and more differentiated ways. As we saw in that
chapter, adolescents are much more capable than chil-
dren of thinking about abstract concepts and consid-
erably more proficient in processing large amounts of
information. These intellectual capabilities affect the
way in which individuals characterize themselves.
Compared with children, who tend to describe them-
selves in relatively simple, concrete terms, adolescents
are more likely to employ complex, abstract, and psy-
chological self-characterizations (Harter, 2011). In
addition, with development comes greater consis-
tency between how individuals describe themselves
and how they actually behave (Davis-Kean et al.,
2008). There is also evidence that adolescents’ ideas
about the sort of person they would like to be (their
“ideal self ”) become more stable over time (Zentner Despite the stereotype of adolescence as a time of psychological
& Renaud, 2007). difficulty, most adolescents have positive self-conceptions.

CHANGES IN THE CONTENT AND people. Instead of saying, “I am shy” or “I am outgoing,”


STRUCTURE OF SELF-CONCEPTIONS an adolescent might say something more complicated,
such as “People don’t think I’m shy, but most of the
Self-conceptions change in structure and content dur- time, I’m really nervous about meeting other kids for
ing the transition from childhood into and through the first time.” Adolescents also recognize that they may
adolescence. Structurally, self-conceptions become come across differently to different people, another
more differentiated and better organized (Byrne & type of differentiation in self-conceptions that does not
Shavelson, 1996). Let’s first consider the idea that self- appear until this point in time—for example, “My par-
conceptions become more differentiated. ents think I’m quiet, but my friends know I really like to
party a lot.” Neuroimaging studies show that adoles-
❚ Differentiation of the Self-Concept In an- cents’ self-conceptions may be particularly sensitive to
swer to the question “Who am I?” adolescents are more the opinions of others (Pfeifer et al., 2009).
likely than children to link traits and attributes that
describe themselves to specific situations, rather than ❚ Organization and Integration of the Self-
using them as global characterizations. Whereas a pre- Concept With this shift toward increased differen-
adolescent might say “I am nice” or “I am friendly,” but tiation in self-conceptions comes better organization
not specify when or under what conditions, an adoles- and integration (Harter, 2011). When children are
cent is more likely to say “I am nice if I am in a good asked to describe themselves, the traits and attributes
mood” or “I am friendly when I am with people I have they list remain somewhat disorganized. Adolescents,
met before.” The realization that their personality is in contrast, are likely to organize and integrate different
expressed in different ways in different situations is one aspects of their self-concept into a more logical, coher-
example of the increased differentiation that character- ent whole. Whereas a younger child may list a sequence
izes the self-conceptions of youngsters as they mature of several traits that appear to be contradictory (“I am
toward adulthood. friendly, and I am shy”), an adolescent will attempt to
There is another way in which self-conceptions be- organize apparently discrepant bits of information into
come more differentiated at adolescence. As opposed to more highly organized statements (“I am shy when I
characterizations provided by children, adolescents’ first meet people, but after I get to know them, I’m usu-
self-descriptions take into account who is doing the ally pretty friendly”).
describing (Harter, 2011). Teenagers distinguish be- Self-conceptions continue to become more psycho-
tween their own opinions of themselves and the views logical well into the high school years. The increased
of others. Suppose you ask a group of youngsters to psychological complexity of self-conceptions may pre-
describe how they behave when they are with other sent some difficulties, though, when adolescents become
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 257 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 257

able to recognize—but not yet quite understand or romantic and dating situations and with classmates,
reconcile—inconsistencies and contradictions in their and they are least likely to put on a false front with close
personality. The proportion of adolescents who give friends. Interestingly, false-self behavior—acting in a
opposite traits in self-descriptions, who feel conflicts way that one knows is inauthentic—occurs less often
over such discrepancies, and who feel confused over with parents than with dates, but more often with par-
such discrepancies increases markedly between 7th and ents than with close friends (Harter, 2011). Although
9th grades and then declines somewhat (Harter & adolescents sometimes say that they dislike false-self
Monsour, 1992). In one study, when asked to reflect on behavior, they also say that sometimes it is acceptable,
contradictions in their personalities, early, middle, and such as when trying to impress another person or hide
late adolescents responded in very different ways, as the an aspect of their personality that others do not like.
following examples illustrate (Harter, 1990, p. 358): You can easily imagine how the ability to put on a false
front would come in handy when meeting someone for
I guess I just think about one thing about myself at a
time and don’t think about the other until the next day.
the first time, for example.
(11–12 years old) Adolescents differ in the degree to which they present
false fronts and in their reasons for doing so. In general,
I really think I am a happy person and I want to be that
adolescents who report less emotional support from
way with everyone, but I get depressed with my family
and it really bugs me because that’s not what I want to be
parents and peers, who have low self-esteem, and who
like. (14–15 years old) are relatively more depressed and hopeless than their
peers are more likely to engage in false-self behavior
You can be shy on a date, and then outgoing with friends
(Impett, Sorsoli, Schooler, Henson, & Tolman, 2008).
because you are just different with different people; you
can’t always be the same person and probably shouldn’t
The connection between false-self behavior and low
be. (17–18 years old) self-esteem runs in both directions; some adolescents
engage in false-self behavior because they are low in self-
Although the recognition that one’s personality is esteem, whereas others experience a drop in self-esteem
multifaceted—even contradictory—may initially cause because they knowingly put on a false front. Depression
some distress, it has a number of advantages in the and hopelessness are highest among adolescents who
long run. Indeed, some psychologists have suggested engage in false-self behavior because they genuinely
that the development of a more complicated view of devalue their true self, in contrast to those who put on
the self is one way that individuals cope with the recog- a false front because they want to please others or be-
nition of their faults and weaknesses, a recognition that cause they are experimenting with different personali-
comes with the increased self-awareness of adolescence ties (Harter, Marold, Whitesell, & Cobbs, 1996).
(“I’m not really a nasty person, I just act mean when
people tease me”). Consistent with this, adolescents
who have more complex self-conceptions are less likely
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
to be depressed (A. Jordan & Cole, 1996).
One additional advantage of having a more differen- When was the last time you put on a false
tiated self-concept is that the adolescent is now able to self? What was your motivation? How did
distinguish among his or her actual self (who the ado- you feel afterward?
lescent really is), ideal self (who the adolescent would
like to be), and feared self (who the adolescent most
dreads becoming). An important aspect of having a
healthy self-concept is having an ideal self to balance a Understanding how self-conceptions change during
feared self. One study found, for example, that delin- adolescence helps to explain why issues of identity
quent adolescents were less likely than nondelinquent begin to take on so much importance at this stage in the
youth to have this sort of balanced view; although de- life span. As self-conceptions become more abstract,
linquent adolescents might dread becoming criminals, and as young people become more able to see them-
for instance, they may not have a positive ideal self (for selves in psychological terms, they become more
example, being successfully employed) to balance this interested in understanding their own personalities and
fear (Oyserman & Markus, 1990). why they behave the way they do. The distress caused by
recognizing one’s inconsistencies may spur identity
❚ False-Self Behavior Another interesting conse- development. You may recall having wondered as a
quence of adolescents’ recognition that they are not
always consistent in their personality concerns their
ability to distinguish between their true and false selves
(that is, their authentic and inauthentic selves). Adoles-
cents are most likely to behave inauthentically in false-self behavior Behavior that intentionally presents a false
impression to others.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 258 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

258 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

teenager about your personality development, the in part because we tend to spend time in environments
influences that shaped your character, and how your that reward and reinforce the traits that draw us to these
personality had changed over time: “Am I more like my settings (B. Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000). As a result, we
father or like my mother? Why do my sister and I seem become more like ourselves every day! There is also evi-
so different? Will I always be so nervous?” Although these dence that, between adolescence and young adulthood,
sorts of questions may seem commonplace to you now, individuals on average become more extraverted, more
in all likelihood, you did not think about these things conscientious, more agreeable, more resilient, and more
until adolescence, when your own self-conceptions be- emotionally stable (Huntsinger & Jose, 2006; Meeus,
came more abstract and more sophisticated. Van de Schoot, Klimstra, & Branje, 2011; B. Roberts,
Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006). There also is evidence
that girls mature earlier than boys, but that boys catch
DIMENSIONS OF PERSONALITY
IN ADOLESCENCE
up over time, so that by the end of adolescence, there are
no gender differences in maturity (Klimstra, Hale, Raaij-
While many researchers have studied adolescent per- makers, Branje, & Meeus, 2009).
sonality development by examining young people’s In sum, a good deal of evidence indicates that many
self-conceptions, others have used standardized inven- core personality traits, such as impulsivity or timidity,
tories designed to assess the most important aspects of are quite stable between childhood and adolescence and
personality. Most researchers now approach the study between adolescence and young adulthood (McCrae et
of personality using the five-factor model (McCrae & al., 2002; B. Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2001; van Aken,
John, 1992). This model is based on the observation Hutteman, & Denissen, 2011). Extraversion appears to
that there are five critical personality dimensions, often be especially stable over time (Hampson & Goldberg,
referred to as the “big five”: extraversion (how outgoing 2006). Although the external manifestations of these
and energetic someone is), agreeableness (how kind or traits may change with age (for example, anxiety may
sympathetic), conscientiousness (how responsible and appear as bed-wetting in early childhood but as ner-
organized), neuroticism (how anxious or tense), and vous talkativeness in adolescence), our basic, underly-
openness to experience (how curious and imaginative). ing traits turn out to be remarkably stable. For example,
Although the five-factor model was developed through individuals who displayed relatively higher levels of
research on adults, it has been successfully applied aggression in preadolescence, temper tantrums during
to adolescents as well (Caspi, 1997; McCrae et al., 2002). childhood, or negative emotions during infancy are
For example, delinquent adolescents are more likely more likely to behave aggressively as adolescents (Caspi,
than their peers to score high in extraversion and low in Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995; Hart, Hofman,
agreeableness and conscientiousness, whereas adoles- Edelstein, & Keller, 1997). Similarly, individuals who
cents who are high achievers in school score high in had difficulty controlling their impulses as preschoolers
conscientiousness and openness (John, Caspi, Robins, are more likely to be impulsive, aggressive, and danger
Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1994). In general, the seeking as adolescents and young adults, whereas indi-
structure of personality appears comparable across viduals who were inhibited as young children tend to be
groups of adolescents from different ethnic back- relatively more timid, anxious, and shy as teenagers.
grounds (Markstrom-Adams & Adams, 1995; Rowe, Not surprisingly, then, individuals who are judged to be
Vazsonyi, & Flannery, 1994). well-adjusted in early and middle childhood tend to be
There are both genetic and environmental influences resilient and competentin adolescence (Gest, 1997;
on individual differences in personality. Individuals may Hart et al., 1997; Shiner, Masten, & Tellegen, 2002).
inherit temperamental predispositions (such as a high Despite popular stereotypes about adolescence as a
activity level or an inclination to be sociable), which are time of “rebirth,” research has not supported the view
observable early in life, and these predispositions may that adolescence is a time of tumultuous upheaval in
“harden” and become organized into personality traits personality.
partially in response to the environment (Caspi, 2000;
Gest, 1997; John et al., 1994). Thus, an active and socia-
ble child who enjoys interacting with others may be re-
warded for doing so and, over time, become extraverted. MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
Longitudinal studies show that both temperament and
Do you see elements of yourself that have
personality become increasingly stable as we grow older,
always been present in your personality?
Thinking about yourself, would you agree
with the statement “The person who enters
adolescence is basically the same as that
five-factor model The theory that there are five basic dimensions
who exits it”?
to personality: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness,
neuroticism, and openness to experience.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 259 21/03/13 10:30 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 259

RECAP STABILITY AND CHANGES


• During the transition into adolescence, self- IN SELF-ESTEEM
conceptions become increasingly complex, As you read in Chapter 2, the stability of a trait (like in-
abstract, and psychological. telligence or self-esteem) has nothing to do with the de-
• Adolescents’ self-conceptions are more differenti- gree to which people change with age, because stability
ated and better organized than those of children. merely refers to the extent to which individuals’ relative
• Although having more complex self-conceptions standing stays more or less the same over time. Height,
may be unsettling to adolescents initially, it ulti- for instance, is a stable trait (tall children tend to become
mately permits them to have a more sophisticated tall adults) that nevertheless changes a great deal with
and more accurate view of themselves. age (individuals grow taller between childhood and
• Adolescents are able to distinguish between their adulthood). Asking whether self-esteem changes during
actual and their imagined selves and between their adolescence (that is, whether on average people’s view of
authentic and false selves. themselves becomes more positive or negative) is not
• There are five basic personality dimensions in ado- the same as asking whether self-esteem is stable during
lescence and adulthood, often referred to as the this period (that is, whether individuals with high self-
big five: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientious- esteem as children are likely to have high self-esteem as
ness, neuroticism, and openness to experience. adolescents). In general, self-esteem tends to become
• Longitudinal studies show strong links between increasingly more stable between childhood and early
early temperament and adolescent per- adulthood, suggesting that adolescents’ feelings about
sonality, and stability in basic themselves gradually consolidate over time, becoming
personality traits less likely to fluctuate in response to different experi-
over time. ences (Erol & Orth, 2011; Trzesniewski, Donnellan, &
Robins, 2003) (see Figure 8.1). Along similar lines, day-
to-day fluctuations in mood tend to become smaller be-
tween early adolescence and late adolescence (Larson,
Moneta, Richards, & Wilson, 2002).

Changes in Self-Esteem
One of the manifestations assumed to result from the MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
“storm and stress” of adolescence involves problems in
The pattern of change in self-esteem
adolescents’ self-esteem—how they evaluate them-
over time shown in Figure 8.1 comes
selves. As you will read, although there is no dramatic
from a random sample of over
drop in self-esteem in adolescence, adolescents’ feelings
7,000 American youth. Do you think
about themselves fluctuate from day to day, particularly
this pattern would be different in
during the early adolescent years. From about 8th grade
other cultures? Why?
on, however, self-esteem remains highly stable. And, de-
spite commentary in the popular press about the exces-
sively high self-esteem characteristic of today’s teenagers,
careful statistical analyses show that there has been no Studies of changes in self-esteem as individuals move
appreciable increase in American adolescents’ self- through adolescence have not yielded consistent find-
esteem during the past several decades (Trzesniewski & ings, partly because researchers have focused on differ-
Donnellan, 2009). ent aspects of individuals’ self-image. As a consequence,

0.5 FIGURE 8.1 Self-esteem increases during ado-


lescence but begins to level off as individuals
0.25 enter young adulthood. (Erol & Orth, 2011)
Self-esteem

-0.25

-0.5
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30
Age (years)
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 260 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

260 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

1.40 1.6
1.30 1.4
Girls
1.20
Average affect

Average affect
1.2
1.10
1
1.00
Boys
0.8
0.90
0.80 0.6

0.70 0.4
5th 6th 7th 8th 9th 10th 11th 12th 5th 6th 7th 8th 9th 10th 11th 12th
Grade Grade
FIGURE 8.2 On average, individuals become less positive in their mood between 5th and 9th grades. Around 10th grade,
however, this decline in mood starts to level off. (Larson et al., 2002)

some studies find that individuals’ feelings about them- have a more unstable self-image. Generally, the differ-
selves become more negative over the course of adoles- ences between preadolescents and early adolescents
cence (e.g., Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, are greater than those between younger and older ado-
2002), but others find that they become more positive lescents, which indicates that the most marked fluctu-
(D. Cole et al., 2001). In general, however, changes in ations in self-image occur during the transition into
self-perceptions (whether positive or negative) are adolescence, rather than over the course of adoles-
greater during early adolescence than during middle or cence itself (Thomaes, Poorthuis, & Nelemans, 2011).
late adolescence; from middle adolescence through Consistent with this, a study of poor, urban students
young adulthood, self-esteem either remains at about in New York City, Baltimore, and Washington, DC,
the same level or increases (Côté, 2009). Although there found that the transition from elementary into junior
is a general trend for individuals’ average mood to be- high school was more likely to disrupt adolescents’
come less positive over the course of adolescence (that self-esteem than was the transition into high school
is, on average, children are usually in a more positive (Seidman & French, 2004).
mood than young adolescents, who are generally in a
better mood than older adolescents), this trend begins
to level off around 10th grade (Larson et al., 2002) (see MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
Figure 8.2). What do you think makes self-esteem
Although adolescence is not a time of storm and more volatile in some individuals than
stress, some evidence suggests that minor problems in others? Do you think that the contribu-
self-image may arise for a brief period during early ad- tors to high self-esteem are the same as
olescence. To fully understand why, it is necessary to the contributors to stable self-esteem?
distinguish among three aspects of adolescents’ self-
image: their self-esteem (how positively or negatively
they feel about themselves), their self-consciousness
(how much they worry about their self-image), and
their self-image stability (how much their self-image Oddly enough, the extent to which an individual’s
changes from day to day) (Simmons, Rosenberg, & self-esteem is volatile is itself a fairly stable trait; that is,
Rosenberg, 1973). young adolescents whose self-image fluctuates a lot
Fluctuations in adolescents’ self-image are most from moment to moment are likely to develop into
likely to occur between the ages of 12 and 14. Com- older adolescents who experience the same thing (Savin-
pared with older adolescents (15 years and older) and Williams & Demo, 1983). Young adolescents with the
with preadolescents (8–11 years old), early adolescents most volatile self-image report the highest levels of anx-
have lower self-esteem, are more self-conscious, and iety, tension, and adjustment problems (Molloy, Ram, &
Gest, 2011; Rosenberg, 1986). This is especially likely
among adolescents who have a great deal of stress in
their day-to-day lives (Tevendale, DuBois, Lopez, &
self-consciousness The degree to which an individual is
preoccupied with his or her self-image. Prindiville, 1997). In other words, having a volatile self-
image may make individuals especially vulnerable to the
self-image stability The degree to which an individual’s self- effects of stress.
image changes from day to day.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 261 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 261

Fluctuations in self-image during early adoles-


cence probably are due to several interrelated factors.
First, the sort of egocentrism that is common in early
adolescence, discussed in Chapter 2, may make young
adolescents painfully aware of others’ reactions to
their behavior. Second, as individuals become more
socially active, they begin to learn that people play
games when they interact, and they learn that it is not
always possible to tell what people are thinking on the
basis of how they act or what they say. This ambiguity
may leave young adolescents—who are relatively
unskilled at this sort of “impression management”—
puzzled and uncomfortable about how they are really
viewed by others. Finally, because of the increased
importance of peers in early adolescence, young ado-
lescents are especially interested in their peers’ opin-
ions of them. For the first time, they may have to
come to terms with contradictions between the mes- Early adolescent girls’ self-esteem is lower, their degree of self-
sages they get from their parents (“That haircut you consciousness is higher, and their self-image is shakier than is the
just got makes you even prettier—you look great with case for boys.
short hair”) and the messages they get from their
peers (“You’d better wear a hat until your hair grows
back!”). Hearing contradictory messages probably
generates a certain degree of uncertainty about one- research on adolescent self-esteem has focused on
self (Rosenberg, 1986). teenagers’ overall feelings about themselves, most re-
searchers today believe that young people evaluate
❚ The Wrong Question? Some researchers have themselves both globally (which may be a good indi-
argued that the question of whether self-esteem is stable cator of general psychological well-being) but also
during adolescence is a poor one, because averages often along several distinct dimensions, such as academics,
hide substantial differences between people. According athletics, appearance, social relationships, and moral
to several studies, some adolescents show very high sta- conduct (Côté, 2009). As a consequence, it is possible
bility in self-esteem over time, whereas others do not for an adolescent to have, say, high self-esteem when it
(Diehl, Vicary, & Deike, 1997; Hirsch & DuBois, 1991; comes to academic abilities, low self-esteem when it
M. Zimmerman, Copeland, Shope, & Dielman, 1997). comes to athletics, and moderate self-esteem when
In one study, the researchers identified four dramati- it comes to physical appearance, social relationships,
cally different patterns of self-esteem over time that or moral conduct.
were followed by youngsters during the transition into
junior high school. Approximately one-third of the ad- ❚ Components of Self-Esteem Even within
olescents were classified as consistently high in self- broad domains of self-esteem (for example, academics,
esteem, and approximately one-sixth were classified as athletics, or social relationships), adolescents may have
chronically low. Half the sample, however, showed im- quite differentiated views of themselves. For example,
pressive patterns of change over just a 2-year period: adolescents’ evaluations of their social competence
About one-fifth were categorized as steeply declining, within the context of their relationships with their par-
and nearly one-third showed a small but significant in- ents may be very different from the way they see them-
crease in self-esteem. When these patterns are averaged, selves in the context of their relationships with teachers,
their differences cancel each other out, resulting in a which in turn may differ from their evaluations of
picture of self-esteem that may look more stable than it themselves in the context of their friendships with peers
is. (As the old joke goes, a statistician can drown in a (Harter, Waters, & Whitesell, 1998). And within the
river that averages three-feet deep.) Not surprisingly, realm of peer relationships, adolescents’ social self-es-
adolescents with better family and peer relationships teem may vary depending on whether they are thinking
are more likely than their peers to maintain positive about their friendships or their romantic relationships
self-esteem or develop enhanced self-esteem over time (Connolly & Konarski, 1994). Therefore, it may be
(Diehl et al., 1997). misleading to characterize an adolescent’s “social self-
Some critics of studies of the stability of self-esteem esteem” as low or high without specifying the relation-
in adolescence also question the validity of examining ship being referred to. The same goes for academic
self-esteem in such a general sense. Although most self-esteem: Because students evaluate their abilities in
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 262 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

262 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

specific subject areas both in comparison to other stu- GROUP DIFFERENCES IN SELF-ESTEEM
dents (“I am terrible at math compared to everyone else
❚ Sex Differences Several studies have shown that
in this class”) and relative to their abilities in other sub-
early adolescent girls are more vulnerable to distur-
ject areas (“I am so much better at math than I am at
bances in their self-image than any other group of
history”), making sweeping statements about an ado-
youngsters. Specifically, early adolescent girls’ self-
lescent’s overall academic self-image is often unwise
esteem is lower, their degree of self-consciousness is
(Arens, Yeung, Craven, & Hasselhorn, 2011; Marsh &
higher, and their self-image is shakier than is the case
Hau, 2004).
for boys. As a consequence, girls also are more likely
Do some aspects of self-esteem contribute more to
than boys to say negative things about themselves, to
an adolescent’s overall self-image than others? Yes, they
feel insecure about their abilities, and to worry about
do. In general, adolescents’ physical self-esteem—how
whether other people like being with them. Sex differ-
they feel about their appearance—is the most impor-
ences in adolescents’ self-perceptions become smaller
tant predictor of overall self-esteem, followed by self-
over the course of adolescence, however (Fredricks &
esteem about relationships with peers (Harter, 1999).
Eccles, 2002).
Less important are self-esteem about academic ability,
Although sex differences in self-esteem are most
athletic ability, or moral conduct. Interestingly, al-
pronounced among White adolescents, similar pat-
though researchers find that adolescents’ physical self-
terns have been found among Hispanic adolescents
esteem is the best predictor of their overall self-esteem,
but not, for the most part, among Black adoles-
adolescents, when asked, say that their physical appear-
cents (Erkut, Szalacha, Garcia Coll, & Alarcon, 2000;
ance is one of the least important contributors to how
Harter, 1999; van den Berg et al., 2010). Studies of
they feel about themselves. In other words, adolescents
Black girls do not find the same sort of self-esteem
may be unaware of the degree to which their self-worth
vulnerability as is found in studies of White girls, in
is based on their feelings about their appearance
part because Black girls do not experience the same
(DuBois, Tevendale, Burk-Braxton, Swenson, & Hard-
drop in body image during puberty (K. Brown et al.,
esty, 2000). It is important to note both that physical
1998) (see Figure 8.3).
self-esteem is a more important influence on overall
Why would girls have greater self-esteem problems
self-esteem among girls than among boys, although
during early adolescence than boys? The answer may be
both genders’ self-esteem is linked to how they feel
related to the special significance of physical appearance
about their appearance (Kistler, Rodgers, Power, Austin,
and acceptance by peers in determining self-esteem, as
& Hill, 2010; Thomaes et al., 2011; van den Berg, Mond,
discussed earlier. Because young girls appear to be more
Eisenberg, Ackard, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2010). Taken
concerned than boys about physical attractiveness, dat-
together, these findings help to explain why there are
ing, and peer acceptance, they may experience a greater
sex differences in the extent to which adolescents’ expe-
number of self-image problems. Because Black girls do
rience self-image difficulties and, as we’ll see in Chapter
not feel as negatively about their appearance as White
13, depression.

FIGURE 8.3 Race differences 4.0


in patterns of change in girls’
physical self-esteem. (K. Brown
et al., 1998)
3.5
Physical self-esteem

3.0 Black girls

White girls
2.5

2.0

1.5
9 10 11 12 13 14
Age at prior birthday
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 263 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 263

or Hispanic girls, they have higher overall self-esteem et al., 2007; Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000). Interestingly,
and show less of a decline in self-esteem over adoles- ethnic differences in self-esteem, favoring Black adoles-
cence (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Malanchuk & Ec- cents, have increased over the past 25 years (perhaps
cles, 1999; Prosser & Carlson, 1999). because ethnic identity has become a more relevant is-
sue in society) and are greater during adolescence than
❚ Ethnic Differences Most research indicates that childhood (perhaps because, as we shall see, ethnic
Black adolescents have higher self-esteem than White identity is a more salient issue during adolescence than
adolescents, who, in turn, tend to have higher self- before) (Twenge & Crocker, 2002).
esteem than Hispanic, Asian, or Native American youth
(Biro, Striegel-Moore, Franko, Padgett, & Bean, 2006;
Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Twenge & Crocker, 2002). MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
Several studies indicate that Asian American adoles-
Studies of ethnic differences in self-esteem, indicating
cents have particularly low self-esteem relative to their
relatively higher self-esteem among Black adolescents
peers (e.g., Herman, 2004), a finding that some re-
and lower self-esteem among Asian adolescents, have
searchers have attributed to higher rates of peer rejec-
almost all been conducted in the
tion (recall the discussion of peer discrimination in
United States. Do you think similar
Chapter 5) (Greene & Way, 2005).
patterns would emerge in research
A number of researchers have asked why Black ado-
conducted in other countries that
lescents have such high self-esteem, given the preva-
have ethnically diverse populations?
lence of prejudice in American society and the generally
disadvantaged position of Black individuals in the
workplace and school, two institutions where individu-
als’ performance influences their self-image. Three
main explanations for the relatively high self-esteem of Ethnic differences also exist in patterns of change in
Black adolescents have been offered. self-esteem during adolescence. In one study of Black,
First, some writers have argued that, despite their en- Latino, and Asian urban adolescents, Black students
counters with racism and prejudice, Black teenagers ben- and biracial students (mainly Black/Latino) had higher
efit from the support and positive feedback of adults in self-esteem in early adolescence, and this remained
the Black community, especially in the family (Gaylord- high throughout the adolescent years. In contrast,
Harden, Ragsdale, Mandara, Richards, & Petersen, 2007). Latino students had relatively lower self-esteem early in
This is not surprising, given the wealth of research show- adolescence, but caught up with their Black peers by the
ing that the approval of significant others is an especially end of high school. Asian students began with the low-
powerful influence on adolescents’ self-esteem—much est self-esteem, and it remained lower than that of other
more so than the opinion of the broader society (e.g., groups over time (see Figure 8.4). Similar patterns were
Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Robinson, 1995; Whitesell,
Mitchell, Kaufman, & Spicer, 2006). 4.6
Second, other researchers suggest that all teenagers—
minority and otherwise—tend to shift their priorities 4.4
over time so that they come to value those activities at
which they excel. In doing so, adolescents are able to pro- 4.2 Black
tect their self-esteem by focusing on areas of strength
instead of weakness. For example, a boy who is an out- 4.0
Bi/multiracial
Self-esteem

standing student but who feels physically unattractive


and does not do well in sports will likely derive positive 3.8
self-esteem from his school achievement and not restrict Latino
his self-evaluation to his looks or performance on the 3.6
playing field. One way that Black adolescents may re-
spond to their relatively poorer school performance is to 3.4 Asian American
change their feelings about the importance of doing well
in school, which weakens the connection between aca- 3.2
demic success and self-esteem.
Finally, several writers have suggested that the very 3.0
strong sense of ethnic identity (which we look at later in 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
this chapter) among Black adolescents serves to en- Age
hance their overall self-esteem (DuBois, Burk-Braxton, FIGURE 8.4 Ethnic differences in patterns of self-esteem
Swenson, Tevendale, & Hardesty, 2002; Gaylord-Harden over time. (Greene & Way, 2005)
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 264 21/03/13 9:09 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

264 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

found in a large national sample of American youth Adolescents who report frequent experiences of being
(Erol & Orth, 2011). Interestingly, and in contrast to insulted, excluded, and teased about their race or ethnic-
studies of White adolescents, there were no sex differ- ity have relatively more psychological problems than do
ences in levels or patterns of change in self-esteem in their peers who face no discrimination (C. H. Caldwell,
this sample of ethnic minority adolescents. Kohn-Wood, Schmeelk-Cone, Chavous, & Zimmerman,
The ethnic diversity of the context in which adoles- 2004; DuBois et al., 2002; Huynh & Fuligni, 2010; Sellers,
cents develop has a substantial impact on their self- Copeland-Linder, Martin, & Lewis, 2006).
image, however. Some research indicates, for example,
that high school students who live in a social environ-
ment or go to a school in which their ethnic or socio- ANTECEDENTS AND CONSEQUENCES
economic group is in the minority are more likely to OF HIGH SELF-ESTEEM
have self-image problems than those who are in the
majority (Rosenberg, 1975). This seems to be true Several researchers have examined the link between
with regard to religion, socioeconomic status, ethnic- self-esteem and adolescent behavior, in an attempt to
ity, and household composition. Black teenagers, for see whether certain sorts of experiences contribute—
example, have a higher opinion of themselves when either positively or negatively—to adolescents’ feelings
they go to schools in which Black students are a major- about themselves. Others have posed the question in
ity than when they attend predominantly White reverse: Does having high (or low) self-esteem lead ad-
schools, where they may feel out of place and under olescents to behave in particular ways?
pressure to play down their cultural heritage. By the
same token, Jewish adolescents have higher self-esteem ❚ Influences on Self-Esteem Regarding the first
in schools in which there are many other Jewish stu- question, studies find that self-esteem is enhanced by
dents than in schools in which Jews compose a small having the approval of others, especially of parents
minority of the student body. Similarly, levels of mal- and peers, and by succeeding in school (DuBois et al.,
adjustment are higher among both Hispanic and non– 2002; Luster & McAdoo, 1995; Ojanen & Perry, 2007;
Hispanic youngsters when they are in the minority in A. Roberts et al., 2000; R. B. Wilkinson, 2004). These
their school (Kaufman, Gregory, & Stephan, 1990). As correlates of high self-esteem have been found in virtu-
you will read later in this chapter, adolescents’ psycho- ally all ethnic and cultural groups, although the self-
logical well-being, including their self-esteem, is ad- esteem of Asian youngsters seems especially influenced
versely affected by discrimination and prejudice. by their academic success (e.g., Szesulski, Martinez, &
Reyes, 1994). Thus, even though there are ethnic differ-
ences in average levels of self-esteem, the correlates of
self-esteem are similar in different ethnic groups (Rowe
et al., 1994).
Adolescents whose self-esteem is too wrapped up
in the approval of others—especially the approval of
peers—may be at risk for developing self-image prob-
lems, since peer acceptance may fluctuate over time,
leading to temporary drops in self-esteem (Harter,
Stocker, & Robinson, 1996). Consistent with this, ad-
olescents who tend to derive their self-esteem rela-
tively more from peers than from teachers or parents
show more behavioral problems and poorer school
achievement (DuBois, Bull, Sherman, & Roberts,
1998; DuBois, Felner, Brand, & George, 1999). In fact,
one explanation for the increase in problem behavior
that takes place over the course of adolescence is that
adolescents tend to look relatively more to their peers
for social support as they get older (DuBois, Burk-
Braxtonet al., 2002).
Adolescents who attend schools in which they are in the ethnic
minority may suffer greater self-esteem problems than their peers ❚ Consequences of High or Low Self-Esteem
who attend schools in which they are in the majority. Although How does having especially high or especially low self-
desegregation may have a positive impact on minority adolescents’
academic achievement, this benefit may be counterbalanced by the
esteem influence adolescents’ adjustment and behavior?
apparently negative impact of desegregation on minority students’ Although it once was believed that enhanced self-esteem
self-image. leads to school success, there actually is little evidence
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 265 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 265

for this, and a good deal of evidence that things go in the • Adolescence is a time of gradual rather than tumul-
opposite direction (e.g., X. Liu, Kaplan, & Risser, 1992; tuous change in individuals’ self-image. For most in-
J. Schmidt & Padilla, 2003). That is, academic success dividuals, global self-esteem is quite stable during
leads to improvements in how adolescents feel about adolescence and, contrary to popular belief, in-
themselves, not the other way around. These findings cast creases slightly. This increase levels off as individu-
doubt on the logic behind programs designed to raise als become young adults.
teenagers’ school performance by increasing their self- • During early adolescence, individuals may experi-
esteem. High self-esteem during adolescence does en- ence periods of heightened self-consciousness, and
hance adolescents’ well-being, however, whereas low their self-image may fluctuate more than during
self-esteem may lead to involvement in deviant activity, other periods.
psychological distress, and victimization, both in the • Generally, males and Black adolescents have higher
short run (Andrews, Alpert, Hops, & Davis, 1996; DuBois self-esteem than females or adolescents from
& Tevendale, 1999; Egan & Perry, 1998; X. Liu et al., 1992) other ethnic groups, although sex differences in
and well into adulthood (Trzesniewski et al., 2006), al- self-esteem are much more pronounced among
though this may be due to the fact that many of the same White teenagers. In the United States, Asian ado-
factors that contribute to high self-esteem in adolescence lescents have especially low self-esteem relative to
(such as doing well in school) are themselves stable over their peers.
time (Boden, Fergusson, & Horwood, 2008). • Across all demographic groups, high self-esteem is
The relationship between low self-esteem and emo- related to parental approval, peer support, and suc-
tional and behavioral problems is complicated (D. Cole, cess in school. Although it is difficult to disentangle
Peeke, Dolezal, Murray, & Canzoniero, 1999; Gerard & cause and effect, high self-esteem is associated with
Buehler, 2004; Tram & Cole, 2000). Low self-esteem is better mental health, whereas low self-
one of several symptoms of depression, but it is not esteem is correlated with a
clear whether depression leads to low self-esteem, or number of emotional
vice versa. The answer, it seems, depends on the indi- problems.
vidual. For about half of all adolescents, depression
leads to low self-esteem. For the other half, however, the
reverse is true—having low self-esteem leads to depres-
sion and other forms of emotional distress (Harter,
1999). One reason that low self-esteem may lead to de-
pression is that adolescents with negative feelings about
themselves are less likely to seek positive feedback from The Adolescent Identity
others (Cassidy, Ziv, Mehta, & Feeney, 2003).
The link between self-esteem and behavior prob-
Crisis
lems (as opposed to emotional problems) is even less Many of literature’s most important novels, from clas-
clear. Although low self-esteem initially may impel sics like Jane Eyre, Portrait of the Artist as a Young Man,
some adolescents toward delinquency, involvement and The Catcher in the Rye to more contemporary sto-
with delinquent peers actually may lead to an increase ries like The Secret Life of Bees, The House on Mango
in self-esteem, perhaps because involvement in delin- Street, The God of Animals, and Everything Is Illumi-
quency earns teenagers approval from certain peers nated, revolve around an adolescent’s identity crisis.
(Dishion, Andrews, & Crosby, 1995; Jang & Thorn- Indeed, the coming-of-age novel is a classic literary
berry, 1998; W. Mason, 2001). Furthermore, adoles- genre.
cents with high self-esteem are more likely to experiment If you were asked to write a novel about your own
with alcohol than are those with low self-esteem identity development, what sorts of things would you
(Scheier, Botvin, Griffin, & Diaz, 2000), most probably mention? Perhaps you would talk about the develop-
because high self-esteem is associated with being in the ment of a sense of purpose, or the clarification of your
more popular social crowds, in which drinking is more long-term plans and values, or the growing feeling of
common. knowing who you really are and where you are headed.
If these are the sorts of things that come to mind when
you think about identity development in adolescence,
RECAP you are thinking about an aspect of development that
• Researchers have differed in their approach to the psychologists refer to as the sense of identity. The dom-
study of self-esteem. While some have studied ad- inant view in the study of adolescent identity develop-
olescents’ general, or global, feelings about them- ment emphasizes precisely these aspects of psychosocial
selves, others have stressed the multidimensional development, and the theorist whose work has been
nature of self-esteem. most influential in this area is Erik Erikson.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 266 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

266 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

ERIKSON’S THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK how we were changed by the experience. In one study of
college undergraduates (McLean, 2005), students were
Erikson’s (1959, 1963, 1968) theory developed out of
asked to describe occasions in the past year in which
his clinical and cross-cultural observations of young
they shared an important memory with another person.
people at various stages of development. He viewed the
One participant described the following event, which
developing person as moving through a series of eight
took place when he was in high school:
psychosocial crises over the course of the life span.
Each crisis, although present in one form or another at I was at my friend’s house one night with my main group
all ages, takes on special significance at a given period of friends. They were all smoking marijuana and drink-
of the life cycle because biological and social forces in- ing. I did not feel comfortable with trying marijuana.
teract to bring the crisis into prominence. Erikson be- They tried hard to get me to try it, but I chose not to. One
lieved that the establishment of a coherent sense of of my friends (my best) supported my choice. I learned
who my real friends were. But more important, I learned
identity—what he called the crisis of identity versus
that I can be strong with my decisions if I choose to, regard-
identity diffusion—is the chief psychosocial crisis of
less of the outside influence.
adolescence.
He then described telling his college friends about the
event:
IDENTITY VERSUS IDENTITY DIFFUSION
The subject of drugs came up and so I explained the
Prior to adolescence, the child’s identity is like patches above story to them. They seemed to enjoy the story and
of fabric that have not yet been sewn together. But by applauded me on my resiliency. Now, my friends here [at
the end of adolescence, these patches will be woven into college] will ask me if I want to do anything and they will
a patchwork quilt that is unique to the individual. The not harass me about it, regardless of my answer. It pays to
maturational and social forces that converge at adoles- be strong when you want to. (McLean, 2005, p. 687, italics
cence force young people to reflect on their place in in original)
society, on the ways that others view them, and on their
In other words, developing an identity is a social as
options for the future.
well as mental process. Erikson placed a great deal of
Achieving a balanced and coherent sense of identity
weight on the role of others (and, especially, on those
is an intellectually and emotionally taxing process. In
individuals who have influence over the adolescent) in
fact, according to Erikson, it is not until adolescence
shaping the adolescent’s sense of self. The adolescent’s
that one even has the mental or emotional capacity to
identity is the result of a mutual recognition between
tackle this task. Erikson believed that the key to resolv-
the young person and society: The adolescent forges an
ing the crisis of identity versus identity diffusion lies in
identity, but, at the same time, society identifies the
the adolescent’s interactions with others. By responding
adolescent.
to the reactions of people who matter, the adolescent
selects and chooses from among the many elements
that could conceivably become a part of his or her adult THE SOCIAL CONTEXT OF IDENTITY
identity. The other people with whom the young per- DEVELOPMENT
son interacts serve as a mirror that reflects back to the
The social context in which the adolescent attempts to
adolescent information about who he or she is and
establish a sense of identity exerts a tremendous impact
ought to be. As such, the responses of these significant
on the nature and outcome of the process. Clearly, if the
others shape and influence the adolescent’s developing
adolescent’s identity is forged out of a recognition on
sense of identity. Through others’ reactions, adolescents
the part of society, society will play an important role in
learn whether they are competent or clumsy, attractive
determining which sorts of identities are possible alter-
or ugly, socially adept or geeky. Perhaps more impor-
natives; and of those identities that are genuine options,
tantly—especially during periods when their sense of
society will influence which are desirable and which are
identity is still forming—they learn from others what
not. As a consequence, the course of identity develop-
they do that they ought to keep doing and what they do
ment will vary in different cultures, among different
that they ought to stop doing.
subcultures within the same society, and over different
One process through which this occurs is via the
historical eras (Kroger, 1993).
sharing of important memories with others. When we
The social context in which an adolescent develops
tell stories to friends or family members about our pre-
also determines to a large extent whether the young-
vious experiences, we do so for a variety of reasons, one
ster’s search for self-definition will take the form of a
of which is to help ourselves understand who we are and
full-blown crisis or a more manageable challenge. Gen-
erally, the more alternatives available to the young per-
identity versus identity diffusion According to Erikson, the son, and the more arenas in which decisions must be
normative crisis characteristic of the fifth stage of psychosocial made, the more difficult establishing a sense of identity
development, predominant during adolescence.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 267 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 267

will be. Growing up in contemporary America, where


adolescents have a range of careers to decide among, for
example, is far more likely to provoke an occupational
identity crisis than is growing up in a small agrarian
community in which each young person continues
farming the family’s land.
The rapid rate of social change in most of the world
has raised new and more complex sets of questions for
young people to consider—questions not only about
occupational plans but also about values, lifestyles, and
commitments to other people. Today, even in some
countries where until recently individuals had little
choice about the life they would lead, adolescents
must ask themselves what sort of work they want to do;
if they want to remain single, live with someone, or
marry; and if and when they want to have children.
Consequently, the likelihood of going through a pro-
longed and difficult identity crisis is probably greater
today, and more prevalent around the world, than it has
ever been.

MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION


During the past 5 years, there have been dramatic po-
litical changes in much of the Arab
world. Do you think that changes in Role experimentation during adolescence often involves trying on
these societies’ “identity” will lead different looks, images, and patterns of behavior. According to the-
orists such as Erik Erikson, having the time and freedom to experi-
to changes in patterns of adoles- ment with different roles is an important prelude to establishing a
cent identity development? coherent sense of identity.

piercings, and a few weeks later he will discard this im-


❚ The Psychosocial Moratorium According to age for that of a preppie. Although many parents worry
Erikson, the complications inherent in identity devel- about their teenage children going through these sorts
opment in modern society have created the need for a of phases, much of this behavior is just normal experi-
psychosocial moratorium—a “time out” during ado- mentation with roles and personalities.
lescence from the sorts of excessive responsibilities and Having the time to experiment with roles is an im-
obligations that might restrict the young person’s pur- portant prelude to establishing a coherent sense of
suit of self-discovery. Adolescents in contemporary identity. But role experimentation can take place
America are given a moratorium of sorts by being en- only in an environment that allows and encourages it
couraged to remain in school for a long time, where (Côté, 2009). Without a moratorium, a full and thor-
they can think seriously about their plans for the future ough exploration of the options and available alterna-
without making irrevocable decisions. tives cannot occur, and identity development will
During the psychosocial moratorium, the adoles- be somewhat impeded. In other words, according to
cent can experiment with different roles and identities Erikson, adolescents must grow into an adult identity,
in a context that permits and encourages this sort of rather than be forced into one prematurely.
exploration. The experimentation involves trying on It is clear, however, that the sort of moratorium
different postures, personalities, and ways of behav- Erikson described is an ideal; indeed, some might even
ing—sometimes to the consternation of the adoles- consider it to be a luxury of the affluent. Many young
cent’s parents, who may wonder why their child’s people—perhaps even most—do not have the eco-
personality seems so changeable. One week, an adoles- nomic freedom to enjoy a long delay before taking on
cent girl will spend hours putting on makeup; the next
week, she will insist to her parents that she is tired of
caring so much about the way she looks. An adolescent psychosocial moratorium A period during which individuals are
boy will come home one day with a shaved head and free from excessive obligations and responsibilities and can
therefore experiment with different roles and personalities.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 268 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

268 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

the responsibilities of adult life. For many youngsters, as going through a single identity crisis, it probably
alternatives do not exist in any realistic sense, and intro- makes more sense to view the phenomenon as a series
spection only interferes with the more pressing task of of crises that may concern different aspects of the
survival. Does the 17-year-old who must drop out of young person’s identity and that may surface—and
school to work a full-time factory job go through life resurface—at different points in time throughout the
without a sense of identity? Do youngsters who cannot adolescent and young-adult years. Indeed, during ado-
afford a psychosocial moratorium fail to resolve the lescence, the feeling of well-being associated with estab-
identity crisis? lishing a sense of identity is often fleeting. Ultimately,
Certainly not. But from an Eriksonian point of view, however, the identity crisis of adolescence, when suc-
the absence of a psychosocial moratorium in some ado- cessfully resolved, culminates in a series of basic life
lescents’ lives—either because of restrictions they place commitments: occupational, ideological, social, reli-
on themselves, restrictions placed on them by others, or gious, ethical, and sexual (Côté, 2009).
their life circumstances—is truly regrettable. The price
these youngsters pay is not the failure to develop a sense
PROBLEMS IN IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
of identity but lost potential. You may know people
whose parents forced them into prematurely choosing a Given the wide variations in developmental histories
certain career or who had to drop out of college and that individuals bring to adolescence and the wide vari-
take a job they really did not want because of financial ations in the environments in which they develop, it is
pressures. According to Erikson, without a chance to not surprising to find differences in the ways in which
explore, to experiment, and to choose among options individuals approach and resolve the identity crisis.
for the future, these young people may not realize all Problems in identity development can result when an
that they are capable of becoming. It is easy to see how individual has not successfully resolved earlier crises or
the broader context in which adolescents grow up af- when the adolescent is in an environment that does not
fects this. Think, for example, of how individuals’ plans provide the necessary period of moratorium. Three
for the future may have had to change during the Great sorts of problems received special attention from
Recession that began in 2008. Erikson: identity diffusion, identity foreclosure, and
negative identity.
RESOLVING THE IDENTITY CRISIS
❚ Identity Diffusion Identity diffusion is charac-
Is establishing a sense of identity something that is con- terized by an incoherent, disjointed, incomplete sense
scious? According to Erikson, it is. It is experienced as a of self. Identity diffusion can vary in degree from a mild
sense of well-being, a feeling of “being at home in one’s state of not quite knowing who one is while in the midst
body,” a sense of knowing where one is going, and an of an identity crisis to a more severe, psychopathologi-
inner assuredness of recognition from those who count. cal condition that persists beyond a normal period of
It is a sense of sameness through time—a feeling of exploration. It is marked by disruptions in the individ-
continuity between the past and the future. ual’s sense of time (some things seem to happen much
faster than they really do, while others seem to take for-
ever); excessive self-consciousness, to the point that it is
difficult to make decisions; problems in work and
school; difficulties in forming intimate relationships
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
with others; and concerns over sexuality. In other
Have you experienced the sort of identity words, identity diffusion is reflected not only in prob-
crisis described by Erikson? If so, when did it lems of identity but also in the areas of autonomy, inti-
happen, and what did it feel like? Was it macy, sexuality, and achievement.
triggered by anything in particular? How, if A classic example of an adolescent in the throes of
at all, were you changed by the experience? identity diffusion is Holden Caulfield in the 1951 novel
The Catcher in the Rye. He has flunked out of several
prep schools, has severed most of his friendships, and
has no sense of where he is headed. At one point in
Establishing a coherent sense of identity is a lengthy the book, for example, walking up Fifth Avenue in
process. Most writers on adolescence and youth believe New York City, Holden says, “Every time I came to the
that identity exploration continues well into young end of a block and stepped off the goddam curb, I had
adulthood. But rather than thinking of the adolescent this feeling that I’d never get to the other side of the
street. I thought I’d just go down, down, down, and
nobody’d ever see me again. Boy, did it scare me”
identity diffusion The incoherent, disjointed, incomplete sense of (Salinger, 1951/1964, pp. 197–198).
self characteristic of not having resolved the crisis of identity.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 269 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 269

❚ Identity Foreclosure Some young people by- please his excessively demanding parents. He feels he is a
pass—either willingly or unwillingly—the period of nobody in his parents’ eyes, so he drops out of school to
exploration and experimentation that precedes the es- play guitar in a band—something his parents vehemently
tablishment of a healthy sense of identity. Instead of oppose. As Erikson pointed out, most adolescents would
considering a range of alternatives, these adolescents rather be somebody “bad” than nobody at all.
prematurely commit themselves to a role, or series of
roles, and settle upon a certain identification as a final
identity. In essence, these individuals are not given—or MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
do not take advantage of—a psychosocial moratorium. Are there any aspects of today’s
For example, a college freshman who made up her environment that might make
mind about becoming a doctor at the age of 13 may the resolution of the identity
enroll in a rigid premed curriculum without consider- crisis especially difficult?
ing other career possibilities. The circumvention of the Can you think of anything
identity crisis is called identity foreclosure. that might be done to
Typically, the roles adopted in the process of identity help facilitate healthy
foreclosure revolve around the goals set for the young identity development?
person by parents or other authority figures. The ado-
lescent may be led into these roles directly or may be
forced into them indirectly by being denied a true psy- RECAP
chosocial moratorium. Perhaps the parents of the • According to Erikson, the major psychosocial issue
would-be doctor have arranged their child’s school of adolescence revolves around the identity crisis—
schedule and summer vacations so that all of her spare coming to terms with who one is and where one is
time is spent taking extra science courses. No time is left headed.
for role experimentation or introspection. Individuals • To resolve the identity crisis, the young person needs
who have bypassed the identity crisis have made com- some time out from excessive responsibilities—a
mitments, but they have not gone through a period of psychosocial moratorium—in order to engage in
experimentation before making them. Identity foreclo- identity exploration and experimentation.
sure is an interruption of the identity development pro- • Some adolescents have trouble resolving the iden-
cess, an interruption that interferes with the individual’s tity crisis. The three most common problems de-
discovery of his or her full range of potentials. scribed by Erikson are identity diffusion, identity
foreclosure, and negative identity.
❚ Negative Identity Occasionally, adolescents ap- • Problems in identity development can result when
pear to select identities that are obviously undesirable the individual has not resolved earlier crises or when
to their parents and their community. The examples are the adolescent is in an environment that
familiar: the daughter of the local district attorney who does not provide the necessary
repeatedly gets into trouble with the law, the son of period of psychosocial
prestigious and successful parents who refuses to go to moratorium.
college, or the child of a devoutly religious family who
insists that he or she is a confirmed atheist. Because the
establishment of a healthy sense of identity is so inti-
mately tied to the recognition of the adolescent by those
who count in her or his life, the adoption of a so-called
Research on Identity
negative identity is a sign that problems in identity de- Development
velopment have arisen. The adolescent who adopts a DETERMINING AN ADOLESCENT’S
negative identity is recognized by those around him or IDENTITY STATUS
her, but not in a way that fosters healthy development.
Usually, selecting a negative identity represents an at- The term identity status refers to the point in the
tempt to forge some sense of self-definition in an envi- identity development process that characterizes an
ronment that has made it difficult to establish an adolescent at a given time. In order to determine an
acceptable identity. This appears to be especially likely individual’s identity status, most researchers have used
when, after repeatedly trying and failing to receive posi-
tive recognition from those who are important in their
identity foreclosure The premature establishment of a sense of
lives, adolescents turn to a different, perhaps more suc-
identity, before sufficient role experimentation has occurred.
cessful, route to being noticed—adopting a negative
identity. Consider this example: The son of successful negative identity The selection of an identity that is obviously
parents is a good student but not quite good enough to undesirable in the eyes of significant others and the broader
community.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 270 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

270 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

an approach adapted from the work of James Marcia and show the lowest level of intimacy with peers
(1966), which focuses on the processes of exploration (Meeus, 2011).
(experimenting with different ideas about occupa-
tions, vales, relationships, and so forth) and commit- ❚ Ways of Resolving the Identity Crisis Several
ment (making choices among various alternatives). researchers have also described the ways in which
Some theorists (e.g., Luyckx, Goossens, & Soenens, different individuals approach the resolution of the
2006) distinguish between two different stages: explo- identity crisis (e.g., Berzonsky, 2004). According to this
ration in “depth” (making a commitment to an iden- framework, it is possible to differentiate among indi-
tity and then exploring one’s options) and exploration viduals who tend to actively seek information and ap-
in “breadth” (exploring one’s options and them mak- proach identity-related decisions with an open mind
ing a further commitment). Others (e.g., Meeus, 2011) (described as having an “informational” orientation),
see identity development as a more dynamic process, those who attempt to conform to family and other
with individuals moving back and forth between com- social expectations and try to get identity-related deci-
mitment and exploration over time. In all of these sions over as quickly as possible (a “normative” orienta-
frameworks, the important point is that healthy iden- tion), and those who tend to procrastinate and avoid
tity involvement involves some degree of experimenta- making identity-related decisions (a “diffuse/avoidant”
tion and exploration before one finalizes one’s choices orientation). Not surprisingly, the informational orien-
about work, love, and lifestyle. tation is more characteristic of identity achievers, the
In identity status research, based on their responses normative orientation is more characteristic of indi-
to an interview or questionnaire, individuals are as- viduals who are identity foreclosed, and the diffuse/
signed to one of four identity states (“state” is the right avoidant orientation is more characteristic of individu-
word, because research shows that during adolescence als who exhibit identity diffusion.
individuals move from state to state, and not necessar- More evidence of this sort comes from a study that
ily in an orderly fashion): (1) identity achievement (the attempted to link classifications based on a measure of
individual has established a coherent sense of iden- identity development with scores on the personality di-
tity—that is, has made commitments after a period of mensions tapped within the five-factor model of person-
exploration), (2) moratorium (the individual is in the ality discussed earlier (Clancy & Dollinger, 1993). As
midst of a period of exploration), (3) identity foreclo- expected, adolescents who were classified as identity
sure (the individual has made commitments but with- achievers were higher in extraversion and less neurotic
out a period of exploration), or (4) identity diffusion than other adolescents; foreclosed adolescents were less
(the individual does not have firm commitments and is open; and diffused adolescents were more neurotic, less
not currently trying to make them). open, and less agreeable. It was not clear from this study
Generally, research employing this approach has whether different personality constellations led to differ-
supported many aspects of Erikson’s theory (Meeus, ent patterns of identity development or, alternatively,
2011). The strongest support comes from studies that whether different patterns of identity development in-
show a pattern of correlations between various traits fluenced subsequent personality. Given what we know
and the different identity statuses that are consistent about the childhood antecedents of personality traits,
with predictions based on Erikson’s model. As you however, the former explanation (that personality affects
might expect, for example, individuals who are in a identity development) seems more likely than the latter.
state of identity achievement are psychologically One of the defining characteristics of individuals
healthier than others on a variety of measures: They who have achieved a coherent sense of identity, at least
score highest on measures of achievement motivation, in contemporary American society, is that they ap-
moral reasoning, intimacy with peers, and career matu- proach life’s decisions with a strong sense of agency—
rity. Individuals in the midst of a moratorium score they take responsibility for themselves, they feel in
highest on measures of anxiety, show the highest levels control of their decisions, and they have confidence
of conflict over issues of authority, and are themselves that they will be able to overcome obstacles along the
the least rigid and least authoritarian. Individuals clas- way (Côté, 2000). In studies of college students from
sified as being in the foreclosure status have been shown different ethnic groups, psychologist James Côté and
to be the most authoritarian and most prejudiced and colleagues have shown that a strong sense of personal
to have the highest need for social approval, the lowest agency is predictive of identity achievement across eth-
level of autonomy, and the greatest closeness to their nic and socioeconomic groups (e.g., Schwartz, Côté, &
parents. Individuals in a state of identity diffusion dis- Arnett, 2005). Being “in charge” of one’s life may be es-
play the highest level of psychological and interper- pecially important in contemporary industrialized so-
sonal problems: They are the most socially withdrawn ciety, where the transition to adulthood is prolonged
and where individuals are faced with a tremendous
number of identity-related decisions. Consistent with
agency The sense that one has an impact on one’s world.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 271 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 271

this, individuals in their late teens or early 20s who,


when asked whether they are adolescents or adults, say
they are not sure, are less likely to have achieved a sense
of identity than are those who are certain that they have
reached adulthood (L. J. Nelson & Barry, 2005). It is not
clear whether having a coherent sense of identity leads
one to think of oneself as an adult or, instead, whether
seeing oneself as an adult leads one to have a more co-
herent sense of identity. But it does seem that becoming
an adult, at least in industrialized society, is a psycho-
logical transition as well as one characterized by enter-
ing the formal roles of adulthood.
What sorts of parenting practices are associated with
different identity statuses? Generally, individuals whose
identity development is healthy are more likely to have
had authoritative homes characterized by warm, but
not excessively constraining relations (Berzonsky, 2004;
W. Beyers & Goossens, 2008; Dumas, Lawford, Tieu, & Most research indicates that the chief period for identity development
Pratt, 2009; Schwartz, Pantin, Prado, Sullivan, & is in late adolescence, when many individuals are enrolled in college.
Szapocznik, 2005). As we saw in Chapter 4, individuals
who grow up in these environments are encouraged to
assert their individuality but remain connected to their frequently observed between groups in the 18- to
families at the same time. Typically, the absence of 21-year-old range. Few consistent differences emerge
parental warmth is associated with problems in making in comparisons of teenagers in the middle adolescent
commitments—the most extreme case being identity years, suggesting that, although self-examination may
diffusion—whereas the absence of parental encourage- take place throughout adolescence, the consolidation
ment of individuality is associated with problems in of a coherent sense of identity does not begin until very
engaging in extensive exploration (Côté, 2009). late in the period (Côté, 2009; Kroger, 2003). The late
teens and early 20s appear to be the critical times for
the crystallization of a sense of identity (Nurmi, 2004;
STUDYING IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
Schwartz, Côté, & Arnett, 2005). Interestingly, although
OVER TIME
individuals engage in more of this sophisticated self-
In order to examine the development of a sense of iden- reflection as they mature through adolescence, at-
tity, researchers have done both cross-sectional studies tempts to speed up this process, by training individuals
(comparing individuals of different ages) and longitu- to think more about how specific life events had played
dinal studies (following the same individuals over a pe- a role in their development, are not effective (Haber-
riod of time). Many of these studies have challenged mas & de Silveira, 2008).
some widely held beliefs about the nature of identity Second, there is less systematic change in identity de-
development in adolescence (Côté, 2009; Meeus, 2011). velopment that originally had been hypothesized. That
First, in contrast to the view that the crisis of iden- is, although we might expect that individuals move from
tity is resolved during adolescence, studies show that a a state of diffusion to a state of foreclosure or morato-
coherent sense of identity generally is not established rium, and then either remain foreclosed or move to a
before age 18, let alone earlier in adolescence, as origi- state of identity achievement, not all individuals follow
nally theorized (Côté, 2009). This is especially true this hypothesized pattern. In a longitudinal study of
among boys, who tend to lag behind girls in identity Dutch youth, nearly 60% of the individuals classified as
development in early and middle adolescence but catch in a state of identity diffusion were no longer classified
up by late adolescence (Klimstra, Hale, Raaijmakers, that way 4 years later, and nearly 75% of individuals who
Branje, & Meeus, 2010). There is clearly a decline with were in the midst of a moratorium at the beginning of
age in the proportion of individuals who are in a state the study were no longer in this category at the later as-
of moratorium or diffusion (Klimstra, et al. 2010; sessment (Meeus, Iedema, & Vollebergh, 1999). But 67%
Meeus, Van de Schoot, Keijsers, Schwartz, & Branje, of individuals who had apparently foreclosed the iden-
2010). But the proportion of individuals who are in a tity development process were in the midst of an identity
state of identity achievement before late adolescence is crisis 4 years later, suggesting that foreclosure may be a
low. In general, when comparisons are made among temporary stage rather than a permanent one, at least
groups of individuals of different ages over the span for some adolescents. Other longitudinal studies of ado-
from 12 to 24, differences in identity status are most lescents have revealed similar findings (Côté, 2009).
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 272 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

272 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Moreover, in these same longitudinal studies, a identity are provoked both by internal factors—
large proportion of individuals who were at one point discontent with one’s life, for example—and by specific
classified as “identity achieved” status later shifted sta- life events or changes in life circumstances, such as mak-
tus over the course of the study, indicating that ing the transition into or out of high school (Kalakoski &
“achievement,” like “foreclosure,” may be temporary Nurmi, 1998; Kroger & Green, 1996). Individuals are able
(Meeus et al., 2010). In the Dutch study, for example, to “make meaning” out of these turning points, by using
half of the adolescents who were classified as identity the event to come to a better understanding of themselves
achieved at the first assessment were not classified this (Kang, Okazaki, Abelmann, Kim-Prieto & Lan, 2010;
way 4 years later. How could some individuals who at McLean & Breen, 2009; McLean, Breen, & Fournier, 2010;
one point had apparently resolved their identity crisis McLean & Pratt, 2006). At least one study finds that giv-
actually not have resolved it—at least, not in any final ing students opportunities to engage in this process
sense? According to some writers, these sorts of regres- through writing assignments helps make students more
sions to a less mature identity status are part of the engaged in class (Faircloth, 2009). Interestingly, when
normal process through which individuals ultimately adults look back on their life and attempt to tell a story
establish a coherent sense of self (Kroger, 2003). That that makes sense, they tend to put more weight on events
is, we should not view the achievement of a sense of that took place during adolescence and young adulthood,
identity in adolescence as a final state, but rather as a a phenomenon that has been described as a “reminis-
step on a long route toward the establishment of a ma- cence bump” (Thorne, 2000).
ture sense of self.
Finally, many individuals who show signs of iden-
tity diffusion early on remain in this state, as do many
individuals who spend time in a state of moratorium. MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
In other words, there are some individuals who are Adults tend to refer back to adolescence more
perpetually confused (at least during adolescence than other periods when creating a narrative about
and young adulthood) about who they are as well as their life. Do you think this is because events during
others who seem to be always exploring and experi- adolescence are more important in
menting with new identities (Meeus et al., 2010). shaping one’s life, because events during
Individuals who are high in anxiety may have an es- adolescence are simply remembered
pecially difficult time (Crocetti, Klimstra, Keijsers, more clearly, because adolescence is the
Hale, & Meeus, 2009). first time that individuals begin creating
a life story, or for some other reason?

MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION


Given the fact that college provides a sort of psycho-
Erikson developed his ideas about
social moratorium for many people, researchers have
identity development in the 1950s. Do
asked whether college attendance facilitates identity de-
you think that this accounts for findings
velopment (Côté, 2009). This has proven to be a difficult
that most identity development occurs
question to answer. While studies have found that the
in late adolescence or early adulthood,
proportion of college students who are classified as
rather than in early or middle adoles-
identity achieved increases from around 20% during
cence, as he proposed?
freshman year to as many as 40% by senior year, whether
this development can be attributed to the college experi-
ence is hard to say, since this increase might have taken
place just as a result of maturation (Pascarella & Teren-
Because little research has focused on the processes zini, 2005). Although in theory one could test this by
through which identity development occurs, the factors comparing identity development among college stu-
associated with changing from one identity status to an- dents with late adolescents who are not enrolled in col-
other are not well understood (Kroger & Green, 1996; lege, in practice this is not easy to do. One problem (in
LaVoie, 1994). Indeed, it is fair to say that psychologists addition to the difficulty researchers find in recruiting
have been much better at describing the various stages samples of noncollege individuals to participate in re-
that adolescents move through over the course of their search studies) is that people are not randomly assigned
identity development than at explaining why or how in- to go to college or not. Thus, even if one were to find that
dividuals’ sense of identity changes when it does. The college students showed relatively greater identity devel-
little research that has been done on this subject indicates opment than nonstudents, this could be due to factors
that turning points in the development of a sense of that differentiate people who go to college from those
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 273 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 273

who do not. As you’ve probably figured out, this is the many White adolescents, especially those from more
same “selection versus socialization” problem we have working-class backgrounds, identify strongly with a
encountered many times. particular ethnic group (such as German, Irish, Italian,
Jewish) and derive part of their overall sense of self
from this identification (J. M. Grossman & Charmaraman,
RECAP 2009; R. Martinez & Dukes, 1997; R. Roberts et al.,
• The predominant influence on the study of identity 1999). Nevertheless, if given a list of labels to identify
development in adolescence has been Erik Erikson, their own ethnic background, White adolescents in
who suggested that the major psychosocial issue of America are less likely than ethnic minority adolescents
adolescence revolves around the identity crisis— to choose labels based on their specific heritage (for ex-
coming to terms with who one is and where one is ample, “German,” “Italian American”) and more likely
headed. to use generic “panethnic” labels (for example, “White”)
• To resolve this crisis successfully, young people or simply to identify themselves as “American” (Fuligni,
need some time out from excessive responsibili- Witkow, & Garcia, 2005). Among immigrant adoles-
ties—a psychosocial moratorium—in order to en- cents, there is considerable vacillation between identify-
gage in identity exploration and experimentation. ing oneself as a member of a broad ethnic category (e.g.,
• Some adolescents have difficulty in successfully re- Latino, Asian) and identifying oneself as a member of a
solving the identity crisis. Among the three most group defined by one’s country of origin (e.g., Mexican,
common problems described by Erikson are iden- Chinese) (Fuligni, Kiang, Witkow, & Baldelomar, 2008),
tity diffusion, identity foreclosure, and negative as well as variability in definitions of how best to main-
identity. tain an identity that merges being a member of one’s
• Most studies indicate that the major developments ethnic group and being a member of the country into
in identity occur in late adolescence and young which the family has immigrated (Ko & Perreira, 2010;
adulthood, rather than earlier during the adoles- Li, 2009; Nguyen & Brown, 2010; Qin, 2009). Language
cent decade. and style of dress are often used by immigrant youth to
• Research has not supported the proposition that make a statement about their identity, as these Hmong
identity development always follows a logical, or- girls explain:
derly sequence. Although some individuals fol-
low expected patterns, others get Kandi: A lot of people aren’t proud of being Hmong and
“stuck” at earlier stages, and if you speak Hmong with them they’ll be like, “Why are
some even appear you speaking Hmong, that’s stupid, speak English. Hello,
you’re in America, speak English.’’
to regress.
Eve: I just hate when they tell you that. Like, I can speak
as much as I want. (Nguyen & Brown, 2010, p. 857)

The Development THE PROCESS OF ETHNIC IDENTITY


DEVELOPMENT
of Ethnic Identity The process of ethnic identity development is similar to
For all individuals who are not part of the majority cul- the process of identity development in general, with an
ture, integrating a sense of ethnic identity into their unquestioning view of oneself often being displaced or
overall sense of personal identity is likely to be an im- upset by a crisis. Often, but not always, the precipitating
portant task of late adolescence, perhaps as important event involves an experience during which the individ-
as establishing a coherent occupational, ideological, or ual encounters prejudice, becomes aware of his or her
interpersonal identity (Chao & Otsuki-Clutter, 2011; group’s underrepresentation in some activity or setting,
Fuligni, Hughes, & Way, 2009; Seaton & Gilbert, 2011). or suddenly feels different from adolescents from other
Over the past three decades, an extensive literature has backgrounds (Syed & Azmitia, 2006). Here’s how one
been amassed on the process through which ethnic Asian American woman described her “awakening”:
identity develops and on the implications of having a
I was on the computer chatting with my friend, and he
strong versus weak sense of ethnic identity for adoles-
was telling me he has plans to go to Japan to visit for va-
cent adjustment and behavior. Ethnic identity has been cation. Then I told him I would like to visit China, and he
studied in samples of Black, Hispanic, Native American, said to me, “China sounds so shady, but you dirty chink
Asian, and White youth (Fuligni et al., 2009; M. D. Jones
& Galliher, 2007; Markstrom, 2011a; D. L. Newman,
2005). In America, White youth generally have a weaker
sense of ethnic identity than their non-White peers, but ethnic identity The aspect of individuals’ sense of identity
concerning ancestry or racial group membership.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 274 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

274 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

would totally fit in anyway.” I told him immediately I was students began to prepare to make the transition from
offended by his comment, but he thought I was joking relatively homogeneous schools into much more di-
about my emotions. I told him to take that comment verse ones (S. E. French, Seidman, Allen, & Aber, 2006).
back because I didn’t find it funny. I felt very offended; Between middle and late adolescence, exploration de-
I was thinking to myself, “Is that how others view Chinese
clines, as individuals begin to develop a more consoli-
people?” (Syed & Azmitia, 2006, p. 1019)
dated identity (Pahl & Way, 2006). As is the case with
Following the crisis, individuals engage in a period identity development more generally, individuals with
of exploration, during which they may immerse them- a strong sense of ethnic identity have better mental
selves in learning about their ethnic heritage. (Increas- health than their peers whose sense of ethnic identity
ingly, a good deal of this exploration occurs online is more diffuse (Seaton, Scottham, & Sellers, 2006; Yip,
[Tynes, 2007]). This process of exploration leads to Seaton, & Sellers, 2006). One reason for this is that a
increases in self-esteem (Umaña-Taylor, Gonzales- strong ethnic identity helps to foster a sense of mean-
Backen, & Guimond, 2009). Eventually, as the value of ing in life, which has been shown to be related to over-
having a strong ethnic identity becomes clear, the indi- all adjustment (Kiang & Fuligni, 2010).
vidual establishes a more coherent sense of personal The development of ethnic identity, like the develop-
identity that includes this ethnic identity (Seaton, ment of identity more broadly, is profoundly affected by
Yip, & Sellers, 2009; Whitehead, Ainsworth, Wittig, & the context in which adolescents live (Tsai & Fuligni,
Gadino, 2009). Consistent with this, a study of ethnic 2012). For instance, patterns of ethnic identity develop-
identity development among students attending ur- ment are affected by the ethnic composition of the
ban public schools found that adolescents’ feelings adolescent’s school, the adolescent’s immediate peer
about their own ethnic group became more positive group, and the extent to which the adolescent has con-
during both early and middle adolescence (when eth- tact with other teenagers from the same or different
nic identity first becomes salient and individuals be- backgrounds (Kiang, Witkow, Baldelomar, & Fuligni,
come immersed in their own culture), but that actual 2010; Nishina, Bellmore, Witkow, & Nylund-Gibson,
identity exploration did not really begin until middle 2010; Yip, Seaton, & Sellers, 2010). Some research also
adolescence, perhaps because it was at that point that indicates that moving through the early stages of ethnic

For individuals who are not part of the majority culture, integrating a sense of ethnic identity into their overall sense of personal identity is
likely to be an important task.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 275 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 275

identity development may be speeded up somewhat McMahon & Watts, 2002). One study ofdrug use found,
when parents take a more deliberate approach to the for instance, that ethnic pride was associated with less
socialization of an ethnic identity (McHale et al., 2006; use among Black, Mexican American, and biracial ado-
Quintana, Castaneda-English, & Ybarra, 1999; Umaña- lescents, but with more use among White adolescents
Taylor, Bhanot, & Shin, 2006; Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, (Marsiglia, Kulis, & Hecht, 2001).
Bámaca, & Guimond, 2009). Ethnic socialization (also The mental health of ethnic minority youth is also
referred to as “racial socialization”) is the process par- affected by their orientation to the mainstream culture.
ents use to attempt to teach their children about their In general, positive mental health among ethnic mi-
ethnic or racial identity and about the special experi- nority adolescents is associated with what might be
ences they may encounter within the broader society as described as “savvy biculturalism”—having a strong,
a result of their ethnic background. Ethnic socialization positive ethnic identity and a healthy awareness of
in minority families typically focuses on at least three the potential for discrimination, but not with outright
themes: understanding and valuing one’s culture, deal- rejection of the mainstream culture (Juang, Nguyen, &
ing with racism, and succeeding in mainstream society Lin, 2006; Pillay, 2005; A. Smith & Lalonde, 2003;
(Chao & Otsuki-Clutter, 2011; Lesane-Brown, 2006; Umaña-Taylor, 2004; Yasui, Dorham, & Dishion, 2004).
Moua & Lamborn, 2010). However, although ethnic so- For example, among ethnic minority youth, academic
cialization by parents may speed up the process of eth- achievement is highest when adolescents feel connected
nic identity development and encourage adolescents to to their ethnic group, are aware of racism, and believe
think positively about their ethnic heritage, whether it that it is important to the people in their life to be
leads to a stronger sense of ethnic identity (i.e., whether academically successful within mainstream society
it makes ethnic background more central to how they (Altschul, Oyserman, & Bybee, 2006). Being aware of
see themselves) isn’t clear—some studies find that it potential racism and mistrusting others are not the
does (e.g., Rivas-Drake, Hughes, & Way, 2009; Umaña- same thing, however; awareness of racism is associated
Taylor, Alfaro, Bámaca, & Guimond, 2009; Umaña- with better achievement, but mistrust is associated with
Taylor & Guimond, 2010), but many do not (e.g., doing more poorly in school (Huynh & Fuligni, 2008).
DeBerry, Scarr, & Weinberg, 1996; Marshall, 1995;
Phinney & Chavira, 1995; Supple, Ghazarian, Frabutt,
Plunkett, & Sands, 2006). Nor is more ethnic socializa-
tion necessarily better; in one study, the best-adjusted MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
Black adolescents came from homes in which their How strong is your own sense of ethnic
mothers provided a moderate number of racial social- identity? Have there been points in your life
ization messages, rather than many or few (Frabutt, when it was especially important? How has
Walker, & MacKinnon-Lewis, 2002). Another found it changed as you’ve gotten older?
that the less frequent communication of highly positive
messages may be most effective (Neblett, Smalls, Ford,
Nguyên, & Sellers, 2009). Ethnic socialization also ❚ Recent Immigrants Several researchers have
occurs indirectly, for example, when parents stress the focused on the special situation of ethnic minority
importance of family obligations (Kiang & Fuligni, youth who are recent immigrants to a new culture
2009; Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, Bámaca, & Guimond, (e.g., Fuligni, Hughes, & Way, 2009; Gonzales, 2011;
2009). Interestingly, having positive attitudes about Phinney, Romero, Nava, & Huang, 2001). Despite the
one’s own ethnic group is correlated with having posi- fact that adolescents who are recent immigrants fre-
tive attitudes about adolescents from other ethnic quently report high levels of academic, familial, social,
groups, suggesting that ethnic socialization may en- and economic stress (Cervantes & Cordova, 2011), for-
hance, rather than upset, interracial relations (Phinney, eign-born ethnic minority adolescents tend to express
Ferguson, & Tate, 1997). In fact, many adolescents with more positive feelings about mainstream American
a strong ethnic identity are members of peer crowds for ideals than do their counterparts whose families have
which ethnicity is not a defining feature; a better predic- been in the United States longer. In addition, in the
tor of whether an adolescent is a member of an ethni- United States, foreign-born and first generation ethnic
cally defined crowd is the proportion of that adolescent’s minority youth (i.e., adolescents whose parents were
friends who are from the same ethnic group (B. Brown, born in a different country) perform better in school
Herman, Hamm, & Heck, 2008). and are less likely to be involved in delinquent behavior
Having a strong ethnic identity is consistently asso-
ciated with higher self-esteem, stronger self-efficacy,
and better mental health among minority youngsters,
more so than among White youth (DuBois et al., 2002; ethnic socialization The process through which individuals
Mandara, Gaylord-Harden, Richards, & Ragsdale, 2009a; develop an understanding of their ethnic or racial background,
also referred to as racial socialization.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 276 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

276 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

or have physical, emotional, and behavioral problems expectations of adults in the community. As one girl
than adolescents from the same ethnic group whose from Little Saigon noted,
parents were born in America (Kulis, Marsiglia, &
You want to do what makes you feel happy or comfort-
Hurdle, 2003; Prado et al., 2009; Rumbaut, 1997), a
able but they put you in this little ethnicity box. Espe-
phenomenon that is known as the immigrant paradox. cially here. Going to church you wear ao dai [traditional
One explanation for this is that ethnic minority immi- Vietnamese dress]. I’m not rebelling but I don’t want to
grants arrive in their new country idealistic about their be another Asian girl in an American crowd. I want to be
prospects, but the longer their family lives in the new myself. I’m not going against my parents or tradition.
context, the more likely they are to become disillu- I want to make my own morals and traditions and it
sioned (Tartakovsky, 2009). Another explanation for makes me happy. (p. 682)
the higher achievement and better mental health of
immigrant adolescents is that newly arrived immi- Adolescents living outside the enclave had more op-
grantparents provide relatively closer supervision of portunities to explore a wider range of alternatives, but
their children (Chao & Otsuki-Clutter, 2011; Harker, were more passive in their identity exploration and of-
2001; Pradoet al., 2009). ten actively fought attempts by their parents to encour-
The process of identity development among adoles- age a strong Vietnamese identity. As one boy put it:
cents from recent immigrant families also depends on My parents expect me to speak Vietnamese consistently.
the context in which the family lives, as a study of Viet- Every now and then they just say that I forgot it and that
namese adolescents from Southern California illus- I don’t know how to speak it anymore. . . . Of course,
trates (Vo-Jutabha, Dinh, McHale, & Valsiner, 2009). I understand it and my parents expect me to be in a Viet
In this study, the researchers compared the identity Club or something. But I mean c’mon, really c’mon.
development of adolescents living in a Vietnamese en- (pp. 683–684)
clave (“Little Saigon”) with those who lived nearby, but
in a more diverse community. For adolescents living
DISCRIMINATION AND ITS EFFECTS
inside the enclave, pressures on them to adopt a strong
Vietnamese identity had both helped focus their iden- The task of developing a coherent sense of identity is
tity development but also constrained it, frustrating much more complicated for minority adolescents than
those teenagers whose behavior conflicted with the for their majority counterparts (Gray-Little & Hafdahl,

Discrimination item Males Females

You are treated with less courtesy than other people.

You are treated with less respect than other people.

You receive poorer service than others at restaurants or stores.

People act as if they think you are not smart.


People act as if they are afraid of you.

People act as if they think you are dishonest.

People act as if they’re better than you are.

You are called names or insulted.

You are threatened or harassed.

You are followed around in stores.

Your teachers treat you with less respect than other students.

Your teachers act as if they think you are not smart.

Your teachers act as if they are afraid of you.

Total percentage reporting any of the above

10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Percent
FIGURE 8.5 The experience of discrimination is common among Black adolescents. (Seaton et al., 2008)
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 277 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 277

Depression Affiliation with Risky and


Perceived
and antisocial antisocial
discrimination
alienation peers behavior
FIGURE 8.6 One pathway linking perceived discrimination to antisocial behavior is through the impact of discrimination on
depression and alienation, which leads adolescents to affiliate with antisocial peers. (Adapted from M. Roberts et al., 2012)

2000; H. Stevenson, Reed, Bodison, & Bishop, 1997). Be- Growing up in a racially isolated, poor neighborhood
cause identity development is profoundly influenced by especially intensifies feelings of discrimination, which
the social context in which the adolescent lives, the increases adolescents’ involvement in violence (Martin
development of minority adolescents must be under- et al., 2011).
stood in relation to the specific context that they face in Individuals vary both in the extent to which they
contemporary society (Byrd & Chavous, 2011; Chao & feel discriminated against and in the extent to which
Otsuki-Clutter, 2011; Garcia Coll et al., 1996; Mark- they are adversely affected by it, and it appears that eth-
strom, 2011a; Rivas-Drake et al., 2009). All too often, nic identity is an important factor (Greene, Way, &
this context includes racial stereotypes, discrimination, Pahl, 2006; Oppedal, Røysamb, & Sam, 2004; Sellers et
and mixed messages about the costs and benefits of al., 2006). Psychologist Robert Sellers and colleagues
identifying too closely with the majority culture (see developed the multidimensional model of racial iden-
Figure 8.5). Immigrant adolescents who speak with an tity to help make sense out of a complex web of find-
accent are stereotyped as “perpetual foreigners,” which ings (Sellers et al., 2006). According to this model, we
can lead to discrimination and victimization (S. Kim, need to take into account three different aspects of ra-
Wang, Deng, Alvarez, & Li, 2011). One recent study of cial identity: racial centrality (how important race is in
American adolescents of Iranian descent revealed that defining individuals’ identity), private regard (how in-
many of these teenagers identified themselves as Persian, dividuals feel about being a member of their race), and
rather than Iranian, because of the negative portrayals of public regard (how individuals think others view their
Iranian individuals in the media (Daha, 2011). Similarly, race). Generally speaking, during adolescence private
it is impossible to fully understand the process of ethnic regard tends to be positive and remain stable across all
identity development among American Indian adoles- ethnic groups, but changes in public regard differ
cents without taking into account the particular history among adolescents from different backgrounds. In one
of American Indians in the United States (Markstrom, study of New York City middle school students, public
2011a). regard increased among Chinese American adoles-
It is well established that individuals—from any cents, but it declined among Black, Puerto Rican, and
group—who report experiencing high levels of dis- Dominican youth (Hughes, Way, & Rivas-Drake, 2011)
crimination suffer psychologically as a result. Many (see Figure 8.7). One possible explanation for this is
studies of Asian, Black, and Latino youth have found that Black and Latino students report that their teach-
that feeling discriminated against is predictive of subse- ers have more negative attitudes toward them than do
quent conduct problems, depression, and lower Asian youth, whose teachers generally see them in a
achievement in school (Benner & Graham, 2011; Brody more favorable light.
et al., 2006; Coker, Elliot, Kanouse, & Grunbaum, 2009; Not surprisingly, adolescents who have experienced
DeGarmo & Martinez, Jr., 2006; Huynh & Fuligni, discrimination firsthand are more likely to believe that
2010; Juang & Alverez, 2010; Romero, Carvajal, Valle, & the public has low regard for their ethnic group (Seaton
Orduña, 2007; Seaton, Caldwell, Sellers, & Jackson, et al., 2009). However, individuals who believe that the
2010; Seaton & Yip, 2009; Smokowski, Bacallao, & Bu-
chanan, 2009). There are all sorts of reasons that feeling
discriminated against may be harmful to one’s mental immigrant paradox The fact that on many measures of
health, but one important process derives from the psychological functioning and mental health, adolescents
effect of discrimination on adolescents’ feelings of who have immigrated more recently to the United States
control: Adolescents who feel discriminated against in score higher on measures of adjustment than adolescents
school report feeling less control over their academic from the same ethnic group whose family has lived in the
achievement, which leads to feelings of depression United States for several generations.
(Lambert, Herman, Bynum, & Ialongo, 2009). Another multidimensional model of racial identity (MMRI)
possibility is that the experience of discrimination leads Sellers’s perspective on ethnic identity, which emphasizes
to depression and alienation, which, in turn, leads ado- three different phenomena: racial centrality (how important
lescents to affiliate with deviant peers, increasing the race is in defining individuals’ identity), private regard
likelihood that they will engage in risky and antisocial (how individuals feel about being a member of their race),
behavior (M. Roberts et al., 2012) (see Figure 8.6). and public regard (how individuals think others feel about
their race).
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 278 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

278 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

4.8
4.4
4.6
4.2
4.4
4.0
4.2
Private regard

3.8

Private regard
4.0
3.6
3.8
3.4

3.6 3.2
African American African American
3.4 Puerto Rican 3.0 Puerto Rican
Dominican Dominican
3.2 Chinese 2.8 Chinese
6th 7th 8th 6th 7th 8th
Grade Grade
FIGURE 8.7 Feelings about one’s own ethnic group (private regard) remain stable during middle adolescence, regardless of
ethnicity. Patterns of change in beliefs about how one’s group is perceived by others (public regard) differ across ethnic
groups, however. (Hughes et al., 2011)

public has low regard for their ethnic group are more potential racism) (Berkel et al., 2009; Delgado, Upde-
sensitive to racial cues, which, in turn, may heighten graff, Roosa, & Umaña-Taylor, 2011; Harris-Britt,
their experience of discrimination; this process may be Valrie, Kurtz-Costes, & Rowley, 2007; Juang & Alvarez,
accentuated in families where parents engage in rela- 2010; Neblett et al., 2008; Smokowski et al., 2009). Hav-
tively more ethnic socialization (Rivas-Drake et al., ing a strong ethnic identity is also protective against the
2009). This is not to say that individuals with height- harmful effects of online discrimination (Tynes,
ened sensitivity to discrimination are simply imagining Umaña-Taylor, Rose, Lin, & Anderson, 2012).
it; rather, individuals with heightened sensitivity may However, the impact of having race as a central part
be better at perceiving more subtle signs of genuine ra- of one’s identity (which, as you now know, is not the
cial bias. Interestingly, one recent study found that the same thing as having high private regard for one’s ethnic
experience of feeling discriminated against adversely group) is complicated: It makes adolescents more sensi-
affects adolescents’ mental health regardless of what tive to discrimination (which hurts their mental health),
they attribute the discrimination to (e.g., ethnicity, but some studies show that it makes them more able to
gender, physical appearance, etc.) (Seaton, Caldwell, cope with it (which helps) (Berkel et al., 2010; C. S.
Sellers, & Jackson, 2010). Brown, Alabi, Huynh, & Masten, 2011; Chavous, Rivas-
How all of this works together to affect adolescents’ Drake, Smalls, Griffin, & Cogburn, 2008; Greene et al.,
mental health is tricky. Not surprisingly, having positive 2006; Kiang, Peterson, & Thompson, 2011; Thomas et
feelings about one’s race is positively linked to psycho- al., 2009), although not all studies find this (e.g., Deng,
logical well-being and protects against the harmful Kim, Vaughan, & Li, 2010; Seaton, Neblett, Upton,
effects of stress and discrimination (Benner & Kim, Hammond, & Sellers, 2011). Believing that the public
2009; C. H. Caldwell, Sellers, Bernat, & Zimmerman, has high regard for one’s ethnic group lifts adolescents’
2004; Galliher, Jones, & Dahl, 2011; Kiang, Yip, school performance, but believing that the public has a
Gonzales-Backen, Witkow, & Fuligni, 2006; Sellers et positive view of one’s race also intensifies the effects of
al., 2006; Thomas, Caldwell, Faison, & Jackson, 2009; discrimination—perhaps because people feel especially
Umaña-Taylor & Updegraff, 2007). Consistent with wounded when they don’t expect to encounter it
this, adolescents whose parents have emphasized the (Hughes, Way, & Rivas-Drake, 2011; Sellers, Copeland-
positive aspects of ethnic socialization (e.g., having Linder, Martin, & Lewis, 2006).
pride in one’s ethnic group) and who have more posi- One study of ethnic identity and academic achieve-
tive family relationships fare better in the face of ment illustrates how racial centrality, private regard,
discrimination than those whose parents have empha- and public regard work together to influence Black
sized the negative (e.g., the need to be wary about adolescents’ school performance and motivation
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 279 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 279

(Chavous et al., 2003). The researchers identified four so little about multiracial adolescents, given the fact
distinct clusters within their sample of 600 students: that in many studies they are impossible to identify.
buffering/defensive (high centrality, high private regard, A study by sociologist Melissa Herman took advan-
low public regard; 29% of the sample), low connected- tage of a unique opportunity to examine what biracial
ness/high affinity (low centrality, high private regard, adolescents do when forced to choose an ethnic identity
low public regard; 21%), idealized (high on all three and whether biracial adolescents who choose one eth-
dimensions; 31%), and alienated (low on all three di- nicity to describe themselves differ from adolescents
mensions; 19%). Alienated students were the most dis- with parents of the same two ethnic backgrounds who
engaged from school and most likely to drop out, while choose the other ethnicity for self-description (Her-
idealized students were more likely to hold positive be- man, 2004). In a survey that I conducted many years
liefs about school. But it was members of the buffering/ ago with Sanford Dornbusch and Bradford Brown, we
defensive group who were the least likely to drop out accidentally asked adolescents about their ethnic back-
and the most likely to be enrolled in college two years ground twice: once when we asked them to tell us the
after graduation. The researchers speculated that the ethnic background of each of their parents, and once
success of this group was due to the combination of when we asked them to identify their own ethnic back-
strong feelings of group pride and a realistic expecta- ground but did not permit them to choose more than
tion of facing discrimination. Sadly, as long as discrim- one category. (To be honest, this was a mistake that we
ination persists in American society, adolescents who overlooked when developing the questionnaire, but the
are its victims will need to protect themselves against its truth is that researchers’ mistakes often lead to more
pernicious effects. interesting discoveries than their deliberate choices.)
Capitalizing on our error, Herman was able to iden-
tify biracial adolescents (based on their descriptions of
MULTIETHNIC ADOLESCENTS
each of their parents) and examine how they described
One understudied group of adolescents for whom de- themselves. For instance, some adolescents with a
veloping a sense of ethnic identity may be especially White father and a Hispanic mother, when forced to
challenging consists of multiethnic youth—adoles- choose, described themselves as White, others said they
cents whose parents are not from the same ethnic or were Hispanic, and a third group chose neither. Two
racial group. Understanding psychological develop- specific findings from this study are noteworthy. First,
ment among multiethnic adolescents has taken on in- individuals with one Black parent were significantly
creased importance as their numbers have grown (Chao more likely to identify themselves as Black than as a
& Otsuki-Clutter, 2011). In 2010, 17 percent of all in- member of the second group. Second, individuals with
fants with a Black parent were born to one Black parent one White and one ethnic minority parent were more
and one White parent, and 34 percent of all infants with likely to identify themselves as a member of the ethnic
an Asian parent were born to one Asian parent and one minority group, regardless of the ethnicity.
White parent (Frey, 2012). Herman then asked whether certain factors, beyond
You’ve probably filled out many forms and applica- the ethnicities of his or her parents, predicted which
tions asking you to indicate your racial or ethnic back- ethnic group an adolescent chose. Although few vari-
ground, and for the majority of individuals, this is not ables operated the same across all ethnic groups, two
an especially challenging task. But what about individ- that did were the adolescent’s physical appearance
uals who are of mixed heritage? Today, when question- (based on ratings of a yearbook photo) and the ethnic
naires ask individuals to provide information on their background of the adolescent’s friends. Biracial adoles-
ethnic background, they typically provide respondents cents who appeared White to the yearbook raters were
with the opportunity to indicate that they are multira- more likely to identify themselves as White, and biracial
cial, by permitting them to select more than one an- adolescents who reported that the majority of their
swer. Prior to 2000, however, even the U.S. Census friends were from an ethnic minority group were less
forced people to choose an identification from one of likely to identify themselves as White.
several broad categories (for example, Asian, Black, La- Developing a consistent ethnic identity is challenging
tino, White, Other). The problem, of course, is that a for biracial adolescents, especially during early adoles-
forced choice of this sort demands that multiracial in- cence (Marks, Patton, & Coll, 2011). Actually, many bi-
dividuals either select one of the ethnic groups or use racial adolescents change their racial identity over time,
the category “Other,” neither of which allows them to switching between adolescence and young adulthood
be identified as multiracial. Moreover, each ethnic cat- from being biracial to being “monoracial” (identifying
egory will include individuals with two parents from
that group as well as multiracial individuals who have
an especially strong identification with one of their
parents’ ethnic heritage. It is little wonder that we know multiethnic Having two parents of different ethnic or racial
backgrounds.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 280 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

280 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

oneself with just one ethnic group); in one study of


Black, White, and biracial Black/White adolescents,
Gender-Role Development
nearly three-quarters of the biracial group changed Although adolescent boys and girls differ in obvious
their self-identification over a 4-year period, which may ways in their physical development, males and females
reflect the ways in which the cultural desirability of be- are far more alike than dissimilar in their attitudes, abil-
ing seen as a member of one ethnic group or another ities, and behaviors. Popular books proclaim that men
fluctuates over time (Terry & Winston, 2010). Although and women are fundamentally different; that men and
this switching has been observed in all ethnic groups, in women come from different “planets”; that males and
one study it was especially common among adolescents females learn, speak, and navigate the world in different
who identified as Native Americans, either exclusively ways; and that adolescent boys and girls need to be
or in combination with a White self-identification schooled and raised in different ways. But the fact of the
(Hitlin, Brown, & Elder, Jr., 2006). The majority of in- matter is that, apart from some obvious physical differ-
dividuals who had identified themselves as both White ences, adolescent males and females actually aren’t all
and Native American when they were adolescents iden- that different (Perry & Pauletti, 2011; Priess & Hyde,
tified themselves only as White when they were young 2011). I’m sorry to disappoint you (if I have), but scien-
adults. However, a large number of adolescents who tific studies of the sexes simply do not support the
identified themselves only as White identified them- claims of popular writers who argue that males and fe-
selves as both White and Native American several years males have brains that are wired in fundamentally dif-
later. Similar patterns of change were seen among other ferent ways, have different perspectives on morality, or
multiracial individuals, but not nearly as often. learn in fundamentally different ways.
Whether large sex differences in adolescent behavior
had existed in the past but have disappeared (certainly
RECAP a possibility, given the fact that men and women faced
• Many researchers have studied the process through different expectations and opportunities in past gener-
which ethnic identity develops and the implications ations) or whether they were just assumed to be larger
of having a strong versus weak sense of ethnic iden- than they were isn’t known. But differences within
tity for adolescent adjustment and behavior. groups of males or females are far more substantial
• The process of ethnic identity development fol- than differences between them. In this and other chap-
lows the process of identity development in gen- ters, I’ve noted when studies have found meaningful sex
eral, with an unquestioning view of the self being differences in the ways in which adolescents develop or
displaced or upset by an event or experience that function. If I haven’t mentioned them, it’s either be-
provokes a period of exploration. Over time, a cause they weren’t reported or weren’t observed.
more coherent sense of ethnic identity emerges. Apart from differences in strength, adolescent males
• Ethnic socialization is the process through which and females do not differ in their abilities, and although
parents attempt to teach their children about their girls are more “people-oriented” and boys are more
ethnic identity and about the special experiences “things-oriented,” the magnitude of sex differences in
they may encounter within the broader society as a interests and attitudes is smaller than most people
result of their ethnic background. think (Priess & Hyde, 2011). The most consistent sex
• Positive mental health among ethnic minority ado- differences are seen in the ways adolescent boys and
lescents is associated with having a strong, positive girls express aggression (males are generally more phys-
ethnic identity and an awareness of the potential ically aggressive than females, who tend to use social or
for discrimination, but not with outright rejection verbal aggression) and intimacy (females are more
of the mainstream culture. likely to express intimacy verbally, whereas males ex-
• Adolescents who report experiencing high levels of press it mainly through shared activities), and in the
discrimination suffer psychologically as a result, extent to which males and females are prone to low self-
but individuals vary both in the extent to which esteem and depression (females are more prone to
they feel discriminated against and in the extent to both). There are few, if any, sex differences in patterns
which they are adversely affected by it. of family relationships, performance on achievement
• One understudied group of adolescents for whom tests, or in the correlates of competence, popularity
developing a sense of ethnic identity may be espe- with peers, and healthy psychological development.
cially challenging are multiethnic youth—
adolescents whose parents are
not from the same eth- GENDER-ROLE SOCIALIZATION DURING
nic or racial ADOLESCENCE
group. Despite the fact that psychological differences between
the sexes are trivial or nonexistent, many individuals
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 281 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 281

Pressures to behave in sex-stereotypic ways may temporarily intensify during middle adolescence.

continue to hold strong beliefs about what is “normal” Not all studies find an increase in gender-stereo-
for males and for females, and psychologists have been typed behavior in middle adolescence (e.g., Jacobs et
interested in the consequences of behaving in ways that al., 2002; McHale, Kim, Whiteman, & Crouter, 2004) or
are stereotypically masculine or feminine. Some studies in pressure to conform to traditional gender roles
have found that pressures to behave in sex-stereotypic (Priess, Lindberg, & Hyde, 2009). One reason for dis-
ways appear to increase temporarily during middle ad- crepancies between studies is that the extent to which
olescence, a phenomenon referred to as the gender in- gender-stereotypic behavior becomes more pro-
tensification hypothesis (Galambos, Berenbaum, & nounced or demanded in adolescence likely depends
McHale, 2009; Lobel, Nov-Krispin, Schiller, Lobel, & on the realm of behavior studied, the developmental
Feldman, 2004). Individuals’ beliefs about gender roles history of the adolescent, and the broader context in
become more flexible as they move through adoles- which the adolescent lives (Daniels & Leaper, 2011;
cence, largely as a result of the cognitive changes of the Galambos, Berenbaum, & McHale, 2009; McHale,
period, but social pressures may drive teenagers toward Shanahan, Updegraff, Crouter, & Booth, 2004; H. M.
more gender-stereotypic behavior; indeed, the impact Watt, 2004). For instance, although individuals become
of environmental factors on gender-role behavior is more traditional in their attitudes about gender roles
much stronger than the impact of the hormonal between early and middle adolescence, this pattern is
changes of puberty (Galambos et al., 2009). As teenag- not universal. One study of changes in gender role at-
ers begin to date, for example, it may become more im- titudes broke the sample down by sex, birth order, and
portant for them to act in ways that are consistent with the sorts of attitudes the parents had. Whereas firstborn
gender-role expectations and that meet with approval boys with brothers and parents with traditional
in the peer group. Boys who do not act masculine
enough and girls who do not act feminine enough may
be less popular with and less accepted by their same-
and opposite-sex peers (T. E. Smith & Leaper, 2006). gender intensification hypothesis The idea that pressures
to behave in sex-appropriate ways intensify during adolescence.
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 282 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

282 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

attitudes about gender become more traditional in nor girls reduce the display of traditionally masculine
their own attitudes over time, second-born girls with traits, such as instrumentality (McHale, Kim, Dotterer,
brothers and less traditional parents did not (Crouter, Crouter, & Booth, 2009) (see Figure 8.8).
Whiteman, McHale, & Osgood, 2007). It is easier for girls to behave sometimes in masculine
ways during adolescence than it is for boys to act occa-
sionally in feminine ways. Consistent with research on
younger children, during adolescence—at least in con-
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
temporary American society—males who do not con-
The gender intensification hypothesis form to traditionally masculine gender-role norms are
was developed more than 30 years ago. judged more deviant than are females whose behavior
Based on your recollection of adoles- departs from exclusively feminine roles. Interestingly,
cence, does it still ring true? however, boys who have a more traditionally masculine
orientation, while higher in self-acceptance than other
boys, are more likely to be involved in various types of
problem behavior—perhaps because part of being mas-
culine in contemporary society involves being “macho”
enough to experiment with delinquency, drugs and al-
MASCULINITY AND FEMININITY cohol, and unprotected sex (Kulis, Marsiglia, & Hurdle,
If expectations to conform to traditional gender stereo- 2003), or because boys who live in difficult environ-
types intensify during adolescence, we would expect ments, where problem behavior is prevalent, adopt a
that boys who are especially masculine and girls who more “macho” posture to survive in the community
are especially feminine would fare better psychologi- (Cunningham, 1999). Conversely, girls who have a more
cally than their peers who behave in gender-atypical traditionally feminine gender-role orientation are more
ways. Do more feminine girls and more masculine boys likely to develop more traditionally feminine sorts of
feel better about themselves? psychological problems, such as disordered eating
Recent research suggests that the answer to this (McHale, Corneal, Crouter, & Birch, 2001).
question may differ for males and females (Galambos, Given that pressures to conform with gender-role
Berenbaum, & McHale, 2009). Although boys and girls norms affect both girls and boys during adolescence,
who behave in gender-typical ways are more accepted why is it that boys suffer greater self-image problems
than their peers whose behavior does not conform with when they deviate from what is viewed as appropriate
gender-role stereotypes, and feel better about them- behavior for their sex? The answer is that although girls
selves as a result of this (Menon, 2011), the costs of be- may be pressured to adopt (or maintain) certain femi-
ing gender-atypical are greater for boys than girls (T. E. nine traits during adolescence, they are not necessarily
Smith & Leaper, 2006). It is not surprising, therefore, to pressured to relinquish all elements of masculinity. In
find that during adolescence boys are likely to cut back contrast, boys are socialized from a very early age not to
on the display of traditionally feminine traits, such as adopt feminine traits and are judged deviant if they
being emotionally expressive, whereas neither boys show signs of femininity. Consistent with this, boys are

Girls
Boys
25 25
24
24
Instrumentality

23
Expressivity

23 22
21
22 20
21 19
18
20 17
7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
Age Age
FIGURE 8.8 During adolescence, boys increasingly avoid displaying stereotypically feminine traits, but comparable pres-
sure on girls to avoid stereotypically masculine traits is much milder. As a consequence, boys show a drop in emotional ex-
pressiveness, but girls do not show a similar decline in instrumentality. (McHale, Kim, Dotterer, Crouter, & Booth, 2009)
ste32118_ch08_251-283.indd Page 283 21/03/13 10:31 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 8 Identity 283

more likely to see themselves as “typical males” than RECAP


girls are to see themselves as “typical females,” more • One aspect of adolescent identity involves gender-role
likely to be content to be male than girls are to be fe- development. Although actual differences between
male, and more pressured to act in stereotypically male adolescent males and females are not substantial, ado-
ways than girls are to act in stereotypically female ways lescents may feel especially strong pressure to adhere
(Egan & Perry, 2001). to stereotypical roles for males and females.
In other words, girls can be highly pressured during • The extent to which individuals’ gender-role behavior
adolescence to behave in feminine ways without neces- changes during adolescence is influenced by many
sarily being punished or labeled deviant for exhibiting factors, including birth order, the presence and sex of
some masculine traits at the same time; thus, for girls, older or younger siblings, and their parents’ attitudes.
having a mixture of masculine and feminine traits is a • In general, among both males and females, many
viable alternative to exclusive femininity. Girls may feel traits traditionally labeled as masculine are associ-
increasingly pressured to dress nicely and to wear ated in adolescence with better adjustment and
makeup when they reach adolescence, but they are greater peer acceptance. As a result, females who
not pressured to give up athletics or other typically have a mix of both masculine and feminine
masculine interests. Boys, however, from childhood on, traits and masculine males report
are pressured not to behave in feminine ways. Their higher self-esteem than
gender-role socialization does not intensify during ado- their peers do.
lescence as much as it does for girls because it is so
intense to begin with.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 284 21/03/13 11:01 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 285 21/03/13 11:01 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
9
Autonomy
AUTONOMY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE
THE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTIONAL AUTONOMY
Emotional Autonomy and Detachment
Emotional Autonomy and Individuation
Research on Emotional Autonomy
Emotional Autonomy and Parenting Practices
THE DEVELOPMENT OF BEHAVIORAL AUTONOMY
Changes in Decision-Making Abilities
When Do Adolescents Make Decisions as Well as Adults?
Changes in Susceptibility to Influence
Ethnic and Cultural Differences in Expectations for Autonomy
Changes in Feelings of Self-Reliance
THE DEVELOPMENT OF COGNITIVE AUTONOMY
Moral Development During Adolescence
Prosocial Reasoning, Prosocial Behavior, and Volunteerism
Political Thinking During Adolescence
Religious Beliefs During Adolescence

285
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 286 21/03/13 11:01 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

286 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

When I got my driver’s license, I looked at it as freedom, the adaptive nature of autonomy-seeking during
getting out from my parents, going places you couldn’t get adolescence, suggesting that the adolescents’ need to
to before because you had to have your parent’s permission distance themselves from their parents has an evolution-
. . . not having to be worried about being dropped off and
ary basis, reflecting an increase in novelty-seeking and
being picked up. Going to the library even or a dance club or a
party and not having to worry about, I don’t know, your
exploration that facilitates reproduction outside of the
parents having to come pick you up. Not necessarily, it wasn’t family (Casey et al., 2010; Steinberg, 2010).
embarrassment for me, it was more, just like okay, I make my Because today’s adolescents spend so much time away
own choices I can leave when I want to, you know, I made this from the direct supervision of adults, either by them-
decision to come here you know I’ll drop you off. I felt more selves or with their peers, learning how to govern their
like an adult but um, I don’t know, I went to the beach, to own behavior in a responsible fashion is a crucial task.
people’s houses, friends’ houses, the movies. (Best, 2006, p. 63)
Given the large numbers of single-parent and two-career
FOR MOST ADOLESCENTS, like the teenager quoted households in many industrialized countries, many young
above, establishing a sense of autonomy is as impor- people are expected to supervise themselves for a good
tant a part of becoming an adult as is establishing a part of the day. Many feel pressured—by parents, by
sense of identity. Becoming an autonomous person—a friends, and by the media—to grow up quickly and to act
self-governing person—is one of the fundamental like adults at an earlier age. Many adolescents who grow
developmental tasks of adolescence. up in poverty feel a different sort of pressure to grow
Although we often use the words autonomy and inde- up—they are expected to take on adult responsibilities to
pendence interchangeably, in the study of adolescence, assist their families during times of need (Burton, 2007).
they mean slightly different things. Independence gener- There is a curious paradox in all of this, however. At
ally refers to individuals’ capacity to behave on their own. the same time that adolescents have been asked to be-
The growth of independence is surely a part of becoming come more autonomous psychologically and socially,
autonomous during adolescence, but, as you will see in they have become less autonomous economically. Be-
this chapter, autonomy has emotional and cognitive as cause of the extension of schooling well into the 20s for
well as behavioral components. In other words, auton- most individuals, and because of the difficulty many
omy is not just about acting independently—it is also young adults have had finding employment during the
about feeling independent and thinking for oneself. recent recession, financial independence may not come
During adolescence, there is a movement away from until long after psychological independence has been
the dependency typical of childhood and toward the au- established. Many young people who are emotionally
tonomy typical of adulthood—not only among human independent find it frustrating to discover that they have
adolescents, but among mammals more generally (re- to abide by their parents’ rules as long as they are being
member that all mammals go through puberty) (Casey, supported economically. They may believe that the abil-
Duhoux, & Cohen, 2010). But the growth of autonomy ity to make their own decisions has nothing to do with
during adolescence is frequently misunderstood. financial dependence. An 18-year-old college freshman
Autonomy is often confused with rebellion, and becom- who has a part-time job, a full load of classes, and a seri-
ing an independent person is often equated with break- ous relationship with his girlfriend may be independent
ing away from the family. This perspective on autonomy in these respects, but he may nonetheless still be living
goes hand in hand with the idea that adolescence is in- at home because he can’t afford to do otherwise. His
evitably a time of stress and turmoil. But as we have seen parents may feel that as long as their son lives in their
in previous chapters, the view that adolescence is a pe- home, they should decide how late he can stay out at
riod of storm and stress has been questioned repeatedly night. But the adolescent may feel that his parents have
by scientific research. The same sort of rethinking has no right to tell him when he can come and go. This sort
taken place with regard to the development of auton- of difference of opinion can be a real source of prob-
omy. Rather than viewing adolescence as a time of rebel- lems and confusion for teenagers and their parents, par-
lion, researchers now see the growth of autonomy during ticularly when they have difficulty agreeing on a level of
adolescence as gradual, progressive, and—although independence for the adolescent (Steinberg, 2011). Dis-
important—relatively undramatic (McElhaney, Allen, agreements over autonomy-related concerns are at the
Stephenson, & Hare, 2009; Zimmer-Gembeck, Ducat, & top of the list of things that provoke quarrels between
Collins, 2011). Moreover, many writers have pointed to adolescents and parents (Laursen & Collins, 2009).
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 287 21/03/13 11:01 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 287

Autonomy as an Adolescent If establishing and maintaining a healthy sense of


autonomy is a lifelong concern, why has it attracted so
Issue much attention among scholars interested in adoles-
cence? When we look at the development of autonomy
Like identity, autonomy is a psychosocial concern that
in relation to the biological, cognitive, and social
surfaces and resurfaces during the entire life cycle.
changes of adolescence, it is easy to see why.
The development of independent behavior begins
long before puberty. Toddlers try to establish an ini-
❚ Puberty and the Development of Autonomy
tial sense of autonomy when they begin to explore
Consider first the impact of puberty. Some theorists
their surroundings on their own and assert their de-
have suggested that the physical changes of early ado-
sire to do as they please—a stage of development so
lescence trigger changes in the young person’s emo-
frustrating to parents that it is often called “the terri-
tional relationships at home (Laursen & Collins,
ble twos.” The toddler who insists on saying “No!” and
2009; Zimmer-Gembeck, Ducat, & Collins, 2011).
the young adolescent who insists on keeping her
Adolescents’ interest in turning away from their par-
whereabouts secret are both demonstrating their
ents and toward their peers for emotional support—
growing sense of independence and autonomy. And
a development that is part of establishing adult
just as psychologists see toddlers’ oppositional behav-
independence—may be sparked by their emerging in-
ior as normal, they also see adolescents’ interest in
terest in sexual relationships and by their concerns
privacy as normal, too—however frustrating that
over such things as dating and intimate friendships.
might be to their parents (McElhaney et al., 2009).
Indeed, from an evolutionary perspective, adolescent
Although childhood and adolescence are important
independence-seeking can be seen as a natural conse-
periods for the development of autonomy, issues of au-
quence of the individual’s sexual and physical matu-
tonomy are not resolved once and for all upon reach-
ration, and “leaving the home” after puberty is
ing young adulthood. Questions about being able to
something that is observed not just in humans, but in
function independently arise whenever individuals
other primates as well (Casey et al., 2010; Steinberg,
find themselves in positions that demand a new degree
2008). In some senses, then, puberty drives the ado-
of self-reliance. Following a divorce, for example,
lescent away from exclusive emotional dependence on
someone who has depended on a spouse over the years
the family. In addition, the changes in stature and
for economic support, guidance, or nurturance must
physical appearance occurring at puberty may pro-
find a way to function more autonomously and more
voke changes in how much autonomy the young per-
independently. During late adulthood, autonomy may
son is granted by parents and teachers. Youngsters
become a significant concern of the person who, after
may be given more responsibility by the adults around
losing a spouse, suddenly finds it necessary to depend
them simply because they look older.
on others for assistance and support.
❚ Cognitive Change and the Development of
Autonomy The cognitive changes of adolescence
also play an important role in the development of au-
tonomy (Albert & Steinberg, 2011b; Zimmer-Gembeck,
Ducat, & Collins, 2011). Part of being autonomous in-
volves being able to make independent decisions. When
individuals turn to others for advice, they often receive
conflicting opinions. For example, if you are trying to
decide between staying home to study for an exam and
going out to a party, your professor and the person
throwing the party will probably give you different ad-
vice. As an adult, you are able to see that each individu-
al’s perspective influences his or her advice. The ability
to see this, however, calls for a level of intellectual ab-
straction that is not available until adolescence. Being
able to take other people’s perspectives into account, to
reason in more sophisticated ways, and to foresee the
future consequences of alternative courses of action all
help the young person to weigh the opinions and sug-
gestions of others more effectively and to reach his or
Being able to drive greatly increases adolescents’ autonomy from her independent decisions. The cognitive changes of
parental control. adolescence also provide the logical foundation for
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 288 21/03/13 11:01 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

288 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

changes in the young person’s thinking about social, Psychologists have described autonomy in three ways
moral, and ethical problems. These changes in thinking (McElhaney et al., 2009; Zimmer-Gembeck, Ducat, &
are important prerequisites to the development of a Collins, 2011). The first characterization involves
system of values based on the individual’s own sense of emotional autonomy—that aspect of independence
right and wrong, and not merely on rules and regula- related to changes in the individual’s close relation-
tions handed down by parents or other authority fig- ships, especially with parents. The second is behavioral
ures (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2011; autonomy—the capacity to make independent deci-
Smetana & Villalobos, 2009). sions and follow through on them. And the third is re-
ferred to as cognitive autonomy (sometimes called
❚ Social Roles and the Development of Autonomy “value autonomy”), which involves having indepen-
Finally, changes in social roles and activities during ad- dent values, opinions, and beliefs.
olescence are bound to raise concerns related to inde-
pendence, as the adolescent moves into new positions
that demand increasing degrees of responsibility and RECAP
self-reliance (Coatsworth & Conroy, 2009; Halpern- • Although the development of autonomy is an im-
Felsher, 2011; Larson, Pearce, Sullivan, & Jarrett, 2007). portant psychosocial issue throughout the life span,
Becoming involved in new roles and taking on new re- it is especially salient during adolescence because
sponsibilities, such as having a job or a driver’s license, of the physical, cognitive, and social changes of the
place the adolescent in situations that require and stim- period.
ulate the development of independent decision mak- • Emotional autonomy (feeling independent) refers
ing. A teenager might not really think much about the to emotional independence in relationships with
responsibilities associated with taking a job, for exam- others, especially parents.
ple, until she actually ends up in one (D. Wood, Larson, • Behavioral autonomy (acting independently) refers to
& Brown, 2009). Choosing whether to drink does not the development of independent decision-making
become an important question until the adolescent be- abilities.
gins to approach the legal drinking age. And deciding • Cognitive autonomy (thinking independently)
what his political beliefs are becomes a more pressing concerns the development of inde-
concern when the young person realizes that he will pendent values, opinions,
soon have the right to vote. and beliefs.

MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION


Many psychologists contend that the
two periods of life during which The Development of
autonomy is an especially salient issue
are early adolescence and toddlerhood. Emotional Autonomy
What do these periods share in com- The relationship between children and their parents
mon that might account for the impor- changes repeatedly over the course of the life cycle.
tance of autonomy during each? Changes in the expression of affection, the distribution
of power, and patterns of verbal interaction, to give a
few examples, are likely to occur whenever important
transformations take place in the child’s or parents’
❚ Three Types of Autonomy We have talked a competencies, concerns, and social roles.
great deal thus far about the need to develop a sense of By the end of adolescence, individuals are far less
autonomy during adolescence. But what does it really emotionally dependent on their parents than they were
mean to be an autonomous or independent person? as children. We can see this in several ways. First, older
adolescents do not generally rush to their parents when
they are upset, worried, or in need of assistance. Sec-
ond, they do not see their parents as all-knowing or
emotional autonomy The establishment of more adultlike and all-powerful. Third, they often have a great deal of
less childish close relationships with family members and peers. emotional energy wrapped up in relationships outside
the family; in fact, they may feel more attached to a
behavioral autonomy The capacity to make independent
decisions and to follow through with them. boyfriend or a girlfriend than to their parents. And fi-
nally, older adolescents are able to see and interact with
cognitive autonomy The establishment of an independent set of their parents as people—not just as their parents. Many
values, opinions, and beliefs.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 289 21/03/13 11:01 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 289

parents find, for example, that they can confide in their


adolescent children, something that was not possible
when their children were younger, or that their adoles-
cent children can easily sympathize with them when
they have had a hard day at work. These sorts of changes
in the adolescent–parent relationship all reflect the de-
velopment of emotional autonomy (McElhaney et al.,
2009; Zimmer-Gembeck, Ducat, & Collins, 2011).

EMOTIONAL AUTONOMY
AND DETACHMENT
❚ Psychoanalytic Theory and Detachment
Early writings about emotional autonomy were influ-
enced by psychoanalytic thinkers such as Anna Freud
(1958), who argued that the physical changes of
puberty cause substantial disruption and conflict
inside the family system. The reason, Freud believed, is
In contrast to the view that tension between adolescents and their
that intrapsychic conflicts that have been repressed
parents is the norm, every major study done to date of family
since early childhood are reawakened at early adoles- relations in adolescence has shown that most teenagers and their
cence by resurgent sexual impulses. (These conflicts parents get along quite well.
revolve around the young child’s unconscious attrac-
tion toward the parent of the other sex and ambivalent
feelings toward the parent of the same sex.) The re- Collins, 2011). Although parents and adolescents
awakened conflicts are expressed as increased tension may bicker more often than they did during earlier
among family members, an increase in arguments, and periods of development, there is no evidence that this
a certain degree of discomfort around the house. As a bickering significantly diminishes closeness between
consequence of this tension, early adolescents are them in any lasting way (W. A. Collins & Steinberg,
driven to separate themselves, at least emotionally, 2006; Laursen & Collins, 2009). Most individuals re-
from their parents, and they turn their emotional port becoming closer to their parents in late adoles-
energies to relationships with peers—in particular, cence, especially after they have made the transition
peers of the opposite sex. Psychoanalytic theorists call into college (Lefkowitz, 2005; McElhaney et al., 2009).
this process of separation detachment, because to In other words, although teenagers and their par-
them it appears as though the early adolescent is ents undoubtedly modify their relationships during
attempting to sever the attachments that have been adolescence, their emotional bonds are by no means
formed during infancy and strengthened throughout severed. This is an important distinction, for it means
childhood. that emotional autonomy during adolescence involves
Freud and her followers viewed detachment, and a transformation, not a breaking off, of family rela-
the accompanying storm and stress inside the family, tionships (Laursen & Collins, 2009). In other words,
as normal, healthy, and inevitable aspects of emo- adolescents can become emotionally autonomous
tional development during adolescence. In fact, Freud from their parents without becoming detached from
believed that the absence of conflict between an ado- them (McElhaney et al., 2009), although achieving
lescent and his or her parents signified that the young this balance without difficulty may be more difficult
person was having problems growing up. in cultural contexts in which individualism is not as
strongly valued as it is in many Western cultures
❚ Research on Detachment Studies of adoles- (Yu, 2011). Interestingly, adolescents who are better
cents’ family relationships have not supported Freud’s able to balance autonomy and connectedness in their
view, however. In contrast to predictions that high relationships with their parents are also better able to
levels of adolescent–parent tension are the norm, that balance autonomy and intimacy in their romantic
adolescents detach themselves from relationships relationships (Taradash, Connolly, Pepler, Craig, &
with their parents, and that adolescents are driven Costa, 2001).
out of the household by unbearable levels of family
conflict, every major study done to date of teenagers’
relations with their parents has shown that most fam-
ilies get along quite well during the adolescent years detachment In psychoanalytic theory, the process through which
(McElhaney et al., 2009; Zimmer-Gembeck, Ducat, & adolescents sever emotional attachments to their parents or other
authority figures.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 290 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

290 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

EMOTIONAL AUTONOMY to which adolescents felt individuated within the


AND INDIVIDUATION relationship with their parents (“There are some
things about me that my parents do not know”).
As an alternative to the classic psychoanalytic per-
Scores on three of the four scales—all except “parents
spective on adolescent detachment, some theorists
as people”—increased over the age period studied.
have suggested that the development of emotional
Generally, early adolescence is a time during which
autonomy be looked at in terms of the adolescent’s
parents’ knowledge of their adolescent’s behavior
developing sense of individuation (Blos, 1967). The
declines, in part because parents are less likely to
process of individuation, which begins during in-
supervise their child and in part because adolescents
fancy and continues into late adolescence, involves a
become less likely to spontaneously disclose informa-
gradual, progressive sharpening of the young person’s
tion to their parents (Grundy, Gondoli, & Salafia,
sense of self as autonomous, competent, and separate
2010; Keijsers, Frijns, Branje, & Meeus, 2009). In one
from her or his parents. Individuation, therefore, has
study, for example, the researchers found that as
a great deal to do with the development of a sense of
adolescents aged the number of their friends whom
identity, in that it involves changes in how adolescents
their parents knew declined significantly, reflecting
come to see and feel about themselves.
greater individuation and privacy (Feiring & Lewis,
Individuation does not involve stress and turmoil,
1993). Other studies show that, with age, adolescents
however. Rather, individuation entails relinquishing
become less likely to say that they have the same opin-
childish dependencies on parents in favor of a more
ions as their parents or that they always agree with
mature, more responsible, and less dependent relation-
them (McElhaney et al., 2009; Zhang & Fuligni, 2006).
ship (McElhaney et al., 2009; Zimmer-Gembeck,
This, in turn, is associated with changes in adoles-
Ducat, & Collins, 2011). Adolescents who establish a
cents’ beliefs about their parents’ authority over them;
healthy sense of autonomy can accept responsibility for
as you read in Chapter 2, adolescents become increas-
their choices and actions instead of looking to their
ingly likely to draw distinctions between aspects of
parents to do it for them (Van Petegem, Beyers,
their life that their parents have the right to regulate
Vansteenkiste, & Soenens, 2012). For example, rather
and those that they think are not really their parents’
than rebelling against her parents’ midnight curfew by
business (Darling, Cumsille, & Martinez, 2008; Laird
deliberately staying out later, a girl who has a healthy
& Marrero, 2011; S. A. Perkins & Turiel, 2007).
sense of individuation might take her parents aside
Important changes in adolescents’ emotional rela-
before going out and say, “This party tonight may last
tionship with their parents occur as well. For example,
longer than midnight. If it does, I’d like to stay a bit
one study, which examined adolescent boys’ reports
longer. Suppose I call you at eleven o’clock and let you
of homesickness during summer camp, found that
know when I’ll be home?”
homesickness—which was experienced by youngsters
as anxiety and depression—became less prevalent
RESEARCH ON EMOTIONAL AUTONOMY during middle adolescence than it had been in early
adolescence or preadolescence (Thurber, 1995). And
Studies show that the development of emotional
adolescents’ willingness to express negative emotions
autonomy is a long process, beginning early in ado-
in front of their parents—for example, anger and
lescence and continuing into young adulthood
sadness—is lower during early adolescence than be-
(McElhaney et al., 2009). In one study (Steinberg &
fore or after, perhaps because keeping some emotional
Silverberg, 1986), a questionnaire measuring four
distance from one’s parents is a part of the individua-
aspects of emotional autonomy was administered to
tion process (Zeman & Shipman, 1997).
a sample of 10- to 15-year-olds. The four compo-
nents were (1) the extent to which adolescents
❚ De-Idealization Children frequently place their
de-idealized their parents (“My parents sometimes
parents on a pedestal; adolescents often knock them off
make mistakes”), (2) the extent to which adolescents
it. Psychologists believe that this de-idealization of
were able to see their parents as people (“My parents
parents may be one of the first aspects of emotional
act differently with their own friends than they do
autonomy to develop, because adolescents may shed
with me”), (3) nondependency, or the degree to
their childish images of their parents before replacing
which adolescents depended on themselves, rather
them with more mature ones. Yet, although middle ado-
than on their parents, for assistance (“When I’ve
lescents are less likely than young adolescents to hold
done something wrong, I don’t always depend on my
onto idealized pictures of their parents, when it comes to
parents to straighten things out”), and (4) the degree
seeing their parents as individuals, 15-year-olds are no
more emotionally autonomous than are 10-year-olds. In
other words, de-idealization is the beginning, not the
individuation The progressive sharpening of an individual’s sense end, of a long process that gradually leads adolescents to
of being an autonomous, independent person.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 291 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 291

that the development of emotional autonomy, and


individuation in particular, may have different psycho-
logical effects on adolescents depending on whether the
parent–child relationship is a close one. Adolescents
who become emotionally autonomous, but who also
feel distant or detached from their parents, score poorly
on measures of psychological adjustment, whereas ado-
lescents who demonstrate the same degree of emotional
autonomy, but who still feel close and attached to their
parents, are psychologically healthier than their peers
(J. Allen, Porter, McFarland, McElhaney, & Marsh,
2007; Lamborn & Steinberg, 1993; Mahoney, Schweder,
& Stattin, 2002).
In one study of Black, Mexican American, and White
adolescents, for example, young people from all ethnic
groups who reported greater feelings of separation from
their parents and higher levels of family conflict (a com-
bination indicative of detachment) also showed increases
in alcohol use over time, whereas alcohol use declined
among those who reported higher levels of individuation
and family cohesion (Bray, Adams, Getz, & Baer, 2001).
Taken together, these studies remind us that it is impor-
tant to distinguish between separating from one’s parents
in a way that nevertheless maintains emotional closeness
in the relationship (which is healthy) and breaking away
As adolescents develop emotional autonomy, they often begin to from one’s parents in a fashion that involves alienation,
question and challenge their parents more frequently. conflict, and hostility (which is unhealthy) (W. Beyers,
Goossens, Vansant, & Moors, 2003). Consistent with this,
lying to one’s parents and concealing undesirable things
from them, which may be more an indicator of detach-
adopt more realistic views of their parents. Even during
ment than of healthy individuation, is associated with
the high school years, adolescents appear to have some
psychological problems (Finkenauer, Engels, & Meeus,
difficulty in seeing their parents as individuals beyond
2002; Frijns, Finkenauer, Vermulst, & Engels, 2005). Not
their roles as parents. This aspect of emotional auton-
surprisingly, as individuals make the transition from
omy may not develop until much later—perhaps not
adolescence into adulthood and work through much of
until young adulthood (Smollar & Youniss, 1985). Seeing
the individuation process, they increasingly see lying to
one’s parents as people also appears to develop later in
their parents as unacceptable (Jensen, Arnett, Feldman, &
adolescents’ relations with their fathers than with their
Cauffman, 2004).
mothers, because fathers seem to interact less often with
their adolescents in ways that permit them to be seen as
❚ What Triggers Individuation? What triggers the
individuals (Smollar & Youniss, 1985).
process of individuation? Two different models have
been suggested (Laursen & Collins, 2009). According to
several researchers, puberty is the main catalyst (e.g.,
Holmbeck, 1996; Steinberg, 2000). Changes in the ado-
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
lescent’s physical appearance provoke changes in the way
When do you think you began seeing your that adolescents are viewed—by themselves and by their
parents as people who were more than just parents—which, in turn, provoke changes in parent–
your parents? child interaction. As we saw in Chapter 4, shortly after
puberty, most families experience an increase in bicker-
ing and squabbling. Some writers have suggested that
this increase in conflict helps adolescents see their par-
ents in a different light and develop a sense of individua-
❚ The Importance of Maintaining the Connec- tion (Steinberg, 1990). Consistent with this, adolescents’
tion Interestingly, and in contrast to the old view that feelings of connectedness to their parents may decline in
adolescents needed to sever their ties with their parents early adolescence, when bickering is more frequent, but
in order to grow up healthily, a number of studies find increase in late adolescence after this temporary period
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 292 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

292 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

of heightened squabbling is over (Pinquart & Silbereisen, with confidence or with trepidation, one fact is certain:
2002). Healthy individuation and positive mental health are
Other authors believe that adolescents’ movement fostered by close, not distant, family relationships
toward higher levels of individuation is stimulated by (McElhaney et al., 2009). Tense family relationships
their social-cognitive development (W. A. Collins, during adolescence indicate problems, not positive
1990; Smetana, 1995a). As you read in Chapter 2, social development. Researchers have found, for example,
cognition refers to the thinking we do about ourselves that adolescents who feel the most autonomous—that
and our relationships with others. The development of is, those who are most likely to feel that they have been
emotional autonomy in adolescence may be provoked granted enough freedom by their parents—are not the
by young people’s development of more sophisticated ones who have severed relationships at home. In fact,
understandings of themselves and their parents. Prior just the opposite is true: Autonomous adolescents
to adolescence, individuals accept their parents’ views report that they are close to their parents, enjoy doing
of themselves as accurate (“My parents think I am a things with their families, have few conflicts with their
good girl, so I must be”). But as individuals develop mothers and fathers, feel free to turn to them for
more differentiated self-conceptions in early and mid- advice, and say they would like to be like their parents
dle adolescence (as you read in Chapter 8), they come (McElhaney et al., 2009). Rebellion, negativism, and
to see that their parents’ view is but one of many—and excessive involvement in the peer group are more
one that may not be entirely accurate (“My parents common among psychologically immature adoles-
think I am a good girl, but they don’t know what I am cents than among mature ones (Steinberg, 1990). Even
really like”). By late adolescence, individuals are able to during college, students who live away from home
see that these apparent discrepancies between their (which is in its own way a type of autonomy)—as
self-conceptions and their parents’ views are perfectly opposed to remaining in their parents’ home and
understandable (“There are sides of me that my par- commuting to school—report more affection for their
ents know and sides of me that they don’t”) (Harter, parents, better communication, and higher levels of
2011). satisfaction in the relationship (Holmbeck, Durbin, &
This is not to suggest that the process of individua- Kung, 1995). In other words, strained family relation-
tion is always a smooth one. Some writers have sug- ships appear to be associated with a lack of autonomy
gested that as adolescents de-idealize their parents, they during adolescence, rather than with its presence
may begin to feel both more autonomous and more (Bomar & Sabatelli, 1996).
insecure—what one research team labeled a “double- At the same time, adolescents whose parents are
edged sword” (S. Frank, Pirsch, & Wright, 1990). That is, emotionally close to the point of being intrusive or
even though the images children have of their parents as overprotective—parents who use a lot of psychological
all-knowing and all-powerful may be inaccurate, the control—may have difficulty individuating from them,
images still provide a degree of emotional comfort. which may lead to depression, anxiety, and diminished
Leaving such images behind can be both liberating and social competence (J. Allen & McElhaney, 2000; Bean,
frightening, for parents as well as teenagers. Indeed, Bush, McKenry, & Wilson, 2003; Holmbeck et al., 2000;
some researchers have found that the development of Loukas, Ripperger-Suhler, & Horton, 2009; S. R. Miller,
emotional autonomy is associated not only with insecu- Tserakhava, & Miller, 2011; Sher-Censor, Parke, &
rity among adolescents but also with increased feelings Coltrane, 2011; Soenens et al., 2007). Adolescents whose
of anxiety and rejection among parents (Hock, Eberly, parents impede the individuation process are more
Bartle-Haring, Ellwanger, & Widaman, 2001; R. Ryan & likely to show signs of anxiety, depression, and other
Lynch, 1989; Steinberg & Steinberg, 1994). Difficulties forms of psychological distress (Barber, 1996; Q. Wang,
in the process of individuation may also arise when ado- Pomerantz, & Chen, 2007). Overprotectiveness may be
lescents push for independence at an earlier age than especially harmful for adolescents who are less compe-
parents are willing to grant it. Adolescents often believe tent to begin with (Thompson & Zuroff, 1999). As a
that individuals should be granted autonomy earlier consequence, the impact of parental psychological con-
than parents do (Ruck, Peterson-Badali, & Day, 2002). trol is even more harmful when it is accompanied by
negative self-evaluations by the adolescent; adolescents
who do not feel good about themselves and who have
EMOTIONAL AUTONOMY
very intrusive parents are especially vulnerable to
AND PARENTING PRACTICES
depression (Pomerantz, 2001). In contrast, around the
Whether provoked by puberty or by the development of world, adolescents whose parents provide support for
more advanced cognitive skills, and whether approached their growing interest in autonomy report being more
satisfied with life than those whose parents do not (see
Figure 9.1) (Ferguson, Kasser, & Jahng, 2011; Lekes,
psychological control Parenting that attempts to control the Gingras, Philippe, Koestner, & Fang, 2010).
adolescent’s emotions and opinions.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 293 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 293

Correlation Between Autonomy Support


Parental Autonomy Support and Life Satisfaction
6.0 0.60

5.5 0.55

5.0 0.50

4.5 0.45

4.0 0.40
Denmark Korea U.S. Denmark Korea U.S.
FIGURE 9.1 Although adolescents in different cultures receive different degrees of parental support for becoming
emotionally autonomous, the correlation between autonomy support and adolescents’ life satisfaction is similar in different
cultural contexts. (Ferguson, Kasser, & Jahng, 2011)

explained, it is not hard for the family to adjust and


MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
modify them as the child matures, physically, emotion-
Studies show that parents’ ally, and intellectually (Smetana & Asquith, 1994).
behavior can help or im- Gradual changes in family relations that permit the
pede the development of young person more independence and encourage more
emotional autonomy. responsibility but that do not threaten the emotional
Based on what you’ve read, bond between parent and child—in other words,
what advice would you give changes that promote increasing emotional autonomy—
the parents of a preadolescent are relatively easy to make for the family that has been
in this regard? flexible and has been making these sorts of modifica-
tions in family relationships all along (Vuchinich,
❚ Emotional Autonomy and Parenting Style As Angeletti, & Gatherum, 1996).
we saw in Chapter 4, adolescents’ development is af- In authoritarian households, where rules are rig-
fected differently by different styles of parenting. In idly enforced and seldom explained, adjusting to ado-
particular, independence, responsibility, and self- lescence is more difficult. Authoritarian parents may
esteem are all fostered by parents who are authorita- see the child’s increasing emotional independence as
tive (friendly, fair, and firm) rather than authoritarian rebellious or disrespectful, and they may resist their
(excessively harsh), indulgent (excessively lenient), adolescent’s growing need for independence rather
or indifferent (aloof to the point of being neglectful). than accepting it. For example, authoritarian parents,
At least some of these differences may be related to on seeing that their daughter is becoming interested
the development of emotional autonomy in different in boys, may implement a rigid curfew in order to
types of households. restrict her social life. Instead of encouraging auton-
In authoritative families, guidelines are established omy, authoritarian parents may inadvertently main-
for the adolescent’s behavior, and standards are up- tain the dependencies of childhood by failing to give
held, but they are flexible and open to discussion. their children sufficient practice in making decisions
Although parents may have the final say when it comes and being responsible for their actions. In essence,
to their child’s behavior, the decision that is reached authoritarian parenting may interfere with adoles-
usually comes after consultation and discussion— cent individuation.
with the child included. In discussing an adolescent’s When closeness, as well as support for autonomy, is
curfew, for example, authoritative parents will sit absent, the problems are compounded. In families in
down with their child and explain how they arrived at which excessive parental control is accompanied by
their decision and why they picked the hour they did. extreme coldness and punitiveness, adolescents may
They will also ask the adolescent for his or her sugges- rebel against their parents’ standards explicitly, in an
tions and consider them carefully in making a final attempt to assert their independence in a visible and
decision. demonstrable fashion (Kakihara, Tilton-Weaver, Kerr,
It is not difficult to see why the sort of give-and-take & Stattin, 2010). One recent study that tracked families
found in authoritative families is well suited to the over the transition into adolescence found that adoles-
healthy development of emotional autonomy. Because cents were more likely to “act out”—to misbehave—
standards and guidelines are flexible and adequately when their parents were overreactive (van den Akker,
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 294 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

294 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Deković, & Prinzie, 2010); another found that the more indicates that the growth of emotional autonomy
frequently parents called their adolescent’s cell phone, is typically smoother and less tumultuous than
the more dishonest the adolescent was (Weisskirch, stereotypes suggest.
2009). Such rebellion is not indicative of genuine emo- • Instead of emphasizing the young person’s need for
tional autonomy, though; it is more likely a demonstra- detachment, contemporary psychologists stress the
tion of the adolescent’s frustration with his or her more gradual process of individuation, which may
parents’ rigidity and lack of understanding. And, as be triggered by the physical and cognitive changes
noted earlier, when adolescents attempt to establish of early adolescence.
emotional autonomy within the context of a cold or • One of the first signs of individuation may be the
hostile family, the effect on their mental health is likely adolescent’s de-idealization of his or her parents.
to be negative (Lamborn & Steinberg, 1993). In fact, • Healthy individuation is fostered by close, not
adolescents from hostile or stressful family environ- distant, family relationships, in which adolescents
ments may do best when they actively detach them- are encouraged to develop and assert their
selves from their parents (Fuhrman & Holmbeck, 1995). individuality.
In both indulgent families and indifferent families, a • Adolescents who are raised in authoritative homes
different sort of problem arises. These parents do not in which their parents are both accepting and
provide sufficient guidance for their children, and as a tolerant of the young person’s individuality enjoy
result, the youngsters do not acquire adequate stan- many psychological advantages over their
dards for behavior. In the absence of parental guidance peers, including a more fully devel-
and rules, permissively reared teenagers often turn to oped sense of emotional
their peers for advice and emotional support—a prac- autonomy.
tice that can be problematic when the peers are them-
selves still relatively young and inexperienced. Not
surprisingly, adolescents whose parents have failed to
provide sufficient guidance are likely to become psy-
chologically dependent on their friends—emotionally
detached from their parents, perhaps, but not genu- The Development of
inely autonomous (Steinberg, 1990). The problems of
parental permissiveness are exacerbated by a lack of
Behavioral Autonomy
closeness, as is the case in indifferent families. Whereas the development of emotional autonomy
Some parents who have raised their children per- is played out mainly in adolescents’ relationships
missively until adolescence are caught off guard by the with their parents, the development of behavioral
consequences of not having been stricter earlier on. The autonomy—the ability to act independently—is seen
greater orientation toward the peer group of permis- both inside and outside the family, in relationships
sively raised adolescents may involve the young person with peers as well as parents. Broadly speaking, behav-
in behavior that his or her parents disapprove of. As a ioral autonomy refers to the capacity for independent
consequence, some parents who have been permissive decision making. More specifically, researchers who
throughout a youngster’s childhood shift gears when he have studied behavioral autonomy have looked at
or she enters adolescence, becoming autocratic in an changes in decision-making abilities, in susceptibility to
attempt to control a youngster over whom they feel the influence of others, and in feelings of self-reliance.
they have lost control. For instance, parents who have
never placed any restrictions on their daughter’s after-
CHANGES IN DECISION-MAKING
noon activities during elementary school may suddenly
ABILITIES
begin monitoring her social life once she enters junior
high school. Shifts like these can be extremely hard on As you read in Chapter 2, the more sophisticated rea-
adolescents—just at the time when they are seeking soning processes employed by adolescents permit them
greater autonomy, their parents become more restric- to hold multiple viewpoints in mind simultaneously,
tive. Having become accustomed to relative leniency, allowing comparisons among the viewpoints to be
adolescents whose parents change the rules in the mid- drawn—an ability that is crucial for weighing the
dle of the game may find it difficult to accept standards opinions and advice of others. In addition, because
that are being strictly enforced for the first time. adolescents are better able than children to think in
hypothetical terms, they are more likely to consider the
RECAP possible long-term consequences of choosing one
• Although adolescence historically has been viewed course of action over another. And the enhanced role-
as a time during which individuals need to break taking capabilities of adolescence permit teenagers to
away from and rebel against their parents, research consider another person’s opinion while taking into
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 295 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 295

account that person’s perspective. This is important in these factors are weighed about evenly (Cauffman et al.,
determining whether someone who gives advice has 2010). Psychologists have now mapped this development
special areas of expertise, particular biases, or vested onto changes in patterns of brain activation, showing
interests that the young person should keep in mind. that the regions of the brain that are especially sensitive
Taken together, these cognitive changes result in im- to reward are more intensely activated during early and
proved decision-making skills and, consequently, in middle adolescence than adulthood, especially when
the individual’s greater ability to behave independently. rewards are being anticipated, as they might be when
adolescents are thinking about how much fun they are
❚ Improvements in Decision-Making Abilities going to have before they head out for an evening
Many studies have documented that important im- (Galvan, 2010; Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie et al., 2010).
provements in decision-making abilities occur during Some of the heightened “reward sensitivity” seen among
middle and late adolescence (Halpern-Felsher, 2011). adolescents is not even conscious (Cauffman et al., 2010).
With age, adolescents become more likely to consider Adolescents are just as consciously aware as adults of the
both the risks and benefits associated with the decisions potential rewards and costs of a decision—they are
they make and more likely to weigh the long-term con- just affected by them differently (Van Leijenhorst,
sequences of their choices, and not just the immediate Westenberg, & Crone, 2008).
ones (Crone & van der Molen, 2007; Halpern-Felsher & Not only are younger adolescents more drawn to
Cauffman, 2001; Steinberg, Graham et al., 2009). These rewards than are adults, but they also seem especially
improvements appear to be due to two separate, but drawn to immediate rewards (Steinberg, 2008). Con-
related developments. sider the following question: Would you rather have
First, there is a decline over the course of adolescence $200 tomorrow or $1,000 a year from now? How about
in the extent to which decisions are influenced by their $600 tomorrow versus $1,000 a year from now? This is
potential to produce an immediate reward (Haydon, a good way to assess how important it is for someone
McRee, & Halpern, 2011). Most situations in which we to get a reward immediately, because individuals who
have to decide among alternative choices (Should I stay are willing to settle for a smaller amount in order to
home and study or go out with my friends? Should we get it sooner are more drawn to immediate rewards.
sneak into the movie theater or stand in line and pay for Figure 9.2 shows the amount of money individuals of
tickets? Should I sleep with my girlfriend right now or different ages would settle for if they received it
wait until tomorrow night, when I’ll make sure to have tomorrow rather than waiting a year, based on a study
a condom?) present a combination of potential rewards my colleagues and I did (Steinberg, Graham et al.,
and potential costs, and what we decide to do is often 2009). As you can see, preadolescents and early adoles-
the result of how strong those rewards and costs are. cents are much more willing to settle for less, as long as
You can imagine that someone who is just thinking they can get it sooner.
about having fun with his friends, the prospect of seeing A second influence on changes in decision making
the movie for free, or how good sex is going to feel concerns individuals’ ability to control their impulses
would act very differently than someone who is think- (Steinberg, Albert et al., 2008; van Duijvenvoorde,
ing about the grade he might get on a test he didn’t Jansen, Bredman, & Huizenga, 2012; Weiser &
study for, what would happen if he got caught, or the Reynolds, 2011). As you know from Chapter 2, regions
possibility of getting his girlfriend pregnant. of the brain that are important for self-regulation are
During early adolescence, individuals are much more still developing during adolescence and early adult-
drawn to the potential benefits of a decision than the hood. This has important implications for decision
potential costs. As they mature, the relative balance of making, because it means that, with age, individuals are
reward and cost changes, so that by late adolescence, better at thinking ahead, imagining and analyzing the

$950 FIGURE 9.2 Younger adolescents are


Amount accepted tomorrow
versus $1,000 in one year

especially drawn to immediate rewards.


$900 This graph shows age differences in the
amount of money individuals would
$850
settle for if they could have it immedi-
$800 ately, versus waiting 1 year for $1,000.
(Steinberg, Graham, et al., 2009)
$750

$700
10–11 12–13 14–15 16–17 18–21 22–25 26–30
Age (years)
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 296 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

296 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

FIGURE 9.3 Age differences 80


in how individuals believe
Talk/deny
one should respond to a 70 Talk/admit
police interrogation when Remain silent
one has committed a crime.
(Grisso et al., 2003)
60

50

Percent
40

30

20

10

0
11–13 14–15 16–17 18+
Age

consequences of their decision, seeking and evaluat- home”), and not the longer-term implications (“If I
ing the advice of others, and making decisions that are confess, this information can be used against me in
not hasty or excessively influenced by their emotions. court) (see Figure 9.3).
As we noted in Chapter 2, the combination of height-
ened reward sensitivity and immature impulse control
WHEN DO ADOLESCENTS MAKE
may lead adolescents to make a lot of risky—even
DECISIONS AS WELL AS ADULTS?
dangerous—decisions.
The recognition that individuals’ decision-making
❚ Legal Decision Making One interesting line of skills improve over the course of adolescence has
research on changes in decision-making abilities has prompted numerous debates about young people’s
examined adolescents’ legal decision making (Grisso et abilities to make decisions in the real world—for
al., 2003; Kambam & Thompson, 2009; M. G. Schmidt, example, with regard to having access to medical care
Reppucci, & Woolard, 2003). In the typical study, without their parents’ approval or functioning as com-
adolescents and adults are presented with vignettes petent defendants in court. Many such debates revolve
involving an individual who had gotten into trouble around where we should draw the legal boundary
with the law and asked how the individual should han- between adolescence and adulthood for things like
dle different situations—being interrogated by the driving, purchasing alcohol or cigarettes, or being tried
police, consulting with an attorney, deciding whether in adult court (Steinberg, 2012).
to plead guilty in return for a lesser sentence versus Part of the difficulty in making decisions about
going to trial, or taking her or his chances on the out- where to draw lines between adolescents and adults is
come. Researchers find that adolescents are less likely that mature decision making is the product of both
than adults to think about the long-term implications cognitive abilities (such as being able to reason logi-
of their decisions, more likely to focus on the immedi- cally) and psychosocial factors (such as being able to
ate consequences, and less able to understand the ways control one’s impulses), and these aspects of develop-
in which other people’s positions might bias their in- ment appear to proceed along somewhat different
terests. For example, when asked what a guilty indi- timetables (Steinberg, Cauffman et al., 2009). For
vidual should do when being interrogated by the example, as you read in Chapter 2, the maturation of
police, younger adolescents are more likely than adults basic cognitive abilities is complete at around age 16.
to say that they should confess (which is not what most On the basis of this, many writers have argued that
attorneys would recommend) rather than remain older adolescents should have the right to seek health
silent (which most attorneys would say is the most care services (including abortions and contraception)
advisable thing to do). The younger adolescents are without parental knowledge or consent. At the same
thinking about the immediate consequences of their time, because there are improvements in things like
actions (“If I tell the police the truth, they’ll let me go impulse control, planning ahead, and risk assessment
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 297 21/03/13 11:02 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 297

well into early adulthood, there is a period during


which adolescents may think like adults but behave in
a much more immature way. Individuals who are
opposed to trying juvenile offenders as adults use this
evidence to argue in favor of treating juveniles who
have committed crimes less harshly than adults
because of their immature judgment (Modecki, 2008;
Owen-Kostelnik, Reppucci, & Meyer, 2006; E. Scott &
Steinberg, 2008).
One way of resolving this problem is to make sure
our treatment of adolescents under the law is aligned
with what we know about adolescent development in
ways that are specific to the legal matters in question
(Steinberg, Cauffman et al. 2009). In other words, if the
skills necessary for one type of decision mature earlier
than those necessary for another, it would make sense
to have a different age boundary for each.
Peers have a powerful influence over adolescents’ tastes in clothes,
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION hairstyles, and other day-to-day decisions.

Based on what you have read about changes in deci-


sion-making abilities in adolescence, should adoles- peer pressure—more so than children or young
cents be treated like adults adults—and as being stubbornly resistant to the influ-
under the law? If you were ence of their parents. But is peer pressure really more
a lawmaker, where would potent during adolescence than during other times in
you draw the line for issues the life cycle?
concerning access to health
care? For issues concerning ❚ The Influence of Parents and Peers Research-
criminal activity? ers have studied conformity and peer pressure during
adolescence by putting adolescents in situations in
which they must choose either between the wishes of
CHANGES IN SUSCEPTIBILITY
their parents and those of their peers or between their
TO INFLUENCE
own wishes and those of others—typically, parents or
As adolescents come to spend more time outside the friends. For example, an adolescent might be told to
family, the opinions and advice of others—not only imagine that he and his friends discover the answer
peers but adults as well—become more important. For sheet to an upcoming test on the floor outside the teach-
example, at a certain point, adolescents seek the advice ers’ lounge. His friends tell him that they should keep it
of friends rather than their parents concerning how to a secret. But the adolescent tells his mother about it, and
dress. They may turn to a teacher or guidance counselor she advises him to tell the teacher. He then would be
for advice about what courses to take in school, instead asked by the researcher to say what he would do.
of bringing such questions home. Understandably, a In general, studies that contrast parents’ and peers’
variety of situations arise in which adolescents may feel influences indicate that in some situations, peers’ opin-
that their parents’ advice may be less valid than the ions are more influential, but that in other situations,
opinions of others. parents’ opinions are more powerful. Specifically, ado-
There also are issues that might be talked over with lescents are more likely to conform to their peers’ opin-
more than one person. For example, a teenage girl who ions when it comes to short-term, day-to-day, and
is trying to decide whether to take a part-time job after social matters—styles of dress, tastes in music, choices
school might discuss the pros and cons with her par- among leisure activities, and so on. This is particularly
ents but also ask friends for their advice. When friends true during the junior high school and early high school
and parents disagree, adolescents must reconcile the years. When it comes to long-term questions concern-
differences of opinion and reach their own indepen- ing educational or occupational plans, however, or to
dent conclusions. issues concerning values, religious beliefs, or ethics,
In situations in which parents and peers give con- teenagers are primarily influenced by their parents
flicting advice, do teenagers tend to follow one group (W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006). In general, when
more often than the other? Adolescents are often por- adolescents’ problems center on a relationship with a
trayed as being extremely susceptible to the influence of friend, they turn to a peer, a preference that becomes
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 298 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

298 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

stronger with age. But adolescents’ willingness to turn 0.7


to an adult for advice with problems—especially those 0.6 High teen susceptibility
that involve getting along with their parents—remains Low teen susceptibility

Target teen drug/alcohol use


0.5
very strong and increases as individuals move toward
late adolescence. This suggests that older adolescents 0.4
are quite willing to turn to adult experts whose advice 0.3
they value (Morrison, Laughlin, Miguel, Smith, & 0.2
Widaman, 1997). In short, adolescents turn for advice
0.1
to different people in different situations (Finken &
Jacobs, 1996; Halpern-Felsher, 2011). 0
–0.1
❚ Responding to Peer Pressure Studies that con- –0.2
trast the influence of peers and adults do not really
reveal all there is to know about peer pressure, however, –0.3
Lower Higher
because most peer pressure operates when adults are Peer drug/alcohol use
absent—when adolescents are at a party, driving home
FIGURE 9.4 The consequence of being susceptible to peer
from school, or hanging out with their friends on pressure depends on the peers one might be influenced by.
weekends. To get closer to this issue, researchers have (Adapted from Allen et al., 2006)
studied how adolescents respond when they must
choose between the pressure of their friends and their
own opinions of what to do. For example, an adoles- Wall, Power, & Arbona, 1993). Of course, the conse-
cent boy might be asked whether he would go along quences of being especially susceptible to one’s peers
with his friends’ pressure to vandalize some property depend on who those peers are. For instance, whereas
even though he did not want to do so (e.g., Bámaca & high susceptibility to peer influence predicts adoles-
Umana-Taylor, 2006). cents’ substance use if their friends are substance users,
In general, most studies using this approach show the same level of susceptibility is not predictive of sub-
that conformity to peers is higher during middle ado- stance use if their friends are not (J. Allen et al., 2006)
lescence than later adolescence (Steinberg & Monahan, (Figure 9.4).
2007). Some studies find that conformity to peers One interesting application of research on adoles-
increases between early and middle adolescence, peak- cents’ susceptibility to social pressure concerns adoles-
ing around age 14, whereas others find no change dur- cents’ false confessions to police officers. In several
ing this time (T. Sim & Koh, 2003) or that preadolescents well-publicized cases involving adolescents who had
are even more susceptible to peer influence than teen- confessed to crimes—such as the case of the Central
agers (Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). The especially Park jogger, in which a group of adolescents confessed
heightened susceptibility to peer pressure around age to assaulting a woman who was running through the
14 is most often seen when the behavior in question is New York City park—it was subsequently discovered
antisocial—such as cheating, stealing, or trespassing— that the juveniles had given false confessions to the
especially in studies of boys (Erickson, Crosnoe, & police (Kassin, 2008). Although most of us find it hard
Dornbusch, 2000). These findings are in line with to understand why someone would confess to a crime
studies of delinquent acts, which are often committed that he or she did not commit, studies that simulate
by boys in groups, during middle adolescence police interrogations have found that it is not at all
(Farrington, 2009). Several studies indicate that, com- unusual for adolescents to do so when questioned
pared with their more autonomous friends, adoles- under pressure. In one such study (Redlich &
cents who are more susceptible to peer pressure to Goodman, 2003), subjects of different ages partici-
engage in delinquent activity actually are more likely pated in an experiment that involved typing letters on
to misbehave (J. Allen, Porter, & McFarland, 2006; a computer as they were read by the experimenter.
Monahan, Steinberg, Cauffman, & Mulvey, 2009). Sus- Before beginning the experiment, subjects were
ceptibility to antisocial peer pressure is also higher instructed not to touch the ALT key, because doing so
among relatively more acculturated Latino adolescents would crash the computer and ruin the experiment.
than their less acculturated peers, and higher among The experiment was rigged, however, so that the com-
Latino adolescents who were born in the United States puter would crash after a set amount of time, even
than those who were born abroad, consistent with re- without the subject’s touching the ALT key. After the
search showing higher rates of delinquency among computer crashed, the experimenter questioned the
more acculturated adolescents (recall the “immigrant subjects about whether they had hit the ALT key. When
paradox” discussed in Chapter 8) (Bámaca & Umaña- subjects denied doing so, the experimenter presented
Taylor, 2006; Umaña-Taylor & Bámaca-Gómez, 2003; false evidence “proving” that the ALT key had been
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 299 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 299

100
Friends
Percent giving false confession
90 88% Watching
80 73%
70
60
50%
50
40
30
20
10
0 Alone
12 and 13 15 and 16 Young adults
Age
FIGURE 9.5 In a controlled experiment, adolescents were
more likely than adults to provide false confessions when con-
fronted with fake “evidence” that they had made a computer
crash when they actually had not. (Redlich & Goodman, 2003)

pressed. The subjects were then questioned once again


about what they had done. In the face of false evidence,
significantly more young adolescents (ages 12–16)
FIGURE 9.6 Adolescents are more susceptible to peer
than young adults confessed to having hit the ALT key, influence than adults are. This figure shows two brain scans,
even when they actually had not (see Figure 9.5). one taken while adolescents were playing a driving game
Although we know that conformity to peer pressure when their friends were watching, and the other taken when
is high during early adolescence, it is not clear why this the adolescents were playing alone. The area surrounded by
is so. One interpretation is that adolescents are more the yellow rectangle is a part of the brain that is activated
susceptible to peer influence during this time because when we experience reward. One reason adolescents may be-
of their heightened orientation toward the peer group. have more recklessly when they are with their friends is that
Experimental studies find, in fact, that during adoles- the presence of peers may make them pay more attention to
cence the mere presence of friends activates brain the potential rewards of a risky choice. (Chein et al., 2011)
regions associated with the experience of reward, but
that no such effect is found when adults are with their the following picture emerges: During childhood, boys
friends, as shown in Figure 9.6 (Chein, Albert, O’Brien, and girls are highly oriented toward their parents and
Uckert, & Steinberg, 2011). Because they care more far less oriented toward their peers, and peer pressure is
about what their friends think of them, they are more not especially strong. As they approach adolescence,
likely to go along with the crowd to avoid being rejected children become somewhat less oriented toward their
(W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006). Consistent with this, parents and more oriented toward their peers, and peer
in one experimental study, adolescents who were led to pressure begins to escalate. During early adolescence,
believe they were interacting in a chat room with either conformity to parents continues to decline, and peer
high-status or low-status peers (manipulated by the pressure continues to rise. It is not until middle adoles-
experimenters in the way the peers’ interests were cence that genuine increases in behavioral autonomy
described) about the acceptability of various illegal or occur, for it is during this time (between the 9th and
risky behaviors were more influenced by the opinions 12th grades) that conformity both to parents and to
of high-status peers (G. L. Cohen & Prinstein, 2006). A peers declines even though peer pressure continues to
different version of the same story is that individuals’ increase.
susceptibility to peer pressure may remain constant
over adolescence, but peer pressure itself may be espe- ❚ Individual Differences in Susceptibility to
cially strong around the time individuals are 14. In Peer Influence Within a group of teenagers of the
other words, adolescent peer groups may exert more same age, some are highly autonomous, others are eas-
pressure on their members to conform than do groups ily influenced by their peers, others are oriented toward
of younger or older individuals, and the pressure may their parents, and still others are swayed by both peers
be strong enough to make even the most autonomous and parents, depending on the situation (Prinstein,
teenagers comply. Brechwald, & Cohen, 2011). Girls are less susceptible to
When we put together findings from research on peer pressure than boys, as are Black adolescents in
peer pressure, peer conformity, and parental influence, comparison to adolescents from other ethnic groups.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 300 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

300 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Asian American adolescents, in contrast, seem espe- but they may be more susceptible to the influence of
cially susceptible to peer pressure, perhaps consistent positive peers. Adolescents from authoritative homes,
with the greater emphasis placed on the importance for example, are less likely than other teenagers to be
of the group over the individual in Asian cultures influenced by having drug-using friends, but they are
(Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). Studies also have more likely than their peers to be influenced by having
found that adolescents from single-parent families, as friends who perform well in school (Mounts &
well as those with less supportive or involved parents, Steinberg, 1995). It is also important to distinguish
appear especially susceptible to antisocial peer pres- between adolescents who are excessively dependent
sure (Farrell & White, 1998; C. Wong, Crosnoe, Laird, on their peers (and who forgo their parents’ rules and
& Dornbusch, 2003). pay less attention to their schoolwork for the sake of
Studies of adolescent brain development are add- being popular with peers) and those who turn to peers
ing to our growing understanding of individuals dif- for counsel but do not ignore their parents’ guidance
ferences in adolescents’ susceptibility to peer influence (Fuligni, Eccles, Barber, & Clements, 2001). Substitut-
(Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011). One recent investiga- ing peers for parents leads to problem behavior; add-
tion found that individuals who showed a pattern of ing peers to the list of persons to whom the adolescent
activity indicating heightened sensitivity to social re- turns for advice, so long as that list includes parents,
jection were less able to resist peer influence (Sebastian does not. In other words, it is detachment from par-
et al., 2011). Another found that being able to resist ents, rather than attachment to peers, that is poten-
peer pressure is associated with stronger connections tially harmful.
between regions of the brain active during decision The ways in which parents and adolescents negoti-
making and other brain regions, suggesting, perhaps, ate changes in behavioral autonomy have implications
that individuals who are more likely to stand up to for adolescents’ adjustment, according to several
their friends may be able to better control the sort of studies (e.g., Fuligni & Eccles, 1993; S. Goldstein,
impulsive, emotional decision making that often oc- Davis-Kean, & Eccles, 2005; T. Sim, 2000). Not sur-
curs in the peer group (Grosbras et al., 2007; Paus, prisingly, adolescents who have less positive relation-
Toro et al., 2008). In another recent brain imaging ships with their parents are more likely to be especially
study, adolescents whose neural activity was indicative peer oriented, to affiliate with negative peers, and to
of better emotion regulation in response to pictures of spend time with friends in unsupervised settings, all
emotional faces also reported more resistance to peer of which heighten adolescents’ risk for problem be-
influence (Pfeifer, Masten, Moore, & Oswald, 2011). havior. But studies also show that parents need to
This brain research is consistent with the idea that a maintain a healthy balance between asserting control
key aspect of positive development in adolescence in- and granting autonomy. Generally, granting too much
volves the deve lopment of self-regulation (Gestdóttir autonomy before adolescents are ready for it or grant-
& Lerner, 2007; Monahan, Steinberg, Cauffman, & ing too little autonomy once adolescents are mature
Mulvey, 2009). enough to handle it creates adolescents who are the
Like emotional autonomy, behavioral autonomy most strongly peer oriented. Adolescents whose par-
appears to be associated with authoritative rather than ents become more authoritarian (that is, stricter and
permissive, authoritarian, or neglectful parenting less likely to permit the adolescent to make decisions)
(W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006). Consistent with this, over time are the most peer oriented of all. Although
the sexual behavior of adolescents who have discussed many parents clamp down on their teenagers’ inde-
sex with their parents is less influenced by peer pres- pendence out of fear that not doing so will allow the
sure than is the behavior of adolescents who have not youngsters to fall under the “evil” influence of the peer
done so (Whitaker & Miller, 2000), and adolescents group, this strategy often backfires. Evidently, having
whose parents strongly disapprove of smoking are less parents limit their autonomy at just the time when
likely to be influenced by their friends’ smoking than more independence is desired and expected makes
are adolescents whose parents do not voice their disap- adolescents turn away from the family and toward
proval (Sargent & Dalton, 2001). Other research finds their friends.
that impulsive adolescents are less likely to consult
with their parents before making decisions (Romich,
ETHNIC AND CULTURAL DIFFERENCES
Lundberg, & Tsang, 2009).
IN EXPECTATIONS FOR AUTONOMY
The situation may be somewhat more complicated
than this, however. It turns out that the impact of hav- The development of behavioral autonomy varies across
ing authoritative parents on adolescents’ susceptibility cultures because of differences in the age expectations
to peer pressure depends on the nature of the peer that adolescents and parents have for independent
pressure in question. Adolescents from authoritative behavior. In general, adolescents’ mental health is best
homes are less susceptible to antisocial peer pressure, when their desire for autonomy matches their
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 301 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 301

expectations for what their parents are willing to grant their perceptions of how much independence their
(Juang, Lerner, McKinney, & von Eye, 1999). For example, friends have (Daddis, 2011).
White adolescents and their parents have earlier
expectations for adolescent autonomy than do Asian ado-
lescents and parents from the same countries (S. Feldman MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
& Quatman, 1988; Rosenthal & Feldman, 1990). Because Suppose you were writing
of this, adolescents from Asian families may be less likely a book for parents of teen-
to seek autonomy from their parents than their White agers. What special advice
counterparts, and White adolescents are less likely than would you give to parents
Asian adolescents to define themselves in terms of their who have recently moved
relationship with their parents (Pomerantz, Qin, Wang, & to the United States about
Chen, 2009). And perhaps because of this, whereas in- coping with peer pressure?
creased autonomy tends to be associated with better emo-
tional functioning among American youth (where being
an independent individual is highly valued), it is less so CHANGES IN FEELINGS
among Asian adolescents (Chun & MacDermid, 1997; OF SELF-RELIANCE
Qin, Pomerantz, & Wang, 2009). A third approach to the study of behavioral autonomy
Surprisingly, in studies of behavioral autonomy, sex focuses on adolescents’ own judgments of how autono-
and birth order differences tend to be very small and mous they are. When adolescents of different ages are
are often inconsistent—contrary to the popular beliefs asked to complete standardized tests of self-reliance, for
that boys are granted more autonomy than girls or that example, the results show that subjective feelings of
later-born adolescents are granted earlier freedom autonomy increase steadily over the adolescent years,
because their older siblings have paved the way and that adolescent girls report feeling more self-reliant
(Wray-Lake, Crouter, & McHale, 2010). Some studies than adolescent boys (W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006);
find sex and birth order differences in the extent to girls also are better able to resist peer pressure, consis-
which parents grant autonomy, but the pattern varies tent with the idea that girls mature psychosocially
depending on the particular constellation of sons and earlier than boys in general (Steinberg & Monahan,
daughters in the household and on the parents’ atti- 2007). This is especially interesting in light of the find-
tudes toward sex roles (more traditionally minded par- ings concerning susceptibility to peer pressure,
ents grant more autonomy to sons than daughters, but discussed earlier, since it indicates that adolescents may
more educated parents grant more autonomy to describe themselves as gaining in self-reliance during a
daughters than sons, perhaps because their daughters period when their susceptibility to peer pressure may
are easier to supervise [Bumpus, Crouter, & McHale, be increasing. Although adults may view adolescents’
2001; Wray-Lake et al., 2010]). Sex differences in the giving in to peer pressure as a sign of diminished au-
extent to which adolescents are granted independence tonomy, adolescents may not see their own behavior in
appear to be especially pronounced within Black this light. Not surprisingly, adolescents who have a
households, where, compared to other ethnic groups, stronger sense of self-reliance report higher self-esteem
boys are given relatively more freedom but girls are and fewer behavior problems (Owens, Mortimer, &
given less (Bulcroft, Carmody, & Bulcroft, 1996; Daddis & Finch, 1996; S. Wolfe & Truxillo, 1996).
Smetana, 2005).
One problem facing many families that have im-
migrated to a new culture is that parents and adoles- RECAP
cents may have very different expectations for what • As individuals mature, they become better able to
an appropriate timetable is with respect to granting seek out and weigh the advice of individuals with
autonomy—although this will depend on the degree different degrees of expertise and to use this infor-
of discrepancy in timetables in the initial and new mation in making independent decisions.
contexts. But, as a rule, because adolescents generally • One controversy involves whether adolescents’
acculturate more quickly to a new culture than do decision-making abilities are mature enough to
parents, a family that has moved from a culture in warrant treating them as adults under the law.
which it is normal to grant autonomy relatively later • Although adolescence is a time of advances in
in adolescence (as in most Asian countries) to one in decision-making abilities, during early adolescence,
which it is normal to grant autonomy relatively ear- youngsters are particularly susceptible to peer pres-
lier (as in the United States) may experience conflict sure, especially on issues concerning day-to-day
as a result of differences in the expectations of adoles- activities. Susceptibility to peer pressure is high
cents and parents (Kwak, 2003). Adolescents’ expecta- during early adolescence but decreases over the
tions for autonomy are shaped to a great extent by high school years.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 302 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

302 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

• Adolescents whose parents are extremely authori- for the exploration of differing value systems, political
tarian or extremely permissive are the most easily ideologies, personal ethics, and religious beliefs.
influenced by their friends, especially in antisocial But the growth of cognitive autonomy is encouraged
contexts. by the development of emotional and behavioral
• Adolescents do not experience this increased suscep- independence as well. As we shall see, some evidence
tibility to peer pressure as a decline in self-reliance. suggests that the development of cognitive autonomy
In fact, self-reports of self-reliance occurs later (between ages 18 and 20) than does the
increase steadily throughout development of emotional or behavioral autonomy,
adolescence. which take place during early and middle adolescence
(W. A. Collins & Steinberg, 2006). The establishment of
emotional autonomy provides adolescents with the abil-
ity to look at their parents’ views more objectively. When
young people no longer see their parents as omnipotent
and infallible authorities, they may seriously reevaluate
The Development of the ideas and values that they accepted without question
as children. Not surprisingly, individuals with a stronger
Cognitive Autonomy sense of cognitive autonomy show more maturity in
other psychological domains as well, such as in the
The development of cognitive autonomy entails
realms of identity development and self-awareness
changes in the adolescent’s beliefs, opinions, and values
(Hart & Fegley, 1995).
and has been studied mainly by looking at how adoles-
As adolescents begin to test the waters of indepen-
cents think about moral, political, and religious issues.
dence behaviorally, they may experience a variety of
Three trends in the development of cognitive auton-
cognitive conflicts caused by having to compare the
omy during adolescence are especially noteworthy.
advice of parents and friends and having to deal with
First, adolescents become increasingly abstract in the
competing pressures to behave in different ways. These
way they think about moral, political, and religious is-
conflicts may prompt young people to consider in
sues. Consider an 18-year-old who is deciding whether
more serious and thoughtful terms what they really
to participate in a disruptive demonstration in his state
believe. For example, during adolescence, individuals
capital against policies he believes indirectly support
become increasingly likely to say that it is permissible
the interests of environmental polluters. Instead of
to lie to one’s parents about disobeying them when
looking at the situation only in terms of the specifics
they think their parents’ advice is immoral (for in-
of the situation, he might think about the implications
stance, if the parents had forbidden their teenager
of knowingly violating the law in general. Second, dur-
to date someone from another race) (S. A. Perkins &
ing adolescence, beliefs become increasingly rooted in
Turiel, 2007). This struggle to clarify values, provoked
general principles. An 18-year-old might say that dem-
in part by the exercise of behavioral autonomy, is a key
onstrating against pollution is acceptable because pro-
component of the process of developing a sense of
tecting the environment is more important than living
cognitive autonomy.
in accord with the law, and so breaking a law is legiti-
mate when the status quo leads to environmental deg-
radation. Finally, beliefs become increasingly founded MORAL DEVELOPMENT DURING
in the young person’s own values, and not merely in a ADOLESCENCE
system of values passed on by parents or other author-
Moral development has been the most widely studied
ity figures. Thus, an 18-year-old may look at the issue of
aspect of cognitive autonomy during adolescence.
environmental protection in terms of what he himself
The study of moral development involves both rea-
believes, rather than in terms of what his parents have
soning (how individuals think about moral dilem-
told him to think.
mas) and behavior (how they behave in situations
Much of the growth in cognitive autonomy can be
that call for moral judgments). Related to this is the
traced to the cognitive changes characteristic of the
study of prosocial behavior, the behaviors individu-
period. With adolescents’ enhanced reasoning capabili-
als engage in to help others (Morris et al., 2011).
ties and the further development of hypothetical think-
ing come a heightened interest in ideological and
Assessing Moral Reasoning The dominant theo-
philosophical matters and a more sophisticated way of
retical viewpoint in the study of moral reasoning has
looking at them. The ability to consider alternate possi-
been a perspective grounded in Piaget’s theory of cog-
bilities and to engage in thinking about thinking allows
nitive development. As you will recall from the discus-
sion in Chapter 2 of changes in thinking processes
during adolescence, the emphasis within the Piagetian
prosocial behavior Behaviors intended to help others.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 303 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 303

view, or cognitive-developmental perspective, is on Perhaps Kohlberg’s best-known dilemma involved a


changes in the structure and organization of thought, man who had to choose between stealing a drug to save
rather than on changes in its content. Theories of mo- his wife or letting his wife remain mortally ill:
rality that stem from the cognitive-developmental
In Europe, a woman was near death from a very bad dis-
viewpoint similarly emphasize shifts in the type of rea- ease, a special kind of cancer. There was one drug that the
soning that individuals use in making moral decisions, doctors thought might save her. It was a form of radium
rather than changes in the content of the decisions that a druggist in the same town had recently discovered.
they reach or the actions they take as a result. Although The drug was expensive to make, but the druggist was
the initial formulation of the cognitive-developmental charging 10 times what the drug cost him to make. He
perspective on morality was presented by Piaget paid $200 for the radium and charged $2,000 for a small
himself, the theory was subsequently expanded by dose of the drug. The sick woman’s husband, Heinz, went
Lawrence Kohlberg, and it is Kohlberg’s work that is to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could
more relevant to the study of moral development dur- only get together about $1,000, which was half of what it
ing adolescence (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009; Smetana & cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying, and
asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the
Villalobos, 2009).
druggist said, “No, I discovered the drug and I’m going to
Researchers assess individuals’ levels of moral rea- make money from it.” Heinz got desperate and broke into
soning by examining their responses to hypothetical the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife. Should the
moral dilemmas about difficult, real-world situations. husband have done that? Was it right or wrong?
These dilemmas are presented either in an interview,
in which case the adolescents’ responses are recorded, ❚ Stages of Moral Reasoning According to
transcribed, and coded (Gibbs, Basinger, Grime, & Kohlberg, whether or not you think that Heinz should
Snarney, 2007), or in a questionnaire, in which adoles- have stolen the drug, or that Louise should tell her
cents respond to the dilemmas in a multiple-choice mother, or that cheating someone is worse than stealing
format (Rest, Narvaez, Bebeau, & Thoma, 1999). Here from a store is less important than the reasoning be-
are examples of the sorts of dilemmas researchers hind your answers. Kohlberg theorized that individuals’
have used: reasoning about moral issues becomes more sophisti-
cated with development. Specifically, Kohlberg sug-
Judy was a 12-year-old girl. Her mother promised her
that she could go to a special rock concert coming to gested that there are three levels of moral reasoning:
their town if she saved up from babysitting and lunch preconventional moral reasoning, which is dominant
money to buy a ticket to the concert. She managed to save during most of childhood; conventional moral rea-
up the $15 the ticket cost plus another $5. But then her soning, which is usually dominant during late child-
mother changed her mind and told Judy that she had to hood and early adolescence; and postconventional
spend the money on new clothes for school. Judy was dis- moral reasoning (sometimes called principled moral
appointed and decided to go to the concert anyway. She reasoning), which emerges sometime during the ado-
bought a ticket and told her mother that she had only lescent or young adult years.
been able to save $5. That Saturday she went to the per- Preconventional thinking is characterized by refer-
formance and told her mother that she was spending the
ence to external and physical events. Preconventional
day with a friend. A week passed without her mother
moral decisions are not based on society’s standards,
finding out. Judy then told her older sister, Louise, that
she had gone to the performance and had lied to her rules, or conventions (hence the label preconventional).
mother about it. Louise wonders whether to tell their Children at this stage approach moral dilemmas in ways
mother what Judy did. that focus on the rewards and punishments associated
Should Louise, the older sister, tell their mother that with different courses of action. One preconventional
Judy lied about the money, or should she keep quiet?

Two young men, brothers, had got into serious trouble.


preconventional moral reasoning According to Kohlberg, the first
They were secretly leaving town in a hurry and needed
level of moral reasoning, which is typical of children and is
money. Karl, the older one, broke into a store and stole
characterized by reasoning that is based on rewards and
$1,000. Bob, the younger one, went to a retired old man
punishments associated with different courses of action.
who was known to help people in town. He told the man
that he was very sick and that he needed $1,000 to pay for conventional moral reasoning According to Kohlberg, the second
an operation. Bob asked the old man to lend him the level of moral development, which occurs during late childhood and
money and promised that he would pay him back when early adolescence and is characterized by reasoning that is based on
he recovered. Really, Bob wasn’t sick at all, and he had no the rules and conventions of society.
intention of paying the man back. Although the old man
didn’t know Bob very well, he lent him the money. So postconventional moral reasoning In Kohlberg’s theory, the stage
Bob and Karl skipped town, each with $1,000. of moral development during which society’s rules and conventions
Which is worse, stealing like Karl or cheating like Bob? are seen as relative and subjective rather than as authoritative; also
called principled moral reasoning.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 304 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

304 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

ought to be questioned and when more important


principles—such as justice, fairness, or the sanctity of
human life—take precedence over established social
norms. For instance, a postconventional response
might be that Heinz should not have stolen the drug
because in doing so he violated an implicit agreement
among members of society—an agreement that gives
each person the freedom to pursue his or her liveli-
hood. However, another principled thinker might re-
spond that Heinz was right to steal the drug because
someone’s life was at stake and because preserving hu-
man life is more important than respecting individual
freedoms. Whereas conventional thinking is oriented
toward society’s rules, postconventional thinking is
founded on more broadly based abstract principles.
For this reason, the development of postconventional
reasoning is especially relevant to the discussion of
cognitive autonomy.
Studies have confirmed Kohlberg’s suggestion that
The ways in which individuals think about moral dilemmas change
during adolescence.
moral reasoning becomes more principled over the
course of childhood and adolescence and that modal
development appears to proceed through the sequence
child might say that Heinz should not have stolen the described in Kohlberg’s theory (N. Eisenberg et al.,
drug because he could have been caught and sent to jail. 2009). Preconventional reasoning dominates the re-
Another might say that Heinz was right to steal the drug sponses of children; conventional responses begin to
because people would have been angry with him if he appear during preadolescence and continue into mid-
had let his wife die. In either case, the chief concern to dle adolescence; and postconventional reasoning does
the preconventional thinker is what would happen to not appear until late adolescence, if at all. According to
Heinz as a result of his choice. Kohlberg’s theory, movement into higher stages of
Conventional thinking about moral issues focuses moral reasoning occurs when the adolescent is devel-
not so much on tangible rewards and punishments as opmentally “ready”—when his or her reasoning is
on how an individual will be judged by others for be- predominantly at one stage but partially at the next
having in a certain way. In conventional moral reason- higher one—and when he or she is exposed to the
ing, special importance is given to the roles people are more advanced type of reasoning by other people,
expected to play and to society’s rules, institutions, and such as parents or peers (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009).
conventions. Individuals behave properly because, in so The development of moral reasoning tends to follow a
doing, they receive the approval of others and help to pattern in which individuals move from periods of
maintain the social order. The correctness of society’s consolidation (in which their reasoning is consistently
rules is not questioned, however—individuals do their at a particular stage of development), into periods of
duty by upholding and respecting the social order. A transition (in which there is more variability in their
conventional thinker might say that Heinz should not stages of reasoning), into new periods of consolida-
have stolen the drug because stealing is against the law. tion (in which their reasoning is consistent, but at a
But another might counter that Heinz was right to steal higher stage than during the previous period of con-
the drug because it is what a good husband is expected solidation) (L. Walker, Gustafson, & Hennig, 2001).
to do. According to most studies of moral reasoning, As is the case with formal operations, the most ad-
the majority of adolescents and adults think primarily vanced stage of cognitive development described by
in conventional terms—they evaluate moral decisions Piaget (see Chapter 2), not all individuals develop the
in terms of a set of rules that people are supposed to capacity to engage in postconventional moral reason-
abide by. ing. Advanced levels of moral reasoning are more com-
Postconventional reasoning is relatively rare. At this mon among children raised in authoritative families in
level of reasoning, society’s rules and conventions are which parents encourage their child to participate in
seen as relative and subjective rather than as absolute family discussions, in which the level of conflict in fam-
and definitive. Individuals may have a moral duty to ily discussions is neither extremely low nor extremely
abide by society’s standards for behavior—but only high, and in which parents expose the adolescent to
insofar as those standards support and serve moral moral arguments that are fashioned at a higher stage
ends. Thus, occasions arise in which conventions than her or his own (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009). There is
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 305 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 305

also some evidence that the development of advanced that parallel their reasoning about the moral dilemmas
moral reasoning among Black youth is facilitated by they actually encounter in everyday life (N. Eisenberg
commitment to traditional African values of spiritual- et al., 2009). And as for the second of these concerns,
ity and community (Woods & Jagers, 2003). many studies indicate that individuals’ behavior is re-
lated to the ways in which they reason about hypothet-
ical moral dilemmas. Although individuals do not
always behave in ways that are absolutely consistent
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
with their moral reasoning, on average, individuals
Did you grow up in a family that engaged in who reason at higher stages behave in more moral
the sorts of discussions thought to promote ways. For example, individuals who are capable of rea-
more advanced levels of moral reasoning? soning at higher stages of moral thought are less likely
What are some examples of the ways in to commit antisocial acts, less likely to cheat, and less
which your family did (or did not) do this? likely to bow to the pressures of others, as well as being
more tolerant, more likely to engage in political pro-
tests, more likely to engage in volunteer activities, and
more likely to assist others in need of help. They are
Although not all individuals enter a stage of post- also more likely to be influential over their friends in
conventional thinking during adolescence, many begin group decisions about moral problems (Gummerum,
to place greater emphasis on abstract values and moral Keller, Takezawa, & Mata, 2008). Conversely, those who
principles (Rest et al., 1999). Moreover, if individuals of reason at lower stages of moral thought are more ag-
different ages are presented with other peoples’ moral gressive, delinquent, accepting of violence, and tolerant
arguments, older individuals are more often persuaded of others’ misbehavior (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009; Stams
by arguments that in Kohlberg’s framework are more et al., 2006).
advanced. Thus, the appeal of postconventional moral
reasoning increases over the course of adolescence,
while the appeals of preconventional and of conven- MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
tional reasoning both decline. Interestingly, the appeal Should schools attempt
of postconventional thinking appears to increase both to facilitate adolescents’
with age and with schooling; most adults reach a pla- moral development as a
teau in moral reasoning after completing their formal part of their curriculum?
education. Although for many years psychologists de- If so, what sorts of activi-
bated whether there were sex differences in the way that ties would be most useful?
individuals approach moral problems, and many popu-
lar books were based on the idea that men and women
think differently about ethical issues, studies have not
supported this (Smetana & Villalobos, 2009).
Of course, moral behavior and moral reasoning do
❚ Moral Reasoning and Moral Behavior It is not always go hand in hand. Most of us have found
one thing to reason about hypothetical moral problems ourselves in situations in which we behaved less mor-
in an advanced way; it is quite another to behave in ally than we would have liked to. Accordingly, we
strict accordance with one’s reasoning. After all, it is should not expect moral behavior to follow exactly
common for people to say one thing (for example, from moral reasoning, because other factors compli-
cheating on a test is immoral) but do another (for ex- cate moral decision making. In tests measuring moral
ample, sneak a peek at a classmate’s test when running reasoning, assessments are made in a social vacuum,
out of time during an exam). Some critics have argued but such vacuums rarely exist in the real world. For
that although Kohlberg’s theory may provide a window example, you probably realize in the abstract that
on how people think about abstract and hypothetical complying with highway speed limits is important be-
dilemmas, or about life-and-death situations, as in the cause such limits prevent accidents, and you may obey
Heinz story, it does not tell us very much about the these limits most of the time. But you may have found
ways people reason about day-to-day problems or be- yourself in a situation in which you weighed your need
have when they find themselves in situations that might to get somewhere in a hurry (perhaps you were late for
evoke moral considerations. an important job interview) against your moral belief
As it turns out, research on Kohlberg’s theory has in the importance of obeying speeding laws, and you
answered these criticisms fairly well. As for the first of decided that in this instance you would behave in a
these concerns, for example, research indicates that way inconsistent with your belief. Situational factors
people reason about life-and-death dilemmas in ways influence moral choices, and they also influence moral
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 306 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

306 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

reasoning. When individuals perceive that they will be (Morris et al., 2011). Over the course of adolescence,
severely hurt by behaving in a morally advanced way for example, individuals come to devalue prosocial acts
(for example, if standing up for someone will lead to that are done for self-serving reasons (for example, re-
severe punishment), they are less likely to reason at a ceiving a reward, returning a favor, or improving their
higher moral level (Sobesky, 1983). Moral reasoning is image) and value those that are done out of genuine
an important influence on moral behavior, but it can- empathy for others, a pattern that has been observed
not be considered out of context. across a variety of cultures (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009).
The correlation between adolescents’ moral reason- During late adolescence, prosocial reasoning becomes
ing and their moral behavior is especially likely to break increasingly advanced, leveling off sometime in the
down when they define issues as personal choices rather early 20s (N. Eisenberg, Cumberland, Guthrie, Murphy,
than ethical dilemmas (for instance, when using drugs is & Shepard, 2005).
seen as a personal matter rather than a moral issue). This Several researchers have asked whether moral and
observation turns out to be especially important for un- prosocial reasoning is facilitated by certain types of par-
derstanding why adolescents’ moral reasoning and risk enting. Generally, the same type of parenting that fa-
taking are for the most part unrelated (N. Eisenberg cilitates the growth of healthy emotional autonomy,
et al., 2009). If individuals consider various risky behav- discussed earlier, also contributes to the development
iors (for example, experimenting with drugs, having un- of prosocial reasoning. Adolescents whose parents en-
protected sex) to be personal decisions rather than moral gage them in discussion, elicit their point of view, and
choices, their moral reasoning will be relatively unim- practice authoritative parenting display more advanced
portant in predicting how they will act. In other words, reasoning than their peers (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009). It
individuals are more likely to engage in risky behavior appears that this type of parenting makes adolescents
(even if it is unethical) when they see the behavior as a more likely to feel sympathy toward others, which
matter of personal taste rather than a question of right in turn prompts prosocial behavior (Carlo, Knight,
and wrong. It is not clear, however, whether viewing risk McGinley, & Hayes, 2011a). In addition, positive par-
taking as a personal choice is likely to lead to more risk enting helps facilitate the development of empathy and
taking, or whether individuals, once they’ve engaged in a emotion regulation, both of which contribute to proso-
risky activity, are likely to redefine the issue as a personal cial development (see Figure 9.7) (Padilla-Walker &
rather than moral one, as a way of justifying their behav- Christensen, 2011).
ior after the fact. In either case, though, this suggests that
interventions designed to stimulate moral reasoning ❚ Prosocial Reasoning and Prosocial Behavior
will have little impact on adolescents’ risk taking if they Adolescents who show more advanced prosocial rea-
fail to convince adolescents that the behavior in ques- soning and who place a high value on prosocial behav-
tion involves a moral and not just a personal choice. ior in fact behave in ways that are consistent with this
Delinquency and aggression are also more common (Hardy, Carlo, & Roesch, 2010). For example, exemplary
among adolescents who score higher on measures of youth—adolescents who have volunteered considerable
moral disengagement (the tendency to rationalize im- amounts of time in service activities—score higher on
moral behavior as legitimate, as when one justifies steal- measures of moral reasoning than do their peers, are
ing from someone as a way of retaliating) (Paciello, Fida, more committed to the betterment of society, and, as
Tramontano, Lupinetti, & Caprara, 2008). children, were made aware of the suffering of those
who are less fortunate (Hart & Fegley, 1995; Matsuba &
PROSOCIAL REASONING, PROSOCIAL
BEHAVIOR, AND VOLUNTEERISM
❚ Changes in Prosocial Reasoning Although Empathy
most research on the development of morality has fo-
cused on what adolescents do under circumstances in
which a law might be broken or a rule violated, re- Positive Prosocial
searchers have increasingly turned their attention to the parenting behavior
study of reasoning and behavior in prosocial situations.
In general, the ways in which individuals think about
prosocial phenomena, such as honesty or kindness, Self-
regulation
become more sophisticated during late adolescence
FIGURE 9.7 Positive parenting promotes prosocial behav-
ior by increasing adolescents’ abilities to regulate their
emotions and empathize with others. (Padilla-Walker &
moral disengagement Rationalizing immoral behavior as Christensen, 2011)
legitimate, as a way of justifying one’s own bad acts.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 307 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 307

Walker, 2005; Yates & Youniss, 1996). Individuals who conducted to measure students’ knowledge and atti-
score high on measures of prosocial moral reasoning tudes on a range of political issues. Most of these stud-
also have been shown to be more sympathetic and em- ies have found that only a small proportion of young
pathic (N. Eisenberg, Carlo, Murphy, & Van Court, 1995), people can be characterized as politically engaged, not
to engage in more prosocial behavior (N. Eisenberg, just in the United States, but around the world. In the
Zhou, & Koller, 2001), and to be less likely to behave United States, election turnouts continue to be lower
violently after having witnessed violence themselves among young people than among adults, and, with
(Brookmeyer, Henrich, & Schwab-Stone, 2005). In gen- the exception of a temporary spark in interest follow-
eral, female adolescents score higher on measures of ing major political and world events (such as the 9/11
prosocial moral reasoning than do male adolescents, as terrorist attack), adolescents’ interest in, and knowl-
do both males and females who are relatively more fem- edge of, political issues is meager (Sherrod & Lauck-
inine (Carlo, Koller, Eisenberg, Da Silva, & Frohlich, hardt, 2009). Experts attribute this in part to the
1996; N. Eisenberg et al., 2001; Morris et al., 2011). widespread absence of civics education in American
Although, as noted earlier, prosocial reasoning be- high schools and to the tendency of adolescents to fo-
comes more advanced over the course of adolescence, cus their civic energies on organizations that they are
research on changes in prosocial behavior during ado- more directly involved with, such as schools, religious
lescence is not as consistent. Some studies find that in- institutions, and extracurricular clubs.
dividuals become more empathic, sympathetic, and Most research on civic engagement in adolescence
helpful as they move into and through adolescence, but has focused on community service. Volunteering in
many do not (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009), and some have community service activities, sometimes referred to as
found that adolescents become less helpful toward oth- service learning, is more common in the United States
ers over the high school years (Carlo, Crockett, Randall, than in most other countries and, in fact, is now re-
& Roesch, 2007). More consistent are research findings quired by many school districts. Researchers have been
indicating that prosocial behavior is fairly stable over interested in both the antecedents of volunteering
time (that is, prosocial children grow up to be prosocial (that is, what leads adolescents to become involved in
teenagers) and across different contexts (that is, adoles- volunteer activities) and its consequences (that is, how
cents who are helpful to classmates in school are more adolescents are affected by volunteering). Several con-
likely than their less prosocial peers to also be helpful to clusions have emerged from this work. First, apart
strangers in the mall). Also, girls are generally more car- from attending a school in which some sort of com-
ing and prosocial than boys, perhaps because parents munity service is required, the best predictors of vol-
emphasize prosocial development more in raising unteerism in adolescence are being actively involved
daughters than sons (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009). Encour- in religion (most probably because many volunteer
aging adolescents to spend time thinking about what’s activities are organized through religious institutions)
important to them seems to increase their tendency to and having parents who are active as volunteers in the
act prosocially (Thomaes, Bushman, de Castro, & community. Volunteers also tend to be female, more
Reijntjes, 2012). And individuals with prosocial friends socially mature, more extraverted, and, not surpris-
tend to behave more prosocially themselves (Barry & ingly, more altruistic (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009).
Wentzel, 2006). It has been more difficult to document the effects
of volunteering on adolescent development because
❚ Civic Engagement One of the most obvious individuals who choose to volunteer are different to be-
ways in which adolescents can demonstrate prosocial gin with from their peers who do not (Atkins, Hart, &
behavior is through various types of civic engagement Donnelly, 2005). Nevertheless, studies of volunteering
(Flanagan & Wray-Lake, 2011; Sherrod & Lauckhardt, that follow adolescents over time indicate that engaging
2009). Civic engagement is a broad term for a cate- in community service leads to short-term gains in so-
gory of activities that reflect involvement in political cial responsibility, increases in the importance individ-
and community affairs, including staying knowledge- uals place on helping others, and increased commitment
able about politics and current affairs, participating in to tolerance, equal opportunity, and cultural diversity
conventional political activities (e.g., contacting a po- (N. Eisenberg et al., 2009; Flanagan, 2004; Lawford,
litical representative about an issue, campaigning for a
candidate, or voting in an election), participating in
alternative political activities (e.g., being part of a
civic engagement Involvement in political and community affairs,
demonstration or a boycott), and engaging in com-
as reflected in knowledge about politics and current affairs,
munity service. Because the minimum age for voting participation in conventional and alternative political activities, and
in most countries is 18 or older, little research has engaging in community service.
been conducted on adolescents’ involvement in politi-
cal activities, although a number of surveys have been service learning The process of learning through involvement in
community service.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 308 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

308 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Pratt, Hunsberger, & Pancer, 2005; Reinders & Youniss, but research on this topic is generally consistent with
2006). There also is some evidence that volunteering in the view that beliefs become more principled, more ab-
adolescence predicts volunteering in adulthood (Hart, stract, and more independent during the adolescent
Donnelly, Youniss, & Atkins, 2007). The extent to which years. This pattern is linked in part to the general cogni-
these effects persist over time depends, in part, on how tive developments of adolescence and in part to the
long the volunteer activity lasts; the shorter the activity, growth of specific expertise, as the adolescent is exposed
the more short-lived the results of participation. to more political information and ideas (Flanagan,
During the past several decades, many school dis- 2004; Torney-Purta, 1990).
tricts began requiring, or considering requiring, com-
munity service of all students. This suggestion was met ❚ Changes in Political Thinking Political think-
with both praise and criticism. Proponents argued that ing changes during adolescence in several important
service activities would help develop concern for the ways (Flanagan, 2004; Torney-Purta, 1990). First, it
community and facilitate adolescents’ prosocial devel- becomes more abstract. In response to the question
opment. Opponents argued that forcing adolescents to “What is the purpose of laws?” for example, 12- and
do something they did not want to do would make them 13-year-olds are likely to reply with concrete answers—
even more negative about community service and less “So people don’t kill or steal,” “So people don’t get
likely to volunteer at later points in their development. hurt,” and so on. Older adolescents, in contrast, are
Some even suggested that turning an activity that some likely to respond with more abstract and more general
adolescents wanted to do into a school requirement statements—“To ensure safety and enforce the gov-
would make the activity less intrinsically rewarding. ernment” or “They are basically guidelines for people.
In order to inform this issue, several studies have I mean, like this is wrong and this is right and to help
been conducted that have compared students who have them understand” (Adelson, 1972, p. 108). Individu-
volunteered for community service with those who als’ understanding of various rights—for example,
have had it forced on them. It does not seem that re- their beliefs about whether children and adolescents
quiring community service makes students develop have the right to have some control over their lives—
negative attitudes about volunteering, regardless of becomes more abstract with age, as well (Ruck,
whether they had been volunteers previously. But the Abramovitch, & Keating, 1998). Additionally, with age,
evidence is mixed with regard to whether the effects are individuals are more likely to judge the appropriate-
different between adolescents who willingly participate ness of having certain rights (for example, freedom
and those who do it only because it is a requirement. of speech) in light of characteristics of the individual
Whereas some studies find that participating in com- (for example, whether the individual is mature enough
munity service activities has positive effects regardless to act responsibly) and the context within which
of whether the participation is voluntary or required by the right is expressed (for example, whether the au-
the adolescent’s school (Hart et al., 2007; J. Schmidt, thority who is regulating speech is a parent or a gov-
Shumow, & Kackar, 2007), another found that partici- ernment official) (Helwig, 1997). Nevertheless, there is
pation had little effect in either group (Henderson, strong support among adolescents for fundamental
Brown, Pancer, & Ellis-Hale, 2007). One important dif- democratic principles such as representation and
ference, however, between students who are forced into majority rule, even among adolescents growing up
community service and those who volunteer is that in countries whose governments do not operate on
volunteers are more likely to continue their service these principles (Helwig, Arnold, Tan, & Boyd, 2007;
work after graduation, suggesting that, whatever the Smetana & Villalobos, 2009).
positive effects of participation, they are not enough to Second, political thinking during adolescence be-
turn adolescents who aren’t especially interested in comes less authoritarian and less rigid (Flanagan &
community work into adults who are (Planty, Bozick, & Galay, 1995). Young adolescents are inclined toward
Regnier, 2006). Perhaps the most reasonable conclusion obedience, authority, and an uncritical, trusting, and
one can draw from these studies is that the potential acquiescent stance toward government. For example,
benefits to the recipients of the adolescents’ service (the when asked what might be done in response to a law
children they tutor, the elderly they visit, or the neigh- that is not working out as planned, the young adoles-
borhoods whose parks they clean up) may be greater cent will “propose that it be enforced more rigorously.”
than the effects on the participants themselves. An older teenager may suggest, instead, that the law
needs to be reexamined and perhaps amended. In con-
trast to older adolescents, younger adolescents are
POLITICAL THINKING DURING
“more likely to favor one-man rule as [opposed to] rep-
ADOLESCENCE
resentative democracy,” show “little sensitivity to indi-
Less is known about the development of political think- vidual or minority rights”; and are “indifferent to the
ing during adolescence than about moral development, claims of personal freedom” (Adelson, 1972, p. 108).
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 309 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 309

Of special significance is the development during 1990). This context includes both the immediate com-
late adolescence of a roughly coherent and consistent munity and the larger social and historical environment.
set of attitudes—a sort of ideology—that does not ap- Thus, minority adolescents, especially those living in en-
pear before this point and that is based on a set of over- vironments in which there are limited economic oppor-
arching principles. These principles may concern a tunities, tend to be more cynical about politics than their
wide range of issues, including civil liberties, freedom White counterparts. Similarly, a study of German youth
of speech, and social equality (Flanagan & Galay, 1995; indicates that adolescents are more likely to develop
Helwig, 1995). As is the case among adults, adolescents’ right-wing (excessively authoritarian) attitudes when
views about political matters—the causes of unem- they are raised by authoritarian parents within a broader
ployment, poverty, or homelessness, for example—are context that promotes antiforeigner and antidemocratic
strongly linked to their social upbringing. Adolescents attitudes (Noack, Kracke, & Hofer, 1994).
from higher social classes tend to attribute unemploy- The importance of context can be clearly seen when
ment, poverty, and homelessness to societal factors adolescents’ civic engagement and political participa-
(“People are unemployed because companies are tion are tracked over time. One recent analysis of 30
moving a lot of jobs to Mexico”), whereas adolescents years of data from a large, nationally representative
from lower-class backgrounds are more likely to attri- sample of American high school seniors identified sev-
bute these problems to individual factors (“People are eral important trends (Syvertsen, Wray-Lake, Flanagan,
poor because they have problems managing money”) Osgood, & Briddell, 2011). As you can see in Figure 9.8,
(Flanagan & Tucker, 1999). since 1990, there has been a steady increase in the pro-
Shifts in all three of these directions—increasing ab- portion of young people who report participating in
straction, decreasing authoritarianism, and increasing community service activities, perhaps because there
use of principles—are similar to the shifts observed in was an increase during this time in the number of
studies of moral development, and they support the schools that required it. The increase was especially no-
idea that cognitive autonomy begins to emerge during table among college-bound seniors. The figure also
late adolescence. The movement away from authori- shows that there was a drop in participation in “con-
tarianism, obedience, and unquestioning acceptance of ventional” civic activities (e.g., voting, contacting
the rulings of authority is especially interesting because elected officials) between 1976 and 1990, but little
it suggests further that an important psychological con- change after that. Participation in alternative political
cern for adolescents involves questioning the values activities (e.g., boycotts, demonstrations) waxed and
and beliefs emanating from parents and other authority waned over the 30-year period.
figures and trying to establish their own priorities.
RELIGIOUS BELIEFS DURING
❚ Political Thinking and Political Behavior As is
ADOLESCENCE
the case with moral development, there may be gaps be-
tween adolescents’ political thinking in hypothetical situ- Despite the fact that religion plays an important role
ations and their actual political attitudes and behavior. In in the lives of many adolescents, the development of
general, the most important influences on the political religious beliefs has been very much neglected by so-
behavior of young people tends to be the social context cial scientists (Burg, Mayers, & Miller, 2011; Clardy &
in which they come of age (Flanagan, 2004; Torney-Purta, King, 2011)

45% FIGURE 9.8 Changes over time in adolescents’


Community involvement in conventional political activities
40% Conventional (e.g., voting), alternative political activities
Alternative (e.g., demonstrations), and community service.
35% (Syvertsen et al., 2011)

30%

25%

20%

15%

10%
1976
1978
1980
1982
1984
1986
1988
1990
1992
1994
1996
1998
2000
2002
2004
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 310 21/03/13 11:03 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

310 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

FIGURE 9.9 The proportion of adoles- 100


cents from different countries who re- 90
port that God is very important in their
80
life. (Based on Clardy & King, 2011)
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0

China
Japan
France
Russia
Great Britain
Australia
South Korea
Italy
Croatia
Canada
Uruguay
Argentina
U.S.
South Africa
Mexico
Egypt
Philippines
Pakistan
Religious beliefs, like moral and political beliefs, Studies of the development of religious beliefs dur-
become more abstract, more principled, and more ing adolescence indicate many parallels with the devel-
independent during the adolescent years. Specifically, opment of moral and political reasoning. During late
adolescents’ beliefs become more oriented toward adolescence, individuals enter into a stage in which
spiritual and ideological matters and less oriented to- they begin to form a system of personal religious
ward rituals, practices, and the strict observance of beliefs, rather than relying solely on the teachings of
religious customs. For example, although close to their parents (P. King & Roeser, 2009). This is reminis-
90% of all American adolescents pray, and 95% be- cent, of course, of adolescents’ transition to principled
lieve in God, a substantial proportion of young people moral reasoning or to the development in middle to late
feel that organized religion does not play a very im-
portant role in their lives (Gallup & Bezilla, 1992;
Holder et al., 2000; Wallace, Forman, Caldwell, &
Willis, 2003). Compared with children, adolescents
place more emphasis on the internal aspects of reli-
gious commitment (such as what an individual be-
lieves) and less on the external manifestations (such
as whether an individual goes to church) (Lopez,
Huynh, & Fuligni, 2011). Adolescence is an important
time for “spiritual questioning, doubting, and creat-
ing” (P. King & Roeser, 2009, p. 447). There are enor-
mous differences around the world in the extent to
which adolescents say that God is important in their
life (see Figure 9.9).

MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION


As shown in Figure 9.9, there is wide cultural variability
around the world in the significance of religion in
adolescents’ lives. To what extent do you think this af-
fects the nature of adolescence more generally? Is the
experience of adolescence as a devel-
opmental period likely to be differ-
ent for young people who grow up
in a context where religion is
important compared to those who
grow up in one where it plays less of During adolescence, religious beliefs become more oriented toward
spiritual and ideological matters and less oriented toward rituals,
a role?
practices, and the strict observance of religious customs.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 311 21/03/13 11:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 311

adolescence of a coherent political ideology. In all like-


lihood, developments in all three domains—moral, po- Not
important 8th Grade
litical, and religious—probably reflect the underlying 10th Grade

Religious importance
growth of cognitive abilities and the shift from concrete 12th Grade
to abstract reasoning that characterizes the adolescent A little
important
transition. As you now know, this fundamental shift in
cognitive ability affects adolescents’ thinking across a
wide variety of topics. Pretty
important
The development of religious beliefs is multifaceted,
but in many ways we can think of religious develop-
Very
ment as having two main components: religiosity important
(which refers to the religious practices one engages in)
and spirituality (which refers to one’s personal quest
for answers to questions about God and the meaning of
life) (P. King, Ramos, & Clardy, 2013). Although both Never
can be part of the process of psychosocial development,

Religious attendance
religiosity may be more important for identity develop-
ment, since it involves the identification of oneself with Rarely
a particular religious group and its practices and beliefs
(not unlike identifying oneself with a particular ethnic Once or
group), whereas spirituality may be more closely linked twice
to the development of cognitive autonomy, since it in- a month
volves the development of a personal meaning system, About
self-awareness, and certain values. For most religious once
a week
adolescents, religiosity and spirituality are deeply inter-
connected, as they are among many Muslim youth (D. C. 0 10 20 30 40 50
French, Eisenberg, Vaughan, Purwono, & Suryanti, Percent
2008). But there are substantial numbers of adolescents
FIGURE 9.10 Age differences in religious activity.
who practice religion without giving much thought to (Wallace et al., 2003)
its spiritual aspects (for instance, adolescents who at-
tend religious services each week or celebrate religious
holidays mainly because their parents expect them to or important role; religious commitment may become
because they enjoy the familiarity and routine of regu- stronger among students who attend a college with a
lar observance) and substantial numbers who devote a religious orientation (Barry & Nelson, 2005).
great deal of time and energy to thinking about spiritual Although some parents interpret this decline in reli-
matters but who don’t identify with an organized reli- giosity as indicating rebellion against the family’s val-
gion or practice customary religious rituals. ues, the development of religious thinking during late
adolescence might be better understood as part of the
❚ Patterns of Religious Involvement Generally, overall development of cognitive autonomy. As the ad-
the stated importance of religion—and especially olescent develops a stronger sense of independence, he or
religiosity—declines somewhat during the adolescent she may leave behind the unquestioning conventional-
years (Koenig, McGue, & Iacono, 2008). Compared ity of earlier religious behavior as a first step toward
with older adolescents, younger ones are more likely to finding a truly personal faith. The adolescent who con-
attend church regularly and to state that religion is im- tinues to comply with her or his parents’ religious be-
portant to them (Wallace et al., 2003) (see Figure 9.10). liefs without ever questioning them may actually be
In addition, church attendance and religious observa- showing signs of immature conformity or identity fore-
tion are more common among rural youth, especially closure, and not spiritual maturity.
those from farm families (V. King, Elder, & Whitbeck,
1997). The early years of college appear to be a time ❚ Individual Differences in Religiosity Although
when some individuals reexamine and reevaluate many individuals generally become less involved in formal
of the beliefs and values they grew up with. For many, religion during adolescence, there are differences among
this involves a decline in regular participation in orga-
nized religious activities (perhaps because the college
religiosity The degree to which one engages in religious practices,
environment doesn’t encourage this) but an increase in like attending services.
spirituality and religious faith (Lefkowitz, 2005). The
religious context of the college environment plays an spirituality The degree to which one places importance on the
quest for answers to questions about God and the meaning of life.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 312 21/03/13 11:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

312 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

adolescents in their degree of religiosity (Dollahite, Lay- Willoughby, 2011; Good, Willoughby, & Fritjers, 2009;
ton, Bahr, Walker, & Thatcher, 2009; Good, Willoughby, Hardy & Raffaelli, 2003; R. K. Jones, Darroch, & Singh,
& Busseri, 2011). According to U.S. surveys, about 85% 2005; McCree, Wingood, DiClemente, Davies, &
of American adolescents report an affiliation with a re- Harrington, 2003; Steinman & Zimmerman, 2004). As
ligious group; of the remaining 15%, about 10% report you read in Chapter 3, religious involvement may play
not being religious, 3% say they are uncertain, and an- an especially important role in buffering inner-city
other 3% describe themselves as atheist or agnostic Black adolescents against the harmful effects of neigh-
(King et al., 2013). About half of all American adoles- borhood disorganization and exposure to violence
cents say that formal religious participation is impor- (Aceves & Cookston, 2007; Benhorin & McMahon,
tant in their life (P. King & Roeser, 2009). Approximately 2008; Brookmeyer et al., 2005; Fowler et al., 2008;
38% of adolescents report weekly attendance at reli- Kliewer et al., 2006; S. T. Li, Nussbaum, & Richards,
gious services, 17% attend once or twice per month, 2007; Rosario et al., 2008), and there also is evidence
and about 45% rarely or never attend services; regular that religious involvement may help protect against the
attendance at religious services drops over the course of adverse effects of family conflict (K. A. Davis & Epkins,
high school. Adolescent religious attendance declined 2009). There is more consistent evidence for the role
gradually during the 1980s and 1990s but has changed that religion plays in preventing problem behavior than
very little since then. About half of U.S. adolescents for its role in promoting positive development.
identify themselves as Protestant (and close to half of Although studies show that religious adolescents are
Protestants identify themselves as conservative Chris- more involved in the community, more altruistic, and
tians), and about one-fifth as Catholic. Adolescent girls more prosocial, other research finds that rates of iden-
are slightly more likely to be religious than adolescent tity foreclosure (see Chapter 8) are higher among reli-
boys (this sex difference is found among adults, too). In gious adolescents (P. King & Roeser, 2009).
general, Black and Latino adolescents are more religious
than youth from other ethnic backgrounds, as are
adolescents who live in the South or Midwest (P. King & MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
Roeser, 2009; C. Smith & Denton, 2005; Wallace et al., A great deal of research indicates that adolescents
2003). Rates of adolescents’ religious participation are who report high levels of religious involvement also
considerably higher in the United States than other report better mental health and lower levels of prob-
parts of the world. In contrast to the 41.9% of American lem behavior. Given this,
youth who participate in an organization sponsored by some writers advocate
a religious group, the figures are 14.4% in Western more religious participa-
Europe, 27.6% in Southern Europe, 19.9% in Asia/ tion in adolescence. Do
Pacific regions, 13.1% in Northern Europe, and 10.3% you agree? Why or why
in Eastern Europe (King et al., 2013). not?

❚ The Impact of Religious Involvement on


Development A growing body of research suggests
that religious adolescents are better adjusted and less Some clues into the reasons that religious involve-
depressed than other adolescents, less likely to engage ment may protect against involvement in problem be-
in premarital sexual intercourse, less likely to use drugs, havior comes from the finding that religiosity rather
and less likely to engage in delinquent behavior (C. Y. than spirituality is the stronger predictor of staying out
Chen, Dormitzer, Bejarano, & Anthony, 2004; Holder et of trouble (M. Good & Willoughby, 2006). Moreover, it
al., 2000; P. King, Carr, & Boiter, 2011; Ludden, 2011; A. does not appear to be attending religious services that
M. Meier, 2003; L. Miller, Davies, & Greenwald, 2000; matters—it is being a part of a community of individu-
Milot & Ludden, 2009; Sallquist, Eisenberg, French, als who share similar values and engage in similar ac-
Purwono, & Suryanti, 2010; Sinha, Cnaan, & Gelles, tivities. In one study of churchgoing rural youth, the
2007). Although some of the apparent positive effects adolescents frequently mentioned participating in
of religious involvement are due to the fact that adoles- youth groups and the relationships they formed with
cents who are involved in religion often have other youth group leaders as important influences on their
positive influences in their life that on their own pro- behavior and development (M. Good & Willoughby,
mote positive development and prevent problem be- 2007). Seen in this light, the connection between reli-
havior (for example, supportive parents, prosocial and gious participation and lower problem behavior is not
religious peers, adults who care about them) (French, surprising, since, as you know from Chapter 5 (and will
Purwono, & Triwahyuni, 2011; P. King & Furrow, 2004), see again in later chapters), among the strongest predic-
being religious in and of itself appears to deter problem tors of adolescents’ involvement in sex, drugs, and de-
behavior and delay the onset of sexual activity (Good & linquency are the behavior and attitudes of their peers.
ste32118_ch09_284-313.indd Page 313 21/03/13 11:04 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 9 Autonomy 313

RECAP • Studies of civic engagement have focused mainly


• According to Kohlberg’s theory, which is the domi- on adolescents’ involvement in community service.
nant perspective on the development of moral Involvement in community service leads to gains in
reasoning, late adolescence is a time of potential social responsibility, tolerance, and the importance
shifting from a morality that defines right and individuals place on helping others.
wrong in terms of society’s rules to one that de- • Changes in the domains of political and religious
fines right and wrong on the basis of one’s own thinking during adolescence also reflect the individ-
basic moral principles. ual’s growing sense of cognitive autonomy. As with
• Adolescents’ moral behavior does not always moral reasoning, political and religious thinking
match their moral reasoning, in part because con- become more abstract, more principled, and more
textual factors influence how they act when they independent, especially during late adolescence.
face moral dilemmas in the real world. • Religious involvement during adolescence lowers
• The ways in which individuals think about prosocial the risk for problem behavior, primarily because
phenomena, such as honesty or kindness, become it helps situate adolescents in
more sophisticated during adolescence, although peer groups of other
there is no consistent evidence that individuals be- well-behaved
come more helpful or kind as they move through adolescents.
adolescence.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 314 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 315 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
10
Intimacy
INTIMACY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON ADOLESCENT INTIMACY
Sullivan’s Theory of Interpersonal Development
Interpersonal Development During Adolescence
Attachment in Adolescence
THE DEVELOPMENT OF INTIMACY IN ADOLESCENCE
Changes in the Nature of Friendship
Changes in the Display of Intimacy
Sex Differences in Intimacy
Changes in the Targets of Intimacy
Friendships With the Other Sex
DATING AND ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS
Dating and the Development of Intimacy
The Development of Dating Relationships
The Impact of Dating on Adolescent Development
INTIMACY AND PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT

315
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 316 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

316 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

My boyfriend and I fell asleep together one night at my therefore have an intimate relationship without having a
house [when I was 15]. We were curled up facing each sexual one. And, by the same token, two people can have
other. I fell asleep looking at his face. I had a dream that a sexual relationship without being especially intimate.
night where I was falling. . . . I looked into his eyes and Although the development of intimacy during ado-
I knew that we were having the same dream. We immedi-
lescence is almost always studied in relation to friend-
ately hugged each other and we both knew what had
ships and romantic relationships with peers, adolescents’
happened simply by looking in each other’s eyes.
intimate relationships are by no means limited to other
(A 21-year-old’s response when asked to recall “a vivid,
highly memorable, personally important memory that teenagers. Parents often have intimate relationships with
‘conveys powerfully how you have come to be the person their adolescent children, especially when the children
you currently are.’”) (McLean & Thorne, 2003, p. 641). have reached a sufficient level of maturity. Siblings, even
with many years between them, are often close confi-
ONE OF THE MOST REMARKABLE things about adoles- dants. Sometimes, young people form intimate relation-
cence is the ways in which close relationships change ships with adults who are not in their immediate family.
during these years. Think about the friendships you had as Obviously, one of the central issues in the study of
a child, and compare them with those you had as a teen- intimacy during adolescence is the onset of dating.
ager. Think about the boyfriends or girlfriends that chil- Although the young person’s initiation into romantic
dren have and the boyfriends or girlfriends adolescents relationships is undoubtedly important, it is not the
have. And think about relationships between parents and only noteworthy change that occurs in close relation-
their children and about how these relationships change ships during adolescence. Adolescence is also an im-
during adolescence. In all three cases, adolescents’ rela- portant time for changes in what individuals look for
tionships are closer, more personal, more involved, and in friends, in their capacity to be intimate with friends
more emotionally charged than children’s. During adoles- of both sexes, and in the way they express closeness
cence, in short, relationships become more intimate. In to others. Indeed, when college students were asked
this chapter, we examine how and why this occurs. what the most important criteria are for deciding
At the outset, we need to draw a distinction between when someone is ready to get married, the capacity
intimacy and sexuality. The concept of intimacy—at for intimacy with another person is the most consis-
least as it is used in the study of adolescence—does not tently mentioned indicator, among both males and
have a sexual or physical connotation. Rather, an intimate females (Carroll et al., 2009) (see Table 10.1). Interest-
relationship is an emotional attachment between two ingly, in this sample of nearly 800 American college
people that is characterized by concern for each other’s students from around the country, who averaged 20
well-being; a willingness to disclose private, and occa- years in age, only 9% of the men and just 5% of the
sionally sensitive, topics; and a sharing of common inter- women said they were ready for marriage, whereas
ests and activities. (An easy way to remember this is 60% of men and 67% of women said they weren’t. The
“caring,” “daring,” and “sharing.”) Two individuals can rest were ambivalent.

TABLE 10.1 When is someone ready to get married?

Percent of American college students


Criterion who say this is necessary

Be able to express feelings in close relationships 98


Be able to listen to others in an understanding way 98
Be able to discuss personal problems with others 98
Be respectful of others when dealing with differences 98
Accept responsibility for the consequences of your actions 97
Committed to a long-term love relationship 96
Avoid aggressive and violent behavior 95
Become less self-oriented, develop greater consideration for others 95
Financially independent from parents and others 91
For a man, become capable of supporting a family financially 91
For a woman, become capable of running a household 91

Source: Carroll et al., 2009.


ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 317 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 317

Intimacy as an Adolescent of good parenting to the development of social compe-


tence (Glick & Rose, 2011).
Issue
❚ Puberty and the Development of Intimacy
Intimacy is an important concern throughout most of
Why do such important changes take place in close re-
the life span. During childhood, not having friends is
lationships during adolescence? Several theorists point
associated with a wide range of psychological and social
to significant links between the development of inti-
problems (Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 2006). And dur-
macy during adolescence and the biological, cognitive,
ing adulthood, having at least one intimate friendship
and social changes of the period (Savin-Williams &
is beneficial to an individual’s health: People who have
Berndt, 1990). Naturally, changes in sexual impulses at
others to turn to for emotional support are less likely to
puberty provoke interest in sex, which leads to the de-
suffer from psychological and physical disorders
velopment of romantic relationships. In addition, with
(Myers, Lindentthal, & Pepper, 1975). Without ques-
puberty and sexuality come new issues and concerns
tion, close relationships are extremely important to
requiring serious, intimate discussions. Some young
people of all ages. Why, then, is the development of in-
people feel hesitant to discuss sex and dating with their
timacy especially important during adolescence?
parents and turn instead to relationships outside the
One reason is that it is not until adolescence that
family. And some of the most intimate conversations
truly intimate relationships—relationships character-
adolescents have with their friends involve their rela-
ized by openness, honesty, self-disclosure, and trust—
tionships with actual or potential romantic partners.
emerge. Although children certainly have important
These sorts of concerns may also prompt the develop-
friendships, their relationships are different from those
ment, perhaps for the first time, of intimate friendships
formed during adolescence. Children’s friendships are
with other-sex peers.
activity oriented; they are built around games and
shared activities. To a child, a friend is someone who
❚ Cognitive Change and the Development of
likes to do the same things he or she does. But teenag-
Intimacy Advances in thinking—especially in the
ers’ close friendships are more likely to have a strong
realm of social cognition—are also related to the devel-
emotional foundation; they are built on the sorts of
opment of intimacy during adolescence (Rote &
bonds that form between people who care about and
Smetana, 2011). As we saw in Chapter 2, the growth of
know and understand each other in a special way
social cognition during adolescence is reflected in the
(Kobak & Madsen, 2011).
young person’s more sophisticated conceptions of so-
cial relationships and in improvements in interpersonal
understanding and communication. These changes
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION permit adolescents to establish and maintain relation-
What things have been special about the re- ships that are characterized by higher levels of empathy,
lationships in your life that you consider to self-disclosure, and sensitivity. Limitations in preado-
be intimate? Do you agree with the asser- lescents’ ability to look at things from another person’s
tion that genuine intimacy in relationships point of view may make intimate interpersonal rela-
does not appear before adolescence? tionships a cognitive impossibility, because it is hard to
be an intimate friend to someone with whom you are
unable to empathize.

Another reason for the importance of intimacy dur- ❚ Changes in Social Roles and the Development
ing adolescence concerns the changing nature of the of Intimacy We can also point to changes in the
adolescent’s social world—during early adolescence, adolescent’s social roles as potentially affecting the de-
the increasing importance of peers in general, and dur- velopment of intimacy. Perhaps most simply, the be-
ing middle and late adolescence, the increasing impor- havioral independence that often accompanies the
tance of other-sex peers in particular (Furman, Brown, transition from childhood into adolescence provides
& Feiring, 1999). In Chapter 9, we looked at the young greater opportunities for adolescents to be alone with
person’s growing orientation toward peers as part of their friends, engaged in intimate discussion. Adoles-
the development of emotional autonomy. In this chap- cents spend more time in conversation with their
ter, we look at changes in adolescent peer relations friends than in any other activity (Dijkstra & Veenstra,
again, but in a different light—as part of the develop- 2011). Moreover, the recognition of adolescents as
ment of intimacy. Although experiences in the family
are important for the initial development of social
skills, experiences in friendships, especially during
adolescence, contribute above and beyond the benefits intimacy The psychosocial domain concerning the formation,
maintenance, and termination of close relationships.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 318 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

318 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

“near adults” may prompt their parents and other


adults to confide in them and turn to them for support.
Theoretical Perspectives on
Shared experiences such as working, as well as the de- Adolescent Intimacy
velopment of emotional autonomy, may help give
The most important theoretical perspectives on the de-
young people and their parents more of a basis for
velopment of intimacy during adolescence are those of
friendship and communication (Youniss & Smollar,
Harry Stack Sullivan (1953a) and various writers who
1985). Finally, changes in the structure of schools dur-
have studied attachment relationships in adolescence
ing early adolescence—often giving younger teenagers
(Kobak & Madsen, 2011; McElhaney, Allen, Stephenson,
more contact with older ones—may promote new
& Hare, 2009). Let’s look at each of these views in turn.
types of peer relationships (Eccles & Roeser, 2009).
During the course of preadolescence and adoles-
cence, relationships are gradually transformed from the SULLIVAN’S THEORY OF INTERPERSONAL
friendly but activity-oriented friendships of childhood DEVELOPMENT
to the more self-conscious, analytical, and intimate re-
Like Erikson, whose theory of adolescent identity de-
lationships of adulthood. In the next section, we exam-
velopment was discussed in Chapter 8, Sullivan took a
ine why and how this transformation occurs.
far less biological view of development than other
thinkers. Instead, Sullivan emphasized the social as-
pects of growth, suggesting that psychological develop-
ment can be best understood when looked at in
RECAP interpersonal terms. Sullivan’s theory focuses on trans-
• In adolescence, the development of intimacy formations in the adolescent’s relationships with oth-
refers to the development of relationships that ers. In particular, the challenges of adolescence (and
are characterized by self-disclosure, trust, and indeed, according to Sullivan, of the entire life cycle)
concern. revolve around trying to satisfy changing interpersonal
• The changes of puberty, which draw young people needs (Buhrmester, 1996).
together around common concerns and which
stimulate interest in romantic relationships, ❚ Stages of Interpersonal Needs Sullivan’s per-
contribute to the development of intimacy. spective starts from the premise that, as children de-
• The development of intimacy is also stimulated by velop, different interpersonal needs surface that lead
the cognitive changes of the period, which allow either to feelings of security (when the needs are satis-
for a more sophisticated understanding of fied) or feelings of anxiety (when the needs are frus-
relationships. trated). Sullivan charted a developmental progression
• Intimate relationships are facilitated by the social of needs, beginning in infancy and continuing through
changes of the period, which provide for more adolescence (see Table 10.2) (Sullivan, 1953b). These
opportunities for adolescents to be alone with changing interpersonal needs define the course of in-
each other and for them to acquire experi- terpersonal development through different phases of
ences that bring them closer to the life span. During middle childhood, for example,
adults. youngsters need to be accepted into peer groups, or
else they feel rejected and ostracized.
In Sullivan’s view, the security that is derived from
having satisfying relationships with others is the glue

TABLE 10.2 Interpersonal needs associated with different developmental eras: Sullivan’s theory

Developmental Epochs Interpersonal Needs

Infancy (0 to 2–3 yrs) Need for contact with people, need for tenderness from mothering one
Early childhood (2–3 to 6–7 yrs) Need for adult participation in child’s play
Middle childhood (6–7 to 8–10 yrs) Need for peer playmates, need for acceptance into peer society groups
Preadolescence (8–10 to 12–14 yrs) Need for intimacy and consensual validation in same-sex chumships
Early adolescence (12–14 to 17–18 yrs) Need for sexual contact, need for intimacy with other-sex partner
Late adolescence (17–18 yrs to adult) Need for integration into adult society

Source: H. S. Sullivan, 1953a.


ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 319 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 319

that holds one’s sense of self together. Identity and


self-esteem are gradually built up through interper-
sonal relationships. Like Erikson, Sullivan viewed
psychosocial development as cumulative: The frustra-
tions and satisfactions individuals experience during
earlier periods affect their later relationships and de-
veloping sense of identity. For instance, the child who
as an infant has her need for contact or tenderness
frustrated will approach interpersonal relationships at
subsequent ages with greater anxiety, a more intense
need for security, and a shakier sense of self. In con-
trast, the infant who has his interpersonal needs met
will approach later relationships with confidence and
optimism.
When important interpersonal transitions arise (for
example, during childhood, when the social world is
broadened to include significant relationships with
peers), having a solid foundation of security in past
As adolescents’ needs for intimacy increase, so might the emphasis
relationships aids in the successful negotiation of the they place on intimacy as an important component of friendship.
transition. An individual who is nervous about form-
ing relationships with others is likely to have trouble
forming new types of relationships, because they
threaten an already shaky sense of security. A child who adolescence, and late adolescence. During preadoles-
does not have a strong sense of security may have many cence, children begin to focus their attention on rela-
friends in elementary school but be too afraid to form tionships with a few close friends, generally of the same
intimate friendships upon reaching preadolescence. sex. It is through these friendships that the need for in-
She may try to maintain friendships like those of child- timacy is first satisfied. With these close friends, the
hood—friendships that focus on playing games, for young person learns to disclose and receive intimate,
example, rather than talking—long after friends have private information and to build caring, mutual friend-
outgrown getting together to “play.” As a result, that ships based on honesty, loyalty, and trust. Sullivan
youngster may be rejected by peers and may feel lonely believed that these relationships could even have a cor-
and isolated. rective influence, helping to repair interpersonal prob-
lems that might have developed during childhood. A
good preadolescent friend, for example, can help some-
INTERPERSONAL DEVELOPMENT DURING
one overcome feelings of insecurity that have devel-
ADOLESCENCE
oped as a result of poor family relationships.
Looking back at the progression of interpersonal needs Not all youngsters feel secure enough as preadoles-
that Sullivan mapped out, we can see that he distin- cents to forge these more mature, intimate friend-
guished between intimacy and sexuality; perhaps more ships. Their feelings of insecurity are so strong that
importantly, he suggested that the need for intimacy— anxiety holds them back. As a result, some youngsters
which surfaces during preadolescence—precedes the never fully develop the capacity to be intimate with
development of romantic or sexual relationships, which others, a limitation that takes its toll on relationships
do not emerge until adolescence. In other words, throughout adolescence and adulthood. In other
Sullivan believed that the capacity for intimacy first de- words, Sullivan felt that forming intimate friendships
velops prior to adolescence and in the context of same- during preadolescence is a necessary precondition to
sex, not other-sex, relationships. This turns out to be forming close relationships as an adolescent or young
one of the most important observations in Sullivan’s adult.
work, because as you will read, the quality of individu- According to Sullivan, the preadolescent era comes
als’ same-sex friendships is predictive of the quality of to an end with the onset of puberty. Early adolescence
their later romantic relationships. One of the main is marked by the emergence of sexuality, in the form of
challenges of adolescence, according to Sullivan, is a powerful, biological sex drive. As a consequence of
making the transition from the nonsexual, intimate, this development, the preferred “target” of the adoles-
same-sex friendships of preadolescence to the sexual, cent’s need for intimacy changes. He or she must be-
intimate, other-sex friendships of late adolescence. gin to make the shift from intimate relationships with
Sullivan divided the years between childhood and members of the same sex to intimate relationships
adulthood into three periods: preadolescence, early with members of the other sex. It is important to note
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 320 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

320 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

that during the historical epoch when Sullivan was intimacy continues well into late adolescence. If the
writing, homosexuality was considered abnormal, and interpersonal tasks of adolescence have been negoti-
like other writers of his era, Sullivan equated normal ated successfully, the young person enters late adoles-
sexual development with the development of hetero- cence able to be intimate, able to enjoy sex, and, most
sexual relationships. Social scientists no longer hold critically, able to experience intimacy and sexuality in
this view, however, and most would say that the cru- the same relationship.
cial interpersonal challenge for the young adolescent
is not the movement from same-sex to other-sex
friendships, but the transition from nonromantic to
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
romantic relationships.
A central proposition in Sullivan’s theory is that we first
Like all interpersonal transitions, the movement
learn how to be intimate within same-
from nonromantic to romantic relationships can be
sex friendships and that later this capac-
fraught with anxiety. For adolescents who do not have a
ity for intimacy is used in romantic
healthy sense of security, it can be scary to leave the
relationships. If this is true, would you
safety of nonsexual friendships and venture into the
expect to see differences in the ways in
world of dating and sexuality.
which males and females express
The overarching challenge of adolescence, accord-
intimacy with romantic partners?
ing to Sullivan, is to integrate an established need for
intimacy with an emerging need for sexual contact in
a way that does not lead to excessive anxiety. Just as
ATTACHMENT IN ADOLESCENCE
Erikson viewed adolescence as a time of experimenta-
tion with different identities, Sullivan saw adolescence In recent decades, a different theoretical perspective has
as a time of experimentation with different types of greatly influenced the study of intimate relationships in
relationships. Some adolescents choose to date many adolescence, one that draws on theories of the develop-
different people to try to find out what they are look- ment of the attachment relationship during infancy
ing for in a relationship. Others get involved very (Kobak & Madsen, 2011). In many ways, the basic ideas
deeply with a boyfriend or girlfriend in a relationship developed by Sullivan (namely, that early relationships
that lasts throughout their entire adolescence. Others set the stage for later ones) were maintained, but a dif-
may have a series of serious relationships. Still others ferent perspective and vocabulary have come to domi-
keep intimacy and sexuality separate. They may de- nate contemporary theory and research on adolescents’
velop close platonic relationships (nonsexual rela- intimate relationships. In order to understand how at-
tionships) with other-sex peers, for example, or they tachment theory is applied to the study of adolescence,
may have sexual relationships without getting very we need to look first at how this construct has been
intimate with their sex partners. And just as Erikson used to understand development in infancy.
viewed role experimentation as a healthy part of the
adolescent’s search for identity, Sullivan viewed the ❚ Attachment in Infancy In writings on infant
adolescent’s experimentation with different types of development, an attachment is defined as a strong and
relationships as a normal way of handling new feel- enduring emotional bond. Virtually all infants form at-
ings, new fears, and new interpersonal needs. For tachment relationships with their mother (and most do
many young people, experimentation with sex and so with their father and other caregivers as well), but
not all infants have attachment relationships of the
same quality. Psychologists differentiate among three
types of infant attachment: secure, anxious-avoidant,
platonic relationships Nonsexual relationships with individuals and anxious-resistant. A secure attachment between
who might otherwise be romantic partners. infant and caregiver is characterized by trust; an
anxious-avoidant attachment is characterized by in-
attachment The strong affectional bond that develops between difference on the part of the infant toward the caregiver;
an infant and a caregiver. and an anxious-resistant attachment is characterized
secure attachment A healthy attachment between infant and by ambivalence. The security of the early attachment
caregiver, characterized by trust. relationship is important, because studies show that in-
fants who have had a secure attachment are more likely
anxious-avoidant attachment An insecure attachment between
to grow into psychologically healthy and socially skilled
infant and caregiver, characterized by indifference on the part of the
infant toward the caregiver. children (Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 1978).
Attachment theory has given rise to two different, but
anxious-resistant attachment An insecure attachment between related, questions about adolescent development. First,
infant and caregiver, characterized by distress at separation and
anger at reunion.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 321 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 321

is there a link between the quality of attachment formed You can throw the ball by clicking on the name or picture of another player
in infancy and mental health or behavior in adolescence?
And, second, can the same three-category framework
used to characterize interpersonal relationships in
infancy—secure, anxious-avoidant, anxious-resistant—
be used to characterize interpersonal relationships in Kimberly Josh
adolescence?

❚ Does Infant Attachment Predict Adolescent


Intimacy? Many theorists who study adolescent de-
You
velopment believe that the nature of individuals’
attachment to caregivers during infancy continues to FIGURE 10.1 Using a computer game called “Cyberball,”
have an influence on their capacity to form satisfying researchers scan adolescents’ brains in order to better
understand their responses to social exclusion.
intimate relationships during adolescence and adult-
(K. Williams et al., 2012)
hood, for two reasons (McElhaney et al., 2009). First,
some theorists have argued that the initial attachment
relationship forms the basis for a more general model
of interpersonal relationships employed throughout Cheung, & Choi, 2012). Participants are told that they
life (Bowlby, 1969). This internal working model de- will be playing a ball-tossing game via the Internet
termines to a large measure whether individuals feel with two other adolescents in other scanners (in actu-
trusting or apprehensive in relationships with others ality, there are no other players). On a screen inside
and whether they see themselves as worthy of others’ the scanner, adolescents see cartoon images repre-
affection. We might think of an internal working senting the other players, as well as a cartoon image of
model as a set of beliefs and expectations people draw their own “hand” (see Figure 10.1). The ball is thrown
on in forming close relationships with others— back and forth among the three players, with the par-
whether they go into relationships expecting accep- ticipant choosing the recipient of his or her throws,
tance or anticipating rejection. According to the and the throws of the other two “players” determined
theory, individuals who enjoyed a secure attachment by the computer. At the beginning of the game, the
relationship during infancy will have a more positive computerized players are equally likely to throw the
and healthy internal working model of relationships ball to the participant or the other player. However, as
during adolescence, whereas individuals who were the task progresses, the other players stop throwing to
anxiously attached as infants will have a less positive the participant. The researchers then compare partici-
model (Kobak & Madsen, 2011; McElhaney et al., pants’ brain activity when they are excluded to that
2009). when they are included. Adolescents high in rejection
Several studies have found that adolescents’ working sensitivity actually show a different pattern of brain
models for their relationships with parents are similar activity in response to exclusion, and those who do so
to their working models of relationships with friends, are more likely to develop symptoms of depression
and adolescents’ working models of relationships with (Masten et al., 2011).
friends are similar to their working models of relation-
ships with romantic partners (e.g., Furman, Simon,
Shaffer, & Bouchey, 2002). In addition, a number of
writers have suggested that individuals who emerge MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
from infancy with an insecure attachment are more Think about your own internal working model of
sensitive to being rejected by others in later romantic relationships. Are there consistencies in the
encounters, a trait that psychologists call rejection ways in which you approach close relation-
sensitivity (N. L. Collins & Feeney, 2004; G. Downey, ships with different people? Would you
Bonica, & Rincón, 1999). Individuals who are high in say that you are high or low in “rejection
rejection sensitivity are more likely to develop symp- sensitivity”?
toms of depression and anxiety, which in turn, lead to
further increases in rejection sensitivity (Marston,
Hare, & Allen, 2010).
In recent years, several teams of neuroscientists
internal working model The implicit model of interpersonal
have studied adolescents’ neural responses to rejec-
relationships that an individual employs throughout life, believed to
tion by imaging their brain activity while playing an be shaped by early attachment experiences.
online game called “Cyberball” (K. Williams, Yeager,
rejection sensitivity Heightened vulnerability to being rejected
by others.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 322 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

322 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

A second reason for the continued importance of How strong is the specific link between infant attach-
early attachment relationships during adolescence is ment and the quality of interpersonal relationships in
that interpersonal development is cumulative: What adolescence and young adulthood? Do individuals who
happens during infancy affects what happens in early were securely attached as infants have more positive
childhood, which affects what happens in middle child- working models of relationships as adolescents or
hood, and so on (Kerns, 1996). In other words, indi- young adults? Studies that have followed individuals
viduals who leave infancy with a secure attachment from infancy all the way through adolescence and be-
may be on a different interpersonal trajectory than yond have yielded conflicting results. Some have shown
those who leave infancy insecure. (Here’s where you considerable continuity from infancy through adoles-
can see similarities between this perspective and cence (e.g., C. Hamilton, 2000; Waters, Merrick,
Sullivan’s.) The only way to examine this proposition is Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2000), but others have
to follow individuals over time and trace their interper- shown no continuity whatsoever (M. Lewis, Feiring, &
sonal development. Rosenthal, 2000; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000).
Numerous studies that have done this show that Some researchers have suggested that individuals’ secu-
insecure infants are more likely to develop psycho- rity of attachment remains stable only in the absence of
logical and social problems during childhood and major life events that could upset the course of inter-
adolescence, including poor peer relationships (e.g., personal development (such as the loss of a parent or
Weinfield, Ogawa, & Sroufe, 1997). It has been hy- parental divorce), and that the lack of continuity ob-
pothesized that these problems in peer relations dur- served in some studies is due to the importance of in-
ing childhood affect the development of social tervening events (Beckwith, Cohen, & Hamilton, 1999;
competence during adolescence—in essence, forming Waters et al., 2000; Weinfield et al., 2000). Others, how-
a link between early experience and later relationships ever, argue that the significance of early attachment for
(Jaffari-Bimmel, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans- later relationships is far outweighed by the importance
Kranenburg, & Mooijaart, 2006). Additionally, several of the experiences the individual has in childhood and
studies indicate that the benefits of positive relations the context in which he or she lives as an adolescent
with peers extend beyond adolescence: Individuals (M. Lewis et al., 2000).
who establish healthy intimate relationships with age-
mates during adolescence are psychologically health- ❚ Attachment in Adolescence In addition to
ier and more satisfied with their lives as middle-aged employing the three-way attachment classification
adults (Hightower, 1990). scheme to study the links among infancy, childhood,
Of course, it is possible for interpersonal develop- and adolescence, attachment theorists have applied
ment to be cumulative without the root cause of this similar classifications to the study of adolescents’ at-
continuity being the individual’s internal working tachments to others (e.g., McElhaney et al., 2009), as
model. Individuals who have positive peer relationships well as to adolescents’ internal working models (e.g.,
in childhood may simply learn how to get along better Kobak, Cole, Ferenz-Gillies, Fleming, & Gamble, 1993).
with others, and this may lead to more positive peer In some of these studies, adolescents’ current relation-
relationships in adolescence, which, in turn, may lead ships with parents and peers are assessed; in others,
to better relationships in adulthood. For instance, one adolescents are asked to recount their childhood expe-
study that followed individuals from birth through riences through the use of a procedure called the Adult
midlife found a cascade of interpersonal connections Attachment Interview (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy,
over time: low-quality parent–child relationships were 1985). The interview focuses on individuals’ recollec-
linked to low-quality parent–adolescent relationships, tions of their early attachment experiences and obtains
which predicted low-quality romantic relationships in information on the ways in which the individual
young adulthood and, later, dissatisfaction with life in recounts his or her childhood history. A variety of
middle age (Overbeek, Stattin, Vermulst, Ha, & Engels, schemes for coding responses to the interview have
2007). Similarly, other research finds that individuals been devised, but most categorize individuals as
with more negative views of themselves disengage from “secure,” “dismissing,” or “preoccupied.”
peers, which may lead to poorer-quality peer relation- Many researchers have found that adolescents in dif-
ships and peer rejection, thereby intensifying their ferent attachment categories differ in predictable ways
negative self-image (M. S. Caldwell, Rudolph, Troop- (McElhaney et al., 2009). Compared with dismissing or
Gordon, & Kim, 2004). preoccupied adolescents, for example, secure adoles-
cents interact with their mothers with less unhealthy
anger and more appropriate assertiveness, suggesting
that they may experience fewer difficulties in establish-
Adult Attachment Interview A structured interview used to ing emotional autonomy (Kobak et al., 1993). Individ-
assess an individual’s past attachment history and “internal working uals with dismissive or preoccupied attachment profiles
model” of relationships.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 323 11/03/13 6:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 323

Peer Security at
competence age 16

Adult romantic
Infant
relationship
attachment
quality

FIGURE 10.2 The quality of infant attachment is linked to adult romantic relationships through effects on social
development in childhood and adolescence. (Simpson et al., 2007)

are more likely to show a range of emotional and be- greater risk for emotional and behavioral problems
havior problems in adolescence, including depression, (Rosenthal & Kobak, 2010). Security of attachment is
maladaptive coping, anxiety, eating disorders, conduct also related to whether and at what age adolescents
problems, and delinquency (e.g., J. Allen, Porter, “leave the nest”: Insecurely attached adolescents are
McFarland, McElhaney, & Marsh, 2007; Kobak, Zajac, more likely to delay moving out or to return to their
& Smith, 2009; Lieberman, Doyle, & Markiewicz, 1999; parents’ home than their more securely attached peers
Seiffge-Krenke & Beyers, 2005; P. Zimmermann & (Seiffge-Krenke, 2006).
Becker-Stoll, 2002). Not surprisingly, adolescents who Keep in mind, however, that while attachment se-
are judged to have had a secure infant attachment have curity is generally highly stable over the adolescent
more stable romantic relationships than their insecure years, it can change if adolescents are living in dys-
counterparts (K. Davis & Kirkpatrick, 1994). Studies functional family situations or under high amounts of
have linked security of attachment in infancy with so- stress (J. Allen, McElhaney, Kuperminc, & Jodl, 2004).
cial competence in childhood, security of attachment In other words, early attachment security is not an
to close friends in adolescence, and positive romantic “inoculation” that protects individuals from psycho-
relationships in adulthood (Nosko, Tieu, Lawford, & logical problems forever, but rather a psychological
Pratt, 2011; Simpson, Collins, Tran, & Haydon, 2007) advantage that increases the probability of developing
(Figure 10.2). Of course, it is hard to say whether social in healthy ways.
competence leads to healthier attachments or vice versa By the same token, the degree of security in an
(most probably, both are true). adolescent’s attachment style interacts with other ex-
Numerous studies also have looked at the quality periences to shape mental health and behavior: Posi-
of adolescents’ current attachments to parents and tive experiences (like having an authoritative parent)
peers as well as their early attachments (McElhaney have even more positive effects among adolescents
et al., 2009). In general, individuals who have secure with a secure style, and negative experiences are not
attachments during adolescence are more socially as harmful (J. Allen, Hauser, O’Connor, & Bell, 2002).
competent, more successful in school, less likely to Similarly, among adolescents with an insecure attach-
engage in substance use, and better adjusted than ment style, negative experiences (like having exces-
their insecure peers (J. Allen et al., 2007; Branstetter, sively intrusive parents) have an even worse effect
Furman, & Cottrell, 2009; Çuhadaroğlu Çetin, Tüzün, than they would otherwise (P. Marsh, McFarland, &
Pehlivantürk, Ünal, & Gökler, 2010; M. L. Cooper, Allen, 2003).
Shaver, & Collins, 1998; Mayseless & Scharf, 2007).
There is also some evidence that adolescents’ attach-
ment to their mother is typically more secure and RECAP
more important, and attachment to their father less • According to Sullivan, the need for intimacy
so, over the entire adolescent period (Doyle, Lawford, emerges in preadolescence and is typically satisfied
& Markiewicz, 2009; Markiewicz, Lawford, Doyle, & through same-sex friendships.
Haggart, 2006). One recent study of the ways in which • In Sullivan’s view, the main challenge of adoles-
adolescents’ multiple attachments varied in impor- cence is to integrate an already established need
tance found that individuals whose attachment to for intimacy with the emerging need for sexual
their father ranked very low—lower even than attach- contact in a way that does not engender excessive
ments to people outside the family—were at relatively anxiety.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 324 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

324 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

• Attachment theory now dominates the study of about loyalty and anxieties over rejection become more
intimate relationships in adolescence. Individuals pronounced and may temporarily overshadow concerns
who enjoyed a secure attachment to their caregiver about intimate self-disclosure (Berndt & Perry, 1990).
during infancy develop a healthier or more secure Consistent with this, the sorts of conflicts adolescents
internal working model of relationships, which is have with their friends change during this time; whereas
thought to permit more satisfying intimate rela- older adolescents’ conflicts are typically over private
tionships during adolescence and adulthood. matters, younger adolescents’ conflicts are often over
• There is also evidence that interpersonal develop- perceived public disrespect (Shulman & Laursen, 2002).
ment is cumulative: Positive experiences in early Girls show a pronounced increase in jealousy over
family relationships contribute to social compe- their friends’ friends during early adolescence (J. Parker,
tence, which facilitates the development Low, Walker, & Gamm, 2005). Girls who have low self-
of intimate relationships with esteem and are high in rejection sensitivity are espe-
peers and romantic cially likely to become jealous of their friends’
partners. relationships with other girls. In some senses, then, in-
timate friendship is a mixed blessing for young adoles-
cent girls—they get the benefits of having confidantes
with whom they can easily talk about their problems,
but their friendships are more fragile and more easily
disrupted by feelings of betrayal. As a consequence,
The Development of girls’ friendships on average do not last as long as boys’
do (Benenson & Christakos, 2003).
Intimacy in Adolescence How might Sullivan have explained this pattern?
Why might loyalty become such a pressing concern for
CHANGES IN THE NATURE OF
girls during the middle adolescent years? One possibil-
FRIENDSHIP
ity is that at this age, girls may start to feel more nervous
❚ Changes in Definitions of Friendship When about their relationships with friends because they are
asked what makes someone a friend, both children and beginning to make the transition into other-sex rela-
adolescents mention things like sharing, helping, and tionships. These transitions, as Sullivan noted, can
common activities, but not until early adolescence do make individuals feel insecure. Perhaps it is anxiety
people mention things like self-disclosure, common in- over dating and heightened feelings of insecurity that
terests, similar attitudes and values, or loyalty. In other cause adolescent girls to temporarily place a great deal
words, in childhood, friendship is defined in terms of of emphasis on the trust and loyalty of their close
companionship; it is not until adolescence that intimacy friends. Indeed, close friends who have highly intimate
is a part of the definition (Buhrmester & Furman, 1987). and exclusive relationships with each other often be-
The fact that conceptions of friendship come to have more aggressively with friends than they do to-
place greater weight on things like intimacy, loyalty, and ward peers who are not their close friends (Grotpeter &
shared values and attitudes during early adolescence is Crick, 1996).
consistent with Sullivan’s theory. As adolescents’ needs
for intimacy increase, so does the emphasis they place
on intimacy as an important component of friendship.
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
The findings are also consistent with what we know
about other cognitive changes during early adolescence. Images of jealous adolescent girls pervade American
As we saw in Chapter 2, adolescents have greater facility movies and television shows that fea-
than children in thinking about abstract concepts such ture teenagers. Do you think this is a
as intimacy and loyalty. And as you know, adolescents’ common phenomenon around the
judgments of others are more sophisticated, more psy- world? Or is it less likely to be found
chological, and less tied to concrete attributes than are in cultures where dating is delayed
those of children. until early adulthood?

❚ Jealousy Several studies indicate that the impor-


tance of intimacy as a defining feature of close friend- Adolescents’ close friendships also are distinguished
ship continues to increase throughout early and middle from their casual friendships in the types of conflicts
adolescence (McNelles & Connolly, 1999; Phillipsen, they have and in the ways in which their conflicts are
1999). But an interesting pattern of change occurs resolved (Laursen, 1995, 1996; Raffaelli, 1997). Although
around age 14. During middle adolescence (between conflicts between adolescents and their close friends are
ages 13 and 15), particularly among girls, concerns less frequent than they are between adolescents and
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 325 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 325

other peers, arguments with close friends are more me” and “I feel free to talk to [my friend] about almost
emotional (that is, lots of anger and hurt feelings). More everything” (Sharabany, Gershoni, & Hofman, 1981).
importantly, though, conflict between close friends is Over the course of adolescence, adolescents’ reports of
more likely to provoke efforts to restore the relationship friendship quality increase steadily. These improve-
than is conflict between casual friends. Nonetheless, ments in friendship quality lead to gains in social com-
some best friendships don’t survive, and others are petence, which in turn lead to further improvements in
“downgraded” from best friend to “good friend” the quality of adolescents’ friendships (Glick & Rose,
(Bowker, 2011). 2011). Although there are ethnic differences in average
levels of friendship quality—Asian American adoles-
cents report more dissatisfaction with their friendships
CHANGES IN THE DISPLAY OF INTIMACY than do other adolescents—the rate of improvement in
In addition to placing greater emphasis on intimacy friendship quality over time is the same (Way & Greene,
and loyalty in defining friendship than children do, 2006). Consistent with Sullivan’s viewpoint, then, dur-
teenagers are also more likely to display intimacy in ing preadolescence and early adolescence, youngsters’
their relationships, with respect to what they know friendships become more personal. And, despite fears
about their friends, how responsive they are, how em- that spending time socializing over the Internet will un-
pathic they are, and how they resolve disagreements. dermine adolescents’ social competence, studies find
that the people adolescents interact with online are
mainly the same people they interact with offline (Reich,
❚ Knowing Who Their Friends Are As individu-
Subrahmanyam, & Espinosa, 2012). As you can see from
als move into and through adolescence, they gain knowl-
Figure 10.3, adolescents use social networking sites pri-
edge about more intimate aspects of their friends’ lives.
marily to communicate with people they have offline
For example, although preadolescents and adolescents
relationships with. Actually, adolescents who use the In-
have comparable degrees of knowledge about character-
ternet a lot for social networking are less socially isolated
istics of their best friends that are not especially personal
than their peers (Smahel et al., 2012).
(such as the friend’s telephone number or birthday),
adolescents know significantly more things about their
friends that might be classified as intimate (such as what
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
their friends worry about or what they are proud of)
(Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990). Along similar lines, Do you remember your first intimate
between the 5th and 11th grades, increasingly more ado- friendship? What made the relationship
lescents agree with such statements as “I know how [my different from other friendships that you
friend] feels about things without his [or her] telling had previously had?

To stay in touch w/ friends i don’t see often


To fill up free time/not be bored
Because all my friends have accounts
To stay in touch w/ relatives and family
To make plans w/ friends i see often
To meet new people/make new friends
To explore interests (music, shows, etc.)
To read private entries/comment on profiles
To share my favorite music and video clips
My friend(s) made it for me
To flirt
To voice my opinions on various topics....
other
0 20 40 60 80 100
FIGURE 10.3 Adolescents use social networking sites primarily to communicate with people they have offline relationships
with. (Reich et al., 2012)
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 326 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

326 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

❚ Caring and Concern Individuals also become their friends with personal problems of all sorts be-
more responsive to close friends, less controlling, and come more centered on providing support for the trou-
more tolerant of their friends’ individuality during ad- bled friend and less aimed at mere distraction (Denton
olescence (Berndt & Perry, 1990; Keller, Edelstein, & Zarbatany, 1996).
Schmid, Fang, & Fang, 1998; Shulman, Laursen,
Kalman, & Karpovsky, 1997). This can be interpreted as ❚ Conflict Resolution The ways in which close
another indicator of their increased capacity for inti- friends resolve conflict also change over the course of
macy. Before preadolescence, for example, children are adolescence. As individuals move from childhood into
actually less likely to help and share with their friends adolescence, and from adolescence into young adult-
than with other classmates (perhaps because children hood, they become more likely to end their disagree-
are more competitive with their friends than with other ments by negotiation (trying to compromise or find a
youngsters and do not want to feel inferior). By about solution that is acceptable to both friends) or disen-
4th grade, children treat their friends and other class- gagement (walking away from the situation) and less
mates similarly when it comes to sharing and coopera- likely to end them with one person coercing or over-
tion. But by the time they have reached 8th grade, powering the other and getting his or her way. Negotia-
friends are more helpful and generous toward each tion is also more common between romantic partners
other than toward other classmates. Experiments in than between friends and more common between close
which individuals play computer games with an anony- friends than between acquaintances (Laursen,
mous partner find increases with age in both trust and Finkelstein, & Betts, 2001).
reciprocity (see Figure 10.4) (van den Bos, Westenberg,
van Dijk, & Crone, 2010). Adolescents are also physically
SEX DIFFERENCES IN INTIMACY
and physiologically responsive to their friends: Studies
show that the behaviors and emotional states of pairs of ❚ How Females Are More Intimate There are
friends are more frequently synchronized, or “on the striking sex differences in intimacy during adolescence.
same wavelength,” than are those of acquaintances, When asked to name the people who are most impor-
even when the friends and acquaintances are engaged tant to them, adolescent girls—particularly in the mid-
in the same task (Field et al., 1992). dle adolescent years—list more friends than boys do,
Perhaps because of this, adolescents show greater and girls are more likely to mention intimacy as a defin-
levels of empathy and social understanding in situa- ing aspect of close friendship. In interviews, adolescent
tions in which they are helping or comforting others. girls express greater interest in their close friendships,
Compared with children, adolescents are more likely to talk more frequently about their intimate conversations
understand and acknowledge how their friends feel with friends, express greater concern about their
when those friends are having problems. Indeed, over friends’ faithfulness and greater anxiety over rejection,
the course of adolescence, individuals’ attempts to help and place greater emphasis on emotional closeness in

70%
Trust
Reciprocity
60%

50%
Proportion of decisions

40%

30%

20%

10%

0%
9 years 12 years 16 years 22 years
Age
FIGURE 10.4 Experiments in which individuals play computer games with an anonymous partner find increases with age in
both trust and reciprocity. (van den Bos, Westenberg, van Dijk, & Crone, 2010)
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 327 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 327

their evaluation of romantic partners (Feiring, 1999;


J. Parker, Low, Walker, & Gamm, 2005). Girls are more
likely than boys to make distinctions in the way they
treat intimate and nonintimate friends and to fight
about relationships; girls prefer to keep their friendships
more exclusive and are less willing to include other
classmates in their cliques’ activities (Bukowski, Sippola,
Gauze, Hoza, & Newcomb, 1993; Raffaelli, 1997). In
conversations, girls are more collaborative, whereas boys
are more controlling (Strough & Berg, 2000).
When self-disclosure is used as the measure of inti-
macy, boys’ friendships with other boys do not ap-
proach girls’ friendships with other girls until late
in adolescence, if at all (McNelles & Connolly, 1999;
Radmacher & Azmitia, 2006; Shulman et al., 1997).
And girls appear to be more sensitive and empathic
than boys, especially when comforting friends who are
distressed and in knowing when their friends are de-
pressed (Swenson & Rose, 2003). In fact, one reason
that girls are more likely than boys to confide in friends
is that girls expect that self-disclosure will make them
feel better, whereas boys expect it to be a waste of time
that will make them feel “weird” (Rose, Schwartz-
Mette, Smith, Asher, Swenson, Carlson, & Waller, 2012).
In these very numerous—and very important—
respects, the expression of intimacy is more advanced
among adolescent girls than among boys (Buhrmester,
1996).
Although this carries many advantages for girls, it By virtually any measure, girls display more intimacy in their
also carries some liabilities. Girls’ mental health is more friendships than do boys.
positively affected than boys’ when things are going
well with their friends, but girls suffer more when
things are going poorly (Flook, 2011). Girls also are Adolescents also have to be careful not to engage in too
more likely than boys to spend excessive time discuss- much “negative feedback seeking” (asking other people
ing each other’s problems—a phenomenon that has to verify their flaws, as in, “My voice is so annoying,
been called co-rumination (A. J. Rose, 2002). Co- right?”); too much of this leads to rejection by others,
rumination, often done in the context of discussing which then only makes people feel even worse (Borelli
problems with romantic relationships, turns out to be & Prinstein, 2006).
a double-edged sword, at least for girls—it brings There are also interesting sex differences in the na-
friends closer, but it also contributes to girls’ depres- ture of conflicts between close friends during adoles-
sion and anxiety (Starr & Davila, 2009). In fact, co- cence. Boys’ conflicts are briefer, typically over issues
rumination makes anxiety and depression “contagious,” of power and control (such as whose turn it is in a
transmitting symptoms between the pair of individu- game); more likely to escalate into physical aggres-
als, either because listening to someone’s problems is sion; and usually resolved without any explicit effort
itself distressing (Smith & Rose, 2011) or through to do so, often by just “letting things slide.” Girls’ con-
“emotional mimicry,” where one person unconsciously flicts, in contrast, are longer, typically about some
takes on the feelings of another (Schwartz-Mette & form of betrayal in the relationship (such as breaking
Rose, 2012). Among boys, co-rumination also im- a confidence or ignoring the other person), and only
proves friendships, but does not increase depression or resolved when one of the friends apologizes (Noakes
anxiety as much or as consistently as it does among & Rinaldi, 2006; Raffaelli, 1997). When friendships
girls (A. J. Rose, Carlson, & Waller, 2007), in part be- end, girls are more adversely affected by the loss of the
cause girls are more likely than boys to get upset when relationship (Bakker, Ormel, Verhulst, & Oldehinkel,
they hear that their friends are having problems (Smith 2010).
& Rose, 2011). This doesn’t mean that adolescents
should avoid talking to friends about their feelings and
problems entirely—they just need to keep it in check.
co-rumination Excessive talking with another about problems.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 328 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

328 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

❚ And How They Aren’t On some measures of result of different patterns of socialization, with fe-
friendship, however, adolescent boys and girls show males being more strongly encouraged to develop and
similar degrees of intimacy. Although girls are more express intimacy—especially verbal intimacy—than
likely to mention self-disclosure when asked to define males. Other factors could be at work, however. Social
close friendship and report more self-disclosure in their pressures on males and females during adolescence are
friendships, boys and girls have equivalent degrees of quite different and may lead to differences in expressions
intimate knowledge about their best friends (McNelles of intimacy in certain types of relationships. As we noted
& Connolly, 1999; Shulman et al., 1997). When boys are in Chapter 8, boys are punished much more for acting in
with their friends, they are just as likely as girls to share feminine ways than girls are for acting in masculine
each other’s emotional state (McNelles & Connolly, ways. This is especially so within ethnic groups that
1999). And although girls are generally more consider- stress the importance of machismo (a strong and some-
ate, sex differences in helpfulness are very small times exaggerated sense of masculinity), as is often the
(N. Eisenberg et al., 2009). case among Mexican Americans (Stanton-Salazar &
It seems safe to say that, at the very least, intimacy is Spina, 2005). One reason that adolescent males may not
a more conscious concern for adolescent girls than for be as intimate in their friendships as adolescent females
boys. But this does not mean that intimacy is absent may be that boys are nervous that expressions of inti-
from boys’ relationships. Rather, they express intimacy macy will be taken as a sign of their lack of masculinity.
in different ways. Boys’ friendships are more oriented
toward shared activities than toward the explicit satis-
faction of emotional needs, as is often the case in girls’ MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
friendships. The development of intimacy between ado- Some sex differences in friendship quality predate
lescent males may be a more subtle phenomenon, re- adolescence—girls are more verbal than boys at an
flected more in shared activities than in self-disclosure, early age, for example—but others, such as differences
even in young adulthood (McNelles & Connolly, 1999; in conflict resolution or in feelings of
Radmacher & Azmitia, 2006) and even in online com- jealousy, do not seem to emerge until
munication (Valkenburg & Peter, 2007). Another possi- this developmental period. What
bility is that the development of close friendships among aspects of the transition into adoles-
males starts at a later age than it does among females. cence differ for girls and boys that
One study found, for instance, that there are substantial might account for sex differences in
sex differences in friendship quality at age 13, but that by intimacy?
age 18 these are gone (see Figure 10.5) (Way & Greene,
2006). Once again we see a familiar pattern: during early
and middle adolescence, girls are more emotionally and Although much research has concluded that girls ex-
socially mature than boys, but by late adolescence, boys perience more intimacy in their relationships than boys
have caught up. (I suspect that many women are shaking do (Buhrmester, 1996; Maccoby, 1990), it is important
their heads at this, but that’s what the research says.) to bear in mind that these studies have been based
mainly on samples of White youngsters. Several studies
❚ Origins of Sex Differences Many theorists of non-White youth suggest that there may not be simi-
have suggested that sex differences in intimacy are the lar patterns of sex differences in intimacy in some eth-
nic groups (DuBois & Hirsch, 1990; D. Jones, Costin, &
Ricard, 1994). Indeed, one study of Black, Asian Ameri-
4.3
can, and Latino adolescents found no sex differences in
4.2
support between friends among African American
4.1 Girls teenagers; slight sex differences among Latino teenag-
friendship quality
Closest same-sex

4.0 ers, with girls reporting more friendship support than


3.9 boys; and large sex differences among Asian American
3.8 teenagers, but with boys reporting more support than
3.7 girls (Way & Chen, 2000). Sanctions against intimate
3.6 Boys disclosure may be especially strong among White males,
3.5 but they may be far less so among their minority
3.4 counterparts.
3.3
13 14 15 16 17 18 19
Age CHANGES IN THE TARGETS OF INTIMACY

FIGURE 10.5 Changes in friendship quality over time. According to Sullivan, adolescence is a time of notewor-
(Way & Greene, 2006) thy changes in the “targets” of intimate behavior. During
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 329 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 329

preadolescence and early adolescence, intimacy with sources of emotional support, by no means do parents
peers is hypothesized to replace intimacy with parents, become unimportant (De Goede, Branje, Delsing, &
and during late adolescence, intimacy with peers of the Meeus, 2009). When adolescents are asked to list the
other sex is thought to take the place of intimacy with important people in their lives—people they care
same-sex friends. Actually, this view appears to be only about, go to for advice, or do things with—the number
somewhat accurate. As we’ll see, new targets of intimacy of peers listed increases over the course of adolescence.
do not replace old ones. Rather, new targets are added to At the same time, however, there are no changes over
old ones. adolescence in the percentage of individuals listing
their mother or father. More importantly, studies indi-
❚ Parents and Peers as Targets of Intimacy Two cate that adolescents who spend a good deal of time
conclusions emerge repeatedly in studies of adoles- with their parents also spend a good deal of time with
cents’ intimacy with parents and peers, at least in the their friends. Thus, rather than drawing distinctions
United States. First, from early adolescence on, teenag- between parent-oriented and peer-oriented adoles-
ers describe their relationships with their best friends cents, it makes more sense to distinguish between ado-
and romantic partners as more intimate than those lescents who have a lot of social contact and enjoy a
with their mother or father (Beaumont, 1996; K. Rice & great deal of support from others (both family and
Mulkeen, 1995). Second, although there may be a slight friends) and those who are socially isolated or lonely
drop in intimacy between adolescents and parents (Fallon & Bowles, 1997; Scholte, van Lieshout, & van
sometime during adolescence, the decline reverses as Aken, 2001).
young people move toward young adulthood. Indeed, one of the most consistent findings to
Consider, for example, the results of one study of age emerge from studies of adolescents’ peer and family
differences in intimate self-disclosure with parents, relationships is that the qualities of these relationships
friends, and romantic partners over the course of child- are closely linked. In other words, we can see features of
hood and young adulthood (Buhrmester, 1996). As adolescents’ relationships with their parents and their
Figure 10.6 indicates, intimacy between individuals and parents’ marital relationship—how close they are, how
their parents declines between the 5th and 10th grades, much they tolerate independence, how they deal with
but increases between 10th grade and young adult- conflict, how much control they assert over their chil-
hood. Intimacy with friends increases steadily through- dren’s friends, and so forth—in their relationships with
out adolescence, although most dramatically during their friends and romantic partners (Connolly &
the early adolescent years. Intimacy with romantic McIsaac, 2009; Taradash, Connolly, Pepler, Craig, &
partners also increases steadily throughout adolescence, Costa, 2001; Updegraff et al., 2004; Way & Chen, 2000).
but in this case, the most dramatic increase takes place On a theoretical level, this provides support for both
during the late high school years. social learning and attachment-based views of adoles-
In other words, while peers become relatively more cent intimacy, in that it suggests that the lessons young
important during adolescence as confidants and people learn in close relationships at home provide a
template for the close relationships they form with
others—in both good and bad ways. One study of Black
5 adolescents found that teenagers whose relationships
with parents were emotionally close but not very indi-
4
viduated tended to stay longer in romantic relation-
Friend
ships, even when the relationships were not very good,
Rated intimate disclosure

suggesting that difficulties in establishing healthy au-


tonomy at home may carry over to romantic relation-
Romantic ships (Smetana & Gettmen, 2006). On a practical level,
partner
3 these findings suggest that one approach to improving
the peer relationships of adolescents who are having
difficulties might be to focus on improving the quality
of their relationships at home (Updegraff, Madden-
Parents Derdich, Estrada, Sales, & Leonard, 2002).
2 Studies of adolescents’ preferences for social support
similarly show that the likelihood of turning to a peer
1 during a time of trouble increases during adolescence,
5 7 10 College Young
adult but that the likelihood of turning to a parent remains
Grade constant. In a study of Black, Hispanic American, and
FIGURE 10.6 Age differences in self-disclosure to parents, White youth, the researchers found that between ages 7
friends, and romantic partners. (Buhrmester, 1996) and 14, the amount of support received from the
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 330 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

330 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

immediate family remained fairly constant, while the modest (Fuligni, Hughes, & Way, 2009). Ethnic minor-
amount of support received from friends increased—a ity American adolescents are more likely to say that it is
pattern seen in all three ethnic groups (M. Levitt, important to respect, assist, and support their family
Guacci-Franci, & Levitt, 1993). In other words, even than are White adolescents (Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam,
though adolescents begin to see their friends as increas- 1999), but ethnic differences in adolescents’ beliefs and
ingly important sources of emotional support, they do expectations appear to be more substantial than ethnic
not cease needing or using their parents for the same differences in how adolescents and their parents actu-
purpose. What seems to occur, instead, is that adoles- ally interact. Indeed, with the exception of families
cents develop preferences for social support that vary as who are very recent immigrants to the United States,
a function of the specific issue. relations between American adolescents and their par-
Interestingly, adolescents may feel freer to express ents look surprisingly similar across ethnic groups
anger during arguments with family members than (Fuligni, 1998).
during arguments with friends, presumably because an- There are important differences between adoles-
ger may lead to the end of a friendship but not to the cents’ relationships with mothers versus fathers, how-
end of a family relationship (Laursen, 1993). Perhaps ever. In general, adolescents interact much more often
because of this, adolescents report more angry feelings with, are closer to, and argue more with their mother
after conflicts with their parents than after conflicts than with their father, a pattern seen among males as
with their friends (Adams & Laursen, 2001). And when well as females and across a variety of cultures (Claes,
asked to recall key events in their past that contributed 1998; C. Cooper, 1994; Fuligni, 1998). Of their two
to their sense of identity, college students’ reminiscences parents, adolescents see their mother as being more
of their relationships with their parents more often em- understanding, more accepting, and more willing to
phasize conflict and separation, whereas their recollec- negotiate, and as less judgmental, less guarded, and
tions of their relationships with their friends more often less defensive (see Figure 10.7). The difference between
emphasize closeness (McLean & Thorne, 2003). perceptions of mothers and fathers is especially large
among girls: As a rule, the mother–daughter relation-
❚ Cultural Differences in Patterns of Intimacy ship tends to be the closest, and the father–daughter
Patterns of adolescents’ relationships with parents and relationship the least intimate, with mother–son and
peers vary across cultures, however, and it may be mis- father–son relationships falling in between (Monck,
leading to generalize results of studies of U.S. or Cana- 1991; Noller & Callan, 1990; K. Rice & Mulkeen, 1995).
dian teenagers to other countries. A comparison of All in all, then, an important transition in intimate
American and Indonesian youth found, for instance, relationships appears to take place sometime between
that Indonesian adolescents ranked parents higher the 5th and 8th grades. During this period, peers be-
in social support and ranked friends lower than was come the most important source of companionship
the case for the American adolescents (D. French, and intimate self-disclosure—surpassing parents and
Rianasari, Pidada, Nelwan, & Buhrmester, 2001). other family members, such as siblings (Buhrmester &
Similarly, a comparison of adolescents from Canada, Furman, 1987; Larson & Richards, 1991). Peers may be-
Belgium, and Italy found that Italian adolescents were come increasingly important as targets of intimacy not
closer to their family, whereas Canadian adolescents simply because they are similar in age but also because
were closer to their friends (Claes, 1998). It may be they do not share the same family with the adolescent.
wrong to automatically assume that American young- As adolescents begin the process of individuation, they
sters are more peer-oriented than their counterparts may need to seek intimacy outside the family as a
in all other cultures, however. In fact, one study found means of establishing an identity beyond their family
that Japanese adolescents (and Japanese boys, in par- role. Although this shift in intimacy is normative, a
ticular) were less likely than American adolescents to shift in primary attachment figures at this age is not:
list their parents—and more likely to list their peers— Adolescents who report that their strongest attachment
as significant others (Darling, Hamilton, & Matsuda, is to a friend or romantic partner are more likely to
1990). Another study, of Dutch youth, found that par- have insecure attachments with their parents (Freeman
ent–adolescent relationships were least positive during & Brown, 2001).
late adolescence, not early adolescence, as has been
found in U.S. samples (van Wel, 1994). ❚ The Different Roles of Parents and Peers
Whereas there are substantial differences among Adolescents also have very different sorts of intimate
ethnic groups within the United States with respect to relationships with parents and peers, and these differ-
patterns of parental discipline (discussed in Chapter 4) ences point to different ways in which mothers, fathers,
and ethnic socialization (discussed in Chapter 8), and friends may contribute to their social development.
differences between ethnic groups in the expression of Even in close families, parent–adolescent relations are
intimacy between adolescents and parents are fairly characterized by an imbalance of power, with parents as
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 331 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 331

Conflict with mother Cohesion with mother


Number of angry discussions 8 3.8
7

Degree of cohesion
6 3.4
in past 2 weeks

5
4 3.0
3
2 2.6
1
0 2.2
Mexican Chinese Filipino European Mexican Chinese Filipino European

6th grade 8th grade 10th grade

Conflict with father Cohesion with father


8 3.8
Number of angry discussions

Degree of cohesion
6 3.4
in past 2 weeks

5
4 3.0
3
2 2.6
1
0 2.2
Mexican Chinese Filipino European Mexican Chinese Filipino European
FIGURE 10.7 Adolescents’ reports of conflict and cohesion with mothers and fathers in four ethnic groups. (Fuligni, 1998)

nurturers, advice givers, and explainers whom adoles- greater when an adolescent also has supportive parents
cents turn to for their experience and expertise. Ado- (Helsen, Vollebergh, & Meeus, 2000).
lescents’ interactions with their friends, in contrast, are The different functions of intimacy with parents
more mutual, more balanced, and more likely to pro- and peers are nicely illustrated in a study of social
vide them with opportunities to express alternative support during a transition into a new school (S. Dunn,
views and engage in an equal exchange of feelings and Putallaz, Sheppard, & Lindstrom, 1987). As you will
beliefs. Conflicts between adolescents and their parents recall from Chapter 6, changing schools during adoles-
are relatively more likely to end with a “winner” and a cence can sometimes be stressful, and social support—
“loser,” whereas conflicts between adolescents and emotional or instrumental assistance from others—can
their friends are relatively more likely to end in com- help buffer adolescents against the potential negative
promise or, at least, equal outcomes (Adams & Laursen, effects of stress (Hauser & Bowlds, 1990). The study
2001). found that support from family members was more
Rather than viewing one type of relationship as predictive of adaptation to the demands of the new
more or less intimate than the other, it is more accurate school, as indexed by grades and attendance, but that
to say that both types of intimacy are important, for support from peers was more predictive of psychologi-
each influences a different aspect of the adolescent’s de- cal well-being, as indexed by low levels of depression
veloping character in important ways. Intimacy with and anxiety. The absence of peer support was espe-
parents provides opportunities to learn from those cially critical for boys, perhaps because girls are more
older and wiser; intimacy with friends provides oppor- likely than boys to seek out other sources of support
tunities to share experiences with individuals who have when their peers do not provide it.
a similar perspective and degree of expertise. Consis- A lack of support from parents or from friends in
tent with this, studies find that adolescents who have school is associated with low self-worth and poorer
strong attachments to both parents and peers are better social adjustment in early adolescence. Social support
adjusted than those who have strong attachments in
one type of relationship but not in the other (Laible,
Carlo, & Raffaelli, 2000). In addition, the positive im- social support The extent to which an individual receives
pact of having supportive friends in adolescence is emotional or instrumental assistance from his or her social
network.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 332 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

332 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

from one source (such as the family) can be especially mothers) remain important confidants, and both
important when other sources of support (such as mothers and fathers continue to be significant influ-
friends) are lacking (Ohannessian, Lerner, Lerner, & ences on the young person’s behavior and decisions.
von Eye, 1994). A supportive family is more important Indeed, even in adolescence, being close to one’s par-
for the healthy adjustment of adolescents who do not ents has a more positive impact on psychological health
have a close friend, whereas support from friends is than being close to one’s friends (Greenberg, Siegel, &
more crucial among adolescents whose family relation- Leitch, 1983). That said, peers take on an increasingly
ships are strained (Gauze, Bukowski, Aquan-Assee, & important role in the individual’s social life over the
Sippola, 1996). Having support from parents, siblings, course of adolescence (B. Brown & Larson, 2009). Al-
or nonschool friends does not fully compensate for a though peers do not replace parents, they clearly con-
lack of support from classmates, though, and having tribute to the adolescent’s social development in a
support from siblings, classmates, or others does not unique and influential way.
fully compensate for a lack of support from parents
(East & Rook, 1992; Gore & Aseltine, 1995). In other ❚ Other Individuals as Targets of Intimacy
words, optimal social development during adolescence Intimacy in sibling relationships is a complicated mat-
may require healthy relationships with both parents and ter and often includes a mix of affection and rivalry
peers (Barber & Olsen, 1997; Eccles, Early, Frasier, (East, 2009). Generally, adolescents say they are less in-
Belansky, & McCarthy, 1997; Vaughan, Foshee, & timate with siblings than with parents or friends
Ennett, 2010; J. F. Young, Berenson, Cohen, & Garcia, (Buhrmester & Furman, 1987). Adolescents fight more
2005). Family relationships and peer relationships in- with brothers and sisters than they do with close friends,
fluence, rather than compete with, each other (Fallon & and their arguments with siblings tend to be resolved
Bowles, 1997; Gavin & Furman, 1996). less by giving in or by letting things slide than through
A study of the differential uses of parents and peers the intervention of parents (Raffaelli, 1997). Over the
as sources of support indicates that whom adolescents course of adolescence, conflict between siblings de-
turn to is likely to be highly dependent upon the specific creases, but this may be due to the fact that siblings
issue at hand, which explains, in part, why adolescents spend less time together in adolescence than they did in
may need support from both parents and peers (Boldero childhood as they become involved in romantic rela-
& Fallon, 1995; Morrison et al., 1997). This particular tionships and extracurricular activities; a better way to
study asked adolescents whom they would turn to if think about it is that sibling relationships become less
they had a substance abuse problem; the respondents intense. Nevertheless, although overt conflict between
could name as many different people as desired (Windle, siblings declines during adolescence, so do warmth and
Miller-Tutzauer, & Barnes, 1991). Among the early ado- closeness; early adolescence appears to be the low point
lescents surveyed, only 43% listed their parents among in sibling relationships, but even college students report
people they would turn to for help, whereas 60% listed ambivalent feelings about their siblings (Stocker,
friends; among the middle adolescents, the proportions Lanthier, & Furman, 1997).
were 39% and 70%, respectively. There also were inter- Comparatively little is known about intimacy with
esting ethnic differences in adolescents’ responses, with members of adolescents’ extended family or with non-
Black and Hispanic American youngsters twice as likely familial adults like teachers or coaches. Actual contact
as White adolescents to report being socially isolated with extended family is infrequent for many adoles-
(that is, having no one to turn to). An absence of social cents, however, because those family members often
support may be especially problematic for ethnic mi- live outside the adolescent’s immediate area (Feiring &
nority youth, who often rely on peers to provide emo- Lewis, 1991). There appears to be a slight increase in
tional support in the face of stress and other difficulties intimacy with extended family members during child-
inherent in living in high-risk environments (Benner, hood but an especially steep drop-off in intimacy with
2011; Stanton-Salazar & Spina, 2005). grandparents and other extended family members
In general, then, the results of research on changes in between childhood and adolescence (Buhrmester &
intimacy with parents and peers as an individual ages Furman, 1987; Creasey & Kaliher, 1994; M. Levitt et al.,
are similar to the findings discussed in Chapter 9 re- 1993). Nevertheless, adolescents benefit from having
garding autonomy: Although the importance of peer grandparents involved in their life (Yorgason, Padilla-
relationships undoubtedly increases during adoles- Walker, & Jackson, 2011).
cence, the significance of family relationships does not Although a decline in intimacy with grandparents is
decline so much as it narrows in focus. Parents do not often observed during adolescence, this may not be as
cease to be important sources of influence or, as we see common among adolescents who are living with a sin-
here, targets of intimacy. Throughout adolescence, par- gle, divorced mother (Hirsch, Mickus, & Boerger, 2002).
ents and adolescents remain close, parents (especially Divorce may be associated with increased contact
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 333 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 333

between adolescents and their grandparents, especially


between the adolescent and his or her maternal grand-
father. Ties to grandmothers are especially strong
among Black adolescents, particularly among girls from
divorced households (Hirsch et al., 2002). Interestingly,
puberty seems to increase intimacy between adolescent
boys from divorced homes and their grandfathers (per-
haps to compensate for diminished contact with their
father), whereas it seems to distance adolescent girls
from their grandfathers (perhaps because of discom-
fort with the girl’s sexuality).
Researchers also have asked whether relationships
between adolescents and nonfamilial adults in schools,
workplaces, or neighborhoods can play a significant
role in teenagers’ lives (Greenberger, Chen, & Beam,
1998; Munsch, Liang, & DeSecottier, 1996). Indeed,
studies suggest that the development of relationships
with nonfamilial adults is a normative part of adoles-
As cross-sex relationships begin to develop, adolescents may mask
cence, not a sign of difficulties at home (Beam, Chen, &
their anxieties by teasing and joking around with members of the
Greenberger, 2002; Rhodes & Lowe, 2009), and that re- other sex.
lationships with positive role models outside the family
contribute to healthy adolescent development above
and beyond the contribution of family relationships ❚ Origins of the “Sex Cleavage” The schism be-
and well into late adolescence (Chang, Greenberger, tween boys and girls during early adolescence results
Chen, Heckhausen, & Farruggia, 2010; Haddad, Chen, from various factors. First, despite whatever changes may
& Greenberger, 2011). Close friendships may develop have taken place in American society with regard to sex-
naturally between adolescents and their teachers or role socialization during the past 50 years, it is still the
work supervisors or can be cultivated through commu- case that preadolescent and early adolescent boys and
nity organizations, such as Big Brothers/Big Sisters, or girls have different interests, engage in different sorts of
similar programs designed to pair young people— peer activities, and perceive themselves to be different
especially those under stress—with supportive and car- from each other (Galambos, Berenbaum, & McHale,
ing adults. Indeed, linking an adolescent with a mentor 2009). The sex cleavage in adolescent friendships results
is one of the most important components of successful more from adolescents’ preferring members of the same
youth programs (Theokas & Lerner, 2006). The benefits sex—and the activities they engage in—than from their
of having a Big Brother or Big Sister may be especially actually disliking members of the other sex, although
significant among adolescents with more difficulties at boys express more positive feelings about their female
home, such as those living in foster care (Rhodes, classmates than vice versa (Bukowski, Gauze, Hoza, &
Haight, & Briggs, 1999). Not all close relationships with Newcomb, 1993; Bukowski, Sippola, & Hoza, 1999).
nonparental adults benefit adolescents’ development, The transitional period—between same-sex non-
however: Adolescent boys who have close friendships sexual relationships and other-sex sexual ones—can be
with young adult men are more likely to engage in anti- a trying time for adolescents. This period usually coin-
social behavior when they perceive their older friends cides with the peer group’s shift from same-sex cliques
as likely to condone or commit antisocial acts them- to mixed-sex crowds, which we examined in Chapter 5.
selves (Greenberger et al., 1998). The interpersonal strains and anxieties inherent in the
transition show up in the high levels of teasing, joking
around, and overt discomfort that young adolescents so
FRIENDSHIPS WITH THE OTHER SEX
often display in situations that are a little too close to
Not until late adolescence do intimate friendships being romantic or sexual. One reason for the mutual
with other-sex peers begin to be important. Studies of physical playfulness that boys and girls engage in is that
preadolescents and young teenagers point to very it is ambiguous enough to be denied as motivated by
strong sex segregation in adolescents’ friendships, genuine sexual interest. Whereas rough play—play
with boys rarely reporting friendships with girls, and fighting—between boys is typically done to show who
girls rarely reporting friendships with boys, at least is dominant, the same behavior between boys and girls
until middle adolescence (Galambos, Berenbaum, & is often semisexual in nature—what some have labeled
McHale, 2009). “poke and push courtship” (Pellegrini, 2003).
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 334 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

334 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

These observations support Sullivan’s claim that inti- in early adolescence is not explicitly in the context of
macy between adolescent boys and girls is relatively slow romance, it likely sets the stage for later romantic expe-
to develop and generally is tinged with an air of sexual- riences (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). The pattern of age
ity. Contrary to his notion that cross-sex intimacy comes differences in other-sex friendships, for example, is
to replace intimacy with peers of the same sex, however, consistent with what we know about age differences be-
researchers have found that intimate friendships be- tween dating partners in early and middle adolescence,
tween adolescents of the same sex are not displaced by with boys generally older than their girlfriends, rather
the emergence of intimacy between adolescent males than the reverse (Montgomery & Sorell, 1998). In addi-
and females (Connolly & Johnson, 1993). Although re- tion, adolescents who have more other-sex friends than
searchers find that the likelihood of other-sex peers ap- their peers early in adolescence tend to enter into ro-
pearing on adolescents’ lists of people who are important mantic relationships at an earlier age and tend to have
to them increases during early and middle adolescence, longer romantic relationships as well (Feiring, 1999).
and although the amount of time adolescents spend This could be due to many factors, including the ado-
with other-sex peers increases as well, the number of lescent’s use of the pool of other-sex friends to “re-
same-sex peers listed also increases or remains constant, hearse” for later romantic relationships or to develop a
and time spent with same-sex peers does not decline social network that is used to meet potential dates later
(Feiring & Lewis, 1991; Richards, Crowe, Larson, & on (Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000; Connolly &
Swarr, 1998; Zimmer-Gembeck, 1999). However, there Goldberg, 1999; Darling, Dowdy, Van Horn, & Caldwell,
are substantial individual differences in patterns of time 1999). In any case, even preadolescents as young as 9
allocation to same- and other-sex relationships; some differentiate between cross-sex relationships that are
adolescents shift their energy from same-sex friends to platonic and those that are romantic (Connolly, Craig,
other-sex relationships early and abruptly, others do so Goldberg, & Pepler, 1999).
gradually over the course of high school, and still others Not all relationships between males and females
do not shift their focus at all (Zimmer-Gembeck, 1999). in adolescence are romantic, of course, and having
Although intimacy between the sexes increases dur- close, other-sex friendships is a common experience
ing early adolescence (Buhrmester & Furman, 1987), (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009; Kuttler, La Greca, &
many adolescents do not list a single other-sex peer as a Prinstein, 1999; Stanton-Salazar & Spina, 2005). Two
significant person in their lives. In middle school, only very different types of adolescents appear to have close
8% of adolescents’ friendships are with members of the other-sex friends—adolescents who are socially com-
other sex; by high school, this figure has risen only to petent and highly popular with same-sex peers, and
13% (Değirmencioğlu & Urberg, 1994). One exception adolescents who are socially incompetent and highly
to this general trend is seen among gay male adoles- unpopular with same-sex peers (Bukowski, Sippola, &
cents, who tend to have more female than male friends Hoza, 1999; Değirmencioğlu & Urberg, 1994; Feiring
(Diamond & Dubé, 2002). & Lewis, 1991). Among boys, having an other-sex
When females do include other-sex peers on their friend compensates for not having same-sex friends,
list of important people, the boys they mention are of- leading to more positive mental health than is seen
ten older and often from another school; when boys list among boys without any friends at all. Among girls,
girls as important friends, they generally are of the however, the results are mixed. Although some studies
same age or younger (Poulin & Pedersen, 2007). Con- have found that “there is no advantage, or perhaps
sistent with this, the increase in time spent with other- there is even a disadvantage, to having a friendship
sex peers that occurs in adolescence takes place much with a boy” (Bukowski et al., 1999, p. 457), others have
earlier among girls than boys—by the time they are in found that, among less sexually advanced girls, having
11th grade, girls are spending 10 hours each week alone platonic friendships with boys is associated with a
with a boy, compared to only 5 hours per week spent by more positive body image—perhaps because these
boys alone with a girl. Young adolescents of both sexes friendships permit girls to feel that boys like them
spend a lot of time thinking about the other sex, but for themselves, without the added cost of feeling
relatively little time with them. Interestingly, as adoles- pressured to have sex (Compian, Gowen, & Hayward,
cents get older, the time they spend thinking about the 2004). Having male friends, however, increases girls’
other sex tends to be increasingly associated with nega- likelihood of being involved in antisocial behavior
tive mood states, perhaps because the fantasies about (Arndorfer & Stormshak, 2008; Mrug, Borch, &
the other sex experienced in early adolescence come to Cillessen, 2011; Poulin, Denalt, & Pedersen, 2011), es-
be replaced by unrequited longings for romantic com- pecially if their male friends are antisocial (Cauffman,
panionship (Richards et al., 1998). Farruggia, & Goldweber, 2008), which makes sense,
given that boys are more likely to be antisocial and
❚ Some Functions of Other-Sex Friendships that adolescents’ behavior often mimics that of their
Although the emergence of close other-sex friendships friends.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 335 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 335

cross-sex friendships are less evident for females than


MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
for males. Many such friendships draw females into tra-
Anecdotal evidence suggests that nonsexual friend- ditional caregiving roles, reinforcing traditional sex-
ships between males and females have become more role stereotypes.
common in recent decades. Did you have
more platonic, other-sex friends when you
were a teenager than your parents did when
they were adolescents? RECAP
• With development, adolescents place more
emphasis on trust and loyalty as defining features
of friendship, become more self-disclosing in their
In general, though, the overall picture suggests that
relationships, and become more responsive and
boys have more to gain from friendships with girls than
sensitive to their friends’ needs.
vice versa. Having an intimate relationship with an
other-sex peer is more strongly related to boys’ general
• Sex differences in the expression of intimacy in
adolescents’ friendships are striking, with girls’ rela-
level of interpersonal intimacy than it is to girls’
tionships being more intimate than boys’ across
(Buhrmester & Furman, 1987); and whereas boys re-
many different indicators. Boys’ friendships do not
port that their friendships with girls are more reward-
lack intimacy, but intimacy is a much more con-
ing than their friendships with other boys, girls do not
scious concern for girls than it is for boys, in part
describe their friendships with boys as more rewarding
because of sex differences in the way they are
than their friendships with other girls (J. Thomas &
socialized.
Daubman, 2001). These findings are not surprising,
given that adolescents’ friendships with girls (regardless
• New types of relationships are added to the
adolescent’s social world without replacing previ-
of whether they themselves are male or female) tend to
ous ones. Beginning in preadolescence, the network
be more intimate and supportive than their friendships
of intimate relationships widens to include peers
with boys (Kuttler et al., 1999).
as well as family members and, sometimes,
A study of peer networks among low-income Mexi-
nonfamilial adults.
can American adolescents in San Diego illustrates how
important platonic friendships with girls can be for
• Many adolescents have platonic friendships with
other-sex peers. Other-sex friendships help to set
adolescent boys in the development of more mature
the stage for the emergence of romantic relation-
relationships. Many boys likened their nonsexual
ships later on.
friendships with girls to relationships with sisters, em-
phasizing the ease with which they could explicitly ask
• Boys may profit psychologically from
other-sex friendships more
for emotional support, something that many adoles-
than girls.
cent males find difficult to request from their same-sex
friends:

Le tengo un cariño así con ella como si fuera también mi


hermana [I have such love for her, as if she were my
sister]. . . . We always support each other, it doesn’t matter
what it is; if she asks me, I help her, and if I ask her, she
helps me, with anything. (Stanton-Salazar & Spina, 2005,
pp. 397–398)
Dating and Romantic
Some boys also discussed the important role their
Relationships
female friends played in helping them manage their Dating plays a very different role in adolescents’ lives
tempers: today than it did in previous times (Connolly &
McIsaac, 2009). In earlier eras, dating during adoles-
You know, I’m the kind of guy that can go around and cence was not so much a recreational activity (as it is
like, you know, get mad and you know . . . siento como que today) as a part of the process of courtship and mate
quiero cingar a alguien [I feel like I wanna fuck somebody selection. Individuals would date in order to ready
up (i.e., to hit and seriously injure someone)]. She’s the
themselves for marriage, and unmarried individuals
person that when she looks at me, you know [giggles], all
would play the field—under the watchful eyes of
those mad feelings go to, like, Oh God!, you know. Like
she can control me, like she’s holding me back, just by chaperones—for a relatively long period before settling
looking at me [laughs]. (pp. 399–400) down (Montgomery, 1996). At the turn of the 20th cen-
tury, most individuals did not marry until their mid-20s
As in studies of non-Latino youth, this study of Mex- (U.S. Census Bureau, 2009b). The first half of the 20th
ican American adolescents indicates that the benefits of century saw a gradual decline in the average age of
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 336 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

336 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

29.0

28.5
28.0

27.5

27.0

26.5

26.0
Median age at first marriage

25.5

25.0
Males
24.5

24.0

23.5

23.0

22.5

22.0
Females
21.5

21.0

20.5

20.0
1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011
FIGURE 10.8 The median age at marriage in the United States declined from 1900 through the mid-1950s but rose markedly
during the second half of the 20th century. (U.S. Census Bureau, 2011)

marriage, however, and as a result, individuals began cohabitation (living together) as a substitute for mar-
dating more seriously at an earlier age. By the mid- riage (Manning, Longmore, & Giordano, 2007).
1950s, the average age at first marriage in the United Only recently have social scientists begun writing sys-
States had fallen to 20 among women and 22 among tematically about adolescents’ romantic relationships
men—which means that substantial numbers of indi- (W. A. Collins, 2003; Connolly & McIsaac, 2009; Furman
viduals were courting during high school and marrying et al., 1999; Mounts & Kim, 2009). However, studies of
during their late adolescent years. adolescent romance indicate that romantic relationships
The function of adolescent dating changed, how- are very common: One-fourth of American 12-year-
ever, as individuals began to marry later and later—a olds, one-half of 15-year-olds, and more than two-thirds
trend that began in the mid-1950s and continues today of 18-year-olds report having had a romantic relation-
(see Figure 10.8). Now, the average age at which people ship in the past 18 months. The average American ado-
marry is considerably later than it was 50 years ago— lescent begins dating around age 13 or 14, although
about age 27 for women and 29 for men (U.S. Census nearly half of all adolescents have at least one date before
Bureau, 2011). In one recent study of a sample of Seat- they turn 12. By age 16, more than 90% of adolescents of
tle individuals, most of whom grew up in low-income both sexes have had at least one date, and during the
neighborhoods, only half of the women and about 40% later years of high school, more than half of all students
of the men were married at age 30 (see Figure 10.9) average one or more dates weekly. Only 15% of high
(Oesterle, Hawkins, Hill, & Bailey, 2010). This, of school students date less than once a month (Feiring,
course, gives high school dating a whole new meaning, 1993). By age 18, virtually all adolescents have dated
because today it is clearly divorced from its function in once, and three-fourths have had at least one steady rela-
mate selection. Adults continue to regulate and moni- tionship (Neemann, Hubbard, & Masten, 1995).
tor adolescent dating in order to prevent rash or impul- Girls tend to become involved with boys who are
sive commitments to early marriage (Laursen & slightly older, whereas boys tend to become involved
Jensen-Campbell, 1999), but in the minds of most with girls who are the same age or younger. Because the
young people, high school dating has little to do with average duration of romantic relationships during the
courtship or marriage. Nor do today’s adolescents see middle high school years is about 6 months, most
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 337 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 337

Women Men
100% 100%

80% 80%

60% 60%

40% 40%

20% 20%

0% 0%
18 21 24 27 30 18 21 24 27 30
Age Age

Attending school Married Attending school Married


Employed full-time Living with children Employed full-time Living with children
FIGURE 10.9 In one recent study of a sample of Seattle individuals, most of whom grew up in low-income neighborhoods,
only half of the women and about 40% of the men were married at age 30. (Oesterle et al., 2010)

adolescents report having experienced a breakup dur- chief developmental task of middle and late adoles-
ing the last year. Perhaps as a way of protecting them- cence. The capacity for intimacy, which initially devel-
selves from more pain than is necessary, most teenagers ops out of same-sex friendships, eventually is brought
say that they were in control of the breakup (either into romantic relationships, which for the vast majority
alone or by mutual agreement) (Connolly & McIsaac, of adolescents are with members of the other sex. In
2009). Nevertheless, as you will read later in this chap- some senses, then, Sullivan viewed relationships be-
ter, the breakup of a romantic relationship is a signifi- tween romantic partners as a context in which intimacy
cant source of distress for many adolescents, and, as you is expressed rather than learned. Consistent with this,
read earlier, the ups and downs of romantic life often the quality of adolescents’ friendships is predictive of
dominate conversations between friends. the quality of their subsequent romantic relationships,
whereas the reverse is not true (Connolly et al., 2000).
Romantic relationships may play a somewhat different
DATING AND THE DEVELOPMENT role in the development of intimacy for females than for
OF INTIMACY males (Feiring, 1999). As we noted earlier, in American
Contemporary discussions of adolescent romance draw
on Sullivan’s theory of interpersonal development, at-
tachment theory, and ecological perspectives on devel-
opment (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). From Sullivan
comes the idea that there is a developmental progres-
sion in individuals’ capacity for intimacy, with the
emergence of romantic relationships occurring after
individuals have experienced emotional closeness
within same-sex friendships. From attachment theory
comes the idea that individuals differ in the quality of
their romantic relationships and that these differences
are paralleled by differences in the relationships indi-
viduals have with parents and peers. And from the eco-
logical perspective comes the idea that romantic
relationships, like all relationships, need to be viewed
within the social context in which they occur.

❚ The Nature and Significance of Romance As


you may recall, Sullivan believed that establishing inti- One of the fundamental developmental tasks of adolescence is to
mate relationships with peers of the other sex was the begin to develop the capacity for intimate, romantic relationships.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 338 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

338 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

society, boys are not encouraged to develop the capacity [have sex] more than I did. . . . she said that I wasn’t
to be emotionally expressive, particularly in their rela- mature enough and you know all that stuff. . . . I was too
tionships with other males. During middle adolescence, young, I was scared, I didn’t know what I was doing I
as we have seen, girls are better than boys at certain types wasn’t ready for it. I think I felt like I was too young. . . .
she was my girlfriend and that’s what she wanted.
of intimacy—self-disclosure and interpersonal under-
(Giordano et al., 2006, p. 281)
standing, for example. Girls, therefore, are more likely
than boys to be capable of being intimate in these ways There are also important cultural differences in how
upon entering a relationship. Some, but not all, studies of adolescents approach dating: In one study, Hispanic
early sexual relationships confirm this: For adolescent adolescents were more likely to emphasize romantic as-
girls more than boys, early sexual relationships are far pects of the relationship and were more willing to ac-
more likely to involve love, emotional involvement, and cept traditional views of the roles of males and females
intimacy (Montgomery, 2005; Shulman & Scharf, 2000). in relationships, whereas Black adolescents were more
In other words, whereas for girls cross-sex relationships pragmatic and egalitarian in their attitudes (Milbrath,
may provide a context for further expression of intimacy, Ohlson, & Eyre, 2009). Compare, for instance, these
for boys they may provide a context for the further devel- very different perspectives on relationships:
opment of intimacy. This notion is consistent with the . . . girls are more into the fairytale kin’ a love, you know.
finding, discussed earlier, that other-sex relationships An’ the happy endings and . . . ROSES! They want roses
may play a more important role in the development of once-in-a-while, too. I mean . . . a red rose, one red rose,
intimacy among boys than among girls, who, on average, it wouldn’t hurt. And to make it like a surprise. You know,
like to leave it in their locker. . . . an’ the girl opens it up
develop and experience intimacy earlier with same-sex
an’ like, “Oh My God!” (Hispanic, 18-year-old female)
friends than boys do (Buhrmester & Furman, 1987). One
(Milbrath et al., 2009, p. 338)
study found that the way a girl interacts with her boy-
friend is more strongly related to the girl’s internal work- Well, he has . . . to tell her how much he care about her
ing model of relationship than the boy’s, perhaps because an’ how much he doesn’t want to hurt her an’ how he
girls’ greater prior experience with intimacy has led them doesn’t care about them, an’ how he’ll lie to them, an’,
to better align how they behave with how they really feel “I’m telling’ you this an’ I’m tryin’ to let chyou know that
(Furman & Simon, 2006). I’m gonna cheat on you becuz I care about chyou an’ I
Although boys’ capacity for intimacy may lag behind don’t wanna lie to you.” . . . he has to have a lot of game.
girls’, it is important not to confuse ability with aspira- (Black, 16-year-old female) (Milbrath et al., 2009, p. 341)
tion. In the past, much was made of the different mean-
ings of romantic relationships to adolescent males and ❚ The Role of Context The age at which dating be-
females, but today it appears that the sexes are more gins is influenced by the norms and expectations in the
similar than different in how their romantic relation- adolescent’s community. Romantic relationships are
ships develop (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). The stereo- more common at a younger age in other industrialized
type of the emotionally stunted but swaggering boy who countries than in North America, but by late adolescence,
enters into a romantic relationship purely for sex and rates of dating are similar. Within the United States and in
uses his power and influence to get it no longer appears Canada, Asian adolescents are less likely than other ado-
accurate, although adolescent couples are more likely to lescents to date, whereas the prevalence of dating is very
have intercourse when the girl reports that her boyfriend similar among Black, Hispanic, American Indian, and
holds the power in the relationship (Giordano, Manning, White adolescents, although some studies find that His-
& Longmore, 2010). Indeed, research shows that boys panic girls start dating at a later age than either Black or
are often more awkward and less confident than the girls White girls (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). In one study, a
they are dating and just as eager to be emotionally close. high proportion of Asian adolescents hid the fact that they
In one study, in which boys were interviewed about their were dating from their parents, because they did not want
relationships, they frequently responded with answers their parents to worry about their school performance:
that contradict the macho stereotype. Here’s a 17-year- They know I have a guy that I spend a lot of time with,
old talking about his girlfriend: but I don’t think they really know how serious it is,
She kept insisting I wasn’t going to work out and I kept because I really don’t want them to know, because if
insisting I wanted to try it and one night, and like I said something ever happened with me in school, if I failed
I couldn’t sleep, and I wrote her a letter, front and back, a class, I know that they would blame him, even though
crying the whole time and then I handed the letter to her it would probably be my fault because he is really
the next morning. . . . It was really emotional, like how supportive and it’s not like I am always with him, but
she hurt me and how it wasn’t right. (Giordano, they would think that. They would think that he is a bad
Longmore, & Manning, 2006, p. 277) influence on me. (18-year-old Asian female) (Lau,
Or how about this 18-year-old: Markhan, Lin, Flores, & Chacko, 2009, p. 104)
I guess she was more mature than I was and I guess I Although early maturers begin dating somewhat earlier
wasn’t on her level you know because she wanted to do it than late maturers (Lam, Shi, Ho, Stewart, & Fan, 2002;
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 339 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 339

FIGURE 10.10 Participation in


Same-gender activities only various romantic activities in early
adolescence. (Connolly et al., 2004)

Mixed-gender affiliative activities


Hang around with
boys and girls

Go to clubs, groups, or sports


activities with boys and girls

Go to dances or parties
with boys and girls

Dating activities
Go out with groups of
boys and girls at night

Go out with a boy (girl) and


a couple of girls (boys)

Go on dates with a
boy (girl) in a group

Girls and boys go on dates

Have a boyfriend (girlfriend) now

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Percent

Neemann et al., 1995), age norms within the adoles- parental monitoring, a desire on the part of the adoles-
cent’s school and peer group are more important in de- cent to escape a difficult home environment, or both is
termining the age at which dating begins than is the not known (Cavanagh, Crissey, & Raley, 2008).
adolescent’s level of physical development. In other
words, a physically immature 14-year-old who goes to ❚ Patterns of Dating “Dating” can mean a variety
school where it is expected that 14-year-olds will date is of different things, of course, from group activities that
more likely to date than is a physically mature 14-year- bring males and females together (without much actual
old who lives in a community where dating is typically contact between the sexes), to group dates in which a
delayed until age 16. Early maturers whose peers are group of boys and girls go out jointly (and spend part
dating are especially likely to date early (Friedlander, of the time in couples and part of the time in the larger
Connolly, Pepler, & Craig, 2007). Sexual activity, how- group), to casual dating in couples, to serious involve-
ever, as we’ll see in Chapter 11, is more strongly influ- ment with a steady boyfriend or girlfriend (Carlson &
enced by biological development (Udry, Billy, Morris, Rose, 2012). Generally, casual socializing with other-sex
Gruff, & Raj, 1985). Dating also begins earlier among peers and experiences in a mixed-sex social network oc-
adolescents who have older siblings, among adolescents cur before the development of romantic relationships
whose parents do not monitor them well, and among (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). As a consequence, more
those with single mothers, especially if the mother adolescents have experience in mixed-sex group activi-
is sexually active herself (Longmore, Manning, & ties like parties or dances than in dating, and more have
Giordano, 2001; Neemann et al., 1995). Family instabil- experience in dating than in having a serious boyfriend
ity (changes in parents’ marital status through divorce or girlfriend or a sexual relationship (see Figure 10.10)
or remarriage) is associated with dating, especially (Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, & Pepler, 2004; O’Sullivan,
among boys, with adolescents from more unstable fam- Cheng, Harris, & Brooks-Gunn, 2007). However, in-
ilies more likely to date and more likely to have multiple volvement in one-on-one romantic relationships does
romantic partners. Whether this is due to less vigilant not replace same-sex or mixed-sex group activities—like
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 340 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

340 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

other aspects of intimacy in adolescence, new forms of increases, and by college, individuals typically name
relationships are added to the adolescent’s repertoire their romantic partner first on a list of significant others
while old ones are retained. The sequence of transition- (up from fourth in grade 7 and third in grade 10)
ing into romantic relationships follows similar patterns (Buhrmester, 1996; Furman & Wehner, 1994).
across ethnic groups, although Asian American youth The ways in which adolescents interact with roman-
appear to make this transition at a somewhat later stage tic partners also changes with development, with in-
than their peers from other backgrounds, consistent creasing willingness to acknowledge, analyze, and work
with other findings on ethnic differences in beliefs through disagreements. (The major sources of conflict
about the appropriate age at which adolescents should between boyfriends and girlfriends are issues related to
begin dating and engaging in other adultlike activities how the dyad is functioning, such as jealousy, neglect,
(Connolly et al., 2004) (see Chapter 9). betrayal, and trust [McIsaac, Connolly, McKenney,
Even for adolescents with a history of intimate Pepler, & Craig, 2008].) One study of Israeli couples’
friendships with same- and other-sex peers, the transi- conflict resolution found that whereas 70% of the ado-
tion into romantic relationships can be difficult. In one lescent couples either denied or dismissed the conflict as
study, in which adolescents were asked to discuss social insignificant, only 20% of the young adults did. Interest-
situations they thought were difficult, themes having to ingly, the adolescents whose conversations looked more
do with communicating with the other sex were men- like those of the young adults were less likely to break up
tioned frequently. Many adolescents discussed diffi- (Shulman, Tuval-Mashiach, Levran, & Anbar, 2006).
culty in initiating or maintaining conversations, in
person (“He will think I am an idiot,” “Sometimes you ❚ Reasons for Dating Prior to middle or late adoles-
don’t know, if you’re like sitting with a guy and you’re cence, dating may be less important for the development
watching a basketball game or something, you don’t of intimacy than it is for other purposes, including estab-
know if you should start talking or if you should just sit lishing emotional and behavioral autonomy from par-
there”) and on the phone (“I think it is hard to call. ents (Dowdy & Kliewer, 1998; Gray & Steinberg, 1999),
After it’s done with, you don’t know how to get off the furthering the development of gender identity (Feiring,
phone”). Others mentioned problems in asking people 1999), learning about oneself as a romantic partner
out (“Asking a girl out on a first date—complete (Furman & Simon, 1999), and establishing and main-
panic!”) or in turning people down (“How about if you taining status and popularity in the peer group (B. Brown,
go on a date and you’re really not interested, but he 1999). For these reasons, younger adolescents’ choice of
keeps calling?”). Still others noted problems in making dating partners may have more to do with how they will
or ending romantic commitments (“You don’t know if be seen by others (for example, as “grown up,” “macho,”
you are going out with someone or if you are just seeing or “popular”) than with the actual quality of the relation-
them,” “It is hard to say, ‘so, are we gonna make a com- ship itself. Consistent with this, between elementary
mitment?’” “I avoided [breaking up] for two weeks be- school and middle school, there is an increase in girls’
cause I was trying to think of what to say”) (Grover & attraction to aggressive boys who stand out in the peer
Nangle, 2003, pp. 133–134). group (Bukowski, Sippola, & Newcomb, 2000).

❚ Phases of Romance The development of inti-


THE DEVELOPMENT OF DATING
macy and more sophisticated social cognitive abilities is
RELATIONSHIPS
paralleled by changes in the ways adolescents think
Dating serves many purposes in adolescence, only one about and behave within romantic relationships. Sev-
of which is the development of intimacy. Indeed, it is eral stage theories of romantic relationships have been
not until late adolescence that dating relationships begin advanced (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). The evolution
to be characterized by a level of emotional depth and of romance in the adolescent’s life proceeds through
maturity that can be described as intimate, and it is not three distinct phases. During the first phase (roughly
until late adolescence that individuals develop genuinely between 11 and 13), adolescents first discover an inter-
deep attachments to individuals other than their parents est in socializing with potential romantic and sexual
(Furman & Simon, 1999; Hazan, 1994; Montgomery, partners. The focus of activity during this phase is pri-
2005). One study comparing the way adolescents inter- marily on learning about themselves, as adolescents
acted with their mother, a close friend, and a romantic broaden their self-conceptions to include seeing them-
partner found that interactions with romantic partners selves as a potential romantic partner to someone else.
were characterized by more conflict and fewer positive Actual romantic relationships tend to be short-lived
interactions than with friends, and more off-task behav- (the average romantic relationship at this age lasts only
ior than with mothers (Furman & Shomaker, 2008). a few weeks) and are frequently based on superficial
Over the course of adolescence, the importance of a infatuations. Indeed, the main purpose of romantic ac-
romantic partner—relative to other relationships— tivity at this age involves establishing, improving, or
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 341 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 341

maintaining peer group status. Success in socializing make the development of intimate relationships—
with the other sex becomes an important determinant whether nonsexual friendships, dating relationships, or
of status in the peer group, and high-status adolescents sexual relationships—far more complicated among
generally start dating before their lower-status peers LGBT youth than among their straight peers. For ex-
(Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). ample, because few LGBT youth have the freedom to
During the second phase (from about 14 to 16), ado- publicly express their romantic and sexual interests in
lescents slowly move toward more meaningful dyadic members of the same sex, they often find it difficult, if
relationships. During this period, dating is very casual not impossible, to engage in many of the social and in-
and often occurs in a group context in which peer net- terpersonal activities that their heterosexual friends are
works start to include couples who have a special rela- permitted to enjoy. Thus, many LGBT youth end up
tionship. Although adolescents are still learning about pursuing sexual activity outside the context of a dating
themselves as romantic and sexual partners and are still relationship, because the prejudices and harassment of
aware of the way their peers view their romantic rela- others may preclude any public display of romantic in-
tionships, they are now sufficiently involved in the emo- timacy with a same-sex partner. At the same time,
tional side of romance for this to completely overshadow for LGBT youth who are even somewhat open about
the personal and status concerns that dominated the their sexual identity, the development of close, nonsex-
earlier phases of romantic involvement. Relationships ual friendships with same-sex peers may be hampered
become a source of passion and preoccupation— by the suspicions and homophobia of others. As one
recalling the themes expressed in popular love songs group of writers explains the special predicament faced
that appeal to teenagers. Although relationships are by LGBT adolescents, “A sexual-minority adolescent
more enduring at this age than they were during early may already be privately plagued by the sense that he or
adolescence, the average romance still lasts only about she is profoundly different from other youths. To have
six months (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). One reason for this differentness acknowledged and perhaps ridiculed
this is that “dating the ‘wrong’ person or conducting by peers may prove intolerable” (Diamond, Savin-
romantic relationships in the ‘wrong’ way can seriously Williams, & Dubé, 1999).
damage one’s standing in the group. . . . This makes it
difficult to sustain relationships that are too heavily ❚ Sex Differences in Partner Preferences There
focused inward, on the quality of the interaction or are both age and sex differences in what adolescents
needs of the couple” (B. Brown, 1999, p. 297). look for in romantic partners, and these differences
Finally, toward the later years of high school (around parallel what is known about age and sex differences in
17 or 18), concerns about commitment begin to move romantic relationships. During middle adolescence,
to the forefront, as adolescents begin to think about the boys are more likely than girls to emphasize physical
long-term survival and growth of their romantic at- attractiveness and girls are more likely than boys to
tachments. Often during this stage, there are tensions place more weight on interpersonal qualities, such as
between partners’ needs for intimacy (which draw them support or intimacy, although controlled studies, in
together) and their needs for autonomy (which distance which characteristics of potential dates are experi-
them). As conceptions of romance develop, adolescents mentally manipulated, find that girls are influenced
come to value commitment and caring as features of more by attractiveness than they think they are (Ha,
relationships that are as important as passion and plea- Overbeek, & Engels, 2010). By late adolescence, how-
sure, if not more so (B. Brown, 1999; Seiffge-Krenke, ever, both sexes emphasize interpersonal qualities, and
2003). Relationships begin to look more like those seen the ingredients of a satisfying relationship are very
among young adults, and couples increasingly spend similar for males and females (and quite similar to
time by themselves, rather than in the larger peer group. those mentioned by adults): passion, communication,
The average romantic relationship at age 18 lasts more commitment, emotional support, and togetherness
than a year (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). (W. A. Collins, 2003; Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, &
Although the progression through the different Pepler, 1999). Interestingly, however, adolescents’ sat-
phases of dating and romance may characterize the de- isfaction with their romantic relationships are not as
velopment of most adolescents, a number of writers negatively affected by such qualities as conflict or pos-
interested in the experiences of lesbian, gay, bisexual, sessiveness as are adults’ (Levesque, 1993).
and transgender (LGBT) adolescents have pointed out
that this picture may be less applicable to LGBT
youth—adolescents who are not exclusively heterosex-
ual (Diamond, 2000; Diamond, Savin-Williams, &
LGBT youth Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth,
Dubé, 1999). Although great strides have been made in sometimes referred to as sexual-minority youth.
increasing the public’s tolerance and understanding of
sexual-minority youth, stigmas and stereotypes still sexual-minority youth Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender
(LGBT) youth.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 342 11/03/13 6:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

342 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

for the adolescent, just that its benefits may only accrue
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
among teenagers who wait until they are a certain age
Traditional, one-on-one dating is far less common (Neemann et al., 1995).
today than in past generations, and relatively more
interaction between males and females, ❚ Early Starters There is considerable evidence
even those who are romantically inter- that entering into a serious romantic relationship be-
ested in each other, occurs in groups fore it is normative (say, before age 15) is associated
than in couples. What implications does with a wide range of negative correlates (Connolly &
it have for the development of inti- McIsaac, 2009). This is probably true for both sexes, but
macy? For preparation for marriage? researchers have focused primarily on girls because
boys are less likely to begin serious dating quite so early.
Even so, the few studies that have looked at early dating
among boys do not show consistent effects.
The links between early dating and poorer mental
THE IMPACT OF DATING ON health have been reported consistently for at least
ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT 50 years. Compared with their peers, girls who begin
When considering the impact of dating on adolescents’ serious dating early are worse off psychologically than
development and mental health, it is important to dif- their peers—less mature socially, less imaginative, less
ferentiate between group and couple activities. Partici- oriented toward achievement, less happy with who they
pating in mixed-sex activity in group situations—going are and how they look, more depressed, more likely to
to parties or dances, for example—has a positive im- engage in disordered eating, less likely to do well in
pact on the psychological well-being of adolescents, school, and more likely to be involved in delinquency,
perhaps because at this stage of development, partici- substance use, and risky behavior (Connolly & McIsaac,
pating in these activities is status enhancing. The im- 2009). Early dating seems to have especially negative
pact of more serious dating is complicated and, as we implications among White girls (Compian et al., 2004),
shall see, may depend a great deal on the adolescent’s girls whose family relationships are more strained
age. Early starters (those who enter into dating relation- (Doyle et al., 2003), girls who date older boys (Haydon
ships well before their peers) and late bloomers (those & Halpern, 2010; Loftus, Kelly, & Mustillo, 2011), and
who do not have a romantic relationship until young girls who are early maturers (Natsuaki, Biehl, & Ge,
adulthood) may both be at risk, although for different 2009). Adolescents who are unpopular with same-sex
reasons and with different consequences. This is not to peers are especially harmed by early serious dating, per-
say that dating is not a valuable interpersonal experience haps because having few same-sex friends makes the
dating relationships excessively important (Brendgen,
Vitaro, Doyle, Markiewicz, & Bukowski, 2002). Re-
search also shows that adolescents who begin dating
early and who have multiple dating partners experience
a drop in the quality of their relationships over time
(W. A. Collins, 2003) and poorer quality relationships
in young adulthood (Madsen & Collins, 2011). Adoles-
cents who begin dating early are also more likely to be
victims of dating violence (Halpern, Spriggs, Martin, &
Kupper, 2009).
A variety of explanations for the link between early
dating and psychological problems have been offered,
but before we get ahead of ourselves, let’s keep in mind
the difficulty in distinguishing between selection and
socialization (Zimmer-Gembeck, Siebenbrunner, &
Collins, 2001). There are all sorts of reasons to believe
that girls with psychological problems are more likely
to get involved in dating relationships at a younger age,
and because we cannot randomly assign some teenagers
to date and others to remain single, we cannot be sure
that early dating actually causes problems. Moreover,
The impact of dating on adolescent development depends on the
age of the adolescent and the intensity of the relationship. Early,
early dating may be part of a larger profile that includes
serious dating may have a negative impact on psychological precocious involvement in many adultlike activities, of-
development. ten because their dating partners are older than they
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 343 11/03/13 6:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 343

are, and there is a good deal of evidence that this sort of In general, adolescents who do not date at all show
“pseudomaturity” is associated with a range of signs of retarded social development and feelings of in-
psychological problems (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). security (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009), while adolescents
Because this profile is itself associated with many fac- who date and go to parties regularly are more popular,
tors known to place adolescents at risk (poor parenting, have a stronger self-image, and report greater accep-
early puberty, or family instability, for example), it is tance by their friends (Connolly & Johnson, 1993).
hard to pinpoint early dating as the culprit. One recent Conversely, stopping or cutting back on dating after
study found, for example, that individuals who had having dated heavily is associated with a drop in self-
poorer-quality relationships prior to adolescence, were image and an increase in symptoms of depression
more likely to be dating at age 15 (Roisman, Booth- (Davies & Windle, 2000).
LaForce, Cauffman, Spieker, & The NICHD Early Child It is not clear, of course, whether age-appropriate
Care Research Network, 2009). dating leads to better social development or whether
That said, it has been suggested that the link between more socially advanced adolescents are simply more
early dating and poor mental health may have some- likely to date; both are probably true. But it does seem
thing to do with pressures on girls to engage in sexual safe to conclude that a moderate degree of dating—and
activity before they are willing or psychologically ready. a delay in serious involvement until age 15 or so—
(Early daters are more likely to be sexually active at a appears to be the most potentially valuable pattern.
younger age [Marin, Kirby, Hudes, Coyle, & Gomez, This conclusion must be tempered by research indi-
2006].) Sexual coercion and date rape are common cating that, as with the case of peer relationships in gen-
during the high school years (B. Brown, 2004; McMaster, eral, characteristics of the romantic partner play a role
Connolly, & Craig, 1997; W. Patton & Mannison, 1995). in shaping the impact of dating on psychological devel-
Although boys may feel peer pressure to become sexu- opment. Adolescents who are not all that popular to
ally active, this may be a very different sort of pressure— begin with, but who date popular peers, gain in popu-
with very different consequences—from what girls feel. larity over time, and adolescents with problems who
Because boys generally begin dating at a later age than date peers whose mental health is good show improve-
girls—and date people who are younger rather than ments in their psychological functioning over time
older—beginning to date in couples may be less (Simon, Aikins, & Prinstein, 2008). It is also the case,
anxiety-provoking for boys, who have the advantage of just as in the selection of friends, that adolescents tend
a few additional years of “maturity.” to select romantic partners with whom they share cer-
tain attributes. (Generally speaking, research suggests
that “birds of a feather flock together” is more often
true than “opposites attract.”) And, as is the case with
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION friendships, dating a romantic partner with a history of
In addition to the possibility that the delinquent behavior leads to more antisocial behavior,
negative impact of early dating on girls’ among both males and females (Herrera, Wiersma, &
mental health is due to pressures to Cleveland, 2011).
have sex, what other explanations occur Regardless of the impact that dating does or doesn’t
to you? have on adolescents’ psychosocial development, studies
show that romance has a powerful impact on their
emotional state. According to several studies, adoles-
cents’ real and fantasized relationships trigger more
strong emotional feelings during the course of a day
❚ To Date, or Not to Date? About 10% of late (one-third of girls’ strong feelings and one-quarter of
adolescents report having had no serious romantic re- boys’) than do family, school, or friends. Not surpris-
lationships, and another 15% have not been in a rela- ingly, the proportion of strong emotions attributed to
tionship that lasted more than a few months (Connolly romantic relationships increases dramatically between
& McIsaac, 2009). In looking at the effects of being a preadolescence and early adolescence, and between
“late bloomer,” it is important to distinguish between early and middle adolescence as well. And although the
adolescents who delay dating because it is culturally majority of adolescents’ feelings about their romantic
normative to do so (as is the case in many Asian relationships are positive, a substantial minority of
American communities) and those who delay because their feelings—more than 40%, in fact—are negative,
they are shy, unattractive, or unpopular (Connolly & involving anxiety, anger, jealousy, and depression
McIsaac, 2009). Although one would think that it is the (Larson, Clore, & Wood, 1999). Consistent with this,
latter group whose development is most at risk, not adolescents who have entered into a romantic relation-
enough research has been done on late bloomers to ship in the past year report more symptoms of depres-
draw definitive conclusions. sion than do those who have not (Joyner & Udry, 2000).
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 344 11/03/13 6:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

344 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

One reason for this may be that many adolescents who ❚ Violence in Dating Relationships Unfortu-
are involved romantically also experience breakups nately, many romantic relationships in adolescence are
during the same time period (Z. Chen et al., 2009b; characterized by hostility, aggression, and abuse
W. A. Collins, 2003), and the breakup of a romantic (B. Brown, 2004). More distressingly, a high proportion
relationship is the single most common trigger of the of young adolescents believe that physical violence in a
first episode of major depression (Monroe, Rohde, relationship is acceptable. In a recent study of more
Seeley, & Lewinsohn, 1999). As you would expect, neg- than 5,000 American 6th-graders, over half said that it
ative emotions associated with being in a relationship was acceptable for a girl to hit her boyfriend if he had
are more common among adolescents who are high in made her mad or jealous, and a quarter of the students
rejection sensitivity (G. Downey et al., 1999) or who thought it was fine for a boy to hit his girlfriend. Nearly
have an insecure working model, especially those who a third of the girls in the sample, and more than 25% of
form preoccupied attachments to romantic partners the boys who either were in, or had been in, a romantic
(because they are unable to fully trust their partner or relationship had been physically aggressive toward their
see themselves as worthy of their partner’s affection) partner (Simon, Miller, Gorman-Smith, Orpinas, &
(Davila, 2008). Sullivan, 2010) (see Figure 10.11). Although more than
Breaking up does not have severe effects on all ado- half of all parents talk to their adolescent about dating
lescents, of course. Those who are most vulnerable to violence, parents are less likely to talk about the topic
the potential negative consequences of ending a rela- than about drugs, alcohol, family finances, money
tionship are adolescents high in rejection sensitivity, management, or even the economy (Rothman, Miller,
those who have experienced a series of breakups, those Terpeluk, Glauber, & Randel, 2011).
who have other sorts of problems (such as binge drink- Estimates vary from study to study, but recent
ing or involvement in delinquency), and, not surpris- national surveys find that about 40% of American ado-
ingly, those who identify themselves as the one who was lescents have been the victim of violence within the
broken up with (rather than the breaker-upper) context of a romantic relationship at one time or an-
(Connolly & McIsaac, 2009). other (Halpern et al., 2009). Dating abuse increases

40
Females
Males
35

30

25
Percent

20

15

10

0
Scratched them

Slapped them
Slammed/held
them against a wall
Kicked them

Pushed/shoved them
Threw something at
them that could hurt
Punched/hit them with
something that could hurt

Scratched them

Slapped them
Slammed/held
them against a wall
Kicked them

Pushed/shoved them
Threw something at
them that could hurt
Punched/hit them with
something that could hurt

Perpetration Victimization
FIGURE 10.11 Proportion of American 6th-graders reporting being victims or perpetrators of various types of dating
violence. (Simon et al., 2010)
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 345 11/03/13 6:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 10 Intimacy 345

between early adolescence and mid-adolescence, and relationships are linked to the models they had been
then becomes somewhat less common (Foshee et al., exposed to at home. For example, adolescents who
2009). Male and female adolescents are equally likely to have witnessed a great deal of conflict between their
be the victims of violence in dating relationships parents (either physical or verbal) report higher levels
(Halpern, Oslak, Young, Martin, & Kupper, 2001; of verbal aggression, physical aggression, and relation-
J. Miller & White, 2003; O’Leary, Slep, Avery-Leaf, & ship difficulties with their romantic partners, both as
Cascardi, 2008), and violence is often associated with perpetrators and victims (Kinsfogel & Grych, 2004;
drinking (Buzy et al., 2004; McNaughton Reyes, Foshee, Simon & Furman, 2010; Tschann et al., 2009). Other
Bauer, & Ennett, 2011). Dating violence is more com- studies have found that adolescents whoare either per-
mon in rural areas than in suburban or urban com- petrators or victims of violence in dating relation-
munities (G. A. Spencer & Bryant, 2000), among ethnic ships are more likely to have had parents who were
minority adolescents, among adolescents from single- abusive, harsh, or behaved inappropriately toward
parent households, among adolescents from lower them (Manchikanti Gómez, 2011; Lavoie et al., 2002;
socioeconomic homes, and is common among both Linder & Collins, 2005). These studies, along with
LGBT adolescents and heterosexual youth (Foshee those discussed earlier about adolescent attachments,
et al., 2009; Freedner, Freed, Yang, & Austin, 2002; suggest that variations in adolescents’ romantic rela-
Halpern et al., 2009). Individuals who are aggressive in tionships may have their origins—at least in part—in
romantic relationships are more likely to have had adolescents’ family experiences.
problems with aggression earlier in life (L. O’Donnell, The main point to keep in mind is that the qualities
Stueve, Myint-U, Duran, Agronick, & Wilson-Simmons, of adolescents’ relationships with others—whether
2006). Being in a violent relationship also increases the with parents, siblings, friends, or romantic partners—
chances of an adolescent girl behaving violently as a are correlated across different types of relationships
young adult (Herrera et al., 2011). Adolescents who (B. Brown, 2004). Adolescents who have supportive and
have been the victims of violence within the context of satisfying relationships at home are more likely to have
a romantic relationship are more likely to be depressed, high-quality friendships, and adolescents who have
contemplate suicide, use illegal drugs, become preg- high-quality friendships are more likely to have high-
nant during adolescence, and drop out of school (Hagan quality romantic relationships. Thus, individuals’ early
& Foster, 2001; Kim & Capaldi, 2004; Silverman, Raj, experiences in the family, in interaction with their cu-
Mucci, & Hathaway, 2001), and many of these prob- mulative experiences with peers during childhood and
lems persist into young adulthood (Adam et al., 2010). preadolescence, affect the nature and quality of their
They are also more likely to be victimized again in the romantic relationships in adolescence (B. Brown, 2004;
future (P. H. Smith, White, & Holland, 2003). W. A. Collins, 2003; Dhariwal, Connolly, Paciello, &
Caprara, 2009), and the quality of adolescents’ family
relationships affect the quality of the romantic relation-
ships they have in young adulthood (R. Conger, Cui,
Bryant, & Elder, 2000; Donnellan, Larsen-Rife, &
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION Conger, 2005).
What can be done to reduce the prevalence of
violence in adolescents’ dating relationships? Is this
something that schools RECAP
should become involved • In general, social activities with the other sex begin
in? in early adolescence as group activities that bring
males and females together, proceed to casual
dating in couples, and later in adolescence pro-
gresses to serious involvement with a steady
romantic partner.
• Paralleling these changes in the context of dating
are changes in the nature and function of dating. As
adolescents develop, dating shifts from a focus on
We also know that adolescents behave in a variety of infatuation and status, to intimacy, and, finally to
ways within dating relationships that are shaped by bonding.
“scripts” for how males and females are expected to • Although early intense dating appears to have
behave—scripts that are learned at home and from the adverse effects on adolescents’ mental health and
mass media (Feiring, 1999; Gray & Steinberg, 1999; behavior, a moderate degree of dating after age 15
Larson, Clore, & Wood, 1999). In general, adoles- or so is associated with better mental health and
cents’ ways of dealing with conflict in their romantic well-being than no dating at all.
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 346 11/03/13 6:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

346 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

• Adolescents who have supportive and satisfying Keep in mind, however, that the effects of having an
relationships at home are more likely to have intimate friendship with someone depend on who that
high-quality friendships, and adolescents who have someone is and what takes place in the relationship.
high-quality friendships are more likely to have high- Being popular is less important than genuinely having
quality romantic relationships. Conversely, friends, and having friends is less important than hav-
adolescents who have been exposed to high levels ing good friendships (Asher, Parker, & Walker, 1996;
of conflict or violence in their family, are Berndt, 1996; Fontaine et al., 2009; Hartup & Stevens,
more likely to experience vio- 1997; Hussong, 2000). Not all friendships are consis-
lence in their own dating tently good friendships. Some friendships provide for
relationships. positive things like self-disclosure, intimacy, and com-
panionship, but others give rise to insecurity, conflict,
jealousy, and mistrust (J. Parker, Low, Walker, &
Gamm, 2005). And, as you know, adolescents who are
close to peers or romantic partners who have antisocial
values or habits are themselves more likely to develop
Intimacy and Psychosocial similar patterns of behavior (Cauffman et al., 2008;
Haynie, Giordano, Manning, & Longmore, 2005;
Development Hussong & Hicks, 2003). It is easy to forget, but it goes
Intimate relationships during adolescence, whether without saying, that not all close relationships foster
with peers or adults, inside or outside the family, or positive developmental outcomes.
sexual or nonsexual, play an important role in young Nevertheless, studies consistently show that individ-
people’s overall psychological development (Hartup & uals with satisfying close friendships fare better than
Stevens, 1997). Close friends serve as a sounding board those without them, not only in adolescence but in
for adolescents’ fantasies and questions about the fu- adulthood as well. Adolescence is an especially impor-
ture. Adolescents often talk to their friends about the tant time in the development of close relationships be-
careers they hope to have, the people they hope to get cause many of the capacities and capabilities that
involved with, and the life they expect to lead after they permit intimacy in adult relationships make their debut
leave home. Friends provide advice on a range of in adolescence.
identity-related matters—from how to act in different
situations to what sorts of occupational and educa-
tional paths to pursue. At least one study finds that hav- RECAP
ing an intimate friendship is more central to adolescents’ • Adolescents who have intimate friendships typi-
mental health than it is to children’s (Buhrmester, cally have better mental health than their peers
1990). It also has been found that intimacy with same- who do not, although we do not know which
sex friends and intimacy with romantic partners make comes first, intimacy or psychological health.
distinct contributions to adolescents’ self-esteem • Experts agree that close peer relationships are an
(Connolly & Konarski, 1994). essential part of healthy social development during
adolescence.
• Any discussion of the impact of close friends on
development must take into account the charac-
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION teristics of these friends.
Do you think changes in family life— • Being popular is less important than genuinely hav-
divorce, remarriage, parental employ- ing friends, and having friends is less important
ment, and so on—have affected the than having good friendships.
nature of intimacy in adolescence?
ste32118_ch10_314-347.indd Page 347 11/03/13 6:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 348 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 349 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
11
Sexuality
SEXUALITY AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE
SEXUAL ACTIVITY DURING ADOLESCENCE
Stages of Sexual Activity
Sexual Intercourse During Adolescence
Changes in Sexual Activity Over Time
THE SEXUALLY ACTIVE ADOLESCENT
Psychological and Social Characteristics of Sexually Active Adolescents
Hormonal and Contextual Influences on Sexual Activity
Parental and Peer Influences on Sexual Activity
Sex Differences in the Meaning of Sex
Sexual Orientation
Sexual Harassment, Rape, and Sexual Abuse During Adolescence
RISKY SEX AND ITS PREVENTION
Contraceptive Use
AIDS and Other Sexually Transmitted Diseases
Teen Pregnancy
Adolescent Parenthood
Sex Education

349
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 350 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

350 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

AMERICAN ADULTS HAVE an ambivalent attitude to- that sex between teenagers is wrong—that only trou-
ward adolescent sexuality. On the one hand, they are bled teenagers do it or that doing it will get normal
fascinated by it—it is nearly impossible to turn on the teenagers into trouble. As a result, until fairly recently,
television and avoid seeing sexual imagery that either the problematic aspects of adolescent sexuality—
depicts or is directed at adolescents (D. Roberts et al., precocious sex, promiscuous sex, unsafe sex, unwanted
2009). On the other hand, adults deplore it—most sex, and so forth—have received far more attention
adults (80%, in fact) say that teenage sex is always or than its normative aspects (Tolman & McClelland, 2011).
almost always wrong (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). This is not to make light of these problems, which for
Adults look, and then they look the other way, and then many adolescents are very real. But it is worth pointing
they look again. Talk about a love–hate relationship! out that the study of other aspects of adolescent psy-
This same ambivalence is reflected in the way that chosocial development is not similarly dominated by
social scientists have studied adolescent sexuality. Sex research on what can go wrong. Imagine if researchers
has always been a popular subject among adolescence interested in identity, autonomy, intimacy, or achieve-
researchers. But rather than try to understand it, most ment studied only negative self-conceptions, angry re-
research focuses simply on enumerating it—counting bellion, failed friendships, or flunking out. As you will
how many people have done which things how often, read, and in contrast to adults’ concerns, most of the
at what age, and with whom. When studies probe more time, sex during adolescence is not associated with
deeply than this, they often start from the premise problems.

Sexuality as an dramatic and more obvious prior to adulthood, it by no


means ceases at the end of adolescence. Nonetheless,
Adolescent Issue most of us would agree that adolescence is a fundamen-
tally important time—if not the most important time
Like other aspects of psychosocial development, sexual-
in the life cycle—for the development of sexuality.
ity is not an entirely new issue that surfaces for the first
There are several reasons for this.
time during adolescence. Young children are curious
about their sex organs and at a very early age derive
❚ Puberty and Adolescent Sexuality Perhaps
pleasure from them (if not what adults would label or-
most obvious is the link between adolescent sexuality
gasm). And, although sexual development may be more
and puberty (Bogin, 2011; Diamond & Savin-Williams,
2011). A substantial increase in the sex drive in early
adolescence is clearly the result of hormonal changes.
Moreover, not until puberty do individuals become
capable of sexual reproduction. Before puberty, chil-
dren are certainly capable of kissing, fondling, mastur-
bating, and even having sexual intercourse, and erotic
feelings are reported by individuals prior to adoles-
cence; sexual feelings do not suddenly switch on at
puberty (Herdt & McClintock, 2000). But not until
puberty can males ejaculate semen or do females ovu-
late, and the fact that pregnancy is a possible outcome
of sexual activity changes the nature and meaning of
sexual behavior markedly—for the adolescent and for
others. Finally, as we saw in Chapter 1, not until puberty
do individuals develop the secondary sex characteris-
tics that serve as a basis for sexual attraction and as
dramatic indicators that the young person is no longer
physically a child.

❚ Cognitive Change and Adolescent Sexuality


Most individuals’ first experiences in sexual relationships occur The increased importance of sexuality at adolescence is
during adolescence. not solely a result of puberty. The cognitive changes of
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 351 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 351

adolescence play a part in the changed nature of sexual- safe sex—sex that avoids pregnancy and sexually trans-
ity as well. One obvious difference between the sex play mitted infections.
of children and the sexual activity of adolescents is that
children are not especially introspective or reflective
about sexual behavior. In contrast, sex during adoles-
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
cence is the subject of sometimes painful conjecture
(“Will she or won’t she?”), decision making (“Should I What role, if any, should school-based sex education
or shouldn’t I?”), hypothetical thinking (“What if he play in the promotion of
wants to do it tonight?”), and self-conscious concern healthy sexuality? Should
(“Am I good-looking enough?”). As we saw in Chapter 10, the focus of sex education
one of the chief tasks of adolescence is to figure out be on preventing sexual ac-
how to deal with sexual desires and how to incorporate tivity during adolescence
sex successfully and appropriately into social relation- or on promoting positive
ships. Much of this task is cognitive in nature, and sexual development?
much of it is made possible by the expansion of intel-
lectual abilities that takes place during the period.
RECAP
❚ Social Roles and Adolescent Sexuality In
• Adolescence is a fundamentally important time in
addition to the influence of puberty and the growth of
the life cycle for the development of sexuality.
sophisticated thinking on sexuality during adolescence,
• The physical and hormonal changes of puberty
the new social meaning given to sexual and dating be-
increase sex drive, change the adolescent’s appear-
havior at this time in the life cycle makes sexuality an
ance, and permit reproduction, all of which affect
especially important psychosocial concern. Adoles-
the development of sexuality.
cence is a turning point in the development of sexuality
• The cognitive changes of adolescence result in the
because it marks the onset of deliberate sexually moti-
increased capacity of the individual to understand
vated behavior that is recognized, both by an adolescent
and think about sexual feelings.
and by others, as primarily and explicitly sexual in
• The transition of the individual into new social
nature. Sexual activity in adolescence is motivated by
roles gives new meaning to sexual behavior in the
more than hormones. For many adolescents (especially
eyes of individuals, society, and social institutions
girls, but boys as well), sex is motivated by love and the
and provides new motives for establishing sexual
desire for the sort of serious emotional relationship
relationships.
that begins to take on features of adult romance. For
• Four aspects of positive sexual development in
many adolescents (especially boys, but girls as well), sex
adolescence are accepting one’s changing body,
is motivated by a desire to enhance their status with
accepting one’s feelings of sexual arousal,
peers (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009).
understanding that sexual activity is
voluntary, and practicing
❚ Positive Sexual Development Although the
safe sex.
history of research on adolescent sexuality has empha-
sized its problematic aspects, in recent years, there has
been increased interest in the factors that lead to posi-
tive sexual development (Diamond & Savin-Williams,
2009). There are four distinct aspects to positive sexual-
ity in adolescence that can serve as the basis for how
parents and educators discuss sex with teenagers Sexual Activity During
(Brooks-Gunn & Paikoff, 1993; Zimmer-Gembeck,
Ducat, & Boislard-Pepin, 2011). First, the adolescent
Adolescence
needs to come to feel comfortable with his or her ma- Given the field’s historical focus on problematic aspects
turing body—its shape, size, and attractiveness. Second, of adolescent sexuality, such as precocious sex (having
the adolescent should accept having feelings of sexual sex at too young an age), promiscuous sex (having sex
arousal as normal and appropriate. Third, the adoles- with too many partners), unwanted sex (having sex
cent needs to feel comfortable about choosing to en- against one’s will), or unsafe sex (having sex than can
gage in—or not to engage in—various sexual activities; result in pregnancy or a sexually transmitted disease),
that is, healthy sexual development involves under- most of the research conducted into the sexual behav-
standing that sex is a voluntary activity for oneself and ior of adolescents has focused on sexual intercourse
for one’s partner. Finally, the adolescent (at least, one (Tolman & McClelland, 2011). With the possible excep-
who is sexually active) must understand and practice tion of oral sex, adults have tended not to worry about
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 352 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

352 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

sexually experienced adolescents—involves activities


other than sexual intercourse, such as kissing and
touching parts of each other’s body (broadly referred to
as “noncoital activity,” or, to use a more familiar term,
“fooling around”) (Tolman & McClelland, 2011).
Moreover, because most individuals do not begin their
sexual experiences with intercourse but progress to-
ward it through stages of gradually increasing intimacy,
it is important to view intercourse as one activity in a
long progression, rather than as an isolated behavior
(Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009).

STAGES OF SEXUAL ACTIVITY


Before we turn to statistics on adolescent sexual activ-
ity, a word of caution is in order. Reports of sexual be-
havior vary markedly as a function of the ways in which
questions are worded and data are collected. When for-
mer president Bill Clinton, referring to his affair with
Monica Lewinsky, infamously said, “I did not have sex-
ual relations with that woman,” what he meant (we
assume) is that he did not have vaginal intercourse with
her. Whether the possibility that they engaged in oral
sex makes his statement false depends on what one takes
the expression “sexual relations” to mean. Similarly,
when adolescents respond to questions asking whether
they have “had sex,” have been “sexually active,” or are
“still a virgin,” it is not clear how they interpret the
question. Is genital touching “sex”? Are you a “virgin”
if you have engaged in anal sex but not vaginal inter-
course? Adolescents, like adults, don’t always agree.
And to make things even more complicated, adoles-
cents distinguish between acts that culminate in or-
gasm (which are more likely to be viewed as leading to
Although most research on adolescent sexuality has focused on a loss of virginity) and those that don’t (Bersamin,
sexual intercourse, adolescents’ initial forays into the world of sex Fisher, Walker, Hill, & Grube, 2007). Moreover, adoles-
typically begin with less intimate sexual activity and gradually
build toward intercourse.
cents who have engaged in a specific behavior are more
likely to say that the behavior doesn’t “count” in their
definition of losing one’s virginity, which means that
sexual behavior other than intercourse, and worries adolescents’ responses to surveys about sex are biased
about oral sex have surfaced only in recent years, in by their actual experience. So bear in mind that all fig-
response to exaggerated media reports about teenagers ures to follow are necessarily approximate.
reporting giving or receiving oral sex promiscuously; Most adolescents’ first experience with sex falls into
in fact, although national surveys show that slightly the category of autoerotic behavior—sexual behavior
more teenagers have had oral sex than intercourse, that is experienced alone. The most common auto-
they also indicate that the vast majority of teenagers erotic activities reported by adolescents are having
who engage in oral sex also engage in sexual inter- erotic fantasies (about three-quarters of all teenagers
course, and that promiscuity is not the norm for either report having sexual fantasies) and masturbation (dif-
activity (Hensel, Fortenberry, & Orr, 2008; L. Lindberg, ferent surveys yield different estimates, depending on
Jones, & Santelli, 2008). the age of the respondents and the wording of the ques-
Although adolescents’ involvement in sexual inter- tions, but about half of all adolescent boys and about
course is an important topic, it is wise to remember that one-fourth of all adolescent girls masturbate prior to
a good deal of the sexual activity of adolescents—even age 18) (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009).
By the time most adolescents reach high school, they
have made the transition from autoerotic behavior to
autoerotic behavior Sexual behavior that is experienced alone, sexual activity that involves another person. By the time
such as masturbation or sexual fantasizing.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 353 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 353

individuals have turned 16, about 80% have engaged in sexual intercourse, national surveys indicate that more
some type of noncoital activity with another person. adolescents are sexually active at an earlier age today
Interestingly, the developmental progression of sexual than several decades ago; however, there has been a
behaviors, from less intimate to more intimate, has not slight decrease in the proportion of sexually experi-
changed very much over the past 50 years, and the se- enced teenagers since the mid-1990s—in other words,
quence in which males and females engage in various slightly fewer adolescents are having sexual intercourse,
sexual activities is remarkably similar. According to re- but those who are do so at a somewhat earlier age
cent, large-scale studies of American adolescents, hold- (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). The best estimates
ing hands comes first, followed (in this order) by we have are that, by the end of their sophomore year in
kissing, making out (kissing for a long time), feeling high school, more than 40% of American adolescents
breasts through clothes, feeling breasts under clothes, have had heterosexual vaginal intercourse (these esti-
feeling a penis through clothes, feeling a penis under mates, which are based on large national surveys, do
clothes or while naked, feeling a vagina through clothes, not include same-sex intercourse or other types of sex,
feeling a vagina under clothes or while naked, and like oral or anal sex). By age 18, this number has risen to
intercourse or oral sex. Some studies find that adoles- about 65% (see Figure 11.1) (Centers for Disease Con-
cents’ first experience with oral sex generally precedes trol and Prevention, 2012). Whatever we might think
their first experience with intercourse, whereas others about these figures, one conclusion is inescapable:
find the reverse (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009; Sexual intercourse during high school is now a part of
Shtarkshall, Carmel, Jaffe-Hirschfield, & Woloski-Wruble, the normative experience of adolescence in America.
2009). One worrisome finding is that most adolescents
report talking about contraception after they first have ❚ Ethnic Differences in Age of Sexual Initiation
intercourse, rather than before (O’Sullivan, Cheng, There are substantial ethnic differences in age of sexual
Harris, & Brooks-Gunn, 2007). For most adolescents, initiation, especially among males (Centers for Disease
this sequence of increasingly advanced behaviors Control and Prevention, 2012). Among Black males,
unfolds gradually over time, but for a significant minor- the average age of first intercourse is 15 (more than
ity, it is compressed into a shorter interval (de Graaf, one-fifth report having had sex for the first time by age
Vanwesenbeeck, Meijer, Woertman, & Meeus, 2009). 13); among White and Hispanic males, it is 16.5; and
The expected timetable for progressive sexual activities among Asian American males, it is 18; in all ethnic
is faster among adolescents who expect a relatively faster groups, the average reported age of first sex is slightly
timetable for achieving autonomy from parents and ex- older among females than males (Centers for Disease
perimenting with drugs and alcohol, suggesting that Control and Prevention, 2012; Upchurch, Levy-Storms,
earlier involvement in more intimate forms of sex may Sucoff, & Aneshensel, 1998). Ethnic differences in the
be part of a larger pattern of earlier involvement in age of sexual initiation are far smaller among females,
“adult” activities (Rosenthal & Smith, 1997). although Hispanic and Asian American females gener-
ally have their first sexual intercourse at a later age than
is the case among their Black and White counterparts
SEXUAL INTERCOURSE DURING
(Grunbaum, Lowry, Kann, & Pateman, 2000). One rea-
ADOLESCENCE
son for the relatively high rate of early sexual activity
❚ Prevalence of Sexual Intercourse Estimates among Black males is the higher proportion of Black
of the prevalence of sexual intercourse among contem-
porary adolescents vary from study to study, depending
100
on the nature of the sample surveyed, the year and
Percent who had sexual intercourse

90 Female
region in which the study was undertaken, the reliabil-
80 Male
ity of the data gathered, and, of course, the wording of
the questions (Santelli, Lindberg, Abma, McNeely, & 70
Resnick, 2000). Some studies suggest that adolescents 60
do not always report their sexual activity honestly or 50
accurately, with males, in particular, tending to over- 40
state their level of activity and females tending to 30
understate it (Kaestle, Halpern, Miller, & Ford, 2005). 20
The following paragraphs summarize what social sci-
10
entists have concluded from recent surveys with these
0
caveats in mind. 9th grade 10th grade 11th grade 12th grade
Although regional and ethnic variations make it FIGURE 11.1 About one-third of American 9th-graders
difficult—if not misleading—to generalize about the have had sexual intercourse. By senior year, about two-thirds
average age at which American adolescents initiate have. (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012)
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 354 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

354 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

youth who grow up in single-parent homes and in poor It is important to note that for many girls, their first
neighborhoods, both of which, as you will read later in sexual experience is forced (Diamond & Savin-Williams,
this chapter, are risk factors for early sexual activity. 2009). Involuntary sex is especially frequent among
Americanized Latino adolescents are more likely than girls who have sex for the first time when they are 13 or
their less acculturated peers to have sex at a younger younger; one-fourth of younger adolescents report that
age, to have multiple sex partners, to contract STDs, their first intercourse was against their will, in contrast
and to become pregnant (Kaplan, Erickson, & Juarez- to 10% of women whose first intercourse was after age 18.
Reyes, 2002; J. Lee & Hahm, 2010; McDonald, Manlove, Moreover, many other young women who report that
& Ikramullah, 2009; Upchurch, Aneshensel, Mudgal, & they had sex voluntarily the first time nevertheless re-
McNeely, 2001), and more acculturated Asian American port that they did not really want to have sex; girls in
girls are more likely to be sexually active than their less relationships where the balance of power favors the boy
Americanized peers (Hahm, Lahiff, & Barreto, 2006). are more likely to have sex than those whose relation-
Studies also indicate that, among all ethnic groups, rates ship is more equal (Giordano et al., 2010). Young girls
of sexual activity are higher among economically disad- whose first partner was 7 or more years older were twice
vantaged youth, although the gap in rates of sexual as likely as others to report having had voluntary but
activity between rich and poor is substantially narrower unwanted intercourse (Abma, Driscoll, & Moore,
now than it was a decade ago, again pointing to the 1998). Young adolescents, both male and female, with a
increasingly normative nature of sexual intercourse significantly older romantic partner are far more likely
among American teenagers (Singh & Darroch, 1999). to have sexual intercourse than those whose partner
The average age at first intercourse varies considerably is the same age (Kaestle, Morisky, & Wiley, 2002;
across nations; one survey of European adolescents Leitenberg & Saltzman, 2000; Loftus & Kelly, 2012).
found that the proportion of 15-year-olds who ever had
intercourse ranged from 15% in Poland to 75% in ❚ Timing of Sexual Initiation Adolescents are
Greenland (Madkour, Farhat, Halpern, Godeau, & more likely to lose their virginity during certain times of
Gabhainn, 2010a). the year than during others. There are two seasonal peaks
in the timing of first intercourse: June and December
(Levin, Xu, & Bartkowski, 2002) (see Figure 11.2). June
and, to a lesser extent, May and July are common months
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION for first intercourse regardless of whether the adoles-
Today’s American teenagers are growing up within cents are romantically involved; December, however, is a
a context in which sexual intercourse before high peak time only among adolescents who are with a seri-
school graduation is considered normative in many ous boyfriend or girlfriend, and particularly so among
segments of society. In other countries, however, sex girls. Several explanations for these seasonal trends have
before age 18 is not normative. How might this change been offered, including the general tendency for people
the nature of adolescence in these to be more sexually active when the weather is very hot
different cultural contexts? How do or very cold and the fact that adolescents have more un-
you think adolescence might be supervised time when they are on summer or winter
different when only a minority of vacation. As for what has been called “the holiday effect”
high school students are sexually (the rise in sexual debuts among romantic partners in
active? December)—well, perhaps you can figure that out for
yourself.

FIGURE 11.2 Adolescents are most 0.8


(Observed-expected)/expected

likely to have intercourse for the first Romantic


0.6 Nonromantic
time during early summer or in
December. (Levin et al., 2002) 0.4
0.2
0.0
–0.2
–0.4
–0.6
Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec
Month of first intercourse
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 355 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 355

CHANGES IN SEXUAL ACTIVITY active before high school is also an important factor in
OVER TIME discussions of sex education, because programs that
do not begin until students’ later years of high school
Attitudes toward premarital intercourse during adoles-
are probably too late for a substantial number of
cence became more liberal beginning in the mid-1960s
young people.
and, especially, during the early 1970s. Not surprisingly,
Finally, the greatest increase in the prevalence of
accompanying this shift in attitudes was an equally
intercourse among adolescents, and the greatest decline
noteworthy shift in sexual behavior (Alan Guttmacher
in the age at first intercourse, has been among females
Institute, 1994).
(Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). Before 1965, there
❚ Recent Historical Trends Three trends are were substantial gaps between the proportions of
of special interest. First, the overall percentage of sexually active boys and girls. Since about 1965, the
American adolescents who had engaged in premarital proportion of sexually experienced high school males
sex accelerated markedly during the early 1970s and has nearly tripled, but the proportion of sexually expe-
again during the late 1980s, and then declined between rienced high school females is about 5 times higher
1995 and 2001; it has remained flat, at a little less than today. Sex differences in rates of sexual intercourse
50%, since then (Centers for Disease Control and today are negligible, especially by age 16 (Centers for
Prevention, 2012; Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009; Disease Control and Prevention, 2012).
Santelli et al., 2000; Santelli et al., 2004). Interestingly, The bottom line: Whether adults approve or not,
the recent decline in the proportion of adolescents who sexual activity has become a normative part of the
have had sexual intercourse is not paralleled by a de- American teenager’s life. And while many parents, ed-
cline in the proportion of adolescents who are having ucators, and other adults are alarmed by sexual activity
other types of sex—in fact, today’s teenagers are far among the young, for most adolescents, sexual involve-
more “active” sexually than were previous generations ment is accompanied by affection, emotional involve-
(Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). It is likely that the ment, and commitment to a relationship. Although
threat of AIDS and other sexually transmitted diseases many high school students are sexually active on a
has led many adolescents to substitute a safer type of regular basis, promiscuity is not the norm (Singh &
sex (such as oral sex, which, although safer than vaginal Darroch, 1999). According to recent data, 80% of sexu-
or anal intercourse, still carries some risk) for inter- ally active high school girls, and 67% of sexually active
course, with what appear to be the desired results: high school boys, had intercourse with only one part-
Adolescents who engage in oral sex, but not sexual ner during the past 3 months, and the proportion of
intercourse, are less likely to feel guilty or used and less adolescents who have had multiple sex partners has
likely to contract a sexually transmitted infection decreased substantially over the past decade, although
(Brady & Halpern-Felsher, 2007). it is still substantial today—15% of contemporary high
Second, the proportion of individuals who have school students have had intercourse with four or
sexual intercourse early in adolescence is substantial. more persons (Centers for Disease Control and Pre-
Although the median age at which adolescents first vention, 2012). Rates of promiscuity vary considerably
engage in intercourse has remained somewhere across localities, however, as Figure 11.3 illustrates.
between 16 and 17 for quite some time, today, one- About one-third of sexually active adolescents have
third of all contemporary American adolescents have had intercourse with someone they are not in a roman-
had intercourse by the time they are 9th-graders, tic relationship with (Manning, Longmore, & Giordano,
and more than 6% have had intercourse by age 13 2005). Although many adults have expressed concern
(Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). about adolescents having sex with someone outside the
Among adolescents who have not had sex, fears of context of a dating relationship, or “hooking up,” these
pregnancy and disease (including HIV/AIDS) are the encounters are usually with someone the adolescent
most common reasons for abstaining (Blinn-Pike, knows well, like a friend or ex-partner (sometimes re-
1999; Loewensen, Ireland, & Resnick, 2004). As we’ll ferred to as “friends with benefits”). And in one-third of
see later, these figures on sexual activity among these “nonromantic” encounters, one of the persons
younger adolescents are noteworthy, because the was hoping that the friendship would turn into (or re-
younger individuals are when they have sex, the more turn to) a romantic relationship (Manning, Giordano,
likely they are to have unprotected sex, exposing them- & Longmore, 2006). In other words, the broad category
selves to the risks of pregnancy and STDs (Diamond & of “casual sex” includes a wide range of behavior, from
Savin-Williams, 2009; Kaestle, et al., 2005). (Because one-night stands to sex with a desired (but not yet com-
Black males initiate sex at an earlier age, they are also mitted) romantic partner.
more likely than other adolescents to engage in risky
sex [Fergus, Zimmerman, & Caldwell, 2007].) The ❚ Sex and Alcohol One particular cause for concern
fact that a large number of adolescents are sexually is that the percentage of adolescents who use alcohol
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 356 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

356 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Proportion with four or more partners 30

25

20

15

10

0
San Los Angeles New York Miami Houston Chicago Charlotte Boston Memphis Philadelphia
Francisco City
FIGURE 11.3 The rate of sexual promiscuity varies considerably across the United States. (Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, 2012)

or other drugs prior to having sex has increased in re- • More than 40 percent of all American 15-year-olds
cent years—in one national survey, about one-fourth of and close to two-thirds of all American 18-year-
American adolescents said they drank or used drugs olds have had sexual intercourse.
before the last time they had sex, a behavior that is rela- • The proportion of individuals who have had sexual
tively more common among White and Hispanic male intercourse before entering high school has risen
adolescents (Centers for Disease Control and Preven- significantly in recent decades.
tion, 2012). Not surprisingly, sexual risk taking is more • The most substantial change in recent
likely when alcohol is involved, mainly because of im- decades has been in the
paired judgment and loss of control (Kiene, Barta, sexual activity of
Tennen, & Armeli, 2009). One study of British adoles- females.
cents found that one-fourth of those interviewed re-
ported having been so drunk during sex that they did
not even remember it (a phenomenon that by no means
is limited to teenagers in England), as these two 16-year-
olds (the first male, the second female) describe:
The Sexually Active
I’ve actually woken up next to a girl and I didn’t have a
clue who she was. And when I woke up I was like, “What’s Adolescent
your name? Like, Who were you?” She explained herself, I
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND SOCIAL
couldn’t remember it, man. I was thinking, “How the hell
couldn’t I remember that?” . . . I don’t remember. CHARACTERISTICS OF SEXUALLY
ACTIVE ADOLESCENTS
I’ve had previous problems with alcohol where I’ve been so
drunk that I can’t actually remember things that have hap- For many years, researchers studied the psychological
pened and the next morning, or the next couple of weeks, and social characteristics of sexually active adolescents
I get told about things and then it [having sex with some- on the assumption that these teenagers were more trou-
one] suddenly comes back and I think, oh my God, what bled than their peers (either before or as a consequence
have I done? (L. M. Coleman & Cater, 2005, p. 656) of becoming sexually active). This view has been re-
placed as sexual activity has become more prevalent
among “normal” adolescents.
RECAP
• Important changes in attitudes toward adolescent ❚ Sexual Activity and Psychological Development
premarital sex occurred during the 1970s and have Numerous studies show that sexual activity during ado-
persisted since that time. Most teenagers today lescence is decidedly not associated with psychological
believe that it is acceptable to have intercourse disturbance (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2011; Tolman
before marriage as long as it takes place within the & McClelland, 2011). Adolescents who become sexually
context of a loving, intimate relationship. active earlier than their peers have levels of self-esteem
• Sexual intercourse, once delayed until early adult- and life satisfaction similar to those of other adoles-
hood, is clearly now a part of the typical adoles- cents (Goodson, Buhi, & Dunsmore, 2006; Vrangalova
cent’s experience, regardless of her or his ethnic or & Savin-Williams, 2011). Losing one’s virginity does not
socioeconomic background. have negative psychological repercussions, either in the
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 357 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 357

6
None Low High
5

0
with mother

with father

attendance

Peer sexual

Sexual

appeal

Sensation
monitoring

connectedness

expectations

Grades

maturity

Sexual
desire
attitudes
Religious

Physical
Closeness

seeking
behavior
Religious
Closeness

Parental

School
School

Characteristic
FIGURE 11.4 Characteristics that distinguish adolescents who are low, moderate, or high in their likelihood of becoming
sexually active. (L’Engle, Jackson, & Brown, 2006)

short or long term (Bingham & Crockett, 1996; Langer, Cavazos-Rehg et al., 2010; Harden & Mendle, 2011a;
Zimmerman, & Katz, 1995), even when one’s sexual Lohman & Billings, 2008; Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand,
debut is outside the context of a romantic relationship 2008). One study that measured adolescents’ likelihood
(Monahan & Lee, 2008). Thus, both the prejudice that of losing their virginity found that individuals who fit
only “troubled” adolescents have sex and the belief that the profile in 7th or 8th grade were 25 times more likely
sexual activity during adolescence leads to later psy- to lose their virginity within the next 2 years than those
chological disturbance are false (Goodson, Buhi, & who did not (L’Engle & Jackson, 2008; L’Engle, Jackson,
Dunsmore, 2006; A. M. Meier, 2007; Spriggs & Halpern, & Brown, 2006) (see Figure 11.4). In the United States,
2008). but not in all countries, early sexual intercourse is also
It is important, however, to distinguish between pre- associated with higher rates of depression (Madkour et al.,
dictors of being sexually active and predictors of engag- 2010b). In contrast, studies of adolescents who become
ing in risky sex (unprotected sex, sex with multiple sexually active at age 16 or later do not find major differ-
partners, etc.), which is associated with the same sorts ences between these youth and their virginal counter-
of psychological and behavioral factors correlated with parts. Less is known about characteristics of individuals
other forms of risk taking, as well as other risky behav- who do not have sex at all during adolescence, but the
iors (L. K. Brown et al., 2006; Crockett, Raffaelli, & most consistent correlate of abstaining from sex until
Shen, 2006; Dogan, Stockdale, Widaman, & Conger, after age 18 is strong religious commitment (Zimmer-
2010; Kirby, 2011; Lansford et al., 2010; Moilanen, Gembeck & Helfand, 2008).
Crockett, Raffaelli, & Jones, 2010a; Price & Hyde, 2011;
Strachman, Impett, Henson, & Pentz, 2009; Van Ryzin,
Johnson, Leve, & Kim, 2011). One factor that does not MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
appear to be correlated with risky sexual behavior, how-
Why do you think the psychological correlates of
ever, is exposure to pornography (Luder et al., 2011).
early sexual intercourse are different from the corre-
Although sexually active adolescents do not differ
lates of sexual intercourse when it is delayed until the
psychologically from those who are not, early sexual
last years of high school?
activity (that is, having intercourse before age 16) is as-
Would you be in favor of
sociated with a more general attitudinal and behavioral
sex education courses
profile that includes more permissive attitudes toward
whose focus was on per-
sex, experimentation with drugs and alcohol, minor
suadingadolescents to wait
delinquency, low levels of religious involvement, lower
until they were 16 before
interest in academic achievement, and a stronger orien-
having intercourse?
tation toward independence (Armour & Haynie, 2007;
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 358 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

358 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

found that engaging in delinquency and in casual sex


are influenced by some of the same genes, but that
delinquency and sex within a romantic relationship are
not, consistent with other research on the genetic bases
of traits like sensation-seeking and impulsivity (Harden &
Mendle, 2011c). Thus, it probably makes sense to view
sexual risk taking as a specific instance of risky behavior
more generally (Madkour, Farhat, Halpern, Godeau, &
Gabhainn, 2010a).
Another factor that affects adolescents’ sexual activ-
ity is the extent to which they are supervised by their
parents or other adults. Most sexual activity between
teenagers takes place in one of the two individuals’
homes—most often, the boy’s. (The third most popular
setting is at the home of a friend.) And the most com-
mon time for adolescents to have sex is not on the
weekend, but on weekdays, after school. Not surpris-
ingly, then, adolescents who are unsupervised after
school and who do not participate in after-school pro-
grams are more likely to be sexually active, more likely
Research shows that adolescents who are sexually active are no to have multiple sexual partners, and more likely to
more likely to have psychological problems than their nonactive contract an STD (Buhi & Goodson, 2007; D. Cohen,
peers. However, engaging in risky sexual activity is associated with
Farley, Taylor, Martin, & Schuster, 2002).
involvement in other forms of reckless behavior.

HORMONAL AND CONTEXTUAL


INFLUENCES ON SEXUAL ACTIVITY
❚ Causation or Correlation? Although many
studies have found a link between early sexual activity One factor that is consistently related to the age at which
and small-scale deviance, the nature of the causal chain adolescents initiate sex is physical maturation; adoles-
is not entirely clear (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009; cents who mature earlier are also likely to have sex ear-
Zimmer-Gembeck, Siebenbruner, & Collins, 2004). lier (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009; Lam et al., 2002;
Several studies show that involvement in deviance B. Miller, Norton, Fan, & Christopherson, 1998). In-
(especially alcohol and drug use, but aggression as well) creased interest in sex at adolescence is likely to have
precedes early involvement with sex (C. Mitchell, social as well as biological causes, however. Specifically,
Whitesell, Spicer, Beals, & Kaufman, 2007; Schofield, adolescents are thought to become interested in sex in
Bierman, Heinrichs, & Nix, 2008; Strachman et al., part because of increases in sex hormones at puberty
2009; T. Williams, Connolly, & Cribbie, 2008), whereas and in part because sexual activity becomes accepted—
others suggest that experimentation with deviant activ- even encouraged—in their peer group. A fuller under-
ity and early sex occur simultaneously and may reflect standing of adolescent sexual behavior necessitates
some common underlying factor, such as the propen- looking at biological and social influences in interaction
sity to take risks (e.g., Bingham & Crockett, 1996; with each other, rather than at either set of influences
Harden, Mendle, Hill, Turkheimer, & Emery, 2008). (In alone (Halpern, 2003). Moreover, the story line is some-
contrast, there is little support for the notion that early what different for males than females.
sex leads to other types of risky or antisocial activity.)
As you’ll read in Chapter 13, many experts believe that ❚ Hormonal Influences Boys’ and girls’ initial in-
a general inclination toward problem behavior and lack terest in sex is influenced primarily by the surge in certain
of impulse control is behind an overarching pattern hormones—testosterone, to be specific—at puberty.
that includes minor delinquency, precocious or pro- Adolescents with higher levels of androgens (testoster-
miscuous sex, disengagement from school, and drug one is an androgen) are more likely than their peers to
and alcohol use (Crockett et al., 2006; Rosenthal, Smith, report masturbating, thinking about sex, and planning
& de Visser, 1999; Valois, Oeltmann, Waller, & Hussey, to have sexual intercourse within the next year. This
1999). Consistent with this view, one recent study hormonal change appears to increase adolescents’ in-
terest in sex as well as their arousal when exposed to
sexual stimuli. This is true for both males and females,
although females’ interest in sex is also influenced by
testosterone One of the sex hormones secreted by the gonads, estrogen.
found in both sexes but in higher levels among males than females.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 359 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 359

Motivation to have sex is one thing; becoming sexu- ❚ The Role of Context Numerous studies show
ally active is another. How important is the rise in tes- that social factors are far more important in influencing
tosterone at puberty in triggering the onset of sexual girls’ involvement in sexual intercourse than boys’
intercourse? The answer differs between boys and girls. (Crockett, Bingham, Chopak, & Vicary, 1996; D. B. Henry,
Among boys, the increased level of androgens is di- Schoeny, Deptula, & Slavick, 2007). Although increases
rectly related to the likelihood of being sexually active in androgens lead to increased interest in sex among
(B. C. Campbell, Prossinger, & Mbzivo, 2005). Younger girls, and although increases in estrogens lead to in-
boys who are more mature biologically are more creased attractiveness to boys, whether this interest and
likely to be sexually active than older boys whose hor- attractiveness are translated into behavior depends
mone levels are lower. Early-maturing boys also are largely on the social environment (Diamond & Savin-
more likely than their peers to download pornography Williams, 2009). Among girls with high levels of andro-
off the Internet (Skoog, Stattin, & Kerr, 2009). gens, for example, those who have sexually permissive
Whether boys actually have sex is not entirely de- attitudes and whose friends are sexually active are more
pendent on their hormone levels, however, because likely to engage in intercourse. But girls whose social
actually having sex depends on how receptive girls are environment is less encouraging of sex—even those
to them. Not surprisingly, boys who are more popular with high levels of androgens—are unlikely to be sexu-
with girls in their school are more likely to initiate sex ally active. In other words, whereas hormones seem to
early than are boys who are less popular with girls. As have a direct and powerful effect on the sexual behavior
you know from Chapter 1, androgens, including testos- of boys, the impact of hormones on the sexual behavior
terone, do not just contribute to increases in boys’ sex of girls seems to depend on the social context.
drive—they also affect height, strength, and the devel- Why might this be? One explanation is that boys de-
opment of secondary sex characteristics, like facial hair. velop in an environment that is more uniformly toler-
(Remember that one role of secondary sex characteris- ant and encouraging of sexual behavior than girls do.
tics is to signal to others that the individual is ready for All that boys need to become sexually active is the bio-
sex.) In other words, increases in androgens lead to logical jolt from the increase in androgens at puberty—
boys’ increased sexual activity both because they in- there is nothing in the environment to hold them back.
crease their sex drive (which may make boys with For girls, however, the environment is more varied.
higher testosterone levels want to have sex more) and Some girls develop within a context that permits and
because they change their physical appearance (which even encourages sexual activity; others do not. Al-
may make them more attractive to girls). though the increase in androgens also provides a jolt to
Hormonal influences on sexual desire and physical the sex drive of the adolescent girl, and although the
appearance are easier to separate in girls. Although an- increase in estrogens makes her more attractive to
drogens also are responsible for increases in girls’ sex boys, if she develops within a context that places strong
drive, a different set of hormones—estrogens—is pri- social controls on sexual activity, this hormonal awak-
marily responsible for changes in their appearance, in- ening will not be translated into sexual activity. Among
cluding breast development. Because of this, it is the most important forces in this context are parents
possible to study whether increased sexual activity and peers.
among girls after puberty is more influenced by in-
creases in their sex drive or by changes in their physical
PARENTAL AND PEER INFLUENCES
appearance (both of which, presumably, influence their
ON SEXUAL ACTIVITY
sexual attractiveness to boys). It turns out that differ-
ences in adolescent girls’ sexual activity have little to do Many researchers have asked whether adolescents who
with differences in their androgen levels but is corre- become sexually active earlier than their peers have dif-
lated with differences in their estrogen levels. Although ferent sorts of relationships with their parents or their
it was once believed that estrogen played an important peers. The answer is clear: Not surprisingly, given the cor-
role in influencing girls’ receptivity to boys’ advances, relation between early sexual activity and other forms of
it turns out that this is not the case (Diamond & problem behavior, most studies have found that
Savin-Williams, 2009). Estrogen influences girls’ sexual adolescents from authoritative homes—that is, homes
activity mainly through its impact on their attractive- where parents are warm, are involved in their adolescent’s
ness to boys. life, and monitor their adolescent’s behavior—are less
Of course, girls’ involvement in sex is not solely de- likely to become sexually active at an early age and less
termined by whether boys want them as sexual part- likely to engage in risky sexual activity, such as having
ners. Girls’ own interest in sex and their receptivity to unprotected sex (Capaldi, Stoolmiller, Clark, & Owen,
boys’ interest in them are also crucial. But in girls, it 2002; de Graaf, Vanwesenbeeck, Meijer, Woertman, &
turns out, these factors are much more determined by Meeus, 2009; Frisco, 2005; Huebner & Howell, 2003; Kan,
context than by biology. Cheng, Landale, & McHale, 2010; Manlove, Logan,
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 360 15/03/13 8:12 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

360 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Positive view
Suboptimal Academic of risk-taking Risk-taking Sexual
parenting disengagement peers peers risk-taking
FIGURE 11.5 The antecedents of risky sexual behavior are similar to the antecedents of risk taking more generally
(Kogan et al., 2011)

Moore, & Ikramullah, 2008). Parent–adolescent conflict parent–adolescent communication about sex is no
is also associated with early sexual activity, especially more open in Europe than in the United States
among adolescents who are relatively more mature phys- (Arnett, 2002). Across cultures, however, adolescents
ically (McBride, Paikoff, & Holmbeck, 2003). These are more likely to be well educated about sex when
strong and consistent links between effective parenting their conversations with their parents are genuinely
and safer sexual behavior have been found across ethnic interactive, rather than dominated by the parents
groups (Biddlecom, Awusabo-Asare, & Bankole, 2009; (Lefkowitz, Romo, Corona, Au, & Sigman, 2000). This
Meschke, Bartholomae, & Zentall, 2000; B. Miller, is important because conversations between mothers
Benson, & Galbraith, 2001). Consistent with the idea that and daughters about sex tend to be more interactive
the underpinnings of risky sexual behavior are the un- than conversations between mothers and sons
derpinnings of risky behavior more generally, one recent (Lefkowitz, Boone, Sigman, & Kit-fong Au, 2002).
study of Black youth found that sexual risk taking was the Perhaps because of this, parent–adolescent communi-
outcome of a pathway that included suboptimal parent- cation about sex is more predictive of daughters’ be-
ing, academic disengagement, and affiliation with risk- havior than of sons’ (McNeely et al., 2002). Although
taking peers (Kogan et al., 2011) (see Figure 11.5). there is not a great deal of research on ethnic differ-
ences in parent–adolescent communication about
❚ Parent–Adolescent Communication A great sex, it does appear as if conversations about sex are
deal of attention has been devoted by researchers to the very infrequent in Asian American homes (J. L. Kim &
study of parent–adolescent communication about sex, Ward, 2007).
although it is quite clear from this research that any It is often assumed that it is beneficial for adoles-
conclusions you might draw about the nature and cents to discuss sex with their parents; however, the ef-
impact of these conversations depends entirely on fect of parent–child communication on adolescents’
whom you ask. Many more parents report communi- sexual behavior depends on who is doing the commu-
cating with their adolescent about sex than vice versa. nicating and what is being communicated. Overall,
In addition, parents often say that they have communi- most studies find that the impact of parent–adolescent
cated about a particular topic (such as AIDS) when communication on the likelihood of an adolescent be-
their teenager says they have not (K. Miller, Kotchick, ing sexually active is very small (B. Miller et al., 2001;
Dorsey, Forehand, & Ham, 1998). Other discrepancies Paikoff et al., 1997), although parent–child communi-
abound as well. For instance, parents underestimate cation specifically about contraception lowers the rate
their adolescents’ sexual activity and unrealistically as- of risky sex (Aspy et al., 2007; Donenberg, Emerson, &
sume that if they disapprove of sexual activity, the ado- Mackesy-Amiti, 2011; Hutchinson, Jemmott, Jemmott,
lescents are not likely to be sexually active; on the other Braverman, & Fong, 2003; K. Miller & Whitaker, 2001),
hand, sexually active adolescents underestimate their especially if the discussions take place before the ado-
parents’ disapproval of sexual activity (Jaccard, Dittus, lescent becomes sexually active (K. Miller, Levin,
& Gordon, 1998). Generally, teenagers are more likely Whitaker, & Xu, 1998). Communication with older sib-
to talk about sex with mothers than fathers, and they lings about safe sex is also effective (Kolburn Kowal &
rate their mothers as better sex educators (DiIorio, Blinn-Pike, 2004).
Kelley, & Hockenberry-Eaton, 1999; S. Feldman & A growing body of research suggests that what is
Rosenthal, 2000; Raffaelli & Green, 2003). Adoles- most important are the attitudes and values communi-
cents also are likely to be more receptive to having cated by parents during discussions of sex and, of course,
multiple conversations over a period of time about the ways in which these attitudes and values are inter-
sex than to having one “big talk” (Martino, Elliott, preted by the adolescent (Bersamin, Walker, Waiters,
Corona, Kanouse, & Schuster, 2008). Fisher, & Grube, 2005; Jaccard, Dodge, & Dittus, 2003b;
Most discussions about sex between parents and Longmore, Eng, Giordano, & Manning, 2009; B. Miller
teenagers focus on issues of safety (AIDS, condom et al., 2001). Among girls with liberal parents, for in-
use) rather than issues of sexual behavior or relation- stance, talking about sex is associated with more sexual
ships (DiIorio et al., 1999; K. Miller, Levin, Whitaker, activity; this is not true among girls with parents who
& Xu, 1998). And, despite the progressive image of disapprove of premarital sex, however (Dittus & Jaccard,
countries like the Netherlands, studies indicate that 2000; Fingerson, 2005; Usher-Seriki, Bynum, & Callands,
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 361 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 361

2008). In Hispanic families, in which parental attitudes early involvement with drugs, alcohol, and minor de-
about sex tend to be relatively more conservative, com- linquency, which, according to some studies, in-
munication with adolescents about values and beliefs creases the likelihood of sex. In other words, it is not
concerning sex is associated with less sexual activity, but family structure per se, but the quality of family re-
the degree to which parents directly caution their teen- lationships in divorced homes, that helps explain
agers against sex does not seem to make a difference why girls from single-parent homes are more sexu-
(Romo, Lefkowitz, Sigman, & Au, 2002). In addition, ally active at an earlier age (E. Davis & Friel, 2001).
adolescents who speak regularly with their parents about Another possibility is that some of the same person-
sex are more likely to turn down unwanted sex when ality characteristics that are associated with adults’
they are pressured by others (Sionéan et al., 2002). More- marital instability, like impulsivity and substance
over, studies find that it is important for parents to abuse, are transmitted genetically from parents to
maintain a close relationship with their teenager after children, making adolescents with divorced parents
the adolescent has become sexually active and to resist more likely to engage in early sex; in other words, it
the temptation to pull away in anger over the teenager’s may not only be what divorced parents do, but who
behavior (G. Ream & Savin-Williams, 2005). they are (Mendle et al., 2009). The fact that the im-
It is important to note that parent–adolescent com- pact of family structure is stronger among girls than
munication about sex is more effective in deterring boys suggests that something in addition to, or in
risky sexual activity than in promoting abstinence, combination with, genetic transmission is likely go-
and even here, the effect that parents have is small. ing on.
Thus, despite some parents’ beliefs that they can pre- Why should growing up in a single-parent home
vent their adolescent’s sexual activity by talking about affect girls’ sexual behavior more than boys’, though?
it, and despite other parents’ fears that talking about At least four possibilities exist. One, as noted earlier,
sex will have the unintended effect of encouraging is that social influences on girls’ sexual behavior are
their teenager’s sexual behavior, studies show that stronger and more varied than are the influences on
parent–adolescent communication about sex has sur- boys’ behavior. Parents simply may not attempt to ex-
prisingly little impact on whether adolescents are sex- ert much control over sons’ sexual activity, regardless
ually active, one way or the other. While it is possible
to teach parents how to talk more effectively to their
teenagers about sex, it is not clear that doing so leads
to dramatic changes in teenagers’ knowledge, atti-
tudes, or behavior (Lefkowitz, Sigman, & Au, 2000).
Adolescents’ opportunity to have sex (for example,
whether they are in a steady relationship or date fre-
quently), their having sexually active friends, and their
use of alcohol and drugs are far better predictors of
early sexual initiation than is parent–adolescent com-
munication (M. Black, Ricardo, & Stanton, 1997;
B. Miller et al., 1997; Whitbeck, Yoder, Hoyt, & Conger,
1999).

❚ Sexual Activity and Household Composition


One family factor that does predict adolescent sexual
involvement, however—especially among girls—is
household composition. Researchers consistently find
that adolescents whose parents are in the process of
divorcing as well as girls who live in single-parent
households—regardless of when (or if) a divorce took
place—are more likely to be sexually active earlier than
their peers (Crockett et al., 1996; Ellis et al., 2003;
Hogan, Sun, & Cornwell, 2000; Lammers, Ireland,
Resnick, & Blum, 2000; B. Miller et al., 1997; Wu &
Thomson, 2001). One hypothesis, consistent with what
we know about divorce and parent–adolescent relation- Talking to teenagers about sex does not increase their likelihood
ships, is that parental divorce temporarily disrupts the of becoming sexually active, but may encourage adolescents to
parent–child relationship, leading the adolescent into practice safe sex.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 362 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

362 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

of whether the household has one parent or two, and they have older siblings who model more sexually
as a result, boys from single- and two-parent homes advanced behavior (East, 2009). Consistent with this,
may be equally likely to be sexually active. Girls’ sex- although religious involvement deters adolescents’
ual behavior, in contrast, may be more subject sexual activity, regular church attendance is associ-
to parental controls. Single-parent homes are typi- ated with delayed sexual activity only among adoles-
cally more permissive than two-parent homes cents whose friends attend the same church (Mott et
(Laursen & Collins, 2009), and this difference in con- al., 1996). Importantly, adolescents whose parents
trol may be enough to make a difference in girls’ sex- discuss sex with them in an open and understanding
ual activity. way are less influenced by having sexually active
A second possibility is that many single-parent peers (Fasula & Miller, 2006; Whitaker & Miller,
mothers are likely to be dating and, in so doing, may 2000).
inadvertently be role models of sexual activity to their Peer influences on adolescents’ sexual activity ap-
adolescents (Whitbeck, Simons, & Kao, 1994). Other re- pear to operate in two different, but compatible,
search shows, for example, that adolescents whose ways. First, when an adolescent’s peers are sexually
mothers had been sexually active at an early age are active, they establish a normative standard that hav-
themselves more likely to begin having sex early (Mott, ing sex is acceptable (Gillmore et al., 2002; Kogan et
Fondell, Hu, Kowaleski-Jones, & Menaghan, 1996). To al., 2011; Sieving, Eisenberg, Pettingell, & Skay, 2006).
the extent that this modeling effect is stronger between One of the reasons that minor drug use is associated
parents and children of the same sex, we would expect with earlier involvement in sexual activity is that
to find a more powerful effect of growing up in a drug use may lead an adolescent to form friendships
single-parent home on the sexual behavior of daughters with a different group of friends, a group that is sex-
than sons. ually more permissive(D. French & Dishion, 2003;
Yet a third possibility is that girls are more likely Whitbeck, Conger, Simons, & Kao, 1993). Consistent
than boys to respond to problems at home by going with this, adolescents’ initiation of sexual activity
outside the family for alternative sources of warmth varies from neighborhood to neighborhood, with
and support; if their family environment is not satisfy- earlier sexual activity more likely in relatively more
ing, girls (whether in divorced homes or not) may be disorganized neighborhoods, where adults have little
more likely than boys to seek the attention of a roman- control over teenagers and where peer groups are
tic partner (Whitbeck, Hoyt, Miller, & Kao, 1992). relatively more powerful (T. Leventhal, Dupéré, &
During or immediately following their parents’ divorce, Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Upchurch, Aneshensel, Sucoff,
girls may seek the support of individuals with whom & Levy-Storms, 1999). Generally, the more individ-
they become sexually involved. ual risk factors there are in an adolescent’s life for
Finally, some researchers suggest that the link be- involvement in early sexual activity (for example,
tween growing up in a single-parent household and drug and alcohol use, poor parental monitoring, sex-
earlier involvement in sex is genetic in a way that is spe- ually active friends, antisocial peers, disengagement
cific to girls. They have shown that the same gene that from school, a disadvantaged neighborhood), the
makes men more likely to leave their family may, when more likely the adolescent is to be sexually active
passed on to daughters, make adolescent girls more (Small & Luster, 1994). The factors that place adoles-
likely to go through puberty early and become sexually cents at risk for early sexual activity are the same
active at an earlier age (Comings, Muhleman, Johnson, across ethnic groups (D. Perkins, Luster, Villarruel, &
& MacMurray, 2002). Small, 1998).
Peers also influence each other’s sexual behavior
❚ Influences Other Than Parents Additional directly, either through communication among
studies have examined the influence of forces other friends (“You haven’t done it yet! What’s the matter
than parents on adolescents’ sexual behavior. Gener- with you?” “You’re thinking of doing what?”) or,
ally, adolescents are more likely to be sexually active more commonly, between potential sex partners.
when their peers are (and more likely to engage in Several studies show that sexual activity spreads
risky sex when their peers do) (East, Felice, & Morgan, within a community of adolescents much like an
1993; D. B. Henry et al., 2007); when they believe that epidemic, with sexually experienced adolescents ini-
their friends are sexually active, whether or not their tiating their less experienced partners into increas-
friends actually are (Babalola, 2004; DiIorio et al., ingly more advanced sex (J. Rodgers & Rowe, 1993).
2001; Prinstein, Meade, & Cohen, 2003); and when Once they become sexually experienced, previously
inexperienced adolescents then “infect” other ado-
lescents. Over time, the percentage of sexually expe-
rienced adolescents within a community grows and
risk factors Factors that increase the likelihood of some behavior grows.
or condition.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 363 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 363

❚ Virginity Pledges Studies of “virginity pledges”


also shed light on the way in which the social context
can influence adolescent sexual activity (Bearman &
Brückner, 2001; Rosenbaum, 2009). Over the past
25 years, several million American adolescents have
taken a virginity pledge, promising to abstain from sex
until they are married. How effective is this? Research
finds that virginity pledges work only for younger
adolescents—they have no effect on high school
students. Among high school students, “pledgers” are
just as likely to have sex (including intercourse, oral sex,
and anal sex) as “nonpledgers.” And longitudinal stud-
ies show that after having sex, adolescents who had
taken a virginity pledge frequently deny having made
one (Hollander, 2006; Rosenbaum, 2006). One study,
which drew on longitudinal data from a large represen-
tative sample of American adolescents, found that 82%
of the adolescents who took a virginity pledge denied
having done so 5 years later (Rosenbaum, 2009)! Ironi- Over the past two decades, several million American adolescents
have taken a virginity pledge, promising to abstain from sex until
cally, one way in which those who take a virginity pledge they are married. Research has found, however, that high school
differ from their nonpledging peers is that those who students who have pledged to remain virgins are just as likely to
take the pledge are less likely to use contraception, have sex as those who haven’t made such pledges.
suggesting that encouraging abstinence may actually
promote unsafe sex (as you will read later, it does)
(Rosenbaum, 2009). Perhaps more interestingly, the SEX DIFFERENCES IN THE
impact of pledging varies as a function of how many MEANING OF SEX
other adolescents in the same school have taken the Any discussion of the psychosocial significance of sexual
pledge. Pledging has little effect in schools in which few experience during adolescence must be sensitive to the
students take virginity pledges (presumably because very substantial sex differences in how early sexual ac-
there is little social support for abstinence) or in schools tivity is experienced. Despite the convergence of males’
in which nearly everyone pledges (because one of the and females’ rates of sexual activity in recent decades,
ways in which pledging works is by allowing those who the early sexual experiences of adolescent boys and girls
pledge to make a statement about their values). By the are still very different and, as a consequence, are imbued
way, it turns out that making a promise to oneself to with very different meanings (Diamond & Savin-
delay becoming sexually active is more effective than Williams, 2009). In other words, the sexual behavior of
making a formal, public pledge (Bersamin, Walker, males and females may be similar, but the sexual social-
Waiters, Fisher, & Grube, 2005). ization of males and females is quite different.
Finally, several studies have examined the role of the
broader environment in influencing adolescent sexual
❚ The Way Boys Feel The typical boy’s first sexual
behavior. Adolescents growing up in poor neighbor-
experience is masturbation in early adolescence (Diamond
hoods, for example, are more likely to engage in early
& Savin-Williams, 2009). At the outset, then, the sexual
sexual activity than adolescents from more affluent
socialization of males typically places sex outside of an
communities (Dupéré, Lacourse, Willms, Leventhal, &
interpersonal context. Before adolescent boys begin dat-
Tremblay, 2008; T. Leventhal et al., 2009). When adoles-
ing, they have generally already experienced orgasm and
cents grow up in poverty, they may see little hope for
know how to arouse themselves. For males, the develop-
the future, and they therefore may be more likely to risk
ment of sexuality during adolescence revolves around ef-
their occupational and economic future by becoming
forts to integrate the capacity to form close relationships
sexually active (Benda & Corwyn, 1998). To a young
into an already-existing sense of sexual capability.
person who believes that the chances of getting a good
Perhaps because of this, at the time of first inter-
job are slim, an early pregnancy does not seem as costly
course, boys are likely to keep matters of sex and inti-
as it might to someone who hopes to complete high
macy separate. Boys often have as their first partner
school, attend college, and secure a good job. Adoles-
someone they just met or describe as a casual date, and
cents’ sexual behavior is more strongly influenced by
it is generally the male partner of a couple who is likely
the values and attitudes of their schoolmates than the
characteristics of their neighborhood, however (Teitler
& Weiss, 2000). sexual socialization The process through which adolescents are
exposed to and educated about sexuality.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 364 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

364 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

to initiate sex (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). These one she says she was in love with at the time (Diamond
findings suggest that the early sexual experiences of & Savin-Williams, 2009). After having intercourse for
males are often interpreted not in terms of intimacy and the first time, she is more likely to encounter disap-
emotional involvement, but in terms of recreation proval or mixed feelings on the part of others in whom
(Hendrick & Hendrick, 1994). Consistent with this, boys she confides (generally, peers) than is the typical boy
are more likely than girls to mention sexual arousal (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). And although the
(rather than emotional factors) as a reason for having majority of girls report more positive than negative
sex (Eyre & Millstein, 1999). And males typically report feelings about their first sexual experience, girls are
that the people to whom they describe their first sexual more likely than boys to report feeling afraid, guilty, and
liaison—most probably, male peers—are overwhelm- worried as well as happy or excited about the experience
ingly approving. Not surprisingly, the most common (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009; Oswald et al., 1994).
immediate reactions among adolescent males to having It is important to keep in mind that differences be-
intercourse for the first time are excitement, satisfac- tween males and females in the meaning of sex are nei-
tion, exhilaration, and happiness (Diamond & Savin- ther inevitable nor consistent across cultures or
Williams, 2009; Oswald, Bahne, & Feder, 1994; ). historical time. Nor is it the case that all adolescent boys
follow the male “script” and all adolescent girls follow
the female “script.” As Lisa Diamond and Ritch Savin-
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION Williams, two experts on adolescent sexuality, noted,
If you are sexually experienced, see if you can “Girls are more sexually oriented and boys more
recall the first time you had sex. What was romantically oriented than previous research might
your immediate reaction? suggest” (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009, p. 514).
Moreover, as they mature, adolescent boys and girls be-
come more similar in their motives to have sex (males
increasingly emphasize the place of sex in an emotional
relationship and place less importance on the role of
❚ The Way Girls Feel The typical girl’s first experi- sex in elevating their social status, whereas females
ence and feelings afterward are likely to be very different. become less likely to justify their interest in sex solely in
Masturbation is far less prevalent among girls than terms of their intimate relationships).
boys, and it is far less regularly practiced (Diamond &
Savin-Williams, 2009). As a consequence, the typical
adolescent girl, in contrast to the typical boy, is more MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
likely to experience sex for the first time with another Can you think of any evolutionary expla-
person. For girls, unlike boys, the development of sexu- nations for the different ways in which
ality involves the integration of sexual activity into an males and females react to their first sex-
already existing capacity for intimacy and emotional ual experiences?
involvement. As a consequence, the girl’s sexual script
is one that, from the outset, tinges sex with romance,
love, friendship, and intimacy. Girls are more likely
than boys to engage in sex in order to enhance an emo-
tional connection (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009;
Hendrick & Hendrick, 1994;). SEXUAL ORIENTATION
Boys and girls also encounter very different social at- ❚ Same-Sex Attraction It is not uncommon for
titudes about sex. Because of the possibility of preg- young adolescents to engage in sex play with members of
nancy, the potential adverse consequences of sexual the same sex, to have sexual fantasies about people of the
activity are far more serious for girls than for boys. For same sex, or to have questions about the nature of their
this reason, society monitors the sexual activity of girls feelings for same-sex peers (Diamond & Savin-Williams,
more carefully, and girls are more likely to be encour- 2009). According to the national (and confidential) Add
aged to approach sex cautiously (Rosenthal, 1994). Girls’ Health survey, about 6% of boys and 13% of girls re-
feelings of sexual desire are “tempered by their knowl- ported having had same-sex attractions, a nonhetero-
edge that girls are not supposed to be sexual or that if sexual orientation, or engaging in same-sex activity
they are, they will be marked as bad and unlovable” during adolescence. A smaller number of adolescents—
(Tolman, 1993, p. 4). Perhaps because of this, girls have between 2% and 5%—identify themselves as gay, les-
an easier time saying no to unwanted sex than boys do bian, or bisexual, and this number increases to about
(R. Zimmerman, Sprecher, Langer, & Holloway, 1995). 10% among adults (IOM and NRC, 2011b). Between 2%
Not surprisingly, then, at the time of first intercourse, and 3% of adolescents describe themselves as “unsure”
the adolescent girl’s sexual partner is likely to be some- of their sexual orientation; when they are a bit older,
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 365 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 365

about two-thirds of these individuals describe them- heterosexual or homosexual orientation, the reality is
selves as exclusively heterosexual (Ott, Corliss, Wypij, much more complicated” (2009, p. 505).
Rosario, & Austin, 2011; Zhao, Montoro, Igartua, & It is important to distinguish between homosexu-
Thombs, 2010). ality as an exclusive preference and homosexuality as
A great deal of confusion about homosexuality results an interest that may exist simultaneously with strong
because people sometimes confuse sexual orientation heterosexual interests (Diamond & Savin-Williams,
(whether one is sexually attracted to individuals of the 2009; Russell, Thompson, & Harris, 2011). Many
same sex, the other sex, or both), sex-role behavior (the people mistakenly view sexual orientation as an
extent to which an individual behaves in traditionally “either-or” attribute, with individuals being either
masculine or feminine ways), and gender identity (the exclusively heterosexual or exclusively homosexual.
gender an individual believes he or she is psychologically, In fact, however, of the individuals who do not
regardless of the sex he or she was labeled at birth, usu- develop an exclusive preference for heterosexual
ally on the basis of the infant’s genitals). There is no con- relationships (about 10% of the adult population),
nection between an adolescent’s sexual orientation and only one-third are exclusively homosexual in their
his or her sex-role behavior or gender identity. Individu- orientation. Twice as many describe themselves as
als with strong, or even exclusive, preferences for homo- bisexual—having both heterosexual and homosexual
sexual relationships exhibit the same range of masculine interests. Between 2% and 3% of adult men and
and feminine behaviors seen among individuals with women describe themselves as exclusively homosex-
strong or exclusive heterosexual interests. In other words, ual (IOM and NRC, 2011b).
exclusively gay men (like exclusively heterosexual men) The development of sexual orientation follows dif-
may act in very masculine, very feminine, or both mas- ferent patterns among sexual minority males and fe-
culine and feminine ways, and the same holds true for males (Saewyc, 2011). Males are more likely to have had
exclusively lesbian and exclusively heterosexual women, same-sex relations before identifying themselves as gay
as it does for bisexual individuals. Along similar lines, or bisexual, whereas the reverse sequence is more char-
gender identity and sexual orientation also have nothing acteristic among females. And whereas more lesbian
to do with each other. Individuals who are attracted to and bisexual females had heterosexual experiences be-
people of the same sex are no more or less likely than fore their first same-sex sexual activity, the opposite is
exclusively heterosexual individuals to have a gender true for males. In addition, females who have had same-
identity that does not match their assigned sex. sex contact during adolescence almost always pursue
Although individuals who describe themselves as same-sex contact in adulthood (whereas the same is not
transgender—that is, whose gender identity does not true for males—only about 60% do) (Diamond &
match the sex they were assigned at birth—are often Savin-Williams, 2009).
grouped for purposes of discussion with lesbian, gay,
and bisexual youth (see Chapter 10), transgender indi- ❚ Origins of Homosexuality Studies of the ante-
viduals report the same variety of sexual orientations as cedents of homosexuality generally have focused on
do other individuals. Information on the size of the two sets of factors: biological influences, such as hor-
transgender population, either in adolescence or adult- mones, and social influences, such as the parent–child
hood, is scant. One recent report estimates that about relationship. More is known about the development of
1 in 100,000 American adults are transsexual women homosexuality among men than among women, but
(individuals who identify themselves as women but the weight of the evidence thus far suggests that an ado-
who were labeled as male at birth) and 1 in 400,000 are lescent’s sexual orientation is likely to be shaped by a
transsexual men (individuals who identify themselves complex interaction of social and biological influences
as men but who were labeled as female at birth) (IOM (Saewyc, 2011; Savin-Williams, 2006).
and NRC, 2011b). Support for the contention that homosexuality is de-
Researchers have not found consistent predictors termined at least partly by biological factors comes from
that distinguish individuals who experiment with two sources. You may recall that in Chapter 1 we distin-
same-sex relations in adolescence and who later iden- guished between the organizational and the activational
tify themselves as gay, lesbian, or bisexual from those
whose experimentation during adolescence is passing
sexual orientation Whether one is sexually attracted to
and who later identify themselves as exclusively hetero-
individuals of the same sex, other sex, or both.
sexual. By the same token, the majority of gay, lesbian,
and bisexual adults engaged in heterosexual activity sex-role behavior Behavior that is consistent with prevailing
during adolescence. As Diamond and Savin-Williams expectations for how individuals of a given sex are to behave.
note, “contrary to the widespread notion that desire, gender identity The gender an individual identifies with.
behavior, and identity coalesce neatly in adolescence
and young adulthood to signal an unambiguously transgender Describing individuals whose gender identity does
not match the sex they were assigned at birth.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 366 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

366 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

rejected as an artifact of popular stereotype and poor re-


search designs. But more carefully designed studies have
offered at least partial confirmation of this notion. Both
bisexual and homosexual adolescents are more likely
than heterosexuals to describe their fathers as distant and
rejecting (Bos, Sandfort, de Bruyn, & Hakvoort, 2008;
Busseri, Willoughby, Chalmers, & Bogaert, 2008). Whereas
gay men are more likely than heterosexuals to report hav-
ing had close and generally positive relationships with
their mothers, lesbians are more likely than heterosexuals
to describe their mothers as cold and unpleasant (A. Bell,
Weinberg, & Hammersmith, 1981). However, gay and les-
bian adolescents are no more likely than heterosexual
youth to have had gay or lesbian parents—in fact, studies
of adolescents with same-sex parents find few differences
between them and their peers with opposite-sex parents
(Patterson, 2009).
Although these studies point to certain factors that
appear more often than not in the early histories of gay,
lesbian, or bisexual individuals, all homosexual indi-
viduals do not have identical developmental histories.
For example, although, on average, homosexuals are
more likely than heterosexuals to describe their parents
in negative terms, not all gay and lesbian individuals
feel this way. Indeed, only about half do, suggesting that
a large number of gay and lesbian individuals had
quite positive family relationships growing up. And,
of course, many heterosexuals describe their parents
Sexual orientation is influenced by a complex interplay of biological in exceedingly negative terms. Similarly, although the
and contextual factors. majority of boys with persistently feminine behavior
preferences may grow up to be gay, a substantial num-
ber of feminine boys do not.
roles of hormones on behavior. The hormonal changes
of puberty activate sexual behavior, but the particular ❚ Development of Sexual Identity Several writ-
pattern of sexual behavior that is activated may depend ers have described the process through which gay, les-
on the way in which hormonal pathways in the brain bian, and bisexual individuals discover, come to terms
were organized early in life. There is suggestive evidence with, and disclose their sexual-minority identity
that gay and lesbian adults may have been exposed pre- (Diamond, 2008; Savin-Williams & Ream, 2007).
natally to certain hormones that, in theory, could affect Although the traditional model of this progression—
sexual orientation and gender-atypical behavioral pref- feeling different as a child, engaging in gender-atypical
erences through their effects on early brain organization behavior, being attracted to members of the same sex
(Saewyc, 2011). Second, some evidence indicates that and uninterested in those of the other sex, realizing
homosexuality has a strong genetic component, since one’s sexual attraction to others of the same sex, and
sexual orientation is more likely to be similar among consciously questioning one’s sexual identity—
close relatives than distant relatives and between identi- describes the experience of many sexual-minority ado-
cal twins than fraternal twins (Savin-Williams, 2006). lescents, it by no means is universal. Indeed, some
Although environmental explanations for this similar- writers have suggested that this may be more applica-
ity cannot be ruled out, chances are that at least some of ble to the development of White gay men than to lesbi-
the predisposition to develop a homosexual orientation ans, bisexual adolescents, or ethnic minority gay men
is inherited (Saewyc, 2011). (Diamond, 1998; Dubé & Savin-Williams, 1999). For
Several studies suggest as well that a higher proportion example, there is evidence that females’ sexual orienta-
of homosexuals than heterosexuals report having had tion may be more fluid than males’, with many bisexual
problems in their early family relationships—specifically, or lesbian late adolescents changing sexual orientation
in their relationship with their father. The stereotype of during young adulthood (Diamond, 2008; Saewyc,
the homosexual’s father as cold and distant once was 2011).
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 367 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 367

that the establishment of gay–straight alliances within


MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
schools have had positive effects on school climate
Should social scientists be interested in
(Walls, Kane, & Wisneski, 2010).
the antecedents of homosexuality? Is it
important to know whether homosexu-
ality is biologically or contextually SEXUAL HARASSMENT, RAPE, AND
determined? Why or why not? SEXUAL ABUSE DURING ADOLESCENCE
❚ Sexual Harassment and Date Rape Although
most research on adolescent sexual activity has focused
on voluntary sexual behavior between consenting indi-
viduals, there is growing public awareness that a large
Society’s prejudice and ignorance about homosexual- proportion of teenagers are sexually harassed and that a
ity likely cause significant psychological distress for significant minority are forced to have sex against their
LGBT adolescents, especially if they encounter hostility will (Chiodo, Wolfe, Crooks, Hughes, & Jaffe, 2009;
from those around them (Saewyc, 2011; T. E. Smith & V. Lee, Croninger, Linn, & Chen, 1996). This latter
Leaper, 2006). As you know by now, the developmental group includes adolescents who have been the victims
tasks in the domains of identity, intimacy, and sexuality of forcible rape by a stranger, sexual abuse within the
present formidable challenges for many teenagers. These family, or date rape—when a young person, typically a
challenges may be exacerbated for sexual-minority ado- woman, is forced by a date to have sex when she does
lescents, who are forced to resolve these issues without not want to. Sexual coercion and sex under the influ-
the same degree of social support as their heterosexual ence of alcohol or drugs are more likely to occur when
peers (Diamond & Lucas, 2004). (Interestingly, adolescents there is a large age difference (3 years or more) be-
are more intolerant about peers behaving in gender- tween a girl and her partner (Gowen, Feldman, Diaz, &
atypical ways than they are about sexual orientation Yisrael, 2004).
[Horn, 2007; Toomey, Ryan, Diaz, Card, & Russell, For all the concern that is expressed about the sexual
2010].) Indeed, many studies have found that a substan- harassment of teenagers over the Internet, adolescents
tial number of LGBT adolescents are harassed, physically are far more at risk at school than online. Recent studies
abused, or verbally abused by peers or adults while grow- indicate that sexual harassment—both cross-sex and
ing up (Berlan, Corliss, Field, Goodman, & Bryn Austin, between members of the same sex—is widespread
2010; Birkett, Espelage, & Koenig, 2009; Saewyc, 2011). within American public schools (Leaper & Brown, 2008;
Abuse of this sort, as well having more distant family V. Lee et al., 1996; McMaster, Connolly, Pepler, & Craig,
relationships, contribute to the relatively higher rates of 2002; Young, Grey, & Boyd, 2009) (see Figure 11.6).
depression, suicide, substance abuse, running away from According to one study of a nationally representative
home, and school difficulties reported by LGBT adoles- sample of middle and secondary school students, more
cents (Almeida, Johnson, Corliss, Molnar, & Azrael, than 80% of girls and 60% of boys reported having
2009; Bontempo & D’Augelli, 2002; Bos et al., 2008; received unwanted sexual attention while in school
Busseri et al., 2008; Cochran, Stewart, Ginzler, & Cauce, (V. Lee at al., 1996). Sexual harassment is especially
2002; Galliher, Rostosky, & Hughes, 2004; Needham, distressing to early-maturing girls, whose physical
2012; Saewyc, 2011) as well as mental health problems maturity already makes them stand apart from their
that persist into adulthood (Toomey et al., 2010). As with peers (S. M. Lindberg, Grabe, & Hyde, 2007).
other types of discrimination, hostility toward LGBT Because the majority of those who had been sexually
youth is greater in some schools than others; rural harassed had themselves harassed others, and because
schools, schools in lower-SES communities, schools with many incidents occurred within full view of teachers
fewer explicit rules for student behavior, and less racially and other school personnel—indeed, a significant
diverse schools tend to have climates that are more percentage of students report having been sexually
hostile toward sexual-minority youth (Kosciw, Greytak, harassed by their teachers—numerous experts have
& Diaz, 2009; Poteat, Espelage, & Koenig, 2009; Sandfort, suggested the need for wholesale changes in the moral
Bos, Collier, & Metselaar, 2010). This has prompted and ethical climate of secondary schools (V. Lee et al.,
many experts to call for more concerted efforts to imple- 1996; Timmerman, 2002; A. M. Young et al., 2009).
ment school-based educational programs designed to This is easier said than done, however; one evaluation
promote tolerance. In 2012, for example, Lady Gaga of a school-based program called Safe Dates found sig-
founded the Born This Way Foundation, whose mis- nificant reductions in psychological abuse and sexual
sion is “to foster a more accepting society, where differ- violence 1 month after the program was implemented,
ences are embraced and individuality is celebrated”
(Born this Way Foundation, 2012). Studies also find

date rape Being forced by a date to have sex against one’s will.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 368 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

368 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Females Males
80
Middle school
70
High school
60

50

40

30

20

10

0
Stared at in a
sexual way
Sexual jokes
Sexual/obscene
phone calls
Sexual/obscene
messages
Kissed, hugged,
touched
Attempted rape

Oral sexb

Rapeb
Something else
sexual
Any sexual
assault

Stared at in a
sexual way
Sexual jokes
Sexual/obscene
phone calls
Sexual/obscene
messages
Kissed, hugged,
touched
Attempted rape

Oral sexb

Rapeb
Something else
sexual
Any sexual
assault
Sexual harassment Sexual assault Sexual harassment Sexual assault
FIGURE 11.6 Percentage reporting sexual victimization. Respondents could report multiple forms of victimization.
Questions about oral sex and rape were not asked of middle school students. (Young, Grey, & Boyd, 2009)

but these effects had disappeared within 1 year (Foshee their experiences, it is difficult to obtain accurate esti-
et al., 2000). Other research, on the histories of indi- mates of the numbers of adolescents who have been
viduals who commit dating violence, indicates that sexually victimized. We do know that adolescent vic-
perpetrators themselves were likely to have been ex- tims of sexual abuse are disproportionately female and
posed to physical punishment and abuse at home poor (Cappelleri, Eckenrode, & Powers, 1993). Accord-
(Basile et al., 2006; Capaldi & Clark, 1998; R. Simons, ing to several studies, between 7% and 18% of adoles-
Lin, & Gordon, 1998). Given that sexual harassment is cents report having had nonvoluntary sexual intercourse
a form of bullying, it is not surprising that studies find before age 18; reports by females are substantially
considerable overlap between adolescents who bully higher than those by males, but it is not known how
others and those who harass others sexually (Espelage, much of this is due to different prevalence rates and
Basile, & Hamburger, 2012). how much to different willingness on the part of fe-
males and males to report having been raped
(Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2009). (These figures on
sexual abuse do not include adolescents who have been
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
physically forced to engage in sexual activity other than
If you were asked to devise a sexual harassment intercourse and, as such, clearly underestimate the pro-
policy for a high school, portion of teenagers who have been sexually abused.)
what would be the main Women who were most likely to have been raped dur-
elements of your policy? ing adolescence were those who lived apart from their
Should policies governing parents before age 16; who were physically, emotionally,
adolescents’ conduct in or mentally impaired; who were raised at or below the
school be the same as those poverty level; or whose parents abused alcohol or used
governing adults in the work- other drugs. Indeed, two-thirds of all women who had
place? three or more of these risk factors were raped as adoles-
cents. In contrast to popular perception, adolescents are
abused (sexually, physically, and emotionally) and ne-
❚ Sexual Abuse Because both perpetrators and glected at a higher rate than are younger children
victims of sexual assaults are often reluctant to admit (Cappelleri et al., 1993).
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 369 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 369

Several studies have examined the psychological orientation, or engaging in same-sex activity during
consequences of having been the victim of sexual adolescence. The higher rate of psychological
abuse during adolescence. Adolescents who have been and behavioral problems observed among sexual-
sexually abused have relatively lower self-esteem, minority youth results from their being harassed by
more academic difficulties, and higher rates of anxi- peers and adults.
ety, fear, eating disorders, and depression (Nagy, Di- • A majority of teenagers report having been sexually
Climente, & Adcock, 1995; D. Perkins, Luster, & Jank, harassed in school, and a significant minority of
2002; Trickett, McBride-Chang, & Putnam, 1994); are young people, particularly females, are forced to
more likely to engage in risky behavior (Tubman, have sex against their will. Adolescents who have
Montgomery, Gil, & Wagner, 2004; Whitbeck, Hoyt, & been sexually abused show higher-than-average
Ackley, 1997); and are more likely to be sexually active, rates of academic difficulties, anxiety, fear, and
have multiple sexual partners, be sexually victimized, depression; are more likely to engage in risky be-
become pregnant as teenagers, and engage in prostitu- havior; and are more likely to be-
tion (Black et al., 2009; Wilson & Widom, 2010; Young, come pregnant as
Deardorff, Ozer, & Lahiff, 2011). Girls who have been teenagers.
chronically sexually abused by their biological father
are at greatest risk for problems (Trickett, Noll,
Reiffman, & Putnam, 2001). There is also some evi-
dence that sexual abuse prior to adolescence may lead
to precocious (that is, very early) puberty (J. Brown,
Cohen, Chen, Smailes, & Johnson, 2004).
At the same time, it is worth noting that there are Risky Sex and Its Prevention
substantial differences among individuals in the ex-
CONTRACEPTIVE USE
tent to which they show problems as a result of hav-
ing been sexually abused and in the form those One reason for the great concern among adults over
problems take (Bauserman & Rind, 1997). Generally, the sexual activity of adolescents is the failure of many
individuals who have been both sexually and physi- sexually active young people to use contraception reg-
cally abused fare worse than those who experience ularly. Among sexually active high school students in
sexual abuse alone. But adolescents who have been the United States, 40% report not having used a con-
sexually abused fare better psychologically when they dom the last time they had sex. Adolescents’ condom
have parents (presumably not the perpetrators of the use increased significantly during the 1990s (from less
abuse) who are authoritative (firm and supportive) than half to close to 60%), but has not changed since
and when they are successful in school (Luster 2003, and some studies have found a slight drop in
& Small, 1997). condom use among teenagers, which experts believe is
linked to recent increases in teen pregnancy (Santelli,
Orr, Lindberg, & Diaz, 2009). Condom use is slightly
RECAP higher among sexually active 9th-graders (62%) than
• In general, sexual activity during adolescence sexually active 12th-graders (56%), most likely be-
does not carry the psychological risks that many cause almost four times as many 12th-grade girls
adults associate with it. Adolescents who are sex- (30%) than 9th-grade girls (8%) are on the birth con-
ually active have psychological profiles that are trol pill. Of course, using the pill prevents pregnancy
similar to, rather than different from, their nonac- but provides no protection against sexually transmit-
tive peers. ted diseases. Condom use is higher among Black ado-
• Some evidence suggests that early sexual activity lescents than White or Hispanic adolescents, whereas
(having intercourse before age 16) is more common proportionately more White adolescents are on the
among teenagers growing up in single-parent pill (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
households and is associated with higher rates of 2012).
problem behaviors, such as drug and alcohol use. Among adolescents who do use contraception, the
• Any discussion of the psychological aspects of most popular method by far is using a condom, the
adolescent sexuality must differentiate between method used by close to 60% of sexually active teen-
the experiences of males and females. Early age couples, followed by the birth control pill, which
sexuality for males is tinged with elements of is used by about one-fifth of couples; this is a signifi-
recreation, whereas for females it is more linked cant change from previous generations of adoles-
to feelings of intimacy and love. cents, who were far more likely to depend on the pill
• About 6% of boys and 13% of girls report having than on condoms (Everett et al., 2000). (About 20%
had same-sex attractions, a nonheterosexual of girls who are on the pill report that their partner
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 370 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

370 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

By far the most important reason many adolescents


fail to use birth control is that their sexual activity is
unplanned (D. Kirby, 2007). This absence of planning
may reflect adolescents’ resistance to admitting that
they are choosing to be sexually active, which may ex-
plain why adolescents who have taken virginity pledges
often do not use contraception when they break their
pledge and have sex (Brückner & Bearman, 2005;
Rosenbaum, 2009) and why conservatively religious
adolescents are less likely to be sexually active but also
are less likely to use birth control if they do have sex
(L. Miller & Gur, 2002). Adolescents’ failure to use con-
traceptives also may reflect the fact that adolescents are
less likely to plan ahead and think about the future con-
sequences of their behavior than adults (Steinberg,
Cauffman, Woolard, Graham, & Banich, 2009). In
either case, going on the pill or purchasing a condom
requires an adolescent to acknowledge that he or she is
One of the reasons for adults’ concern about adolescent sexual ac-
having or is going to have sexual relations and engage in
tivity is the failure of many sexually active teenagers to use contra-
ception regularly. some advance planning.
Consistent with this, studies show that one of the
best predictors of condom use is the individual’s intent
to use a condom and willingness to communicate
uses a condom as well [Santelli et al., 1997].) With-
about it with his or her partner (Sheeran, Abraham, &
drawal, a highly ineffective method of preventing
Orbell, 1999; Tschann & Adler, 1997; Widman, Welsh,
pregnancy, and a practice that provides no protection
McNulty, & Little, 2006). Interventions designed to
against STDs, unfortunately is still used by a large
strengthen adolescents’ intentions and their ability to
number of teenagers. Nearly 60% of sexually active
communicate with their partner about contraception,
teenagers have relied on withdrawal at least once
and not just increase their knowledge, have been
(Horner et al., 2009).
shown to be effective in promoting condom use, even
Researchers estimate that the risk of teen preg-
within high-risk populations (DiClemente et al., 2004;
nancy is about half due to the absence of contracep-
Jemmott, Jemmott, Fong, & McCaffree, 1999).
tive use and about half due to failed contraceptive use,
A second reason for adolescents’ failure to use con-
which is more frequent among adolescents than adults
traception, although a far less important one, is that
(Blanc, Tsui, Croft, & Trevitt, 2010; Santelli, Morrow,
some adolescents can’t afford birth control or don’t
Anderson, & Lindberg, 2006). A large proportion of
know where to obtain it (D. Kirby, 2007). Lack of ac-
condom users do not use condoms correctly, for ex-
cess (or perceived lack of access) is an especially im-
ample (e.g., not putting the condom on before first
portant barrier among younger adolescents, who may
entry or not holding onto the condom while with-
feel uncomfortable discussing their sexual activity
drawing) (Oakley & Bogue, 1995), and many adoles-
with parents or other adults whose help or consent
cents who might benefit from using emergency
may be necessary in order to obtain birth control.
contraception (the “morning after pill,” or “Plan B”)
Having ready access to a free, confidential family
do not know how to use it properly (Mollen et al.,
planning service that does not require parents’ con-
2008). The rate of adolescent pregnancy is substan-
sent is a strong predictor of whether adolescents will
tially higher in the United States than in other indus-
use contraceptives consistently or at all (Averett, Rees,
trialized countries—despite the fact that the rate of
& Argys, 2002; Blake et al., 2003; S. Ryan, Franzetta, &
teenage sexual activity in the United States is compa-
Manlove, 2007). One study found that teen pregnan-
rable to that in other countries (Darroch, Singh, &
cies and childbearing rose after the imposition of a
Frost, 2001).
requirement that adolescents get their parents’ con-
sent in order to obtain contraception (Zavodny,
❚ Adolescents’ Reasons for Not Using Contra- 2004), and surveys of sexually active teenagers who
ception Why do so few adolescents use contracep- use contraceptives indicate that one-fifth of them
tion regularly and effectively? Social scientists point to would stop using them if they had to notify their par-
three broad factors: lack of planning, lack of access, and ents in order to get them (R. K. Jones, Purcell, Singh,
lack of knowledge. & Finer, 2005).
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 371 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 371

MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION


safe sex in particular, so that when adolescents become
sexually active, they find it easier to plan ahead without
Think back to your own sexual experience as an adoles-
feeling guilty. Finally, it is important that adolescents be
cent. If you were sexually active, what factors influenced
encouraged to consider the potential future conse-
your use (or lack of use) of contraception?
quences of an unplanned pregnancy or a sexually trans-
Given the fact that many adolescents’ sexual
mitted disease. One way to do this is to engage
experiences are unplanned, what steps might
adolescents in school, which may improve their aspira-
society take to increase safe sex?
tions for the future (D. Kirby, 2007). Adolescents who
do not believe that a pregnancy will be an impediment
to their future goals are less likely to take steps to avoid
getting pregnant (Jumping-Eagle, Sheeder, Kelly, &
Finally, many young people are insufficiently edu- Stevens-Simon, 2008).
cated about sex, contraception, and pregnancy, which
may leave them misinformed about when and how to
AIDS AND OTHER SEXUALLY
use contraception (D. Kirby, 2007; S. Ryan et al., 2007).
TRANSMITTED DISEASES
For example, although it is important for adolescents to
understand the need to use contraception every time Helping youngsters understand sex, pregnancy, and
they have sex, a very large proportion of teenagers who contraception is an important goal of sex education
have had sex with contraception have also had sex with- programs for adolescents. Helping them avoid the
out contraception (Arnett & Balle-Jensen, 1993; risks of sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) (also
Gillmore, Morrison, Lowery, & Baker, 1994). That said, referred to as sexually transmitted infections, or STIs)
knowledge alone does not seem to be sufficient to pro- is another. STDs are caused by viruses, bacteria, or
mote contraceptive use; individuals must be motivated parasites that are transmitted through sexual contact.
to use contraception as well as know why they need to About 3 million adolescents are diagnosed with a sex-
(Sheeran, Abraham, & Orbell, 1999). ually transmitted infection each year (Ozer & Irwin,
Given all these reasons—lack of planning, lack of 2009). Some of the most common STDs among ado-
access, and lack of knowledge—it is not surprising that lescents are gonorrhea and chlamydia (both caused
one of the best predictors of contraceptive use is the by a bacterium), herpes and human papillomavirus
adolescent’s age: Older teenagers are better at thinking (HPV) (both caused by a virus), and trichomoniasis
ahead, less guilty about having sex, more likely to be (caused by a parasite) (Forhan et al., 2009. One recent
able to discuss contraception with their partner, and study of a representative sample of American adoles-
better able to grasp the potential negative consequences cent females found that one-fourth of all young
of an unwanted pregnancy (Sheeran et al., 1999). Rela- women between the ages of 14 and 19, and nearly 40%
tively younger women are even less likely to use contra- of sexually active women of this age, had at least one
ception if their partner is older and more likely, as a of these five infections, with HPV by far the most
result, to contract an STD (Bauermeister, Zimmerman, prevalent (Forhan et al., 2009). These infections pose a
Xue, Gee, & Caldwell, 2009; Pettifor, O’Brien, MacPhail, significant health risk to young people, because they
Miller, & Rees, 2009; S. Ryan, Franzetta, Manlove, & are associated with increased rates of cancer and infer-
Schelar, 2008). tility. Countries vary considerably in rates of STD in-
fection, with the United States having one of the
❚ Improving Contraceptive Behavior Taken to- highest rates in the world (Panchaud, Singh, Feivelson,
gether, research suggests that there is a great deal that & Darroch, 2000).
adults can do to improve the contraceptive behavior of
adolescents (R. K. Jones, Biddlecom, Hebert, & Mellor,
2011b). First, adults can see that contraceptives are
sexually transmitted disease (STD) Any of a group of infections—
made accessible to the young people who feel they need
including HPV, gonorrhea, trichomoniasis, herpes, chlamydia, and
them. Second, parents and schools should provide sex
AIDS—passed on through sexual contact.
education at an early enough age to instruct young peo-
ple in the fundamentals of contraceptive use before, gonorrhea A sexually transmitted infection caused by a bacterium.
rather than after, adolescents have become sexually ac-
chlamydia A sexually transmitted infection caused by a bacterium.
tive; such education should be aimed at strengthening
adolescents’ intentions to use contraception and not herpes A sexually transmitted infection caused by a virus.
just their contraceptive knowledge. Third, parents can
human papillomavirus (HPV) One of several viruses that causes a
be more open and responsive in the ways in which they sexually transmitted disease.
communicate with their teenagers about sex, and about
trichomoniasis A sexually transmitted infection caused by a
parasite.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 372 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

372 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

❚ HIV/AIDS During the 1980s, a new and far more, influenced by their perceptions of benefits (for
more serious STD commanded the world’s attention: example, the fun of having different partners or the
AIDS, or acquired immune deficiency syndrome. The physical sensation of unprotected intercourse) as it is
virus that causes AIDS, human immunodeficiency by their perceptions of costs (for example, the risks
virus (HIV), is transmitted through bodily fluids, espe- of contracting an STD) (R. Levinson, Jaccard, &
cially semen, during sex, or blood when drug users Beamer, 1995). Adolescents have sex because they
share needles. AIDS itself has no symptoms, but HIV want intimacy with their partner, status with their
attacks the body’s immune system, interfering with the peers, and, of course, physical pleasure (Ott, Millstein,
body’s ability to defend itself against life-threatening Offner, & Halpern-Felsher, 2006). Even adolescents
diseases like pneumonia or cancer. Because there is a who know they are vulnerable to infection are less
long period of time between HIV infection and the ac- likely to protect themselves when they feel negatively
tual manifestation of illness, however—sometimes as about using condoms, when they are positively in-
long as 10 years—infected adolescents are likely to be clined toward risk taking, and when their friends are
asymptomatic carriers of the HIV virus who may de- actively engaged in risky sex (Romer et al., 1994;
velop AIDS in young adulthood; indeed, during the last Serovich & Greene, 1997; Shoop & Davidson, 1994;
decade, cases of AIDS increased by 7% among adoles- St. Lawrence, Brasfield, Jefferson, Allyene, & Shirley,
cents, but by 47% among 20- to 24-year-olds (Ozer & 1994). One large-scale evaluation of a media cam-
Irwin, 2009). paign targeted at Black youth (who are four times
Although the incidence of AIDS in the United more likely than other youth to have an STD) and
States was initially concentrated within two groups, designed to promote the messages that using con-
gay men and drug users who use needles, surveys in- doms would make sex more worry-free and therefore
dicate that the transmission of AIDS through hetero- more enjoyable, that waiting to have sex was a way of
sexual activity is a clear danger to male and female showing respect for one’s partner, and that a “steady
adolescents and is especially prevalent among inner- partner is a safe partner” was shown to be effective in
city minority youngsters (Andrinopoulos, Kerrigan, changing adolescents’ attitudes and condom use
& Ellen, 2006; Rangel, Gavin, Reed, Fowler, & Lee, (Romer et al., 2009).
2006). Nor is HIV infection limited to poor, inner- Short of abstinence, the best way for teenagers to
city adolescents (Gayle et al., 1990). Given what we protect themselves against contracting HIV and
know about adolescents’ sporadic condom use, many other STDs is by using condoms during sex;
straight, bisexual, and gay youth are all at high risk for adolescents who consistently use condoms are half as
HIV (Rotheram-Borus, Marelich, & Srinivasan, 1999). likely as those who do not to contract an STD
HIV infection is particularly high in Sub-Saharan (Crosby, DiClemente, Wingood, Lang, & Harrington,
Africa, where it has reached epidemic proportions 2003). Educating young people about the risk factors
(Hindin & Fatusi, 2009). associated with AIDS is also important, because
The chances of contracting HIV are greatest among studies show that adolescents who believe that they
individuals who use drugs, have unprotected sex, have are at risk for HIV infection and who are motivated
many sexual partners, and already have another STD to avoid the risk are more likely to take precautions
(such as gonorrhea) (Elkington, Bauermeister, & during intercourse (DiClemente et al., 1992; Hausser
Zimmerman, 2010; Hein, Dell, Futterman, Rotheram- & Michaud, 1994; Jemmott et al., 1999; D. Orr &
Borus, & Shaffer, 1995; Lowry et al., 1994). Because Langefeld, 1993; Rotheram-Borus & Koopman,
these risk factors are more common among young peo- 1991). Increasing adolescents’ perceptions of vulner-
ple than adults, the risk of HIV infection among ado- ability to HIV infection is not sufficient to motivate
lescents is substantial. Accordingly, in recent years, them to use condoms, however (Gerrard, Gibbons, &
efforts have been made to develop AIDS education pro- Bushman, 1996).
grams specifically aimedat teenagers (e.g., DiClemente
et al., 2004; Fang, Stanton, Li, Feigelman, & Baldwin,
TEEN PREGNANCY
1998; Jemmott et al., 1999).
Given the high rate of sexual activity and poor record
❚ Protecting Against STDs Adults often forget of contraceptive use among contemporary adoles-
that adolescents’ sexual behavior is as much, if not cents, it comes as little surprise to learn that many
young women become pregnant before the end of
adolescence. Each year, about 750,000 American ado-
AIDS (acquired immune deficiency syndrome) A disease, caused
lescents become pregnant—giving the United States
by a virus transmitted by means of bodily fluids, that devastates the
immune system. the highest rate of teen pregnancy in the industrial-
ized world (Ventura, Abma, Mosher, & Henshaw,
HIV (human immunodeficiency virus) The virus associated with 2008). The rate of unintended pregnancy is far
AIDS.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 373 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 373

greater among adolescents than adults once age dif-


ferences in sexual activity are taken into account
(Finer, 2010).

MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION


Although rates of sexual activity are
no higher in the United States than
in many other industrialized coun-
tries, rates of STDs, teen pregnancy,
and teen childbearing are. What
factors do you think contribute to
this?

❚ Prevalence of Teen Pregnancy Recent statis-


tics indicate that nearly one-third of American young
women become pregnant at least once by age 20, al-
though rates of teen pregnancy vary considerably by
ethnicity: The rate is twice as high among Black youth
as among White youth, and the rate among Hispanic
teenagers is 3 times as high as among White youth; the
lowest rate of teen pregnancy is among Asian adoles- Each year, about 750,000 American adolescents become pregnant—
giving the United States the highest rate of teen pregnancy in the
cents (B. Hamilton, Martin, & Ventura, 2007). These
industrialized world.
rates are lower than they were in the past several de-
cades, in part because of increased contraceptive use
and in part because fewer younger teenagers are sexu-
ally active, although the rate of teen pregnancy has risen Ozer, & Tschann, 2003), this option is not chosen
very slightly in recent years (Kaye, 2009; Santelli, Orr, equally often within all segments of the adolescent
Lindberg, & Diaz, 2009; Wingo, Smith, Tevendale, & population. Unplanned pregnancies are much more
Ferré, 2011). Whether this is a temporary blip in the likely to be terminated by abortion among young
curve or the beginning of a reversal of the long-standing women who are academically successful and ambi-
downward trend is not known, but many experts are tious, who come from middle- or upper-class families,
worried. whose parents are well educated, who live in wealthier
Keep in mind that not all adolescent pregnancies neighborhoods, whose significant others support the
result in childbirth. The proportion of teen pregnan- decision to terminate the pregnancy, and who are
cies that are aborted differs from country to country, more inclined to seek psychological counseling (P. K.
from a low of about 20% in Ireland to a high of close Coleman, 2006; B. Miller & Moore, 1990; South &
to 70% in Sweden (Singh & Darroch, 2000). In the Baumer, 2001). An important factor accounting for
United States, about one-third of all teenage pregnan- the racial and socioeconomic differences in adolescent
cies are aborted, and slightly more than 15% end in childbearing, therefore, is that White and middle-class
miscarriage (Ventura et al., 2008). Thus, approxi- adolescent women perceive themselves as having more
mately half of teenage pregnancies in the United States to lose—economically and in terms of their careers—
end in abortion or miscarriage, and about half result by having a child so early in life than do their minority
in the birth of an infant who will be raised by his or and poor counterparts.
her mother (with or without the help of a partner or Several studies have examined whether teenagers
other family members). Among American adolescents who choose to abort an unwanted pregnancy are
who carry their pregnancy full term, very few put the harmed psychologically by the experience. The consen-
baby up for adoption. sus among experts is that they are not (Adler, Ozer, &
Tschann, 2003). Indeed, several studies indicate that
❚ Abortion There are important differences be- pregnant teenage women who abort their pregnancy
tween pregnant teenagers who do and do not seek are significantly better off, psychologically as well as so-
abortion. Although studies show that teenagers can cially and economically, than comparable women who
make well-reasoned decisions about abortion (Adler, choose to give birth to their child, both in the United
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 374 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

374 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

States (Zabin, Hirsch, & Emerson, 1989) and abroad ambivalent, and not unequivocally negative, about the
(Bailey et al., 2001). Among the most important differ- prospect of having a child (Jaccard, Dodge, & Dittus,
ences between pregnant adolescents who abort their 2003a). For example, consider the following response
pregnancy and those who do not is that young women from a 17-year-old Australian girl, when asked about
who terminate their pregnancy by abortion are less her pregnancy: “I knew it was going to happen, like I
likely over the next 2 years to experience a subsequent didn’t stop it from happening so if it was going to hap-
pregnancy and more likely to practice contraception. pen it was going to happen and if it didn’t, it didn’t”
Given the apparent psychological and economic (J. L. Smith, Skinner, & Fenwick, 2011, p. 628). She
benefits of terminating an unwanted adolescent preg- doesn’t explicitly say that she wanted to get pregnant,
nancy, it is easy to understand why many social scien- but she doesn’t say that she didn’t want it, either.
tists have questioned the wisdom of court decisions Adolescents who are ambivalent about childbearing
designed to restrict adolescents’ access to abortion ser- or who believe that having a child will be a positive
vices (Blum, Resnick, & Stark, 1990). While some stud- experience are less likely to use contraception effec-
ies show that laws requiring parental notification or tively (Unger, Molina, & Teran, 2002; Zabin, Astone, &
limiting access to legal abortion do, in fact, result in Emerson, 1993). Thus, while the vast majority of sexu-
fewer terminated pregnancies among adolescents ally active teenagers do not actively wish to become
(Joyce & Mocan, 1990; J. Rogers, Boruch, Stoms, & pregnant, a significant minority feel less troubled by
DeMoya, 1991), not all studies reach the same conclu- the prospect of early parenthood than do their peers,
sion (e.g., Henshaw, 1995). An analysis of the parental and these youngsters are more likely to risk pregnancy
notification law in Texas found that it was associated by having unprotected sex. As one team of authors
with an overall decrease in abortion, but an increase in wrote, “Adolescent childbearing is more an unintended
late-term abortion (Joyce, Kaestner, & Colman, 2006). result of risky behaviors than a result of rational choice”
Another study found that policies limiting access to (Trent & Crowder, 1997, p. 532). Research also indi-
abortion lead to higher rates of unintended childbear- cates that the younger sisters of adolescent mothers
ing, especially among Black, Hispanic, and poor youth may be more likely to become adolescent parents
(Coles, Makino, Stanwood, Dozier, & Klein, 2010). One themselves, in part because the older sisters may com-
possible reason for these inconsistent findings is that municate some acceptance of early motherhood (East,
states requiring parental notification also tend to have 2009).
high levels of religious involvement, both of which are
associated with a lower teen abortion rate (Tomal, ❚ The Role of the Father A number of studies
2001). have focused on the male partners of pregnant adoles-
cents. In general, research indicates that these males
❚ Causes of Teen Pregnancy Many myths per- share a number of distinguishing characteristics that
meate discussions of the causes of adolescent pregnancy differentiate them from their peers who have not gotten
and complicate what is actually a fairly simple matter. a teenager pregnant. Most important is the fact that
The most important differences between young women they are relatively more likely to have problems with
who do and do not become pregnant during adoles- self-esteem, school, work, aggression, drugs and alco-
cence are in their sexual activity and contraceptive use. hol, and the law, and to have fathered a child previously
As you have read, sexual activity among American (Fagot, Pears, Capaldi, Crosby, & Leve, 1998; Males &
young people is high while contraceptive use is spo- Chew, 1996; Miller-Johnson, Winn, Coie, Malone, &
radic and inadequate. Although some evidence indi- Lochman, 2004; Thornberry, Smith, & Howard, 1997).
cates that Black and Hispanic teenagers are more likely They are also more likely to have been the child of an
than White teenagers to say they intend to have a baby adolescent father (Sipsma, Biello, Cole-Lewis, &
at an early age, the large racial difference in teenage Kershaw, 2010). Much has been made in the popular
childbearing is due mainly to racial differences in media about the age gap between teenage mothers and
unintended pregnancies (Mosher & Bachrach, 1996; the men who have fathered their children, but the pro-
Trent & Crowder, 1997). portion of teenage births fathered by adult men (that is,
Deep down inside, though, do adolescents who be- age 20 or older) actually has declined over the past 40
come pregnant actually want to have a baby? This has years, and the age difference between teenage mothers
been an extremely difficult question for social scientists (most of whom are 18 or 19) and their sexual partners
to answer. According to national surveys, 85% of births is generally about 2 or 3 years—a gap not substantially
to women ages 15–19 are unintended, a figure suggest- different from the age gap between husbands and wives
ing that the vast majority of adolescent mothers did not in the general population (Elo, King, & Furstenberg,
become pregnant intentionally. Yet, studies that plumb 1999). Moreover, girls who choose to get involved with
the issue a bit deeper find that many young women who older partners have more psychological problems than
say they do not want to become pregnant are actually girls who do not, well before the relationship begins
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 375 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 375

(Young & D’Arcy, 2005). Nevertheless, adolescent girls acceptable within the White and Hispanic communities
who have sex for the first time before 16 with a man (M. Buchanan & Robbins, 1990). In general, however,
who is more than 3 years older are less likely to use con- teenage fathers receive little in the way of supportive
traception and more likely to bear a child (Manlove, services or assistance in becoming responsible parents
Terry-Humen, & Ikramullah, 2006). Regardless of the (Kiselica & Sturmer, 1993).
age difference between mother and father, however, the
higher rates of problem behavior among the male part-
ADOLESCENT PARENTHOOD
ners of pregnant adolescents help to explain why mar-
riage may not be the best response to pregnancy for It is important to distinguish between pregnancies and
teenage women, as we’ll see shortly. actual births—a distinction that often is lost in debates
Although many of their problems precede the preg- over the consequences of teenage pregnancy. Because of
nancy, young men’s educational development and the many pregnant adolescents choosing abortion, the
mental health are adversely affected by fathering a child birthrate among teenage women is far lower than it
early in life, even if they do not marry the child’s mother would otherwise be, and it may surprise you to learn
(Furstenberg, Brooks-Gunn, & Chase-Lansdale, 1989; that the birthrate among adolescent women today is
Nock, 1998; Sigler-Rushton, 2005; Vera Institute of considerably lower than it was in previous eras. Con-
Justice, 1990). Men who impregnate adolescent women trary to the popular idea that teenage childbearing has
are more likely to drop out of school and to report feel- reached epidemic proportions in this country, relatively
ing anxious and depressed as young adults than their more women gave birth to an infant before reaching
peers. The adverse effects of teenage fatherhood appear adulthood in previous decades than do so today—and
to be greater among White and Hispanic men than by a large margin.
among Black men, however, perhaps because teenage Nevertheless, the rate of teenage births in the United
fatherhood is more disruptive and is seen as less States continues to be more than twice as high as in
Canada; 3 times greater than in Ireland; 5 times greater
than in France; 10 times greater than in Japan; and 17
times greater than in Korea (UNICEF, 2001). All sorts of
explanations for the astoundingly high rate of teenage
childbearing in the United States have been offered
(often, the finger is pointed at the mass media, which are
blamed for pretty much everything adolescents do that
adults disapprove of), but the answer can probably be
found in Figure 11.7, which shows teenage childbearing
rates in different industrialized countries alongside rates
of income equality and educational attainment. Gener-
ally speaking, the greater the gap between rich and poor,
and the lower the rate of school attendance among young
people, the higher the rate of teen childbearing. Given
the fact that the United States leads the list in income
inequality and comes in 25th (out of 28) in school en-
rollment, it is little surprise that we lead the industrial-
ized world in teen childbearing (UNICEF, 2001).
Rates of teenage childbearing vary markedly across
ethnic and socioeconomic groups. Middle-class women
are far more likely to abort their pregnancies than are
poor women, and as a consequence, the problem of
teenage childbearing is densely concentrated among
economically disadvantaged youth (Russell, 1994).
Because minority adolescents are more likely to grow
up poor, teenage childbearing is especially prevalent in
non-White communities.
Part of the controversy surrounding teenage child-
bearing is linked to the public’s concern about the
large number of teenage mothers who spend ex-
Because adolescent mothers are more likely than adult mothers to
tended periods of time on welfare. Among White ad-
be both unmarried and poor, their children are at greater risk of olescents, nearly two-thirds of all births occur outside
developing a variety of psychological and social problems. of marriage, but a large proportion of these births
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 376 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

376 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Income 15- to 19-year- occur within the context of cohabitation; among


Teenage inequality olds not in Black adolescents, virtually all childbirths are out of
birthrate index education (%) wedlock, and relatively few of these occur even among
Korea 2.0 21.4 cohabiting couples (Manning & Landale, 1996). The
rate for Hispanic teenagers falls somewhere in be-
Japan 4.6 tween; interestingly, young Mexican American
women are more likely to bear their first child within
Switzerland 5.5 35.5 15.9 marriage, whereas young Puerto Rican women are
more likely to bear children out of wedlock but within
Netherlands 6.2 30.2 14.0
the context of cohabitation. This suggests that cul-
Sweden 6.5 25.3 13.9 tural attitudes toward marriage and cohabitation in-
fluence the context of childbearing in important ways
Italy 6.6 35.9 30.2 (East & Blaustein, 1995; Manning & Landale, 1996;
Milan et al., 2006).
Spain 7.9 32.4 23.5 Because minority youth are more likely to experi-
Denmark 8.1 24.6 19.9
ence problems such as school failure or unemploy-
ment, early childbearing is likely to take place in the
Finland 9.2 24.8 17.9 context of limited social and economic resources. In-
deed, the main reason for the high rate of nonmarital
France 9.3 32.4 12.2 childbearing among Black teenagers is the higher
proportion of adolescents growing up in single-
Luxembourg 9.7 26.9 parent homes (Bumpass & McLanahan, 1987), which
experience more economic stress. In addition, many
Belgium 9.9 27.7 13.9 poor, young, Black women believe that it is perfectly
normal to become a mother while still a teenager and
Greece 11.8 35.6 22.4 to become a grandmother before turning 40 (Perez-
Norway 12.4 25.7 13.6 Febles, Allison, & Burton, 1999), norms that may be
handed down from one generation to the next
Germany 13.1 30.0 11.7 (Hardy, Astone, Brooks-Gunn, Shapiro, & Miller,
1998).
Austria 14.0 30.4 23.8 Because teenage childbearing tends to go hand in
hand with a variety of other problems—the most crit-
Czech Republic 16.4 25.8 25.1
ical of which is poverty—it is extremely difficult to
Australia 18.4 33.7 18.4 know whether any problems of teenage mothers or
their children result from the mother’s young age or
Ireland 18.7 34.6 19.3 from other, correlated factors. Separating the effects
of early childbearing from poverty is a matter of more
Poland 18.7 35.8 18.6 than theoretical importance: If early childbearing is,
in fact, a problem in and of itself, it becomes impor-
Canada 20.2 31.7 22.0 tant to direct preventive programs at deterring adoles-
cent pregnancy (either by discouraging sexual activity
Portugal 21.2 38.2 23.8 or by encouraging effective contraceptive use) and
childbearing (by encouraging adoption and abor-
Iceland 24.7 20.3
tion). But if poverty, not the mother’s age, is the key,
Hungary 26.5 25.0 24.6 an entirely different set of strategies is called for,
aimed not at youngsters’ sexual behavior but at all in-
Slovak Republic 26.9 26.2 dividuals’ economic circumstances. It is extremely im-
portant, therefore, to ask whether and in what ways a
New Zealand 29.8 37.0 28.3 mother’s age at the time she gives birth affects her and
UK 30.8 36.6 30.5 her child’s well-being.

USA 52.1 40.6 25.8


❚ Children of Teen Mothers Many of the prob-
FIGURE 11.7 Countries characterized by greater income lems that plague children born to adolescent mothers
inequality and lower school enrollment have higher teen result primarily from the environment of poverty and
birthrates. (UNICEF, 2001) single parenthood in which these children are raised
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 377 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 377

and from other qualities that often characterize young children are at greater risk of developing a variety of
women who become teen parents (such as poor school psychological and social problems. Indeed, many of the
achievement), rather than from the mother’s youth problem behaviors seen among children of adolescent
(e.g., Kalil & Kunz, 2002; J. A. Levine, Emery, & Pollack, mothers are prevalent among poor children growing
2007; Pittard, Laditka, & Laditka, 2008). In other words, up in single-parent homes generally. In other words,
infants born to middle-class adolescents differ little the greater incidence of problems among offspring of
from their counterparts born to older middle-class adolescent mothers may reflect the overall environ-
mothers, and infants born to poor adolescents are simi- ment in which the children grow up, rather than the
lar to children born to equally poor adults. ways in which they are raised. Although in theory we
One important exception to this general similarity can separate the effects of poverty on children from the
between the children of adolescent and adult mothers effects of adolescent childbearing, in reality, the two
is that adolescent mothers—even of similar socioeco- usually go together, and the end result is that children
nomic status—may perceive their babies as being es- born to adolescent mothers are more likely than other
pecially difficult and may interact with their infants children to suffer the effects both of malnutrition—in
less often in ways that are known to be beneficial to the womb as well as in the world—and of environmen-
the child’s cognitive and social development (Coley & tal deprivation.
Chase-Lansdale, 1998; Schellenbach, Whitman, &
Borkowski, 1992; Sommer et al., 1993). To what ex-
tent this directly jeopardizes the child’s development ❚ Consequences for Teen Mothers Studies of
is not known, although studies suggest that children the long-term consequences of adolescent parenthood
born to adolescent mothers are more likely to have indicate that the problems associated with it may
school problems, more likely to be involved in misbe- actually be greater for the mothers than for their
havior and delinquent activity, more likely themselves children. In general, women who bear children early
to be sexually active at an early age, and more likely to suffer disruptions in their educational and occu-
become an adolescent parent (Campa & Eckenrode, pational careers (Fergusson & Woodward, 2000;
2006; Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998; Conseur, Rivara, Hofferth, Reid, & Mott, 2001; Klepinger, Lundberg, &
Barnoski, & Emanuel, 1997; Hofferth & Reid, 2002; Plotnick, 1995; Otterblad Olausson, Haglund, Ring-
Wakschlag et al., 2000). In general, and for reasons back Weitoft, & Cnattingius, 2001), the consequences
that are not known, the cognitive and psychosocial of which often continue into midlife (Lounds Taylor,
problems of children born to adolescent mothers 2009). Not only are adolescent mothers more likely to
grow increasingly more apparent with age (that is, the come from a poor background and to have a history of
differences between children born to teen versus academic difficulties, but they are also more likely to
adult mothers are more evident in adolescence than remain poor than their equally disadvantaged peers
infancy). Again, though, studies show that the adverse who delay childbearing until after their schooling is
outcomes of being born to an adolescent mother— completed (Mollborn, 2007; K. Moore et al., 1993;
even outcomes not visible until the children have R. Richardson, 1996). It is important to bear in mind,
reached young adulthood—are attributable both to however, that many adolescent mothers were low-
characteristics of young women who are likely to be- achieving students before becoming pregnant, and the
come teen parents (for example, the adverse effects of limited educational attainment of teenage mothers is
being raised by someone who is poorly educated) and at least partly due to factors that were in play long
to the circumstances that characterize the family en- before the pregnancy, perhaps even during early child-
vironments of young mothers (for example, the ad- hood (Fergusson & Woodward, 2000; Frisco, 2008;
verse effects of growing up in poverty) (Jaffee, Caspi, Miller-Johnson et al., 1999; Roenkae & Pulkkinen,
Moffitt, Belsky, & Silva, 2001; Pogarsky, Lizotte, & 1998; Russell, 2002; Shearer et al., 2002). In short,
Thornberry, 2003). Not surprisingly, then, adolescent poverty and low achievement are both causes and
mothers who were relatively more intelligent and bet- consequences of early childbearing.
ter adjusted before the birth of their infant have Having a child early in life does not inevitably cast in
greater parenting skills later (Mylod, Whitman, & concrete a life of poverty and misery for the mother and
Borkowski, 1997; O’Callaghan, Borkowski, Whitman, her youngster, however. Studies show that there is con-
Maxwell, & Keogh, 1999), and children whose adoles- siderable diversity among teenage mothers in the routes
cent mothers are better educated, married, and better that their adult lives take (Ahn, 1994; Coley &
off financially do better in school than those whose Chase-Lansdale, 1998; Oxford, Gilchrist, Gillmore, &
mothers are less accomplished and single (Luster, Lohr, 2006). One study identified three distinct groups:
Bates, Fitzgerald, Vandenbelt, & Key, 2000). a problem-prone group (15% of the sample), who had
Because adolescent mothers are more likely than chronic problems in many areas of life, including
adult mothers to be both unmarried and poor, their antisocial behavior; a psychologically vulnerable group
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 378 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

378 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

(42%), who had relatively high rates of mental health 1997; Furstenberg et al., 1987b; Kalmuss & Namerow,
problems but who were able to transition into adult 1994). Moreover, teenage marriage is very likely to end
roles with some degree of success; and a normative in divorce, which itself is an additional stressor on the
group (43%), who defied common stereotypes of ado- mother and child.
lescent mothers as doomed to failure and poverty and Adolescent mothers therefore cannot always look to
who were able to make a successful transition to adult- the father of the child to help break the cycle of poverty
hood (Oxford et al., 2005). Some research also suggests that afflicts many young parents. However, they can, in
that the long-term consequences of early childbearing many cases, look to their own parents for support, and
may not be as negative among Black adolescents as this may be an effective strategy for some (J. Stevens,
among Whites or Hispanics, especially among Black 1988). Teenage mothers who move in with their own
youth living in communities in which early childbear- family for a short time—a practice far more common
ing is accepted as normative (K. Moore et al., 1993; among Black than among Hispanic or White adoles-
E. Smith & Zabin, 1993). cents—are more likely to enjoy educational and occu-
In general, young mothers who remain in, or return pational success than their counterparts who live on
to, high school and delay subsequent childbearing fare a their own, because the family’s help allows the young
great deal better over the long run—as do their children— mother to return to school or find employment
than their counterparts who drop out of school or have (B. Miller & Moore, 1990; Roye & Balk, 1996; Trent &
more children relatively early on (Furstenberg, Morgan, Harlan, 1994). Without this help, many young mothers
Moore, & Peterson, 1987a; Leadbeater, 1996; Upchurch must drop out of school and find and pay for child care,
& McCarthy, 1990). Indeed, remaining in school and which often is more costly than the income their low-
living at home with one’s parents significantly dimin- paying jobs generate. Without a high school diploma,
ishes the chances of a second unwanted pregnancy these women have little chance of improving their eco-
(Manlove, Mariner, & Papillo, 2000). Marriage, in con- nomic situation and, consequently, of improving the
trast, tends to be a high-risk strategy (Furstenberg, opportunities for their child.
Morgan, & Allison, 1987b). In some cases, when a stable Although having the support of her own family is
relationship is formed and economic resources are important for the adolescent mother’s development and
available, marriage improves the mother’s and the well-being, living with her family of origin for an ex-
child’s chances for life success; this seems to be especially tended period after having a baby is not uniformly ben-
true for women who marry somewhat later. In other eficial, as several studies of three-generational Black
cases, however, a hasty decision to marry in the absence families show. On the negative side, when the adolescent
of a stable relationship and economic security actually mother lives with her own mother, the living arrange-
exacerbates many other problems (Teti & Lamb, 1989). ment may undermine the development of her own par-
enting skills and increase the risk of getting pregnant
❚ After the Baby Is Born Research on the conse- again (Chase-Lansdale, Brooks-Gunn, & Zamsky, 1994;
quences of adolescent childbearing also suggests that Gillmore, Lewis, Lohr, Spencer, & White, 1997; Spieker
many of the negative effects of having children early can & Bensley, 1994), and problems in the relationship be-
be prevented or at least minimized by lessening the dis- tween the adolescent and her mother can adversely af-
ruptive economic impact of teenage parenthood on fect the teen parent’s mental health (A. Davis & Rhodes,
young women’s lives (Sandfort & Hill, 1996). What do 1994; East & Felice, 1996; Musick, 1994). On the positive
we know about the factors that work toward this end? side, living with one’s mother is associated with contin-
First, it is clear that marrying the father of the child may ued schooling, which confers long-term economic ad-
place the adolescent mother at greater risk if the father is vantages (Spieker & Bensley, 1994). Interestingly, several
not capable of supporting himself economically, much studies have found that support from the adolescent’s
less his family. Studies show, in contrast, that if the father father, in addition to that of her mother, may be espe-
is able to find a good job and remain employed, he can cially beneficial (A. Davis, Rhodes, & Hamilton-Leaks,
be an important source of psychological and economic 1997; Oyserman, Radin, & Benn, 1993).
support and a healthy influence on the mother and
child. Given the characteristic problems of male part-
ners of adolescent mothers that we discussed earlier, MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
however, it is all too likely that marriage may diminish, Based on what you’ve read
rather than enhance, an adolescent mother’s economic in this section, what advice
circumstances. In addition, marriage places the adoles- would you give a 17-year-
cent mother at greater risk of having another child rela- old young woman who was
tively soon, which further jeopardizes her already pregnant and unsure about
precarious economic situation and is one of the factors what she should do?
most likely to worsen her other problems (Apfel & Seitz,
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 379 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 11 Sexuality 379

One fact is certain, though: Adolescent mothers who it is trying to achieve. A recent comprehensive review of
receive social support fare better, are better parents, and more than 50 curriculum-based programs concluded
have healthier children than do adolescent mothers that carefully constructed educational interventions
who lack support (Barratt, Roach, Morgan, & Colbert, can delay the initiation of sex and reduce rates of risky
1996; Riggs, Holmbeck, Paikoff, & Bryant, 2004; Turner, sexual activity among adolescents (D. Kirby & Laris,
Sorenson, & Turner, 2000). Studies suggest that a lack of 2009). Generally speaking, programs are more effective
support is an especially dire problem among poor His- in reducing risky sex than in reducing sexual activity
panic adolescent mothers (Wasserman, Rauh, Brunelli, more generally. But experts agree that successful inter-
Garcia-Castro, & Necos, 1990). Taken together, these ventions must do more than provide information about
and other studies suggest that the best arrangement for contraception, STDs, and pregnancy. They must also
a teenage mother may be to live independently from her teach adolescents how to refuse unwanted sex and avoid
own parents but rely on them for emotional support unintended sex, increase adolescents’ motivation to en-
and child care (Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998). gage in safe sex, and change perceptions about peer
Because it is so important for young mothers to have norms and attitudes (Anderman et al., 2011). These ap-
an adequate income and a chance for adequate employ- proaches, collectively, are referred to as comprehensive
ment, many policymakers have called for changes in the sex education. Although many adults understandably
ways that schools and other social institutions treat preg- have been concerned that teaching adolescents how to
nant students and changes in the provision of day care. engage in safe sex sends a message encouraging more
Among the most important are adaptations in school teenagers to become sexually active, evaluations of
schedules and the development of school-based child care effective comprehensive sex education programs (even
centers so that pregnant students can remain in school af- those that distribute condoms to teenagers) show that
ter the birth of their child; the expansion of subsidized this is not the case (D. Kirby & Laris, 2009). Evaluations
child care for young mothers who are out of school so that of school-based clinics, which make contraceptive ser-
the economic benefits of having a job are not outweighed vices more accessible to adolescents, do not appear to
by the costs of child care; and the expansion of family increase sexual activity (a concern of many parents),
planning services to adolescent mothers so that they can but neither do they have the hoped-for effect on con-
prevent yet another pregnancy. Unfortunately, evaluations traceptive use.
of programs aimed at enhancing teen mothers’ employ- We also know a fair amount about what doesn’t
ability, decreasing their reliance on welfare, or preventing work. Programs designed to increase safe sex (which
their subsequent pregnancies have been largely disap- includes both abstinence as well as condom use) are far
pointing (Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998), although occa- more effective than those that emphasize abstinence
sional successes have been reported (e.g., Solomon & alone. Indeed, careful evaluations of abstinence-only
Liefeld, 1998). sex education programs have shown unequivocally
Although there are stories of young women whose that they are not successful, either in changing adoles-
lives are not devastated by early childbearing, in gen- cents’ sexual behavior or in reducing rates of preg-
eral, studies suggest that the successes are young women nancy or STDs (D. Kirby, 2007; P. K. Kohler, Manhart,
who have avoided poverty, rather than achieving great & Lafferty, 2008). Indeed, abstinence-only education
economic success. Although the picture of adolescent programs cause an increase in teen pregnancy and
parenthood appears less uniformly dire than typically childbearing (Stanger-Hall & Hall, 2011; Yang &
painted in the media, there is still consensus among ex- Gaydos, 2010). Programs that attempt to reduce sex-
perts that it is important to try to prevent teenage preg- ual risk taking by reducing other forms of risky behav-
nancy and childbearing. ior (such as delinquency or substance use) also are not
effective, consistent with the notion that sexual risk
taking, while correlated with other types of risk tak-
SEX EDUCATION
ing, may have some unique causes (Santelli, Carter,
Each year, millions of dollars are spent attempting to Orr, & Dittus, 2009). The evidence on other types of
prevent teen pregnancy and the spread of sexually programs designed to reduce unsafe sex, such as those
transmitted diseases. Many adolescents receive some
sort of classroom instruction about sex—whether
through high school health classes, biology classes, comprehensive sex education Programs that not only provide
classes designated exclusively for the purpose of sex information about contraception, STDs, and pregnancy but also
teach adolescents how to refuse unwanted sex and avoid
education, or educational programs administered
unintended sex, increase their motivation to engage in safe sex, and
through youth or religious organizations. Do these pro- change perceptions about peer norms and attitudes.
grams do any good?
The answer to this question is complicated and abstinence-only sex education Programs that encourage
depends on the nature of the program and the outcome adolescents to avoid sexual activity but that do not provide
information about safe sex.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 380 15/03/13 8:13 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

380 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

designed to encourage safe sex indirectly, by facili- • About half of all teenage pregnancies result in the
tating positive youth development, is inconclusive birth of a child who is raised by the teen parent;
(D. Kirby, 2007). the majority of other pregnancies are aborted.
Teenagers are not harmed psychologically by
aborting their pregnancy, but studies of the conse-
RECAP
quences of teenage childbearing indicate that both
• Most experts agree that the primary reason so few short- and long-term problems for the teenage
adolescents use contraceptives regularly is that using mother may be considerable.
contraception requires the sort of long-term plan- • While there are occasional success stories, teenage
ning that many young people are reluctant or unable parents are more likely than their peers to experi-
to engage in. In addition, some adolescents are insuf- ence disruptions in their educational and occupa-
ficiently educated about pregnancy and contracep- tional development. Adolescent mothers who have
tion, and they seldom anticipate having intercourse social support from family or friends and who are
until they become sexually active on a regular basis. able to complete high school fare far better than
• Because of their erratic use of condoms, several mil- those who do not.
lion sexually active adolescents contract a sexually • Careful evaluations of sex education programs find
transmitted disease each year. Cases of HIV infection, that comprehensive sex education, which combines
the most serious sexually transmitted disease, have information about how to practice safe sex with
been increasing among adolescents and young adults. practical instruction in how to avoid unwanted and
• The United States has the highest rate of teen unplanned sex, can be effective. In contrast, absti-
childbearing in the industrialized world. About nence-only education has been shown
750,000 American teenagers become pregnant to be entirely ineffective.
annually. Experts believe that adolescent pregnancy
is more likely to be an unintended result of risky
sexual behavior than the result of a rational choice
to become pregnant.
ste32118_ch11_348-381.indd Page 381 15/03/13 8:14 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 382 15/03/13 7:39 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 383 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
12
Achievement

ACHIEVEMENT AS AN ADOLESCENT ISSUE


ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVES AND BELIEFS
Achievement Motivation
The Importance of Beliefs
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ACHIEVEMENT
The Influence of the Home Environment
The Influence of Friends
EDUCATIONAL ACHIEVEMENT
The Importance of Socioeconomic Status
Ethnic Differences in Educational Achievement
Changes in Educational Achievement Over Time
Dropping Out of High School
OCCUPATIONAL ACHIEVEMENT
The Development of Occupational Plans
Influences on Occupational Choices

383
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 384 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

384 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

BECAUSE ADOLESCENCE IS typically a time of prepa- force is best negotiated. For the college student con-
ration for the roles of adulthood, considerable atten- templating a career as a therapist who works with teen-
tion has been paid to the development and expression agers, for instance, is it better to major in some sort of
of achievement during these years. Broadly defined, counseling or to follow a more general, liberal arts
achievement concerns the development of motives, course of study? How early is it necessary to decide
capabilities, interests, and behavior that have to do which specific profession to specialize in (e.g., counsel-
with performance in evaluative situations. More spe- ing, social work, psychology, psychiatry)? Is it necessary
cifically, the study of achievement during adolescence to go to graduate school right away, or is it better to get
has focused on young people’s performance in educa- some work experience before applying? These are all
tional settings and on their hopes and plans for future difficult questions to answer. And they are made more
scholastic and occupational careers. Since most young difficult because the nature of work, and the prepara-
people form their first realistic educational and voca- tion one needs for specific careers, changes so rapidly
tional plans during adolescence, researchers have long in contemporary society.
been interested in the factors that play the greatest Finally, achievement is a particularly important issue
role in influencing individuals’ futures. in the study of adolescence in contemporary society
Achievement is a particularly important consideration because individuals vary so widely in levels of educa-
in the study of adolescence in contemporary society. tional and occupational success (Byrnes, 2011). By the
Industrialized societies place an extraordinary emphasis end of high school, many adolescents demonstrate a
on achievement, competition, and success. During child- high enough level of academic achievement to enter
hood and adolescence, youngsters are continually tested selective colleges and universities; at the other extreme
to determine how they stand scholastically in relation to are a sizable number of their peers who enter adult-
their peers. In most industrialized societies, the amount hood unable even to read a newspaper or understand a
of education a person has completed and the job he or bus schedule. Although three-quarters of adolescents
she holds—two of the most important indicators of in the United States today complete high school and
achievement—provide a basis for the individual’s self- go on to college, 10% leave high school before graduat-
conceptions and image in the eyes of others. ing (the figure is even higher in many inner-city school
A second reason for the importance of achievement districts).
in the study of adolescence concerns the range and Similar disparities exist in the world of work: Most
rapidly changing nature of the choices faced by today’s youth make the transition from school to work without
young people. Adolescents in modern societies are a great deal of difficulty, but a significant number expe-
confronted with an array of difficult occupational and rience frustrating unemployment. Even within the pop-
educational decisions before they turn 25. Beyond such ulation of young people who enter the labor force,
fundamental questions as what type of career to fol- there is considerable variation in earnings and in occu-
low and whether to continue with schooling after high pational status. Many important questions in the study
school, adolescents must think about what specific of adolescent achievement, therefore, concern factors
sorts of jobs should be pursued within a particular ca- that distinguish between young people who are
reer path, what kind of educational preparation would successful—however success is defined—and those
be most appropriate, and how their entry into the labor who are not.

Achievement as an and success on tests of knowledge and ability. Concerns


over achievement continue throughout adulthood as well.
Adolescent Issue Like their younger counterparts, adults often place a pre-
mium on success, and in American society, what one does
Development in the realm of achievement neither begins
for a living is an important part of one’s identity.
nor ends during adolescence. Educational institutions—
Achievement during the adolescent years, though,
even for young children—stress performance, competition,
merits special attention for several reasons. First, the
fact that adolescence is a time of preparation for adult
achievement The psychosocial domain concerning behaviors and work roles raises questions about the nature of the
feelings in evaluative situations.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 385 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 385

preparation young people receive and the processes before turning 18, but only if they have their parents’
through which they sort themselves (or are sorted) into permission to do so.) Moreover, it is during adolescence
the occupational roles that may influence the remain- that most individuals decide whether they want to pur-
der of their lives. Many of the factors that narrow an sue postsecondary education or enter a full-time job
individual’s educational options and vocational alter- directly from high school. All these decisions have im-
natives are prominent during the high school and col- portant implications for the sort of choices and plans
lege years, and it is important to ask how such options the adolescent will make in the future, which, in turn,
are defined and at what age educational and occupa- will influence his or her earnings, lifestyle, identity, and
tional decisions are made. subsequent psychosocial development.
How might the biological, cognitive, and social changes
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
of adolescence affect the ways in which individuals re-
spond in achievement-related situations?
Adults disagree about the extent to which achieve-
ment is, and should be, made an especially salient is-
❚ Puberty and Achievement Although the bio-
sue among adolescents. Some contend that we place
logical changes of puberty are less obvious influences
too much pressure on
on achievement than are the cognitive and social tran-
teenagers to succeed in
sitions of the period, they may nevertheless affect the
school. Others argue that
development of achievement in important ways. As you
we do not place enough.
will read, the transition into secondary school is usually
Are there ways to stress the
marked by a temporary drop in individuals’ motivation
importance of doing well in
to achieve, and some of this may be related to puberty,
school without stressing ado-
because it introduces new issues (like dating and sex)
lescents out?
into the adolescent’s mix of concerns. To the extent that
puberty changes what’s important for maintaining sta-
Second, although differences in school performance tus in the peer group, it may lead some adolescents to
and achievement are apparent as early as the 1st grade, worry about whether trying too hard to do well will
not until adolescence do individuals begin to fully ap- make them less attractive to their classmates. In addi-
preciate the implications of these differences for imme- tion, puberty intensifies differences between males and
diate and future success. Children’s occupational plans females, and one impact of this is to make individuals
are to a large extent made on the basis of fantasy and think about what is “appropriate” achievement-related
passing interests, without any realistic assessment of behavior for each of the sexes.
their practicality or feasibility. Not until adolescence do
individuals begin to evaluate their occupational choices ❚ Cognitive Change and Achievement The in-
in light of their talents, abilities, opportunities, and the tellectual changes of the period are important influ-
performance of those around them. For example, high- ences on achievement as well. Certain subjects, like
achieving students who attend schools in which average algebra, may demand the use of the sorts of higher-order
achievement levels are high do not feel as good about cognitive skills that don’t fully mature until adoles-
themselves as their high-achieving counterparts in cence. Perhaps more important, not until adolescence
schools with lower average achievement levels (H. Marsh, are individuals cognitively capable of seeing the long-
Kong, & Hau, 2000). term consequences of their educational and occupa-
Third, the educational and occupational decisions tional choices or of realistically considering the range of
made during adolescence are more numerous, and the scholastic and work possibilities open to them. Thus, a
consequences of such decisions more serious, than the second reason for the prominence of achievement-
decisions made during childhood. For example, in most related issues during adolescence relates to the advent
elementary schools, although children may be grouped of more sophisticated forms of thinking. The ability to
by ability—groupings that have implications for subse- think in hypothetical terms, for example, raises new
quent achievement—they generally are all exposed to achievement concerns for the individual (“Should I go
fairly similar curricula and have few opportunities to to college after I graduate, or should I work for a
veer from the educational program established by their while?”); it also permits the young person to think
school system. In high school, however, students can through such questions in a logical and systematic fash-
select how much science and math they want to take, ion (“If I decide to go to college, then . . .”).
whether they wish to study a foreign language, whether
they want to pursue an academic or vocational track— ❚ Social Roles and Achievement The main rea-
even whether they want to remain in school once they son that many achievement-related issues take on new
have reached the legal age for leaving school. (In most significance during adolescence, however, involves the
states in the United States, students can leave school social transition of the period. In virtually all societies,
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 386 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

386 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

adolescence is the period when important educational


and occupational decisions are made, and society has
Achievement Motives
structured its educational and work institutions ac- and Beliefs
cordingly. In most industrialized societies it is not until
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION
adolescence that individuals attain the status necessary
to decide whether they will continue or end their for- There is no question that success is partly determined
mal education. Similarly, it is not until adolescence that by sheer ability. But as many writers have pointed out, it
individuals are allowed to enter the labor force in an takes more than talent to succeed—it also takes desire
official capacity, since child labor regulations typically and determination (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, &
prohibit the formal employment of youngsters under Kelly, 2007). Individuals differ in the extent to which
the age of 14 or so. In other words, the transition from they strive for success, and this differential striving—
school to work—one of the central issues in the study which can be measured independently of ability—helps
of achievement during adolescence—is a socially de- to account for different degrees of actual achievement
fined transition, a passage that society has determined (Wigfield et al., 2011). Two students may both score
will be negotiated during adolescence. equally on an intelligence or aptitude test, but if one
In this chapter, we look at the nature of achievement student simply tries much harder than the other to do
during the adolescent years. As you will see, the extent well in school, their actual grades will probably differ.
to which an adolescent is successful in school and in As someone who has been advising undergraduate and
preparing for work is influenced by a complex array of graduate students for more than three decades, I can
personal and environmental factors. We begin with a assure you that the difference between those who are
look at one set of factors that reliably differentiates ado- successful and those who are not usually has much
lescents who are successful from their peers who are more to do with their drive and capacity for self-
not: how motivated they are to achieve and their beliefs direction than with their intelligence—an observation
about the causes of their successes and failures. that is borne out by scientific study (Andersson &
Bergman, 2011; Duckworth & Seligman, 2005).
In recent years, there has been growing interest in
RECAP understanding the social and personality factors that
predict success in school and work, especially things
• Achievement is an important issue during adoles-
cence because society typically designates adoles-
cence as a time for preparation for adult work
roles, because individuals now can understand the
long-term implications of their educational and
career decisions, and because during adolescence
schools begin making distinctions among individu-
als that potentially have profound effects on their
long-term occupational development.
• The fundamental changes of adolescence affect
the development of achievement in several ways.
Puberty may affect the development of achieve-
ment by introducing new psychological and
interpersonal concerns, which may affect the
adolescents’ priorities and behavior in school.
• The cognitive changes of adolescence allow individu-
als to engage in longer-term, hypothetical thinking and
planning about their educational and occupational
futures, as well as enable them to study subjects that
require more sophisticated cognitive abilities.
• The transition into new social roles is probably the
most important influence on achievement in ado-
lescence. Society has structured the worlds of
school and work so that major deci-
sions about school and
work take place in Individuals who are intrinsically motivated strive to achieve
adolescence. because of the pleasure they get out of learning and mastering the
material. Generally speaking, they perform better in school than
students who are extrinsically motivated.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 387 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 387

like self-control and persistence. In one now-classic important (Klassen et al., 2009; Midgley, Arunkumar, &
study conducted nearly 50 years ago, researchers pre- Urdan, 1996; Midgley & Urdan, 1995). Some students
sented preschoolers with the choice between receiving a actually want to appear uninterested in school because
single marshmallow immediately or waiting 15 minutes in some contexts this presentation may garner more re-
to get two of them. The children who were able to wait spect and admiration from peers than academic success.
were far more likely than the others to be successful in Others want to make sure that they have an excuse for
school throughout childhood and adolescence, and, as poor performance other than a lack of ability (Nurmi,
adults, at work (Mischel, in press). In fact, motivation Onatsu, & Haavisto, 1995). Still others may downplay
becomes a more and more important determinant of the importance of academics as a response to their poor
success during adolescence, as individuals increasingly performance (Gibbons, Benbow, & Gerrard, 1994).
are expected to take charge of their own educational These students may use various self-handicapping
careers. By the time one enters college, doing well is strategies—such as joking around in class, procrastina-
influenced as much by conscientiousness as it is by in- tion, turning in incomplete homework, or partying
telligence (Poropat, 2009). excessively the night before a big exam—as a way of
self-protection (“I failed the test because I didn’t try
❚ Fear of Failure Being motivated to achieve is hard, not because I’m stupid”) or as a means of enhanc-
only part of the story, however. Even students who are ing their self-presentation (“I’m too cool to care about
determined to succeed are sometimes so afraid of fail- doing well in school”). Although self-handicapping is
ing that their strong achievement motivation is under- common among both males and females, there are sex
mined. Fear of failure, which is often manifested in differences in the ways in which adolescent girls and
feelings of anxiety during tests or in other evaluative boys undermine their own success in school: Boys who
situations, can interfere with successful performance. self-handicap tend to attribute their poor performance
Generally, when the achievement situation involves an to a lack of effort, whereas girls are more likely to men-
easy task, and when a little anxiety helps to focus atten- tion emotional problems (Warner & Moore, 2004). A
tion (if, for example, the task is boring), a moderate number of writers have drawn special attention to the
amount of anxiety may improve performance by in- use of self-handicapping strategies among ethnic mi-
creasing one’s concentration. The anxiety generated by nority youth, who may disengage from school because
a strong fear of failure interferes with successful perfor- they perceive their long-term prospects as being limited
mance, however. This is often the case in situations in by discrimination and prejudice (Mickelson, 1990;
which the task involves learning something new or R. Taylor, Casten, Flickinger, Roberts, & Fulmore, 1994).
solving a complex problem—like many tasks faced by
adolescents in school settings. Studies of adolescents ❚ Achievement Goal Orientation Two individ-
from affluent backgrounds who are under strong pres- uals can be equally motivated to achieve, but for very
sure to do well in school find that it isn’t so much par- different reasons. Psychologists draw a distinction be-
ents’ pressure to do well that creates mental health tween mastery motivation (similar to intrinsic motiva-
problems, but rather, parents’ criticism when expecta- tion) and performance motivation (similar to extrinsic
tions are not achieved (Luthar, Shoum, & Brown, 2006). motivation). Individuals who have a strong mastery ori-
An adolescent’s achievement motivation and her or entation strive to achieve because of the pleasure they
his fear of failure work together to pull the individual get out of learning and mastering the material. Indi-
toward (or repel the individual from) achievement situ- viduals who are mainly performance oriented strive to
ations. Individuals with a relatively strong need for achieve because of the rewards they get for performing
achievement and a relatively weak fear of failure are well (typically, good grades) and the punishments they
more likely to actively approach challenging achieve- receive for performing poorly. Generally speaking, in-
ment situations—by taking more difficult classes, for dividuals with a strong mastery orientation perform
example—and to look forward to them. In contrast, better in school than those whose motivation is mainly
those whose fear of failure is relatively intense and
whose need for achievement is relatively weak will
dread challenging situations and do what they can to underachievers Individuals whose actual school performance is
lower than what would be expected on the basis of objective
avoid them. Many students who have trouble persisting
measures of their aptitude or intelligence.
at tasks and who fear failure become underachievers—
students whose grades are far lower than one would self-handicapping Deliberately behaving in ways that will likely
expect based on their intellectual ability. interfere with doing well, in order to have an excuse for failing.
mastery motivation Motivation to succeed based on the pleasure
❚ Self-Handicapping Distinguishing between stu- one will experience from mastering a task.
dents whose underachievement is due mainly to anxiety
and those who underperform for other reasons is performance motivation Motivation to succeed based on the
rewards one will receive for successful performance.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 388 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

388 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

driven by performance goals, because intrinsically influences subsequent achievement (students who take
motivated individuals are more confident about their more challenging math classes perform better on math
ability and more likely to persist in the face of failure tests), and achievement, in turn, influences students’ be-
(Eccles & Roeser, 2011). One worrisome observation is liefs about their abilities (students who do well on math
that there appears to be a drop in students’ mastery mo- tests come to see themselves as better math students), a
tivation as they transition from elementary into sec- cycle is set in motion in which students’ beliefs, abilities,
ondary school (Bong, 2009; Wang & Pomerantz, 2009), and actual achievement have a reciprocal influence on
in part because teachers themselves become more per- each other (H. Marsh & Yeung, 1997). One of the most
formance-oriented and less mastery-oriented during interesting applications of this idea involves what psy-
this time (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). chologists call stereotype threat.
You read earlier that adults—parents and teachers,
for instance—can affect adolescents’ degree of achieve- ❚ Stereotype Threat Students’ beliefs about their
ment motivation. Adults also can affect the extent to abilities and, as a consequence, their performance can
which an adolescent’s achievement motives are more also be affected by situational factors operating when
aimed at mastery or more targeted toward performance they take an exam. When students are told that mem-
(Kim, Schallert, & Kim, 2010; Murayama & Elliot, bers of their ethnic group usually perform poorly on a
2009). When adults attempt to control an adolescent’s particular test (for example, before the test is adminis-
achievement behavior by rewarding good grades (such tered, students are told that previous studies have shown
as by giving prizes or money), punishing bad grades that members of their ethnic group do not score as well
(such as by restricting privileges), or excessively super- as other students), their performance actually suffers,
vising their performance (such as by constantly check- whereas the reverse is true when students are told that
ing up on their homework), adolescents are more likely members of their ethnic group usually perform better
to develop a performance orientation and, as a result, than others (Steele, 1997). This stereotype threat effect
are less likely to do well in school. In contrast, adoles- has also been demonstrated to depress females’ perfor-
cents whose parents and teachers encourage their mance when they are told before taking a test that males
autonomy, provide a cognitively stimulating home en- usually outperform females (Keller, 2002). To the extent
vironment, and are supportive of school success (with- that adolescents believe widely held stereotypes about
out rewarding it concretely) are more likely to develop ethnic or sex differences in ability (for example, that
a strong mastery orientation and tend to perform bet- boys are just better at math than girls or that Asians are
ter in school as a consequence (Gottfried, Marcoulides, more intelligent than individuals from other ethnic
Gottfried, & Oliver, 2009; Kim et al., 2010). groups), their achievement may be enhanced or depressed,
depending on how they think they are expected to per-
form. For instance, biracial students who identify them-
THE IMPORTANCE OF BELIEFS
selves as Black or Hispanic (groups often stereotyped as
How we behave in achievement situations is also influ- poor achievers) achieve lower grades in school than
enced by our beliefs about our abilities and our chances those with identical backgrounds who identify them-
for success and failure. You may have a very strong selves as White or Asian (groups often stereotyped as
need for achievement, but if you are put into a situa- high achievers) (Herman, 2009).
tion in which you see little likelihood of succeeding, Because stereotypes can change over time, the
you will behave very differently than if you are in a strength of the stereotype threat effect should vary as
situation in which you think your odds of doing well well. One study found, for instance, that the impact of
are good. For this reason, researchers have studied ado- stereotype threat on Black students’ peformance weak-
lescents’ beliefs about achievement, and not simply ened considerably both after Barack Obama received
their motives. the Democratic presidential nomination and after he
Many studies show that adolescents make judgments became the first Black president in U.S. history, pre-
about their likelihood of succeeding or failing and exert sumably because the worldwide attention devoted to
different degrees of effort accordingly (Dweck, 2002). his accomplishments weakened the stereotype of Blacks
For example, adolescents’ choices of what classes to take as less capable (Marx, Ko, & Friedman, 2009).
in school are influenced by their beliefs about their abil- Although not all studies have found an “Obama effect”
ities. Students who believe that they are good at math, (Aronson, Jannone, McGlone, & Johnson-Campbell,
for instance, will take more, and more difficult, math 2009), there is reason to think that changing views of
courses than their peers. But because course selection male and female intellectual ability have affected adoles-
cent girls’ test performance. For many years experts ex-
pressed special concern about the achievement motives
stereotype threat The harmful effect that exposure to and beliefs of adolescent girls, particularly with regard to
stereotypes about ethnic or sex differences in ability has on student performance in math and science, but more studies have
performance.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 389 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 389

shown that many previously observed sex differences


have gotten much smaller or have disappeared (Vermeer,
Boekaerts, & Seegers, 2000). One possible reason for this
is that stereotypes about sex differences in cognitive abil-
ity have weakened considerably. Indeed, in contrast to
several decades ago, when raters in experiments judged
successful females as less likable, less attractive, and less
likely to be happy, similar experiments conducted more
recently show the opposite effect, perhaps because we
have become that much more accustomed to seeing suc-
cessful girls and women in a variety of settings and en-
deavors (Quatman, Sokolik, & Smith, 2000).
With improvements in girls’ achievement—across
all subject areas—the achievement gap between males
and females in math and science is closing. When sex
differences in these subjects are reported today, they are
very slight, especially in the United States (E. Evans,
Schweingruber, & Stevenson, 2002; Leahy, 2001). In-
Although there are many exceptions, on average, Asian American
deed, observations that boys are not doing as well in adolescents outperform their peers from other ethnic groups.
school as girls, that boys are disciplined more in school
than girls, and that boys are more likely to perceive their
schools and teachers as unfair (Nichols & Good, 1998; In contrast, students who believe that intelligence is
Pomerantz, Altermatt, & Saxon, 2002) have prompted malleable approach achievement situations from a dif-
numerous writers to express concern about the achieve- ferent perspective. These students are more likely to be
ment problems of boys (Sommers, 2000). Sex differ- intrinsically than extrinsically motivated; for them, sat-
ences in educational attainment have grown in recent isfaction comes from mastering the material, not sim-
years, with females far outnumbering males on Ameri- ply from gaining a good evaluation. They are also far
can college campuses (T. Lewin, 2006). Sex differences less affected by their level of confidence, because they
in educational attainment, favoring females, are espe- are less concerned about their performance. Whether
cially great among Black adolescents (J. King, 2006; assured or insecure, these students exert extra effort
Saunders, Davis, Williams, & Williams, 2004). and seek out challenges, because they are motivated by
learning rather than by performing (Purdie, Hattie, &
❚ Beliefs About Intelligence Other studies suggest Douglas, 1996).
that the way in which adolescents think about intelli- These newer models of the psychological aspects of
gence in general (in addition to how they view their own achievement during adolescence illustrate how stu-
capability) also enters into the achievement equation; dents’ beliefs (about the nature of ability in general and
what’s especially crucial is whether intelligence is thought the nature of their own ability in particular) influence
of as something that is fixed or as something that is their motivation, which, in turn, influences their per-
changeable (Stipek & Gralinski, 1996). These studies formance (Bandura et al., 1996; Bassi, Steca, Delle Fave,
show that three factors interact to predict students’ be- & Caprara, 2007; K. Gordon, 1995; Legault, Green-
havior in school: whether the student believes that intel- Demers, & Pelletier, 2006; Simpkins, Davis-Kean, &
ligence is fixed or malleable, whether the student is Eccles, 2006). Their performance, in turn, influences
oriented more toward performance or mastery, and their beliefs about their competence (T. Williams &
whether the student is confident about his or her abilities, Williams, 2010) (Figure 12.1). Understanding how
or, as some theorists have put it, has a strong sense of self-
efficacy (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli,
1996). The way in which these factors are combined has a
significant impact on actual achievement. Beliefs about Academic
self-efficacy performance
Students who believe that intelligence is fixed tend to
be oriented toward their performance and to be greatly
FIGURE 12.1 Individuals’ sense of self-efficacy influences
affected by their degree of confidence (Stipek & Gralinski, their academic performance, which further shapes their
1996). If they are confident about their abilities, they tend sense of self-efficacy. (T. Wllliams & Williams, 2010)
to work hard and to seek out challenges. If they are inse-
cure, however, they tend to give up easily and to feel help-
less. In other words, if you believe that intelligence is
fixed, you’d better have confidence in your own abilities. self-efficacy The sense that an individual has some control over
his or her life.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 390 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

390 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

these forces work together has important implications “Come on, Jamaal, you can do this,” and stuff like
for teachers, because, as you will read, adolescents’ mo- that. . . . I don’t know what it does, but it’s just like extra
tives and beliefs are influenced by the context in which comfort to me. (p. 295)
they are educated. In other words, there are specific Compare this sentiment to that expressed by Tanisha, a
steps teachers can take that will help bring out the best classmate of Jamaal’s who is low in self-efficacy:
in their students.
Some of my friends tell me about [algebra], and, you
❚ The Importance of Context Although stu- know, some of their tests that they told me about, they
said it was hard. But, you know, that’s kind of what makes
dents’ orientation toward mastery versus performance is
me feel like I’m not going to do good in it. Because, like,
determined in part by psychological factors, the educa-
if they can’t do it, then I probably can’t do it. (p. 297)
tional context matters as well. When classroom condi-
tions change so that performance becomes more
important than learning, students’ motives and beliefs ❚ Attributions for Success and Failure How
change as a result. You have probably experienced this students interpret their successes and failures is also
when you enrolled in a course in which the instructor important. Researchers who are interested in what are
stressed grades rather than mastery of the material. This called achievement attributions have studied how the
sort of emphasis brings out the worst in students— explanations that individuals give for their success or
literally. Under some circumstances, performance goals failure influence their performance (Dweck, 2002). Ac-
make students more extrinsically motivated, more inse- cording to these theorists, individuals attribute their
cure about their abilities, more hesitant to challenge performance to a combination of four factors: ability,
themselves, and less likely to ask for help to improve effort, task difficulty, and luck. When individuals suc-
their performance (R. Newman & Schwager, 1995). ceed and attribute their success to internal causes such
This is especially likely when students are motivated as their ability or effort, they are more likely to ap-
mainly by trying to avoid looking stupid (which dimin- proach future tasks confidently and with self-assurance.
ishes their performance) rather than by trying to com- If, however, they attribute their success to external
pete with and outperform their classmates (which factors outside their control, such as luck or an easy
enhances their performance) (Pintrich, 2000). In task, they are more likely to remain unsure of their
classrooms in which teachers are very performance- abilities. Not surprisingly, scholastically successful in-
oriented (rather than mastery-oriented), students, on dividuals, who tend to be high in achievement motiva-
average, feel more alienated from school, have lower tion, are likely to attribute their successes to internal
feelings of self-efficacy, and are more likely to engage in causes (Durik, Vida, & Eccles, 2006; Randel, Stevenson,
self-handicapping behavior (Kalil & Ziol-Guest, 2008; & Witruk, 2000; Swinton, Kurtz-Costes, Rowley, &
Patall, Cooper, & Wynn, 2010; Urdan, 2004). Okeke-Adeyanju, 2011).
Students’ feelings of self-efficacy influence, and are
influenced by, their experiences, the messages they re-
ceive from teachers and parents, and the ways in which MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
they compare themselves to their classmates (Usher,
When you succeed in school, to what do you
2009). Here is how Jamaal, an 8th-grade student who is
attribute your success? When you fail, how
high in self-efficacy, described his reaction to the per-
do you explain your failure? Have you had
formance of a high-achieving classmate:
teachers who influenced your beliefs about
I was real mad when Stacey had got a 100. I wasn’t really your own abilities?
mad, but I was kind of jealous. Like I envied it. Like I
wished I was that one that got 100. I mean sometimes,
now that, that’s one of the things that give me the extra
drive. That gives me the extra determination to work How adolescents interpret their failures is also im-
harder in math to get good grades like everyone else. portant in influencing their subsequent behavior. Some
(Usher, 2009, p. 295) youngsters try harder in the face of failure, whereas oth-
ers withdraw and exert less effort. When individuals at-
When he felt challenged by a math problem, Jamaal tribute their failures to a lack of effort, they are more
would give himself pep talks: likely to try harder on future tasks (Dweck, 2002). But
I’ll be like, “Come on.” I’ll be thinking about different individuals who attribute their failure to factors that
ways to solve problems and stuff like that. I’ll be saying, they feel cannot be changed (such as bad luck, lack of
intelligence, or task difficulty) are more likely to feel
helpless and to exert less effort in subsequent situations.
Suppose, for instance, a student takes the SAT and
achievement attributions The beliefs an individual holds about receives a combined score of 1,000. He then is told by
the causes of her or his successes and failures.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 391 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 391

his guidance counselor that the SAT is a measure of in- 3.00


telligence, that intelligence is fixed, and that his score 2.90
reflects how smart he is. The counselor tells the student 2.80
2.70 Experimental
that he can retake the test if he wants to but that he

Math grades
should not expect to score much higher than 1,000. 2.60
2.50
Now imagine a different student, who also scores 1,000
2.40
on the test. She is told by her guidance counselor that 2.30 Control
effort has a great deal to do with scores on the SAT and 2.20
that she can raise her score by trying harder. In all likeli- 2.10
hood, the next time these students take the test, the first 2.00
student will not try as hard as the second student, be- Time 1 Time 2 Time 3
(Pre-intervention) (Post-intervention)
cause he is more likely to feel helpless.
FIGURE 12.2 A recent experiment found that teaching
Students who are led to believe that their efforts do
adolescents that intelligence is malleable can head off
not make a difference—by being told, for example, that the decline in school performance often seen during the
they are stupid or that the work is too difficult for transition into secondary school. (Blackwell et al., 2007)
them—develop what psychologists call learned help-
lessness: the belief that failure is inevitable (Dweck,
2002). As a result of learned helplessness, some students increase in their use of self-handicapping strategies
try less hard than their peers, and they do not do as well (Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 2001; Pintrich, 2000).
as they might. Students who suffer from learned help- In addition, studies find that individuals’ beliefs about
lessness and who use a lot of self-handicapping strate- intelligence change as they move into and through ad-
gies tend not only to perform worse in school but also olescence, with older students more likely to view intel-
to have more overall adjustment problems than their ligence as stable (Ablard & Mills, 1996) and to endorse
peers (Määta, Nurmi, & Stattin, 2007; Määta, Stattin, & dysfunctional attributions (e.g., attributing failure to a
Nurmi, 2002). Research on adolescents’ attributions for lack of ability, rather than a lack of effort) (Swinton
success and failure suggests that, instead of dismissing et al., 2011).
low-achieving students as having “low needs for One experiment demonstrated that this decline does
achievement” or “low intelligence,” teachers and other not have to be inevitable (Blackwell et al., 2007). The
school personnel can help students achieve more by help- researchers randomly assigned students enrolled in
ing them learn to attribute their performance to factors 7th-grade math classes to one of two groups: an experi-
that are under their own control (Blackwell, Trzesniewski, mental group, which received two class sessions on how
& Dweck, 2007; Hudley, 1997; Usher, 2009). it is possible to “grow your intelligence” and on how
experience can actually affect brain development, and a
❚ The Drop in Motivation During the Transition control group, which was taught strategies to improve
Into Secondary School One of the most interest- memory. Figure 12.2 shows the effects of the interven-
ing applications of research on achievement-related tion. As you can see, while both groups showed a com-
beliefs has been in studies of changes in adolescents’ parable drop in math grades initially, after the
academic motivation during the transition from ele- intervention, the group that was taught that intelligence
mentary school to junior high school (Eccles & Roeser, is malleable improved, whereas the control group con-
2011). As you read in Chapter 6, studies find that stu- tinued to decline.
dents’ motivation and school performance decline
when they move into secondary school (Eccles &
Rosser, 2009; Elmen, 1991; Yoon, Eccles, Wigfield, & RECAP
Barber, 1996). Why might this be? Among the other • Adolescents’ achievement motivation and their fear
important changes that take place during this school of failure work together to pull them toward or
transition is a shift on the part of teachers toward a repel them from achievement situations. A strong
more performance-oriented style of instruction and need for achievement is facilitated by parenting
evaluation. Elementary school teachers tend to stress that combines the basic elements of authoritative-
the importance of mastering the material. During sec- ness with high expectations for success.
ondary school, however, students discover that more of • Although some students underachieve because
an emphasis is placed on grades. This shift in emphasis they have an intense fear of failure, which makes
undermines many students’ intrinsic motivation and them anxious, others engage in “self-handicapping”
their self-confidence, which, in turn, diminishes their
performance. Indeed, during the early years of high
school, there is a general decline in adolescents’ feelings
of self-efficacy and in their mastery motivation, and an learned helplessness The acquired belief that an individual is not
able to influence events through his or her own efforts or actions.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 392 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

392 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

strategies in order to appear nonchalant about and in which team teaching is used more frequently
school. Often, self-handicapping is done so that (V. Lee & Smith, 1993). Unfortunately, many school
students have an excuse for poor performance districts, plagued with shrinking tax bases, are charac-
other than a lack of ability. terized by decaying school buildings, outdated equip-
• Some students perform poorly because they have ment, and shortages of textbooks and teachers. In some
been led to believe that members of their ethnic schools, problems of crime and discipline have become
group or gender are inherently less able than others, so overwhelming that attention to these matters has
a phenomenon that is called “stereotype threat.” taken precedence over learning and instruction. In
• Contemporary theories tend to stress the interac- short, many young people who genuinely want to suc-
tion of motives, beliefs, attributions, and goals as ceed are impeded not by a lack of talent or motivation
influencing adolescents’ achievement orientation. but by a school environment that makes their academic
Adolescents who believe that ability is malleable, success virtually impossible. Students who attend
who are motivated by intrinsic rather than extrinsic schools with a high concentration of poor, minority
rewards, who are confident about their abilities, students are especially disadvantaged, as are students
and who attribute their successes and failures to who attend schools with a high proportion of students
effort rather than to things they can’t control from single-parent families (Bankston & Caldas, 1998;
achieve more in school than their peers. Pong, 1998).
• Teachers can help improve their students’ achieve-
ment by creating environments that stress mastery
THE INFLUENCE OF THE HOME
over performance, by helping students attribute their
ENVIRONMENT
successes and failures to how hard they
work, and by stressing that in- The school, of course, is not the only environment that
telligence is malleable makes a difference in adolescents’ achievement, and few
rather than fixed. would argue that schools should accept full responsibil-
ity for adolescents who do not succeed at a level conso-
nant with their abilities. If anything, the evidence
suggests that important aspects of the home environ-
ment are better predictors of adolescents’ academic
MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION achievement than are features of the school environ-
Given what you’ve just read about achieve- ment (Azmitia, Cooper, & Brown, 2009; Steinberg,
ment motives and beliefs, how would you 1996). Researchers have studied three ways in which the
characterize your own approach to achieve- adolescent’s home may influence his or her level of
ment. Are you oriented to mastery or per- achievement (see Figure 12.3).
formance? Do you believe that intellectual
abilities are fixed or malleable? ❚ Parental Values and Expectations Adoles-
cents’ achievement is directly related to their parents’
values and expectations (Jodl, Michael, Malanchuk, Ec-
cles, & Sameroff, 2001). Parental encouragement of aca-
Environmental Influences demic success may be manifested in a number of ways,
all of which have been shown to be beneficial to adoles-
on Achievement cents’ school performance. First, parents who encourage
Ability, beliefs, and motivation may play a large role in school success set higher standards for their child’s
influencing individual performance, but opportunity school performance and homework, and they have
and situational factors also have a great deal to do with higher aspirations for their child, which, in turn, con-
achievement (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). Many of the dif- tribute to school success (Luthar et al., 2006; Patrikakou,
ferences in academic or occupational achievement that 1996). Parents’ and adolescents’ expectations influence
are observed among adolescents result not from differ- each other over time, so that adolescents who grow up
ences in their abilities, motives, or beliefs but from dif- with parents who expect a lot come to expect a lot of
ferences in the schools and classrooms where their themselves (Y. Zhang, Haddad, Torres, & Chen, 2011b),
abilities and motives are expressed. whereas low parental expectations can contribute to a
As we noted in Chapter 6, school environments dif- self-fulfilling prophecy, leading to poor achievement
fer markedly—in physical facilities, in opportunities (Wood, Kurtz-Costes, Rowley, & Okeke-Adeyanju,
for pursuing academically enriched programs, and in 2010). One arena in which parental expectations may
classroom atmospheres. For example, students are play an especially important role is in the realm of
more engaged and successful in schools that are more mathematics, where traditional views of gender roles
personal, less departmentalized, and less rigidly tracked, may lead adolescent boys and girls to develop very
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 393 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 393

the family in Mexican culture (Trusty, Plata, & Salazar,


Parents’ 2003; Woolley, Kol, & Bowen, 2009). The way in which
values and parents are involved matters, however: Studies show
expectations that encouraging and expecting achievement in school
and being involved in school-based activities are both
effective forms of parental involvement, whereas help-
ing with homework is not (N. E. Hill & Tyson, 2009).
Interestingly, parental involvement in schooling has a
more substantial effect when the adolescent attends a
Adolescent
school in which a large proportion of other students’
achievement
parents are involved as well (Darling & Steinberg, 1997;
Pong, 1998).

❚ Authoritative Parenting A second way in


which parents influence student achievement is more
Authoritative Cultural
parenting capital
indirect—through their general approach to parenting.
Numerous studies have shown that authoritative
parenting—parenting that is warm, firm, and fair—is
linked to school success during adolescence, as indexed
FIGURE 12.3 Parents influence adolescent achievement
by better performance, better attendance, higher expec-
through three mechanisms: their values and expectations,
the way they parent, and the cultural capital they provide
tations, more positive academic self-conceptions, and
in the home environment. stronger engagement in the classroom (Y. Li, Lerner, &
Lerner, 2010; Murray, 2009; Sirin & Rogers-Sirin, 2004;
Steinberg, Lamborn, Darling, Mounts, & Dornbusch,
different sorts of self-conceptions that inspire male stu- 1994; Wentzel, 1998). In contrast, parenting that is
dents to develop relatively stronger interests in math especially punitive, harsh, overcontrolling, or inept is as-
from an early age (Frenzel, Goetz, Pekrun, & Watt, 2010; sociated with lower school engagement and diminished
Nagy et al., 2010). achievement (R. Clark, Dogan, & Akbar, 2003; Eamon,
Second, parents who encourage school success also 2005; Melby & Conger, 1996; Q. Wang, Pomerantz, &
have values that are consistent with doing well in school, Chen, 2007). Interestingly, extreme parental permis-
and they structure the home environment to support siveness, not authoritarianism, is associated with higher
academic pursuits so that the messages children receive rates of dropping out of school (Rumberger, Ghatak,
from their teachers are echoed at home (Benner, Poulos, Ritter, & Dornbusch, 1990). Authoritative par-
Graham, & Mistry, 2008; Jodl et al., 2001). Studies show enting has also been shown to help adolescents adjust to
that even high school students profit from having par- middle school (Bronstein et al., 1998) and help poorly
ents who help them learn more effective time manage- performing early adolescents turn their academic per-
ment strategies and healthier work habits (Xu, 2004). formance around in high school (Catterall, 1998).
Finally, parents who encourage success are likely to Why do adolescents achieve more in school when
be more involved in their child’s education—more they come from authoritative homes? One reason is
likely to attend school programs, help in course selec- that authoritative parenting promotes the development
tion, maintain interest in school activities and assign- of a healthy achievement orientation—including an em-
ments, and the like—all of which contribute to students’ phasis on mastery and a healthier attributional style—
success (Benner et al., 2008; Bogenschneider, 1997; N. E. which, in turn, enhances adolescent school performance
Hill et al., 2004; Shumow & Miller, 2001). Parental in- (Aunola, Stattin, & Nurmi, 2000; Bronstein, Ginsburg, &
volvement in schooling may make academics seem both Herrera-Leavitt, 2000; Duchesne & Ratelle, 2010; Glasgow,
more important and more conquerable to the adoles- Dornbusch, Ritter, Troyer, & Steinberg, 1997). This is in
cent, which may enhance the young person’s academic part because authoritative parents are more likely them-
self-conceptions (Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994; Ibañez, selves to hold healthier beliefs about their child’s
Kuperminc, Jurkovic, & Perilla, 2004); it may also send achievement and less likely to be overly controlling—
an important message to teachers (Kuperminc, Darnell, two factors that strengthen adolescents’ work ethic and
& Alvarez-Jiminez, 2008). In contrast, parental disen- intrinsic motivation (Arbeton, Eccles, & Harold, 1994;
gagement from school may make students themselves Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994). Having a strong work
more likely to disengage and do poorly (Roeser, Lord, & orientation enhances achievement both directly, as we
Eccles, 1994). Parental involvement seems to be an espe- saw earlier, and indirectly, through the positive impres-
cially strong influence on the achievement of Mexican sion it makes on teachers (Farkas, Grobe, & Shuan,
American youth, perhaps because of the importance of 1990).
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 394 15/03/13 7:40 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

394 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Schools in the United States vary considerably in the resources their students have access to.

In general, these findings are in line with a good deal level of academic achievement than is the quality of the
of research suggesting that consistent, authoritative par- physical facility of the school they attend, the background
enting is associated with a wide array of benefits to the and training of their teachers, or the level of teachers’ sala-
adolescent, including higher achievement motivation, ries paid by the school district (Armor, 1972). A number
greater self-esteem, and enhanced competence (Steinberg, of researchers have also shown that the extent to which the
2001). Authoritative parents also tend to have more in- adolescent’s parents provide the youngster with what is
volvement in school activities, which is associated with called cultural capital—by exposing the adolescent to art,
scholastic success, although parents’ involvement both music, literature, and so forth—exerts a positive impact
affects and is affected by adolescents’ achievement on achievement above and beyond the effects of the par-
(Crosnoe, 2001; Juang & Silbereisen, 2002; Steinberg, ents’ own level of education (Buechel & Duncan, 1998;
Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992). Students also perform bet- Roscigno & Ainsworth-Darnell, 1999).
ter when the values and expectations they encounter at Several researchers have asked whether adolescents’
home are consistent with those they encounter in school school achievement is affected by genetic factors. Inter-
(Arunkumar, Midgley, & Urdan, 1999). estingly, evidence on the heritability of school achieve-
ment suggests that while intelligence and cognitive
❚ The Quality of the Home Environment A achievement both have strong genetic components (and
third mechanism of familial influence is through the influence grades through this mechanism), school per-
quality of the home environment provided. Studies formance is highly influenced by environmental factors,
have shown that the quality of an adolescent’s home both inside and outside the family (W. Johnson, McGue,
environment—as measured simply in terms of the pres- & Iacono, 2006; Teachman, 1997). With this in mind, it is
ence of such items as a television, dictionary, encyclopedia, important to point out that a disheartening number of
newspaper, vacuum cleaner, and other indicators of family young people in this country live in overcrowded, inade-
income—is more strongly correlated with youngsters’ quate housing and come from families that are under
severe economic and social stress—so much so that pa-
rental encouragement and involvement are often under-
cultural capital The resources provided within a family through mined by neighborhood conditions (Gonzales, Cauce,
the exposure of the adolescent to art, music, literature, and other Friedman, & Mason, 1996). It is extremely difficult for a
elements of “high culture.”
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 395 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 395

parent to provide a supportive home environment day-to-day school behaviors, such as doing homework
when he or she is under severe economic stress. Stress at and exerting effort in class (Kurdek, Fine, & Sinclair,
home, in turn, spills over into the adolescent’s school 1995; Midgley & Urdan, 1995; Steinberg, 1996). That is,
life, leading to academic problems and diminished although parents are stronger influences on long-range
achievement (Flook & Fuligni, 2008). One recent study educational plans, what adolescents do in school on a
found, for example, that adolescents from homes in daily basis is more affected by their friends. Indeed, one
which there had been a lot of family instability did not of the main reasons that adolescents growing up in
do as well as other students when they attended schools poor neighborhoods achieve less is that they are often
that had a large proportion of high-achieving students; surrounded by peers who are disengaged from school
in schools with fewer high achievers, coming from an (South, Baumer, & Lutz, 2003).
unstable family environment mattered much less When most of us think about the influence of ado-
(Cavanagh & Fomby, 2012). lescents’ peers on achievement, we tend immediately to
Put succinctly, many American youngsters do not think of the ways in which peers undermine academic
grow up in an atmosphere that is conducive to aca- success. But contrary to the notion that the influence of
demic achievement. Many communities lack what the peer group is always negative, the impact of friends
social scientists sometimes call social capital—the on adolescents’ school performance depends on the
support, encouragement, and involvement of adults academic orientation of the peer group. Having friends
necessary to facilitate youngsters’ success in school who earn high grades and aspire to further education
(J. Coleman & Hoffer, 1987). Social capital, which is can enhance adolescents’ achievement, whereas having
strengthened when families have strong ties to other friends who earn low grades or disparage school success
families in the community, has been shown to be an may interfere with it (Steinberg, 1996). Students whose
important contributor to success in school, above and friends are more engaged in school are themselves more
beyond the contribution of adolescents’ family income, engaged and less likely to drop out (R. Ream & Rumberger,
their parents’ education, or their household composi- 2008). Friends also influence course selection and appear
tion (Pong, 1997, 1998). Not surprisingly, students to play an especially important role in girls’ decisions to
whose families lack social capital are more likely to have take math and science classes (Riegle-Crumb, Farkas, &
difficulty in school (Teachman, Paasch, & Carver, 1997). Muller, 2006).
In contrast, students who can draw on resources Students’ grades change over time in relation to the
provided not only by the family, but by friends, men- grades of their friends (Mounts & Steinberg, 1995).
tors, and teachers, stand a far better chance of succeed- Students with best friends who achieve high grades in
ing in school; one recent study of undocumented school are more likely to show improvements in their
immigrant Latino college students described the way own grades than are students who begin at similar levels
that these individuals created “families” of support that of achievement but whose friends are not high achiev-
extended well beyond their immediate family (En- ers. Peers also exert a small but significant influence on
riquez, 2011). each other’s college plans. Among low-achieving adoles-
cents, for example, those with high-achieving friends
are more likely to plan to continue their education than
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION are those with low-achieving friends. The causal direc-
What sorts of programs for parents existed in the tion works the other way, too: When adolescents’ grades
district in which you attended high school? In view of go up, they tend to befriend more high-achieving class-
what we know about the home mates, but when their grades drop, they tend to become
environments of high- friends with lower-achieving peers (Flashman, 2012;
achieving students, how Véronneau, Vitaro, Brendgen, Dishion, & Tremblay,
likely are these sorts of 2010).
activities to succeed? What Although peers can influence achievement for better
do you think would motivate or for worse, many observers have noted that in the
parents to become more contemporary United States, the influence of the peer
involved in their adolescent’s culture on academic achievement is far more negative
schooling? than positive (Bishop, Bishop, Gelbwasser, Green, &
Zuckerman, 2003; Steinberg, 1996). Perhaps because of
this, adolescents with an extremely high orientation
toward peers tend to perform worse in school (Fuligni &
THE INFLUENCE OF FRIENDS
Eccles, 1993). Conversely, adolescents who are neglected
There is also evidence that friends influence adoles-
cents’ achievement. Indeed, some studies suggest that
friends are the most salient influences on adolescents’ social capital The interpersonal resources available to an
adolescent or family.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 396 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

396 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

by their peers often have a stronger academic orienta- Mason, 1996; Kurdek et al., 1995; Steinberg, 1996).
tion than relatively more popular students (Luthar & These studies show that the family environment has an
McMahon, 1996; Wentzel & Asher, 1995). As they move effect on adolescents’ choice of friends, and this, in
into middle school, adolescents become increasingly turn, can influence school achievement (B. Brown,
worried about their friends’ reactions to success in Mounts, Lamborn, & Steinberg, 1993). In addition,
school; one study found, for example, that by 8th grade having friends who value school can positively affect
students did not want their classmates to know that academic achievement, even among teenagers who do
they worked hard in school, even though they knew not come from nonauthoritative homes; having aca-
that it would be helpful to convey this impression to demically oriented peers is especially beneficial to ado-
their teachers (Juvonen & Murdock, 1995). lescents from single-parent homes, where parental
Doing well in school does not have to come at the involvement in schooling is typically lower (Garg,
cost of having a decent social life, however. A study in Melanson, & Levin, 2007). By the same token, having
which adolescents maintained daily diaries of their friends who disparage school success may offset the
time use sheds interesting light on the differences be- benefits of authoritative parenting (Steinberg, 1996).
tween high- and low-achieving students in how they Rather than asking whether family or friends influence
spend their time (Witkow, 2009). Not surprisingly, stu- adolescents’ school performance, it may make more
dents who earn higher grades than their peers spend sense to ask how these two forces—along with the in-
more time studying, both on weekdays and on week- fluence of the school itself—work together.
ends. But as Figure 12.4 shows, a key difference between A study examining the influence of peers and par-
the groups is in how much—and when—they spend ents on school achievement in 12 different countries
time with their friends. As you can see, high-achieving shows that the broader context in which schooling
students spend less time with their friends than do low- takes place can affect the degree to which peers and par-
achieving students on weekdays, but not on weekends. ents influence adolescents’ achievement. Peers and par-
This suggests that high-achieving students are able to ents more strongly influence student achievement in
maintain an active social life by allocating their time countries where schools serve more heterogeneous
more judiciously during the week. As you read in groups of students, as in the United States. In countries
Chapter 5, adolescents tend to have friends whose ori- where students with different long-term educational
entation toward school is similar. In all likelihood, one aspirations attend different schools (that is, in coun-
reason that high-achieving students spend less time tries like Germany, where there are separate schools for
with friends on weekdays is that their friends are also adolescents who are planning to go to college versus
busy studying. those who are not), peers and parents are less influen-
A number of researchers have studied how the influ- tial (Buchmann & Dalton, 2002).
ences of parents and peers operate together to affect In sum, although psychological factors play a key
adolescents’ achievement (Fletcher, Darling, Steinberg, role in determining occupational and scholastic
& Dornbusch, 1995; Gonzales, Cauce, Friedman, & success, it is important to take into account the broader

1.8 1.8
1.6 1.6
Average time with friends (hours)
Average study time (hours)

1.4 1.4
1.2 1.2
1.0 1.0
0.8 0.8
0.6 0.6
0.4 0.4
0.2 0.2
0 0
Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend
Time spent studying Time spent socializing

Low GPA Mid GPA High GPA


FIGURE 12.4 High- and low-achieving students allocate their time spent studying and socializing differently. (Witkow, 2009)
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 397 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 397

environment in which individuals pursue their educa- are, not surprisingly, interrelated, but they are less tied
tional and occupational careers (Gonzales, Cauce, to each other than you might expect.
Friedman, & Mason, 1996; Li, Lerner, & Lerner, 2010; No single factor adequately accounts for differences
Steinberg, 1996). Moreover, distinguishing between in adolescents’ levels of educational achievement. Gen-
motivational and environmental factors is hard be- erally, intellectual ability—as assessed by IQ tests—is
cause they typically go hand in hand. Living in an en- highly correlated with performance on achievement
vironment that offers few opportunities for success tests (hardly a surprise, since IQ tests and achievement
induces feelings of learned helplessness, which, in tests are designed to tap similar abilities). But grades in
turn, leads individuals to feel that exerting any effort to school—and to an even greater extent, educational
succeed is futile. Attending school in an environment attainment—are influenced by a wider range of factors
in which achievement is not encouraged engenders at- than simply the adolescent’s intellectual abilities.
titudes and beliefs inconsistent with striving to do Grades, for example, are influenced by teachers’ judg-
well. Rather than viewing achievement during adoles- ments of students’ mastery of the material, and these
cence as being determined by one single factor, such as judgments may be influenced by teachers’ evaluations
ability or motivation, it is more accurate to say that of students’ efforts and behaviors in the classroom
patterns of achievement are the result of a cumulative (Farkas, Grobe, & Shuan, 1990). How many years of
process that includes a long history of experience and school an adolescent completes is likely to be influ-
socialization in school, in the family, in the peer group, enced by his or her family background and living cir-
and in the community. cumstances, as well as by school performance. Two
adolescents may have similar grade point averages, but
if one comes from a poor family and cannot afford to
go to college, the two will have different levels of educa-
RECAP tional attainment. Even as early as elementary school,
• In addition to the influence of beliefs, motives, at- for example, many inner-city youth have very limited
tributions, and goals, individuals’ levels of achieve- occupational expectations, and these low expectations
ment are affected by the social context in which affect their educational achievement and attainment
they develop. (T. Cook et al., 1996).
• Adolescents perform better and are more engaged Regardless of what influences it, educational attain-
in school when they come from homes in which ment has important implications for subsequent earn-
their parents value and expect scholastic success, ings (Ceci & Williams, 1999). The gap in earnings
practice authoritative parenting, and provide a between high school graduates and college graduates is
home environment that is high in cultural capital. considerable, and this is true across all ethnic groups.
• In addition, adolescents whose friends support When they enter the labor force, individuals with a
academic achievement perform better in school college degree earn twice as much per year as do indi-
than do peers whose friends disparage doing well viduals with only a high school diploma (U.S. Census
in school. Bureau, 2009a). This state of affairs has led many to call
• Researchers now understand that patterns of for educational policies that encourage all students to
achievement are the result of a cumulative process “shoot for the stars.” Although some experts had specu-
that includes a long history of experience and lated that encouraging all students to strive to go to
socialization in school, in the family, college would ultimately have negative effects on the
in the peer group, and in mental health of students whose expectations are unre-
the community. alistic, this has not proven to be true (Domina, Conley,
& Farkas, 2011; Reynolds & Baird, 2010). It is impor-
tant, however, that adolescents who are encouraged
to go to college be given the information and skills
they need in order to fulfill their plans and succeed
(Roderick, Coca, & Nagaoka, 2011; Rosenbaum, 2011).

Educational Achievement school performance A measure of achievement


Educational achievement is usually defined in one of based on an individual’s grades in school.
three ways: school performance (the grades students
academic achievement Achievement that is
earn in school), academic achievement (their perfor- measured by standardized tests of scholastic ability
mance on standardized tests), or educational attain- or knowledge.
ment (the number of years of schooling they complete).
These different measures of educational achievement educational attainment The number of years of
schooling completed by an individual.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 398 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

398 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

THE IMPORTANCE OF on IQ tests. The disadvantages of poorer youngsters in


SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS achievement test scores persist—and may even in-
crease—throughout elementary and secondary school
❚ A Head Start for the More Affluent One of
(Rouse, Brooks-Gunn, & McLanahan, 2005). Because
the most powerful influences on educational achieve-
progress in high school depends so heavily on having a
ment is the socioeconomic status of the adolescent’s
solid foundation of basic academic competence, ado-
family. Five decades of studies have shown over and
lescents who enter secondary school without having
over that middle-class adolescents score higher on basic
mastered basic academic skills quickly fall behind, and
tests of academic skills, earn higher grades, and com-
some leave high school before graduating.
plete more years of schooling than their working-class
and lower-class peers (Muller, Stage, & Kinzie, 2001;
Schoon, Parsons, & Sacker, 2004). Adolescents who ❚ Early Intervention One bit of encouraging news
come from lower socioeconomic levels are more likely on this front comes from long-term evaluations of inter-
to score lower than their more advantaged peers on ventions designed to improve the academic achievement
standardized tests of achievement (Sackett, Kuncel, of very poor youngsters who, by virtue of their poverty,
Arneson, Cooper, & Waters, 2009). Although some of are at high risk for academic failure (F. Campbell &
the socioeconomic gaps in school achievement have Ramey, 1995; A. Reynolds & Temple, 1998). In these eval-
narrowed, disparities in achievement between the social uations, researchers compared groups of adolescents who
classes remain strong, and the importance of socioeco- had participated in an intensive educational program
nomic status in determining educational achievement during their preschool and elementary school years with
remains substantial across all ethnic groups (Goyette & matched samples of adolescents who had had the pre-
Xie, 1999; Goza & Ryabov, 2009; S. Hanson, 1994; Kao school intervention only, the elementary school interven-
& Tienda, 1998; Lucas, 1996; Teachman & Paasch, tion only, or no educational intervention at all. The
1998). Socioeconomic status also influences adolescent interventions were targeted at improving the children’s
achievement through neighborhood processes. Poor school skills and at strengthening the links between par-
Black students who live in neighborhoods with a rela- ents and their child’s school.
tively higher proportion of middle-class neighbors Long-term evaluations show that individuals who
place more value on education and try harder in school participated in preschool interventions (with or without
than comparably poor students who live in disadvan- participation in elementary school interventions) per-
taged neighborhoods (Ceballo, McLoyd, & Toyokawa, form significantly better in school during adolescence
2004; E. B. Stewart, Stewart, & Simons, 2007). than those who did not (F. Campbell & Ramey, 1995). In
Socioeconomic status, therefore, influences both one study, participating in both the preschool and the
educational achievement and attainment and, as a con- elementary school programs provided additional advan-
sequence, influences occupational achievement as well. tages over the preschool program alone (A. Reynolds,
However, keep in mind that variations within socioeco- Temple, Robertson, & Mann, 2001). Interestingly, how-
nomic categories are often as substantial as differences ever, adolescents who had been in the elementary school
between categories. Not all youngsters from affluent program but not the preschool program had no advan-
backgrounds go further in school than adolescents tages over those who had been in no intervention at all
from poorer families, and many youngsters from eco- (F. Campbell & Ramey, 1995). These findings suggest
nomically disadvantaged households go on to earn col- that intervening prior to entering first grade may be ex-
lege and postgraduate degrees. tremely important in preventing long-term academic
One big reason that family background is related to problems among impoverished adolescents and that ex-
educational achievement is that children from lower so- tended participation in educational programs may be
cioeconomic levels are more likely to enter elementary better than short-term participation. Consistent with
school scoring low on tests of basic academic compe- this, longitudinal research shows that school difficulties
tence (Rouse, Brooks-Gunn, & McLanahan, 2005). as early as kindergarten are predictive of poor school
These initial differences reflect both genetic and envi- performance in adolescence (Hamre & Pianta, 2001).
ronmental factors. Middle-class adults generally have One reason for the relatively poorer school perfor-
higher IQs than lower-class adults, and this advantage is mance of disadvantaged youth, therefore, is that these
passed on to their children—both through inheritance youngsters begin school at a distinct academic disad-
and through the benefit that middle-class youngsters vantage. A second reason for the disparity is stress, how-
receive from growing up under more favorable envi- ever—both before and during adolescence. Adolescents
ronmental conditions (C. S. Chen, Lee, & Stevenson, who come from lower-class backgrounds experience
1996; Teachman, 1996). Affluent youngsters receive more stressful life events, report more daily hassles, and
better health care and better nutrition, for example, attend schools with more negative climates (DuBois,
both of which contribute to their higher performance Felner, Meares, & Krier, 1994; Felner et al., 1995; Gillock
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 399 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 399

& Reyes, 1999; Pungello, Kupersmidt, Burchinal, &


Patterson, 1996). Stress has been shown to adversely
affect adolescents’ mental health, well-being, and school
performance (DuBois, Felner, Brand, Adan, & Evans,
1992; Felner et al., 1995).

❚ Parental Involvement Some studies also sug-


gest that parents from higher social classes are more
likely to be involved in their adolescent’s education,
especially through formal parent–teacher organiza-
tions, like the PTA or PTO (Shumow & Miller, 2001).
Middle- and upper-middle-class parents are also more
likely to have information about their child’s school, to
be responsive to their child’s school problems, and to
help select more rigorous courses for their child to take
(Crosnoe & Huston, 2007). Because adolescents whose
parents are involved in their schooling perform better
than those whose parents are not involved, youngsters
Adolescents whose parents are involved in their schooling perform
from higher social classes may achieve more in school better than adolescents whose parents are not. One reason that
than their less advantaged peers in part because of students from higher social classes do better in school is that their
their parents’ more active involvement (Henry, Cava- parents tend to be more involved.
nagh, & Oetting, 2011; V. Lee & Croninger, 1994). In
addition, parents with greater economic resources are
able to provide their children with more cultural capi-
Although more research on successful students from
tal, which, as we have seen, is an important contributor
poor backgrounds is sorely needed, several findings
to school success (Roscigno & Ainsworth-Darnell,
suggest that what might be most important is social
1999).
support for academic achievement: the presence of
warm and encouraging parents who raise their children
authoritatively, take an interest in their children’s aca-
MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION demic progress, and hold high aspirations for their
In light of the profound impact that children’s educational attainment, as well as the avail-
socioeconomic status has on student ability of peers who support and encourage academic
achievement, what would you suggest success (Brody, Stoneman, & Flor, 1995; Goza & Ryabov,
as policies or practices to raise the 2009; Melby, Conger, Fang, Wickrama, & Conger, 2008;
achievement of poor youth? Think Steinberg, 1996; Sui-Chu & Willms, 1996). In other
about people you know who overcame words, positive relations at home and the encouragement
economic disadvantage and were highly of significant others can in some circumstances over-
successful in school (perhaps you are come the negative influence of socioeconomic disad-
in this category). To what would you attribute this vantage.
success?
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN EDUCATIONAL
ACHIEVEMENT
Socioeconomic differences in school achievement
obviously reflect the cumulative and combined effects Among the most controversial—and intriguing—
of a variety of influences, and it is simplistic to explain findings in research on adolescents’ achievement are
social class differences in achievement without consid- those concerning ethnic differences in school success.
ering these factors simultaneously. What is perhaps On average, the educational achievement of Black and
more interesting—and more worthy of scientific Hispanic students—virtually however indexed—lags
study—is the question of what it is about the many behind that of White students, and all three groups
youngsters from economically disadvantaged back- achieve less in school than do Asian students. Although
grounds who are successful that accounts for their over- some of these differences can be attributed to socioeco-
coming the tremendous odds against them. Put nomic differences among these ethnic groups, the group
concretely, the successful college student who comes disparities persist even after socioeconomic factors are
from an environment of severe economic disadvantage taken into account (Fuligni, Hughes, & Way, 2009). The
has had to overcome incredible barriers. academic superiority of Asian students tends to emerge
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 400 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

400 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

during the transition into junior high school—when future are very similar across ethnic groups (Chang,
most other students’ grades typically decline—and it Chen, Greenberger, Dooley, & Heckhausen, 2006).
persists through high school and into college (Fuligni, If anything, it may be adolescents’ fear of failure,
1994; Fuligni & Witkow, 2004). What has been most rather than their desire (or lack of desire) to succeed, that
intriguing to social scientists is the observation that matters most (Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992).
Black and Hispanic students have educational aspira- Asian youngsters not only believe in the value of school
tions and attitudes that are similar to those of Asian and success but also are very anxious about the possible neg-
White students but significantly poorer academic skills, ative repercussions of not doing well in school, in terms
habits, and behavior (Ainsworth-Darnell & Downey, of both occupational success and their parents’ disap-
1998). If Black and Hispanic students ostensibly have pointment (Eaton & Dembo, 1997; Herman, 2009;
the same long-term goals as other students, why do they Steinberg, 1996). Moreover, many Asian youth believe
not behave in similar ways? that the only way they can succeed in mainstream
American society is through educational achievement
❚ False Optimism Rather Than Realistic Pessi- (Sue & Okazaki, 1990). Interestingly, Asian students’
mism Several theories have been advanced to explain sense of obligation to their parents—a factor frequently
this finding. One set of theories involves the perceptions suggested as a reason for their high rates of school
that adolescents have about the likely payoff of hard success—does not seem to play a very important role in
work in school. Some writers, for instance, have argued predicting school achievement. If anything, being ex-
that even though they have high aspirations in the ab- pected to assist the family by performing household
stract, many minority youth do not believe that educa- chores and other family work—something that is espe-
tional success will have substantial occupational payoff cially salient in Asian and Hispanic households—has a
for them, because discrimination and prejudice will negative impact on school performance (Fuligni, Tseng,
limit their actual opportunities (Mickelson, 1990). & Lam, 1999; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009).
Although intuitively appealing, this theory has not re-
ceived convincing empirical support (Fuligni et al., ❚ The Burden of “Acting White” Another pop-
2009; Herman, 2009). It is true that adolescents who be- ular explanation for ethnic differences in achievement
lieve they have been victims of discrimination, or who is that some ethnic minority students underperform in
believe that their opportunities for occupational success school because they are stigmatized for “acting White”
are unfairly constrained by society, achieve less in school if they try to do well and, as a consequence, develop an
and report more emotional distress than do peers who “oppositional” identity that is hostile to doing well in
do not hold these beliefs (C. Fisher, Wallace, & Fenton, school (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986). This view has not
2000; R. Taylor et al., 1994; P. Wood & Clay, 1996). It is held up, however, perhaps because the extent of this
also true that students who are more confident about negative peer pressure varies from school to school and
and oriented to the future do better in school (Beal & from peer group to peer group (e.g., Akom, 2003;
Crockett, 2010; W. T. Brown & Jones, 2004; Oyserman, D. Downey & Ainsworth-Darnell, 2002; Farkas, Lleras,
Bybee, & Terry, 2006). It is not true, however, that Black & Maczuga, 2002; Goza & Ryabov, 2009; Horvat &
or Hispanic youngsters are more likely than other Lewis, 2003; Tyson, Darity, & Castellino, 2005). Black
adolescents to believe that their opportunities for suc- peer groups are not all the same, and while some may
cess are blocked (Ainsworth-Darnell & Downey, 1998; disparage academic achievement, many admire it.
D. Downey & Ainsworth-Darnell, 2002; Kao & Tienda, Here’s how one academically successful Black student
1998; Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992). Indeed, replied when asked if students from different peer
several studies indicate that Black and Hispanic youth groups treated her differently:
have more optimistic beliefs and positive feelings about
I think, yes. The black people who, say, aren’t as smart
school than other students (e.g., Ainsworth-Darnell &
as me or Renée or whoever else they say, “Oh, you act
Downey, 1998; D. Downey, Ainsworth, & Qian, 2009;
white” because we’re in high classes or whatever, and
Shernoff & Schmidt, 2008; Voelkl, 1997). Actually, some that really upsets me. They say we talk white. I don’t even
research suggests that beliefs about the likelihood of fu- have like proper English or whatever, but they say we talk
ture discrimination may motivate adolescents to per- white because we use all these big words and everything.
form better in school (perhaps because they feel that To me, that’s total ignorance.
they will need to be even better prepared than others
to overcome prejudicial treatment), but that feeling dis- When asked to describe her own friends, however, she
criminated against in the present, by classmates or said:
teachers, hinders academic achievement (perhaps by They’re always, “I’m so proud of you. You have a job,
causing psychological distress or hopelessness) (Eccles you’re still in school, and you’re not pregnant.” All this
& Roeser, 2009; Mattison & Aber, 2007). All in all, other good stuff. But it makes me feel good. (Horvat &
however, adolescents’ hopes and aspirations for the Lewis, 2003, pp. 270–272)
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 401 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 401

Another successful student from the same school noted ❚ The Success of Immigrants Of course, as is the
that even her friends who had dropped out of school in case with research on socioeconomic differences in ed-
the 8th or 9th grade were supportive of her achievement: ucational achievement, research on ethnic differences
indicates that there are large and important variations
They just treat me as one of them. School’s not a subject
within as well as between ethnic groups. First, there are
that really comes up as far as my neighborhood is
concerned. They’ll say, “How was your day at school?” differences in educational achievement among young-
“Fine.” They accept me for what I am and what I do. If I sters from different countries of origin who may be
am smart, I am smart. It seems they really congratulated classified together by researchers into the same larger
me, if anything, especially now. So many of my friends ethnic group for purposes of statistical comparison. For
have told me, “I’m so proud of you.” Basically pushing me example, although both groups are classified as Asian,
on. (Horvat & Lewis, 2003, p. 272) Chinese American adolescents have much higher aca-
demic achievement than Filipino Americans; similarly,
❚ Ethnic Differences in Beliefs An alternative there are large differences in academic achievement
account of ethnic differences in achievement stresses among Puerto Rican, Cuban American, and Mexican
differences in ethnic groups’ beliefs about ability. We American adolescents, all of whom are classified as His-
noted earlier that adolescents who believe that intelli- panic (Fuligni et al., 2009).
gence is malleable are more likely to be intrinsically moti- Second, studies of ethnic minority youngsters show
vated and, as a consequence, academically successful. It is that foreign-born adolescents, as well as those who are
therefore interesting to note that Asian cultures tend to children of immigrants, tend to achieve more in school
place more emphasis on effort than on ability in explain- than do minority youngsters who are second- or third-
ing school success and are more likely to believe that all generation Americans, a finding that has now emerged
students have the capacity to succeed (H. Stevenson & in many studies of Asian, Latino, and Caribbean youth
Stigler, 1992). By and large, students from Asian back- (Fuligni et al., 2009; Kao, 1999; Lansford, 2011; Rong
grounds tend to be more invested in mastering the mate- & Brown, 2001). One explanation for the so-called
rial than in simply performing well—an orientation that, immigrant paradox has been that part of becoming
as we saw earlier, contributes to school success (J. Li, acculturated to American society—at least among
2006). It is also important to note that Asian students— teenagers—may be learning to devalue academic success.
both in the United States and in Asia—spend significantly There is also some evidence that the higher school
more time each week on homework and other school- achievement of immigrant youth—at least among ado-
related activities, and significantly less time socializing lescents who have immigrated from Mexico—may be
and watching television, than do other youth (Asakawa & due to the higher quality of the schooling they receive
Csikszentmihalyi, 2000; Caplan, Choy, & Whitmore, before coming to the United States (Padilla & Gonzalez,
1992; Fuligni & Stevenson, 1995; Steinberg, 1996). 2001). As psychologist Andrew Fuligni has pointed out,
You may be interested to know that, contrary to the exceptional achievement of immigrant youth is all
popular belief, Asian students do not pay a price for the more remarkable in light of the fact that these
their superior achievement in terms of increased anxi- adolescents typically have much greater family obliga-
ety, depression, stress, or social competence; the suicide tions—providing financial support to their parents, for
rate among American teenagers is substantially higher instance—than their American-born peers (Fuligni &
than it is among Japanese youth, for example (X. Chen, Witkow, 2004). Indeed, the stronger sense of family
Rubin, & Li, 1997; Crystal et al., 1994; C. King, Akiyama, obligation felt by immigrant youth may actually con-
& Elling, 1996). However, there is some evidence that tribute to, rather than interfere with, school success
the long hours of studying that Korean high school (van Geel & Vedder, 2011).
seniors must endure while in “examination hell”—the
period of preparation for that country’s college entrance
exam—may take its toll on adolescents’ mental health
(M. Lee & Larson, 2000). During regular periods of MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
school in the United States, however, Asian students’ Many immigrant adolescents in the United States
moods while studying are significantly more positive achieve more in school than their counterparts from
than those of other students (Asakawa & Csikszentmih- the same ethnic group who were born in America—
alyi, 1998), and the links between academic motivation despite the fact that adolescents
and various indices of happiness and adjustment are who are immigrants often arrive
stronger among Asian adolescents than other youth without proficiency in English or
(Asakawa & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Among Asian familiarity with American culture.
students more than their peers from other ethnic How do you account for this?
groups, then, engagement in academics is linked to
positive emotion and well-being.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 402 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

402 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

550

Verbal
Score

500

Mathematical

450
1968 1972 1976 1980 1984 1988 1992 1996 2000 2004 2008 2012
FIGURE 12.5 Changes in SAT scores over time. (College Board, 2012)

Third, and most important, studies indicate that non-Hispanic White adults have completed high
within all ethnic groups, students achieve more when school, compared with 82% of Blacks and 88% of Asian
they feel a sense of belonging to their school, when Americans, but only 62% of Hispanic adults have (U.S.
they see the connection between academic accom- Census Bureau, 2011). In light of the rapidly increasing
plishment and future success, when their friends and size of the Latino population in the United States, the
parents value and support educational achievement, gap in educational attainment between Latinos and
and when their parents are effective monitors of their non-Latinos is one of the most important challenges
children’s behavior and schooling (Alva, 1993; C. S. facing American educational institutions.
Chen et al., 1996; Chun & Dickson, 2011; Conchas,
2001; Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; D. Ford and ❚ More Schooling, But Less Learning Trends in
Harris, 1996; Luthar, 1994; Mandara, Varner, Greene, academic achievement (what students know) have not
& Richman, 2009b; Murdock, 1994; Reyes & Jason, paralleled trends in educational attainment (how many
1993; Steinberg, 1996). The especially close and sup- years of schooling they have completed), however. In
portive relationships characteristic of immigrant fami- other words, although more students are staying in
lies likely contributes to school success (Suárez-Orozco, school longer, they are not necessarily learning more.
Rhodes, & Milburn, 2009). For example, as Figure 12.5 indicates, between 1970
and 1980, average scores on the Scholastic Assessment
CHANGES IN EDUCATIONAL Test (SAT) declined by about 35 points on the verbal
ACHIEVEMENT OVER TIME portion and 20 points on the math. Scores remained
more or less flat between 1980 and 1990, when math
As we noted in Chapter 6, more students are going on (but not verbal) scores began to rise. Verbal and math
to postsecondary education today than ever before. scores both dropped slightly from 2005 on. (The one
Today, three-fourths of high school graduates enroll in exception to this general trend is among Asian students,
college, two-thirds of them immediately after gradua- whose scores on both subscales have risen substantially
tion (National Center for Education Statistics, 2012). in the last 10 years.) Moreover, the gap in SAT scores
Although ethnic differences in educational attainment between Black and Hispanic students on the one hand,
have narrowed over the past 40 years, there remain sub- and Asian and White students on the other, remains
stantial gaps in attainment between White and non- substantial and virtually unchanged over the past de-
White individuals, and especially between White and cade (College Board, 2012).
Hispanic individuals. Thus, whereas about 35% of all The relatively poor showing of American adoles-
non-Hispanic White adults ages 25 and over, and 50% cents on standardized tests of achievement has been
of all Asian adults of this age, are college graduates, carefully documented in a series of reports based on the
only 20% of Black adults and only 14% of Hispanic National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP).
adults are (U.S. Census Bureau, 2011). Discrepancies in This national assessment of student achievement is
rates of high school graduation between Hispanic and conducted by the federal government in order to track
non-Hispanic Americans are also substantial—90% of trends in educational achievement over time. Because
the NAEP tests have been administered regularly for
nearly 40 years, it is possible to compare the achieve-
National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) A periodic ment levels of today’s adolescents with their counter-
testing of American 4th-, 8th-, and 12th-graders by the federal parts 4 decades ago.
government, used to track achievement.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 403 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 403

many White and Asian students score in the proficient


MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION
range as do Hispanic students. The gaps are even larger
International achievement comparisons indicate that in math—about 8 times as many White and Asian stu-
American students fare just about as well as students dents are competent in algebra as are Black or Hispanic
from other countries when they are in elementary students (National Center for Education Statistics,
school, but that the gap widens as students move into 2006b). Obviously, this gap has important implications
middle school and is quite substantial for the labor market success of adolescents from differ-
by the time of high school, where ent ethnic groups.
American students are among the
lowest-achieving in the industrial- ❚ International Competitions Although the
ized world. Why do you think this is poor achievement test performance of Black and Latino
the case? students is certainly cause for concern, there is no rea-
son to be sanguine about the performance of White
high school students. Since the 1970s, their achievement
test scores have remained more or less stagnant, and
According to recent NAEP reports, over the past 45 U.S. scores on standardized tests of math and science are
years adolescent achievement in reading, writing, math, mediocre in comparison with scores of other industrial-
and science has improved only slightly among 13-year- ized countries. More interestingly, the gap between
olds and not at all among 17-year-olds, despite massive American students’ performance and that of students
national efforts at education reform. This is consistent from other countries widens as they move from elemen-
with other trends in achievement data, which show that tary to middle to high school. That is, when elementary
American students have been making gains at the ele- school students from around the world are compared,
mentary school level, holding ground at the middle American students fare just about as well as students
school level, and losing ground at the high school level from other countries. When middle school students are
(J. Lee, 2010). Contemporary 17-year-olds, for exam- compared, Americans perform more poorly than their
ple, score no better than their counterparts did in the counterparts from most industrialized Asian coun-
early 1970s in reading or math, and worse than their tries, but at a level comparable to young adolescents
counterparts did in science. Among 13-year-olds, read- from most western European countries, and consider-
ing scores in 2011 were only marginally better than they ably better than adolescents from less developed nations
had been in the 1970s, and science scores showed no (National Center for Education Statistics, 2009). But
improvement between 1970 and 2000. In fact, science when the comparison focuses on high school students,
scores actually declined in the late 1990s and have re- the gap between the United States and other countries is
mained flat since then. The only bit of good news has substantial (J. Schmidt, 2003). According to recent in-
been in math scores among 13-year-olds, with scores ternational comparisons, 15-year-olds in the United
improving steadily since the 1970s (National Center for States ranked slightly above average in reading, well be-
Education Statistics, 2011a). Perhaps more importantly, low average in science, and near the bottom of the list in
most analyses of the NAEP data indicate that the mod- math (OECD, 2010) (see Table 12.1).
est gains in achievement that have occurred during re- The relatively poor performance of American high
cent years have been in relatively simple skills. Only a school students in international competitions persists
handful of students, at any age, or in any subject area, despite the fact that spending on education in the
score at a level that is designated “advanced” or “supe- United States is among the world’s highest (Walberg,
rior,” whereas large proportions of students score in the 1998). Despite how much is spent, at 4-year American
lowest category, “below basic.” When gains have been colleges and universities, 20% of entering college
made (and they are generally very small), they tend to freshmen require some sort of remedial education in
be in the proportion of students who have moved from order to do college-level work; at community colleges,
the basic into the “proficient” range, rather than from half of all entering students do. As a result, employers
“proficient” to “advanced.” and postsecondary educational institutions alike
As is the case in rates of high school graduation and today devote vast amounts of money to remedial
SAT scores, the gap in achievement test performance education—colleges and universities spend an esti-
among ethnic groups narrowed during the 1970s, but a mated $3 billion annually covering subject matter that
wide disparity still exists. The achievement gap did not students should have mastered before graduating from
shrink at all during the 1980s, widened a bit during the high school (Complete College, 2012). And, as as-
1990s, and has not changed since then (Rampey, Dion, tounding as it may sound, more than 80% of college
& Donahue, 2009). About 3 times as many White and students enrolled in remedial education graduated
Asian students score in the proficient range in writing, from high school with a GPA of 3.0 or better (Strong
for example, as do Black students, and nearly twice as American Schools, 2008).
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 404 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

404 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

TABLE 12.1 An International Education Test


Results of 2009 PISA (Program for International Student Assessment) test of 15-year-old students in
65 countries.

SCIENCE PISA READING PISA MATH PISA


SCORE SCORE SCORE

Shanghai, China* 575 Shanghai, China 556 Shanghai, China 600


Finland 554 Korea 539 Singapore 562
Hong Kong, China 549 Finland 536 Hong Kong, China 555
Singapore 542 Hong Kong, China 533 Korea 546
Japan 539 Singapore 526 Taiwan 543
Korea 538 Canada 524 Finland 541
New Zealand 532 New Zealand 521 Liechtenstein 536
Canada 529 Japan 520 Switzerland 534
Estonia 528 Australia 515 Japan 529
Australia 527 Netherlands 508 Canada 527
Netherlands 522 Belgium 506 Netherlands 526
Taiwan 520 Norway 503 Macao, China 525
Germany 520 Estonia 501 New Zealand 519
Liechtenstein 520 Switzerland 501 Belgium 515
Switzerland 517 Poland 500 Australia 514
Britain 514 Iceland 500 Germany 513
Slovenia 512 United States 500 Estonia 512
Macao, China 511 Liechtenstein 499 Iceland 507
Poland 508 Sweden 497 Denmark 503
Ireland 508 Germany 497 Slovenia 501
Belgium 507 Ireland 496 Norway 498
Hungary 503 France 496 France 497
United States 502 Taiwan 495 Slovakia 497
AVERAGE SCORE 501 Denmark 495 AVERAGE SCORE 497
Czech Republic 500 Britain 494 Austria 496
Norway 500 Hungary 494 Poland 495
Denmark 499 AVERAGE SCORE 494 Sweden 494
France 498 Portugal 489 Czech Republic 493
Iceland 496 Macao, China 487 Britain 492
Sweden 495 Italy 486 Hungary 490
Austria 494 Latvia 484 Luxembourg 489
Latvia 494 Slovenia 483 United States 487
Potugal 493 Greece 483 Ireland 487

*In the study, China was represented by the city Shanghai and by the administrative regions Hong Kong
and Macao.

Source: Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, 2010.

If more American students are remaining in high very, very little time is spent on writing; that there has
school, and so many are going on to college, why are been a pervasive decline in the difficulty of textbooks;
their achievement test scores so low according to abso- that parents are not encouraging academic pursuits at
lute, historical, and international standards? Experts home; that students are not spending sufficient time on
suggest several reasons: that teachers are not spending their studies outside of school; that students are permit-
enough time on basic instruction in the classroom, and ted to choose what courses they take; and that students
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 405 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 405

know that, thanks to “grade inflation,” they can earn Statistics, 2012). There are huge variations in dropout
good grades without working very hard (D. Hayes, rates from region to region, however; indeed, in some
Wolfer, & Wolfe, 1996; Kiuhara, Graham, & Hawken, urban districts, 50% of students leave school prema-
2009; Owen, 1995; Public Agenda, 1997; J. Schmidt, turely (Swanson, 2009). Black and American-born His-
2003; Steinberg, 2003). panic youngsters drop out of high school at nearly
twice the rate of White youngsters, and 5 times more
frequently than Asian youngsters, but foreign-born
DROPPING OUT OF HIGH SCHOOL
Hispanic youngsters drop out at more than 3 times the
There was a time when leaving high school before grad- rate of American-born Hispanic youth (see Figure 12.6)
uating did not have the dire consequences that it does (National Center for Education Statistics, 2012).
today. With changes in the labor force, however, have
come changes in the educational requirements for en- ❚ Correlates of Dropping Out Given the find-
try into the world of work. Today, educational attain- ings on educational achievement discussed earlier, the
ment is a powerful predictor of adult occupational other correlates of dropping out come as no surprise. In
success and earnings. Not surprisingly, high school addition to the higher prevalence of dropping out
dropouts are far more likely than graduates to live at or among foreign-born Hispanic youngsters, adolescents
near the poverty level, to experience unemployment, to who leave high school before graduating are more likely
depend on government-subsidized income mainte- to come from lower socioeconomic levels, poor com-
nance programs, to become pregnant while still a teen- munities, large families, single-parent families, permis-
ager, and to be involved in delinquent and criminal sive or disengaged families, and households where little
activity (Manlove, 1998; Rumberger, 2012). reading material is available. In short, adolescents who
Because there are different ways of counting drop- drop out of school are more likely to come from back-
outs, different studies often report very different fig- grounds with limited financial, social, and cultural
ures. For example, many students drop out of school capital (K. L. Alexander, Entwisle, & Kabbani, 2001;
temporarily but return in their early 20s and obtain a L. E. Davis, Ajzen, Saunders, & Williams, 2002; Pong &
diploma or GED—so while these students would be Ju, 2000; Rumberger, 2012).
classified as dropouts at the age of 17, they would be Coupled with this disadvantage in background, ado-
classified as graduates if they were surveyed just a few lescents who drop out of high school also are more
years later. Based on the definition used by the U.S. De- likely to have a history of poor school performance, low
partment of Education—the proportion of individuals school involvement, multiple changes of schools, poor
aged 16 to 24 who are not enrolled in school and who performance on standardized tests of achievement and
have not earned a high school diploma or GED—the intelligence, negative school experiences, and a variety
dropout rate has declined steadily over the past half of behavioral problems, some of which contribute to
century, to about 8% (National Center for Education academic failure and some of which result from it

50

40

30
Percent

20

10

*
0
White Black Hispanic Asian Pacific AmericanTwo or White Black Hispanic Asian Pacific American Two or
Islander Indian/ more Islander Indian/ more
Alaska races Alaska races
Native Native
Native-born Foreign-born
Race/ethnicity

*Reporting standards not met (too few cases).


FIGURE 12.6 Dropout rates vary as a function of ethnicity and place of birth. (National Center for Education Statistics, 2012)
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 406 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

406 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

In some urban school districts, well over 50% of students drop out of high school.

(Battin-Pearson et al., 2000; Crosnoe, 2006; K. L. history of poor school performance), there is neverthe-
Henry, Knight, & Thornberry, 2012; Hirschfield, 2009; less diversity within this population. According to one
Rumberger, 2012). Many high school dropouts had to extensive study of Canadian students (Janosz, LeBlanc,
repeat one or more grades in elementary school; indeed, Boulerice, & Tremblay, 2000), there are at least four dis-
having been held back is one of the strongest predictors tinct groups of dropouts: (1) quiet dropouts (whose
of dropping out (Rumberger, 2012). histories and personal characteristics actually look very
In other words, dropping out of high school is not so similar to those of students who do not drop out of
much a discrete decision made during the adolescent years school, but who appear somewhat withdrawn—they
as it is the culmination of a long process (Rumberger, almost seem to “fade out” rather than drop out, perhaps
2012; Sweeten, Bushway, & Paternoster, 2009). Specific as a result of depression), (2) disengaged dropouts
factors may instigate a student’s final decision to leave (whose dropping out appears mainly to be the result of
school—a suspension for misbehavior, a failed course, low commitment to school and poor academic motiva-
an unintended pregnancy, the lure of a job—but by and tion), (3) low-achiever dropouts (whose dropping out
large, dropping out is a process characterized by a is primarily the result of very poor school perfor-
history of repeated academic failure and increasing mance), and (4) maladjusted dropouts (whose drop-
alienation from school (Henry et al., 2012). While ping out is part of a larger constellation of behavioral
programs designed to enhance adolescents’ academic and psychological problems). The idea that different
skills have been largely unsuccessful in preventing developmental histories lead to dropping out is impor-
dropping out, one approach that has met with success tant to the design of preventive interventions, because
has focused on involving at-risk adolescents in service it suggests that different sorts of programs may work
learning and in guided discussions of their life options, for different sorts of students. For instance, dropping
which may help them see how important it is to gradu- out early in high school is more likely to be the result of
ate from high school (J. Allen, Philliber, Herrling, & disciplinary problems at school, whereas dropping out
Kuperminc, 1997). later in high school is more likely to be driven by the
Although adolescents who drop out of school often desire to work (Stearns & Glennie, 2006). Other studies
share certain characteristics in common (for example, a indicate that it is important to distinguish between
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 407 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 407

students who temporarily drop out of school but re- in school and complete more years of schooling
turn at some later point and obtain their GED—as do than do their less advantaged counterparts.
between one-third and one-half of all dropouts—and • There are ethnic differences in educational achieve-
those who leave and never return (Entwisle, Alexander, ment above and beyond those attributable to
& Olson, 2004). socioeconomic status. In the United States, Asian
adolescents outperform White students, who, in
turn, do better than Black, Hispanic, or American
Indian students. One reason for the superior per-
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION formance of Asian students is that they are more
Based on what we know likely to hold the sorts of beliefs about achieve-
about the causes and ment that are predictive of success in school in all
consequences of drop- ethnic groups.
ping out, what steps • The low level of educational achievement among
should be taken to reduce American youth has been a concern for several de-
the dropout rate? cades. Although some gains in scores on standard-
ized tests of achievement were reported during the
mid-1980s, with the exception of early adolescents’
math scores, achievement by and large has not
❚ School Factors Although most research on the improved since then, and American high school
causes of dropping out has focused on characteristics of students continue to fare poorly in international
adolescents who leave school prematurely, some studies comparisons.
have focused on the schools that dropouts leave • Dropouts are more likely than their peers to be
(Rumberger, 2012). In general, dropping out is less foreign-born Hispanic youth, to come from eco-
likely from schools where the environment is orderly, nomically disadvantaged backgrounds, to come
where academic pursuits are emphasized, and where from single-parent homes, and to have had a poor
the faculty is supportive and committed (V. Lee & record of educational achievement throughout
Burkam, 2003). Students who are at particularly high their school years. Dropping out is not so much a
risk of dropping out (low-achieving, economically dis- decision that is made during adolescence
advantaged, and foreign-born Hispanic youth) are as it is the culmination of
helped especially by having teachers who are sources of a long process that
social support and guidance (Croninger & Lee, 2001; begins early on.
Enriquez, 2011). Dropout rates are also higher in larger
schools that group students according to ability and
that fail a relatively high proportion of students in the
early years of high school (Roderick & Camburn, 1999).
Consistent with this, some research suggests that drop-
Occupational Achievement
out rates may be reduced in some cases by permitting School, rather than work, is the setting in which achieve-
students who are having educational difficulties to ment is most often studied by contemporary scholars
change schools, rather than leaving school altogether interested in adolescence. Although many individuals in
(V. Lee & Burkam, 1992). Although some educators previous generations began their occupational careers
have expressed concern about the recent trend toward during adolescence, this is very rare today in most in-
toughening graduation requirements and ending social dustrialized societies, where the majority of individuals
promotion—the practice of promoting students from pursue some form of postsecondary education before
one grade to the next on the basis of age rather than entering into full-time work. With the exception of ap-
actual achievement—evaluations of policies such as the prenticeships, which are popular in only a handful of
use of high school exit examinations to determine European countries, the work individuals perform dur-
whether students can graduate show that they do not ing adolescence is rarely relevant to their future careers; as
increase the rate of dropping out or differentially affect we saw in Chapter 7, it is mainly a means of earning
minority and White students (Bishop, 2001; Warren, spending money. Work experiences in adolescence, in gen-
Grodsky, & Lee, 2008; Warren & Jenkins, 2005). eral, have little or no impact on adolescents’ plans or
aspirations for adult work, especially among students from
RECAP nonpoor families (Entwisle, Alexander, & Olson, 2005;
• Socioeconomic status is an extremely powerful in-
fluence on educational achievement. Generally, ad- social promotion The practice of promoting students from one
olescents from higher social classes perform better grade to the next automatically, regardless of their school
performance.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 408 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

408 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

M. Johnson, 2002), although it is possible that the small occupational plans originally focused on adolescence,
minority of adolescents who hold “good” jobs may learn changes in the labor force, requiring individuals to
something about their career interests from them. Ac- complete more and more education before beginning
cording to one analysis, fast food, restaurant, and retail their careers, have made young adulthood a far more
jobs provide the fewest opportunities to build career- important phase than adolescence for the development
related skills, whereas office and clerical jobs are among of occupational plans. For many individuals, the devel-
the best (Staff, Messersmith, & Schulenberg, 2009). opment of occupational plans may not take place until
Although we often think of school and work as sepa- the final years of college, and deciding on a specific
rate domains, achievement is one aspect of psychoso- career may not even begin until well after college grad-
cial development during adolescence that links them uation. As we saw in Chapter 3, many developmental
together. The number of years of schooling an individ- challenges that in the past were faced during the teenage
ual completes is the single best indicator of his or her years are now delayed until the 20s.
eventual occupational success (Arum & Hout, 1998). It
is not simply that adolescents benefit in the labor force
INFLUENCES ON
by having a high school diploma or a college degree.
OCCUPATIONAL CHOICES
Although these credentials matter, research shows that
each year of education—even without graduating— What makes one individual choose to become an
adds significantly to occupational success. In other attorney and another decide to be a teacher? Why do
words, individuals who have completed 3 years of col- some students pursue careers in psychology while
lege earn more money, on average, than individuals others major in engineering? Researchers have long
who have completed only 1 year, even though neither been interested in the reasons that individuals end up
group has a college degree in hand. in certain careers (Neuenschwander & Kracke, 2011).
Researchers who are interested in occupational
achievement during adolescence have examined several ❚ Work Values When you think about your future
issues, including the ways in which young people make work, what will you look for in a job? Work values refer
decisions about their careers and the influences on their to the sorts of rewards individuals seek from their jobs
occupational aspirations and expectations. We begin (e.g., M. Johnson, 2002). For example, are you most
with a look at the development of adolescents’ occupa- interested in making a lot of money, in having a secure
tional plans. job, or in having a job that permits you to have a lot of
vacation time? According to most theories of work val-
ues, seven basic types of work rewards define individu-
THE DEVELOPMENT OF als’ work values: extrinsic rewards (earning a high
OCCUPATIONAL PLANS income), security (enjoying job stability), intrinsic re-
The development of occupational plans during adoles- wards (being able to be creative or to learn things from
cence can be viewed in many respects as paralleling, or work), influence (having authority over others or
even as part of, the identity development process power over decision making), altruistic rewards (help-
(Skorikov & Vondracek, 2007; Staff et al., 2009). As with ing others), social rewards (working with people you
developing a coherent sense of identity, the develop- like), and leisure (having an opportunity for free time
ment of occupational plans follows a sequence that or vacation). Individuals choose jobs based on the rela-
involves an examination of one’s traits, abilities, and tive importance of these various work rewards to them
interests; a period of experimentation with different (see Table 12.2).
work roles; and an integration of influences from one’s
past (primarily, identification with familial role mod-
els) with one’s hopes for the future. And as is also the TABLE 12.2 People look for different things in a job.
case with identity development, the development of an Which types of rewards are most important to you?
occupational identity is profoundly influenced by the
Type of Reward Example
social environment in which it takes place.
Changes in the broader environment in which ado- Extrinsic Earning a good income
lescents develop—in this case, changes in the need for Security Enjoying job stability
and accessibility of higher education—have exerted a Intrinsic Having opportunities for creativity
powerful influence on the developmental course of Influence Wielding power over others
occupational planning. Although many theories of
Altruistic Helping others
Social Enjoying one’s coworkers
work values The particular sorts of rewards an individual looks for Leisure Having opportunities for vacation
in a job (extrinsic, intrinsic, social, altruistic, security, influence, or time off
leisure).
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 409 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 409

Several writers have noted that many contemporary fixed during adolescence, but continue to develop and
adolescents have unrealistic and overly ambitious ideas change during the adult years (M. Johnson, 2002).
about the rewards they will derive from their future Indeed, one of the most important influences on per-
work. One set of authors noted that a very large pro- sonality development during adulthood is work itself.
portion of adolescents aspire to levels of work rewards Thus, through working in a job that emphasizes certain
that they are highly unlikely to attain (Schneider & personality characteristics, requires certain abilities, or
Stevenson, 1999). One specific problem is that adoles- provides certain types of rewards, individuals begin to
cents tend to rate almost all work rewards very highly, change their personality, skills, and values. Consequently,
optimistically believing that they can find jobs that sat- a job that seems like a bad match during early adulthood
isfy multiple rewards simultaneously. When they actu- may, over time, become a good match, as the individual
ally enter their first full-time adult jobs, though, they grows and changes in response to the work environment.
soon discover that it is difficult, if not impossible, to For example, someone may not be especially interested
have a career in which, say, one makes a lot of money, is in a job that involves a lot of social interaction but may,
creative, helps other people, enjoys job security, and has because of a tight job market, end up in a teaching
a lot of free time. Over the course of young adulthood, position after graduating from college. Over time, the
then, one of the most important changes that occurs in more he interacts with students, the more appealing the
the domain of occupational development is that indi- interpersonal aspects of the job may become. Eventually,
viduals become both somewhat disillusioned and more he may come to feel that having opportunities for social
focused on what they want from a job, abandoning the interaction on the job is very important.
unrealistic notion that one can “have it all” (B. Roberts, A second problem with theories of career choice that
O’Donnell, & Robins, 2004). As adolescents move into emphasize adolescents’ work values is that they may
young adulthood, the degrees to which they value the underestimate the importance of other factors that in-
extrinsic, altruistic, and social rewards of jobs, which fluence and shape vocational decisions, most impor-
are all strongly valued when individuals are seniors in tantly, the social context in which adolescents make
high school, decline most dramatically, whereas the career decisions (Staff et al., 2009). Many career deci-
values they place on intrinsic rewards and job security, sions are influenced more by individuals’ beliefs about
which are also strong at the end of high school, remain what sorts of jobs are accessible or “appropriate” for
strong (M. Johnson, 2002) (see Figure 12.7). them than by their interests and preferences (M. Johnson,
There are important limitations to theories of career 2002). It is all well and good, for example, for an adoles-
choice that are based solely on reward preferences as- cent to discover that he is well suited for a career in
sessed in adolescence. First, interests and abilities are not medicine, but the realization is of little value if his fam-
ily cannot afford the cost of college or medical school.
Or an adolescent girl may discover through taking a
vocational preference test that she is well suited for
4.0 work in the area of construction or building, but she
may find that her parents, peers, teachers, and potential
employers all discourage her from following this ave-
3.5 nue of employment because they think it is not appro-
priate for women. One study found that early adolescent
Importance

Mexican American girls, in particular, were more likely


3.0 than Black or White girls to have stereotypically female
career goals (N. E. Hill, Ramirez, & Dumka, 2003).
Put most simply, career choices are not made solely on
2.5
the basis of individual preference; they are the result of an
interaction among individual preferences, social influ-
ences, and important forces in the broader social environ-
2.0
18 20 22 24 26 28 30 ment. It is to these influences and forces that we now turn.
Age (years)
Intrinsic Altruistic Extrinsic ❚ The Influence of Parents and Peers No influ-
Security Social Influence ence on occupational choice is stronger than socioeco-
Leisure nomic status, and as a result, adolescents’ occupational
FIGURE 12.7 As adolescents move into young adulthood, ambitions and achievements are highly correlated with
the value they place on the extrinsic, altruistic, and social the ambitions and achievements of those around them
rewards of a job declines, whereas the value they place on (Dubow, Huesmann, Boxer, Pulkkinen, & Kokko, 2006).
intrinsic job rewards and job security remains strong. Youngsters from middle-class families are more likely
(Johnson, 2002) than their less advantaged peers to aspire to and enter
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 410 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

410 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

middle-class occupations. Socioeconomic status also in-


fluences work values, with individuals from higher classes
more likely to value intrinsic rewards and influence and
less likely to value extrinsic rewards and security. The
importance of social class as a determinant of what people
look for in their jobs is strong and constant throughout
adolescence and young adulthood (M. Johnson, 2002).
A variety of explanations have been offered for the
fit between adolescents’ ambitions and the socioeco-
nomic status of those around them. First, and perhaps
most important, occupational attainment—the pres-
tige or status an individual achieves in the world of
work—depends strongly on educational attainment
An individual’s choice of occupation is influenced by many factors,
(Elmore, 2009). As we saw earlier, educational attain-
including the work values he or she has. Contextual factors, such as
ment is greatly influenced by socioeconomic status. job opportunities, are important as well.
Thus, because middle-class adolescents are likely to
complete more years of schooling than their lower-class
peers, economically advantaged adolescents are more
likely to seek and enter higher-status occupations. Fourth, parents, siblings, and other important
Second, middle-class parents, as noted earlier, are sources of influence serve as models for adolescents’
more likely to raise their children in ways that foster the occupational choices (Barber & Eccles, 1992). Although
development of strong achievement orientation and some young people establish career choices through the
career exploration (Kracke, 2002). The development of explicit rejection of their parents’ careers, the weight of
achievement motivation, which has an impact on the evidence suggests that adolescents’ and parents’
school performance, also has an impact on youngsters’ vocations are more similar than different, particularly
occupational ambitions—both directly (in that indi- when the adolescent’s family relationships have been
viduals with strong needs for achievement will express warm and close and when strong identifications have
these needs by aspiring to occupations that provide op- formed between the adolescent and his or her parents.
portunities to achieve status or wealth) and indirectly, Finally, parents—and, to a lesser extent, peers—
through the effects of achievement motivation on aca- influence adolescents’ occupational plans by establish-
demic achievement (in that youngsters who are suc- ing a value context in which certain occupational
cessful in school are likely to be encouraged to seek choices are encouraged and others are discouraged
higher-status occupations and engage in identity explo- (Kohn, 1977). Middle-class families and middle-class
ration). Indeed, parents influence their adolescents’ schools encourage children to value autonomy, self-
career aspirations mainly by influencing their educa- direction, and independence—three features that are
tional achievement (Jodl et al., 2001). more likely to be found in middle-class than in working-
Third, the same opportunities that favor economically class jobs. The children are told, implicitly and explicitly,
advantaged youngsters in the world of education—better how important it is to have freedom, power, and status.
facilities, more opportunities for enrichment, greater Adolescents who have been raised to value attributes
accessibility of higher education—also favor middle- that are characteristic of middle-class jobs, not surpris-
class youngsters in the world of work. Because their ingly, will seek those attributes when they plan their
parents are more likely to work in positions of power careers. They will look for jobs that offer independence
and leadership, middle-class youngsters often have im- and power. In working-class families, in contrast,
portant family connections and sources of information children are more likely to be raised to value obedience
about the world of work that are less available to young- and conformity—two characteristics that are highly
sters from poorer families. In addition, coming from a valued in most working-class jobs. For youngsters from
family that is economically well off may provide an ad- this socioeconomic background, jobs that appeal to
olescent with more time to explore career options and these values will be relatively more attractive. They will
to wait for an especially desirable position, rather than have been raised to value such things as job security and
having to take the first job that becomes available out of not having to worry too much about making high-
economic necessity. This advantage is particularly im- pressured decisions. Indeed, to many working-class
portant during periods of economic downturn, when it youngsters, the high-stress world of the business execu-
is relatively more difficult to find employment. tive is not at all an attractive career possibility.

❚ The Broader Context of Occupational Choice


occupational attainment A measure of achievement based on Adolescents’ occupational choices are made, of course,
the status or prestige of the job an individual holds.
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 411 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 12 Achievement 411

within a broader social context that profoundly influ- say that if they had enough money, they would not
ences the nature of their plans (Staff et al., 2009). work; that they are less willing to work overtime to
Whereas in the past, for example, female adolescents make sure their job was done well; and that it is impor-
had lower educational and occupational expectations tant to have a job that allows sufficient time for leisure
than males, the reverse is true today (Mello, 2008). At (Wray-Lake, Syvertsen, Briddell, Osgood, & Flanagan,
different times, different employment opportunities 2009).
arise, and young people—particularly by the time they
reach the end of their formal schooling—are often very
aware of the prospects for employment in different MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
fields. Indeed, one study of inner-city youngsters found
Think about your own occupational development.
that many had developed ideas about their future job
What factors have most influenced your choices?
prospects by the time they were in 2nd grade (T. Cook
How did your occupational plans change
et al., 1996). Understandably, young people often tailor
during adolescence? How similar are they
their plans in response to what they perceive as the fu-
now to the ones you had when you were
ture needs and demands of the labor market and the
just entering high school?
acceptability of given occupational choices within their
community. In addition, whether an adolescent’s occu-
pational expectations are actually realized depends on
many factors that are not in his or her control (as any- Unfortunately, adolescents also tailor their plans
one graduating during the economic downturn of based on their beliefs regarding which jobs society
2008–2009 could certainly attest). Today’s young peo- says are “acceptable” for individuals of particular social
ple see work as a less central part of life than their coun- class, ethnicity, or sex (M. Johnson, 2002) (see Figure
terparts did in the past. Contemporary young people, 12.8). For example, although sex differences in the
for example, are more likely than past generations to value adolescents place on extrinsic rewards are small,

150,000
Physics
138,874
3,846 Earth, atmospheric, and
2,399 ocean sciences
3,338
Chemical engineering
14,894 Mechanical engineering
120,000
Electrical engineering
Civil engineering
16,438 Chemistry
Mathematics and statistics
8,819 Computer sciences
88,371
Number of degrees

90,000 1,024 Agricultural sciences


1,678 1,743 5,636
2,017
2,109
Biological sciences
2,499 8,724
5,614
6,827

60,000 7,944
34,652
8,915

8,781
30,000
48,001

31,347

0
Women Men
FIGURE 12.8 Bachelor’s degrees earned in selected science and engineering fields, by gender, 2007. Although gender roles have
changed considerably in recent decades, there remain large sex differences in occupational choices. (Hill, Corbett, & Rose, 2010)
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 412 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

412 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

there are significant sex differences in the extent to college campuses, have come to play an increasingly
which individuals value power (associated with mascu- important role in individuals’ career decision making,
line jobs, and more valued by males) and altruism (as- because the rapid pace at which the labor market
sociated with feminine jobs, and more valued by changes has made it less likely that adolescents will be
females) (M. Johnson, 2002; Weisgram, Bigler, & Liben, able to obtain accurate information from their family
2010). Moreover, many more adolescent girls than boys (Staff et al., 2009).
express concern about having to balance family and
work demands in adulthood, and this further affects
their occupational decision making, since they may be RECAP
reluctant to pursue careers that they believe will inter- • The development of occupational plans follows a
fere with family life (Cinamon & Rich, 2002). Perhaps process that is similar to the process of identity de-
because of this, adolescent boys’ occupational expecta- velopment, with a period of experimentation and ex-
tions are predictive of what they actually end up doing ploration leading to increasingly more specific plans.
as adults, whereas this is not the case for adolescent girls • Adolescents’ career choices are influenced by a
(Mello, 2008). number of factors, including their work values, their
One problem faced by all young people in making social background, and their perceptions of the la-
career plans is obtaining accurate information about bor market and their potential place within it.
the labor market needs of the future and the appropri- • As is the case with educational achievement, occu-
ate means of pursuing positions in various fields. The pational attainment is strongly influenced by socio-
majority of young people do not have educational plans economic status.
that are consistent with the educational requirements • Occupational plans are also influenced by the
of the jobs they hope to enter, and many adolescents are broader context, including adolescents’ percep-
overly optimistic about their chances for success tions of what occupations are “appropri-
(Chang et al., 2006; Schneider & Stevenson, 1999). One ate” for them given their sex,
goal of career educators is to help adolescents make ethnicity, and social
more informed and more realistic choices about their background.
careers and to free them from stereotypes that con-
strain their choices. Career counselors, especially on
ste32118_ch12_382-413.indd Page 413 15/03/13 7:41 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 414 15/03/13 7:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 415 15/03/13 7:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

CHAPTER
13
Psychosocial Problems
in Adolescence
SOME GENERAL PRINCIPLES ABOUT PROBLEMS IN ADOLESCENCE
PSYCHOSOCIAL PROBLEMS: THEIR NATURE AND COVARIATION
Comorbidity of Externalizing Problems
Comorbidity of Internalizing Problems
SUBSTANCE USE AND ABUSE
Prevalence of Substance Use and Abuse
Causes and Consequences of Substance Use and Abuse
Prevention and Treatment of Substance Use and Abuse
EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS
Categories of Externalizing Problems
Developmental Progression of Antisocial Behavior
Changes in Juvenile Offending Over Time
Causes of Antisocial Behavior
Prevention and Treatment of Externalizing Problems
INTERNALIZING PROBLEMS
The Nature and Prevalence of Depression
Sex Differences in Depression
Suicide
Causes of Depression and Internalizing Disorders
Treatment and Prevention of Internalizing Problems
STRESS AND COPING

415
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 416 15/03/13 7:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

416 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

ALTHOUGH THE VAST MAJORITY of young people As you now know, the stereotype of adolescence
move through the adolescent years without experiencing as a period riddled with difficulty is not accurate. We
major difficulty, some encounter serious psychological should not gloss over the fact that many healthy ado-
and behavioral problems that disrupt not only their lescents at one time or another experience self-
lives but also the lives of those around them, such as doubt, family squabbles, academic setbacks, or broken
substance abuse, delinquency, and depression. Moreover, hearts, of course. But it is important to keep in mind
these problems indirectly touch the lives of all of us, either as we look at psychosocial problems during adoles-
directly, through the personal contact we may have with cence that there is an important distinction between
a troubled young person, or indirectly, through increased the normative, and usually transitory, difficulties en-
taxes for community services or heightened anxiety about countered by many young people and the serious psy-
the safety of our neighborhoods. In this final chapter, we chosocial problems experienced by a minority of
look at some of the more serious psychological problems youth.
we typically associate with adolescence.

Some General Principles their patterns of socializing, their drinking declines


(Staff, Schulenberg et al., 2010). Similarly, although the
About Problems in vast majority of teenagers do something during adoles-
cence that is against the law, very few of these young
Adolescence people go on to have criminal careers. In a period of
The mass media like nothing more than to paint ex- development during which it is normal, even expected,
treme pictures of the world in which we live. This exag- that individuals will seek independence and explore
gerated worldview is obvious in the presentation of themselves and their relationships with others, it is
teenage problem behavior. Rarely are popular portray- hardly surprising that some of the experimentation in
als of behavioral disorders, psychological distress, or which individuals engage is risky (Hayden et al., 2011;
drug use accurate: A breakup with a boyfriend is fol- A. Siegel & Scovill, 2000; Steinberg, 2008). In fact,
lowed that evening by a suicide attempt. An after-school adolescents who experiment occasionally with risky
prank develops into a life of crime. A weekend of heavy behavior report a quality of life that is more similar to
drinking fades into a commercial, and when the pro- that reported by adolescents who abstain from risk
gram returns, the adolescent is on his way to a life of taking entirely than it is to the quality of life reported
addiction, delinquency, and school failure. Those of you by frequent risk takers (Topolski et al., 2001).
for whom adolescence was not that long ago know that
these “facts” about adolescent problem behavior are
rarely true. But we are so often bombarded with images MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
of young people in trouble that it is easy to be fooled Why do you think stereotypes of problem behavior in
into believing that “adolescence” equals “problems.” adolescence differ from
One of the purposes of this chapter is to put these reality? What are some
problems in perspective. It is therefore helpful, before dangers of holding on to
we look at several specific sorts of problems in detail, to the stereotypic view?
lay out some general principles about adolescent psy-
chosocial problems that apply to a range of issues.

❚ Most Problems Reflect Transitory Experi-


mentation First, let’s distinguish between occasional
experimentation and enduring patterns of dangerous ❚ Not All Problems Begin in Adolescence Second,
or troublesome behavior. Rates of occasional, usually we need to distinguish between problems that have their
harmless, experimentation far exceed rates of enduring origins and onset during adolescence, and those that
problems. For example, the majority of adolescents ex- have their roots during earlier periods of development
periment with alcohol sometime before high school (Drabick & Steinberg, 2011). It is true, for example, that
graduation, and the majority will have been drunk at some teenagers fall into patterns of criminal or delinquent
least once; but, as we’ll see, relatively few teenagers de- behavior during adolescence, and for this reason, we tend
velop drinking problems or permit alcohol to adversely to associate delinquency with the adolescent years. But
affect their schooling or personal relationships, and as most teenagers who have recurrent problems with the law
they move into adulthood, get married, and change had problems at home and at school from an early age; in
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 417 15/03/13 7:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 417

during adolescence suffered from other types of psycho-


logical distress, such as excessive anxiety, as children (Flynn
& Rudolph, 2011; Graber & Sontag, 2009; Puelo, Settipani,
Crawley, Beidas, & Kendall, 2011). According to a study of
more than 10,000 American adolescents, although one-
third of all teenagers report having had an anxiety disorder
by age 18, almost all of these individuals had developed an
anxiety disorder before turning 12. Similarly, of the 20% of
adolescents who reported having had a behavioral disorder
at some point in time, two-thirds had the first occurrence
before entering adolescence (Merikangas et al., 2010) (see
Figure 13.1). In other words, simply because a problem
may be displayed during adolescence does not mean that it
is a problem of adolescence.

❚ Most Problems Do Not Persist Into Adulthood


Third, it is important to remember that many, although
not all, of the problems experienced by adolescents are
relatively transitory in nature and are resolved by the be-
ginning of adulthood, with few long-term repercussions
in most cases. Substance abuse, delinquency, and eating
disorders are three good examples of problems that tend
to follow this pattern: Rates of drug and alcohol use, de-
linquency, and disordered eating are all higher within the
adolescent population than in the adult population, but
It is important to differentiate between occasional experimentation most individuals who abused drugs and alcohol, com-
with risky or unhealthful activities and enduring patterns of mitted delinquent acts, or were bulimic as teenagers
troublesome behavior. grow up to be sober, law-abiding adults without eating
disorders. Individuals for whom problem behavior per-
some samples of delinquents, the problems were evident as sists into adulthood are likely to have had a problematic
early as preschool (Farrington, 2009; Hartung, Lefner, & childhood as well as a problematic adolescence. The fact
Fedele, 2011). Many individuals who develop depression that some of the problems of adolescence seem to fade

40
Anxiety
Behavior
35
Mood
Substance
30
Cumulative percent

25

20

15

10

0
4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age in years
FIGURE 13.1 Anxiety disorders and behavioral disorders typically begin during childhood, whereas mood disorders and
substance abuse first appear during adolescence. (Merikangas et al., 2010)
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 418 15/03/13 7:32 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

418 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

away with time does not make their prevalence during


adolescence any less significant, but it should be kept in
Psychosocial Problems:
mind when you read or hear reports about the inevitable Their Nature and
decline of civilization at the hands of contemporary
youth. Covariation
Clinical practitioners (psychologists, psychiatrists, social
❚ Problems During Adolescence Are Not Caused workers, and counselors) and other experts on the devel-
by Adolescence Finally, problem behavior during opment and treatment of psychosocial problems during
adolescence is virtually never a direct consequence of adolescence typically distinguish among three broad cat-
the normative changes of adolescence itself. Popular egories of problems: substance abuse, externalizing disor-
theories about “raging hormones” causing opposi- ders, and internalizing disorders (Achenbach & Edelbrock,
tional or deviant behavior have no scientific support 1987). Substance abuse refers to the maladaptive use of
whatsoever, for example, nor do the widely held beliefs drugs, including legal drugs like alcohol or nicotine; illegal
that problem behaviors are manifestations of an inher- drugs like marijuana, cocaine, or ecstasy; and prescription
ent need to rebel against authority or that bizarre be- drugs such as stimulants or sedatives. Externalizing dis-
havior results from an identity crisis. As you learned in orders are those in which the young person’s problems
previous chapters, the hormonal changes of puberty are turned outward and are manifested in behavioral
have only a modest direct effect on adolescent behav- problems (some writers use the expression “acting out” to
ior; rebellion during adolescence is atypical, not nor- refer to this set of problems). Common externalizing
mal; and few adolescents experience a tumultuous problems during adolescence are delinquency, antisocial
identity crisis. When a young person exhibits a serious aggression, and truancy. Internalizing disorders are
psychosocial problem, such as depression, the worst those in which the young person’s problems are turned
possible interpretation is that it is a normal part of inward and are manifested in emotional and cognitive
growing up. It is more likely a sign that something is distress, such as depression or anxiety.
wrong. Although we often think of adolescent substance abuse
as an externalizing disorder, research indicates that it is just
as likely to accompany depression and other internalizing
RECAP disorders as it is to be a part of “acting out” behavior (Lien,
• The vast majority of individuals do not develop Sagatun, Heyerdahl, Søgaard, & Bjertness, 2009; Verona &
serious psychological or social problems during the Javdani, 2011). We are simply more likely to be aware of
adolescent years, but a significant minority do. substance abuse problems when they are seen among ado-
• It is important to distinguish between experimen- lescents who are antisocial (such as a rowdy group of drunk
tation and enduring patterns of behavior, between delinquent youth) than when they occur in the context of
problems that have their origins during adoles- internalizing problems (such as a depressed teenager who
cence and those that do not, and between prob- drinks herself to sleep each night). Because substance
lems that are transitory and those that persist into abuse problems co-occur, or are comorbid, with both ex-
adulthood. ternalizing and internalizing problems, and because many
• Serious problem behavior during adolescence adolescents who experiment with drugs have neither inter-
is rarely a direct consequence of nalizing nor externalizing problems, we look at substance
the normative changes abuse as a separate category of problem behavior.
of adolescence While the distinction between internalizing disorders
itself. and externalizing disorders is useful for organizing infor-
mation about psychosocial problems during adoles-
cence, it is important to bear in mind that some
adolescents experience problems in both domains simul-
taneously (Cosgrove, Rhee, Gelhorn, Boeldt, & Hewitt,
substance abuse The misuse of alcohol or other drugs to a degree 2011). That is, some adolescents who engage in delin-
that causes problems in the individual’s life.
quency or show other behavior problems also suffer
externalizing disorders Psychosocial problems that are from depression (Loeber & Burke, 2011; Kiesner, 2002;
manifested in a turning of the symptoms outward, as in aggression Sheidow et al., 2008). And, as noted earlier, many
or delinquency. depressed or anxious adolescents as well as many antiso-
internalizing disorders Psychosocial problems that are
cial adolescents also abuse drugs and alcohol (Ansary &
manifested in a turning of the symptoms inward, as in depression Luthar, 2009; Armstrong & Costello, 2002; Flisher et al.,
or anxiety. 2000; Repetto, Zimmerman, & Caldwell, 2008; Xue,
Zimmerman, & Cunningham, 2009). Many research-
comorbid Co-occurring, as when an individual has more than one ers believe that it is important to distinguish among
problem at the same time.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 419 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 419

adolescents who exhibit one specific problem without ❚ Problem Behavior Syndrome Researchers
any others (for example, depressed adolescents who do have devoted a great deal of attention to studying the
not have other internalizing or externalizing problems), covariation among externalizing problems during ado-
adolescents who exhibit more than one problem within lescence, and a number of theories about the origins of
the same general category (for example, violent delin- what some experts call problem behavior syndrome
quent youth or anxious-depressed youth), and adoles- have been proposed. The most widely cited perspective,
cents who exhibit both internalizing and externalizing now nearly 40 years old, comes from the work of social
problems (for example, depressed delinquents). These psychologist Richard Jessor and his colleagues (Jessor &
adolescents may have followed very different pathways to Jessor, 1977). According to Jessor, the underlying cause
deviance and may require very different types of treat- of externalizing problems during adolescence is uncon-
ment. In general, studies show that multiproblem teen- ventionality in both the adolescent’s personality and
agers have had far worse family experiences than those the social environment. Unconventional individuals are
with one problem (Aseltine, Gore, & Colten, 1998; tolerant of deviance in general, are not highly con-
R. Chen & Simons-Morton, 2009; Ge, Best, Conger, & nected to educational or religious institutions, and are
Simons, 1996). Co-occurring internalizing and external- very liberal in their views. Unconventional environ-
izing problems are more often seen among females than ments are those in which a large number of individuals
among males (Kofler et al., 2011; Ritakallio et al., 2008). share these same attitudes. Unconventional individuals
One explanation for this is that conduct problems mean in unconventional environments are more likely to en-
different things when they are displayed by boys and girls. gage in a wide variety of risk-taking behavior, including
Among boys, conduct problems are, in some senses, an experimentation with illegal drugs, risky sex, delin-
extreme version of behavior that is considered “normal” quent activity, and even risky driving (Brack, Brack, &
among males (who are expected to be more aggressive Orr, 1996; M. L. Cooper, Wood, Orcutt, & Albino, 2003;
and unruly). In contrast, for a girl to be diagnosed with a Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996). A comparison of adoles-
conduct problem, she has to depart in a very significant cents in the United States and China found that the
way from the norm. Accordingly, girls who have exter- same factors heighten or diminish adolescents’ risk for
nalizing problems are usually much more disturbed in a problem behavior in both countries (Jessor et al., 2003).
wide variety of ways, including being depressed or anx- Although Jessor’s theory does not specifically hy-
ious. In contrast, many boys with conduct problems are pothesize about the origins of unconventionality, a
“normal” in other respects (Wiesner & Kim, 2006). number of possibilities have been proposed. One set of
theories emphasizes the biological underpinnings of
COMORBIDITY OF EXTERNALIZING risk taking or unconventionality and argues that a pre-
PROBLEMS disposition toward deviance may actually be inherited
One of the reasons it is helpful to differentiate between (e.g., Mednick, Gabrielli, & Hitchings, 1987; Rowe,
internalizing and externalizing disorders is that the Rodgers, Meseck-Bushey, & St. John, 1989). A second
specific problems within each broad category are often view stresses biologically based differences (either in-
highly intercorrelated. Delinquency, for example, is of- herited or acquired through experience) among indi-
ten associated with problems such as truancy, defiance, viduals in arousal, sensation seeking, and fearlessness
sexual promiscuity, academic difficulties, and violence (e.g., Dunlop & Romer, 2010; Ortiz & Raine, 2004). Yet
(Farrington, 2009). All these problems are different a third view emphasizes the early family context in
sorts of manifestations of a lack of impulse control, and which deviance-prone children are reared and frames
adolescents who engage in these behaviors are often problem behavior as a sort of adaptive response to a
described as “undercontrolled” (Martel et al., 2009; hostile environment (Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper,
Robins, John, Caspi, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1991). Indeed, many writers have argued that some
1996; S. E. Young et al., 2009). types of antisocial behavior, especially those that in-
volve risk taking, actually make a lot of evolutionary
sense (Ellis et al., 2012).
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
How might the recognition that there is considerable ❚ Problem Clusters An alternative to the view
comorbidity of problems in that an underlying trait drives all problem behavior has
adolescence change our been proposed by sociologist Denise Kandel. She ar-
approaches to prevention gues that different types of deviance have distinctly dif-
and intervention? ferent origins, but that involvement in a given problem

problem behavior syndrome The covariation among various


types of externalizing disorders believed to result from an
underlying trait of unconventionality.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 420 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

420 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Externalizing Academic Internalizing


problems in problems in problems in
childhood adolescence adulthood

FIGURE 13.2 One explanation for comorbidity is that problems in one domain can create problems in another.
(Masten et al., 2005)

behavior may itself lead to involvement in a second one. overstating the case for a single problem behavior “syn-
Thus, problem behaviors may cluster together not be- drome” (Farrell, Sullivan, Esposito, Meyer, & Valois, 2005;
cause of a common underlying trait like unconvention- Willoughby, Chalmers, & Busseri, 2004). They note that
ality, but because involvement in some problematic although engaging in one type of problem behavior in-
activities (such as drug and alcohol use) leads to in- creases the likelihood of engaging in another, the overlap
volvement in others (such as delinquency) (Bingham & among behavior problems is far from perfect. Indeed, in
Shope, 2004; Glaser, Shelton, & van den Bree, 2010; one study, it was found that the vast majority of delin-
Hussong, Curran, & Moffitt, 2004; Malone, Taylor, & quents are not serious drug users (D. Elliott, Huizinga, &
Marmorstein, 2004; W. Mason & Windle, 2002; Menard, 1989). Other studies suggest that it is impor-
Ridenour et al., 2002; Rohde, Lewinsohn, Kahler, Seeley, tant to differentiate between problem behavior that
& Brown, 2001). Some writers have talked about “cas- adults disapprove of but that many adolescents consider
cading” effects, where one sort of problem leads to an- normative (such as smoking, drinking, having sex) ver-
other, which leads to a third (Bornstein, Hahn, & sus problem behavior that both adults and adolescents
Haynes, 2010; Burrell & Roosa, 2009; Burt & Roisman, view as serious (such as violent crime) (Sullivan, Childs,
2010; Ingoldsby, Kohl, McMahon, & Lengua, 2006; & O’Connell, 2010; Willoughby et al., 2004; Zweig,
Lynne-Landsman, Bradshaw, & Ialongo, 2010a; Mun, Lindberg, & McGinley, 2001). Context matters, too: One
Windle, & Schainker, 2008; Rogosch, Oshri, & Cicchetti, recent international comparison found that adolescent
2010; Siebenbruner, Englund, Egeland, & Hudson, alcohol use was predictive of violence in Scandinavia
2006). For example, one analysis of individuals fol- and Eastern Europe, but not in Mediterranean countries,
lowed from preadolescence into adulthood found in part because adolescents in Mediterranean countries
that externalizing problems in childhood led to aca- are less likely to drink to intoxication and more likely to
demic difficulties in adolescence, which, in turn, led to drink in settings where adults are present (Felson,
internalizing problems in adulthood (see Figure 13.2) Savolainen, Bjarnason, Anderson, & Zohra, 2011).
(A. Masten et al., 2005).

❚ Social Control Theory According to a third COMORBIDITY OF INTERNALIZING


view, social control theory (Gottfredson & Hirschi, PROBLEMS
1990), individuals who do not have strong bonds to There is also a good deal of comorbidity in internaliz-
society’s institutions—such as the family, school, or ing disorders, which tend to have in common the sub-
workplace—will be likely to behave unconventionally in jective state of distress. For example, depressed
a variety of ways. Thus, the clustering of different prob- adolescents are more likely than their peers to experi-
lem behaviors may stem not from a problem “in” the ence anxiety, panic, phobia, obsessional thinking,
person (such as a biological predisposition toward risky suicidal ideation, eating disorders, and various psycho-
behavior), but from an underlying weakness in the indi- somatic disturbances (that is, physical problems that
vidual’s attachment to society. This underlying problem have psychological causes) (Graber & Sontag, 2009;
leads to the development of an unconventional attitude, J. G. Johnson et al., 2002; Masi et al., 2004; Measelle,
to membership in an unconventional peer group, or to Stice, & Hogansen, 2006; Skitch & Abela, 2008; Zaider,
involvement in one or several problem behaviors that Johnson, & Cockell, 2002). Some experts question
may set a chain of problem activities in motion. Social whether it even makes sense to consider some of these
control theory helps to explain why behavior problems problems as separate entities when speaking about
are not just clustered together but are far more preva- children or adolescents (for example, to draw a distinc-
lent among poor, inner-city, minority youngsters. tion between anxiety and depression) because rates of
comorbidity are so high (Graber & Sontag, 2009).
❚ Overstating the Case? Finally, a number of
Just as different externalizing problems are hypoth-
researchers stress that we should be careful about
esized to reflect an underlying antisocial syndrome,
various indicators of internalizing problems may be
social control theory A theory of delinquency that links deviance
with the absence of bonds to society’s main institutions. thought of as different manifestations of a common
underlying factor. This factor is referred to as negative
negative emotionality The presumed underlying cause of inter- emotionality (Barrocas & Hankin, 2011; Wetter &
nalizing disorders, characterized by high levels of subjective distress.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 421 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 421

Hankin, 2009). Individuals who are high in negative football games and sitcoms that many of these same
emotionality—who become distressed easily—are at viewers watch tell them, no less subtly, that having a
greater risk for depression, anxiety disorders, and a good time with friends is virtually impossible without
range of internalizing problems, as are individuals who something alcoholic to drink. Many celebrities who
are anhedonic, or low in positive emotionality, who are are idolized by teenagers speak out against cocaine and
especially prone to depression. As is the case with the marijuana, but many equally famous stars admit to
underpinnings of externalizing problems, the under- using these same drugs. Tobacco companies spend
pinnings of internalizing problems are believed to have enormous amounts of money marketing cigarettes to
both biological and environmental origins, including teenagers, and research shows that adolescents are espe-
high levels of biological reactivity to stress (Katainen, cially drawn to cigarette advertisements that appear to
Raeikkoenen, & Keltikangas-Jaervinen, 1998; Susman, be aimed at teenagers (Arnett, 2001; Biener & Siegel,
Dorn, Inoff-Germain, Nottelmann, & Chrousos, 1997). 2000). Tobacco and alcohol use are common in music
These common underpinnings contribute to stability videos and often linked to sex, and more often than
in predispositions toward internalizing problems over not, the lead performer is the individual doing the
time (Bardone, Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, & Silva, 1996). drinking, smoking, and lovemaking (DuRant et al.,
In this chapter, we examine the nature, prevalence, 1997). Although there is not conclusive evidence that
consequences, and amelioration of the three sets of exposure to alcohol or cigarette advertising causes
problems often seen during adolescence: substance adolescents to start drinking or smoking—advertising
abuse, antisocial behavior and other externalizing is mainly effective in increasing brand recognition
problems, and depression and other internalizing prob- (National Research Council, 2004)—the messages
lems. In each case, we ask four central questions: (1) these ads communicate is that these are pleasurable
What is the nature of this sort of problem in adoles- activities (hardly a surprise, since the ads are designed
cence? (2) How many, and which, young people have to get people to buy the products).
these problems? (3) What do we know about factors The mixed signals sent to young people about drugs
that contribute to these problems? and, (4) What ap- reflect the inconsistent way that we view these sub-
proaches to prevention and intervention appear to have stances as a society: Some drugs (like alcohol or Adder-
the most promise? all) are fine, as long as they are not abused, but others
(like marijuana or ecstasy) are not; some drinking
(enough to relax at a party) is socially appropriate, but
RECAP too much (enough to impair an automobile driver) is
• Experts distinguish among three categories of prob- not; some people (those over 21) are old enough to
lems in adolescence: substance abuse, internalizing handle drugs, but others (those under 21) are not. It is
problems, and externalizing problems. easy to see why teenagers do not follow the dictates of
• There is a great deal of covariation, or “comorbid- their elders when it comes to alcohol and other drugs.
ity,” among different psychosocial problems during How, then, should we view substance use and abuse
adolescence within and across these broad among teenagers, when our backdrop is a society that
categories. much of the time tolerates, if not actively encourages,
• Adolescents who engage in delinquency are more adults who use these same substances?
likely than their peers to be truant, to engage in As with most of the problem behaviors that are
precocious sexual behavior, and to commit acts of common during adolescence, discussions of teenage
aggression—a pattern that has been termed substance use are often filled more with rhetoric than
“problem behavior syndrome.” reality. The popular stereotype of contemporary young
• Adolescents who are high in negative emotionality people is that they use and abuse a wide range of drugs
are more likely to suffer from depres- more than their counterparts did previously, that the
sion, feel anxious, and report main reason adolescents use drugs is peer pressure, and
other symptoms of that the “epidemic” level of substance use among
distress. American teenagers is behind many of the other prob-
lems associated with this age group—including aca-
demic underachievement, early pregnancy, suicide, and
crime. The simplicity of these assertions is certainly
tempting—after all, what could be more reassuring
than to identify the “real” culprit (drugs) and the “real”
Substance Use and Abuse
Our society sends young people mixed messages about
drugs and alcohol. Television programs aimed at pre-
adolescents urge viewers to “Just Say No!” but the anhedonic Having difficulty experiencing positive emotions, a risk
factor for depression.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 422 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

422 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Alcohol and marijuana remain the main drugs of choice among American adolescents.

causes (peers) of all the maladies of young people? And (L. D. Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg,
what could be even more comforting than the belief 2012a), is an excellent source of information about pat-
that, if we simply teach young people to “say no” to terns of adolescent drug and alcohol use, at least among
their peers, these problems will disappear? young people who have not dropped out of school.
Unfortunately, what we might like to believe about (The latest survey results can be accessed at www
adolescent substance use is not necessarily correct. As we .monitoringthefuture.org.)
shall see, there are grains of truth to many of the popular
claims about the causes, nature, and consequences of ❚ Drugs of Choice The surveys consistently indi-
teenage substance use and abuse, but there are many cate that alcohol is by far the most commonly used and
widely held misconceptions about the subject, too. abused substance, in terms of both prevalence (the
percentage of teenagers who have ever used the drug)
and recency of use (the percentage of teenagers who
PREVALENCE OF SUBSTANCE
have used the drug within the last month), followed by
USE AND ABUSE
marijuana and tobacco. By the time they are seniors in
Each year since 1975, a group of researchers from the high school, 70% of teenagers have tried alcohol, 46%
University of Michigan has surveyed a nationally repre- have smoked marijuana, and 40% have smoked ciga-
sentative sample of about 15,000 American high school rettes. After marijuana and tobacco, however, the per-
seniors on several aspects of their lifestyle and values, centage of young people who have tried various other
including their use and abuse of a variety of drugs. drugs drops precipitously, and only about 9% of teen-
Beginning in 1991, comparable samples of 8th- and agers have used an illicit drug other than marijuana
10th-graders were added to the annual survey. Because within the last month (L. D. Johnston et al., 2012a)
of the size and representativeness of the sample of re- (see Figure 13.3). And, notwithstanding all the media
spondents, this survey, called Monitoring the Future attention given in recent years to fears that adolescents
will mimic the steroid use of professional athletes,
many fewer high school seniors have ever used steroids
Monitoring the Future An annual survey of a nationwide sample (2%) than have tried smokeless tobacco (17%) or
of American 8th-, 10th-, and 12th-graders, mainly known for its data taken prescription drugs, like Percocet or Vicodin, for
on adolescent substance use.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 423 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 423

school seniors smoke daily, marijuana is used daily by


Alcohol 7% of teenagers, and daily use of alcohol is rare (only
Cigarettes 2% of seniors drink daily). However, about 20% of all
seniors, 15% of all 10th-graders, and 6% of all 8th-
Marijuana
graders report having abused alcohol (had more than
Narcotics other
than heroin
five drinks in a row, sometimes called binge drinking)
Amphetamines at least once during the past 2 weeks (L. D. Johnston
et al., 2012a). Also worrisome is that 14% of high school
Inhalants seniors report having driven a car after drinking at least
Tranquilizers once in the past month (Centers for Disease Control
Hallucinogens and Prevention, 2012).
(LSD, “shrooms”) Taken together, the findings from these surveys cast
Barbiturates doubt on some of the most fervently held stereotypes
Cocaine about adolescent drug use in the United States. Many
adolescents who drink do so to excess—a quarter of all
Ecstasy
seniors and nearly 15% of all sophomores have been
Steroids drunk at least once in the last month (L. D. Johnston
Heroin et al., 2012a). But only a very small proportion of young
people have serious drug dependency problems (which
0 20 40 60 80 100 would lead to daily use) or use hard drugs at all. More-
Percent ever used over, it is very unlikely that drug and alcohol use lurks
FIGURE 13.3 Percentages of American high school seniors behind the wide assortment of adolescent problems for
who have ever used various drugs. (L. D. Johnston et al., 2012a) which it is so frequently blamed. Rather, the pattern
suggests that most adolescents have experimented with
alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco; that many have used
recreational use (7%) (J. A. Ford, 2009; Schepis & one or more of these drugs regularly; that alcohol is
Krishnan-Sarin, 2009; L. Wu, Ringwalt, Mannelli, & clearly the drug of choice among teenagers (a substan-
Patkar, 2008). Although alcohol and tobacco use among tial proportion of whom drink to excess); and that most
adolescents in most European countries is substantially teenagers have not experimented with other drugs.
higher than it is in the United States, twice as many
American than European adolescents regularly use
illicit drugs (mainly marijuana) (Wadley, 2012). ❚ Changes in Substance Use Over Time The
Monitoring the Future study has also been used to chart
changes over time in adolescent substance use, and recent
MAKING THE CULTURAL CONNECTION administrations of the survey have given experts both
cause for relief and cause for concern (L. D. Johnston
The use of illicit drugs is more common among
et al., 2012a) (see Figure 13.4). Marijuana use, which
American teenagers than their
had been on a steady decline since the late 1970s, rose
European counterparts, but adoles-
quite sharply during the mid-1990s, has not declined to
cent smoking and drinking are more
its former levels, and appears to be on the rise again;
common in Europe. Why do you
especially worrisome is the continuing increase in the
think this is?
proportion of high school students who get high daily
(perhaps this is related to the problem of poor achieve-
ment discussed in Chapter 12). Alcohol use, which
declined steadily during the 1980s (from more than
Prevalence statistics, especially those that tap
70% of seniors drinking monthly to about 50%), has
whether an individual has ever tried the substance in
declined more slowly since then. One bit of very
question, tell us little about the nature and extent of
good news is that teen smoking, which increased during
drug use from the standpoint of adolescents’ health and
the 1990s, declined dramatically—probably because
well-being. It is one thing to have tried alcohol or
there has been a gargantuan increase in the price of
marijuana; it is something else to use either of these
cigarettes during the past two decades—although
substances so often that one’s life and behavior are
the decline has pretty much leveled off at around
markedly affected.
20%. Despite the massive amounts of money spent on
One of the best ways to examine this issue is to look
at the percentage of young people who report using
various substances daily or nearly daily. Daily use is in-
frequent, even among older teens. About 10% of high binge drinking Consuming five or more drinks in a row on one
occasion, an indicator of alcohol abuse.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 424 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

424 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

100
Alcohol
90 Cigarettes
Marijuana/hashish
80
Any illicit drug other than marijuana
70

60
Percent

50

40

30

20

10

0
1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010
FIGURE 13.4 Over-time trends in the proportion of high school seniors who report having used various drugs in the 30 days
preceding the survey. (L. D. Johnston et al., 2012a)

antismoking education, the high price of cigarettes is a drunk at least once. Curiously, while young adolescents’
far more powerful deterrent to teen smoking (Gruber & attitudes toward drinking and smoking have gotten
Zinman, 2001; Pampel & Aguilar, 2008). In fact, expo- progressively more negative over the years, their views
sure to some antismoking and antidrug ads may in- of marijuana have not changed.
crease adolescents’ drug use (Hornik, Jacobsohn, Orwin, Rates of substance use among 8th-graders are im-
Piesse, & Kalton, 2008; Wakefield et al., 2006). portant to watch, because the chances of becoming ad-
Although pundits and political commentators fre- dicted to alcohol or nicotine are dramatically increased
quently claim to have discovered the “real” reason for when substance use begins prior to age 15 (G. C. Patton,
changes in rates of adolescent substance use, no one Coffey, Carlin, Sawyer, & Wakefield, 2006). Because the
really knows why rates of adolescent substance use fluc- typical adolescent who smokes cigarettes begins around
tuate over time, except, perhaps, because of fluctua- the 7th or 8th grade, looking at changes in the number
tions in price and availability. We know that adolescents’ of 8th-graders who smoke is a good way of forecasting
drug use fluctuates with changes in their perceptions of rates of smoking among adults in the future. Fortu-
how harmful and disapproved of drug use is, but scien- nately, although the decline in the rate of smoking
tists have not been able to determine what influences among 8th-graders had halted, the rate started to drop
these perceptions, although it is likely that the messages again a few years ago (L. D. Johnston et al., 2012a).
teenagers receive about drugs—from parents, teachers,
and mass media—are important. Interestingly, the ❚ Drugs and the Adolescent Brain Researchers
once-existent gender gap in drug use, with males more have long speculated that, because the brain is still very
likely to use and abuse drugs, has all but disappeared malleable early in adolescence, experimentation with
for alcohol, marijuana, and cigarettes, and has nar- drugs is more harmful then than later in development.
rowed substantially for other drugs as well (R. Johnson Experimental research in which scientists have com-
& Gerstein, 1998). pared the brains of animals exposed to drugs either
Perhaps the most encouraging finding to emerge in close to the time of puberty or after reaching full matu-
recent surveys is that experimentation with drugs is less rity, has illuminated some of the specific neurobi-
common among younger teens than it had been in the ological pathways that explain why the potential for
past (L. D. Johnston et al., 2012a). In the mid-1990s, addiction is much greater in adolescence than adult-
about 25% of all 8th-graders reported drinking at least hood (Brenhouse, Sonntag, & Andersen, 2008; Hardin
once a month; by 2011, only half as many young adoles- & Ernst, 2009; Schochet, Kelley, & Landry, 2004, 2005;
cents did. Three times as many 8th-graders were regular Sturmhöfel & Swartzwelder, 2004; Volkow & Ting-Kai,
smokers in the 1990s than is the case today. Neverthe- 2005). In order to understand what these studies say,
less, one-third of all 8th-graders have tried alcohol, one we need to digress slightly and revisit adolescent brain
in six has tried marijuana, and one in six has been development.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 425 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 425

As you read in Chapter 2, changes in the limbic sys- & Winter, 2006). Similar evidence suggests that indi-
tem that take place during adolescence affect receptors viduals who begin smoking regularly before age 14 are
for dopamine, one of the neurotransmitters that at greater risk for nicotine dependence as adults than
influence our experience of pleasure. We experience are those who start in late adolescence (Orlando,
things like great sex or fabulous food as enjoyable be- Tucker, Ellickson, & Klein, 2004), and, as well, that early
cause they result in higher levels of dopamine in the use of inhalants is associated with higher rates of abuse
brain; these higher levels permit more electrical activity and dependence (L. Wu, Pilowsky, & Schlenger, 2004).
through the synapses that connect the circuits in the Experimental studies comparing the consequences of
brain that regulate feelings of pleasure. drug exposure during adolescence and adulthood have
Certain drugs make users feel good primarily be- confirmed this pattern (Zickler, 2004).
cause they affect the same receptors that are sensitive to The increased vulnerability of the adolescent brain
the dopamine that is in the brain naturally. In fact, the to the addicting effects of alcohol is compounded by
molecules of addictive drugs are so similar to dopamine the fact that adolescents don’t feel the negative conse-
molecules that dopamine receptors act in the same way quences of drinking as profoundly as adults do (this
in their presence as they do in the presence of actual can only be studied in animals, because researchers are
dopamine. As a result, when drugs enter the brain not allowed to give teenagers alcohol). Studies compar-
(which is where they go whether they enter the body ing juvenile rodents with adult rodents find that juve-
through the mouth, nose, or blood vessels), they are niles can drink more than adults before they become
“read” by dopamine receptors as the real thing. On the tired or have their reflexes slow, and the unpleasant
positive side, this makes the user feel good (the same consequences of drinking too much (otherwise known
way that natural dopamine does)—which, of course, is as a hangover) are less intense among juveniles than
why people take drugs. The problem, though, is that adults. To make matters worse, juveniles feel the posi-
frequent drug use during adolescence appears to inter- tive effects of alcohol more intensely than adults—
fere with the normal maturation of the brain’s dopa- alcohol makes juvenile rodents want to socialize, for
mine system. The animal studies referred to earlier have instance, but it makes adults want to be left alone
shown that experiences in early adolescence, when the (Spear, 2010, 2011b).
limbic system is changing naturally, can permanently Although the short-term effects of alcohol are less
affect the way the dopamine system functions (Spear, severe in adolescents than adults, there is solid evidence
2011b). (As you know, various brain systems and re- that the lasting effects of alcohol on brain functioning
gions change during different periods of development, are worse in adolescence than in adulthood—again, be-
and it is during periods of change that these brain cause the brain is more vulnerable to influences during
systems are most easily and irreversibly affected by out- periods of plasticity. One area of the adolescent brain
side influences.) Repeated exposure to drugs during that appears especially vulnerable to the harmful effects
this period of heightened malleability in the limbic sys- of alcohol is the hippocampus, which is important for
tem can affect the brain in ways that make it necessary memory and, along with the prefrontal cortex, for “put-
to use drugs in order to experience normal amounts of ting the brakes” on impulsive behavior (Squeglia,
pleasure. Jacobus, & Tapert, 2009; Sturmhöfel & Swartzwelder,
How many exposures to a drug does it take to per- 2004; E. Walker et al., 2004). Alcohol also has harmful
manently alter the adolescent brain’s dopamine system? effects on the development of regions of the brain in-
No one knows for sure, and the answer varies from volved in higher-order cognitive abilities, such as plan-
person to person, largely because of genetic factors (this ning and decision making, and in self-regulation
is why some people are more likely to develop addic- (Butler, 2006; Nasrallah et al., 2011; Nasrallah, Yang, &
tions than others) (Laucht et al., 2008). What we now Bernstein, 2009). Although there is some evidence that
know, though, is that this permanent alteration in the the harmful neurobiological consequences of drinking
dopamine system is more likely to happen in adoles- in early adolescence can be reversed, the fact that early
cence, when the limbic system is still malleable, than in exposure is more likely to lead to addiction and long-
adulthood, when it is less changeable. term use indicates that interventions designed to pre-
If this is true, we should expect to find that exposure vent substance abuse should begin prior to adolescence.
to drugs during adolescence is more likely to lead to
addiction than is exposure during adulthood. And, in ❚ Ethnic Differences in Substance Use Several
fact, studies show that, compared with individuals who national surveys have examined ethnic differences in
delay drinking until they are 21, people who begin rates of adolescent substance use and abuse. In general,
drinking in early adolescence (before age 14) are 7 times White adolescents are more likely to use drugs and
more likely to binge drink as teenagers and 5 times
more likely to develop a substance abuse or dependence
disorder at some point in their life (Hingson, Heeren, dopamine A neurotransmitter especially important in the brain
circuits that regulate the experience of reward.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 426 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

426 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

alcohol than minority youngsters, especially Black and passes through the gate, however, is influenced by
Asian youth (P. Chen & Jacobson, 2012; L. D. Johnston many factors beyond his or her previous patterns of
et al., 2012b). Rates of drug use among Hispanic adoles- drug use, including the period in which he or she
cents are comparable to those of White youngsters, and grows up. For example, progression from tobacco and
use among American Indian adolescents is the high- alcohol to marijuana and other illegal drugs was far
est (Chassin, Hussong, & Beltran, 2009; L. D. Johnston more common among people who were born around
et al., 2012b; Whitbeck, Yu, Johnson, Hoyt, & Walls, 1960 than among people born before 1950 or after 1970
2008). In general, foreign-born and less Americanized (Golub & Johnson, 2001).
minority youngsters—whether Asian or Hispanic in Studies that followed adolescents over time have
background—use alcohol, drugs, and tobacco at a identified several distinct developmental trajectories
lower rate than do American-born and more accultur- of alcohol, tobacco, and drug use (Chassin et al., 2009;
ated immigrant youth, suggesting that part of becom- Flory, Lynam, & Milich, 2004; Patrick & Schulenberg,
ing an “American” teenager means experimenting with 2011; Sher, Gotham, & Watson, 2004; Windle & Wiesner,
drugs (Delva et al., 2005; Georgiades, Boyle, Duku, & 2004). In one study, six distinct groups were identified.
Racine, 2006; Gfroerer & Tan, 2003; Hahm, Lahiff, & Nonusers (who made up one-third of the sample)
Guterman, 2004; Nagoshi, Marsiglia, Parsai, & Castro, rarely experimented with substances at any point in
2011). Indeed, the rate of drug use among immigrant adolescence. Alcohol experimenters (25% of the sam-
adolescents is half the rate of use among adolescents ple) first tried alcohol early in adolescence and contin-
from the same ethnic group who were born in the ued to drink occasionally, but did not try other drugs
United States (K. Harris, 1999). and did not increase their drinking over time. Low
escalators (5%) began using substances early in adoles-
❚ Does Substance Use Follow a Particular cence and increased their use slowly but steadily over
Progression? Researchers have also been interested time. Early starters (6%) showed very high substance
in the sequence through which adolescents experiment use in early adolescence and escalated gradually over
with different drugs. In general, young people experi- time, so that by the end of high school they were smok-
ment with beer and wine before trying cigarettes or ing and drinking frequently and experimenting with
hard liquor, which precedes marijuana use, which, in drugs. Late starters (20%) used substances infrequently
turn, precedes the use of other illicit drugs. However, during early adolescence but increased their use rapidly
although experimentation may follow this sequence, during high school—so much so that by the end of
this does not mean that alcohol use or smoking high school their substance use was similar to that of
invariably lead to marijuana use, or that marijuana use the early starters. Finally, high escalators (8%) showed
necessarily leads to experimentation with harder drugs moderate use in early adolescence, escalated rapidly
(van Leeuwen et al., 2011). In fact, there is little evi- between early and middle adolescence, and continued
dence to support the idea that marijuana is an inevita- to increase their use throughout high school (Zapert,
ble stepping-stone to hard-drug use (much appears to Snow, & Tebes, 2002).
depend on how frequently marijuana is used) (Treaster,
1994).
CAUSES AND CONSEQUENCES
The fact that there is a fairly standard sequence of
OF SUBSTANCE USE AND ABUSE
drug use, however, suggests that virtually all users of
hard drugs have also tried alcohol, cigarettes, and In looking at the causes and consequences of substance
marijuana and, moreover, that one way to prevent use and abuse in adolescence, it is especially important
adolescents from experimenting with more serious to keep in mind the distinction between occasional ex-
drugs might be to stop them from drinking, smoking, perimentation and problematic use.
and using marijuana. In fact, studies show that adoles-
cents who have not experimented with alcohol or ❚ Users, Abusers, and Abstainers Because the
marijuana by the time they are in their 20s are unlikely majority of adolescents have experimented with alco-
ever to use these or any other drugs (K. Chen & Kandel, hol and marijuana, it is likely that there are plenty of
1996). For this reason, tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana normal, healthy young people who have used these
are considered gateway drugs, in the sense that they drugs at least once. In fact, several studies indicate that
represent a gate through which individuals pass on the adolescents who experiment with alcohol and
way to using harder drugs. Whether an individual marijuana are no worse adjusted than their peers who
abstain from alcohol and marijuana (Alex Mason &
Spoth, 2011; Scheier & Botvin, 1998; J. S. Tucker, Ellickson,
gateway drugs Drugs that, when used over time, lead to the use Collins, & Klein, 2006). This substantial body of re-
of other, more dangerous substances. search shows quite clearly that it is important to differ-
entiate among four groups of adolescents: frequent
developmental trajectories Patterns of change over time.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 427 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 427

drug users (for example, at least once a week); hard-


drug users (that is, drugs other than alcohol, tobacco,
or marijuana); those who experiment with marijuana
and alcohol but who do not use them frequently (that
is, no more than once a month); and those who ab-
stain (Connell, Gilreath, Aklin, & Brex, 2010; Hughes,
Power, & Francis, 1992; C. Mitchell et al., 1996; Wills,
McNamara, Vaccaro, & Hirky, 1996). Experimenters
and abstainers score higher on measures of psycho-
logical adjustment than frequent users. Longer-term
follow-up studies also show that moderate alcohol use
during adolescence does not have negative long-term
effects (Haller, Handley, Chassin, & Bountress, 2010;
Paschall, Freisthler, & Lipton, 2005). In contrast, ciga-
rette use during adolescence has more harmful long-
term health consequences because nicotine is a more
addictive drug, and its use is more likely to persist into
middle adulthood (K. Chen & Kandel, 1996; Elders,
Perry, Eriksen, & Giovino, 1994; Pierce & Gilpin, 1996).
These results do not mean that occasional experi-
Studies of juvenile mice have furthered our understanding of the
mentation with drugs during adolescence leads to bet-
impact of drinking on adolescent brain development.
ter adjustment, of course. In fact, research shows that
the psychological advantages observed among adoles-
cents who experiment with alcohol and marijuana were unstable, stubborn, and inattentive. In other words,
evident when these individuals were younger children drug and alcohol abuse during adolescence is often a
(Shedler & Block, 1990). Taken together, though, the symptom of prior psychological disturbance.
studies suggest that moderate alcohol and marijuana Substance abuse during adolescence, whatever its
use has become normative among adolescents in con- antecedents, is associated with a host of other prob-
temporary society (however troublesome some adults lems. Young people who abuse alcohol, tobacco, and
may find this), that these substances are typically used other drugs are more likely to experience problems
in social situations, and that better-adjusted and more at school; suffer from psychological distress and de-
interpersonally competent young people are likely to pression; become involved in dangerous or deviant
participate in social activities in which alcohol and activities, including crime, delinquency, and truancy;
other drugs are present (Ludden & Eccles, 2007; Scheier and engage in unprotected sexual activity. As adults,
& Botvin, 1998). Relative to experimenters, abstainers— they are more likely to have physical health prob-
and “irrational” abstainers, in particular—tend to be lems, experience unemployment and out-of-wedlock
overcontrolled, narrow in their interests, anxious, and childbearing, and develop substance abuse problems
inhibited (Shedler & Block, 1990). (Aseltine, Schilling, James, Glanovsky, & Jacobs, 2009;
Chassin et al., 2009; Green & Ensminger, 2006; Holmen,
❚ Predictors and Consequences of Substance Barrett-Connor, Holmen, & Bjermer, 2000; Kandel,
Abuse Substance abuse (using drugs in a way that Johnson, Bird, & Canino, 1997; Stuart & Green, 2008;
causes significant problems at home, school, work, or L. Wu & Anthony, 1999). Alcohol and other drugs are
with the law) is a different matter (see Table 13.1). Ado- often implicated in adolescent automobile crashes, the
lescents who are frequent users of alcohol, tobacco, and leading cause of death and disability among American
other drugs score lower on measures of psychological teenagers (Lang, Waller, & Shope, 1996; O’Malley &
adjustment as teenagers and were more likely to be Johnston, 1999), and in other fatal and nonfatal acci-
maladjusted as children. Indeed, a team of researchers dents, such as drownings, falls, and burns (Hingson &
who followed a sample of individuals from preschool Zha, 2009). Adolescent substance abusers also expose
into young adulthood report that, at age 7, individuals themselves to the long-term physical health risks of
who would later become frequent drug users as adoles- excessive drug use; in the case of cigarettes, alcohol,
cents were described as “not getting along well with and marijuana, these risks are substantial and well
other children, not showing concern for moral issues . . . documented—among them, cancer, heart disease, and
not planful or likely to think ahead, not trustworthy kidney and liver damage. It is also now well established
or dependable . . . [and] not self-reliant or confident” that heavy cigarette smoking during adolescence can
(Shedler & Block, 1990, p. 618). As 11-year-olds, these exacerbate feelings of emotional distress and lead to
individuals were described as deviant, emotionally depression and anxiety disorders (Chassin et al., 2009).
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 428 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

428 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

TABLE 13.1 Proposed DSM–V diagnostic criteria for alcohol use disorder

A. A problematic pattern of alcohol use leading to clinically significant impairment or distress.


B. Two (or more) of the following occurring within a 12-month period:
1. Alcohol is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than was intended
2. There is a persistent desire or unsuccessful effort to cut down or control alcohol use
3. A great deal of time is spent in activities necessary to obtain alcohol, use the substance, or recover from its effects
4. Recurrent alcohol use resulting in a failure to fulfill major role obligations at work, school, or home (e.g., repeated
absences or poor work performance related to alcohol use; substance-related absences, suspensions, or expulsions
from school; neglect of children or household)
5. Continued alcohol use despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated
by the effects of the substance
6. Important social, occupational, or recreational activities are given up or reduced because of alcohol use
7. Recurrent alcohol use in situations in which it is physically hazardous (e.g., driving an automobile or operating a
machine when impaired by substance use)
8. Alcohol use is continued despite knowledge of having a persistent or recurrent physical or psychological problem
that is likely to have been caused or exacerbated by the substance
9. Tolerance, as defined by either or both of the following:
a. A need for markedly increased amounts of alcohol to achieve intoxication or desired effect
b. Markedly diminished effect with continued use of the same amount of the substance
10. Withdrawal, as manifested by either of the following:
a. The characteristic withdrawal syndrome for alcohol (refer to Criteria A and B of the criteria set for Withdrawal)
b. The same (or a closely related) substance is taken to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms
11. Craving or a strong desire or urge to use alcohol

Source: American Psychiatric Association, 2013.

❚ Risk Factors for Substance Abuse Which ad- peers. These characteristics include anger, impulsivity,
olescents are most likely to become substance abusers? inattentiveness, and sensation seeking (Chassin et al.,
Generally, four sets of risk factors—psychological, fa- 2009; Malone, Van Eck, Flory, & Lamis, 2010; Tapert,
milial, social, and contextual—for substance abuse have Baratta, Abrantes, & Brown, 2002; Wilens et al., 2011;
been identified, and the more risk factors that are pres- Wills, Sandy, Yaeger, & Shinar, 2001; M. Wong et al.,
ent for an individual, the more likely she or he is to use 2006). Many of these traits have a strong genetic com-
and abuse drugs (O’Loughlin, Karp, Koulis, Paradis, & ponent, although the effects of having a genetic pro-
DiFranza, 2009; Ostaszewski & Zimmerman, 2006; pensity toward substance use can be diminished by a
Petraitis, Flay, & Miller, 1995). These same risk factors context that discourages smoking or drinking (Brody et
have been found across a variety of studies and in sam- al. 2009; D. Li et al., 2011; Park, Sher, Todorov, & Heath,
ples of adolescents from a wide range of ethnic and 2011) (see Figure 13.5). In addition, individuals who
socioeconomic backgrounds, although there is some have more tolerant attitudes about drug use (and about
evidence that family factors may be more influential deviance in general) are at greater risk for drug abuse
in early adolescence and peer factors more so in middle (Schulenberg, Wadsworth, O’Malley, Bachman, &
adolescence (M. J. Cleveland, Feinberg, Bontempo, & Johnston, 1996; Petraitis et al., 1995), as are those who
Greenberg, 2008). In other words, the factors that place expect alcohol or other drugs to improve their social
an adolescent at risk for substance abuse are more or relationships (Griffin, Epstein, Botvin, & Spoth, 2001;
less the same regardless of the adolescent’s sex, social G. Smith, Goldman, Greenbaum, & Christiansen,
class, or ethnicity (Choi, Harachi, Gillmore, & Catalano, 1995). Even as children, for example, individuals who
2005). As one team of researchers points out, this is eventually become heavy drinkers as adolescents expect
good news, because it suggests that preventive inter- alcohol to have positive effects on them (M. Dunn &
ventions do not need to be specifically tailored to dif- Goldman, 1998).
ferent subgroups of adolescents (Hu, Davies, & Kandel, Second, individuals with distant, hostile, or conflicted
2006). family relationships are more likely to develop substance
The first set of risk factors is psychological. Individ- abuse problems than are their peers who grow up in
uals with certain personality characteristics—which close, nurturing families (Dishion, Capaldi, & Yoerger,
typically are present prior to adolescence—are more 1999; J. A. Ford, 2009; Kilpatrick et al., 2000; King,
likely to develop drug and alcohol problems than their Molina, & Chassin, 2009; Sale et al., 2005). Drug-abusing
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 429 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 429

0.25 FIGURE 13.5 Supportive and involved


No genetic risk parenting can mitigate the effects of having
Genetic risk a genetic propensity for substance abuse.
0.20 (Brody et al., 2009)
Substance use rate of increase

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.00

–0.05
Low High
Involved-supportive parenting

youngsters are also more likely than their peers to have drug use easier (Chassin et al., 2009; Stanley, Henry, &
parents who are excessively permissive, uninvolved, Swaim, 2011). Important contextual factors are the
neglectful, or rejecting (Chassin et al., 2009; Jarvinen & availability of drugs, the community’s norms regarding
Østergaard, 2009; Tobler & Komro, 2010). In addition, drug use, the degree to which drug laws are enforced,
they are more likely to come from homes where one or and the ways in which drug use is presented via the
more other family members (parents or siblings) mass media (K. Allison et al., 1999; X. Li, Stanton, &
smoke, drink, or use drugs; there are both genetic and Feigelman, 2000; Petraitis et al., 1995). A New Zealand
family environmental influences on adolescent alcohol study found, for example, that lowering the minimum
and drug use (Chassin et al., 2009; Gilman et al., 2009; purchasing age for alcohol significantly increased the
K. G. Hill, Hawkins, Catalano, Abbott, & Guo, 2005; rate of alcohol-related car crashes among younger driv-
Nofziger & Lee, 2006). One explanation for especially ers (Kypri et al., 2006). Binge drinking and drinking
high rates of substance use among affluent suburban while driving are higher among adolescents who live in
teenagers is that their parents often are tolerant of this neighborhoods with relatively more retail outlets for al-
behavior (Botticello, 2009; Luthar & Goldstein, 2008). cohol (M.-J. Chen, Gruenewald, & Remer, 2009a; Resko
Third, individuals with substance abuse problems et al., 2010; Truong & Sturm, 2009), and smoking is
are more likely to have friends who use and tolerate the more common among adolescents who live in neigh-
use of drugs (Alli, Amialchuk, & Dwyer, 2011; G. M. borhoods with relatively more stores that sell cigarettes
Barnes, Hoffman, Welte, Farrell, & Dintcheff, 2006; (McCarthy et al., 2009; Novak et al., 2006) or attend
Bray, Adams, Getz, & McQueen, 2003; Ennett et al., schools where a high proportion of other students
2006; J. Hill, Emery, Harden, Mendle, & Turkheimer, smoke (Sabiston et al., 2009).
2008; I. J. Kim, Zane, & Hong, 2002; Parsai, Voisine, Researchers have also identified important protec-
Marsiglia, Kulis, & Nieri, 2009). As you read in Chapter tive factors that decrease the likelihood of adolescents’
5, whether and how often adolescents use drugs is an engaging in substance abuse (Jessor, Van Den Bos,
important defining characteristic of peer groups— Vanderryn, Costa, & Turbin, 1995). Among the most
abstainers tend to have other abstainers as friends, and important protective factors are positive mental health
users tend to be friends with other users. Drug-using (including high self-esteem and the absence of depres-
adolescents seek drug-using peers, and drug-using sion), high academic achievement, engagement in
peers encourage even more drug use among their school, close family relationships, and involvement in
friends (Chassin, Presson, Todd, Rose, & Sherman, religious activities (Albers & Biener, 2003; Bryant,
1998; Schulenberg et al., 1999). Substance-using ado- Schulenberg, O’Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, 2003;
lescents who have many substance-using friends may L. C. Jordan & Lewis, 2005; Sanchez, Opaleye, Chaves,
also overestimate how common substance use is Noto, & Nappo, 2011). These protective factors appear
because they are so much more likely to see other peo- to operate over and above the effects of the risk factors
ple engaged in it (Unger & Rohrbach, 2002).
Finally, adolescents who become substance abusers
are more likely to live in a social context that makes protective factors Factors that limit individual vulnerability
to harm.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 430 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

430 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

discussed previously. As with the factors that place self-esteem, for example, or who are gainfully employed
adolescents at risk for substance abuse, the protective will be less likely to use drugs. In other programs, the
factors identified operate similarly among adolescents intervention is directly focused on preventing drug use.
from different ethnic groups and explain why some These programs include information-based efforts (in
groups of adolescents use drugs more than others do which adolescents are educated about the dangers of
(G. Barnes & Farrell, 1992; Ennett et al., 2008; Flannery, drugs), social skills training (in which adolescents are
Vazsonyi, & Rowe, 1996). One of the reasons for the taught how to turn down drugs), and some combina-
lower rate of drinking among Black youth, for example, tion of informational and general psychological inter-
is that their parents are less likely to drink or to tolerate vention (in which adolescents are educated about drug
adolescent drinking (Peterson, Hawkins, Abbott, & abuse and exposed to a program designed to enhance
Catalano, 1994). their self-esteem, for instance) (Newcomb & Bentler,
1988).
PREVENTION AND TREATMENT
OF SUBSTANCE USE AND ABUSE
MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
Efforts to prevent substance use and abuse among
teenagers focus on one of three factors: the supply of Why do you think that interventions designed to
drugs, the environment in which teenagers may be change individuals’ knowledge about drug use and its
exposed to drugs, and characteristics of the potential consequences have been such failures?
drug user (Newcomb & Bentler, 1988). Although a How do you explain the fact
good deal of government spending and media atten- that many individuals con-
tion have been devoted to the first of these approaches— tinue to engage in behav-
attempts to control or limit the availability of iors that they know are
drugs—the consensus among experts is that it is more dangerous or unhealthy,
realistic to try to change adolescents’ motivation to use such as smoking?
drugs and the environment in which they live, since it
has proven virtually impossible to remove drugs to-
tally from society. Indeed, two of the three most com-
monly used and abused drugs—cigarettes and Generally, the results of research designed to evalu-
alcohol—are both legal and widely available, and laws ate these sorts of individual-focused approaches have
prohibiting the sale of these substances to minors are not been encouraging (Dielman, 1994; H. Leventhal &
not well enforced (Centers for Disease Control and Keeshan, 1993). Careful evaluations of Project DARE,
Prevention, 2006). Research does show, however, that for example—the most widely implemented drug edu-
raising the price of alcohol and cigarettes does reduce cation program in the United States—show that the
adolescents’ use of them (Bishai, Mercer, & Tapales, program is largely ineffective (Ennett, Tobler, Ringwall,
2005; M. Grossman, Chaloupka, Saffer, & Laixuthai, & Flewelling, 1994). Experts are now fairly confident
1994; Gruber & Zinman, 2001), and that raising the that drug education alone, whether based on rational
minimum legal drinking age from 18 to 21 likely led to information or scare tactics, does not prevent drug use.
a decline in binge drinking among teenagers (but not This is reminiscent of research on sex education, which,
young adults) (Grucza, Norberg, & Bierut, 2009). At- as we saw in Chapter 11, has shown that informational
tempts to enforce laws governing the purchase of programs are simply not effective on their own. As a
cigarettes are less effective, in part because many ado- rule, educational programs may change individuals’
lescents obtain cigarettes through means other than knowledge, but they rarely affect their behavior.
purchasing them from stores (for example, bumming Research on the effectiveness of drug testing in schools
them from older friends, stealing them from parents) has yielded inconsistent findings (James-Burdumy,
(Fichtenberg & Glantz, 2002; Pokorny, Jason, & Goesling, Deke, & Einspruch, 2012; Yamaguchi,
Schoeny, 2006). Johnston, & O’Malley, 2003).
Many different types of drug abuse prevention inter- The most encouraging results have been found in
ventions have been tried, either alone or in combination. programs that do not focus only on the individual ad-
In programs designed to change some characteristic of olescent but rather combine some sort of social com-
the adolescent, drug use is targeted indirectly either by petence training with a communitywide intervention
attempting to enhance adolescents’ psychological aimed not only at adolescents but also at their peers,
development in general or by helping adolescents de- parents, and teachers (Fletcher, Bonell, & Hargreaves,
velop other interests and participate in other activities 2008; J. Hawkins et al., 2008; Liddle, Rowe, Dakoff,
that will make drug use less likely. The idea behind Henderson, & Greenbaum, 2009; M. Siegel & Biener,
these sorts of efforts is that adolescents who have high 2000). These multifaceted efforts have been shown to
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 431 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 431

be effective in reducing adolescents’ use of alcohol, • Adolescents who abuse alcohol and other drugs
cigarettes, and other drugs, especially if the programs are more likely to be impulsive, to come from
begin when youngsters are preadolescents and con- hostile family environments, to have friends
tinue well into high school (Bruvold, 1993; Dielman, who use drugs, and to live in a context in which
1994; Ellickson, Bell, & McGuigan, 1993; Flynn et al., drugs are easily available and their use is
1994; Perry et al., 1996). Overall, most experts agree tolerated.
that efforts designed simply to change the potential • The most promising interventions for substance
adolescent drug user without transforming the envi- abuse problems in adolescence are those
ronment in which the adolescent lives are not likely that target the adolescent’s social
to succeed. Despite their intuitive appeal, efforts to environment as well as
help adolescents “Just Say No” have been remarkably the individual.
unsuccessful.
One of the problems with all prevention programs
is that they often do not distinguish between drug use
and drug abuse. Trying to stop teenagers from ever
using alcohol, for instance, is both unlikely to suc-
ceed and probably not a very wise allocation of re-
sources, whereas preventing binge drinking or drunk Externalizing Problems
driving are far more important—and attainable—
goals. Generally, experts distinguish among three main cate-
Distinguishing between use and abuse is also impor- gories of externalizing problems in adolescence: con-
tant in treatment. Some experts worry that adolescents duct disorder, aggression, and delinquency. Although
who are mistakenly enrolled in treatment programs these three classes of problems are highly interrelated,
(because their parents have overreacted to the adoles- their definitions differ.
cent’s normative and probably harmless experimenta-
tion with drugs) may end up more alienated and more
distressed—and more likely to become drug abusers— CATEGORIES OF EXTERNALIZING
as a result of the “treatment.” Evaluations of treatment PROBLEMS
programs for adolescents who are genuine drug abusers Conduct Disorder The first category of externaliz-
suggest that efforts that involve the adolescent’s family, ing problems is conduct disorder, which is a clinical
and not just the teenager, are more likely to be success- diagnosis that refers to a repetitive and persistent pat-
ful (Liddle et al., 2009). Unfortunately, many adoles- tern of antisocial behavior in which the rights of others
cents who would benefit from substance abuse or age-appropriate societal norms are violated and
treatment, especially those from ethnic minority where, as a result of this behavior, the individual has
groups, do not receive it, often because they can’t afford problems in social relationships, school, or the work-
it or have inadequate health insurance (Cummings, place (see Table 13.2) (Farrington, 2009). (A related,
Wen, & Druss, 2011). but less serious, diagnosis is oppositional-defiant dis-
order, which refers to behavior that is spiteful, angry,
and argumentative, but not necessarily aggressive.) An
estimated 6%–16% of adolescent males and 2%–9% of
RECAP adolescent females have conduct disorder (Farrington,
• Among contemporary adolescents, alcohol, 2009. Estimates of the prevalence of conduct disorder
marijuana, and tobacco are clearly the drugs of have been increasing in recent years, although it is hard
choice. Although rates of drinking and smoking to know whether this is due to increases in the num-
marijuana are high, fewer teens are smoking bers of children who genuinely have the disorder or to
cigarettes than ever before. increases in the number of badly behaving children
• Experts have expressed particular concern about who are diagnosed. Conduct disorder is very stable be-
the use of substances early in adolescence, espe- tween childhood and adolescence—about half of all
cially in light of new brain research indicating that
the potential for addiction and for other harmful
physical consequences is significantly greater when
the first use of substances occurs before age 14. oppositional-defiant disorder A disorder of childhood and
adolescence characterized by excessive anger, spite, and
• Research indicates a clear need to distinguish stubbornness.
between experimentation with alcohol and
marijuana (which has not been shown to be conduct disorder A repetitive and persistent pattern of antisocial
harmful) and regular or heavy use (which has). behavior that results in problems at school or work, or in
relationships with others.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 432 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

432 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

TABLE 13.2 Proposed DSM–V diagnostic criteria for conduct disorder

A. A repetitive and persistent pattern of behavior in which the basic rights of others or major age-appropriate societal norms
or rules are violated, as manifested by the presence of at least three of the following 15 criteria in the past 12 months from
any of the categories below, with at least one criterion present in the past 6 months:

Aggression to people and animals 1.


Often bullies, threatens, or intimidates others
2.
Often initiates physical fights
3.
Has used a weapon that can cause serious physical harm to others (e.g.,
a bat, brick, broken bottle, knife, gun)
4. Has been physically cruel to people
5. Has been physically cruel to animals
6. Has stolen while confronting a victim (e.g., mugging, purse snatching,
extortion, armed robbery)
7. Has forced someone into sexual activity
Destruction of property 8. Has deliberately engaged in fire setting with the intention of causing
serious damage
9. Has deliberately destroyed others’ property (other than by fire setting)
Deceitfulness or theft 10. Has broken into someone else’s house, building, or car
11. Often lies to obtain goods or favors or to avoid obligations (i.e., “cons”
others)
12. Has stolen items of nontrivial value without confronting a victim (e.g.,
shoplifting, but without breaking and entering; forgery)
Serious violations of rules 13. Often stays out at night despite parental prohibitions, beginning before
age 13 years. (In adults, often violates rules of family life, e.g., neglects
basic needs of a child.)
14. Has run away from home overnight at least twice while living in parental
or parental surrogate home, or once without returning for a lengthy
period. (In adults, often violates major societal norms, e.g., rulings of the
court or conditions of parole/probation or rules of a public agency or
residential setting.)
15. Is often truant from school, beginning before age 13 years. (In adults or
adolescents not in school, often violates rules of the workplace, e.g.,
chronic work absenteeism without acceptable reason.)
B. The disturbance in behavior causes clinically significant impairment in social, academic, or occupational functioning.

Source: American Psychiatric Association, 2013.

individuals who are diagnosed with it as children are Individuals who have been diagnosed with conduct
also diagnosed with it as teenagers, and many had op- disorder and who persist in their antisocial behavior
positional-defiant disorder when they were younger. after age 18 may subsequently be diagnosed with anti-
One reason for this is that the risk factors for these two social personality disorder, which is characterized by
disorders are pretty much the same (Boden, Fergusson, a lack of regard for the moral or legal standards of the
& Horwood, 2010). community and a marked inability to get along with
others or abide by societal rules. Some individuals
antisocial personality disorder A disorder of adulthood with antisocial personality disorder are psychopaths—
characterized by antisocial behavior and persistent disregard for the individuals who are not only antisocial in their behav-
rules of society and the rights of others. ior but manipulative, superficially charming, impulsive,
and indifferent to the feelings of others, a cluster of
psychopaths Individuals who are not only antisocial but also
characteristics referred to as callous-unemotional
manipulative, superficially charming, impulsive, and indifferent to
the feelings of others.
(CU) traits (Frick & White, 2008; Shirtcliff et al., 2009).
Because the terms “antisocial personality disorder”
callous-unemotional traits (CU) A cluster of traits characteristic and “psychopath” imply a deep-seated personality
of psychopathic individuals, which includes a lack of empathy and problem that is unlikely to change, experts advise against
indifference toward the feelings of others.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 433 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 433

applying them to people younger than 18, because, as ❚ Juvenile Offending The third main category of
you will read, most individuals who engage in antiso- externalizing problems is juvenile offending, which in-
cial behavior as teenagers do not continue to do so after cludes delinquency (crimes committed by minors that
their mid-20s. Social scientists disagree about whether are dealt with in the juvenile justice system) and crimi-
it is possible to identify “juvenile psychopaths” or “fledg- nal behavior (crimes that are dealt with in the criminal
ling psychopaths”—individuals who, despite their youth, justice system, regardless of the age of the offender), and
exhibit many of the same characteristics as adult psycho- status offenses, a special category of delinquent acts
paths and are likely to grow into them. Some contend that are not against the law for adults but that neverthe-
that it is possible to do so (e.g., M. A. Campbell, Porter, & less violate established codes of conduct for juveniles,
Santor, 2004; Frick, Kotov, Loney, & Vasey, 2005; Lynam like truancy or running away from home (Woolard &
et al., 2009; Salekin, 2008), while others note that some of Scott, 2009). Unlike conduct disorder or aggression,
the distinguishing features of adult psychopaths that are which are defined in terms of behavior, juvenile offend-
considered pathological (impulsivity, irresponsibility, in- ing is defined legally. A large proportion of juvenile of-
stability in romantic relationships) may be transient fenders have conduct disorder, and most are aggressive,
characteristics that reflect immaturity, not pathology but not all adolescents who have conduct disorder or
(Edens, Skeem, Cruise, & Cauffman, 2000; Skeem & who are aggressive are juvenile offenders, because that
Cauffman, 2003; Vincent, Vitacco, Grisso, & Corrado, depends entirely on whether they have broken the law.
2003). Nevertheless, some adolescents have stronger psy- Both violent crimes (such as assault, rape, robbery,
chopathic tendencies than others (Edens, Marcus, & and murder) and property crimes (such as burglary,
Vaughn, 2011), and while not all adolescents who score theft, and arson) increase in frequency between the pre-
high on measures of CU traits grow up to be adult psy- adolescent and adolescent years, peak during the late
chopaths, they are more likely to commit crimes, as ado- high school years (slightly earlier for property than for
lescents and as adults (McMahon, Witkiewitz, Kotler, & violent crimes), and decline during young adulthood.
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2010; The so-called age–crime curve has been remarkably
Moran et al., 2009; Stickle, Kirkpatrick, & Brush, 2009). stable over time and is consistently seen around the
Many experts now believe that practitioners should dis- world (Piquero, Farrington, & Blumstein, 2003). In the
tinguish between conduct-disordered adolescents with United States, almost one-third of arrests for serious
CU traits and those without them. crimes involve a suspect under 18, and individuals under
18 account for one-sixth of all violent crimes (Federal
❚ Aggression A second category of externalizing Bureau of Investigation, 2009) (see Figure 13.6). The on-
problems is aggression, which is behavior that is done set of serious delinquency generally begins between the
to intentionally hurt someone. “Aggression” is a very ages of 13 and 16 (Farrington, 2009).
broad term that includes physical fighting, relational
aggression, and intimidation, and it can be either in- MAKING THE SCIENTIFIC CONNECTION
strumental (planned) or reactive (unplanned). It is very The age-crime curve is found all over
difficult to estimate the prevalence of aggression during the world. How do you account for this,
adolescence, because the category is so far-reaching. in light of the fact that the contexts in
Virtually everyone has done something aggressive at one which adolescents develop vary so
time or another, and about one-third of high school much?
students report having been in a fight during the past
year (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
2012). Most psychologists are concerned with adoles-
cents whose aggression is persistent and causes serious
injury to others. Aggressive behavior actually declines
aggression Acts done to be intentionally harmful.
over the course of childhood and adolescence—in sheer
quantity, the most aggressive period of development is juvenile offending An externalizing problem that includes
the preschool years, when children frequently hit, kick, delinquency and criminal behavior.
or bite each other—although aggression committed by delinquency Juvenile offending that is processed within the
adolescents is generally more serious than that com- juvenile justice system.
mitted by children (Bongers, Koot, van der Ende, &
criminal behavior Crimes that are dealt with in the criminal
Verhulst, 2004). Like conduct disorder, aggression is justice system, regardless of the age of the offender.
also very stable over time, although much more so in
boys than girls (Broidy et al., 2003). One likely reason status offenses Violations of the law that pertain to minors but
not adults.
for this sex difference is that aggressive little girls are
more often forced to curtail their bad behavior than ag- age–crime curve The relationship between chronological age and
gressive little boys are. offending, showing that the prevalence of offending peaks in late
adolescence.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 434 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

434 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

600

500
Violent index crime arrests
per 100,000 population

400

300

200

100

0
2

4
14

4
ov nd
15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23
24
10
–1

–3

–4
–3

–5

–6
–5
–2

–4
13–
r⬍

a
10

30

35

50

55
25

er
60
40

45

65
de
Un

FIGURE 13.6 Age differences in violent criminal activity. (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2009)

DEVELOPMENTAL PROGRESSION less serious forms of overt behavior; and most chroni-
OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR cally rebellious teenagers were oppositional children.
Of course, some juveniles commit all three types of
Antisocial behavior can take different forms: authority
acts; generally, the more serious an adolescent’s behav-
conflicts (such as truancy or running away from home),
ior is in one category, the more likely he or she is to have
covert antisocial behavior (such as stealing), and overt
displayed the others. That is, most adolescents who
antisocial behavior (such as attacking someone with
commit violent crimes have also engaged in covert and
a weapon). Within these broad categories, there are
authority-related antisocial behavior, but not all ado-
some fairly predictable progressions (Farrington, 2009;
lescents who have conflicts with authority or who en-
Loeber & Burke, 2011). Authority conflicts usually first
gage in covert antisocial behavior are necessarily
appear as stubborn behavior, which escalates into defi-
aggressive (Van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Koot, & Tremblay,
ance and disobedience, and then progresses to more se-
2009). The authority conflict pathway almost always
rious signs of problems with authority, such as truancy
starts in childhood (contrary to the stereotype, few
and running away from home. Covert antisocial behav-
people suddenly develop serious authority problems
ior typically begins with acts like lying and shoplifting;
for the first time as teenagers). The covert and overt
progresses to property damage, such as vandalism; and
pathways, in contrast, can begin either in childhood or
then to more serious property crimes, such as burglary.
in adolescence—and, as you will read, individuals
Overt antisocial behavior generally first presents itself as
whose antisocial behavior begins in childhood are very
fighting or bullying, which escalates to things like gang
different from those whose antisocial behavior doesn’t
fighting and, ultimately, to violent criminal activity.
start until adolescence (Moffitt, 2006).
This is not to say that all bullies grow up to be vio-
lent criminals or that all stubborn preschoolers end up
running away from home as teenagers. But the reverse CHANGES IN JUVENILE
is almost always true: Virtually all violent juveniles have OFFENDING OVER TIME
a history of escalating aggressive behavior; most adoles-
When social scientists track changes in antisocial
cents who commit serious property crimes started with
behavior over time, they generally look at juvenile
offending, because statistics are kept on the numbers of
juveniles arrested each year and the crimes with which
authority conflicts A type of antisocial behavior characterized by they have been charged. Between 1965 and 1988, and
stubbornness and rebelliousness. especially after 1984, arrests for the most serious violent
crimes—murder, rape, armed robbery, and aggravated
covert antisocial behavior A type of antisocial behavior
characterized by misdeeds that are not always detected by others,
assault—increased substantially among young people.
such as lying or stealing. After 1993, violent crime among young people declined
dramatically; it began to rise very slightly in the middle
overt antisocial behavior A type of antisocial behavior of the first decade of the new century, but started to
characterized by aggression toward others.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 435 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 435

Arrests per 100,000 juveniles ages 10–17 FIGURE 13.7 Rates of juvenile crime
500 in the United States are lower than
they have been at any time in the past
400 30 years. (Office of Juvenile Justice and
Delinquency Prevention, 2011)
Violent crime index
300

200

100

0
81 83 85 87 89 91 93 95 97 99 01 03 05 07 09
Year

decline again in 2006. As of 2009, violent crime among Delinquency Prevention, 2011). If female offending re-
adolescents was at its lowest level since 1980 (Office of mained relatively flat, but male offending dropped by
Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, 2011) more than 50%, the ratio of male-to-female offending
(see Figure 13.7). Property crime among adolescents is would be cut in half. Regardless of the size or causes of
also less prevalent today than in 1980. the gender gap in arrests, there is good evidence that
violent females have significantly more mental health
❚ Bad Girls Much attention has been devoted to problems than do violent males, consistent with the no-
what appears to be a substantial reduction in the gender tion that gender-inappropriate displays of aggression
gap in serious offenses over the past several decades may be indicative of greater maladjustment (Cauffman,
(Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2009). Although anti- 2008). There is also good evidence that girls’ antisocial
social behavior is still far more common among males behavior may be especially influenced by the boys
than females, the male-to-female ratio in juvenile ar- who are their friends and boyfriends (Arndorfer &
rests today (for violent crime, it is about 4 to 1) is about Stormshak, 2008; Cauffman, Farruggia, & Goldweber,
half of what it was in 1980 (Office of Juvenile Justice 2008; Lonardo, Giordano, Longmore, & Manning, 2009).
and Delinquency Prevention, 2011). However, it is not
clear whether this change is mainly due to changes in ❚ Adolescents as Crime Victims Violent crime
actual offending or to changes in arrest practices. among young people is a significant source of worry to
Changes in arrest rates can occur without there being adults, of course. But crime is also a significant source
any changes in actual offending (if, for example, the
police crack down on crime, more people will be ar-
rested, even if more people are not offending), and
studies that rely on official statistics often reach conclu-
sions different from those that rely on police or court
records (Farrington, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber,
2003). In fact, an analysis of data on actual offending
found that there has not been an increase in violent acts
committed by adolescent girls; rather, girls are simply
being arrested more frequently for the same things they
did in the past but were not arrested for (Steffensmeier,
Schwartz, Zhong, & Ackerman, 2005). Analyses of un-
derage drinking among girls have reached a similar
conclusion, namely, that although the proportion of
girls who drink has not increased over time, there has
been a disproportionate increase in girls who are ar-
rested for underage drinking (Zhong & Schwartz, 2010).
It also appears that the ratio of males to females who
have been arrested has changed not so much because of
an increase in female offending, but because the drop in
juvenile offending since 1993 was much steeper for In the United States, almost one-third of arrests for serious crimes
males than females (Office of Juvenile Justice and involve a suspect under 18.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 436 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

436 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

of worry to adolescents themselves, who are the age (Farrington, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003).
group most likely to be victims of crimes such as theft, Underreporting results from the fact that many adoles-
robbery, rape, and assault. Indeed, adolescents are 2.5 cents commit offenses that are undetected by authori-
times more likely than adults to be the victim of a non- ties or that are handled outside official reporting
fatal violent crime (Snyder & Sickmund, 2006). Victims procedures—for example, when an adolescent who is
of violent crimes are more likely to report a wide range caught shoplifting is reprimanded by the storekeeper
of problems than are other adolescents, including post- instead of being referred to the police. Selective report-
traumatic stress disorder, depressed mood, sleep depri- ing results from the fact that poor and minority
vation, and academic difficulties, and they are more youngsters are more likely to be arrested and, if con-
likely themselves to engage in aggression and antisocial victed, to be treated more harshly than other young-
behavior (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995; Cooley- sters who commit similar offenses, so that official
Quille & Lorion, 1999; Moses, 1999). Adolescents living statistics may artificially inflate the proportion of
in single-parent homes in the inner city are dispropor- crimes committed by poor, minority youth (Bridges &
tionately likely to be the victims of violent crime; al- Steen, 1998; Chauhan, Reppucci, Burnette, & Reiner,
though Black and Hispanic adolescents are more likely 2010; Farrington et al., 2003; Rodriguez, 2010). One
to be victimized than White adolescents, this is due to experiment found that people hold such strong nega-
the higher proportion of non-White adolescents living tive stereotypes about Black males that when provided
in single-parent homes in poor neighborhoods (Snyder with information about a crime and asked to evaluate
& Sickmund, 2006; Wright & Younts, 2009). Indeed, for the perpetrator, individuals who were unconsciously
many adolescents growing up in the inner city, gang led to believe that the offender was Black were signifi-
violence and victimization are chronic problems. cantly more likely than those who were not to rate him
Among 15- to 19-year-olds, for example, homicide ac- as likely to reoffend in the future and as deserving of
counts for 40% of all deaths among Blacks and 23% of harsh punishment, an effect that was consistent re-
all deaths among Hispanic Americans, but 4% of White gardless of the race of the rater (S. Graham & Lowery,
deaths (National Center for Health Statistics, 2008). 2004). Racial bias is especially strong in the processing
Most researchers agree that violence and aggression of relatively more minor crimes; when a very serious
among youth are strongly linked to poverty for a num- crime, like armed robbery, is committed, juveniles of
ber of reasons. First, when families live in impoverished different ethnic backgrounds are likely to receive simi-
neighborhoods, parents are less effective in nurturing lar treatment (Cauffman, Piquero, Kimonis, Steinberg,
and monitoring their children, and this diminished ef- & Chassin, 2007).
fectiveness leads to increased aggression and crime An alternative to relying on official records is to go
(Snyder & Sickmund, 2006). Second, concentrated pov- to adolescents directly and ask them about their
erty upsets the social fabric of a neighborhood, making involvement in various criminal or status offenses.
it more difficult for adults and social institutions to Several researchers have done this, promising the re-
provide the guidance and supervision that adolescents spondents anonymity and confidentiality. The results
need (Sampson, 1992). Third, in many inner-city com- of these surveys do not necessarily provide a more ac-
munities devastated by unemployment, aggression is curate picture of juvenile crime, but they certainly sug-
used by males to demonstrate their standing and gest a different one. Two conclusions are especially
power—characteristics that are typically demonstrated interesting.
in middle-class communities through occupational First, a very large proportion of adolescents—
success (M. Wilson & Daly, 1985). Finally, the wide- between 60% and 80%, depending on the survey
spread prevalence of guns in inner-city neighborhoods sample—report having engaged in delinquent behavior
changes the sorts of interactions that take place when at one time or another; nearly one-third of American
adolescents fight, transforming what might have been 17-year-old boys have committed a violent crime in the
aggressive disputes into lethal exchanges (P. J. Cook & past year, and nearly half of all males report being re-
Ludwig, 2004; D. Wilkinson & Fagan, 1996). The sig- sponsible for an assault sometime during adolescence
nificance of neighborhood influences on violence was (Farrington, 2009). Second, ethnic differences in the
confirmed in a experiment, in which poor families with prevalence of actual offending are smaller than what
adolescents were randomly selected to be relocated into you might think based on official records (Farrington
better neighborhoods: After their relocation, rates of et al., 2003). More minority than White youth admit to
violent behavior among the juveniles dropped signifi- having committed a serious crime, but ethnic differ-
cantly (J. R. Kling, Ludwig, & Katz, 2005). ences in self-reported offending are far smaller than
ethnic differences in rates of arrest. There also are so-
❚ Official Statistics Versus Adolescents’ Reports cial class and neighborhood differences in rates of se-
Official figures about adolescent crime both underre- rious criminal activity, and because minority youth
port and selectively report rates of juvenile offending are overrepresented among the poor, they are also
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 437 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 437

overrepresented among those who commit crimes out that virtually everybody desists from crime by
(McNulty & Bellair, 2003). Delinquency is by no means midlife, so that there really is no such thing as “life-
limited to poor adolescents, however. One-third of ad- course-persistent” offending (Sampson & Laub, 2003).
olescents in affluent neighborhoods report involve- Nevertheless, experts agree that the causes and conse-
ment in violent and serious delinquency (J. M. Beyers, quences of delinquent behavior that begins during
Loeber, Wikström, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2001). childhood or preadolescence are quite different from
Although studies indicate that most adolescents— those of delinquency that begins—and typically
regardless of their social backgrounds—do something ends—during adolescence or young adulthood (e.g.,
that violates the law at one time or another, the vast Dandreaux & Frick, 2009; Moffitt, 2006; van der Geest,
majority of teenagers who violate the law do so only Blokland, & Bijleveld, 2009). Although many more
once, and not violently. In fact, a relatively small num- males than females are life-course-persistent offend-
ber of adolescents—between 5% and 10%, depending ers, the risk factors for early-onset antisocial behavior
on the study—account for most serious criminal activ- are similar for the sexes (Fergusson & Horwood, 2002;
ity (Piquero et al., 2003). It is important, therefore, in Storvoll & Wichstrøm, 2002).
thinking about the causes of delinquent behavior, to One important ramification of Moffitt’s theory is
distinguish between delinquent behavior that is serious that it is extremely difficult to predict which antisocial
and chronic and delinquent behavior that is less worri- adolescents will persist in their bad behavior solely on
some. As you will see momentarily, these two sets of the basis of their behavior during adolescence. Indeed,
delinquent behavior have very different antecedents social scientists who have attempted to assess juve-
(Moffitt, 2006). nile offenders’ risk for future reoffending based solely
on their adolescent characteristics have a remarkably
poor track record (Mulvey et al., 2004). According to
CAUSES OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR Moffitt’s model, it is necessary to have information on
In general, the earlier an adolescent’s “criminal career” the juvenile’s behavior and history before adolescence in
begins—in particular, if it begins before adolescence— order to predict whether her or his offending is likely to
the more likely he or she is to become a chronic of- be adolescence-limited or life-course-persistent, be-
fender, to commit serious and violent crimes, and to cause the best predictor of continued offending in
continue committing crimes as an adult (Farrington, adulthood is the presence of serious antisocial behavior
2009). Conversely, the older an adolescent is when the in childhood.
delinquent activity first appears, the less worrisome his
or her behavior is likely to become. For purposes of ❚ Life-Course-Persistent Offenders Many stud-
discussion, therefore, it is helpful to distinguish be- ies indicate that youngsters whose problems with the
tween youngsters who begin misbehaving before ado- law begin before adolescence are often psychologically
lescence and those whose delinquent activity first troubled. Most of these delinquents are male, many
appears during adolescence. are poor, and a disproportionate number come from
homes in which divorce has occurred (Farrington,
❚ Two Types of Offenders One of the most in- 2009). More importantly, a large and consistent body of
fluential ways of characterizing these two groups of research shows that chronic delinquents typically come
delinquents has been suggested by psychologist Terrie from disorganized families with hostile, inept, or
Moffitt (2006), who distinguishes between life-course- neglectful parents who have mistreated their children
persistent offenders and adolescence-limited offend- and failed to instill in them proper standards of be-
ers. The first group demonstrates antisocial behavior havior or the psychological foundations of self-control
before adolescence, is involved in delinquency during (Compton, Snyder, & Schrepferman, 2003; Dogan,
adolescence, and is at great risk for continuing crimi- Conger, Kim, & Masyn, 2007; Patterson, DeGarmo, &
nal activity in adulthood. The second group engages Knutson, 2000). There is some evidence that exposure
in antisocial behavior only during adolescence; some to harsh parenting may adversely affect the developing
adolescence-limited offenders become involved in child’s brain chemistry—in particular, the activity of
crime relatively early in adolescence, whereas others serotonin receptors—which may increase the risk of
begin during mid-adolescence (Fergusson & Horwood, antisocial behavior (Pine et al., 1996).
2002). Some researchers have suggested that there are
other groups of offenders as well (for example, indi-
life-course-persistent offenders Individuals who begin
viduals who do not start offending until adolescence
demonstrating antisocial or aggressive behavior during childhood
but who continue on into adulthood, and those who
and continue their antisocial behavior throughout adolescence and
display antisocial behavior as children but desist before into adulthood.
adulthood) (Piquero et al., 2003; Veenstra, Lindenberg,
Verhulst, & Ormel, 2009), and others have pointed adolescence-limited offenders Antisocial adolescents whose
delinquent or violent behavior begins and ends during adolescence.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 438 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

438 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Adverse Early harsh Poor school Conduct School Low parent Deviant Adolescent
context parenting readiness problems failure monitoring peers violence

Birth 0–5 Grade K Post–Grade K Grade 1 Grades 1–4 Post–Grade 4 Post–Grade 7 Grades 10–11
FIGURE 13.8 Adolescent violence is often the long-term consequence of a process that begins early in life.
(Dodge et al., 2008)

As Figure 13.8 illustrates, serious adolescent vio- Second, studies show that many children who become
lence is typically the result of a cascade that begins early persistent offenders have problems in self-regulation—
in life (Dodge, Greenberg, Malone, & Conduct Prob- they are more impulsive, less able to control their anger,
lems Prevention Research Group, 2008). You have al- and more likely than their peers to suffer from atten-
ready read about many components of this chain in tion deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Calkins &
previous chapters on family and peer relationships. Keane, 2009; A. Carroll et al., 2006; B. Henry, Caspi,
Early economic disadvantage in the home leads to Moffitt, Harrington, & Silva, 1999; Patterson et al.,
harsh and inconsistent parenting, which leads to cogni- 2000; Sourander et al., 2006). ADHD is primarily bio-
tive and social deficits. These deficits, in turn, lead to logical in origin, strongly influenced by genes, and
conduct problems, which contribute to peer rejection characterized by impulsivity, inattentiveness, restless-
and academic failure in elementary school and, over ness, and inappropriately high levels of activity, espe-
time, a reduction in parental supervision and monitor- cially in learning situations (Greven, Rijsdijk, & Plomin,
ing. Once poorly monitored, adolescents tend to drift 2011). Although ADHD does not directly cause antiso-
into antisocial peer groups, which heightens their in- cial behavior, it does elevate the risk for other family
volvement in violence. and academic problems, which, in turn, increase the
The idea that family factors may underlie chronic likelihood of an adolescent developing externalizing
delinquency—because of genetic factors, environmen- problems (Bussing, Mason, Bell, Porter, & Garvan,
tal influences, or both—is supported by observations 2010; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999; Patterson et al., 2000;
that preadolescent delinquency tends to run in families Sibley et al., 2011). The current thinking is that chroni-
(Farrington, 2009; Thornberry, Freeman-Gallant, cally conduct-disordered adolescents are born with
Lizotte, Krohn, & Smith, 2003). Many adolescents who strong biological predispositions toward antisocial be-
have been in trouble with the law from an early age have havior, some of which are genetic in origin, including
siblings and parents who have had similar problems low levels of serotonin (which diminish their ability to
(Farrington, 2009). Although studies have identified delay gratification), an emotional system that is easily
genetic influences on all types of antisocial behavior, aroused and difficult to regulate, and a temperament
aggression is especially heritable (Deater-Deckard & that makes them hard to control (Dodge & Pettit, 2003;
Plomin, 1999; Eley, Lichtenstein, & Stevenson, 1999). Guo, Roettger, & Cai, 2008; Vloet, Konrad, Huebner,
There is also considerable evidence that, apart from Herpetz, & Herpetz-Dahlmann, 2008). One recent
family factors, certain characteristics distinguish brain imaging study found that the connections be-
persistently delinquent youngsters from their peers at a tween brain regions that are important for impulse con-
relatively early age. First and most importantly, children trol are less well developed among juvenile offenders
who become delinquent—especially those who engage (Shannon et al., 2011). Poor self-regulation is an espe-
in violence—have histories of aggressive and antiso- cially potent risk factor for continued problem behav-
cial behavior that were identifiable as early as age 8 ior (P. Chen & Vazsonyi, 2011; T. W. Gardner, Dishion,
(Broidy et al., 2003; Farrington, 2009; Kokko, Pulkkinen, & Connell, 2008; Monahan, Steinberg, Cauffman, &
Huesmann, Dubow, & Boxer, 2009; Patterson, Forgatch, Mulvey, 2009).
Yoerger, & Stoolmiller, 1998). Although this has been Many researchers have examined the biological un-
confirmed in hundreds of studies, it is important to derpinnings of chronic antisocial behavior. There is
keep in mind that the majority of children who have considerable evidence that antisocial adolescents, espe-
histories of aggressive behavior problems do not grow cially those who are callous and unemotional, have a
up to be delinquent. (If this seems confusing, think significantly lower resting heart rate than other youth,
about it this way: The majority of delinquents probably which may indicate a biologically inherited tendency
have eaten in fast-food restaurants at some point in toward fearlessness (L. Baker et al., 2009; Bimmel, van
their childhood, but the majority of children who eat in IJzendoorn, Bakersman-Kranenburg, Juffer, & De Geus,
fast-food restaurants do not grow up to be delinquent.) 2008). In addition, CU adolescents show a blunted
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 439 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 439

biological response to emotional and painful stimuli, as


evidenced both in neuroimaging studies (Blair, 2007),
and in studies of stress reactivity, where researchers
measure changes in cortisol levels as an indicator of
how much distress the individual feels (as you read in
Chapter 1, when individuals are stressed, their cortisol
levels rise) (Haltigan, Roisman, Susman, Barnett-Walker,
& Monahan, 2011; Susman, 2006). It has been suggested
that because individuals high in CU traits don’t experi-
ence distress as easily or often as others, they are less
likely to empathize with others or behave prosocially
(Shirtcliff et al., 2009) All CU adolescents do not be-
come delinquents, however; it also takes a willingness to
engage in antisocial activity, sometimes referred to as
“moral disengagement” (Hyde, Shaw, & Moilanen, 2010;
Shulman, Cauffman, Piquero, & Fagan, 2011). More-
over, it is important to note that the identification of the
biological underpinnings of problematic functioning
does not necessarily mean that they are inborn or hard-
wired. Child maltreatment, for example, has been shown
to affect children’s stress reactivity, which may be one
reason that abused and neglected children are at greater
risk for developing subsequent behavior problems
(Trickett, Negriff, Ji, & Peckins, 2011).
Third, and probably as a result of these biological
inclinations, children who become chronically delin-
quent are more likely than their peers to score low on
standardized tests of intelligence and neuropsycho-
logical functioning and to perform poorly in school Some aggressive adolescents are prone to having a “hostile
(Cauffman, Steinberg, & Piquero, 2005; B. Henry et al., attributional bias”—they are more likely to interpret ambiguous
1999; Pajer et al., 2008; Raine et al., 2005). Some of this interactions with others as intentionally hostile.
is due to genetic factors, but some is also due to condi-
tions surrounding their birth and prenatal care. A dis- trustworthy than others (Sharp, HA, & Fonagy, 2011),
proportionate number of persistently violent adolescents and there is evidence that some adolescents have more
were born to poor mothers who abused drugs during positive views about using aggression as a means to solve
pregnancy and had medical complications during de- problems and that this inclination, in combination with
livery that likely affected their baby’s neuropsychologi- a hostile attributional bias, leads to aggressive behavior
cal and intellectual development (Arseneault, Tremblay, that is almost automatic (Griffith Fontaine, Salzer Burks,
Boulerice, & Saucier, 2002; Brennan, Hall, Bor, Najman, & Dodge, 2002). Unfortunately, a recent evaluation of
& Williams, 2003; J. Liu, Raine, Wuerker, Venables, & Fast Track, a program designed to prevent disruptive be-
Mednick, 2009; Piquero & Chung, 2001). havior by improving individuals’ social skills, found that
Research by psychologist Kenneth Dodge and his positive effects of the program observed when the par-
colleagues into the cognitive aspects of chronic antiso- ticipants were in elementary school were not maintained
cial behavior indicates that especially aggressive young- in adolescence (Lochman et al., 2010).
sters are likely to suffer from a tendency toward what Because aggressiveness, impulsivity, hyperactivity,
has been called a hostile attributional bias (Dodge & and intelligence are relatively stable traits over child-
Pettit, 2003; Fontaine, Yang, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, hood, there is a great deal of continuity in problem be-
2008; Lansford et al., 2006). Individuals with a hostile haviors over time. Studies that have followed individuals
attributional bias are more likely than their peers to from childhood through adolescence and into adult-
interpret ambiguous interactions with other children hood find very high correlations between behavior
as deliberately hostile and to react aggressively. What problems at one point in time and antisocial behavior
might be viewed by the average adolescent as an later in life (Farrington, 2009; Fergusson, Lynskey, &
innocent and accidental bump on the basketball court Horwood, 1996; B. Henry, Caspi, Moffitt, & Silva, 1996;
may be interpreted as an intentional shove by someone
with a biased viewpoint, and it may lead to a fight.
Adolescents with a hostile attributional bias are also less hostile attributional bias The tendency to interpret ambiguous
interactions with others as deliberately hostile.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 440 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

440 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Lussier, Farrington, & Moffitt, 2009). As noted earlier, adolescence that he or she is not troubled. These indi-
this does not mean that all individuals who show anti- viduals’ serious offending may be limited to adoles-
social behavior early invariably show it later—in fact, cence, but their other problems may persist into early
the majority do not. Nevertheless, many chronically an- adulthood.
tisocial adolescents grow up to be adults who persist in The main risk factors for adolescence-limited of-
their antisocial behavior and who are at increased risk fending are well established: poor parenting (especially
for other problems as well, such as substance abuse and poor monitoring) and affiliation with antisocial peers
depression (Wiesner, Kim, & Capaldi, 2005; Wiesner & (Burt, McGue, & Iacono, 2009; Goodnight, Bates,
Windle, 2006). Newman, Dodge, & Pettit, 2006; Lacourse, Nagin, &
Tremblay, 2003; McCabe, Hough, Wood, & Yeh, 2001;
❚ Adolescence-Limited Offenders In contrast Monahan, Steinberg, & Cauffman, 2009; Wiesner,
to youngsters who begin their delinquent behaviors Capaldi, & Kim, 2012; Wissink, Deković, & Meijer,
prior to adolescence (and who often continue their an- 2009). The first of these (poor parenting) usually leads
tisocial behavior into adulthood), those who begin dur- to the second (hanging around with antisocial peers)
ing adolescence do not ordinarily show signs of serious (Dishion, Patterson, Stoolmiller, & Skinner, 1991; Laird,
psychological abnormality or severe family pathology Criss, Pettit, Dodge, & Bates, 2008), often through the
(Moffitt, 2006; Van Lier, Wanner, & Vitaro, 2007). How- impact of poor parenting on school problems. Adoles-
ever, it appears that some individuals are genetically cents who have problems in school start spending time
inclined to experience a greater-than-average increase with antisocial peers, which leads to violence and other
in sensation-seeking during early adolescence, and this types of antisocial behavior (Dishion, Véronneau, &
contributes to increased delinquency as well (Harden, Myers, 2010). Influences on adolescence-limited of-
Quinn, & Tucker-Drob, 2012). Typically, the offenses fending are virtually identical for males and females
committed by these youngsters do not develop into se- and among adolescents from different ethnic groups
rious criminality, and these individuals do not commit (Choi, Harachi, Gillmore, & Catalano, 2005; Fergusson
serious violations of the law after adolescence, although & Horwood, 2002; Fite, Wynn, & Pardini, 2009;
they may be more likely to have subsequent problems Malsonado-Molina, Piquero, Jennings, Bird, & Canino,
with drugs and alcohol (Nagin, Farrington, & Moffitt, 2009; S. Miller, Malone, & Dodge, 2010).
1995). In general, individuals who are involved in ado- As you read in Chapter 7, one of the strongest pre-
lescence-limited antisocial activities have learned the dictors of delinquency and other forms of problem be-
norms and standards of society and are far better so- havior is the extent to which the adolescent spends
cialized than life-course-persistent antisocial individ- unsupervised time in unstructured activities with
uals. Nor do adolescence-limited offenders show the peers—activities like hanging out, cruising around, and
sorts of temperamental difficulties and neuropsycho- going to parties (Osgood, Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman,
logical problems seen among life-course-persistent & Johnston, 1996). Arrest statistics show that most de-
offenders (Moffitt, Caspi, Harrington, & Milne, 2002). linquent activity occurs in group situations in which
Interestingly, in contrast to the greatly dispropor- adolescents are pressured by their friends to go along
tionate number of males who make up the life-course- with the group (Zimring, 1998). It is not coincidental
persistent population (10 times more of these offenders that the peak years of susceptibility to peer pressure
are males than females), the ratio of males to females overlap with the peak years for this sort of delinquency.
whose delinquency begins in adolescence is much Indeed, one of the central tenets of Moffit’s theory is
smaller (about 1.5 to 1) (Moffitt & Caspi, 2001). that adolescence-limited offending is largely done in an
Although adolescence-limited offenders do not effort to impress other adolescents with one’s bravado
show the same degree of pathology as life-course- and independence from adult authority; nondelin-
persistent offenders, they do have more problems both quent youth may mimic antisocial peers to increase
during adolescence and in early adulthood than youth their status and popularity (Moffitt, 2006; Rebellon,
who are not at all delinquent (Aguilar, Sroufe, Egeland, 2006). (As you read in Chapter 5, some forms of antiso-
& Carlson, 2000; X. Chen & Adams, 2010; Roisman cial behavior can increase popularity.)
et al., 2010). Indeed, a long-term follow-up of individuals
who had earlier been classified as life-course-persistent
PREVENTION AND TREATMENT
offenders, adolescence-limited offenders, or neither
OF EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS
found that as young adults the adolescence-limited of-
fenders had more mental health, substance abuse, and Given the important differences between the causes of
financial problems than individuals who had not been life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antiso-
delinquent at all as teenagers (Moffitt et al., 2002). It cial behavior, it makes sense that these two groups of
would be incorrect, therefore, to assume that just be- adolescents would be best served by different sorts of
cause an adolescent’s antisocial behavior is limited to preventive and after-the-fact interventions. In order to
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 441 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 441

lower the rate of chronic antisocial behavior, experts climate in ways that discourage antisocial behavior
argue that we need mainly to prevent disruption in and encourage prosocial behavior (Beets et al., 2009;
early family relationships and head off early academic Kellam, Ling, Merisca, Brown, & Ialongo, 1998). Finally,
problems through a combination of family support by treating delinquency seriously when it occurs—by
and preschool intervention (Loeber & Farrington, making sure that an adolescent knows that misbehav-
2000; Tolan & Gorman-Smith, 2002; Tremblay, Pagani- ior has definite consequences—we can deter her or him
Kurtz, Mâsse, Vitaro, & Pihl, 1995; Yoshikawa, 1994). from doing the same thing again in the future (E. Scott
There is also some evidence that interventions designed & Steinberg, 2008). Treating juvenile offending seri-
to improve the transition into school and work roles in ously does not require that we incarcerate juveniles for
young adulthood may prove helpful (Roisman, Aguilar, long periods, however; this has been shown to be inef-
& Egeland, 2004; Stouthamer-Loeber, Wei, & Loeber, fective in deterring future crime (Loughran et al.,
2004). 2009).
These sorts of preventive strategies are easier pro-
posed than done, however. Our society is hesitant to
intervene to prevent family problems, and we typically RECAP
wait until we see a sign of trouble in a family before act- • Experts distinguish among three broad categories
ing. Unfortunately, research shows that the outlook for of externalizing problems in adolescence: conduct
delinquents who have begun criminal careers early is disorder, aggression, and juvenile offending.
not very good, although recent evaluations of a variety Although there is considerable overlap among
of interventions that follow evidence-based practices them, they have different definitions. Some
(that is, programs that have a proven track record) have individuals with conduct disorder are callous
been encouraging (Lipsey, 2009). This is the case for and unemotional, which increases their risk for
treatment approaches that employ individual psycho- continued behavior problems.
therapy, family-based interventions, and diversion • Antisocial behavior takes three forms: authority-
programs designed to remove delinquents from the related conflicts, covert antisocial behavior, and
juvenile justice system and provide them with alterna- overt antisocial behavior. Within each of these
tive opportunities for productive behavior. One evalua- types, there are predictable patterns of progression
tion of a family-based prevention program found the from less serious to more serious forms.
strongest effects among adolescents who had a genetic • Although the juvenile crime rate has declined
predisposition for problem behavior (Brody, Beach, considerably from its peak in the early 1990s,
Philibert, Chen, & Murry., 2009). In contrast, interven- adolescents still account for a disproportionately
tions that group antisocial youth together tend to be high number of crimes, including violent crimes.
less effective, because they may inadvertently foster • It is important to distinguish between life-course-
friendships among delinquent peers, and more antiso- persistent offenders, whose antisocial behavior
cial adolescents may teach less antisocial ones some begins before adolescence and continues into
of the “tricks of the trade” (Mager, Milich, Harris, & adulthood, and adolescence-limited offenders,
Howard, 2005). whose antisocial behavior typically begins and
The prognosis for delinquents whose antisocial be- ends during the teenage years.
havior is adolescence-limited is considerably better. • Life-course-persistent offenders typically have
Many juvenile offenders simply “age out” of crime; as histories of early family problems, childhood
they settle into adult roles, following a criminal lifestyle aggression, and neuropsychological deficits, as
becomes more difficult and less attractive (Massoglia & well as strong biological predispositions toward
Uggen, 2010). Because they have internalized a basic antisocial behavior.
foundation of norms and moral standards, it is easier to • Adolescence-limited offenders, in contrast, are
help these youngsters control their own behavior and normal adolescents whose problems usually stem
stop misbehaving. Four types of strategies have been from poor parental monitoring and affiliation with
proposed. First, we can help youngsters by teaching antisocial peers.
them how to learn to resist peer pressure and to settle • Recent evaluations suggest that
conflicts without resorting to aggression (Conduct evidence-based programs can be
Problems Prevention Research Group, 1999). Second, effective in treating
by training parents to monitor their children more ef- delinquency.
fectively, we can minimize the number of opportunities
adolescents have to engage in peer-oriented misbehav-
ior (Forgatch, Patterson, Degarmo, & Beldavs, 2009).
Third, by intervening within classrooms, schools, and
neighborhoods, we may be able to alter the broader evidence-based practices Programs and practices that have a
proven scientific basis.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 442 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

442 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Internalizing Problems so sad and hopeless so often that they stop engaging in
their usual activities, and each year, 16% of this age
Most individuals emerge from adolescence confident, group seriously contemplate committing suicide (Cen-
with a healthy sense of who they are and where they are ters for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012).
headed. But in some instances, the changes and de- Fewer individuals report a pattern of depressive
mands of adolescence may leave a teenager feeling help- symptoms that includes a wider range of symptoms
less, confused, and pessimistic about the future. than sadness alone. About 8% of American teenagers
Although minor fluctuations in self-esteem during met the DSM diagnostic criteria for a depressive disor-
early adolescence are commonplace—as you read in der during the past year (see Table 13.3). Some studies
Chapter 8—it is not normal for adolescents (or adults, estimate that as many as 15% of individuals will experi-
for that matter) to feel a prolonged or intense sense ence at least one bout of depression by the age of 18
of hopelessness or frustration. Such young people (Merikangas et al., 2010).
are likely to be psychologically depressed and in need Depressed mood, depressive syndrome, and
of professional help. Depression is by far the most depressive disorder all become more common over
significant internalizing problem that has its onset in adolescence, in part because of the increasing preva-
adolescence. Many adolescents have experienced lence of stressful events during the adolescent years
bouts of severe anxiety as well, but these generally make (Graber & Sontag, 2009) and in part because the cog-
their first appearance in childhood (Merikangas et al., nitive changes of adolescence permit the introspection
2010). and rumination that often accompany depression
(Avenevoli & Steinberg, 2001). There is also a signifi-
cant decline in positive mood over the adolescent
THE NATURE AND PREVALENCE
years (Weinstein, Mermelstein, Hankin, Hedeker, &
OF DEPRESSION
Flay, 2007).
In its mild form, depression is the most common psy- There is an especially dramatic increase in the preva-
chological disturbance among adolescents (Graber & lence of depressive feelings around the time of puberty;
Sontag, 2009). Although we typically associate depres- depression is one-third as common during childhood
sion with feelings of sadness, there are other symptoms as it is during adolescence (Graber & Sontag, 2009).
that are important signs of the disturbance, and sadness One intriguing idea links the increase in depression to
alone without any other symptoms may not indicate the same changes in the brain’s dopamine system that
depression in the clinical sense of the term. Depression increase the vulnerability to alcohol and other drugs
has emotional symptoms, including dejection, de- (Davey, Yücel, & Allen, 2008). According to this view,
creased enjoyment of pleasurable activities, and low the coincidence of increased reward seeking caused by
self-esteem. It has cognitive symptoms, such as pessi- this brain change with changes in the adolescent’s social
mism and hopelessness. It has motivational symptoms, world leads to an intensification in adolescents’ desire
including apathy and boredom. Finally, it usually has for the rewards of intimate friendships and romantic
physical symptoms, such as a loss of appetite, diffi- relationships. When these rewards don’t materialize,
culty sleeping, and loss of energy. The symptoms of adolescents may become frustrated and depressed.
major depression are the same in adolescence as in Symptoms of depression increase steadily throughout
adulthood and among males and females, although, as adolescence and then start to decline—making late ad-
you’ll read, there are large sex differences in the preva- olescence the period of the life span with the highest
lence of the illness (Lewinsohn, Pettit, Joiner, & Seeley, risk for the disorder (Wight, Sepúlveda, & Aneshensel,
2003). 2004). One reason that depression declines after late
adolescence is that individuals report a significant de-
❚ Mood, Syndromes, and Disorder Many peo- cline in stress during this period (J. S. Brown, Meadows,
ple use the term depression imprecisely, to refer to a & Elder, Jr., 2007; Seiffge-Krenke, Aunola, & Nurmi,
wide range of affective problems. Psychologists believe 2009).
that it is important to distinguish among depressed
mood (feeling sad), depressive syndromes (having mul-
tiple symptoms of depression), and depressive disorder
(having enough symptoms to be diagnosed with the MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
illness) (Graber & Sontag, 2009). According to one
Scientists speculate that one reason for the decline in
large-scale survey, 30% of all high school students feel
depression as individuals transition into
adulthood is a drop in the level of stress
they experience. Do you feel less stressed
depression A psychological disturbance characterized by low
out now than you did as a teenager?
self-esteem, decreased motivation, sadness, and difficulty in finding
pleasure in formerly pleasurable activities.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 443 15/03/13 7:33 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 443

TABLE 13.3 Proposed DSM–V diagnostic criteria for mild depressive disorder (dysthymic disorder)

A. Depressed mood for most of the day, for more days than not, as indicated either by subjective account or observation by
others, for at least 2 years. (In children and adolescents, mood can be irritable and duration must be at least 1 year.)
B. Presence, while depressed, of two (or more) of the following:
1. Poor appetite or overeating
2. Insomnia or hypersomnia
3. Low energy or fatigue
4. Low self-esteem
5. Poor concentration or difficulty making decisions
6. Feelings of hopelessness
C. During the 2-year period (1 year for children or adolescents) of the disturbance, the person has never been without the
symptoms in Criteria A and B for more than 2 months at a time.
D. Over the last 2 years there is at least one period of 2 months or longer during which the full criteria for a Major
Depressive Episode are not met.
E. There has never been a Manic Episode, a Mixed Episode, or a Hypomanic Episode, and criteria for current Cyclothymic
Disorder are not met.
F. The disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a chronic Psychotic Disorder, such as Schizophrenia or
Delusional Disorder.
G. The symptoms are not better accounted for by the direct physiological effects of a substance (e.g., a drug of abuse, a
medication) or another medical condition (e.g., hypothyroidism).
H. The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of
functioning.

Source: American Psychiatric Association, 2013.

One reason the incidence of depression is greater among adolescent girls than boys is that girls are more likely to react to stress by ruminating
about the problem and feeling helpless.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 444 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

444 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

Some studies also indicate that there have been his- steeper among females, perhaps because they experience
torical increases in the prevalence of depression and a greater drop in stress as they leave adolescence behind
other signs of internalized distress, especially among (Galambos, Barker, & Krahn, 2006; Ge, Natsuaki, &
adolescents, with the rates increasing in each generation Conger, 2006; Meadows, Brown, & Elder, Jr., 2006;
(Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, & Fischer, 1993). Indeed, Stoolmiller, Kim, & Capaldi, 2005).
one analysis found that the average American child in Psychologists do not have a definitive explanation
the 1980s reported feeling more anxious than child psy- for the emergence of sex differences in depressive disor-
chiatric patients did in the 1950s; increases in stressors der at adolescence. Some evidence indicates that females
such as divorce and crime seem to have played a major are more susceptible than males to genetic influences on
role (Twenge, 2000). Several studies have also indicated depression, such that even when males and females in-
that there may be ethnic differences in the prevalence of herit the same genetic predisposition toward depression
depression during adolescence, with significantly more from their parents, that predisposition is more likely to
Hispanic teenagers reporting depressive symptoms than be manifested among girls (Jacobson & Rowe, 1999),
their White, Black, or Asian peers, especially within sam- but it is not known why this is the case. More likely,
ples of girls (Roberts, Roberts, & Chen, 1997; J. Siegel, changes in social relationships around the time of
Aneshensel, Taub, Cantwell, & Driscoll, 1998). puberty may leave girls more vulnerable than boys to
some forms of psychological distress (with some indi-
viduals inheriting a stronger predisposition than oth-
SEX DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION
ers), and depression may be a stereotypically feminine
One of the most consistent findings in the study of ado- way of manifesting it.
lescent depression involves the emergence of a very
large sex difference in rates of depression in early ado- ❚ Gender Roles More specifically, social scientists
lescence (Kessler et al., 2012). As Figure 13.9 indicates, speculate that the emergence of sex differences in de-
before adolescence, boys are somewhat more likely to pression has something to do with the social role that
exhibit depressive symptoms than girls, but after pu- the adolescent girl may find herself in as she enters the
berty, the sex difference in prevalence of depression world of boy–girl relationships (Wichstrøm, 1999). As
reverses. From early adolescence until very late in adult- you read in previous chapters, this role may bring
hood, twice as many females as males suffer from de- heightened self-consciousness over her physical ap-
pressive disorder, and females are somewhat more likely pearance and increased concern over popularity with
than males to report depressed mood (Holsen, Kraft, & peers. Since many of these feelings may provoke help-
Vitterso, 2000; Twenge & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2002; lessness, hopelessness, and anxiety, adolescent girls may
Wade, Cairney, & Pevalin, 2002). The increased risk for be more susceptible to depressive feelings. To make
depression among girls emerges during puberty, rather matters worse, pressures on young women to behave in
than at a particular age or grade in school (Angold, sex-stereotyped ways, may lead girls to adopt some be-
Costello, & Worthman, 1998; G. C. Patton et al., 2008). haviors and dispositions—passivity, dependency, and
Although sex differences in major depression persist fragility, for example—that they have been socialized to
beyond adolescence, reports of depressive symptoms believe are part of the feminine role and that may con-
tend to diminish in early adulthood, but the decline is tribute to their depressed mood. Consistent with this,

FIGURE 13.9 Depressed mood among 2.1


males and females as a function of age.
(Wichstrøm, 1999) Boys
2
Mean depressed mood scores

Girls

1.9

1.8

1.7

1.6

1.5
12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
Age
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 445 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 445

studies show that depression in girls is significantly The third explanation emphasizes girls’ generally
correlated with having a poor body image and being greater orientation toward and sensitivity to interper-
low in masculinity (Bearman & Stice, 2008; Eberhart, sonal relations (Cyranowski & Frank, 2000; Guyer,
Shih, Hammen, & Brennan, 2006; Seiffge-Krenke & McClure-Tone, Shiffrin, Pine, & Nelson, 2009; Hankin,
Stemmler, 2002; Wichstrøm, 1999). Stone, & Wright, 2010). Sex differences in levels of
The gender-role hypothesis is only one explanation the hormone oxytocin may both encourage females
for sex differences in the prevalence of depression to invest more in their close relationships and make
during adolescence. Three other accounts focus on them more vulnerable to the adverse consequences of
sex differences in the degree to which adolescence is relational disruptions and inter-personal difficulties
stressful, in the ways of coping with stress, and in vul- (Bakker, Ormel, Verhulst, & Oldehinkel, 2010). Con-
nerability to different types of stress (Cyranowski & sistent with this, research indicates that girls are
Frank, 2000; Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994; much more likely than boys to develop emotional
Rudolph & Hammen, 1999). problems as a result of family discord or problems
with peers (Crawford, Cohen, Midlarsky, & Brook,
❚ Stress, Rumination, and Sensitivity to Others 2001; Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Blatt, & Hertzog, 1999;
The link between stress and depression during adoles- Seiffge-Krenke & Stemmler, 2002; Washburn-Ormachea,
cence is well documented among both males and fe- Hillman, & Sawilowsky, 2004). Because adolescence is
males; individuals who experience more stress are more a time of many changes in relationships—in the fam-
vulnerable to depression and other internalizing prob- ily, with friends, and with romantic partners—the
lems (Ge, Natsuaki, Neiderhiser, & Reiss, 2009). But capacity of females to invest heavily in their relation-
there is evidence that early adolescence is generally a ships with others may be both a strength and a source
more stressful time for girls than boys (Charbonneau, of vulnerability.
Mezulis, & Hyde, 2009; Rudolph & Hammen, 1999).
This is because the bodily changes of puberty, especially MAKING THE PRACTICAL CONNECTION
when they occur early in adolescence, are more likely to In light of what we know
be stressful for girls than boys; because girls are more about the likely causes of
likely than boys to experience multiple stressors at the sex differences in depres-
same time (for example, going through puberty while sion, what preventive
making the transition into junior high school); and be- interventions should be
cause girls are likely to experience more stressful life targeted at young adoles-
events than boys (Graber & Sontag, 2009; Vaughan & cent girls?
Halpern, 2010).
Second, there is evidence that girls are more likely
than boys to react to stress by turning their feelings SUICIDE
inward—for instance, by ruminating about the prob-
lem (sometimes with a friend) and feeling helpless— ❚ Prevalence of Suicide According to recent na-
whereas boys are more likely to respond either by tional surveys, in any given year, close to 10% of Ameri-
distracting themselves or by turning their feelings out- can female high school students and about 6% of males
ward, in aggressive behavior or in drug and alcohol attempt suicide; nearly one-third of these attempts are
abuse (Calvete & Cardeñoso, 2005; Cox, Mezulis, & serious enough to require treatment by a physician or
Hyde, 2010; Grant et al., 2004; Jose & Brown, 2008; Pa- nurse. Twice as many adolescents think about killing
padakis, Prince, Jones, & Strauman, 2006). As we noted themselves than actually attempt suicide—referred to
in Chapter 10, girls’ greater tendency to co-ruminate as suicidal ideation—but the majority of these have
likely contributes to their greater risk for depression gone so far as to make a plan (Centers for Disease Con-
(Stone, Hankin, Gibb, & Abela, 2011). As a result, even trol and Prevention, 2012). Suicidal ideation increases
when exposed to the same degree of stress, girls are during early adolescence, peaks around age 15, and
more likely to respond to the stressors by becoming de- then declines (see Figure 13.10) (Rueter & Kwon, 2005).
pressed (Angold et al., 1996; Hankin, Mermelstein, & Adolescents who attempt to kill themselves usually
Roesch, 2007; Rudolph & Hammen, 1999). This differ- have made appeals for help and have tried but failed to
ence in the ways that boys and girls react to stress helps get emotional support from family or friends. They re-
explain why the prevalence of externalizing disorders is port feeling trapped, lonely, worthless, and hopeless
higher in boys, while the prevalence of internalizing (Kidd, 2004).
disorders is higher in girls. Girls who are more able to
tolerate conflicts in their relationships are less likely to oxytocin A hormone known to influence emotional bonding to
develop internalizing symptoms (Gunlicks-Stoessel & others.
Powers, 2008).
suicidal ideation Thinking about ending one’s life.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 446 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

446 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

FIGURE 13.10 Suicidal ideation peaks in 0.20


middle adolescence and then declines.
(Rueter & Kwon, 2005) 0.18 Younger cohort

0.16

0.14

Suicidal ideation rate


0.12

0.10

0.08
Older cohort
0.06

0.04

0.02

0
12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23
Age

The adolescent suicide rate among 15- to 19-year- have engaged in NSSI at least once, and that the preva-
olds increased alarmingly between 1950 and 1990, fu- lence has been rising in recent years (Jacobson & Gould,
eled by the increased use of drugs and alcohol and the 2007). The most common reasons given for NSSI are to
increased availability of firearms (Judge & Billick, reduce feelings of tension, anger, anxiety, or depression
2004). The rate peaked and declined somewhat during or to prompt feelings when none exist. Less is known
the 1990s, as new forms of antidepressant medication about this group of adolescents than about those who
were more widely prescribed to adolescents (Zametkin, attempt suicide, but at least one study found that levels
Alter, & Yemini, 2001; Zito et al., 2002). You may have of depressive symptomatology among adolescents who
read that suicide is a leading cause of death among attempt to harm themselves, as well as risk factors for
young people, but this is primarily because very few depression, are lower than among suicidal youth but
young people die from other causes, such as disease. higher than among individuals who have not engaged in
Although the rate of suicide rises rapidly during the self-injury (Brausch & Gutierrez, 2010).
middle adolescent years, it continues to rise throughout
adulthood, and suicide is a much more common cause ❚ Risk Factors for Suicide Systematic studies
of death among adults than it is among young people, have identified four established sets of risk factors for
largely because very few suicide attempts by adolescents attempting suicide during adolescence, and they are
are successful. The most common method of suicide similar for males and females and among Black, His-
among adolescents is with a firearm, followed by hang- panic, and White adolescents: having a psychiatric
ing. Drug overdoses and carbon monoxide poisoning problem, especially depression or substance abuse; hav-
are also common (Judge & Billick, 2004). The suicide ing a history of suicide in the family; being under stress,
rate is highest among American Indian and Alaskan especially in the areas of achievement and sexuality;
Native adolescents and lowest among Black and White and experiencing parental rejection, family disruption,
adolescents; rates among Hispanic, and Asian adoles- or extensive family conflict (Judge & Billick, 2004;
cents fall in between these extremes (Goldston et al., Schilling, Aseltine, Glanovsky, James, & Jacobs, 2009;
2008). Swahn, Bossarte, & Sullivent, 2008; Xing et al., 2010).
Adolescents who have one of these risk factors are sig-
❚ Non-Suicidal Self-Injury Many adolescents nificantly more likely to attempt suicide than their
do not contemplate suicide, but commit acts of non- peers, and adolescents who have more than one risk
suicidal self-injury (NSSI), such as deliberately burning factor are dramatically more likely to try to kill them-
or cutting themselves (Nock, Prinstein, & Sterba, 2009). selves. Adolescents who have attempted suicide once
Some studies estimate that nearly 25% of adolescents are at risk for attempting it again (Lewinsohn, Rohde, &
Seely, 1994). Adolescents are also more likely to attempt
suicide if one of their friends or someone else in their
community has committed suicide (Bearman & Moody,
non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) Deliberate attempts to hurt
2004; R. X. Liu, 2006).
oneself in nonlethal ways, including cutting or burning one’s skin.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 447 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 447

CAUSES OF DEPRESSION AND opment of depression, vulnerability to depression tends


INTERNALIZING DISORDERS to run in families—adolescents with a depressed parent
are 3 times as likely to develop depression as are other
A variety of theories have been proposed to account for youth (Graber & Sontag, 2009).
the onset of depression and other types of internalizing Other researchers have focused more on the cogni-
problems during adolescence, and the current consen- tive style of depressed individuals, suggesting that peo-
sus is that internalizing problems are likely to result ple with tendencies toward hopelessness, pessimism,
from interacting environmental conditions and indi- and self-blame are more likely to interpret events in
vidual predispositions rather than either set of factors their lives in ways that lead to the development of de-
alone. Today, most experts endorse a diathesis–stress pression (Cole et al., 2011; Clements, Aber, & Seidman,
model of depression, which suggests that depression 2008; Hilt, McLaughlin, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010;
may occur when individuals who are predisposed toward Kaslow, Adamson, & Collins, 2000; Kercher & Rapee,
internalizing problems (the term “diathesis” refers to this 2009; Prinstein & Aikins, 2004). These sorts of cogni-
predisposition) are exposed to chronic or acute stress- tive sets, which may be linked to the ways in which chil-
ors that precipitate a depressive reaction (Auerbach, dren think they are viewed first by parents and later by
Eberhart, & Abela, 2010; Hilsman & Garber, 1995; peers, develop during childhood and are thought to
Lewinsohn, Joiner, & Rohde, 2001). Individuals with- play a role in the onset of depression during adoles-
out the diathesis—who are not predisposed toward cence (Abela & Hankin, 2011; D. Cole & Jordan, 1995;
depression—are able to withstand a great deal of stress, D. Cole, Martin, & Powers, 1997). As with the case of
for instance, without developing any psychological stress-reactivity, there is some evidence that individu-
problems. In contrast, individuals who have strong pre- als’ cognitive style is stable and moderately heritable
dispositions toward the disorder may become depressed (Lau & Eley, 2008; Rudolph & Klein, 2009).
in the face of mildly stressful circumstances that most
of us would consider to be normal. Research has fo- ❚ The Stress Researchers who have been more
cused on both the diathesis and the stress—on identify- concerned with the stress component of the diathesis–
ing individual predispositions toward depression and stress model—that is, with environmental influences
on identifying the environmental circumstances likely on depression—have focused on three broad sets of
to precipitate the disorder. stressors (Aseltine, Gore, & Colten, 1994; Lewinsohn,
Gotlib, & Seeley, 1997; Lewinsohn, Roberts et al., 1994).
❚ The Diathesis Two categories of predispositions By and large, these factors have been found to be linked
have received the most attention. First, because depres- to depression among adolescents from very different
sion has been found to have a strong genetic compo- cultures (Greenberger, Chen, Tally, & Dong, 2000b;
nent, it is believed that at least some of the diathesis is S. Kim & Ge, 2000; Xia & Qian, 2001). First, depression
biological in origin and may be related to problematic is more common among adolescents from families
patterns of neuroendocrine functioning (neuroendo- characterized by high conflict and low cohesion, and
crine refers to hormonal activity in the brain and ner- rates are higher among adolescents from divorced
vous system). In particular, researchers have focused on homes (Graber & Sontag, 2009; Hamza & Willoughby,
the predisposition toward intense problems in the reg- 2011; Karevold, Røysamb, Ystrom, & Mathiesen, 2009).
ulation of activity in one or more of the hypothalamic- Second, depression is more prevalent among adoles-
pituitary axes, including those involving the adrenal, cents who are unpopular, have poor peer relations, or
thyroid, gonadal, and somatotropic axes (Graber & have friends who are depressed (Agoston & Rudolph,
Sontag, 2009; Spies, Margolin, Susman, & Gordis, 2011; Conley & Rudolph, 2009; Mendle, Harden,
2011). This biological predisposition may make it dif- Brooks-Gunn, & Graber, 2012; Sontag & Graber, 2010;
ficult for individuals to regulate their emotions, which, Van Zalk, Kerr, Branje, Stattin, & Meeus, 2010). Third,
in turn, may make them susceptible to depression and depressed adolescents report more chronic and acute
other psychological problems (Silk et al., 2007). Indi- stress than nondepressed adolescents do (Garber &
viduals who are prone to intense activation of the Cole, 2010; Graber & Sontag, 2009). These psychosocial
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis, in factors may both contribute to and exacerbate the devel-
particular, are more biologically reactive to stress than opment of negative cognitive sets (Garber, Robinson, &
others, and they are more prone to depression and Valentiner, 1997). There is also evidence that academic
other internalizing disorders. Scientists have recently
discovered that abnormalities in one of several genes, in diathesis–stress model A perspective on disorder that posits that
particular, may make some individuals more likely to problems are the result of an interaction between a preexisting
develop depression in the face of stress (Caspi et al., condition (the diathesis) and exposure to a stressful event or
2003; Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Sturge-Apple, 2007). Not condition.
surprisingly, given the role that genes play in the devel-
neuroendocrine Hormonal activity in the brain and nervous
system.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 448 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

448 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

difficulties are correlated with depression, especially


among adolescents from Asian cultures, in which rela-
tively more stress may be placed on achievement (Chan,
1997; Greenberger, Chen, Tally, & Dong, 2000b). Simi-
lar findings on the connection between academic pres-
sure and depression have emerged from studies of
affluent suburban youth in the United States (Luthar &
Becker, 2002). Although all sorts of stressors have
been linked to the onset of psychological problems
(Steinberg & Avenevoli, 2000), the single most com-
mon trigger of the first episode of major depression in
adolescence is the breakup of a romantic relationship
(Monroe, Rohde, Seeley, & Lewinsohn, 1999).
You read earlier that the prevalence of depression
rises during adolescence. Can diathesis–stress models
of depression account for this increase? For the most
part, they can. Biological theorists can point to the
hormonal changes of puberty, which are likely to
have implications for neuroendocrine activity; as you
read in Chapter 1, one of the effects of pubertal hor-
mones is to make individuals more sensitive to stress
(Edwards, Rose, Kaprio, & Dick, 2011; E. Walker,
Sabuwalla, & Huot, 2004). Depression and negative
affect among both boys and girls are correlated
with various hormones known to change at puberty Contrary to myth, suicide attempts by adolescents are rarely
(Angold, Costello, Erkanli, & Worthman, 1999; Graber impulsive reactions to specific events. Rather, adolescents who attempt
& Sontag, 2009). Many studies show that the increase to kill themselves typically have had a history of depression and have
not been able to find emotional support from family or friends.
in depression in adolescence is more closely linked to
puberty than age (Conley & Rudolph, 2009; Hayward,
Gotlib, Schraedley, & Litt, 1999), although it is diffi- TREATMENT AND PREVENTION
cult to pinpoint puberty as the cause of the problem, OF INTERNALIZING PROBLEMS
since many other changes typically occur around the
same time (such as the transition out of elementary The treatment of depression during adolescence is very
school). Cognitive theorists can point to the onset of similar to its treatment at other points in the life span.
hypothetical thinking at adolescence, which may re- Clinicians use a wide range of approaches, including
sult in new (and perhaps potentially more depress- biological therapies employing antidepressant medica-
ing) ways of viewing the world (Kuhn, 2009). And tion (these address the neuroendocrine problem, if one
theorists who emphasize environmental factors draw exists); psychotherapies designed to help depressed ad-
attention to the new environmental demands of ad- olescents understand the roots of their depression, to
olescence, such as changing schools, beginning to increase the degree to which they experience reinforce-
date, or coping with transformations in family rela- ment in their daily activities, or to change the nature of
tionships—all of which may be stressful (Graber & their cognitive set; and family therapy, which focuses
Sontag, 2009; Morris, Ciesla, & Garber, 2010). Thus, on changing patterns of family relationships that may
there are many good reasons to expect that the preva- be contributing to the adolescent’s symptoms. A com-
lence of depression would increase as individuals pass prehensive review found that psychotherapy with
from childhood into adolescence. Individuals who depressed and anxious adolescents has a significant,
develop internalizing disorders such as depression but modest, effect; although most of the treatment
and anxiety in adolescence are at elevated risk to studies employed cognitive-behavioral therapy, the re-
suffer from these problems as adults (Lewinsohn, view found that other evidence-based practices (e.g.,
Rohde, Seeley, Klein, & Gotlib, 2003; Pine, Cohen, interpersonal therapy) were equally effective (Weisz,
Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998). McCarty, & Valeri, 2006). Importantly, however, the re-
searchers found that evidence-based approaches are
superior to approaches that do not have a scientific ba-
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) A class of sis, which, unfortunately, are widely used (Weisz et al.,
antidepressant medications that has proven to be effective with 2009). With regard to antidepressants, research has
adolescents suffering from internalizing problems, such as confirmed the effectiveness of a class of drugs called
depression. selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in the
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 449 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e CHAPTER 13 Psychosocial Problems in Adolescence 449

treatment of depression in adolescence (Graber & Sontag, both relatively common stressors (such as breaking up
2009). These medications are also effective in treating with a romantic partner) and relatively severe ones
other types of internalizing problems, such as anxiety (such as exposure to war trauma, terrorism, or natural
disorders and social phobias, especially when used in disasters) (Gershoff, Aber, Ware, & Kotler, 2010; La
conjunction with cognitive-behavioral therapy (Brent Greca & Silverman, 2009).
et al., 2008; Kennard et al., 2009; Walkup et al., 2008). Yet, for some adolescents, the very same sources
and levels of stress do not seem to be associated with
psychological or physical upset at all. Thus, although
RECAP we tend to think of stress as having negative effects on
• Depression is the most common internalizing our well-being, the connection between stress and
disorder of adolescence, afflicting as many as 15% dysfunction is not clear-cut. Some adolescents show
of adolescents by the time they are 18. enormous resilience in the face of enormous adver-
• Significant sex differences in depression emerge in sity (Compas & Reeslund, 2009; Luthar, Cicchetti, &
early adolescence that are maintained throughout Becker, 2000; Masten, 2001). What makes some adoles-
adulthood. Females are more likely to be depressed cents more vulnerable to the effects of stress than oth-
than males, although experts disagree about why ers? Psychologists point to three sets of factors.
this is the case. First, the effect of any one stressor is exacerbated if it
• Nearly 10% of adolescent girls and 5% of boys have is accompanied by other stressors. Studies show that
attempted suicide, and twice this number have stress tends to have a multiplicative effect: An adoles-
seriously thought about killing themselves. cent who faces two stressors at the same time (parental
• Depression is the result of an interacting set of divorce and school change, for example) is more than
environmental conditions (especially stress) and twice as likely to experience psychological problems as
individual predispositions, or diatheses (including someone who has experienced only one of the two
biological and cognitive predispositions). stressors (Compas & Reeslund, 2009; Forehand, Biggar, &
• Depression during adolescence can be successfully Kotchick, 1998).
treated through medication and Second, adolescents who have other resources—
psychotherapy, which are either internal resources, such as high self-esteem,
especially effective healthy identity development, high intelligence, or
if combined. strong feelings of competence, or external resources,
such as social support from others—are less likely
to be adversely affected by stress than their peers
(Compas & Reeslund, 2009; Luthar, 1991; Masten
et al., 1999; Steinberg, Dahl et al., 2006). Adolescents
Stress and Coping with close friends and good social skills seem to be
better able to handle stressors such as parental divorce
Nearly half of all adolescents report difficulty in coping or starting junior high school than are teenagers who
with stressful situations at home or at school (Gans, lack close friendships or have fewer interpersonal
1990). These stressors include major life changes (such resources. Most importantly, teenagers who have
as going through parental divorce, changing schools, or warm and close family relationships are less likely to be
having someone in the family suddenly become seri- distressed by a stressful experience than are teenagers
ously ill), chronically stressful conditions (such as pov- without such familial support (Weist, Freedman,
erty, a disabling illness, or constant family conflict), and Paskewitz, Proescher, & Flaherty, 1995). Indeed, studies
day-to-day hassles (such as school exams, fights with consistently show that the presence of a close parent–
friends, and arguments with siblings and parents) adolescent relationship is probably the single most
(Compas & Reeslund, 2009). important factor in protecting adolescents from psy-
Research indicates that stress can affect individuals chological harm (Resnick et al., 1997). The importance
in different ways (Steinberg & Avenevoli, 2000). For of social support as a buffer against the adverse effects
some teenagers, stress can lead to internalized disorders, of stress has been documented in studies of adoles-
such as anxiety, depression, headaches, and indiges- cents around the world.
tion—even compromised immune system functioning Finally, some adolescents use more effective coping
(Birmaher et al., 1994). For others, the consequences strategies than do others. Specialists who study coping
of stress are externalized, in behavior and conduct strategies distinguish between strategies that involve
problems. For still others, the impact of stress is mani- taking steps to change the source of the stress, called
fested in drug and alcohol abuse. These links between
stress and psychosocial problems have been docu-
mented in studies of youngsters from all ethnic groups resilience The ability of an individual to continue to function
and family backgrounds and among youth exposed to competently in the face of adversity or stress.
ste32118_ch13_414-450.indd Page 450 15/03/13 7:34 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

450 PART 3 Psychosocial Development During Adolescence

primary control strategies, and those that involve ef- Petersen, 1995; L. D. Scott & House, 2005; Seiffge-Krenke &
forts to adapt to the problem, called secondary control Klessinger, 2000; Tolan, Gorman-Smith, Henry, Chung,
strategies (Compas & Reeslund, 2009). For instance, if & Hunt, 2002; Weisz, Francis, & Bearman, 2010).
you are very worried about an upcoming exam in this
course, a primary control strategy might be to form a MAKING THE PERSONAL CONNECTION
study group with other students in order to review the
How do you cope with stress? Are you
material, whereas a secondary control strategy might
more likely to use primary or secondary
be to go out to a movie or for a run in order to relax
control strategies?
yourself.
In some situations, secondary control strategies are
quite effective. These tend to be stressful situations that
are clearly uncontrollable, such as being diagnosed with RECAP
a serious illness or learning that one’s parents are getting • For some adolescents, exposure to chronic or
a divorce (Hall, Chipperfield, Perry, Ruthig, & Goetz, severe stress can result in psychological or physical
2006). In these instances, trying to distract and calm difficulties, but for others, the very same sources
oneself may help alleviate some of the stress, and trying and levels of stress do not seem to be associated
to exert control over an uncontrollable situation may with upset at all.
only make matters worse (Jaser et al., 2007). But, in • In general, the effect of stress is exacerbated if it is
general, research shows that adolescents who employ accompanied by other stressors, if the adolescent
primary control strategies are less vulnerable to the det- lacks sufficient internal or external resources, or if
rimental health consequences of stress, especially if the the adolescent has poorly developed coping skills.
source of the stress is controllable (Clarke, 2006). Studies • Coping with stress by trying to change the source
of a wide range of male and female adolescents from of stress (if it is controllable) or to adapt to
different ethnic groups show that individuals who use the problem (if it is not) is generally
primary or secondary control strategies are better ad- more effective than coping
justed, less depressed, and less likely to have behavioral through distraction
problems than those who react to stress through disen- or avoidance.
gagement or avoidance (Compas & Reeslund, 2009;
Dumont & Provost, 1999; Herman-Stahl, Stemmler, &

primary control strategies Coping strategies in which an


individual attempts to change the stressor.
secondary control strategies Coping strategies that involve
attempts by the individual to adapt to the stressor.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-1 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Glossary
A anxious-resistant attachment An insecure attachment between
infant and caregiver, characterized by distress at separation and
abstinence-only sex education Programs that encourage adoles-
anger at reunion.
cents to avoid sexual activity but that do not provide information
about safe sex. attachment The strong affectional bond that develops between
an infant and a caregiver.
academic achievement Achievement that is measured by stan-
dardized tests of scholastic ability or knowledge. attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) A biologically
based psychological disorder characterized by impulsivity, inatten-
achievement The psychosocial domain concerning behaviors
tiveness, and restlessness, often in school situations.
and feelings in evaluative situations.
authoritarian parents Parents who use punitive, absolute, and
achievement attributions The beliefs an individual holds about
forceful discipline, and who place a premium on obedience and
the causes of her or his successes and failures.
conformity.
activational role (of hormones) The process through which
authoritative parents Parents who use warmth, firm control, and
changes in hormone levels, especially at puberty, stimulate changes
rational, issue-oriented discipline, in which emphasis is placed on
in the adolescent’s behavior, appearance, or growth.
the development of self-direction.
adolescence The second decade of life.
authority conflicts A type of antisocial behavior characterized by
adolescence-limited offenders Antisocial adolescents whose de- stubbornness and rebelliousness.
linquent or violent behavior begins and ends during adolescence.
autobiographical memory The recall of personally meaningful
adolescent growth spurt The dramatic increase in height and past events.
weight that occurs during puberty.
autoerotic behavior Sexual behavior that is experienced alone,
adolescent health care A field of study and health care devoted to such as masturbation or sexual fantasizing.
understanding the health care needs of individuals during the sec-
autonomy The psychosocial domain concerning the develop-
ond decade of life.
ment and expression of independence.
adrenarche The maturation of the adrenal glands that takes place
during adolescence.
Adult Attachment Interview A structured interview used to assess B
an individual’s past attachment history and “internal working model” baby boom The period following World War II, during which the
of relationships. number of infants born was extremely large.
age grading The process of grouping individuals within social Bar (Bas) Mitzvah In Judaism, the religious ceremony marking
institutions on the basis of age. the young person’s transition to adulthood.
age of majority The designated age at which an individual is rec- basal metabolism rate The minimal amount of energy used by
ognized as an adult. the body during a resting state.
age–crime curve The relationship between chronological age and behavioral autonomy The capacity to make independent deci-
offending, showing that the prevalence of offending peaks in late sions and to follow through with them.
adolescence. behavioral decision theory An approach to understanding ado-
agency The sense that one has an impact on one’s world. lescent risk taking, in which behaviors are seen as the outcome of
aggression Acts done to be intentionally harmful. systematic decision-making processes.
AIDS (acquired immune deficiency syndrome) A disease, caused behavioral genetics The scientific study of genetic influences on
by a virus transmitted by means of bodily fluids, that devastates the behavior.
immune system. big fish–little pond effect The reason that individuals who
androgens A class of sex hormones secreted by the gonads, found attend high school with high-achieving peers feel worse about
in both sexes, but in higher levels among males than females follow- themselves than comparably successful individuals with lower-
ing puberty. achieving peers.
anhedonic Having difficulty experiencing positive emotions, a binge drinking Consuming five or more drinks in a row on one
risk factor for depression. occasion, an indicator of alcohol abuse.
anorexia nervosa An eating disorder found chiefly among young binge eating disorder An eating disorder characterized by a pat-
women, characterized by dramatic and severe self-induced weight loss. tern of binge eating that is not accompanied by drastic attempts to
lose weight.
antisocial personality disorder A disorder of adulthood charac-
terized by antisocial behavior and persistent disregard for the rules body mass index (BMI) A measure of an individual’s body
of society and the rights of others. fat, the ratio of weight to height; used to gauge overweight and
obesity.
anxious-avoidant attachment An insecure attachment between
infant and caregiver, characterized by indifference on the part of the brain function Patterns of brain activity.
infant toward the caregiver. brain structure The physical form or shape of the brain.
G-1
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-2 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

G-2 Glossary

bulimia An eating disorder found primarily among young childhood and early adolescence and is characterized by reason-
women, characterized by a pattern of binge eating and extreme ing that is based on the rules and conventions of society.
weight loss measures, including self-induced vomiting. correlation The extent to which two things vary systematically
with each other.
C cortisol A hormone produced when a person is exposed to stress.
callous-unemotional traits (CU) A cluster of traits characteristic covert antisocial behavior A type of antisocial behavior charac-
of psychopathic individuals, which includes a lack of empathy and terized by misdeeds that are not always detected by others, such as
indifference toward the feelings of others. lying or stealing.
causation The correlation between two things attributable to the criminal behavior Crimes that are dealt with in the criminal jus-
effect one thing has on the other. tice system, regardless of the age of the offender.
charter schools Public schools that have been given the auton- criminal justice system The system of courts and related institu-
omy to establish their own curricula and teaching practices. tions developed to handle adult crime.
child protectionists Individuals who argued, early in the 20th critical thinking Thinking that involves analyzing, evaluating,
century, that adolescents needed to be kept out of the labor force in and interpreting information, rather than simply memorizing it.
order to protect them from the hazards of the workplace. cross-sectional study A study that compares two or more groups
chlamydia A sexually transmitted infection caused by a bacterium. of individuals at one point in time.
circumcision A procedure in which some part of the genitals is crowds Large, loosely organized groups of young people, com-
cut and permanently altered. posed of several cliques and typically organized around a common
civic engagement Involvement in political and community af- shared activity.
fairs, as reflected in knowledge about politics and current affairs, cultivation theory A perspective on media use that emphasizes
participation in conventional and alternative political activities, the impact media exposure has on individuals.
and engaging in community service. cultural capital The resources provided within a family through
cliques Small, tightly knit groups of between 2 and 12 friends, the exposure of the adolescent to art, music, literature, and other
generally of the same sex and age. elements of “high culture.”
co-rumination Excessive talking with another about problems. curvilinear pattern In statistical analyses, a pattern of relations
cognitive autonomy The establishment of an independent set of between two variables that resembles a U-shaped or an inverted
values, opinions, and beliefs. U-shaped curve.

cognitive-developmental view A perspective on development, cyberbullying Bullying that occurs over the Internet or via cell
based on the work of Piaget, that takes a qualitative, stage-theory phones.
approach.
cohort A group of individuals born during the same general his- D
torical era.
date rape Being forced by a date to have sex against one’s will.
collective efficacy A community’s social capital, derived from its
members’ common values and goals. deductive reasoning A type of logical reasoning in which one
draws logically necessary conclusions from a general set of prem-
comorbid Co-occurring, as when an individual has more than
ises, or givens.
one problem at the same time.
delayed phase preference A pattern of sleep characterized by
comprehensive high school An educational institution that
later sleep and wake times, which usually emerges during puberty.
evolved during the first half of the 20th century, offering a varied
curriculum and designed to meet the needs of a diverse population delinquency Juvenile offending that is processed within the juve-
of adolescents. nile justice system.
comprehensive sex education Programs that not only provide depression A psychological disturbance characterized by low
information about contraception, STDs, and pregnancy but also self-esteem, decreased motivation, sadness, and difficulty in finding
teach adolescents how to refuse unwanted sex and avoid unin- pleasure in formerly pleasurable activities.
tended sex, increase their motivation to engage in safe sex, and detachment In psychoanalytic theory, the process through which
change perceptions about peer norms and attitudes. adolescents sever emotional attachments to their parents or other
concrete operations The third stage of cognitive development, authority figures.
according to Piaget, spanning the period roughly between age 6 and developmental trajectories Patterns of change over time.
early adolescence. diathesis–stress model A perspective on disorder that posits
conduct disorder A repetitive and persistent pattern of antisocial that problems are the result of an interaction between a preexist-
behavior that results in problems at school or work, or in relation- ing condition (the diathesis) and exposure to a stressful event or
ships with others. condition.
continuous transitions Passages into adulthood in which adult diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) A technique used to produce im-
roles and statuses are entered into gradually. ages of the brain that shows connections among different regions.
conventional moral reasoning According to Kohlberg, the digital divide The fact that adolescents from different economic
second level of moral development, which occurs during late and cultural groups have differential access to digital technology.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-3 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Glossary G-3

discontinuous transitions Passages into adulthood in which externalizing disorders Psychosocial problems that are mani-
adult roles and statuses are entered into abruptly. fested in a turning of the symptoms outward, as in aggression or
disordered eating Mild, moderate, or severe disturbance in eat- delinquency.
ing habits and attitudes.
divided attention The process of paying attention to two or more
F
stimuli at the same time.
false-self behavior Behavior that intentionally presents a false
dopamine A neurotransmitter especially important in the brain
impression to others.
circuits that regulate the experience of reward.
familism An orientation toward life in which the needs of one’s
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex The outer and upper areas of the
family take precedence over the needs of the individual.
front of the brain, important for skills such as planning ahead and
controlling impulses. family systems theory A perspective on family functioning that
emphasizes interconnections among different family relationships
dyscalculia Impaired ability in arithmetic.
(such as marital, parent–child, sibling).
dysgraphia Impaired ability in handwriting.
feedback loop A cycle through which two or more bodily func-
dyslexia Impaired ability in reading or spelling. tions respond to and regulate each other, such as that formed by the
hypothalamus, the pituitary gland, and the gonads.
E female genital mutilation The cutting or removal of the clito-
ris, performed in some cultures as part of the initiation of female
early adolescence The period spanning roughly ages 10–13, cor-
adolescents.
responding roughly to the junior high or middle school years.
five-factor model The theory that there are five basic dimensions
ecological perspective on human development A perspective on
to personality: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neu-
development that emphasizes the broad context in which develop-
roticism, and openness to experience.
ment occurs.
flow experience The experience of high levels of both concentra-
educational attainment The number of years of schooling com-
tion and interest at the same time.
pleted by an individual.
electroencephalography (EEG) A technique for measuring elec- formal operations The fourth stage of cognitive development,
trical activity at different locations on the scalp. according to Piaget, spanning the period from early adolescence
through adulthood.
emerging adulthood The period spanning roughly ages 18–25,
during which individuals make the transition from adolescence to foster care A placement in a temporary living arrangement when
adulthood. a child’s parents are not able to provide care, nurturance, or safety.

emotional autonomy The establishment of more adultlike and functional connectivity The extent to which multiple brain regions
less childish close relationships with family members and peers. function at the same time, which improves during adolescence.
endocrine system The system of the body that produces, circu- functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) A technique
lates, and regulates hormones. used to produce images of the brain, often while the subject is per-
forming some sort of mental task.
epiphysis The closing of the ends of the bones, which terminates
growth after the adolescent growth spurt has been completed. future orientation The extent to which an individual is able and
inclined to think about the potential consequences of decisions and
estrogens A class of sex hormones secreted by the gonads, found
choices.
in both sexes, but in higher levels among females than males follow-
ing puberty.
ethnic identity The aspect of individuals’ sense of identity con- G
cerning ancestry or racial group membership.
gangs Organized peer groups of antisocial individuals.
ethnic socialization The process through which individuals de-
gateway drugs Drugs that, when used over time, lead to the use of
velop an understanding of their ethnic or racial background, also
other, more dangerous substances.
referred to as racial socialization.
gender identity The gender an individual identifies with.
ethnography A type of research in which individuals are ob-
served in their natural settings. gender intensification hypothesis The idea that pressures to be-
have in sex-appropriate ways intensify during adolescence.
event-related potentials (ERP) Changes in electrical activity in
areas of the brain in response to specific stimuli or events. generational dissonance Divergence of views between adoles-
evidence-based practices Programs and practices that have a cents and parents that is common in families of immigrant parents
proven scientific basis. and American-born adolescents.

exo-system In the ecological perspective on human development, gifted students Students who are unusually talented in some as-
the layer of the environment that does not directly contain the devel- pect of intellectual performance.
oping person but that affects the setting in which the person lives. glands Organs that stimulate particular parts of the body to re-
experience sampling method (ESM) A method of collecting data spond in specific ways to particular hormones.
about adolescents’ emotional states, in which individuals are paged gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) neurons Specialized
and asked to report on their mood and activity. neurons that are activated by certain pubertal hormones.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-4 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

G-4 Glossary

gonads The glands that secrete sex hormones: in males, the testes; initiation ceremony The formal induction of a young person
in females, the ovaries. into adulthood.
gonorrhea A sexually transmitted infection caused by a bacterium. instrumental aggression Aggressive behavior that is deliberate
graduated driver licensing A licensing system in which full adult and planned.
driving privileges are not granted all at once to teen drivers, but internal working model The implicit model of interpersonal re-
phased in over time. lationships that an individual employs throughout life, believed to
be shaped by early attachment experiences.
internalizing disorders Psychosocial problems that are manifested
H
in a turning of the symptoms inward, as in depression or anxiety.
herpes A sexually transmitted infection caused by a virus.
Internet addiction A disorder in which an individual’s use of the
HIV (human immunodeficiency virus) The virus associated Internet is pathological, defined by six symptoms: salience, mood
with AIDS. change, tolerance, withdrawal, conflict, and relapse and reinstatement.
hormones Highly specialized substances secreted by one or more intimacy The psychosocial domain concerning the formation,
endocrine glands. maintenance, and termination of close relationships.
hostile attributional bias The tendency to interpret ambiguous inventionists Theorists who argue that the period of adolescence
interactions with others as deliberately hostile. is mainly a social invention.
HPG (hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal) axis The neurophysio-
logical pathway that involves the hypothalamus, the pituitary gland,
and the gonads. J
human papillomavirus (HPV) One of several viruses that causes junior high school An educational institution designed during
a sexually transmitted disease. the early era of public secondary education, in which young adoles-
hypothalamus A part of the brain that controls the functioning cents are schooled separately from older adolescents.
of the pituitary gland. juvenile justice system A separate system of courts and related
institutions developed to handle juvenile crime and delinquency.
juvenile offending An externalizing problem that includes delin-
I quency and criminal behavior.
iatrogenic effects Unintended adverse consequences of a treat-
ment or intervention.
identity The domain of psychosocial development involving self- L
conceptions, self-esteem, and the sense of who one is. late adolescence The period spanning roughly ages 18–21, cor-
identity diffusion The incoherent, disjointed, incomplete sense responding approximately to the college years.
of self characteristic of not having resolved the crisis of identity. learned helplessness The acquired belief that an individual is not
identity foreclosure The premature establishment of a sense of able to influence events through his or her own efforts or actions.
identity, before sufficient role experimentation has occurred. learning disability A difficulty with academic tasks that cannot
identity versus identity diffusion According to Erikson, the nor- be traced to an emotional problem or sensory dysfunction.
mative crisis characteristic of the fifth stage of psychosocial devel- leptin A protein produced by the fat cells that may play a role in
opment, predominant during adolescence. the onset of puberty.
imaginary audience The belief, often brought on by the heightened LGBT youth Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth, some-
self-consciousness of early adolescence, that everyone is watching and times referred to as sexual-minority youth.
evaluating one’s behavior. life-course-persistent offenders Individuals who begin demon-
immigrant paradox The fact that on many measures of psycho- strating antisocial or aggressive behavior during childhood and
logical functioning and mental health, adolescents who have immi- continue their antisocial behavior throughout adolescence and into
grated more recently to the United States score higher on measures adulthood.
of adjustment than adolescents from the same ethnic group whose limbic system An area of the brain that plays an important role in
family has lived in the United States for several generations. the processing of emotional experience, social information, and re-
indifferent parents Parents who are characterized by low levels of ward and punishment.
both responsiveness and demandingness. long-term memory The ability to recall something from a long
individuation The progressive sharpening of an individual’s time ago.
sense of being an autonomous, independent person. longitudinal study A study that follows the same group of indi-
indulgent parents Parents who are characterized by responsive- viduals over time.
ness but low demandingness, and who are mainly concerned with
the child’s happiness.
information-processing perspective A perspective on cognition M
that derives from the study of artificial intelligence and attempts to macro-system In the ecological perspective on human develop-
explain cognitive development in terms of the growth of specific ment, the outermost layer of the environment, containing forces
components of the thinking process (such as memory). such as history and culture.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-5 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Glossary G-5

mainstreaming The integration of adolescents who have educa- neuroendocrine Hormonal activity in the brain and nervous system.
tional handicaps into regular classrooms. neurons Nerve cells.
mastery motivation Motivation to succeed based on the pleasure neurotransmitters Specialized chemicals that carry electrical im-
one will experience from mastering a task. pulses between neurons.
media practice model A perspective on media use that empha- new media Digital media typically accessed via computers,
sizes the fact that adolescents not only choose what media they are smartphones, or other Internet-based devices.
exposed to but also interpret the media in ways that shape their
nonshared environmental influences The nongenetic influences in
impact.
individuals’ lives that make them different from people they live with.
melatonin A hormone secreted by the brain that contributes to
non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) Deliberate attempts to hurt one-
sleepiness.
self in nonlethal ways, including cutting or burning one’s skin.
menarche The time of first menstruation, one of the most im-
portant changes to occur among females during puberty.
mentalizing The ability to understand someone else’s mental O
state. occupational attainment A measure of achievement based on
meso-system In the ecological perspective on human develop- the status or prestige of the job an individual holds.
ment, the layer of the environment formed by two or more immedi- oppositional-defiant disorder A disorder of childhood and ado-
ate settings, as in the home–school linkage. lescence characterized by excessive anger, spite, and stubbornness.
metacognition The process of thinking about thinking itself. orbitofrontal cortex The region of the brain located directly be-
micro-systems In the ecological perspective on human develop- hind the eyes, important for the evaluation of risk and reward.
ment, the immediate settings in which adolescents develop, such as organizational role (of hormones) The process through which
the family or the peer group. early exposure to hormones, especially prenatally, organizes the
middle adolescence The period spanning roughly ages 14–17, brain or other organs in anticipation of later changes in behavior or
corresponding to the high school years. patterns of growth.
middle school An educational institution housing 7th- and 8th- ovaries The female gonads.
grade students along with adolescents who are 1 or 2 years younger. overt antisocial behavior A type of antisocial behavior charac-
midlife crisis A psychological crisis over identity believed to oc- terized by aggression toward others.
cur between the ages of 35 and 45, the age range of most adoles- oxytocin A hormone known to influence emotional bonding to
cents’ parents. others.
Monitoring the Future An annual survey of a nationwide sample
of American 8th-, 10th-, and 12th-graders, mainly known for its
data on adolescent substance use. P
moral disengagement Rationalizing immoral behavior as legiti- parental demandingness One of two important dimensions of
mate, as a way of justifying one’s own bad acts. parenting; demandingness refers to the degree to which the parent
expects and insists on mature, responsible behavior from the child.
multidimensional model of racial identity (MMRI) Sellers’s
perspective on ethnic identity, which emphasizes three different parental responsiveness One of the two important dimensions
phenomena: racial centrality (how important race is in defining of parenting; responsiveness refers to the degree to which the parent
individuals’ identity), private regard (how individuals feel about responds to the child’s needs in an accepting, supportive manner.
being a member of their race), and public regard (how individuals parietal cortex A region of the brain that is especially important
think others feel about their race). for working memory.
multiethnic Having two parents of different ethnic or racial participant observation A research technique in which the re-
backgrounds. searcher “infiltrates” a group of individuals in order to study their
myelination The process through which brain circuits are insu- behavior and relationships.
lated with myelin, which improves the efficiency of information peak height velocity The point at which the adolescent is grow-
processing. ing most rapidly.
peer groups Groups of individuals of approximately the same age.
N perceived popularity How much status or prestige an individual has.
National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) A peri- performance motivation Motivation to succeed based on the re-
odic testing of American 4th-, 8th-, and 12th-graders by the federal wards one will receive for successful performance.
government, used to track achievement. personal fable An adolescent’s belief that he or she is unique
negative emotionality The presumed underlying cause of in- and therefore not subject to the rules that govern other people’s
ternalizing disorders, characterized by high levels of subjective behavior.
distress. pheromones A class of chemicals secreted by animals that stimu-
negative identity The selection of an identity that is obviously late certain behaviors in other members of the species.
undesirable in the eyes of significant others and the broader pituitary gland One of the chief glands responsible for regulating
community. levels of hormones in the body.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-6 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

G-6 Glossary

platonic relationships Nonsexual relationships with individuals relational aggression Acts intended to harm another through the
who might otherwise be romantic partners. manipulation of his or her relationships with others, as in malicious
positive youth development The goal of programs designed to gossip.
facilitate healthy psychosocial development and not simply to deter religiosity The degree to which one engages in religious practices,
problematic development. like attending services.
possible selves The various identities an adolescent might imag- resilience The ability of an individual to continue to function
ine for him- or herself. competently in the face of adversity or stress.
postconventional moral reasoning In Kohlberg’s theory, the response inhibition The suppression of a behavior that is inap-
stage of moral development during which society’s rules and con- propriate or no longer required.
ventions are seen as relative and subjective rather than as authorita-
reverse causation Relationship in which the correlation between
tive; also called principled moral reasoning.
two things is due not to the first thing causing the second, but to the
preconventional moral reasoning According to Kohlberg, the second causing the first.
first level of moral reasoning, which is typical of children and is
risk factors Factors that increase the likelihood of some behavior
characterized by reasoning that is based on rewards and punish-
or condition.
ments associated with different courses of action.
rite of passage A ceremony or ritual marking an individual’s
prefrontal cortex The region of the brain most important for
transition from one social status to another, especially marking the
sophisticated thinking abilities, such as planning, thinking ahead,
young person’s transition to adulthood.
weighing risks and rewards, and controlling impulses.
routine activity theory A perspective on adolescence that views
premature affluence Having more income than one can manage
unstructured, unsupervised time with peers as a main cause of
maturely, especially during adolescence.
misbehavior.
preoperational period The second stage of cognitive develop-
ment, according to Piaget, spanning roughly ages 2–5.
primary control strategies Coping strategies in which an indi-
S
vidual attempts to change the stressor.
scaffolding Structuring a learning situation so that it is just
problem behavior syndrome The covariation among various
within the reach of the student.
types of externalizing disorders believed to result from an underly-
ing trait of unconventionality. scarification The intentional creation of scars on some part or
parts of the body, often done as part of an initiation ceremony.
prosocial behavior Behaviors intended to help others.
school performance A measure of achievement based on an indi-
protective factors Factors that limit individual vulnerability to
vidual’s grades in school.
harm.
psychological control Parenting that attempts to control the ad- school vouchers Government-subsidized vouchers that can be
olescent’s emotions and opinions. used for private school tuition.

psychopaths Individuals who are not only antisocial but also ma- schools within schools Subdivisions of the student body within
nipulative, superficially charming, impulsive, and indifferent to the large schools created to foster feelings of belongingness.
feelings of others. secondary control strategies Coping strategies that involve at-
psychosocial Referring to aspects of development that are both tempts by the individual to adapt to the stressor.
psychological and social in nature, such as developing a sense of secondary education The system of middle schools, junior high
identity or sexuality. schools, and high schools.
psychosocial moratorium A period during which individuals are secondary sex characteristics The manifestations of sexual ma-
free from excessive obligations and responsibilities and can there- turity at puberty, including the development of breasts, the growth
fore experiment with different roles and personalities. of facial and body hair, and changes in the voice.
puberty The biological changes of adolescence. secular trend The tendency, over the past two centuries, for indi-
viduals to be larger in stature and to reach puberty earlier, primarily
because of improvements in health and nutrition.
Q
secure attachment A healthy attachment between infant and
quinceañera An elaborate sort of “coming-out” celebration for caregiver, characterized by trust.
adolescent girls that is practiced in many Latino communities.
selective attention The process by which we focus on one stimulus
while tuning out another.
R selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) A class of antide-
reactive aggression Aggressive behavior that is unplanned and pressant medications that has proven to be effective with adolescents
impulsive. suffering from internalizing problems, such as depression.
reference groups A group against which an individual compares self-conceptions The collection of traits and attributes that indi-
him- or herself. viduals use to describe or characterize themselves.
rejection sensitivity Heightened vulnerability to being rejected self-consciousness The degree to which an individual is preoc-
by others. cupied with his or her self-image.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-7 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Glossary G-7

self-efficacy The sense that an individual has some control over social redefinition The process through which an individual’s
his or her life. position or status is redefined by society.
self-esteem The degree to which individuals feel positively or social support The extent to which an individual receives emo-
negatively about themselves. tional or instrumental assistance from his or her social network.
self-fulfilling prophecy The idea that individuals’ behavior is in- sociometric popularity How well-liked an individual is.
fluenced by others’ expectations for them. spirituality The degree to which one places importance on the
self-handicapping Deliberately behaving in ways that will likely quest for answers to questions about God and the meaning of life.
interfere with doing well, in order to have an excuse for failing. spurious causation Relationship in which the correlation be-
self-image stability The degree to which an individual feels that tween two things is due to the fact that each of them is correlated
his or her self-image changes from day to day. with some third factor.
sensation seeking The pursuit of experiences that are novel or standards-based reform Policies designed to improve achieve-
exciting. ment by holding schools and students to a predetermined set of
sense of identity The extent to which individuals feel secure standards measured by achievement tests.
about who they are and who they are becoming. status offense A violation of the law that pertains to minors but
sensorimotor period The first stage of cognitive development, not adults.
according to Piaget, spanning the period roughly between birth and statutory rape Sex between two individuals, even when it is con-
age 2. sensual, when at least one of the persons is below the legal age of
consent; in the United States, the specific age of consent varies from
serotonin A neurotransmitter that is especially important for the
state to state.
experience of different moods.
stereotype threat The harmful effect that exposure to stereotypes
service learning The process of learning through involvement in
about ethnic or sex differences in ability has on student perfor-
community service.
mance.
set point A physiological level or setting (e.g., of a specific hor-
student engagement The extent to which students are psycho-
mone) that the body attempts to maintain through a self-regulating
logically committed to learning and mastering the material rather
system.
than simply completing the assigned work.
sex-role behavior Behavior that is consistent with prevailing ex-
substance abuse The misuse of alcohol or other drugs to a degree
pectations for how individuals of a given sex are to behave.
that causes problems in the individual’s life.
sexual orientation Whether one is sexually attracted to individu-
suicidal ideation Thinking about ending one’s life.
als of the same sex, other sex, or both.
synapse The gap in space between neurons, across which neu-
sexual socialization The process through which adolescents are
rotransmitters carry electrical impulses.
exposed to and educated about sexuality.
synaptic pruning The process through which unnecessary con-
sexual-minority youth Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender
nections between neurons are eliminated, improving the efficiency
(LGBT) youth.
of information processing.
sexuality The psychosocial domain concerning the development
and expression of sexual feelings.
sexually transmitted disease (STD) Any of a group of infections— T
including HPV, gonorrhea, trichomoniasis, herpes, chlamydia, and Tanner stages A widely used system that describes the five stages
AIDS—passed on through sexual contact. of pubertal development.
shared environmental influences Nongenetic influences that teenager A term popularized about 50 years ago to refer to young
make individuals living in the same family similar to each other. people; it connoted a more frivolous and lighthearted image than
sibling deidentification The process through which siblings de- did “adolescent.”
liberately try to be different from each other. temporal cortex A region of the brain that is especially impor-
sibling rivalry Competition between siblings, often for parental tant for social cognition.
attention. testes The male gonads.
social capital The interpersonal resources available to an adoles- testosterone One of the sex hormones secreted by the gonads,
cent or family. found in both sexes but in higher levels among males than females.
social cognition The aspect of cognition that concerns thinking theory of mind The ability to understand that others have be-
about other people, about interpersonal relations, and about social liefs, intentions, and knowledge that may be different from one’s
institutions. own.
social control theory A theory of delinquency that links deviance tracking The practice of separating students into ability groups,
with the absence of bonds to society’s main institutions. so that they take classes with peers at the same skill level.
social conventions The norms that govern everyday behavior in transgender Describing individuals whose gender identity does
social situations. not match the sex they were assigned at birth.
social promotion The practice of promoting students from one trichomoniasis A sexually transmitted infection caused caused
grade to the next automatically, regardless of their school performance. by a parasite.
ste32118_glo_G1-G8.indd Page G-8 28/03/13 2:45 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

G-8 Glossary

U working memory That aspect of memory in which information


is held for a short time while a problem is being solved.
underachievers Individuals whose actual school performance is
lower than what would be expected on the basis of objective mea-
sures of their aptitude or intelligence. Y
uses and gratification approach A perspective on media use that youth Today, a term used to refer to individuals ages 18–22; it
emphasizes the active role users play in selecting the media to which once referred to individuals ages 12–24.
they are exposed.

Z
V
zero tolerance A get-tough approach to adolescent misbehavior
ventromedial prefrontal cortex The lower and central area at the that responds seriously or excessively to the first infraction.
front of the brain, important for gut-level decision making.
zone of proximal development In Vygotsky’s theory, the level of
viral marketing A way of promoting products or services by en- challenge that is still within the individual’s reach but that forces an
couraging individuals to pass information on to others. individual to develop more advanced skills.

W
work values The particular sorts of rewards an individual looks
for in a job (extrinsic, intrinsic, social, altruistic, security, influence,
leisure).
ste32118_cred_C1-C4.indd Page C-1 14/05/13 5:08 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Credits
TEXT CREDITS Fig 3.1, p. 92: From W. Osgood, G. Ruth, J. Eccles, J. Jacobs, & B. Barber, 2005,
“Six Paths to Adulthood,” in R. Settersten, F. Furstenberg, Jr., & R. Rumbaut
Fig 1.1, p. 23: From M. Grumbach, J. Roth, S. Kaplan & R. Kelch, 1974, (Eds.), On the Frontier of Adulthood, pp. 320–355. Chicago: University of
“Hypothalamic-Pituitary Regulation of Puberty in Man: Evidence and Con- Chicago Press. Reprinted with permission.
cepts Derived from Clinical Research,” in Control of the Onset of Puberty, Fig 3.2, p. 93: From N.L. Galambos, E.T. Barker, & H.J. Krahn, 2006,
M. Grumbach, G. Grave & F. Mayer (eds.) Copyright © 1974 Lippincott “Depression, Self-Esteem, and Anger in Emerging Adulthood: Seven-Year
Williams & Wilkins. Reprinted by permission. Trajectories,” Developmental Psychology, 42, pp. 350–365. Copyright © 2006
Fig 1.2, p. 25: From L. Dorn, R. Dahl, H. Woodward, & F. Biro, 2006, “Defin- by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted by permission of the
ing the Boundaries of Early Adolescence: A User’s Guide to Assessing Puber- American Psychological Association and the author.; 94 : From N.L. Galambos,
tal Status and Pubertal Timing in Research with Adolescents,” Applied E.T. Barker, & H.J. Krahn, 2006, “Depression, Self-Esteem, and Anger in
Developmental Science, 1, pp. 30–56. Reprinted with permission from Taylor Emerging Adulthood: Seven-Year Trajectories,” Developmental Psychology,
& Francis and the author. 42, pp. 350–365. Copyright © 2006 by the American Psychological Associa-
tion. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association
Fig 1.3, p. 27: From W. Marshall, 1978, “Puberty,” in Human Growth, Vol. 2, F. and the author.
Faulkner & J. Tanner (eds.). Copyright © 1978 Plenum Publishers. Reprinted
with kind permission from Springer Science and Business Media. Table 3.1, p. 94: From J.E. Schulenberg, A.L. Bryant, & P.M. O’Malley, 2004,
“Taking Hold of Some Kind of Life: How Developmental Tasks Relate to
Table 1.1, p. 29: From B. Goldstein, Introduction to Human Sexuality, Copy- Trajectories of Well-Being During the Transition to Adulthood,” Develop-
right © 1976. Reprinted by permission of Star Publishing Company, ment and Psychopathology, 16(4), p. 1124. Reprinted with the permission of
Belmont, CA. Cambridge University Press.
Fig 1.5, p. 30: From N. Morris & J. Udry, 1980, “Validation of a Self-Admin- Fig 3.8, p. 109: Reprinted from V.J. Ebin, C.D. Sneed, D.E. Morisky, M. Rotherman-
istered Instrument to Assess Stage of Adolescent Development,” Journal of Borus, A.M. Magnusson, & C. Malotte, 2001, “Acculturation and Interrelation-
Youth and Adolescence, 9, pp. 271–280. Reprinted with kind permission from ships Between Problem and Health-Promoting Behaviors Among Latino
Springer Science and Business Media. Adolescents,” Journal of Adolescent Health, 28, Fig. 1., pp. 62–72, with permission
Fig 1.6, p. 31: From W. Marshall & J. Tanner, 1969, “Variations in the Pattern from the Society for Adolescent Medicine and Elsevier.
of Pubertal Change in Girls,” Archive of Disease of Childhood, 44, 130. BMJ Fig 3.9, p. 111: Welcome to the Neighborhood? Long-Term Impacts of Mov-
Publishing Group. Reprinted with the permission of BMJ Publishing Group. ing to Low-Poverty Neighborhoods on Poor Children’s and Adolescents’
Fig 1.7, p. 34: From P. Eveleth & J. Tanner, 1976, Worldwide Variation in Outcomes by Fauth et al., Journal of Research on Adolescence, 2007, p. 270,
Human Growth. Cambridge University Press. Figure 3. Cambridge University Press.

Fig 1.9, p. 38: Snell et al., Sleep and the Body Mass Index and Overweight Fig 3.11, p. 113: From H.L. Chung & L. Steinberg, 2006, “Relations Between
Status of Children and Adolescents. Child Development, 2007, Vol 78, Num- Neighborhood Factors, Parenting Behaviors, Peer Deviance, and Delin-
ber 1, Page 315, Figure 2. Reprinted by permission. quency Among Serious Juvenile Offenders,” Developmental Psychology, 42,
pp. 319–331. Copyright © 2006 by the American Psychological Association.
Fig 1.11, p. 44: From D. Dick, R. Rose, R. Viken & J. Kaprio, 2000, “Pubertal Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association and the
Timing and Substance Use: Associations Between and Within Families Across author.
Late Adolescence,” Developmental Psychology, 36(2), pp. 180–189. Copyright ©
2000 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted by permission of Fig 4.1, p. 123: From R. Larson, M. Richards, G. Moneta, G. Holmbeck &
American Psychological Association and the author. E. Duckett, 1996, “Changes in Their Families From Ages 10 to 18: Disengage-
ment and Transformation,” Developmental Psychology, 32, pp. 744–754.
Fig 1.12, p. 46: “Prevalence of Obesity Among U.S. Children and Adolescents Copyright © 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted by
by Age Group and Selected Period, 1963–2002,” from: http://www.cdc.gov/ permission of the American Psychological Association and the author.
nchs/products/pubs/pubd/hestats/overwght99.htm
Fig 4.2, p. 126: From “Stage-environment fit during adolescence: Trajectories
Fig 1.13, p. 47: Nader et al., JAMA, July 16, 2008, Vol 300, p. 303, Figure 3. of family relations and adolescent outcomes” by Gutman, Leslie Morrison;
Fig 2.1, p. 64: Processing Speed in Childhood and Adolescence: Longitudinal Eccles, Jacquelynne S. in Developmental Psychology, March 2007. Reprinted
Models for Examining Developmental Change by Kail et al., Child Develop- by permission of American Psychological Association.
ment, 2007, Vol 78, p. 1767. Figure 2, top panel only. Reprinted by permission. Table 4.1, p. 131: Reprinted with the permission of Simon and Schuster.
Fig 2.4, p. 70: Why Do Many Psychiatric Disorders Emerge During Adoles- Adult Publishing Group from The Ten Basic Principles of Good Parenting, by
cence? By Paus et al., from Nature Review Neuroscience 2008, Vol. 9, p. 953, Laurence Steinberg, Ph.D. Copyright © 2004 by Laurence Steinberg, Ph.D.
Figure 4. Reprinted by permission. Fig 4.5, p. 134: Longitudinal Course and Family Correlates of Sibling Relation-
Fig 2.5, p. 75: From N. Bayley, 1949, “Consistency and Variability in the ships From Childhood Through Adolescence by Ji-Yeon et al., Child Develop-
Growth of Intelligence from Birth to Eighteen Years,” Journal of Genetic Psy- ment, 2006, p. 1753, Figure 1.
chology, 75, pp. 165–196. Reprinted with the permission of the Helen Dwight Fig 4.6, p. 137: From “Parental monitoring moderates the importance of
Reid Educational Foundation. Published by Heldref Publications, 1319 genetic and environmental influences on adolescent smoking” by Dick,
Eighteenth Street, NW, Washington, DC 20036-1802. Copyright © 1949. Danielle M.; Viken, Richard; Purcell, Shaun; Kaprio, Jaakko; Pulkkinen,
Fig 2.6, p. 87: From “Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as Lea; Rose, Richard J. from Journal of Abnormal Psychology, February 2007.
indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model” by Reprinted by permission of American Psychological Association.
Steinberg, Laurence; Albert, Dustin; Cauffman, Elizabeth; Banich, Marie; Fig 4.8, p. 139: From “On the relationship between family structure and anti-
Graham, Sandra; Woolard, Jennifer in Developmental Psychology, November social behavior” by Apel & Kaukinen from Criminology, Vol. 46, pp. 35–69
2008. Reprinted by permission of American Psychological Association. (Figure 1, p. 49). Used by permission of American Society of Criminology.

C-1
ste32118_cred_C1-C4.indd Page C-2 14/05/13 5:08 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

C-2 Credits

Fig 4.10, p. 146: From R. Conger, K. Conger, G. Elder, Jr., F. Lornez, R. Simons Fig 8.3, p. 262: Reprinted from K. Brown, R. McMahon, F. Biro, P. Cranford,
& L. Whitbeck, 1993, “Family Economic Stress and Adjustment of Early G. Schrieber, S.L. Similo, M. Waclawiw & R. Striegel-Moore, 1998, “Changes
Adolescent Girls,” Developmental Psychology, 29, pp. 206–219. Copyright © in Self Esteem in Black and White Girls Between the Ages of 9 and 14 years:
1993 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted by permission of The NHLBI Growth and Health Study,” Journal of Adolescent Health, 23,
the American Psychological Association and the author. pp. 7–19, with permission from the Society for Adolescent Medicine and
Elsevier.
Fig 5.1, p. 152: From R. Larson & M. Richards, 1998, “Waiting for the Week-
end,” New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, Winter, No. 82, Fig 8.4, p. 263: From M.L. Greene & N. Way, 2005, “Self-Esteem Trajectories
pp. 37–51. John Wiley & Sons. Inc. Among Ethic Minority Adolescents: A Growth Curve Analysis of the Patterns
and Predictors of Change,” Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15, p. 165.
Fig 5.3, p. 158: From R. Larson & M. Richards, 1991, “Daily Companionship
Reprinted by permission of Blackwell Publishing.
in Late Childhood and Early Adolescence: Changing Developmental Con-
texts,” Child Development, 62, pp. 284–300. Reprinted with permission of P. 256: From K.C. McLean, 2005, “Late Adolescent Development: Narrative
the Society for Research in Child Development and Blackwell Publishing. Meaning Making and Memory Telling,” Developmental Psychology, 41,
Fig 5.4, p. 161: An Observational Study of Early Heterosexual Interaction at pp. 683–691. Copyright © 2005 by the American Psychological Association.
Middle School Dances by Pellegrini & Long, Journal of Research on Adolescence, Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association and the
2007, p. 629, bottom panel only. Reprinted by permission of Wiley-Blackwell. author.
Fig 8.8, p. 282: The Development of Gendered Interests and Personality
Fig 5.5, p. 164: From B. Brown, 1990, “Peer Groups,” in At the Threshold: The
Qualities From Middle Childhood Through Adolescence: A Biosocial Analy-
Developing Adolescent, S. Feldman & G. Elliot (eds.), Harvard University
sis by McHale et al., from Society for Research in Child Development 2009,
Press. Reprinted by permission.
Child Development, p. 488 (Figure 1) and p. 489 (Figure 3). Reprinted by
Fig 5.6, p. 166: From “Developmental changes in gender composition of permission.
friendship networks in adolescent girls and boys” by Poulin, François;
Fig 9.2, p. 295: From L. Steinberg & S. Silverberg, 1986, “The Vicissitudes of
Pedersen, Sara in Developmental Psychology, November 2007. Reprinted by
Autonomy in Early Adolescence,” Child Development, 57.
permission of American Psychological Association.
Fig 9.3, p. 296: From T. Grisso, L. Steinberg, J. Woolard, E. Cauffman, E. Scott,
Fig 5.7, p. 168: From M.L. Greene, N. Way, & K. Pahl, 2006, “Trajectories of
S. Graham, F. Lexcen, N. Reppucci, & R. Schwartz, 2003 , “Juveniles’ Compe-
Perceived Adult and Peer Discrimination Among Black, Latino, and Asian
tence to Stand Trial: A Comparison of Adolescents’ and Adults’ Capabilities as
American Adolescents: Patterns and Psychological Correlates,” Develop-
Trial Defendants,” Law and Human Behavior, 27, pp. 333–363. Reprinted with
mental Psychology, 42, p. 227, Fig 2. Copyright © 2006 by the American
kind permission from Springer Science and Business Media.
Psychological Association. Reprinted by permission.
Fig 9.4, p. 298: Allen et al., Development and Psychopathology, 2006, p. 164,
Fig 5.11, p. 181: Developmental Trajectories of Bullying and Associated Fac- Figure 1. Reprinted by permission of Cambridge University Press.
tors by Pepler et al., Child Development, 2008, p. 331, Figure 1. Reprinted by
permission. Fig 9.5, p. 299: From A. Redlich & G. Goodman, 2003, “Taking Responsibil-
ity for an Act Not Committed: The Influence of Age and Suggestibility,” Law
Fig 6.1, p. 189: From D. Tanner, 1972, Secondary Education. New York: Macmillan; and Human Behavior, 27(2), pp. 141–156. Reprinted with kind permission
William T. Grant Foundation Commission on Work, Family, and Citizenship. from Springer Science and Business Media.
(1988). The Forgotten Half: Non-College Youth in America. Washington, D.C.:
William T. Grant Foundation Commission on Work, Family, and Citizenship. Table 10.2, p. 318: From Conceptions of Modern Psychiatry, by Henry
Stack Sullivan. Copyright © 1940, 1945, 1947, 1953 by the William Alanson
Fig 6.2, p. 198: The Transition to High School as a Developmental Process White Psychiatric Foundation. Used by permission of W.W. Norton &
Among Multiethnic Urban Youth by Benner & Graham, Child Develop- Company, Inc.
ment, 2009, p. 368, Figure 6. Reprinted by permission.
Fig 10.1, p. 323: From “Attachment and the experience and expression of emo-
Fig 6.5, p. 207: Parent involvement in the academic adjustment of Latino tions in romantic relationships: A developmental perspective”by Simpson,
middle and high school youth: Teacher expectations and school belonging Jeffry A.; Collins, W. Andrew; Tran, SiSi; Haydon, Katherine C. in Journal of
as mediators by G.P. Kuperminc in Journal of Adolescence, Vol 31 (2008), Personality and Social Psychology, February 2007. Reprinted by permission of
pp. 469–483, Figure 1, p. 474. Reprinted by permission. American Psychological Association.
Fig 6.6, p. 208: From R. Larson & M. Richards, 1998, “Waiting for the Week- Fig 10.3, p. 328: From N. Way, & M.L. Greene, 2006, “Trajectories of Per-
end,” New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, Winter, No. 82, ceived Friendship Quality During Adolescence: The Patterns and Contex-
pp. 37–51. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. tual Predictors,” Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16(2), p. 308. Reprinted
Fig 7.5, p. 225: Staff et al., From Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. Copy- by permission of Blackwell Publishing.
right © 2008 by Wiley-Blackwell. Reprinted by permission. Fig 10.6, p. 329: From D. Buhrmester, 1996, “Need Fulfillment, Interpersonal
Fig 7.6, p. 230: From R. Larson & M. Richards, 1991, “Daily Companionship Competence and the Developmental Contexts of Early Adolescent Friend-
in Late Childhood and Early Adolescence: Changing Developmental Con- ship,” in The Company They Keep: Friendship in Childhood and Adolescence,
texts,” Child Development, 62. Reprinted with permission of the Society for W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb & W. Hartup (eds.), pp. 158–185. Cambridge
Research in Child Development and Blackwell Publishing. University Press. Reprinted with the permission of Cambridge University
Press.
P. 254: From S. Harter, 1999, The Construction of the Self. New York: Guil-
Fig 10.7, p. 331: From A. Fuligni, 1998, “Authority, Autonomy, and Parent-
ford. Reprinted by permission of Guilford Publications, Inc.
Adolescent Conflict and Cohesion: A Study of Adolescents from Mexican,
P. 256: Quote from S. Harter, 1990, “Identity and Self-Development,” in At Chinese, Filipino and European Backgrounds,” Developmental Psychology,
the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent, S. Feldman & G. Elliott (eds.), 34, pp. 782–792. Copyright © 1998 by the American Psychological Associa-
pp. 352–387. Harvard University Press. Reprinted by permission. tion. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association
Fig 8.2, p. 260: From R. Larson, G. Moneta, M. Richards & S. Wilson, 2002, and the author.
“Continuity, Stability and Change in Daily Emotional Experience Across P. 332: From R.D. Stanton-Salazar & S.U. Spina, 2005, “Adolescent Peer Net-
Adolescence, Child Development, 73(4), pp. 1151–1165. Reprinted with per- works as a Context for Social and Emotional Support,” Youth and Society,
mission of the Society for Research in Child Development. 36(4), pp. 397–398; pp. 399–400. Reprinted with permission.
ste32118_cred_C1-C4.indd Page C-3 14/05/13 5:08 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Credits C-3

Fig 11.2, p. 354: Copyright © 2002 by the National Council on Family Rela- Images; Chapter 3 Opener: © Comstock/PunchStock; p. 87: © Radius
tions, 3989 Central Avenue, NE, Suite 550, Minneapolis, MN 55421. Reprinted Images/Photolibrary; p. 90: Courtesy of Seventeen Magazine. Photo by
by permission of the National Council on Family Relations and Blackwell Francesco Scavullo.; p. 96: © Brand X Pictures; p. 98: © Jeremy Woodhouse/
Publishing. AGE Fotostock; p. 100: © Carol Beckwith & Angela Fisher/HAGA/Image
P. 356: From L.M. Coleman & S.M. Cater, 2005, “A Qualitative Study of the Works; p. 105: © Creatas/PunchStock; p. 111: © AP Photo/Gaas.
Relationship Between Alcohol Consumption and Risky Sex in Adolescents,”
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, pp. 649–661. Part 2

Fig 11.6, p. 376: From Innocent Report Card (UNICEF, 2001). Copyright © Opener: © Don Farrall/Getty Images; Chapter 4 Opener: © Royalty-Free/
2001 by UNICEF. Reprinted with permission. Corbis; p. 122: © Fuse/Getty Images; p. 124: © Bob Thomas/Getty Images;
p. 127: © Ryan McVay/Getty Images; p. 131: © gulfimages/Alamy; p. 133:
Fig 12.2, p. 391: Implicit Theories of Intelligence Predict Achievement Across © Michelle D. Bridwell/PhotoEdit; p. 135: © Photodisc/AGE Fotostock;
an Adolescent Transition: A Longitudinal Study and an Intervention” by Lisa S. p. 140: © John Moore/Getty Images; p. 141: © Image Source/PunchStock;
Blackwell et al., from Child Development, 2007, p. 257, Figure 3. Reprinted by p. 145: © Ingram Publishing; Chapter 5 Opener: © Robert Kohlhuber/Getty
permission. Images; p. 153: © Bob Daemmrich/The Image Works; p. 154: © Gabriela
Fig 12.4, p. 396: Academic Achievement and Adolescents’ Daily Time Use in Medina/Blend Images LLC; p. 159: © Angela Hampton Picture Library/
the Social and Academic Domains by Witkow from Journal of Research on Alamy; p. 161: © Stockbyte/Punchstock; p. 165: © Bob Daemmrich/The
Adolescence, 2009, pp. 162–163, Figures 1 and 2. Reprinted with permission. Image Works; p. 167: © Collection/SuperStock; p. 172: © BananaStock/
PunchStock; p. 174: © Stockbyte/PictureQuest; p. 177: © Image Source/
P. 400: From E.M. Horvat & K.S. Lewis, 2003, “Reassessing the Burden of
Alamy; p. 181: © SW Productions/Photodisc/Getty Images; Chapter 6
‘Acting White’: The Importance of Peer Group in Managing Academic
Opener: © Don Farrall/Getty Images; p. 190: © The Granger Collection;
Success,” Sociology of Education, 76(4), pp. 270–272.
p. 192: © Purestock/SuperStock; p. 194: © AP Photo/Medford Mail Tribune,
Fig 13.6, p. 434: Testing an Idealized Dynamic Cascade Model of the Devel- Jim Craven; p. 200: © Photodisc/Getty Images; p. 204: © Bob Daemmrich/
opment of Serious Violence in Adolescence by Dodge et al., Child Develop- PhotoEdit; p. 206: © BananaStock/PictureQuest; p. 209: © David Lassman/
ment, 2008, p. 1911, Figure 2. Reprinted by permission. Syracuse Newspapers/The Image Works; Chapter 7 Opener: © Justin A.
Fig 13.8, p. 438: Sex Differences in Adolescent Depression: Stress Exposure Morris/Getty Images; p. 220: © Digital Vision; p. 223: © Peter Hvizdak/The
and Reactivity Models by Hankin et al., Child Development, 2007, p. 288, Image Works; p. 226: © J. Hardy/PhotoAlto; p. 228: © Peter Hvizdak/The
Figure 2. Reprinted by permission. Image Works; p. 231: © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./John Flournoy,
photographer; p. 232: © Royalty-Free/Corbis; p. 237: © Bob Daemmrich/
Fig 13.9, p. 444: From L. Wichstrøm, 1999, “The Emergence of Gender The Image Works; p. 242: © James Woodson/Getty Images; p. 245: © JGI/
Difference in Depressed Mood During Adolescence: The Role of Intensi- Jamie Grill/Getty Images; p. 247: © Purestock/PunchStock.
fied Gender Socialization,” Developmental Psychology, 35, pp. 232–245.
Copyright © 1999 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted Part 3
by permission of the American Psychological Association and the author.
Opener: © Comstock Images/JupiterImages; Chapter 8 Opener: © The
Fig 13.10, p. 446: From M.A. Rueter & H. Kwon, 2005, “Developmental Trends
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Ken Karp photographer; p. 254: © Royalty-Free/
in Adolescent Suicidal Ideation,” Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15, p. 211.
Corbis; p. 256: © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Ken Karp, photographer;
Reprinted by permission of Blackwell Publishing.
p. 261: © Angela Hampton Picture Library/Alamy; p. 264: © Thinkstock/Getty
Images; p. 267: © Getty Images/SW Productions; p. 271: © Purestock; p. 274: ©
PHOTO CREDITS Lee Snider/The Image Works; p. 281: © Syracuse Newspapers/Li-Hau Lan/The
Image Works; Chapter 9 Opener: © Photographer’s Choice/Getty Images;
Front Matter p. 287: © BananaStock/PunchStock; p. 289: © Ingram Publishing; p. 291: ©
Page vi: Courtesy of Laurence Steinberg; p. viii (top): © Don Farrall/Getty BananaStock/PunchStock; p. 297: © Reed Kaestner/Corbis; p. 299: Courtesy of
Images; p. viii (middle): © Don Farrall/Getty Images; p. viii (bottom): Laurence Steinberg; p. 304: © PhotoAlto; p. 310: © Brand X Pictures/Punch
© Comstock Images/JupiterImages; p. ix: © Don Farrall/Getty Images; p. x: Stock; Chapter 10 Opener: © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Erin Melloy;
© Don Farrall/Getty Images; p. xii: © Comstock Images/JupiterImages; p. 319: © Bill Bachmann/The Image Works; p. 327: © Thinkstock/Getty Images;
p. xviii (top): © Adam Gault/Getty Images; p. xviii (bottom): © J. Hardy/ p. 333: © Design Pics/Hammond HSN; p. 337: © PhotoAlto; p. 342: © J. Hardy/
PhotoAlto; p. xix: © Photodisc/Getty Images; Introduction Opener: © PhotoAlto; Chapter 11 Opener: © 2009 Jupiterimages Corporation; p. 350: ©
Comsotck Images/Jupiterimages; p. 5: © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Patrick Sheandell/Photo Alto; p. 352: © Stockbyte/Punchstock; p. 358: © Banana
Inc./Christopher Kerrigan, photographer; p. 8: © Fuse/Getty Images; p. 9: Stock/PunchStock; p. 361: © SW Productions/Getty Images; p. 363: © Johannes
© Ryan McVay/Getty Images; p. 12: © Patrick Sheandell/Photo Alto; p. 16: Kroemer/VISUM/The Image Works; p. 366: © Digital Vision/PunchStock;
© Dave Moyer. p. 370: © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.; p. 373: © BananaStock/Punch-
Stock; p. 375: © BananaStock/PunchStock; Chapter 12 Opener: © Comstock
Part 1 Images/JupiterImages; p. 386: © Bananastock/AGE Fotostock; p. 389: © Gary
Opener: © Don Farrall/Getty Images; Chapter 1 Opener: © Comstock Conner/PhotoEdit; p. 394: © Bob Daemmrich/The Image Works; p. 399: © SW
Images; p. 24: © Glow Images; p. 37: © Comstock/age fotostock; p. 38: Productions/Brand X Pictures/Getty Images; p. 406: © Jim West/Alamy; p. 410:
© Corbis/age fotostock; p. 41: © Jeff Greenberg/The Image Works; p. 42: © Lars A. Niki; Chapter 13 Opener: © Comstock/PunchStock; p. 417: © Banana
© Fancy Collection/SuperStock; p. 47: © Image Source/Corbis; Chapter 2 Stock/PunchStock; p. 422: © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Gary He, pho-
Opener: © Don Farrall/Getty Images; p. 59: © Rhoda Sidney/The Image tographer; p. 427:© Adam Gault/Getty Images; p. 435: © Chet Gordon/The
Works; p. 62 (left): © Photodisc/Getty Images; p. 62 (right): © Tim Pannell/ Image Works; p. 439: © Mikael Karlsson; p. 443: © Jetta Productions/Walter
Corbis; p. 66: © Brookhaven National Laboratory/Getty Images; p. 69: Hodges/Getty Images; p. 448: © Rubberball/Getty Images.
© Steve Gschmeissner/SPL/Science Source; p. 73: © i love images/city break/ Repeating design element (pen):© Burke/Triolo/Brand X Pictures/Jupiter
Alamy; p. 77: © S. Olsson/PhotoAlto; p. 80: © RubberBall Productions/Getty Images.
ste32118_cred_C1-C4.indd Page C-4 14/05/13 5:08 PM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-1 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Name Index
Note: Page numbers followed by f Alarcon, O., 262 Anderman, L. H., 196, 198 Asakura, T., 181
refer to figures. Alasker, F., 222 Andersen, S. L., 424 Asante, K., 245
Albano, A. M., 449 Anderson, A. L., 39, 92f, 108, 233, Asarnow, J. R., 449
Alberga, A., 244 234, 234f, 235, 420 Asato, M., 68
A Albers, A. B., 429 Anderson, C. A., 242, 278 Aseltine, R. H. J., 214, 419, 427,
Albersheim, L., 322 Anderson, J. E., 66, 355, 370 446, 447
Abbott, R. D., 112, 406, 429, 430
Albert, D., 65, 69, 81f, 81–82, 287, Anderson, M., 210 Aseltine, R., Jr., 332
Abecassis, M., 179
295, 299, 299f Anderson, S. E., 32 Asher, S., 176, 327, 346, 396
Abela, J. R. Z., 420, 445, 447
Alberts, A., 59 Andersson, H., 386 Ashman, A., 438
Abelmann, N., 272
Albino, A., 419 Ando, M., 181 Asmussen, L., 140–141
Aber, J. L., 197, 198, 222, 264, 274,
Albrecht, A. K., 132 Andrews, D., 265 Aspy, C. B., 360
402, 447, 449
Albrecht, C., 145 Andrews, J., 265 Asquith, P., 132, 293
Aber, M. S., 203, 400
Albrecht, H., 42, 43 Andrews, J. A., 42 Astone, N., 374, 376
Ablard, K., 391
Albrecht, R., 42 Andrews-Hanna, J., 69 Astor, R. A., 168, 178, 209
Abma, J., 353–355, 372, 373
Albus, K. E., 114 Andrinopoulos, K., 372 Atkins, R., 307, 308
Abraham, C., 370, 371
Alexander, C., 246 Aneshensel, C. S., 36, 353, 354, 362, Attia, E., 50
Abrahamson, A., 136
Alexander, K. L., 199–200, 215, 228, 442, 444 Attie, I., 40, 49
Abramovitch, R., 78, 308
405, 407 Ang, R. P., 387 Au, T. K., 360, 361
Abrams, C. L., 199, 393
Alexandersson, A., 107 Angeleri, R., 24 Auerbach, R. P., 447
Abrams, D. B., 429
Alex Mason, W., 426 Angeletti, J., 293 Aunola, K., 393
Abrantes, A. M., 428
Alfaro, E. C., 275 Angold, A., 444, 445, 448 Austin, E., 262
Acebo, C., 38
Alink, L., 177 Angus, R., 236 Austin, L., 79
Aceves, M. J., 114, 181, 312
Allen, J. P., 12, 132, 133, 134, 175, Ansary, N. S., 418 Austin, S. B., 49, 345, 365
Achenbach, T., 418
176, 197, 206, 206f, 233, 239, Anstine, P., 431 Avenevoli, S., 417, 417f, 442, 444,
Acierno, R., 428
245, 246, 286, 287, 288, 289, Anthony, C., 142 448, 449
Ackard, D., 262
290, 291, 292, 298, 298f, 318, Anthony, J. C., 312, 427 Averett, S. L., 370
Ackerman, J., 435
321, 322, 323, 406 Antonishak, J., 97, 133, 176 Avery-Leaf, S., 345
Ackley, K., 369
Allen, L., 198, 222, 264, 274 Antonucci, T., 44 Avigad-Spitz, L., 134
Acock, A., 140, 441
Allen, M. L., 46 Antshel, K., 202 Awusabo-Asare, K., 360
Adalbjarnardottir, S., 130–131
Allen, N., 124 Apel, R., 139f, 139–140, 227, 228 Axinn, W. G., 126
Adam, E., 145
Allen, N. B., 442 Apfel, N., 378 Azmitia, M., 273–274, 327, 328, 392
Adam, E. K., 38f, 53
Allen, R., 38 Apperly, I., 76 Azrael, D., 367
Adamczyk-Robinette, S. L., 132
Adams, G., 258, 429 Allen, V., 44 Aquan-Assee, J., 332
Adams, J., 291 Alli, M. M., 429 Aquilino, W., 133, 144–145
Allison, K. W., 171, 193, 376, 429 Arbess, G., 49 B
Adams, K., 419
Adams, M., 440 Allison, P., 378 Arbeton, A., 393 Babalola, S., 362
Adams, R. E., 120, 126, 172, 181, Allred, E., 421 Arbona, C., 298 Babson, K. A., 43
330, 331 Allwood, M., 114 Archambault, I., 179 Bacallao, M., 276, 278
Adamson, L. B., 447 Allyene, E., 372 Archibald, A., 48, 50 Bachman, J. G., 226–228, 234, 416,
Adan, A., 147, 398, 399 Almeida, D., 131, 444 Archibald, M. E., 362 422–424, 423f, 424f, 426, 428,
Adcock, A., 369 Almeida, J., 367 Ardelt, M., 135 429, 440
Adegoke, A., 41 Almerigi, J. B., 236, 236f Arel, S., 11 Bachrach, C., 374
Adelson, J., 308 Alpert, A., 265 Arend, R., 320 Bäckström, T., 169
Adler, N., 53, 370, 373 Alter, M. R., 446 Arens, A., 262 Badger, G., 431
Agliata, D., 247 Altermatt, E., 389 Argys, L. M., 370 Badger, S., 91, 316, 316f
Agoston, A. M., 447 Althaus, F., 102 Arias, D. F., 92 Baer, J., 126, 127
Agras, W. S., 49, 50 Altheimer, I., 113 Arias, I., 368 Baer, P. E., 291
Agronick, G., 345 Altschul, I., 275 Arim, R. G., 33 Bagley, E., 108, 109, 113, 146
Aguilar, B., 440, 441 Alva, S., 402 Armeli, S., 356 Bagwell, C., 172, 183
Aguilar, J., 424 Alvarado, R., 149 Armistead, L., 115, 142 Bahne, J., 364
Ahmed, R., 429 Alvarez, A., 276, 278 Armor, D., 394 Bahr, H., 311–312
Ahmed, S., 114, 312 Alvarez, R., 276 Armour, S., 357 Bailey, J. A., 336, 337f
Ahn, N., 377 Alvarez-Jimenez, A., 207, 207f, 393 Armstrong, B., 209 Bailey, P. E., 373–374
Aikins, J. W., 172, 178, 343, 447 Amaral, D., 71 Armstrong, D., 39 Baird, A., 82
Ainsworth, A. T., 274 Amato, P. R., 133, 139–143 Armstrong, T. D., 418 Baird, C. L., 397
Ainsworth, J. W., 112, 400 Ambridge, B., 64 Arndorfer, C. L., 334, 435 Baisch, E., 142, 345
Ainsworth-Darnell, J. W., 143, 394, American Psychiatric Association, Arndt, S., 113 Bajaj, A., 58
399, 400 428f, 432f, 443f Arneson, J. J., 398 Bakan, D., 16, 189
Ajanaku, S., 397, 411 American Psychological Association Arnett, J. J., 6, 91–92, 101, 159, 270, Baker, L., 438
Ajzen, I., 405 Zero Tolerance Task Force, 210 271, 291, 360, 371, 421 Baker, L. A., 136
Akbar, N. J., 393 Ames, A., 178 Arnold, M. L., 132, 308 Baker, S., 371
Akers, C. A., 425 Amialchuk, A., 429 Aronson, J., 388 Baker, T., 211
Akiyama, M., 401 Amiot, C. E., 232 Arseneault, L., 80, 439 Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.,
Aklin, W. M., 427 Ammon, N. Y., 28 Arthur, M., 112, 227, 228 322, 438
Akom, A. A., 400 Analitis, F., 179 Arthur, M. W., 430 Baker-Smith, E. C., 194, 196
Akré, C., 357 Anbar, S., 340 Aruguete, M., 28 Bakes, M., 233
Aksglaede, L., 34 Anda, R. F., 131 Arum, R., 408 Bakken, J., 122, 170
Alabi, B. O., 278 Anderman, E., 193–195, 197, Arunkumar, R., 387, 394 Bakken, N., 196
Alan Guttmacher Institute, 355 203, 379 Asakawa, K., 401 Bakker, M. P., 327, 445

I-1
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-2 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-2 Name Index

Baldelomar, O., 273, 274 Bates, L., 377 Benjet, C., 41 Bingham, C. R., 356–359, 361,
Baldwin, R., 372 Batterink, L., 45 Benn, R., 378 420, 429
Bales, S., 17 Battin-Pearson, S., 406 Benner, A. D., 146, 198, 198f, 207, Birch, L., 282
Balfanz, R., 193 Bauer, D. J., 93, 345 276, 278, 332, 393 Bird, H. R., 418, 427, 440
Balk, S., 378 Bauer, K., 51 Bennett, C., 82 Birkett, M., 367
Ballard, M., 146 Bauer, R., 48 Bensley, L., 378 Birmaher, B., 449
Balle-Jensen, L., 371 Bauermeister, J. A., 228, 371, 372 Benson, B., 360 Birnbaum, A. S., 244
Balsa, A., 175 Bault, N., 78 Benson, M., 142, 450 Biro, F., 22, 25f, 34, 262, 262f, 263
Balsano, A., 110 Bauman, K. E., 158, 166, 167, Benson, P., 136, 232, 235 Bishai, D., 430
Baltes, B., 114 172, 173, 345, 367–368, 429, Bentler, P., 430 Bishop, A., 276
Balu, S., 38 430, 449 Ben-Zur, H., 80 Bishop, D., 66
Bámaca, M. Y., 275, 298 Baumer, E. P., 113, 373, 395 Berchtold, A., 357 Bishop, J., 208, 395, 407
Bámaca-Gómez, M. Y., 298 Baumert, J., 201, 393 Berenbaum, S., 166, 281, 282, 333 Bishop, J. L., 112, 175
Bamford, H., 239 Baumrind, D., 129 Berends, M., 200 Bishop, M., 395
Bandiera, F., 276 Bauserman, R., 369 Berenson, G., 46 Biswas, A., 438
Bandura, A., 13f, 15, 389 Bayer, P., 169 Berenson, K., 332 Bitz, B., 77
Banich, M., 69, 72, 78, 80, 81, 81f, Bayley, N., 41, 75f Berg, C., 327 Bjarnason, T., 420
97, 255, 295f, 295–297, 370 Beach, S., 428, 429f, 441 Berge, J., 47, 51 Bjermer, L., 427
Banister, E., 123 Beal, S. J., 400 Bergman, L. R., 386 Bjertness, E., 418
Bank, L., 135 Beals, J., 358, 427 Berkel, C., 278 Black, M. C., 361, 368, 369, 372
Bankole, A., 360 Beam, C., 138 Berkhof, J., 37 Black, S. J., 131
Bankston, C. L., III, 392 Beam, M. R., 333 Berkowitz, R. I., 48 Blackwell, L. S., 52, 391, 391f
Banny, A. M., 178 Beaman, J., 112 Berlan, E. D., 367 Blades, N. M., 370
Bao, W., 147 Beamer, L., 372 Berliner, D., 193 Blair, J. J., 438–439
Baratta, M. V., 428 Bean, J. A., 263 Berman, E., 197 Blais, J. J., 246
Barbaranelli, C., 389 Bean, R. A., 130, 292 Bernasconi, S., 35 Blake, S. M., 370
Barber, B. K., 7, 91, 130, 132, 140, Beardsall, L., 134 Bernat, D. H., 278–279 Blakemore, S.-J., 59, 65, 66, 71, 76,
142, 149, 164, 197, 200, 249, Bearman, P. S., 363, 370, Berndt, T., 169, 183, 317, 78, 126, 255, 300
292, 300, 332, 391, 410 446, 449 324–326, 346 Blanc, A. K., 370
Barber, B. L., 232, 233 Bearman, S. K., 49, 445, 450 Berninger, V., 201, 202 Blanton, H., 172
Barboza, G. E., 181 Beaulieu, C., 67 Berns, G., 177 Blatt, S., 197, 445
Barch, D. M., 64 Beaumont, S., 329 Bernstein, I. L., 425 Blatt-Eisengart, I., 24, 133
Bardone, A., 421 Beaver, K. M., 132, 136 Beron, K., 181 Blaustein, E., 376
Barg, F. K., 370 Bebeau, M., 303, 305 Berry, S., 242 Bleakley, A., 242
Baril, M. E., 134 Bechara, A., 69 Bersamin, M., 352, 360, 363 Blinka, L., 245, 325
Barker, E. D., 41, 92, 93, 93f, 131, Beck, J., 232 Bertini, N., 420 Blinn-Pike, L., 135, 355, 360
176, 178, 434, 444 Becker, B. E., 174–175, 448, 449 Berzonsky, M., 270, 271 Block, J., 427
Barkley, R., 202 Becker, J., 36, 37 Best, A. L., 286 Blodgett Salafia, E., 132
Barnes, A. R., 141 Becker, K., 425 Best, C., 428 Blokland, A., 131, 437
Barnes, G. M., 232, 332, 429, 430 Becker-Stoll, F., 323 Best, K., 43, 419 Blomfield, C. J., 233
Barnes, R., 203 Beckwith, L., 322 Betancourt, L., 72, 81 Bloom, F., 71
Barnett, T., 145 Becnel, J. N., 233 Bettencourt, A., 171 Blos, P., 14, 290
Barnett, T. A., 46 Beelman, A., 167, 168 Beuhring, T., 52 Blozis, S. A., 146
Barnett, T. E., 228 Beets, M. W., 244, 441 Beurhing, T., 360 Blum, R., 48, 52, 53, 149, 360, 361,
Barnett-Walker, K., 439 Begg, D. J., 53, 429 Beyers, J. M., 113, 437 374, 449
Barnoski, R., 377 Behrmann, M., 71 Beyers, W., 271, 290, 291, 323 Blumenthal, H., 43
Bar-On, Z., 134 Beidas, R., 417 Beyth-Marom, R., 79 Blumler, J., 240
Barr, A., 125 Bejarno, J., 312 Bezerra, M. F., 373–374 Blumstein, A., 433, 437
Barr, S., 48 Belanger, M., 46 Bezilla, R., 310 Blunden, S., 38
Barratt, M., 379 Belansky, E., 332 Bhanot, R., 275 Blyth, D., 43
Barrera, M., Jr., 125 Beldavs, Z. G., 441 Bhapkar, M., 32 Blythe, T., 58
Barreto, R. M., 354 Bell, A., 366 Bianchin, L., 35 Bo, W., 91
Barrett-Connor, E., 427 Bell, D., 114 Bibbins-Domingo, K., 45 Bobek, D., 110
Barrett-Singer, A. T., 137 Bell, J., 59 Biddle, B., 193 Bobkowski, P., 11, 237, 239–242, 248
Barrios, L., 210 Bell, K., 323 Biddlecom, A. E., 360, 371 Boden, J., 43, 265, 432
Barrocas, A. L., 420 Bell, L., 438 Biederman, J., 428 Bodison, P., 276
Barry, C. M., 91, 198, 271, 307, 311 Bell, R., 431 Biehl, M. C., 42, 79–80, 342 Boekaerts, M., 389
Barry, H., 98 Bellair, P., 436–437 Biello, K. B., 374 Boelcke-Stennes, K., 420
Barta, W. D., 356 Belliston, L. M., 133 Biely, E., 241 Boeldt, D., 418
Bartholomae, S., 360 Bellmore, A. D., 28, 175, 177, 178, Biener, L., 421, 429, 430 Boerger, R., 332, 333
Bartko, W. T., 222 179, 274 Bierman, K. L., 176, 182, Boergers, J., 429
Bartkowski, J., 354, 354f Belsky, J., 33, 40, 143, 377, 419 358, 439 Bogaert, A. F., 366, 367
Bartle-Haring, S., 292 Beltran, I., 12, 426–429 Bierut, L. J., 430 Bogenschneider, K., 393
Barton, B., 34 BeLue, R., 46 Biggar, H., 449 Bogin, B., 7, 23, 27, 28f, 29,
Barton, E., 178 Benbenishty, R., 168 Biglan, A., 181 31–33, 350
Bascoe, S., 10, 121, 122, 129, 129f, Benbensihty, R., 209 Bigler, R. S., 412 Bogue, E., 370
131, 276–277 Benbow, C., 387 Bijleveld, C., 437 Bohnert, A. M., 232
Basile, K. C., 368 Benda, B., 363 Bijvank, M., 243 Boislard-Pepin, M.-A., 351
Basinger, K. S., 303 Bender, H., 133 Billick, S. B., 446 Boiter, C., 312
Bassi, M., 389 Bender, K. A., 179 Billings, A., 357 Boivin, M., 179
Bateman, C., 428 Benedict, R., 13f, 16–17, 95, 103 Bills, D., 227 Boldero, J., 332
Bates, J. E., 76, 113, 128, 132, 132f, Benefield, T., 345 Billy, J., 339 Boles, M., 446
136, 143, 178, 235, 256, 346, Benenson, J. F., 324 Bimmel, N., 438 Bolland, J. M., 114
357, 361, 393, 439, 440 Benhorin, S., 114, 312 Bingenheimer, J. B., 110, 114 Bolling, D., 176–177
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-3 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-3

Bomar, J., 292 Braet, C., 129 Brown, J., 11, 128, 232, 237, Burt, S. A., 126, 136, 141, 440
Bona, G., 35 Braga, A., 210 239–242, 245, 248, 369 Burton, E., 50
Bond, L., 207, 444 Brahmbhatt, S. B., 64 Brown, J. D., 242, 357, 357f Burton, L., 275, 286, 376, 429
Bonell, C., 430 Brainerd, C., 82 Brown, J. R., 236, 288, 392 Burwell, R. A., 135
Boney-McCoy, S., 436 Brame, B., 433, 438 Brown, J. S., 280, 442, 444 Bush, K. R., 133, 292
Bong, M., 388 Brame, R., 227, 228 Brown, K., 262, 262f Bushman, B. J., 242, 243, 307, 372
Bongers, I. L., 433 Brammer, M., 69 Brown, L. K., 357, 370 Bushwall, S., 144
Bonica, C., 321, 344 Branch, C., 169 Brown, P. J., 232, 387, 392 Bushway, S. D., 227, 228, 406
Bonneau, K., 168 Brand, S., 147, 232, 264, Brown, R. A., 420 Busseri, M. A., 311–312, 366,
Bontempo, D. E., 367, 428 398, 399 Brown, S. A., 144, 428 367, 420
Bookheimer, S. Y., 63, 65 Branje, S. J. T., 126, 127, 132, Brown, S. D., 308 Bussey, K., 182
Boone, E., 232 170–173, 258, 271–272, 290, Brown, W. T., 400 Bussing, R., 438
Boone, T. L., 360 329, 447 Browning, C. R., 112, 113, 233 Butler, H., 207
Bootcheck, J., 156 Branstetter, S. A., 323 Brubacher, J., 211–212 Butler, K., 425
Booth, A., 37, 142, 143, 281, Brasfield, T., 372 Brückner, H., 363, 370 Buzi, F., 35
282, 282f Bratt, C., 233 Brugman, D., 305 Buzy, W. M., 345
Booth, M. Z., 215 Braun, H., 204 Brumberg, J., 36, 48 Bybee, D., 275, 400
Booth-LaForce, C., 343 Brauner, J., 175 Brunelli, S., 379 Bynner, J., 7, 93, 109
Bor, W., 439 Brausch, A. M., 446 Bruno, Z. V., 373–374 Bynum, M. S., 276, 360
Borch, C., 334 Braverman, P., 113, 360 Brunson, L., 203 Byrd, C., 276
Borelli, J. L., 327 Bray, J., 109, 291, 429 Brush, L. N., 433 Byrne, B., 256
Borge, A. I. H., 176, 178 Brechwald, W. A., 164, 165, 172, Bruvold, W., 431 Byrne, M., 124
Borkowski, J., 191, 377 299, 300 Bryant, A. L., 92, 94f, 110, 172, 429 Byrnes, J., 81, 384
Bornstein, M. H., 128, 136, 420 Bredman, J. C., 295 Bryant, C. M., 46, 345
Born this Way Foundation, 367 Breen, A. V., 272 Bryant, F., 379
Borofsky, L. A., 37, 59, 71, Brendgen, M., 169, 171, 172, 183, Bryant, S. A., 345 C
256, 321 342, 395 Bryn Austin, S., 367 Cadinu, M., 169
Boruch, R., 374 Brener, N. D., 368 Bucci, M., 169 Cadoret, R., 113
Borzekowski, D., 245 Brenhouse, H. C., 424 Buchanan, C. M., 36, 37, 128, 132, Cai, L., 172, 429
Bos, H., 143, 366, 367 Brennan, P. A., 439, 445 133, 140, 141, 143, 145, 197 Cai, T., 136, 438
Bose, K., 132 Brennan, R. T., 44 Buchanan, L., 173 Cairney, J., 444
Bosker, R., 206 Brent, D., 449 Buchanan, M., 375 Cairns, B. D., 112, 173, 175
Bosmans, G., 129 Brenzel, B., 193 Buchanan, R., 276, 278 Cairns, R., 53, 173, 232
Bossarte, R. M., 446 Breslau, N., 39 Buchmann, C., 168, 183, 396 Cajochen, C., 38
Botticello, A. L., 429 Brex, R. A., 427 Bucholz, K. K., 357 Caldas, S., 392
Botvin, G. J., 265, 426–428 Briddell, L., 224, 309, 309f, 411 Budde, J. P., 136 Caldwell, C. H., 44, 228, 264,
Botvin, M., 172 Bridges, G., 436 Budge, S., 114 276–279, 277f, 310–312,
Bouchey, H. A., 137, 321 Bridges, M., 141, 143 Buechel, F., 394 311f, 355, 371, 418
Bouffard, S., 130 Briggs, E., 203, 333 Buehler, C., 131, 142, 265 Caldwell, K., 198
Boufous, S., 79 Briggs, M. L., 135 Buhi, E. R., 356–358 Caldwell, L. L., 235, 334
Boulerice, B., 406, 439 Bright, D., 39 Buhrmester, D., 134, 183, 318, 324, Caldwell, M. S., 322
Bountress, K., 427 Brinch, C., 75, 215 327–330, 329f, 332, 334, 335, Calkins, S. D., 135, 438
Bourdony, C., 32 Brindis, C. D., 113 338, 340, 346 Call, K. T., 53
Boutelle, K., 46 Briskman, J., 433 Buka, S. L., 53, 115, 429 Callahan, R., 200
Bowen, F., 179 Brisson, D., 179 Bukowski, W. M., 169, 171, 172, 179, Callan, V., 330
Bowen, G. L., 112, 393 Brodsky, N., 72, 81 183, 317, 332–334, 340, 342 Callands, T. A., 360
Bowen, N. K., 112 Brody, G. H., 42–44, 113, 115, Bulcroft, K., 301 Calvert, C., 58
Bower, J., 438 130, 131, 134, 146, 147, 276, Bulcroft, R., 301 Calvert, S., 248
Bowers, E. P., 235 278, 360, 360f, 362, 399, 428, Bull, C., 264 Calvete, E., 445
Bowker, A., 173 429f, 441 Bullock, B., 132, 135, 171 Camburn, E., 407
Bowker, J. C., 166, 325 Broh, B. A., 233 Bumpass, L., 376 Cameron, J., 71
Bowlby, J., 321 Broidy, L., 433, 438 Bumpus, M. F., 128, 301 Cameron, R. P., 49
Bowlds, M., 331 Bromnick, R., 59 Bunge, S., 63 Camou, S., 193
Bowles, T. V., 329, 332 Bronfenbrenner, U., 9, 10 Burchinal, M., 75, 178, 399 Campa, M. I., 377
Boxer, A., 42 Bronik, M., 63 Burdette, A., 46 Campbell, B., 24, 49
Boxer, P., 142, 409, 438 Bronstein, P., 199, 393 Bureau of Labor Statistics, 213, Campbell, B. C., 359
Boyce, T., 53 Brook, J. S., 243, 243f, 420, 445 213f, 223f, 229 Campbell, F., 75, 398
Boyce, W., 33, 112 Brookmeyer, K. A., 114, 175, Burg, S., 309 Campbell, H. S., 429
Boyd, C. J., 367, 368f, 419 307, 312 Burger, K., 45 Campbell, I., 66
Boyd, D., 308 Brooks-Gunn, J., 24, 35, 37, 40–44, Burgess, G., 69 Campbell, M. A., 433
Boyd-Zaharias, J., 195 48–50, 110–115, 111f, 141, 232, Burk, W. J., 132, 172 Campbell, M. E., 167, 168
Boyer, T. W., 81 233, 339, 351, 353, 361–363, Burkam, D., 144, 407 Campbell, S., 440
Boyle, M. H., 111, 426 375, 376, 378, 398, 447 Burk-Braxton, C., 262–264, 275 Campione-Barr, N., 122, 126,
Bozick, R., 308 Brown, B., 12, 122, 152–154, Burke, J. D., 132, 418, 434 127, 134
Bozzola, M., 35 156–160, 162–165, 164f, 167, Burks, V. S., 346 Campton, W., 420
Braciszewski, J., 114 169–174, 183, 245, 273, 275, Burman, B., 142 Canino, G., 427, 440
Brack, C., 419 279, 317, 325, 330, 332, 336, Burnett, S., 59, 71, 76, 78, 255 Cannon, M., 107
Brack, G., 419 341, 343–345, 377, 378, 394, Burnette, M., 436 Cansler, E., 233
Bradford, K., 142 396, 400 Burns, J., 170 Cantillon, D., 115
Bradley, R. H., 46, 47f, 113, 244 Brown, C., 441 Burraston, B., 135 Cantwell, D., 444
Bradshaw, C. P., 181, 182, Brown, C. S., 278, 367 Burrell, G., 146, 420 Canzoniero, A., 265
182f, 420 Brown, E. C., 430 Burstein, M., 417, 417f, 442 Capaldi, D. M., 92, 129, 171, 345,
Brady, S., 355 Brown, F., 401 Burt, C., 112, 113 359, 368, 374, 428, 440, 444
Braeges, J., 141 Brown, I., 445 Burt, K. B., 93, 420 Caplan, M., 182
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-4 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-4 Name Index

Caplan, N., 401 Centers for Disease Control and Chin, C. S., 120 Coatsworth, J. D., 101, 236, 250, 288
Cappelleri, J., 368 Prevention, 78–79, 353, 353f, Chiodo, D., 367 Cobb, B. K., 361
Capra, C., 177 355–356, 356f, 369, 423, 430, Chipperfield, J. G., 450 Cobbs, G., 257
Caprara, G. V., 306, 345, 389 433, 442, 445 Chisholm, L., 105 Coca, V., 397
Carbonaro, W., 200, 204 Cepeda, A., 170 Choi, W. S., 39, 321, 321f Cochran, B. N., 367
Card, N. A., 173, 175, 177, Cerda, M., 115 Choi, Y., 125, 428, 440 Cockell, S. J., 420
178, 367 Cervantes, R., 275 Chong, W. H., 387 Cogburn, C., 278
Cardeñoso, O., 445 Chacko, M. R., 338 Choo, H., 238 Cohen, A., 65, 69, 70f
Carels, R., 203 Chadwick, M., 68 Chopak, J., 359, 361 Cohen, D., 234, 358
Carey, M. P., 370, 372 Chalmers, H., 366, 367, 420 Chou, C., 46 Cohen, G., 362
Carlin, J. B., 207, 444 Chaloupka, F. J., 83, 424, 430 Choudhury, S., 76 Cohen, G. L., 175, 299
Carlo, G., 146, 306, 307, 331 Chamberlain, P., 148 Chowdhury, A., 131 Cohen, M., 286, 287
Carlsmith, J., 144 Champion, J. E., 450 Choy, M., 401 Cohen, P., 243, 243f, 332, 369,
Carlson, C., 263 Chan, A., 173 Christakos, A., 324 420, 445
Carlson, E., 198, 440 Chan, C. S., 110 Christakou, A., 69 Cohen, P. K., 102–103
Carlson, W., 327, 339 Chan, D., 448 Christensen, K. J., 306, 306f Cohen, R., 178, 397, 411
Carlson Jones, D., 28 Chandler, M., 60 Christensen, M., 147 Cohen, S., 53, 322
Carmel, S., 353 Chandra, A., 242 Christiansen, B., 428 Coie, J. D., 76, 176, 178, 183, 374,
Carmody, D., 301 Chang, E. S., 333, 400, 412 Christopher, F. S., 43 377, 439
Carnagey, N. L., 242 Chang, V. W., 46 Christopherson, C., 358 Coiro, M., 143
Carolan, B. V., 194 Chao, R. K., 125, 133, 273, 275, Christou, C., 63 Coker, T., 276
Caronongan, P., 130 276, 279 Chrousos, G. P., 31, 421 Colaco, B., 46
Carpenter, C., 47 Chao, W., 170 Chu, B. C., 448 Colbert, K., 379
Carpenter, J., 431 Chapman, D. P., 131 Chua, A., 133 Colditz, G., 53
Carr, A., 312 Charbonneau, A. M., 445 Chuang, S., 121, 133 Cole, A., 134
Carr, P. J., 114 Charlton, K., 215 Chumlea, W., 32 Cole, D. A., 257, 260, 265, 447
Carrano, J., 233 Charman, T., 76 Chun, H., 402 Cole, H., 322
Carroll, A., 438 Charmaraman, L., 273 Chun, Y.-J., 141, 301 Cole-Lewis, H., 374
Carroll, J. S., 91, 316, 316f Charnigo, R., 433 Chung, C. Y., 201–202 Coleman, J., 15–16, 156, 204, 395
Carroll, M. D., 45 Chase-Lansdale, P. L., 53, 131, 143, Chung, G. H., 126 Coleman, L. M., 356
Carskadon, M., 37–39 145, 146, 375, 377–379 Chung, H. L., 113, 113f, 115, 439 Coleman, P. K., 373
Carson, D., 131 Chassin, L., 12, 39–40, 172, 173, Chung, I., 131 Coles, M. S., 374
Carstensen, L., 123 426–429, 436, 437 Chung, J., 439 Coley, R. L., 128, 131, 132, 143, 146,
Carter, C. S., 64 Chauhan, P., 110, 436 Chung, K., 450 235, 377, 379
Carter, J., 50 Chaves, T. V., 429 Church, J. A., 65, 69, 70f Coll, C. G., 279
Carter, M., 379 Chavira, V., 275 Church, M., 397, 411 Collaer, M., 25, 26
Carter, R., 44 Chavous, T. M., 264, 276, 278–279 Church, R., 189, 190, 211 College Board, 402, 402f
Carter, S., 108 Cheadle, J., 141 Chyi-In, W., 146 Collier, K. L., 367
Carvajal, S. C., 276 Chein, J., 81–82, 299, 299f Ciairano, S., 172 Collins, A. L., 425
Carver, K., 197, 204, 395 Chen, C. S., 126, 131, 173, 333, 392, Cicchetti, D., 420, 447, 449 Collins, J. L., 369–370, 372
Cascardi, M., 345 398, 400–402, 412, 447, 448 Ciesla, J. A., 448 Collins, M. H., 447
Casella, R., 210 Chen, C. Y., 312 Cillessen, A. H. N., 174–176, 334 Collins, N. L., 321, 323
Casey, B. J., 8, 67–70, 286, 287 Chen, E., 114 Cinamon, R. G., 412 Collins, R. L., 242, 426
Cash, T. F., 50 Chen, H., 50, 79, 102–103, 292, Claes, M., 330 Collins, W. A., 12, 40, 120, 121,
Cashman, S. Y., 149 301, 369 Clampet-Lundquist, S., 111 126–128, 130, 133, 136, 183,
Casper, D., 173 Chen, K., 426, 427 Clancy, S., 270 198, 256, 286–292, 297,
Caspi, A., 44, 72, 75, 113, 136, Chen, L., 167, 328, 329 Clardy, C., 309–312, 310f 299–302, 323, 323f, 336, 341,
141, 258, 265, 377, 419, 421, Chen, M.-J., 429 Clark, J., 140 342, 344, 345, 358, 362
438–440, 447 Chen, P., 426, 438 Clark, J. J., 425 Colman, S., 374
Cassidy, J., 265, 322 Chen, R., 209, 419 Clark, M. A., 429 Colón, Y., 110
Castaneda-English, P., 275 Chen, R.-M., 34 Clark, R., 130, 393 Colten, M., 419, 447
Casteel, M., 63 Chen, S.-K., 34 Clark, S., 359, 368 Coltrane, S., 292
Castellino, D. R., 165, 393, 400 Chen, W-I., 114 Clarke, A. T., 450 Comacho, T., 372
Casten, R., 387, 400 Chen, X., 205, 367, 401, 440 Clarke, G., 449 Comings, D. E., 362
Castillo, M., 50 Chen, Y., 131, 273, 278, 428, 429f, Clarke-Pearson, K., 246 Compas, B. E., 146, 449–450
Castro, F. G., 127, 426 441, 444 Clark-Kauffman, E., 146–147 Compian, L., 40, 334, 342
Catalano, R. F., 106, 107f, 112, 406, Chen, Y.-F., 114, 360, 360f, 362 Clasen, D., 164 Complete College, 403
428–430, 440 Chen, Z., 344 Claus, E., 69, 80, 81, 295 Compton, K., 437
Catanese, D., 48 Cheng, M. M., 339, 353 Clauson, J., 199, 393 Compton, S. N., 449
Cater, S. M., 356 Cheng, Y.-H. A., 359 Clay, W., 400 Conant, J., 191
Catsambis, S., 201 Chen-Mok, M., 373–374 Clemans, K. H., 43, 181 Conchas, G. Q., 402
Catterall, J., 393 Cheong, P., 240 Clements, M., 447 Conduct Problems Prevention
Cauce, A., 171, 367, 394, 396, 397 Cherlin, A. J., 113, 143, 146 Clements, P., 300 Research Group, 182–183, 433,
Cauffman, E., 24, 33, 50, 51, 72, Chermack, S. T., 429 Cleveland, H., 113, 172 438, 438f, 441
78–81, 81f, 96, 97, 97f, 133, Cherney, I. D., 78 Cleveland, H. H., 343, 345 Conger, K. J., 134, 135, 142, 146,
169, 172, 210, 227, 228, 255, Chesir-Teran, D., 264 Cleveland, K., 132 146f, 399
291, 295f, 295–298, 296f, Chesney, M., 53 Cleveland, M. J., 113, 130, 428 Conger, R. D., 42–44, 112, 128, 130,
300, 334, 343, 346, 370, 433, Chessor, D., 201 Clingempeel, W., 145 134, 135, 142, 145, 146, 146f,
435–440 Cheung, C., 321, 321f Clinkinbeard, S. S., 39 170, 197, 345, 357, 361, 362,
Cavanagh, S. E., 141, 145, 339, 395 Cheung, L., 53 Clinton, B., 352 393, 399, 419, 437, 444
Cavanagh, T. M., 399 Chew, K., 374 Clore, G., 343, 345 Conklin, H. M., 63, 65, 68
Cavazos-Rehg, P. A., 357 Chilcoat, H. D., 227, 228 Cnaan, R. A., 312 Conley, A., 397
Ceballo, R., 398 Childs, K. K., 179, 420 Cnattingius, S., 377 Conley, C. S., 447, 448
Ceci, S., 75, 397 Child Trends, 193 Coatsworth, D., 132 Connell, A. M., 131, 438
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-5 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-5

Connell, C. M., 427 Craven, R., 201, 262 Cutrona, C., 42, 43, 113 Dearing, E., 130
Connell, F. A., 416 Crawford, I., 429 Cvijanovich, N. Z., 52 Deater-Deckard, K., 129, 136, 438
Connell, J., 402 Crawford, P., 34, 262, 262f Cyranowski, J., 445 De Bellis, M. D., 65, 71
Conner, B. T., 48 Crawford, T. N., 445 DeBerry, K., 275
Connolly, J. A., 12, 160, 168, 181, Crawley, S., 417 de Boer, H., 206
181f, 246, 261, 289, 324, Crean, H. F., 131 D de Bruin, W. B., 79
327–329, 334–346, 339f, Creasey, G., 332 de Bruyn, E., 175, 366, 367
Daddis, C., 121, 122, 131, 301
358, 367 Cribbie, R., 358 de Castro, B. O., 307
Daha, M., 276
Connor-Smith, J. K., 448 Crick, N., 177, 178, 324 Decety, J., 71
Dahinten, V. S., 33
Conrad, J., 176, 178 Criss, M. M., 123, 128, 132, Deck, A., 143
Dahl, A., 278
Conroy, D. E., 236, 288 135, 440 DeClemente, R., 370
Dahl, R. E., 5, 13, 25f, 26, 34, 37, 64,
Conseur, A., 377 Crissey, S. R., 339 Dee, T., 52
69–71, 80, 449
Conway, K., 445 Crnic, K., 276 Deen, B., 176–177
Dakof, G. A., 430, 431
Cook, E., 142 Crocetti, E., 272 DeFries, J., 141
Dallago, L., 112
Cook, L. J., 52 Crocker, J., 263 De Fruyt, F., 258
Dallal, G. E., 32
Cook, P. J., 436 Crockett, L. J., 82, 128, 129, 307, DeGarmo, D. S., 145, 276, 437,
Dalton, A., 39
Cook, T., 115, 147, 169, 397, 411 356–359, 361, 400 438, 441
Dalton, B., 183, 396
Cook, W. L., 132 Croft, J., 39, 39f De Geus, E., 438
Dalton, M., 300
Cooksey, E. C., 161 Croft, T. N., 370 Degnan, K., 129
Daluga, N., 243
Cookston, J. T., 114, 312 Croll, J., 49 De Goede, I. H. A., 126, 329
Daly, B. P., 48
Cooley-Quille, M., 436 Crone, E. A., 26, 59, 63, 64, 69, 70, de Graaf, H., 353, 359
Daly, M., 436
Cooper, A., 34 72, 73, 79, 295, 326, 326f DeHart, G., 33
Damhorst, M. L., 48
Cooper, C. R., 330, 392 Croninger, R. G., 194, 214, 367, Deike, R., 261
Damsgaard, M., 180, 180f
Cooper, H., 110, 215, 390 399, 407 Deke, J., 430
Dandreaux, D. M., 437
Cooper, M. L., 323, 419 Cronquist, J. L., 48 Daniel, D. B., 57 Deković, M., 293–294, 305,440
Cooper, S. R., 53, 398 Crooks, C., 367 Daniel, J. H., 310, 312 de la Haye, K., 172
Copeland, L., 261 Crosby, A. E., 149 Daniels, D., 136, 137 de la Rosa, M., 276
Copeland-Linder, N., 114, 264, Crosby, L., 265, 374 Daniels, E., 281 DeLay, D., 120–122, 125–127
277, 278 Crosby, R. A., 361, 372 Danoff-Burg, S., 115 Delgado, M. Y., 278
Coplan, J., 437 Crosnoe, R., 46, 141, 145, 147, 169, Dapretto, M., 37, 59, 71, 256, 321 Del Giudice, M., 24
Corbett, C., 411f 171, 173, 200, 203, 298, 300, D’Arcy, H., 374–375 Del Guidice, M., 419
Corbin, W. R., 106 394, 399, 406 Darity, W., 165, 400 DeLisi, M., 136
Cordova, D., 275 Crouter, A. C., 37, 124, 127, Darling, N., 77, 121, 132, 209, 235, Dell, R., 372
Coricelli, G., 78 128, 134, 134f, 135, 137, 250, 290, 330, 334, 393, 396 Delle Fave, A., 389
Corley, R. C., 418 167, 196, 209, 231, 233, Darling-Hammond, L., 191, 193, Delsing, M. J. M. H., 159, 329
Corley, R. P., 419 235, 275, 281, 282, 282f, 199, 200 Delva, J., 50, 426
Corliss, H. L., 365, 367 301, 329 Darnell, A., 207, 207f, 393 DelVecchio, W. F., 258
Corneal, D., 282 Crow, S., 49–50 Darroch, J. E., 312, 354, 355, 370, DeMaris, A., 144
Cornell, D., 195, 206 Crowder, K., 374 371, 373 DeMaster, D., 63
Cornwell, G., 361 Crowe, P., 334 Darwin, C., 13 Dembo, M. H., 400
Corona, R., 360 Crowell, J., 322 Da Silva, M., 307 de Meer, G., 112
Corrado, R. R., 433 Crowley, M., 176–177 Datta, S., 371 Demetriou, A., 63
Cortina, K., 11 Crozier, J., 439 Dauber, S., 199–200 Demo, D., 140, 260
Corwyn, R. F., 113, 363 Cruise, K., 433 Daubman, K. A., 335 DeMoya, D., 374
Cosgrove, V. E., 418 Crump, A., 179, 181 D’Augelli, A. R., 367 Denault, A.-S., 161, 334
Costa, F. M., 45, 47, 419, 429 Crusto, C., 53 Davey, C. G., 442 Deng, S., 276, 278
Costa, M., 289, 329 Cruz, I., 52 David Hawkins, J., 336, 337f Deng, Y., 169
Costa, P. T., Jr., 258 Crystal, D., 77, 401 Davidson, A., 203 Denissen, J. J. A., 132, 258
Costanzo, P. R., 175 Csikszentmihalyi, M., 36, 184, 231, Davidson, P., 372 Dennis, M., 69
Costello, E. J., 418, 444, 445, 448 250, 401 Davies, M., 312, 418, 428 Dennison, R. P., 101, 144
Costin, S., 328 Cubbin, C., 113 Davies, P., 10, 121, 122, 129, 129f, Denton, K., 326
Côté, J. E., 6, 7, 93, 255, 260, 261, Çuhadaroğ lu Çetin, F., 323 131, 141, 142, 276–277, 343 Denton, M., 312
267, 268, 270–272 Cui, L., 417, 417f, 442 Davies, P. L., 66 Deptula, D. P., 359, 362
Cottier, L., 420 Cui, M., 142, 345 Davies, S., 312, 370, 372 DeRose, L. M., 44
Cottrell, L., 170, 323 Cullen, F., 226 Davila, J., 143, 327, 344 Deschenes, E., 170
Council on Communications and Cumberland, A., 306 Davis, A., 378 DeSecottier, L., 333
Media, 246 Cummings, E., 146 Davis, B., 47, 265 de Silveira, C., 271
Couperus, J. W., 67 Cummings, J. R., 431 Davis, E., 361 Desjardins, T. L., 178
Courtney, M., 178 Cumsille, P., 77, 121, 132, 137, Davis, K., 128, 275, 323 D’Esposito, M., 63
Covay, E., 204 250, 290 Davis, K. A., 312 Detmar, S., 179
Cox, M., 10 Cunning, D., 245 Davis, K. C., 243 Devine, D., 142
Cox, S. J., 445 Cunningham, M., 282 Davis, L. E., 389, 405 de Visser, R., 358
Coxson, P., 45 Cunningham, R. M., 418 Davis, M., 176 deVries, M. W., 37
Coy, K., 40 Cunningham-Williams, R., 420 Davis, W., 59, 79 de Winter, A., 112
Coyl, D. D., 40 Cupp, P. K., 379 Davis-Kean, P., 256, 300, 389 de Zwaan, M., 49, 50
Coyle, K. K., 113, 343 Curran, J., 372 Davison, T., 48 Dhariwal, A., 345
Coyne, S., 242 Curran, P. J., 172, 173, 420 Dawes, N. P., 236 Diaczok, D., 23
Craig, A., 33 Currie, C., 180, 180f Day, D. M., 292 Diamant, A. L., 46
Craig, I., 72, 447 Curtin, L., 45 Day, L., 135 Diamond, L., 12, 167, 334,
Craig, W. M., 181, 181f, 246, Curtis, T., 361 Day, R., 146, 242 341, 350–356, 358, 359,
289, 329, 334, 339f, 339–341, Curtner-Smith, M., 171 Değ irmencioğ lu, S., 160, 172, 363–368
343, 367 Cusick, P. A., 160 173, 334 Dias, J. J., 46
Crane, D. R., 125, 130 Cuthbertson, C., 102 Dean, J. M., 52 Diaz, D., 369, 373
Crapanzano, A., 179 Cutler, G. B., Jr., 31 Deardorff, J., 33, 43, 125, 369 Diaz, E. M., 367
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-6 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-6 Name Index

Diaz, R. M., 367 Downey, G., 179, 321, 344 East, P., 134–135, 137, 332, 362, 374, Emmett, P., 34
Diaz, T., 265 Dowshen, S., 34 376, 378 Emslie, G., 449
Dick, D., 42, 43, 44f, 136, 137f Doyle, A. B., 142, 323, 342 Eaton, D., 39, 39f Eng, A. L., 360
Dick, D. M., 448 Doyle, A. C., 245 Eaton, M. J., 400 Engels, R. C. M. E., 159, 172, 238,
Dick, R., 427 Dozier, A., 374 Eberhart, N. K., 445, 447 246, 291, 322, 341
Dickson, G., 402 Drabick, D., 416 Eberly, M., 292 Engles, R., 291
Dickson, N., 421 Drapela, L. A., 42 Ebin, V. J., 109, 109f Englund, M. M., 420
DiClemente, R. J., 243, 312, 361, Draper, P., 419 Eccles, J. S., 7, 36, 37, 91, 115, 126, Ennett, S. T., 112, 158, 166, 167, 172,
369, 370, 372 Dregan, A., 39 126f, 133, 137, 145–147, 164, 173, 177, 332, 345, 429, 430
Diehl, L., 261 Driscoll, A., 354, 444 189, 196–200, 205–207, Enright, R., 18, 88
Dielman, T., 261, 429–431 Drumm, P., 64 214–215, 222, 229, 232, 233, Enriquez, L. E., 395, 407
Dietsch, B., 429 Druss, B. G., 431 249, 260, 262, 263, 281, 300, Ensminger, M. E., 112, 113, 199,
DiFranza, J., 428 Dryfoos, J., 191 318, 332, 388–393, 395, 400, 228, 427
DiIorio, C., 360, 362 Du, M.-L., 34 410, 427 Entwisle, B., 167
Dijkstra, J. K., 10, 152, 157, 158, 161, Du, S., 245 Eckenrode, J. J., 245, 368, 377 Entwisle, D. R., 199–200, 203, 228,
175, 176, 317 Duan, Q., 344 Edelbrock, C., 418 405, 407
Dilks, R. J., 48 Dubas, J. S., 41, 131, 132 Edelstein, W., 258, 326 Epkins, C., 312
Dinh, K. T., 276 Dubé, E. M., 167, 334, 341, 366 Edenberg, H. J., 357 Epstein, J. A., 428
Dinkes, R., 209 Dube, S. R., 131 Edens, J. F., 433 Epstein, L., 45
Dintcheff, B. A., 429 Dubner, S., 191 Eder, D., 175 Epstein, R., 15
Dion, G. S., 403 Dubocovich, M. L., 38 Edin, K., 111 Erath, S. A., 176, 357
DiPietro, S., 195 DuBois, D., 110, 147, 260–265, 275, Edman, J., 28 Erel, O., 142
Dishion, T., 123, 128, 131, 132, 135, 328, 398, 399 Edmonston, B., 377, 378 Erhart, M., 179
169–173, 206, 265, 275, 362, Dubow, E. F., 409, 438 Edwards, A. C., 448 Erickson, K., 149, 171, 298
395, 419, 428, 438, 440 Ducat, W., 12, 286–290, 351 Edwards, J. N., 134 Erickson, L., 7, 91
Dittus, P. J., 360, 374, 379 Duchesne, S., 393 Edwards, K., 212 Erickson, P., 354
Dixon, L., 48 Duckett, E., 122, 123f Edwards, T. C., 416 Eriksen, M., 427
Dmitrieva, J., 131 Duckworth, A. L., 386 Egan, E., 112, 227, 228 Erikson, E., 13f, 14, 265–271, 319
Dobkin, P., 129, 172 Dudley, W. N., 362 Egan, S., 265, 283 Erkanli, A., 448
Dockray, S., 39 Due, P., 180, 180f Egeland, B., 322, 420, 440, 441 Erkut, S., 262
Dodge, K. A., 76, 113, 128, 132, 132f, Due-Tonnessen, P., 67, 68f Ehrenreich, S., 238 Ernst, M., 69, 101, 424
143, 178, 235, 346, 357, 361, Dufur, M., 143 Ehringer, M. A., 418 Erol, R. Y., 259, 259f, 263–264
393, 438f, 438–440 Duhoux, S., 286, 287 Eichen, D. M., 48 Erwin, E. H., 171, 193
Dodge, T., 172, 360, 374 Dukes, R., 170, 273 Eijnden, R. J. J. M., 246 Esbensen, F., 169, 170
Dogan, R. R., 393 Duku, E., 426 Einspruch, E., 430 Escalante, K. A., 144
Dogan, S. J., 357, 437 Dulin, A., 181 Eisenberg, M. E., 48–51, 262, 362 Esparza, P., 110
Doi, Y., 39 Dumas, T. M., 271 Eisenberg, N., 12, 78, 132, 288, Espelage, D. L., 169, 172, 215,
Dolezal, S., 265 Dumka, L. E., 125, 409 302–307, 311, 312, 328 367, 368
Dollahite, D., 311–312 Dumont, M., 450 Eisenberger, N. I., 37, 71, 321 Espenshade, T. J., 201–202
Dollinger, S., 270 Dumontheil, I., 76, 300 Eitel, P., 179, 181 Espinosa, G., 325, 325f
Dolliver, M., 248 Duncan, G., 111, 112, Elder, G. H., Jr., 16, 44, 100, 128, Espinoza, G., 11, 181
Domina, T., 397 146–147, 394 145–147, 146f, 170, 197, 203, Esposito, L. E., 420
Dominguez, A., 146 Duncan, G. J., 38f 231, 280, 311, 345, 442, 444 Esser, G., 425
Donahue, K. L., 143 Duncan, P., 199, 393 Elders, M., 427 Essex, M., 33
Donahue, P. L., 403 Dunger, D., 34 Eley, T., 438, 447 Estell, D. B., 112, 175
Donelly, W. O., 142 Dunifon, R., 142 Elkind, D., 58, 59 Estrada, A., 329
Donenberg, G. R., 360 Dunlap, K. G., 134 Elkington, K. S., 372 Estrada, S., 79–80
Dong, M., 131 Dunlop, S. M., 419 Elkins, I., 126, 136 Ethier, K., 376
Dong, Q., 173, 419, 447, 448 Dunn, J., 134, 137 Ellen, J. M., 372 Eubanks, J., 242
Donnellan, M., 259, 265, 345 Dunn, M., 428 Ellickson, P. L., 425, 426, 431 Eurostat, 106, 106f
Donnellan, M. B., 142 Dunn, S., 331 Elling, K., 401 Evans, A. D., 76
Donnelly, T. M., 307, 308 Dunsmore, S. C., 356, 357 Elliot, A. J., 388 Evans, E., 147, 389, 398, 399
Donnerstein, E., 238–243 Duong, H. T., 39 Elliot, D., 113, 228 Evans, M. A., 245
Dupéré, V., 110, 112, 113, Elliott, D., 420 Evans, M. K., 39
D’Onofrio, B. M., 132, 141,
362, 363 Elliott, M. N., 46, 242, 276, 360 Evans, W., 52
143, 361
Dupree, D., 130 Ellis, B., 33, 361, 419 Eveleth, P., 22, 33, 34f
Dooley, D., 400, 412
Duran, R., 345 Ellis, C., 73 Everett, S. A., 369
Dorham, C. L., 275
DuRant, R., 172, 421, 429 Ellis, N., 210 Eyal, E., 241
Dormitzer, C. M., 312
DuRant, R. H., 310, 312 Ellis-Hale, K., 308 Eyre, S. L., 338, 364
Dorn, L. D., 22, 24–26, 25f, 31–34,
Durbin, D., 292 Ellwanger, P., 292
36, 39, 421
Durbin, M., 372
Dornbusch, S. M., 140, 141, Elmen, J., 391 F
Duriez, B., 132
143, 144, 149, 171, 183, 209, Elmore, R., 188, 189, 191, 196, 410
Durik, A. M., 390 Fagan, J., 436, 437, 439, 441
227, 279, 298, 300, 393, 394, Elo, I., 374
Durkin, K., 438 Fagot, B., 374
396, 400 Elonheimo, H., 438
Durston, S., 67, 68 Fair, D. A., 65, 69, 70f, 438
Dorsey, S., 360 El-Sheikh, M., 146
Dweck, C. S., 388, 390–391, 391f Fairbank, J., 107
Dosenbach, N., 65, 69, 70f Elwert, F., 112
Dworkin, J., 157 Faircloth, B., 272
DosReis, S., 446 Emans, S., 421
Dwyer, D. S., 429 Faison, N., 278
Dotterer, A., 196, 205, 209, 233, 275, Emanuel, I., 377
282, 282f Emerson, E., 360 Falissard, B., 39
Douaud, G., 68 E Emerson, M., 373–374 Falk, R., 60
Douglas, G., 389 Emery, C. R., 377 Fallon, B. J., 329, 332
Dowdy, B. B., 334, 340 Eamon, M. K., 393 Emery, R. E., 42, 138, 141, 143, Fan, S., 338, 358
Downes, B., 48 Earls, F., 44, 53, 112 358, 429 Fan, X., 147, 206, 358
Downey, D., 143, 207, 400 Early, D., 332 Emery, S., 424 Fang, F., 326
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-7 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-7

Fang, G., 326 Field, T., 326 Ford, C. A., 353, 355 Fried, M., 99
Fang, J., 292 Fields, J., 138 Ford, D., 402 Fried, M., 99
Fang, S., 399 Fiese, B., 47 Ford, J. A., 423, 428 Fried, R., 428
Fang, X., 131, 372 Figner, B., 81 Ford, K., 275, 278 Friedenberg, E., 15, 215
Fanti, K., 175 Figueredo, A., 419 Ford, T., 433 Friedlander, L. J., 339
Farah, M., 81 Filardo, E., 166 Fordham, C., 165, 400 Friedman, K., 241
Farb, A., 231, 232 Finch, M., 301 Forehand, R., 115, 142, 360, 449 Friedman, N. P., 419
Farhat, T., 354, 357, 358 Fincham, F., 142, 143 Forgatch, M., 145, 438, 441 Friedman, R., 388, 394, 396, 397
Faris, R., 172, 429, 430 Fine, M., 145, 395, 396 Forhan, S., 371 Friedman, S., 33
Farkas, G., 393, 395, 397, 400 Finer, L. B., 370, 373 Forman, T. A., 310–312, 311f Friel, L. V., 361
Farley, F., 79, 80 Fingerson, L., 360 Forman, Y., 235 Friend, S., 51
Farley, T., 234, 358 Finkelhor, D., 181, 246, 247, 436 Formichella, C. M., 114 Frijns, T., 126, 170–171,
Farmer, T. W., 112, 175, 196 Finkelstein, B. D., 326 Formoso, D., 125 290, 291
Farrell, A. D., 114, 171, 177, 193, Finkelstein, J., 445 Forrest-Bank, S., 179 Frisch, R., 33
300, 420 Finken, L., 298 Forrester, K. K., 181 Frisco, M. L., 141, 359, 377
Farrell, M. P., 232, 429, 430 Finkenauer, C., 126, 291 Fortenberry, J., 352 Fritjers, J., 312
Farrelly, M. C., 243 Finn, A., 63 Fortner, M., 124 Frohlich, C., 307
Farrington, D. P., 12, 172, 298, 417, Finn, J., 195 Fortson, E. N., 113 Fröjd, S., 42
419, 431, 433–441 Finnerty, K., 241 Fosco, G. M., 142 Fromme, K., 106
Farruggia, S. P., 333, 334, 346, 435 Fischer, S., 444 Foshee, V. A., 112, 172, 177, 332, Frost, J. J., 370
Fasick, F., 88–90 Fischhoff, B., 59, 79 345, 367–368, 429, 430 Fry, A., 63
Fasula, A. M., 362 Fishbein, M., 242 Foss, R. D., 83 Fry, M. D., 235
Fatusi, A. O., 372 Fisher, C., 400 Foster, D. L., 24, 25 Fugelsang, J., 82
Fauber, R. L., 48 Fisher, D., 352, 360, 363 Foster, E., 24, 70, 177 Fuhrman, T., 294
Fauth, R. C., 111, 111f, 232 Fisher, L., 142 Foster, H., 44, 345 Fujimoto, K., 46
Fay, K., 233 Fisher, M., 48 Foster, T., 326 Fukuda, S., 63
Fedele, D., 417 Fisher, P., 148 Foster, W., 110 Fuligni, A. J., 39, 59, 107, 108,
Feder, M., 364 Fite, P. J., 440 Fournier, M., 272 125–127, 169, 200, 211, 256,
Federal Bureau of Investigation, Fitzgerald, H., 377, 428 Fowler, F., 133 264, 273–276, 278, 290, 300,
433, 434f, 435 Fitzpatrick, K. M., 181 Fowler, M. G., 372 310, 330, 331f, 395, 399–401
Federal Interagency Forum Fjell, A., 67, 68f Fowler, P., 114, 148, 312 Fulker, D., 129
on Child and Family Flach, C., 433 Fox, N., 101 Fulkerson, J. A., 48, 49
Statistics, 109f Flaherty, B. P., 77, 121, 221, 227 Frabutt, J. M., 275 Fulmore, C., 387, 400
Feeney, B. C., 265, 321 Flaherty, L., 449 Frala, J. L., 43 Fung, D., 238
Fegley, S., 302, 306 Flammer, A., 222 Francis, D., 427 Furbey, M., 79
Feigelman, S., 372, 429 Flanagan, C., 197, 210, 224, Francis, L. A., 46 Furman, W., 134, 160, 182, 317, 321,
Feinberg, I., 66 307–309, 309f, 411 Francis, S. E., 450 323, 324, 330, 332, 334–338,
Feinberg, M. E., 113, 134, 135, Flanagan, K. S., 176 Franck, K., 142 340, 345
137, 428 Flanders, W., 46 Frank, E., 445 Furr-Holden, C., 114
Feiring, C., 290, 317, 322, 326–327, Flannery, D., 36, 40, 209, 258, Frank, J., 425 Furrow, J., 312
332, 334, 336, 337, 340, 345 264, 430 Frank, K. A., 141, 169 Furstenberg, F. F. J., 93
Feivelson, D., 371 Flashman, J., 169, 395 Frank, S., 51, 292 Furstenberg, F., Jr., 5, 6, 88, 91,
Feldlaufer, H., 198 Flavell, E., 58 Frankenberger, K. D., 59 105–106, 108, 137, 139, 147,
Feldman, A. F., 221, 222f, 231, 281 Flavell, J., 58 Franklin, J., 172 223, 374, 375, 378
Feldman, J., 74 Flay, B. R., 428, 429, 441, 442 Franko, D. L., 263 Fussell, M., 53
Feldman, R., 113, 114, 312 Flegal, K. M., 45 Franzetta, K., 370, 371 Futterman, D., 372
Feldman, S. S., 142, 291, 301, Fleming, C. B., 106, 107f Franzoi, S., 176
360, 367 Fleming, J. S., 391 Frasier, K., 332
Felice, M., 362, 378 Fleming, W., 322 Fredricks, J. A., 232, 262
G
Felitti, V. J., 131 Fletcher, A. C., 132, 231, 396, 430 Fredriksen, K., 39 Gabhainn, S., 354, 357, 358
Felix, E., 114, 209 Flewelling, R. L., 228, 430 Fredstrom, B. K., 181 Gable, R. K., 196
Felner, R., 147, 264, 398, 399 Flickinger, S., 387, 400 Freed, L. H., 345 Gabrielli, W., 419
Felson, R. B., 420 Flieller, A., 62 Freedman, A., 449 Gadino, B., 274
Fenton, R. E., 400 Flisher, A. J., 418 Freedman-Doan, C., 198 Gage, J. C., 234
Fenwick, J., 374 Flom, J., 33 Freedman-Doan, P., 227, 228 Gager, C. T., 231
Fenzel, L., 198 Flook, L., 126, 327, 395 Freedner, N., 345 Gahr, J. L., 43
Ferenz-Gillies, R., 322 Flor, D., 147, 399 Freeman, H., 147, 330 Gaines, C., 133
Fergus, S., 355 Flores, E., 142, 345 Freeman-Gallant, A., 438 Galambos, N., 93f
Ferguson, C., 242 Flores, G., 338 Freisthler, B., 427 Galambos, N. L., 6, 39, 41, 91–93,
Ferguson, D., 275 Flory, J., 33 Freitas, A., 179 101, 131, 132, 166, 170, 234,
Ferguson, M., 66 Flory, K., 426, 428 French, D. C., 176–178, 311, 312, 281, 282, 333, 444
Ferguson, Y., 292, 293f Flynn, B., 431 330, 362 Galay, L., 308, 309
Fergusson, D., 43, 143, 183, 265, Flynn, M., 417 French, M., 175 Galbraith, J., 372
361, 377, 419, 432, 437–440 Fobil, J., 245 French, S. A., 48, 49 Galbraith, K. A., 360
Feron, F. J., 37 Foehr, U., 220, 237–239, 241–244, French, S. E., 198, 260, 274 Galliher, R. V., 273, 278, 367
Ferrar, K., 46 247, 350 Freng, A., 170 Gallimore, R., 170
Ferré, C., 373 Folkman, S., 53 Frenn, K., 24, 70 Galloway, T., 75, 215
Ferrell, R., 33 Fomby, P., 395 Frenzel, A. C., 393 Gallup, G., 310
Ferrer, E., 64, 64f Fonagy, P., 439 Freud, A., 14, 289 Galvan, A., 69, 70, 295
Ferrero, J., 34, 41 Fondell, M., 362 Freud, S., 14 Galvez, M. P., 34
Ferris, J., 33 Fong, G. T., 360, 370, 372 Frey, S., 38 Galvin, D. M., 227
Fichtenberg, C. M., 430 Fontaine, R. G., 76, 346, 439 Frey, W., 279 Gamble, W., 322
Fida, R., 306 Forbes, E., 26, 70 Frick, P. J., 179, 432, 433, 437 Gamm, B. K., 324, 326–327, 346
Field, A. E., 367 Forbes, E. E., 37, 447 Fried, J., 437 Gamoran, A., 200, 207–208
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-8 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-8 Name Index

Gano-Overway, L., 235 Ghazarian, S. R., 128, 132, 275 Gonzalez, R., 401 Greenberg, M. T., 113, 182, 197,
Gans, J., 449 Ghetti, S., 63 Good, M., 311–312 332, 428, 438, 438f, 439
Garber, J., 198, 232, 447, 448 Giannetta, J., 72, 81 Good, T., 389 Greenberger, E., 126, 131, 173, 225,
Garcia, C., 273 Gibb, B. E., 445 Goodenow, C., 370 333, 400, 412, 447, 448
Garcia, J., 332 Gibbons, F. X., 42, 43, 59, 113, 130, Goodman, E., 147, 310, 312, 367 Green-Demers, I., 389
Garcia, M., 449 131, 276, 277f, 360, 360f, 362, Goodman, G., 298, 299f Greene, K., 372
Garcia-Castro, M., 379 372, 387 Goodman, M., 89–90, 105 Greene, M., 53
Garcia-Coll, C., 113, 262, 276 Gibbons, K., 49 Goodnight, J. A., 136, 440 Greene, M. L., 167, 168f, 263, 263f,
Garcia-Reid, P., 193 Gibbs, J. C., 303, 305 Goodson, P., 356–358 277, 278, 325, 328, 328f
Garcia-Tunon, M., 372 Giedd, J. N., 65, 70, 70f, 136 Goosby, B. J., 113 Greene, N., 402
Gardner, H., 58, 73, 74 Gil, A. G., 369 Goossens, L., 58, 132, 270, 271, 291 Greene, W., 367–368
Gardner, J. F., 446 Gilbert, A., 273 Goran, M., 28 Greenfield, P., 11
Gardner, M., 81, 114–115, 232, 233 Gilchrist, L. D., 377–378 Gordis, E. B., 142, 447, 448 Greenley, R. N., 51, 52
Gardner, T. W., 438 Giles, W. H., 131 Gordon, K., 102–103, 389 Greenspan, L. C., 34
Gardner, W., 439 Giletta, M., 172 Gordon, L., 368 Greenstein, D., 68
Garg, R., 396 Gilliam, F., 17 Gordon, R. A., 172, 377 Greenwald, P., 326
Gargiulo, J., 40 Gillin, C. J., 39 Gordon, V., 360 Greenwald, S., 312
Gariglietti, G., 58 Gillman, S., 141 Gore, S., 214, 332, 419, 447 Greenwood, G., 360
Garmezy, N., 449 Gillmore, M. R., 362, 371, 377–378, Gorman-Smith, D., 112, 113, Gregory, A., 206, 206f, 209
Garnefski, N., 149 428, 440 147, 170, 344, 344f, 418, Gregory, E., 124
Garner, R., 156 Gillock, K., 197, 398–399 441, 450 Gregory, W., 264
Garnier, H., 170 Gilman, R., 181 Gortmaker, S. L., 49, 53 Greusing, S., 38
Garrett, S. B., 132 Gilman, S. E., 429 Gostisha, A. J., 432, 439 Greven, C. U., 438
Gartrell, N., 143 Gilpin, E., 427 Gotcsik, M., 370 Grey, M., 367, 368f, 419
Garvan, C., 438 Gilreath, T. D., 427 Gotham, H. J., 426 Greytak, E. A., 367
Garver, K., 57, 63 Gil-Rivas, V., 131 Gotlib, I., 447, 448 Griffin, K. W., 265, 428
Gaskin, A., 108 Gingras, I., 292 Gottfredson, D., 195 Griffin, T., 278
Gasser, L., 77 Ginsberg, S., 59 Gottfredson, M., 420 Griffith Fontaine, R., 439
Gastic, B., 222 Ginsburg, G., 199, 393 Gottfried, A. E., 388, 391 Grigg, W., 204
Gathercole, S. E., 64 Ginzler, J. A., 367 Gottfried, A. W., 388, 391 Griggs, C., 24, 70
Gatherum, A., 293 Giordano, P. C., 336, 338, 339, 346, Gottlieb, B. H., 92 Grime, R., 303
Gau, S. S., 39 354, 355, 360, 435 Gottlieb, S., 371 Griskevicius, G., 419
Gaudreau, P., 232 Giorgio, A., 68 Goudriaan, A., 238 Grisso, T., 96, 97f, 296, 296f, 433
Gault-Sherman, M., 128 Giovino, G., 427 Gould, J. B., 113 Grobe, R., 393, 397
Gauze, C., 332, 333 Girgus, J., 445 Gould, M., 246, 446 Grodsky, E., 407
Gavin, L., 332, 372 Glaberson, W., 4 Gould, M. S., 181 Grogan-Kaylor, A., 50
Gawlik, A., 31, 34 Glanovsky, J. L., 427, 446 Gowen, L. K., 40, 48, 334, 342, 367 Grolnick, W., 134, 393
Gaydos, L. M., 379 Glantz, S. A., 430 Goyette, K., 398 Grosbras, M., 300
Gayle, H., 372 Glaser, B., 420 Goza, F., 398–400 Gross, E. F., 181
Gaylord-Harden, N., 263, 275 Glasgow, K., 393 Grabe, S., 367 Gross, L., 240
Ge, X., 32, 33, 42–44, 113, 130, Glass, J., 428 Graber, J. A., 12, 24, 35, 37, 41–44, Grossman, J. M., 273
131, 146, 342, 344, 419, 444, Glauber, A., 344 48–50, 181, 417, 420, 442, 445, Grossman, M., 430
445, 447 Glenn, D., 130 447–449 Grotevant, H. D., 126, 147
Gebelt, J. L., 42 Glennie, E. J., 406 Graf, A. R., 432, 439 Grotpeter, J., 324
Gecas, V., 124 Glenwright, M., 60 Grafman, J., 243 Grover, R. L., 340
Gee, G. C., 371 Glick, G. C., 317, 325 Graham, C., 33 Grube, J., 352, 360, 363
Geier, C., 70 Glover, G., 69 Graham, R., 71 Gruber, J., 53, 79, 83, 424, 430
Gelbwasser, L., 395 Glover, S., 207 Graham, S., 28, 41, 72, 78, 80, Gruber, M., 107, 444
Gelhorn, H. L., 418 Gnagy, E. M., 438 81, 81f, 96, 97, 97f, 177–179, Grucza, R. A., 430
Geller, B., 431 Goate, A., 136 181, 198, 198f, 203, 255, 276, Gruen, J., 201
Gelles, R. J., 312 Godeau, E., 354, 357, 358 295f, 295–297, 296f, 370, 393, Gruenewald, P. J., 429
Gennetian, L. A., 146–147 Goesling, B., 430 405, 436 Gruff, T., 339
Gentile, D., 238, 239f, 242 Goetz, T., 393, 450 Graham-Bermann, S., 147 Grumbach, M., 23f
Gentry, M., 196 Gökler, B., 323 Gralen, S., 28, 50 Grunbaum, J., 276, 353
Gentsch, J., 181, 238 Goldberg, A., 334, 339f, 339–341 Gralinski, J., 389 Grundy, A., 132, 290
George, G., 264 Goldberg, L. R., 258 Granberg, E., 28 Grych, J. H., 142, 345
Georgiades, K., 111, 426, 444 Golden, N., 48 Grange, C. M., 360, 360f, 362 Guacci-Franci, N., 330, 332
Gerard, J. M., 142, 265 Goldfield, G., 244 Granger, D., 24, 37, 70, 177 Guberman, S., 75
Gerber, M., 232 Goldman, M., 428 Granic, I., 123 Gudiño, O., 110
Gerber, S. B., 195 Goldscheider, F., 106 Granillo, M., 50 Guerino, P., 209
Gerbner, G., 240 Goldsmith, P. A., 203 Grant, K., 445 Guerra, N. G., 180, 181
Germán, M., 125 Goldstein, A. S., 429 Gravelle, M., 196 Guerry, J., 172
Gerrard, M., 59, 113, 130, 276, 277f, Goldstein, B., 29f Gravitt, P., 102 Guest, A., 232
362, 372, 387 Goldstein, S., 300 Gray, D. L., 379 Guillen, E., 48
Gerris, J. M., 127, 131 Goldston, D., 446 Gray, G., 419 Guimond, A. B., 274, 275
Gershoff, E. T., 449 Goldweber, A., 334, 346, 435 Gray, H., 53 Guion, K., 134
Gershoni, R., 325 Goleman, D., 73 Gray, M., 340, 345 Guivernau, M. R., 235
Gerstein, D., 424 Golub, A., 426 Gray-Little, B., 203, 263, 276 Guldi, M., 109
Gest, S. D., 258, 260, 449 Gomez, C. A., 343 Green, F., 58 Gullone, E., 80
Gestsdóttir, S., 236, 236f, 300 Gondoli, D., 51, 124, 132, 290 Green, J., 444 Gummerum, M., 305
Gettman, D. C., 127, 329 Gonzales, N. A., 43, 125, 129, 133, Green, K., 272 Gunlicks-Stoessel, M., 445
Getz, J., 109, 291, 429 171, 278, 394, 396, 397 Green, K. M., 427 Gunnar, M., 24, 70
Gfroerer, J. C., 108, 426 Gonzales, R., 275 Green, S. M., 360, 377, 395 Gunnoe, M., 133, 145
Ghatak, R., 393 Gonzales-Backen, M. A., 274, 278 Greenbaum, P. E., 428, 430, 431 Gunter, W., 196
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-9 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-9

Guo, F., 344 Hambarsoomian, K., 46 Hart, D., 258, 302, 306–308 Helms, L., 227
Guo, G., 45, 136, 242, 438 Hamburger, M. E., 368 Harter, S., 11, 254, 256, 257, 261, Helsen, M., 331
Guo, J., 429 Hamby, S. L., 181 262, 264, 265, 292 Helwig, C., 77, 308, 309
Guo, M., 173 Hamill, S., 124 Hartmark, C., 102–103 Hemingway, F., 438
Gur, M., 370 Hamilton, B., 373 Hartney, C., 100 Hemphill, S. A., 444
Gurevitch, M., 240 Hamilton, C., 322 Hartos, J., 52 Henderson, A., 308
Gurin, G., 170 Hamilton, H. A., 145 Hartung, C., 417 Henderson, C., 142
Güroğlu, B., 26, 73, 179 Hamilton, M., 88 Hartup, W. W., 179, 183, 346 Henderson, C. E., 430, 431
Gustafson, P., 304 Hamilton, S., 88, 330 Harwood, R., 182 Henderson, H., 129
Gustafsson, H., 10 Hamilton-Leaks, J., 378 Hasebe, Y., 121, 122 Henderson, S., 137, 138, 140,
Guterman, N., 109, 426 Hamm, J. V., 167, 169, 196, 275 Haselager, G. J. T., 179 144–145
Guth, A., 38 Hamm, M., 74 Hasselhorn, M., 262 Hendrick, C., 364
Guthertz, M., 326 Hammen, C. L., 445 Hastings, J. S., 204 Hendrick, S., 364
Guthrie, H., 28 Hammersmith, S., 366 Hastorf, A., 144 Henkel, R. R., 169, 172
Guthrie, I. K., 306 Hammond, W., 278 Hathaway, J. E., 345 Hennessy, M., 242
Gutierrez, P. M., 446 Hammons, A., 47 Hattie, J., 389 Hennig, K. H., 304
Gutierrez, R., 205 Hampson, S. E., 258 Hatzenbuehler, M., 181 Henning, A., 77
Gutman, L., 113, 126, 126f, Hamre, B. K., 398 Hatzichristou, C., 176 Henninghausen, K., 198
196, 199 Hamza, C. A., 128, 447 Hatzinger, M., 232 Henrich, C. C., 114, 307, 312
Guyer, A., 71, 148, 445 Han, W., 81 Hau, K., 201, 202, 262, 385 Henrich, C. C., 114, 175, 197
Guyll, M., 18 Handley, E., 427 Hauser, S., 323, 331 Henriksen, L., 220, 237, 239,
Handwerger, K., 24, 70, 177 Hausser, D., 372 241–244, 247, 350
Hankin, B. L., 420–421, 442, Havel, M., 38 Henry, B., 258, 438, 439
H 445, 447 Havighurst, R., 15 Henry, D., 112, 113, 170, 450
Ha, C., 439 Hannan, P. J., 49–51, 247 Haviland, A. M., 227, 228 Henry, D. B., 147, 359, 362, 418
Ha, T., 322, 341 Hansbrough, E., 242 Haviland, M. L., 243 Henry, K. L., 399, 406, 429
Haas, E., 172 Hansen, D. M., 38, 233 Hawk, S., 170–171 Hensel, D., 352
Haavisto, T., 387 Hanson, S., 398 Hawken, L. S., 405 Henshaw, S., 372–374
Habermas, T., 271 Hanson, T., 144 Hawkins, D. N., 112, 139, 143, Henson, J. M., 257, 357, 358
Haberstick, B. C., 419 Hansson, K., 136 227, 228 Heraux, C. G., 181
Hadd, V., 429 Hao, J. H., 446 Hawkins, J. D., 112, 126, 131, 406, Herdman, M., 179
Haddad, E., 333, 392 Harachi, T. W., 125, 428, 440 429, 430 Herdt, G., 24, 26, 350
Hadjiyannakis, S., 244 Harackiewicz, J. M., 208 Hawley, P. H., 175 Herman, K. C., 276
Hafdahl, A., 263, 276 Hardaway, C., 108, 109, 113, 146 Hay, C., 113, 181 Herman, M., 115, 167, 263, 275,
Hafen, C., 169 Harden, K. P., 41, 44, 81, 136, 357, Hayden, H., 51 279, 388, 400
Hafen, M., Jr., 125 358, 361, 429, 440, 447 Haydon, A., 295, 416 Herman-Giddens, M., 32, 34
Hafsteinsson, L. G., 130–131 Hardesty, J. L., 262–264, 275 Haydon, A. A., 342 Herman-Stahl, M., 450
Hagan, J., 179, 345 Hardiman, M., 66 Haydon, K. C., 323, 323f Hernández, A. M., 92
Hagewen, K. J., 222 Hardin, M., 424 Hayes, D., 405 Hernandez, D., 235
Haggart, N., 323 Harding, D. J., 112 Hayes, K. L., 370 Hernandez, P., 148
Haglund, B., 377 Hardway, C., 39, 125 Hayes, R., 129, 306 Hernandez-Guzman, L., 41
Hahm, H. C., 109, 354, 426 Hardy, D., 376 Hayford, S. R., 93 Herpertz, S., 438
Hahn, C.-S., 420 Hardy, K. K., 245 Haynes, O. M., 420 Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., 69, 438
Haight, W., 333 Hardy, S. A., 306, 312 Haynie, D. L., 44, 113, 132, 135, 159, Herrenkohl, T. I., 112, 126, 131
Haines, J., 49–51, 247 Hare, A. L., 12, 132–134, 245, 172–173, 179, 181, 346, 357 Herrera, C., 110
Hair, E. C., 132 286–290, 292, 318, 321–323 Hayslip, B., 142 Herrera, D., 197
Haj-Yahia, M. M., 131, 168 Hare, T. A., 69 Hayward, C., 40, 48–50, 334, Herrera, V. M., 343, 345
Haj-Yahia, Y., 131 Harel-Fisch, Y., 180, 180f 342, 448 Herrera-Leavitt, I., 393
Hakvoort, E., 366, 367 Hargis, P., 223 Hazan, C., 340 Herrling, S., 406
Halder, I., 33 Hargreaves, D., 48 He, J., 417, 417f, 442 Hershberg, T., 105–106, 223
Hale, L. E., 201–202 Hargreaves, J., 430 He, M., 125 Hertzman, C., 53
Hale, S., 63 Hariri, A., 5, 13 Healton, C. G., 243 Hertzog, C., 445
Hale, W. W., 258, 271, 272 Harker, K., 276 Healy, B., 326 Herwehe, S., 39
Halgunseth, L., 133 Harkness, A., 75 Heard, H. E., 142 Herz, D., 148
Hall, D. M., 149 Harlan, S., 378 Heath, A. C., 141, 428 Herzog, D., 53
Hall, D. W., 379 Harold, G., 134, 136, 142 Heatherton, T. F., 243 Hetherington, E. M., 128, 133, 134,
Hall, G., 446 Harold, R., 393 Heaton, J. A., 39 136–138, 140, 141, 143–145
Hall, G. S., 13, 13f, 17 Harper, G., 170 Hebert, L., 371 Hetland, J., 180, 180f
Hall, J., 69, 439 Harré, N., 52 Hebert, M., 345 Hevey, C., 134
Hall, N. C., 450 Harrington, H., 72, 438–440, 447 Hechinger, F., 196 Hewitt, J. K., 418
Haller, M., 427 Harrington, K. F., 312, 361, 370, 372 Hecht, M. L., 275 Heyerdahl, S., 418
Halliday-Scher, K., 160 Harris, C., 170 Heck, D., 167, 275 Hibbert, J. R., 131
Halligan, S. L., 178–179 Harris, D., 18, 34 Heck, R. H., 200 Hicks, L., 197
Hallinan, M., 167, 200, 204, 205 Harris, J., 149, 154, 171 Heckhausen, J., 333, 400, 412 Hicks, R. E., 172, 346
Hallquist, S., 102 Harris, J. I., 402 Hedeker, D., 442 Hightower, E., 322
Halpern, C., 35, 49, 102, 295, 416 Harris, K., 45, 102, 426, 449 Heeren, T., 425 Hill, C., 411f
Halpern, C. T., 342, 344, 345, Harris, K. M., 339, 353 Heffner, V., 247 Hill, D., 352
353–355, 357, 358, 445 Harris, M., 169, 170 Heilbron, N., 172, 178 Hill, E., 361
Halpern-Felsher, B. L., 33, 79–80, Harris, M. J., 441 Hein, K., 53, 372 Hill, J., 429
288, 295, 298, 355, 372 Harris, R., 365 Heinrichs, B., 358 Hill, J. E., 358
Halpern-Meekin, S., 140 Harris, T. L., 310, 312 Helenius, H., 438 Hill, J. P., 7, 40
Halpert, J., 114 Harris-Britt, A., 278 Helfand, M., 357 Hill, K. G., 112, 131, 336, 337f,
Haltigan, J. D., 439 Harrison, H. M., 178, 179 Helmond, P., 201 406, 429
Ham, A., 360 Harrison, K., 247 Helms, H., 329 Hill, L., 262
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-10 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-10 Name Index

Hill, M., 378 Horner, J., 248, 370 Hutchinson, D., 51 Jaffee, S., 377
Hill, N. E., 209, 393, 409 Hornik, R., 424 Hutchinson, M. K., 360 Jaffe-Hirschfield, D., 353
Hillier, L., 49 Horton, K. D., 206, 292 Hutteman, R., 258 Jager, J., 256
Hillman, S. B., 445 Horvat, E. M., 165, 400–401 Huynh, V. M., 264, 275, 276 Jagers, R. J., 305
Hills, N. K., 243 Horwood, J., 43, 265, 432 Huynh, V. W., 278, 310 Jahng, S., 292, 293f
Hilsman, R., 447 Horwood, L., 361, 437, 439, 440 Hwang, K., 69 Jain, U., 449
Hilt, L. M., 181, 447 Houck, C., 357 Hyde, J., 280, 281 James, A., 427, 446
Himes, J., 32 Hough, R., 440 Hyde, J. S., 357, 367, 445 James, L., 74
Hindin, M. J., 372 Houghton, S., 438 Hyde, L. W., 123, 439 James, S., 68
Hine, T., 16, 90, 220 Houltberg, B., 12, 288, 302, 306, 307 Hyman, C., 176, 183, 377 James-Burdumy, S., 430
Hines, M., 25, 26 House, L. E., 450 Jamieson, P., 248
Hingson, R., 425, 427 Houston, S. M., 63, 65 Jang, S., 265
Hinshaw, S. P., 63, 181 Hout, M., 408
I Jank, W., 369
Hipp, J., 430 Houts, R. M., 33, 46, 47f, 244 Iacoboni, M., 71 Jannone, S., 388
Hiraga, Y., 171 Hoven, C. W., 418 Iacono, W. G., 126, 136, 141, 311, Janosz, M., 179, 406
Hirky, A., 427 Hovinga, M. E., 377 394, 440 Jansen, B., 59, 81
Hirsch, B., 261, 328, 332, 333 Howard, A., 6, 91, 441 Ialongo, N. S., 114, 227, 276, Jansen, B. R. J., 295
Hirsch, M., 373–374 Howard, G., 374 420, 441 Jansen, E., 177
Hirschfield, P., 111, 406 Howard, K., 114 Iannotti, R., 47 Jansen, M., 300
Hirschi, T., 420 Howard Caldwell, C., 145 Ibañez, G. E., 393 Janssen, I., 38
Hitchings, B., 419 Howe, G. W., 134, 137 Ickovics, J., 376 Jansson, S. M., 132
Hitlin, S., 280 Howell, L. W., 359 Iedema, J., 271 Jarrett, R., 147, 236, 288
Hjalmarsson, R., 169 Howell, W., 204 Igartua, K., 365 Jarvinen, M., 429
Ho, A., 12, 386 Hoyt, D., 147, 361, 362, 369, 426 Iglowstein, I., 39 Jarvis, P., 199
Ho, A. Y., 175 Hoyt, L., 53 Ikramullah, E. N., 354, Jaser, S. S., 450
Ho, L., 338, 358 Hoza, B., 333, 334 359–360, 375 Jason, L., 402
Hochberg, Z., 31, 34 Hrabosky, J. I., 50 Imai-Matsumura, K., 63 Javdani, S., 418
Hock, E., 292 Hsu, C., 401 Impett, E. A., 257, 357, 358 Jazbec, S., 69
Hockenberry-Eaton, M., 360 Hu, M., 428 Ingoldsby, E. M., 420 Jefferson, K., 372
Hodge, S. A., 196, 198 Hu, P., 362 Ingram, F., 372 Jemal, A., 46
Hodges, E. V. E., 175, 178, 179 Huan, V. S., 387 Inhelder, B., 14 Jemmott, J. B., III, 360, 370, 372
Hoeve, M., 131 Huang, B., 34, 147 Inoff-Germain, G., 31, 421 Jemmott, L. S., 360, 370, 372
Hofer, M., 309 Huang, D., 275 Insabella, G., 141, 143 Jenkins, F., 204
Hoffer, T., 204, 395 Huang, F., 206 Institute of Medicine (IOM), 46, 47, Jenkins, K. N., 407
Hofferth, S. L., 377 Huang, L., 133 52, 78, 364, 365 Jenkins, R., 276
Hoffman, J. H., 429 Huang, S., 48, 276 Ireland, M., 49, 355, 361 Jenkins, S., 69
Hoffman, J. P., 113, 140 Huang, Y., 437 Irwin, C., 52, 53, 371, 372 Jenni, O., 38, 39
Hoffman, K. L., 134 Hubbard, J., 336, 339, 342, 449 Irwin, K., 209 Jennings, W. G., 440
Hofman, J., 325 Hudes, E. S., 343 Isaacs, J., 175 Jensen, L. A., 291
Hofmann, V., 258 Hudley, C., 178, 203, 391 Isakson, K., 199 Jensen-Campbell, L., 336
Hogan, D., 361 Hudson, K., 420 Ispa, J., 133 Jenson, J. M., 179
Hogansen, J. M., 420 Huebner, A. J., 231, 359 Ivanova, K., 141 Jenssen, B., 243
Hogue, A., 172, 173 Huebner, C. E., 416 Ivers, R., 79 Jeong, Y.-J., 141
Holcombe, R., 197 Huebner, T., 438 Ivory, J., 242, 246 Jessor, R., 45, 47, 419, 429
Holder, D. W., 310, 312 Huesmann, L. R., 243, 256, 409, 438 Jessor, S., 419
Holderness, C., 40 Huettel, S., 65, 71 J Jester, J. M., 419, 428
Holland, L. J., 345 Hughes, D., 108, 205, 273, 275–278, Jeynes, W., 140, 141, 204
Hollander, D., 363 278f, 330, 399–401 Jaccard, J., 172, 360, 372, 374 Ji, J., 42, 439
Hollenstein, T., 123 Hughes, H. K., 367 Jack, F., 63 Jia, Y., 205
Hollingshead, A., 15 Hughes, J. L., 449 Jackson, C., 121, 242, 357, 357f Jiang, D., 181, 181f
Hollist, D. R., 113 Hughes, L. A., 233 Jackson, D., 64 Jiang, X., 428
Holloway, B., 110 Hughes, R., 367 Jackson, J., 276, 277f, 278, 332 Jimerson, S., 198
Holloway, C., 364 Hughes, S., 427 Jackson, J. S., 44 Jodl, K. M., 137, 145, 323, 392,
Holmbeck, G. N., 39–40, 51, 52, Huh, D., 46, 132 Jackson, L., 244–246 393, 410
122, 123f, 142, 291, 292, 294, Huizenga, H. M., 59, 81, 295 Jackson, S., 51 John, O., 258, 419
360, 379 Huizinga, D., 80, 113, 420 Jackson, T., 50 Johnson, A. B., 334, 343, 357
Holmen, J., 427 Huizink, A. C., 426 Jackson-Newsom, J., 128, 132, 133 Johnson, B., 95
Holmen, T., 427 Hulleman, C. S., 208 Jacobi, C., 49, 50 Johnson, B. D., 426
Holsboer-Trachsler, E., 232 Hunsberger, B., 307–308 Jacobs, D., 427, 446 Johnson, C., 112, 147
Holsen, I., 444 Hunt, J., 249 Jacobs, J., 7, 91, 298 Johnson, C. A., 81
Holstein, B., 180, 180f Hunt, M., 450 Jacobs, J. E., 120, 232, 260, 281 Johnson, D., 28, 37
Holt, M. K., 169, 172 Hunter, S., 132 Jacobs, S., 144 Johnson, D. W., 205
Holtzman, D., 372 Huntsinger, C. S., 258 Jacobsohn, L., 424 Johnson, E., 39
Homer, J., 175 Huot, R., 24, 425, 448 Jacobson, C., 446 Johnson, J., 369, 427
Hommeyer, J., 292 Hurdle, D., 276, 282 Jacobson, K., 136, 444 Johnson, J. G., 243, 243f, 420
Hong, S., 429 Hurley, D., 138 Jacobson, K. C., 426 Johnson, J. P., 362
Hook, E., 370, 372 Hurrelmann, K., 105 Jacobson, L., 137 Johnson, K., 426
Hooper, C. J., 63, 65, 68 Hurt, H., 72, 81 Jacobson, N., 112 Johnson, M., 71, 226, 227,
Hopf, D., 176 Hurt, T. R., 278 Jacobs-Quadrel, M., 79 407–412, 409f
Hoppe, M. J., 362 Hussey, J., 358 Jacobus, J., 425 Johnson, R., 28, 424
Hops, H., 265, 447 Hussey, M., 34 Jacques-Tiura, A., 114 Johnson, R. M., 367
Horan, P., 223 Hussong, A. M., 12, 172, 346, 420, Jacquez, F. M., 447 Johnson, R. T., 205
Horn, E. J., 135 426–430 Jaffari-Bimmel, N., 322 Johnson, S., 292
Horn, S. S., 77, 162, 177, 367 Huston, A. C., 113, 200, 399 Jaffe, P., 367 Johnson, W., 136, 394
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-11 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-11

Johnson-Campbell, T., 388 Kambam, P., 296 Kelly, M. C., 77 Kirby, D. B., 113, 343, 357, 370, 371,
Johnston, L. D., 234, 416, 422–424, Kamenetz, A., 105 Kelly, S., 199 379–380
423f, 424f, 426–430, 440 Kaminski, J. W., 149 Keltikangas-Jaervinen, L., 421 Kirby, J. B., 145
Johnston, M., 203 Kan, M. L., 359 Kendall, P., 417 Kirk, D., 112
Joiner, T. E., Jr., 442, 447 Kanaya, T., 122 Keniston, K., 90 Kirkpatrick, L., 323
Jolles, D., 63 Kandel, D., 419–420, 426–428 Kennard, B. D., 449 Kirkpatrick, N. M., 433
Jones, B. L., 357 Kane, P., 232 Kenneavy, K., 242 Kiselica, M., 375
Jones, D., 28, 328 Kane, S. B., 367 Kenny, G., 244 Kistler, M., 262
Jones, D. C., 51 Kang, H., 272 Kenyon, D. B., 101 Kit-fong Au, T., 360
Jones, D. J., 115 Kann, L., 353, 369–370, 372 Keogh, D., 377 Kiuhara, S. A., 405
Jones, J., 199, 449 Kanouse, D. E., 242, 276, 360 Kercher, A., 447 Kivlighan, K., 24, 70, 177
Jones, J. M., 400 Kantor, H., 193 Kerns, K., 322 Klaczynski, P. A., 57, 62
Jones, L., 246, 247 Kao, G., 398, 400, 401 Kerns, K. A., 134 Klahr, A., 126
Jones, M., 245 Kao, M., 362 Kerr, D., 129 Klasen, L., 68
Jones, M. C., 41 Kaplan, A., 50 Kerr, M., 44, 132, 169, 171, 231, 293, Klassen, R. M., 387
Jones, M. D., 273, 278 Kaplan, C., 354 359, 447 Klauer, S., 81, 82f
Jones, N., 53 Kaplan, H., 265 Kerrigan, D., 372 Klaver, J., 96
Jones, N. P., 445 Kaplan, N., 322 Kershaw, T., 374, 376 Kleibeuker, S., 63
Jones, R., 8, 70, 107, 352 Kaplan, R., 108, 109, 113, 146 Keshavan, B., 70, 70f Klein, D. J., 425, 426, 447, 448
Jones, R. K., 312, 370, 371 Kaplan, S., 23f Kessler, R., 444 Klein, H. W., 45, 46
Jones, R. M., 40 Kaprio, J., 32, 42, 43, 44f, 136, Kett, J., 16, 98, 103, 105, 223 Klein, J., 195, 243
Jones, S. M., 114, 131, 175 137f, 448 Key, J., 377 Klein, J. D., 374
Jones Harden, B., 148 Karevold, E., 447 Khadr, O., 4 Kleinman, M., 181, 246
Jonkman, K., 174–175 Karp, I., 46, 428 Khoo, A., 238 Kleitman, S., 227, 233
Jonkman, L., 63 Karpovsky, S., 326–328 Khoo, S., 134 Klepinger, D., 377
Jordan, A., 238–240, 242, 243, Karremans, J. C., 176 Khoury-Kassibri, M., 209 Klerman, L. V., 53, 377
257, 447 Karriker-Jaffe, K., 112, 177, 345 Kiang, L., 167, 273–275, 278 Klessinger, N., 450
Jordan, L. C., 429 Kasen, S., 243, 243f, 420 Kidd, S. A., 445 Kliewer, W., 114, 177, 312, 340
Jose, P. E., 258, 445 Kasim, R., 428 Kiecolt, J., 134 Klimstra, T., 258, 271, 272
Joshi, G., 428 Kaslow, N. J., 447 Kiene, S. M., 356 Kling, J. R., 111, 436
Joshi, H., 109 Kasser, T., 292, 293f Kiesner, J., 169, 176, 418 Klingberg, T., 63, 65
Josse, G., 73 Kassin, S., 298 Kigozi, G., 102 Klomek, A. B., 181
Jouriles, E. N., 345 Kassir, S., 437 Kilbourne, B., 372 Kloska, D., 429
Jovonen, J., 179 Katainen, S., 421 Kilburn, J., 242 Klump, K. L., 48, 50
Joyce, T., 374 Kataoka, S., 110 Kilgore, S., 204 Knafo, A., 121
Joyner, K., 343 Katz, E., 240 Killen, J., 48, 49 Knecht, A., 172
Jozefowicz-Simbeni, D., 114, 312 Katz, J., 356–357 Killen, M., 77 Knight, E. D., 369
Ju, D., 141, 405 Katz, L. F., 111, 436 Killeya-Jones, L., 102 Knight, G., 278, 306
Juang, L., 275, 276, 278, 300–301, Katz, M., 98, 103, 105 Killeya-Jones, L. A., 175 Knight, G. P., 125
394 Katzman, D., 48 Killoren, S., 170 Knight, K. E., 406
Juarez, L., 113 Kaufman, C., 263 Kilpatrick, D. G., 419, 428 Knoester, C., 113, 132
Juarez-Reyes, M., 354 Kaufman, C. E., 358 Kim, D. Y., 322 Knox, V., 146–147
Judge, B., 446 Kaufman, J., 210 Kim, H., 23, 92, 345 Knutson, M., 437, 438
Juffer, F., 322, 438 Kaufman, K., 264 Kim, H. K., 357, 419, 440, 444 Knutson, N., 145
Jumping-Eagle, S., 371 Kaukinen, C., 139f, 139–140 Kim, H. S., 336 Ko, H., 401
Jung, J., 240 Kawabata, Y., 177 Kim, I. J., 42, 43, 429 Ko, L., 273
Juonala, M., 46 Kawai, E., 172 Kim, J., 134f, 135, 170, 241, 275, Ko, S., 388
Jurkovic, G., 393 Kaye, K., 373 281, 282, 282f, 397, 411 Kobak, R., 317, 318, 320–323
Jussim, L., 206 Kazis, R., 109 Kim, J.-I., 388 Koch, G., 32, 367–368
Juul, A., 34 Keane, E., 427 Kim, J. L., 360 Kochel, K., 179, 179f
Juvonen, J., 43, 179, 181, 203, Keane, S. P., 135, 438 Kim, J.-Y., 233 Koenig, B. W., 215, 367
209, 396 Keating, D., 8, 53, 56, 61–64, 74, 78, Kim, K., 128, 145, 437 Koenig, L., 311
308, 449 Kim, M., 235, 388 Koerner, S., 144
Keefe, K., 169 Kim, M. J., 106, 107f Koerner, S. S., 101
K Keel, P. K., 48, 50 Kim, S., 130, 146, 276, 447 Koestner, R., 292
Kabbani, N. S., 405 Keeling, R., 372 Kim, S.-B., 227 Koff, E., 41
Kackar, H., 308 Keeshan, P., 53, 430 Kim, S. Y., 125, 146, 278 Kofler, M. J., 419
Kadosh, K., 59, 71, 76, 255 Keijsers, L., 126, 132, 170–171, Kim, Y., 240 Kogan, M. D., 234
Kaestle, C. E., 353–355 271–272, 290 Kimberg, C., 52 Kogan, S., 276
Kaestner, R., 374 Keith, B., 140 Kimonis, E., 436 Kogan, S. M., 360, 360f, 362
Kahler, C. W., 420 Kelch, R., 23f Kim-Prieto, C., 272 Koh, S., 298
Kahn, R., 53 Kellam, S., 441 King, C., 401 Kohen, D. E., 141
Kahneman, D., 79, 82 Keller, G., 450 King, J., 389 Kohl, G. O., 420
Kail, R. V., 64, 64f Keller, J., 388 King, K. M., 428 Kohlberg, L., 303–305
Kakihara, F., 293 Keller, M., 258, 305, 326, 449 King, P., 309–312, 310f Kohler, J. K., 147
Kalakoski, V., 272 Kelley, A., 424 King, R., 49, 374 Kohler, P. K., 379
Kaliher, G., 332 Kelley, M., 114, 360 King, R. A., 418 Kohls, G., 69
Kalil, A., 133, 146, 147, 205, 377, 390 Kelly, A., 48 King, V., 139, 143, 145, 311 Kohn, M., 410
Kaljee, L., 372 Kelly, B. C., 342, 354 Kingston, B., 113 Kohn-Wood, L. P., 264, 278–279
Kalman, Z., 326–328 Kelly, D., 386 Kinney, D., 158, 160, 162 Koivisto, A.-M., 42, 419
Kalmuss, D., 378 Kelly, F. D., 128 Kinsfogel, K. M., 345 Kokko, K., 409, 438
Kaltiala-Heino, R., 42, 419 Kelly, H., 133 Kinzie, J., 398 Kol, K. L., 393
Kalton, G., 424 Kelly, L. S., 371 Kipke, M., 53, 147 Kolaric, G., 41, 101
Kam, C., 63 Kelly, M., 362 Kipp, K. H., 63 Kolbe, L. J., 48, 369, 372
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-12 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-12 Name Index

Kolburn Kowal, A., 135, 360 Kwak, K., 301 Larson, R., 5, 10, 18, 36, 41, 88, 90, Lejuez, C., 114
Koller, O., 201 Kwon, H., 445, 446f 98, 104, 108, 122, 123f, 126, Lekes, N., 292
Koller, S., 307 Kypri, K., 53, 429 128, 140–141, 152, 152f, 153, LeMare, L., 178, 179
Komro, K. A., 429, 431 157, 158f, 184, 188, 208f, 222, Lemmens, J., 238
Konarski, R., 160, 261, 334, 224, 229, 230, 230f, 232, 233, Lempers, J. D., 135
337, 346 L 236, 237, 239, 250, 259, L’Engle, K. L., 242, 357, 357f
Kong, C., 385 LaBar, K. S., 71 260, 260f, 288, 330, 334, Lengua, L. J., 132, 420
Konijn, E. A., 242, 243 Lachman, M., 123 343, 345, 401 Lenhart, A., 238, 238f, 245
Konrad, K., 69, 438 Lacourse, É., 112, 113, 123, 363 Larson, R. W., 236, 288 Lenhart, L., 78
Koopman, C., 372 Lacourse, R., 440 Latendresse, S. J., 130, 136 Lenox, K., 176, 183
Koot, H., 170–171, 183 Ladd, G., 179, 179f Lau, A., 110 Lenroot, R. K., 68, 136
Koot, H. M., 433, 434 Ladewski, B., 144 Lau, J., 447 Leon, G., 48
Korn, M., 144 Laditka, J., 377 Lau, M., 338 Leonard, G., 300
Kort-Butler, L. A., 222 Laditka, S., 377 Laub, J., 437 Leonard, S., 329
Korzeniewski, S. J., 181 Ladouceur, C. D., 64 Laucht, M., 425 Leone, P., 429
Kosciw, J. G., 367 Ladouceur, D., 69 Lauckhardt, J., 109–110, 307 Lerch, J., 68
Kosterman, R., 126 Laetz, V., 429 Laughlin, J., 298, 332 Lerner, J. V., 235, 236, 236f, 300–301,
Kotchick, B., 360, 449 Laeyendecker, O., 102 Laursen, B., 40, 120–122, 125–127, 332, 393, 397
Kotler, J. A., 449 Lafferty, W. E., 379 169, 286, 287, 289, 291, 324, Lerner, R. M., 5, 198, 206, 233,
Kotler, J. S., 433 LaGrange, B., 447 326–328, 330, 331, 336, 362 235, 236, 236f, 300, 332, 333,
Kotler, L., 243, 243f, 420 La Greca, A. M., 51, 107, 160, 161, Lavoie, F., 345 393, 397
Kotov, R., 433 164, 178, 179, 334, 335, 449 LaVoie, J., 272 Lesane-Brown, C., 275
Koulis, T., 428 Lahey, B. B., 132, 172, 377 Lawford, H., 271, 307–308, 323 Lescano, C., 357
Kowal, A. K., 135, 137 Lahiff, M., 109, 354, 369, 426 Lawler, M., 49, 50f Lesko, N., 18
Kowaleski-Jones, L., 115, 142 Laible, D., 331 Layton, E., 311–312 Leslie, F., 248
Kowaleski-Jones, P., 362 Laird, J., 149, 300 Lazar, N., 57, 63 Lester, J., 173
Kracke, B., 42, 43, 309, 408, 410 Laird, R., 121, 128, 132, 132f, 178, Lazarus, N., 372 Lesthaeghe, R., 91
Kraemer, H. C., 49, 50 290, 440 Leadbeater, B. J., 178, 182, 197, 232, Leung, M.-C., 173, 196
Kraft, P., 444 Laixuthai, A., 430 378, 445 Leve, C., 374
Krahn, H., 92, 93, 93f Lake, M., 146 Leaf, W., 52 Leve, L., 33, 357
Kramer, J. R., 357 Lalonde, R. N., 275 Leahy, E., 389 Leventhal, B. L., 377
Kramer, L., 134, 135, 137 Lam, L. T., 39 Leaper, C., 281, 282, 367 Leventhal, H., 53, 430
Kramer, R. A., 418 Lam, M., 330, 400 Leatherdale, S. T., 46 Leventhal, T., 110–113, 111f, 115,
Krasnovsky, F., 372 Lam, T., 338, 358 Lebel, C., 67 128, 132, 362, 363
Krawchuk, L. L., 387 Lamarche, V., 172 LeBlanc, A., 113 Levesque, R., 341
Kreager, D. A., 80, 169, 172, 174, 233 Lamb, K., 144 LeBlanc, L., 210 Levin, E., 396
Krei, M., 213 Lamb, M., 45, 378 LeBlanc, M., 114, 406 Levin, M., 354, 354f, 360
Kreider, H., 130 Lambert, K., 196 Le Blanc, R., 429 Levine, J. A., 377
Kriepe, R., 48 Lambert, L. E., 197 Lebolt, A., 179 Levine, L., 248
Krier, M., 398 Lambert, M., 46 Ledsky, R., 370 Levine, M., 28, 50, 51, 247
Krishnakumar, A., 142 Lambert, S. F., 114, 203, 276 Lee, A. M., 41, 48 Levine, P., 80
Krishnan-Sarin, S., 423 Lamberty, G., 276 Lee, B., 169 Levinson, R., 372
Kroger, J., 266, 271, 272 Lamborn, S., 142, 171, 209, 275, Lee, C., 225 Levitt, J., 330, 332
Krohn, M. D., 438 291, 294, 393, 396 Lee, D., 45, 108 Levitt, M., 330, 332
Krueger, F., 243 Lamis, D. A., 428 Lee, H., 45, 429 Levitt, S., 191
Krueger, R. F., 136 Lamkin, R., 112 Lee, J., 40, 212, 226, 227, 354, Levran, E., 340
Kruse, M. I., 106 Lammers, C., 361 357, 403 Levy, V., 18
Kubitschek, W., 204 Lan, S., 272 Lee, J. C., 228, 407 Levy-Storms, L., 353, 362
Kuehnle, T., 38 Landale, N. S., 359, 376 Lee, K., 76 Lewin, K., 13f, 15, 16
Kuhn, D., 4, 56, 59, 61–64, 71, 448 Landry, C., 424 Lee, L. M., 372 Lewin, T., 389
Kuhn, E. S., 121 Lane, D. R., 59, 379 Lee, M., 401 Lewin-Bizan, S., 235
Kulin, H., 32 Lang, D., 370, 372 Lee, P., 32 Lewinsky, M., 352
Kulis, S., 275, 276, 282, 429 Lang, F., 123 Lee, S., 81, 82f, 125, 144, 169 Lewinsohn, P. M., 41, 44, 344, 420,
Kumanyika, S. K., 46 Lang, S., 427 Lee, S. Y., 398, 402 442, 444, 446–448
Kumar, A., 239 Langefeld, C., 372 Lee, V., 144, 194–197, 214, 367, 392, Lewis, J., 376
Kumpfer, K. L., 53, 149 Langer, D. A., 114, 448 399, 407 Lewis, K. S., 165, 400–401
Kuncel, N. R., 398 Langer, J. A., 205 Lee, W., 428 Lewis, M., 36, 49, 290, 322, 332, 334
Kung, E., 292 Langer, L., 356–357, 364 Lee, Y., 51 Lewis, M. L., 429
Kunkel, D., 241 Langhout, R. D., 110 Leech, S., 429 Lewis, R., 264, 277, 278
Kunnen, S. E., 123 Langley, J. D., 53, 429 Leech, T. G. J., 46 Lewis, S., 378
Kunz, J., 377 Lansford, J. E., 108, 128, 132, 136, Leen-Feldner, E. W., 43 Lewis, T., 142, 369
Kupanoff, K., 128 143, 175, 256, 357, 393, 401, 439 Leets, L., 77 Li, D., 238, 401, 428
Kupchik, A., 210 Lansu, T. A. M., 175, 176 Leffert, N., 182 Li, J., 58, 273, 276, 278, 401
Kuperminc, G. P., 175, 197, 207, Lanthier, R. P., 332 Lefkowitz, E., 289, 311, 360, 361 Li, K. K., 441
207f, 323, 393, 406, 445 Lanz, M., 127 Lefner, E., 417 Li, S. T., 114, 312
Kupersmidt, J. B., 114, 130, 178, 399 Lanza, S., 260, 281 Legault, L., 389 Li, X., 170, 344, 372, 429
Kupfer, D., 449 Lapsley, D., 18, 88 Legters, N., 193 Li, Y., 141, 145, 198, 206, 233,
Kupper, L. L., 342, 344, 345 Largo, R., 39 Lehman, S. J., 144 393, 397
Kurdek, L., 145, 395, 396 Laris, B. A., 379 Leibenluft, E., 59, 63, 69, 71, Lian, B. E., 114
Kurlychek, M., 95 Larsen, J. K., 172 101, 126 Liang, S., 333
Kurowski, C. O., 134 Larsen-Rife, D., 345 Leiderman, P., 144 Liau, A., 238
Kurtz-Costes, B., 278, 390–392 Larson, J., 12, 125, 152, 154, Leininger, L., 439 Liben, L. S., 412
Kusche, C., 182 156–158, 160, 162–164, 169, Leitch, C., 332 Lichenstein, P., 438
Kuttler, A. F., 160, 161, 334, 335 172, 174, 183, 332 Leitenberg, H., 354 Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A., 123
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-13 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-13

Lictenstein, P., 136 Lord, H., 171, 229, 234 Macmillan, R., 179 Markus, H., 255, 257
Liddle, H. A., 430, 431 Lord, S. E., 147, 197, 393 MacMurray, J. P., 362 Marmorstein, N. R., 420
Lieberman, M., 323 Lorenz, F. O., 128, 142, 145, 146, MacPhail, C., 371 Marold, D., 257
Lieberman, M. D., 59, 256 276, 277f MacPherson, L., 114 Marrero, M., 132, 290
Liefeld, C., 379 Lorion, R., 436 Maczuga, S., 400 Marrocco, F., 181
Lien, L., 418 Lorr, M., 156 Madden, M., 245 Marsh, H., 201, 202, 227, 233, 262,
Light, R., 195–196 Losoya, S. H., 441 Madden, P. A., 141 385, 388
Lightwood, J., 45 Loth, K., 47 Madden-Derdich, D., 329 Marsh, P., 175, 291, 323
Lin, H., 338 Louden, L., 142 Madkour, A. S., 354, 357, 358 Marshall, J., 148
Lin, J., 278 Loughlin, S., 248 Madon, S., 18, 206 Marshall, L., 360
Lin, K., 368 Loughran, T. A., 441 Madsen, S. D., 91, 316f, 316–318, Marshall, S., 275
Lin, W., 240 Louis, K., 198 320, 321, 342 Marshall, S. W., 83
Lin, Y., 275 Loukas, A., 122, 197, 206, 292 Mager, W., 441 Marshall, W., 27f, 31f
Lindberg, L., 352, 353, 355, 369, Lounds Taylor, J., 377 Maggs, J. L., 39, 41, 101, 234, Marsiglia, F. F., 127, 275, 276, 282,
370, 373, 420 Lovato, C. Y., 429 416, 429 426, 429
Lindberg, S. M., 281, 367 Loveless, T., 193, 204 Magnussen, C., 46 Marston, E. G., 132, 321
Lindemeier, L., 36, 40 Low, C., 324, 326–327, 346 Magnusson, A. M., 109, 109f Marta, E., 250
Lindenberg, S., 175, 176, 437 Lowe, K., 205 Magnusson, D., 44 Martel, M. M., 419
Lindentthal, J., 317 Lowe, S., 110, 333 Maguin, E., 112 Martelon, M. K., 428
Linder, G., 367–368 Lowery, B., 436 Magyar, T. M., 235 Marti, C. N., 48
Linder, J. R., 242, 345 Lowery, C., 371 Maharaj, S., 168 Martin, D., 234, 358
Lindsay, L., 142 Lowry, R., 39, 39f, 353, 369–370, 372 Maher, C. A., 46 Martin, J., 129, 260, 373, 447
Lindstrom, R., 331 Lozano, D. I., 438 Mahoney, A., 142 Martin, M., 10, 121, 122, 129, 129f,
Ling, G., 205 Lu, L. H., 63, 65 Mahoney, J., 11, 171, 209, 220, 229, 131, 276–277
Ling, T., 132 Lu, Y., 429 231–235, 291 Martin, M. J., 142
Ling, X., 441 Lubell, K., 246 Maimon, D., 112, 233 Martin, P., 264, 277, 278
Link, B. G., 46 Lubienski, C., 204 Main, M., 322 Martin, S. L., 342, 344, 345
Linn, E., 367 Lubienski, S. T., 204 Makino, K. K., 374 Martinez, A., 264
Linver, M., 50 Lucas, S., 200, 367, 398 Malanchuk, O., 263, 392, 393, 410 Martinez, C. R. J., 276
Linver, M. R., 141, 233 Luce, K. H., 245 Maldonado-Molina, M. M., Martinez, C. R., Jr., 125
Lippold, M., 197 Luciana, M., 63, 65, 68 276, 440 Martínez, M. L., 77, 121, 290
Lipsey, M., 441 Lucky, A., 34 Males, M., 374 Martinez, R., 170, 273
Lipton, R. I., 427 Ludden, A. B., 312, 427 Malone, P. S., 374, 428, 438f, Martino, S. C., 242, 360
Liston, C., 67, 68 Luder, M. T., 357 438–440 Marttunen, M., 42, 419
Litrownik, A. J., 148, 369 Lüdtke, O., 174–175, 201, 393 Malone, S. M., 420 Marx, D., 388
Litt, I. F., 245, 448 Ludwig, J., 111, 436 Malotte, C., 109, 109f Masi, G., 420
Little, K. C., 370 Lun, J., 206, 206f Malti, T., 77 Maslowsky, J., 416
Little, S. A., 198 Luna, B., 57, 63, 68–70 Manchikanti Gomez, A., 345 Mason, C. A., 131, 133, 171, 394,
Little, T., 128, 132 Lundberg, S., 300, 377 Mancini, J. A., 231 396, 397
Little, T. D., 175, 177 Luo, Q., 131, 173 Mandara, J., 140, 263, 275, 402 Mason, D. M., 438
Liu, C., 77 Luo, X.-P., 34 Manera, V., 24 Mason, W., 265, 420
Liu, J., 428, 439 Lupinetti, C., 306 Manhart, L. E., 379 Mason, W. A., 126
Liu, L., 34 Lussier, P., 439–440 Manke, B., 136 Mason-Singh, A., 12, 386
Liu, R. X., 446 Luster, T., 264, 362, 369, 377 Manlove, J., 354, 359–360, 370, 371, Mâsse, L., 172, 273, 441
Liu, X., 265 Luthar, S., 130, 232, 387, 392, 396, 375, 378, 405 Massoglia, M., 441
Lizotte, A. J., 377, 438 402, 429, Mann, E. A., 398 Masten, A. S., 93, 101, 147, 258, 336,
Lleras, C., 400 448, 449 Mann, N. C., 52 339, 342, 420, 449
Lobel, O., 281 Luthar, S. S., 174–175, 418 Mannelli, P., 423 Masten, C. L., 37, 59, 71, 256, 278,
Lobel, T. E., 281 Lutz, A., 395 Mannering, A., 148 300, 321
Lochman, J., 176, 183, 374, 439 Luyckx, K., 132, 270 Mannes, M., 232 Masten, S. V., 83
Loeber, R., 113, 131, 132, 170, 172, Lynam, D. R., 44, 76, 113, 426, Mannheim, K., 13f, 15, 16 Masyn, K., 437
377, 418, 433–437, 439, 441 433, 439 Manning, W. D., 144, 336, 338, 339, Mata, J., 305
Loehlin, J. C., 136 Lynch, J., 292 346, 354, 355, 360, 376, 435 Matas, L., 320
Lynch, P. J., 242 Mannison, M., 343 Mateika, J. H., 38
Loewenson, P. R., 355
Lyndon, S. T., 181 Manson, S., 427 Mathiesen, K. S., 447
Loftus, J., 342, 354
Lynne-Landsman, S. D., 42, 420 Manuck, S., 33 Matjasko, J. L., 221, 222f, 231, 232
Logan, C., 359–360
Lynskey, M., 419, 439 Marcia, J., 269–270 Matricciani, L., 38
Lohman, B. J., 146, 357
Lyons, A., 445 Marcoen, A., 58 Matsuba, M. K., 306–307
Lohr, M. J., 377–378
Lytle, L. A., 244 Marcoulides, G. A., 388 Matsuda, S., 330
Lohrmann, D., 370
Lollis, S., 178, 179 Marcus, D. K., 433 Matsueda, R. L., 80
Lonardo, R. A., 435 M Marczak, M., 124 Matthews, D., 74
London, A. S., 146–147 Marecek, J., 167 Matthews, K., 114
London, S. J., 428 Ma, H.-M., 34 Marelich, W., 372 Matthews, L. S., 134
Loney, B., 433 Ma, J., 46 Margolin, G., 142, 447 Matthews, M., 386
Long, J., 24, 70, 93 Määttä, S., 391 Marin, B. V., 343, 345 Mattison, E., 400
Long, J. D., 161, 161f, 420 Maccoby, E., 128, 129, 136, 140, 141, Mariner, C., 378 Matusko, N., 44
Longest, K. C., 228 143, 145, 166, 328 Marino, R., 107 Matza, L. S., 114, 130
Longmore, M. A., 336, 338, 339, MacDermid, S. M., 301 Markham, C., 338 Maughan, B., 433
346, 354, 355, 360, 435 MacDonald, W., 144 Markiewicz, D., 142, 323, 342 Maumary-Gremaud, A., 377
Lopez, A. B., 310 MacDougall, J., 367–368 Marks, A. K., 279 Maxwell, K., 198
Lopez, C., 260 Mackesy-Amiti, M. E., 360 Marks, H. M., 207–208 Maxwell, S., 260, 377
Lopez, E. M., 170 Mackey, E. R., 51 Markstrom, C., 8, 95, 98, 101, Mayberry, M. L., 215
Lopez, S., 237 Mackey, K., 132 273, 276 Mayers, R., 309
Lopez-Larson, M., 66 MacKinnon-Lewis, C., 171, 275 Markstrom-Adams, C., 258 Mayes, L., 176–177
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-14 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-14 Name Index

Mayeux, L., 175, 176 McLachlan, K., 96 Merrick, S., 322 Millstein, S. G., 79, 80, 372
Mays, D., 232 McLanahan, S., 144, 376, 398 Merskin, D., 40 Millstein, S. S., 364
Mays, G., 169 McLaughlin, B., 210 Merten, D., 176 Milne, B. J., 440
Mays, S., 171 McLaughlin, K. A., 107, 181, 447 Merten, M., 46, 245 Milnitsky-Sapiro, C., 121
Mayseless, O., 131, 323 McLean, K., 266, 272, 316, 330 Mervielda, I., 129 Milot, A., 312
Mazziotta, J., 71 McLellan, J. A., 109–110 Merz, J. L., 432, 439 Minden, J. A., 418
Mbwara, J., 362 McLeroy, K., 360 Mesch, G. S., 166, 246 Minowa, M., 39
Mbzivo, M., 359 McLoyd, V. C., 53, 108, 109, 113, Meschke, L. L., 360 Mirabell, J., 38
McAdoo, H., 264, 276 146, 398 Meseck-Bushey, S., 419 Mischel, W., 387
McAloney, K., 114 McMahon, R., 262, 262f Messer, S., 445 Mistry, R. S., 147, 207, 393, 429
McAuley, E., 244 McMahon, R. J., 420, 433, 439 Messeri, P., 243 Mitchell, C., 263, 427
McAuley, T., 64 McMahon, S., 114, 275, 312 Messersmith, E., 209, 220, 222, 225f, Mitchell, C. M., 358
McBride, C. K., 360 McMahon, T., 396 225–228, 408, 409, 411, 412 Mitchell, E., 51
McBride-Chang, C., 369 McMaster, L., 343, 367 Messner, S., 114 Mitchell, J., 244
McCabe, K., 440 McMorris, B. J., 106, 107f Metselaar, M., 367 Mitchell, K., 143, 181, 246, 247
McCabe, M. P., 48, 49, 51 McNamara, G., 427 Metzger, A., 127 Mitru, G., 38
McCaffree, K., 370, 372 McNamara Barry, C., 316, 316f Metzler, C. W., 181 Miu, A., 242
McCart, M. R., 419 McNaughton Reyes, H. L., 345 Meuss, W., 170–171 Miyake, A., 419
McCartan, C., 114 McNealy, K., 37, 71, 321 Meyer, A., 193 Mizuno, K., 63
McCarthy, G., 71 McNeely, C., 132, 149, 353–355, 360 Meyer, A. L., 420 Mocan, N., 374
McCarthy, J., 209, 378 McNelles, L., 324, 327, 328 Meyer, J. R., 297 Modecki, K. L., 297
McCarthy, K., 147, 197, 332 McNulty, J. K., 370 Mezulis, A. H., 445 Modell, J., 89–90, 105–106, 223
McCarthy, W. J., 429 McNulty, T., 436–437 Michael, A., 392, 393, 410 Moffit, R. A., 146
McCarty, C. A., 448 McQueen, A., 109, 429 Michalska, K., 71 Moffitt, T. E., 44, 72, 75, 113, 258,
McClelland, S. I., 350–352, 356 McQuillan, G., 371 Michaud, P. A., 357, 372 265, 377, 419–421, 434,
McClintock, M., 24, 26, 33, 350 McRae, C., 143 Mickelson, R., 387, 400 437–440, 447
McClure, E., 59, 63, 69, 71, 101, 126 McRee, A-L., 295, 416 Mickus, M., 332, 333 Moghaddam, B., 70
McClure-Tone, E., 71, 445 McRee, N., 42 Middlestadt, S., 48 Mohr, P., 172
McCord, J., 171 McRitchie, S. L., 46, 47f, 244 Midgley, C., 194, 196–199, 387, Moilanen, K. L., 198, 357, 439
McCormick, A., 360 McRoy, R. G., 147 394, 395 Molcho, M., 112
McCormick, S., 39–40 Mead, M., 16–17, 104 Midlarsky, E., 445 Molgaard, V., 276
McCoy, J., 134, 183, 203 Meade, C., 362 Miech, R. A., 46 Molina, B. S. G., 39–40, 428, 438
McCrae, R., 258 Meadows, S. O., 442, 444 Miguel, S. S., 298, 332 Molina, G., 374
McCree, D. J., 312 Meares, H., 398 Mihalic, S., 228 Molinari, L., 39
McCrystal, P., 114 Measelle, J. R., 420 Mikami, A. Y., 206, 206f, 239, Mollen, C. J., 370
McDade, T., 53 Mecklinger, A., 63 245, 246 Molloy, L. E., 260
McDaniel, B., 78, 132, 288, Medeiros, B. L., 131, 143 Miklikowska, M., 132 Molnar, B. E., 115, 367
303–307, 328 Mednick, S., 419, 439 Milam, A., 114 Molock, S., 446
McDill, E., 163 Meece, D., 235 Milan, S., 376 Monahan, K. C., 81, 112, 163, 169,
McDonald, J. A., 354 Meerkerk, G.-J., 246 Milantoni, L., 420 172, 198, 226–228, 239, 242,
McDonald, R., 345 Meeus, W. H. J., 126, 127, 132, 159, Milbrath, C., 338 298, 300, 301, 357, 438–440
McDonald, R. M., 133 172–173, 258, 270–272, 290, Milburn, M., 402 Monck, E., 330
McDuff, P., 345 291, 329, 331, 353, 359, 447 Miles, B., 148 Mond, J., 262
McElhaney, K. B., 12, 133, 134, 175, Meheula, L., 239 Milich, R., 426, 441 Moneta, G., 122, 123f, 233, 259,
176, 286–292, 318, 321–323 Mehta, C., 157, 166 Miliotis, D., 147 260, 260f
McFarland, C., 291, 323 Mehta, T. G., 265 Millepiedi, S., 420 Monk, C., 63, 69
McFarland, F., 175 Meier, A. M., 312, 357 Miller, B., 147, 201, 202, 358, 360, Monroe, S. M., 344, 448
McFarland, F. C., 298, 298f, 323 Meier, M., 113 361, 373, 378 Monsour, A., 257
McGee, R. O., 258 Meijer, A. M., 440 Miller, C. J., 292 Montgomery, M. J., 334, 335, 338,
McGinley, K., 420 Meijer, S., 353, 359 Miller, D., 81 340, 369
McGinley, M., 306 Mekos, D., 137, 231 Miller, E., 344 Montgomery, S., 147
McGloin, J., 172 Melanson, S., 396 Miller, J., 345 Montoro, R., 365
McGlone, M., 388 Melby, J., 171, 393, 399 Miller, J. D., 393, 399 Montplaisir, J., 39
McGue, M., 126, 136, 141, 311, Melde, C., 169, 170 Miller, K., 300, 360 Moody, J., 167, 446
394, 440 Meldrum, R., 181 Miller, K. E., 232 Mooijaart, A., 322
McGuigan, K., 431 Mello, Z. R., 411, 412 Miller, K. S., 362 Moor, B., 26
McGuire, S., 136 Mellor, R., 371 Miller, L., 309, 312, 370 Moore, C., 133
McHale, J. P., 276 Melnick, M. J., 232 Miller, M., 362 Moore, K. A., 132, 231, 354,
McHale, S. M., 37, 124, 127, 128, Memmo, M., 235 Miller, N., 99, 104 359–360, 373, 377, 378
134, 134f, 135, 137, 166, 167, Menaghan, E., 362 Miller, S., 175, 344, 344f, 440 Moore, M., 210
196, 209, 231, 233, 235, 275, Menard, S., 420 Miller, S. R., 292 Moore, M. J., 232
281, 282, 282f, 301, 329, Mendle, J., 33, 34, 41, 42, 44, 357, Miller, T., 428, 429 Moore, M. R., 143
333, 359 358, 361, 429, 447 Miller, W. C., 353, 355, 371 Moore, S., 40, 80, 177, 387
McHugh, S., 135 Mendoza-Denton, R., 181 Miller, X., 376 Moore, W., 71
McIntosh, A., 300 Menning, C. L., 144 Miller-Johnson, S., 75, 374, 377 Moore, W., III, 300
McIsaac, C., 329, 334–344 Menon, M., 282 Miller-Tutzauer, C., 332 Moors, E., 291
McIssac, C., 12 Menson, R., 212 Millrood, D. L., 38 Moos, R., 205
McKay, K., 114 Mercer, D., 430 Mills, B., 79–80 Morales, L. S., 46
McKenney, K., 340 Merchant, M. J., 450 Mills, C., 391 Moran, P., 433
McKenry, P. C., 292 Mercy, J., 53 Mills, C. J., 258 More, D., 238
McKinney, C., 130 Merikangas, K. R., 39, 417, 417f, 442 Mills, K. L., 438 Morgan, A., 112
McKinney, J., 300–301 Merisca, R., 441 Mills, M., 141 Morgan, K., 379
McKinney, K., 43 Merlo, J., 180, 180f Millsap, R. E., 43 Morgan, M., 240, 362
McKnight-Eily, L., 39, 39f Mermelstein, R. J., 442, 445 Millstein, R., 292 Morgan, S. P., 378
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-15 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-15

Morgano, E., 169 Myers, M. W., 440 Neumark-Sztainer, D., 47–51, Nygård, C., 38
Morin, A. J. S., 179 Myint-U, A., 345 247, 262 Nylund, K., 177
Morisi, T., 223 Mylod, D., 377 Newby-Clark, I., 92 Nylund-Gibson, K., 274
Morisky, D. E., 109, 109f, 354 Newcomb, A. F., 183, 333, 340
Morris, A., 12, 17, 78, 122, 132, Newcomb, M., 430
288, 302–307, 328 N Newcomb, M. D., 406 O
Morris, A. K., 57, 62 Nadeem, E., 41, 110 Newman, B., 13, 163 Oakes, J., 199, 200
Morris, J. E., 235 Nader, P. R., 46, 47f, 244 Newman, D., 40 Oakley, D., 370
Morris, M. C., 448 Nagaoka, J., 397 Newman, D. L., 273 Obama, B., 191–192, 388
Morris, N., 30f, 339 Nagin, D., 131, 438, 440 Newman, J. P., 440 Obando, P., 114, 312
Morrison, D. M., 362, 371, Nagin, D. S., 227, 228 Newman, K., 228 Obeidallah, D., 44, 310, 312
377, 378 Nagoshi, J. L., 127, 426 Newman, P., 13, 163 Oberlander, S. E., 369
Morrison, G. M., 298, 332 Nagy, G., 393 Newman, R., 390 Obradović, J., 93, 420
Morrison Gutman, L., 115, 133 Nagy, S., 369 Newmann, F., 207–208 O’Brennan, L. M., 181
Morrissey-Kane, E., 53 Najman, J. M., 439 Newton, M., 235 O’Brien, K., 371
Morrow, B., 355, 370 Namerow, P., 378 Ng-Mak, D., 113, 114, 312 O’Brien, L., 81–82, 295, 295f,
Morrow, C., 326 Nangle, D. W., 340 Nguyen, D. T., 142 299, 299f
Morrow, J., 50 Nansel, T. R., 47, 179, 181, 234 Nguyên, H., 275, 278 O’Brien, M., 46, 47f, 135, 244
Mortimer, J. T., 7, 91, 108, Nappo, S. A., 429 Nguyen, H. H., 275 O’Callaghan, M., 377
225–228, 301 Narasimham, G., 57 Nguyen, J., 273 Ochner, C., 48, 49, 51
Mory, M., 158, 160 Nardos, B., 65, 69, 70f Niccolai, L., 376 O’Connell, A., 379
Moscicki, A., 243 Narkunas, J., 372 NICHD Early Child Care Research O’Connell, D., 420
Moses, A., 436 Narvaez, D., 303, 305 Network, 33, 343, 440 O’Connor, T., 129, 141, 323
Mosher, W., 372–374 Nash, K., 225 Nichols, S., 389 O’Donnell, D., 114
Moshman, D., 97 Nasrallah, N. A., 425 Nicolson, N. A., 37 O’Donnell, L., 345
Mosteller, F., 195–196 Nass, C., 239 Nicotra, E., 176 O’Donnell, M., 409
Motl, R. W., 244 Nation, M., 53, 112, 132 Niemelä, S., 438 Oehmke, J., 181
Mott, F. L., 161, 362, 377 National Center for Children in Nieri, T., 429 Oelsner, J., 197
Moua, M., 275 Poverty, 138, 138f Nieto, M., 203 Oeltmann, J., 358
Mount, K. K., 416 National Center for Education Nigg, J. T., 419, 428 Oesterle, S., 336, 337f, 430
Mounts, N., 132, 165, 170–172, 300, Statistics, 102, 189, 193, 204, Nije Bijvank, M., 242 Oetting, E. R., 399
336, 393, 395, 396 211, 211f, 212, 212f, 226, 402, Nishina, A., 28, 177, 179, 181, 203, Oettinger, G., 227
Mouw, T., 167 403, 405, 405f 209, 274 Office of Juvenile Justice and
Mrug, S., 114, 134, 334 National Center for Health Nix, R. L., 358 Delinquency Prevention,
Mucci, L. A., 345 Statistics, 436 Nixon, C. L., 178 434–435, 435f
Mucci, M., 420 National Coalition for the Nixon, E., 49, 50f Ofner, S., 372
Mudgal, J., 354 Homeless, 147 Noack, P., 222, 309 Ogawa, J. R., 322
Mueller, A. S., 169 National Heart, Lung, Blood Insti- Noakes, M. A., 327 Ogbu, J., 165, 400
Muhleman, D., 362 tute Growth and Health Study Nock, M. K., 175, 446 Ogden, C., 45
Muhlenbruck, L., 215 Research Group, 53 Nock, S., 375 Ogle, J. P., 48
Muise, A. M., 49 National Institute of Child Health Nofziger, S., 429 Ogletree, M. D., 40
Mukherjee, P., 167 and Human Development Noguera, P., 209 Ohannessian, C., 332
Mulhall, P., 147, 205, 398, 399 Early Child Care Research Net- Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 444, 445, 447 O’Hare, E. D., 63, 65
Mulkeen, P., 329, 330 work, 33, 343, 440 Noll, J., 33 Ohida, T., 39
Muller, C., 141, 169, 395 National Research Council (NRC), Nolle, K., 209 Ohlson, B., 338
Muller, P. A., 398 18, 46, 47, 52, 78, 104, 188, 224, Noller, P., 330 Ojanen, T., 264
Mulvey, E. P., 210, 298, 300, 437, 225, 227, 364, 365, 421 Norberg, K. E., 430 Okazaki, S., 272, 400
438, 441 Natriello, G., 215 Nord, C., 377, 378 O’Keefe, G., 246
Mulvihill, B. A., 377 Natsuaki, M. N., 32, 33, 42, 342, Northstone, K., 34 Okeke-Adeyanju, N., 390–392
Mun, E. Y., 420 444, 445 Norton, E., 175 O’Koon, J., 445
Munfakh, J., 246 Nava, M., 275 Norton, M., 358, 361 Olatunji, A. N., 228
Munsch, J., 333 Neal, J., 175 Norton, R., 79 Oldehinkel, A. J., 327, 445
Munson, S., 45, 46 Neblett, E., Jr., 108, 275, 278 Nosko, A., 323 Olds, T., 38, 46
Murakami, J., 446 Neckerman, H., 173 Notaro, P. C., 110, 142 O’Leary, K. D., 345
Murayama, K., 388 Necos, B., 379 Noto, A. R., 429 Oliveira, C. M., 373–374
Murdock, T., 196, 198, 396, 402 Needham, B., 46, 169, 173 Nottelmann, E., 421 Oliver, P. H., 388
Murphy, B. C., 306, 307 Needham, B. L., 367 Nottelmann, E. D., 31 Olmsted, M., 50
Murray, C., 393 Neemann, J., 147, 336, 339, 342 Noussair, C., 177 O’Loughlin, J., 46, 428
Murray, C. B., 140 Negriff, S., 41–44, 439 Novaira, H., 23 Olsen, J., 132, 197, 332
Murray, D., 48 Neiderhiser, J. M., 32, 136, 137, 445 Novak, J. D., 130 Olson, L., 228, 407
Murray, L., 110 Neidert, L., 91 Novak, S. P., 53, 113, 429 Olsson, C., 444
Murray, N., 265 Neighbors, B., 142 Nov-Krispin, N., 281 Olweus, D., 179
Murrelle, L., 114, 312 Nelemans, S., 11, 260, 262 Nowlin, P., 393 O’Malley, P. M., 92, 94f, 227, 228,
Murry, V. M., 42, 44, 131, Nelson, C. A., 67, 71 Ntoumanis, N., 233 234, 416, 422–424, 423f, 424f,
276, 277f, 278, 428, Nelson, E., 59, 63, 69, 71, 101, Nucci, L., 121, 122 426–430, 440
429f, 441 126, 445 Nucci, M. S., 121, 122 Oman, R. F., 360
Musick, J., 378 Nelson, I. A., 222 Nuñez, A., 212 Onatsu, T., 387
Musleh, K., 131 Nelson, L. J., 91, 271, 311, 316, 316f Nuno, V. L., 206 O’Neal, K. K., 112, 175
Must, A., 32 Nelson, S., 132, 171 Nurius, P., 255 O’Nell, T., 427
Mustanski, B. S., 32 Nelson-Mmari, K., 52 Nurmi, J.-E., 101, 255, 271, 272, Ong, A. D., 125
Mustard, C., 111 Nelwan, P., 330 387, 391, 393 Ostergaard, J., 429
Mustillo, S. A., 342 Ness, A., 34 Nurnberger, J., 357 Opaleye, E. S., 429
Myers, D., 377, 378 Neubauer, S. A., 161 Nussbaum, K. M., 114, 312 Op de Macks, Z., 26
Myers, J., 317 Neuenschwander, M., 408 Nuutila, A., 438 Openshaw, A., 243
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-16 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-16 Name Index

Oppedal, B., 277 Padmanabhan, A., 70 Paulozzi, L., 210 Pettit, G. S., 76, 113, 128, 132, 132f,
Oransky, M., 167 Pagani, L. S., 171, 179 Paus, T., 65–68, 70, 70f, 300 143, 178, 235, 346, 357, 361,
Orbell, S., 370, 371 Pagani-Kurtz, L., 441 Paxton, S. J., 49–50 393, 438–440
Orbuch, T. L., 126 Page, M., 109 Payne, A., 207 Pettit, J., 442
Orcutt, H. K., 419 Page-Gould, E., 181 Pea, R., 239 Petty, C., 428
Ordaz, S., 70 Pahl, K., 167, 168f, 206, 274, Pearce, M. J., 114, 131 Pevalin, D. J., 444
Orduña, M., 276 277, 278 Pearce, N., 236, 288 Pexman, P., 60
Organization for Economic Coop- Paikoff, R. L., 35, 37, 39–40, 48, 351, Pearl, R., 175 Peyser, H., 143
eration and Development 360, 379 Pears, K., 129, 374 Pfefferbaum, B., 107
(OECD), 403, 404f Pajer, K., 34, 439 Pechmann, C., 248 Pfeifer, J. H., 37, 59, 71, 126, 255,
Orlando, M., 425 Palardy, G., 204, 214 Peckins, M., 439 256, 300, 321
Ormel, J., 112, 175, 327, 426, Palen, L., 250 Pedersen, N. L., 136 Phares, V., 48
437, 445 Paley, B., 134 Pedersen, S., 107, 161, 166, 166f, Phelan, J. C., 46
Ormrod, R. K., 181 Palladino, G., 90 176, 178, 222, 334 Phelan, P., 203
Orpinas, P., 344, 344f Pallas, A., 215 Pederson, S., 264 Phelps, E., 233, 235, 236, 236f
Orr, D., 352, 372, 419 Pallock, L., 142 Peebles, R., 245 Philibert, R., 428, 429f, 441
Orr, M., 369, 373, 379 Palmer, P., 81 Peeke, L., 260, 265 Philip, C., 278
Ortega, L. A. G., 149 Palmgren, C., 79 Peeters, M., 175 Philippe, F. L., 292
Orth, U., 259, 259f, 263–264 Pampel, F. C., 424 Pehlivantürk, B., 323 Philips, K. J., 178–179
Ortiz, J., 419 Pan, W., 428 Pekrun, R., 393 Philliber, S., 406
Orwin, R., 424 Pancer, S. M., 307–308 Pelham, W. E., 438 Phillips, M. L., 37, 115
Osaki, Y., 39 Panchaud, C., 371 Pelkonen, M., 419 Phillips, P. E., 244, 425
Osborne, L. N., 110 Pantin, H., 271, 276 Pellegrini, A. D., 161, 161f, 333 Phillipsen, L. C., 324
Osborne, M. I., 245 Papadakis, A. A., 445 Pellerin, L. A., 206 Phinney, J., 125, 273, 275
Oser, F., 58 Papadakis, S., 46 Pelletier, L., 389 Piacentini, J., 449
Osgerby, B., 90, 220, 248 Papandonatos, G., 24, 70, 177 Pelphrey, K., 176–177 Piaget, J., 13f, 14, 61–62, 302–304
Osgood, D. W., 7, 91, 92f, 108, 127, Papillo, A. R., 231, 378 Peltzer, J., 69 Pianta, R. C., 206, 206f, 398
134f, 135, 137, 172–173, 224, Paradis, G., 428 Pemberton, S., 142 Pickering, S. J., 64
227, 233–235, 234f, 260, 281, Pardini, D. A., 132, 170, 440 Pena, C., 428 Pidada, S., 177, 330
282, 309, 309f, 411, 440 Pardun, C. J., 242 Peña-Alampay, L., 132 Piehler, T., 157, 171
Oshri, A., 420 Parente, M., 234 Penn, J., 69 Pieper, C., 53
Oslak, S., 49, 345 Parfenoff, S., 360 Penner, E., 96 Pierce, J. P., 39, 427
Osofsky, J., 107 Parides, M., 50 Pentz, M. A., 357, 358 Pierce, L., 419
Osswald, K., 300 Park, A., 428 Pepler, D. J., 181, 181f, 246, Piesse, A., 424
Ostapenko, L., 69 Parke, R. D., 292 289, 329, 334, 339f, Pietiläinen, K. H., 32
Ostaszewski, K., 428 Parker, A., 79 339–341, 367 Pihl, R., 441
Ostby, Y., 67, 68f Parker, J., 176, 183, 317, 324, Pepper, M., 317 Pike, A., 136
O’Sullivan, L. F., 339, 353 326–327, 346 Percy, A., 114 Piko, B., 181
Oswald, H., 364 Parker, K. F., 102, 103, 103f, 107, 110 Perez-Edgar, K., 129 Pilgrim, C., 131, 172
Oswald, T., 71, 300 Parker, S., 246 Perez-Febles, A., 376, 429 Pilla, R. S., 179
Otsuki-Clutter, M., 133, 273, 275, Parker, W. D., 258 Perilla, J., 393 Pillay, Y., 275
276, 279 Parra, C. E., 69 Perkins, D., 112, 232, 362, 369 Pilowsky, D. J., 425
Ott, M. A., 372 Parrish, J., 101 Perkins, D. D., 205 Pimentel, E., 225
Ott, M. Q., 365 Parsai, M., 127, 426, 429 Perkins, S. A., 122, 290, 302 Pinderhughes, E. E., 148, 439
Otterblad Olausson, P., 377 Parsons, S., 109, 398 Perreira, K., 273 Pine, D. S., 59, 63, 69, 71, 126, 129,
Ouimet, M., 81, 82f Parvanta, S., 245 Perron, M., 300 437, 445, 449
Overbaugh, K., 233 Pascal, S., 179 Perry, A. A., 50 Pineda, A. Q., 447
Overbeek, G., 246, 322, 341 Pascarella, E., 272 Perry, C., 131, 427, 431 Pinney, S. M., 34
Overgaauw, S., 26 Pasch, L. A., 142, 345 Perry, C. L., 48 Pinquart, M., 291–292
Overpeck, M. D., 179, 234 Paschall, M. J., 228, 427 Perry, D., 179, 264, 265, 280, 283 Pintrich, P., 390, 391
Overton, W., 62 Paskewitz, D., 449 Perry, G., 39, 39f Pipes McAdoo, H., 113
Owen, J., 208, 405 Pasley, K., 124 Perry, R. P., 450 Piquero, A. R., 433, 436, 437,
Owen, L., 129, 173, 359 Pastore, M., 112, 132, 169 Perry, T., 324, 326 439–441
Owen-Kostelnik, J., 297 Pastorelli, C., 389 Persson, A., 231 Pirie, P., 431
Owens, A., 203 Patall, E. A., 390 Peskin, H., 43 Pirsch, L., 292
Owens, T., 301 Pate, R., 244 Peter, J., 238, 245, 246, 328 Pitner, R. O., 168
Oxford, M. L., 377–378 Patel, M., 429 Petersen, A., 41–43, 53, 450 Pitskel, N., 176–177
Oyserman, D., 257, 275, 378, 400 Pateman, B., 353 Pittard, W., 377
Petersen, A. C., 263
Ozcan, M., 227 Paternoster, R., 227, 228, 406 Pittet, I., 357
Petersen, J. H., 34
Ozer, E. J., 52, 53, 369, 371–373 Pati, C., 131 Pittman, L. D., 131, 146
Petersen, S., 69
Patkar, A. A., 423 Peterson, C., 386 Planty, M., 308
P Patrick, D. L., 416 Peterson, D., 170 Plata, M., 393
Patrick, H., 206 Peterson, G. W., 133 Platsidou, M., 63
Paasch, K., 197, 204, 395, 398 Patrick, M. E., 426 Peterson, J., 53 Plomin, R., 129, 136, 137, 141, 438
Pabon, E., 170 Patrikakou, E., 392 Peterson, J. L., 167, 278, 378 Plotnick, R., 377
Paciello, M., 306, 345 Patten, C. A., 39 Peterson, P., 204, 430 Plumert, J., 64
Pack, R., 170 Patterson, C. J., 147–148, 178, Peterson-Badali, M., 292 Plunkett, S. W., 275
Packaged Facts, 248 366, 399 Petit, D., 39 Pogarsky, G., 377
Padgett, J., 263 Patterson, G. R., 123, 169, 170, 437, Petraitis, J., 428, 429 Poll, P., 420
Padilla, A. M., 401 438, 440, 441 Petrie, C., 210 Pollack, C., 39
Padilla, B., 265 Patton, F., 279 Petropoulos, L., 114 Pollack, H., 377
Padilla-Walker, L. M., 122, 242, 306, Patton, G. C., 207, 444 Petrovich, J., 49 Pollack, S., 69
306f, 332 Patton, W., 343 Pettifor, A., 371 Pomerantz, E. M., 292, 301, 388,
Padilla-Walker, L. P., 146 Pauletti, R., 280 Pettingell, S., 362 389, 393
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-17 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-17

Pomery, E., 113, 130 Putnam, F., 33, 369 Ready, D. D., 194, 196 Richardson, B., 53
Pong, S., 141, 392, 393, 395, 405 Puttler, L. I., 419, 428 Reagan, P., 34 Richardson, F., 73
Poorthuis, A., 11, 260, 262 Ream, G., 361, 366 Richardson, J., 142
Porfeli, E., 7, 91 Ream, R., 395 Richardson, R., 377
Poropat, A. E., 387 Q Reardon, S. F., 53, 429 Richman, S., 402
Porter, M. R., 175, 291, 298, Rebellon, C. J., 440 Ricken, J., 38
298f, 323 Qi, X. Y., 446 Reckdenwald, A., 110 Rickman, A., 5, 10, 18, 88, 90, 98,
Porter, P., 438 Qian, M., 447 Redden, D. T., 377 104, 108
Porter, S., 433 Qian, Z., 400 Reddy, R., 39, 197, 205 Ridenour, T., 420
Post, L. A., 181 Qin, D., 125, 167, 273 Redlich, A., 96, 298, 299f Rideout, V., 238, 239, 244
Poteat, P. V., 367 Qin, L., 301
Reed, C., 372 Riedel, A. A., 53
Poulin, F., 161, 166, 166f, 169, Qouta, S., 131
Reed, J., 276 Riegle-Crumb, C., 169, 395
171–173, 176, 334 Quadrel, M., 59, 79
Rees, D. I., 370 Rierdan, J., 41
Poulos, G., 393 Quatman, T., 301, 389
Rees, H., 371 Riggs, L., 379
Poulsen, C., 300 Queiroz, I., 373–374
Rees, J., 48 Rigney, G., 38
Poulton, R., 265 Quillian, L., 167, 168
Reese, E., 63 Rigon, F., 35
Power, J., 65, 69, 70f Quinn, T., 102, 440
Reese-Weber, M., 134 Rigsby, L., 163
Quintana, S., 275
Power, T., 262, 298, 427 Reeslund, K. L., 449–450 Rijsdijk, F. V., 438
Quittner, A., 52
Powers, B., 447 Regnier, M., 308 Riley, J. R., 420
Powers, J., 368 Reich, K., 58 Rimpelä, A., 38
Powers, J. L., 245 R Reich, S., 245, 325, 325f Rinaldi, C. M., 327
Powers, S. I., 445 Reichhardt, T., 242 Rincón, C., 179, 321, 344
Pozen, D., 169 Raabe, T., 167, 168 Reid, L., 377 Rind, B., 369
Prado, G., 271, 276 Raaijmakers, Q., 258, 271 Reijneveld, S. A., 112, 426 Rinehart, P., 149
Pramstaller, P., 38 Rabin, B., 449 Reijntjes, A., 307 Rinelli, L., 144
Pratt, M. W., 132, 271, 272, Rabiner, D., 78 Reinders, H., 308 Ringback Weitoft, G., 377
307–308, 323 Racine, Y., 111, 426 Reiner, S., 436 Ringwalt, C. L., 423, 430
Prelow, H., 115 Radin, N., 378 Reiser, L., 41 Ripperger-Suhler, K. G., 206, 292
President’s Science Advisory Radmacher, K., 327, 328 Reiss, D., 32, 133, 134, 136–138, 140, Risch, S., 145
Committee, 191 Radovick, S., 23 144–145, 445 Rissanen, A., 32
Presley-Cantrell, L., 39, 39f Raeikkoenen, K., 421 Reiter, E., 34, 40, 42 Risser, W., 265
Presnell, K., 48 Raffaelli, M., 312, 324, 327, 331, Remer, L. G., 429 Ritakallio, M., 419
Presson, C., 429 332, 357, 358, 360 Renaud, O., 256 Ritter, P., 144, 393
Preusser, D., 52 Ragsdale, B. L., 263, 275 Rende, R., 429 Rivara, F., 377
Pribesh, S., 207 Rahdar, A., 181 Renk, K., 130 Rivas-Drake, D., 275–278, 278f
Price, C. D., 233 Raichle, M. E., 438 Repetti, R., 132 Rivera, A. C., 222
Price, C. L., 200 Raine, A., 419, 433, 438, 439 Repetto, P. B., 418 Rivera, W., 170
Price, J. M., 346 Rainie, L., 245 Repinski, D., 182 Rivers, S., 80
Price, M. N., 357 Raj, A., 345 Reppucci, N. D., 97, 110, 296, 297, 436 Rizza, M. G., 196
Priess, H., 280, 281 Raj, M., 339 Reshef-Kfir, Y., 80 Roach, M., 379
Prince, R. P., 445 Raley, R. K., 339 Resko, S. M., 429 Roalson, L., 197
Prindiville, S., 260 Ram, N., 260 Resnick, H., 428 Robbins, C., 375
Prinstein, M. J., 164, 165, 172, Ramanan, J., 234 Resnick, M. D., 48, 52, 353, 355, 361, Roberts, A., 264
175, 178, 299, 300, 327, Ramchand, R., 227 374, 449 Roberts, A. L., 115
334, 335, 343, 362, Ramey, C., 75, 398 Rest, J., 303, 305 Roberts, B., 258, 409
446, 447 Ramirez, C., 409 Restrepo, A., 141 Roberts, C., 273, 444
Prinzie, P., 293–294 Ramirez, M., 114, 147, 312, 449 Retty, R., 377 Roberts, C. R., 39
Proctor, L., 148 Ramirez-Valles, J., 113 Reuman, D., 197, 201 Roberts, D., 142, 147, 220, 237–239,
Proescher, E., 449 Ramos, J., 311, 312 Reyes, B. T., 135, 264 241–244, 247, 350, 387, 400
Prom, E., 114, 312 Rampey, B. D., 403 Reyes, O., 197, 398–399, 402 Roberts, M., 264, 276, 277f
Pronk, R., 177 Ramsden, S., 73 Reyna, V., 79–80, 82 Roberts, R. E., 39, 273, 444, 447
Prosser, E., 263 Rance, M., 239 Reynolds, A., 398 Robertson, A., 107
Prossinger, H., 359 Rancourt, D., 172 Reynolds, B. M., 43, 295 Robertson, D. L., 398
Provost, M., 450 Randall, B. A., 307 Reynolds, C., 438 Robertson, L., 53
Pruessner, J., 69 Randall, E. T., 232 Reynolds, E., 114 Robins, G., 172
Public Agenda, 208, 405 Randel, B., 390 Reynolds, J. R., 397 Robins, L., 420
Puddy, R. W., 149 Randel, J., 344 Reynolds, L., 445 Robins, R., 258, 259, 265, 409, 419
Puelo, C., 417 Rangel, M. C., 372 Rhee, S. H., 418 Robinson, M. G., 205
Pugatch, D., 357 Rankin, J., 59 Rhodes, J. E., 39, 110, 197, 205, 333, Robinson, N., 263, 264, 447
Puhse, U., 232 Rankin, L. A., 101 378, 402 Robinson, T., 48
Pulaski, S., 243 Rao, P., 173 Rianasari, M., 330 Robinson, W. L., 170
Pulgiano, D., 115 Rapee, R. M., 51, 447 Ricard, R., 328 Roche, A., 32
Pulkkinen, L., 32, 42, 136, 137f, 377, Raphaelson, Y. E., 114 Ricardo, I., 361, 372 Roche, K. M., 113, 128, 132, 228
409, 438 Rasanen, M., 32 Ricciardelli, L. A., 49, 51 Roche, L., 201
Pullman, A. W., 429 Raskauskas, J., 181 Rice, F., 136 Rodas, C., 143
Punamäki, R., 38, 131 Ratelle, C., 393 Rice, K., 329, 330 Roderick, M., 397, 407
Pungello, E., 75, 399 Rauch, K., 156 Rich, L. M., 227 Rodgers, J. L., 132, 362, 419
Purcell, A., 370 Raudenbush, S. W., 53, 112, 429 Rich, M., 421 Rodgers, K., 141, 262
Purcell, K., 245 Rauh, V., 379 Rich, Y., 412 Rodine, S., 360
Purcell, S., 136, 137f Ravens-Sieberer, U., 179 Richard, P., 173 Rodkin, P., 175
Purdie, N., 389 Ravitch, D., 191, 214 Richards, M. H., 41, 42, 50, 114, 122, Rodriguez, A. U., 146
Purtell, K. M., 108, 109, 113, 146 Rawls, A., 212 123f, 126, 128, 152, 152f, 157, Rodriguez, N., 436
Purwono, U., 311, 312 Raymond, M., 144 158f, 208f, 230, 230f, 259, 260, Rodriguez, O., 170
Putallaz, M., 331 Raynor, S. R., 142 260f, 263, 275, 312, 330, 334 Roe, K., 239
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-18 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-18 Name Index

Roehlkepartain, E., 235 Rowe, R., 433 Saltzman, L. E., 368 Schleepen, T., 63
Roelofsma, P., 243 Rowen, J., 138 Salzer Burks, V., 439 Schlegel, A., 5, 87, 98
Roenkae, A., 377 Rowley, S. J., 145, 278, 390–392 Salzinger, S., 113, 114, 312 Schlenger, W. E., 227, 425
Roenneberg, T., 38 Roye, C., 378 Sam, D. L., 277 Schmeelk-Cone, K. H., 264,
Roesch, L., 445 Rozin, P., 48 Sambrano, S., 428 278–279
Roesch, R., 96 Ruan, W., 179 Samela-Aro, K., 6 Schmid, C., 326
Roesch, S., 148 Rubia, K., 69 Sameroff, A., 147, 392, 393, 410 Schmidt, J. A., 207, 265, 308, 400,
Roesch, S. C., 306, 307 Rubin, K., 129, 178, 179, 317, 401 Sameroff, A. J., 115, 133 403, 405
Roeser, R., 189, 194, 197–199, 205, Ruchkin, V., 114, 131 Sampson, R., 111, 112, 436, 437 Schmidt, M. G., 296
207, 214–215, 310, 312, 318, Ruck, M., 77, 78, 292, 308 Sánchez, B., 110 Schmidt, M. H., 425
388, 391–393, 400 Rudolph, K. D., 179, 179f, 322, 417, Sanchez, Z. M., 429 Schmitz, M. F., 127
Roettger, M. E., 438 445, 447, 448 Sanders, L., 142 Schneider, B., 200, 232, 409, 412
Rogers, I., 34 Rudy, D., 133 Sandfort, J., 378 Schneiders, J., 37
Rogers, J., 374 Rudy, W., 211–212 Sandfort, T. G., 366, 367 Schnurr, P., 428
Rogers, M., 142 Rueter, M., 126, 445, 446f Sandi, L., 114, 312 Schochet, T., 424
Rogers-Sirin, L., 393 Ruggiero, K. J., 419 Sands, T., 275 Schoenmakers, T., 238
Rogosch, F. A., 420, 447 Ruggles, S., 142 Sandstrom, M. J., 175, 176 Schoeny, M. E., 359, 362
Rohde, P., 48, 344, 420, 444, Ruiz, S., 133 Sandy, J. M., 428 Schofield, H. T., 358
446–449 Rumbaut, R., 5, 6, 88, 276 Santelli, J. S., 113, 352, 353, 355, Schofield, J., 166
Rohrbach, L., 429 Rumberger, R., 204, 214, 393, 395, 369–370, 373, 379 Schofield, T. J., 142
Roiser, J., 300 405–407 Santesso, D. L., 68 Scholte, R. H. J., 172, 175, 179,
Roisman, G. I., 93, 101, 228, 343, Rusby, J. C., 181 Santinello, M., 112, 132, 205 246, 329
420, 439–441 Russell, S. T., 128, 197, 365, 367, Santor, D., 433 Schommer, M., 58
Rombouts, S. A. R. B., 63, 69, 375, 377 Saporito, S., 204 Schonfeld, I. S., 181
70, 295 Russell, T., 228 Sargent, J., 300 Schooler, D., 241, 257
Rome, E., 421 Ruth, G., 7, 91 Sargent, J. D., 243 Schoon, I., 109, 398
Romer, D., 72, 81, 248, 370, Ruthig, J. C., 450 Sarraj, E., 131 Schraedley, P., 448
372, 419 Rutter, M., 129 Sarwer, D. B., 48 Schranz, N., 46
Romero, A. J., 273, 276 Ruzic, L., 69 Sasabe, T., 63 Schreiber, G., 262, 262f
Romero, I., 275 Ryabov, I., 398–400 Saucier, J., 439 Schrepferman, L., 437
Romero, S., 52 Ryan, A. M., 159, 183, 206 Saudino, K., 136 Schubert, C., 32
Romich, J., 300 Ryan, C., 367 Saunders, B. E., 419, 428 Schubert, C. A., 441
Romo, L. F., 360, 361 Ryan, G., 210 Saunders, J., 389, 405 Schulenberg, J. E., 92, 94f, 209, 220,
Rong, X., 401 Ryan, J., 148 Saunders, T., 244 222, 225f, 225–228, 408, 409,
Rook, K., 332 Ryan, N. D., 37, 447 Savage, M., 28 411, 412, 416, 422–424, 423f,
Roosa, M. W., 43, 125, 133, 146, Ryan, R., 292 Savin-Williams, R. C., 12, 183, 260, 424f, 426, 428, 429
278, 420 Ryan, S., 370, 371 317, 325, 341, 350–356, 358, Schuler, R., 36
Rosario, M., 113, 114, 312, 365 Rφysamb, E., 277, 447 359, 361, 363–366, 368 Schultz, L., 39
Roscigno, V., 394, 399 Ryu, S., 225 Savolainen, J., 420 Schuster, M. A., 46, 181, 234,
Rose, A. J., 174, 178, 317, 325, 327, Sawalani, G. M., 177 358, 360
339, 411f Sawilowsky, S. S., 445 Schutz, R., 27–28
Rose, C., 278
S Sawyer, A. L., 181 Schvaneveldt, P., 361
Rose, H. A., 141 Saarni, L., 38 Sawyer, R., 370 Schwab-Stone, M. E., 114, 131,
Rose, J., 429 Sabatelli, R., 292 Saxon, J. L., 389 307, 312
Rose, R. J., 32, 42, 43, 44f, 136, Sabiston, C. M., 46, 429 Saylor, K., 179, 181 Schwager, M., 390
137f, 448 Sabo, D., 232 Scabini, E., 127 Schwartz, J., 435
Rose, S., 74 Sabuwalla, Z., 24, 425, 448 Scales, P., 232, 235 Schwartz, O., 124
Rosen, L., 181, 238 Sacchitelle, C., 129 Scanlan, L., 233 Schwartz, S., 170–171,
Rosenbaum, J. E., 213, 363, 370, 397 Sachs, J., 195–196 Scanlan, T., 233 271–272, 276
Rosenberg, F., 260 Sacker, A., 109, 398 Scaramella, L., 135, 170 Schwartz, S. E. O., 110
Rosenberg, M., 260, 261, 264 Sackett, P. R., 398 Scarr, S., 275 Schwartz, S. H., 121
Rosenbloom, S. R., 207, 209–210 Sadek, S., 181 Schachar, R., 69 Schwartz, S. J., 270, 271
Rosenblum, G., 36, 49 Saenz, D., 125, 278 Schaefer, D. R., 233 Schwartz-Mette, R., 327
Rosenfield, D., 345 Saewyc, E. M., 365–367 Schaefer-Rohleder, L., 107 Schwarz, D. F., 370
Rosenthal, D., 301, 353, 358, Safer, D. J., 446 Schaible, L. M., 113 Schweder, A. E., 235, 291
360, 364 Saffer, H., 424, 430 Schainker, L. M., 420 Schweder, R., 93
Rosenthal, N. L., 323 Safron, J., 227, 228 Schallert, D. L., 388 Schweingruber, H. A., 133, 389
Rosenthal, S., 322 Sagar, H., 166 Scharf, M., 131, 134, 323, 338 Scott, E., 86, 87, 96, 97, 97f, 296,
Roseth, C. J., 205 Sagatun, A., 418 Schebendach, J., 48 296f, 297, 433, 441
Rostosky, S. S., 367 Sagrestano, L., 39–40 Scheidt, P., 179 Scott, L., 28
Rote, W., 76, 317 Salafia, E., 51 Scheier, L., 265, 426, 427 Scott, L. D., 450
Roth, J., 23f Salafia, E. B., 290 Schelar, E., 371 Scott, M., 143
Roth, J. L., 110, 232, 233 Salazar, C. F., 393 Schellenbach, C., 377 Scottham, K., 274
Roth, T., 39 Salazar, L., 243 Schepis, T. S., 423 Scovill, L. C., 18, 416
Rothen, N., 38 Salazar, L. F., 370, 372 Scherf, K., 71 Seal, J., 183
Rotheram-Borus, M., 109, 372 Sale, E., 428 Schiamberg, L. B., 181 Sears, H., 41, 101
Rotherman-Borus, M., 109f Salekin, R. T., 433 Schiefelbein, V. L., 39 Seaton, E. K., 146, 203, 273, 274,
Rothman, E. F., 344 Salem, D., 142 Schiff, A., 38 276–278, 277f
Rouse, C. E., 398 Sales, L., 329 Schiller, D., 281 Sebastian, C., 59, 71, 76, 255, 300
Rousseau, J.-J., 17 Salinger, J. D., 268 Schiller, K., 198 Secker-Walker, R., 431
Rovner, A., 47 Sallquist, J., 312 Schiller, K. S., 169, 200 Seegers, G., 389
Rowe, C. L., 107, 430, 431 Salmivalli, C., 179 Schilling, E. A., 427, 446 Seegers, V., 39
Rowe, D., 136, 258, 264, 362, 419, Salsberry, P., 34 Schindler, H. S., 128, 131, 132 Seeley, J. R., 41, 44, 344, 420, 442,
430, 444 Saltzman, H., 354 Schlaggar, B., 69 444, 446–448
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-19 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-19

Seepersad, S., 222 Sheridan, M., 63 Simons, R. L., 28, 42–44, 112, 113, Sneed, M., 191
Seff, M., 124 Sherman, M., 264 131, 142, 146, 170, 278, 360, Snell, E. K., 38f
Segalowitz, S. J., 66, 68 Sherman, S., 429 360f, 362, 368, 398, 419 Snow, D. L., 426
Seghete, K., 69 Shernoff, D. J., 231, 400 Simons-Morton, B., 52, 81, 82f, 179, Snyder, A. Z., 438
Seguin, J., 80 Sherrill, J. T., 449 181, 209, 419 Snyder, F. J., 441
Seidman, E., 197, 198, 222, 260, 264, Sherrod, L., 109–110, 307 Simpkins, S., 130, 209, 220, Snyder, H., 166, 436
274, 447 Shew, M. L., 360 231–235, 389 Snyder, J., 135, 437
Seiffge-Krenke, I., 58, 323, 341, Shi, H., 338, 358 Simpson, J. A., 323, 323f Sobesky, W., 306
445, 450 Shiffrin, N., 71, 445 Sims, B., 157 Sobolewski, J. M., 141, 142
Seitz, V., 378 Shih, J. H., 445 Sinclair, R., 395, 396 Soeken, K., 446
Self-Brown, S., 114 Shih, T., 168, 206 Singer, M., 209 Soenens, B., 132, 270, 290
Selfhout, M., 172–173 Shimek, J. S., 345 Singh, P., 182 Soet, J. E., 362
Seligman, M. E. P., 386 Shin, N., 275 Singh, S., 312, 354, 355, 370, Sogaard, A. J., 418
Sellers, R. M., 264, 274–278, 277f Shinar, O., 428 371, 373 Sogolow, E., 369–370
Sells, C., 53 Shiner, R. L., 258 Sinha, J. W., 312 Sohoni, D., 204
Selman, R., 206 Shing, Y. L., 78 Sinopoli, K. J., 69 Sokolik, E., 389
Senserrick, T., 79 Shipman, K., 290 Sionéan, C., 361 Soli, A. R., 135
Sentse, M., 132 Shirley, A., 372 Sippola, L., 332–334, 340 Solomon, R., 379
Seo, D., 48 Shirtcliff, E. A., 33, 432, 439 Sipsma, H., 374 Somerville, L., 8, 70
Sepúlveda, J. E., 442 Shiyko, M. P., 44 Sirin, S. R., 393 Sommer, K., 377
Serlin, R., 88 Sholt, M., 131 Sisk, C. L., 24, 25 Sommers, C., 389
Serna, I., 199, 200 Shomaker, L. B., 340 Skakkebæk, N. E., 34 Somsen, R., 64
Seroczynski, A., 260 Shoop, D., 372 Skay, C., 362 Song, A., 71
Serovich, J., 372 Shope, J. T., 261, 420, 427, 429 Skeem, J., 433 Sonntag, K. C., 424
Serwadda, D., 102 Shoulberg, E. K., 137 Skinner, B. F., 15 Sontag, L. M., 12, 24, 43, 181, 417,
Sesma, A., Jr., 235 Shoum, K. A., 232, 387, 392 Skinner, M., 169, 170, 440 420, 442, 445, 447–449
Settersten, R., Jr., 5, 6, 88, 115 Shtarkshall, R. A., 353 Skinner, S. R., 374 Soong, W., 39
Settipani, C., 417 Shuan, Y., 393, 397 Skitch, S. A., 420 Sorell, G. T., 334
Seybolt, D., 53 Shulman, E., 80, 81, 295 Skoog, T., 44, 359 Sorensen, S., 53
Shadish, W., Jr., 397, 411 Shulman, E. P., 439 Skorikov, V. B., 408 Sorenson, A. M., 379
Shaffer, J., 92f, 108, 233–235, 234f Shulman, S., 12, 134, 324, 326–328, Skriner, L., 148 Sorsoli, C., 241
Shaffer, L., 321 338, 340 Skup, M., 69 Sorsoli, L., 257
Shaffer, N., 372 Shumow, L., 234, 308, 393, 399 Slap, G., 147 Sourander, A., 438
Shahar, B., 197 Sibley, M. H., 438 Slavick, J. T., 359, 362 South, S. J., 113, 159, 373, 395
Shahinfar, A., 114 Sickmund, M., 436 Slep, A. M. S., 345 Southam-Gerow, M. A., 448
Shanahan, L., 127, 135, 137, 281 Siebenbruner, J., 342, 358, 420 Slomkowski, C., 134 So Wa, N., 125
Shanahan, M. J., 7, 91, 93, 221, Siegel, A., 18, 416 Slooter, L., 78 Sowell, E. R., 63, 65
227, 228 Siegel, D., 372 Slora, E., 32, 34 Spanoudis, G., 63
Shannon, B. J., 438 Siegel, J., 36, 332, 444 Sloutsky, V., 57, 62 Spear, L., 8, 65–70, 425
Shao, T., 77 Siegel, M., 421, 430 Slowiaczek, M., 393 Spencer, G. A., 345
Shapera, W., 292 Siegel, R. S., 178, 179 Slutske, W. S., 113, 141 Spencer, M., 378, 402
Shapiro, X., 376 Siegle, G., 69 Smahel, D., 11, 245, 325 Spencer, M. B., 207
Shapka, J. D., 33 Siegler, R., 64 Smailes, E., 369 Spencer, S. V., 166
Sharabany, R., 325 Sieving, R. E., 360, 362 Small, D., 45 Spicer, P., 263, 358
Sharkey, P., 111 Sigler-Rushton, W., 375 Small, S., 235, 362, 369 Spieker, S., 343, 378
Sharma, A., 136 Sigman, M., 360, 361 Smalls, C., 108, 109, 113, 146, Spies, L. A., 447
Sharp, C., 439 Signorelli, N., 240 275, 278 Spijkerman, R., 238, 246
Sharp, E. H., 250 Silbereisen, R. K., 42, 43, 104, 108, Smetana, J. G., 57, 76–78, 121, 122, Spina, S., 328, 332, 334, 335
Sharpe Potter, J., 362 172, 291–292, 394 126, 127, 131–134, 141, 288, Spinrad, T., 78, 132, 288,
Shattuck, A., 181 Silk, J., 69, 122, 128, 447 292, 293, 301, 303, 305, 308, 303–307, 328
Shavelson, R., 256 Silk, J. S., 37 317, 329 Spirito, A., 172
Shaver, K., 120 Sillanmäki, L., 438 Smiciklas-Wright, H., 28 Spitznagel, E. L., 357, 420
Shaver, P. R., 323 Silva, P., 32, 33, 44, 75, 377, 421, Smith, A., 245, 275, 353, 358 Spoth, R., 18, 170
Shaw, D. S., 123, 128, 132, 135, 198, 438, 439 Smith, B., 198 Spoth, R. L., 426, 428
439, 447 Silva, P. A., 258 Smith, C., 312, 323, 374, 438 Spotts, E. L., 136
Shaw, H., 46, 48–51 Silva, S. G., 449 Smith, D. C., 298, 332 Sprecher, S., 364
Shaw, P., 68 Silverberg, S., 124, 290 Smith, E., 378 Spriggs, A. L., 342, 344, 345, 357
Shaywitz, B., 201 Silverman, A., 96 Smith, G., 428 Springer, J. F., 428
Shaywitz, S., 201 Silverman, J., 345 Smith, J., 194, 195, 197, 214, 392 Sprondel, V., 63
Shearer, D. L., 377 Silverman, W. K., 449 Smith, J. L., 374 Spruijt-Metz, D. S., 45–47, 46f
Shedler, J., 427 Silverthorn, N., 110 Smith, K., 389 Squeglia, L. M., 425
Sheeder, J., 371 Sim, T., 132, 238, 298, 300 Smith, M., 234 Sφrensen, K., 34
Sheehan, H. C., 215 Simi, P., 39 Smith, P. H., 345 Srinivasan, S., 372
Sheeran, P., 370, 371 Similo, S., 262, 262f Smith, R., 327 Sroufe, A., 322
Sheidow, A. J., 147, 418 Simmens, S., 137 Smith, R. A., 373 Sroufe, L. A., 198, 320, 322, 440
Shek, D., 128 Simmons, J., 124 Smith, T., 234 St. George, I., 32, 33
Shelton, K. H., 136, 420 Simmons, R., 43, 177–178, 260 Smith, T. E., 281, 282, 367 St. John, C., 419
Shen, Y., 128, 357, 358 Simon, T., 147 Smith, T. M., 205 St. John, R., 142
Shepard, D., 114 Simon, T. R., 210, 344, 344f, 368 Smokowski, P., 276, 278 St. Lawrence, J., 372
Shepard, S. A, 306 Simon, V. A., 172, 321, 338, 340, Smolak, L., 28, 50, 51 Staff, J., 209, 220, 222, 225f,
Sheppard, B., 331 343, 345 Smoll, F., 27–28 225–228, 408, 409, 411,
Sher, K. J., 426, 428 Simons, L., 28, 113, 130, Smollar, J., 291, 318 412, 416
Sheras, P., 206 276, 277f Snarney, J., 303 Stage, F. K., 398
Sher-Censor, E., 292 Simons, L. G., 142 Sneed, C. D., 109, 109f Stams, G. J., 305
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-20 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-20 Name Index

Standish, A. R., 48 Stipek, D., 389 Swanson, C., 405 Terenzini, P., 272
Stanger-Hall, K. F., 379 Stockdale, G. D., 142, 357 Swanson, S. A., 417, 417f, 442 Terpeluk, A., 344
Stangl, D., 445 Stockdale, L., 242 Swarr, A., 50, 334 Terracciano, A., 258
Stanley, L. R., 429 Stocker, C., 264 Swartzwelder, H., 424, 425 Terranova, A., 179
Stanton, B., 170, 361, 372, 429 Stocker, C. M., 135, 332 Sweeney, J., 57, 63 Terry, K., 400
Stanton-Salazar, R., 328, 332, Stoltz, A. D., 181 Sweeten, G., 406 Terry, M., 33
334, 335 Stoms, G., 374 Swenson, L. P., 178, 262–264, Terry, R., 176, 183, 280
Stanwood, N. L., 374 Stone, G., 142 275, 327 Terry-Humen, E., 375
Stark, T., 374 Stone, J., 11, 223, 224, 227 Swenson, R., 115 Terry-McElrath, Y., 424
Starr, L. R., 327 Stone, L. B., 445 Swinton, A. D., 390, 391 Tershakovec, A. M., 48
Stattin, H., 44, 132, 169, 171, 231, Stone, M., 164, 249 Swisher, R., 210 Terwilliger, R., 68, 70
234, 235, 291, 293, 322, 359, Stoneman, Z., 134, 147, 399 Sy, S., 227, 228 Teslovich, T., 70
391, 393, 447 Stoolmiller, M., 92, 148, 169, 170, Syed, M., 273–274 Teti, D., 378
Stearns, E., 168, 406 171, 359, 438, 440, 444 Syvertsen, A., 210, 224, 309, Tevendale, H. D., 260, 262–265,
Steca, P., 389 Stormshak, E., 128, 206 309f, 411 275, 373
Stedman, L., 193 Stormshak, E. A., 334, 435 Szalacha, L. A., 262 Thapar, A., 136
Steele, C. M., 388 Storr, C., 228 Szapocznik, J., 271 Thatcher, J., 311–312
Steele, J., 240 Storvoll, E. E., 437 Szczypka, G., 424 Thayer, S., 167, 170, 329
Steen, S., 436 Story, M., 48–51 Szeszulski, P., 264 Theokas, C., 110, 231, 236,
Steffensmeier, D., 435 Stout, M., 210 Szmukler, G., 49 236f, 333
Steffes, J., 34 Stouthamer-Loeber, M., 170, Sznitman, S., 372 Thijs, J., 201
Steglich, C., 172, 175 172, 258, 419, 433, 435–437, Szwedo, D. E., 239, 245, 246 Thφgersen-Ntoumanis, C., 233
Stein, D. G., 49 439, 441 Thoma, S., 303, 305
Stein, J., 41, 107, 170 Strachan, M. K., 418 Thomaes, S., 11, 260, 262, 307
Steinberg, A. R., 48 Strachman, A., 357, 358 T Thomas, J., 335
Steinberg, L., 5, 12, 17, 18, 24, 33, Strang, N., 69 Tach, L., 140 Thomas, L. A., 69, 71, 207
40, 50, 51, 57, 65, 68–70, 72, Strasburger, V., 238–240, 242, 243 Taiarol, J., 178 Thomas, M., 71, 73
78–83, 81f, 91, 96, 97, 97f, 100, Straub, D. M., 243 Takahashi, A., 148 Thomas, O., 278
113, 113f, 115, 120, 122–124, Strauman, T. J., 445 Takezawa, M., 305 Thomas, S. L., 200
126, 128–133, 131f, 136, 144, Straus, M., 146 Tally, S., 447, 448 Thombs, B. D., 365
149, 160, 163–165, 169, Strenziok, M., 243 Talmud, I., 166, 246 Thompson, C., 296
171–173, 182, 183, 191, 193, Striegel-Moore, R., 34, 263 Tamnes, C., 67, 68f Thompson, E., 365
198, 207–210, 225–228, 235, Strong American Schools, 403 Tan, D., 77, 308 Thompson, J. K., 48
239, 242, 255, 286, 287, Stroud, L., 24, 70, 177 Tan, G., 300 Thompson, N., 232
289–292, 294–302, 295f, 296f, Strough, J., 157, 166, 327 Tan, L. L., 108, 426 Thompson, P. J., 243
299f, 340, 345, 370, 392–397, Stuart, E. A., 427 Tanaka, M., 63 Thompson, R., 169, 292
399–402, 405, 416, 419, Stucky, B. D., 177 Tang, C. S., 41 Thompson, T. L., 167, 278
436–442, 448, 449 Stueve, A., 345 Tanihata, T., 39 Thomson, E., 144, 361
Steinberg, S. J., 143 Sturge-Apple, M., 447 Tanner, D., 189, 189f, 190 Thornberry, T. P., 265, 374, 377,
Steinberg, W., 123, 124, 292 Sturm, R., 429 Tanner, J., 22, 29, 31f, 32, 33, 34f 406, 438
Steiner, H., 96 Sturman, D., 70 Tanner-Smith, E. E., 42 Thorne, A., 272, 316, 330
Steinman, K. J., 312 Sturmer, P., 375 Tantleff-Dunn, S., 247 Thornton, A., 126
Stemmler, M., 445, 450 Sturmhöfel, S., 424, 425 Tao, F. B., 446 Thurber, C., 290
Stephan, W., 264 Su, X., 142 Tapales, A., 430 Tienda, M., 398, 400
Stephens, C. M., 132 Suárez-Orozco, C., 402 Tapert, S. F., 425, 428 Tieu, T.-T., 271, 323
Stephens, L., 143 Subrahmanyam, K., 11, 325, 325f Taradash, A., 289, 329 Tiezzi, L., 209
Stephenson, J., 12, 133, 134, Succop, P. A., 34 Tartakovsky, E., 276 Tiggemann, M., 48
286–290, 292, 318, 321–323 Suchindran, C., 35, 49, 112, 177, 345 Tasopoulos-Chan, M., 132 Tilton-Weaver, L. C., 41, 101,
Stephenson, S. C. R., 53, 429 Sucoff, C., 353, 362 Tate, J., 275 170, 293
Sterba, S. K., 446 Sue, S., 400 Taub, B., 444 Timmerman, G., 367
Sternberg, M., 371 Sugden, K., 72, 447 Taylor, A., 51, 72, 447 Ting-Kai, L., 424
Sternberg, R., 73–74 Sui-Chu, E., 399 Taylor, C., 48, 49 Tippetts, A. S., 53, 429
Sternberg, R. J., 58 Sullivan, C. J., 420 Taylor, D., 38 Tobian, A., 102
Stettler, N., 46 Sullivan, H. S., 183, 318f, 318–320 Taylor, I. M., 233 Tobler, A. L., 429
Stevens, A., 109 Sullivan, P., 236, 288 Taylor, J., 420 Tobler, N., 430
Stevens, E. A., 172 Sullivan, S., 271 Taylor, K., 245 Todd, M., 429
Stevens, J., 378 Sullivan, T., 114, 193, 344, 344f Taylor, L., 107 Todorov, A. A., 428
Stevens, N., 346 Sullivan, T. N., 177, 420 Taylor, N., 46 Tokoyawa, T., 113
Stevenson, D., 200, 409, 412 Sullivent, E. E. I., II, 446 Taylor, R., 142, 146, 147, 387, 400 Tolan, P. H., 112, 113, 147, 170, 418,
Stevenson, H. W., 276, 389, 390, Sun, P., 81 Taylor, S., 234, 358 441, 450
398, 401, 402 Sun, R., 361 Taylor, T. J., 170 Tolman, D. L., 241, 257, 350–352,
Stevenson, J., 438 Sun, Y., 141, 142, 145, 147 Teachman, J., 197, 204, 394, 356, 364
Stevenson, M., 79 Sunwolf, 77 395, 398 Tolou-Shams, M., 357
Stevens-Simon, C., 371 Supple, A. J., 133, 275 Teachman, J. D., 145 Tolson, J., 160, 173
Stewart, A. J., 367 Suris, J. C., 357 Tebes, J. K., 426 Tomal, A., 374
Stewart, E. A., 398 Suryanti, T. A., 311, 312 Tehranifar, P., 33 Tomasik, M., 104, 108
Stewart, E. B., 398 Susman, E., 22, 24–26, 32–34, 36, Tein, J.-Y., 125, 278 Tompsett, C., 114, 312
Stewart, S. D., 143 37, 421, 439 Teitler, J. O., 115, 363 Tonev, S., 449
Stewart, S. M., 338, 358 Susman, E. J., 31, 39, 41, 42, 43, 44, Tellegen, A., 101, 258, 449 Tong, Y., 136
Stice, E., 45, 46, 48–51, 129, 132, 439, 447 Telzer, E. H., 400 Toomey, R. B., 367
172, 173, 420, 445 Suzuki, K., 39 Temple, J., 398 Toomey, T., 431
Stickle, T. R., 433 Swahn, M. H., 446 Tennen, H., 356 Topitzes, D., 115
Stigler, J., 401 Swaim, R. C., 429 Teran, L., 374 Topolski, T. D., 416
Still, E., 92 Swank, P. R., 345 ter Bogt, T. F. M., 159 Torney-Purta, J., 308, 309
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-21 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-21

Toro, P., 114, 148, 312 U van der Werf, M., 206 Videon, T. M., 143
Toro, R., 300 Van de Schoot, R., 258, 271–272 Viding, E., 300
Torquati, J., 36, 40 Uckert, K., 299, 299f van Dijk, E., 326, 326f Viechtbauer, W., 258
Torres, B., 392 Udry, J. R., 30f, 35, 49, 339, 343 Van Doorn, M. D., 127 Vieno, A., 112, 132, 205
Tottenham, N., 67, 68 Uggen, C., 441 van Duijvenvoorde, A., 81, 295 Vigfusdottir, T. H., 51
Touchette, E., 39 Uhls, Y., 11 van Dulmen, M., 147, 235 Viken, R. J., 32, 43, 44f, 136, 137f
Tough, P., 193 Umaña-Taylor, A. J., 274, 275, Van Eck, K., 428 Viljoen, J. L., 96
Toumbourou, J. W., 444 278, 298 Vanfossen, B., 199 Villalobos, M., 76–78, 121, 288, 303,
Townsend Betts, N., 326 Ünal, F., 323 van Geel, M., 401 305, 308
Toyokawa, T., 398 Underwood, M., 181, 238 Vangeepuram, N., 34 Villarreal, V. M., 175
Trainor, C. D., 43 Unger, J., 147, 374, 429 van Geert, P. L. C., 123 Villarruel, F., 362
Tram, J. M., 260, 265 UNICEF, 375, 376f van Holst, R., 238 Vincent, G. M., 433
Tramontano, C., 306 United Nations, 154f, 188, 188f Van Horn, M. L., 112, 227, 228, 334 Visser, I., 81
Tramonte, L., 33 U. S. Census Bureau, 102, 103, 106, Van Hulle, C. A., 132, 361 Vitacco, M. J., 432, 433, 439
Tran, S., 323, 323f 108, 138, 153, 223, 335–336, Van Hulst, A., 46 Vitaro, F., 39, 112, 113, 169, 171,
Trautwein, U., 174–175, 201, 393 336f, 397, 402 van Ijzendoorn, M. H., 177, 172, 176, 178, 179, 183, 210,
Treaster, J., 426 U. S. Department of Education, 322, 438 342, 345, 363, 395, 434,
Treboux, D., 322 191, 192 van Leeuwen, A. P., 426 440, 441
Trejos-Castillo, E., 6, 133 U. S. Senate Committee on the Judi- Van Leeuwen, K. G., 129 Vitiello, B., 449
Tremblay, R. E., 39, 80, 112, ciary, 237 Van Leijenhorst, L., 69, 70, 79, 295 Vitterso, J., 444
113, 129, 171, 172, 210, Upchurch, D., 353, 354, 362, 378 Van Lier, P. A. C., 183, 434, 440 Vloet, T. D., 438
345, 363, 395, 406, 433, 434, Updegraff, K. A., 128, 134, 135, 167, van Lieshout, C. M., 179, 329 Voas, R. B., 53, 429
438–441 170, 233, 278, 281, 329 Van Meel, C., 69, 70, 295 Voelkl, K., 400
Trent, K., 374, 378 Upton, R., 278 van Os, J., 37 Vogt Yuan, A. S., 46
Trentacosta, C. J., 123 Urban, T., 57, 63 VanOss Marin, B., 142 Voisine, S., 429
Treutlein, J., 425 Urberg, K. A., 131, 160, 172, Van Petegem, S., 290 Vo-Jutabha, E. D., 276
Trevitt, J. L., 370 173, 334 van Rooij, A., 238 Volkow, N., 424
Trickett, E., 429 Urdan, T., 194, 387, 390, 394, 395 Van Rooij, T., 238 Vollebergh, W. A. M., 271, 331, 426
Trickett, P. K., 33, 42, 44, Usher, E. L., 390, 391 van Rosmalen, L., 305 Von Bank, H., 160, 164
369, 439 Usher-Seriki, K., 360 Van Ryzin, M., 33, 357 von der Pahlen, B., 419
Tristan, J., 132 Uwer, R., 66 Vansadia, P., 145 Vondracek, F. W., 408
Triwahyuni, A., 312 Vansteenkiste, M., 132, 290 von Eye, A., 235, 300–301, 332
Troop-Gordon, W., 322 van Wel, F., 330 von Suchodoletz, W., 66
Troyer, L., 393 V Vanwesenbeeck, I., 353, 359 Voss, H., 69
Trudeau, L., 18 Van Zalk, M. H. W., 447 Voss, K., 69
Vaccaro, D., 427
Truman, B., 372 Vargas, W., 146–147 Votruba-Drzal, E., 128, 132
Valdez, A., 170
Truong, K. D., 429 Varner, F., 402 Vrangalova, Z., 356
Valente, T., 46
Truss, A. E., 447 Vasant, I., 291 Vuchinich, R., 145
Valentin, P., 58
Trusty, J., 393 Vasey, M., 433 Vuchinich, S., 145, 293, 441
Valentine, J., 110, 215
Truxillo, C., 301 Vasquez-Suson, K., 176 Vullin-Reynolds, M., 171
Valentiner, D., 447
Trzesniewski, K. H., 259, 265, Vaughan, C. A., 332, 445 Vygotsky, L., 75
Valeri, S. M., 448
391, 391f Valkenburg, P. M., 238, 245, 246, 328 Vaughan, J., 311
Tsai, K. M., 274 Vaughan, P. W., 278
Tsang, K., 300
Valle, F., 276
Vaughan, R., 209
W
Vallerand, R. J., 232
Tschann, J., 142, 345, 370, 373 Valois, R. F., 358, 420 Vaughn, L. B., 142 Waasdorp, T., 182, 182f
Tseng, V., 125, 127, 330, 400 Valrie, C. R., 278 Vaughn, M. G., 136, 433 Wadden, T. A., 48
Tseng, W., 177 Valsiner, J., 276 Vazsonyi, A. T., 6, 131, 133, 258, 264, Wade, E., 132
Tserakhava, V., 292 Vanable, P. A., 370, 372 430, 438 Wade, T. J., 444
Tsui, A. O., 370 Van Acker, R., 175 Veblen-Mortenson, S., 431 Wadley, J., 423
Tsur, A., 134 van Aken, C., 329 Vedder, P., 401 Wadsworth, K., 428
Tubman, J. G., 369 van Aken, M., 258 Veenstra, R., 10, 112, 141, 152, 157, Wadsworth, M. E., 146
Tucker, C., 127, 134, 135, 137, 231, van Aken, M. A. G., 132 158, 161, 175, 176, 317, 437 Wagner, E. F., 369
235, 309 Van Court, P., 307 Velanova, K., 69 Wagner, K. D., 449
Tucker, J. S., 425, 426 van de Eijnden, R., 238 Velderman, M. K., 179 Waite, E. B., 135
Tucker-Drob, E. M., 81, Vandell, D. L., 209, 220, 231–235 Velez, W., 211 Waite, L., 106
136, 440 van de Meer, E., 243 Veltman, D., 238 Waiters, E., 360, 363
Turbin, M. S., 45, 47, 419, 429 van de Mheen, D., 238 Venables, P. H., 439 Waizenhofer, R., 128, 132
Turiel, E., 121, 122, 290, 302 van den Akker, A., 293–294 Ventura, S., 355, 372, 373 Wakefield, M., 424
Turkheimer, E., 42, 110, 136, 141, Vandenbelt, M., 377 Vera Institute of Justice, 375 Wakschlag, L. S., 377
358, 361, 429 Van Den Berg, D., 428 Verhulst, F. C., 112, 175, 327, 426, Walberg, H., 403
Turner, H. A., 181 van den Berg, P., 247, 262 433, 437, 445 Wald, M., 213
Turner, J., 379 Van Den Bos, J., 429 Verkuyten, M., 78, 201 Walden, B., 126, 136
Turner, P., 101 van den Bos, W., 73, 326, 326f Verma, S., 152, 153, 188, 222, 224 Waldfogel, J., 81
Turner, R., 379 van den Bree, M. B. M., 420 Vermeer, H. J., 389 Waldman, I. D., 132, 178
Turner, T., 176 Van den Bulck, J., 39 Vermeiren, R., 114 Waldron, M., 136
Tuval-Mashiach, R., 340 van den Eijnden, R., 238, 246 Vermulst, A. A., 127, 238, 246, Walhovd, K., 67, 68f
Tuvblad, C., 438 van der Aa, N., 246 291, 322 Walker, A., 311–312
Tüzün, Z., 323 Vanderbilt-Adriance, E., 447 Verona, E., 418 Walker, A. M., 275
Tweed, D., 445 van der Ende, J., 433 Véronneau, M.-H., 169, 395, 440 Walker, A. R., 324, 326–327, 346
Twenge, J., 263, 444 van der Geest, V., 437 Vesely, S. K., 360 Walker, D., 176, 346
Tynes, B., 245, 274, 278 van der Laan, P., 131, 305 Vest, A. E., 233 Walker, E., 24, 242, 425, 448
Tyrka, A. R., 49 van der Molen, M. W., 64, 295 Vezina, L., 345 Walker, L. J., 304, 306–307
Tyson, D., 209, 393 Vanderryn, J., 429 Vicary, J., 261, 359, 361 Walker, M., 39
Tyson, K., 165, 400 VanderValk, I., 132 Vida, M., 390 Walker, R. S., 31, 34
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-22 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-22 Name Index

Walker, S., 352, 360, 363 Weinberg, R., 275 Wikstroem, P. O., 113 Witkow, M. R., 169, 211, 222, 273,
Walker-Barnes, C. J., 131, 133, 171 Weinfield, N., 198, 322 Wikström, P. H., 437 274, 278, 396, 396f, 400, 401
Walkup, J. T., 449 Weinstein, J. M., 204 Wilcox, W., 133 Witruk, E., 390
Wall, J., 298 Weinstein, R. S., 137, 209 Wilens, T. E., 428 Witsberger, C., 106
Wall, M., 47, 48, 51 Weinstein, S. M., 442 Wiley, D. J., 354 Witthoft, S., 144
Wallace, J. M., 310–312, 311f Weiser, J., 295 Wilhelm, M. S., 206 Wittig, M. A., 274
Wallace, J. M., Jr., 426 Weisgram, E. S., 412 Wilkening, F., 60 Witvliet, M., 183
Wallace, S. A., 400 Weiss, C., 194, 196 Wilkinson, D., 114, 436 Wodtke, G., 112
Wallace, S. R., 144 Weiss, C. C., 115, 363 Wilkinson, R. B., 264 Woertman, L., 353, 359
Wallander, J. L., 377 Weiss, H., 130 Wilkinson-Lee, A. M., 206 Wolak, J., 181, 246, 247
Wallenius, M., 38 Weissberg, R., 182 Willard, J., 18 Wolf, A., 53
Waller, E. M., 178, 327 Weisskirch, R. S., 294 Willcutt, E. G., 419 Wolfe, A., 23
Waller, J., 358 Weist, M., 449 Willett, J. B., 128 Wolfe, D. A., 367
Waller, P., 427 Weist, M. D., 114 Williams, A., 46, 245 Wolfe, M., 405
Walls, M., 426 Weisz, J. R., 448, 450 Williams, C., 431 Wolfe, S., 301
Walls, N. E., 367 Weitlauf, A. S., 447 Williams, G., 439 Wolfer, L., 405
Walsh, B., 50 Wells, A., 199, 200 Williams, J. H., 389 Wolff, J. M., 82
Walsh, D. A., 242 Wells, E. A., 362 Williams, K. R., 180, 181, 321, 321f, Wolfson, A., 39
Walsh, S., 134 Wells, K., 147 389, 389f Woloski-Wruble, A., 353
Walter, H., 209 Welner, K. G., 194, 196 Williams, L. R., 128, 129 Wong, C., 149, 300
Walters, R., 15 Welsh, D. P., 142, 370 Williams, M., 38 Wong, I., 387
Walton, K., 258 Welte, J. W., 429 Williams, N., 226 Wong, M., 428
Walton, M. A., 429 Wen, H., 431 Williams, P., 51, 52 Woo, J., 68
Wan, Y. H., 446 Weng, C., 276, 277f Williams, R., 167 Wood, D., 236, 288, 392
Wandersman, A., 53 Wentzel, K. R., 178, 198, 205, 206, Williams, S., 32, 33, 231, 360 Wood, G., 343, 345
Wang, C., 419 307, 393, 396 Williams, S. K., 128 Wood, P., 400
Wang, F., 10 Werch, C. E., 232 Williams, S. T., 146 Wood, P. A., 440
Wang, J., 47, 81, 82f Werner, N., 172, 178 Williams, T., 358, 389, 389f, 405 Wood, P. K., 419
Wang, L., 65, 71 Wertheim, E., 49 Williams, W., 58, 75, 397 Woods, E., 421
Wang, M., 28, 128, 197, 206 Weseter, K., 209 Williamson, D., 449 Woods, L. N., 305
Wang, Q., 81, 292, 301, 388, 393 West, T. C., 193 Williams-Wheeler, M., 132 Woodward, H., 25f, 34
Wang, W., 439 Westenberg, M., 326, 326f William T. Grant Foundation Woodward, L., 143, 183, 377
Wang, Y., 45, 181, 276 Westenberg, P. M., 69, 70, 79, 295 Commission on Work, Family, Woodward, M., 79
Wanner, B., 172, 440 Westhoven, V., 292 Citizenship, 189, 189f Woolard, J., 72, 78, 81, 81f, 86, 87,
Ward, E., 46 Westlye, L., 67, 68f Williford, A. P., 179 96, 97, 97f, 255, 295f, 295–297,
Ward, L. M., 241, 242, 360 Wetter, E. K., 420–421 Willis, D. S., 310–312, 311f 296f, 370, 433
Ware, A., 449 Wewerka, S., 24, 70 Willms, J. D., 33, 112, 113, 363, 399 Woolley, M. E., 393
Wargo, J., 176 Whalen, D. J., 69, 447 Willoughby, B., 91 Worden, J., 431
Warneke, C., 53 Wharton, M., 181 Willoughby, B. J., 316, 316f Worthman, C. M., 24, 444, 445, 448
Warner, S., 387 Whitaker, D., 300, 360, 362 Willoughby, T., 128, 311–312, 366, Wray-Lake, L., 224, 301, 307, 309,
Warr, M., 132 Whitbeck, L., 112, 146, 147, 361, 367, 420, 447 309f, 411
Warren, C., 369–370 362, 369, 426, 446 Wills, T., 427, 428 Wright, B. R. E., 436
Warren, J. R., 227, 407 Whitbeck, L. B., 311 Wilmer, C., 130 Wright, J., 226
Warren, M. P., 24, 40, 42, 43, 48 White, A. M., 231 Wilsdon, A., 362 Wright, J. P., 132, 136
Waschbusch, D. A., 438 White, H. R., 106, 107f Wilson, C., 172 Wright, K., 132
Washburn-Ormachea, J. M., 445 White, J. W., 345 Wilson, H. W., 369 Wright, P. A., 445
Wasik, B., 276 White, K. S., 300 Wilson, J., 234, 440 Wright, V., 292
Wasserman, G., 379, 437 White, N., 58, 63, 345 Wilson, J. L., 245 Wu, C., 125
Wasserman, R., 32 White, R., 278, 378 Wilson, M., 436 Wu, L., 227, 361, 423, 425, 427
Watanabe, Y., 63 White, S. F., 432 Wilson, R., 248 Wuerker, A., 439
Waterman, P., 209 Whitehead, K. A., 274 Wilson, S., 5, 10, 18, 88, 90, 98, 104, Wunderlich, M., 52
Waters, E., 322 Whiteman, S., 134, 167, 281, 282 108, 259, 260, 260f Wynn, P., 440
Waters, P., 261 Whitenton, K., 50 Wilson, S. M., 292 Wynn, S. R., 390
Waters, S. D., 398 Whitesell, N., 257, 261, 263 Wilson-Ahlstrom, A., 235 Wypij, D., 365
Watkins, K., 68 Whitesell, N. R., 358 Wilson-Simmons, R., 345
Watson, A. L., 426 Whiteside, T., 449 Windle, M., 114, 134, 141, 332, 343,
Watt, H. M. G., 281, 393 Whitlock, J. L., 245 420, 426, 440 X
Watt, T. T., 194–195 Whitman, T., 377 Windsor, R., 370
Winfree, J. L., 169 Xia, G., 447
Watts, R., 275 Whitmore, J., 401
Winfree, L., Jr., 170 Xiao, L., 81
Way, N., 39, 108, 167, 168f, 197, Wibbelsman, C. J., 142, 345
Xie, Y., 398
205–207, 209–210, 263, 263f, Wichstrφm, L., 42, 437, 444f, Wingo, P. A., 373
Wingood, G. M., 312, 361, 370, 372 Xing, C., 446
273–278, 278f, 325, 328f, 444–445
Wingrove, T., 96 Xing, X. Y., 446
328–330, 399–401 Wickrama, K. A. S., 46, 399
Winmill, L., 68 Xu, F., 371
Wearing, H., 64 Widaman, K. F., 142, 292, 298,
Winn, D. C., 374, 377 Xu, J., 393
Weaver, S. R., 125 332, 357
Winston, C., 280 Xu, X., 354, 354f, 360
Weber, E., 81 Widman, L., 370
Xue, Y., 371, 418
Weber, L. M., 193 Widom, C. S., 369 Winter, C., 171
Weems, C., 107 Wiebe, R. P., 172 Winter, M., 425
Weerman, F., 172 Wiersma, J. D., 343, 345 Winter, T., 32 Y
Weesie, J., 172 Wiesner, M., 419, 426, 440 Wiseman, R., 177–178
Wehner, E., 340 Wigfield, A., 12, 197, 260, 281, Wishard, A., 170 Yaeger, A., 428
Wei, E., 441 386, 391 Wisneski, H., 367 Yamaguchi, R., 430
Wei, Y., 33, 81 Wiggins, R. D., 109 Wissink, I. B., 175, 440 Yancey, A., 36
Weinberg, M., 366 Wight, R. G., 442 Witkiewitz, K., 433 Yang, C., 76, 346, 439
ste32118_nndx_I1-I23.indd Page I-23 01/05/13 12:18 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Name Index I-23

Yang, L., 39 Yonelinas, A., 63 Zamboni, G., 243 Zheng, M., 438
Yang, S., 77 Yoon, J. S., 178 Zametkin, A. J., 45, 46, 446 Zhong, H., 435
Yang, T. W. H., 425 Yoon, K., 391 Zamsky, E., 378 Zhou, Q., 307
Yang, W., 72 Yorgason, J. B., 332 Zane, N., 429 Zick, C., 220–221, 221f, 224
Yang, X., 344 Yoshikawa, H., 205, 222, 441 Zanolie, K., 64, 69, 70, 295 Zickler, P., 425
Yang, Y., 345 You, S., 209 Zapert, K., 426 Zickuhr, K., 245
Yang, Z., 379 Young, A. M., 367, 368f, Zarahn, E., 63 Zillman, D., 242
Yannett, N., 199, 393 374–375, 419 Zarbatany, L., 326 Zima, B., 147
Yap, M., 124 Young, J. F., 332 Zarrett, N., 11, 209, 220, Zimiles, H., 144
Yarger, R. S., 63, 65, 68 Young, M., 345, 361 231–235 Zimmer, C., 245
Yasui, M., 275 Young, M. E. D., 369 Zavodny, M., 370 Zimmer-Gembeck, M., 12, 177,
Yates, A., 28 Young, S. E., 419 Zayas, L., 446 286–290, 334, 342, 351,
Yates, M., 23, 306–307 Youngstrom, E., 114 Zee, P. C., 38 357, 358
Yau, J., 121, 132, 133, 141 Youniss, J., 109–110, 291, Zeiders, K., 278 Zimmerman, G., 114
Ybarra, M. L., 181, 246 306–308, 318 Zeiders, K. H., 125 Zimmerman, M. A., 110, 113, 142,
Ybarra, V. C., 275 Younts, C. W., 436 Zeidman, J., 357 145, 172, 228, 261, 264,
Yeager, D., 321, 321f Ystrom, E., 447 Zeira, A., 168, 209 278–279, 312, 355, 371, 372,
Yeates, K., 439 Yu, H., 203 Zeldin, S., 115 418, 428, 429
Yeh, H., 135 Yu, J. J., 289 Zeman, J., 290 Zimmerman, R., 356–357,
Yeh, M., 440 Yu, K., 179, 181 Zentall, S. R., 360 364, 379
Yemini, T., 446 Yu, M., 426 Zentner, M., 256 Zimmermann, P., 323
Yeo, L. S., 387 Yu, T., 357 Zha, W., 427 Zimring, F., 97, 440
Yeung, A., 262, 388 Yuan, A. S. V., 145 Zhai, F., 245 Zinman, J., 53, 424, 430
Yeung, D. Y., 41 Yücel, M., 442 Zhang, D., 438 Ziol-Guest, K., 205, 390
Yeung, R., 182 Yurkelun-Todd, D. A., 66 Zhang, H., 419 Zito, J. M., 446
Yin, J., 183 Zhang, J., 48, 344 Ziv, Y., 265
Yip, T., 125, 203, 274, 276–278 Z Zhang, Q., 206 Zohra, I. T., 420
Yisrael, D. S., 367 Zhang, W., 290 Zollo, P., 248
YMCA, 120, 235 Zabin, L., 373–374, 378 Zhang, Y., 392 Zolotor, A. J., 369
Yodanis, C., 146 Zaff, J. F., 231 Zhang, Z., 81, 82f Zoon, C. K., 45, 46
Yoder, K., 361 Zahn-Waxler, C., 432, 439 Zhao, W., 48 Zucker, R., 428
Yoerger, K., 171, 428, 438 Zaider, T., 420 Zhao, X., 245 Zuckerman, A., 395
Yohalem, N., 235 Zajac, K., 323, 419 Zhao, Y., 365 Zuroff, D. C., 292
Yokum, S., 45 Zakin, D., 43 Zheng, H., 429 Zweig, J. M., 420
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-24 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

Subject Index
Note: Page numbers followed by personality dimensions in, 258 after-school programs, 231, 234 anxious-avoidant attachment, 320
f refer to figures; those followed by proportion of population around age-crime curve, 433, 434f anxious-resistant attachment, 320
t indicate tables. Bold page numbers the world, 154f age grading, 153 apprenticeships, 98, 105
indicate key terms. as social invention, 16, 88–95 agency, 270–271 armed robbery, 436
as “storm and stress” period, 13, age of majority, 87 arrest rates, gender differences in,
17–18, 39–40, 120–122, 126, age segregation, 156, 166, 196–197 435–436
A 259, 260, 289 aggression, 136, 433. See also Asian youth
as term, 5–6, 90 violence academic achievement and,
ability grouping, 200–201. See also adolescent development, 3–18 electronic media and, 238, 399–403
tracking abortion, 87, 97 achievement in, 384–386. See also 239, 243 achievement gap and, 193
decisions concerning, 296 achievement hostile attributional bias and, clique membership and, 167,
teen pregnancy and, 373–374 adolescence, defined, 5–6 178–179, 439 168, 168f
abstainers, 426–427, 429 attachment in, 321–323, 345 iatrogenic effects and, 171 college enrollment of, 211, 212f
abstinence, 379 behavioral genetics and, 135–138 moral reasoning and, 306 crowd membership and, 165
abstinence-only sex education, 379 biological perspective on. See popularity and, 175 current trends in home leaving,
abstract thinking, 58, 61–62, 86 biological perspective preventing, 178 107
abusers, 426–427 boundaries of adolescence and, rejection and, 176, 177–179 dating relationships of, 340, 343
academic achievement, 397–407 5–7 relational, 177–179 differences in self-esteem, 263,
brain crowd and, 159, 160, contexts of adolescence, 9–11 sexual activity and, 342, 343, 358 263f, 264
164, 165 developmental tasks in, 94t stability and changes in, 439–440 disordered eating among, 49–50
changes over time, 402–405 electronic media and, 244–247 types of, 175 dropping out, 405
common interests among employment and, 226–228 video games and, 238 employment opportunities, 224
friends, 169 extracurricular activities and, agreeableness, 258 ethnic composition of U.S., 109f
dropping out, 405–407 231–233 AIDS/HIV, 52, 53, 355, 360, 372 ethnic identity development,
ethnic differences in, 399–402 family relationships and, Alaska Native youth. See American 273–274, 276, 277
ethnic identity and, 278–279 128–135, 149 Indian/Alaska Native youth expectations for behavioral
experience in school and, framework for studying, 7–13 alcohol. See substance use and autonomy, 301
188–189 free time and, 249–250 abuse false optimism and, 400
factors in, 397 fundamental changes in, 7–8 altruism, in occupational choice, family relations and, 126–127
impact of tracking on, 200–201 impact of dating on, 342–346 411–412 generational dissonance, 125
peer group and, 183 impact of religious involvement American Indian/Alaska Native parent-teenager conflict, 121
socioeconomic status (SES) and, on, 312 youth school violence and, 209–210
398–399 interpersonal development in, adolescent views of themselves, sex differences in friendship, 328
achievement, 383–412 318–320 101 sexual activity and, 353–354, 360
as adolescent issue, 384–386 multidisciplinary approach to, dating relationships of, 338 single parenthood and, 138
beliefs in, 388–392 4–5 differences in self-esteem, 263 stress and, 114, 448
educational. See academic perspectives on adolescence, 4–17 dropout rates, 109 substance use and abuse, 426
achievement psychosocial development, educational reform and, 191 susceptibility to peer influence,
environmental influences on, 11–12. See also psychosocial ethnic identity development, 299–300
392–397 development 273, 276 assessment
family influence on, 204, 206, school and, 215–216 initiation ceremonies, 95 Advanced Placement (AP)
207, 398–399 stages in, 6–7, 29–31 obesity among, 45–46 programs, 223–224
motivation for, 386–388 stereotypes versus scientific study suicide among, 53 international competitions,
nature of, 384 in, 17–18, 77, 280–282, 282f transition to adulthood, 108 403–405, 404t
occupational, 384, 385, 407–412 theoretical perspectives on, 13–17 unemployment rate, 109 measuring IQ, 73, 74–75, 75f, 439
in psychosocial development, 11, adolescent egocentrism, 58–59, 261 Americanization process, 108–109, National Assessment of
12, 87 adolescent growth spurt, 26–28 168, 190, 354 Educational Progress
selection versus socialization adolescent health care, 51–53 anabolic steroids, 49 (NAEP), 402–403
and, 172 disordered eating treatment, 51 anal intercourse, 352, 355 SAT (Scholastic Assessment Test),
achievement attributions, 390 mortality causes, 52 androgens, 23–24, 37, 358–359 390–391, 402, 403
achievement gaps, 191–192, nutrition, 33, 46, 47 anger, 330. See also aggression; athletic performance
193, 389 obesity prevention and conflict; violence extracurricular activities, 231–233
achievement tests, 192–193 treatment, 47–48 anhedonic, 421 jock crowd and, 159–160, 164,
“acting out” behavior, 418 paradox of adolescent health, anorexia nervosa, 48, 49–51 165, 232
acting White, 400–401 51–52 anthropological theories, 16–17 sex differences in, 27–28, 28f
activation role (of hormones), 25 promoting health, 51–52, 53 antidepressants, 202 attachment
actual self, 257 adolescent-limited offenders, antisocial activity, 169–171, 234 in adolescence, 321–323, 345
ADHD (attention deficit/hyperac- 437, 440 adolescent-limited offenders, detachment versus, 289, 291, 294
tivity disorder), 202, 438 adolescent marginality, 15 437, 440 in family relationships, 133–134
adolescence Adolescent Society, The developmental progression, 434 in infancy, 320–323, 323f
“adult” activities and, 95–97, (Coleman), 156 life-course-persistent offenders, to mothers versus fathers, 323
100–101, 104 adoptive parents, 138, 147 437–440 nature of, 320
alternative terms for, 90–95 adrenarche, 24 antisocial personality disorder, types of, 320–321
autonomy as issue of, 287–288 “adult” activities, 95–97, 432–433 to value of school, 203, 232–233
elongation of, 87–88 100–101, 104 anxiety attention, in information-process-
as emerging adulthood, 6–7, Adult Attachment Interview, 322 childhood, 417, 417f ing perspective, 63, 64–65
90–95 Advanced Placement (AP) friendship and, 327 attention deficit/hyperactivity
identity as issue of, 254–255 programs, 223–224 poverty and, 146 disorder (ADHD), 202, 438
identity crisis, 14, 254, 265–269 African American youth. See sexual abuse and, 369 Australia, employment
nature of, 5–6 Black youth social support and, 331–332 opportunities, 224
I-24
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-25 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Subject Index I-25

authoritarian parents, 130, 133, 206 endocrine system in, 22–26 Board of Education v. Mergens, 97 selection versus socialization
behavioral autonomy and, 300 genetic and environmental body dissatisfaction, 36, 49 and, 172
emotional autonomy and, influences in, 32–35 depression and, 445 victimization and, 172, 176,
293–294 health and health care, 51–53 disordered eating and, 28, 48–51 179–182
political thinking and, 308–309 homosexuality in, 365–366 mass media and, 247–248
authoritative parents, 130–132, 145, obesity in, 28, 45–48 obesity and, 28, 45–48
206, 209, 234 psychological impact of puberty, platonic relationships and, 334
achievement and, 393–394, 396 35–45 body mass index (BMI), 45, 49, 50 C
attachment and, 323 social impact of puberty, 35–45 Born This Way Foundation, 367
emotional autonomy and, 293 callous-unemotional (CU) traits,
somatic development in, 26–32 boundaries of adolescence, 5–7,
identity achievement and, 271 432–433
timing and tempo of puberty, 95–97
nature of, 131–132 Canada
32–35, 41–44 boys. See also gender-role develop-
power of authoritative parenting, adolescent parenthood, 375
biosocial theories, Hall’s theory of ment; sex differences
130–131 bullying study, 181–182
recapitulation, 13, 17 feminine gender roles, 282–283
prosocial reasoning and, 306 dropping out, 406
biracial youth friendship with the other sex and,
authority conflicts, 434 employment opportunities, 224
differences in self-esteem, 263f 333–335
autobiographical memory, 63 ethnic segregation in adolescent
identity development, 279–280 gender-role development,
autoerotic behavior, 29, 41, peer relations, 168
birth control. See contraception 280–283 car accidents, 52, 81–83, 82f, 427,
352–353, 363, 364 birth order, 301 intimacy and, 328
automobile accidents, 52, 81–83, 429, 431
bisexuality, 366 meaning of sex and, 363–364 caring/compassion
82f, 427, 429, 431 Black youth. See also ethnic peer influence and, 299–300
autonomy, 11, 12, 285–313 in friendship, 326
differences prosocial moral reasoning in positive youth development,
as adolescent issue, 287–288 academic achievement and, and, 307
behavioral autonomy, 288, 236t
399–400, 403 sexual activity and, 353f, Catcher in the Rye, The (Salinger),
294–302 achievement gap and, 191–192, 353–354, 363–364
cognitive autonomy, 288, 268
193 sexual maturation in, 29, 41–42 causation, 240–241
302–313 adolescent parenthood and, Boys and Girls Clubs, 235
emotional autonomy, 288–294 censorship, 97
375–376, 378 brain development, 4–5, 65–72. Central Park jogger case, 298
establishing, 108 body dissatisfaction and, 28 See also cognitive perspective
in family relationships, 133–134 character, in positive youth devel-
clique membership and, attention deficit/hyperactivity opment, 236t
importance of, 286 167–168, 168f disorder (ADHD), 202, 438
independence versus, 286 charter schools, 193, 203–204
college enrollment of, 211, 212f basic operations of brain, 66–67 cheerleaders, 175–176
peer group and, 183 crime victimization and, 436 brain function changes in adoles-
types of, 288 childhood
crowd membership and, 165 cence, 65–66, 69–71 adolescence as transition
dating relationships of, 338 brain structure changes in from, 90
differences in self-esteem, adolescence, 65–66, 67–69 adolescence versus, 89
262–264, 263f exposure to media violence, 243 anxiety in, 417, 417
B disengagement from school, impact of peer influence, attachment in, 322
baby boom generation, 102–103, 198–199 299–300 children of teen mothers,
153, 155t disordered eating among, 49 implications for adolescent 376–377
bad girls, 435 educational reform and, 191 behavior, 71–72 toddler independence, 287
Bar (Bas) Mitzvah, 99, 100 ethnic composition of U.S., 109f neuroimaging studies in, 45–46, child protectionists, 89–91
basal metabolism rate, 45 ethnic identity development, 273, 65–66 Children of the Great Depression,
behavioral autonomy, 288, 294–302 276, 277 sensitivity to emotional signals, 155t
changes in susceptibility to influ- expectations for behavioral au- 126 chlamydia, 371
ence, 297–300 tonomy, 301 sex differences in, 66 chronological perspective
cultural differences in expecta- false optimism and, 400 social brain, 71 boundaries of adolescence, 6t
tions for, 300–301 family relations and, 126–127 substance use and abuse and, stages of development, 6–7
decision-making abilities, grandparents and, 333 424–425 circumcision, 102
294–297 homicide among, 53 brain function civic engagement, 307–308
ethnic differences in expectations multiethnic youth, 279–280 changes during adolescence, civil liberties, 78
for, 300–301 nutrition and, 28 65–66, 69–71 clarity of social redefinition,
feelings of self-reliance, 301 “Obama effect” and, 388–389 correlation between behavior 100–103
timing of, 302 obesity among, 45–46, 46f changes and, 71–72 adolescent views of themselves,
behavioral decision theory, 79 parenting styles and, 132–133 nature of, 65–66 101
behavioral genetics, 135–138 parents as targets of intimacy, “brains,” 159, 160, 164, 165 circumcision controversy, 102
in adolescent development, 136 329–330 brain structure in previous eras, 102–103
sibling differences and, 137 peer groups and, 157 changes in adolescence, 65–66, social redefinition, 100–101
behavioral therapy, 47–48 religious involvement of, 312 67–69 in traditional cultures, 101–102
behaviorism, 15 school violence and, 209–210 correlation between behavior classroom climate, 205–211
beliefs sex differences in friendship, 328 changes and, 71–72 characteristics of good class-
achievement and, 388–392 sexual activity and, 353–354, 355 nature of, 65–66 rooms, 214–215
religious, 309–312, 310f single parenthood and, 138, 139 Brazil, mathematical skills of street school violence, 209–210
Big Brothers/Big Sisters, 333 social support and, 142, 332–333 merchants, 75 student engagement, 205–206,
big fish-little pond effect, 201–202 stereotype threat and, 388 breast development, 29–30, 31f, 34, 207–209
binge drinking, 423, 425, 429–431 stress and neighborhood 40–41, 353, 359 teacher expectations in, 120,
binge eating disorder, 48–49 conditions, 113–114 Brown v. Board of Education of 206–207, 391
biological perspective, 21–53. susceptibility to peer influence, Topeka, 203 variations in class size, 195–196
See also puberty 299–300 Brown v. Entertainment Merchants, cliques, 42, 166–174
adolescent health care in, 51–53 timing and tempo of puberty, 243 changes over time, 161–162, 163
biological transitions of 32, 34 bulimia, 48, 49–51 common interests of, 168–171
adolescence, 7, 9 total media exposure, 238 bullying crowds versus, 159–160, 164
boundaries of adolescence, 6t transition to adulthood, 108 rejection and, 176 exclusion from, 177–178
disordered eating in, 28, 48–51 unemployment rate, 109 responses to, 182 meanness in, 176, 177–178
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-26 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-26 Subject Index

cliques—Cont. non-college-bound students, peer groups, 10. See also peer intelligence and, 75
nature of, 158 213–214 groups parenting styles in, 130–131,
same-sex, 333 peer group and, 395–397 risk-taking behavior in, 80–82. 132–133
selection versus socialization and, transition from high school to See also risk-taking behavior curvilinear pattern, 230–231
171–173 college, 212–213, 289 schools, 11. See also school custody arrangements, 143–144
similarity among members, Columbine High School shootings, sexual activity and, 359. See also “Cyberball,” 321, 321f
166–168 210 sexual activity cyberbullying, 180–181, 246
in structuring social networks, Coming of Age in Samoa (Mead), thinking and, 76–83
158–159 104 work and leisure activities, 11.
cocaine, 421 commitment, 341 See also leisure activities; D
cognition changes, 56–61 community work
adolescent relativism, 60–61 extracurricular activities and, 232 continuity of social redefinition, date rape, 343, 367–368
development of intimacy, 317 mentoring programs, 110 103–107 dating, 335–346. See also romantic
metacognition, 58–59 neighborhood conditions and in contemporary society, relationships
rejection by peers and, 176–177 adolescent development, 103–104 age of, 335–336, 339
risk-taking behavior, 59 110–115 current trends in home leaving, context in, 338–339
thinking about abstract concepts, schools in, 214 106–107 development of dating relation-
58, 61–62, 86 unstructured leisure time and, in previous eras, 105–106 ships, 340–342
thinking about possibilities, 234–235 in traditional cultures, 104–105 impact on adolescent develop-
56–58 community service, 307–308 continuous transitions, 103 ment, 342–346
thinking about thinking, 58–59 communitywide intervention, contraception, 87, 97, 353 intimacy in, 316, 317, 337–340
thinking in multiple dimensions, 430–431 condom use, 350, 360, 369–371, patterns of, 339–340
59–60 comorbidity, 50, 418–421 372, 379 puberty and, 28, 35, 40, 51
violations of expectations externalizing disorders, 418–419 decisions concerning, 296 reasons for, 340
and, 127 internalizing disorders, 420–421 teen pregnancy and, 374–375 single parenthood and, 362
cognitive autonomy, 288, 302–313 competence use of, 369–371 violence in, 342–345, 344f,
civic engagement and competence training programs, virginity pledges and, 363, 370 367–368
volunteerism, 306–308 182–183 conventional moral reasoning, death. See also suicide
moral development, 302–306 performance versus, 62 303, 304 adolescent mortality, 52
political thinking in, 308–309 in positive youth development, coping, stress and, 449–450 homicide, 52, 53, 436
prosocial reasoning and behavior, 236t correlation, 240–241 death penalty, 96, 97
302, 306–307 componential intelligence, 74 cortisol, 24, 439 decision making, 294–300
religious beliefs in, 309–312 comprehensive high school, 190, co-rumination, 327, 445 behavioral decision theory, 79
timing of, 302 194–196 covert antisocial behavior, 434 changes in decision-making
cognitive-developmental view, comprehensive sex education, 379 criminal behavior, 433. See also legal abilities, 294–297
61–62 compulsory education, 16, 75 perspective legal, 296
cognitive perspective, 55–83 age grading and, 153 adolescents as criminal defen- parental influence on, 297–298
achievement in, 385 origins of, 189–190 dants, 96–97 peer group influence on, 297–300
adolescent brain, 65–72 work and, 223 work and, 226, 227–228 risk-taking behavior and, 78–83
adolescent risk taking, 78–83 concern, in friendship, 326 criminal justice system, 95–97 deductive reasoning, 57, 61–62
boundaries of adolescence, 6t concrete operations period, 61 critical thinking, 191, 213 de-idealization, 290–291, 292
changes in cognition, 56–61, condom use, 350, 360, 369–371, cross-cultural perspective delayed phase preference, 37–39
66–71 372, 379 on adolescent parenthood, 375 delinquency, 12, 433–437. See also
cognitive transitions of adoles- conduct disorder, 431–433, biological transitions in, 22 risk-taking behavior
cence, 7–8, 9 432t, 438 disordered eating, 49–50 age differences in values and
development of autonomy in, conduct problems, 171, 179 intimacy in, 330 priorities, 80
287–288 confidence, in positive youth devel- parenting style in, 130–131 age of onset, 416–417
identity development in, 255 opment, 236t parent-teenager conflict in, 121 athletic participation and, 233
individual differences in intelli- conflict on teenage employment, 224 early maturation and, 42, 43–44
gence, 72–76 adolescence as “storm and stress” cross-sectional studies, 36 electronic media and, 237
Internet and cognitive develop- period, 13, 17–18, 39–40, crowds iatrogenic effects and, 171
ment, 246 120–122, 126, 259, 260, 289 changes during adolescence, involvement in antisocial activity,
sexuality and, 350–351 in divorce, 141, 143–144 157–158 169–170
social cognition in adolescence, among friends, 324–325, 326, 327 changes over time, 161–163 moral reasoning and, 306
76–78 impact of family, 291 cliques versus, 159–160, 164 parenting styles and, 132
theoretical perspectives on ado- intergenerational, 15–16, 120–121, ethnicity and, 165 personality dimensions, 258
lescent thinking, 61–65 123–124, 291–292, 331 exclusion from, 162–163 selection versus socialization and,
cohabitation, 336, 375–376 marital, 141, 142–144 nature of, 159 172–173
cohorts, 153–154, 155t parent-teenager, 15–16, 120–122, as reference groups, 164–165 single parenthood and, 144
baby boom generation, 102–103, 126–127, 291–292 social map of adolescence, 163 sleep patterns and, 39
153, 155t in romantic relationships, sorting into, 159–160 work and, 226, 227–228
nature of, 98 340, 345 waxing and waning of, 162–163 depression, 12, 136, 442–445.
collective efficacy, 112–113, 235 sibling, 137 CU (callous-unemotional) traits, See also suicide
college connection, in positive youth 432–433 adolescent, 417f, 418
Advanced Placement (AP) development, 236t cultivation theory, 240 bullying and, 181
programs, 223–224 conscientiousness, 258 cultural capital, 394–395, 399 causes, 447–448
big fish-little pond effect, consolidated identity, 274 cultural perspective crime victimization and, 436
201–202 contexts of adolescence, 9–11 on adolescent sexuality, 360 dating behavior and, 343–344
characteristics of, 211–212 achievement and, 390. See also boundaries of adolescence, 6t development during adolescence,
college-bound students, 211–213 achievement cultural differences in patterns of 417
enrollment growth, 211, 211f, 212f dating and, 338–339. See also intimacy, 330 disordered eating and, 49, 50, 51
identity development and, dating on dating, 338 early maturation and, 44
272–273 families, 10. See also family expectations for behavioral electronic media and, 246
impact on earnings, 397 occupational choice, 410–412 autonomy, 300–301 of ethnic minority youth, 276
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-27 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Subject Index I-27

false self-behavior, 257 dopamine, 69, 425, 442 thinking in multiple dimensions ethnography, 162
friendship and, 327 dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and, 60 dynamics of popularity, 175–176
genetic factors in, 72 68–69 video games, 97, 238, 239t, 240, of social interaction and peer
marital conflict and, 142 dropping out, 405–407 242–244 culture, 162
mild depressive disorder, 443t big fish-little pond effect, 202 violations of expectations event-related potentials (ERPs), 66
as mood, syndrome, and correlates of, 405–407 and, 127 evidence-based practices, 441, 448
disorder, 442 dropout rates, 109 youth culture and, 156 exercise, 46, 47f
nature and prevalence of, employment and, 227, 228–229 emerging adulthood, 6–7, 90–95 exo-systems, 10
442–445 impact of, 213, 215 features of, 91 experience sampling method
neighborhood conditions parenting style and, 393 lifestyle patterns of, 92f (ESM), 230–231
and, 114 school factors in, 407 psychological versus social, 93 experiential intelligence, 74
obesity and, 46 teen pregnancy and, 375, 378 psychological well-being in, extended family, 332–333
parenting styles and, 131 types of dropouts, 406 92–95 externalizing disorders, 418,
poverty and, 146, 147 drug education, 430 as universal, 91–92, 103 431–441
rejection and, 178 drugs. See substance use and abuse emotional autonomy, 288–294 categories of, 431–433
selection versus socialization dyscalculia, 201 detachment and, 289, 291, 294 causes of antisocial behavior,
and, 172 dysgraphia, 201 individuation and, 290, 291–292 437–440
self-esteem and, 265 dyslexia, 201 intimacy and, 318 changes in juvenile offending
sex differences in, 444–445 parenting practices and, 292–294 over time, 434–437
sexual abuse and, 369 research on, 290–294 comorbidity, 418–419
sexual activity and, 357 E timing of, 302 developmental progression of
social support and, 331–332 Emotional Intelligence (Goleman), 73 antisocial behavior, 434
desegregation early adolescence, 6 emotional mimicry, 327 prevention and treatment,
ethnic, 203 early intervention programs, emotional perspective 440–441
mainstreaming in classrooms, 398–399, 441 boundaries of adolescence, 6t problem behavior syndrome, 419
201–202 early maturity, 42, 43–44, 338–339 risk-taking behavior in, 80–82 problem clusters, 419–420
detachment, 289. See also early starters, 342–343, 357 empathy, 306, 326, 327, 439 social control theory, 420
attachment eating disorders. See disordered “empty nest” stage, 124 extracurricular activities, 195, 202,
emotional autonomy and, 289, eating endocrine system, 22–26. See also 209, 214, 215–216, 221–222,
291, 294 ecological perspective on human hormones 222f, 227
importance of maintaining development, 9–11, 115 adrenarche, 24 athletic performance, 231–233
connections, 291 educational achievement. See hormonal feedback loop, 23–24 impact on development,
psychoanalytic theory and, 289 academic achievement menarche, 22, 30–31, 33–35, 231–233
research on, 289 educational attainment, 397 40–41 extraversion, 258
developmental readiness, 43 educational perspective, boundaries engagement of students, 205–206, extrinsic motivation, 390
developmental trajectories, 426 of adolescence, 6t 207–209
diathesis-stress model, 447–448 egocentrism, 58–59, 261 environmental factors
diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), ejaculation, 29, 40, 41 achievement and, 392–397 F
65–66 electroencephalography (EEG), 66 in adolescent development, 136
digital divide, 238 electronic media in pubertal maturation, 33–35 Facebook, 244, 246
discontinuous transitions, 103 adolescent consumer and, 248 epiphysis, 26 facial expressions, social brain and,
discrimination adolescent development and, Eriksonian theory, 14, 266–269, 320 71
academic achievement and, 400 244–247 estrogens, 23–24, 37, 358 false-self behavior, 257–258
ethnic identity development, adolescent parenthood, 375 ethnic differences. See also minority familism, 125
276–279 aggression and, 238, 239, 241 groups and specific groups family, 10, 119–149. See also fathers;
dishonesty, 291, 294 body dissatisfaction of adolescent in academic achievement, mothers; parents; siblings
disordered eating, 28, 48–51. girls and, 247–248 399–402 achievement and, 204, 206, 207,
See also obesity clarity of social redefinition in adolescent parenthood, 398–399
anorexia nervosa, 48, 49–51 and, 102 375–376, 378 adolescence as “storm and stress”
binge eating disorder, as context of adolescence, 11 in age of sexual initiation, period, 13, 17–18, 39–40,
48–49 cyberbullying, 180–181, 246 353–354, 355 120–122, 126, 259, 260, 289
bulimia, 48, 49–51 dating relationships and, 345 clique membership and, 167–168 adolescent development and,
gender and, 282 disordered eating and, 48 crowd membership and, 165 128–135, 149
mass media influences on, 48 free time and, 220 effects of desegregation, 203 autonomy and attachment in,
sexual abuse and, 369 friendship and, 244–246 in expectations for behavioral 133–134
treating, 51 impact of exposure to controver- autonomy, 300–301 behavioral genetics and, 135–138
divided attention, 63 sial content, 241–244 parenting styles and, 130–131, changes in family needs and
divorce, 138, 139, 140–142 importance of adolescents, 90 132–133 functions, 124–125
adaptation to, 141 media saturation, 237–238 in self-esteem, 263–264 in changing society, 138–140
conflict and stress in, 141, new media, 237, 241–242 in substance use and abuse, continuity of social redefinition
143–144 obesity and, 244–245 425–426 and, 105–106
custody following, 143–144 online discrimination, 278 in timing and tempo of puberty, disordered eating and, 50–51
effects of, 140, 141–142 patterns of media use, 237–238, 32, 43 divorce, 140–142, 143
extended family role in, 239t ethnic identity, 273–280 family systems theory, 123
332–333 psychosocial problems in academic achievement and, generation gap and, 15–16,
genetic influences on, 141 adolescence, 416 278–279 120–121, 123–124
longer-term effects of, 143 sexuality and, 237, 239, 241–242, development of, 273–276 importance of, 149
quality of family relationships, 246–247, 350 discrimination and its effects, influences on pubertal
140–141 sleep patterns and, 38, 39 276–279 maturation, 33, 39–40
sexual activity in household and, social networking sites, 325f multidimensional model of racial marital conflict, 141, 142–144
361–362 substance use and abuse and, identity (MMRI), 277–278 midlife stage for parents, 40, 93,
stress and, 141, 447 243–244, 421 multiethnic adolescents, 263f, 123–124
youth culture and, 156 theories of media influence and 279–280 parenting styles, 113, 113f,
domestic violence, 142 use, 239–241 ethnic socialization, 275 129–133
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-28 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-28 Subject Index

family—Cont. selection versus socialization and, relational aggression and, 176, hooking up, 355
parent-teenager conflict, 171–173 177–178 hormones, 22–26
120–122, 126 self-disclosure in, 324, 327, 328, sexual activity and, 353f, adolescent sexuality and, 351,
parent-teenager relations, 17–18 338, 346 353–354, 364 358–359
poverty, 145–147 similarity between friends, sexual maturation in, 29–31, homosexuality and, 366
remarriage, 138, 139, 143, 144–145 171–173 42–44 hormonal feedback loop, 23–24
role in friendship, 170–171 stability of adolescent, 173 susceptibility to peer influence, HPG axis, 23–25
sex differences in family substance use and abuse, 429 299–300 influence on adolescent
relationships, 127–128 friends with benefits, 355 timing and tempo of puberty, 32 development, 25–26, 33,
sibling relationships in, 134–135 functional connectivity, 69, 70f glands, 23 35–40, 448
special family forms, 138, functional magnetic resonance gonadotropin-releasing hormone melatonin and sleep patterns,
147–148 imaging (fMRI), 65–66 (GnRH) neurons, 23, 25 37–40
in substance use and abuse, fundamental changes of gonads, 23 sex hormones, 23f, 23–24
428–429 adolescence, 7–8 gonorrhea, 371, 372 hostile attributional bias, 178–179,
transformations in family rela- biological transitions, 7, 9, Google, 244 439
tions, 125–127 21–53 gossip, 175, 178 HPA (hypothalamic-pituitary-
Family Guy (TV program), 60 cognitive transitions, 7–8, 9, graduated driver licensing, 52, 83 adrenocortical) axis, 447
Family Stress Model, 146 55–83 grandparents, 142, 332–333 HPG axis, 23–25
family systems theory, 123 social transitions, 8, 9, 85–115 Greatest generation, 155t human papillomavirus (HPV), 371
fathers. See also family; mothers; future orientation, 255 Great Recession, 107, 224, 248, 411 Hurricane Katrina, 107
parents guns hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocor-
adolescent relationships lethal school violence, 210 tical (HPA) axis, 447
with, 330 G neighborhood conditions hypothalamus, 24
attachment to, 323 and, 114 hypothetical thinking, 57–58
homosexuality and, 366 gambling, 95 suicide and, 446
teen pregnancy and, 374–375, 378 gangs and gang violence, 436
feared self, 257 nature of, 169–170 I
fearlessness, 438–439 selection versus socialization and, H
fear of failure, 387 172–173 iatrogenic effects, 171
feedback loops, 23–24 gateway drugs, 426 Happy days generation, 155t ideal body types, 51
female genital mutilation, 102 gender differences Harlem Children’s Zone, 193 ideal self, 257
feminine gender roles, 282–283 in arrest rates, 435–436 Hazelwood v. Kuhlmeier, 97 identity, 11–12, 253–283. See also
finances. See also poverty; socioeco- in religiosity, 312 health care. See adolescent peer groups; social redefinition
nomic status (SES); work gender identity, 365 health care adolescent identity crisis, 14, 254,
changes in family, 125 gender intensification hypothesis, height 265–269
effects of financial strain, 146 281 adolescent growth spurt, 26–28 as adolescent issue, 254–255
electronic media and adolescent gender-role development, 280–283. lifetime trends in, 74 burden of “acting White,”
consumer, 248 See also sex differences as stable characteristic, 259 400–401
money management skills, 226 depression and, 444–445 Heinz dilemma (Kohlberg), changes in self-conception,
premature affluence, 226 femininity in, 282–283 303–304 256–259
firearms. See guns masculinity in, 282–283 herpes, 371 changes in self-esteem, 259–265
Five C’s of positive youth develop- occupational achievement and, high school. See secondary changes throughout life cycle,
ment, 235–236, 236t 409, 411f education 254–255
five-factor model, 258 socialization in, 280–282 Hispanic youth. See Latino youth crowd membership and, 164–165
flow experience, 231 generational dissonance, 125 historical perspective ethnic identity development,
formal jobs, 225 generation gap, 15–16, 120–121, adolescence in, 16, 89–91 273–280
formal operations period, 61–62, 123–124 origins of adolescence, 153–154 gender-role development,
304–305 Generation Y, 154, 155t HIV/AIDS, 52, 53, 355, 360, 372 280–283
foster care, 138, 148, 333 Generation Z, 154, 155t Hodgson v. Minnesota, 97 peer group and, 183
France, adolescent parenthood, 375 genetic factors holiday effect, 354 research on identity develop-
fraternities, 98 achievement and, 394 home environment. See also family; ment, 269–273
free time, 229–231 in adolescent development, 136 fathers; mothers sexual orientation and, 364–367
adolescent development and, behavioral genetics, 135–138 achievement and, 392–395 identity achievement, 270–272
249–250 in brain development, 71–72 parental expectations, 392–393 identity crisis, 14, 265–269
time use in contemporary in divorced families, 141 parental values, 392–393 Erikson’s theoretical framework,
America, 220–222 in marital instability, 361 quality of, 394–395 266–269
time use in other countries, 222 in pubertal maturation, 32–33 home leaving midlife, 40, 123–124, 254
Freudian theory, 14 Gen X, 154, 155t current trends in, 106f, 106–107 occupational, 267
friendship. See also cliques; crowds; Getting By (Public Agenda), 208 “empty nest” stage, 124 problems in identity develop-
intimacy; peer groups gifted students, 201–202 “launching” children in puberty, ment, 268–269
achievement and, 395–397 girls. See also gender-role develop- 88, 98, 106f resolving, 268, 270–271
caring and concern in, 326 ment; sex differences homework, 222, 223–224, 401 social context of identity devel-
changes in nature of, 324–325 bad girls, 435 homicide, 52, 53, 436 opment, 266–268
common interests among friends, body dissatisfaction and, 28, 36, homosexuality, 365–366 identity diffusion, 268, 270–272
168–171 49, 247–248 HIV/AIDs and, 52, 53, 355, identity versus, 266
conflict in, 324–325, 326, 327 friendship with the other sex and, 360, 372 identity foreclosure, 269, 270, 312
electronic media and, 245, 246 333–335 intimacy and, 319–320 identity status, 269–271
intimacy in, 316, 324–325, gender-role development, lesbian, gay, bisexual, and identity versus identity diffusion,
333–335 280–283 transgender (LGBT) youth, 266
jealousy in, 324–325, 344 intimacy and, 324, 326–328 341, 366–367 iGeneration, 154, 155t
knowing who friends are, 325 jealousy of, 324 lesbian and gay parents, 138, imaginary audience, 58–59
in network of relationships, 135 meaning of sex and, 364 147–148 immigration
and the other sex, 333–335 prosocial moral reasoning Hong Kong, parent-teenager academic achievement and, 401
popularity versus, 176 and, 307 conflict, 121 compulsory education and, 190
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-29 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Subject Index I-29

ethnic identity development, 273, suicide, 445–446 Latino youth. See also ethnic school violence and, 209–210
275–276 internal working model, 321 differences social cognition and, 78
expectations for behavioral Internet. See also electronic media academic achievement and, zero tolerance policies in
autonomy, 301 adolescent development and, 399–403 schools, 210
generational dissonance, 125 244–246 achievement gap and, 191–192, leisure activities, 11, 229–236
immigrant paradox, 276, 401 cyberbullying, 180–181, 246 193 expansion of, 224
“perpetual foreigner” status, 276 digital divide, 238 adolescent parenthood and, 376 experience sampling method
substance use and abuse, 426 Internet addiction, 245–246 clique membership and, 167, 168f (ESM), 230–231
success of immigrants, 401–402 Internet predators, 237, 246–247 college enrollment of, 211, 212, free time and, 220–222, 229–231,
youth employment and, 224 viral marketing, 248 212f 249–250
impression management, 261 interpersonal perspective, 318–320 crime victimization and, 436 mood and, 229–231
impulse control, 295–297, 438 boundaries of adolescence, 6t current trends in home promoting positive youth
impulsiveness, 81, 358, 361 interpersonal development leaving, 107 development, 235–236
income inequality, 108 during adolescence, 319–320 dating relationships of, 338 structured, 231–233
independence stages of interpersonal needs, differences in self-esteem, unstructured, 233–235
autonomy versus, 286 318–319 262–263, 263f, 264 leptin, 25
development of, 287 interracial relations, 275 disengagement from school, 198 lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender
indifferent parents, 130, 206, 293, 294 intimacy, 12, 315–346 disordered eating among, 48, 49 (LGBT) youth, 341, 366–367
individual differences as adolescent issue, 317–318 dropping out, 109, 405 lesbian and gay parents, 138,
in adolescent intelligence, 72–76 attachment and, 320–323 educational reform and, 191 147–148
in effects of divorce, 141–142 changes in display of, 325–326 ethnic composition of U.S., 109f LGBT youth, 341, 366–367
in pubertal maturation, 32–33 changes in targets of, 328–333 ethnic identity development, 273, life-course-persistent offenders,
in religiosity, 311–312 characteristics of, 316, 317 276, 277 437–440
in susceptibility to peer influence, cultural differences in patterns false optimism and, 400 life cycle
299–300 of, 330 generational dissonance, 125 identity changes over, 254–255
transition into secondary school, in dating, 316, 317, 337–340 initiation ceremonies, 98, 98f, 100 independence over, 287
198–199 in dating and romantic involvement in antisocial limbic system, 425
individuation, 290 relationships, 335–346 activity, 170 risk and reward and, 69–70
emotional autonomy and, 290, in friendship, 316, 324–325, obesity among, 45–46, 46f role of, 68
291–292 333–335 parenting styles and, 132–133 social brain and, 71
triggers of, 291–292 importance of, 317 parents as targets of intimacy, logical thinking, 82, 86
indulgent parents, 130, 206, nature of, 11, 317 329–330 longitudinal studies, 36
293, 294 peer group and, 183 school violence and, 209–210 long-term memory, 63
Industrial Revolution, 16, sex differences in, 326–328 sex differences in friendship, 328 Lumi people (New Guinea), 22
89–90 Sullivan’s theory of interpersonal sexual activity and, 353–354, 361
infancy, attachment in, 320–322, 323f development, 318–320, 322, single parenthood and, 138, 139
informal jobs, 225 324, 325, 328–329, 334, 337 social support and, 332 M
information-processing perspective, introspection, 58–59 stereotype threat and, 388
62–65 inventionists, 16, 88–95 substance use and abuse, 426 machismo, 328
attention in, 63, 64–65 invulnerability, 79–80 teen pregnancy and parenthood, macro-systems, 10
memory in, 63 IQ tests, 73, 74–75, 75f, 439 378, 379 magnet schools, 203–204
metacognition in, 64–65 Iraq war, 107 timing and tempo of puberty, 32 mainstreaming, 201–202
nature of, 63 Ireland, adolescent parenthood, 375 total media exposure, 238 majority, age of, 87
organization in, 64 transition to adulthood, 108 makeup, 99
speed in, 64 unemployment rate, 109 marijuana, 421–424, 427
initiation ceremonies J laws, 78 marriage, 91, 101, 103, 108. See also
in contemporary society, 95, learned helplessness, 391 divorce; parents
97–100 Japan, adolescent parenthood, 375 learning disability, 201 age of, 335–336
in traditional cultures, 101–102 jealousy, 324–325, 344 attention deficit/hyperactivity conflict in, 141, 142–144
insecure attachment, 322, 323 Jewish youth disorder (ADHD), 202 masculine gender roles, 282–283
Inside High School (Cusick), 160 circumcision, 102 big fish-little pond effect, mass media. See electronic media
insomnia, 39 differences in self-esteem, 264 201–202 mastery motivation, 387–388
instrumental aggression, 175 initiation ceremonies, 99, 100 types, 201 mastery orientation, 390
intelligence, 72–76, 136 “jocks,” 159–160, 164, 165, 232 learning theories, 14–15 masturbation, 29, 41, 352–353,
academic achievement and, junior high school, 196 behaviorism, 15 363, 364
397, 398 secondary versus elementary social learning theory, 15, 329 mathematics
beliefs about, 389–390 schools, 197–199 Leave It to Beaver (TV program), 102 achievement gap between males
culture and, 75 transition to secondary school, legal perspective and females, 389
Gardner’s theory of multiple 196–197, 391 adolescents as criminal Brazilian street merchants
intelligences, 74 juvenile justice system, 95–96 defendants, 96–97 and, 75
improving with education, 215 juvenile offending, 433, 434–437 boundaries of adolescence, 6t, gender roles and, 392–393
measuring IQ, 73, 74–75, 439 95–97 National Assessment of
Sternberg’s “triarchic” theory, decision making in, 96–97, 296f, Educational Progress
73–74 K 296–297, 298–299, 374 (NAEP), 402–403
intergenerational conflict, 15–16, false evidence, 298–299 peer group achievement and, 395
120–121, 123–124, 291–292, 331 kissing, 352, 353 inconsistent adolescent legal school reform and, 191
internalizing disorders, 418, Korea, adolescent parenthood, 375 status, 97 thinking about possibilities in, 56
442–449 juvenile justice system, 95–96 maturational deviance
causes, 447–448 juvenile offending in, 433, 434–437 hypothesis, 43
comorbidity, 420–421 L lethal school violence, 210 Mean Girls (movie), 178
depression, 442–445, 447–448 minimum legal drinking age, 53, meanness, 176, 177–178
negative emotionality, 420–421 late adolescence, 6 288, 430 media. See electronic media
prevention and treatment, late bloomers, 343 police questioning under media practice model, 240
448–449 latency stage, 14 pressure, 298–299 melatonin, 37–40
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-30 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-30 Subject Index

memory transition to adulthood and, of adolescent brain development, emotional autonomy and,
in information-processing 108–110 45–46, 65–66 293–294
perspective, 63 mixed-sex groups, 160–161 methods used, 65–66 patterns of intimacy and, 330
long-term memory, 63 mnemonic devices, 58, 64 of obesity, 45–46 parents. See also family; fathers;
mnemonic devices, 58, 64 Monitoring the Future study, neurons, 66 mothers; single parenthood
working memory, 63, 64–65 422, 423–424 neurotransmitters, 66 achievement and, 392–393
menarche, 22, 30–31, 33–35, moods. See also anxiety; depression New Hope program, 113 adolescent parenthood, 375–379
40–41 adolescent, 417f, 418 new media, 237, 241–242. See also adoptive, 138, 147
mental health. See also depression; leisure activities and, 229–231 electronic media antisocial activity and, 170–171
substance use and abuse puberty and, 36–37 No Child Left Behind Act, 191–192 communication with adolescents
abortion and, 373–374 stability and changes in, 260, 260f noncoital activity, 352–353 about sexuality, 360–361
age of onset of psychiatric moral development, 302–306 nonshared environmental influ- emotional autonomy and,
disorders, 70f assessing moral reasoning, ences, 136 292–294
continuity of social redefinition 302–303 non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI), 446 foster care, 138, 148, 333
and, 106–107 Kohlberg’s theory of moral North Africa, female genital mutila- influence on adolescent decision
crowd membership and, 164 reasoning, 303–305 tion, 102 making, 297–298
dating relationships and, moral behavior and, 305–306 Northern Ireland, impact of influence on adolescent sexuality,
343–344 Piagetian theory of, 302–303, violence, 114 359–362
disordered eating and, 50, 51 304–305 Norway, compulsory schooling intergenerational conflict,
electronic media and, 244, moral disengagement, 306 in, 75 15–16, 120–121, 123–124,
245–246 mortality, adolescent, 52 nurturance rights, 78 291–292, 331
in emerging adulthood, 92–93, mothers. See also family; fathers; nutrition involvement in education of
93f parents obesity and, 46, 47 children, 399
of ethnic minority youth, adolescent relationships with, 330 in pubertal maturation, 33 midlife crisis, 40, 123–124, 254
275–276, 278 attachment to, 323 monitoring by, 127, 339
fear of failure, 387 homosexuality and, 366 occupational achievement and,
friendship and, 327, 346 teen pregnancy and, 364, 369, O 409–410
parenting styles and, 131, 294 370, 372–379 as targets of adolescent intimacy,
of parents, 124 motivation “Obama effect,” 388–389 329–332
poverty and, 147 achievement, 386–388 obesity, 28, 45–48 as targets of intimacy, 327–338
rejection and, 178–179 performance, 387–388 correlates and consequences of, parietal cortex, 67
risk and reward and, 69–70 multidimensional model of racial 45–46 participant observation
selection versus socialization identity (MMRI), 277–278 electronic media and, 244–245 changes in clique and crowd
and, 172 multidimensional thinking, 59–60 preventing and treating, 47–48 structure over time,
sexual abuse and, 369 multidisciplinary perspective, 4–5 secular trend in, 34 161–163
sexual identity and, 367 multiethnic youth obsessive-compulsive disorder, 50 nature of, 160
sleep patterns and, 39 differences in self-esteem, 263f occupational achievement, 384, 385, part-time employment, 223, 223f,
teen pregnancy and, 377–378 identity development, 279–280 407–412 224, 227, 286
mentalizing, 76, 77 multiple-choice questions, 63 development of occupational PATHS (Promoting Alternative
mentoring programs, 110 multiple intelligences theory plans, 408 Thinking Strategies), 182–183
meso-systems, 10 (Gardner), 74 influences on occupational peak height velocity, 26
metacognition, 58–59 music choices, 408–412 peer groups, 10, 151–184. See also
adolescent egocentrism, extracurricular activities, 231 occupational attainment, 410 friendship; siblings
58–59, 261 music videos, 241, 242, 421 occupational identity crisis, 267 achievement and, 395–397
in information-processing violent songs, 242 Odd Girl Out (Simmons), 177–178 changes during adolescence,
perspective, 64–65 myelination, 67, 68–69 openness to experience, 258 157–158, 160–163
nature of, 58 operant conditioning, 15 cliques, 158–159, 160–163,
methylphenidate (Ritalin), 202 opposite-sex friendships, 173 166–174
micro-systems, 10 N oppositional-defiant disorder, cohorts, 153–154, 155t
middle adolescence, 6 431–432 crowds, 157–158, 159–165
middle school, 196 National Assessment of Educational oral sex, 351–352, 353, 355 dating and, 339–340
secondary versus elementary Progress (NAEP), 402–403 orbitofrontal cortex, 69 decision making and, 297–300
schools, 197–199 Native Americans. See American organismic theories differences in self-esteem,
sexual harassment in, 367 Indian/Alaska Native youth Eriksonian theory, 14, 266–269, 263, 264
transition to secondary school, negative emotionality, 420–421 320 friendship with the other sex,
196–197, 391 negative feedback seeking, 327 Freudian theory, 14 333–335
midlife crisis, 40, 123–124, 254 negative identity, 269 Piagetian theory, 14 importance of, 125
midlife stage, 40, 93, 123–124 neighborhood conditions, 110–115 organization, of information pro- individual susceptibility to, 81
Millennial generation, 154, 155t adolescent sexual activity and, cessing, 64 intimacy and, 317
minority youth. See also American 362, 363 organizational role (of hormones), nature of, 152, 153–163
Indian/Alaska Native youth; collective efficacy, 112–113, 235 25 in network of relationships, 135
Asian youth; Black youth; La- effects of school desegregation, ovaries, 23 occupational achievement and,
tino youth 203 overcrowding, 196 409–410
Americanization process and, impact of stress, 113–114 overprotectiveness, 292 origins in contemporary society,
108–109, 168, 190, 354 limited access to resources, overt antisocial behavior, 434 153–154
education in inner cities, 193 114–115 oxytocin, 445 popularity and, 156, 174–176
ethnic composition of processes of neighborhood as problem versus necessity,
schools, 203 influences, 112–115 154–157
ethnic identity development, relocation programs, 111–112, P psychosocial development and,
273–280 436 183–184
parenting styles and, 132–133 “nerds,” 159, 162, 165 parental demandingness, 129–130 pubertal maturation and, 40
political behavior and, 309 neuroendocrine problems, parental responsiveness, 129–130 rejection and, 176–179
school violence and, 209–210 447, 448 parenting styles, 113, 113f, 129–133 self-image changes and, 261
tracking and, 199, 207 neuroimaging studies achievement and, 393–394, 396 sexual activity and, 183, 362–363
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-31 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Subject Index I-31

student engagement in school family relations and, 123–124, adolescent sexuality and, 350, racism, 274–275
and, 208–209 125–126 351, 358 rape
as targets of adolescent intimacy, in occupational choice, 411–412 body dissatisfaction in, 28, 36, 49 date rape, 343, 367–368
330–332 sexual violence and, 354 changes in peer group during, 158 statutory rape, 87
unstructured leisure activities precocious sex, 351 dating and, 28, 35, 40, 51 reactive aggression, 175
and, 234–235, 440 preconventional moral reasoning, depression and, 445 reading, National Assessment of
victimization and harassment, 303–304 development of autonomy in, 287 Educational Progress (NAEP),
179–183 prefrontal cortex, 67–69, 72, 425 development of intimacy in, 317, 402–403
penis, 29, 30f, 102 pregnancy, 364 319–320 recapitulation, 13, 17
People’s Republic of China, parent- contraception and, 369–371 disordered eating in, 48–51 reference groups
teenager conflict in, 121 drug abuse during, 439 in ecological perspective on hu- crowds as, 164–165
perceived popularity, 174–175 sexual abuse and, 369 man development, 9, 115 nature of, 164–165
Percocet, 422–423 teen pregnancy, 364, 369, 370, endocrine system in, 22–26 reinforcement, 15
performance, competence 372–379 family relationships in, 39–40, rejection, 176–179
versus, 62 premature affluence, 226 126–127 impact of, 178–179
performance motivation, 387–388 preoperational period, 61 first ejaculation in, 29, 40–41 intimacy and, 326–327
performance orientation, 390 preschool intervention programs, in Freudian theory, 14 rejection sensitivity, 321, 344
personal fables, 59, 79–80 398–399, 441 hormonal influences in, 25–26, relational aggression, 177–178
personality primary control strategies, 449–450 33, 35–40, 448 relational aggression, 177–179
dimensions in adolescence, 258 priorities, age differences in, 80 identity development in, 254–255 relativism, 60–61
as multifaceted, 257 probability, 60 immediate impact of, 36–40 religion
in substance use and abuse, 428 problem behavior syndrome, 419 “launching” children in, 88, 98, adolescent sexual activity
pheromones, 33 problem solving 108f and, 362
physical activity, 46, 47f deductive reasoning in, 57, 61–62 menarche in, 22, 30–31, 33–35, beliefs during adolescence,
Piagetian theory, 14, 61–62, hypothetical thinking in, 57–58 40–41 309–312, 310f
302–303, 304–305 memory in, 63 moodiness in, 36–37 impact of religious involvement
piercing, 99, 267, 267f social problem-solving programs, obesity and, 45–48 on development, 312
PISA (Program for International 182–183 peer relationships in, 40 individual differences in
Student Assessment), 404t thinking about possibilities in, physical manifestations of, 22, religiosity, 311–312
pituitary gland, 23 56–58 26–32 patterns of religious
platonic relationships, 320, Project DARE, 430 psychological and social impact involvement, 311
333–335 promiscuous sex, 351, 352, of, 35–45 religious ceremonies, 98–100
political thinking, 308–309 355, 356f rise of compulsory education volunteering and, 307
changes in, 308–309 property crime, 433 and, 16 religiosity, 311–312
political behavior and, 307, 309 prosocial behavior, 302, 306–307 sequence of physical changes relocation programs, 111–112, 436
voting rights, 95, 104, 307 prosocial reasoning in, 29f remarriage, 138, 139, 143, 144–145
popularity, 156 changes in, 306–307 sexual maturation during, 29–31, remedial education, 403
aggression and, 175 prosocial behavior and, 302, 41–44 reminiscence bump (Thorne), 272
dating behavior and, 343 306–307 sleep patterns in, 37–39 residential arrangements, 91
determinants, 174–175 prostitution, 369 social transitions during, 86 resilience, 449
dynamics of, 175–176 protective factors, 429–430 as “storm and stress” period, 13, response inhibition, 69
friendship versus, 176 pseudomaturity, 41, 342–343 17–18, 39–40, 120–122, 126, responsibility
helping unpopular teens, psychoanalytic theory, 14, 289 259, 260, 289 agency and, 270–271
182–183 psychological control, 292–294 as term, 7, 22 autonomy and, 290
rejection versus, 176–179 psychopaths, 432–433 timing and tempo of, 32–35, employment and, 226–227
types of, 174–175 psychosocial crises, 14 41–44 reverse causation, 240–241
pornography, 240, 246–247, 357 psychosocial development, 11–12. as trigger for individuation, reward sensitivity, decision making
positive youth development, See also social perspective 291–292 and, 295
235–236 achievement in, 11, 12, 87. triggers of, 24–25 rights, 78
possibilities, thinking about, 56–58 See also achievement pubic hair, 29–31, 30f, 31f risk factors, 362
possible selves, 255 autonomy in, 11, 12. See also punishment, 15 for substance use and abuse,
postconventional moral reasoning, autonomy pyschosocial problems, 415–450 428–430
303, 304–305 free time and, 249–250 comorbity, 418–421 for suicide, 446
posttraumatic stress disorder, identity in, 11–12. See also externalizing disorders, 418, risk-taking behavior, 78–83. See also
114, 436 identity 419–420, 431–441 substance use and abuse; teen
poverty, 145–147 intimacy in, 11, 12, 346. See also general principles, 416–418 pregnancy; tobacco use
achievement gap and, 193 intimacy internalizing disorders, 418, age differences in values and pri-
adolescent health and, 53 peer group and, 183–184. See also 420–421, 442–449 orities, 80
adolescent parenthood and, 376 peer groups stress and coping, 449–450 behavioral decision theory, 79
effects of financial strain, 146 problems in, 12 substance use and abuse. See car accidents and, 52, 81–83, 82f,
extent of, 138–140 psychosocial maturity in substance use and abuse 427, 429, 431
impact of chronic poverty, 146–147 adulthood, 101 contextual influences on risk
inner city schools, 193 puberty and, 35–45 taking, 80–82
neighborhood conditions and school in adolescent, 215 Q emotional influences on risk
adolescent development, sexual activity and, 356–357, 358 taking, 80–82
110–115 sexuality in, 11, 12 Queen Bees and Wannabees invulnerability and, 79–80
relocation of families, psychosocial moratorium, 267–268, (Wiseman), 178 logic and intuition in, 82
111–112, 436 270–272 quinceañera, 98, 98f, 100 moral reasoning and, 306
transition to adulthood and, psychosocial problems, age of onset, personal fables and, 59, 79–80
108–109 416–417 reducing adolescent risk taking,
youth employment and, 228 puberty R 82–83
power achievement and, 385 response inhibition and, 69
adolescent marginality and, 15 adolescent growth spurt, 26–28 Race to the Top, 192 risk and reward and, 69–71
in dating relationships, 338 adolescent health care and, 51–53 racial socialization, 275 risky sex, 357, 358, 360, 369–380
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-32 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-32 Subject Index

risk-taking behavior—Cont. mentoring programs, 110 self-descriptions, 257 in intimacy, 326–328


self-control and, 69–71, 72 non-college-bound students, self-determination rights, 78 in meaning of sex, 363–364
sensation seeking, 81, 81f, 213–214 self-disclosure in occupational achievement,
358, 440 reform efforts, 190–193, 223–224 in friendship, 324, 327, 328, 409, 411f, 411–412
risky sex, 369–380 role of, 192–193 338, 346 in rate of sexual intercourse, 355
adolescent parenthood, 375–379 school size, 194–196 with parents, 329, 330 in relational aggression, 176,
contraceptive use, 369–371 secondary education, 188 self-efficacy, 389f, 389–390, 390 177–178
sex education, 371, 379–380, 430 social organization of schools, in romantic partner preferences,
self-esteem, 255, 259–265
sexually transmitted diseases 194–205 341
(STDs), 52, 53, 80, 102, 355, tracking in, 196, 199–202 body dissatisfaction and, 28, in self-esteem, 262–263
369, 371–372, 379 violence in, 209–210 36, 49 in sexual maturation in puberty,
teen pregnancy, 364, 369, 370, school performance, 397 bullying and, 181 29–31
372–379 school reform, 190–193 components of, 261–262 social redefinition and, 98–99
Ritalin (methylphenidate), 202 school size, 194–196, 201–202 consequences of high or low, in somatic development, 22,
rite of passage, 8, 98–100 schools within schools, 195 264–265 26–32
romantic relationships. See also dat- school violence, 209–210 ethnic differences in, 263–264 sex education, 371, 379–380, 430
ing; intimacy; sexual activity school vouchers, 193 in ethnic identity development, sex-role behavior, 365
age of, 336–337 science 274 sex segregation, in cliques, 166–167
changes in peer group and, achievement gap between males false self-behavior, 257–258 sexting, 247
160–161 and females, 389 family relationships and, 133–134 sexual abuse, 368–369
friendship with the other sex National Assessment of group differences in, 262–264 sexual activity, 339, 351–364
and, 334 Educational Progress influences on, 264 changes over time, 355–356
nature and significance of (NAEP), 402–403 internalizing disorders and, 442 condom use, 350, 360, 369–371,
romance, 337–338 peer group achievement and, 395 obesity and, 46 372, 379
phases of romance, 340–341 school reform and, 191 patterns over time, 261 context in, 359
prevalence of, 336–337 thinking about possibilities in, puberty and, 36, 41–44 hormonal influences on, 351,
selection versus socialization 56–57 sex differences in, 262–263 358–359
and, 172 secondary control strategies, 450 sexual abuse and, 369 household composition and,
sex differences in partner secondary education sexual activity and, 356–357 351–352
preferences, 341 classroom climate, 195–196, stability and changes in, 259–262 parental influences on, 360–362
Roper v. Simmons, 96, 97 205–211, 214–215 substance use and abuse and, 430 peer group influences on,
routine activity theory, 233–234 college-bound students, 211–213 transition into middle school/ 362–363
rumination, 445 comprehensive high school, 190, junior high, 197–198 promiscuous sex, 351, 352, 355,
running away, 95 194–196 self-fulfilling prophecy, 120, 356f
compulsory, 16, 75, 153, 206–207 psychological development and,
189–190, 196, 223 self-handicapping, 387, 390, 391 356–357, 358
S dropping out, 109, 202, 213, 215, self-image stability, 260–261 sexual orientation and, 364–367
227, 228–229, 405–407 self-reflection, 271 small-scale deviance and, 358
Safe Dates, 367–368 elementary education versus, self-regulation, 438 stages of, 352–353
safe sex, 360 197–199 in decision making, 295–296 substance use and abuse and,
same-sex groups, 160–161, 173, 333, extracurricular activities, 195, peer influence and, 300 355–356, 358, 361, 362
337. See also cliques 202, 209, 214, 215–216, self-reliance, 131, 287, 301 types, 351–354
Sandy Hook Elementary School 221–222, 222f, 227, 231–233 sensation seeking, 81, 81f, 358, 440 violence and, 342, 343, 358
shootings, 210 historical growth of, 211 sense of identity, 255 virginity pledges, 363, 370
sarcasm, 60 international enrollment in, 188f sensorimotor period, 61 sexual harassment, 367–369
SAT (Scholastic Assessment Test), nature of, 188 serotonin, 69, 438 sexual intercourse, 351–352
390–391, 402, 403 non-college-bound students, service learning, 307–308 during adolescence, 353–354
scaffolding, 75 213–214 set point, 23 ethnic differences in age of
scarification, 99, 102 school size, 194–196 Seventeen magazine, 90, 90f initiation, 353–354
Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT), sexual harassment and, 367 sex differences prevalence of, 353
390–391, 402, 403 transition from junior high/ in academic achievement, 389 sexuality, 349–380
school, 11, 187–216. See also middle school, 196–199, 391 in adjustment to remarriage, as adolescent issue, 350–351
achievement; college; second- transition to college, 212–213, 289 144–145 early maturation and, 43–44
ary education secondary sex characteristics, in athletic performance, 27–28, early starters, 342–343, 357
adolescent development and, 25–26, 29–31, 40–41, 359 28f, 43 electronic media and, 239,
215–216 second-class citizens, 15 241–242, 350
in body dissatisfaction, 28
age grouping, 196–197 secular trend Internet predators, 237, 246–247
alternatives, 193, 203–204 in pubertal maturation, 33–35 in brain development, 66 parental influences on, 360–362
characteristics of good schools, as term, 34 in brain organization, 25 peer influences and, 183, 362–363
214–215 secure attachment, 320, 320–323 in depression, 444–445 in psychosocial development,
classroom climate, 195–196, selective attention, 63 in development of secondary sex 11, 12
205–211, 214–215 selective serotonin reuptake inhibi- characteristics, 25–26, risky sex, 369–380
class size, 195–196 tors (SSRIs), 448–449 29–31, 40–44 sex differences in meaning of sex,
college-bound students, 211–213 self-assurance, 131 expectations for behavioral 363–364
common interests among self-awareness, 257 autonomy, 301 sexual abuse, 368–369
friends, 169 self-care, 234–235, 286 in family relationships, 127–128 sexual activity during adoles-
compulsory education, 16, 75, self-conceptions, 255, 256–259 gender-role development, cence, 351–359
153, 189–190, 196, 223 differentiation of self-concept, 256 280–283, 409, 411f sexual harassment, 367–368
dropout rates, 109 false-self behavior, 257–258 in hormone influence on adoles- sexual maturation in puberty,
ethnic composition, 203 organization and integration of cent development, 25–26 29–31
extension of schooling, 286 self-concept, 256–257 in identity development over sexual orientation, 364–367
inner city, 193 self-consciousness, 58–59, 255, 260, time, 271–273 sexually transmitted diseases
junior high/middle school, 442, 444 initiation ceremonies in tradi- (STDs), 52, 53, 80, 102, 355,
196–199, 367 self-control, 69–71, 72 tional cultures, 101–102 369, 371–372, 379
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-33 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

www.mhhe.com/steinberg10e Subject Index I-33

sexual-minority youth, 341 changes in status during adoles- spurious causation, 240–241 social support and, 332
sexual orientation, 364–367 cence, 95–97 standardized testing, 191–192 suicide and, 446
development of sexual identity, elongation of adolescence, 87–88 standards-based reform, 192–193 unstructured leisure activities
366–367 on identity development, Stanford-Binet, 73 and, 234–235
nature of, 365 266–268 status offenses, 95, 433 work and, 227–228
origins of homosexuality, neighborhood conditions and statutory rape, 87 suicidal ideation, 445–446
365–366 adolescent development, STDs (sexually transmitted suicide, 52, 53, 136, 442
same-sex attraction, 364–365 110–115 diseases), 52, 53, 80, 102, 355, poverty and, 147
sexual socialization, 363–364 social redefinition in, 86–87, 369, 371–372, 379 prevalence of, 445–446
shared environmental influences, 97–99 stepfamilies, 143, 144–145 risk factors for, 446
136 social transitions of adolescence, stereotype threat, 388–389 summer school programs, 215
shoplifting, 436 8, 9, 85–115 stimulant medications, 202 synapses, 66–67
sibling deidentification, 137 transition into adulthood, “storm and stress” period, 13, synaptic pruning, 67–68, 68f
sibling rivalry, 137 99–110 17–18, 39–40, 120–122, 126, systems theory, 10
siblings social promotion, 191, 407 259, 260, 289
adolescent sexuality and, 360, 362 social redefinition stress
conflict among, 137 “adult” activities and, 95–97, coping and, 449–450 T
intimacy and, 316, 332 100–101, 104 depression and, 442, 444,
in network of relationships, clarity of, 100–103 445, 447 Tanner stages, 29–31
134–135 continuity of, 103–107 diathesis-stress model, 447–448 tattoos, 99
occupational achievement elements of transition to in divorce, 141, 447 teachers
and, 410 adulthood, 102–103 genetic factors in, 72 curricular requirements of, 214
same-sex versus mixed-sex dyads, nature of, 86 neighborhood conditions and, expectations for students, 120,
134, 134f process of, 97–99 113–114 206–207, 391
Simpsons, The (TV program), 60 psychosocial development and, posttraumatic stress disorder, in good schools, 214–215
single parenthood, 138, 139, 86–87 114, 436 junior high versus elementary,
144–145. See also divorce social roles poverty and, 145–147 197–198
achievement and, 396 achievement and, 385–386 preschool intervention programs, respect for students, 205–206
adolescent parenthood and, in development of autonomy, 398–399 social support and, 333, 407
376–377 288 risk and reward and, 69–71 teenager, 90, 93–94, 220
adolescent sexual activity, in development of intimacy, stress-reactivity and, 438–439, teen pregnancy, 364, 369, 370,
353–354, 361–362 317–318 447 372–379
antisocial peer influence and, 300 gender-role development, work and, 228 abortion, 373–374
autonomy of children and, 286 280–283 stress hormones, 24, 439 adolescent parenthood, 375–379
sexual activity in household and, identity development and, 255 student engagement, 205–206, causes of, 374
361–362 sexuality and, 351 207–209 children of teen mothers,
“sleeper” effects of divorce, 143 social skills, bullying and, 181 substance use and abuse, 12, 52, 376–377
sleep patterns social support, 331–332 421–431 consequences for teen mothers,
influences on, 37–39 grandparents and, 142 adolescent brain and, 424–425 377–378
obesity and, 46 poverty and, 147 alcohol use disorder, 428t prevalence of, 373
smoking. See tobacco use socioeconomic status (SES), 15. behavioral decision theory television. See also electronic media
social brain, 71 See also poverty and, 79 body dissatisfaction and, 247–248
social capital, 204, 395 academic achievement and, binge drinking, 423, 425, clarity of social redefinition
social class, 15 398–399 429–431 and, 102
social cognition, 76–78 adolescent parenthood and, causes and consequences of, content of soap operas, 241, 242
adolescent individuation and, 375–379 426–430 thinking in multiple dimensions
292 early intervention programs, changes over time, 423–424 and, 60
civil liberties, 78 398–399, 441 continuity of social redefinition violent programming, 243
intimacy and, 317 education in inner cities, 193 and, 106 temperament, 141–142
laws, 78 engagement/disengagement from disordered eating and, 50 temporal cortex, 67–68
nature of, 76 school, 199, 215 drugs of choice, 422–423 10 Basic Principles of Good Parent-
rights, 78 occupational achievement early maturation and, 42, ing, The (Steinberg), 131t
social conventions, 77–78 and, 410 43–44, 44f testes, 23
theory of mind, 76 selection versus socialization electronic media and, 243–244, 421 testosterone, 35, 37, 358–359
thinking about relationships, 77 and, 173 ethnic differences in, 425–426 theater productions, 232
social competence tracking and, 199–200 genetic factors in, 72 theoretical perspectives, 13–17
attachment and, 322–323 victimization and, 179–180 HIV/AIDs and, 372 on adolescent thinking, 61–65
social competence training, youth employment and, 228 minimum legal drinking age, 53, anthropological theories, 16–17
430–431 sociological theories, 15–16 288, 430 autonomy in, 289
social control theory, 420 adolescent marginality, 15 nature of substance abuse, 418 biosocial theories, 13
social conventions, 77–78 intergenerational conflict, 15–16 peer group influence on, 298f Eriksonian theory of identity, 14,
social exclusion, attitudes toward, sociometric popularity, 174–175 predictors and consequences 266–269, 320
77, 162–163, 177–178 somatic development, 22, 26–32 of, 427 Freudian theory, 14
socialization adolescent growth spurt, 26–28 during pregnancy, 439 historical theories, 16
ethnic, 278 body dissatisfaction and, 28, prevalence, 422–426 on identity development,
gender-role, 280–282 36, 49 prevention and treatment of, 266–269
selection versus, 171–173 sex differences in muscle and fat, 430–431 information-processing
sex differences in friendship, 328 27–28, 28f, 43 progression of, 426 perspective, 62–65
sexual, 363–364 sororities, 98 reducing, 83 on intelligence, 73–74
social learning theory, 15, 329 South Park (TV program), 60 risk factors, 428–430 on intimacy, 318–324
social perspective speed, of information processing, selection versus socialization and, Kohlberg’s theory of moral
adolescence as social invention, 64, 74 172–173 development, 303–305
88–95 spillover effect, 232 sexual activity and, 355–356, 358, learning theories, 14–15
boundaries of adolescence, 6t spirituality, 311 361, 362 organismic theories, 14
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-34 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

I-34 Subject Index

theoretical perspectives—Cont. sale of violent video games, W continuity of social redefinition


Piagetian theory, 14, 61–62, 242–243 and, 104, 105
302–303, 304–305 school desegregation, 203 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale dropping out and, 227, 228–229
sociological theories, 15–16 unsafe sex, 351 (WAIS-III), 73 education and, 108, 223–224
Sullivan’s theory of interpersonal unwanted sex, 351 Wechsler Intelligence Scale for emerging adulthood and, 92f, 93
development, 318–320, 322, users, 426–427 Children (WISC-IV), 73 formal versus informal jobs, 225
324, 325, 328–329, 334, 337 uses and gratification weight Industrial Revolution, 16, 89–90
theory of mind, 76 approach, 240 adolescent growth spurt, 26–28 Industrial Revolution and, 16,
tobacco. See also substance use and body dissatisfaction and, 28, 36, 49 89–90
abuse disordered eating and, 28, 48–51 minimum-wage jobs, 214, 224
tobacco use, 136, 421 V obesity and, 28, 45–48 occupational achievement, 384,
disordered eating and, 49 White youth. See also ethnic 385, 407–412
early maturation and, 43–44, 44f values differences occupational attainment, 410
electronic media and, 243–244 age differences in, 80 academic achievement and, occupational identity crisis, 267
prevalence of, 422–425 generation gap and, 15–16, 399–403 part-time employment, 223, 223f,
progression of substance use, 120–122, 392–393 achievement gap and, 193 224, 227, 286
426, 427 parental, 392–393 adolescent parenthood and, problem behavior and, 226,
reducing, 53, 83 work values, 408–409 375–376 227–228
work and, 227–228 ventromedial prefrontal cortex, clique membership and, 167–168 rise and fall of student worker,
toddlers, independence of, 287 68–69 college enrollment of, 211, 212f 223–224
tracking, 199–202 Vicodin, 422–423 crime victimization and, 436 self-care of children of working
effects on student achievement, victimization, 179–183 crowd membership and, 165 mothers, 234–235
200–201 bullying and, 172, 176, 179–182 dating relationships of, 338, 342 sex differences in, 98
ethnic and socioeconomic crime, 435–436 differences in self-esteem, teenage employment in other
background in, 199, 207 dating violence, 342, 343, 262–263, 264 nations, 224
impact of track placements, 200, 344–345, 367–368 disordered eating among, 49 unemployment, 109, 213f,
215–216 education in inner cities dropping out, 405 228–229
multitrack programs, 196 and, 193 ethnic composition of U.S., 109f working memory, 63, 64–65
pros and cons of, 199–200 school size and, 195 ethnic identity development, 273 work values, 408–409
students at the extremes, 201–202 school violence and, 209–210 expectations for behavioral au- writing, National Assessment of
traditional cultures video games, 97, 238, 239t, 240, tonomy, 301 Educational Progress (NAEP),
clarity of social redefinition in, 242–244 family relations and, 126–127 402–403
101–102 violence multiethnic youth, 279–280
continuity of social redefinition dating, 342–345, 344f, 367–368 obesity among, 46, 46f
in, 104–105 gangs and, 169–170, parenting styles and, 132–133 Y
transgender, 365 172–173, 436 parents as targets of intimacy,
triarchic theory of intelligence neighborhood conditions 329–330 youth, 90–91, 93–94
(Sternberg), 73–74 and, 114 peer groups and, 157 youth culture, 156–157, 163
trichomoniasis, 371 prosocial moral reasoning poverty and, 139 age segregation and, 156, 166
truancy, 95 and, 307 sex differences in friendship, 328 impact of, 156–157
two-career households, autonomy school, 209–210 sexual activity and, 353–354 orientation toward, 169
of children and, 286 sexual, 354, 367–369 social support and, 332 Youth Internet Safety Survey, 246
video game, 238, 239t, 240, teen pregnancy and parenthood, youth movement, 90–91
242–243 378 YouTube, 244
U violent crime, 52, 53, 433, 436–437 timing and tempo of puberty,
violent songs, 242 32, 40
underachievers, 387 viral marketing, 248 work, 11, 222–229 Z
underperforming schools, 191 virginity pledges, 363, 370 adolescent development and,
unemployment, 109, 213f, 228–229 voice development, 29 226–228 zero tolerance, 210
U.S. Department of Justice, 246 volunteerism, 306–308 adolescent work environment, zone of proximal development
U.S. Supreme Court voting rights, 95, 104, 307 225–226 (Vygotsky), 75
legal status of adolescents, 96–97 voucher programs, 204 apprenticeships, 98, 105
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-35 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-36 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-37 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles
ste32118_sndx_I24-I38.indd Page I-38 01/05/13 12:24 AM user /204/MH01960/ste32118_disk1of1/0073532118/ste32118_pagefiles

You might also like