Robinson 2011

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86

DOI 10.1007/s10643-011-0496-3

Teaching Practices that Promote Motor Skills in Early Childhood


Settings
Leah E. Robinson • E. Kipling Webster •

S. Wood Logan • W. Amarie Lucas •


Laura T. Barber

Published online: 1 December 2011


 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract Early childhood educators, especially those in Keywords Movement program  Motor development 
preschool centers, are often expected to design and Mastery motivation  Early childhood  Motor skills
implement movement programs. However, these individ-
uals may not have been taught these skills during their
education. The purpose of this study was to determine if Introduction
early childhood majors could successfully be taught to
implement a mastery climate instructional approach to Movement and physical activity contribute to a child’s
promote motor skill development to preschool-age chil- quality of life (Piek et al. 2008), enhance intellectual
dren. Twenty undergraduate Early Childhood Education development and academic achievement (Dwyer et al.
majors, who completed a Motor Development Across the 2001; Tremblay et al. 2000), and are associated with a
School Years course, designed and implemented a move- healthy body weight (Logan and Getchell 2010; Logan
ment program to develop preschoolers’ locomotor and et al. 2011b). Children learn by exploring their environ-
object control skills. Fourteen preschoolers participated in ment through movement (Clark 2007). Movement is the
an 11-week movement program. Preschoolers’ motor per- catalyst that supports their interaction with others and their
formance was assessed with the Test of Gross Motor surroundings. In early childhood, movement is based on the
Development—2nd Edition prior to and following the performance of motor skills (i.e., fundamental motor skills)
intervention. Results indicate significant improvements in such as running, jumping, throwing, and catching. Motor
total performance (p \ .05) and locomotor skills skills are the building blocks for more advanced movement
(p = .037). Findings indicate that with guidance and (Clark and Metcalfe 2002) and are critical toward a child’s
training, early childhood majors can effectively design and current and future participation in movement-related
implement a mastery-based movement program that experiences (i.e., physical activity and sports; Clark 1994).
enhances motor skill competence. The findings also dem- Due to the critical importance of movement in a child’s
onstrate the need and value for early childhood majors to life, several educational agencies support the inclusion of
receive training in the design and implementation of movement and play programs in early education settings.
movement programs. This paper provides a conceptual The National Association for the Education of Young
framework and practical recommendations to assist teach- Children (NAEYC) is one of those agencies concerned
ers and educators with implementing a mastery climate with upholding the highest level of standards with regard to
movement programs. early childhood education programs. Accreditation by
NAEYC requires that programs provide quality care in a
number of different areas including the facilitation of
physical development through structured movement expe-
L. E. Robinson (&)  E. K. Webster  S. W. Logan  riences (NAEYC 2008). Furthermore, the Council on
W. A. Lucas  L. T. Barber
Physical Education for Children (COPEC 1992) encour-
Department of Kinesiology, Auburn University,
2050 Memorial Coliseum, Auburn, AL 36849, USA ages teachers to provide instruction that is developmentally
e-mail: lerobinson@auburn.edu and instructionally appropriate to the learner. Teachers

123
80 Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86

need to provide young children with learning experiences Archer 1988; Elliot and Dweck 1988). A mastery goal
that recognize individual differences while providing orientation is associated with the desire to increase com-
accommodations for the developmental level of all the petence through learning for the purpose of mastering tasks
students. It is important that early childhood educators and supports that effort and hard work lead to positive
implement the most effective teaching practices that pro- outcomes. Performance goal orientation is based on com-
vide learning opportunities and promote success for all parison to self-referenced standards (Ames 1992a, b;
students. Dweck and Leggett 1988). If mastery goals are salient,
Additionally, it is imperative that educators be mindful children tend to use adaptive motivational strategies, seek
that children in a given classroom will demonstrate a range out challenges, and display positive outcomes of cognition
of motor skill abilities and will learn at different rates. For and affect such as enjoyment (Ames and Archer 1988).
instance, not every child in a class will be able to strike a Mastery learners are characterized by the desire to develop
pitched ball with a bat. This task might be too advanced or and acquire new skills. The individuals are intrinsically
below one’s developmental skill level. This task requires motivated, which leads to progress and eventual mastery of
modification for success and learning to occur. To modify the skill.
this task, a teacher can incorporate a larger ball, a ball that Mastery climate is a child-centered and high autonomy
is placed on a batting tee, or a wider bat to decrease the (e.g., independence) instructional approach and believes
level of difficulty. Also, the instructional approach can be that children should not have learning situations imposed
manipulated to provide all children the opportunity to work on them (Ames 1992a, b). This learning approach
and learn at their own pace of skill development. encourages children to navigate their own learning expe-
Early childhood movement programs provide children rience. Mastery climate supports that children in this
the opportunity to develop basic movement patterns that environment become knowledgeable of their own devel-
lead to the acquisition of more advanced and sport-specific opmental status, and that learners will select tasks that meet
skills. Evidence suggests that early childhood teachers may their developmental needs (Maehr 1983). Various devel-
not be prepared to implement high-quality movement opmentally appropriate tasks, which range in difficulty
programs (Breslin et al. 2008). It is unclear if early child- level, are provided and children choose their desired path
hood educators receive the necessary amount of training of engagement and learning. Mastery climate uses a scaf-
and experience during undergraduate studies or profes- folding approach where the teacher models the desired
sional development courses to effectively implement learning strategy or task, then gradually shifts responsi-
movement programs. Therefore, it is imperative to provide bility to the child.
evidence-based research so training programs can suc- The approach centers around six key structures; task,
cessfully prepare future classroom teachers with the nec- authority, recognition, grouping, evaluation, and time;
essary knowledge and skills needed to design and better known as TARGET (Epstein 1988, 1989). In gen-
implement high-quality movement programs. eral, the TARGET structure provides children with:
• High variety of challenging and novel tasks
What is Mastery Climate?
• High participation in instructional decisions
• Option to establish and modify rules
Mastery climate is an instructional approach that has been
• Self and private recognition
used within various classroom, sport, and physical educa-
• Choice of grouping
tion settings and provides developmentally appropriate
• Flexible choice of activity station
instruction in a success-oriented environment. The
• No time restriction per station
instructional approach supports that learning is driven by
and through a child’s motivation (Ames 1992a, b; Nicholls A step-by-step guide of how to implement the TARGET
1984). Children either take a performance goal orientation structure in a classroom is located in the section titled:
or a mastery goal orientation approach toward learning ‘‘Getting Started … How to Implement ‘Mastery’ in your
(Ames 1992a, b). A performance goal orientation is asso- Learning Environment.’’
ciated with the desire to demonstrate competence to others
and views ability level as the contributing factor to the Does Mastery Motivational Climate Work?
development of competence. Also, performance is based
on comparison to normative standards (i.e. others; Ames Research indicates that a mastery climate is an effective
and Archer 1988). If performance goals are emphasized, instructional approach that promotes positive attitudes and
individuals tend to exhibit maladaptive motivational strat- self-perception towards movement, social interaction,
egies such as attributing failure to ability, negative affect, problem-solving skills, and higher participation in physical
and a decrease in task persistence (Ames 1992b; Ames and activity (Robinson 2011; Robinson and Goodway 2009;

123
Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86 81

Robinson et al. 2010). With regard to motor skill devel- motor skill activity. For example, for a striking station,
opment, participation in a mastery climate improves fun- children could strike a stationary large ball off of a tee
damental motor skills in young children (Martin et al. (low), strike a stationary small to mid-sized ball off of a tee
2009; Robinson and Goodway 2009; Valentini and Rudisill (moderate), or strike a tossed or pitched ball (high). Thus,
2004). This approach also leads to higher retention of learners had six activities (i.e., two skills at three levels of
motor skill learning (Robinson and Goodway 2009; difficulty) available to them on each intervention day.
Valentini and Rudisill 2004). The literature suggests that Following the theoretical approach of a mastery climate,
mastery climates result in developmental changes in pre- the teacher was a facilitator of learning and children were
schoolers’ motor skills (Martin et al. 2009; Robinson and provided with optimal control of their engagement in the
Goodway 2009; Valentini and Rudisill 2004). However, all movement program. They were allowed to choose which
of these interventions have been conducted and imple- activity to engage in, the amount of time spent within an
mented by trained researchers and not by practitioners or activity, the level of difficulty within that activity, and
practitioners-in-training. From a practical standpoint, a choice of whom to engage with (alone, with a partner, or in
mastery climate is effective and facilitates many benefits to small groups).
children. Thus, it is important to determine the feasibility
of training future early childhood teachers to implement a
Motor Skill Instruction
mastery climates movement programs. Therefore, the
purpose of this study was to determine if Early Childhood
The intervention was supervised by two trained Ph.D.
Education majors could be trained to successfully imple-
students in Kinesiology. Twenty Early Childhood
ment a mastery climate movement program that promotes
Education majors who completed a course titled ‘‘Motor
motor skill development in preschool-age children.
Development during the School Years’’ served as the
instructors of the movement program. The semester-long,
15-week course focused on the design and implementation
Methods
of developmentally appropriate motor skill activities and
experiences for preschool-age children. During this time,
Participants and Setting
the Early Childhood Education majors were trained in the
implementation of a mastery climate and observed the
Fourteen children (8 boys; 6 girls) between the ages of
execution of this instructional approach. The student
3–5 years old (M = 4.61 ± 0.46 years) from a university-
majors were placed into groups under the guidance of two
based early learning center in the southeast region of the
doctoral students and a professor and implemented a
United States participated in the study. In this sample, 6
mastery climate preschool movement program.
children were Caucasian (42.9%), 6 were Asian (42.9%), 1
was African American (7.1%), and 1 was of mixed decent
(7.1%). Instrumentation

Movement Program All children completed the Test of Gross Motor Develop-
ment-2nd edition (TGMD-2; Ulrich 2000) pre- and post-
An 11-week movement program was implemented to movement program to assess the improvement of motor
improve fundamental motor skills. The program specifi- skill development. The TGMD-2 is a qualitative assess-
cally focused on the following motor skills: running, ment and includes two subscales: locomotor and object
jumping, sliding, galloping, leaping, hopping, striking, control skills. The locomotor subscale assesses the ability
dribbling, kicking, throwing, catching, and underhand to run, gallop, hop, leap, horizontal jump, and slide. The
rolling. The movement program consisted of 22 sessions object control subscale assesses the ability to throw, catch,
(two per week) each lasting for 30 min. Each session dribble, kick, strike, and underhand roll a ball. Children
included a 3-min warm-up, 24-min of movement instruc- completed one practice trial and two formal trials. For each
tion, and a 3-min cool down. The movement program formal trial, a child received a zero if a performance cri-
lasted a total of 660 with 528 min dedicated to motor skill terion was not met and a one if a criterion was met. Scores
instruction. Motor skill instruction included time for were summed and converted to age and gender specific
teaching the critical elements of the motor skills, intro- percentile scores according to the TGMD-2 manual (Ulrich
duction of the motor skills, and skill practice. For each 2000). Children were videotaped during data collection and
session, two motor skill activities (i.e., stations) were the primary investigator scored each child through video
available to the preschoolers. Each station provided three analyses. Intra-rater reliability ([90%) was established by
levels of difficulty (i.e., low, moderate, and high) for each an independent rater.

123
82 Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86

Procedures

Institutional review board approval, parental consent, and


child assent were obtained for this research project prior to
data collection. Overall, children were included for data
analyses if they had no physical, intellectual, or develop-
mental disability, which would inhibit the understanding of
instructions and completion of the motor skill assessment.
On the day of assessment, children entered the motor
development laboratory and were provided with an over-
view of the TGMD-2. Data collection occurred on two
separate days and each child completed one subscale of the
TGMD-2 per day. Immediately following the movement
program, the same procedure was followed to administer a
post-assessment of the TGMD-2.
Fig. 1 Results indicate improvement in TGMD-2 scores at pre- and
Data Analysis post-intervention for boys, and girls separately

Three paired sample t tests were conducted to determine trained to successfully implement a mastery climate
pre- and post-intervention changes of motor skill devel- movement program that promotes motor skill development
opment. Changes on total performance of the TGMD-2 and in preschool-age children. Our results suggest that with
each subscale (object control and locomotor skills) were instruction, guidance, and direction, Early Childhood
examined. Due to the small sample size and exploratory Education majors are effective in understanding the con-
nature of the study, no adjustment for alpha level was cepts to teach a mastery climate movement program.
calculated. Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Specifically, the majors were able to design and implement
version 17.0.1 for Windows was used for data analyses. a mastery climate movement program that improved pre-
schoolers’ motor skills. Our findings support that training
required to implement a mastery climate in a movement
Results program setting can be provided within a short time frame.
Early childhood educators are expected to have knowledge
Children’s mean percentile values at pre-intervention were concerning children’s growth and maturational patterns
as follows: object control skills (M = 30.7; SD = 21.9); and processes. Thus, it seems feasible that only a few hours
locomotor skills (M = 28.7; SD = 23.7); and total is sufficient for educators to learn the theoretical principles
TGMD-2 performance (M = 26.8; SD = 23.7). At post- of a mastery climate and how to implement this climate in
intervention, mean percentile values were as follows: early childhood populations.
object control skills (M = 41.7; SD = 26.2); locomotor A central component of Piaget’s developmental theory
skills (M = 36.5; SD = 23.7); and total TGMD-2 perfor- of learning (i.e. adaptation) is the importance of partici-
mance (M = 37; SD = 27.7). A paired samples t test pation of the learner. Piaget asserted that learners must be
revealed a significant improvement in mean percentile on active in the environment and not treated as if they were
total performance of the TGMD-2 from pre- to post- empty vessels (London 1988). Adaptation occurs through
intervention (t(13) = 3.0, p \ .05). Two additional paired assimilation, the incorporation of new events into preex-
samples t tests indicated a significant improvement in isting knowledge; and accommodation, changing existing
locomotor skills (t(13) = 1.9, p = .037), and a non-sig- knowledge to integrate new information (Shaffer and Kipp
nificant improvement in object control skills (t(13) = 2.3, 2010). Participation in a mastery-based movement program
p = .079). See Fig. 1 for a visual representation of provides the opportunity for young learners to learn new
improvement from pre- to post-intervention on total per- skills through assimilation and accommodation. For
formance of the TGMD-2. example, a novice mover may not understand how to throw
an object using a mature form of movement due to limited
previous experiences. However, participation in a devel-
Discussion opmentally appropriate movement program that provides
task variations and several situations to practice a motor
The purpose of this study was to determine if Early skill allows a child to build a repertoire (i.e. assimilation)
Childhood Education early childhood majors could be of experiences. As a result of practice, new knowledge is

123
Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86 83

integrated into past experiences and children are able to these principles for the implementation of physical activity
apply the skill of throwing to novel situations (i.e. and motor skill development programs (Robinson and
accommodation). Piaget also emphasized the importance of Goodway 2009; Valentini and Rudisill 2004). The TAR-
active learning environments and a learner-centered GET structure provides a conceptual framework for cre-
approach (London 1988). A mastery motivational climate ating a mastery climate in the learning environment. 1.
encourages children to make engagement related decisions
1. The TASK dimension refers to the design of the
while teachers serve as a guide to stimulate children’s
learning activities/environment, task structure, and
interest. Specifically, mastery climates encourages children
lesson (Epstein 1989). This structure specifically
to explore and learn on their own. Furthermore, a mastery
relates to how the tasks are presented to the learner
climate endorses success for children of all skill levels. In a
(Ames 1992a, b). Epstein (1989) insists that a mastery
mastery-based movement program children are encouraged
climate only emerges when the activity involves a
to explore, question, experiment, search, and discover their
variety of tasks for the learning experience. Addition-
own solutions to become a skilled mover.
ally, the learner must find the task challenging,
There were limitations to the present study. The precise
interesting, and be able to make his/her own decisions
number of hours required for training was not recorded.
as it relates to task engagement. The task component in
This would have provided critical information with regard
a mastery climate challenges all learners through the
to specific recommendations of the amount of time
implementation of motivating tasks and offering a
required for early childhood educators to learn how to
variety of tasks at various levels of difficulty to meet
implement a mastery climate. Additionally, there was no
the needs and interests of the learning.
assessment on student learning or outcome behaviors of the
2. AUTHORITY relates to the interaction of the children
Early Childhood Education majors; only motor skill per-
and teacher in decision making within the learning
formance of the preschool children was assessed as an
environment (Epstein 1989). Often in a learning envi-
outcome measure. Future studies should emphasize the
ronment, authority or the ‘‘decision-making process’’ is
teacher training aspect, but first it was imperative to test the
made by the teacher with little input from the child. In a
feasibility of training individuals to effectively implement
mastery climate, authority is fostered by allowing
a preschool movement program. The next phase of research
children to actively participate in choices and decisions
needs to focus more on the teacher training aspect and
that relate to learning. Providing children with authority
provide more specific details to support replication of the
increases engagement within the learning environment
training program. Another limitation is the small sample
(Grolnick and Ryan 1987). The children’s authority
size. Generalizations regarding the effectiveness of a stu-
makes the learner responsible for her/his own learning
dent-led mastery climate movement programs to large
and shifts the locus of control from the teacher to the
groups of children should not be made. Finally, there was
student, which encourages autonomy.
no control group to allow comparison of the benefits of a
3. The RECOGNITION structure focuses on informal
mastery climate to another teaching approach or to no
and formal rewards, incentives, and praise that are
participation in a movement program. However, a recent
used and distributed by teachers to facilitate motiva-
meta-analysis suggests little to no improvement in motor
tion (Ames 1992a, b). Within a mastery climate, the
skill performance in children assigned to a control group of
teacher must be aware of providing individual incen-
a motor skill intervention study (Logan et al. 2011a).
tives for students and not to the class as a whole. The
Despite the limitations, we provide initial evidence that
teacher must avoid social comparison of children,
supports the effectiveness of a student-led mastery climate
which is prevalent in many reward systems (e.g.,
intervention to promote motor skills of preschool-age
public posting) in physical education settings. Recog-
children and that this approach could be implemented
nition of accomplishments and efforts take place in a
successfully by early childhood majors.
private forum between the teacher and child. When
recognition is private, the child’s sense of pride and
Getting Started … How to Implement ‘‘Mastery’’ satisfaction is derived from doing his/her best and not
in Your Movement Program from outperforming others. It is also important to
provide opportunities for all students to receive
The key to implementing a mastery climate within your recognition and rewards (Ames 1992a, b).
classroom centers on the six dimensions of TARGET 4. GROUPING as a structure centers around the class-
(Epstein 1988, 1989). These TARGET structures were room environment being an extension of society where
initially developed and designed for classroom environ- individual differences in ability, experience, gender,
ments, but researchers in motor behavior have adapted and background are often used to structure grouping

123
84 Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86

patterns. A goal of a mastery climate is to establish an to have a healthy body weight (Logan and Getchell 2010;
environment where individual differences are accepted Logan et al. 2011b; Trost et al. 2003). The weight status of
and all children develop a sense of belonging. Thus, children is a concern for not only the United States, but
grouping is child-centered and based on the learner’s also for countries around the world. Data from the World
preference. This will allow the formation of heteroge- Health Organization suggest that about 42 million children
neous cooperative groups that foster peer interaction under the age of five are overweight worldwide (WHO
and use individual and group activity to encourage 2010). Although the exact etiology of childhood obesity is
children to work effectively with others during an complex, physical inactivity is a component. For children
intervention (Epstein 1989). In a mastery climate, the to engage in physical activity they need to possess basic
formation and disbanding of a group is a free flowing competence in fundamental motor skills, and children learn
event. Children are not assigned to a group and groups fundamental motor skills through well-designed and plan-
form and break up based on the individual desires of ned movement programs. Research indicates that mastery
the child. climates are an effective approach (Martin et al. 2009;
5. The EVAULATION structure focuses on methods that Robinson 2011; Robinson and Goodway 2009; Valentini
are used to assess, monitor, judge, and measure and Rudisill 2004).
children’s behavior and learning. Learners are more Furthermore, researchers have provided consistent evi-
likely to adopt a mastery goal orientation when the dence that participation in a mastery-based movement
evaluation structure is based on improvement, personal program results in positive changes in children’s self-per-
progress, participation, and effort (Ames 1992a, b). ceptions (Robinson et al. 2009; Valentini and Rudisill
Evaluation must focus on the process of acquiring 2004), fundamental motor skill development (Amui 2006;
knowledge and skills and should help students focus Martin et al. 2009; Robinson and Goodway 2009), and
on the process of learning, not the product of learning. greater engagement of physical activity (Parish et al. 2007;
One means of achieving this form of evaluation is with Wadsworth et al. 2010. It is important to note that the
portfolio assessments. These types of assessments literature indicates that boys tend to display higher motor
document the students’ individual strengths, weak- competence compared to girls (Barnett et al. 2009; Graf
nesses, abilities, and skill level (Whorthen 1993). et al. 2004). For these findings, boys did demonstrate more
Thus, to create a mastery climate, it is essential to proficient motor skills, but the differences were not sig-
implement an evaluation structure that emphasizes the nificant. Thus, early childhood education teachers should
individual child’s progress and mastery of learning. be aware of this difference and be mindful of encouraging
6. The TIME structure embodies the appropriateness of girls to participate and learn during a planned movement
the workload, the pace of instruction, and time program. Given the concern of childhood obesity, it is
allotment for children to complete learning activities important for research to provide recommendations of the
and assignments (Epstein 1989). From a developmen- best teaching practices that promote positive health-related
tal perspective, children are motivated for different outcomes.
reasons and learn at individual rates (Payne and Isaacs Mastery motivational climate is an effective instruc-
2007). Thus, teachers must create learning experiences tional method that fosters a child-centered achievement
that are tailored to the individual need of the child. environment. This instructional approach provides children
This occurs by allowing children to self-direct their with a variety of tasks and freedom of choice with
own pace and is not determined by the teacher or the emphasis on learning at an individual level. A recent meta-
progress of other students (Epstein 1989). In a mastery analysis provides evidence that the ability to make choices
climate, there is no set time allocated to a specific skill, increases intrinsic motivation, effort, task persistence, and
rather, each child spends different amounts of time perceived competence of children (Patall et al. 2008).
engaged in skills based on his/her individual Providing choice to children is at the core of promoting a
preferences. mastery climate in early childhood educational settings.
While it is acknowledged that teachers must have class-
room structure, especially when providing children the
Conclusion opportunity to engage in physical activity and develop
motor skills, children should be provided as many oppor-
The development of fundamental motor skills is associated tunities within that structure to make choices related to
with psychological, physiological, and behavioral benefits engagement. A major limitation of previous research is that
(see Lubans et al. 2010 for a review). Evidence suggests only researchers have implemented mastery-based move-
that preschoolers that participate in physical activity and ment programs. Findings of the present study suggest that
are competent in fundamental motor skills are more likely not only researchers but Early Childhood Education majors

123
Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86 85

can also promote motor skill development through devel- Epstein, J. L. (1989). Family structures and student motivation: A
opmentally and instructionally appropriate movement developmental perspective. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.),
Research on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 259–295). San
programs. Overall, a mastery climate is an innovative and Diego, CA: Academic Press.
exceptional pedagogy for teaching children motor skills Graf, C., Koch, B., Kretschmann-Kandel, E., Falkowski, G., Christ,
and encouraging physical activity. Early childhood edu- H., & Coburger, S. (2004). Correlation between BMI, leisure
cators can rejuvenate a movement program based on habits and motor abilities in childhood (CHILT-Project). Inter-
national Journal of Obesity, 28, 22–26.
principles of mastery learning. Such programs not only Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in children’s
give young children the greatest opportunity to discover the learning: An experimental and individual difference investiga-
benefits of movement and physical activity for themselves tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 890–898.
but also hold promise for shaping an enduring commitment Logan, S. W., & Getchell, N. (2010). The relationship between motor
proficiency and body composition in children with and without
to more healthy, active ways of living. dyslexia. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81(4),
518–523.
Logan, S. W., Robinson, L. E., Wilson, A. E., & Lucas, W. A.
(2011a). Getting the fundamentals of movement: A meta-
analysis of the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in
References young children as assessed by the Test of Gross Motor
Development. Child: Care, Health and Development. doi:
Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01307.x.
motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in Logan, S. W., Scrabis-Fletcher, K., Modlesky, C., & Getchell, N.
sport and exercise (pp. 161–176). Champaign, IL: Human (2011b). The relationship between motor skill proficiency and
Kinetics. body mass index in preschool children. Research Quarterly for
Ames, C. (1992b). Classroom: Goals, structures, and student moti- Exercise and Sport, 82(3), 442–448.
vation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271. London, C. (1988). A Piagetian constructivist perspective on curric-
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: ulum development. Reading Improvement, 27, 82–95.
Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal Lubans, D. R., Morgan, P. J., Cliff, D. P., Barnett, L. M., & Okely, A.
of Educational Psychology, 80, 260–267. D. (2010). Fundamental movement skills in children and
Amui, H. N. (2006). The effects of two instructional approaches on adolescents. Sports Medicine, 40(12), 1019–1035.
the object control skill of children considered disadvantaged. Maehr, M. L. (1983). On doing well on science. Why Johnny no
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University, longer excels, why Sarah never did. In S. G. Paris, G. M. Olson,
Columbus, OH. & H. W. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and motivation in the
Barnett, L. M., van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L. O., & classroom (pp. 179–210). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Beard, J. R. (2009). Childhood motor skill proficiency as a Martin, E. H., Rudisill, M. E., & Hastie, P. A. (2009). Motivational
predictor of adolescent physical activity. Journal of Adolescent climate and fundamental motor skills performance in a natural-
Health, 44, 252–259. istic physical education setting. Physical Education and Sport
Breslin, C. M., Morton, J. R., & Rudisill, M. E. (2008). Implementing Pedagogy, 14, 227–240.
a physical activity curriculum into the school day: Helping early National Association for the Education of Young Children. (2008).
childhood teachers meet the challenge. Early Childhood Educa- Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood pro-
tion Journal, 35, 429–437. gram serving children from birth through age 8: A position
Clark, J. E. (1994). Motor development. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), statement of the National Association for the Education of Young
Encyclopedia of human behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 163–190). San Children. Washington, D.C: National Association for the
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Education of Young Children.
Clark, J. E. (2007). On the problem of motor skill development. Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of
Journal of Physical Education Recreation & Dance, 78(5), ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance.
39–44. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.
Clark, J. E., & Metcalfe, J. S. (2002). The mountain of motor Parish, L. E., Rudisill, M. E., & St Onge, P. M. (2007). Mastery
development: A metaphor. In J. E. Clark & J. H. Humphrey motivational climate: Influence on physical play heart rate and
(Eds.), Motor development: Research and review (Vol. 2, intensity in African American toddlers. Research Quarterly for
pp. 62–95). Reston, VA: National Association for Sport and Exercise and Sport, 78, 171–178.
Physical Education. Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of
Council on Physical Education for Children. (1992). Retrieved April choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-
15, 2006, from http://www.aahperd.org/copec. analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2),
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to 270–300.
motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95(2), Payne, V. G., & Isaacs, L. D. (2007). Human motor development: A
256–273. lifespan approach (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Dwyer, T., Sallis, J. F., Blizzard, L., Lazarus, R., & Dean, K. (2001). Piek, J. P., Dawson, L., Smith, L. M., & Gasson, N. (2008). The role
Relation of academic performance to physical activity and of early fine and gross motor development on later motor and
fitness in children. Pediatric Exercise Science, 13, 225–238. cognitive ability. Human Movement Science, 27, 668–681.
Elliot, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to Robinson, L. E. (2011). Effect of a mastery climate motor program on
motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social object control skills and perceived physical competence in
Psychology, 54(1), 5–12. preschoolers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82,
Epstein, J. L. (1988). Effective schools or effective students? Dealing 355–359.
with diversity. In R. Hawkins & B. MacRae (Eds.), Policies for Robinson, L. E., & Goodway, J. D. (2009). Instructional climates in
America’s public schools (pp. 89–126). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. preschool children who are at-risk. Part I: Object-control skill

123
86 Early Childhood Educ J (2012) 40:79–86

development. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 80, preschool children. International Journal of Obesity, 27,
533–542. 834–839.
Robinson, L. E., Reynolds, J., Wadsworth, D. D., & Logan, S. W. Ulrich, D. A. (2000). The test of gross motor development (2nd ed.).
(2010). The effects of physical play instructional climates on Austin, TX: Pro-ed.
physical activity in preschool-age children [Abstract]. Science Valentini, N. C., & Rudisill, M. E. (2004). Motivational climate,
and Sports, 25, 4–13. motor-skill development, and perceived competence: Two
Robinson, L. E., Rudisill, M. E., & Goodway, J. D. (2009). studies of developmentally delayed kindergarten children. Jour-
Instructional climates in preschool children who are at-risk. Part nal of Teaching in Physical Education, 23, 216–234.
II: Perceived physical competence. Research Quarterly for Wadsworth, D., Robinson, L. E., Rudisill, M., Logan, S. W., Morera,
Exercise and Sport, 80, 543–551. M., & Daly, C. (2010). The influence of motivational climates on
Shaffer, D. R., & Kipp, K. (2010). Developmental psychology: the physical activity in rural, African American school-age
Childhood and adolescence (8th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: children [Abstract]. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
Brooks/Cole. 32(suppl), S52.
Tremblay, M. S., Inman, J. W., & Willms, J. D. (2000). The Whorthen, B. R. (1993). Critical issues that will determine the future
relationship between physical activity, self-esteem, and aca- of alternative assessments. Phi Kappan Delta, 74(6), 444–454.
demic achievement in 12-year-old children. Pediatric Exercise World Health Organization. (2010). Childhood overweight and
Science, 12, 312–324. obesity on the rise. Retrieved November 7, 2011, from http://
Trost, S. G., Sirard, J. R., Dowda, M., Pfeiffer, K. A., & Pate, R. R. www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/childhood/en/index.html.
(2003). Physical activity in overweight and nonoverweight

123

You might also like