Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 1

294 R. M. SHAPLEYAND C.

ENROTH-CUGELL

i ooo" ' caused by quantal fluctuations. One must suppose


| I ' \ ]

that this high frequency noise is immune to the


adaptational effects of the mean level o f
illumination. Perhaps the noise which limits
detection of high frequency responses is post-
D-R
retinal. In any case, a complete and adequate
explanation for Kelly's "linear range" requires
future research.

2OO
~oo ['- 2 cpd lg70 o
<3 3. GAIN AND CONTRAST GAIN IN
500 O.S c:pd
R E T I N A L G A N G L I O N CELLS
2 [ ] r I I i~ RECTILINEAR PATTERNS
Subj. TZ
IC)~ a;" ~,~ I (d) At the outset of this section on the physiology
of retinal adaptation we concentrate on retinal
ganglion cells, the output stage of the retina. All
10 o
o
information which flows from the retina to the
20 __ ~>
brain about the visual appearance of the outside
5or ° °° °° °o o world passes along the axons of these ganglion cells.
100 ; The evidence of retinal adaptation in the activity
200 of these neurons allows us to establish a link
8 ¢pd D-R 12 cpd O-R
~ T l , ] [ i J I I r
between the visual, perceptual function of light
1 2 5 10 20 50 1 2 5 10 20 50
adaptation and the underlying retinal mechanisms.
TEMPORAL FREQUENCY (Hz)
We will further concentrate our attention on two
FIG. 20. Threshold illumination as a function of temporal kinds of retinal ganglion cells in the cat's retina, the
frequency at different spatial frequencies. The threshold X and Y cells (see Appendix 2), because most is
illumination AB was the amplitude of sine grating which known about them. Comparison with the retinas
could just be seen on a mean illumination of B td. In each
of the four panels of the figure, the four different curves are of other species and with human vision will be made
f r o m m e a s u r e m e n t s at the following m e a n retinal frequently. As in the Introduction, we stress the
illuminations: 36 td (arrowheads), 114 td (triangles), 360 td importance of a hierarchy of gain control
(diamonds), and 1140 td (circles). The test target was a pale-
green CRT which subtended 7 deg; it was viewed monocularly mechanisms at different sites in the retina.
and fixated. The gratings were modulated in time with a Furthermore, the role of retinal gain controls in
sinusoidal waveform; the temporal frequency o f the making the retina respond to contrast will be made
modulation is plotted on the horizontal axis. Measurements
from four spatial frequencies are shown: 0.5 c deg -1, evident.
2 c deg -1, 8 c deg -~, and 12 c deg-L Thresholds, which are One can speak about the g a i n of retinal ganglion
separated from those at other mean illuminations by a factor cells because their impulse rate variation caused by
which is equal to the ratio of the mean levels, conform to
Weber's Law, and have a W written next to the curve. increments (or decrements) of illumination are
Thresholds which rise like the square root o f the mean level proportional to the magnitude of the increment (or
are labeled D - R for the de Vries-Rose law, synonymous with decrement) over a considerable range of response
the square root law. From Kelly (1972).
amplitude. This is illustrated by Fig. 22 (Shapley
and Kaplan, unpublished). The stimuli were fine
be quantal noise and must be independent of mean gratings which stimulated the center of the receptive
light level. This is not implausible. Most sources of field. (In this initial discussion we will be dealing
noise in the visual system, e.g. channel opening and with the gain of the center only, but will consider
closing in neuronal membranes, or spontaneous the gain of the surround below.)
transmitter release, should have wide-band The ratio of the change in impulse rate with
components. These components may be relatively change in stimulus magnitude is the gain, G
larger at high temporal frequencies than at low,
compared to the light evoked neural shot noise G = dR/dI. (19a)

You might also like