Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 6

http://acrwebsite.

org/volumes/7134/volumes/v18/NA-18

Personal Rites of Passage and the Reconstruction of Self

John W. Schouten, Iowa State University


[ to cite ]:

John W. Schouten (1991) ,"Personal Rites of Passage and the Reconstruction


of Self", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 18, eds. Rebecca H.
Holman and Michael R. Solomon, Provo, UT : Association for Consumer
Research, Pages: 49-51.
[ direct url ]:

http://acrwebsite.org/volumes/7134/volumes/v18/NA-18
Advances in Consumer Research Volume 18, 1991 Pages 49-51

PERSONAL RITES OF PASSAGE AND THE RECONSTRUCTION OF SELF

John W. Schouten, Iowa State University

[This report is abstracted from the forthcoming paper "Selves in Transition:


Symbolic Consumption in Personal Rites of Passage and Identity
Reconstruction" (Schouten 1991).]

INTRODUCTION

Recent research has established the importance of consumption activities to the


construction and maintenance of self and identity (cf. Belk 1988, Solomon
1983). However, much work is still needed if we are to understand the complex
relationships between consumer behaviors and the psychosocial needs of
changing, growing human beings. To the extent that the self-concept is created
and comprehended through symbolic acts of consumption, the changes in self-
concept that accompany human development may be wrought at least partially
through the disposition and acquisition of consumer goods.

Solomon (1983), recognizing the importance of consumer goods in the learning


and performance of social roles, articulated the need for further research on role
transitions. Andreasen (1984) subsequently presented evidence that life status
changes are linked to changes in lifestyle and the willingness to try new brands
or products. Despite these intriguing beginnings, the consumer behavior
literature has yet to come to grips with life transitions and self-concept change
as holistic phenomena influencing and influenced by acts of disposition and
acquisition.

1
In an in-depth, qualitative study of the consumption of cosmetic surgery
(Schouten 1991) it is observed that through the surgical disposition of one
physical attribute and the acquisition of another a person may symbolically shed
one identity in favor of another. The process whereby this occurs is explained
via van Gennep's (1960) conceptualization of rites of passage with special
attention is given to the "identity play" of liminal consumers.

RITES OF PASSAGE

Van Gennep (1960) observed that important role transitions generally consist of
three phases: 1) separation, in which a person disengages from a social role or
status, 2) transition, in which the person adapts and changes to fit new roles,
and 3) incorporation, in which the person integrates the new role or status into
the self. Victor Turner (1969) described the transitional or liminal phase as a
limbo between a past state and a coming one, a period of personal ambiguity, of
non-status, and of unanchored identity. In primal societies, culturally prescribed
rituals (rites of passage) provided individuals an experience of "communitas" or
shared psychological support throughout major status passages. In the modern,
secular world, however, people often experience liminoid states (cf. Turner
1974) devoid of such supportive rites. Left to their own devices to cope with
difficult transitions and ambiguous self-concepts people appear to create
personal rites of passage through symbolic acts of disposition and acquisition,
and, in so doing, to construct new concepts of self.

SEPARATION: THE DISPOSITION OF IDENTITY

Much of the time people's self-concepts are relatively equilibrated -- they know
who they are and they feel fairly stable and comfortable in their various social
roles (Levinson 1978). There are times, however, when the relative stability of
such states is upset by changes in the environment or from within an individual
(cf. Adams, Hayes and Hopson 1976; Levinson 1978). The reconstruction of
identity begins with separation from some role, relationship, or other key
component of the extended self (cf. Belk 1988). Separation often occurs literally
in time and space, triggered by some external force or event (e.g. the loss of a
job or the death of a family member) that causes a shift in one's major roles.
Separation may also be a subjective experience triggered by an internal force or
psychological need (e.g. a fear of aging or a yearning for intimacy) that leads to
the rejection of a particular aspect of the self. The loss or rejection of an
important component of the self-concept is often finalized symbolically by the
disposition of possessions that act as reminders of the former self. Such
dispositional acts may serve a cleansing or stabilizing function (cf. Young 1990),
thereby creating "fresh start" opportunities. However, acts of separation in and
of themselves do not constitute a completed transitions. Instead they usher in
the period of flux and self-concept plasticity known as a liminoid state.

2
TRANSITION: LIMINALITY AND IDENTITY PLAY

Liminal people face the task of reconstructing congruous, integrated self-


concepts. If they have experienced unbidden separation from key roles, they
must create new roles or emphasize existing roles to fill the gaps. If they have
emotionally rejected traits or aspects of self with which they are dissatisfied,
they may seek some way of excising them and replacing them with more
desirable traits. In either case they begin to formulate possible selves (cf.
Markus and Nurius 1986), i.e., they envision themselves as they might possibly
become. An important characteristic of liminoid states is the tendency to play
and experiment with new categories of meaning (Turner 1974). Liminal people
appear to be more prone than others to engage in "identity play," that is, to
formulate, elaborate, and evaluate possible selves with an eye to self-change or
self-completion (cf. Wicklund and Gollwitzer 1982).

Possible selves may begin as loosely articulated mental constructs or vague


images. Some may be dismissed as flights of fancy, but ultimately others will be
"fleshed out" through mental elaboration and then evaluated as to their
desirability and the feasibility of their actualization. Identity play often involves
the experimental consumption of goods and services, especially those offering
symbolic or hedonic benefits. The various self-schemas that an individual "tries
on" will tend to reflect personal goals, values and/or fantasies, and they may
also incorporate themes and styles from popular culture, myth, and the media.
Identity play, at least that which is publicly enacted, tends to be limited by one's
perceptions of social constraints and shaped by social interaction (cf. Mead
1934).

The amount of time and energy a person invests in elaborating a possible self
appears to vary according to such factors as the magnitude of the contemplated
self-change, the level of perceived risk it entails, and the individual's imaginative
tendencies.- Extensive elaboration appears to give a possible self greater
motivating power. The investment of psychic energy required by the elaboration
process may lead to cathexis of the possible self, or the contemplated self-
schema may simply become more believable as it is made more specific and
detailed.

The perceived attainability of a possible self also affects its motivating power. If
the likelihood of attaining a possible-self is perceived as too low, motivation to
actualize it is diminished. The attainability of a particular self-state depends on
such factors as one's personal resources and one's orientation to such social
constraints as may exist. Perceived attainability may, therefore, change
significantly during a role transition as social and financial conditions change.
Failure to actualize a desired and otherwise attainable self may owe to a lack of
self-efficacy (cf. Bandura 1977; Nuttin 1984), i.e., an inability to convert will to

3
action. A possible self that is sufficiently desirable and attainable will likely
become actualized.

INCORPORATION: THE ACQUISITION OF IDENTITY

People can respond to their possible selves in one of three ways: 1) with
inaction, 2) with active rejection, or 3) with actualization and the incorporation of
the possible self into a revised self-concept. Inaction results when possible
selves are insufficiently desirable, undesirable, or plausible to motivate action,
or when approach-avoidance conflicts result (cf. Levin 1935). The short term
consequence of inaction is continued liminality. Rejection occurs when a
possible self is deemed unattainable, undesirable, or incongruent with other
aspects of the self-concept. The rejection of a possible self leads to continued
liminality and the probable formulation of yet another possible self. Failure to
make a successful passage results in prolonged liminality. Painfully prolonged
liminoid states have been observed in conjunction with psychological
impediments to normal development (Shorter 1987), permanently debilitating or
stigmatizing handicaps (Murphy 1987), and the inability to let go of past roles or
statuses (Levinson 1978; Roberts 1988).

Ultimately, the more desirable and plausible a possible self seems to the
individual, the more motivating power it wields and the more likely it is to be
actualized (Markus and Nurius 1986). Actualization may occur via the
consumption of instrumental goods and services as the individual goes about
accumulating the appropriate symbols of the new self (cf. Solomon 1983).
Incorporation occurs as the symbols are cathected (Belk 1988) or cultivated
(Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981) as part of the new identity.
Successful incorporation leads a person out of the liminoid state with a revised
self-concept and an increased sense of self-congruity.

CONCLUSION

Consumption activities, including disposition and acquisition, play vital roles in


the restoration of harmony to an ambiguous, incongruous, or dissatisfying self-
concept. Personal rites of passage fashioned with consumer goods and
services aid in the symbolic disposition of lost or rejected identities, in
constructive identity play, and in the incorporation of new components of the
extended self. Major role transitions are crucial times in determining the
direction and quality of consumers' lives, but little is yet known about the
consumption behaviors of liminal people or the importance of consumer
behaviors to human growth and change. This report and the study from which it
derives attempt, through a synthesis of anthropological and psychological
perspectives, to move in that direction.

REFERENCES

4
Adams, John, John Hayes, and Barrie Hopson (1976), Transition:
Understanding & Managing Personal Change, London: Martin Robinson & Co.

Andreasen, Alan R. (1984), "Life Status Changes and Changes in Consumer


Preferences and Satisfaction," Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (December),
784-794.

Bandura, Albert (1977), "Self-Efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral


Change," Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.

Belk, Russell W. (1988), "Possessions and the Extended Self," Journal of


Consumer Research, 14 (September), 139-168.

Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly and Eugene Rochberg Halton (1981), The Meaning of


Things: Domestic Symbols and the Self, New York: Cambridge University
Press.

Levinson, Daniel J. (1978), The Seasons of a Man's Life, New York: Ballantine.

Lewin, Kurt (1935), A Dynamic Theory of Personality, New York: McGraw-Hill.

Markus, Hazel and Paula Nurius (1986), "Possible Selves," American


Psychologist, Vol. 41, No. 9, 954-969.

Mead, George Herbert (1934), Mind, Self, and Society, Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.

Murphy, Robert F. (1987), The Body Silent, New York: Holt.

Nuttin, Joseph (1984), Motivation, Planning, and Action: A Relational Theory of


Behavior Dynamics, Trans. R. P. Lorion and J. E. Dumas, Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.

Roberts, Scott D. (1988), Resource Reallocation and Other Effects of Structural


Unemployment, Doctoral Dissertation, University of Utah, Department of
Marketing.

Schouten, John W. (1991), "Selves in Transition: Symbolic Consumption in


Personal Rites of Passage and Identity Reconstruction," Journal of Consumer
Research, 17 (March), forthcoming.

Shorter, Bani (1987), An Image Darkly Forming, New York: Routledge & Kegan.

Solomon, Michael R. (1983), "The Role of Products as Social Stimuli: A


Symbolic Interaction Perspective," Journal of Consumer Research, 10
(December), 319-329.

Turner, Victor W. (1969), The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure,


Chicago: Aldine.

5
Turner, Victor W. (1974), "Liminal to Liminoid in Play, Flow, and Ritual: An Essay
in Comparative Symbology," Rice University Studies, 60 (3), 53-92.

van Gennep, Arnold (1960), The Rites of Passage, trans. M. B. Vizedom and G.
L. Caffee, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Wicklund, Robert A. and Peter M. Gollwitzer (1982), Symbolic Self-Completion,


Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Young, Melissa Martin (1990), Possession Centrality to Self, Perceptions of


Control, and the Experience of Disposition, Master's Thesis, University of
Arizona, Department of Marketing.

----------------------------------------

You might also like