Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

ANTHROPOS

113.2018: 21 – 38

Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies


Warren Shapiro

Abstract. – Fifteen complaints are lodged against the so-called ceptualisation of kinship as a system of ties estab-
“new kinship studies” inspired by David Schneider. The main lished through procreation, they are, in fact, para-
argument of these studies, that they get at indigenous apprecia- sitic upon it (Holy 1996: ​167).
tions, as contrasted with pre-Schneiderian analyses, supposedly
entrapped in a Eurocentric model, is shown to be without merit. [T]he idea of Western bio-essentialized folk con-
On the contrary, these latter analyses, far from assuming a pro- cepts of kinship being endlessly ethnocentrically
creative base for kinship worldwide, regularly discovered it in project onto non-Western cultures by ethnogra-
the field. Schneiderian kinship studies are shown to be grossly phers and kinship theorists is itself a kind of an-
deficient from a scholarly standpoint, and to aspire to hegemony thropological myth (Wilson 2016: ​573).
in the academy. [kinship, history of anthropology, the culture of
academia, scholarly responsibility, “radical” feminism] Creativity is predicated on a system of rules and
forms, in part determined by intrinsic human ca-
Warren Shapiro, Emeritus Professor of Anthropology at Rut- pacities. Without such constraints, we have arbi-
gers University. – He has carried out fieldwork among the Ab- trary and random behavior, not creative acts. …
original people of northeast and central Arnhem Land, Austra- [I]t would be an error to think of human freedom
lia. – He has been writing on kinship and other subjects for over solely in terms of absence and constraint (Chom-
four decades. Among his publications are “Social Organization sky 1975: ​133).
in Aboriginal Australia” (Canberra 1979) and “Denying Biolo-
gy. Essays on Gender and Pseudo-Procreation” (co-edited with
Uli Linke, Lanham 1996), and dozens of other articles. See also The self-styled “new kinship studies” are expressly
References Cited.
indebted to David Schneider’s writings – particular-
ly his study of American kinship (1968), his Mor-
gan Centennial essay (1972), and most of all his
[A]ll systems of social relationships recognized by “Critique” (1984) of previous kinship scholarship.
anthropologists cross-culturally as kin relationships Although particular analyses do not all share the de-
are rooted in parturition (Goodenough 2001: ​217). ficiencies enumerated below, this common indebt-
edness, it seems to be, is sufficient to regard these
[I]n many primitive tribes the terms used for the
immediate members of the family are either distin-
studies as constituting a “school of thought.” This
guished from the same terms in the extended sense appellation is quite deliberate: I shall argue that this
by the addition of some particle, or terms corre- “scholastic” character has a stark Medieval quali-
sponding to “own” are used … Family is family, ty, specifically, that it ignores (or is ignorant of) an
whatever the system of relationship … (Golden- enormous quantity of pertinent evidence; that it also
weiser 1937: ​301). ignores elementary logical and semantic operations;
Although the studies which suggest that kinship that it denigrates its scholarly opponents as “Euro-
in many cultures is defined not only by genealogy centric,” at the same time claiming, utterly errone-
but also by a code of conduct, particularly con- ously; that it presents “the natives’ point of view”
duct expressing sharing of food, land and services, (Geertz 1983: ​55); that it substitutes for empirical-
may seem to challenge the anthropological con- ly and logically sound analysis a Manichean model

https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
22 Warren Shapiro

of humanity which is remarkably compatible with informants. Consider Carsten’s own analysis of her
Biblical eschatology, including a messianic view of Malay materials in the light of those of other schol-
Schneider and a hopelessly obsolete biology/culture ars. She claims that the Malay village she studied
dichotomy; that it substitutes for the alleged author- has “undivided kinship” (1995: ​115), but she pro-
ity of a scientific establishment a very real authori- vides little data on kin classification. She does tell
tarian plan of its own; that it violates principles of us, however, that “villagers perceive a continuum of
scholarly debate, either by ignoring its opponents or relatedness, from the ‘close’ … to the ‘distant’ …”
by responding ad hominem. (1990: ​272). Other sources concur with this latter
For present purposes I  shall mostly ignore finding, indicating, in fact, that Malay kinship is de-
Schneider’s scholarship itself: I intend to deal with cidedly “divided.” Superficially, the kinship termi-
this at length in a future project.1 I shall focus in- nology conforms to the generational pattern of Mur-
stead on his by now considerable legion of admirers, dock (1949) and others. But Banks makes the key
who present themselves as advocates of a performa­ point that “Malayan [kin] terms tend to be appended
tive view of human kinship, where kin (or kinship- by … marker-affixes indicating that the individual
like) ties are formed by non-procreative criteria, in question is ‘like a …’ 5 but not a definitive repre-
such as commensality (Carsten 1995), name-shar- sentative of the Malayan category” (1974: ​47; em-
ing (Nuttall 1994), and consociation (Weston 1991). phasis added). Thus, for example, only one’s geni-
Although I have dealt with particular flaws of the tor is referred to by the lexically unmarked “father”
performativist view elsewhere,2 the present analy- term; other “fathers” are lexically marked accord-
sis is intended to be more comprehensive. I pre­sent ing to their degree of collaterality. Thus, one’s FB
it as a series of complaints, to wit: is, in Malay parlance, “father one degree removed,”
father’s male first cousin, “father two degrees re-
(1)  “There is minimal and/or sophomoric attention moved,” etc. Malays, Banks (1974: ​51) concludes,
given to kinship terminologies.” Despite the seem- “thus distinguish degrees of cousinship in their own
ingly endless celebrations of a “return” to kinship generation much as American English informants
studies inspired by Schneider, what has decidedly do,” except that “the concept of collateral distance
not returned is the study of kinship terminologies, … is applied … in all generations and not simply
the foundational subject of the entire discipline in the in [one’s] own.” Moreover, actual parents and chil-
United States (Shapiro 2012: ​394), thanks to Lewis dren are said to be the “real” members of their re-
Henry Morgan’s herculean labors (Morgan 1871), spective kin classes (Banks 1983: ​59). This is a re-
and an important part of anthropology abroad.3 This current finding in systems of kin classification, as
is much less true of scholars influenced by Schnei- Goldenweiser (see my second epigraph) pointed out
der early on.4 But subsequent studies of performed many years ago, and, as such, it largely demolishes
kinship give the analyses of systems of kin classi- the claims of the performativists.
fication short shrift. Weston (1991: ​xiv) dismisses To continue: Full siblings are similarly said to be
such studies as “arcane.” Similarly, Carsten (2004: ​ the “real” members of their kin classes but half-sib-
16) voices what I take to be a generally held view lings are lexically marked – i.e., even at this close a
among advocates of a performative approach when genealogical position secondary semantic status is
she writes that “studies of kin classification became in effect. Wilder (1982: ​86) notes Malay expressions
a highly technical and specialized area, quite di- which he translates as “mother’s side” and “father’s
vorced from the … everyday experience of kinship.” side” and which are employed in kin classifica-
In other words, kinship terminologies are not tion. Banks (1974: ​51; 1983: ​57, 60) and McKin-
what one might call, following Geertz (1983: ​57 f.), ley (1983: ​345) provide examples of kin reckoning,
“experience near.” But they come much closer if one all of which involve parent/child and sibling/sibling
considers certain details readily elicited from our links, both genealogical and terminological. Read
(2018) has called attention to the cross-cultural oc-
currence of the latter, exemplified in English by the
  1 I  shall mention Marshall Sahlins’ recent contribution to a
general theory of kinship (Sahlins 2013) only occasionally.
rule, “I call the wife of anyone I call ‘uncle’ ‘aunt’.”
For a more comprehensive critique of his book, see Shapiro Note that such rules manifest native principles of
(2018). kin class extension.6
  2 See Shapiro (2008, 2009a, 2009b, 2010, 2011b, 2012, 2013,
2014, 2015a, 2015b, n. d.).
  3 E.g. Kohler (1975 [1897]), Lubbock (1872), Starcke (2012   5 Throughout this essay, I use single quotes to indicate glosses,
[1889]). i.e., exact or approximate translations of foreign terms.
  4 E.g. Basso (1973), Inden and Nicholas (1977), Silverman   6 Such rules are unlike those associated especially with Har-
(1971). old Scheffler, which rely partly on genealogical positions that

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 23

Banks (1983: ​80, note 2) reports an expression information on lexical marking or other forms of
used “to refer to a group of all one’s blood kins- subclassification, such as we have seen in the Ma-
men.” Wilder (1982: ​97) notes another, which he lay case. But he calls attention to the teknonymous
translates as “near kin” and which, he notes else- practice whereby “[s]iblings start to call their mar-
where (1991: ​132), distinguishes such people from ried female siblings ‘mother of,’ rather than ‘sibling’
“kinsmen a little distant” and “distant kinsmen.” … after they have borne their first child  ” (2003: ​66,
Those outside this sphere are likely to be labeled by emphasis added). It is hard to see this as anything
a term which Djamour (1959: ​24), Massard (1991: ​ but a practice based on procreative kinship. More-
141), and McKinley (1983: ​336) translate as “unre- over, this particular use of the “mother” term sug-
lated” or “stranger.” Massard (1991: ​141) notes an gests that its primary meaning is based on actual
expression pertaining to a whole village: she trans- maternity, rather than its “classificatory” counter-
lates it as “the people here are all relatives.” But in part – something which is true of all other teknony-
the same paragraph she provides an expression by mous usages with which I am familiar.7
which neighbors who are not kin are rendered as A third example is a recent analysis of Lako-
“like brothers and sisters” (emphasis added). ta Sioux kinship, wherein the author initially as-
Carsten makes much of the hearth used in com- serts, that “Sioux people continue to make relatives
mon by the women of a household, but this Com- throughout their lives who are classed with, and not
munitarianism is decidedly limited. Thus, in a po- differentiated from, those they had at birth” (DeMal-
lygynous marriage, according to Djamour (1959: ​ lie 1994: ​133; emphasis added). Then, two pages
30), “[t]he children of the newer wife refer to the later, he mentions “linguistic forms that differen­
father’s older wife as … [‘]stepmother[’], whereas tiated between one’s biological parents and other
those of the older wife refer to their father’s new- mothers and fathers ” (135; emphasis added). But
er wife as … [‘]young mother[’]. The wives them- he provides nothing resembling a detailed seman-
selves refer to each other’s children by their com- tic analysis of Lakota kinship terminology. Earlier
mon husband as … [‘]stepchildren[’] …” There is scholars, however, were more thorough, presenting
a great deal more in Carsten’s corpus that contra- data that confirmed the latter statement but not the
dicts her claim of “undivided kinship” among Ma- former. Thus, Walker (1914: ​104) noted that one’s
lays: I shall have a bit more to say on this later. The actual parents and siblings are distinguished from
interested reader should see Shapiro (2011b) for a others in their respective kin classes by a modifi-
fuller analysis. er which he renders as “own,” which is logically
Similarly, writing on the Reite of the Rai Coast of comparable to Malay “real.” By contrast, these oth-
Papua New Guinea, and in a volume whose purpose ers are said to be members of their respective kin
is to “deconstruct” the idea of genealogy, James classes in a secondary sense: they are “considered
Leach (2009: ​188) insists that “[t]o draw a line from as” mothers, fathers, etc. (Walker 1914: ​96 f.). Close
father to son (in a kinship diagram) and say that this procreative kin are said to be “of blood,” but others
is a kinship connection is meaningless in Reite, for are only “considered of blood” (97). A study of the
the role of the father is not to pass on some com- neighboring Teton Lakota by Hassrick (1944) re-
ponent of substance to the son … but – through his veals native rules of kin class extension, e.g., let any
work – to establish the conditions for the latter’s woman married to a man I call “father” be called
growth on the land.” But in his book-length state- “mother.” Such rules, as we shall see, are common
ment on these matters, Leach tells us that such co- ethnographically.
operative labor is “modelled on procreation” (Leach
2003: ​29, emphasis added). Moreover, he renders (2)  “Comparable pertinent data on systems of kin
the kinship terminology via conventional genealog- classification, which, if considered, would have ob-
ical diagrams (2003: ​57 ff.), though he presents no viated the performativist position, have been in the
ethnographic record literally since Morgan’s day.
But pace supra, performativist scholars seem to be
are likely foreign to native understandings. Schneider (1989)
unaware of it.” Thus Morgan noted that, among the
dismissed them as “virtuoso manipulations.” But there is
more to the extensionist position than this: see esp. Scheffler Cree of the eastern Canadian Subarctic, the term
(1972) for a response to such criticism. Throughout this essay for FB is not the “father” term in its unmarked form
my employment of such terms as “focal” and “focality” fol- but rather that term with a lexical marker. Morgan
lows native distinctions – as indeed Scheffler did. Lounsbury, translated this marker as “step-” and, consequently,
by contrast, simply assumed the logical priority of close pro-
creative kin: thus his well-known analysis of Trobriand kin
classification (Lounsbury 1965) takes no account of native   7 E.g., Firth (1936: ​130), Geertz and Geertz (1975: ​85–94),
distinctions noted by Malinowski and others (Shapiro n. d.). Kroeber (1917: ​70).

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
24 Warren Shapiro

rendered the pertinent native term as “stepfather” pending on relative age. This is to say that parent
(Morgan 1871: ​208). In other words, in Cree the fa- enjoys focal status in his/her class, other members
ther is what semanticists call the focal or seman- being defined relative to him or her. This is a com-
tically central member of a larger “father” class, mon feature in so-called “classificatory” terminolo-
while the FB is a secondary member of that class. gies: Wake (1889: ​476–479), drawing on Morgan’s
A less technical but perhaps more comprehensible tabulations in “Systems,” noted it for Tamil-speak-
way of putting it is to say that the FB is fatherish, or ers in South India just after Morgan’s passing, and
like a father, while the father is the Real McCoy. But similar reports have come to us since.9 Moreover, in
Morgan was entirely unaware that such evidence, of Hoebel’s words, “The br[other] category receives
which the Cree provide only one of his examples in several striking extensions” (1939: ​448). “A wife-
“Systems,” utterly undermines his communitarian absconder and the aggrieved husband,” he tells us,
scheme of the progression of family forms more ful- “call each other ‘brother,’ though until a legal set-
ly spelled out in “Ancient Society” (Morgan 1877). tlement has been made … they are dangerous ene-
For these examples suggest a singling out of one’s mies.” “This terminological practice seems to stem
father, mother, siblings, and children  – in other from the notion that the males who share a married
words, one’s nuclear family – from other members woman sexually should be brothers …” (448; em-
of their respective kin classes. As we saw in the Ma- phasis added). Hence the native extension rule, “Let
lay case, English does the same – only more obvi- any man cavorting with my wife be terminological-
ously, i.e., without resorting to lexical marking. ly equated with my brother.” Moreover, “[t]he insti-
Speck (1918), basing his analysis on longer field- tution of formal friendship among men also entails
work and covering both the Cree and neighboring the use of the brother terminology. The friend …
populations, confirmed the “stepfather” finding and takes the status of his ‘brother’ in the relationship
added that the reciprocal terms for a man’s broth- system of his comrade’s family …” (448). Hence a
er’s son and brother’s daughter are themselves lexi- further native extension rule, “Let my formal friend
cally marked versions of the “son” and “daughter” be terminologically equated with my brother.”
terms, respectively. In his words they are “terms Speck’s findings on the Cree and their neighbors
derived from those for son and daughter” (151). and Hoebel’s on the South Plains/Great Basin were
These are all terms of reference. As for “terms in- by no means unusual for their time.10 Indeed, they
volving address within the immediate family,” these have continued to be reported, including in many of
are “shortened forms of the non-vocatives denoting the classic ethnographic cases (Shapiro 2018).
endearment” (153), comparable to English “mom-
my” and “daddy.” Moreover, in some of these pop- (3)  “Ideologies of substance-sharing, which, if con-
ulations the “mother” term is derived from terms sidered, would have also obviated the performativist
meaning “to suckle” and “breast” (153; compare position, have been in the ethnographic record since
English “mama” and “mammary”), and, more di- Tylor’s day. Again, it goes almost unrecognized in
rectly related to procreation, it means literally “the the new kinship literature.” Although not nearly so
one who bore me” (156, note 3). The correspond- abundantly analyzed as kinship terminologies, they
ing “father” term is translatable as “my generator” point in the same direction. Over a hundred and fif-
(156, note 1).8 Finally, in some of the groups ad- ty years ago Tylor (1865: ​300–313) brought to the
joining the Cree, the “cousin” terms are lexical- attention of social theory “the couvade,” a practice
ly marked versions of the “sibling” terms, and the exemplified especially in Aboriginal Latin America
marker means “not by blood descent” (157, note 4; in which a child is held, Tylor emphasized, to be in a
emphasis added). Again, as in the Malay case, lo- post-partum bond of substance with its father. Sub-
cal notions of kinship are not so very different from sequent research, especially in Amazonia but else-
what we find in English. where as well,11 has shown that the mother shares
A further example pertains to some of the Native this tie too. Doja puts it cogently:
American peoples of the southern Great Plains and
to adjoining populations of the Great Basin. ­Hoebel Most ethnographic accounts of couvade insist that both
(1939: ​447 f.) notes that one’s parents’ same-sex parents are protecting the infant’s vigor and assisting its
siblings are subclassed relative to the linking par­ fast growth through fasting [and other observances]. But
ent, as “big” or “small” versions of the latter, de-
  9 E.g., Beck (1972: ​216), Nayacakalou (1955: ​48), Strathern
(1980).
  8 Westermarck (1894: ​88 f.), writing on other populations, was 10 See, e.g., Freire-Marreco (1914), Harrington (1912), Junod
apparently the first to note the common use of procreative no- (1912: ​222–226), Kroeber (1917), Walker (1914).
tions in the construction of kin terms. 11 E.g., DaMatta (1973: ​279 f.), Doja (2005), Valentine (2002).

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 25

it is important to stress that violations of the taboo not lings, who have drunk the same milk. [Others] brought up
only harm the child but can also turn against the father or in one house who have shared meals … could technically
mother. … Both father’s and mother’s fasting and inac- marry. In fact they are very unlikely to, because to do so
tivity strongly identify them with the newborn with whom would carry connotations of incest (1992: ​38).
they form a community of substance (2005: ​930; empha-
sis added). And elsewhere Carsten (1990: ​271) tells us that
“[s]hared consumption is epitomized above all
Doja (2005: ​931) further points out that some- by siblings …” The obvious conclusion, surely, is
times a wider sphere of kin is emphasized. But this that the “fundamental” means of generating kin-
is not true in all cases, and when it is, the injunctions ties among Malays is indeed procreation.12 This is
to more distant kin are attenuated, on the grounds also the case if we move from the begging of life to
that they are held to share less substance with the its end. Thus, among the Kayapó of central Brazil,
child (see esp. Aijmer 1992: ​8 and associated ref- “those mourning the death of a member of their im-
erences). These points are entirely lost on Becker- mediate family (for example, a spouse, sibling, or
man and Valentine (2002: ​3), in their grand claim child) have their hair cut short” (Turner 1980: ​117).
that Amazonian “partible paternity” – the idea that This is apparently because
several acts of intercourse are deemed necessary to
create a child, whose mother may have extra-mari- [p]arents are thought to be connected to their children,
tal lovers – challenges the “One Sperm, One Fertil- and siblings to one another, by a tie that goes deeper than
ization Doctrine” espoused by “Western” science, a mere social or emotional bond. The tie is imagined as
itself dependent on “our common Western view of a sort of spiritual continuation of the common physical
[single] paternity as universal.” As we shall see, substance that they share though conception … Although
spouses lack the intrinsic biological link of blood rela-
such charges of ethnocentrism and the parochial- tions, their sexual relationship constitutes a libidinal com-
ism of science are common in the new kinship stud- munity that is its counterpart. In as much as both sorts of
ies. The thing to note now is that Beckerman and biological relationship are cut off by death, cutting off the
Valentine utterly misrepresent what we know about hair, conceived as an extension of the biological … self,
Amazonian social theory, including the couvade. As is the symbolically appropriate response to the death of a
I have shown (Shapiro 2009b: ​41 f.), throughout the spouse as well as a child (Turner 1980: ​117).
area primary couvade linkages to the child pertain
to the mother and her husband, who enjoys the most
(4)  “Local notions of spiritual animation have been
sexual access to his wife and is, therefore, most like-
misinterpreted by performativists.” Thus, Sahlins
ly to be the actual father of her child; her extra-mar-
(2013: ​4 f.), drawing on Godelier (2011: ​229–280),
ital lovers are deemed to be much less closely tied to
writes of a “third party” in ideologies of concep-
the child. As one Amazonian ethnographer puts it,
tion – i.e., a spiritual one in addition to the moth-
“the most direct bodily connections are those rein-
er and father. In this connection, it is surely no ac-
forced on a daily basis among parents and children
cident that a group of scholars intent on divorcing
who sleep and eat together” (Conklin 2001: 118).
kinship from biology (see below) should seize upon
Carsten’s Malay analysis is relevant here as well.
some of the well-known claims of an alleged “igno-
Her claim is that, in the village she studied, “ideas
rance of physiological paternity,” as Sahlins (1976: ​
surrounding co-eating and sharing are as funda-
37–39), Carsten (2000: ​8), and Franklin (1997: ​
mental … as are ideas about procreation” (1991: ​
33–43) have all done. But none of these claims
425). This allegation rests on the idea that, just as
has withstood further analysis. Thus, Malinowski
blood is generated by procreation, so too is it held to
(1916: ​407) initially claimed that in the Trobriands
come about through wet-nursing and commensality,
“the state of knowledge … is just at the point where
because food is said to be transformed into blood
there is a vague idea as to some nexus between
within the body. This way of putting it suggests that
sexual connection and pregnancy, whereas there
the latter two sources of blood are modelled on the
is no idea whatever concerning the man’s contri-
first – a conclusion consistent with Carsten’s own
bution towards the new life which is being formed
statement that
in the mother’s body.” Conception is held to occur,
[J]ust as relatedness [i.e., kinship] is thought of in terms
12 In her initial publications, Carsten was nearly silent on the
of a continuum – one is more or less distantly related … role Malay fathers are held to play in procreation, but others
we find a parallel in terms of substance. … Mothers and have had more to say on the matter (see Shapiro 2011b: ​144).
their offspring and full siblings are most closely related. Indeed, Laderman (1987: ​75) tells us that the fetus is said
… More distant than full siblings but still close enough to originate in the father’s brain. Carsten (2004: ​129) seems
for marriage to be incestuous are those, such as foster sib- more recently to echo this.

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
26 Warren Shapiro

he tells us, when a spirit-child enters a woman, her 115). Further, it is considered offensive to the an-
husband only “opening the way” through repeated cestors if a married couple copulates in the vicinity
copulations (412 f.). But even in this classic arti- of the men’s house and/or just after they – the an-
cle Malinowski unwittingly presents contrary evi- cestors – have been propitiated, for to do so would
dence. Thus he describes a ritual bath undergone by “antagonize” them (115, 118). There is thus much
a woman “four of five months after the first symp- the same spiritual/carnal opposition as we found in
toms of pregnancy” (404). The ceremony, he tells the Trobriand case.
us, “is connected with the incarnation of the spir- Sahlins (2013: ​9) observes that “the Eskimo-
it children” (405), and he further notes that “[t]he speaking peoples must be the world champions of
view taken by one of my informants was that dur- postnatal kinship”, and it is clear that recent ethno-
ing the first stage of pregnancy the … [spirit-child] graphic studies of these peoples have played a large
has not really entered the woman’s body. … Then, role in making a case for the performativist posi-
during the ceremonial bathing, the spirit child en- tion.14 Nutall’s materials on an Inuit population in
ters the body of the woman” (405). In “The Sexual Greenland (1992, 1994, 2000) are especially perti-
Life of Savages” these data are repeated (Malinow- nent, partly because they supply a great deal of infor-
ski 1929: ​225), but we also learn that “[p]regnancy mation on Inuit naming practices and their connec-
is first diagnosed by the swelling of the breasts and tion to kin classification. Indeed, Nuttall (1994: ​133)
the darkening of the nipples. At this time a wom- insists that “name beliefs [among Greenland Inuit]
an may dream that the spirit of one of her kinsmen raise questions … for kinship theory in general.”
brings her the child from the … [spirit] world to Naming here is a form of ensoulment: an individ-
be reincarnated” (211); emphasis added). In other ual is said to be a reincarnation of a deceased rela-
words, the woman is already pregnant when entered tive whose name he or she is given (Nuttall 1992: ​
by the spirit-child. This is made plainer by Read 60, 67–69, 89; 1994: ​127). This is so much the case
(1918). Writing only two years after Malinowski’s that names influence the employment of kin terms:
initial formulation, he states expressly that spirit- thus, for example, a boy who is given the name of
entry is held to occur at fetal quickening. Thus what his maternal grandfather may call his own moth-
we are dealing with is not a conception ideology at er “daughter” (Nuttall 1992: ​68, 131). Moreover,
all: rather, it is a doctrine about the generation of he may assume the kin terminological position of
the spiritual aspect of the person, and, as such, it someone else who has this name: he may call that
is comparable to baptism in Christianity, as well as person’s wife “wife,” etc. So we have another native
other metaphysical ideas which Aijmer (1992) has extension rule, viz. Let anyone my name-giver or
dubbed animation. name-sharer called (or calls) X be called X by me.
It is, therefore, unsurprising that spirit-entry is But this is less impressive than it seems. Here are
held to be antithetical to physical generation. Thus Nuttall’s words:
Glass, drawing on more recent research in the Tro-
This does not mean that actual kinship, that is, kinship
briands, tells us that “[t]he Trobrianders were very based on putative biology is entirely forgotten about.
guarded about articulating their knowledge of Non-biological ties … are ultimately recognized as fic-
[physiological] paternity for fear of offending ‘the tive when sexual relationships and inheritance are taken
ears of the spirits’ ” (1986: ​60, emphasis added).13 into consideration. The equivalence of [kin] terminology
Another example of spiritual animation cited by does not mean the equivalence of blood, so there are no
Sahlins (2013: ​82–84) more recently pertains to the incest taboos that apply between an individual and the
Mae-Enga of the Papua New Guinea Highlands. actual kin of his/her name-sharer … Furthermore, inheri-
Here Meggitt (1965a: ​163) tells us that the father’s tance also recognizes the importance of genealogical con-
contribution to the fetus is openly acknowledged nection. A son … inherits … from his father. … [B]ut
but what is emphasized instead is entry into the (those connected through naming) have no claim … to
property (Nuttall 1992: ​68, 131; see also Nuttall 1992: ​
mother’s body by the spirit of a patri-clan ancestor.
92; 2000: ​44, 46).
As with the Trobriands, this entry is held to occur
not at conception but a fetal quickening (Meggitt In short, among Greenland Inuit kin ties through
1965a: ​163). Moreover, such ancestors are placated naming are modeled on those based on procreation.
in male secret/sacred ritual, held in a men’s house This being so, they do not have the significance for
from which women are barred (Meggitt 1965b: ​ kinship theory that Nuttall claims.

13 This is a highly truncated version of a more comprehensive


study of Trobriand kinship currently in preparation. See Sha- 14 E.g. Bodenhorn (2006), Fienup-Riordan (2005: ​219–221),
piro (1996) for remarkable parallels in Aboriginal Australia. Nuttall (1992).

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 27

(5)  “Elementary semantic operations are unrecog- Note the likenings – “like a spiritual parent,” “like
nized, with the result that analysis is deeply flawed dating your brother”: in each case the focus is a bio-
by sloppy conceptualization.” Thus, Chock (1974: ​ logical relative. Note also the use of lexical markers
33), in her study of Greek-American spiritual kin- (“brothers and sisters of the oil,” “spiritual moth-
ship, insists that “[s]piritual kinship in Greek-Amer- er,” “spiritual father” – indicating specialization and
ican culture is of equal symbolic status as [sic] derivation –, and again the focus is biological rela-
blood … kinship” (emphasis added). I suspect she tionship. Finally, note the extension of marital pro-
means that both forms of kinship are symbolic, that hibitions, almost certainly based on comparable ex­
“blood kinship” for these people is a “construct” tension of notions of relationship. The conclusion
and not an unmediated reading of nature. This bit seems to me inescapable that, if we are to use the
of triteness is a regular – and uncontestable – claim expression “equal symbolic status” with any rigor,
in the “new kinship studies,” 15 and it is entirely be- Chock’s thesis is entirely false, a distortion of the
side the point. The real empirical issue is wheth- way Greek-Americans represent the world.
er, for Greek-Americans, one of these two forms Weston’s analyses of gay/lesbian families in the
of kinship provides a model for the other, and it is San Francisco Bay Area turns Chock’s claim, so
quite clear from the data Chock presents that “blood to say, upside down, but effectively says the same
kinship” does indeed serve as a model for spiritual thing, and is equally mistaken. Weston (1995: ​99)
kinship. Here are some of the parallels she adduces claims that gay families are “just as real” as hetero-
in tabular form, supplemented by my own clarifi- sexual families. The expression, like Chock’s “equal
cations: symbolic status,” is used rhetorically, without rigor-
ous meaning, and is, therefore, beyond contestation.
physical birth spiritual rebirth
The real issue is the semantic status of such a fam-
womb baptismal basin
blood (of parturition) water (of baptism)
ily: as I have pointed out elsewhere (Shapiro 2010)
umbilicus cut hair cut it is quite plainly a derivative one, and its focus is
heterosexual families (see also Peletz 1995: ​348,
Chock (1974: ​44)explicates by telling us that 364). Why else is it labelled a family? There are
other examples of semantic modeling in Weston’s
informants … remarked on the birth symbols of baptism,
corpus; for details again see Shapiro (2010). There
including the similarity between the shape and function
of the baptismal basin and the womb. The child is spiri- is as well as solid evidence of some of her infor-
tually reborn from its water [basin] … [B]its of [its] hair mants’ grappling with what they rightly construe to
are cut off. These symbols thus contrast with the symbols be a heterosexual model. Here is Weston’s summary
of physical birth. of their position:

It seems fairly clear from this that her informants Why speak of lovers, friends, or even children as kin?
modeled spiritual rebirth on physical birth, that they “We” … should develop “our” own terminology to de-
likened the former to the latter. This interpretation scribe “our” experiences, rather than adopting “their”
(heterosexual) language (1992: ​122 f.).
is supported by the way these informants depicted
godparents, with whom a relationship is established This would seem to suggest a recognition by these
at baptism: informants that modeling is in status nascendi, and
People say that godparents are “like spiritual parents,” that its base is heterosexual kinship: there is consid-
… A godparent is a “spiritual mother” or a “spiritual fa- erable evidence for this in Weston’s corpus (again see
ther.” Furthermore, children who have been baptized by Shapiro 2010). It also suggests an apparently minor-
the same person are believed to be related as “brothers ity opinion that gay people should stake out a path of
and sisters of the oil” … This expression refers to the fact their own, rather than imitate an established kinship
that their godparent anointed them with olive oil at their model – which, I submit, is precisely what they do.
baptisms. They cannot marry. … In the same way, one A third and final example is provided by an up-
cannot marry the child of one’s godparent … The prohi- dating of Ward Goodenough’s Truk analysis by Mar-
bition is sometimes extended to a sibling’s godparent’s shall (1976), who provides a description of various
child … [One informant] said that she had once dated forms of performed “siblingship” on Truk without
the son of her brother’s godmother and “there was a big
any recognition that all of them, as Goodenough’s
commotion … because it was like dating your brother”
(Chock 1974: ​38 f.). classic analysis makes plain (1951: ​99 f.), are se-
mantically derived from procreative siblingship, ei-
ther by lexical marking or some other process (but
15 See, e.g., Carsten (2000: ​10 f.), Franklin (2001), Yanagisako see Marshall 1983: ​206). Here is what is apparently
and Delaney (1995: ​13–15). Goodenough’s last statement on the m ­ atter:

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
28 Warren Shapiro

When people enter into nonkin relationships to which a Manichean one, thus creating what D’Andrade
they give the rights and duties of a kin relationship, they (1995) has called ‘a moral model’.” The structure
may analogically extend kin terms to these relationships. of this model is as follows:
Thus Americans may say, “We are like brothers.” In …
[Truk], people who are close friends will say publicly performative kinship : procreative kinship :: ​Rest : West :: ​
“We are in a … [“sibling”] relationship” … In America Good : Evil
and … [Truk] alike, people recognize that these relation-
To which should be added
ships are modeled on genealogically based relationships
and that they are not “true” in this latter sense (Goode- communal kinship :: individual kinship
nough 2001: ​210; see also Scheffler 2001: ​166 f.).
Thus McKinnon (2005a: ​59) laments a Lost Par-
Marshall, for his part, enters the comparative adise characterized by “an expansive understanding
arena as well, claiming that various communities of kinship,” which, she argues, survives in the Third
studied by other scholars “are all reported to de- World of Latin American peasantry in the form of
fine kinsmen primarily on the basis of performance” compadrazgo, which is of course historically de-
(Marshall 1977: ​651). But the words “define” and rived from the westernmost part of Western Eu-
“primarily” are used without rigor, probably mean- rope. Elsewhere (McKinnon 2005b) she instances
ing “posit” in the first instance and, in the second, “classificatory” kinship terminologies, which, she
something like “has a lot of.” At any rate, I know of contests, provide for “a multiplicity of mothers”
no ethnographic case in which local definitions of (2005b: ​112), as opposed to maternal singularity in
kinship do not assign semantic primacy to procre- the West; but she is utterly oblivious to the immense
ative notions, as these are locally posited: most of this evidence for lexical marking in such systems, as
essay is a partial demonstration of this proposition. well as such English expressions as godmother,
step-mother, and Mother Superior (see Shapiro
(6)  “Related, calling attention to the fabricated or 2008 for a more detailed critique). Zimmer-Ta­ma­
nonfocal nature of performative kinship is seen by koshi (2001: ​192) writes of “a relatively rigid, West-
performative scholars not as a matter of semantic ern biological view of kinship.” And Weston (1991: ​
analysis but as one of denigration and ethnocen- 196) complains of “the genealogical logic of scar-
trism, and it therefore stimulates, utterly unnec- city and uniqueness.” All this is preposterous: it is
essarily, adversarial rhetoric.” Hence Weston’s re- as if performative scholars have never heard of such
mark, noted above, about gay kinship being “just Western notions, whereby members of a trade union
as real” as its heterosexual analogue. Hence, too, are “brothers and sisters”; whereby all Christians,
Schneider’s charging me with ethnocentrism for especially those “born again” as adults, are “broth-
using the rubric “pseudo-procreation” (Schneider ers and sisters in Christ”; or as if all the evidence
1989; Shapiro 1988), as if the Aboriginal Australian cited in this essay on the centrality of procreative
rites noted by Hiatt (1971), wherein elements of the kinship in the non-Western World did not exist.
male and female contributions to reproduction are
mimicked, were not derived from these elements, (8)  Related, the very positing of such a non-exis-
or as if, pace supra, Greek-Americans did not mod- tent gulf indicates that, far from representing non-
el their ideas about spiritual kinship on their ideas Western ethnography in ‘indigenous terms’ (Carsten
about physical kinship. A handy summary of this 1997: ​292)  – the grandest of the grand claims of
mistaken attitude is provided by Marshall (1977: ​ performative scholarship –, “the more recent per-
644), who writes as follows: “Kinship ties that are formative scholars misrepresent this ethnography.”
neither consanguineal nor affinal have been called I have already documented this in the present es-
many things in the anthropological literature (for say, as well as in several other places (see above).
example, ‘fictive,’ ‘pseudo,’ ‘ritual,’ ‘artificial’), all I would add here only that, for performativists, the
of which imply that these relationships are some- time-honored tasks of serious inquiry into human
how not ‘real’.” But there is nothing wrong with variation and the unity of humankind are abandoned
these labels insofar as they imply native modeling, in favor of the promulgation of a moral model.
which they do, and I know of no anthropologist who
uses them in a pejorative way. (9)  “Related, subscription to this paradigm excus-
es performativists of the scholarly responsibility of
(7)  “Having thus created a non-existent gulf be- commanding any of the kinship literature published
tween procreative and performative kinship, more between Engels and Schneider, so it is hardly sur-
recent performative scholars mistakenly marry this prising that the latter is viewed in virtually messian-
distinction to a West/Rest one, and, even worse, to ic terms.” Thus Faubion (2001: ​5) suggests that “the

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 29

anthropological study of kinship might be divided Her misanalysis is by no means unique. Thus
into ‘pre-Schneiderian’ and ‘post-Schneiderian’ pe- an alleged “new anthropological view” of the near-
riods.” Other commentators16 are only slightly less universality of the nuclear family by Collier and
enthralled. Many of the key figures in the history her associates (Collier et al. 1992) maintains that
of kinship studies – Westermarck, Kroeber, Lowie, the Mundurucú of central Brazil, studied by Robert
Murdock, Goodenough, Lounsbury, and Scheffler, and Yolanda Murphy (Murphy 1960; Murphy and
for example – are effectively banished from the cur- ­Murphy 1974), lack the nuclear family because vil-
riculum of the “new kinship studies.” All the egre- lage men reside largely in a special “men’s house,”
gious flaws in Schneider’s scholarship17 are ignored. with women resident, apparently collectively, in
Knight (2008: ​69–70) and Weismantel (1995) repre- separate dwellings. This recalls the Mae-Enga case,
sent the logical outcome of these tendencies, evok- noted above. But this “collective” residence is de-
ing the image of a vast right-wing conspiracy bent on cidedly compromised by several factors. First, a man
hiding from the anthropological world the insights regularly visits the women’s quarters, and when he
of Marx and Engels. Some idea of how absurd this does he associates exclusively with his own wife and
argument is can be gleaned from Shapiro (2009a). their dependent children, providing them with meat
from the hunt and fish from the catch. Second, he
(10)  “Related, there is recurrent appeal in the per- leaves his personal possessions near his wife’s ham-
formativist literature to some of the unsustainable mock, not in the men’s house. Third, when ill, he
claims of so-called ‘radical feminism’.” 18 In par- leaves the men’s house and resides with his wife, so
ticular, Friedrich Engels’ “The Origin of the Fam- that she can minister to him. Finally, there are vari-
ily, Private Property, and the State” (1972 [1884]) ous symbolic expressions of the unity of husband
is accorded a venerated place in the new kinship and wife, along with their common children. So, as
studies. Engels (1972: ​113) imagined that at an ear- with Carsten’s Malays, the all-female collective is
lier “stage” in human “prehistory” “the position of an illusion. As for the men’s house it-self, it is not
women [was] not only free, but honorable,” and just a place where men sleep and spend many of
he linked this high ranking with the absence of the their waking hours. It is a locale from which women
nuclear family  – the bête noir of “radical” femi- are absolutely barred, on penalty of gang rape, and
nism – and “mother right.” So – as if to instantiate within it are held rituals of a very high order of mi-
his words – Carsten emphasizes that her Malays live sogyny. That the traditional Mundurucú should be
in houses composed partly of matrilineally-related held up as exemplifying a feminist paradise seems
women who, she claims, are “central to the politi- therefore nothing short of delusional.
cal process” (1997: ​18). She ignores completely the Even more revealing is what has happened to
compartmentalization of these houses into nuclear those Mundurucú who have become involved in the
families (Shapiro 2011b: ​143 f.). As for the “politi- Brazilian rubber trade. This involvement has led to
cally central” claim, it is gainsaid by a mass of data, the disappearance of both the men’s house and the
including one of the photographs in her magnum allegedly collective female residences, and it has
opus: its caption is “Men vote at a village meeting” been instigated by the women, who plainly prefer
(Carsten 1997: ​141), and, accurately enough, it con- to reside permanently with their husbands and chil-
tains not a single woman. dren. The men, for their part, tend to wax nostalgic
about the former regime. So the suggestion, made
16 E.g., Bamford and Leach (2009: ​9), Brettell (2001: ​48), Ter- by Robert Murphy (1959) himself, is that the rela-
rell and Modell (1994: ​158). tively isolated nuclear family encourages the dimin-
17 E.g., Kuper (1999: ​133 ff.), Scheffler (1976), Wallace (1969). ishment of sexism, and that a regime of “extended
18 The temporal and geographical proximity to Morgan – he
lived in Rochester, New York – is remarkable, especially in family” households is what encourages the mistreat-
the light of Morgan’s incipient feminism. Thus he writes as ment of women (Shapiro 2011a).
follows: “It … remains an enigma that a race, with endow- All this, of course, is directly contrary to Engels.
ments great enough to impress their mental life upon the These two cases show with remarkable clarity that
world, should have remained essentially barbarian in their
treatment of the female sex … Women were not treated with
the conjoined fantasies of the all-female collective
cruelty … within the range of the privileges allowed them; and the absence of the nuclear family readily trump
but their education was superficial … and their inferiority anything resembling accurate rendering or reading
was inculcated as a principle … The wife was not the com- of ethnographic materials. What’s more, Murphy
panion and the equal of her husband … The wife is neces- seems to have been on the right track. To see this,
sarily the equal of her husband in dignity, in personal rights
and in social position” (Morgan 1877: ​474 f.). all one has to do is to consider where – and under
But I am unaware of any scholarly treatment of the mat- what conditions – the modern feminist movement
ter. emerged. The place, apparently by common agree-

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
30 Warren Shapiro

ment, was Seneca Falls, New York:19 It was not munity in which she carried out fieldwork, “there is
Moscow or Beijing or a village in Amazonia or the absolutely no privileging of the relationship a child
Papua New Guinea Highlands. It occurred more or has with the genitor and genitrix over others who
less simultaneously with the European political rev- are called parents” (1995: ​691), and that “[o]ften …
olutions of 1848 – more than three decades before the reverse is the case …” (691), but we are given
Engels wrote, nearly seven decades before L ­ enin no idea as to how often, and under what conditions,
replaced the tyranny of the czar with the Tyranny and there is absolutely nothing on kin classification.
of the People, but only fifteen years before the ab- In any case, her book on this community (Weis­
olition of slavery in the United States. Among its man­tel 1988) is not at all consistent with these
more or less immediate predecessors were chang- claims. Here she notes that “[t]he nuclear family is
es in British law which allowed women to inherit the conceptual basis for the … household” (169),
their fathers’ private property (Tobias 1997: ​15–17). and that there are “extended and fictive kin” (171).
All this seems lost on “radical” feminists – includ- Moreover, she tells us that “[a] man or woman re-
ing their representatives in the new kinship stud- fers to his/her mother-in-law or father-in-law as
ies – who, like other utopian intellectuals, want the ‘mother’ and ‘father’,” respectively, but the recipro-
world made perfect the day before yesterday. cal terms are not the unmarked “child” terms, or any
An especially outrageous example of radical lexically marked version thereof. Rather, they are
feminism’s influence on the “new kinship studies” is purely affinal terms (171): hence it is not true that all
provided by one of the most regularly cited articles others called by parental terms are undistinguished
in the performativist corpus, i.e., Weismantel’s es- from actual parents. These affinal “parents” are said
say on adoption in a village in the Ecuadorian high- to be “not really kin” (171), nor are they behavior-
lands, which claims to find a “masculinist bias” in ally like real parents; on the contrary, they are far
pre-Schneiderian kinship studies (1995: ​692). She less nurturant than exploitative: Weismantel (1988: ​
expands, in what I regard as the single most irre- 171–174) spends some time instancing examples of
sponsible statement in all the performative literature: callous treatment of sons- and daughters-in-law by
[O]ne aspect of this masculinist heritage that has re-
the parents of their spouses. Moreover, there is con-
mained largely unexamined is the emphasis upon sexual siderable stigma – very “bourgeois,” this – attached
intercourse as the single moment in which paternity be- to producing a child out of wedlock, even if the cou-
comes embodied. The authors of classical kinship thereby ple marries before the child is born (170).
universalized a heterosexual masculine perspective, de-
rived from traditional bourgeois life, in which men de- (11)  “Related, performative scholars claim, quite
fined their role in the family primarily in terms of sex- mistakenly, that certain ethnographic findings dem-
ual access to the wife and a distanced authority over the onstrate the unimportance of procreative kinship in
children. Extended nonsexual physical contact, especially at least some non-Western communities.” The claim
with children …, or nurturance for other family members, by Storrie (2003: ​408) that the Hoti of the Venezue-
was defined as feminine and demeaning … (Weismantel
lan rainforests “do … not recognize any idea of ge-
1995: ​697).
nealogical connection between persons” is surely
Weismantel (1995) claims that all this “is alien the most astonishing example; but it is gainsaid by
to the experience” of the people among whom she his employment of native theories of parent/child
worked. Let me suggest that it is also alien to the linkage in questioning informants (410 f.); and then
experience of a very large number of Westerners, by his list of Hoti kin terms, including the informa-
“bourgeois” or otherwise. I cannot pronounce upon tion that “parent” terms, when used in the possessive
the numbers involved, just as Weismantel has no form, isolate the “biological father” and “the biolog-
idea as to how many “bourgeois” families fit her ical mother” (412). Also pertinent is the proposition,
stereotype: apparently, she sees no need to cite any fairly widely found in the performative literature,
historical or ethnographic research on such fami- that, just as performative ties can be maximized in
lies. Her article consists almost entirely of actual practice, so procreative ones can be minimized or
instances showing that people get attached to those neglected altogether. This is, supposedly, especial-
who adopt them in early childhood, which is hardly ly true with regard to adoption, wherein the seeking
news, and recurrent bashes at “bourgeois” under- out of birthparents by adoptees is put down by per-
standings of sociality. She claims that, in the com- formativists to a Western obsession with biological
connection.20 But as I have shown elsewhere (Sha-
19 “I cannot bring myself to call these women ‘radical’ femi-
nists, as they are usually labeled, because I do not believe 20 E.g. Bowie (2004), Terrell and Modell (1994), Weismantel
they go to the root of anything” (Patai 1998: ​136). (1995).

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 31

piro 2015a), adoption in most of the world involves the People and gets his comeuppance in the volume
lexical marking  – i.e., it is generally regarded as in which Cassidy’s essay appears.23 More, the fol-
a secondary form of kinship, birthparents are fre- lowing excerpt from one of Marx’s letters to Engels
quently reluctant to give up their children and virtu- appears at least twice in the performativist litera-
ally always retain ties to them, including the ability ture (Sahlins 1976: ​101 f.; Yanagisako and Delaney
to reclaim them (see also Silk 1987). 1995: ​5): “It is remarkable how Darwin recognizes
The flexibility of non-Western kinship is a recur- among beasts and plants his English society with
rent theme in the “new kinship” literature.21 There its division of labour [read, diversification], com-
is indeed such flexibility, in both parts of the world petition, opening of new markets [niches], ‘inven-
in fact, but it has its limits: relatively distant procre- tions’ [variations], and the Malthusian ‘struggle for
ative ties can be ignored or even cut, but usually not existence.’ ”
close ones.22 The plain fact is that most human be- So Darwin’s thought occurred in a particular so-
ings do not just forget about their parents, siblings, cial and economic environment. It’s unclear how
or children. this distinguishes him from anyone else, including
Marx and the new kinship scholars. Thus, Wilson
(12) “Performative scholars subscribe to a ‘cul- (2016: ​573) turns the tables on the performativists
ture’/​‘biology’ dichotomy which is utterly obsolete.” by arguing that
Thus, Sahlins (1976: ​22), in his supposed critique a conception of kinship indeed has been projected from
of sociobiology, distinguishes between “a naturally “the West” to “the Rest” in [performative] kinship present
given set of ‘blood relationships’ ” and “a culturally … This conception of kinship reflects the shift in kinship
variable system of meaningful categories”; and in structures in the West in the 1960s and 1970s … [This
his recent (and also supposed) synthesis on kinship shift was exemplified by] working mothers, the spread of
(2013), he declares flatly that human kinship is “cul- contraception, skyrocketing divorce rates, Brady Bunch
ture,” not “biology” – as if these expressions point- families, communal living and free love [and] sexual lib-
ed to anything more than an Everyperson’s innate/ eration … Combined with developing technologies of re-
learned dichotomy, abandoned in the hard sciences production, those in the West had new ways to live …
for at least four decades (Lehrman 1970). McKin- That [new] conception of kinship was then projected
onto societies subject to past colonial and imperial in-
non (2005b: ​127) claims that evolutionary psychol-
fluence … .
ogy insists upon “universal forms of behavior,” in
apparent ignorance of the reliance on probability But of course the purpose of the quotation from
calculi in virtually all research in this field. And she Marx is not illumination but denigration. Let me
adds (117), in fine “deconstructionist” style, that it suggest instead that Darwin is a rather bad choice
exemplifies a “restrictive understanding of kinship for an argument that people (except, of course, “de-
… that is a reflection of Western upper-class con- constructionists”) are just parrots for an established
cerns.” She fails utterly to see its indebtedness to the regime, as well as, pace Cassidy, the contention that
idea on brain modularity of Noam Chomsky, not a his sense of genealogy “reduces individuals and
man generally associated with “reactionary” politi- events to the playing out of inevitable properties.”
cal views. Cassidy (2009) marries this antiquated Performativists need to recall the title of Dar-
view of “biology” with the pre-Schneiderian con- win’s most famous book “On the Origin of Spe-
cern with “the genealogical model,” about which cies by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preser-
she says the following: vation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life”
(1859), as well as the importance of differential re-
It reduces individuals and events to the playing out of … production and speciation in his overall theory. This
inevitable properties … It presents the contingent as nec- is the very opposite of Cassidy’s implication that
essary, and in doing so provides evidence supporting the
his model of evolution “presents the contingent as
most conservative of interpretations of the present. Much
of its utility depends on an ability to separate the mod- necessary.” Such a preformationist model (Rich-
el from society, to allocate it a place within “nature” … ards 1992) is the one proffered by Morgan and other
(2009: ​24  f.). “classical evolutionists” (Carneiro 2003). Although
Darwin could never fully rid himself of it,24 this is
No one is spared the all-seeing eye of “decon- not why he is generally considered the greatest fig-
struction”: even Darwin is exposed as An Enemy of
23 Bamford and Leach (2009: ​6), Cunningham (2009: ​112), In-
21 E.g., Bodenhorn (2000), Carsten (1995), DeMallie (1994). gold (2009: ​208).
22 See, e.g., Beattie (1971), Marshall (1977: ​659), Weston 24 E.g., Bowler (2009: ​163 f.), Gruber (1981: ​181–184, 197–
(1991: ​43–75). 199), Richards (1988: ​137).

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
32 Warren Shapiro

ure in the history of biology. The radical distinction erature on native science apart from the reproduc-
between the two senses of “evolution” is widely ap- tive process.28 Non-Western people are viewed as
preciated by historians of social theory.25 Moreover, corseted in “cultures,” devoid of any scientific tra-
far from echoing his Zeit­geist, Darwin was quite ditions of their own, and as having no interest in
aware that his formulations, especially on the muta- Western science; in truth, this is not very far from
bility of species, would offend extant religious sen- the worst Victorian and pre-Victorian images of
sibilities (Gruber 1981: ​202–204, 209–213). Indeed, “savagery.” All knowledge, after all, is held to be
as Bowler (1986: ​41) has argued, his “making nat- “constructed” – except the knowledge of “decon-
ural development an essentially haphazard … pro- structionists,” which, to them at least, is quite real.
cess” “challenged the most fundamental values of Consider the sheer pretentiousness of the following
the Victorian era.” remarks:
The shoe, really, is mostly on the other foot: it
is performativists like Sahlins and McKinnon who [T]he presumption of the structuring importance of the
subscribe to a performationist model (to which they “facts of life” as they are defined by Western biological
add certain Manichean quality). For them “culture” science [has] enormous theoretical consequences … The
production of sexual differences, the maintenance of het-
is imagined as replacing “biology,” and as antitheti-
erosexuality, the operation of an a priori domain of “nat-
cal to it. At the same time, “biology” is construed as ural fact,” [and] the presumption of a reproductive telos
something apart from “learning” or “socialization,” at the base of social organization all … evade critical rec­
somehow “inherent” in individuals, and “its” con- ognition … as a result of being so taken-for-granted [that]
sequences, unless countered by “culture,” are seen they remain invisible. … [T]hese features … of a centu-
as inevitable. This has absolutely nothing to do with ry of anthropological debate [on kinship] can be directly
current conceptualizations of the organism/envi- traced to the post-Darwinian worldview of Euro-Ameri-
ronment interface in biology:26 it is, again, nothing can anthropology (Franklin 1997: ​49, emphasis in origi-
more than an Everyperson’s ideology. nal).
With this antediluvian view of biology, perfor-
mativists claim that those who stress procreative (13) “Performative scholars participate in what
kinship ideas are advocates of a “reductive” (or “es- might be dubbed as ‘feudal’ academic community,
sentialist” or “biologized”) view of the human situ- entirely at odds with the universalistic values that
ation, views which, we are to believe, held in uni- academic institutions are supposed to live by.” They
son by the Western scientific “establishment” and repeatedly cite one another – and in often lauda-
Western folk theory.27 It is, of course, nothing if not tory terms, as if each one had made a serious con-
voguish these days to “deconstruct” various scien- tribution to knowledge, one which is beyond ques-
tific claims, or even the whole scientific/medical en- tion. Thus, Carsten (2013: ​245), self-involved in the
terprise (Gross and Levitt 1994). Such “deconstruc- sort of literary flourish common among performa-
tion,” it bears noting, should not be confused with tivists, judges Sahlins’ recent synthesis (2013) to be
earnest history-of-ideas scholarship, which serious- “wonderful” and “intuitively graspable – not as an
ly examines theories in their social and intellectual analytic abstraction, as many definitions of kinship
contexts. Its only purposes, as noted, are denigration seem to be, but in a way that palpably makes sense
and rhetorical one-upmanship, and it carries the im- of a whole range of human experience.” Schnei-
plication that those who “deconstruct” are part of der’s analysis of Yapese kinship is described as
an elite class who have somehow managed to sur- “brilliant” (Terrell and Modell 1994: ​158), despite
pass the biographical and social constraints that en- the fact that it has been called into question by sev-
trap the rest of us and see into Ultimate Reality. A eral scholars.29 On the other side of the emotion-
corollary of all this, widely maintained, as we have al spectrum, Weismantel’s vitriolic contribution, as
seen, in the performativist literature, is that biolo- I have already noted, is regularly cited, but hers is
gy too is “culture,” implying in turn that there is no only the starkest expression of an overall hostili-
reason to grant it privileged status. Thus, not only ty to contrary views. The most common response
can the old kinship studies be ignored, “reductive” to such views is to ignore them. Scholarly debate,
as they are, but so too can a very considerable lit- when it occurs, is almost invariably ad hominem, ei-
ther blatantly or by assigning to adversaries unfash-
25 E.g., Carneiro (2003: ​171 f.), Nisbet (1969: ​159–208), Rich- ionable political views (“arch-conservative”), social
ards (1992).
26 E.g., Barkow (2006), Oyama (1985), Pinker (2002: ​100–
102). 28 E.g., Brown (1984), Goodenough (1996), Sanga and Ortalli
27 E.g., Franklin (2001), Franklin and McKinnon (2001), Mc­ (2004).
Kinnon (2005a, 2005b). 29 E.g., Labby (1976), Lingenfelter (1975), Shimizu (1991).

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 33

class positions (“bourgeois”), or gender attributes cated pseudo-scholars will have in their classrooms
(“masculinist”).30 far more devotees than students with a right to ques-
tion; the latter, if tolerated at all, will likely be sent
(14) “Since ‘biology’ implies inevitability, ‘cul- for “sensitivity training.”
ture,’ its Manichean counterpart, implies untram-
meled freedom.” In practice this amounts to the un-
trammeled freedom of alleged authorities to dictate Concluding Remarks
people’s lives. This utterly disrespects the complex-
ity of the human nervous system as well as human In one of the very many self-congratulatory “synthe-
creativity and initiative (see my last epigraph), in- ses” attempted by performative scholars, Schweit-
cluding the ability to question authority. As noted, zer (2000: ​214) writes of “the ‘outdated’ nature of
performative scholars see themselves as part of a pre-Schneiderian kinship studies.” But such a trium-
larger “deconstructionist” project, allegedly collab- phant tone, as I believe I have shown, is warranted
orating with other “revolutionary” movements to only if one conflates scholarly progress with intel-
free the world from the constraints of capitalism, lectual sloppiness and academic faddishness. The
the nuclear family, and the erotic and power lusts of performativists’ position seems reasonable to some,
White heterosexual males. But here, as elsewhere, because they ignore systems of kin classification or
their “ethnography” is deeply flawed: by my lights have sophomoric views of their semantic structure,
the greatest power on a university campus these and because they have no idea of the overwhelm-
days lies with the local Sexual Harassment Office, ing evidence for the extensionist position in the ear-
seconded by the political intolerance of both fac- ly literature and beyond – indeed, even in much of
ulty and students, programs peddling “diversity” their own published work. The “cutting edge” they
(but insisting on conformity), and the almost com- have generated aligns what was once anthropolo-
plete “adaptability” of administrators (since the late gy’s gem with Afrocentrism, Goddess and matriar-
1960s, in fact) to extant fads. chy theory, the idea that gender is merely a “social
construct,” and other hokums that have been devel-
(15)  Last, and worst of all, “all this bodes badly for oping since the 1960s.
the future of kinship studies – indeed, for education All this is bad enough, but in addition, performa-
at large, which in any case has been on a downhill tivists have married it to utterly discredited collec-
course since the late 1960s.” This is because perfor- tivist theories of human sociality and the unsustain-
mative scholars have come to constitute part of an able West/Rest dichotomy that drives those theories.
Establishment on university campuses. Even with They are, I think it fair to say, part of a Brave New
the best of intentions, university administrators, not Academic World in which professorships – even the
being (except in rare cases) kinship experts, will be- status of “public intellectual” – are granted, based
lieve that those who claim to be so actually are – all less on scholarly achievement and more on preten-
the more so when they are willing – more than will- tious writing, faux commitment to hopelessly failed
ing to marry their “expertise” with current trends social and political ideas, gender (female, transgen-
in social thought, particularly “postmodernism” and der), sexual preference (gay/lesbian), racial catego-
“radical” feminism, which appeal to students and ry (non-White, in the process of becoming, at least
potential students and, thus, help such institutions to in the United States, non-Asian as well), misrepre-
meet their bottom lines. Much the same can be said senting The Other (as Primitive and/or Matriarchal
for funding agencies, whose grants depend largely Communists), and, of course, the ability to “decon-
on “expert” advice. In this way, such an Establish- struct” what one knows next to nothing about.
ment becomes self-perpetuating: it is looked to for Finally, it seems to me long overdue that we
advice of this sort, including what to do about hir- abandon the absurd argument that procreative kin-
ing, promotion, and student admission. Finally, and ship is a Western perversion of the “essentially”
also self-perpetuating, a professoriate of under-edu- collective nature of human sociality, allegedly to
be found in one part or another of the non-Western
30 The one exception that comes to mind is a debate between world. Focality and extension in kin classification
Linda Watts and myself (subsequent to Shapiro 2009a), are not fabrications of a vast right-wing conspira-
which, though heated, was nonetheless scholarly, if I may cy bent on hiding from us the “insights” of Fried-
say so. It is, I think, significant that Watts is one of the few rich Engels, or David Schneider, or Marshall Sah-
performative scholars who takes systems of kin classification
seriously, and who is familiar with the older literature. The lins: they have been in the ethnographic record for
“arch-conservative” charge was leveled against me by Sah- over a century. We find them whenever one of our
lins (2012). informants tells us that his or her genetrix is the

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
34 Warren Shapiro

“true” member of her kin class, and whenever he or Beckerman, Stephen, and Paul Valentine
she notes that he/she calls a particular man “father” 2002 Introduction. The Concept of Partible Paternity among
because his/her “true” father called him “brother.” Native South Americans. In: S. Beckerman and P. Valen-
tine (eds.), Cultures of Multiple Fathers. The Theory and
This being so, the suggestion is that the nuclear Practice of Partible Paternity in Lowland South America;
family is something to which human beings are in- pp. 1–13. Gainesville: University of Florida Press.
clined by their species heritage and not the dispos- Bodenhorn, Barbara
able product of a particular “stage of society” or a 2000 “He Used to Be My Relative.” Exploring the Basis of
particular economic regime or “arch-conservative” Relatedness among Iñupiat of Northern Alaska. In: J.
social views. The number of exceptions to its near- Carsten (ed.), Cultures of Relatedness. New Approaches
to the Study of Kinship; pp. 128–148. Cambridge: Cam-
universality in the ethnographic record on the Third bridge University Press.
World can be counted on the fingers of one hand. 2006 Calling into Being. Naming and Speaking Names on
More urgently, there are exceptions all around us Alaska’s North Slope. In: G. vom Bruck and B. Boden-
in the shape of “alternative family forms.” But even horn (eds.), The Anthropology of Names and Naming;
pp. 140–156. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
this expression suggests modeling, and it should be
clear by now what this model is. The real achieve- Bowie, Fiona
2004 Adoption and the Circulation of Children. A Compara-
ment of the performativists, though I doubt that they tive Perspective. In: F. Bowie (ed.), Cross-Cultural Ap-
would ever admit it, is not that they show that the proaches to Adoption; pp. 3–20. London: Routledge.
nuclear family is absent or unimportant in some Bowler, Peter J.
communities but, quite to the contrary, that it is so 1986 Theories of Human Evolution. A Century of Debate,
important nearly everywhere that people model oth- 1844–1944. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
er social ties on it. 2009 Evolution. The History of an Idea. Berkeley: University
of California Press. [25th Anniversary Ed.]
Brettell, Caroline B.
References Cited 2001 Not That Lineage Stuff. Teaching Kinship into the Twen-
ty-First Century. In: L. Stone (ed.), New Directions in
Anthropological Kinship; pp. 48–70. Lanham: Rowman
Aijmer, Göran
& Littlefield.
1992 Introduction. Coming into Existence. In: G. Aijmer (ed.),
Coming into Existence. Birth and Metaphors of Birth; Brown, Cecil H.
pp. 1–19. Gøteborg: Institute for Advanced Studies in So- 1984 Language and Living Things. Uniformities in Folk Clas-
cial Anthropology. sification and Naming. New Brunswick: Rutgers Univer-
sity Press.
Bamford, Sandra, and James Leach
2009 Introduction. Pedigrees of Knowledge. Anthropology and Carneiro, Robert L.
the Genealogical Method. In: S. Bamford and J. Leach 2003 Evolutionism in Cultural Anthropology. A Critical His-
(eds.); pp. 1–23. tory. Boulder: Westview Press.
Bamford, Sandra, and James Leach (eds.) Carsten, Janet
2009 Kinship and Beyond. The Genealogical Model Reconsid- 1990 Women, Men, and the Long and Short Term of Inher-
ered. New York: Berghahn Books. (Fertility, Reproduc- itance in Pulau Langkawi, Malaysia. Bijdragen tot de
tion, and Sexuality, 15) Taal-, Land- en Volkenkunde 146: ​270–288.
1991 Children in Between. Fostering and the Process of Kin-
Banks, David J. ship in Pulau Langkawi, Malaysia. Man (N. S.) 26: ​425–
1974 Malay Kinship Terms and Morgan’s Malayan Terminolo- 443.
gy. The Complexity of Simplicity. Bijdragen tot de Taal-, 1995 The Substance of Kinship and the Heat of the Hearth.
Land- en Volkenkunde 130: ​44–68. Feeding, Personhood, and Relatedness among Malays in
1983 Malay Kinship. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study of Pulau Langkawi. American Ethnologist 22: ​223–241.
Human Issues. 1997 The Heat of the Hearth. The Process of Kinship in a Ma-
Barkow, Jerome H. lay Fishing Community. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
2006 Introduction. Sometimes the Bus Does Wait. In: J. H. 2000 Introduction. Cultures of Relatedness. In: J. Carsten (ed.),
Barkow (ed.), Missing the Revolution. Darwinism for Cultures of Relatedness. New Approaches to the Study
Social Scientists; pp. 3–59. Oxford: Oxford University of Kinship; pp. 1–36. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press. Press.
2004 After Kinship. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Basso, Ellen B. 2013 What Kinship Does – and How. HAU – Journal of Eth­
1973 The Kalapalo Indians of Central Brazil. New York: Holt, nographic Theory 3/2: ​245–251.
Rinehart, and Winston.
Cassidy, Rebecca
Beattie, John H. M. 2009 Arborescent Culture. Writing and Not Writing Racehorse
1971 “Cutting Kinship” in Bunyoro. Ethnology 10: ​211–214. Pedigrees. In: S. Bamford and J. Leach (eds.); pp. 24–49.
Beck, Brenda E. F. Chock, Phyllis Pease
1972 Peasant Society in Koṅku. A Study of Right and Left 1974 Time, Nature, and Spirit. A Symbolic Analysis of Greek-
Subcastes in South India. Vancouver: University of Brit- American Spiritual Kinship. American Ethnologist 1: ​
ish Columbia Press. 33–47.

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 35

Chomsky, Noam Franklin, Sarah


1975 Reflections on Language. New York: Random House. 1997 Embodied Progress. A Cultural Account of Assisted Con-
ception. London: Routledge.
Collier, Jane, Michelle Z. Rosaldo, and Sylvia Yanagisako 2001 Biologization Revisited. Kinship Theory in the Context
1992 Is There a Family? New Anthropological Views. In: B. of the New Biologies. In: S. Franklin and S. McKinnon
Thorne (ed.), Rethinking the Family. Some Feminist (eds.); pp. 302–328.
Questions; pp. 31–48. Boston: Northeastern University
Press. Franklin, Sarah, and Susan McKinnon (eds.)
2001 Relative Values. Reconfiguring Kinship Studies. Durham:
Conklin, Beth A.
Duke University Press.
2001 Consuming Grief. Compassionate Cannibalism in an Am-
azonian Society. Austin: University of Texas Press. Freire-Marreco, Barbara
Crocker, Jon Christopher 1914 Tewa Kinship Terms from the Pueblo of Hano, Arizona.
1985 Vital Souls. Bororo Cosmology, Natural Symbolism, and American Anthropologist 16: ​269–287.
Shamanism. Tucson: The University of Arizona Press. Geertz, Clifford
Cunningham, Hilary 1983 Local Knowledge. Further Essays in Interpretive Anthro-
2009 Genes, Mobilities, and the Enclosures of Capital. Con- pology. New York: Basic Books.
testing Ancestry and Its Applications in Iceland. In: S. Geertz, Hildred, and Clifford Geertz
Bamford and J. Leach (eds.); pp. 111–137. 1975 Kinship in Bali. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
D’Andrade, Roy G. Glass, Patrick
1995 Moral Models in Anthropology. Current Anthropology 1986 The Trobriand Code. An Interpretation of Trobriand War
36: ​399–408. Shield Designs. Anthropos 81: ​47–63.
Da Matta, Roberto Godelier, Maurice
1973 A Reconsideration of Apinayé Social Morphology. In: 2011 The Metamorphoses of Kinship. London: Verso.
D. R. Gross (ed.), Peoples and Cultures of Native South
America. An Anthropological Reader; pp. 277–291. Gar- Goldenweiser, Alexander
den City: Doubleday. 1937 Anthropology. An Introduction to Primitive Culture. New
York: F. S. Crofts.
Darwin, Charles
1859 On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, Goodenough, Ward H.
or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for 1951 Property, Kin, and Community on Truk. New Haven:
Life. London: John Murray. Yale University Press. (Yale University Publications in
DeMallie, Raymond J. Anthropology, 46)
1994 Kinship and Biology in Sioux Culture. In: R. J. DeMallie 1996 Navigation in the Western Carolines. A Traditional Sci-
(ed.), North American Indian Anthropology. Essays on ence. In: L. Nader (ed.), Naked Science. Anthropological
Society and Culture; pp. 125–146. Norman: University Inquiry into Boundaries, Power, and Knowledge; pp. 29–
of Oklahoma Press. 42. New York: Routledge.
2001 Conclusion. Muddles in Schneider’s Model. In: R. Fein-
Djamour, Judith berg and M. Ottenheimer (eds.), The Cultural Analysis
1959 Malay Kinship and Marriage in Singapore. London: Ath- of Kinship. The Legacy of David M. Schneider; pp. 205–
lone Press. (London School of Economics; Monographs 218. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
on Social Anthropology, 21)
Gross, Paul R., and Norman Levitt
Doja, Albert 1994 Higher Superstition. The Academic Left and Its Quar-
2005 Rethinking the Couvade. Anthropological Quarterly 78: ​ rels with Science. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University
917–950. Press.
Engels, Friedrich Gruber, Howard E.
1972 The Origin of the Family, Private Property, and the State 1981 Darwin on Man. A Psychological Study of Scientific Cre-
in the Light of the Researches of Lewis H. Morgan. (With ativity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [2nd Ed.]
an Introd. and Notes by E. Burke Leacock.) New York:
International Publishers. [1884] Harrington, John P.
1912 Tewa Relationship Terms. American Anthropologist 14: ​
Faubion, James D. 472–498.
2001 Introduction. Toward an Anthropology of the Ethics of
Kinship. In: J. D. Faubion (ed.), The Ethics of Kinship. Hassrick, Royal B.
Ethnographic Inquiries; pp. 1–28. Lanham: Rowman & 1944 Teton Dakota Kinship System. American Anthropologist
Littlefield. 46: ​338–347.
Fienup-Riordan, Ann Hiatt, Lester R.
2005 Wise Words of the Yup’ik People. We Talk to You Be- 1971 Secret Pseudo-Procreation Rites among the Australian
cause We Love You. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Aborigines. In: L. R. Hiatt and C. Jayawardena (eds.),
Press. Anthropology in Oceania. Essays Presented to Ian Hog-
bin; pp. 77–88. San Francisco: Chandler Publishing.
Firth, Raymond
1936 We, the Tikopia. A Sociological Study of Kinship in Hoebel, E. Adamson
Primitive Polynesia. (With a Preface by B. Malinowski.) 1939 Comanche and HзKandika Shoshone Relationship Sys-
London: George Allen & Unwin. tems. American Anthropologist 41: ​440–457.

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
36 Warren Shapiro

Holy, Ladislav Malinowski, Bronislaw


1996 Anthropological Perspectives on Kinship. London: Pluto 1916 Baloma. The Spirits of the Dead in the Trobriand Islands.
Press. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 46: ​353–
430.
Inden, Ronald B., and Ralph W. Nicholas 1929 The Sexual Life of Savages in North Western Melanesia.
1977 Kinship in Bengali Culture. Chicago: University of Chi- An Ethnographic Account of Courtship, Marriage, and
cago Press. Family Life among the Natives of the Trobriand Islands,
Ingold, Tim British New Guinea. London: Routledge.
2009 Stories against Classification. Transport, Wayfaring, and Marshall, Mac
the Integration of Knowledge. In: S. Bamford and J. 1976 Incest and Endogamy on Namoluk Atoll. Journal of the
Leach (eds.); pp. 193–213. Polynesian Society 85: ​181–197.
Junod, Henri A. 1977 The Nature of Nurture. American Ethnologist 4: ​643–
1912 The Life of a South African Tribe. Neuchâtel: Attinger 662.
Frères. 1983 Sibling Sets as Building Blocks in Greater Trukese Soci-
ety. In: M. Marshall (ed.), Siblingship in Oceania. Studies
Knight, Chris in the Meaning of Kin Relations; pp. 201–224. Lanham:
2008 Early Human Kinship Was Matrilineal. In: N. J. Allen University Press of America. (ASAO Monograph, 8)
(eds.), Early Human Kinship. From Sex to Social Repro-
duction; pp. 61–82. London: Blackwell. Massard, Josiane
1991 Kinship and Exchange Practices in a Malay Village. In: F.
Kohler, Josef Hüsken and J. Kemp (eds.), Cognation and Social Orga-
1975 On the Prehistory of Marriage. Totemism, Group Mar- nization in Southeast Asia; pp. 137–147. Leiden: KITLV
riage, Mother Right. (Transl. from the German by R. H. Press. (Verhandelingen van het Koninklijk Institute voor
Barnes and R. Barnes; ed. and with an Introd. by R. H. Taal-, Land- en Volkenkunde, 145)
Barnes.) Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [1897]
McKinley, Robert
Kroeber, Alfred L. 1983 Cain and Abel on the Malay Peninsula. In: M. Marshall
1917 Zuñi Kin and Clan. New York: American Museum of Nat- (ed.), Siblingship in Oceania. Studies in the Meaning of
ural History. (Anthropological Papers of the American Kin Relations; pp. 335–387. Lanham: University Press of
Museum of Natural History, 18/2) America. (ASAO Monograph, 8)
Kuper, Adam McKinnon, Susan
1999 Culture. The Anthropologists’ Account. Cambridge: Har- 2005a Neo-Liberal Genetics. The Myths and Moral Tales of
vard University Press. Evolutionary Psychology. Chicago: Prickly Paradigm
Press.
Labby, David 2005b On Kinship and Marriage. A Critique of the Genetic
1976 The Demystification of Yap. Dialectics of Culture on a and Gender Calculus of Evolutionary Psychology. In: S.
Micronesian Island. Chicago: University of Chicago McKinnon and S. Silverman (eds.), Complexities. Be-
Press. yond Nature & Nurture; pp. 106–131. Chicago: Univer-
Laderman, Carol sity of Chicago Press.
1987 Wives and Midwives. Childbirth and Nutrition in Ru- Meggitt, Mervyn J.
ral Malaysia. Berkeley: University of California Press. 1965a The Lineage System of the Mae-Enga of New Guinea.
(Comparative Studies of Health Systems and Medical Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd.
Care, 7) 1965b The Mae Enga of the Western Highlands. In: P. Lawrence
and M. J. Meggitt (eds.), Gods, Ghosts, and Men in Mela-
Leach, James
nesia. Some Religions of Australian New Guinea and the
2003 Creative Land. Place and Procreation on the Rai Coast of
New Hebrides; pp. 105–131. Melbourne: Oxford Univer-
Papua New Guinea. New York: Berghahn Books.
sity Press.
2009 Knowledge as Kinship. Mutable Essence and the Signif-
icance of Transmission on the Rai Coast of Papua New Morgan, Lewis H.
Guinea. In: S. Bamford and J. Leach (eds.); pp. 175–192. 1871 Systems of Consanguinity and Affinity of the Human
Family. Washington: Smithsonian Institution. (Smithso-
Lehrman, Daniel S. nian Contributions to Knowledge 17/2)
1970 Semantic and Conceptual Issues in the Nature-Nurture 1877 Ancient Society, or Researches in the Lines of Progress
Problem. In: L. A. Aronson (ed.), Development and Evo- from Savagery through Barbarism to Civilization. New
lution of Behavior. Essays in Memory of T. C. Schneirla; York: Henry Holt.
pp. 17–52. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Murdock, George P.
Lingenfelter, Sherwood G. 1949 Social Structure. New York: Free Press.
1975 Yap. Political Leadership and Culture Change in an Is-
land Society. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii. Murphy, Robert F.
1959 Social Structure and Sex Antagonism. Southwestern
Lounsbury, Floyd G. Journal of Anthropology 15: ​89–98.
1965 Another View of the Trobriand Kinship Categories. In: 1960 Headhunter’s Heritage. Social and Economic Change
E. A. Hammel (ed.), Formal Semantic Analysis; pp. 142– among the Mundurucú Indians. Berkeley: University of
185. [Special Issue of American Anthropologist 67/5.2] California Press.
Lubbock, John Murphy, Yolanda, and Robert F. Murphy
1872 On the Development of Relationships. Journal of the 1974 Women of the Forest. New York: Columbia University
Royal Anthropological Institute 1: ​1–29. Press.

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
Fifteen Complaints against the New Kinship Studies 37

Nayacakalou, R. R. Scheffler, Harold W.


1955 The Fijian System of Kinship and Marriage. Part 1. Jour­ 1972 Baniata Kin Classification. The Case for Extensions.
nal of the Polynesian Society 64: ​44–55. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 28: ​350–381.
1976 The “Meaning” of Kinship in American Culture. Anoth-
Nisbet, Robert A. er View. In: K. H. Basso and H. A. Selby (eds.), Meaning
1969 Social Change and History. Aspects of the Western Theo- in Anthropology; pp. 57–91. Albuquerque: University of
ry of Development. New York: Oxford University Press. New Mexico Press.
(Galaxy Book, 313) 2001 Remuddling Kinship. The State of the Art. Zeitschrift für
Nuttall, Mark Ethnologie 126/2: 181–174.
1992 Arctic Homeland. Kinship, Community, and Develop- Schneider, David M.
ment in Northwest Greenland. Toronto: University of 1968 American Kinship. A Cultural Account. Englewood
Toronto Press. Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
1994 The Name Never Dies. Greenland Inuit Ideas of the Per- 1972 What Is Kinship All About? In: P. Reining (ed.), Kinship
son. In: A. Mills and R. Slobodin (eds.), Amerindian Re- Studies in the Morgan Centennial Year; pp. 32–63. Wash-
birth. Reincarnation Belief among North American In- ington: Anthropological Society of Washington.
dians and Inuit; pp. 123–135. Toronto: University of 1984 A Critique of the Study of Kinship. Ann Arbor: The Uni-
Toronto Press. versity of Michigan Press.
2000 Choosing Kin. Sharing and Subsistence in a Greenlandic 1989 Australian Aboriginal Kinship. Cultural Construction,
Hunting Community. In: P. P. Schweitzer (ed.), Dividends Deconstruction, and Misconstruction. Man (N. S.) 24: ​
of Kinship. Meanings and Uses of Social Relatedness; 165–166.
pp. 33–60. London: Routledge. Schweitzer, Peter P.
Oyama, Susan 2000 Concluding Remarks. In: P. P. Schweitzer (ed.), Divi-
1985 The Ontogeny of Information. Developmental Systems dends of Kinship. Meanings and Uses of Social Related-
and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ness; pp. 207–217. London: Routledge.
Shapiro, Warren
Patai, Daphne
1988 Ritual Kinship, Ritual Incorporation, and the Denial of
1998 Heterophobia. Sexual Harassment and the Future of Fem-
Death. Man (N. S.) 23: ​275–297.
inism. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield.
1996 The Quest for Purity in Anthropological Inquiry. In: W.
Peletz, Michael G. Shapiro and U. Linke (eds.), Denying Biology. Essays on
1995 Kinship Studies in Late Twentieth-Century Anthropol- Gender and Pseudo-Procreation; pp. 167–189. Lanham:
ogy. Annual Review of Anthropology 24: ​343–372. University Press of America.
2008 What Human Kinship Is Primarily About. Toward a Cri-
Pinker, Steven tique of the New Kinship Studies. Social Anthropology
2002 The Blank Slate. The Modern Denial of Human Nature. 16: ​137–153.
New York: Viking. 2009a A. L. Kroeber and the New Kinship Studies. Anthropo­
logical Forum 19/1: ​1–20.
Read, Carveth
2009b Partible Paternity and Anthropological Theory. The Con-
1918 No Paternity. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Insti­
struction of an Ethnographic Fantasy. Lanham: Univer-
tute 48: ​146–154.
sity Press of America.
Read, Dwight W. 2010 The Old Kinship Studies Confronts Gay Kinship. A
2018 The Extension Problem. Resolution through an Unex- Critique of Kath Weston. Anthropological Forum 20/1: ​
pected Source. In: W. Shapiro (ed.), Focality and Exten- 1–18.
sion in Kinship. Essays in Memory of Harold W. Scheff­ 2011a The Nuclear Family versus the Men’s House. A Re-Ex-
ler; pp. (not yet fixed). Canberra: The Australian National amination of Mundurucú Sociality. Anthropological Fo­
University Press. rum 21/1: ​57–75.
2011b What Is Malay Kinship Primarily About? Or, the New
Richards, Robert J. Kinship Studies and the Fabrication of Ethnographic Fan-
1988 The Moral Foundations of the Idea of Evolutionary tasy. In: D. Jones and B. Milicic (eds.), Kinship, Lan-
Progress. Darwin, Spencer, and the Neo-Darwinians. In: guage, and Prehistory. Per Hage and the Renaissance in
M. H. Nitecki (ed.), Evolutionary Progress; pp. 129–148. Kinship Studies; pp. 141–151. Salt Lake City: University
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. of Utah Press.
1992 The Meaning of Evolution. The Morphological Construc- 2012 Anti-Family Fantasies in “Cutting-Edge” Anthropologi-
tion and Ideological Reconstruction of Darwin’s Theory. cal Kinship Studies. Academic Questions 25/3: ​394–402.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 2013 The Nuclear Family and its Derivations. That’s What
Sahlins, Marshall D. Kinship Is. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Ox­
1976 The Use and Abuse of Biology. An Anthropological Cri- ford 5/2 (N. S.): ​171–193.
tique of Sociobiology. Ann Arbor: University of Michi- 2014 Contesting Marshall Sahlins on Kinship. Oceania 84/1: ​
gan Press. 19–37.
2012 Comment. Birth Is the Metaphor. Journal of the Royal 2015a New Muddles in the Models. Why Schneiderian Kinship
Anthropological Institute (N. S.) 18: ​673–677. Studies Have It All Wrong, with Special Reference to
2013 What Kinship Is – and Is Not. Chicago: University of Adoptive Kinship. Paper Presented at the Annual Meet-
Chicago Press. ings of the American Anthropological Asssociation, Den-
ver. [to be published in Structure and Dynamics]
Sanga, Glauco, and Gherardo Ortalli (eds.) 2015b Not “From the Natives’ Point of View”. Why the New
2004 Nature Knowledge. Ethnoscience, Cognition, and Utility. Kinship Studies Need the Old Kinship Terminologies.
New York: Berghahn Books. Anthropos 110: ​1–13.

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.
38 Warren Shapiro

2018 Hal Scheffler versus David Schneider and His Admir- Wake, C. Staniland
ers in the Light of What We Now Know about Trobriand 1889 The Development of Marriage and Kinship. London:
Kinship. In: W. Shapiro (ed.), Focality and Extension in George Redway.
Kinship. Essays in Memory of Harold W. Scheffler; pp.
Walker, James R.
(not yet fixed). Canberra: The Australian National Uni-
1914 Oglala Kinship Terms. American Anthropologist (N. S.)
versity Press.
16: ​96–109.
Shimizu, Akitoshi Wallace, Anthony F. C.
1991 On the Notion of Kinship. Man (N. S.) 26: ​377–403. 1969 Review of Schneider 1968. American Anthropologist 71: ​
Silk, Joan B. 100–106.
1987 Adoption and Fosterage in Human Societies. Adaptations Watts, Linda K.
or Enigmas? Cultural Anthropology 2/1: ​39–49. 2009 Discussion pursuant to Shapiro 2009a. In: C. Alès, L. K.
Watts, and W. Shapiro, Discussion of “A. L. Kroeber and
Silverman, Martin G.
the New Kinship Studies” 1. Anthropological forum 19: ​
1971 Disconcerting Issue. Meaning and Struggle in a Reset-
22–27.
tled Pacific Community. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press. Weismantel, Mary J.
1988 Food, Gender, and Poverty in the Ecuadorian Andes. Phil-
Speck, Frank G. adelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
1918 Kinship Terms and the Family Band among the North- 1995 Making Kin. Kinship Theory and Zumbagua Adoptions.
eastern Algonkian. American Anthropologist 20/2: ​143– American Ethnologist 22: ​685–709.
161.
Westermarck, Edward A.
Starcke, Carl N. 1894 The History of Human Marriage. London: Macmillan.
2012 The Primitive Family in Its Origin and Development. [2nd Ed.]
London: Forgotten Books. [1889]
Weston, Kath
Storrie, Robert 1991 Families We Choose. Lesbians, Gays, Kinship. New
2003 Equivalence, Personhood, and Relationality. Processes of York: Columbia University Press. [2nd Ed. 1997]
Relatedness among the Hoti of Venezuelan Guiana. Jour­ 1992 The Politics of Gay Families. In: B. Thorne and M. Yalom
nal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N. S.) 9: ​407– (eds.), Rethinking the Family: Some Feminist Questions;
428. pp. 119–139. Boston: Northeastern University Press.
1995 Forever Is a Long Time. Romancing the Real in Gay
Strathern, Andrew Kinship Ideologies. In: S. Yanagisako and C. L. Delaney
1980 Melpa Kinship Terms. In: E. A. Cook and D. O’Brien (eds.), Naturalizing Power: Essays in Feminist Cultural
(eds.), Blood and Semen. Kinship Systems of Highland Analysis; pp. 87–110. New York: Routledge.
New Guinea; pp. 329–370. Ann Arbor: The University of
Michigan Press. Wilder, William D.
1982 Communication, Social Structure, and Development in
Terrell, John, and Judith Modell Rural Malaysia. A Study of Kampung Kuala Bera. Lon-
1994 Anthropology and Adoption. American Anthropologist don: Athlone Press. (Monographs on Social Anthropol-
96: ​155–161. ogy, 56)
1991 Kinship, Community, and the Structure of Pahang Malay
Tobias, Sheila
Kindreds. In: F. Hüsken and J. Kemp (eds.), Cognation
1997 Faces of Feminism. An Activist’s Reflections on the
and Social Organization in Southeast Asia; pp. 125–136.
Women’s Movement. Boulder: Westview Press.
Leiden: KITLV Press. (Verhandelingen van het Konink-
Turner, Terrence lijk Institut voor Taal-, Land- en Volkenkunde, 145)
1980 The Social Skin. In: J. Cherfas and R. Lewin (eds.), Not Wilson, Robert
Work Alone: A Cross-Cultural View of Activities Super- 2016 Kinship Past, Kinship Present. Bio-Essentialism in the
fluous to Survival; pp. 112–140. Beverly Hills: Sage Pub- Study of Kinship. American Anthropologist 118: ​570–
lications. 584.
Tylor, Edward B. Yanagisako, Sylvia J., and Carol L. Delaney
1865 Researches into the Early History of Mankind and the 1995 Naturalizing Power. In: S. Yanagisako and C. L. Delaney
Development of Civilization. London: John Murray. (eds.), Naturalizing Power. Essays in Feminist Cultural
Analysis; pp. 1–22. New York: Routledge.
Valentine, Paul
2002 Fathers That Never Exist. Exclusion of the Role of Zimmer-Tamakoshi, Laura
Shared Father among the Curripaco of the Northwest 2001 Development and Ancestral Gerrymandering. Schnei-
Amazon. In: S. Beckerman and P. Valentine (eds.), Cul- der in Papua New Guinea. In: R. Feinberg and M. Ot-
tures of Multiple Fathers. The Theory and Practice of Par- tenheimer (eds.), The Cultural Analysis of Kinship. The
tible Paternity in Lowland South America; pp. 178–191. Legacy of David M. Schneider; pp. 187–203. Urbana:
Gainesville: University Press of Florida. University of Illinois Press.

Anthropos  113.2018
https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2018-1-21
Generiert durch Mariá Morando, am 13.07.2018, 16:54:35.
Das Erstellen und Weitergeben von Kopien dieses PDFs ist nicht zulässig.

You might also like