Countering Embarrassment-Avoidance by Taking An Observer's Perspective

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Motivation and Emotion

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9673-7

ORIGINAL PAPER

Countering embarrassment-avoidance by taking an observer’s


perspective
Li Jiang1   · Aimee Drolet2 · Carol A. Scott2

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
The fear of embarrassment can have harmful effects in many important consumer domains (e.g. health and financial), espe-
cially for high public self-consciousness (PUBSC) consumers. This research examines how adopting the perspective of an
observer interacts with trait PUBSC to influence embarrassment-avoidance. Study 1 demonstrates that individuals high in
PUBSC (vs. not) are more likely to take an actor’s perspective and to feel personal distress when viewing an ad with an
embarrassment appeal. Studies 2–3 show that seeing oneself as an observer is a helpful strategy for combatting embarrass-
ment-avoidance for high PUBSC individuals. This process is effortful and requires cognitive resources. Together, Studies
1–3 demonstrate the power of our theory to explain, predict, and modify embarrassment-avoidance among individuals most
likely to anticipate and avoid embarrassment.

Keywords  Embarrassment-avoidance · Empathy · Perspective-taking · Personal distress · Public self-consciousness

Introduction to be aware of oneself as a social object (Fenigstein et al.


1975). High public self-consciousness (HPUBSC) corre-
As consumers, we often forgo opportunities to help our- sponds to heightened feelings of being the focus of attention
selves, and others, in order to avoid embarrassment (e.g. (Fenigstein 1984), i.e. being in the social “spotlight”, and of
Helweg-Larsen and Collins 1994). Our fear of embarrass- paranoia (von Gemmingen et al. 2003). HPUBSC consumers
ment prevents us from admitting we do not know how a appear to be more concerned than others are with how they
product, such as a mortgage or birth control, works. It pre- are regarded by others (e.g., Froming et al. 1990; Miller and
vents us from asking advice about what we should do, for Cox 1982), and are more prone to embarrassment-avoidance
example, about our mounting mortgage bills and unplanned (Lau-Gesk and Drolet 2008).
pregnancies. In many cases, if we are to help ourselves, and How can we help individuals, especially high PUBSC
others, we must overcome our fear of embarrassment in individuals, to counter their tendency toward embarrass-
social situations (Foss and Crenshaw 1978). ment-avoidance? We suspect one of the reasons that high
This overwhelming tendency to avoid embarrassment PUBSC are particularly prone to embarrassment avoidance
is especially true for people who are high in public self- is that they may be more likely to take an actor’s (versus an
consciousness (PUBSC). PUBSC is defined as the tendency observer’s) perspective in an embarrassing situation. They
tend to perceive themselves to be in the social “spotlight”
and focus too much on the situation (Fenigstein 1984). If we
* Li Jiang can get high PUBSC individuals to focus less on the actor’s
ljiang2@andrew.cmu.edu perspective and take an observer’s perspective, we may be
Aimee Drolet able to help them counter embarrassment avoidance. The
adrolet@anderson.ucla.edu goal of the present research is to propose and test a potential
Carol A. Scott strategy for reducing embarrassment-avoidance, taking an
cscott@anderson.ucla.edu observer’s perspective.
1 The present research builds on three streams of past lit-
Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
erature. First, the work of Epley, Gilovich, and Savitsky sug-
2
UCLA Anderson School of Management, Los Angeles, gests when focusing too much on the actor’s perspective,
CA 90095‑1481, USA

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Motivation and Emotion

people fail to take into account observers’ empathy (Epley individuals. Specifically, we examine constructs proposed
et al. 2002). This research reveals that observers tend to to underlie embarrassment-avoidance, and demonstrate the
make kinder judgments than actors expect when observ- need for sufficient cognitive resources to enable an actor
ers notice actors’ potentially embarrassing blunders (Epley to adopt an observer’s perspective. In so doing, we extend
et al. 2002; Savitsky et al. 2001).1 This bias in judgment the literature on empathy-neglect, the effects of taking an
was termed “empathy-neglect.” Focusing too much on the observer’s perspective, rumination on negative emotions,
actor’s perspective, people may underestimate the degree to and public self-consciousness (PUBSC).
which others have experienced similarly embarrassing pre-
dicaments and thus others’ ability to empathize with them.
Second, we draw on literature on the effects of rumination Theoretical foundations
on negative self-thoughts. This research suggests that “step-
ping out of the self” is an effective strategy for interrupting Embarrassment‑avoidance
an undue focus on oneself, which past studies has shown,
intensifies negative emotions including depression and anxi- Researchers define embarrassment as a commonly-occur-
ety (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema 1991). On a similar theme, tak- ring, short-lived negative emotional response that arises
ing an observer’s perspective may be an effective strategy to from a threat to the public self in the presence of an audi-
avoid too much focus on the embarrassing situation. Taking ence, real or imagined (Miller and Leary 1992). A person
the perspective of an observer shifts the actor’s attention experiences embarrassment when he or she publicly violates
away from the self and to those factors that spur observers norms, displays out-of-role behaviors, or is incompetent,
to have empathy, tolerance, and kindness. in turn triggering unwanted and unflattering evaluations
Third, we draw on the literature on the personality vari- that threaten the person’s public identity (Edelmann 1987;
able public self-consciousness (PUBSC), which is corre- Modigliani 1968; Parrott and Smith 1991; Schlenker 1980).
lated with both the anticipation of embarrassment and the Much past research has focused on the triggers of embar-
tendency toward embarrassment-avoidance (Edelmann rassment (Keltner and Buswell 1997), how it relates to other
1985). Based on this literature, we propose that high PUBSC self-conscious emotions such as shame (Miller and Tang-
individuals are more likely to engage in embarrassment- ney 1994), and how it correlates with various individual-
avoidance because of their increased tendency to picture difference variables such as cultural background (Singelis
themselves in embarrassing situations and feel personally and Sharkey 1995).
distressed. Other research has shown that people will go to great
The main goal of our research, then, is to investigate a lengths to avoid embarrassment (Miller 2007) and the
potential strategy for reducing embarrassment-avoidance, accompanying threat to their social image. They may mini-
taking an observer’s perspective. We propose that the key mize or explain away the embarrassing behavior (Modigliani
to reducing embarrassment-avoidance is to induce actors (or 1971) or simply avoid potentially-embarrassing situations,
those who imagine themselves to be actors) in a situation for example by purchasing embarrassing products online (vs.
to see the situation from the perspective of observing the in-store) or by buying products aimed at preventing embar-
embarrassing situation rather than experiencing it. rassing situations (e.g. adult diapers) (Bell 2009; Brack-
We test our proposed strategy in three studies, focus- ett 2004; Lau-Gesk and Drolet 2008; Moore et al. 2006).
ing on perhaps the most challenging consumer segment: Accordingly, a strategy for overcoming the fear of embar-
individuals who exhibit a high level of chronic public self- rassment, and perhaps the emotional distress that accompa-
consciousness (HPUBSC).2 Our results demonstrate the nies it, would be useful for reducing embarrassment-avoid-
effectiveness of our strategy for reducing embarrassment- ant behaviors (e.g., not using a condom, hiding sensitive
avoidance (and not merely judgments) for high PUBSC information from a doctor, etc.).
Most relevant to our construction of such a strategy to
reduce embarrassment-avoidance is the psychological
1
research on empathy-neglect, which shows that individu-
 A second judgment bias, the ‘spotlight’ effect, refers to actors’
als’ fears of embarrassment and social disapproval are often
tendency to overestimate the degree to which observers notice their
embarrassing blunders; see research by Epley and colleagues. We unfounded (Epley et  al. 2002).3 Although actors often
do not investigate the spotlight effect here. Instead, we focus on the
segment of consumers who chronically feel as though they are under
observation (i.e., in the spotlight).
2 3
  We use the LPUBSC designation as a shortcut for saying low to   A reduction in embarrassment-avoidance might also be achieved by
medium PUBSC consumers—or not high PUBSC—consumers. reducing the degree to which individuals feel that others will notice
LPUBSC is not the focus of the paper because they do not have their behavior, i.e., that they are in the ‘spotlight’ (Gilovich et  al.
embarrassment avoidance behavior in the first place. 2000). In this paper, however, we focus only on empathy-neglect.

13
Motivation and Emotion

feel or expect to feel embarrassed, observers often do not interactions with sales people, cues in the environment, et
notice embarrassing blunders. And, even when observers cetera. An observer’s perspective may reduce self-focus and
do notice, they are generally forgiving. For example, studies in turn personal distress arising from an embarrassing situa-
have shown that people asked to imagine publicly tripping tion, and thus reduce embarrassment-avoidance.4
a security alarm, failing a test, arriving at a party without a
gift, or being introduced as someone who bed-wets believe Moderating role of public self‑consciousness
observers will judge them more harshly than observers actu-
ally do (Savitsky et al. 2001). Actors, focus too much on Not all actors are equivalent in terms of their expectation of
their perspective, appear not to take observers’ empathy into 1987 experiencing embarrassment. In particular, a person’s
account; they exhibit empathy-neglect (Epley et al. 2002). tendency toward public self-consciousness is associated
This bias appears to be fairly robust and difficult to counter. with expectations of being embarrassed and embarrassment-
Several processes have been proposed to explain why avoidance. PUBSC is defined as the tendency to be aware of
actors overestimate others’ harshness. The most common oneself as a social object (Fenigstein et al. 1975). High pub-
explanation appears to be that empathy-neglect is due to a lic self-consciousness (HPUBSC) corresponds to heightened
failure on the part of the actor to process information from feelings of being the focus of attention (Fenigstein 1984), i.e.
the perspective of others (Epley et al. 2002). Egocentric, being in the social “spotlight”, and of paranoia (von Gem-
people focus on their blunders and do not consider other mingen et al. 2003). HPUBSC individuals appear to be more
factors that influence observers’ judgments (c.f., Epley 2014, concerned than others are with how they are regarded by
pp. 111–115; Savitsky et al. 2001). Accordingly, embarrass- others (e.g., Miller and Cox 1982), and thus more suscepti-
ment-avoidance may result from too much attention being ble to experiencing personal distress due to an embarrassing
paid to the self for an embarrassing situation. First, actors situation.
may underestimate the frequency with which others also Past research confirms that HPUBSC consumers are
experience embarrassment and thus underestimate others’ more prone to embarrassment-avoidance and so less likely
potential empathy. Second, actors may engage in “naïve to engage in potentially embarrassing behaviors. For exam-
cynicism,” the tendency to believe that others will not be ple, Lau-Gesk and Drolet (2008) found that ads for a prod-
as understanding as they themselves would be (Epley et al. uct aimed at preventing embarrassing situations (e.g., gas
2002; Kruger and Gilovich 1999). What is needed, then, is a prevention product) incurred higher purchase intentions of
way to disrupt actors’ focus on themselves and augment this the product among individuals higher in PUBSC. Further,
focus with an observer’s view of the situation. a study by Froming et al. (1990) confirmed that HPUBSC
Past research on rumination also shows that a heavy individuals exhibit embarrassment-avoidance in part due to
focus on oneself can intensify negative emotions because their greater sensitivity to the negative evaluations of oth-
it focuses people on episodic information concerning the ers. Specifically, participants were paid to sing “The Star-
specific chain of events and emotions experienced (Kross Spangled Banner” in front of (1) a friend, (2) a stranger with
2009; Kross et al. 2005). Alternatively, less focus on oneself whom they did not expect to interact with in the future, or
can weaken negative emotions such as distress. For example, (3) a stranger with whom they did expect to interact with in
people felt less personal distress when asked to “take a few the future. The longer participants sang, the more money
steps back and move away from their experience” versus they earned. Regardless of audience type, participants with
“relive the situation as if it is happening.” They also had higher levels in PUBSC sang for a shorter time compared to
more construing thoughts (e.g. “I understand why the fight participants with lower levels of PUBSC who curtailed their
happened; it might have been irrational but I understand his singing only when the audience was composed of strangers
motivation now”) and fewer ruminative thoughts (Kross and with whom they expected to interact with again.
Ayduk 2008). Research also suggests that self-distancing To demonstrate that HPUBSC participants tend to focus
buffers people against blood pressure reactivity (Ayduk and on themselves as an actor in an embarrassing situation and
Kross 2008) and future negative affect (Ayduk and Kross
2008). In other words, an actor can benefit by taking another
4
perspective, i.e., one of an observer to the situation.   We are aware of the literature on vicarious (i.e. empathetic) embar-
We propose that, similar to the self-regulation of negative rassment, which reveals that observers of an actor committing a
social blunder often experience that actor’s emotional distress (Edel-
emotions, taking an observer’s perspective in an embarrass- mann and McCusker 1986; Miller 1987). We focus on somewhat
ing situation will make a consumer step out of the situation, different aspects of embarrassing situations. Specifically, whereas
focus on the bigger picture (possibly kinder judgments), and vicarious embarrassment focuses on what an observer may feel while
reduce embarrassment-avoidance. Taking an observer’s per- witnessing an actor commit a social blunder, we focus on how the
observer will evaluate the actor. In brief, we believe that our work
spective is an implementable and credible intervention. An represents a novel extension of the vicarious embarrassment litera-
observer’s perspective can be primed through advertising, ture.

13
Motivation and Emotion

exhibit heightened susceptibility to feelings of personal dis- We hypothesize that the effort involved in taking the
tress, we conducted a pilot study that examined the correla- perspective of an observer requires cognitive resources.
tion between PUBSC and the fantasy and personal distress Indeed, cognitive load can diminish people’s ability to
subscales of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis imagine how others perceive the situation (Davis et al.
1980, 1983). One hundred-three participants were recruited 1996). Similarly, we propose that people need cognitive
from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (Mage = 35; 53% female) resources to step out of themselves and take an observer’s
to complete an “Opinion Survey” which contained both role, and thus a lack of cognitive resources would reduce
the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis 1980, 1983), or the effect of an observer’s perspective.
the “IRI” as well as a measure of PUBSC (Fenigstein et al.
1975). The IRI contains 28 questions (anchors 1 = does not
Hypotheses
describe me well; 7 = describe me very well) that measure
different aspects of empathy, 7 items of which measure fan-
The results of the above-mentioned pilot study suggest
tasizing tendencies (e.g. “When I am reading an interesting
that PUBSC is positively associated with fantasizing,
story or novel, I imagine how I would feel if the events in the
which implies that HPUBSC (vs. not) are more likely to
story were happening to me”) and 7 items measure personal
put themselves in the perspective of an actor. Study 1, in a
distress (e.g., “In emergency situations, I feel apprehen-
correlational manner, tests the hypothesis that:
sive and ill-at-ease,” “Being in a tense emotional situation
scares me,” and “I sometimes feel helpless when I am in the
H1a  PUBSC is associated with an increased tendency to
middle of a very emotional situation”). The PUBSC scale
take the actor’s perspective in an embarrassing situation;
consists of seven items that measure the extent to which par-
PUBSC is associated with a decreased tendency to take an
ticipants consider themselves as social objects (Fenigstein
observer’s perspective in an embarrassing situation.
et al. 1975). Example items for the PUBSC scale include
“I usually worry about making a good impression” and “I
H1b  PUBSC is associated with increased embarrassment
am concerned about what other people think about me.” To
and personal distress.
ensure that participants were unaware of the survey’s pur-
pose, several filler tasks were placed between the PUBSC
Studies 2 and 3 test the effect of taking an observer’s
and IRI scales. In support of the above theorizing, we found
perspective, in both a health and an advertising context
a significant positive correlation between PUBSC and Fan-
respectively, on embarrassment-avoidance, and multiple
tasy sub-scale (r = .30, p < .0024) and between PUBSC and
measures to illuminate the underlying mechanism of these
Personal Distress sub-scale (r = .31, p < .002).
effects. Specifically, we hypothesize that:
Taking the perspective of an observer
H2a  PUBSC is associated with increased embarrassment-
avoidance (control condition).
The present research examines how taking an observer’s
perspective reduces self-focus and can counter embarrass-
H2b  Taking the perspective of the observer causes HPUBSC
ment-avoidance among individuals who expect and fear
individuals (but not others) to exhibit less embarrassment-
embarrassment, namely HPUBSC individuals. It is impor-
avoidance (Observer’s Perspective condition).
tant to note that taking an observer’s perspective is not to
be confused or equated with the more general, traditional
concept of perspective-taking, a term that usually refers to Effortful process
any attempt to overcome one’s own perspective by consider-
ing another’s potentially-different perspective (Davis et al. The process of taking the perspective of the observer pre-
1996; Gilovich et al. 2000; Nickerson 1999). Traditionally, sumably requires attentional resources and effort to redi-
perspective-taking describes the active attempt to understand rect one’s own perspective. Individuals can take an observ-
an actor’s thoughts with respect to a third object. In other er’s perspective only if sufficient cognitive resources are
words, perspective-taking refers to taking the perspective available to them. Thus, one key goal of Studies 3 is to
of the actor, not taking the perspective of the observer. In tease apart less effortful processing vs. more effortful con-
the present research, taking the perspective of an observer trolled processing presumably associated with countering
describes the attempt to imagine oneself as an observer to it by taking the perspective of an observer.
an (embarrassing) event (not the actor in it), which in turn Cognitive load has been shown to disrupt more con-
influences the salience of a now-wider set of information, sciously-controlled processes, causing people to rely
including information that observers are typically kinder in on more automatic processes and a narrowed focus of
their evaluations of embarrassed others.

13
Motivation and Emotion

attention on immediate, local information (e.g., Drolet and university participated in Study 1 (Mage = 31.0, SD = 9.1;
Luce 2004; Drolet et al. 2009).5 When cognitive resources 44.3% female; approximately 7.1% Asian or Asian Ameri-
are limited, HPUBSC individuals may not be able to take can, 70.0% Caucasian, 22.9% Hispanic, African American,
an observer’s perspective to counter empathy-neglect, and Pacific Islander, and mixed/other race). Nine people failed
HPUBSC individuals will seek to avoid embarrassment to answer all of the questions, leaving a final sample of 171
even more. In short, load blocks the effect of an observer’s participants.
perspective on embarrassment-avoidant behavior. Based Participants examined an ad for Beano, a gas-prevention
on this reasoning, we hypothesize: product. The ad portrayed a situation in which a person acci-
dentally farts in a yoga class while doing the downward fac-
H3  Cognitive load moderates the effects of observer’s per- ing dog position (see “Appendix”). The ad read:
spective-taking. Under load (vs. no load), HPUBSC indi-
Rip. Accidentally passing gas in front of classmates is
viduals who take the role of an observer will exhibit greater
one of the most embarrassing experiences. Guaranteed
embarrassment-avoidance.
to linger forever. Try Beano to avoid future embar-
rassment.
Essentially, under load, individuals will revert to their
chronic response patterns. This last hypothesis is tested in After reading the ad, participants answered several ques-
Study 3. tions regarding the perspective they took when they read
In summary, we conducted Studies 1–3 in order to better the ad. Three questions assessed the extent to which par-
understand how to reduce undue embarrassment-avoidance ticipants took the actor’s perspective (“When you read the
among HPUBSC individuals since they are more likely to ad, to what extent did you imagine yourself being the actor
be negatively affected by an embarrassing situation than are who farted in the scene?” “When you read the ad, to what
other individuals, namely individuals with medium and low extent did you put yourself in the scene and imagine this
levels of PUBSC. Past research has not much examined the happening to you?” “When you read the ad, to what extent
drivers of embarrassment-avoidance among individuals who did you think of yourself?”, 1 = A little, 7 = A lot). The three
are not high in PUBSC, instead treating individuals who are questions were highly correlated and were averaged to cre-
high in PUBSC as a clinical population. In the General Dis- ate an actor’s perspective index (α = .77). Another question
cussion, we offer several observations about this group we captured the extent to which participants took an observer’s
refer to as “LPUBSC individuals,” though it includes indi- perspective (“When you read the ad, to what extent did you
viduals with moderate levels of PUBSC as well as individu- imagine yourself outside of the picture”; 1 = A little; 7 = A
als with low levels of PUBSC, and several suggestions for lot).
future research with respect to this less well-defined group. Next, participants completed two items that assessed their
personal distress when they read the ad (“When you read
the ad, to what extent did you feel personal uneasiness?”
Study 1 “When you read the ad, to what extent did you feel personal
discomfort?” 1 = A little; 7 = A lot). The two questions were
Study 1 is a correlational study to demonstrate an empirical averaged to create a personal distress index (α = .96). Par-
foundation of our intervention: HPUBSC individuals tend ticipants reported their level of embarrassment in response
to take an actor’s perspective and tend to feel more personal to the ad using a 7-point scale (“When I read the ad, I felt
distress (control condition) which in turn makes them more embarrassed”; 1 = Not at all agree; 7 = Agree). Then, par-
prone to embarrassment-avoidance. We introduced partici- ticipants answered two questions measuring their inten-
pants to a potential embarrassing situation, and we measured tions to purchase the gas-prevention products (“Would you
actor’s and observer’s perspectives, personal distress, and consider buying this gas prevention product?”, “Would you
embarrassment-avoidant tendencies. want to purchase this product?”, α = .96) using a 7-point
scale (1 = Not at all; 7 = Definitely). Last, participants rated
Method and stimuli their tendency to pass gas in public (1–7 scale; 1 = Never,
7 = Very often). Responses to this question served as a poten-
One hundred and eighty people from an online panel of stu- tial covariate.
dents, staff, and local residents of a large public West Coast At the end of the survey session, participants completed
the Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss (1975) public self-con-
sciousness (PUBSC) sub-scale and supplied demographic
5 information. We conducted covariate analyses for age,
 Unawareness, unintentionality, uncontrollability, and high effi-
ciency are four underlying qualities of automaticity (see Bargh 1994). ethnicity, and gender. However, no significant differences
Evidence of one of these qualities is said to indicate automaticity. emerged (ts < 1) in Study 1, or in Studies 2–3.

13
Motivation and Emotion

Results samples revealed indirect-only mediation (Zhao et al. 2010).


We ran a regression with purchase intentions as the depend-
We followed Aiken et al. (1991) method and mean-centered ent variable and both PUBSC and personal distress as inde-
the level of PUBSC scores (M = 4.43, SD = 1.33). We then pendent variables. Estimation revealed that, controlling for
conducted a regression analysis (SAS PROC SURVEYREG) PUBSC, personal distress was a significant predictor of
in order to examine participants’ responses to the embarrass- embarrassment-avoidance (β = .31; t(168) = 4.43, p < .0001);
ing situation with respect to the centered level of PUBSC. controlling for personal distress, the direct effect of PUBSC
Although we have made directional predictions, we report on embarrassment-avoidance became marginally-significant
the (more conservative) two-tail t-tests. (β = .196; t(168) = 1.83, p = .07). The indirect path (β = .12,
95% CI [.04, .23]) had a 95% confidence interval that did
H1a  Hypothesis H1a suggests that PUBSC is positively not include 0. Therefore, we conclude that personal distress
related to the tendency to take the actor’s perspective mediated the effect of PUBSC on embarrassment-avoidance.
and negatively related to the tendency to see oneself as HPUBSC individuals’ tendency toward embarrassment-
an observer in embarrassing situations. We ran a regres- avoidance appears driven, at least in part, by higher personal
sion with the actor’s perspective as the dependent variable distress experienced by HPUBSC individuals in an embar-
and (continuous) PUBSC as an independent variable. The rassing situation.
analysis revealed a main effect of PUBSC (F(1, 169) = 3.90,
p = .05). The results became more significant if we included Discussion
the tendency to pass gas in public as a covariate (F(1,
168) = 4.93, p < .03). We reported the results below without Study 1, along with the pilot study, provides an empirical
the covariate. HPUBSC individuals were more likely to take foundation for our proposed intervention: HPUBSC indi-
an actor’s perspective (β = .19, t(169) = 1.97, p = .05) and viduals are more likely to imagine themselves to be in an
less likely to take an observer’s perspective (β = − .39, t(169) embarrassing situation, experience greater personal distress
= − 2.40, p < .02). These results are consistent with H1a. at its prospect, and then are more likely to seek to avoid
embarrassment. These results are supportive of H1a and
H1b  Consistent with H1b, a regression with the personal H1b.
distress index as the dependent variable and PUBSC as an Studies 2–3 build on these results by investigating the
independent variable revealed a main effect of PUBSC (F(1, effects of taking an observer’s perspective on embarrass-
169) = 5.34, p < .03) on personal distress such that HPUBSC ment-avoidance. Specifically, studies 2–3 test whether a
individuals felt more personal distress when they read the ad cue to take the perspective of an observer decreases embar-
of the embarrassing situation (β = .40, t(169) = 2.31, p < .03). rassment-avoidance among HPUBSC individuals. Since
We also found a main effect of embarrassment. HPUBSC HPUBSC individuals’ higher embarrassment-avoidance may
(vs. LPUBSC) individuals felt more embarrassment when be driven partly by greater personal distress or egocentric
reading the ad (β = .35, t(169) = 1.96, p < .05). attention, an observer’s perspective may reduce embarrass-
ment-avoidance by reducing personal distress. Study 3 also
Embarrassment‑avoidance provides insights as to the effortful nature of processing
information from the standpoint of an observer and provides
In this specific consumer context, higher intentions to pur- direct evidence as to process via thought protocols.
chase embarrassment-prevention products indicate greater
embarrassment-avoidance. A regression with purchase
intentions as the dependent variable and PUBSC as the Study 2
independent variable revealed a main effect of PUBSC
(F(1, 169) = 8.16, p < .005). HPUBSC individuals were Study 2 tests the hypothesis (H2) that a cue to take the
more likely to purchase embarrassment-prevention products perspective of an observer will have a helpful effect on
in order to forestall embarrassment (β = .31, t(169) = 2.86, HPUBSC individuals. Again, taking the perspective of an
p < .005). observer should make one’s own perspective as an observer
salient. By stepping out of oneself and viewing the embar-
Personal distress as mediator rassing situation from an observer’s perspective, HPUBSC
individuals should realize that others will not judge them as
We tested whether personal distress mediated the effect of harshly and so HPUBSC individuals should experience less
PUBSC on embarrassment-avoidance (purchase intentions) personal distress. In brief, HPUBSC individuals should dis-
using Hayes’ mediation model (2013, Model 4). Preacher play less embarrassment-avoidance when they are encour-
and Hayes’s (2008) SAS macro with 10,000 bootstrapped aged to focus on their role as observers rather than actors

13
Motivation and Emotion

(H2). Because individuals who are not high in PUBSC 7


are less likely to feel embarrassment in the first place, we

VOLUNTEER INTENTIONS
6 5.78
hypothesize that taking an observer’s perspective will have 5.15
no effect on them. 5 4.59

4 3.40
Method and stimuli
3

One hundred and seven students at a large West Coast uni- 2

versity participated in Study 2 for a $20 payment (M age = 1


19.9, SD = 1.5; 74.0% females). The ad read: LOW PUBSC HIGH PUBSC

CONTROL OBSERVER PERSPECTIVE


We are looking for volunteers to talk to researchers
about how to improve communication lines between
doctors and their patients about sensitive healthcare Fig. 1  Mean volunteer intentions by question-order and PUBSC
issues. For example, people who should get tested for (Study 2). Higher volunteer intentions correspond to more embarrass-
ment-avoidance
HIV or need treatment for genital herpes frequently
opt not to seek help from doctors, family members
and friends. You will be asked several personal ques- items). Participants then answered demographic questions.
tions during the interview. The goal of this research We suspected that low-income individuals might volunteer
is to identify ways to make it easier for people to talk for the interview for $50 even if they would feel embar-
more openly about such sensitive topics and thereby rassed, so we included income as a covariate in the analyses
feel more comfortable seeking treatment. You will reported for this study.6 Below we report the two-tail t tests
be paid $50 in exchange for your time and insights with an alpha level of 0.05.
(approximately 1 h).
After participants examined the volunteer ad, they Results
answered one of two versions of the survey corresponding
to the ad. The two different versions of the survey asked the In this study, we used volunteer intentions as our main
same set of questions but in a different order. Past research dependent variable. Higher volunteering intentions to dis-
has shown that different ordering of the same set of ques- close embarrassing information correspond to less embar-
tions can differentially affect responses (Sudman et al. 1996). rassment-avoidance. Note, different from the dependent
One version of the survey served as a control condition. variables used in studies 1 and 3 (i.e., purchase intentions
Participants in the control condition were first asked about for embarrassment-prevention products), the dependent
their intentions to volunteer (1–7, not at all/definitely vol- variable used in Study 2 (i.e., volunteer intentions) is nega-
unteer). Here, per H2a, we expected to find that higher lev- tively correlated with the underlying construct of embar-
els of PUBSC correlate with lower intentions to volunteer, rassment-avoidance. We conducted a regression analy-
a manifestation of embarrassment-avoidance. Participants sis (SAS PROC SURVEYREG), exploring participants’
then indicated how they expected to feel during the inter- responses to the embarrassing volunteer opportunity from
view (1 = not at all, 7 = very comfortable, nervous, embar- the mean-centered level of PUBSC (M = 3.76, SD = .54)
rassed, confident, calm), how they expected the researchers and question-order condition (control vs. observer perspec-
to react towards them as volunteers (1 = not at all, 7 = very tive). Analysis revealed a highly significant two-way inter-
positive, favorable, good; α = .97), and how they would react action effect of PUBSC and question-order condition (F(1,
towards other volunteers in the study (1 = not at all, 7 = very 102) = 4.23, p < .04). The two main effects were not signifi-
positive, favorable, good; α = .98). cant (ps > 0.25). Estimated means for volunteer intentions
The second version represented the observer’s perspec- are provided below using + 1SD and − 1SD to depict the
tive condition. Participants in this condition first indicated nature of any interactions that emerged. Figure 1 depicts
how they would react towards the volunteers if they were the these results using this criterion.
researchers. They then indicated (in order) their intentions The results of Study 2 support our hypotheses. In the con-
to volunteer, how they expected to feel during the interview, trol condition, volunteer intentions were significantly lower
and how they expected the researchers to react towards them
as volunteers. In sum, participants answered the four ques-
tions in a different order. 6
  The effect of including income as a covariate did not change the
At the end of the survey session, participants completed significant level of the two-way interaction: (F(1, 102) = 3.80,
Fenigstein et al.’s (1975) public self-consciousness scale (7 p < .05), or main effect (ps > 0.05).

13
Motivation and Emotion

among HPUBSC individuals (+ 1 SD) versus LPUBSC7 to demonstrate the cognitive toll of adopting an observer’s
individuals (− 1 SD) (MLPUBSC = 5.78 vs. MHPUBSC = 3.40; perspective. We expected load to interfere with taking the
t(102) = − 2.32, p < .03), consistent with H2a. According to perspective of an observer and to reverse the effect of an
H2b, HPUBSC (vs. not HPUBSC) individuals will respond observer’s perspective on embarrassment-avoidance.
differently to a cue to take the perspective of an observer.
According to our theory, because an observer’s perspective
can make HPUBSC individuals step out of themselves and Study 3
reduce personal distress, HPUBSC individuals should dis-
play less embarrassment-avoidance (i.e., higher volunteer Study 3 examines the mechanism of the effect of taking an
intentions) when taking an observer’s perspective (vs. con- observer’s perspective on countering embarrassment-avoid-
trol). Because individuals who are not high in PUBSC do ance. We introduced cognitive load to moderate the effect of
not easily feel embarrassed in the first place, a cue to take an observer’s perspective. We expect cognitive load to interfere
observer’s perspective may simply be ineffective at reducing the effect of observer’s perspective. We also collected open-
embarrassment-avoidance. Consistent with H2b, an analysis ended thinking protocols on self-related thoughts.
across question–order conditions showed that the observer
perspective-taking prime increased HPUBSC participants’ Participants and procedure
intentions to volunteer (MHPUBSC,observer’s perspective = 5.15
vs. MHPUBSC,control = 3.40; t(102) = 1.97, p = .05), making Study 3 participants were two hundred and twenty under-
HPBUSC participants less embarrassment-avoidant. When graduate students at a large public West Coast university
participants took an observer’s perspective, those with (Mage = 20.0; 75.7% female). All participants received a
higher levels of PUBSC (+ 1 SD) displayed the same level survey packet with a cover page instructing them to pro-
of embarrassment-avoidance as those with lower levels of vide feedback on an ad for a real product targeted towards
PUBSC (− 1 SD) (MHPUBSC = 5.15 vs. MLPUBSC = 4.59; college-aged people like themselves. We created two ads
t(102) = .94, p = .35). However, taking the perspective for a real-world flatulence prevention brand (i.e., Beano),
of an observer did not affect LPUBSC participants’ vol- with a photo used by Lau-Gesk and Drolet (2008, Study 2)
unteer intentions (M LPUBSC, observer’s perspective  =  4.59 vs. that depicted four college-age individuals at a party sharing
MLPUBSC,control = 5.78; t(102) = 1.57, p = .12).8 a couch. A male is sitting alone, slouching at one end of a
sofa. The side of his head is resting on his hand. His head
Discussion is turned slightly down. At the other end of the sofa, three
females are sitting together. One of the females is looking
The results of Study 2 support our theorizing. Taking the nervously sidelong at the male. The other two females are
perspective of an observer appears to be a helpful strategy to in animated conversation. The consumers in this photo dis-
combat undue embarrassment-avoidance among HPUBSC played nonverbal behaviors that signaled embarrassment
individuals but not among individuals who do not embarrass (Keltner and Buswell 1997). Participants in the control con-
easily (i.e. individuals not high in PUBSC). dition read the following ad copy:
Study 3 builds on the results of studies 1–2 in two impor-
Rip. Accidentally passing gas in front of a crush is one
tant ways. First, Study 3 tests our theorizing in another
of the most embarrassing experiences. Guaranteed to
important consumer context, advertising, which generally
linger forever.
follows different social rules compared to volunteering.
Second, in order to examine our proposed process in more Participants in the observer’s perspective-taking condition
detail, we collected process measures, such as the perceived read the same ad copy along with an additional sentence:
empathy participants expect from others and participants’
Others will know what it’s like. Put yourself in their
open-ended responses relating to an egocentric focus. Third,
shoes…would you giggle? Would you be horrified?
we tested cognitive load as a moderating variable in order
Would you stare?
To ensure that taking an observer’s perspective could suc-
7
 Again, we use the LPUBSC designation as a shortcut for saying cessfully make people think others would not judge them
low to medium PUBSC consumers—or not high PUBSC—consum- harshly, we conducted a pretest with 164 undergraduate stu-
ers. dents (Mage = 24.0; 52% female). Half of participants received
8
 The results change slightly if income is not included as a the control-ad copy and the other half of participants received
covariate (intentions: MHPUBSC,observer’s perspective  =  4.91 vs. the observer’s-perspective ad copy. All participants were asked
MHPUBSC,control  =  3.39; t(102) = 1.75, p = .08; intentions:
MLPUBSC,observer’s perspective  =  4.76 vs. MLPUBSC,control  =  6.08; to read the ad copy and answer two questions about the empa-
t(102) = 1.64, p = .11). thy they would expect from others if they were to pass gas in

13
Motivation and Emotion

public (1–7 scale, 1 = not agree at all; 7 = agree): “If I pass gas lower on this index than those exposed to the control ad
aloud, people would put themselves in my shoes”; “If I passed (Mobserver’s perspective = 3.83 vs. Mcontrol = 4.34, t(212) = 1.96,
gas aloud, people would empathize with me”. The PUBSC p = .05).
scale was embedded among questions at the end of the survey. Two independent coders assessed the elaborateness of
Regression analysis found a highly-significant two-way participants’ thoughts that served as a manipulation check
interaction between ad condition and PUBSC (F(1,160) = 3.98, for the influence of load on thinking processes. Decision
p < .05). Consistent with past research, HPUBSC participants makers who are under load rely relatively more on auto-
(+ 1 SD) in the control condition expected less empathy from matic processes and locally-provided information rather
others (β = − .46, t(160) = − 2.30, p < .02). Taking an observ- than stored information (Ward and Mann 2000). Coders
er’s perspective caused HPUBSC participants to expect the were instructed to rate the thoughts along two items using a
same level of empathy as participants not high in PUBSC 7-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very) that measured: (1) the
did (-1 SD) (β = .27, t(160) = .87, p < .39). The cue to take an degree to which participants provided thoughts elaborating
observer’s perspective led HPUBSC participants to expect beyond simply describing the ad based on the picture or copy
more empathy, compared to HPUBSC participants in the con- it used; and (2) the degree to which participants provided
trol condition (mobserver’s perspective = 3.79 vs. Mcontrol = 3.14, thoughts indicative of gut reactions to the ad. The latter item
t(160) = − 2.51, p = .01). However, the cue did not affect the was reverse-scored to create an elaboration index (r = .86).
amount of empathy participants not high in PUBSC expected Initial inter-rater reliability was high (r = .85). Inconsisten-
(Mobserver’s perspective = 3.59 vs. Mcontrol = 3.48, t(160) = − .41, cies between the coders were discussed until agreement was
p = .48). Therefore, the pretest results suggest taking an reached. Analysis found only a main effect of load consistent
observer’s perspective could successfully make HPUBSC with expectation: load participants tended to elaborate less
participants think others have more empathy, providing a (Mload = 2.35 vs. Mno load = 3.42, t(212) = 4.47, p < .0001),
foundation for Study 3. indicating the manipulation of load was successful.
In the main study (Study 3), approximately half of the
participants in each ad condition received a cognitive load
manipulation before being asked to provide feedback on Purchase intentions
the print ad they were shown (Ward and Mann 2000). Par-
ticipants in the load (vs. no load) condition were asked to Study 3 measures the same dependent variable tested in
study 20 words for 2 min and keep them in mind when they Study 1: purchase intentions for embarrassment-preven-
answered questions regarding the ad. At the end of the study, tion products. Purchase intentions for products aimed
they were asked to write down as many of the 20 words they at preventing embarrassment correspond to increased
could remember. All participants were exposed to one of the embarrassment-avoidance.
two ads described above and then answered two questions We analyzed the effects of mean-centered PUBSC
that assessed purchase intentions. Participants then provided (M = 3.80, SD = .58), ad condition, and load on partici-
open-ended responses; they were instructed to write down pants’ purchase intentions. Regression analysis revealed
any thoughts or feelings that occurred to them while read- the predicted three-way interaction among PUBSC, ad
ing the ad. This task was followed by a manipulation check type, and load on the purchase intention index (r = .75, F(1,
question assessing whether participants viewed the ad as 212) = 11.11, p < .001) and a marginally-significant two-
relatively self-related versus other-related, the feelings they way interaction between PUBSC and ad condition (F(1,
experienced while viewing the ad (PANAS scale; Watson 212) = 3.62, p < .06). The two-way interaction emerged
et al. 1988), and potential covariates including their own ten- between PUBSC and load for the control condition (F(1,
dency to pass gas in public and the feelings that arise from 212) = − 2.67, p < .008) and the observer’s perspective con-
such situations. Last, participants completed the PUBSC dition (F(1, 212) = 2.19, p < .03).
scale and provided demographic information. Our hypotheses were again confirmed. In the no load
control condition, purchase intentions for the gas preven-
Results and discussion tion product were significantly higher among HPUBSC
participants (+ 1 SD) participants than among LPUBSC9
Manipulation checks participants (− 1 SD) (MLPUBSC = 2.56 vs. MHPUBSC = 4.45;
t(212) = 2.40, p < .015). This suggests that HPUBSC
The manipulation check analysis yielded a significant
main effect of ad type on the actor index (“When I read
the ad, I imagined that I was the person who accidentally 9
 Again, we use the LPUBSC designation as a shortcut for saying
passed gas”; 1 = disagree, 7 = agree). Participants exposed low to medium PUBSC consumers—or not high PUBSC—consum-
to the ad encouraging them to become an observer scored ers.

13
Motivation and Emotion

7 load condition (MHPUBSC,observer’s perspective,load  =  3.50 vs.


M HUPBSC,observer’s perspective,no load  =  2.01 t(212) = − 1.89,
PURCHASE INTENTIONS
6
p < .06).
5 4.45 We also found that load reversed the harmful effects of
4 3.55
taking the perspective of an observer among participants
NO LOAD
2.93 with lower levels of PUBSC. Among these individuals,
3 2.56 LOAD purchase intentions were lower in the load condition than
2 in the no load condition (MLPUBSC,observer’s perspective = 2.96
1 vs. MLUPBSC,observer’s perspective,no load = 4.36, t(212) = 2.04,
LOW PUBSC HIGH PUBSC p < .04). In fact, purchase intentions among both
CONTROL HPUBSC and LPUBSC participants in the load,
observer’s perspective condition did not diff in their
intentions in the no load, control (non-intervention)
7
condition (M LPUBSC,observer’s perspective,load   =  2.96 vs.
PURCHASE INTENTIONS

6 ­M LPUBSC,control,no load   =  2.56; t(212) = .64, p < .52, and


5 MHPUBSC,observer’s perspective,load = 3.50 vs. MHPUBSC,control,no load
4.35
= 4.45; t(212) = − 1.30, p < .19). These results for HPUBSC
4 3.50
2.96
NO LOAD
participants under load are consistent with our hypotheses.
3 We found lower purchase intentions among HPUBSC
LOAD
2.01
2 participants in the load, control condition vs. the no
1
load, control condition (MHPUBSC,control,no load  =  4.45 vs.
LOW PUBSC HIGH PUBSC MHPUBSC,control,load  =  2.93; t(212) = 2.32, p < .02). Load
OBSERVER PERSPECTIVE-TAKING appears to have blocked cognitively-demanding processes,
namely the self-monitoring HPUBSC people usually engage
in to compare their behavior vis-a-vis social standards. Con-
Fig. 2  Mean purchase intentions by ad type by cognitive load by
PUBSC (Study 3)
sequently, HPUBSC participants in the control condition
exhibited less embarrassment-avoidance under load.

individuals do indeed exhibit more embarrassment-avoid- Thoughts


ance, supporting H2a and replicating Study 2’s findings.
Further, results for the no load, observer’s perspec- To illuminate the process that more self-focus and personal
tive condition were consistent with H2b. Under no distress lead to more embarrassment-avoidance among
load, taking the perspective of an observer was effec- HPUBSC participants in the control condition, and that an
tive at reducing HPUBSC participants’ purchase observer’s perspective reduces self-focus and personal dis-
intentions (M HPUBSC,observer’s perspective,no load  =  2.01 vs. tress, two independent coders classified participants’ open-
M HPUBSC,control,no load  =  4.45; t(212) = 1.08, p < .006). ended thoughts into three categories (inter-rater reliabil-
When HPUBSC participants had sufficient cognitive ity = .85): (1) a “self-related” thinking score, i.e. the extent
resources to perspective-take, they displayed less embar- to which participants related the embarrassing situation to
rassment-avoidance. See Fig. 2. Because individuals who self (vs. others), given the embarrassing situation depicted
are not high in PUBSC do not easily feel embarrassed, in the ad (1 = other-related/not related to self; 7 = highly
we expected that the cue to take an observer’s perspec- related to self); (2) other ad-related thoughts; and (3) irrel-
tive would have no effect on their purchase intentions. evant thoughts. No significant differences emerged for the
Unexpectedly, however, we found taking an observer’s latter two categories of thoughts (ps > 0.30).
perspective increased lower PUBSC participants’ pur- Most pertinent to our theorizing regarding process are the
chase intentions (MLPUBSC,perspective-taking no load = 4.36 vs. results for the first category. We found a significant three-
MLPUBSC,control,no load = 2.56; t(212) = − 2.44, p < .002), i.e., way interaction among PUBSC, ad condition, and load on
increased embarrassment-avoidance. We will discuss this participants’ self-related thoughts (F(1, 212) = 6.89, p < .01).
finding in detail in the “General discussion”. There was also a significant main effect of ad condition
In addition, consistent with H3, load reversed the help- (F(1,212) = 7.79, p < .006). Further analysis revealed a sig-
ful effects of taking the perspective among HPUBSC nificant two-way interaction between PUBSC and load in the
participants. Among HPUBSC individuals in the observ- control condition (F(1,212) = − 2.90, p = .004) but not in the
er’s perspective condition, purchase intentions were observer’s perspective condition (F(1,212) = 1.11, p = .27).
marginally-higher in the load condition than in the no In the no load, control condition, self-related thoughts

13
Motivation and Emotion

7 resources are available to take an observer’s perspective.


The ability to adopt the perspective of an observer is essen-
SELF-RELATED THINKING
6
tial. Under load, participants generated fewer self-related
5 thoughts, but embarrassment-avoidance is unaffected by that
4.01
4 emotion-as-information.
3.21 NO LOAD
3
Consistent with the view that load disrupts cogni-
LOAD
2.15 tive processes associated with HPUBSC, load decreased
2
1.31 self-thoughts relative to HPUBSC participants in the no
1 load, control condition (MHPUBSC,control,no load  =  4.01 vs.
LOW PUBSC HIGH PUBSC
MHPUBSC,control,load = 2.15; t(212) = 2.44, p < .02). In line with
CONTROL
the unexpected finding of greater embarrassment-avoidance
among LPUBSC individuals in the load, control condition,
7 LPUBSC individuals exhibited more self-related thoughts
SELF-RELATED THINKING

6 (MLPUBSC,control,no load = 1.31 vs. MLPUBSC,control,load = 3.21;


5
t(212) = 2.34, p = .02).
In summary, the results of Study 3 support our theoriz-
4 NO LOAD ing about how taking the perspective of an observer reduces
2.84
3
LOAD embarrassment-avoidance among HPUBSC individuals and
2 1.36 1.44
1.88
how cognitive load moderates these effects; for these indi-
1
viduals, taking an observer’s perspective reduces personal
LOW PUBSC HIGH PUBSC distress in embarrassing situations. Study 3 provides evi-
OBSERVER PERSPECTIVE-TAKING dence for the proposed process and its consequences.

Fig. 3  Mean self-related thoughts by ad type by cognitive load by


PUBSC (Study 3) General discussion

This research adds to an accumulating body of work on


were significantly higher among HPUBSC (+ 1 SD) versus embarrassment and embarrassment-avoidance. Building
LPUBSC (− 1 SD) participants (MHPUBSC,noload = 4.01 vs. on past research demonstrating that, in comparison to indi-
MLPUBSC,noload = 1.31; t(212) = 2.09, p < .041). This finding viduals with medium or low levels of PUBSC, HPUBSC
is consistent with the notion that HPUBSC participants are individuals expect more embarrassment and exhibit more
more likely to envision oneself in the embarrassing situa- embarrassment-avoidant behavior. As a point of departure
tion. See Fig. 3. (Lau-Gesk and Drolet 2008), the present research offers the
Consistent with our theorizing, when HPUBSC partici- first comprehensive examination of the relationship between
pants adopted the observer’s perspective and had sufficient PUBSC and embarrassment-avoidance, and offers an imple-
cognitive resources, taking an observer’s perspective reduced mental intervention to counter embarrassment-avoidance. As
self-related thinking (MObserver’s perspective,no load = 1.44 vs. Study 1 demonstrates, HPUBSC individuals tend to imag-
Mcontrol,no load = 4.01; t(212) = 3.15, p < .002). Taking an ine themselves as an actor in an embarrassing situation and
observer’s perspective caused no load LPUBSC partici- experience more distress. We demonstrated through media-
pants to engage in directionally more, albeit not statisti- tion analysis that personal distress underlies HPUBSC indi-
cally-significantly more self-related thoughts than they did viduals’ exaggerated embarrassment-avoidance tendency.
in the control condition (Mobserver’s perspective,no load = 2.84 vs. Further, as shown in studies 2 and 3, HPUBSC individuals
Mcontrol,no load = 1.31, t(212) = − 1.59, p < .11). This finding are more likely to exhibit embarrassment-avoidance. The
is consistent with the view that individuals who are not high present research also introduces an intervention to counter
in PUBSC typically do not put themselves in the picture and embarrassment-avoidance among consumers. Studies 2 and
thus would usually generate fewer self-related thoughts. 3 demonstrate the power of taking the perspective of an
Across observer’s perspective conditions, there were observer to an embarrassing situation on the incidence of
no effects of load or PUBSC on self-related thoughts. In self-related thoughts, and offer a way to counteract embar-
comparison to findings with respect to purchase intentions, rassment-avoidance among HPUBSC individuals.
these findings suggest that, while the content of thoughts The present research extends past research on the effects
can be changed by priming instructions, differences in of vicarious embarrassment. Specifically, the literature on
purchase intentions arise only when ample cognitive vicarious (i.e. empathetic) embarrassment indicates that

13
Motivation and Emotion

observers of an embarrassed actor often experience that his/her social identity is not threatened) and is able to real-
actor’s emotional distress. They empathize with the actor, ize others will not judge him negatively. Future research
and feeling embarrassed they will turn away (Edelmann and may want to look into, under what circumstances taking an
McCusker 1986; Miller 1987). This literature implies that observer’s perspective can lead to greater embarrassment
if an actor adopts the perspective of an observer, then the avoidance instead.
actor will still continue to experience embarrassment. How- Future research might consider other ways in which indi-
ever, past research suggests that this vicarious or empathetic viduals’ self-related thoughts can be redirected to dampen
embarrassment is qualitatively different from the embarrass- HPUBSC individuals’ expectations of overly-harsh observer
ment actors experience. For example, Miller (1987) intro- evaluations. For example, results for the load, control (no
duced an important distinction when he defined empathetic intervention) condition imply that load thwarts HPUBSC
embarrassment as embarrassment that is felt for another even individuals’ ability to engage in their usual self-monitoring
though one’s own social identity has not been threatened. (i.e., fewer self-related thoughts; Study 3) which in turn
And, Layton (2016) argues that not only must the observer causes empathy-neglect and embarrassment-avoidance.
experience an emotion that closely matches the actor’s Furthermore, future research may want to examine
(i.e., embarrassment) in order to qualify as an empathetic the group of individuals who are low, versus not-high or
response but also the observer must realize this emotion is medium PUBSC individuals, in greater detail. We hypoth-
unrelated to his own circumstances. This literature relates to esized that taking an observer’s perspective would have no
our own theorizing about the lack of personal distress one effect on what we termed ‘LPUBSC’ individuals because
feels when one adopts the perspective of an observer versus these individuals are less easily embarrassed in the first
actor. Observers may feel embarrassed but are unlikely to place. However, we found that taking an observer’s perspec-
think that their own social standing is at stake. This is a key tive can increase embarrassment-avoidance among LPUBSC
difference between actors and observers and is related to (vs. HPUBSC) individuals (directional in Study 2 and sig-
our hypothesis regarding how observers would evaluate the nificant in Study 3). It may be that lower PUBSC individuals
actor. Our research proposition is not whether observers will tend not to perceive themselves as being in the spotlight, but
experience vicarious embarrassment. Rather, we consider that taking an observer’s perspective switches the spotlight
how observers will evaluate an actor regardless of whether on and grows embarrassment-avoidance. This is consistent
they feel embarrassment on behalf of the actor. with Froming et al.’s (1990) finding LPUBSC people behave
It is not entirely clear what a vicarious embarrassment similarly to HPUBSC people when under a spotlight.
account would predict with respect to observers’ evalua- For lower PUBSC participants in the control conditions,
tions of embarrassed actors. Past research demonstrates that the load manipulation appears to have increased self-related
actors, as opposed to observers, focus on their own behav- thoughts and embarrassment-avoidance. Although a firmer
ior and tend to assume the worst in terms of interpersonal explanation for this unexpected finding awaits further
evaluations. Regardless of vicariously experienced embar- empirical experiment, lower PUBSC participants in Study
rassment, observers’ judgments or actors are consistently 3 responded as though the load manipulation instructions
kinder than actors generally expect, perhaps because observ- caused them to focus more on social actions than they would
ers who are not personally threatened they be sympathetic. have otherwise. Alternatively, it may be that lower PUBSC
As Miller (1987) shows, empathetic embarrassment is more participants tend to engage in self-protective thinking that
likely if observers are susceptible to embarrassment them- is blocked by working-memory load. The underlying cogni-
selves. Such observers tend to view embarrassed actors more tive processes for individuals who are not high in PUBSC
kindly. If an actor is able to view the situation as an observer have not been studied previously. Rather, researchers have
would, the kinder evaluations of the observer may become focused on HPUBSC individuals as a clinical population
salient; actors will then realize that observers are generally and not individuals with low or medium levels of PUBSC.
kinder than they expect. Thus, our main hypothesis is that Importantly, load’s effects in Study 3 on self-related
encouraging an actor to take the viewpoint of the observer thoughts and purchase intentions demonstrate that tak-
will counteract actor’s tendency to anticipate harsher evalu- ing an observer’s perspective is an effortful endeavor. In
ations. In summary, it is important to note that ‘taking the this way, the present research adds to the growing body
perspective of an observer’ is a fundamentally different tack of research that shows how cognitive load can exacerbate
than ‘trying to predict how observers will evaluate actors’ individuals’ reliance on information stored in memory and
since actors generally do not predict observers’ evaluations their chronic tendencies. Under load, taking the perspec-
of them very well. Instead, actors need to consider the expe- tive of the observer lead to fewer self-related thoughts, in
rience of an observer (vs. their own). Our data is consistent particular fewer thoughts relating the embarrassing situ-
with the hypothesis that when the actor takes an observer’s ation to themselves. Future research might benefit from
perspective, he/she distances himself from the situation (e.g., explicitly adopting a dual processing view and exploring

13
Motivation and Emotion

the automatic vs. effortful tendencies associated with that will successfully universally reduce embarrassment-
PUBSC levels in detail (Evans 2008). avoidance is more complicated than devising strategies to
Our results have significant implications for marketers increase embarrassment-avoidance since consumers will
given the frequency with which embarrassment-avoidance react differently to persuasion tactics depending on their
forms the basis for attempts to motivate consumers to buy level of PUBSC and the amount of available cognitive
a wide variety of products from laundry detergents (“ring resources.
around the collar”), to dishwashing liquid (unsightly spots
on dishes), and even to cars (avoiding the embarrassment Acknowledgements  The authors would like to thank Loraine Lau-
Gesk for contributions to an earlier version of this research.
of an unfavorable evaluation by the neighbors). Beyond
this, however, our research is relevant to those situations
in which marketers want to inoculate consumers against a
fear of embarrassment and encourage them to take actions
Methodological details appendix:
they might otherwise avoid. These situations frequently
Countering embarrassment‑avoidance
occur. Examples include getting an embarrassing but
among consumers
potentially life-saving medical test, asking a technician
See Fig. 4.
“dumb” questions that will increase customer satisfaction
with a purchase, or adopting an innovative and socially-
visible but potentially-risky product that might invite pub-
lic ridicule. Our research shows that devising strategies

Fig. 4  Ad—Study 1

13
Motivation and Emotion

References Helweg-Larsen, M., & Collins, B. E. (1994). The UCLA multidimen-


sional condom attitudes scale: Documenting the complex deter-
minants of condom use in college students. Health Psychology,
Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression:
13(3), 224.
Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form
Ayduk, Ö, & Kross, E. (2008). Enhancing the pace of recovery: Self-
and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122(3), 250.
distanced analysis of negative experiences reduces blood pressure
Kross, E. (2009). When the self becomes other. Annals New York Acad-
reactivity. Psychological Science, 19(3), 229–231.
emy of Sciences, 1167(1), 35–40.
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness,
Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2008). Facilitating adaptive emotional analy-
efficiency, intention, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer
sis: Distinguishing distanced-analysis of depressive experiences
Jr. & T. K. Srull (Ed.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd edn.,
from immersed-analysis and distraction. Personality and Social
pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology Bulletin, 34(7), 924–938.
Bell, J. (2009). Why embarrassment inhibits the acquisition and use of
Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does
condoms: A qualitative approach to understanding risky sexual
not hurt distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of
behaviour. Journal of Adolescence, 32(2), 379–391.
negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16(9), 709–715.
Brackett, K. P. (2004). College students’ condom purchase strategies.
Kruger, J., & Gilovich, T. (1999). “Naive cynicism” in everyday theo-
The Social Science Journal, 41(3), 459–464.
ries of responsibility assessment: On biased assumptions of bias.
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual dif-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 743.
ferences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in
Lau-Gesk, L., & Drolet, A. (2008). The publicly self-consciousness
Psychology, 10, 85.
consumer: Prepared to be embarrassed. Journal of Consumer Psy-
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy:
chology, 18(2), 127–136.
Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality
Layton, J. (2016). The odd empathy of vicarious embarrassment. https​
and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126.
://scien​ce.howst​uffwo​rks.com. Accessed 23 Feb 2016.
Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. (1996). Effect of
Miller, L. C., & Cox, C. L. (1982). For appearances’ sake: Public self-
perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A
consciousness and makeup use. Personality and Social Psychol-
merging of self and other. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ogy Bulletin, 8(4), 748–751.
chology, 70(4), 713.
Miller, R. S. (1987). Empathic embarrassment: Situational and per-
Drolet, A., & Luce, M. F. (2004). The rationalizing effects of cognitive
sonal determinants of reactions to the embarrassment of another.
load on emotion-based trade-off avoidance. Journal of Consumer
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1061–1069.
Research, 31(1), 63–77.
Miller, R. S. (2007). Is embarrassment a blessing or a curse. In: J. L.
Drolet, A., Luce, M. F., & Simonson, I. (2009). When does choice
Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious
reveal preference? Moderators of heuristic versus goal-based
emotions: Theory and research (pp. 245–262).
choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 137–147.
Miller, R. S., & Leary, M. R. (1992). Social sources and interactive
Edelmann, R. J. (1985). Individual differences in embarrassment: Self-
functions of emotion: The case of embarrassment. In M. S. Clark
consciousness, self-monitoring and embarrassibility. Personality
(Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology, Vol. 14. Emo-
and Individual Differences, 6(2), 223–230.
tion and social behavior (pp. 202–221). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Edelmann, R. J. (1987). The psychology of embarrassment. New York:
Sage Publications Inc.
Wiley.
Miller, R. S., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). Differentiating embarrassment
Edelmann, R. J., & McCusker, G. (1986). Introversion, neuroticism,
and shame. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13(3),
empathy and embarrassibility. Personality and Individual Differ-
273–287.
ences, 7(2), 133–140.
Modigliani, A. (1968). Embarrassment and embarrassability. Sociom-
Epley, N. (2014). Mindwise: Why we misunderstand what others think,
etry, 31, 313–326.
believe, feel, and want. New York: Vintage.
Modigliani, A. (1971). Embarrassment, facework, and eye contact:
Epley, N., Savitsky, K., & Gilovich, T. (2002). Empathy neglect: Rec-
Testing a theory of embarrassment. Journal of Personality and
onciling the spotlight effect and the correspondence bias. Journal
Social Psychology, 17(1), 15–24.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 300.
Moore, S. G., Dahl, D. W., Gorn, G. J., & Weinberg, C. B. (2006). Cop-
Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judg-
ing with condom embarrassment. Psychology, Health & Medicine,
ment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59,
11(1), 70–79.
255–278.
Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—
Fenigstein, A. (1984). Self-consciousness and the overperception of
what others know: Imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psy-
self as a target. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
chological Bulletin, 125(6), 737.
47(4), 860.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private
on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting
chology, 100(4), 569.
and Clinical Psychology, 43(4), 522.
Parrott, W. G., & Smith, S. F. (1991). Embarrassment: Actual vs. typi-
Foss, R. D., & Crenshaw, N. C. (1978). Risk of embarrassment and
cal cases, classical vs. prototypical representations. Cognition &
helping. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Jour-
Emotion, 5(5–6), 467–488.
nal, 6(2), 243–245.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling
Froming, W. J., Corley, E. B., & Rinker, L. (1990). The influence of
strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple
public self-consciousness and the audience’s characteristics on
mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–891.
withdrawal from embarrassing situations. Journal of Personality,
Savitsky, K., Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Do others judge us as
58(4), 603–622.
harshly as we think? Overestimating the impact of our failures,
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (2000). The spotlight effect
shortcomings, and mishaps. Journal of Personality and Social
in social judgment: An egocentric bias in estimates of the salience
Psychology, 81(1), 44.
of one’s own actions and appearance. Journal of Personality and
Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management. Grove, CA: Brooks/
Social Psychology, 78(2), 211.
Cole Publishing Company.

13
Motivation and Emotion

Singelis, T. M., & Sharkey, W. F. (1995). Culture, self-construal, and Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don’t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating
embarrassability. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 26(6), under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
622–644. ogy, 78(4), 753.
Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Thinking about Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
answers: The application of cognitive processes to survey meth- validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
odology. New York: Jossey-Bass. PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Von Gemmingen, M. J., Sullivan, B. F., & Pomerantz, A. M. (2003). 54(6), 1063.
Investigating the relationships between boredom proneness, para- Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and
noia, and self-consciousness. Personality and Individual Differ- Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of
ences, 34(6), 907–919. Consumer Research, 37(2), 197–206.

13

You might also like