Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation

journal homepage: www.archives-pmr.org


Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2015;96:1935-44

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Add-on Effects of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic


Stimulation on Subacute Aphasia Therapy: Enhanced
Improvement of Functional Communication and Basic
Linguistic Skills. A Randomized Controlled Study
Ilona Rubi-Fessen, MSc,a Alexander Hartmann, MD,a Walter Huber, PhD,b
Bruno Fimm, PhD,b Thomas Rommel, MD,a Alexander Thiel, MD,c Wolf-Dieter Heiss, MDd
From the aRehaNova Rehabilitation Hospital, Cologne, Germany; bDepartment of Neurology, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany; cDepartment
of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; and dMax Planck Institute for Metabolism Research, Cologne, Germany.

Abstract
Objective: To determine to what extent repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) combined with speech and language therapy
improves functional communication and basic linguistic skills of individuals with subacute aphasia.
Design: Randomized, blinded, and sham-controlled study.
Setting: Neurologic rehabilitation hospital.
Participants: Participants (NZ30) with subacute aphasia after stroke.
Interventions: During a 2-week treatment period, half of the participants received 10 sessions of 20-minute inhibitory 1-Hz rTMS over the right
inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 45), and the other half received sham stimulation. Directly thereafter, all the participants underwent 45
minutes of speech and language therapy.
Main Outcome Measures: Aachen Aphasia Test, Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT), a naming screening, and subscales of
the FIM, all assessed the day before and the day after treatment period.
Results: The participants who received real rTMS significantly improved with respect to all 10 measures of basic linguistic skills and functional
communication, whereas sham-treated participants significantly improved in only 6 of 10 measures (paired t tests, P<.05). There was a significant
difference in the gains made by the 2 groups on 5 of 10 measures including functional communication (ANELT) (repeated-measures analysis of
variance, P.05).
Conclusions: For the first time, this study has demonstrated that basic linguistic skills as well as functional communication are bolstered by
combining rTMS and behavioral language therapy in patients with subacute aphasia.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2015;96:1935-44
ª 2015 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine

Aphasia is a common neurologic sequela of stroke that restricts inhibitory rTMS is applied to the right hemisphere in order to
communicative abilities and quality of life. Recently, noninvasive increase the language activity of the remaining left hemisphere
brain stimulation techniques such as repetitive transcranial mag- structures by suppressing competing right hemisphere language
netic stimulation (rTMS)dinducing changes in brain activity by activation.1,2,10-15 Although the discussion about processes un-
modulating cortical excitabilitydhave been used to optimize the derlying successful aphasia recovery is still ongoing,3 functional
outcome of behavioral speech and language therapy (SLT).1-9 reintegration of preserved language-related areas of the left hemi-
rTMS has been shown to improve language behavior in persons sphere seems to provide an effective mechanism of language re-
with aphasia after stroke.1,2,10-16 In most of these studies, covery17-20 in many, but not in all cases.3,21,22 Up to now, few
studies have investigated the influence of rTMS on the reorgani-
Supported in part by the Walter and Marga Boll Foundation and the Wolf-Dieter Heiss-
Foundation.
zation of language functions in the subacute stage of aphasia.23-29
Disclosures: none. Recent studies have combined rTMS with behavioral language

0003-9993/15/$36 - see front matter ª 2015 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2015.06.017
1936 I. Rubi-Fessen et al

therapy. Abo et al16 reported on the favorable effects of an after enrollment. Finally, 30 participants (nZ15 in each group)
inhibitory rTMS protocol and SLT in patients with chronic took part in the study. A participants’ flow diagram is shown
aphasia. These findings have been supported by other studies,26-28 in figure 1.
where a positive effect on naming and overall linguistic perfor-
mance was found by combining SLT and rTMS in patients with
subacute aphasia. In another study, Khedr et al29 observed Language data
enhanced language recovery of participants who received dual-
The day before and the day after the treatment period, each
hemisphere stimulation and SLT in comparison to sham-treated
participant was administered various language tests by a certified
participants with aphasia up to 12 weeks poststroke. By
speech and language therapist who was blinded to group alloca-
contrast, Seniów23 and Waldowski24 and colleagues found only
tion and not involved in behavioral therapy. One of the tests was
moderate benefits by combining rTMS with SLT in sub-
the AAT, which includes an evaluation of spontaneous language
acute aphasia.
production and 5 subtests: Token Test, repetition, written lan-
The strongest and most consistent effects of rTMS on language
guage, and naming, as well as auditory and written comprehen-
function were reported for picture naming.1,2,12-14 Even though the
sion. Severity of aphasia (profile height) was determined by
ultimate goal of language rehabilitation is to restore communi-
weighted T scores of all AAT subtests. In addition, a naming
cation in daily life, there is still limited knowledge about the in-
screening was administered, consisting of 60 items selected from
fluence of rTMS on functional communication.12-15 Thus, this
the Snodgrass and Vanderwart picture naming inventory.32 The
present study aims to determine how rTMS combined with SLT
Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT)33 was
might improve both basic linguistic skills and functional
chosen to provide information about functional dimensions of
communication in individuals with subacute aphasia. This study
linguistic behavior. The ANELT is a valid instrument for assessing
partially incorporates previously published data27 but has been
functional communication34 and consists of 10 familiar daily life
expanded by including additional participants. Furthermore, re-
scenarios to which the participant has to respond verbally. Leaving
sults are reported from the full range of behavioral testing,
aside the B-scale, which measures motor speech skills, only the
including an additional test of naming and standardized assess-
semantic A-scale was analyzed to measure the effectiveness of
ments of functional communication.
conveying a message.
Functional communication (including verbal and nonverbal
communication) was further assessed by an internationally
Methods approved proxy-rating scale, the FIM.35 Items related to
communicative behavior (comprehension and expression) were
Participants used. The rating was performed by clinical employees (nursing
staff) who were neither involved in the study nor informed about
A total of 151 participants with first-ever stroke within the terri- study selection and group assignment.
tory of the left middle cerebral artery were recruited between 2008
and 2013 while they were inpatients in the RehaNova Rehabili-
tation Hospital in Cologne, Germany. Further inclusion criteria Neuroimaging and rTMS sessions
were as follows: (1) subacute aphasia with testability for the
According to group allocation, half of the participants (real
Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT)30; (2) poststroke period up to 16
stimulation/rTMS group) received 20 minutes of 1-Hz rTMS over
weeks (but most were 4e6wk poststroke); (3) right handedness as
the right triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus (center of
measured by the Laterality Questionaire31; (4) German as the first
Brodmann area 45), whereas the other half of the participants
language; and (5) age between 55 and 85 years. Exclusion criteria
(sham group) received the same stimulation over the vertex. Both
were (1) prior symptomatic cerebrovascular accidents; (2)
groups were given an intensity of 90% of the individual resting
neurodegenerative or psychiatric disease; (3) epilepsy; and (4)
motor threshold. The resting motor threshold was defined as the
auditory or visual deficits that might impair testing.
minimum stimulator output that elicited a visible contraction on
Written consent was obtained from all subjects. The study
the first dorsal interosseus muscle of the unaffected hand in more
protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Cologne
than 5 of 10 stimulation trials. Stimulation parameters were in
University and the Federal Office for Radiation Protection
accordance with the guidelines suggested by Wassermann.36
(Salzgitter, Germany). Patients’ characteristics are shown in table 1.
Before stimulation, T1-weighted, diffusion-weighted, and T2
A total of 111 patients were excluded because they did not
fluid-attenuated inversion recovery MRI images were obtained to
meet the criteria. For 40 individuals, allocation to either real or
locate the optimal coil position. The respective brain areas were
sham stimulation was performed by a computer-generated allo-
stimulated using a Magstim Rapid2 stimulatora with a double 70-
cation sequence and concealed by means of consecutively
mm coil. The stimulation point was determined using reference
numbered sealed envelopes. Ten persons discontinued the study
lines defined on the reconstruction of the respective patient’s head
during initial magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or withdrew
from the MRIs, which were then transferred to the patient’s head
(for details, see Weiduschat et al37). This method has an accuracy
List of abbreviations: of 10mm when compared with neuronavigated methods.
AAT Aachen Aphasia Test
ANELT Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test
ANOVA analysis of variance
Behavioral language therapy
MRI magnetic resonance imaging
rTMS sessions were immediately followed by 45 minutes of SLT
SLT speech and language therapy
conducted by an experienced speech and language therapist
rTMS repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation
blinded to the participant’s group assignment. Above all, therapy

www.archives-pmr.org
Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on aphasia 1937

Table 1 Patient characteristics

Participant Stimulation Lesion Infarct


No. Group Sex Age (y) Duration (d) Aphasia* Severityy Fluency Locationz Volume (mm3)
1 Real F 59 49 Wernicke Moderate Fluent P 21,008
2 Real F 66 77 Anomic Mild Fluent Sc 10,376
3 Real F 59 43 Global Severe Nonfluent P 23,858
4 Real M 82 20 Wernicke Moderate Fluent Sc/A 44,481
5 Real F 73 60 Wernicke Severe Nonfluent Sc 13,910
6 Real F 70 17 Wernicke Severe Fluent P 16,060
7 Real M 75 56 Anomic Mild Fluent Sc 10,693
8 Real F 73 17 Broca Mild Nonfluent Sc 3,623
9 Real M 75 24 Wernicke Mild Fluent Sc 17,350
10 Real M 74 20 Wernicke Moderate Fluent P/A 76,302
11 Real F 58 31 Wernicke Moderate Fluent Sc 20,093
12 Real M 61 38 Global Moderate Nonfluent Sc/P 18,914
13 Real F 73 27 Anomic Mild Fluent P 26,823
14 Real F 55 45 Wernicke Severe Fluent P 26,909
15 Real F 66 88 Broca Moderate Nonfluent Sc/P 17,896
16 Sham M 60 46 Broca Mild Nonfluent P 30,904
17 Sham M 67 94 Broca Moderate Nonfluent Sc 16,415
18 Sham M 61 49 Global Severe Nonfluent Sc/A/P 90,829
19 Sham M 67 46 Wernicke Moderate Fluent A 38,976
20 Sham F 65 68 Broca Moderate Nonfluent Sc 3,652
21 Sham M 70 50 Global Severe Nonfluent Sc 11,060
22 Sham M 78 21 Anomic Mild Fluent A 21,929
23 Sham F 65 28 Wernicke Moderate Fluent Sc 6,260
24 Sham F 80 74 Wernicke Moderate Fluent A 13,521
25 Sham F 73 59 Wernicke Mild Fluent Sc/A 21,854
26 Sham F 83 20 Wernicke Mild Fluent P 16,202
27 Sham M 67 22 Anomic Moderate Fluent P 19,369
28 Sham F 73 37 Anomic Mild Fluent A 4,141
29 Sham M 66 70 Anomic Mild Fluent Sc/P 6,617
30 Sham M 69 47 Broca Moderate Nonfluent Sc MV
Abbreviations: A, anterior; F, female; M, male; MV, missing value; P, posterior; Sc, subcortical.
* Aphasia diagnosis according to AAT.
y
Severity according to AAT profile height.
z
A, P, and Sc part of left middle cerebral artery territory (see also supplemental fig S1, available online only at http://www.archives-pmr.org/).

focused on reactivation of word retrieval as required in tasks such and language therapists, who were specifically introduced to the
as oral and written picture naming, picture description, and principles above. The treatment of each participant was normally
writing from memory. To ensure as much consistency of SLT and performed by a single therapist. Training items were selected
treatment fidelity between subjects and across therapists as according to type and severity of the individual aphasic symptoms
possible, the treatment was conducted according to the following and extended after repeated correct naming performance.
principles: (1) main focus on oral naming; (2) preactivation of
word finding by receptive tasks (oral and written word-picture
matching); (3) application of increasing cues to support deliberate Data analysis
lexical retrieval as validated by Abel et al38 for the German lan-
guage; (4) consolidation of successful verbal naming by additional All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 20b).
written naming (providing increasing written cues: initial Independent-sample t tests were applied to identify group differ-
grapheme, initial syllable, word anagram, complete word); (5) ences at baseline, and paired t tests were used to assess therapy
variation of training stimuli progressing from single object to outcome for each group. To evaluate differences in language
related action pictures as provided by the Everyday Life Activities improvement between groups, behavioral scores pre- and post-
set39; and (6) exclusion of holistic or nonverbal facilitation treatment were analyzed using 2-way repeated-measures analysis
methods, or both, that might primarily involve right hemisphere of variance (ANOVA), with the factors group and time. The
functions.40,41 Kolmogorov-Smirnov test revealed normal distribution of age,
All therapy plans were developed by the same experienced duration of disease, and behavioral data. Effect sizes were deter-
speech and language therapist, who was blinded to group alloca- mined using partial h2 according to Cohen’s criteria.42 The level
tion and not involved in conducting the treatment. During the of statistical significance was set at aZ5% for all t tests
course of the study, treatment was provided by a total of 7 speech and ANOVAs.

www.archives-pmr.org
1938 I. Rubi-Fessen et al

Fig 1 Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials flow diagram. Abbreviation: rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Results influence therapy outcome, were sufficiently well balanced be-


tween the 2 groups (see table 1). Supplemental figure S1 (avail-
able online only at http://www.archives-pmr.org/) outlines the
Baseline characteristics method of defining lesion location.
At baseline, the mean performance of the sham group was
quantitatively slightly better regarding all language measures Changes in language and communication
except for the AAT subtest written language. However,
independent-sample t tests did not reveal any significant group Table 3 lists the mean values of outcome measures at baseline and
differences with respect to age (mean age  SD: rTMS, posttreatment, SDs, and gains across time. Individual results for
67.98.12y; sham, 69.66.67y), disease duration (mean time  all participants are listed in supplemental table S1 (available on-
SD poststroke: rTMS, 41.4721.51d; sham, 48.7321.57d), line only at http://www.archives-pmr.org/).
lesion size (mean volume  SD: rTMS, 23,21917,395mm3; Given the influence of behavioral therapy and spontaneous
sham, 21,55222,369mm3), and aphasia severity (mean  SD recovery, both groups improved their language and communica-
AAT profile height [T score]: rTMS, 50.527.15; sham, tive skills during the 2 weeks of therapy. There was a significant
51.607.71). Furthermore, for each language measure, Pearson main effect of time on all measures: AAT profile score
correlations between time postonset and language performance at (F1,28Z61.015, P<.000, partial s2Z.685), AAT Token Test
baseline were found to be insignificant, thus indicating no sig- (F1,28Z11.120, PZ.002, partial s2Z.284), AAT repetition
nificant association between lower baseline performance and (F1,28Z23.847, P<.001, partial s2Z.460), AAT written language
shorter duration of aphasia (table 2). Other characteristics such as (F1,28Z25.647, P<.001, partial s2Z.478), AAT naming
aphasia type (rTMS: anomic nZ3, Broca nZ2, Wernicke nZ8, (F1,28Z33.195, P<.001, partial s2Z.542), AAT comprehension
global aphasia nZ2; sham: anomic nZ4, Broca nZ4, Wernicke (F1,28Z21.404, P<.001, partial s2Z.433), naming screening
nZ5, global aphasia nZ2), sex (rTMS: female nZ10, male nZ5; (F1,28Z17.775, P<.001, partial s2Z.388), ANELT A-scale
sham: female nZ6, male nZ9), and lesion location (rTMS: (F1,28Z43.632, P<.001, partial s2Z.609), FIM comprehension
subcortical nZ6, posterior nZ5, mixed nZ4; sham: subcortical (F1,28Z7.993, PZ.009, partial s2Z.222), and FIM expression
nZ5, posterior nZ3, anterior nZ4, mixed nZ3), which might (F1,28Z9.164, PZ.005, partial s2Z.247).

www.archives-pmr.org
Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on aphasia 1939

Table 2 Comparison of sample characteristics and language baseline between groups


r (nZ30) Time
Sample Characteristics and Poststroke x
Language Tests Pre-Real Stimulation Pre-Sham Independent-Samples t Test* Language Baseline P*
Age (y) 67.98.12 69.606.67 t28Z.639, PZ.528
Time poststroke (d) 41.4721.51 48.7321.57 t28Z.924, PZ.363
Infarct volume (mm3) 23,21917,395 21,55222,369 t28Z.225, PZ.824
AAT profile heighty 50.527.15 51.607.71 t28Z.398, PZ.693 .500 .739
AAT subtest Token Testz 51.337.51 54.6711.46 t28Z.943, PZ.354 .238 .206
AAT subtest repetitionz 52.8010.35 53.139.18 t28Z.093, PZ.926 .038 .841
AAT subtest written languagez 50.537.01 48.877.17 t28Z.644, PZ.525 .061 .750
AAT subtest namingz 47.076.14 50.209.91 t28Z1.041, PZ.307 .024 .898
AAT subtest comprehensionz 48.535.30 51.0713.02 t28Z.698, PZ.494 .127 .504
Naming screeningx (0e60) 32.8718.43 39.2719.67 t28Z.920, PZ.366 .125 .509
ANELT A-scalex (10e50) 28.0011.48 29.6715.08 t28Z.340, PZ.736 .294 .115
FIM comprehensionx (1e7) 4.271.39 4.401.18 t28Z.283, PZ.779 .035 .853
FIM expressionx (1e7) 3.271.34 2.801.15 t28Z1.027, PZ.313 .202 .283
NOTE. Values are mean  SD or as otherwise indicated.
* Two-sided.
y
Weighted T scores.
z
T scores based on AAT norms for aphasia.
x
Raw score.

Although no significant main effect for group was found, significant interactions were found on AAT repetition and the
ANOVA statistical analyses demonstrated significant differences FIM ratings.
in the interactions between group and time, thereby indicating Figures 2 and 3 illustrate the interaction between group and
higher gains in the real stimulation group with respect to the time of the linguistic and communicative measures. The interac-
following measures: AAT profile score (F1,28Z8.91, PZ.006, tion effect for AAT profile height and AAT subtest naming
partial s2Z.239), AAT written language (F1,28Z4.84, PZ.036, demonstrated a medium effect size. For all other measures besides
partial s2Z.147), AAT naming (F1,28Z8.23, PZ.006, partial AAT subtest repetition and the FIM rating subscale expression, a
s2Z.240), AAT comprehension (F1,28Z4.66, PZ.040, partial small to medium effect size was found. Supplemental appendix S1
s2Z.143), and ANELT A-scale (F1,28Z4.19, PZ.050, partial (available online only at http://www.archives-pmr.org/) gives an
s2Z.130). Insignificant trends were found on AAT Token example for changes in functional communication.
Test (F1,28Z3.31, PZ.080, partial s2Z.106) and naming Both at baseline and posttherapy, no significant differences
screening (F1,28Z3.73, PZ.064, partial s2Z.117). No were found between the 2 groups (independent-sample t tests).

Table 3 Changes in language performance and functional communication


Real Stimulation nZ15 Sham nZ15
Paired t Paired t
Test (Pre Test (Pre
Language Test Pre- Post- Gain vs Post)* Pre Post Gain vs Post)*
y
AAT profile height 50.527.15 54.937.88 4.412.63 <.001 51.607.71 53.587.80 1.981.76 <.001
AAT Token Testz 51.337.51 54.739.26 3.403.48 .001 54.6711.46 55.6710.59 1.003.74 .159
AAT repetitionz 52.8010.35 55.739.09 2.933.81 .005 53.139.18 55.479.83 2.341.72 <.001
AAT written languagez 50.537.01 55.279.19 4.734.35 <.001 48.877.17 50.736.82 1.872.56 .007
AAT namingz 47.076.14 53.537.77 6.473.25 <.001 50.209.91 52.2710.89 2.074.73 .057
AAT comprehensionz 48.535.30 52.936.79 4.403.70 <.001 51.1312.98 52.6711.99 1.603.40 .045
Naming screening (0e60)x 32.8718.43 40.9318.59 8.078.67 .002 39.2719.67 42.2719.35 3.005.50 .023
ANELT A-scale (10e50)x 28.0011.48 34.2012.09 6.204.59 <.001 29.6715.08 32.9314.84 3.263.13 <.001
FIM comprehension (1e7)x 4.271.39 4.931.28 0.671.05 .013 4.401.18 4.601.40 0.200.56 <.094
FIM expression (1e7)x 3.271.34 3.801.21 0.530.83 .013 2.801.15 3.071.39 0.270.59 .052
NOTE. Values are mean  SD or as otherwise indicated.
* One-sided.
y
Weighted T scores.
z
T scores based on AAT norms for aphasia.
x
Raw score.

www.archives-pmr.org
1940 I. Rubi-Fessen et al

AAT Profile Height AAT Token Test AAT Repetition AAT Written Lang
60 60 60 60
58 58 58 58
56 56 56 56
54 54 54 54
Weighted T-scores

52 52 52 52

T-scores

T-scores
T-scores
50 50 50 50
48 48 48 48
46 46 46 46
44 44 44 44
42 42 42 42
40 40 40 40
Real Stimulation Sham Real Stimulation Sham Real Stimulation Sham Real Stimulation Sham

pre post p=.006 pre post p=.080 pre post p=.582 pre post p=.036

AAT Naming AAT Comprehension Naming Screening


60 60 50
58 58
56 56 45

54 54
40
52 Raw Score (0-60)
52
T-scores
T-scores

50 50 35
48 48
30
46 46
44 44
25
42 42
40 40 20
Real Stimulation Sham Real Stimulation Sham Real Stimulation Sham

pre post p=.006 p=.064


pre post p=.040 pre post

Fig 2 Language performance regarding pre- and posttreatment. The graphs show mean values at baseline and after a 2-week treatment for the
real stimulation group and sham group with respect to profile height of the AAT, AAT subtests, and naming screening. P values refer to group-by-
time interaction in 2-way repeated-measures ANOVAs. Error bars represent SEs of the mean. Abbreviations: Lang, language; post, posttreatment;
pre, pretreatment.

Fig 3 Communicative measures regarding pre- and posttreatment. Graphs show mean values at baseline and after a 2-week treatment for the
real stimulation group and sham group on A-scale of the ANELT, and subtests of the FIM. P values refer to group-by-time interaction in 2-way
repeated-measures ANOVAs. Error bars represent SEs of the mean. Abbreviations: post, posttreatment; pre, pretreatment.

www.archives-pmr.org
Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on aphasia 1941

Fig 4 Group differences regarding gains in language performance between pre- and posttreatment. The bracket symbol indicates a significant
difference between the gains of both groups; P values from ANOVA group-by-time interaction. Error bars represent SEs of the mean. Abbreviations:
COMP, comprehension; NAM, naming; REP, repetition; Stim, stimulation; TT, Token Test; WRIT, written language.

However, post hoc analyses within groups across baseline and study, rTMS stimulation was directed at the pars triangularis of the
posttherapy (paired t tests) revealed significant improvements for right inferior frontal gyrus with the aim of enhancing residual
all 10 measures for the real stimulation group (see table 3). In the language function of the damaged left hemisphere. The negative
sham group, significant improvements were found for 6 of 10 outcome of Seniów might have been caused by different methods
measures: AAT profile height, AAT repetition, AAT written lan- of placing the rTMS coil. As demonstrated previously, the surface
guage, AAT comprehension, naming screening, and ANELT. distance measurement method,37 which we used in the present
Furthermore, the gain between baseline and posttreatment per- study, is superior to the 10e20 method used by Seniów in local-
formance for almost all measures was higher (fig 4) in the real izing the inferior frontal gyrus. The importance of localizing the
stimulation group than in the sham group. target area within the inferior frontal gyrus has recently been
The differences in the gain between the 2 groups are plotted in outlined.13,43 The variable effect of rTMS on language functions
figure 4. Here, only the AAT subtests are given, because their T may also be attributed to differences in behavioral testing or
scores can be compared with the general performance of the treatment. In the present study, written language was explicitly
aphasia population. used during aphasia therapy. Given the high linguistic specificity
of reading and writing, this may have led to strong left hemisphere
enhancement, leading specifically to improvement of written
Discussion language functions.

The present sham-controlled study investigates the effect of


Improvements across different language functions
inhibitory rTMS combined with SLT on recovery from subacute
aphasia and provides detailed behavioral data about changes of and communication
basic linguistic skills and functional communication after a 10-day Although rTMS has been shown to enhance picture naming,1,2,12-14
treatment period. A relatively short treatment period of SLT the current study has found additional significant add-on effects
combined with rTMS led to significantly higher gains in basic with respect to the AAT subtests of auditory and written
linguistic skills (AAT profile score, AAT subtests Token Test, comprehension, reading and writing, and for functional
written language, naming, and comprehension) and in functional communication as measured by the ANELT. For the commu-
communication (ANELT) for the real stimulation group as nicative items of the FIM, no significant add-on effect was
opposed to the sham group. found even though the real stimulation group showed significant
enhancements on the FIM subscales of comprehension and
rTMS and language therapy in subacute aphasia expression. By contrast, the sham group showed only quanti-
tative yet insignificant improvements.
To date, only a few other group studies have combined rTMS and Apart from naming, the most significant effects were found for
SLT during the subacute stage of aphasia. Using a design similar written language processing, which might be related to content
to our study, Seniów23 found no significant add-on effect for a 3- and methods of behavioral therapy. The training focused on active
week inhibitory rTMS protocol combined with SLT in persons oral and written word-finding rather than on passive imitation of
with subacute aphasia with respect to naming, repetition, and verbal stimulation. These characteristics of the behavioral therapy
comprehension. In both the aforementioned study and the present might also be responsible for the lack of difference between the 2

www.archives-pmr.org
1942 I. Rubi-Fessen et al

groups with respect to repetition, which was not part of the par- incorporate larger and carefully matched samples to allow
ticipants’ therapy plans. detailed analyses of different subgroups and the application of
A transfer of linguistic skills into everyday communication consistent behavioral therapy tailored to the type and severity of
was not specifically trained during the 2-week period. Neverthe- aphasia. Finally, we do not have information about the stability of
less, communicative functions improved as well, which most the observed advantages, because participants were not followed
likely reflects a generalization effect. Generalization of semantic up beyond the immediate posttreatment period. However, studies
and phonologic treatment effects in poststroke aphasia on func- of chronic aphasia provide first evidence that language benefits
tional communication has been described by Doesborgh et al.44 observed after administration of rTMS may remain stable or even
They conclude that improvement in basic language functions increase after several month post-rTMS.1,10-13 Even so, the early
transfers to improvement in functional communication skills. With advantages observed in the real rTMS group suggest that this
respect to chronic aphasia, it was also shown that rTMS without approach coupled with SLT is a promising and encouraging
succeeding SLT may support the transfer of specific linguistic beginning tool of rehabilitation.
skills to communicative abilities.13-15 Medina et al15 demonstrated
improved functional communication after applying rTMS, and
they attributed these effects to enhanced lexical-semantic access. Conclusions
Although we observed the strongest add-on effect on naming, we The present study delivers further evidence that combining 1-Hz
did not find a correlation between any naming task and the applied rTMS with SLT leads to significant add-on treatment effects in the
communicative assessment, the ANELT A-scale. Apparently, subacute stage of aphasia. As indicated by the results, the outcome
communicative transfer cannot be limited merely to improved of behavioral therapy is enhanced not only for a variety of basic
lexical retrieval. linguistic skills but also for functional communication. Longitu-
The role of left Broca’s area in lexical retrieval and its rele- dinal studies are required to evaluate the long-term stability of
vance for picture naming have been investigated in numerous these benefits.
studies in subjects with and without aphasia.45,46 Furthermore,
imaging studies have reported activation of the left Broca’s area in
phonologic and syntactic language tasks, both in receptive and Suppliers
expressive modalities.47-49 Thus, the left Broca’s area and espe-
cially its more anterior parts (Brodmann area 45) seem to act like a. Magstim Rapid2 stimulator; The Magstim Company Ltd.
a bottleneck for a wide range of language functions, integrating b. SPSS version 20; IBM Corp.
lexical-semantic, phonologic, and syntactic information from the
ventral and dorsal stream of language processing.49-51 Therefore,
reintegrating left Broca’s area into language processing apparently Keywords
leads to improvements across linguistic tasks and modalities.27
Aphasia; Language therapy; Rehabilitation; Transcranial magnetic
This positive effect might be enhanced during the early period
stimulation
of aphasia, which is characterized by increased neuronal recruit-
ment in the inferior frontal gyrus of the undamaged right hemi-
sphere.17 Reducing this overactivation of the right hemisphere by Corresponding author
inhibitory rTMS within the first months poststroke might trigger
left hemisphere activation not only of Broca’s area but of the Ilona Rubi-Fessen, MSc, RehaNova Rehabilitation Hospital,
whole residual distributed neural network of language within the Ostmerheimerstr. 200, 51109 Cologne, Germany. E-mail
left hemisphere, resulting in the observed generalization of lin- address: rubi-fessen@rehanova.de.
guistic skills to functional communication.
Acknowledgments
Study limitations
We thank the speech and language therapists of the RehaNova
Although we could show the add-on effect of an inhibitory rTMS Rehabilitation Hospital, Cologne, Germany, for their assistance in
protocol on recovery from aphasia in a comparatively large cohort performing the speech and language therapy.
of patients with subacute aphasia, our study has limitations. The
study design did not allow differentiation between language
improvement resulting from behavioral therapy or from sponta- References
neous recovery. Implementing language-independent control tasks
in the assessment battery would support the differentiation be- 1. Naeser MA, Martin PI, Nicholas M, et al. Improved picture naming
tween language-specific training effects and overall spontaneous in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca’s area: an open-
recovery. However, most of our participants were included about protocol study. Brain Lang 2005;93:95-105.
4 to 6 weeks poststroke. Up to this point, most of the spontaneous 2. Barwood CH, Murdoch BE, Whelan BM, et al. The effects of low
recovery should have taken place, and further improvement might frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and
be more related to language therapy.52 Another limiting factor is sham condition rTMS on behavioral language in chronic non-fluent
aphasia: short term outcomes. NeuroRehabilitation 2011;28:113-28.
the heterogeneity of our sample with respect to lesion size, lesion
3. Hamilton RH, Chrysikou EG, Coslett B. Mechanisms of aphasia
location, and the type and severity of aphasia. These character- recovery after stroke and the role of noninvasive brain stimulation.
istics were relatively evenly distributed between our 2 experi- Brain Lang 2011;118:40-50.
mental groups; former studies, however, have shown that 4. Shah PP, Szaflarski JP, Allendorfer J, Hamilton RH. Induction of
especially lesion location could have an influence on language neuroplasticity and recovery in post-stroke aphasia by non-invasive
recovery supported by rTMS.11,12 Future studies should brain stimulation. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:1-17.

www.archives-pmr.org
Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on aphasia 1943

5. Devlin JT, Watkins KE. Stimulating language: insights from TMS. 26. Weiduschat N, Thiel A, Rubi-Fessen I, et al. Effects of repetitive
Brain 2007;130:610-22. transcranial magnetic stimulation in aphasic stroke: a randomized
6. Andoh J, Martinot JL. Interhemispheric compensation: a hypothesis controlled pilot study. Stroke 2011;42:409-15.
of TMS-induced effects on language-related areas. Eur Psychiatry 27. Thiel A, Hartmann A, Rubi-Fessen I, et al. Effects of noninvasive
2008;23:281-8. brain stimulation on language networks and recovery in early post-
7. Chrysikou EG, Hamilton R. Noninvasive brain stimulation in the stroke aphasia. Stroke 2013;44:2240-6.
treatment of aphasia: exploring interhemispheric relationships and 28. Heiss WD, Hartmann A, Rubi-Fessen I, et al. Noninvasive brain
their implications for neurorehabilitation. Restor Neurol Neurosci stimulation for treatment of right- and left-handed poststroke apha-
2011;29:375-94. sics. Cerebrovasc Dis 2013;36:363-72.
8. Miniussi C, Cappa SF, Cohen LD, et al. Efficacy of repetitive 29. Khedr EM, Abo El-Fetoh N, Ali AA, et al. Dual-hemisphere repet-
transcranial magnetic stimulation/transcranial direct current stimu- itive transcranial magnetic stimulation for rehabilitation of poststroke
lation in cognitive neurorehabilitation. Brain Stimul 2008;1:326-36. aphasia: a randomized, double-blind clinical trial. Neurorehabil
9. Torres J, Drebing D, Hamilton R. TMS and tCDS in post-stroke Neural Repair 2014;28:740-50.
aphasia: integrating novel treatment approaches with mechanisms 30. Huber W, Poeck K, Weniger D, Willmes K. Aachen Aphasia Test
of plasticity. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2013;31:501-15. (AAT). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1983.
10. Martin P, Naeser MA, Theoret H, et al. Transcranial magnetic 31. Salmaso D, Longoni AM. Problems in the assessment of hand
stimulation as a complementary treatment for aphasia. Semin Speech preference. Cortex 1985;21:533-49.
Lang 2004;25:181-91. 32. Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M. A standardized set of 260
11. Martin P, Naeser MA, Ho M, et al. Overt naming fMRI pre- and post- pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, famil-
TMS: two nonfluent aphasia patients, with and without improved iarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn 1980;6:
naming post-TMS. Brain Lang 2009;11:20-35. 174-215.
12. Naeser MA, Martin PI, Treglia E, et al. Research with rTMS in the 33. Blomert L. Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test
treatment of aphasia. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2010;28:511-29. (ANELT). Lisse: Swets Test Services; 1997.
13. Hamilton RH, Sanders L, Benson J, et al. Stimulating conversation: 34. de Jong-Hagelstein M, van de Sandt-Koenderman WM, Prins ND,
enhancement of elicited propositional speech in a patient with et al. Efficacy of early cognitive-linguistic treatment and
chronic nonfluent aphasia following transcranial magnetic stimula- communicative treatment in aphasia after stroke: a randomized
tion. Brain Lang 2010;113:45-50. controlled trial (RATS-2). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;
14. Barwood CH, Murdoch BE, Whelan B, et al. Improved receptive and 82:399-404.
expressive language abilities in nonfluent aphasic stroke patients 35. Granger CV, Hamilton BB, Linacre JM, Heinemann AW, Wright BD.
after application of rTMS: an open protocol case series. Brain Stimul Performance profiles of the Functional Independence Measure. Am J
2012;5:274-86. Phys Med Rehabil 1993;72:84-9.
15. Medina J, Norise C, Faseyitan O, Coslett HB, Turkeltaub PE, 36. Wassermann EM. Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic
Hamilton RH. Finding the right words: transcranial magnetic stim- stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International
ulation improves discourse productivity in non-fluent aphasia after Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stim-
stroke. Aphasiology 2012;26:1153-68. ulation, June 5-7, 1996. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1998;
16. Abo M, Kakuda W, Watanabe M, Morooka A, Kawakami K, 108:1-16.
Senoo A. Effectiveness of low-frequency rTMS and intensive speech 37. Weiduschat N, Habedank B, Lampe B, et al. Localizing Broca’s area
therapy in poststroke patients with aphasia: a pilot study based on for transcranial magnetic stimulation: comparison of surface distance
evaluation by fMRI in relation to type of aphasia. Eur Neurol 2012; measurements and stereotaxic positioning. Brain Stimul 2009;2:
68:199-208. 93-102.
17. Saur D, Lange R, Baumgaertner A, et al. Dynamics of language 38. Abel S, Schultz A, Radermacher I, Willmes K, Huber W. Decreasing
reorganization after stroke. Brain 2006;129:1371-84. and increasing cues in naming therapy for aphasia. Aphasiology
18. Heiss WD, Thiel A. A proposed regional hierarchy in recovery of 2005;19:831-48.
post-stroke aphasia. Brain Lang 2006;98:118-23. 39. Stark J. ELA Everyday life activities. Set 1-3 photo series. New York:
19. Zahn R, Huber W, Drews E, et al. Recovery of semantic word pro- Psychology Pr; 1994.
cessing in transcortical sensory aphasia: a functional magnetic 40. Springer L. Therapeutic approaches in aphasia rehabilitation. In:
resonance imaging study. Neurocase 2002;8:376-86. Stemmer B, Whitaker HA, editors. Handbook of the neuroscience of
20. Rosen HJ, Petersen SE, Linenweber MR, et al. Neural correlates of language. 1st ed. London: Elsevier; 2008. p 397-406.
recovery from aphasia after damage to left inferior frontal cortex. 41. Schlaug G, Marchina S, Norton A. From singing to speaking:
Neurology 2000;55:1883-94. why singing may lead to recovery of expressive language
21. Turkeltaub PE, Coslett HB, Thomas AL, et al. The right hemi- function in patients with Broca’s aphasia. Music Percept 2008;
sphere is not unitary in its role in aphasia recovery. Cortex 2012; 25:315-23.
48:1179-86. 42. Cohen JA. power primer. Psychol Bull 1992;112:155-9.
22. Jarso S, Li M, Faria A, et al. Distinct mechanisms and timing of 43. Naeser MA, Martin PI, Theoret H, et al. TMS suppression of right
language recovery after stroke. Cogn Neuropsychol 2013;30:454-75. pars triangularis, but not pars opercularis, improves naming in
23. Seniów J, Waldowski K, Lesniak M, et al. Transcranial magnetic aphasia. Brain Lang 2011;119:206-13.
stimulation combined with speech and language training in early 44. Doesborgh SJ, van de Sandt-Koenderman MW, Dippel DW, et al.
aphasia rehabilitation: a randomized double-blind controlled pilot Effects of semantic treatment on verbal communication and linguistic
study. Top Stroke Rehabil 2013;20:250-61. processing in aphasia after stroke. A randomized controlled trial.
24. Waldowski K, Seniów J, Lesniak M, Iwanski S, Czolonskowa A. Stroke 2004;35:141-6.
Effect of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation 45. Thiel A, Schuhmacher B, Wienhard K, et al. Direct demonstration of
on naming abilities in early-stroke aphasic patients: a prospective, transcallosal disinhibition in language networks. J Cereb Blood Flow
randomized, double-blind sham-controlled study. Scienti- Metab 2006;26:1122-7.
ficWorldJournal 2012;2012:518568. 46. Schnur TT, Schwartz MF, Kimberg DY, et al. Localizing interference
25. Kindler J, Schumacher R, Cazzoli D, et al. Theta burst stimulation during naming: convergent neuroimaging and neuropsychological
over the right Broca’s homologue induces improvement of naming in evidence for the function of Broca’s area. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
aphasic patients. Stroke 2012;43:2175-9. 2008;106:322-7.

www.archives-pmr.org
1944 I. Rubi-Fessen et al

47. Thompson-Schill SL, D’Esposito M, Aguirre GK, Farah MJ. Role of 50. Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, et al. Ventral and dorsal
left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a pathways for language. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008;105:
reevaluation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94:14792-7. 18035-40.
48. Heim S, Opitz B, Müller K, Friederici AD. Phonological processing 51. Hickok G, Poeppel D. Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for
during language production: fMRI evidence for a shared production- understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language.
comprehension network. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2003;16:285-96. Cognition 2004;92:67-99.
49. Friederici AD. The brain basis of language processing: from structure 52. Robey R. A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes in the treatment of
to function. Physiol Rev 2011;9:1357-92. aphasia. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998;41:172-87.

www.archives-pmr.org
Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on aphasia 1944.e1

Supplemental Appendix S1 Clinically


Relevant Differences
Patient’s example of the ANELT
Assessing the ANELT, the investigator presents 10 everyday
communicative situationsdfor example, a doctor’s visit. The pa-
tient’s task is to produce an adequate verbal reaction. The level of
communicative effectiveness is determined by the adequacy with
which relevant information is communicated (A-scale). Five levels
of ability are differentiatedd“not at all” (1 point), “a little” (2
points), “medium” (3 points), reasonable” (4 points), and “good”
(5 points)dwith a maximum of 50 points.

Example (see audio files)


Examiner: Wir sind in einem Geschäft und sie möchten einen
Fernseher kaufen. Ich bin die Verkäuferin: “Was kann ich für Sie
tun, gnädige Frau?“ (You are in a store and want to buy a tele-
vision. I am the salesperson here: Can I help you?)

Supplemental Fig S1 Definition of lesion location: The vascular ANELT pretreatment (scored 3 points)
territory of the middle cerebral artery (MCA) was defined following Participant: Ja was haben Sie denn Schönes da für mich. In den ..
standard vascular territory maps. The cortical region was the total of den neuen.. Fer. Fernsehen, haben sie da schon welche, die ganz
all cortical gray matter regions within the MCA territory, and it was neu.. die geneust..ach.. die geneu.die neusten schon gesehen
subdivided along the central sulcus into an anterior and posterior MCA haben? (Roughly translated [errors intended]: What do you have
territory. All white matter within the MCA territory, as well as the nice for me? In the.. the new.. tele..television, do you have of
basal ganglia and thalamus, was included in the subcortical MCA them, which are very new.. the newe.. oh.. the new.. the newest
territory (see supplemental fig S1 for clarification of vascular territory already have seen?)
borders). This basically corresponds to the typical vascular occlusion
syndromes causing aphasia: Rolandic and pre-Rolandic artery occlu- ANELT posttreatment (scored 4 points)
sions (anterior M2 branches of the MCA) causing predominantly Participant: Ja ich brauche neues Fernsehen. Sie müssen mal mir
expressive aphasia syndromes and the posterior M2 branches (arteria ein schönes zeigen, was gefällt ihnen das am Besten für mich -
gyri angularis and arteria supramarginalis) mainly causing sensory und wie teuer kostet das? (Roughly translated [errors intended]:
aphasia syndromes, while occlusion of the deep M1 segment perfo- Yes, I need new television. You have to show me a nice one, what
rators causes subcortical infarcts and the related aphasia symptoms. do you like best for me - and how expensive costs that?)

www.archives-pmr.org
1944.e2
Supplemental Table S1 Behavioral changes in basic language skills and functional communication
AAT Naming ANELT FIM
Token Test Written Compre- Compre-
Profile (Reversed)y Repetition Language Naming hension Screening A-Scale Expression hension
Participant Height* (Max 50) (Max 150) (Max 90) (Max 120) (Max 120) (Max 60) (10e50) (1e7) (1e7)
No. Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post
1 46.38 52.29 10 24 70 88 37 65 80 103 88 99 57 59 24 39 5 5 4 4
2 63.14 66.07 46 47 146 147 86 85 88 106 84 100 46 54 34 46 2 3 4 5
3 43.45 44.65 15 15 71 54 15 20 12 45 59 73 22 30 19 20 2 3 5 5
4 54.71 57.75 18 18 146 146 65 82 40 63 65 62 9 28 19 22 2 2 2 3
5 45.11 49.40 17 23 97 120 15 20 51 78 49 75 38 32 19 18 2 2 5 5
6 35.98 43.10 9 6 0 59 10 32 0 31 49 46 9 8 19 25 3 3 4 4
7 56.97 65.02 39 48 141 147 64 82 90 100 96 100 46 56 48 50 4 5 5 5
8 55.72 63.75 36 45 130 138 75 87 89 106 77 98 56 59 38 48 5 5 7 7
9 55.74 63.37 44 48 140 146 53 77 65 102 83 99 35 53 44 48 3 4 3 4
10 46.93 49.01 6 13 125 128 22 24 36 62 52 55 22 38 17 27 3 3 3 4
11 49.60 56.49 24 39 111 128 53 74 51 95 80 92 28 54 30 41 2 5 3 6
12 49.33 49.83 29 30 124 125 36 32 20 26 61 84 3 4 14 20 2 3 4 4
13 58.82 62.68 26 33 145 143 75 83 99 111 93 94 49 56 47 49 6 6 7 7
14 43.60 45.99 10 12 78 90 16 29 14 43 77 80 18 27 24 31 4 4 4 7
15 52.30 54.48 37 41 123 123 43 55 77 88 82 96 55 56 24 29 4 4 4 4
16 55.18 59.57 50 50 99 110 47 61 90 111 114 113 54 60 40 44 2 2 7 7
17 52.68 51.59 50 45 83 92 57 53 50 57 105 108 39 48 17 18 2 2 6 6
18 35.81 36.81 5 10 5 15 0 0 0 0 47 44 0 0 10 10 1 1 4 4
19 48.42 51.94 14 17 127 132 47 63 19 55 36 52 18 15 12 15 3 3 3 3
20 48.79 48.67 15 15 122 121 22 22 79 75 44 51 41 39 18 24 2 2 3 3
21 36.83 41.50 6 14 33 68 0 12 0 0 34 54 0 0 10 10 1 1 3 2
22 61.19 64.73 45 50 136 145 69 76 111 110 96 102 60 60 49 49 5 5 5 6
23 51.12 53.63 23 19 135 142 17 29 83 93 61 69 40 39 42 44 3 3 5 5
24 51.13 53.23 20 24 131 135 44 56 73 81 62 71 37 39 25 33 4 4 4 5
25 55.69 56.45 14 13 148 150 60 54 49 56 65 48 30 42 26 35 3 3 4 4
26 56.58 56.02 34 34 136 138 52 55 106 96 64 74 53 54 45 47 3 5 4 5
27 54.37 55.99 38 40 123 125 56 63 84 95 107 109 55 60 45 48 4 4 5 5
28 56.58 60.11 30 35 144 147 60 69 89 95 90 95 49 49 43 43 3 4 3 3
29 63.37 65.43 50 50 135 136 77 80 110 116 111 110 59 60 47 50 4 5 5 6
30 46.26 48.03 16 26 80 97 43 45 50 51 54 64 54 51 16 24 2 2 5 5
www.archives-pmr.org

Abbreviation: Max, maximum.

I. Rubi-Fessen et al
* Weighted T scores.
y
Raw scores for all other subtests of AAT, ANELT, and FIM.

You might also like