Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/303717499

The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness on


Whistleblowing Intention

Article  in  Journal of Business Ethics · June 2016


DOI: 10.1007/s10551-016-3215-6

CITATIONS READS

10 676

5 authors, including:

Paul Andon Clinton Free


UNSW Sydney UNSW Sydney
21 PUBLICATIONS   324 CITATIONS    36 PUBLICATIONS   574 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Radzi Jidin Gary S. Monroe


University of Tasmania UNSW Sydney
4 PUBLICATIONS   11 CITATIONS    26 PUBLICATIONS   616 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Accounting and the Business of Sport: Playing the Numbers Game - Special issue call for papers from Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal View project

Interview-Based Research in Accounting 2000-2014: A Review View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Clinton Free on 13 July 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


J Bus Ethics
DOI 10.1007/s10551-016-3215-6

The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness


on Whistleblowing Intention
Paul Andon1 • Clinton Free1 • Radzi Jidin1 • Gary S. Monroe1 • Michael J. Turner2

Received: 4 August 2015 / Accepted: 15 May 2016


 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

Abstract Many jurisdictions have put regulatory strategies the intention to whistleblow is moderated by perceptions of
in place to provide incentives and safeguards to whistle- the seriousness of the wrongdoing.
blowers to encourage whistleblowing on corporate
wrongdoings. One such strategy is the provision of a Keywords Whistleblowing  Sarbanes–Oxley Act  Dodd–
financial incentive to the whistleblower if the complaint Frank Act  Financial incentives  Seriousness  Moral
leads to a successful regulatory enforcement action against awareness
the offending organization. We conducted an experiment
using professional accountants as participants to examine
whether such an incentive encourages potential whistle- Introduction
blowers to report an observed financial reporting fraud to a
relevant external authority. We also examine the effect of In spite of the enormous investment organizations
perceived seriousness of the wrongdoing on accountants’ throughout the world have made into fraud detection tools,
intention to whistleblow. We find that a financial incentive processes, and structures, research has consistently identi-
results in a higher intention to whistleblow to a relevant fied whistleblowers as the most important source of fraud
external authority. We also find that perceptions of the detection (ACFE 2016).1 This is perhaps unsurprising
seriousness of the wrongdoing are significantly and posi- given the complexity and low public visibility of much
tively associated with accountants’ intention to whistle- fraudulent activity within organizations. Since the financial
blow to a relevant external authority. We find a significant scandals of the early millennium in particular, regulators
interaction between the provision of a financial incentive throughout the world have underscored the importance of
and the perceived seriousness of the wrongdoing on the whistleblowing in detecting and deterring organizational
intention to report the wrongdoing externally. The intention misconduct and corporate fraud (Feldman and Lobel 2010).
to report the financial reporting fraud externally is higher In the US and elsewhere, a centerpiece of regulatory efforts
when the level of perceived seriousness is higher, regard- has been the implementation of financial incentives to
less of the availability of a financial incentive. However, reward whistleblowers. However, the impact of these
when the perceived level of seriousness is lower, the incentives on potential whistleblowers, particularly
presence of financial incentive results in a higher intention accounting professionals who are well positioned to iden-
to report the financial reporting fraud externally. These tify and report fraud, is not well understood (Pope and Lee
findings indicate that the impact of a financial incentive on 2013).

& Paul Andon 1


Although there is no agreed-upon definition of whistleblowing, the
p.andon@unsw.edu.au
term has been used within the business vernacular to describe an act
1 whereby one exposes wrongdoing (Jubb 1999; Perry 1998). Near and
UNSW Business School, UNSW Australia, Sydney, Australia
Miceli (1985) define whistleblowing as organization members’
2
UQ Business School, The University of Queensland, disclosure of illegal, immoral, or illegitimate practices under the
St Lucia, Australia control of their employers, to parties who may be able to effect action.

123
P. Andon et al.

The aim of this study is to investigate the way that Research looking at the relationship between percep-
financial incentives designed to encourage whistleblowing tions of seriousness, financial incentives, and the intention
influence professional accountants’ intention to whistle- to whistleblow has been scant and mixed. Feldman and
blow. This paper is motivated by two key factors. The first Lobel (2010) found a significant interaction between
is the considerable recent activity on the part of regulators financial incentives and the perceived ‘severity’ of mis-
internationally to provide incentives for whistleblowers as conduct whereby those who viewed the misconduct to be
corporate fraud social enforcement has become a key fea- relatively insignificant were likely to be influenced by
ture of regulatory policy (Feldman and Lobel 2010). A financial incentives, while there was no difference for those
central element of recent US legislation in the area has who viewed the misconduct to be severe. However, it
been the creation of financial incentives for whistleblowers should be noted that their study involved a different
who volunteer original information that results in sanctions wrongdoing, respondent population, and reward manipu-
in the form of monetary fines. Perhaps most notably, the lation than examined in the current study. Moreover, in an
Wall Street Reform and Consumer Protection Act 2010 examination of accounting and non-accounting fraud cases
(commonly referred to as the Dodd–Frank Act), enacted in in the US between 1996 and 2004, Dyck et al. (2010,
the aftermath of the global financial crisis, empowered the p. 2215) conclude that ‘‘monetary incentives for fraud
Securities Exchange Commission (SEC) to make monetary revelation appear to play a role regardless of the severity of
awards available to whistleblowers whose information the fraud’’ (emphasis added). Given concerns regarding the
leads to successful SEC actions in certain circumstances.2 mixed evidence, this research seeks to examine these
Outside of the US, financial rewards for whistleblowers effects using professional accountants currently employed
have been introduced in South Korea, Canada, Mexico, and in an accounting role and currently working in the US. Our
to a lesser extent China and India. Despite the clear policy study contributes to the literature by adding to our under-
and organizational benefits that flow from an improved standing of the effectiveness of financial incentives in
understanding of the factors that facilitate whistleblowing, promoting the reporting of wrongful acts by professional
the field remains relatively understudied (Feldman and accountants in relation to the very type of fraud they are
Lobel 2010). likely to discover, financial statement fraud.
The second motivating factor is the role of accountants We conduct an experiment using professional accoun-
as key stakeholders in the detection and prevention of tants as participants to examine whether a financial
organizational fraud and misconduct (Andon et al. 2015). incentive, similar to that enacted as part of the Dodd–Frank
In its most recent 2016 Report to the Nations, the Asso- Act passed in 2010, encourages potential whistleblowers to
ciation of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE) found that report an observed financial reporting fraud to external
more occupational frauds originated in accounting depart- authorities. We also examine whether the perceived seri-
ments than in any other business unit (16.6 %). Further, ousness (operationalized in terms of harm and moral
over 75 % of the occupational frauds they analyzed were wrongfulness) of the wrongdoing affects the intention to
committed by individuals working in seven departments: whistleblow. We find that the presence of a financial
accounting, operations, sales, executive/upper manage- incentive results in a higher intention to whistleblow to a
ment, customer service, purchasing, and finance (ACFE relevant external authority than when no financial incentive
2016). These findings, which mirror similar findings else- is present. We also find that perceptions of the seriousness
where (see, for example, Friedrichs 2002), focus attention of the wrongdoing are significantly and positively associ-
on a band of roles and responsibilities that have a close ated with accountants’ intention to whistleblow to a rele-
connection to the accounting cycle. Consequently, indi- vant external authority. We further find a significant
viduals employed in accounting-related roles have a unique interaction between the provision of a financial incentive
opportunity to detect fraud. Accountants are also frequently and the perceived seriousness of the wrongdoing on the
in a position to directly or indirectly observe manipulated intention to report the wrongdoing externally. The intention
accounting records and circumvented internal controls. to report the financial reporting fraud to a relevant external
Therefore, the whistleblowing intentions of professional authority is higher when the level of perceived seriousness
accountants are an important, and currently under-re- is higher, regardless of the availability of a financial
searched, element of fraud detection and prevention. incentive. However, when the perceived level of serious-
ness is lower, the presence of a financial incentive results in
a higher intention to report the financial reporting fraud
2
These incentives are required to be between 10 and 30 % of the externally than when no financial incentive is present.
monetary penalties collected. This initiative represents a significant
These findings indicate that the impact of a financial
escalation not only in the incentives offered to whistleblowers, but
also an acknowledgement of the increased strategic regulatory incentive on the intention to whistleblow is moderated by
importance attached by the US Government to whistleblowers. perceptions of the seriousness of the wrongdoing.

123
The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness…

The remainder of this paper is structured as follows. and Civil Rights Commission (ACRC) to provide
‘‘Regulatory Attempts to Encourage Whistleblowing’’ whistleblowers with rewards of up to US$2 million for
section presents a brief overview of regulatory attempts to information that contributes directly to recovering or
encourage whistleblowing. ‘‘Prior Research’’ section sum- increasing revenues or reducing expenditures for public
marizes the experimental literature that examines factors agencies, and also may recommend awards for whistle-
that affect the likelihood of an individual reporting a blowing that has served the public interest (OECD 2012).
wrongdoing. This is followed by ‘‘Hypothesis Develop- The Canada Revenue Agency (CRA) instituted the Stop
ment’’ section, which develops the hypotheses tested in our International Tax Evasion Program, under which the CRA
study. Our research method is discussed in ‘‘Research will reward whistleblowers up to 15 % of international tax
Method’’ section, which is then followed by the presenta- recoveries exceeding CA$100,000 (Fekete 2013). Mexico
tion of our results in ‘‘Results’’ section. The final section introduced a whistleblower incentive scheme focused on
summarizes our study, its contribution to the literature, and money-laundering (Wolf 2011). Other, less comprehensive
potential limitations of our research. variations of whistleblower incentives are also evident in
China and India (Freshfields Bruckhaus Deringer LLP
2013). As indicated in Table 1, there is considerable vari-
Regulatory Attempts to Encourage ation among G20 countries with respect to financial
Whistleblowing incentives.

Across the world’s largest economies, regulators have


increasingly sought to draw upon tools to provide support Prior Research
to whistleblowing. Regulated financial incentives designed
to encourage whistleblowing are becoming a more accep- Experimental research published in accounting, business
ted response to the challenges of eliciting information ethics, and other fields is beginning to identify important
about illegitimate corporate conduct (Braithwaite 2013; characteristics of whistleblowing intentions and behavior.
Bucy 2002a, b). The US has perhaps the most mature Recent work has examined various procedures, protections,
provisions for whistleblower incentives. Under the US and incentives that can be shaped by organizations and
False Claims Act, ‘‘qui tam’’ actions allow a whistleblower regulatory authorities to influence whistleblowing inten-
to receive up to 30 % of monies recovered by government tions and actions. Several procedural elements have been
from offenders in cases of public sector fraud, subject to identified to have a positive impact on whistleblowing
certain conditions. In 2006, amendments were made to the intentions, including formal reporting structures (Brennan
US Internal Revenue Code to provide for economic and Kelly 2007), policies and mechanisms incorporating
incentives aimed at encouraging whistleblowers to provide higher levels of procedural, distributive, and interactional
information to the Internal Revenue Service on large-scale justice (Seifert et al. 2010), anti-retaliation protections
tax fraud (over US$2 million). More recently, and specif- (Keenan 1990; Liyanarachchi and Adler 2011; Liya-
ically focusing on corporate fraud, additional financial narachchi and Newdick 2009), and prior evidence of strong
incentives were enacted as part of the Dodd–Frank Act organizational responses to whistleblowing (Curtis and
passed in 2010. This Act founded the United States Secu- Taylor 2009; Taylor and Curtis 2013). Anonymous
rities and Exchange Commission (SEC) Office of the reporting has been found to increase the frequency of
Whistleblower, which is charged with administering the reporting (Atkinson et al. 2012); however, this literature
reward of financial incentives to individuals who report has yielded mixed findings, suggesting that the availability
breaches of federal securities laws leading to a successful of anonymous reporting channels is no better than pro-
SEC enforcement action against the offending organization tected identity reporting protections (Curtis and Taylor
of more than US$1 million. To qualify for a financial 2009) and do not affect the intention to report generally
incentive between 10 and 30 % of funds recovered, the (Pope and Lee 2013). Kaplan et al. (2012) report that in the
whistleblower must voluntarily provide ‘‘original’’ infor- absence of prior experience of retaliation, respondents did
mation (i.e., the information must have been derived from not differ in their intention to report between anonymous
‘‘independent knowledge or analysis’’) that is not already and non-anonymous reporting channels, while experience
known to the SEC. The amount of the incentive paid within of retaliation lowered whistleblowing intentions through
the percentage range is informed by a range of criteria set non-anonymous reporting channels, but did not increase
out in the Act. anonymous reporting intentions. A small stream of papers
Other countries have also adopted legislated financial has begun to investigate the impact of financial incentives.
incentives for whistleblowers. In South Korea, The Anti- Xu and Ziegenfuss (2008) and Pope and Lee (2013)
Corruption Act passed in 2001 allows the Anti-Corruption highlight a positive relationship between incentives and

123
P. Andon et al.

Table 1 A global survey of whistleblowing provisions (G20 nations Hypothesis Development


listed in order of GDP)
Nation External financial incentives In brief, we expect that both the presence of a financial
incentive and high perceptions of seriousness are positively
Public sector laws Private sector laws
related to the intention to whistleblow. We further propose
European Union 3 3 that this positive relationship between financial incentives
United States 3 1 and intention to whistleblow will be moderated by the
China 3 2 perceived seriousness of observed wrongdoing.
Japan 3 3
Germany 3 3
France 3 3 The Impact of Financial Incentives
United Kingdom 3 3
Brazil 3 3 Offering individuals financial incentives by a regulator to
Russia 3 3 report externally (i.e., to an authority outside their employ-
Italy 3 3 ing organization) has been found to have a significant
India 3 2 influence on whistleblowing intentions (Feldman and Lobel
Canada 3 2 2010; Pope and Lee 2013). While broader social–psycho-
Australia 3 3
logical research may suggest that whistleblowing intentions
Mexico 3 2
could be animated by a sense of organizational loyalty, and
to benefit their organization, available evidence indicates
South Korea 3 1
that there is significant under-reporting of wrongdoing
Indonesia 3 3
(Ethics Resource Center 2013; Miceli and Near 1992).
Turkey 3 3
In situations such as this, where regulators seek to motivate
Saudi Arabia 3 3
reporting in the observed absence of an intrinsic motivation
Argentina 3 3
to do so, it has been long argued that a financial reward is a
South Africa 3 3
potent motivational tool (see, for example, Callahan and
Ratings 1 very/quite comprehensive, 2 somewhat/partially compre- Dworkin 1992; Porter et al. 1976). Accordingly, as descri-
hensive, 3 absent/not at all comprehensive bed above, in recent years regulators throughout the world
have set up schemes whereby whistleblowers who volunteer
original information to external authorities are able to
reporting, but other studies find this to be a complex receive financial bounties. These financial incentives to
association. Feldman and Lobel (2010) find evidence that whistleblow are believed to foster opportunistic motivation
the effect of incentives can be impacted by elements such to reveal wrongdoing where they might not have otherwise
as the size of the incentive, duties to report, internal (Schmidt 2005), in a manner that may be likened to the
motivation to report, and gender (males being more effects of an employee performance bonus (Callahan and
affected by incentives). Dworkin 1992). This is important given the observation that
Thus, the emerging literature on financial incentives many instances of illegitimate corporate conduct go unre-
suggests that the effects of such incentives, while holding a ported (Miceli and Near 1992).
lot of promise, are complex in terms of how they directly Research generally lends support to the positive impact
and indirectly affect whistleblowing intentions and actions. of financial incentives on whistleblowing intentions. Xu
However, despite Miceli and Near’s (1992) call for further and Ziegenfuss (2008) report that the likelihood of internal
research examining the effectiveness of financial rewards on auditors reporting wrongdoing is increased when incentives
people’s intention to whistleblow, research on the effec- are offered by their employing organization. Pope and Lee
tiveness of financial incentives in the private sector is (2013) report similar findings from their study using MBA
‘‘woefully lacking’’ (Pope and Lee 2013, p. 602). Our study students. However, evidence from Feldman and Lobel
adds to the existing academic and practitioner literature by (2010) and Brink et al. (2013) is more equivocal. While
adding to our understanding of the effectiveness of financial Feldman and Lobel (2010) report that financial incentives
incentives in promoting the reporting of wrongful acts, and were the most significant mechanism influencing reporting
certain variables that are likely to impact on the effective- intentions, they also found that participants predicted the
ness of such measures. The following section draws on the reporting intentions of a stranger to be more affected by
literature reviewed to formulate hypotheses, which in turn financial incentives than their own reporting intentions (a
elaborates in more detail how we seek to extend extant social desirability response bias effect), that males were
understanding of financial incentives and whistleblowing. more affected by financial incentives than females, that

123
The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness…

high internal motivation diminishes the impact of financial H2 There is a positive association between perceptions of
incentives, and that in certain circumstances financial the seriousness of a wrongdoing and the intention to report
incentives can ‘crowd out’ internal reporting motivation, a wrongdoing externally.
resulting in lower reporting intentions than if no reward
were offered at all. Brink et al. (2013) find that the impact
of financial incentives is also conditioned by the strength of The Moderating Impact of Seriousness
evidence of wrongdoing at the whistleblower’s disposal. in the Presence of Financial Incentives
Overall, the general implication of this literature is that
financial incentives are likely to have a positive impact on Whether a whistleblower is motivated by intrinsic or
whistleblower intentions to report wrongdoings. Based on extrinsic factors may depend on the context. Based on
current claims, we hypothesize that financial incentives intrinsic–extrinsic motivation theory, we expect that the
will result in a higher intention to report a financial provision of a financial incentive is unlikely to have an
reporting fraud to a relevant external authority. effect on the intention to report a wrongdoing if a wrong-
doing is perceived as very serious. In contrast, if the
H1 The presence of a financial incentive will result in a wrongdoing is not perceived as very serious, then the
higher intention to report a wrongdoing externally. financial incentive is likely to have an effect on the
intention to report a wrongdoing. Feldman and Lobel
Perceptions of Seriousness and Intrinsic Motivations (2010) argue that monetary rewards can affect the level of
reporting a wrongdoing unless the underlying wrongdoing
Motivations to report wrongdoing can be divided into intrinsic is perceived as being in some way morally offensive. When
and extrinsic motivations (Harackiewicz and Sansone 2000). the whistleblower attaches an ethical significance to the act
Extrinsic motivation is linked to actions that are driven by of reporting, monetary rewards are less consequential in
external commands or rewards such as payments (Harack- determining his or her actions because the intrinsic moti-
iewicz and Sansone 2000). Alternatively, intrinsic motivation is vation to right the wrongdoing is the whistleblower’s pri-
the driver when behavior is chosen from within the individual mary motivation. Fehr and Falk (2002) claim that where
out of a sense of moral or civic duty (Deci et al. 1999a; Kasser people attribute their behavior to external rewards, they
and Ryan 1996). Intrinsic motivation was originally focused on have a tendency to discount any consideration of intrinsic
some inherent interest in a task (see, Deci et al. 1999a). Over incentives for their behavior and that extrinsic incentives
time, the concept has expanded to include many additional drive their decision about whether to report the wrongdo-
intrinsic motivations such as morality, duty, legitimacy, fair- ing. Across a range of experimental tasks, tangible rewards
ness, loyalty, and identity (Feldman 2011). Alternatively, have been shown to have the potential to undermine
extrinsic motivation was originally focused on monetary intrinsic motivation (Deci et al. 1999a, b), resulting in
rewards (see Deci et al. 1999a) but has since been expanded to considerable long-term counterproductive results (Deci
include various legal, verbal, social, and organizational mech- et al. 1999a). Feldman and Lobel (2010) found a significant
anisms that seek to influence people to engage in socially interaction between financial incentives and the perceived
desirable behavior (Feldman 2011). ‘severity’ of misconduct whereby those who viewed the
Scholars have suggested that the perceived seriousness misconduct to be relatively insignificant were likely to be
of wrongdoing is a predictor of an individual’s intrinsic influenced by financial incentives, while there was no
motivation to blow the whistle. A wrongdoing that is per- difference for those who viewed the misconduct to be
ceived as more serious is more likely to be reported to a severe.
relevant authority (Clinard and Yeager 1983; Dozier and In summary, intrinsic–extrinsic motivation theory sug-
Miceli 1985; Near et al. 2004; Taylor and Curtis 2010). gests that people are more likely to report a wrongdoing
The perceived seriousness of a wrongdoing should affect that is perceived as serious irrespective of any financial
the intention to report the wrongdoing externally. Drawing incentive to report the wrongdoing. In contrast, a financial
on data from a diverse panel of working adults in the US incentive is likely to have an effect on the likelihood of
using a theft case example, Feldman and Lobel (2010) find reporting a wrongdoing when the wrongdoing is perceived
that the intention to whistleblow externally is associated as less serious.
with what they term ‘severity.’ Accordingly, we expect a
positive association between perceptions of the seriousness H3 Perceived seriousness of observed wrongdoing mod-
of a wrongdoing and the intention to report a financial erates the relationship between financial incentives and the
statement fraud externally. intention to report a wrongdoing externally.

123
P. Andon et al.

Research Method Participants were next exposed to the manipulated


treatment, i.e., information whether a financial incentive
This section of the paper describes our experimental was (was not) available for reporting the financial reporting
materials, measurement of variables, selection of our par- fraud. The financial incentive available manipulation read
ticipants, response rate, and analysis of demographic data. as follows:
Under current law, the US Securities and Exchange
Experimental Design and Measurement of Variables
Commission (SEC) provides a financial reward to
individuals who volunteer original information to a
We used a between-subject quasi-experimental design with
relevant external authority that results in monetary
one manipulated variable (financial incentive) and one
sanctions against the offending company that exceed
measured variable (perceived seriousness of the wrongdo-
$1,000,000. The amount of the financial reward will
ing). Financial incentive was manipulated at two levels
be between 10 and 30 % of such monetary sanctions
(financial incentive or no financial incentive) and perceived
collected by the securities regulator.
seriousness of the wrongdoing was measured rather than
manipulated. Participants were presented with a case sce- The no financial incentive available manipulation read
nario, based on a combination of recent real-life financial as follows:
statement frauds, where they were asked to assume the role
Under current law, there are no provisions for paying
of a certified public accountant (CPA) employed as a senior
financial rewards to individuals who volunteer orig-
accountant within the accounting department of AgFoods
inal information to the US Securities and Exchange
Inc., a public company in the business of grain marketing
Commission that result in monetary sanctions against
and exporting. The experimental materials stated ‘‘One
the offending company.
evening, while working late on a set of files to meet a
reporting deadline, you discovered information indicating Following this, participants were asked to indicate how
that the financial controller of AgFoods has made a series likely they thought it was that they would formally report
of accounting entries recognizing fictitious revenue totaling the fictitious revenue entries to a relevant external authority
$50,000,000. After being unable to find any supporting using a nine-point scale anchored by 1 = very unlikely and
documentation, you raised the matter with the financial 9 = very likely (Report Externally). These responses are
controller who was dismissive of your concerns. It is clear used as the dependent variables in our subsequent analyses.
to you that these entries have caused AgFoods to materially Participants were then asked to answer a number of
misrepresent its reported earnings. Based on recent cases, other questions including a question used to measure our
you expect that the SEC would impose sanctions in the test variable regarding participants’ perceptions of the
form of a fine of approximately $5,000,000 if the company seriousness of the wrongdoing (Seriousness). To capture
is investigated and found guilty of fraudulent financial participants’ perceptions of the seriousness of the wrong-
reporting.’’3 Participants were also informed that doing, they were asked to assess the seriousness of the
‘‘AgFoods has a dedicated hotline in place for employees fictitious accounting entries using a nine-point scale
to report any concerns regarding accounting, auditing and anchored by 1 = not serious and 9 = very serious.4
internal control matters in a confidential and anonymous As proposed by Schultz et al. (1993), the Model of
manner,’’ and that ‘‘there are no explicit protections under Discretionary Reporting posits that, in addition to the
current law against an employer retaliating against an perceived seriousness of a wrongdoing, the decision to
individual presenting an allegation of illegal behavior report a questionable act is also influenced by the perceived
either internally to their employer or externally to the rel- personal costs (or benefits) of reporting. Participants were
evant government authority.’’ asked to assess the costs (and benefits) of reporting the
fictitious revenue entries to the relevant external authority
3
Great care was taken to develop a realistic scenario framed around a using a nine-point scale anchored by 1 = no cost (benefit)
case of financial statement fraud. Real-life cases as reported in and 9 = very high cost (benefit). For our subsequent
national newspapers formed the basis for the scenario used in our analyses, we use the net cost minus benefit score by sub-
experimental material. The scenario was piloted with the assistance of
tracting the cost of reporting responses from the benefit of
(1) 3 persons from industry and (2) 9 university academics
independent to the research. Utilizing a case vignette to manipulate
situational variables is consistent with prior studies of whistleblowing
4
(Mesmer-Magnus and Viswesvaran 2005; Miceli et al. 2008). It is We also asked participants to assess the level of harm and moral
acknowledged, however, that using a brief hypothetical scenario wrongfulness associated with the fictitious accounting entries. We do
cannot adequately capture the richness of the real-life environment, not include the level of harm and the level of moral wrongfulness as
the formal and informal structures and networks within firms, covariates in our models as they were highly correlated with the level
personal ambitions, integrity, and much else besides. of seriousness.

123
The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness…

reporting responses for the relevant assessments of external Table 2 Questionnaire survey response pattern
reporting. Total
Participants were also asked to indicate the amount of
ethics-related training they had undertaken in their career Accepted invitation and passed screening (working the 177
U.S. in an accounting role)
using a nine-point scale anchored by 1 = no ethics training
and 9 = a large amount of ethics training. They also Less incomplete 39
reported whether they held a professional accounting des- Completed 138
ignation; their level of satisfaction with their current job Completion rate 77.97 %
using a nine-point scale; age; gender; and whether they Less failed manipulation checks 58
had, as part of their work experience, discovered a person Total usable responses 80
or persons of greater authority engaging in potentially Response rate 45.20 %
wrongful behavior.
Response Rate
Participants
The survey response pattern is shown in Table 2. Invita-
Participants are professional accountants currently
tions were sent out by Qualtrics to a targeted audience of
employed in an accounting role and currently working in
respondents registered as professional accountants in the
the US who were members of a national panel obtained by
Qualtrics respondent database. At the researcher’s request,
Qualtrics Inc., an international online research company.5
Qualtrics sent the questionnaire to respondents over the age
Respondents were recruited to the panels through two
of 25. Age 25 was chosen as it would be unlikely that
methods: self-registration or third-party (e.g., airline mile
respondents could be a qualified professional accountant at
clubs) recruiting. Participants receive cash-equivalent
any lower age. Two screening out questions were also
rewards which can be redeemed for gift cards, airline
included, which were first that the respondent had to be
miles, magazine subscriptions, etc. The recruitment cam-
currently employed in an accounting role, and second that
paign typically appeals to some combination of incentives,
the respondent had to be currently working in the US. A
self-expression, fun, social comparison, curiosity, or con-
total of 177 respondents passed the initial screening con-
venience (Poynter and Comley 2003). Like most online
ditions. However, 39 were eliminated because the survey
panels, the panel sample was recruited using non-proba-
was incomplete. This resulted in a total of 138 completions,
bility recruitment methods (see, Baker et al. 2010). The
a 77.97 % completion rate. Within the 138 completions,
sample was chosen randomly from the panel. Survey
however, 58 respondents failed the manipulation check
respondents opened a link to the online survey with help
question, leaving a total of 80 usable responses, a 45.20 %
from Qualtrics’ survey administrators. Qualtrics received a
response rate.6
payment for access to their panel and handled the com-
pensation of respondents.
Descriptive Statistics
Professional accountants were chosen as the sampling
group because they are key stakeholders in the detection
Data on demographic factors including age and gender
and prevention of organizational fraud and misconduct,
were collected. Questions were also posed concerning
particularly in relation to financial statement fraud. Further,
whether as part of the respondent’s work experience they
they have a high level of practical experience working in a
had ever discovered a person or persons engaging in
professional environment. In addition, our experimental
potentially wrongful behavior and if they currently held a
materials are about an accountant who discovered infor-
professional accounting designation. Descriptive statistics
mation concerning a wrongdoing involving fraudulent
regarding these categorical demographic questions posed
financial reporting. As such, professional accountants are
are displayed in Table 3.
likely to have experienced ethical dilemmas such as those
Table 3 highlights that sample respondents were evenly
contained within the questionnaire. Therefore, professional
spread across working-age categories [25–34: 19 (23.8 %);
accountants are considered to be appropriate participants in
35–44: 18 (22.5 %); 45–54: 21 (26.3 %)], with only a
our study.
small proportion of respondents being aged over 65
[65–74: 5 (6.3 %); and over 75: 1 (1.3 %)]. While a
majority of the sample respondents were female (65 %),
5
Qualtrics panels have been used in several related fields such as
6
business ethics (e.g., Ferguson et al. 2014), marketing and logistics These failure rates are similar to those in other studies using
(e.g., Albaum et al. 2014), psychology (e.g., Wright and Carlucci electronic survey methods (e.g., Andrews et al. 2003; Oppenheimer
2011), and environmental decision making (e.g., Rosoff et al. 2013). et al. 2009).

123
P. Andon et al.

Table 3 Demographic categorical descriptive statistics of the Results


participants
Category Number of participants Percentage Recall that our financial incentive test variable was
manipulated, whereas our test variable for participants’
Total participants 80 100
perceptions of the seriousness of the wrongdoing was
Age
measured. To capture participants’ perceptions of the
25–34 19 23.8
seriousness of the wrongdoing, participants were asked to
35–44 18 22.5 assess the seriousness of the fictitious accounting entries
45–54 21 26.3 using a nine-point scale anchored by 1 = not serious and
55–64 16 20 9 = very serious. The mean and standard deviation for this
65–74 5 6.3 variable were 7.91 and 1.943, respectively, while the
Over 75 1 1.3 median is 9 (not tabulated). As shown in Panel A of
Gender Table 4, 77.6 % of participants assessed the level of seri-
Male 28 35 ousness as either 8 or 9, whereas the rest of the participants
Female 52 65 (22.4 %) assessed the level of seriousness between 1 and 7.
Professional accounting designation The hypotheses are tested using ANCOVAs. The
Yes 56 70 dependent variable for the testing of H1, H2, and H3 is
No 24 30 measured using the participants’ responses for a question
Prior experience of wrongful behavior about how likely it is that they would report the fictitious
Yes 27 33.8 entries to a relevant external authority using nine-point
No 43 53.8 scales where 1 = very unlikely and 9 = very likely. Apart
Not sure 10 12.5 from the two test variables, Financial Incentive and Seri-
ousness, we also include four covariates in our analyses:
(i) Age, (ii) Gender, (iii) Costs–benefits, and (iv) Ethics
this broadly accords with the proportion of women now
training.8
employed in accounting-related roles (for example, the
Recall that H1 addresses the effect of a financial
Bureau of Labor Statistics reports that women make up
incentive on accountants’ intention to report the wrong-
63 % of all accountants and auditors in the US7). A total of
doing externally. The ANCOVA results reported in Panel
56 (70 %) of our sample respondents indicated that they
C of Table 5 show a significant main effect for Financial
held a professional accounting designation. Finally, 27
Incentive on accountants’ intention to report the financial
(33.8 %) of our sample had prior experience of observing
reporting fraud externally (F = 4.848, p = 0.016 one-
wrongful behavior as compared to 43 (53.8 %) who had
tailed). Consistent with H1, the means reported in Table 5,
not, and 10 (12.5 %) who were unsure. In addition to these
Panel A indicate that accountants’ intention to report the
categorical questions, two additional demographic ques-
financial reporting fraud externally is significantly higher
tions were posed. First, respondents were asked to rate the
when there is a financial incentive compared to when there
amount of ethics-related training that they had undertaken
is no financial incentive (mean difference = 0.878,
in their career, posed on a nine-point scale with 1 being no
p = 0.016 one-tailed). In line with this, parameter esti-
ethics training and 9 being a large amount of ethics train-
mates in Panel D of Table 5 show a negative coefficient for
ing. The mean for this item was 5.95 with a standard
Financial Incentive (B = -3.959, standard error 1.798).
deviation of 2.470. Finally, participants were asked to
This suggests that accountants are less likely to report a
indicate how satisfied they were with their current job,
wrongdoing externally when there is no financial incentive
anchored on a nine-point scale with 1 being very dissatis-
to do so relative to when there is such an incentive. Overall,
fied and 9 very satisfied. The mean for this item was 7.24
the findings support H1.
with a standard deviation of 1.795.
8
Prior research suggests the potential influence of these variables on
individual’s intention to whistleblow. For example, Liyanarachchi
and Adler (2011) provide evidence of the influence of age and gender
on accountants’ likelihood to whistleblow. Besides, Kaplan et al.
(2009) demonstrate the association between gender and individuals’
intention to report fraudulent financial reporting. Also, as discussed
earlier in this section, Schultz et al. (1993) suggest that perceived
7
Current Population Survey, Bureau of Labor Statistics, ‘‘Table 11 personal costs (or benefits) of reporting influenced individual’s
Employed Persons by Detailed Occupation, Sex, Race, and Hispanic decision to report a questionable act. Further, prior studies posit that
or Latino Ethnicity,’’ 2014. Available at http://www.bls.gov/cps/ ethics training program could influence accountants’ intention to
cpsaat11.pdf (Accessed 22 July 2015). whistleblow (e.g., Decker and Calo 2007; Vadera et al. 2009).

123
The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness…

Table 4 Assessment of
Panel A: frequency table
seriousness of wrongdoing
Level of seriousness Frequency Percent Cumulative percent

1—not serious 3 3.8 3.8


4 4 5.0 8.8
5 2 2.5 11.3
6 4 5.0 16.3
7 5 6.3 22.5
8 13 16.3 38.8
9—very serious 49 61.3 100.0
Total 80 100.0

H2 considers the effect of accountants’ perception of the These findings show the roles of seriousness of wrongdoing
seriousness of the wrongdoing on their intention to report a in moderating the effect of financial incentive on accoun-
wrongdoing externally. Panel B of Table 5 shows that there tants’ intention to whistleblow externally, thereby provid-
is a positive correlation between accountants’ perception of ing support for H3. In the presence of this significant
the seriousness of the wrongdoing and their intention to interaction effect, the main effect for Financial Incentive
report it externally (Pearson correlation = 0.447, needs to be interpreted with caution. The findings
p = 0.000 two-tailed). The ANCOVA results reported in demonstrate that the effect of financial incentive on the
Panel C of Table 5 show a significant main effect for intention to report the wrongdoing externally depends on
Seriousness on the intention to report externally the perception of the seriousness of the wrongdoing.
(F = 10.977, p = 0.000). Consistent with our expectation,
parameter estimates presented in Panel C of Table 5 show Results for the Covariates
a positive coefficient (B = 0.184, standard error = 0.170).
These results show a significant positive association Although not related to our hypotheses, we find that three
between the accountants’ perception of the seriousness of of the four covariates included in our analyses as presented
the wrongdoing and their intention to report the financial in Table 5, Panel C, are significant. Recall that the results
reporting fraud externally. In other words, accountants’ presented in Table 5 are for the intention to report a
intention to report the wrongdoing to a relevant external wrongdoing externally. The three significant covariates are
authority is significantly higher when the perceived level of Age (F = 3.900, p = 0.026 one-tailed), Ethics Training
seriousness is high relative to when the perceived level of (F = 7.738, p = 0.004, one-tailed), and Gender
seriousness is low. These findings provide support for H2. (F = 8.019, p = 0.003). The Costs–benefits variable is not
Recall that H3 addresses the interaction effect between significant. The parameter estimates presented in Panel D
Financial Incentive and Seriousness on the intention to of Table 5 show that older accountants are more likely to
report the financial reporting fraud externally. Panel C of report a wrongdoing externally than younger accountants,
Table 5 shows a significant interaction between Financial which is consistent with the findings in prior research
Incentive and Seriousness on the intention of reporting the (Liyanarachchi and Adler 2011). The parameter estimate
financial reporting fraud externally (F = 3.162, p = 0.040 for Ethics Training shows that accountants who have
one-tailed). As predicted, parameter estimates in Panel D undertaken more ethics-related training during their career
of Table 5 indicate a positive coefficient for the interaction have a higher intention to report a questionable act exter-
between Financial Incentive and Seriousness (B = 0.389, nally than those who have undertaken less training. This is
standard error = 0.219). The interaction between Financial consistent with suggestions in prior research that more
Incentive and Seriousness is depicted in Panel E of ethics-related training would lead to a higher likelihood of
Table 5. The parameter estimate for the interaction and the reporting questionable act (e.g., Decker and Calo 2007;
graph depicted in Panel E of Table 5 indicates that Vadera et al. 2009). The results also show that the intention
accountants’ intention to report the financial reporting of reporting a wrongdoing to an external authority is higher
fraud externally is significantly higher when the level of among female accountants than male accountants. This is
perceived seriousness is higher, regardless of the avail- generally consistent with the findings in prior research that
ability of a financial incentive. However, when the level of female participants are more likely to report a wrongdoing
seriousness is lower, the presence of a financial incentive and fraudulent financial reporting than male participants
increases the intention to report the wrongdoing externally. (Kaplan et al. 2009)

123
P. Andon et al.

Table 5 Intention to report externally


Panel A: mean for financial incentive
Financial incentive Mean Std. error

Yes 5.701 .323


No 6.579 .304
Panel B: correlation between seriousness and intention to report externally
Intention to report externally

Seriousness
Pearson correlation .447
Sig. (two-tailed) .000
N 80

Panel C: ANCOVA results


Source Type III sum of squares df Mean square F Significance (one-tailed)

Corrected model 144.475 7 20.639 5.966 .000


Intercept 0.541 1 0.541 0.156 .347
Age 13.491 1 13.491 3.900 .026
Costs–benefits 0.008 1 0.008 0.002 .481
Ethics training 26.770 1 26.770 7.738 .004
Gender 27.740 1 27.740 8.019 .003
Financial incentive 16.772 1 16.772 4.848 .016
Seriousness 37.972 1 37.972 10.977 .000
Financial incentive 9 seriousness 10.938 1 10.938 3.162 .040
Error 249.075 72 3.459
Total 3594.000 80
Corrected total 393.550 79

Panel D: parameter estimates


Parameter Expected sign B Std. error

Intercept 3.056 1.489


Age .344 .174
Costs–benefits .003 .062
Ethics training .243 .088
Gender -1.284 .453
Financial incentive - -3.959 1.798
Seriousness ? .184 .170
Financial incentive 9 seriousness ? .389 .219

123
The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness…

Table 5 continued
Panel E: interaction between financial incentive and seriousness

The dependent variable is the participant’s response to the following question: How likely do you think it is that you would formally report
the fictitious revenue entries to the relevant external authority? Responses are captured using a nine-point scale anchored by 1 = very
unlikely and 9 = very likely. Age is the participant’s age. Costs–benefits is the difference between the participant’s response to the
questions asking the participants to assess the costs and benefits of reporting the fictitious revenue entries to the relevant external authority
using nine-point scales anchored by 1 = no cost (benefit) and 9 = very high cost (benefit). Ethics training is the amount of ethics-related
training participants had undertaken in their career using a nine-point scale anchored by 1 = no ethics training and 9 = a large amount of
ethics training. Gender is the participant’s gender. Financial incentive is a manipulated variable for financial incentive available or no
financial incentive available. Seriousness is a continuous variable based on the participant’s assessment of the seriousness of the
wrongdoing using a nine-point scale where 1 = not serious and 9 = very serious
R2 = .367 (adjusted R2 = .306)

Summary and Conclusions We also find a significant interaction between the provision
of a financial incentive and the perceived seriousness of the
Many jurisdictions have put regulatory strategies in place wrongdoing on the intention to report the financial
to provide incentives and safeguards to whistleblowers to reporting fraud to a relevant external authority. The
encourage whistleblowing on corporate wrongdoings. One intention to report the financial reporting fraud externally is
such strategy is the provision of a financial incentive to the higher when the level of perceived seriousness is higher,
whistleblower if the complaint leads to a successful regu- regardless of the availability of a financial incentive.
latory enforcement action against the offending organiza- However, when the perceived level of seriousness is lower,
tion. We conducted an experiment using professional the presence of financial incentive results in a higher
accountants as participants to examine whether such an intention to report the financial reporting fraud to a relevant
incentive encourages potential whistleblowers to report an external authority. These findings indicate that the impact
observed accounting fraud to a relevant external authority. of a financial incentive on accountants’ intention to
We also examine the effect of perceived seriousness of the whistleblow externally is moderated by perceptions of the
wrongdoing on accountants’ intention to whistleblow. We seriousness of the financial statement fraud. Given the
find that a financial incentive results in a higher intention to significant interaction effect between financial incentive
whistleblow to a relevant external authority. We also find and seriousness, the main effect of financial incentive on
that perceptions of the seriousness of the wrongdoing are the intention to report the financial reporting fraud exter-
significantly and positively associated with accountants’ nally needs to be carefully interpreted. The results imply
intention to whistleblow to a relevant external authority. that the influence of financial incentive on the intention to

123
P. Andon et al.

report a wrongdoing externally is subject to the perceived was not applicable in this research as all responses were
level of seriousness. made at the same time.
Our findings have important implications for regulatory Our data were collected in late 2014, which is after the
policy in relation to whistleblowing. They suggest that the implementation of the Dodd–Frank Act. As a result, the
imposition of incentives is likely to importantly increase experimental design involved treatments that require par-
the number of allegations, particularly in relation to inci- ticipants to assume actual legislative provisions pertaining
dents that may be regarded as of lower significance. This to financial incentives and anti-retaliation protection cur-
implies a stronger flow of potentially frivolous reports and rently operating in the US that were not in effect. We did
it is worth noting that the Office of the Whistleblower has however include clear instructions for all participants,
received 14,116 whistleblower tips9 but paid financial reinforced by robust manipulation check questions. Finally,
incentives (totaling $54 million) to only 22 whistleblowers we recognize the potentially confounding effects possible
between August 2011 and September 2015 (SEC 2015). as a consequence of the uneven and small sample sizes and
This increase in tip-offs has imposed a heavy and expen- the lack of variation observed in perceived seriousness
sive administrative load on the Office. However, a number measure, with 77.6 % of participants assessing the level of
of options are available to regulators to mitigate the risk of seriousness as either 8 or 9 on a 9-point scale.
frivolous claims including stiff penalties for bad faith or
frivolous reports, particularly for serial whistleblowers. Opportunities for Future Research
These include meaningful penalties in the form of signifi-
cant fines for frivolous or vexatious allegations that are More generally, although economically and socially criti-
clearly publicized and strong initial screening processes to cal, the decision to blow the whistle for financial statement
readily ascertain the likelihood of prosecution. On balance, fraud remains in many ways mysterious. Promising ave-
given the social and economic costs of financial statement nues for further research stem from the findings of this
fraud, we argue that the benefits of increasing the intention study. The first relates to further understanding of how
to whistleblow outweigh the additional costs of processing seriousness is perceived in the context of occupational
reports. fraud, and its interaction with incentives to whistleblow. In
this paper, we rely on a holistic assessment of seriousness
Limitations from participants. Additional analysis could manipulate
elements of harm (e.g., impacts of changes in the dollar
Potential limitations to our study arise because only pro- value of the offending) and moral wrongfulness (e.g., by
fessional accountants currently working in the US who manipulating details of the case such as the motivation for
registered their interest with Qualtrics Panels were inclu- the offending) to better pinpoint how incentives connect to
ded in the sample. As not all professional accountants specific elements of seriousness perception. Second, this
currently working in the US are registered with Qualtrics, study focused on the commission of financial statement
our sample of respondents may not be representative of the fraud—further research could look into the relevance of
population. Research has shown differences to exist in our findings in relation to other forms of occupational fraud
motivation for questionnaire participation among panelists and white-collar crime (e.g., forms of bribery and corrup-
(Albaum et al. 2014). Panelists who answer questions about tion). Third, this paper highlights the value of incentives in
a sensitive topic can be less motivated to participate in encouraging whistleblowing, but also alerts us to the
every motivational aspect except for incentives (other potential cost in terms of an increase in spurious reporting.
motivational aspects include interest, to give an opinion, Consequently, research into efficient and effective filtering
curiosity, enjoyment, helping, recognition, and obligation). screens that limit the cost to organizations and govern-
Given that some scenarios involved the respondent being ments of spurious whistleblowing would add to the findings
offered a financial incentive to report a wrongdoing, there presented in this paper.
may be a bias given that respondents could be predisposed
toward incentives. As such, it cannot be assumed that the Acknowledgments Funding for this study was received from Char-
tered Accountants Australia and New Zealand and the Accounting &
findings will be representative of the overall population of Finance Association of Australia and New Zealand.
professional accountants in the US. One of the standard
tests for non-response bias, early versus late respondents,
References

9 ACFE. (2016). Report to the Nations on Occupational Fraud and


The SEC received 3923 whistleblower tips in fiscal year 2015; 3620
Abuse: 2016 Global Fraud Study, Association of Certified Fraud
tips in 2014; 3238 tips in 2013; 3001 tips in 2012; and 334 tips in
Examiners.
2011 (SEC 2015).

123
The Impact of Financial Incentives and Perceptions of Seriousness…

Albaum, G., Roster, C. A., & Smith, S. M. (2014). Topic sensitivity Feldman, Y. (2011). The complexity of disentangling intrinsic and
and research design: Effects on internet survey respondents’ extrinsic compliance motivations: Theoretical and empirical
motives. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 26(1), insights from the behavioral analysis of law. Washington
147–161. University Journal of Law & Policy, 35(3), 10–51.
Andon, P., Free, C., & Scard, B. (2015). Pathways to accountant Feldman, Y., & Lobel, O. (2010). The incentives matrix: The comparative
fraud: Australian evidence and analysis. Accounting Research effectiveness of rewards, liabilities, duties, and protections for
Journal, 28(1), 10–44. reporting illegality. Texas Law Review, 88(6), 1151–1211.
Andrews, D., Nonnecke, B., & Preece, J. (2003). Electronic survey Ferguson, J. L., Ellen, P. S., & Bearden, W. O. (2014). Procedural and
methodology: A case study in reaching hard-to-involve internet distributive fairness: Determinants of overall price fairness.
users. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, Journal of Business Ethics, 121(2), 217–231.
16(2), 185–210. Freshfields Bruckhaus Deringer LLP. (2013). What makes a good
Atkinson, K. E., Jones, R. C., & Eduardo, M. (2012). Whistle whistleblowing policy and why it is important.
blowing, hotlines and the impacts of Sarbanes Oxley and Dodd– Friedrichs, D. O. (2002). Occupational crime, occupational deviance,
Frank. International Research Journal of Applied Finance, and workplace crime: Sorting out the difference. Criminal
3(12), 1731–1738. Justice, 2(3), 243–256.
Baker, R., Blumberg, S. J., Brick, J. M., Couper, M. P., Courtright, Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (2000). Rewarding competence:
M., Dennis, J. M., et al. (2010). Research synthesis: AAPOR The importance of goals in the study of intrinsic motivation. In
report on online panels. Public Opinion Quarterly, 74(4), C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic
711–781. motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance.
Braithwaite, J. (2013). Flipping markets to virtue with qui tam and San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
restorative justice. Accounting, Organizations and Society, Jubb, P. (1999). Whistle-blowing: a restrictive definition and inter-
38(6), 458–468. pretation. Journal of Business Ethics, 12(1), 77–94.
Brennan, N., & Kelly, J. (2007). A study of whistleblowing among Kaplan, S., Pany, K., Samuels, J., & Zhang, J. (2009). An examination
trainee auditors. The British Accounting Review, 39(1), 61–87. of the association between gender and reporting intentions for
Brink, A. G., Lowe, D. J., & Victoravich, L. M. (2013). The effect of fraudulent financial reporting. Journal of Business Ethics, 87(1),
evidence strength and internal rewards on intentions to report 15–30.
fraud in the Dodd–Frank regulatory environment. Auditing: A Kaplan, S. E., Pany, K., Samuels, J., & Zhang, J. (2012). An
Journal of Practice & Theory, 32(3), 87–104. examination of anonymous and non-anonymous fraud reporting
Bucy, P. H. (2002a). Information as a commodity in the regulatory channels. Advances in Accounting, incorporating Advances in
world. Houston Law Review, 39(4), 905–978. International Accounting, 28(1), 88–95.
Bucy, P. H. (2002b). Private justice. Southern California Law Review, Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American
76(1), 1–160. dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals.
Callahan, E. S., & Dworkin, T. M. (1992). Do good and get rich: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(3), 280–287.
Financial incentives for whistleblowing and the False Claims Keenan, J. P. (1990). Upper-level managers and whistleblowing:
Act. Villanova Law Review, 37(2), 273–336. Determinants of perceptions of company encouragement and
Clinard, M., & Yeager, P. (1983). Corporate crime. New York: Free information about where to blow the whistle. Journal of Business
Press. and Psychology, 5(2), 223–235.
Curtis, M. B., & Taylor, E. Z. (2009). Whistleblowing in public Liyanarachchi, G., & Adler, R. (2011). Accountants’ whistle-blowing
accounting: Influence of identity disclosure, situational context, intentions: The impact of retaliation, age and gender. Australian
and personal characteristics. Accounting and the Public Interest, Accounting Review, 21(2), 167–182.
9(1), 191–220. Liyanarachchi, G., & Newdick, C. (2009). The impact of moral
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999a). A meta-analytic reasoning and retaliation on whistle-blowing: New Zealand
review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards evidence. Journal of Business Ethics, 89(1), 37–57.
on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125(6), 627–668. Mesmer-Magnus, J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2005). Whistleblowing in
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999b). The undermining organizations: An examination of correlates of whistleblowing
effect is a reality after all—Extrinsic rewards, task interest, and intentions, actions and retaliation. Journal of Business Ethics,
self-determination: Reply to Eisenberger, Pierce, and Cameron 62(3), 277–297.
(1999) and Lepper, Henderlong, and Gingras (1990). Psycho- Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. (1992). Blowing the whistle: The
logical Bulletin, 125, 692–700. organizational and legal implications for companies and
Decker, W. H., & Calo, T. J. (2007). ‘Observers’ impressions of employees. Riverside, NJ: Lexington Books.
unethical persons and whistleblowers. Journal of Business Miceli, M. P., Near, J. P., & Dworkin, T. M. (2008). Whistle-blowing
Ethics, 76(3), 309–318. in organizations. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Dozier, J. B., & Miceli, M. P. (1985). Potential predictors of whistle Near, J. P., & Miceli, M. P. (1985). Organizational dissidence: the
blowing: A prosocial behavior perspective. The Academy of case of whistle-blowing. Journal of Business Ethics, 4(1), 1–16.
Management Review, 10(4), 823–836. Near, J. P., Rehg, M. T., Van Scotter, J. R., & Miceli, M. P. (2004).
Dyck, A., Morse, A., & Zingales, L. (2010). Who blows the whistle Does type of wrongdoing affect the whistle-blowing process.
on corporate fraud? The Journal of Finance, 65(6), 2213–2253. Business Ethics Quarterly, 14(2), 219–242.
Ethics Resource Center. (2013). National Business Ethics Survey OECD. (2012). Protection of whistleblowers: Study on whistleblower
2013, Ethics and Compliane Initiative. protection frameworks, compendium of best practices and
Fehr, E., & Falk, A. (2002). Psychological foundations of incentives. guiding principles for legislation. The Organisation for Eco-
European Economic Review, 46(4), 687–710. nomic Co-operation and Development.
Fekete, J. (2013). Whistleblowers will get cash rewards for helping Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, N. (2009). Instruc-
nab tax cheats. Retrieved July 24, 2015, from http://www.canada. tional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase
com/news/Whistleblowers?will?cash?rewards?helping?cheats/ statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
8133660/story.html. 45(4), 867–872.

123
P. Andon et al.

Perry, N. (1998). Indecent exposures: Theorizing whistleblowing. Seifert, D. L., Sweeney, J. T., Joireman, J., & Thornton, J. M. (2010).
Organization Studies, 19(2), 235–257. The influence of organizational justice on accountant whistle-
Pope, K. R., & Lee, C. C. (2013). Could the Dodd–Frank Wall Street blowing. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 35(7),
Reform and Consumer Protection Act of 2010 be helpful in 707–717.
reforming corporate America? An investigation of financial Taylor, E. Z., & Curtis, M. B. (2010). An examination of the layers of
bounties and whistle-blowing behaviors in the private sector. workplace influences in ethical judgments: Whistleblowing
Journal of Business Ethics, 112(4), 597–607. likelihood and perseverance in public accounting. Journal of
Porter, L. W., Lawler, E. E., & Hackman, J. R. (1976). Behavior in Business Ethics, 93(1), 21–37.
organizations. New York: McGraw-Hill. Taylor, E. Z., & Curtis, M. B. (2013). Whistleblowing in audit firms:
Poynter, R., & Comley, P. (2003). Beyond online panels. In: Organizational response and power distance. Behavioral
Proceedings of the ESOMAR technovate conference, Research in Accounting, 25(2), 21–43.
Amsterdam. Vadera, A. K., Aguilera, R. V., & Caza, B. B. (2009). Making sense
Rosoff, H., Siko, R., John, R., & Burns, W. J. (2013). Should I stay or of whistle-blowing’s antecedents: Learning from research on
should I go? An experimental study of health and economic identity and ethics program. Business Ethics Quarterly, 19(4),
government policies following a severe biological agent release. 553–586.
Environment Systems & Decisions, 33(1), 121–137. Wolf, B. (2011). Mexico sets money-laundering whistleblower
Schmidt, M. (2005). ‘‘Whistle blowing’’ regulation and accounting program. Retrieved October 8, 2015, from http://www.reuters.
standards enforcement in Germany and Europe—An economic com/article/2011/04/06/mexico-whistleblower-idUSN0620658
perspective. International Review of Law and Economics, 25(2), 120110406.
143–168. Wright, D. B., & Carlucci, M. E. (2011). The response order effect:
Schultz, J. J., Johnson, D., Morris, D., & Dyrnes, D. (1993). An People believe the first person who remembers an event.
investigation of the reporting of questionable acts in an Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 805–812.
international setting. Journal of Accounting Research, 31(Sup- Xu, Y., & Ziegenfuss, D. (2008). Reward systems, moral reasoning,
plement), 75–103. and internal auditors’ reporting wrongdoing. Journal of Business
SEC. (2015). 2015 Annual Report to Congress on the Dodd–Frank Psychology, 22(4), 323–331.
Whistleblower Program. US Securities and Exchange
Commission.

123

View publication stats

You might also like