Dylan Versoes

You might also like

Download as doc, pdf, or txt
Download as doc, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 4

What You See Is What You Get

Photofest

In “A Clockwork Orange,” Malcolm McDowell's character was forced to watch movies


without blinking.
By MANOHLA DARGIS
Published: July 8, 2011
Linkedin
Sign In to E-Mail
Print
Reprints
Share

IN “The Invisible Gorilla,” a book about what we see and what we think we see (it
came out in paperback in June), two cognitive psychologists, Christopher Chabris and
Daniel Simons, describe an experiment they performed with a chess grandmaster named
Patrick Wolff. They briefly showed him a diagram of a chess position from an “obscure
master game,” gave him a set of pieces and asked him to re-create the position on an
empty board from memory. He did so almost perfectly and then repeated his
performance.
Enlarge This Image

Berlin International Film Festival

Perhaps it's a lack of pattern recognition, not taste, that makes some people frustrated
with Bela Tarr movies like “Turin Horse.”
Enlarge This Image

Film still from Paramount Vantage; Analysis by Tim J. Smith

Researchers used kinder methods to track where viewers' eyes settled during a scene
from “There Will Be Blood.”
Enlarge This Image

Enlarge This Image

“By recognizing familiar patterns,” they wrote, “he stuffed not one but several pieces
into each of his memory slots.” Perhaps surprisingly, he couldn’t do the same with
random arrangements on the board: “His memory was no better than that of a beginner,
because his chess expertise and database of patterns were of little help.”

Recognizing patterns is part of the film critic’s tool kit along with a good pen to take
notes in the dark. You have to take in a lot of information when you watch a movie just
once. The easy stuff is usually the story (boy meets girl) and characters (Romeo and
Juliet). The tricky part, when I get to scribbling, is everything else, including how the
boy and girl met and what happened next. (That’s the plot.) Was the lighting soft or
hard, the editing fast or slow, the camera shaky or smooth, the acting broad or not?
Also: Did they dance like Fred and Ginger, shoot like Angelina and Brad? Was it a
musical (but funny) or a comedy (with dancing)? Mostly, how does the narrative work?

Moviegoers fed a strict Hollywood diet may find themselves squirming through, say, a
film by the Hungarian director Bela Tarr less because of the subtitles than because of the
long takes during which little is explained. The same may hold true for those who watch
“The Tree of Life” and want Terrence Malick to connect the dots overtly among his
characters, the dinosaurs and the trippy space images. Other moviegoers may just go
with the flow. They, like critics — who ideally are open to different types of narratives,
having watched nonmainstream, sometimes difficult cinema in school, at festivals, for
pleasure and for work — may have developed specific cognitive habits.

People walk and talk in movies like Mr. Tarr’s “Werckmeister Harmonies” (2000) but
not necessarily in ways that many moviegoers may immediately understand; the films
don’t conform to familiar type. Classical Hollywood, in the theorist David Bordwell’s
wonderful phrase, is “an excessively obvious cinema.” Obvious if complex, as laid out
in “The Classical Hollywood Cinema: Film Style & Mode of Production to 1960,” the
landmark 1985 book he wrote with Janet Staiger and Kristin Thompson in which that
phrase appears. As its title suggests, this tour de force makes the case that classical
Hollywood (1917-60) employed specific stylistic techniques (like point of view and
“invisible” editing) and arranged narrative logic, time and space in a particular way so
we could understand movies.

In the years since, Mr. Bordwell has shifted gears somewhat, and he and another
collaborator, Noel Carroll, became instrumental in introducing cognitive theory into
cinema studies. It’s impossible not to see value in work that examines how movies make
sense to us: cognitive studies remind you that we actively make meaning of our visual
world, that we take in information, fragments of reality, and process them to understand
it — and the films — before us.

The experiment that gives “The Invisible Gorilla” its title reveals something about how
much we see and don’t. More than a decade ago Professors Chabris and Simons made a
short film in which players passed around basketballs. They then asked viewers silently
to count the number of passes made by the players in white and ignore those made by
the players in black. That sounds unremarkable except that about midway through, a
woman in a gorilla suit walked in, faced the camera and pounded her chest before
exiting. She was on the scene for nine whopping seconds but only about half the people
watching the video saw the gorilla: they expected to see the players passing the ball, and
that’s all they saw.

“We think we should see anything in front of us,” Professors Chabris and Simons write,
“but in fact we are aware of only a small portion of our visual world at any moment.”

The viewers who didn’t see the gorilla suit made an error in perception that
psychologists call “inattentional blindness.” As the professors write, “When people
devote their attention to a particular area or aspect of their visual world, they tend not to
notice unexpected objects, even when those unexpected objects are salient, potentially
important and appear right where they are looking.” We are, they write, subject to all
kinds of everyday illusions, including those produced by another phenomenon, “change
blindness.” This is when you don’t notice obvious changes, like continuity flubs.
Professors Chabris and Simons even quote a script supervisor, who despite her
profession doesn’t always notice such errors when she’s watching a film because “the
more into the story I am, the less I notice things that are out of continuity.”

The stronger the pull a narrative has on us, the more we’re hooked. If we don’t notice
some disruptions — like the crew member wearing shades on deck near Johnny Depp in
the first “Pirates of the Caribbean” movie — it’s probably because we’re busy following
the action or watching Mr. Depp’s face. (We’re hard-wired to respond to faces.)
Filmmakers use numerous strategies to keep us watching. As Mr. Bordwell recently
wrote on his blog, davidbordwell.net, “perceptually, films are illusions, not reality;
cognitively, they are not the blooming, buzzing confusion of life but rather simplified
ensembles of elements, designed to be understood.” Both the real world and earlier
movies we’ve seen teach us how to look at films: we look at movies and understand
them through their norms.

Filmmakers employ an arsenal of narrative strategies to hook and keep your attention.
In February a psychological researcher in Britain, Tim Smith, posted an experiment on
Mr. Bordwell’s blog that illustrated how a filmmaker can focus your gaze. Using an eye-
tracking technology to trace the movements of pupils (when they’re somewhat fixed or
darting about), Dr. Smith was able to map what viewers looked at when they watched a
somewhat static interlude from Paul Thomas Anderson’s “There Will Be Blood” (2007).

“Viewers think they are free to look where they want,” Dr. Smith writes, “but, due to
the subtle influence of the director and actors, where they want to look is also where the
director wants them to look.”

What happens, though, if a director doesn’t direct your gaze in familiar ways, shuns
classic compositions on the one hand or fast cuts and close-ups on the other, plays with
or disrupts narrative norms? What happens to even those enthusiastic moviegoers who
— much like that chess master who was able to re-create chess pieces from memory
because he recognized familiar patterns — know how Hollywood movies work? Maybe
some moviegoers who reject difficult films don’t, like the chess master who didn’t
recognize random positions, have the necessary expertise and database patterns to
understand (or stick with) these movies. When they watch them, they’re effectively
(frustrated) beginners and don’t like that feeling.

I e-mailed Mr. Bordwell to ask what he thought about the idea that at least part of the
difficulty some viewers have with some films may be a matter of habits of cognition
and visual perception. “Narrative is our ultimate top-down strategy in watching a
movie,” he wrote back, “specifically, I think, classical narrative principles.”

The narrative keeps us watching, in other words. But,“when nothing is happening, or


when the shot is distant or prolonged — we can’t so easily apply our narrative
schemas,” he continued. “If you don’t have other schemas in your mental kit, your
perception is just lost. As you suggest, the viewer has to retune her perception.
“Once you do, if the filmmaker is skillful, all kinds of stuff open up. To me Bela Tarr
movies have tremendous suspense! It’s like learning to enjoy brushwork in an abstract
painting.”

Not everyone is open to abstract painting or Mr. Tarr’s long, beautiful films, but
perhaps some of this resistance is fueled by cognitive habit rather than so-called taste. A
friend who likes to show children avant-garde films at a cinematheque he helps run says
that when he screens Bruce Baillie’s transporting short “All My Life” (1966), some of
them stand up and sway along, some even clap. It’s a short film, just a three-minute pan
of a flower-draped fence, and the children seem to bliss out on the images of the blooms
and sky, and the purity of the Ella Fitzgerald song that gives the movie its title. (That is
until another child mocks them.)

The children who love the film may not understand it, but they embrace it. And if their
eyes remain open, in time they may not just dance to unfamiliar films, but also find
pleasure in unlocking their meanings.

A version of this article appeared in print on July 10, 2011, on page AR13 of the New
York edition with the headline: What You See Is What You Get.

You might also like