Journal Estetica Dental

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

RESEARCH AND EDUCATION

Influence of light source, polarization, education, and training


on shade matching quality
Jacqueline A. Clary, DMD, MSD,a Joe C. Ontiveros, DDS, MS,b Stanley G. Cron, MSPH,c and
Rade D. Paravina, DDS, MS, PhDd

Color appearance is an impor- ABSTRACT


tant parameter for patient Statement of problem. Many factors influence the quality of shade selection, and isolating how
acceptance of dental restora- significantly each of these factors influences results is difficult.
tions. The esthetic demands
Purpose. The purpose of this in vitro study was to compare results of shade matching using
of patients and dental pro-
handheld lights with or without a polarizing filter with results obtained using a professional viewing
fessionals have additionally booth and to analyze the influence of education and training on shade selection outcome.
elevated the importance of
accurate shade selection. Pub- Material and methods. A total of 96 third-year dental students (evaluators) were randomly
separated into 4 groups. Each group was assigned 1 of 2 handheld shade-matching devices
lished studies cite several sour-
(lights) with or without a polarizing filter. Each group performed a shade matching exercise
ces of shade matching errors, using the handheld light or a professional viewing booth. The exercise consisted of matching
some controlled by the oper- shade tabs placed in a typodont to a commercial shade guide. Each group repeated this
ator and others out of the procedure 4 times over a 9-week period. A lecture on shade matching was presented at the fifth
operator’s control.1-3 These week of the study, between “before” and “after” shade matching procedures.
sources include shade match- Results. Shade matching scores with handheld lights (7.8) were higher than scores of shade
ing conditions (starting from matching with the viewing booth (7.2). The mean scores for before (7.2) and after (7.8) shade
the lighting), methods and matching (with education and training in between) were significantly different. The combined ef-
tools, shade guides, education fect of light and education and training improved the shade matching score by 1.2, from 6.8 in the
and training, color deficiency, before sessions using the viewing booth to 8.0 in the after sessions using handheld lights.
A 21% increase in the number of evaluators who selected 1 of 4 best matches was recorded, 10%
and numerous physiological
for handheld lights versus viewing booth after education and training versus before sessions and
and psychological factors (fa- 11% between after sessions using handheld lights versus before sessions using viewing booth.
tigue, nutrition, medications).
Clinicians must pay attention Conclusions. Within the limits of the study, the shade matching scores with handheld lights
were significantly better than the results obtained using a viewing booth (P<.01). Using a
to shade matching variables,
handheld light with or without a polarizing filter did not influence shade matching results.
such as an accurate and repro- Mean shade matching scores were significantly better after education and training (P<.01).
ducible light sources and color Light combined with education and training resulted in the greatest increase in shade
education and training. matching quality. (J Prosthet Dent 2016;-:---)
Shade matching should be
performed using standardized ideal daylight conditions sought because of their universal nature and broad
with a correlated color temperature range of 5000 K spectrum of wavelengths.1,4-6 However, these conditions
(D50) to 7500 K (D75).4 These color temperatures are are rare in natural lighting. The color temperature of

a
Resident, Department of Restorative Dentistry and Prosthodontics, University of Texas School of Dentistry at Houston, Houston, Texas.
b
Professor, Department of Restorative Dentistry and Prosthodontics, University of Texas School of Dentistry at Houston, Houston, Texas.
c
Instructor, Research Center for Nursing Research, School of Nursing, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas.
d
Professor, Department of Restorative Dentistry and Prosthodontics, and Director, Houston Center for Biomaterials and Biomimetics, University of Texas School
of Dentistry at Houston, Houston, Texas.

THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY 1


2 Volume - Issue -

Clinical Implications
The importance of color appearance in the esthetics
of dental restorations highlights the need for
accurate, high-quality shade matching. Comparison
of results obtained with 2 different handheld
shade-matching lights (with or without a polarizing
filter) to a gold standard control strongly suggested
the suitability of these handheld lights for clinical
use. Furthermore, completion of an online color
education and training program significantly
improved shade matching quality.

Figure 1. Evaluator performing shade matching in viewing booth using


daylight is always changing and can range from 1000 K to Vita Linearguide 3D-Master shade guide.
20 000 K.2 This makes it difficult to rely on natural
daylight to provide the “ideal” color temperature when
influenced shade matching results. The null hypotheses
the dental healthcare provider is selecting a shade for the
were that shade matching results would not be influ-
restoration. Adding to this, research has demonstrated
enced by the type of light or education and training.
that natural light does not necessarily produce better
results than color-corrected artificial light.1,5,7 Handheld
MATERIAL AND METHODS
lights designed specifically for color matching in dentistry
are available and may be a better option than completer A total of 96 evaluators, third-year dental students of the
reliance on natural daylight or office lighting.8,9 University of Texas School of Dentistry at Houston,
Education and training have been found to affect participated in the study. Committee for the Protection of
shade selection quality.3,6,10 Color matching, like many Human Subjects and institutional review board approval
other aspects of dentistry, requires a fine skill and should were obtained. A professional viewing booth (Judge II;
be practiced regularly. There is a difference between GretagMacbeth) was used as the control (Fig. 1). Two
experience and education and training in dental color handheld devices for visual shade matching (color-
matching. It has been reported that senior dental stu- matching lights) were used (Rite-Lite 2; AdDent; and
dents are expected to be more successful because of their SmileLite; Smile Line USA), each with and without a
gradually increasing knowledge and clinical experi- detachable polarizing filter (Fig. 2). Student evaluators
ence.11,12 Probably a combination of dental education were divided into 4 equal groups, each consisting of 25%
and training produced better shade matching results. of the class in alphabetical order. Color matching lights
Traditionally, women are considered more able to select were randomized by groups via coin flip to group I, Rite-
shades accurately than men can.5,13-17 However, a Lite 2 (without polarizing filter) (RL); group II, Rite-Lite 2
number of studies have identified no significant influence (with polarizing filter) (RLP); group III, SmileLite
of sex on shade matching results.4,16,18 (without polarizing filter) (SL); and group IV, SmileLite
The results of visual shade matching using color (with polarizing filter) (SLP). According to manufac-
measuring devices must be evaluated as the reproduc- turer’s instructions, the evaluators were instructed to
ibility of instrumental shade matching has been reported hold the SmileLite 10 cm away from the tooth on which
to be much higher than that of standardized visual they were performing the shade selection and 5 cm away
methods.19 Information on instrumental color differences when using the Rite-Lite 2. One of 4 shade tabs (task
also enables comparison with visual thresholds (percep- tabs): B1, A2, A3, and A4 (Vita classical; Vita Zahnfabrik)
tibility and acceptability threshold),20-22 which is important was positioned at the maxillary central incisor socket of a
for the interpretation of the results of visual shade typodont with adjacent anterior teeth removed from the
matching. Finally, colorimetric findings allow dental typodont. A gray background was used in all situations.
shade guides to be compared; selecting and using the The evaluators used another shade guide (Vita Line-
shade guide with the smallest coverage error (corresponds arguide 3D Master; Vita Zahnfabrik) to match the shade
to the best color match to natural teeth) is best.23-29 of each of the 4 task tabs. The order of shade selection is
The purpose of this study was to determine whether presented in Table 1. In the sessions before education
use handheld lights with or without a polarizing filter and training, half of the evaluators performed the exer-
influenced shade matching results more than using a cise using the viewing booth (VB) first and the handheld
viewing booth and whether education and training lights (HH) second, while the other half of the evaluators

THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY Clary et al


- 2016 3

Figure 2. Handheld shade-matching lights: A, Rite-Lite without polarizing filter. B, Rite-Lite with polarizing filter. C, Smilelite without polarizing filter.
D, Smilelite with polarizing filter.

performed the exercise in the reverse order. The evalu- Table 1. Order of shade selection with handheld lights and viewing
ators had an opposite order of shade-matching lights in booth
the sessions after education and training. Every student Week Group/Task Group/Task

performed shade matching a total of 16 times; there were 1 I/RL III/VB


2 II/RLP IV/VB
4 shade matchings in each of four sessions, 8 before and
3 III/SL I/VB
8 after education and training.
4 IV/SLP II/VB
Education and training included a lecture on the ba-
5 Lecture on color and color training
sics of color in dentistry and clinical color matching. 6 I/VB III/SL
During the week after the lecture (and before continuing 7 II/VB IV/SLP
with the “after” part of the experiment), all evaluators 8 III/VB I/RL
completed Dental Color Matcher (www.scadent.org), an 9 IV/VB II/RLP
online education and training program for esthetic Group I/RL: Rite-Lite 2 without polarizing filter; Group II/RLP: Rite-Lite 2 with polarizing filter;
dentistry. The training program included color vision Group III/SL: SmileLite without polarizing filter; Group IV/SLP: SmileLite with polarizing filter;
VB, viewing booth.
screening (normal versus deficient), a lecture with in-
formation on color in dentistry (light source parameters,
metamerism, surrounding colors, time and length for
shade selection, patient position, translucency/trans- differences in color (DE*ab) were calculated as24:
parency, and other topics), a variety of color matching DE*ab = x[(DL*2) + (Da*2) + (Db*2)]1/2.
exercises, and a 12-question quiz at the end of the pro- The smallest DE*ab value was assigned 10 points (best
gram. For numerical calculation in the experiment, an match), the second highest was assigned 9 points (sec-
ordinal score was assigned to shade-matching perfor- ond best match), and so on, until 1 point for the tenth
mance based on color differences between the task tab best match. Selections of shades worse than the tenth
and the chosen commercial shade guide tab. The CIELab best match were assigned 0 points.

Clary et al THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY


4 Volume - Issue -

Table 2. Mean (±SD) color differences between CIELAB (DE*ab) and Table 3. Estimated mean ±SE values for shade-matching lights,
CIEDE2000 (DE00) for best match (10 points), second best match (9 education, and training
points), and up to tenth best match (1 point)a Effect Estimate Standard Error
Points DE*ab DE00 Shade-matching light
10 1.4 (0.7) 1.2 (0.8) VB 7.2 0.1
9 2.6 (0.3) 1.8 (0.3) HH 7.8 0.1
8 3.3 (0.5) 2.3 (0.8) Education and training
7 3.6 (0.4) 2.2 (0.5) Before 7.2 0.1
6 4.7 (1.5) 3.4 (1.5) After 7.8 0.1
5 4.8 (1.5) 3.1 (1.2) Sex
4 5.4 (1.2) 3.5 (0.9) Women 7.5 0.1
3 5.5 (1.1) 4.1 (1.4) Men 7.5 0.1
2 6.4 (1.3) 4.5 (1.0) Group
1 6.9 (1.5) 4.9 (0.8) I 7.5 0.2
a
CIEDE2000 differences were calculated based on CIELab order of matches. II 7.5 0.2
III 7.5 0.2
IV 7.5 0.2
In addition, color differences between the task tabs Shade Tab
and matched tabs were calculated according to a more A2 7.1 0.1
recent and advanced CIEDE2000 formula, as follows25: A3 7.4 0.1
sffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi A4 7.6 0.1
     
DL0 2 DC0 2 DH 0 2 DC0 DH 0 B1 7.7 0.1
DE00 = + +RT
KL S L KC S C KH SH KC S C K H S H HH, handheld; VB, viewing booth.

Results were analyzed using SAS version 9.4 soft-


ware (SAS) for Windows (Microsoft). Repeated mea-
matching scores in Table 4. Estimated mean scores of
sures analyses with linear mixed models were used to
shade matching results with handheld lights were 7.8
compare shade-matching performance scores across
(0.1) and in the viewing booth 7.2 (0.1), with those of the
multiple student observations. Model residuals
handheld lights being significantly higher (P<.01). Dif-
confirmed that the scores approximated a normal dis-
ferences between handheld lights and viewing booth
tribution. The main effects included in the first model
scores did not vary significantly by group (P=.15).
were light (handheld and viewing booth), education
The estimated mean scores of shade matching results
and training (before and after), group, and shade tab
for before and after shade matching (with education and
(B1, A2, A3, and A4). Interaction terms were added to
training in between) were 7.2 (0.1) and 7.8 (0.1),
the model to test if any differences by repeated effects
respectively. The after score was found to be significantly
varied by group. Further models tested for the effect of
higher (P<.01). The before and after education and
sex, the interactions of sex with group, and the repeated
training scores did not vary significantly by group
effects.
(P=.41).
To supplement descriptive and analytical statistics
The combined effect of light and education and
and provide clinically relevant perspective on the re-
training improved the shade matching score by 1.2, from
sults, numeric color differences between task tabs as
6.8 in the before sessions using the viewing booth to 8.0
selected matches were interpreted through a 50:50%
in the after sessions using handheld lights.
perceptibility threshold of DE*ab=1.2 and DE00=0.8 and
A 10% increase was recorded in the number of
a 50:50% acceptability threshold of DE*ab=2.7 and
evaluators who selected 1 of 4 best matches using
DE00=1.8.20,21
handheld lights versus a viewing booth, whereas an
11% increase was recorded for after education and
RESULTS
training versus before (Table 5). Finally, there was a
Mean (±SD) color differences in CIELab (DE*ab) and 21% increase in the number of evaluators who selected
CIEDE2000 (DE00) for best match (10 points), second 1 of 4 best matches in after sessions using handheld
best match (9 points), up to the tenth best match (1 lights than in before sessions using the viewing booth
point) are presented in Table 2. The correlation between (Table 5).
color differences CIELab and CIEDE2000 was R=.986. No significant overall differences in shade matching
The correlation between visual scores and color differ- results by sex were recorded (P=.98): women = 7.5 (0.1),
ences was R=.985 for DE*ab and R=.982 for DE00. men = 7.5 (0.1). Differences between viewing booth and
Estimated mean (±standard error [SE]) scores are handheld lights were not found to vary by sex (P=.06).
presented in Table 3, and the model for predicting shade Although differences between shade tab scores were

THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY Clary et al


- 2016 5

Table 4. Model for prediction of shade matching scores Table 5. Percentage of participants who selected best shade match
Effect Num DF Den DF F P Pointsa VB HH B A B/VB B/HH A/VB A/HH
Light 1 1414 24.94 <.01 10 21.6 27.3 22.5 26.4 20.1 25.0 23.2 29.7
Education and Training 1 1414 24.31 <.01 9 22.4 30.7 25.4 27.7 18.5 32.3 26.3 29.2
Sex 1 88 0.00 .99 8 15.4 11.5 12.5 14.3 14.8 10.2 15.9 12.8
Group 3 88 0.02 1.00 7 4.4 4.2 3.0 5.6 3.4 2.6 5.5 5.7
Target Tab 3 1414 4.02 .01 6 7.9 6.1 7.3 6.8 8.3 6.3 7.6 6.0

Den DF, denominator degrees of freedom; F, F value; Num DF, numerator degrees of freedom. 5 4.2 3.9 4.0 4.0 4.4 3.6 3.9 4.2
4 14.2 9.4 13.8 9.8 16.7 10.9 11.7 7.8
3 5.1 2.9 5.3 2.6 7.3 3.4 2.9 2.3
found to vary by group (P=.04), this interaction did not 2 1.7 2.5 2.7 1.4 2.3 3.1 1.0 1.8

significantly vary by sex (P=.38). 1 2.9 1.6 3.4 1.0 4.2 2.6 1.6 0.5

A, after education; A/HH, handheld results after education; A/VB, viewing booth results after
education; B, before education; B/HH, handheld results before education; B/VB, viewing booth
DISCUSSION results before education; HH, handheld; VB, viewing booth.
a
The smallest DE*ab value was assigned 10 points (best match), the second highest was
The null hypotheses that the type of light and edu- assigned 9 points (second best match), and so on, until 1 point for the tenth best match.
cation and training would not influence the shade
matching results were rejected; the null hypothesis
regarding the influence of sex was not rejected. The affordability. Further research might evaluate the in-
objective of the study was to evaluate the effect that fluence of different lights and polarizing filters on the
different light sources, polarization, education and shade selection of natural teeth because the optical
training, and sex have on shade matching results. properties of enamel and dentin are different from
Several studies reported that clinicians were inconsis- those of the shade tabs.
tent in shade matching.2,5,6,14 Factors such as age, Education and training on color matching plays an
experience, eye fatigue, and physiological conditions important role in shade matching accuracy. Previous
like deficient color vision may lead to bias and in- studies have concluded that dental care professionals
consistencies.1,15 In this study, data were collected need to participate in hands-on courses, continuing
from third year predoctoral dental students. Dental education classes, and other training programs.3,6,14
students are an appropriate group for shade matching Comparisons between the percentages of evaluators
research as the majority are generally young, healthy who chose 1 of the 4 best matches were performed,
adults without medical disorders that can affect their because the mean scores ranged from 7 to 8, which
shade matching competency.5 Target tabs A2 A2, A3, corresponded to the third and fourth best match, and
and A4 were selected because of their frequency in the because the majority of evaluators (63% to 74%)
population and their variety in lightness (medium- selected 1 of the 4 best matches. Data support previous
light, medium-dark, and dark).26 Finally, tab B1 was findings that color matching results are improved by
selected because it is a light shade and therefore the education and training.14 More research should be
most desirable shade for patients.16 conducted in the area of retraining with standardized
Shade selection should be performed under ideal exercises, as merely shade matching on patients daily
natural light conditions.4 These conditions are difficult does not necessarily improve or maintain skills. Com-
to achieve. Previous research has shown that shade puter programs that engage the participant could also
matching results are better with artificial color-corrected be a way of improving education for many dental
light than under natural light.1 Natural daylight was healthcare providers.
not used as the control because it is too variable and This study supports previous research that reported
unreliable.2 In this study, handheld lights were found that men and women overall scored the same when it
to be better than the control (a statically significant comes to quality of color matching.14 Sex differences
improvement of 7.8 compared with 7.2, respectively). across the various shade tabs were similar among the
One possible explanation for this is that the evaluators 4 groups. The use of different handheld lights with
were focused solely and directly on the shade matching and without polarizing filters did not affect the results by
area observed through a small window of the handheld sex.
light. This could have reduced background distraction The greatest improvement in shade matching quality
and increased focus on the shade-matching exercise. was recorded when light was combined with education
These are clinically favorable results (additionally, and training. When these parameters were evaluated
having a viewing booth in a clinical setting is not together, the average match with a handheld light after
practical). The advantages of the evaluated handheld education was 8.0 compared with 6.8 seen with the
lights compared with other available shade-matching viewing booth before education. The same is true for
aids include their portability, user friendliness, and the percentage of evaluators who selected one of the

Clary et al THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY


6 Volume - Issue -

4 best matches (7-10 points), with a 21% improvement CONCLUSIONS


shown with the handheld light after education com-
Within the limits of this in vitro study, the following
pared with the viewing booth before education. These
conclusions were drawn:
data should motivate clinicians to incorporate color-
corrected light sources into their practices, seek out 1. Mean shade matching scores with handheld lights
proper education on how to match shades, and take were significantly better than the results obtained
an active role in applying their knowledge. With this using a viewing booth (P<.01). Polarizing filters did
proactive approach, clinicians can achieve more esthetic not influence the shade matching results.
restorations and have fewer costly remakes that can 2. Mean shade matching scores were significantly
require additional chairside time and affect patient better after education and training (P<.01).
satisfaction. 3. Light combined with education and training
There is no exact match in clinical shade matching, increased the quality of shade matching.
and this scenario was simulated in the experimental
design of this study. The term “coverage error”
(DECOV) represents the mean of best matches between
REFERENCES
a set of natural teeth and the tabs of the evaluated
shade guides. Studies of shade range coverage re- 1. Joiner A. Tooth colour: a review of the literature. J Dent 2004;32(suppl 1):
3-12.
ported that the coverage error of all dental shade 2. Culpepper WD. A comparative study of shade-matching procedures.
guides was greater than the 50:50% acceptability J Prosthet Dent 1970;24:166-73.
threshold of DE*ab =2.7,20,21 the only exception being a 3. Okubo SR, Kanawati A, Richards MW, Childress S. Evaluation of visual and
instrument shade matching. J Prosthet Dent 1998;80:642-8.
study that reported an DECOV of 2.7 for Vita 3D 4. Paravina RD. Evaluation of a newly developed visual shade-matching
apparatus. Int J Prosthodont 2002;15:528-34.
Master,27 and that Vita 3D Master had the smallest 5. Curd FM, Jasinevicius TR, Graves A, Cox V, Sadan A. Comparison of the
DECOV value of all shade guides, meaning that it shade matching ability of dental students using two light sources. J Prosthet
Dent 2006;96:391-6.
matches the color of human teeth the best. This is why 6. Hammad IA. Intrarater repeatability of shade selections with two shade
Vita Linearguide 3D-Master was selected as the shade guides. J Prosthet Dent 2003;89:50-3.
matching tool in this study, in which the first and 7. Brewer JD, Wee A, Seghi R. Advances in color matching. Dent Clin North
Am 2004;48:341-58.
second best matches (10 and 9 points, respectively) 8. AdDent, Inc. Rite-lite 2 shade matching unit. Updated 2013. Available at:
www.addent.com/rite-lite-2/. Accessed November 2014.
were at or below this threshold in both CIELab and 9. Smile Line USA. Smile litedshade matching for the specialist. Updated 2014.
CIEDE2000. Only the best matches for task tabs A2 Available at: smilelineusa.com/smile-lite-shade-matching-for-the-specialist.
Accessed November 2014.
and A3 were lower than the CIELab perceptibility 10. Paravina RD, O’Neill PN, Swift EJ, Nathanson D, Goodacre CJ. Teaching of
threshold of 1.2. This is a major difference from color in predoctoral and postdoctoral dental education in 2009. J Dent
traditional colorimetric studies where DE*ab can 2010;38(suppl 2):e34-40.
11. Paul S, Peter A, Pietrobon N, Hammerle CH. Visual and spectrophotometric
approach or equal a score of 0. shade analysis of human teeth. J Dent Res 2002;81:578-82.
12. Capa N, Malkondu O, Kazazoglu E, Calikkocaoglu S. Evaluating factors that
This study tried to simulate a clinical procedure and affect the shade-matching ability of dentists, dental staff members and
determine shade matching success using terms under- laypeople. J Am Dent Assoc 2010;141:71-6.
13. Donahue JL, Goodkind RJ, Schwabacher WB, Aeppli DP. Shade color
standable and meaningful to dental clinicians (best discrimination by men and women. J Prosthet Dent 1991;65:699-703.
match, second best match, and so on.). An additional 14. Haddad HJ, Jakstat HA, Arnetzl G, Borbely J, Vichi A, Dumfahrt H, et al.
Does sex and experience influence shade matching quality? J Dent
reason for this was that large differences among the 2009;37(suppl 1):e40-4.
corresponding matches were recorded in this study; for 15. Winkler S, Boberick KG, Weitz KS, Datikashvili I, Wood R. Shade matching
by dental students. J Oral Implantol 2006;32:256-8.
example, color differences for 4 best Vita 3D-Master 16. Hassel AJ, Koke U, Schmitter M, Beck J, Rammelsberg P. Clinical effect of
matches to task tabs were 2.2, 0.6, 1.0, and 1.6. Many different shade guide systems on the tooth shades of ceramic-veneered
similar discrepancies were found in DE*ab values. Also, restorations. Int J Prosthodont 2005;18:422-6.
17. McMaugh DR. A comparative analysis of the colour matching ability of
the sixth best match for one task tab had smaller DE*ab dentists, dental students, and ceramic technicians. Aust Dent J 1977;22:
165-7.
than the third best match for another task tab. In terms 18. Lagouvardos PE, Diamanti H, Polyzois G. Effect of individual shades on
of clinical realities, shade matching was more “suc- reliability and validity of observers in colour matching. Eur J Prosthodont
Restor Dent 2004;12:51-6.
cessful” in the latter case, as evaluators had only 2 19. Kröger E, Matz S, Dekiff M, Tran BL, Figgener L, Dirksen D. In vitro com-
better options in the given circumstances. Therefore, parison of instrumental and visual tooth shade determination under different
the usage of DE*ab values alone could have been illuminants. J Prosthet Dent 2015;114:848-55.
20. Paravina RD, Ghinea R, Herrera LJ, Della Bona A, Igiel C, Linninger M, et al.
misleading in this experiment. It was concluded that the Color difference thresholds in dentistry. J Esthet Restor Dent 2015;
27(suppl 1):S1-9.
most appropriate way of expressing the outcomes of 21. Ragain JC, Johnston WM. Color acceptance of direct dental restorative ma-
this study was to use visual scores as the primary terials by human observers. Col Res Appl 2000;25:278-85.
22. Chu SJ, Trushkowsky RD, Paravina RD. Dental color matching instruments
parameter. These scores were associated with numerical and systems. Review of clinical and research aspects. J Dent 2010;38(suppl 2):
color differences, which enabled comparisons with e2-16.
23. Li Q, Yu H, Wang YN. In vivo spectroradiometric evaluation of colour
other studies and interpretation with and through vi- matching errors among five shade guides. J Oral Rehabil 2009;36:
sual thresholds. 65-70.

THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY Clary et al


- 2016 7

24. Cocking C, Cevirgen E, Helling S, Oswald M, Corcodel N, Rammelsberg P, 29. Sharma G, Wu W, Dalal E. The CIEDE2000 color-difference formula:
et al. Colour compatibility between teeth and dental shade guides Implementation notes, supplementary test data, and mathematical obser-
in quinquagenarians and septuagenarians. J Oral Rehabil 2009;36: vations. Color Res Appl 2005;30:21-30.
848-55.
25. Bayindir F, Kuo S, Johnston WM, Wee AG. Coverage error of three Corresponding author:
conceptually different shade guide systems to vital unrestored dentition. Dr Rade D. Paravina
J Prosthet Dent 2007;98:175-85. 7500 Cambridge St
26. Paravina RD, Majkic G, Imai FH, Powers JM. Optimization of tooth color and Houston, TX 77054
shade guide design. J Prosthodont 2007;16:269-76. Email: rade.paravina@uth.tmc.edu
27. Analoui M, Papkosta E, Cochran M, Matis B. Designing visually optimal
shade guides. J Prosthet Dent 2004;92:371-6. Acknowledgments
28. Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage. Colorimetry: technical report. The authors thank Bharat Rana, BDS, for help with data collection.
CIE publication no. 15. 3rd ed. Vienna: Bureau Central de la CIE;
2004. Copyright © 2016 by the Editorial Council for The Journal of Prosthetic Dentistry.

Clary et al THE JOURNAL OF PROSTHETIC DENTISTRY

You might also like