Metaanalysis Donation Organ

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Original Clinical ScienceçGeneral

Community-Based Interventions and Individuals'


Willingness to be a Deceased Organ Donor:
Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Andrew T. Li,1,2,3 Germaine Wong,2,3,4 Michelle Irving,2,3 Stephen Jan,5 Allison Tong,2,3 Angelique F. Ralph,2,3
and Kirsten Howard2,6

Background. Widespread in-principle community support for organ donation does not necessarily translate to individuals be-
coming organ donors after death. Previous studies have identified factors that influence individuals' decisions to become organ
donors, which may be effectively targeted by interventions. We aimed to describe and evaluate the effectiveness of community-
based interventions to increase the willingness of individuals to be a deceased organ donor. Methods. We systematically reviewed
all randomized controlled trials (RCTs), non-RCTs (NRCTs), and before-after studies that assessed the impact of interventions on
increasing the willingness to be a deceased organ donor (measured as commitment to donate and/or intention to donate). We
searched MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, and CINAHL, without language restriction, to December 2013 and the reference lists
of the included articles. We conducted a risk of bias assessment using the Cochrane risk of bias tools and assessed confidence
in the evidence using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation framework. Results. We
identified 63 studies (11 RCTs, 8 cluster-RCTs, 4 NRCTs, 8 cluster-NRCTs, 27 before-after studies) with over 170000 participants.
Overall, the quality of the evidence was low. Participants who received a broad range of community-based interventions were more
likely to commit as donors (7 cluster-RCTs; 6015 participants; relative risk, 1.70; 95% confidence interval [95% CI], 1.22-2.36;
I2 = 94%, P = 0.002), and had higher levels of willingness to donate (3 RCTs, 393 participants; standardized mean difference,
0.29; 95% CI, 0.01-0.56; I2 = 45%; P = 0.04) than those who did not receive the interventions, but not the intention to donate
(315 participants; relative risk, 1.19; 95% CI, 0.94-1.51; P = 0.14). Conclusions. Community partnerships and active learning
community-based interventions may be effective in increasing the commitment, but not intentions to donate. However, the overall
risk of bias for was high, and this may have led to overestimation of the relative treatment effects of these interventions.
(Transplantation 2015;99: 2634–2643)

F or many patients with end-stage organ failure, transplan-


tation is the only lifesaving treatment option. However,
a critical shortage of donor organs exists worldwide, and
In many countries, strong in-principle support for organ
donation in the community does not always translate into a
personal decision to become an organ donor. 5,6 The factors
in most countries, deceased organ donation rates are inade- that influence such decisions may be multifactorial and in-
quate to address the growing demand for organs. On aver- clude knowledge about organ donation, personal beliefs,
age, more than 1 person in the United Kingdom1 and such as altruism or fear of medical neglect; external influ-
almost 15 people in the United States die each day on the ences, such as family and culture; emotional influences, such
transplant waiting list.2 In Australia, the median waiting time as grief or apathy; and the prevailing institutional and policy
on dialysis for a donor kidney is 3.3 years.3 The rates of de- context, such as the complexity of the consent system.6 These
ceased organ donation vary greatly internationally, from factors may represent potential levers for intervention to in-
36.5 donors per million populations in Croatia to 0.09 do- crease the community's willingness to become a deceased or-
nors per million population in Morocco. 4 gan donor. A considerable variety of approaches have been
taken across and within countries to increase deceased organ
Received 12 February 2015. Revision received 20 May 2015.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Accepted 3 June 2015.
1
K.H., S.J., M.I., and G.W. designed the study. A.T.L., A.F.R., G.W., and A.T. conducted
Monash School of Medicine, Monash University, Victoria, Australia. the data extraction and analyses. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the
2
Sydney School of Public Health, University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia. analyses. A.T.L. drafted the article. All authors contributed to the writing and review of
3
Centre for Kidney Research, The Children's Hospital at Westmead, New South the article.
Wales, Australia. Correspondence: Germaine Wong, Centre for Transplant and Renal Research,
4
Centre for Transplant and Renal Research, Westmead Hospital, New South Wales, Westmead Hospital, Cnr Hawkesbury Rd & Darcy Rd, Westmead New South Wales
Australia. 2145, Australia. (germaine.wong@health.nsw.gov.au).
5
The George Institute for Global Health, Camperdown, New South Wales, Australia. Supplemental digital content (SDC) is available for this article. Direct URL citations
6
appear in the printed text, and links to the digital files are provided in the HTML text
The Institute for Choice, University of South Australia, North Sydney, New South of this article on the journal’s Web site (www.transplantjournal.com).
Wales, Australia.
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
The PAraDOx study is funded by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project
Grant (DP0985187). The funders’ played no role in the research or the decision ISSN: 0041-1337/15/9912-2634
to publish. DOI: 10.1097/TP.0000000000000897

2634 www.transplantjournal.com Transplantation ■ December 2015 ■ Volume 99 ■ Number 12

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


© 2015 Wolters Kluwer Li et al 2635

donation. The types of interventions for increasing community donor on an organ donor registry, driver's licence, donor
willingness to be a deceased organ donor that have been stud- card, or other official record, and/or communicate to the next
ied are diverse and include mass media campaigns, educa- of kin that one wishes to become an organ donor.
tional materials, and community partnerships, among others.
We aimed to identify and evaluate the totality of evidence re- Risk of Bias
garding the effectiveness of community-based interventions Two authors (A.T.L. and A.F.R.) independently assessed
to increase deceased organ donation. the risk of bias and resolved all discrepancies by consensus
for all included studies. For trials, we used the Cochrane Risk
of Bias Tool, 8 and for before-after studies, we used a modi-
MATERIALS AND METHODS
fied version of the Cochrane Effective Practice and Organi-
We conducted a systematic review based on standard methods sation of Care Group suggested risk of bias criteria for
and reporting in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items interrupted time series studies. 9
for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses statement. 7
Quality of Evidence
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
We assessed the quality of the evidence informing the sum-
We included studies (randomized controlled trials [RCTs],
mary estimates using the Grading of Recommendations As-
non-RCTs [NRCTs], and before-after studies) that quantita-
sessment Development and Evaluation guidelines. 10
tively assessed the impact of interventions to increase the will-
ingness of members of the community to become solid organ Data Synthesis and Statistical Analyses
donors after death. We included studies that used individual We expressed results from individual trials and before-
level assignment (RCTs and NRCTs) and those that used after studies as risk ratios (RR) with 95% confidence inter-
group level assignment (cluster RCTs [CRCTs] and cluster vals (95% CI) for dichotomous outcomes and as standard-
NRCTs). We imposed no restrictions on article language. ized mean differences (SMD) with 95% CI for continuous
We excluded abstracts and non-research articles. outcomes. We obtained the summary estimates of binary out-
Search Strategies comes using the DerSimonian and Laird random effects
We searched MEDLINE, Embase, PsycInfo and CINAHL model. We considered P values ≤0.05 to be statistically sig-
from inception to December 2013. MeSH terms and key- nificant. We quantified heterogeneity using the χ2 test and
words for organ donation, interventions, and comparative I2 statistic and used preplanned subgroup analyses by inter-
studies (eg, “RCT”) were used. The search strategies are pro- vention types and target populations, and retrospective sub-
vided in Appendix 1, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TP/B187. group analyses by geographic location of studies and study
quality, to explore the possible sources of heterogeneity.
Data Extraction Where possible, we undertook a sensitivity analysis based
Two authors (A.T.L. and M.J.I.) independently assessed all on the quality of the studies and assessed publication bias
titles and abstracts for eligible studies and discarded those using funnel plots. We conducted all analyses using Review
that did not meet the inclusion criteria. We extracted data Manager 5. Where appropriate, we also calculated the num-
on study design, geographic location, setting, sample size, ber needed to treat from the population attributable risk
participants, interventions and comparators, and the rates (PAR) using the following formula, where Pe represents the
of commitment to donate and intention to donate. Where total proportion of participants who were exposed to the in-
outcome data were reported in a form that could not be terventions, and RRe represents the relative risks (RRs) given
meta-analyzed, we contacted corresponding authors to seek by the meta-analyses:
the relevant data including the number of events and group
sample sizes for the dichotomous outcomes, and the mean es- 1 1 1 þ Pe ðRRe ‐1Þ
NNT ¼ ¼ ¼
timates, standard deviations, and group sample sizes for the PAR Pe ðRRe ‐1Þ Pe ðRRe ‐1Þ
1þPe ðRRe ‐1Þ
continuous outcome.
Outcomes Measures RESULTS
The 2 prespecified outcomes that we assessed were com- Characteristics of Studies
mitment to donate and intention to donate. We defined com- We included 49 articles, incorporating a total of 63 stud-
mitment to donate as documentation of being a donor on an ies as some articles reported findings on multiple studies
organ donor registry, driver's licence, donor card, or other (Figure 1). All articles were from primary sources. The study
official record, communication to the next of kin that one characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Five studies did
wishes to become an organ donor and/or otherwise self- not report their sample size. The remaining 58 studies in-
reporting that one is an organ donor. volved 174,279 participants. Thirty-one studies were trials
Intention to donate could be reported as a dichotomous (8 RCTs, 11 CRCTs, 4 NRCTs, 8 cluster NRCTs) and 32
outcome (“positive intent to donate”) or a continuous out- were before-after studies. In total, 42 of 63 studies (67%)
come (“willingness to donate”). We defined positive intent were included in the meta-analyses. The majority of the stud-
to donate as any action or statement that indicates one is will- ies originated from the United States, and with an average
ing to be an organ donor, document being a donor on an follow-up time of 60.5 days (Table 1).
organ donor registry, driver's licence, donor card, or other of-
ficial record, and/or communicate to the next of kin that they Risk of Bias
wish to become an organ donor, but without committing to Appendix 2 (SDC, http://links.lww.com/TP/B187) shows
donation. We defined willingness to donate as self-assessed the risk of bias assessments for all included studies. Of the
willingness to become an organ donor, document being a 31 trials, 11 (35%) were judged as high risk of bias, 18 trials

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


2636 Transplantation ■ December 2015 ■ Volume 99 ■ Number 12 www.transplantjournal.com

FIGURE 1. Process of studies selection.


as unclear risk of bias (58%), and 2 trials (6%) as low risk of controlled trials or before-after studies, for which the risk of bias
bias. No trials blinded participants or personnel to the inter- varied between unclear and high. The consistency of the esti-
ventions. Sixteen trials (52%) had high risk of reporting bias. mates of the effects was also variable, with heterogeneity
Allocation concealment was adequate in 9 trials (29%). The ranging from low to high. Publication bias was present.
methods of randomization were adequate in 7 trials (23%), Hence, the overall quality of the evidence was low, indicating
unclear in 14 trials (45%), and inadequate in 10 trials (32%). the confidence we placed in the effect estimates was limited,
Overall, 8 of the 32 before-after studies (25%) were con- and the true estimates were likely to be different from the es-
sidered as high risk of bias, 23 studies as unclear risk of bias timate of effects.10
(72%), and a single study as low risk of bias (3%).
Types of Interventions
Visual assessment of funnel plots (not shown) showed that
the studies were distributed asymmetrically around the com- The included studies examined a diverse range of interven-
bined effect size, suggesting publication bias (Table 2). tions (n = 12). Education was a key strategy in the majority
of interventions, conveying factual, and/or emotive messages
Quality of Evidence relating to the need for organs and/or the process of organ
Table 3 summarizes the quality of evidence for the key find- donation. These interventions included advertising, active learn-
ings of our review. The findings were based on randomised ing, community partnerships, a computer-tailored intervention,

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


© 2015 Wolters Kluwer Li et al 2637

TABLE 1. to lead discussion on organ donation within their church


Characteristics of included studies members. The computer-tailored intervention (n = 1, 2%)
used a computer program that asked participants questions
Characteristic No. Studies (%) relating to donation and provided feedback based on their in-
Geographic location put. Educational materials (n = 9, 14%) included written ma-
United States of America 46 (73.02) terials such as brochures, videos, and websites that promoted
Netherlands 4 (6.35) organ donation. Interventions at DMV offices (n = 4, 6%)
Germany 3 (4.76) encompassed a range of educational interventions conducted
Turkey 3 (4.76) at DMVs (or equivalent government agency) offices to en-
United Kingdom 3 (4.76) courage customers to commit as deceased organ donors at
Colombia 1 (1.59) these locations where their decisions can be recorded. Promo-
Croatia 1 (1.59) tional materials and staff training to improve communication
South Africa 1 (1.59) with the public about organ donation were a few examples of
Sweden 1 (1.59) such interventions. Workplace interventions (n = 3, 5%) in-
Year of publication cluded educational interventions conducted in the workplace
≤2000 2 (3.2) setting. Some methods had multiple main components (n = 6,
2001-2005 10 (15.9) 10%), for example, utilization of passive learning combined
2006-2010 32 (50.8) with opportunistic registration procedures. Other nonspecific
≥2011 19 (30.2) interventions did not disseminate facts or arguments in favor
Study design of donation. This category encompassed anticipated regret,
RCT 8 (12.7) (n = 3, 5%) in which participants were asked to consider their
CRCT 11 (17.5) regret if they were to not register as organ donors, and opportu-
NRCT 4 (6.3) nistic registration procedures, (n = 2, 3%), whereby the partici-
CNRCT 8 (12.7) pants were given an opportunity to register immediately.
Before-after study 32 (50.8) The specific interventions used in each included study are sum-
Sample size marized in Appendix 3, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TP/B187.
0-500 38 (60.3)
500-1000 4 (6.3) Efficacy of Interventions
1000-10,000 15 (23.8)
Randomized Controlled Trials
>10,000 1 (1.6)
Not stated 5 (7.9) The types of interventions assessed in the meta-analyses in-
Setting 26 (41.3) cluded anticipated regret (n = 2) and educational materials
General community (n = 1). Participants who received community-based inter-
Schools 11 (17.5) ventions experienced a statistically significant improvement
Universities 12 (19.0) in the willingness to donate compared to those that did not
Churches 3 (4.8) receive the interventions (3 studies, 393 participants: SMD,
Primary care 2 (3.2) 0.29; 95% CI, 0.01-0.56; I2 = 45%; P = 0.04) (Figure 2A).
Motor registry 4 (6.3) Participants who received a personalized letter from the Ohio
Military 1 (1.6) Secretary of State (89413 participants: RR, 1.90; 95% CI,
Workplaces 3 (4.8) 1.78-2.02; P < 0.001) and those who received a personalized
Online 1 (1.6) letter and a brochure (89625 participants: RR, 1.91; 95%
Outcomes CI, 1.80-2.03; P < 0.001) experienced a significant improve-
Commitment to donate 36 (57.1) ment in the commitment to donate compared to those who
Intention to donate 34 (54.0) only received a brochure alone. No significant differences
Follow-up time ( mean, SD), d 60.5 (113.3) were observed in the proportion of participants committing
c
to deceased organ donation between those who received a
Nonrandomized controlled trial.
CNRCT indicates cluster non-RCT. personalized letter and a brochure and those who received
a personalized letter only (85790 participants: RR, 1.01;
95% CI, 0.96-1.06; P = 0.73).11 There were also no signifi-
educational materials, education by health professionals, inter- cant differences in the proportion of participants' expressing
ventions at Department of Motor Vehicles (DMV) offices, passive a positive intention to donate between those who received
learning, and workplace interventions. Advertising interventions passive education compared to those that did not (315 partic-
were disseminated through media, such as television, radio, print, ipants: RR, 1.19; 95% CI, 0.94-1.51; P = 0.14).12 Three ad-
billboard, and/or online (n = 5, 8%). The majority of advertising ditional RCTs13–15 assessed the positive intent to donate,
interventions were targeted at ethnic minorities (n = 4, 80%). Ac- but only 2 of these studies reported a significant increase
tive learning (n = 5, 8%) involved participants to complete tasks in the proportion of participants' expressing a positive inten-
that promote and educate organ donation. This contrasted with tion to donate between the intervention and comparator
interventions that involved passive learning (n = 14, 22%), such groups. The types of interventions found to be effective in-
as presentations and school lessons. cluded educational materials on organ donation compared
Community partnerships (n = 9, 14%) involved lay mem- with education materials on avoiding the common cold15
bers from specific communities to promote organ donation, and passive learning combined with a computer-tailored
such as engagement with African-American church leaders intervention.14

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


2638 Transplantation ■ December 2015 ■ Volume 99 ■ Number 12 www.transplantjournal.com

TABLE 2.
Outcomes of meta-analyses

Outcome Studies Participants P I2 Outcome (summary estimate)


RCTs
Intention to donate (willingness to donate) 3 393 0.04 45% SMD, 0.29 (0.01-0.56)
CRCTs
Commitment to donate 7 6015 0.002 94% RR, 1.70 (1.22-2.36)
NRCTs
Commitment to donate 2 320 0.91 73% RR, 1.05 (0.45-2.45)
Before-after studies
Commitment to donate 15 3709 <0.001 78% RR, 1.51 (1.22-1.86)
Intention to donate (positive intent to donate) 10 9572 0.01 86% RR, 1.14 (1.03-1.26)
Intention to donate (willingness to donate) 5 645 <0.001 76% SMD, 0.54 (0.23-0.86)

Cluster-Randomized Controlled Trials donor cards compared with mass media only.20 Workplace
The types of interventions assessed included in the meta- intervention also resulted in a significant increase in the rate
analyses were community partnership (n = 2), opportunis- of signing donor cards when compared with no intervention.21
tic registration procedures (n = 2), educational materials Workplace intervention comprising of educational materials
(n = 1), intervention at DMVoffices (n = 1), and a workplace and onsite visits by staff was associated with a significant
intervention (n = 1). Participants who received community- increase in self-reported changes in donor status among
based interventions experienced a significant improvement nondonors compared with no intervention (odds ratio, 1.32;
in the commitment to donate compared to those not exposed P < 0.001) When the same intervention was compared with
(7 studies, 6015 participants: RR, 1.70; 95% CI, 1.22-2.36; another intervention comprising only educational materials,
I2 = 94%; P = 0.002) (Figure 2A). a similar result was reported (odds ratio, 1.17; P < 0.03).22
Additionally, a single study that evaluated the effectiveness of
community partnership reported a significant improvement in Before-After Studies
the willingness to donate compared to those that did not receive Twenty-six before-after studies were included in the meta-
the interventions (1043 participants: RR, 0.21; 95% CI, 0.09- analyses. After the interventions were introduced, a signifi-
0.33; P < 0.001).16 Interventions in the form of passive learning cant improvement in the proportion of participants commit-
did not increase the proportion of participants with positive in- ting as donors (15 studies, 3709 participants: RR, 1.51; 95%
tent to donate (187 participants: RR, 1.16; 95% CI, 0.80-1.67; CI, 1.22-1.86; I2 = 78%; P < 0.001), intending to become
P = 0.44). 17 Also, an intervention at DMVoffices was more ef- donors (10 studies, 9572 participants: RR, 1.14; 95% CI,
fective in improving the proportion of participants committed 1.03-1.26, I2 = 86%, P = 0.01,), and in the willingness to do-
to being an organ donor compared with passive display of or- nate (5 studies, 645 participants: SMD, 0.54; 95% CI, 0.23-
gan donation materials. 14 0.86, I2 = 76%, P < 0.001) was observed in the participants
(Figure 2B). The types of interventions assessed for commit-
Non–Randomized Controlled Trials ment to donate included community partnership (n = 6), ac-
tive learning (n = 4), passive learning (n = 3), advertising
Anticipated regret (n = 1) and education by health profes-
(n = 1), and education by health professional (n = 1). The
sional (n = 1) were the 2 major forms of interventions included
types of interventions assessed for positive intent to donate
in the meta-analysis. There were no significant differences in
included advertising (n = 5), passive learning (n = 4), and ed-
the participants' commitment to donate between the inter-
ucational materials (n = 1). The types of interventions
vention and control arms (2 studies, 320 participants: RR,
assessed for willingness to donate included active learning
1.05; 95% CI, 0.45-2.45; I2 = 73%; P = 0.91). Two other
(n = 2), educational materials (n = 1), passive learning (n =1),
studies that examined the benefits of passive learning (17, 18)
and intervention that comprised both passive learning and
did not report a significant increase in the willingness to do-
opportunistic registration procedures (n = 1).
nate and positive intent to donate.
Five other before-after studies examined changes in the
commitment to donate.23–27 Two studies that focused on ed-
Cluster NRCTs ucational materials (n = 1) and passive learning combined
Participants who were shown a videotaped presentation with opportunistic registration procedures (n = 1) reported
containing demographic information about potential organ a significant increase in the proportion of participants agree-
recipients were more likely to take a donor sticker and card ing and committing to being donors compared with baseline
than participants who were shown the same video without results.24,25 Two studies examined changes in the intention to
the demographic information (328 participants: RR, 1.34; donate28,29 and one reported a significant improvement in
95% CI, 1.12-1.61; P = 0.002).18 On the contrary, passive the willingness to donate using educational materials.28
learning with classroom education did not improve the posi-
Subgroup Analyses
tive intent to donate (69 participants: RR, 0.73; 95% CI,
0.40-1.33; P = 0.31).19 A combined intervention of mass me- Types of Intervention
dia and interpersonal approach was associated with a signif- Compared with those not exposed to any form of interven-
icant improvement in the number of participants signing tion, active partnership with the communities was associated

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


© 2015 Wolters Kluwer

TABLE 3.
Grading of recommendations assessment, development, and evaluation evidence profile of studies for commitment to donate and intention to donate after community-based
interventions

Quality of assessment (decrease in quality score) Summary of findings


Summary outcome Quality of
No. studies (type of study) Risk of bias/quality of eEvidence Consistency Directness Precision Publication Bias (95% CI)* evidence
Community-based interventions (all types)
7 CRCTs All CRCTs; mostly unclear risk of bias (+3) Large inconsistency; I2 = 94% (−2) Direct (0) No serious imprecision Suspected publication bias (−1) Commitment to donate
RR, 1.70 (1.22-2.36) Very low
Active learning
4 Before-after studies Lack RCTs; unclear risk of bias (+2) No inconsistency; I2 = 0% Direct (0) No serious imprecision Suspected publication bias (−1) Commitment to donate
96.4% (91.6-98.5%) Very low
Anticipated regret
2 RCTs All RCTs; mostly unclear risk of bias (+3) Some inconsistency; I2 = 32% (−1) Direct (0) No serious imprecision Suspected publication bias (−1) Willingness to donate
SMD, 0.39 (0.09-0.69) Low
Community partnership
2 CRCTs All CRCTs; high risk of bias (+2) Some inconsistency; I2 = 42% (−1) Direct (0) No serious imprecision Suspected publication bias (−1) Commitment to donate
RR, 3.00 (2.14-4.20) Very low
Advertising
5 Before-after studies Lack RCTs; unclear risk of bias (+2) No inconsistency; I2 = 0% Direct (0) No serious imprecision No publication bias (0) Positive intent to donate

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


RR, 1.00 (0.97-1.03) Low
Li et al
2639
2640 Transplantation ■ December 2015 ■ Volume 99 ■ Number 12 www.transplantjournal.com

FIGURE 2. A, Meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials for commitment to donate and intention to donate. B, Meta-analysis of before-after
studies for commitment to donate and intention to donate.

with a significant increase in the proportion of participants CI, 0.09-0.69; I2 = 32%; P = 0.010). Participants who re-
documenting as committed donors (2 CRCTs, 4043 partici- ceived interventions that required active learning, such as im-
pants: RR, 3.00; 95% CI, 2.14-4.20; I2 = 42%; P < 0.001; plementing their own donation campaigns, experienced
PAR, 49.6%). Interventions that involved anticipated regret improved commitment to donate after being exposed to the
were associated with an improvement in the willingness to interventions (4 before-after studies, 322 participants: RR,
donate compared to questionnaires that did not use antici- 2.82; 95% CI, 2.13-3.72; I2 = 0%; P < 0.001). There was
pated regret (2 RCTs, 284 participants: SMD, 0.39; 95% no significant improvement in the proportion of participants

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


© 2015 Wolters Kluwer Li et al 2641

FIGURE 2, continued. B. Meta-analysis of before-after studies for commitment to donate and intention to donate.

with positive intentions to donate after the introduction of 4536 participants: RR, 1.83; 95% CI, 0.99-3.37; I2 = 95%;
the advertising interventions (5 before-after studies, 6380 par- P = 0.05; PAR, 29.2%). A significant improvement in the
ticipants: RR, 1.00; 95% CI, 0.97-1.03; I2 = 0%; P = 0.93). commitment to donate (3 CRCTs, 4199 participants: RR,
2.28; 95% CI, 1.22-4.27; I2 = 91%; P = 0.01) was observed
after exclusion of a single study in which ethnically targeted
Targeted Populations educational materials with a religious focus were compared
Interventions targeted at ethnic minorities did not show a with ethnically targeted educational materials without a reli-
significant improvement in commitment to donate (4 CRCTs, gious focus.

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


2642 Transplantation ■ December 2015 ■ Volume 99 ■ Number 12 www.transplantjournal.com

Interventions that did not target specific ethnic groups also latter, of translating a broad willingness to donate into a for-
resulted in a significant improvement in commitment to mal commitment, but are less consistently effective in shifting
donate (3 CRCTs, 1479 participants: RR, 1.43; 95% CI, people's attitudes toward donation. The implication of this is
1.03-1.98; I2 = 88%; P = 0.03). that intervention through community programs is best suited
to encouraging individuals already disposed to organ dona-
Geographic Location tion to formally register and in doing so, more generally, cap-
The majority of included studies (n = 46, 73%) were orig- italizing on the high level of support for organ donation that is
inated from the United States. There were no significant dif- known to exist in many countries. However, another possible
ferences in outcomes, such as willingness to donate, the explanation for these findings is that actions such as registra-
commitment to donate or positive intent to donate between tion and notifying family members are more objective and
studies conducted in the United States and those conducted hence may be more conclusively measured than studies that in-
elsewhere. cluded behavioural intentions.
Overall, our findings are broadly consistent with findings
Study Quality of previous reviews that suggest specific interventions may
Only 1 study30 was judged to have low-risk bias when con- be effective in increasing the willingness to be a deceased or-
sidering the commitment to donate as an outcome. This study gan donor,32,33 in the general community and ethnic minor-
reported a smaller but statistically significant improvement in ities.32,34 Additionally, this review quantifies the potential
commitment to donate between the intervention and control effectiveness of several specific intervention types, namely
arms (1 study, 952 participants: RR, 1.16; 95% CI, 1.08- community partnerships, active learning, and anticipated re-
1.24; P < 0.001). Similarly, the single RCT31 that reported gret, in improving the community willingness to be a deceased
willingness to donate as an outcome and was deemed to have organ donor.
a low risk of bias reported no significant differences in willing- Our study has several strengths. The inclusiveness of
ness to donate between intervention and controlled arm. our review enables it to assess the overall effectiveness of
community-based interventions in general and to compare
Adverse Events across different types of interventions and target populations.
Adverse events were not reported in any of the included We have also used the standardized approach of conducting
studies. a systematic review whereby a systematic search of medical
databases, data extraction and analysis, and trial quality as-
DISCUSSION sessment by 2 independent reviewers was conducted based
There was considerable variation in the types of community- on a prespecified protocol. Our review has several limita-
based interventions used to promote and improve the rates of tions. First, the mean follow-up time of all included studies
deceased donation in the general population. Broadly, interven- is short (60.5 days) so longer term outcomes are not avail-
tions included advertising, active learning, anticipated regret, able. Additionally, the included studies were of generally
community partnerships, a computer-tailored intervention, edu- low quality. Publication bias may also exist leading to an
cational materials, education by health professionals, interven- overestimation of treatment effect because trials or observa-
tions at DMV offices, opportunistic registration procedures, tional data with more favourable outcomes are more likely
passive learning, workplace interventions, and interventions to be published than studies with negative results. Although
with multiple main components. Evidence from RCTs sug- enquiry of the authors did not reveal additional information,
gested that community-based interventions, such as community there were insufficient studies to formally evaluate such bias.
partnerships and anticipated regret were effective in improving Systematic bias, such as performance bias and sampling bias,
the commitment to donate. Participants who received an inter- also exists when observational studies, particularly before
vention were 1.7 times (95% CI, 1.22-2.36) more likely to com- and after studies, were used to evaluate the effectiveness of
mit to being an organ donor compared to those that did not an intervention. We acknowledge that the limited number
receive any specific intervention. However, the overall risk of of participants in the included studies may preclude accurate
bias for individual studies was high, and this may have led to assessment of heterogeneity beyond chance. Our subgroup
overestimation of the true treatment effects of the interventions. analyses, although limited by the small number of included
In general, strategies that involved community partnership studies (n = 2-5), showed that the heterogeneity may partly
and active participation involving participants achieved the be explained by the effects of intervention types.
greatest impact in promoting deceased organ donation. Inter- Data were sparse in a number of areas. Reporting of
ventions that included active partnership with the community potential harms was only reported in a single study.30 In
increased the number of committed donors by approximately addition, given the relatively small number of studies avail-
50%, compared with no intervention. Specific interventions able in the meta-analyses, we were unable to explore all
that targeted ethnic communities with culturally sensitive outcomes across all study designs, or to perform detailed
approaches were also effective in increasing the number of subgroup analyses to explore interactions between the
committed donors by 29.2% compared to those who did types of interventions on the treatment efficacy between
not receive the interventions. the various types of intervention. Additionally, our review
The studies examined in this review highlight how the deci- included only studies that directly reported on changes in
sion to become an organ donor consists of 2 distinct steps: the willingness to be a deceased organ donor and did not
first, the adoption of in-principle support for donation and sec- consider indirect outcomes such as knowledge or opinions
ond, the translation of this support to a formal commitment about donation. The extent to which changes in these indi-
through registration. Our findings suggest that community- rect outcomes translate into actual increases in deceased
based interventions are generally effective in relation to the organ donations remains unclear.

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


© 2015 Wolters Kluwer Li et al 2643

Overall, the evidence suggests that interventions may be ef- 13. Reubsaet A, Brug J, De Vet E, et al. The effects of practicing registration of
organ donation preference on self-efficacy and registration intention: an
fective in improving the willingness to be a deceased organ
enactive mastery experience. Psychol Health. 2003;18:585–594.
donor, however the quality of the current evidence is poor. 14. Reubsaet A, Brug J, Nijkamp MD, et al. The impact of an organ dona-
Well-powered and well-designed RCTs are needed to evalu- tion registration information program for high school students in the
ate the impact of opportunistic registration procedures, antic- Netherlands. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60:1479–1486.
ipated regret, and active learning on improving the number 15. Vinokur AD, Merion RM, Couper MP, et al. Educational web-based inter-
vention for high school students to increase knowledge and promote
of committed donors in the community. Process evaluations positive attitudes toward organ donation. Health Educ Behav. 2006;33:
alongside these intervention studies are also needed to iden- 773–786.
tify the barriers and facilitators to commit as organ donors 16. Andrews AM, Zhang N, Magee JC, et al. Increasing donor designation
and the impact that specific types of interventions may ulti- through black churches: results of a randomized trial. Prog Transplant.
2012;22:161–167.
mately have on deceased organ donation rates.
17. Rodrigue JR, Krouse J, Carroll C, et al. A department of motor vehicles
intervention yields moderate increases in donor designation rates. Prog
Transplant. 2012;22:18–24.
CONCLUSIONS 18. Singh M, Katz RC, Beauchamp K, et al. Effects of anonymous information
about potential organ transplant recipients on attitudes toward organ
Overall, we found a lack of consistency in methods and out- transplantation and the willingness to donate organs. J Behav Med.
come measures in studies. The overall quality of the evidence is 2002;25:469–476.
also low. Despite this, existing evidence suggests that commu- 19. Weaver M, Spigner C, Pineda M, et al. Knowledge and opinions about or-
nity partnerships, anticipated regret, and active learning may gan donation among urban high school students: pilot test of a health ed-
ucation program. Clin Transplant. 2000;14:292–303.
be promising approaches to increase individuals' willingness
20. Morgan SE, Stephenson MT, Afifi W, et al. The University Worksite Organ
to be a deceased organ donor in both the general community Donation Project: a comparison of two types of worksite campaigns on
and ethnic minorities. Future studies in the forms of well- the willingness to donate. Clin Transplant. 2011;25:600–605.
powered randomised controlled trials comparing the various 21. Morgan SE, Miller J, Arasaratman LA. Signing cards, saving lives: an eval-
forms of interventions, using consistent outcome measures, uation of the worksite organ donation promotion project. Commun
Monogr. 2002;69:253.
are likely to have a major impact on improving our confi- 22. Morgan SE, Harrison TR, Chewning LV, et al. The effectiveness of high-
dence in the treatment effectiveness of these techniques. and low-intensity worksite campaigns to promote organ donation: the
workplace partnership for life. (Author abstract)(Report). Commun Monogr.
2010;77:341–356.
23. D'Alessandro AM, Peltier JW, Dahl AJ. Use of social media and college
student organizations to increase support for organ donation and advo-
REFERENCES cacy: a case report. Prog Transplant. 2012;22:436–441.
1. NHS Blood and Transplant. Organ Donation and Transplantation 24. Fahrenwald NL, Belitz C, Keckler A. Outcome evaluation of 'sharing the gift
Activity Report 2012/13. London: NHS; 2013. Available at: http://www. of life': an organ and tissue donation educational program for American
organdonation.nhs.uk/statistics/transplant_activity_report/current_activity_ Indians. Am J Transplant. 2010;10:1453–1459.
reports/ukt/activity_report_2012_13.pdf. 25. Siegel JT, Alvaro EM, Crano WD, et al. Passive-positive organ donor reg-
2. Based on OPTN data as of 17 January 2014. istration behavior: a mixed method assessment of the IIFF Model. Psychol
3. Wright J, Narayan S. Analysis of kidney allocation during 2013. 2014. Health Med. 2010;15:198–209.
4. International Registry in Organ Donation and Transplantation. IRODaT 26. Harrison TR, Morgan SE, King AJ, et al. Saving lives branch by branch: the
Newsletter 2012. Available at: http://www.irodat.org/img/database/grafics/ effectiveness of driver licensing bureau campaigns to promote organ do-
newsletter/IRODaT Newsletter 2012.pdf. nor registry sign-ups to African Americans in Michigan. J Health Commun.
5. The Gallup Organisation. National Survey of Organ and Tissue Donation 2011;16:805–819.
Attitudes and Behaviors. Rockville, MD: Health Resources and Services 27. Zaramo CE, Morton T, Yoo JW, et al. Culturally competent methods
Administration; 2005. to promote organ donation rates among African-Americans using venues
6. Irving MJ, Tong A, Jan S, et al. Factors that influence the decision to be an of the Bureau of Motor Vehicles. Transplant Proc. 2008;40:1001–1004.
organ donor: a systematic review of the qualitative literature. Nephrol Dial 28. Merion RM, Vinokur AD, Couper MP, et al. Internet-based intervention to
Transplant. 2012;27:2526–2533. promote organ donor registry participation and family notification. Trans-
7. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J; PRISMA Group, et al. Preferred reporting plantation. 2003;75:1175–1179.
items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. 29. Meier D, Schulz KH, Kuhlencordt R, et al. Effects of an educational seg-
BMJ. 2009;339:b2535. ment concerning organ donation and transplantation. Transplant Proc.
8. Higgins J, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Inter- 2000;32:62–63.
ventions. T.C. Collaboration; 2012. 30. Thornton JD, Alejandro-Rodriguez M, León JB, et al. Effect of an iPod
9. Cochrane Effective Practice and Organisation of Care Group. Suggested video intervention on consent to donate organs: a randomized trial. Ann
risk of bias criteria for EPOC reviews. 2012. Intern Med. 2012;156:483–490.
10. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist GE; GRADE Working Group, et al. GRADE: 31. McDonald DD, Ferreri R, Jin C, et al. Willingness to communicate organ
an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of rec- donation intention. Public Health Nurs. 2007;24:151–159.
ommendations. BMJ. 2008;336:924–926. 32. Feeley TH, Moon S-i. A meta-analytic review of communication cam-
11. Quick BL, Bosch D, Morgan SE. Message framing and medium consider- paigns to promote organ donation. Commun Rep. 2009;22:63–73.
ations for recruiting newly eligible teen organ donor registrants. Am J 33. Li AH, Rosenblum AM, Nevis IF, et al. Adolescent classroom education on
Transplant. 2012;12:1593–1597. knowledge and attitudes about deceased organ donation: a systematic
12. Smits M, van den Borne B, Dijker AJ, et al. Increasing Dutch adolescents' review. Pediatr Transplant. 2013;17:119–128.
willingness to register their organ donation preference: the effectiveness of 34. Deedat S, Kenten C, Morgan M. What are effective approaches to in-
an education programme delivered by kidney transplantation patients. Eur creasing rates of organ donor registration among ethnic minority popula-
J Public Health. 2006;16:106–110. tions: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2013;3:e003453.

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

You might also like