Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [Richard Douglass]

On: 30 August 2014, At: 08:35


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Journal of Positive Psychology: Dedicated to


furthering research and promoting good practice
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20

Meaning in life and work: A developmental perspective


a a a
Blake A. Allan , Ryan D. Duffy & Richard Douglass
a
Department of Psychology, University of Florida, PO Box 112250, Gainesville, FL
32611-2250, USA
Published online: 26 Aug 2014.

To cite this article: Blake A. Allan, Ryan D. Duffy & Richard Douglass (2014): Meaning in life and work: A developmental
perspective, The Journal of Positive Psychology: Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice, DOI:
10.1080/17439760.2014.950180

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.950180

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2014
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.950180

Meaning in life and work: A developmental perspective


Blake A. Allan*, Ryan D. Duffy and Richard Douglass
Department of Psychology, University of Florida, PO Box 112250, Gainesville, FL 32611-2250, USA
(Received 4 February 2014; accepted 9 July 2014)

This study examined linear and nonlinear relations between age and the presence and search for meaning in life and
examined if these relations were moderated by the presence of meaning in work. Age did not significantly relate to the
presence of meaning in life, but age had a significant, negative linear relation with the search for meaning in life. More-
over, work meaning moderated the quadratic relation between age and life meaning. Specifically, people high in work
meaning demonstrated negative quadratic curves, with high life meaning during middle adulthood, and people low in
work meaning had positive quadratic curves, with low life meaning during middle adulthood. Work meaning also moder-
ated the linear relation between age and the search for meaning in life with people highest in work meaning showing the
strongest negative relation between age and the search for meaning in life. Limitations and implications for future
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

research are discussed.


Keywords: life meaning; work meaning; lifespan development; positive psychology

Researchers in positive psychology have increasingly some studies have used Erikson’s (1950) theory of
become interested in meaning in life as a fundamental psychosocial development as a guide (e.g. Ang &
component of human well-being (Dik, Duffy, & Jiaqing, 2012), because it suggests that middle adulthood
Eldridge, 2009; Pratt & Ashforth, 2003; Steger, Dik, & may be a particularly salient period in which meaning
Duffy, 2012). Studies have also begun to examine how can be cultivated. During middle adulthood (defined by
the presence and search for life meaning change and Erikson as ages 40–60), Erikson argues that people enter
develop over the lifespan (e.g. Steger, Oishi, & Kashdan, the developmental period of generativity vs. stagnation.
2009), and some studies have employed Erikson’s During this stage, people struggle to find a way to con-
(1950) theory of psychosocial development to understand tribute and give back to the world. Generative adults
differences in life meaning across different age groups dedicate themselves to activities that will outlive the self
(Ang & Jiaqing, 2012). However, no studies have exam- and successfully find ways to contribute to the next
ined nonlinear relations of the presence or search for generation (Kotre, 1984; McAdams, de St. Aubin, &
meaning in life to age or how meaning in specific life Logan, 1993). Supporting this argument, studies have
domains might alter these relations. This study aimed to found adults in midlife are more likely than younger and
address these deficits by testing nonlinear curves of life older adults to have generative concerns and actions
meaning and investigating if meaning in work moderates (McAdams et al., 1993).
the relation between age and the presence and search for Creating generative projects that outlive the self may
meaning in life. The goal was to provide a more reliable provide a unique conduit to experienced meaning in life
understanding of the amount of life meaning in different and, not surprisingly, some researchers consider gener-
age groups and to gain further insights into factors that ativity a crucial source of life meaning (Emmons, 2003;
help or hinder people’s ability to live meaningful lives. Schnell & Becker, 2006). This suggests that people may
experience a boost in meaning in life during middle
adulthood when they are focused on generative concerns
Theoretical framework and actions. Furthermore, the increase in the presence of
The presence of meaning in life is broadly defined as the life meaning could in turn decrease people’s search for
subjective experience that one’s life is significant and meaning in life. This prediction has been partially sup-
valuable (Steger, Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006). ported in the literature. Empirical studies in this area
Although many studies have examined the relation have found positive relations between age and meaning
between age and meaning in life, few have incorporated in life, with older adults reporting higher levels of life
a theoretical framework to understand why experiences meaning than younger adults (Reker, 2005; Steger et al.,
of meaning may differ across the lifespan. However, 2009; Van Ranst & Marcoen, 1997). However, Steger

*Corresponding author. Email: ballan3@ufl.edu

© 2014 Taylor & Francis


2 B.A. Allan et al.

et al. (2009) qualified this relation by reporting a rise in sub-domain of meaning that acts as a potential source of
meaning in life during the ages of 45–64, consistent with meaning in life (Ebersole & DePaola, 2001; Emmons,
Erikson’s generativity vs. stagnation stage. Curiously, in 2003; Fegg et al., 2007; Steger & Dik, 2009). This is
Steger, Oishi, and Kashdan’s study, meaning in life con- supported by several studies that have asked participants
tinued to climb into older adulthood, when generative the sources of their life meaning, finding common
behaviors are hypothesized to decline (McAdams et al., responses including relationships, religion, service, and
1993), and there was a dip in meaning in life during work (Baum & Stewart, 1990; DeVogler & Ebersole,
young adulthood (i.e. 25–44). Other studies have found 1981; Emmons, 2005; Fegg et al., 2007).
this life meaning dip to occur during midlife, defined as Following from this conceptualization, researchers
40–59 by Morgan and Robinson (2013) and 40–49 by have argued that experiencing meaning in work trans-
Fegg, Kramer, Bausewein, and Borasio (2007). Finally, lates into greater meaning in life (Steger & Dik, 2009),
only one study evaluated the search for meaning in life an assertion supported by several studies showing that
across the lifespan and found it to peak in young adult- participants consistently report work as a major source
hood then steadily decline throughout life (Steger et al., of meaning (e.g. Fegg et al., 2007). Few studies have
2009). In summary, these studies provide somewhat examined both life meaning and work meaning in the
mixed results about middle adulthood being a peak stage same study, but Duffy, Allan, Autin, and Bott (2013)
for experiencing meaning in life. found a correlated of 0.49 between the two variables in
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

Although these studies provide insight into the devel- working adults. Several studies have also illustrated how
opment of life meaning across the lifespan, they collec- well-being in the work domain can affect meaning in the
tively have two limitations. First, the studies have life domain. For example, workaholism and work–life
created somewhat arbitrary age categories. For example, conflict are negatively associated with purpose in life,
although Steger et al.’s (2009) categories of 18–24, and work enjoyment is positively associated with pur-
25–44, 45–64, and 65+ fall nicely into Erikson’s stages, pose in life (Bonebright, Clay, & Ankenmann, 2000).
they necessarily lose information and do not allowing Finally, adolescents who report purposeful career goals
the testing of nonlinear curves. This may be one expla- also report higher meaning in life (Yeager & Bundick,
nation as to why contradictory information exists regard- 2009). Therefore, there is some reason to suspect that
ing the dip of meaning in life during some point in meaning in work may translate into higher meaning in
midlife. Specifically, there may be a core group of peo- life.
ple who are being captured in different groups. For
example, the midlife dip has been observed in 25–44,
40–59, and 40–49 years old (Fegg et al., 2007; Morgan The present study
& Robinson, 2013; Steger et al., 2009), which all include Building off Erickson’s theory and previous mixed find-
the ages 40–44. Therefore, this could potentially be the ings, this study had two main goals: (i) to test linear and
typical age when meaning in life is lower. Although age nonlinear relations of age and the presence and search
groupings are consistent with evaluating stage theories, for meaning in life, and (ii) to examine if work meaning
the testing of nonlinear curves could further this knowl- moderated the relation between age and the presence and
edge by investigating the shape of the relation between search for meaning in life. These goals were assessed by
age and meaning in life with years being the variable of studying a large and diverse group of employed adults.
interest. This would also help clarify exactly when mean- First, in two hierarchical regressions, we regressed the
ing in life is higher or lower. For example, a nonlinear linear, quadratic, and cubic terms of age onto the pres-
curve may show a dip around the ages of 40–44. Sec- ence of meaning in life and the search for meaning in
ond, none of these studies have evaluated the impact that life. We hypothesized that, similar to patterns found in
subdomains of meaning might have on the development other studies, a positive quadratic curve would be signifi-
of life meaning. This gives a one dimensional view of cant (Steger et al., 2009). Specifically, life meaning
the development of life meaning, without considering would start at moderate levels, dip in young adulthood
how this might be different for diverse individuals. between ages of 25 and 45 then begin to rise steadily
Specifically, meaning in work may play a role in altering thereafter. In contrast, we hypothesized that the search
life meaning’s trajectory across the lifespan. for meaning in life would show the mirror image: a neg-
ative quadratic curve peaking in young adulthood (Steger
et al., 2009).
Life meaning and work meaning Second, in step two of the hierarchical regressions,
Meaning in work refers to the subjective experience that we entered the product terms of meaning in work and
one’s work has significance, facilitates personal growth, each of the age terms (i.e. linear, quadratic, and cubic)
and contributes to the greater good (Steger et al., 2012). predicting both the presence of meaning in life and the
Conceptually, meaning in work is considered a search for meaning in life. As discussed above, meaning
The Journal of Positive Psychology 3

in work may contribute to higher levels of meaning in Instruments


life, which may decrease the search for meaning in life Life meaning
(Steger & Dik, 2009). Following from this, we predicted
The degree to which participants perceived meaning in
that participants reporting higher levels of meaningful
life was measured by the Presence subscale of the five-
work would show no relation between age and life
item Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ; Steger et al.,
meaning whereas people low in work meaning would
2006). Participants responded to items on a seven-point
show a pronounced, positive quadratic curve (Steger &
scale ranging from 1 (absolutely untrue) to 7 (absolutely
Dik, 2009). In essence, we predicted work meaning
true). Item responses were summed to calculate total
would provide a buffer against the young adulthood dip
scores, where higher scores indicated greater perceived
in life meaning and explain the bump in midlife. We also
meaning in life. Example items include ‘I have a good
hypothesized that the search for meaning in life would
sense of what makes my life meaningful,’ and ‘My life
mirror this result: those high in work meaning would
has a clear sense of purpose.’ In regard to validity,
have no relation between age and the search for mean-
Steger et al. (2006) found the Presence subscale
ing, and those low in work meaning would have a pro-
positively correlated with life satisfaction, positive emo-
nounced, negative quadratic relation between age and the
tions, extraversion, agreeableness, and intrinsic religios-
search for meaning in life (Steger & Dik, 2009; Steger
ity. The presence subscale has been shown to have high
et al., 2009).
internal consistency reliability with coefficient alphas
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

ranging from the low to high 0.80s (Steger et al., 2006;


Method Strack, 2007). In the present study, the estimated internal
consistency reliability was α = 0.92.
Participants
The sample consisted of 926 adults recruited from
Mechanical Turk (MTurk). Participants ranged in age Work meaning
from 18 to 67 (M = 32.57, SD = 10.56) and self-identi- The degree to which participants felt their work was
fied as female (N = 469, 50.6%), male (N = 450, meaningful was measured using the 10-item Work as
48.6%), and transgender (N = 6, 1%). In terms of race/ Meaning Inventory (WAMI; Steger et al., 2012). Sample
ethnicity, participants mainly self-identified as White/ items include ‘I have found a meaningful career,’ and
European American/Caucasian (N = 720, 77.8%), with ‘The work I do serves a greater purpose.’ Participants
remaining participants identifying as African/African- answered items on a seven-point scale ranging from 1
American (N = 69, 7.5%), Hispanic/Latina/o American (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Points from
(N = 54, 5.8%), Asian/Asian American (N = 37, 4.0%), each item were summed to calculate a total score, with
Asian Indian (N = 19, 2.1%), American Indian/Native higher scores representing higher levels of work mean-
American (N = 11, 1.2%), Pacific Islander (N = 2, ing. In the instrument development study, Steger et al.
0.2%), Arab American/Middle Eastern (N = 1, 0.1%), (2012) found the scale to correlate in the expected direc-
and Other (N = 11, 1.3%). In terms of education, 10% tion with overlapping variables, such as career commit-
(N = 92) had a high school education or less, 2.7% ment (0.68), presence of life meaning (0.57), job
(N = 25) had a trade or vocational school diploma, satisfaction (0.56), and calling (0.54). Furthermore,
34.5% (N = 319) had some college, 37.4% (N = 346) Steger et al. (2012) found the WAMI to have high inter-
had a college degree, and 15% (N = 139) had a profes- nal consistency reliability (α = 0.93), and in the present
sional or graduate degree. Of the sample, 74% (N = 683) study, the estimated internal consistency reliability was
of participants were employed fulltime and 26% α = 0.95.
(N = 240) were employed part-time. The sample cap-
tured a wide range of occupations with 457 unique job
titles represented. The most frequently reported job titles Procedure
included manager/director (7.4%; N = 69), teacher Data for this study were collected as part of the Career
(4.2%; N = 39), administrative assistant (3.9%; N = 36), and Life Longitudinal Study (CALLS). The CALLS is a
salesperson (3.4%; N = 31), business owner (3.2%; five-wave survey that obtained data from working
N = 30), analyst (2.9%; N = 27), engineer (2.4%; American adults in three month intervals over the course
N = 22), customer service representative (2.1%; N = 19), of one year. In the present study, we used data from only
researcher/scientist (1.7%; N = 16), server (1.5%; the first wave. In total, 1034 participants completed the
N = 14), clerk (1.3%; N = 12), cook (1.3%; N = 12), first wave of the study. However, 83 participants were
nurse (1.2%; N = 11), accountant (1.1%; N = 10), and missing data on at least one of the study variables (i.e.
cashier (1.0%; N = 9). age, life meaning, and work meaning). Given the large
4 B.A. Allan et al.

sample size, these cases were deleted listwise. As Measurement invariance


described below, one participant was also removed as an Unlike the MLQ (Steger et al., 2009), the invariance of
outlier. Finally, 24 participants were not currently the WAMI has not been tested with different age groups,
employed so were removed for not meeting inclusion so conducting analyses of the WAMI is premature with-
criteria. The final sample was a total of 926 participants. out checking if the WAMI model is consistent for people
The CALLS used MTurk to collect data for all five in various age groups. Therefore, as a preliminary analy-
waves of the study. MTurk is an online participant sis, we tested the invariance of the WAMI with the same
source that allows people to complete surveys for mone- Erikson age groups used by Steger et al. (2009). Testing
tary compensation, although most respondents reports the measurement invariance of the WAMI involved
completing surveys mostly for enjoyment (Buhrmester, establishing the fit of the measure using confirmatory
Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). Recent reviews and studies factor analysis and successively constraining parameters
examining MTurk have largely concluded that it pro- across age groups to evaluate if this reduces the fit of
duces valid data that are comparable to laboratory and the model (Little, 2008). A reduction in the fit of the
other internet recruitment methods (Buhrmester et al., model suggests that the measure’s structure does not
2011; Sprouse, 2011). A link including an informed con- operate consistently for different age groups (Little,
sent document and the survey itself was posted on 2008). The first step is to establish the configural model
MTurk, and in order to participate, participants had to (i) where there are no constraints placed on the parameters.
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

be over the age of 18, (ii) reside within the USA, and This model represents the average fit of the model across
(iii) be employed at least part-time. Participants were all age groups. Next, in weak factorial invariance, all
given $0.50 for each wave of the survey completed, factor loadings are constrained to be the same for each
which is on par with typical amounts offered to partici- group. Finally, in strong factorial invariance, both the
pants on MTurk. factor loadings and the indicator means are constrained
to be the same for each group.
To evaluate the models we used confirmatory factor
Results analysis with maximum likelihood estimation in AMOS
Preliminary analyses 18 (Arbuckle, 2007). We chose fit indices that minimized
None of the study variables had skewness or kurtosis likelihood of Type 1 and Type II error (Hu & Bentler,
values over one, and all except age appeared normally 1999). These included the chi-square test (χ²), the com-
distributed upon visual inspection. Age was somewhat parative fit index (CFI), the root mean squared residual,
positively skewed, but since skewness and kurtosis were and the root mean square error of approximation
not unreasonably high, age was not transformed. All (RMSEA). A significant χ² can indicate poor fit; how-
variables were inspected for outliers. Only one variable ever, this test is unreliable and inflated in larger samples
had a value greater 3.5 standard deviations from the (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Criteria for the CFI have
mean (age = 71). This outlier was removed. ranged from less conservative (CFI ≥ 0.90) to more con-
As discussed above, this study required the creation servative (CFI ≥ 0.95; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Quintana &
of quadratic and cubic age terms. In order to reduce mul- Maxwell, 1999; Weston & Gore, 2006). Similarly, crite-
ticollinearity among terms, the shared variance between ria for the RMSEA has ranged from less conservative
linear, quadratic, and cubic age was removed. This was (≤0.10) to more conservative (RMSEA ≤ 0.08; Hu &
done for the quadratic curve by regressing age onto age- Bentler, 1999; Quintana & Maxwell, 1999; Weston &
squared and saving the unstandardized residuals. For the Gore, 2006). Researchers have noted the need to be ten-
cubic term, age and age-squared were regressed onto tative when using these criteria as strict cut-offs, and
age-cubed, and the unstandardized residuals were saved. other factors such as sample size and model complexity
The residual variables were used as the quadratic and should be considered (Weston & Gore, 2006).
cubic terms. This process ensures that these variables We used the same age groups employed by Steger
represent only the variance unique to the quadratic and et al. (2009): 18–24, 25–44, 45–64, and 65+. However,
cubic terms. In step two, we entered the product terms since there were only four participants in the last group,
of z-transformed age and the three age terms. Linear age we combined it with the 45–64 group, which created
and quadratic age were unrelated to meaningful work three final groups (young adulthood, middle adulthood,
(0.04) and the presence of meaning (0.02) but was and older adulthood). Items were loaded on their respec-
significantly and negatively related to the search for tive subscale factors (i.e. four items for the Positive
meaning (−0.15). Meaningful work was significantly Meaning subscale, three items for the Meaning-Making
related to the presence (0.57) and search (−0.16) for subscale, and three items for the Great Good subscale),
meaning in life. which then loaded on a second-order Work Meaning fac-
tor. For consistency, we allowed the same two errors to
The Journal of Positive Psychology 5

correlate as in the scale development paper (Steger et al.,


2012).
The configural model had acceptable fit indices,
χ²(90, N = 926) = 807.04, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.92; and
RMSEA = 0.09, p < 0.001. Moreover, the fit of the
model did not significantly erode for the weak invariance
model, χ²(108, N = 926) = 823.72, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.92;
and RMSEA = 0.09, p < 0.01, or the strong invariance
model, χ²(126, N = 926) = 880.59, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.91;
and RMSEA = 0.08, p < 0.01. Therefore, because the
factor structure of the WAMI was consistent across the
three age groups, we deemed it acceptable to continue
with the analyses.

The presence of meaning in life


As discussed above, we tested the relation between age
Figure 1. Work meaning moderating the quadric relation of
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

and the presence of meaning in life in a two-step, hierar-


chical regression. In step one, we entered the linear, qua- age and the presence of meaning in life.
dratic, and cubic age terms. In step two, we added the
work meaning and the work meaning by age product Therefore, the linear relation between age and the search
terms. Table 1 displays the results of this regression. In for meaning was moderated by work meaning. Figure 3
step one, the cubic relation between age and meaning in depicts this moderation.
life was positive and significant, although the omnibus
F-statistic was not significant. In step two, work meaning
and the work meaning by quadratic age term were signif- Discussion
icant. Therefore, work meaning moderated the quadratic The goals of the current study were to (i) test linear and
relation between age and the presence of meaning in life. nonlinear relations between age and the presence and
Figure 1 depicts this moderation. search for meaning in life and (ii) examine if work
meaning moderates the relation between age and the
presence and search for meaning in life. Building off
The search for meaning in life Erickson’s theory of lifespan development, findings from
The same regression was conducted for the search for the current study provide insight on how work meaning
meaning in life. As seen in Table 2, the cubic relation and life meaning, and the interaction of the two, function
between age and the search for meaning was significant at different stages of life. These findings serve to repli-
and negative. Figure 2 depicts this relation. In step two, cate previous research (Steger et al., 2009) but are also
linear age, work meaning, and the linear age by work novel in that they demonstrate the relation of age to life
meaning product term were all significant and negative. meaning in linear, cubic, and quadratic fashions and

Table 1. Hierarchical regression examining work meaning as a moderator of the relation between age and the presence meaning in
life.

B β SE B R R² R²Δ F
Step 1
Linear age 0.01 0.01 0.02
Quadratic age −0.002 −0.03 0.002
Cubic age 0.000 0.06 0.000 0.06 0.004 0.004 1.21
Step 2
Linear age 0.001 0.01 0.02
Quadratic age 0.000 −0.002 0.002
Cubic age 0.000 0.02 0.000
Work meaning* 0.27 0.56 0.01
Linear age × work meaning −0.07 −0.01 0.21
Quadratic age × work meaning* −0.004 −0.07 0.002
Cubic age × work meaning −0.000 −0.01 0.000 0.57 0.33 0.32* 63.51*
*p < 0.05.
6 B.A. Allan et al.

Table 2. Hierarchical regression examining work meaning as a moderator of the relation between age and the search meaning in
life.

B β SE B R R² R²Δ F
Step 1
Linear age* −0.11 −0.16 0.02
Quadratic age 0.000 −0.003 0.002
Cubic age* 0.000 −0.07 0.000 0.17 0.03 0.03* 9.01*
Step 2
Linear age* −0.10 −0.15 0.02
Quadratic age 0.000 0.004 0.002
Cubic age 0.000 −0.04 0.000
Work meaning* −0.08 −0.16 0.02
Linear age × work meaning* −0.54 −0.07 0.26
Quadratic age × work meaning −0.002 −0.03 0.002
Cubic age × work meaning 0.000 0.002 0.000 0.24 0.06 0.03* 7.73*
*p < 0.05.

reveal work meaning as a moderator between age and


Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

meaning in life.
Partially mirroring findings from Steger et al. (2009),
the current study found no significant linear or nonlinear
relations between age and the presence of meaning in
life, but age had a negative linear relation with the
search for meaning in life. This result does not support
the theoretical predictions of Erikson’s (1950) generativi-
ty vs. stagnation stage and further adds to the mixed
results of several studies evaluating the relation between
age and meaning in life (Fegg et al., 2007; Morgan &
Robinson, 2013; Reker, 2005; Steger et al., 2009; Van
Ranst & Marcoen, 1997). These studies have found dif-
ferent peaks and valleys in meaning in life across the
lifespan, with almost all finding that meaning in life is at
its highest and the search for meaning it at its lowest for
Figure 2. The cubic relation between age and the search for older adults (Steger et al., 2009). The current study only
meaning in life. replicated the latter finding. Part of this confusion is
likely due to these studies using different measures of
meaning in life. However, this study’s results for the
presence of meaning in life were still different from
Steger et al. (2009), which also used the MLQ. This dif-
ference may be due to differences in samples. For exam-
ple, Steger et al. (2009) used an international sample of
adults, who may or may not have been working, while
this study used a sample of only working adults living in
the USA. Regardless, this nonreplication plus the fact
that meaning in life had no relation with age in our study
suggests that third variables may moderate the relation
between age and meaning in life.
The current study found that work meaning func-
tioned as one of these moderators, partially explaining
the lack of a basic relation between age and life mean-
ing. According to the Bureau of Labor Statistics, individ-
uals spend more time working on weekdays than any
other life activity, including sleep (8.7 h a day; BLS,
Figure 3. Work meaning moderating the linear relation of age 2013). Therefore, it is not surprising that the meaning
and the search for meaning in life. individuals experience at work would have a large effect
The Journal of Positive Psychology 7

on overall life meaning. What might be surprising, how- The relation of work meaning to the search for life
ever, is the manner in which these two variables are meaning by age is also informative. Here, a linear rela-
related in different age groups. The relation of age and tion best fits the data, with the search for life meaning
life meaning for individuals with low work meaning was being lower for older adults regardless of an individual’s
U-shaped, with life meaning being low for individuals in level of work meaning. However, the slope of this rela-
their prototypical working years (ages 20–50) and adults tion is more pronounced for individuals high in work
over 50 reporting higher life meaning. An opposite pat- meaning, especially compared to those low in work
tern existed with individuals high in work meaning, with meaning. Although having and searching for life mean-
life meaning highest for those in their prototypical work- ing are not mutually exclusive (studies have shown that
ing years and lower for those over 50. individuals can feel their life is meaningful and also be
These results exemplify the role that the presence, or searching for more of it; Steger et al., 2012), our results
absence of, work meaning may play in promoting an suggest that individuals who have found meaning in
overall sense of life meaning. For individuals high in work are much less likely to be searching for general life
work meaning, work may represent the dominant way meaning, especially for middle and older adults. Work
one cultivates life meaning (Emmons, 2005; Steger may play such a central role in life meaning that the
et al., 2012). Subsequently, it may be that during these need to find other sources of meaning is low. The slope
prototypical working years, people high work in meaning for those low in work meaning is almost flat, suggesting
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

have plenty of opportunities to contribute to their life the search for life meaning is relatively the same regard-
meaning through work. However, for both high and low less of age group.
work meaning groups, participants in their 60s experi- These results may have important implications for
enced similar levels of life meaning. The reasons for this the study of life meaning for people in different age
finding are largely speculative, but people in the second groups, particularly for individuals high in work mean-
half of life may have more difficulty extracting meaning ing. Although in our sample the presence of life meaning
from work, which in turn may result in this group expe- did not differ as a function of age, individuals in their
riencing lower levels of life meaning. For example, the prototypical working years (ages 20–50) who find mean-
sources of meaning associated with work, such as recog- ing in work were more likely to experience heightened
nition for achievement, meeting basic needs, and materi- levels of life meaning. This age range encompasses a
alism, are lower in older adults (DeVogler & Ebersole, period of life which, on average, is stressful for work
1981; Prager, 1996), and some sources outside of work, and family reasons. In this sense, work meaning might
such as maintaining values and religious practices, are be seen as a buffer, helping individuals maintain a high
higher during this period (Prager, 1996). Similarly, older sense of life meaning regardless of other pressures. How-
adults may be less concerned with generative actions, ever, older adults with high work meaning may not
which may be primarily preformed at work, so work experience the same heightened levels of life meaning as
might lose its potency to influence life meaning younger adults. Work meaning may be less related to life
(McAdams et al., 1993). In either case, work meaning meaning at this time because other sources of meaning
may be stronger for middle age and older adults, but the are more impactful. As such, life domains that bring
relation between work meaning and life meaning may be individual meaning may differ by age group. But where
lower for older adults relative to those in middle age. a potential blind spot occurs is how work meaning
Future studies should explore different moderators and relates to the search for life meaning.
mediators to further elucidate this finding. As evidenced in our data, individuals highest in work
Contrary to people high in work meaning, those low meaning are the lowest in searching for life meaning.
in work meaning may not receive the same type of life Work may be such an all-encompassing source of life
meaning benefit from work. Therefore, for people with meaning for individuals high in work meaning that the
low work meaning in their prototypical working years, need to search for new sources is nonexistent. Older
work might be taxing and uninteresting. Perhaps, work- adults in particular may never have developed the moti-
ing in a low work meaning job almost nine hours a day vation or skills to look for other sources of meaning.
is a drain on overall life meaning. Intriguingly, life The finding that older adults that are low and high in
meaning was higher for individuals older than 50. Our work meaning have relatively the same level of life
data suggests that participants in their 60s (those high meaning life suggests that no matter how much work
and low in work meaning) display relatively the same meaning an individual has, it may be important to be
amount of life meaning. Like the high work meaning continually searching for other meaning sources.
group, older adults low in work meaning may be more Finally, this study was based in a developmental per-
likely to find life meaning in other activities, perhaps as spective, namely, that individuals in different age groups
sources of meaning change during this time (Ebersole & might experience different levels of work and life mean-
DePaola, 2001; Prager, 1996). ing. This developmental perspective assumes that these
8 B.A. Allan et al.

potential differences would exist for a single individual yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Sci-
as they enter different age groups (e.g. if we assessed a ence, 6, 3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980
DeVogler, K. L., & Ebersole, P. (1981). Adults’ meaning in
40-year olds search for life meaning now, we would
life. Psychological Reports, 49, 87–90. doi:10.2466/
assume that 30 year later when he was 70 it would be pr0.1981.49.1.87
lower). However, this perspective does not account for Dik, B. J., Duffy, R. D., & Eldridge, B. (2009). Calling and
potential generational or cohort effects. For example, it vocation in career counseling: Recommendations for pro-
might be that the older adults in our sample come from moting meaningful work. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 40, 625–632. doi:10.1037/
a generation that is not prone to searching for meaning –
a0015547
this variable is low now and would have been low if we Duffy, R. D., Allan, B. A., Autin, K. L., & Bott, E. M. (2013).
assessed these participants 30 years ago. Longitudinal Calling and life satisfaction: It’s not about having it, it’s
research would be required to understand whether a about living it. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60,
developmental perspective offers the best explanation for 42–52. doi:10.1037/a0030635
Ebersole, P., & DePaola, S. (2001). Meaning in life categories
these differences, but considering a generational perspec-
of later life couples. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplin-
tive as an alternative seems prudent given our cross ary and Applied, 121, 185–191. doi:10.1080/
sectional data. 00223980.1987.9712656
Emmons, R. A. (2003). Personal goals, life meaning, and
virtue: Wellsprings of a positive life. In C. M. Keyes &
Limitations and future directions J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

life well-lived (pp. 105–128). Washington, DC: American


The results and conclusions from this study need to be Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10594-005
considered in light of a number of important limitations. Emmons, R. A. (2005). Striving for the sacred: Personal goals,
First and foremost, as noted above, this study was cross life meaning, and religion. Journal of Social Issues, 61,
sectional and examined separate individuals at different 731–745. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00429.x
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York, NY:
life time points. Doing so allowed the tracking of trends
W. W. Norton.
between variables and age, but it is impossible to make Fegg, M. J., Kramer, M., Bausewein, C., & Borasio, G. D.
any claims about causality or the life track of specific (2007). Meaning in life in the Federal Republic of
individuals. Only long-term longitudinal studies evaluat- Germany: Results of a representative survey with the sche-
ing single cohorts as they age can address how meaning dule for meaning in life evaluation (SMiLE). Health and
Quality of Life Outcomes, 5, 59–67. doi:10.1186/1477-
in life truly develops of over time. Second, data from
7525-5-59
this study were collected using an online data collection Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes
service, MTurk. Although numerous articles have dem- in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria ver-
onstrated the validity of data collected using this method sus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Mul-
(Buhrmester et al., 2011; Sprouse, 2011), it is difficult to tidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. doi:10.1080/
10705519909540118
determine the generalizability of results gained form this
Kotre, J. N. (1984). Outliving the self: How we live on in
type of sample. Third, this study only examined work future generations. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
meaning as a potential predictor of life meaning, limiting Little, T. D. (2008, April). Factorial invariance revisited: Are
our conclusions to the links between these two variables. we there yet? Invited 3-hour workshop at the society for
It will be important that future research examines how applied multivariate research, Kansas City, MO.
McAdams, D. P., de St. Aubin, E., & Logan, R. L. (1993).
work meaning links to life meaning after accounting for
Generativity among young, midlife, and older adults. Psy-
other sources of life meaning. chology and Aging, 8, 221–230. doi:10.1037/0882-
7974.8.2.221
Morgan, J., & Robinson, O. (2013). Intrinsic aspirations and
References personal meaning across adulthood: Conceptual interrela-
Ang, R. P., & Jiaqing, O. (2012). Association between caregiv- tions and age/sex differences. Developmental Psychology,
ing, meaning in life, and life satisfaction beyond 50 in an 49, 999–1010. doi:10.1037/a0029237
Asian sample: Age as a moderator. Social Indicators Prager, E. (1996). Exploring personal meaning in an age-differ-
Research, 108, 525–534. doi:10.1007/s11205-011-9891-9 entiated Australian sample: Another look at the sources of
Arbuckle, J. L. (2007). Amos 18 user’s guide. Chicago, IL: meaning profile (SOMP). Journal of Aging Studies, 10,
SPSS. 117–136.
Baum, S. K., & Stewart, R. B. (1990). Sources of meaning Pratt, M. G., & Ashforth, B. E. (2003). Fostering meaningful-
through the lifespan. Psychological Reports, 67, 3–14. ness in working and at work. In K. S. Cameron, J. E.
doi:10.2466/PR0.67.5.3-14 Dutton, & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational
Bonebright, C. A., Clay, D. L., & Ankenmann, R. D. (2000). scholarship (pp. 309–327). San Francisco, CA: Berrett-
The relationship of workaholism with work-life conflict, Koehler.
life satisfaction, and purpose in life. Journal of Counseling Quintana, S. M., & Maxwell, S. E. (1999). Implications of
Psychology, 47, 469–477. recent developments in structural equation modeling for
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). counseling psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 27,
Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, 485–527. doi:10.1177/0011000099274002
The Journal of Positive Psychology 9

Reker, G. T. (2005). Meaning in life of young, middle-aged, in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood. The
and older adults: Factorial validity, age, and gender invari- Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 43–52. doi:10.1080/
ance of the Personal Meaning Index (PMI). Personality 17439760802303127
and Individual Differences, 38, 71–85. doi:10.1016/ Strack, K. M. (2007). A measure of interest to logotherapy
j.paid.2004.03.010 researchers: The meaning in life questionnaire. The Interna-
Schnell, T., & Becker, P. (2006). Personality and meaning in tional Forum for Logotherapy, 30, 109–111.
life. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 117–129. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.030 statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
Sprouse, J. (2011). A validation of Amazon Mechanical Turk U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2013,
for the collection of acceptability judgments in linguistic June 20). American time use survey – 2012 results: News
theory. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 155–167. release. Retrieved from http://www.bls.gov/news.release/
doi:10.3758/s13428-010-0039-7 pdf/atus.pdf
Steger, M. F., & Dik, B. J. (2009). If one is looking for mean- Van Ranst, N., & Marcoen, A. (1997). Meaning in life of
ing in life, does it help to find meaning in work? Applied young and elderly adults: An examination of the factorial
Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1, 303–320. validity and invariance of the life regard index. Personality
doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01018.x and Individual Differences, 22, 877–884. doi:10.1016/
Steger, M. F., Dik, B. J., & Duffy, R. D. (2012). Measuring S0191-8869(97)00011-1
meaningful work: The work and meaning inventory Weston, R., & Gore, P. A. (2006). A brief guide to structural
(WAMI). Journal of Career Assessment, 20, 322–337. equation modeling. The Counseling Psychologist, 34, 719–
doi:10.1177/1069072711436160 751. doi:10.1177/0011000006286345
Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The Yeager, D. S., & Bundick, M. J. (2009). The role of purposeful
Downloaded by [Richard Douglass] at 08:35 30 August 2014

meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the presence of work goals in promoting meaning in life and in schoolwork
and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psy- during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24,
chology, 53, 80–93. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80 423–452. doi:10.1177/0743558409336749
Steger, M. F., Oishi, S., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Meaning in
life across the life span: Levels and correlates of meaning

You might also like