Merritt 1987

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research Volume 30, 539-552, December 1987

S T O R Y G R A M M A R A B I L I T Y IN C H I L D R E N W I T H A N D W I T H O U T
LANGUAGE DISORDER: STORY GENERATION, STORY
RETELLING, AND STORY COMPREHENSION
D O N N A D I S E G N A MERRITT BETTY Z. LILES
The University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT

Twenty language-impaired and unimpaired children ages 9:0 to 11:4 participated in three story tasks. The children generated
three original stories, retold two adventure stories, and then answered two sets of comprehension questions after each retelling.
Stein and Glenn's (1979) story grammar rules were adapted and used to analyze the narratives. The generated and retold stories
produced by the language-disordered children contained fewer complete story episodes, a lower mean number of main and
subordinate clauses per complete episode, and a lower frequency of use of story grammar components than those of the control
group. The story hierarchies produced by both groups were highly similar, though, in both story generation and story retelling.
The groups also did not differ in their understanding of the factual details of the retold stories, but did differ significantly in their
comprehension of the relationships linking the critical parts of the stories together. The results are discussed relative to cognitive
organizational deficits of language-impaired children.

The communication problems of older language-disor- character then makes attempts at solving the problem
dered children have begun to receive increased attention (attempt), meeting with success or failure (direct conse-
in the literature (Ahram & Nation, 1980; Hall & Tomblin, quence). Depending on the outcome, the protagonist may
1978; King, Jones, & Lasky, 1982; Nippold & Fey, 1983; try another strategy or enlist the aid of other characters,
Wallach & Lee, 1980). It has been established that chil- and the story usually ends with the character's emotional
dren identified as language impaired continue to function response to what has occurred (reaction).
with the long-term effects of the language disorder, even A subset of these six story components comprise an
with appropriate intervention and maturity. Specifically, episode structure representing the most salient unit of
they have difficulty forming verbal abstractions and per- analysis in stories. Episodes are comprehended at an
forming the logical operations required to interpret the intuitive level (van Dijk, 1981), are consistent across
complex relationships expressed in language. They also various story grammars (Mandler & Johnson, 1977;
have deficits in formulating and producing spoken lan- Rumelhart, 1975; Stein & Glenn, 1979; Thorndyke, 1977),
guage, and these difficulties are reflected in poor aca- and have been found to have psychological reality
demic progress and social failure (Bennett & Runyan, (Glenn, 1978; Haberlandt, Berian & Sandson, 1980; Yussen,
1982; Maxwell & Wallach, 1984; Stark, 1975). 1982). A complete episode has been described by Stein
Concurrent with this awareness of the long-term effects (1979) as a behavioral sequence containing:
of language disorder has been a shift in emphasis from an 1. some reference to the motivation or purpose of the
analysis of words and sentences to longer linguistic units, character's behavior (an initiating event or an inter-
including discourse. The intent of this investigation was nal response);
to examine one form of discourse, story narratives, in 2. an overt goal-directed action (an attempt) and;
older language-impaired children. Story narratives are 3. the attainment or nonattainment of the goal (a direct
joined together in predictable rule-governed ways with consequence).
stable organizational patterns representing specific types Setting information and reactions provide additional
of temporally and causally related information. These information in stories but are not crucial to the complete
rules, referred to as a story grammar, form a cognitively episode structure (Peterson & McCabe, 1983). If either a
based framework, or "schema," that aids the speaker in motivating state, an attempt, or a consequence is missing
generating a story and guides the listener in the compre- from an event sequence, then the episode is judged
hension and retelling of stories (Mandler & Johnson, incomplete.
1977; Rumelhart, 1975; Stein & Glenn, 1979). The spontaneously-generated stories of children have
Stein and Glenn (1979) have specified a set of story been analyzed using developmental models (Applebee,
grammar rules that include a network of story compo- 1978; Botvin & Sutton-Smith, 1977), and a story grammar
nents and the relations linking the components together. approach. Stein and Glenn (1982) found that normally
A prototypical story usually involves an animate or inan- achieving school-age children were able to produce goal-
imate protagonist in a particular time, location, or context based episode stories, but that even some sixth graders
(setting information) who faces a physical obstacle, moral generated nonepisodic sequences at times. Story gram-
dilemma, environmental occurrence or personal problem mars have been used extensively to study story retelling
(the initiating event). The character responds to the and story comprehension in normal children, with groups
situation and a p/an is devised (internal response). The ranging from preschoolers through college age students

© 1987, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 539 0022-4685/87/3004-0539501.00/0

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
540 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 539-552 December 1987

(Mandler & Johnson, 1977; Rumelhart, 1975; Stein & comparison of story retelling and story generation may
Glenn, 1979). In general, children by age 6 years have contribute to some conclusions regarding the relative
been found to be able to retell and comprehend canonical usefulness of one of the tasks.
stories well; normally developing 9-year-olds have been In addition to comparing the control and language-
found to produce the primary components of a story disordered children's use of a story grammar across story
grammar. Even young children appear to rely on the generation and story retelling, the children were inter-
episode structure to guide them in story tasks. viewed using two sets of questions structured to assess
Story generation ability of disordered groups has not their understanding of the stories presented in the retell-
been investigated extensively, with the exception of Roth ing task. These questions were administered to deter-
and Spekman's (1986) finding that a story grammar anal- mine whether the story content or vocabulary used in the
ysis is sensitive to the kinds of organizational difficulties retelling task was beyond any subject's level of under-
noted in learning-disabled students. In contrast, story standing or attention (Set I) and whether the children's
retelling and comprehension ability of disordered popu- knowledge of questions structured to represent a story
lations has received more attention, including research grammar (Set II) would prove related to their production.
with the deaf (Gaines, Mandler, & Bryant, 1981), men-
tally retarded children (Bacon & Rubin, 1983; Luftwig &
Greeson, 1983), and learning-disabled students (Feagans METHOD
& Short, 1984; Weaver & Dickinson, 1979), with these
disordered populations routinely retelling less story in- DESIGN
formation.
Investigations of narrative ability specific to language- Children's use of a story grammar was measured in
impaired children indicate that they are able to compre- three tasks including story generation, story retelling, and
hend and produce logical relationships across events less story comprehension. Two groups of 20 children first
well than normal children (Graybeal, 1981; Johnston, generated three original stories and then retold two
1982; Liles, 1985; Merritt & Liles, 1984) and that they narratives, with the order of presentation counterbal-
display poorer linguistic cohesion across sentences anced in both tasks. Story comprehension followed each
(Liles, 1985). Although the previous investigations indi- of the two retelling attempts and included sets of factual
cate that language-impaired children do indeed present information and story grammar questions. Stein and
deficits in narrative ability, there remain questions as to Glenn's (1979) story grammar model was adapted and
whether these data fully account for the types of deficits expanded to analyze the children's stories and their
observed in this disordered population. responses to the comprehension questions.
As previously noted, adequate production and compre-
hension of a story grammar depend on the logical se-
quence of cognitively based story structures or "sche- SUBJECTS
mas" (Stein & Glenn, 1979). These structures guide the
individual's interpretations, expectations, and inferences Given previous investigations indicating that normally
about the possible relationships expressed in a story. developing children by age 9 years are able to generate,
Although the relationships are naturally realized through retell, and comprehend stories, 40 children between the
information related to a particular story, the structures are ages of 9:0 (years:months) and 11:4 were chosen as
not considered to be completely dependent on specific subjects, including 22 boys and 18 girls. All of the
content. The purpose of this investigation was to examine children were from white, middle class suburban families
these cognitive structures in the narratives of 9- to 11- and were attending one of eight public elementary
year-old language-impaired children and compare them schools.
with narratives of control subjects who were not language
impaired. If the use of story grammar organization distin-
guishes the groups, we may conclude that some aspects of Language-disordered children
the language-impaired children's poorer narrative ability
can be attributed to less than adequate use of these The 20 children composing the language-disordered
cognitively-based organizational structures. More specif- group had a documented history of language impairment.
ically, some language-disordered children's apparent lan- They had been diagnosed as preschoolers or in the early
guage impairment may be related to a cognitively-based primary grades, and they continued to manifest persistent
organizational deficit. communication problems that required the services of a
Previous investigations of narrative ability have fo- speech-language pathologist in ongoing management
cused on either story generation or story retelling. The programs. In most cases, the etiology of the language
present investigation was intended to present a more disorder was unknown, but their deficits did not include
complete verification of children's use of a story grammar vision or hearing loss, seizure disorder, attention deficit
by evaluating both story generation and story retelling. If disorder requiring medication, known organic/neurologi-
the logical structures in stories represent a cognitively cal problems, or primary psyehosocial problems. All of
based schema, then the children's use of these structures the children had good speech intelligibility and normal
should reveal some similarities across tasks. In addition, a intelligence as measured by a Performance IQ on the

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
MERRITT & LILES: Story Grammar Ability 541

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children--Revised thought was obviously lost. Too much structure (e.g.,
(Wechsler, 1974) within one standard deviation of the word lists) provided too many constraints and yielded
mean. Although many of the children were considered short story samples usually including a main character
"at risk" academically by their respective classroom and a few random events without a logical goal, plan, or
teachers, none were receiving learning disability support. resolution.
The children were chosen from caseloads based on The following three stems were used in the story
diagnostic test results indicating a mild to moderate generation task:
language impairment. Tests that had been most com- 1. "Once upon a time, two friends were in a deep and
monly administered in earlier efforts to diagnose and dark c a v e . . . "
assess language were the following: Peabody Picture 2. "One day, a pilot was flying a plane through the
Vocabulary Test-Revised (Dunn & Dunn, 1981); Test towering m o u n t a i n s . . . "
of Language Development-Intermediate (Hammill & 3. "Once, there was a family who were in the hot
Newcomer, 1982); The Word Test (Jorgensen, Barrett, desert..."
Huisingh, & Zaehman, 1981); Detroit Tests of Learning
Aptitude-2 (Hammill, 1985); Developmental Sentence
Analysis (Lee, 1974); and Clinical Evaluation of Lan- Story Retelling
guage Functions (Semel & Wiig, 1980).
The mean age of the 20 language-disordered children Two additional multiple episode adventure stories,
was 10:2. Age and gender were distributed as follows: Buried Alive and Shipwrecked, were chosen from the
9:0-9:6 (4 girls, 3 boys); 9:7-10:0 (2 girls, 1 boy); same Xerox Education series for the story retelling task.
10:1-10:6 (2 girls, 2 boys); 10:7-11:0 (0 girls, 3 boys); The stories represented a text consistent with the internal
11:1-11:4 (1 girl, 2 boys). structure of story grammar and also had a high interest
value. Each story contained a protagonist facing an envi-
ronmental obstacle. It was necessary for the protagonist to
Control Subjects formulate a plan and make multiple attempts at resolving
the situation. Ultimately, the plan was successful in each
An additional 20 children composed the control group story with positive consequences in the end.
and were drawn from the same eight public schools as the The stories (see Appendix A) were rewritten to shorten
language-disordered group. They were matched with the the presentation time needed and to simplify the vocab-
language-disordered children for age and gender, with a ulary. Each consisted of four episodes, 45 clauses, and
mean age of 10:2 and the same distribution of boys and bad an average of 28 sentences (with a range of 27 to 29).
girls. The children also had normal visual acuity and The stories were analyzed using the Dale-Chall formula
hearing sensitivity and average intelligence as measured (Dale & Chall, 1948), and had a readability level at or
by group IQ tests administered within their respective below the fourth grade. A story grammar analysis was also
schools. According to their classroom teachers, all of the completed on each story using the method described by
children were average students who did not demonstrate Stein and Glenn (1979). This analysis yielded the number
any particular academic difficulties or exceptional abili- of distinct story components in each story grammar cate-
ties. None had received at any time either special educa- gory (i.e., the number of setting statements, reaction
tion or speech and language support services. statements, etc.).

MATERIALS
Story Comprehension

Story Generation Story comprehension involved two sets of questions for


each of the adventure stories used in the story retelling
Story stems (N. L. Stein, personal communication, task. Eight questions were designed for each story to
March 12, 1984; Stein & Glenn, 1982) were used as evaluate comprehension of factual information about the
stimuli in the story generation task. The stems were characters and events to measure the child's overall
derived from three stories selected from a collection of attention to the specific details of the story. Eight addi-
multiple episode adventure tales written for elementary tional questions were designed for each story to measure
school children and distributed by Xerox Education Pub- story grammar knowledge. These questions were struc-
lieations (Verdick, 1972, 1973, 1978). They consisted of a tured to evaluate the child's understanding of the causal
protagonist and setting information designed to evoke a relations in the story in the event that these relationships
potential conflict that would require a goal-based se- were not expressed in the retellings (see Appendix B).
quence of events. Story stems were chosen for use as
stimuli in the story generation task because pilot data had
indicated that language-disordered children in this age PROCEDURE
range could not generate a story if either too little or too
much structure was imposed. A lack of structure Each child completed all three story tasks in one sitting
prompted lengthy stories in which the child's train of and was tested individually in an isolated room in school

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
542 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 539--552 December1987

by a single examiner. The generated and retold stories pleted after the retelling of the demonstration story and
and the question/answer interviews were audio-tape re- following the two test stories.
corded. Following a short period of conversation to gain
rapport, the first task was presented.
SCORING METHOD
Task 1--Story Generation The generated and retold stories and the responses to
the comprehension questions were transcribed verbatim.
The story generation task was presented first for all The generated and retold protocols were separated into
subjects and was introduced by informally asking the main and subordinate clauses and then categorized into
child for assistance in composing stories for younger story components using adaptations of Stein and Glenn's
children. A demonstration story stem ("One day, a soldier procedure (1979) described in Appendices C, D, and E.
was in a thick jungle . . .") was presented as a training
The story grammar analysis of the three generated and
procedure following these instructions:
two retold stories yielded six raw scores for each story
"I'm going to tell you the first part of a story and I want you including one score for each of the total number of
to make up the rest of it. Take your time and think about occurrences of the six story components (settings, initiat-
what might happen next. Then, tell me a good story." ing events, internal responses, attempts, direct conse-
quences, and reactions). An episode analysis was also
The three story stems were presented one at a time in a done on the generated and retold stories, yielding the
counterbalanced order. The children were allowed as total number of complete and incomplete episodes for
much time as necessary to formulate the stories, and short each story. An episode sequence was judged complete if
breaks were given between the presentations. it contained at least one initiating event or internal
response plus an attempt and a direct consequence.
Task 2--Story Retelling Responses to both the factual information and story
grammar comprehension questions were scored as cor-
The stories for the story retelling task were presented rect or incorrect. For each child, a total score of 16 correct
by the examiner via a black and white video tape on a was possible for the factual information questions and 16
portable 9-inch Sony T.V. The task was introduced by correct for the story grammar questions. Specific criteria
saying to each child: were established for each probe that included the most
salient information required to answer the question ade-
"I'm going to be on the T.V. telling you some stories. quately. Responses that circumvented the criteria were
Watch the screen and listen carefully to each one, because scored as errors, as were answers contradicting the orig-
when I'm finished, I want you to tell the story back to me.
I'll be asking some questions about the story too, so listen inal story.
carefully." Story comprehension was reported as two raw scores,
one representing the total number of correct responses to
A demonstration story was presented first for practice. the factual information questions averaged across the two
When the video tape was completed, the examiner retold stories, and the second raw score reflecting the
paused for 5 s and then requested a retelling by saying to total number of correct responses to the story grammar
the child: questions, also averaged across the two stories.

"Now you tell me the story."


Scoring Reliability
No verbal reinforcement was given during the retellings,
but encouragement and enthusiasm were expressed with
Twenty-five percent of each of the generated and retold
head nods, et cetera. A short break was provided, and
stories were independently analyzed by a second exam-
then the two test stories, Buried Alive and Shipwrecked,
iner to determine an interjudge reliability of 85.9% for the
were presented one at a time in a counterbalanced order
story generation procedure and 94.8% for the story retell-
following the same procedure as the demonstration item.
ing procedure. Twenty-five percent of the comprehen-
sion questions were also rescored by a second examiner,
Task 3--Story Comprehension and a 94.3% reliability was established.

The sixteen comprehension questions designed for


each story in Task 2 followed each story retelling. The RESULTS
task was introduced by telling the child:
"Now I want you to answer some questions about the AGE
story. Listen carefully, and give me the best answer you
can.
When the groups were both separated and combined,
No time limitations were imposed and the questions were the results of Pearson product moment correlation coef-
repeated once upon request. This procedure was com- ficients indicated no significant correlations between age

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
MERRITT & LILES: Story Grammar Ability 543

TABLE 1. Use of story components in story generation by control and language-disordered


children.

Language-Disordered
Control Group (N=20) Group (N=20)
Story Grammar Components Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
Settings 3.5 1.7 1-8 2.7 2.3 0-10
Initiating Events 5.2 3.0 1-15 4.6 1.8 1-9
Internal Responses .8 .6 0-2 .5 .5 0-2
Attempts 4.0 2.5 1-10 2.6 1.3 1-5
Direct Consequences 5.1 2.7 2-11 3.4 1.5 1--6
Reactions .2 .3 0-1 .2 .4 0-1

and any of the dependent variables in question (p > .05) components, F (5, 34) = 1.72, p > .05, during story
for story generation, story retelling, or story comprehen- generation. The cell means outlined in Table 1, however,
sion. Regardless of group membership, the tasks de- indicate that the unimpaired and language-disordered
signed for this investigation were not developmental in children were different in their generation of the six story
nature over the age range of the children. grammar components. With the exception of reaction
information, each mean score for the control group was
higher than that of the language-impaired group.
STORY GENERATION An episode analysis of the generated stories (see Table
2) indicated that the groups were significantly different in
the number of complete episodes produced per story, t
An analysis of the generated stories produced by the (38) = 2.29, p < .05, with the control group scoring a
control and language-disordered children yielded data higher mean (2.1) than the language-disordered group
pertaining to the frequency of use of the six story gram- (1.4). The groups were also significantly different in the
mar components, the number of complete and incomplete number of incomplete episodes generated, t (38) = 3.19,
episodes produced, the number of main and subordinate p < .01. The language-disordered children produced an
clauses, the length of story episodes, and story hierarchy; average of .8 incomplete episodes per story as compared
that is, the relative frequency of use of the six story with .3 for the control group.
grammar components. Descriptive statistics relative to
When asked to generate their own stories, the two
the frequency of use of the story components are pre-
groups of children were not significantly different in the
sented in Table 1. Table 2 represents an episode analysis
total number of main and subordinate clauses produced, t
of the story generation data.
To determine if the groups were significantly different (38) = 1.11, p > .05. As displayed in Table 2, though the
in the frequency of use of the six story grammar compo- control group produced more clauses in story generation
nents during story generation, a two-way (2 x 6) repeated (M = 21.4) than the language-disordered children (M =
measures analysis of variance was executed. A significant 17.9), the differences were not statistically significant.
Between Subjects Main Effect for Group was found, F (1, Episode length in story generation, defined as the
38) = 3.97, p = .05, indicating that the control children mean number of main and subordinate clauses per com-
generated significantly more of the six story grammar plete and incomplete episode, was also investigated by
components than the language-impaired children. computing the number of clauses produced in episodes
There was no interaction effect (Group X Category) divided by the number of episodes in each story and for
between group membership and the six story grammar each child. The control children produced a mean of 10.0

TABLE 2. Story generation episode analysis for control and language-disordered groups.

Language-Disordered Group
Control Group (N=20) (N=20)
Episode Analysis Mean SD Range Mean SD Range t
Number of Clauses 21.4 11.4 9-57 17.9 8.1 7-38 1.11
Number of Complete 2.1 1.2 1-5 1.4 .7 0-3 2.29*
Episodes
Number of Incomplete .3 .4 0-1 .8 .7 0-3 3.19"*
Episodes
Mean Number of Clauses 10.0 2.5 9.6 3.6 .36
Per Complete Episode

df = 38
*p < .05
**p < .01

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
544 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 539-552 D e c e m b e r 1987

clauses per complete episode, as compared with 9.6 for means displayed in Table 4 indicate that for each of the
the language-disordered group, t (38) = .36, p = .72. A six story grammar components the mean score was higher
comparison between the groups of the mean number of for the control group than for the language-disordered
main and subordinate clauses per incomplete episode group.
could not be computed; as only ten of the control subjects As Table 5 also illustrates, the groups were found to be
produced incomplete episodes, as compared with 18 significantly different in the number of complete epi-
children in the language-impaired group. sodes produced during story retelling, t (38) = 2.02, p =
The hierarchy of story components in story generation .05, with the control children scoring a higher mean
was examined to determine the relative frequency of use number of complete episodes (2.7) than the language-
of the six story grammar components produced by the disordered children (2.1). Number of incomplete epi-
unimpaired and language-disordered children (See Table sodes was not found to differentiate the groups, t (38) =
3). The control group used initiating events most fre- .88, p > .05. The control children averaged .9 incomplete
quently, followed by direct consequences, attempts, set- episodes per story as compared with 1.0 for the language-
tings, internal responses, and reactions. The story hierar- disordered children.
chy of the language-disordered children was slightly T-test results comparing the number of clauses pro-
different than that of the unimpaired children. The lan- duced by the control and language-disordered groups of
guage-impaired group also generated initiating events children in story retelling indicated significant differ-
most often, followed by direct consequences. Settings ences relative to the total number of main and subordi-
followed by attempts were then produced, as compared nate clauses produced, t (38) = 2.47, p < .02.
with attempts followed by settings for the control group. Analysis of the length of the episodes produced in story
Internal responses and reactions, in that order, completed retelling yielded significant differences between the two
the story hierarchy for the language-disordered children groups in the mean number of main and subordinate
as it did for the control children. The differences between clauses per complete episode, t (38) = 2.27, p = .03, with
the groups were minimal in their story hierarchies pro- the control children producing a mean of 8.6 clauses per
duced, as a Kendall's Tau coefficient of .99 indicated complete episode as compared with 7.4 for the language-
strong positive agreement between the two groups. disordered group. The groups were not found to differ
significantly in the mean number of main and subordinate
clauses per incomplete episode in stow retelling. The
STORY RETELLING language-disordered children averaged 6.8 clauses per
incomplete episode, only slightly higher than the control
The data collected from the story retelling task were group's average of 6.1, t (38) = 1.81, p = .08.
analyzed in the same manner as that described for the Story hierarchies of the two groups were examined in
story generation task and yielded results pertaining to the stow retelling as in story generation to determine if the
same variables (i.e., frequency of use of the story grammar groups were significantly different in the relative fre-
quency of use of the six story grammar categories. The
components, number of episodes, number of clauses,
hierarchies were found to be identical as the frequency
length of story episodes, and story hierarchy). Table 4
lists the frequency of use of the six story grammar com- data in Table 6 indicates. Attempts were retold most
often, followed by initiating events and direct conse-
ponents for both groups, and Table 5 presents an episode
quences. Settings, internal responses and reactions were
analysis of the story retelling data.
retold less frequently and in that order of occurrence for
Using a two-way (2 x 6) repeated measures analysis of
variance, a significant Between Subjeets Main Effect for both groups. A Kendall's Tau of 1.00 can be computed by
Group was found, F (1, 38) = 7.71, p < .05, indicating that rank ordering the components, but the finding is obvious
that a perfect positive correlation exists between the two
the control children were able to retell significantly more
story grammar components than the language-disordered groups in their hierarchy of use of the story categories.
group. No significant interaction effect between group
and the six story grammar components (Group x Gate-
gory) was found, F (5, 34) = .61, p = .69, however, the cell STORY COMPREHENSION

TABLE 3. Hierarchies of story components in story generation No significant differences were found between the
representing mean frequency of use.
groups in the number of correctly answered factual infor-
mation questions, t (38) = .78, p > .05, as Table 7
Control G r o u p Language-Disordered
Group illustrates. The control children had only a slightly higher
Initiating Events 5.2 Initiating Events 4.6 mean number of correct responses (6.4) than the lan-
Direct 5.1 Direct 3.4 guage-disordered group (6.1).
Consequences Consequences In contrast to these findings, there were significant
Attempts 4.0 Settings 2.7 differences between the groups in the number of cor-
Settings 3.5 Attempts "2.6 rectly answered story grammar questions, t (38) = 4.53, p
Internal Responses .8 Internal Responses .5
Reactions .2 Reactions .2 < .001. The control group scored significantly better on
the questions designed to measure comprehension of the

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
MERRITT & LILES: Story Grammar Ability 545

TABLE 4. Use of story components in story retelling by control and language-disordered


children.

Language-Disordered
Control Group (N=20) Group (N=20)
Story Grammar Components Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
Settings 3.5 1.1 2--6 2.3 1.3 0-5
Initiating Events 4.6 1.3 2-7 3.9 1.4 1-7
Internal Responses 3.1 1.3 0-5 2.0 1.5 0-5
Attempts 4.8 1.4 3-7 4.2 1.5 2-7
Direct Consequences 4.2 1.6 2-7 3.3 1.3 1-6
Reactions 1.9 1.4 0-6 1.1 .5 0-2

dr= 38
*p < .05
**p < .02

relationships expressed in the story (M = 6.1) than the the story components was identical in story retelling and
language-impaired group (M = 4.3). highly similar in story generation. The two groups of
children also did not differ in their ability to answer
factual information questions after story retelling, but the
SUMMARY ability to respond to story grammar comprehension ques-
tions correctly did distinguish the groups.

A general comparison between the groups' perfor-


mances in the three story tasks indicated both differences DISCUSSION
and similarities. Regardless of whether the story task
involved generating a new story or retelling a story, the
control children produced more complete episodes and a
greater frequency of story grammar components than did USE OF STORY GRAMMAR
the language-disordered children. Total number of main
and subordinate clauses produced also distinguished the The finding that language-impaired children used
groups in story retelling, but not in story generation. This fewer complete episodes and story grammar components
indicated that the language-disordered children talked than the unimpaired children may be interpreted to mean
less than the unimpaired children when a story model that the language-impaired children demonstrated a less
had been presented. Mean number of main and subordi- effective use of story grammar organization. While this
nate clauses per complete episode followed the same finding is significant, of equal interest is the manner in
pattern as number of clauses produced; this variable which the children organized the story components into
distinguished the control from the language-disordered episode units in the tasks.
groups in story retelling, but again not in story genera- Both groups of children produced complete and incom-
tion. plete episodes in their generated and retold stories, but
The two groups could not be differentiated by the story the language-impaired children organized their episodes
hierarchies that they produced in either story generation into incomplete units more often than the control group.
or story retelling. The groups' relative frequency of use of An analysis of the groups' incomplete episodes may

TABLE 5. Story retelling episode analysis for control and language-disordered groups.

Control Group Language-Disordered Group


(N=20) (N=20)
Episode Analysis Mean SD Range Mean SD Range t
Number of Clauses 28.0 7.3 1742 22.7 6.5 973 2.47**
Number of Complete Episodes 2.7 .8 14 2.1 1.0 0-4 2.02*
Number of Incomplete Episodes .9 .6 0-2 1.0 .7 0-3 .88
Mean Number of Clauses Per 8.6 1.6 7.4 1.6 2.27*
Complete Episode
Mean Number of Clauses Per 6.1 1.7 6.8 4.6 1.80
Incomplete Episode

df = 38
*p < .05
**p < .02

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
546 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 539--552 D e c e m b e r 1987

TABLE 6. Hierarchies of story components in story retelling the listener questions who did what to whom and with
representing mean frequency of use. what motivation.
In the majority of the language-disordered children's
Language- stories, incomplete episodes occurred at the beginning of
Control Disordered the story generation attempts as the children appeared to
Group Group
have difficulty getting going with the task. Once they
Attempts 4.8 Attempts 4.2 imposed a structure on the story they were more likely to
Initiating Events 4.6 Initiating Events 3.9 generate complete episodes with appropriate direct con-
Direct Consequences 4.2 Direct Consequences 3.3 sequences. It was apparent when this framework was not
Settings 3.5 Settings 2.3 created, for in those narratives the generated stories were
Internal Responses 3.1 Internal Responses 9~.0
Reactions 1.9 Reactions 1.1 short and the episode structure weak.
Similar to the deseription of incomplete episodes in the
story generation task, direct consequence statements
provide some insight into what aspects of the episode were again found to be one of the story grammar compo-
structure differ across the groups. nents most often omitted in story retelling, with the
During story generation, the incomplete episodes pro- language-disordered children having a tendency to aban-
duced by the language-disordered children usually con- don episodes without a logical consequence for the char-
tained a series of loosely joined initiating events com- acter's attempts. For example, in the third episode of
bined with irrelevant information. The initiating events Buried Alive the air inside the truck is running out
established the goal of the episode, and then attempt (initiating event), the main character remembers he has a
statements were interspersed and causally linked to the blowtorch at hand 0nternal response), he lights the torch
events. The episode was then most often abandoned and cuts a hole in the roof (attempts), and then breathes
without a logical ending and with direct consequence the fresh air that pours in (direct consequence). A lan-
guage-disordered child retold this episode, in a some-
information virtually nonexistent. For example, a lan-
what summarized form, up to the point where the char-
guage-disordered child generated a story with multiple
acter made a hole in the roof and melted the snow, but
initiating events in which children in a desert find a
then abruptly went on to retell episode four. The retold
camel. The camel becomes wild and steps on one Of the stories of individual control children did not follow this
ehiidren, and the goal of bringing the child to the hospital specific pattern. Their incomplete episodes usually intro-
is established. One attempt statement involves bringing duced one or more initiating events, but then rambled off,
the hurt child home to his family, but the episode ends at appearing as though the child could not remember what
that point. The listener may infer that the child's medical came next. This did not oceur primarily with only one
needs will be met, but a direct consequence statement story or with one particular episode, and in all eases the
reflecting this is not made, and thus the episode unit is control children regained their sense of the story and
incomplete. In another example, a language-impaired went on to correctly retell the remaining parts.
child generated a short story in which children in a cave It may be postulated that the language-impaired chil-
become cold at night (an initiating event). They cover dren included less direct consequence information in
themselves with blankets (attempt) that they had the their retold stories beeause they retailed or compre-
forethought to bring (setting), but that ended the episode hended this information less well, but this interpretation
sequence. The children observed a bat in the cave and is not supported by the children's responses to the inter=
were eventually rescued, but these events were stated as view questions. Both groups of children gave correct
factual occurrenees not causally linked to the attempts, answers to about six of the eight factual questions follow-
and subsequently not scored as direct consequenees. mg story retelling. This indicated that they attended to
Because few causal links were established in the story, and r e m e m b e r e d the stories equally well, including those
factual details found within direct consequence state-
ments. An analysis of the story grammar questions indi-
TABLE 7. T-test results comparing the control and language- cates a similar Outcome. Although the language-disor-
disordered groups' correct responses to comprehension ques- dered children comprehended fewer of the relationships
tions. expressed in the story grammar components (See Table
7), the specific questions represented by direct conse-
Control quence story grammar eomponents were answered with
Group Language-Disordered about the same degree of accuracy by both groups.
(N=20) Group (N=20)
Mean SD Mean SD t These descriptions of episode structures in the stories
of the language-impaired children indicate that this level
Factual Information 6.4 1.7 6.1 1.3 0.8 of narrative organization was weak. It should be noted
Questions however, that both groups used an overall story organi-
Story Grammar 6.1 1.1 4.3 1.3 4.5*
Questions zation based on the episode structure. As previously
reported, the two groups' story hierarchies, or relative
dr= 38 frequency of use of the story grammar components, were
*p < .001 highly correlated, with attempts, initiating events, and

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
MERRITT & LILES: Story Grammar Ability 547

direct consequences appearing foremost; these hierar- STORY THEMES


chies were identical or highly similar across groups and
across tasks. Although a comparison of themes used in the story
This interpretation of the story hierarchies used by generation task was not a formal question of this investi-
both groups in both story tasks is compatible with Stein gation, an interesting consistency between the groups
and Glenn's (1979) results. Three of the critical compo- was noted. Both the language-disordered and the control
nents of an episode, initiating events, attempts, and direct children primarily used traditional themes in their sto-
consequences, composed the three most frequently used ries. The "Pilot" story most often involved running out of
story grammar components for both the unimpaired and fuel or turbulent weather, losing control of the plane,
language-disordered children. These three eomponents stalling, a crash, and then a rescue attempt. The "Family"
were also recalled by Stein and Glenn's (1979) subjects story involved a basic survival theme most of the time, as
most often, following the major setting. This finding the characters ran out of food and water, built shelter from
indicates that the episode, as defined in this investiga- the elements, lost their children, and saw mirages. The
tion, appears to be a powerful unit that guides children in "Cave" story brought out the most imaginative story
generating and retelling stories. Additional story gram- lines. The children met with animals and bizarre crea-
mar components used within the episode structure serve tures, but also found gold, overcame boulders that im-
to elaborate and develop these basic behavioral se- peded their return home, and suffered numerous injuries,
quences, making the goals and outcomes more under- and many of the stories ended with the ehildren's parents
standable or interesting. admonishing them.
The stems were designed to activate an adventure
theme for the children and this was accomplished. Most
STORY LENGTH characters were rescued from their situations, although a
few died and one was elaborately buried. Miraculous
It is not an uncommon observation that language- recoveries also took plaee, and "magic" accounted for
impaired children produce less quantity in their narra- many of the events that occurred, for example, a tele-
tives than unimpaired children (Liles, 1985). However, phone appearing in the middle of the desert as the family
the data in the present investigation represent some is about to perish. The impact of television shows and
refinement of this observation. Story length, as measured movies was also apparent in both the language-impaired
by number of main and subordinate clauses, did discrim- and control children's generated stories. The Indiana
inate the language-impaired from the control children, Jones theme was popular, as well as "shoot-outs" and
but only in the story retelling task. In story generation, fantasy sequences.
both groups produced approximately the same number of
main and subordinate clauses, even though the language-
disordered children produced fewer story components CONCLUSIONS
and fewer complete story episodes. Analyses of individ-
ual narratives of the language-impaired children indi- The findings of this investigation indicate that while
cated that clauses outside the story grammar structure both unimpaired and language-impaired children appear
contained extraneous information often having no bear- to be guided in their overall story organization by the
ing on the plot or goal. These results indicate that while logical structures defined in story grammar, language-
the language-impaired children generated as much quan- disordered children are less effective in their use of the
tity as their normal peers, they said less qualitatively. grammar. This conclusion is supported by the groups'
In contrast to these results from story generation, in similar story hierarchies, but a lower frequency of use of
story retelling the language-disordered children were story grammar components and complete episode struc-
found to talk less than the control children. They not only tures by the language-disordered children is a pattern
produced fewer complete episodes and story grammar found to be consistent in both story generation and story
components, but also produced fewer main and subordi- retelling. Additional support for this conclusion is evi-
nate clauses. The groups also differed in the mean num- denced in the results of the story comprehension task.
ber of main and subordinate clauses produced per com- Although the older language-impaired children compre-
plete episode in story retelling, with the unimpaired hended and reealled story details as well as the control
children producing more story grammar components children, they had difficulty understanding questions
within complete episode units. Again, this result is con- structured to assess relationships between the story parts.
trasted by the performance in story generation; for in that Graybeal (1981) concludes that language-impaired chil-
task, when the language-impaired children produced dren retell less story information because they r e m e m b e r
complete episodes, they were similar in length to those less of the story. The data from this investigation indicate
generated by the control children. This contrast in the that the language-disordered children attended to the
groups across tasks relative to the length of complete stories as well as the unimpaired children and generally
episodes may be interpreted to mean that the control r e m e m b e r e d the details of events. The language-im-
children were able to use the structure provided them in paired children had a poorer use of their story knowledge,
the story retelling task better than the language-impaired though, as reflected in their less adequate comprehension
children. of the relationships between the story parts and the lower

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
548 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 539-552 D e c e m b e r 1987

frequency of complete episodes produced. This perform- of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 463-469.
ance by the language-impaired group indicates that they GLENN, C. G. (1978). The role of episodic structure and story
length in children's recall of simple stories. Journal of Verbal
do not have a strong c o m m a n d of the overriding story Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 229-247.
structure although they appear to have adequate storage GKaYBEAL, C. M. (1981). Memory for stories in language-im-
and recall capability. paired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 269-283.
Although a statistical verification needs to be pursued, HABE/~LANDT, K., BEPaAN, C., & SANDSON,J. (1980). The epi-
sode schema in story processing. Journal of Verbal Learning
it appears that there is no substantial advantage to a story
and Verbal Behavior, 19, 635-650.
generation task over story retelling. The availability of a HALL, P.K., & TOMBLIN, J. B. (1978). A follow-up study of
story model in story retelling contributes to stronger children with articulation and language disorders. Journal of
interjudge reliability and also allows the use of compre- Speech and Hearing Disorders, 43, 227-241.
hension questions, facilitating a more complete represen- HAMMILL, D. D. (1985). Detroit tests of learning aptitude-2.
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
tation of a child's ability. This type of information may be HAMMILL, D. D. & NEWCOMER, P. L. (1982). Test of language
particularly useful in profiling language abilities across development-Intermediate. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
individual children, particularly in a mild to moderately JOHNSTON, J. R. (1982). Narratives: A new look at communica-
impaired population. tion problems in older language-disordered children. Lan-
guage, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 13, 144--155.
The results of this investigation indicate that a story JORGENSEN, C., BARRETT, M., HUISINGH, R., & ZACHMAN, L.
grammar analysis does characterize some aspects of lan- (1981). The word test. Moline, IL: LinguiSystems.
guage disorder. It may be most useful as a means of KING, R. R., JONES, C., & LASKY, E. (1982). In retrospect: A
assessing the degree to which language-impaired chil- fifteen-year follow-up report of speech-language disordered
dren are able to integrate causal and temporal relations children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
13, 24-32.
within the context of stories. The possibility however, LEE, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, IL:
that these cognitively based organizational deficits are Northwestern University Press.
demonstrated in other verbal or nonverbal communica- LILES, B. Z. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and
tive abilities should encourage researchers to pursue this language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Research, 28, 123-133.
area of inquiry. LUFTIG, R. L., & GREESON, L. E. (1983). Effects of structural
importance and idea saliency on discourse recall of mentally
retarded and nonretarded pupils. American Journal of Mental
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Deficiency, 87, (4), 414-421.
MANDLER, J. M., & JOHNSON, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of
This investigation was made possible by a grant from The things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychol-
University of Connecticut Research Foundation. The authors ogy, 9, 111-151.
would like to thank the students and staff of the Andover, MAXWELL, S.E., & WALLACH, G.P. (1984). The language-
Colchester, and East Hartford, Connecticut, school systems for learning disabilities connection. In G.P. Wallach & K.G.
their cooperation and participation in this project. The contribu- Butler (Eds.), Language learning disabilities in school-age
tions of Robert J. Duffy, Jacqueline Sachs, and Ross Buck are also children (pp. 15-34). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
acknowledged, and appreciation is expressed to Nancy Stein for MER~TT, D. D., & LILES, B. Z. (1984, November). Story recall
methodological assistance and Jack Davis for direction in statis- and comprehension in older language-disordered children.
tical planning and interpretation. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the American
Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Los Angeles, CA.
NIPPOLD, M. A., & FEY, S. H. (1983). Metaphoric understanding
REFERENCES in preadolescents having a history of language acquisition
difficulties. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
AHRAM, D.M., & NATION, J.E. (1980). Preschool language Schools, 14, 171-180.
disorders and subsequent language and academic difficulties. PETERSON, C., & McCABE, A. (1983). Developmental
Journal of Communication Disorders, 13, 159-170. psyeholinguisties: Three ways of looking at a child's narra-
APPLEBEE, A. N. (1978). The child's concept of story. Chicago: tive. New York: Plenum Press.
The University of Chicago Press. ROTH, F.P., & SPEKMAN, N.J. (1986). Narrative discourse:
BACON, E. H., & RUBIN, D. C. (1983). Story recall by mentally Spontaneously generated stories of learning-disabled and nor-
retarded children. Psychological Reports, 53, 791-796. mally achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing
BENNETT, C. W., & RUNYAN, C. M. (1982). Educator's percep- Disorders, 51, 8-23.
tions of the effects of communication disorders upon educa- RUMELHART, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In
tional performance. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services D.G. Bobrow & A.M. Collins (Eds.), Representation and
in Schools, 13, 260-263. understanding: Studies in cognitive science (pp. 265-303).
BOTVlN, G. J., & SUTTON-SMITH,B, (1977). The development of New York: Academic Press.
structural complexity in children's narratives. Developmental SEMEL, E.M., & WIIG, E.H. (1980). Clinical evaluation of
Psychology, 13, 377-388. language functions. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill Pub-
DALE, E., & CHALL, J. (1948). A formula for predicting readabil- lishing Co.
ity. Educational Research Bulletin, 27, 11-20, 28. STARK, J. (1975). Reading failure: A language-based problem.
DUNN, L. M., & DUNN, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabu- Asha, 17, 83P.~-834.
lary test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Ser- STEIN, N. L. (1979). How children understand stories. In L. Katz
vice. (Ed.), Current topics in early childhood education (Vol. 2, pp.
FEAGANS,L., & SHORT, E. J. (1984). Developmental differences 261-290). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
in the comprehension and production of narratives by reading STEIN, N.L., & GLENN, C.G. (1979). An analysis of story
disabled and normally achieving children. Child Develop- comprehension in elementary school children. In R.O.
ment, 55, 1727-1736. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp.
GAINES, R., MANDLER,J. M., & BRYANT, P. (1981). Immediate 53-120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
and delayed story recall by hearing and deaf children. Journal STEIN, N. L. & GLENN, C. G. (1982). Children's concept of time:

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
MERRITT & LILES: Story Grammar Ability 549

The development of a story schema. In W. Friedman (Ed.), 28. cut a hole in the roof, (Attempt)
The developmental psychology of time (pp. 255-282). New 29. and melted the snow above the hole. (Direct Consequence)
York: Academic Press. 30. Sunlight and fresh air poured in. (Direct Consequence)
THORNDYKE, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehen- 31. Jim was relieved, (Reaction)
sion and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 32. but he knew (Reaction)
9, 77-110. 33. it would take a 10ng time for all that snow to melt. (Reaction)
VAN DIJK, T. A. (1981). Episodes as units of discourse analysis. In 34. A whole week went by. (Setting)
D. Tannen (Ed.), Analyzing discourse: Text and talk (pp. 35. One day, two state police officers saw an exhaust pipe
177-195). Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press. sticking out of the snow. (Initiating Event)
VERDICK, M. (Ed.). (1972). Amazing adventures. Middletown, 36. They thought (Internal Response)
CT: Xerox Education Publications. 37. that the driver of the truck might be dead. (Internal Re-
VErtO~CK, M. (Ed.). (1973). Nine daring adventures. Middletown, sponse)
CT: Xerox Education Publications. 38. The officers took shovels out of their ear, (Attempt)
VERDICK, M. (Ed.). (1978). Buried alive. Middletown, CT: Xerox 39. and started digging the snow. (Attempt)
Education Publications. 40. About ten minutes later, they reached the door, (Direct
WALLACH, G. P., & LEE, A. D. (1980). So you want to know what Consequence)
to do with language disabled children above the age of six. 41. and pulled it open. (Direct Consequence)
Topics in Language Disorders, 1, 99-113. 42. Jim smiled at the officers. (Reaction)
WEAVER, P. A., & DICKINSON, D. K. (1979). Story comprehen- 43. He was tired, (Reaction)
sion and recall in dyslexic students. Bulletin of the Orton 44. and he was hungry. (Reaction)
Society, 29, 157-171. 45. But he was alive! (Direct Consequence)
WECHSLER, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for childrere--
Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation. STORY #2---8 HIPWRECKED
YUSSEN, S. R. (1982). Children's impressions of coherence in
narratives. Advances in Reading~Language Research, 1, 1. Once there were three brothers (Setting)
245-281. 2. who often fished together in the ocean. (Setting)
3. They were good sailors, (Setting)
Received June 18, 1986 4. and usually were gone from home for only a short time.
Accepted April 13, 1987 (Setting)
5. One day, they all fell asleep on their boat. (Initiating Event)
Requests for reprints should be sent to Donna DiSegna 6. While they slept, (Initiating Event)
Merritt, 21 Robin Rd, Willimantic, CT 06226. 7. the anchor broke loose, (Initiating Event)
8. and the boat drifted away in the dark night. (Initiating
Event)
9. It finally crashed against some rocks. (Initiating Event)
APPENDIX A 10. The boys woke up frightened, (Internal Response)
11. but then saw an island about a mile from the wrecked boat.
STORIES USED IN THE STORY RETELLINGTASK (Initiating Event)
STORY #1--BURIED ALIVE 12. They swam for their lives, (Attempt)
13. and finally all reached the island. (Direct Consequence)
1. Jim had been a truckdriver for twenty years. (Setting) 14. The boys were grateful to be alive, (Reaction)
2. He was a very careful driver, (Setting) 15. but they knew they were lost. (Reaction)
3. and he never took chances. (Setting) 16. In the beginning, life on the island was very hard, (Setting)
4. One day it had been snowing for several hours. (Initiating 17. The boys couldn't find fresh water or food. (Initiating Event)
Event) 18. But they knew (Internal Response)
5. The roads were getting bad, (Initiating Event) 19. they could survive (Internal Response)
6. and Jim could hardly see (Initiating Event) 20. if they worked together. (Internal Response)
7. where he was going. (Initiating Event) 21. First, they looked for coconuts. (Attempt)
8. He wanted to get home safely. (Internal Response) 22. Then they caught birds with their bare hands, (Attempt)
9. So, he looked for a wide place at the side of the road, 23. and cooked them over an open fire. (Attempt)
(Attempt) 24. They always had enough to eat and drink, (Direct Conse-
10. pulled over his eighteen-wheeler, (Attempt) quence)
11. and fell fast asleep. (Direct Consequence) 25. and never felt hungry again. (Reaction)
12. He was finally able to relax. (Reaction) 26. The blazing sun was always hot on the island. (Setting)
13. Jim woke up many hours later. (Setting) 27. But one day the rainy season began. (Initiating Event)
14. It was dark inside the truck, (Setting) 28. The brothers knew (Internal Response)
15. but his watch said it was morning. (Setting) 29. they had to build a shelter. (Internal Response)
16. The snow on the truck was keeping the sun out. (Initiating 30. They searched the island, (Attempt)
Event) 31. and found parts of their wrecked boat. (Direct Consequence)
17. Jim knew (Internal Response) 32. They tied the wood together, (Attempt)
18. he was trapped. (Internal Response) 33. and built a simple cabin. (Direct Consequence)
19. First, he turned on the windshield wipers. (Attempt) 34. They were happier inside the cabin. (Reaction)
20. Then he tried to push open the door. (Attempt) 35. and kept dry when the rain came. (Direct Consequence)
21. But the wipers and the door wouldn't budge. (Direct Con- 36. The boys still dreamed every night of returning home to
sequence) their family. (Setting)
22. Jim started to worry. (Reaction) 37. One day, they spotted a ship. (Initiating Event)
23. By noontime, it was getting harder and harder to breathe. 38. They became excited, (Internal Response)
(Initiating Event) 39. and set fire to some large bushes. (Attempt)
24. The air in the truck was running out. (Initiating Event) 40. The black smoke rose high in the sky, (Direct Consequence)
25. Jim remembered (Internal Response) 41. and the ship's captain spotted it. (Direct Consequence)
26. he had a blowtorch in the back of the truck. (Internal 42. He ordered his men to go ashore, (Direct Consequence)
Response) 43. where the sailors were welcomed by the three brothers.
27. He lit it, (Attempt) (Reaction)

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
550 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 539-552 D e c e m b e r 1987

44. They shouted their thanks. (Reaction) 1. Read each story in its entirety and then separate it into
45. After fifteen long months on the island, they were finally statements, which are defined as either a main clause or a
going home. (Direct Consequence) subordinate clause.
2. Divide the story into episodes, with the goal or problem of
each episode being identified as a preliminary step.
APPENDIX B 3. Include a statement in the analysis if it meets the criteria
established for one of the six story components listed in
Appendix D.
4. Multiple statements can be scored in each category as tong
STORY COMPREHENSION QUESTIONS as they are connected logically by "AND," " T H E N , " or
STORY #1--BuBIED ALIVE " C A U S E " relations, as described by Stein and Glenn
(1979).
5. When a statement is expanded upon later in the generated
Factual Information Questions story, only the expanded version is scored unless additional
story information is expressed. When this occurs, both
1. What was Jim's job? statements are scored•
2. What was the weather like in this story? 6. Statements are scored even if they are a repetition of the
3. What did Jim do with his truck when the roads got bad? information conveyed in the stem. These statements are
4. What happened when Jim turned on the windshield wip- always scored as Settings.
ers? 7. If a statement meets the criteria for two story components
5. What did Jim do with the blowtorch? (e.g., a Setting and an Initiating Event) then the statement
6. How long was Jim in the truck? is scored as both categories.
7. Who found Jim? 8. A statement is not included in the analysis if any one of the
8. How did the police officers get Jim out of the truck? following conditions is noted:
a) a general comment unrelated to the story, e.g., "I got it,"
Story Grammar Questions "This is hard," "Ok," etc.,
b) repetition of a thought,
1. Why did Jim pull his truck over? c) an unfinished statement that conveys an incomplete
2. Why was Jim able to relax at the beginning of the story? thought, e.g., " H e wanted, so, he wanted, then he
3. Why was it so dark in the truck? wanted..."
4. Why did Jim worry when he discovered the door wouldn't d) false starts, e.g., "And they found parts of t h e i r . . . No, I
budge? mean...
5. Why did Jim get out the blowtorch? e) formal endings, e.g., "That's it," "The e n d , " etc.,
6. Why did Jim feel better after the hole was cut? f) statements in whieh the information is not specific
7. Why did the police officers think Jim might be dead? enough to determine an appropriate story category, ei-
8. Why did the police officers start digging? ther because the referrent is unclear, for example, "And
they took him to this place so they could look at it," or
STORY #So---SHIPWRECKED because the information cannot be interpreted, for exam-
ple, "And then, one day, this old lady was in a pilot."
g) extraneous information, usually descriptive, that does
Factual Information Questions not fit into the story grammar analysis.
h) statements that are contradictory, for example, "The
1. What were the brothers doing on the boat? family didn't have much water," "So then they went in
2. What happened when the boys fell asleep? the house to take a bath."
3. How did the boys get to the island?
4. What did the boys eat?
5. How long were the boys on the island?
6. What did the brothers see one day in the ocean? APPENDIX D
7. How did the boys signal the ship?
8. What did the ship's captain do?
STORY COMPONENT CRITERIA
Story Grammar Questions
1. In the beginning of the story, why were the boys fright- Setting Category
ened?
2. Why did the brothers start swimming? A statement is categorized as a S E T T I N G if:
3. Why were the boys grateful when they got to the island? 1. a major or minor character is introduced, (e.g., "Jim had
4. Why did the brothers work together? been a truckdriver for twenty years."), or
5. Why didn't the boys starve? 2. a location is described, or
6. Why did the boys need to build a cabin? 3. additional information is presented that conveys the habit-
7. Why did the ship's captain see the boys? ual social context (e.g., " T h e boys were usually gone from
8. Why did the captain order his men to go ashore? home for only a short time."), physical context (e,g, "'Life on
the island was hard."), or temporal context of the story, (e.g.,
"A week went by."), or
APPENDIX C 4. a character's habitual state is noted; the state may not have
been caused by any previous occurrence, and may not
cause a subsequent event to happen, (e.g., "'He was a very
careful driver.").
STORY GENERATION SCORING PROCEDURE
Initiating Event Category
The following procedural guidelines were adapted from Stein
and Glenn's (1979) story grammar to analyze the generated A statement is scored as an I N I T I A T I N G E V E N T if it begins
stories: a goal-based episode sequence in the story and causes the main

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
MERRITT & LILES: Story Grammar Ability 551

character to respond. Initiating Events include three types of ally, a Reaction preceeds a Direct Consequence, but the causal
information: connection between the statements needs to be apparent.
1. A character's action or an eyent (e.g., "The boys couldn't Reactions usually occur at the end of an episode, but they can
find fresh water or food."). also be inserted at other points, for example, if a character pauses
2. Natural occurrences, which are changes in the physical to reconsider a consequence and then proceeds.
environment not caused by an animate being (e.g., "One
day it had been snowing for many hours.").
3. Internal events, including a character's internal perception
of an external event (e.g., "One day they spotted a ship."),
or, changes in the character's internal physiological state APPENDIX E
(e.g., "'By noontime, it was getting harder and harder to
breatheY)~
Setting and Initiating Events are distinguished from each STORY RETELLING SCORING PROCEDURE
other in that the Setting provides the context for the story and the
Initiating Event always evokes an immediate response from the Statements elicited during the story retelling task are included
character. in the analysis if they meet the criteria for a specific story
Statements that are general events and do not lead to a goal are category presented in Appendix D and if the following two
not scored, for example, "And then they drank w a t e r . . , and then conditions are met:
they ate . . . . " 1. The statement has to have occurred in the original story. An
exact replica is not required, but the retold statement needs
Internal Response Category to contain the same semantic content as the first version.
For example, " H e Was not dead," conveys the same mean-
A statement is characterized as an INTERNAL RESPONSE if ing as, "'He was alive." Also, if details are omitted, e.g.,
it meets three criteria: numbers, specific times, etc., but the same story informa-
1. it describes the character's psychological state including tion is expressed, then the statement is scored.
emotions, goals, desires, intentions, or thoughts, for exam- 2. The retold statement also needs to express the same story
ple, " H e wanted to get home safely," or, "But they knew information, i.e., the same story component (e.g., Initiating
they could survive if they worked together," or, "They Event), as the original version.
thought that the driver of the truck might be dead," AND, Five additional general scoring procedures are also routinely
2. it is causally related to an Initiating Event in the story, followed:
AND, 1. Only one statement is scored when a child uses two or more
3. it leads to a plan sequence. clauses to express information that had been presented in
only one statement in the original story.
Attempt Category 2. If a child uses one clause to express information conveyed
in two separate statements from the original story, e.g., "'So
A statement is categorized as an A T T E M P T if it represents a he pulled over and fell asleep," and two distinct story
character's overt action toward resolving the situation or achiev- categories are expressed, in this ease, an Attempt plus a
ing a goal. For example, in Buried Alive, the main character Direct Consequence, the statement is scored as both cate-
attempts to get air into his truck by lighting a blowtorch and gories.
cutting a h01e in the roof. 3. When a statement is expanded upon later in the retold
There needs to be a direct causal link or enablement relation story, or self-corrected, only the expanded/corrected ver-
between the Attempt and either the Initiating Event or Internal sion is scored.
ReSponse that Usually preceeds it, or a direct causal link or 4. Word f n d i n g errors are not penalized, e.g., "Bill" for "Jim,"
enablement relation between the Attempt and subsequent Di- "bus" for "truck," "blowerthing" for "blowtorch," "hook"
rect Consequence. for "anchor," etc. (Graybeal, 1981).
5. Syntax errors, e.g., "And they dig," for "They started
Direct Consequence Category digging the snow," are also not penalized.
Statements are not included in the analysis if any one of the
A statement is categorized as a D I R E C T C O N S E Q U E N C E if following conditions is noted:
it marks the direct attainment or nonattainment of the character's i. a general comment or question unrelated to the story,
goal and is the result of one or more Attempt statements. A Direct 2. repetition of a thought,
Consequence usually leads to a character's reaction, but this may 3. an unfinished statement that conveys an incomplete
be unstated in the story. thought,
Direct COnsequences include three types of information: 4. false starts,
1. natural occurrences that influence the resolution of the 5. formal endings,
story by facilitating or impeding attainment of the charac- 6. unclear statements in which the information is not specific
ter's goal, (e.g., "One day the rainy season began."). enough, irrelevant, or contradictory to the original story,
2. a character's action that results in either the attainment of a 7. extraneous information not presented in the original story,
goal or a change in the sequence of events. For example, in e.g., additional conflicts, plans, etc.,
Shipwrecked, the children built a simple cabin to meet 8. statements that convey only part of the information in the
their goal of being sheltered from the rain. original story. For example, "And so they got up," is not
the same as, "The boys woke up frightened," as the
3. End Sta~es, (e.g., "They Were happy inside the cabin.").
Internal Response of fear is not conveyed.
9. statements conveying information that was assumed or
Reaction Category implied in the original story, for example, " O n e days Jim
was riding along," is implied in the first story.
REACTIONS define how a character feels about the attain- 10. statements presented in the wrong sequence such that a
ment or nonattainment 0fa goal (e.g., "Jim was relieved."), what different intent and story category is expressed relative to
the character thinks about it (e.g., "They knew they were lost."), the original story. Occasionally, a child expresses the
or an action that is emotional (e.g., "They shouted their sequence of events in an order different from the Original
thanks."). story but consistent with the meaning of the story, for
A Reaction statement is causally linked to a Direct COnse- example, expressing the Initiating E v e n t and then the
quence, which is usually the preeeeding statement. Occasion- Setting, or a Reaction followed by a Direct Consequence

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
552 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30 552-558 D e c e m b e r 1987

end state. These statements are scored as correct if the on an island," (a Direct Consequence), and several state-
causal link is established. On other occasions, the se- ments later says, "But then they saw the island (an Initi-
quence of events is wrong, the correct story category is ating Event). The statements are not scored if this occurs
expressed in the statements, but the story line is not because the relationship between the story parts is not
logical. For example, a child who says, "Then they went appropriate.

© 1987, American-Speech-Language-Hearing Association 552 0022-4685/87/3004-0552501.00/0

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 05/28/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like