Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232989513

Predation on snakes of Argentina: Effects of coloration and ring pattern on


coral and false coral snakes

Article  in  Studies on Neotropical Fauna and Environment · December 2006


DOI: 10.1080/01650520600630725

CITATIONS READS

22 312

3 authors, including:

Gerardo C. Leynaud Felix B Cruz


National University of Cordoba, Argentina Centro Científico Tecnológico Conicet Comahue
49 PUBLICATIONS   540 CITATIONS    74 PUBLICATIONS   1,575 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Analyzing distributional patterns of amphibians and reptiles to inform conservation decisions, with special focus on Argentina. View project

Wildlife trade, emphasis on herps View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Felix B Cruz on 02 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Studies on Neotropical Fauna and Environment, December 2006; 41(3): 183–188

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Predation on snakes of Argentina: Effects of coloration and ring pattern


on coral and false coral snakes

CARLOS M. BUASSO1, GERARDO C. LEYNAUD1 & FELIX B. CRUZ2


1
Centro de Zoologı́a Aplicada, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Argentina, and 2CONICET – CRUB, Universidad
Nacional del Comahue, San Carlos de Bariloche, Argentina

(Received 5 March 2005; accepted 2 February 2006)

Abstract
The occurrence of coral snake coloration among unrelated venomous and non-venomous snake species has often been
explained in terms of warning coloration and mimicry. In Argentina, no field tests have been conducted to confirm this
mimetic association between one venomous coral species (Micrurus phyrrocryptus, Elapidae) and two non-venomous snake
species with a similar color pattern (Lystrophis pulcher and Oxyrhopus rhombifer, Colubridae). The aims of this work were to
test for the possible aposematic or cryptic function of the ring pattern and coloration of coral snakes and false coral snakes
from central Argentina, and to analyse whether the pattern is effective throughout the year. Predation on snakes was
estimated by using non-toxic plasticine replicas of ringed venomous and non-venomous snakes and unbanded green snakes
placed along transects in their natural habitat during the dry and rainy season. Ringed color pattern was attacked by
predators despite the background color. One of the replica types was attacked more than expected during the dry season,
suggesting that both shape and width of rings may influence the choice by predators. The reaction of predators towards
replicas that mimic snake species with ringed patterns is independent of the geographical region, and we can conclude that
mimicry characteristics are quite general when the true models are present in the area.

Keywords: Aposematic coloration, coral snakes, mimicry, predation, snakes

Introduction association between them (Greene & McDiarmid,


1981; Brodie & Janzen, 1995; Zug et al., 2001).
Venomous snakes, especially some species of the Encounters between predator and prey are rarely
family Elapidae, are common models for mimetic observed in nature. Experimental tests to determine
associations (Pough et al., 1998). The presence of whether colored snakes are avoided by predators and
bright, contrasting color bands in species of these whether such predators distinguish between the
venomous snakes and in many species of the family ringed and ringless patterns have been conducted
Colubridae (non-venomous or of low toxicity) adjust using indirect evidence. Smith (1975) studied the
to the Batesian model of mimetism (Greene & reactions of unexperienced birds on plain, long-
McDiarmid, 1981; Campbell & Lamar, 1989). This itudinally striped and ringed snake replicas in the lab
model is based on the assumption that, by being and gathered evidence for the innate avoidance of
similar to venomous snakes with aposematic color replicas painted with the coral snake pattern. Field
patterns, non-venomous snakes are also avoided by experiments performed with artificial snake replicas
possible predators (Yanosky & Chani, 1988; later demonstrated that free-ranging birds avoid all
Campbell & Lamar, 1989; Brodie III & Moore, ringed patterns found on sympatric snake species in
1995; Greene, 1997; Pough et al., 1998). Parallel Costa Rican rain forests (Brodie III & Janzen, 1995).
variation in the coloration of non-venomous and Plasticine replicas of coral snakes have been used by
venomous snakes throughout different geographical Brodie III (1993) to gather data on rates of
areas provides strong evidence for a mimetic predation in nature. The soft plasticine, which

Correspondence: G. C. Leynaud, Centro de Zoologı́a Aplicada, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Rondeau 798, CC 122, 5000 Córdoba, Argentina.
Fax: +54 351 4332055. Email: gleynaud@efn.uncor.edu
ISSN 0165-0521 print/ISSN 1744-5140 online # 2006 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/01650520600630725
184 C. M. Buasso et al.

retains the imprint of any predation attempt, can be predators through the marks left on the artificial
even used to identify predators. Using this method, replicas.
Brodie III (1993) demonstrated for the first time
that the coral snake pattern reduces avian predation
on replicas of both true coral snakes and coral snake Materials and methods
mimics. Thus, he showed a clear relationship Study area
between the aposematic coloration and the conse-
quence of being avoided by predators (Brodie III, Field work was conducted between October and
1993; Brodie Jr & Brodie III, 1980). However, if a November 2001 (dry season) and March and April
replica resembles a model not present in the area it 2002 (rainy season), in Taninga (31u209S, 65u49W),
will not be recognized as a dangerous prey and will department of Pocho, province of Córdoba
not evoke avoidance (Brodie & Janzen, 1995; (Argentina). The area corresponds to the Chaco
Pfennig et al., 2001). Therefore, only in areas where phytogeographic province, characterized by a
predators coexist with an aposematically colored strongly seasonal climate (Bucher, 1982). Micrurus
venomous snake (5model) may the mimics profit by pyrrhocryptus, Lystrophis pulcher, Oxyrhopus rhombifer
being avoided by predators. and Philodryas patagoniensis inhabit this region, and
In Argentina several snake species are similar to are commonly seen in the field, however, no data are
coral snakes of the genus Micrurus in ringed pattern available on their abundance and we therefore
and coloration. The only species of venomous coral arbitrarily assumed that the four species were equally
snake in the province of Córdoba is Micrurus abundant.
pyrrhocryptus, with triads of black bands separated
by white or yellowish rings alternating with red Data collection and statistical analysis
bands of the same width as the central black band
Replicas of Micrurus pyrrhocryptus, Lystrophis pulcher,
(Cei, 1993). Two non-venomous colubrid snakes of
Oxyrhopus rhombifer and Philodryas patagoniensis
similar size and coloration, Lystrophis pulcher and
(ringless green snake) were formed using pre-colored
Oxyrhopus rhombifer, occur in Córdoba (Cei, 1993).
(red, black, white and green) non-toxic plasticine
No specific field studies have been conducted in
(Figure 1). Replicas were 250 mm long and 10 mm
Argentina to analyze the possible benefits of the
in diameter, approximately the size of adult speci-
mimetic association between these venomous and
mens. They were threaded on to a wire frame and
non-venomous species, as well as of the aposematic
anchored to the ground with a fishing line. The
function of coloration of coral snakes or their
replicas were allowed to air for 48 h before being
mimics, although this is a key point to understanding
placed along transects, so that animals were not
the evolution of this mimicry complex.
attracted or repelled by the odour of the plasticine.
Considering the long dry period at our study area Thirty-six replicas, nine of each of the four types,
(Bucher, 1982) and the fact that the pressure exerted were placed along line transects at 10 m intervals in a
on snakes by predators may increase as a conse- random order. Presence and position of marks of
quence of a decrease in plant cover or as a attacks on the replicas were checked every 15 days.
consequence of a lower prey availability in this Replicas were then reshaped or replaced when
critical period of the year, it is reasonable to think necessary, and relocated to a new transect. A total
that predators respond differently to the encounter of 12 transects were used, six in the dry and six in the
with snakes during the dry season. Therefore, we wet season.
decided to analyze predation on the different species The replicas with ringed color pattern were placed
over the dry and wet seasons. on two different backgrounds to check whether
The general objective of this work was to predators responded to the aposematic effect of
determine if artificial replicas of coral snakes coloration by not biting or whether replicas were not
(Micrurus pyrrhocryptus) and of non-venomous attacked because they were camouflaged on the
snakes with a similar color pattern are avoided by natural background due to a cryptic effect of the
predators in central Argentina. Specifically we aimed color pattern. Therefore, half of the replicas of each
to: (1) test the aposematic or cryptic function of species were placed on white paper sheets to
coloration and ring pattern on the body of Micrurus enhance the contrast and thus visibility of the
pyrrhocryptus, Lystrophis pulcher and Oxyrhopus rhom- colored replicas, whereas the rest were placed
bifer; (2) compare predation frequency on replicas of directly on natural ground. The role of aposematic
the three species mentioned and of a non-mimic coloration was analyzed using 262 contingency
snake (ringless: Philodryas patagoniensis); (3) check tables (x2, p,0.05).
seasonal differences in predation among the species Differences in the number of replicas attacked
considered; and (4) analyze the behavior of snake per type (independently of the background) were
Coral snake mimicry in Argentina 185

Figure 1. Color patterns of the four snakes (left) and the respective plasticine replicas (right). (A) Micrurus phyrrocryptus; (B) Lystrophis
pulcher; (C) Oxyrhopus rhombifer; (D) Philodryas patagoniensis.
186 C. M. Buasso et al.

number of replicas attacked and the sequence of


days that they were exposed (Figure 2) shows an
ascending straight line, indicating that the number of
attacks did not decrease throughout the experiment.
Although the replicas were exposed for a relatively
long period of time, predators did not become
accustomed to their presence.

Influence of background color


There were no significant differences in the number
of attacks to replicas placed on natural ground or on
white paper (Table I), showing that the replicas were
noticed on both backgrounds.
Figure 2. Accumulated number of attacks after a sequence of 90
sampling days.
Response of predators to coloration of artificial replicas
analyzed using Friedman test. A multiple compar- The frequency of attacks on replicas was strongly
ison test (Dunn’s test, p,0.05) was used when influenced by the ring pattern and body coloration.
significant differences among replicas were detected. Significant differences were detected in the number
The frequency of marks recorded on the ends (at of attacks received by the different types of replica
30 mm or less of each end) or at mid-body of the (Friedman test, x2514.91, p50.002). The number
models was analyzed using 262 contingency tables of marks found on the unbanded replicas (48%) was
(x2, p,0.05). significantly greater than on the three replicas with
Marks can be easily distinguished through their ringed patterns (M. pyrrhocryptus, 18%; L. pulcher,
shape and size: birds leave a V-shaped mark and 14%; O. rhombifer, 20%). The latter did not show
mammals leave a tooth mark or a U-shaped mark. significant differences among them (Dunn’s test,
Marks other than those made by typical predators of p,0.05).
snakes (opossums, foxes, skunks, hawks, owls or The analysis of the data obtained in each season
large tyrant birds) such as rodents were discarded. indicated that the number of attacks recorded
showed significant differences among replica types
(Friedman test, x259.404, p50.024 for the rainy
Results season and x258.647, p50.034 for the dry season).
The number of attacks on replicas with ringed
Attacks on replicas by predators
patterns in the rainy season did not differ from each
Of a total of 432 replicas placed in 12 transects, 77 other and was significantly lower than the number of
(17.8%) showed marks of attacks by predators. Of attacks on the plain replicas (Figure 3). In the dry
all the imprints recorded on the surface of the season, however, attacks recorded on the replicas of
replicas, 37 corresponded to birds and 40 to O. rhombifer and P. patagonienis did not differ
carnivorous mammals. Among the potential preda- significantly, and were greater than those recorded
tors of snakes in the sampling area we observed the on replicas of M. pyrrhocryptus and L. pulcher
following bird species: Milvago chimango, Polyborus (Dunn’s test, p,0.05) (Figure 3).
plancus (Falconidae), Buteo polyosoma (Accipitridae)
and Pitangus sulphuratus (Tyrannidae); and the
Position of attacks
following mammals: Didelphis albiventris
(Didelphidae), Dusicyon griseus (Canidae) and The number of marks on the ends of the replicas was
Conepatus castaneus (Mustelidae). The accumulated significantly higher (p,0.05) than on the mid-body

Table I. Comparison of the number of attacks to ringed snake replicas placed on different backgrounds (262 contingency table, x2 test,
p,0.05).

No. of replicas attacked

Type of ringed replica White paper Natural background x2 p

Micrurus phyrrocryptus 8 6 0.08 0.77


Lystrophis pulcher 5 6 0.00 1.00
Oxyrhopus rhombifer 6 9 0.31 0.57
Coral snake mimicry in Argentina 187

snake species with ringed patterns is independent of


the geographical region. Consequently, we can
conclude that mimicry characteristics are quite
general when the true models are present in the
area. Pfennig et al. (2001) first demonstrated that
coral snake mimicking species are not protected
when living in regions where their models are absent.
The fact that background color had no influence
on the number of bite marks indicates that coral
snake replicas were equally visible on both highly
contrasting white paper and natural ground. Thus,
we can reject the hypothesis that the colored
ring pattern does have a cryptic or disruptive
effect making detection of the replicas difficult for
Figure 3. Seasonal variation in the number of attacks to replicas predators.
representing four species exposed in 12 successive transects in the Predators may detect subtle differences between
rainy and dry seasons of 2001/2002. Different letters indicate the ring patterns of the snake species. This conclu-
significant differences (lowercase letters: rainy season; uppercase sion is suggested by the higher number of attacks to
letters: dry season) (Dunn’s test, p,0.05).
replicas of O. rhombifer during the dry season. This
species is more yellowish than the other species and
in three of the four different types of replicas (M. its black rings are rhomboids. Consequently, O.
pyrrhocryptus, L. pulcher, O. rhombifer and P. patago- rhombifer is less similar to the model M. pyrrhocryp-
nienis) (Table II). Seven replicas with numerous tus. These results suggest that both shape and width
marks throughout the body were discarded. of the central ring may influence the choice of prey
made by predators and the risk taken. The impor-
Discussion tance of ring width in the comparison of the
proportion of attacks between coral snakes and false
The occurrence of marks on the replicas indicates coral snakes has also been pointed out in other
that vertebrate predators were not able to readily studies (Greene & McDiarmid, 1981; Hinman et al.,
distinguish between the plasticine and true snakes. 1997).
Since the frequency of attacks during the sampling
Predation level was generally higher in the rainy
period did not decrease, predators apparently did
season. However, the proportion of aposematic
not seem to learn that replicas were inedible items.
replicas attacked in the dry season was higher, but
The accumulation of marks on the ends of the
this was due to the significant increase in attacks to
replicas showed that predators attacked them as they
O. rhombifer replicas, suggesting that predators
would do with true, potentially dangerous snakes.
would take higher risks when either food availability
Thus, our experiments did not appear to alter the
is lower or when the need for food is higher, for
behavior of predators.
example in birds during the breeding season in
The difference in the proportion of attacks
spring (Codesido & Bilenca, 2004). We can there-
between plain replicas and those with color ringed
fore conclude that L. pulcher is a better mimic of M.
pattern suggests an advantage of possessing the
pyrrhocryptus than O. rhombifer, at least during the
ringed ‘‘coral snake’’ pattern to avoid predation.
dry season in Córdoba.
Thus, our findings agree with a similar study on
coral and false coral snake species from Costa Rica
(Brodie III, 1993). These results also show that the Acknowledgments
reaction of predators towards replicas that mimic
We would like to thank M. Chiaraviglio, J. Navarro,
Table II. Position of bite marks recorded on snake replicas (262
M. Martella and an anonymous reviewer for their
contingency table, x2 test, p,0.05). suggestions during the preparation of the manu-
script. Anne Zillikens, E. Buasso and J. Brasca
Position of mark improved the English version.
Type of replica Body end Mid-body x2 p

Micrurus phyrrocryptus 12 2 11.57 0.007 References


Lystrophis pulcher 6 3 0.89 0.340
Oxyrhopus rhombifer 13 2 13.30 0.003 Brodie ED Jr, Brodie ED III. 1980. Differential avoidance of
Philodryas patagoniensis 26 6 22.50 0.000 mimetic salamanders by free ranging birds. Science
208:181–183.
188 C. M. Buasso et al.

Brodie ED III. 1993. Differential avoidance of coral snake banded Greene HW, McDiarmid RW. 1981. Coral snake mimicry: does it
patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica. occur? Science 213:1207–1212.
Evolution 47:227–235. Greene HW. 1997. Snakes: the evolution of mystery in nature.
Brodie ED III, Janzen FJ. 1995. Experimental studies of Hinman KE, Throop HL, Adams KL, Dake AJ, McLauchlan KK,
coral snake mimicry: generalized avoidance of ringed snake McKone MJ. 1997. Predation by free-ranging birds on partial
patterns by free-ranging avian predators. Funct Ecol coral snake mimics: the importance of ring width and color.
9:186–190. Evolution 51:1011–1014.
Brodie ED III, Moore A. 1995. Experimental studies of coral Pfennig DW, Harcombe WR, Pfennig KS. 2001. Predation:
snake mimicry: do snakes mimic millipedes? Anim Behav frequency-dependent Batesian mimicry. Nature 410:323.
49:534–536. Pough FH, Andrews RM, Cadle JE, Crump ML, Savitzky AH,
Bucher EH. 1982. Chaco and Caatinga—South American arid Ketwood D, editors. 1998. Herpetology. Upper Saddle River
savannas. Woodlands and tickets. In: Huntley BJ, Walker BH, (NJ): Prentice Hall. 577 p.
editors. Ecology of tropical savannas. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Smith SH. 1975. Innate recognition of coral snake pattern by a
p. 48–80. possible avian predator. Science 187:759–760.
Campbell JA & Lamar WW. 1989. The venomous reptiles of Latin Smith SH. 1977. Coral-snake pattern recognition and stimulus
America. Comstock Publ. Assoc., Ithaca, NY. 425 p. generalization by naive great kiskadees (Aves: Tyrannidae).
Cei JM. 1993. Reptiles del noroeste, nordeste y este de la Nature 265:535–536.
Argentina. Herpetofauna de las selvas subtropicales, Yanosky AA, Chani JM. 1988. Possible dual mimicry of Bothrops
puna y pampas. Turin: Mus. Reg. Sci. Nat. Monograph. and Micrurus by the colubrid Lystrophis dorbignyi. J Herpetol
Turin; 14:1–949. 22:222–224.
Codesido M, Bilenca D. 2004. Variación estacional de un Zug GR, Vitt LJ, Caldwell JP, editors. 2001. Herpetology: an
ensamble de aves en un bosque subtropical semiárido del introductory biology of amphibians and reptiles. San Diego
Chaco Argentino. Biotropica 36:544–554. (CA): Academic Press. 630 p.

View publication stats

You might also like