Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Connectedness to nature, place attachment and conservation behaviour: Testing


connectedness theory among farmers
Elizabeth Gosling, Kathryn J.H. Williams*
Department of Resource Management and Geography, Melbourne School of Land and Environment, University of Melbourne, 221 Bouverie Street, Parkville, Vic., 3010, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The aim of this study was to explore the relationship between pro-environmental behaviour and two
Available online 25 January 2010 forms of emotional association: attachment to place and connectedness to nature. This relationship is
explored in the context of farmers’ management of native vegetation on their properties. A postal survey
Keywords: was conducted to measure the extent to which farmers felt connected to nature and to their property.
Connectedness to nature The survey also measured vegetation management behaviours and associated valued outcomes. A total of
Place attachment
141 farmers in northwest Victoria, Australia, completed the survey. Results showed that vegetation
Biodiversity protection
Landholders
protection behaviours increased with connectedness to nature (CNS), although CNS accounted for only
a modest amount of the variation in behaviour. Place attachment was not related to management
behaviours. Further analysis suggested that the relationship between CNS and management behaviour
was mediated by importance given to environmental benefits of vegetation management. The findings
lend weight to studies showing a relationship between CNS and more simple conservation behaviours
(e.g. recycling). The findings are also consistent with frameworks suggesting that emotional association
with nature leads to an expanded sense of self and greater valuing of non-human species, and so to pro-
environment behaviour. This demonstrates the importance of using a range of instrumental and more
affective strategies to promote conservation behaviours.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction concerns. This study examines whether these affective connections


to place and nature also help to explain farmers’ management of
Native vegetation on private land has an important role in native vegetation.
providing regional biodiversity. Individual landholders therefore Research into farmer pro-environmental behaviour has tradi-
play a critical role in managing this biodiversity (Barr & Cary, 2000; tionally focused on the importance of financial drivers (Williams,
Harrington, Lane, & Mercer, 2006). Human-environment 2003). However, there is increasing recognition that economic
researchers, along with behaviour change practitioners have factors are not the only factors driving decision-making in agri-
consequently sought to understand the reasons why rural land- culture (Maybery et al., 2005). A number of recent Australian
holders do and do not protect native vegetation on their own studies demonstrate the mix of economic, cultural and emotional
properties (Maybery, Crase, & Gullifer, 2005; Williams & Cary, factors that shape vegetation management practices including tree
2002). To date, both research and practice have tended to empha- planting and protection of remnant native vegetation (Harrington
sise instrumental and financial motivations for native vegetation et al., 2006; Seabrook, McAlpine, & Fensham, 2008; Smith, 2008).
management. More recent research has shown that less tangible There is interest in understanding the role of emotion in environ-
motivations, including place attachment and connectedness to mentally significant behaviour (Pooley & O’Connor, 2000) and
nature, are important drivers of environmentally significant action some exploration of the role of emotions in land management
(Kals, Schumacher, & Montada, 1999; Vaske & Kobrin, 2001). This behaviours (see for example Carr, 2002; Harrison, Burgess, & Clark,
relationship has mainly been demonstrated for simple private 1998). Despite this, there is scant research available to understand
sphere behaviours such as switching off unnecessary lights and how emotional attachments to place and nature may influence
citizenship behaviours such as petitioning for environmental native vegetation management on farms.
In other contexts, there is growing evidence that environmen-
tally significant action increases with affective attachments to and
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ61 3 8344 3094. identification with nature and place. Connectedness to nature is the
E-mail address: kjhw@unimelb.edu.au (K.J.H. Williams). extent to which an individual feels that he or she is a part of nature

0272-4944/$ – see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.005
E. Gosling, K.J.H. Williams / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304 299

(Schultz, 2001). Kals et al., (1999) showed how affinity with nature of this relationship. Attachments to place and nature are thought
effectively predicted (with r2 values between .25 and .39) willing- to expand one’s identity or self-definition (Clayton & Opotow,
ness to undertake simple conservation behaviors such as taking 2003; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Schultz et al., 2004). As closeness to
public transport or petitioning for environmental protection. In nature or place increases, so does empathy and willingness to
testing the validity of a Connectedness to Nature Scale, Mayer and help (Mayer & Frantz, 2004). A number of researchers have
Frantz (2004) found positive correlations with environmental shown how this relates to more traditional value-based or
behaviour (two separate studies reported correlations of .44 and cognitive hierarchy models of environmentally significant
.45). Dutcher, Finley, Luloff, and Johnson (2007) found connectivity behaviour. Researchers often distinguish between value orienta-
with nature was a significant predictor of environmental behaviour, tions that give greatest importance to self, the wider human
accounting for 10 per cent of variance in behaviour. Clayton (2003) community, or the biosphere (for example Stern & Dietz, 1994).
observed a slightly broader concept, Environmental Identity, which Schultz (2000) argues that the value someone places on an object
incorporates both emotional association and identity with nature, will depend on the extent to which he or she includes that object
but also agreement with policies that protect nature, and extent of within his or her sense of self. A person who feels interconnected
interactions with nature. She demonstrated that measures of with the natural world may have an expanded sense of self that
Environmental Identity have significant positive correlations (r ¼ includes other non-human living beings, leading to greater
.64) with environmentally responsible behaviour such as turning biospheric concerns. In contrast, individuals who feel less con-
off lights or donating to environmental organisations. Not all nected to nature tend to value a narrower range of objects that
studies have reported support for this relationship, however. reflect self-interest (Schultz, 2000).
Schultz, Shriver, Tabanico, and Khazian (2004) used an Implicit This theory appears to suggest a mediating role for environ-
Association Test (IAT) to measure association with nature, and mental concern in explaining the relationship between environ-
found no significant relationships between IAT scores and mentally significant behaviour and connectedness to place and
behaviour. nature. That is, pro-environment behaviour increases with
There is also considerable interest in the relationship between connectedness to nature or place, but this is brought about through
place attachment and environmental behaviours. Place attachment an expanded concern for things other than oneself (Fig. 1; see also
is a positive connection or emotional bond between a person and Schultz, 2002). To date there is only partial and inconsistent
a particular place (Williams & Vaske, 2003). Vaske and Kobrin evidence for this relationship. For example, Schultz et al. (2004)
(2001) demonstrate that place attachment predicts environmen- demonstrates a positive relationship between biospheric concerns
tally responsible behaviour (r2 ¼ .40), with place identity (symbolic and an implicit association with nature, and a negative relationship
significance of place) mediating the relationship between place between implicit connections with nature and egoistic concerns.
dependence (functional reliance on place) and environmentally This provides evidence for the first part of this mediated relation-
responsible behaviour. Most behaviour indicators used in this study ship, showing that connectedness with nature is associated with
were simple household conservation behaviours, but the measure stronger environmental concern. A small number of studies have
did include at least one place-based or land management behav- demonstrated that manipulation of sense of self through perspec-
iour: participating in a community clean up of a natural area. tive-taking activities can also lead to stronger environmental
Stedman (2002) surveyed American lakeside property owners and concern (Bragg, 1996; Schultz, 2000). This provides evidence for
found that respondents with higher levels of place attachment a causal direction in the relationship between identity with place or
were more willing to act in ways that protected the lake setting’s nature and behaviour, but does not clarify whether environmental
quality, e.g. vote for laws that prevent water quality decline. Simi- concern plays a mediating role.
larly, Halpenny (2006) surveyed visitors to a Canadian national Berenguer (2007) perhaps comes closest to exploring the overall
park, and found that place attachment was a strong predictor (r ¼ relationship. He found that students instructed to take the
.57) of visitors’ park-specific behaviours, e.g. picking up litter left by perspective of a tree or bird expressed more positive attitudes
other people. towards the environment and displayed greater helping behaviour,
Along with demonstrating more generally the relationship allocating more of a student association’s funds to environmental
between place attachment and behaviour, these studies also projects. He found empirical support for a model in which attitudes
provide some indication that affective ties may shape low cost towards the tree or bird mediate the relationship between behav-
land protection behaviours such as removing litter. However, no iour and empathy. The attitude observed in this instance was ‘.to
research has considered the role of place attachment and what extent do you feel a moral obligation to help the [bird or
connectedness to nature in higher cost land protection behaviours tree]?’ This appears to combine elements of personal norms for
such as tree planting, fencing areas of native vegetation for action (Stern, 2000) and the kinds of valued objects or motivations
protection, or setting aside areas of native vegetation from agri- on which Schultz (2000) bases his observations.
cultural production. This study will investigate how well This study seeks to extend the understanding of the relationship
emotional connections to nature and to place predict these types between affective connections and pro-environment behaviour in
of behaviour. two ways. First, we examine whether connectedness to nature and
In exploring this relationship it is important to understand the place partly account for behaviours to protect native vegetation.
reasons why connections to nature and place might result in Second, we examine whether environmental concern mediates the
more positive environmental behaviours. Theorists have pre- relationship between behaviour and connectedness to nature and
sented a relatively consistent account of the psychological basis place.

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the mediating role of values in the relationship between connectedness to nature or place and pro-environment behaviour.
300 E. Gosling, K.J.H. Williams / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304

2. Methods 2.3. Questionnaire

A postal survey was conducted during June and July 2008 in The questionnaire contained five sections: on-farm native
a rural area where over the last decade there has been a concerted vegetation management (measuring behaviour and behavioural
community effort to revegetate cleared land. The postal survey intentions), valued objects associated with native vegetation
measured landholders’ place attachment, connectedness to nature, management, place attachment, connectedness to nature and
valued objects associated with vegetation management and on- demographics.
farm management of native vegetation. Connectedness to nature was measured by adapting scales
developed by Mayer and Frantz (2004) and Dutcher et al. (2007).
2.1. Study area Pre-testing identified that many items on these scales were not
suitable for a rural Australian context. Following the input from
The study area mainly comprised the Hindmarsh Shire – a local eight farmers, some scale items were revised and others discarded.
government area in northwest Victoria, Australia. This is a large The final scale comprised eight items designed to measure how
area, covering around 7600 km2, that has a relatively small pop- much farmers generally felt a part of nature (Table 1). Participants
ulation of around 6100 people. It is a farming region with an responded on a five-point scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree, 5 ¼ strongly
economy based on grain and sheep production (Hindmarsh Shire agree).
Council, 2008). Freehold titles were first established in the area in Place attachment was measured by adapting items from scales
the 1870s (Longmire, 1985); many landholders in the region are used by Halpenny (2006), Stedman (2002) and Vaske and Kobrin
third or fourth generation farmers. The study area also included (2001). The final scale comprised nine items designed to measure
properties surrounding Kaniva, a small town in the neighbouring the positive emotional bond that a landholder has with their
West Wimmera Shire. Typical of many parts of Australia, the property (Table 2). Again, participants responded on a five-point
Hindmarsh Shire has been extensively cleared for agriculture and scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree, 5 ¼ strongly agree).
there is a pressing need to protect and restore native vegetation in The measure of native vegetation management valued objects
the landscape. For the last 10 years a shire-wide program has or concerns was based on Schultz’s (2000) approach to measuring
provided landholders with financial incentives to protect and egoistic, altruistic and biospheric concerns for environmental
replant native vegetation on their property. Because many land- protection. Respondents were asked how important each of the
holders have subsequently been involved in revegetation, the area items (e.g. ‘protecting native birds’, ‘increasing profit on my farm’)
provided an opportunity to investigate the role of more emotional were for their decision-making about how they manage native
motives for on-farm conservation. vegetation on their property (Table 3). Respondents ranked items
on a seven-point scale (1 ¼ not important, 7 ¼ greatest
importance).
2.2. Sampling and participants
To measure native vegetation protection a list was devised of
eight actions that protect remnant vegetation, such as ‘reduce
Landholder names were obtained using a community directory
stocking to encourage regeneration of plants’, ‘leave fallen timber
and the telephone directory. This produced a sampling frame of 453
on the ground for wildlife habitat’, and ‘plant native trees and
property addresses with the corresponding landholder names. It is
shrubs to improve degraded areas’. These actions were based on the
likely that the sampling frame captured a high proportion of all
survey questions of Dettmann, Hamilton, and Curtis (2000).
landholders in the area. A random sample of 300 landholders was
Respondents indicated how often they carried out each action on
used. Nine of these landholders turned out not to be members of
a five-point scale (1 ¼ never, 5 ¼ always). Participants were asked if
the study population (e.g. the questionnaires were returned to
they had replanted native vegetation on their farm in the past, and
sender). In total 141 useable surveys were returned; a 48.5 per cent
if so, what the total area of this replanting was. Behavioural
response rate.
intentions were also measured. Participants were asked if they
Of the 141 respondents 115 (82 per cent) were male and 20 (14
planned to replant native trees and shrubs on their property in the
per cent) were female. Almost three-quarters (74.4 per cent) of
future and, if so, what area of land they would replant if time and
respondents were at least 45 years old. Property size ranged from
money were not an issue. The area of both past and future
18 to 4000 ha with an average of 1092 ha (S.D. ¼ 783 ha). Their
replantings was converted to a percentage of property size.
property provided the main source of income for 85 per cent of
respondents. A comparison to Australian Bureau of Statistics
census data (www.abs.gov.au) suggests that the demographic 2.4. Pre-testing and survey procedure
characteristics of respondents are similar to farmers in the study
area generally, except that female farmers were under- The questionnaire was pre-tested with a small convenience
represented. sample of eight farmers. These landholders helped identify unclear

Table 1
Factor loadings for items on connectedness to nature scale. Components extracted by PCA with varimax rotation.

Item Component

1 2
I often feel that I am a part of nature. .76 .18
I often feel close to the natural world around me. .73 .28
I never feel a personal bond with things in my natural surroundings like trees, wildlife or the view on the horizon. .74 .07
I often feel disconnected from nature. .64 .26
My own welfare is linked to the welfare of the natural world. .64 .16
I recognise and appreciate the intelligence of other living things. .61 .26
When I think about my place on the planet, I consider myself to be at the top of a pecking order among all living things. .04 .90
E. Gosling, K.J.H. Williams / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304 301

Table 2 component explaining 52 per cent of the variance. Table 2 contains


Factor loadings (unrotated) for items on place attachment scale. Components the factor loadings for each item of the place attachment scale.
extracted by PCA.
PCA was used to explore the structure of the native vegetation
Item Component 1 management valued object items. A non-orthogonal rotation
I am happiest when I’m on my farm. .83 method (direct oblimin method) was used to allow factors to be
My farm is my favourite place to be. .83 correlated, consistent with Schultz (2000). Two components with
I really miss my farm when I’m away too long. .77
eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were extracted, accounting for 63 per
I feel my farm is a part of me. .76
My farm is the best place to do the things I enjoy. .73 cent of the total variance. The correlation between these compo-
My farm reflects the type of person I am. .69 nents was r ¼ .26. Two items: ‘increasing the wellbeing of myself
I wouldn’t want to farm anywhere else. .65 and my family’ and ‘looking after the land for future generations’,
I feel I can be myself when I’m on my farm. .62 loaded highly on both components (Table 3). Items that loaded
As far as I’m concerned there are better places to .55
spend time than on my farm.
highly on the first component reflected concern for environmental
and landscape enhancement outcomes. Items that loaded highly on
the second component reflected concern for more anthropocentric
or poorly worded questions, and the questionnaire was revised outcomes. We called component one ‘environmental concern’ and
accordingly. For the main survey, questionnaires were mailed to component two ‘concern for human welfare’. Some analyses
farmers with a cover letter and a postage paid envelope for reported later use sub-scales calculated from mean values of
returning questionnaires. Two weeks later a thank you/reminder responses to the items that loaded on each component. In these
letter was mailed to the full sample. cases items loading highly on both factors were omitted from these
scales. Other analyses use factor scores. Cronbach’s alpha provides
further evidence of the reliability of these scales (for environmental
2.5. Data analysis concern a ¼ .85, and a ¼ .81 for concern for human welfare).
Although the scale did not consist of three dimensions as we had
Observations for place attachment, connectedness to nature and originally intended (egoistic, altruistic and biospheric concerns),
valued object items had very little missing data. Where only one to the structure is consistent with other work that has found a two
two items were missing from any scale, values were imputed using dimensional structure to environmental concern (e.g. Gagnon
the estimation maximisation method. There were more missing Thompson & Barton, 1994) and still allowed clear identification of
data associated with native vegetation management behaviour biospheric concerns.
items. Seven respondents had no remnant vegetation on their The eight questions on management of remnant vegetation
property, and were unable to answer questions about management measured a wide range of behaviours. Some of these (such as seed
of this vegetation. These cases were omitted from relevant analyses. collection) required much effort or skill and others (such as leaving
Five respondents had missing data for a single item; for these cases dead wood on the ground) very little. Unsurprisingly therefore, the
data were imputed using the estimation maximisation method. eight items were not strongly related (a ¼ .61). Three behaviours
Principal Components Analysis (PCA) was used to examine the were selected to form a single measure, hereafter called the
structure of the connectedness to nature, place attachment and remnant vegetation protection score. The reliability of this scale
native vegetation management valued object scales. was acceptable (a ¼ .73). PCA suggested a single component
The reliability of the initial connectedness to nature scale was structure for this scale, accounting for 65% of variation. Item load-
fairly low (a ¼ .68). PCA, using an orthogonal rotated solution, ings on this factor were as expected: ‘reduce stocking to encourage
identified two components, which together explained 56 per cent regeneration of plants’ (.76), ‘plant native trees and shrubs to
of the total variance. All items loaded highly on the first component improve degraded areas’ (.79), ‘fence these areas to manage stock
except for: ‘when I think about my place on the planet, I consider access’ (.87).
myself to be at the top of a pecking order among all living things’, There are several approaches to test for mediation between
which loaded negatively (Table 1). This was the only item to load variables (see Iacobucci, 2008; McKinnon, Fitzgerald, & Fritz, 2007;
highly on the second component. This item was excluded from the and Preacher & Hayes, 2008 for reviews). While structural equation
connectedness to nature scale, which improved the scale’s reli- modelling is the preferred method (Iacobucci, 2008) it requires
ability (a ¼ .78). a large sample size – around 200 cases where data have some non-
The place attachment scale had high reliability (a ¼ .87). Inter- normal characteristics and communalities are not high (Hair,
pretation of the PCA suggested the scale consisted of one Tatham, Anderson, & Black, 2006), as was the case for the data
examined here. Traditional regression approaches to testing
mediation are also widely used and accepted (Iacobucci, 2008;
Table 3 McKinnon et al., 2007). Evidence for mediation exists when there
Factor loadings for items on native vegetation management valued objects scale. is: (a) a significant relation between the predictor (in this study,
Components extracted by PCA with oblimin rotation.
factor scores of connectedness to nature or place attachment items)
Scale item Component and the mediator (in this study, factor score of environmental
1 2 concern items); (b) a significant relation between the criterion (in
Protecting and improving wildlife habitat .89 .03
this study, the remnant vegetation protection score) and the
Protecting native birds .85 .13 mediator; and (c) when the effect of the mediator is controlled, the
Contributing to the health of the landscape .68 .32 relation between the criterion and the predictor becomes signifi-
Increasing the visual appeal of the district .68 .17 cantly smaller (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Iacobucci, 2008). This was
Improving the natural environment .78 .24
tested using three regressions: (1) the criterion was regressed on
Looking after the land for future generations .54 .40
Increasing the wellbeing of myself and my family .49 .50 the predictor; (2) the mediator was regressed on the predictor; and
Providing benefits to others in my community .27 .58 (3) the criterion was regressed on both the mediator and the
Contributing to the prosperity of the district .15 .72 predictor. Sobel’s z test was conducted to compare the size of the
Increasing profit on my farm .16 .91 relation between predictor and criterion in the presence and
Improving the property value of my farm .18 .86
absence of the mediator (Iacobucci, 2008; Preacher & Hayes, 2008).
302 E. Gosling, K.J.H. Williams / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304

3. Results Table 5
Spearman’s rho correlation coefficients between place attachment and remnant
vegetation protection and replanting variables.
In the native vegetation management valued objects scale, the
mean response to each item was above four (the mid-point of the Place attachment P-value N
scale), indicating that all the valued objects tended to be important (factor score) (2-tailed)

to respondents (Table 4). Respondents generally placed the highest Remnant vegetation .03 .71 134
protection (factor score)
importance on ‘looking after the land for future generations’, and
Past replanting as % of .08 .35 129
the least importance on ‘contributing to the prosperity of the property size
district’. While both components within the scale rated highly, Future replanting as % of .08 .40 123
respondents generally expressed greater environmental concern property size
than concern for human welfare.
The mean score on the place attachment scale was 3.9 (out of
a maximum of five; S.D. ¼ .70), indicating that many respondents was not significant when environmental concern was also included
had a strong attachment to their property. Respondents expressed in the regression, mediation may be deemed ‘complete’ (Iacobucci,
a similar level of connectedness to nature; the mean score for this 2008).
scale was also 3.9 (S.D. ¼ .66). Regressions were also conducted to test for mediation of envi-
Place attachment did not correlate significantly with any of the ronmental concern between place attachment factor scores and
behaviour measures (Table 5). Connectedness to nature scores remnant vegetation protection factor scores. The first requirement
correlated significantly with two of the three behaviour measures. for evidence of mediation was not met. When place attachment
The strongest correlation was between connectedness to nature factor score was regressed on environmental concern, no signifi-
and remnant vegetation protection score (Table 6). There was also cant relationship was found (B ¼ .14, S.E. ¼ .08, p ¼ .09).
a modest but significant correlation between connectedness to
nature and intended replanting.
There was a moderately-strong correlation between connect- 4. Discussion
edness to nature and environmental concern (Table 7). Connect-
edness to nature and concern for human welfare did not correlate. Although the correlations are only modest, results suggest that
In contrast, place attachment displayed modest correlations with connectedness to nature does relate to how farmers manage native
both environmental concern and concern for human welfare. vegetation on their farm. This supports existing research demon-
Regressions were conducted to test mediation of environmental strating a relationship between measures of emotional association
concern between connectedness to nature factor scores and or identification with nature and other forms of pro-environment
remnant vegetation protection factor scores. All requirements for behaviour (Clayton, 2003; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Schultz, 2001).
evidence of mediation were met. The first requirement for evidence While the correlation between connectedness and remnant vege-
of mediation was met by the significant relation between tation protection score is comparable to correlations that Mayer
connectedness to nature score and environmental concern score (B and Frantz (2004) and Schultz (2001) reported, other correlations
¼ .51, S.E. ¼ .07, p < .001). The second requirement was met by the are smaller than those reported elsewhere. The results do not
significant coefficient between native vegetation management indicate causality in this relationship. Farmers who feel a sense of
score and environmental concern score (B ¼ .29, S.E.>¼.10, p < kinship with nature may be more motivated to care for wildlife by
.001). The third requirement was also met; when remnant vege- protecting areas of native bush on their farm. Alternatively,
tation protection score was regressed onto both environmental carrying out such actions may cause farmers to feel more connected
concern and connectedness to nature scores, the relation between to the natural world. More research is needed to establish a causal
criterion and predictor was lower (B ¼ .13, S.E. ¼ .10, p ¼ .19) than relationship between connectedness to nature and pro-environ-
the direct relationship (B ¼ .26, S.E. ¼ .09, p < .001). The z test mental behaviours in this context.
indicated that the difference between these coefficients was Results suggest that many farmers in the study area have formed
significant (Sobel z ¼ 2.73, p < .001). Since the relation between an emotional attachment to their property that goes beyond its
connectedness to nature and remnant vegetation protection score value as a place to grow crops and run livestock. This attachment,
however, did not appear to relate to the measures of on-farm pro-
Table 4 environment behaviour. This is not consistent with research on
Mean response for each item on the native vegetation management valued objects place attachment and pro-environment behaviours conducted in
scale and the two sub-scales, where 0 ¼ not important, 7 ¼ greatest importance. other settings (Halpenny, 2006; Stedman, 2002; Vaske & Kobrin,
N Mean Std. dev. 2001).
Environmental concern (sub-scale) Several factors need to be considered in interpreting these
Contributing to the health of the landscape 141 5.85 1.24 results. As noted earlier, past studies have mostly looked at the
Protecting native birds 141 5.62 1.45
Increasing the visual appeal of the district 141 5.27 1.55
Improving the natural environment 141 5.22 1.46 Table 6
Protecting and improving wildlife habitat 141 5.17 1.53 Spearman’s rho correlation coefficients between connectedness to nature factor
Sub-scale mean 141 5.43 1.14 score and remnant vegetation protection and replanting variables.
Concern for human welfare (sub-scale) Connectedness to P-value N
Improving the property value of my farm 141 4.97 1.86 nature (factor score) (2-tailed)
Increasing profit on my farm 141 4.47 2.15
Remnant vegetation protection .27** .00 131
Providing benefits to others in my community 141 4.30 1.70
(factor score)
Contributing to the prosperity of the district 141 4.25 1.82
Past replanting as % of .17 .06 127
Sub-scale mean 141 4.50 1.51
property size
Items not included in sub-scales Future replanting as % of .23* .01 121
Looking after the land for future generations 141 6.15 1.14 property size
Increasing the wellbeing of myself and my family 141 5.59 1.64
**p < .01, *p < .05.
E. Gosling, K.J.H. Williams / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304 303

Table 7
Correlations (Spearman’s rho) between connectedness to nature, place attachment, environmental concern and concern for human welfare (factor scores).

Connectedness to nature Place attachment Environment values Human values


Connectedness to nature 1 N ¼ 138
Place attachment .25** N ¼ 137 1 N ¼ 140
Environmental concern .53** N ¼ 138 .21* N ¼ 140 1 N ¼ 141
Concern for human welfare .03 N ¼ 138 .22** N ¼ 140 .24** N ¼ 141 1 N ¼ 141

**p < .01, *p < .05.

relationship between identity with place or nature and general pro- (Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Schultz, 2002; Schultz et al., 2004). This
environmental behaviours, such as recycling, switching off lights in finding may appear to be in tension with empirical observations
empty rooms, and walking or cycling instead of driving. Even where reported by both Mayer and Frantz (2004) and Clayton (2003).
place-specific behaviours have been observed (e.g. Vaske & Kobrin, Clayton (2003) used partial correlations to explore the relationship
2001), these have generally been less demanding behaviours (e.g. between Environmental Identity, values, attitudes and behaviour,
voting for environmental laws, removing litter, and joining a group while Mayer and Frantz (2004) used similar analyses to examine
to protect the place). Compared to these general behaviours, both the relationship between connectedness to nature, scores on the
on-farm replanting and remnant vegetation protection are highly New Environmental Paradigm (NEP) scale, and environmental
constrained by contextual factors such as income, time and behaviour. In both cases the independent variable (Environmental
equipment. Stern (2000) notes that when a behaviour is more Identity or connectedness to nature) continued to predict behav-
difficult or costly with regard to time and money, its association iour, even when the mediating variable (values or NEP) was
with attitudinal factors (e.g. environmental concern or connect- controlled. While these findings might appear to discount a medi-
edness to nature) is weaker. The influence of association with ating relationship between the independent variable and behav-
nature and place may be less apparent in the face of such contextual iour, Preacher and Hayes (2008) warn against using partial
constraints. correlations to test for mediation. Consistent with this, Mayer and
It should also be noted that some studies on place attachment Frantz (2004) and Clayton (2003) conclude only that this pattern of
have measured behavioural intentions rather than actual behaviour results demonstrates that the concepts are distinct.
(Halpenny, 2006; Stedman, 2002). While intent to carry out an The findings do not support the related proposition that envi-
action may be a good predictor of that behaviour, many external ronmental concern also plays a mediating role between place
factors can prevent behavioural intentions from being translated attachment and native vegetation management behaviour.
into actual behaviour (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Therefore place However, given the issues discussed earlier about the nature of the
attachment is likely to correlate more strongly with behavioural place attachment measure used in this study, it is premature to
intentions than with actual behaviour. reject this theory outright. This relation needs formal testing in
Finally, this study measured place attachment in a different other settings.
context to overseas research. For most respondents the study Because there are some limitations to the approach used to test
measured attachment to their workplace – the place from which for mediation, these findings should be generalised with care.
they earn their living. The need to run a profitable business Iacobucci (2008) notes the benefits of structural equation model-
complicates the relationship between place attachment and place- ling approaches, which account for measurement issues and allow
specific behaviours– especially in a region that has suffered from 10 simultaneous solution of regression equations. Further research is
years of below average rainfall. This was a common theme in the required, with larger sample sizes, to confirm this relationship. It
additional comments section of the questionnaire, for example one would also be helpful to include more direct observation of
respondent wrote: behaviour as well as further testing of scales measuring connect-
edness to nature and place.
‘Profits have to come first like any other business. Costs are too
great so we have to get the most out of the land yield-wise as we
can. If we didn’t have to rely so much on money we would be
5. Conclusion
glad to help nature much more.’
An important implication is that farmers have multiple goals for The results suggest that connectedness to nature does partly
their properties and complex beliefs about what it means to care for account for farmers’ attitudes about tree-planting and their
one’s property. The hypothesis that attachment to place should management of remnant vegetation. This confirms that more
induce a strong stewardship ethic and environmental concern, intrinsic values and attitudes do play a role in on-farm manage-
which should in turn lead to greater protection and replanting of ment of native vegetation. Respondents’ high ranking of environ-
native vegetation, assumes that all farmers believe that trees are mental concern reinforces the importance of non-financial values
good for the land. This may not be the case. For example, some and motives to farmers’ decision-making. From a practical
farmers who feel strongly attached to their property and aim to perspective, this study has important implications for engaging
look after their land may see patches of native vegetation as farmers in conservation behaviours. Results highlight the need to
a source of pest animals. Rather than protecting or replanting trees, speak to all values and motives – including affective ties to nature –
they may instead express their stewardship in other ways, such as when promoting native vegetation protection and restoration
adopting soil conservation practices or controlling weeds and pests. among farmers. For example, people working with farmers to
Further research, observing a broader range of environmental protect and restore native vegetation on private land should
behaviours, is needed to examine the relationship between place emphasise the benefits of such actions for wildlife and nature more
attachment and pro-environmental behaviours on farms. generally. This follows the approach advocated by Schultz and
The study provides evidence that environmental concern Zelezny (2003), who argue that reframing environmental messages
mediates the relationship between connectedness to nature and to be congruent with peoples’ values is a pragmatic way to
native vegetation management behaviour. These findings align encourage pro-environmental behaviour. Future research could
with the theoretical model described by numerous researchers focus on developing strategies to foster or harness connectedness
304 E. Gosling, K.J.H. Williams / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 298–304

to nature among farmers to potentially increase remnant vegeta- Kals, E., Schumacher, D., & Montada, L. (1999). Emotional affinity toward nature as
a motivational basis to protect nature. Environment and Behavior, 31(2),
tion protection and replanting activities.
178–202.
The findings are consistent with the broadly agreed theoretical Longmire, A. (1985). Nine creeks to Albacutya: A history of the shire of Dimboola.
framework proposing that identification with nature leads to an North Melbourne: Shire of Dimboola/Hargreen Publishing Company.
expanded sense of self and greater valuing of non-human species, Maybery, D., Crase, L., & Gullifer, C. (2005). Categorising farming values as economic,
conservation and lifestyle. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(1), 59–72.
and so to pro-environment behaviour. Further research is needed to Mayer, F. S., & Frantz, C. M. P. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: a measure
test this relationship in other settings, and to further understand of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental
causal relationships between identity with place and nature, Psychology, 24(4), 503–515.
McKinnon, D., Fairchild, A., & Fritz, M. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of
environmental concern and pro-environment behaviour. Psychology, 58, 593–614.
References Pooley, J., & O’Connor, M. (2000). Environmental education and attitudes: emotion
and beliefs are what is needed. Environment & Behaviour, 32(5), 711–723.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Contemporary approaches to assessing
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in mediation in communication research. In A. F. Hayes, M. D. Slater, & L. B. Snyder
social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical consider- (Eds.), The sage sourcebook of advanced data analysis methods for communication
ations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. research (pp. 13–54). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Barr, N., & Cary, J. (2000). Influencing improved natural resource management on Schultz, P. W. (2000). Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective
farms. Kingston: Department of Agriculture, Fisheries and Forestry – Australia, taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3),
Commonwealth of Australia. ACT. 391–406.
Berenguer, J. (2007). The effect of empathy in proenvironmental attitudes and Schultz, P. W. (2001). The structure of environmental concern: concern for self,
behaviours. Environment and Behavior, 39(2), 269–283. other people, and the biosphere. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21(4),
Bragg, E. (1996). Towards ecological self: deep ecology meets constructionist self 327–339.
theory. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, 93–108. Schultz, P. W. (2002). Inclusion with nature: understanding the psychology of
Carr, A. (2002). Grass roots and green tape: Principles and practices of environmental human-nature interactions. In P. Schmuck, & P. W. Schultz (Eds.), The psychology
stewardship. Sydney: Federation Press. of sustainable development (pp. 61–78). New York: Kluwer.
Clayton, S. (2003). Environmental identity: a conceptual and an operational defi- Schultz, P. W., Shriver, C., Tabanico, J. J., & Khazian, A. M. (2004). Implicit connec-
nition. In S. Clayton, & S. Opotow (Eds.), Identity and the natural environment tions with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(1), 31–42.
(pp. 45–66). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. (2003). Reframing environmental messages to be
Clayton, S., & Opotow, S. (2003). Introduction: identity and the natural environ- congruent with American values. Human Ecology Review, 10(2), 126–136.
ment. In S. Clayton, & S. Opotow (Eds.), Identity and the natural environment (pp. Seabrook, L., McAlpine, C., & Fensham, R. (2008). What influences farmers to keep
1–24). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. trees? A case study from the Brigalow Belt, Queensland, Australia. Landscape
Dettmann, P. D., Hamilton, S. D., & Curtis, A. L. (2000). Understanding landholder and Urban Planning, 84(3–4), 266–281.
values and intentions to improve remnant vegetation management in Australia: Smith, F. P. (2008). Who’s planting what, where and why–and who’s paying? An
the box-ironbark case study. Journal of Sustainable Agriculture, 16(3), 93–105. analysis of farmland revegetation in the central wheatbelt of Western Australia.
Dutcher, D. D., Finley, J. C., Luloff, A. E., & Johnson, J. B. (2007). Connectivity with Landscape and Urban Planning, 86, 66–78.
nature as a measure of environmental values. Environment and Behavior, 39(4), Stedman, R. C. (2002). Toward a social psychology of place: predicting behavior
474–493. from place-based cognitions, attitude, and identity. Environment and Behavior,
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An intro- 34(5), 561–581.
duction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Stern, P. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior.
Gagnon Thompson, S. C., & Barton, M. A. (1994). Ecocentric and anthropocentric atti- Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407–424.
tudes toward the environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14(2),149–157. Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of
Hair, J. F., Tatham, R. L., Anderson, R. E., & Black, W. (2006). Multivariate data analysis Social Issues, 50, 65–84.
(6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Vaske, J. J., & Kobrin, K. C. (2001). Place attachment and environmentally respon-
Halpenny, E. A. (2006). Environmental behaviour, place attachment and park visi- sible behavior. Journal of Environmental Education, 32(4), 16–21.
tation: A case study of visitors to point pelee national park. PhD thesis, Ontario, Williams, D. R., & Vaske, J. J. (2003). The measurement of place attachment: val-
Canada: University of Waterloo. idity and generalizability of a psychometric approach. Forest Science, 49(6),
Harrington, C., Lane, R., & Mercer, D. (2006). Learning conservation: the role of 830–840.
conservation covenants in landscape redesign at project Hindmarsh, Victoria. Williams, K. J. H. (2003). The biodiversity we want to maintain and the reasons we
Australian Geographer, 37(2), 187–209. want to maintain it. In J. Crosthwaite, Q. Farmer-Bowers, & C. Hollier (Eds.), Land
Harrison, C. M., Burgess, J., & Clark, J. (1998). Discounted knowledges: farmers’ and use change – YES! – But will biodiversity be OK? Proceedings of a conference at
residents’ understanding of nature conservation goals and policies. Journal of Attwood, Victoria, August 2002. Melbourne: Department of Sustainability and
Environmental Management, 54, 305–320. Environment (CD ROM).
Hindmarsh Shire Council. (2008). Official website of the Hindmarsh Shire council. Williams, K. J. H., & Cary, J. W. (2002). Landscape preference, ecological quality and
viewed 01.09.08. <http://www.hindmarsh.vic.gov.au/>. biodiversity protection. Environment and Behavior, 34(2), 258–275.
Iacobucci, D. (2008). Mediation analysis. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.

You might also like