Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology

ISSN: 0167-482X (Print) 1743-8942 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ipob20

Anxiety sensitivity and anxiety as correlates of


expected, experienced and recalled labor pain

Doris Curzik & Natasa Jokic-Begic

To cite this article: Doris Curzik & Natasa Jokic-Begic (2011) Anxiety sensitivity and anxiety as
correlates of expected, experienced and recalled labor pain, Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics &
Gynecology, 32:4, 198-203, DOI: 10.3109/0167482X.2011.626093

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.3109/0167482X.2011.626093

Published online: 31 Oct 2011.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 267

Citing articles: 17 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ipob20
Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology, 2011; 32(4): 198–203
Copyright © 2011 Informa UK, Ltd.
ISSN 0167-482X print/ISSN 1743-8942 online
DOI: 10.3109/0167482X.2011.626093

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Anxiety sensitivity and anxiety as correlates of expected, experienced


and recalled labor pain

Doris Curzik1 & Natasa Jokic-Begic2


1
Department of Psychiatry, University Hospital Center Zagreb, Croatia and 2Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities
and Social Sciences, University of Zagreb, Croatia

There has been a good deal of research on the role of and harmful. In other words, anxiety sensitivity, defined as a
anxiety sensitivity in pain perception, but only recently have tendency to fear anxiety-related symptoms due to the belief
investigators begun to assess its role in labor pain. The aim of that there will be some negative outcome as a result of hav-
this study was to investigate the nature of this relationship as ing those symptoms, may be a factor significantly related to
well as the relationship of state and trait anxiety with labor labor pain. In this single research study [4] that investigated
pain. Assessments of maximum and average labor pain were the role of anxiety sensitivity in labor pain, the authors found
completed in three different time periods (before, during and that anxiety sensitivity shared a significant relationship with
immediately after labor, and 1 month postpartum). Anxiety maximum sensory and affective labor pain, but that state
and anxiety sensitivity measures were completed during the and trait anxiety (measured by the state-trait anxiety inven-
late stage of pregnancy. A total of 46 primiparous healthy tory (STAI)) did not correlate significantly with labor pain.
pregnant women, carrying a single child, participated in the Anxiety sensitivity predicted both maximum pain during
study. State anxiety correlated significantly with maximum labor and sensory aspects of pain above and beyond demo-
(r = 0.352, p < 0.01) and average (r = 0.325, p < 0.05) labor pain graphic and social factors as well as other theoretically impor-
expectancies, whereas trait anxiety correlated significantly tant psychological factors (e.g. depression and state anxiety).
with maximum labor pain expectancies (r = 0.306, p < 0.05). Similar results were obtained by Spice et al. [5], who found
During labor, only the physical concerns dimension of anxiety that anxiety sensitivity significantly predicted elevated fear of
sensitivity shared a significant relationship with sensory pain childbirth even after controlling for parity and trait anxiety.
(r = 0.292, p < 0.05). In conclusion, anxiety shares a significant Currently, Norton and Asmundson’s amended fear-avoid-
relationship with labor pain expectancies only, whereas the ance model [6] provides the best explanation for why anxiety
physical concerns dimension of anxiety sensitivity correlates sensitivity seems to contribute to higher labor pain intensi-
significantly with sensory pain during labor. These data clarify ties. They argue that symptoms of physiological arousal (e.g.
the role of anxiety and anxiety sensitivity in the experience of increased heart rate, increased blood flow to voluntary mus-
labor pain. Clinical implications are discussed. cles) may produce bodily sensations such as muscular tension
that elicit misinterpretations of the nature and meaning of the
Keywords:  anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, childbirth, pain sensations. These misinterpretations are seen as the product of
individual differences in the propensity to respond with fear
to sensations that are anxiety provoking (e.g. anxiety sensitiv-
Introduction
ity and/or negative affect). Those with high anxiety sensitivity
Trait anxiety, defined as a stable tendency to respond with will catastrophically misinterpret the physiological sensations
state anxiety in the anticipation of threatening situations, is of anxiety, provoked by the anticipation of, or exposure to,
commonly proposed as one of the main psychological factors a pain-provoking situation, as evidence of impending harm
contributing to higher labor pain intensity [1,2]. Yet, studies and/or pain [7]. In turn, these catastrophically misinterpreted
have thus far failed to obtain consistent data to support this bodily sensations can lead to the avoidance of associated situ-
hypothesis [3,4]. Moreover, Lang et al [4]. suggest that labor ations. Indeed, research has demonstrated that women who
pain may not be influenced by trait anxiety, but rather by the are prone to pain anticipated and experienced more intense
woman’s fear that those experiences of pain are threatening labor pain and had poorer postpartum physical recovery [8].

Correspondence: Dr. Natasa Jokic-Begic PhD., Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Zagreb, I. Lucica
3, Zagreb, 1000 Croatia. E-mail: njbegic@ffzg.hr

198
Anxiety sensitivity and labor pain  199
According to Reiss  [9], anxiety sensitivity facilitates a fear Participants
response during specific anxiety-evoking situations as it may To participate, women had to be healthy primiparas, who
exacerbate concerns about physiological responses leading to were not experiencing any acute pain and had no chronic
increased fear and avoidance of pain-related activities as well pain problems (i.e. chronic back pain) at the first stage of the
as further increasing physiological reactivity (i.e. a vicious study. All women were between 37 and 40 weeks of pregnancy,
cycle of fear and anxiety leading to more physiological reac- and carrying a single child. Of 60 women who enrolled in the
tivity). Indeed, healthy persons with high anxiety sensitivity study, 46 completed all three stages of the study and thus were
demonstrated greater interpretive bias and more negative pain included in this analysis. None of these women received epi-
experiences relative to persons with low anxiety sensitivity, and dural analgesia in any stage of their labor. However, as a part of
interpretive biases were related to worse affective pain experi- the regular procedure, all of the women who enrolled this study
ences [10]. In the context of labor, women with high levels of received 50 mg of spasmoanalgetic meperidine during labor.
anxiety sensitivity would be more likely to be hypervigilant In the second stage of the study, 11 women were excluded from
for both pain and other bodily sensations and to catastrophi- the study because of cesarean delivery and/or complications
cally misinterpret them as more threatening and dangerous. during labor. The indications for cesarean section (or labor
Consequently, this may lead to avoidance behaviors such as not related complications) included different severe medical con-
taking part in childbirth classes and/or asking more frequently ditions such as placenta previa, dispoportio cefalopelvina and
for C-section in order to avoid anxiety and pain. asfixio fetus imminens, but none was performed for reasons
Pain anticipation and pain avoidance seem to be important such as unbearable labor pain. These women were excluded
factors when considering the role of trait and state anxiety and from the study because they did not reach the pushing stage of
anxiety sensitivity in labor pain. Therefore, establishing the the labor and therefore maximum labor pain assessments (that
role of trait and state anxiety and anxiety sensitivity in correla- constituted the first part of the study’s second stage) could not
tion to expected, experienced and recalled labor pain should be taken. The average labor pain assessments immediately
also provide a better insight in understanding the role of psy- after labor could also not be taken in this group because of the
chological factors in labor pain. qualitatively different nature of labor and pain experienced.
Previous studies have to some extent explored the role of Three women did not show up at the control examination 1
anxiety (i.e. trait anxiety, dental anxiety) in expected, expe- month postpartum and were also excluded from the study.
rienced and recalled pain (i.e. dental pain, chronic low back
pain). It seems that trait anxiety, defined as a tendency to Procedure
respond with fear in situations perceived as harmful, is related Women were recruited during late prenatal examinations.
to pain expectancies rather to experienced pain [11–15]. On They were briefly informed about the research by their gyne-
the other hand, Terry et al. [16] found that preoperative state cologist at the end of their examination and were directed to
and trait anxiety correlated significantly with actual, but not a room where they were more completely informed about the
with expected and recalled postoperative pain. research procedure by one of the investigators. Once informed
To our knowledge, there is not yet any research that has consent had been given, women completed the Spielberger’s
investigated the role of trait and state anxiety and anxiety state-trait anxiety inventory, The anxiety sensitivity index (ASI)
sensitivity in labor pain expectancies, nor in recalled labor and provided estimates of expected average and maximum
pain. Thus, the aim of this study was to investigate the role of labor pain intensities on a scale from 1 to 10 (1 = no pain,
trait and state anxiety and anxiety sensitivity as correlates of 10 = unbearable pain). Demographic information about age,
expected, experienced and recalled labor pain. education, household income, marital status, and marriage/
In their research, Lang et al. [4] emphasized that one of the relationship satisfaction (on a scale from 1 to 10, with a higher
limitations of their study was that labor pain was not mea- number indicating higher level of satisfaction) was also pro-
sured immediately after labor, which could have lead to dis- vided. That completed the first stage of the study. The second
tortions in labor pain assessments. This research presents an stage of the study consisted of two parts. In the first part − after
attempt to eliminate this methodological limitation by taking the first stage of labor− before pushing (later in text referred as
labor pain assessments during and immediately after labor. In during labor) participants assessed maximum labor pain and
research conducted by Spice et al. [5] the role of trait anxiety completed the short-form McGill pain questionnaire. In the sec-
and anxiety sensitivity in fear of childbirth, rather than fear of ond part (immediately after the baby was born), assessments of
pain, was explored. average labor pain were taken. Assessments of actual maximum
We generally hypothesized that trait and state anxiety would labor pain were taken on a scale from 1 to 10 and each of the 15
be related to labor pain expectancies and that anxiety sensitiv- words from the short-form McGill pain questionnaire (MPQ-SF)
ity would be significantly correlated with actual labor pain. Swere read aloud by the investigator, and were assessed on a scale
from 0 to 3 (0 = none, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, 3 = severe). Average
labor pain was assessed on a scale from 1 to 10. Finally, in the
Methods
third stage of the study, recalled labor pain was assessed 1 month
This study was approved by the Zagreb Faculty of Humanities postpartum after the first postnatal examination by completing
and Social Sciences’ Institutional Review Board. All women the short form McGill pain questionnaire as a measure of recalled
provided informed consent. The research was performed at maximum labor pain intensity, and providing estimates of maxi-
Osijek Clinical Hospital Centre. mum and average labor pain intensity on scales from 1 to 10.

Copyright © 2011 Informa UK Ltd.


200  D. Curzik & N. Jokic-Begic
Assessment measures SD = 10.0) showed normal distribution and Cronbach’s α was
In this research, the following measures of psychopathology 0.90. Research supports a hierarchical factor structure of the
were used: the STAI, the ASI and the MPQ-SF. ASI consisting of three lower-order factors measuring physi-
cal concerns, mental incapacitation (psychological) concerns,
The state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI) and social concerns [19].
The STAI [17] is a widely used measure of general anxiety. It
has demonstrated good reliability, with a median test-retest The short-form McGill pain questionnaire (MPQ-SF)
reliability coefficient for the trait version of 0.76. The reliabil- Finally, The MPQ-SF [20] was used in the present research,
ity coefficients for the state version is lower (0.33), which is which is the most commonly used measure of labor pain and
understandable because it is designed to reflect fluctuations provides information about both sensory and affective aspects
in anxiety  [17]. The results in our sample (N = 60; STAI-S: of pain [21]. Sensory aspects of pain refer to temporal, spa-
M = 41.62, SD = 8.9; STAI-T: M = 37.77, SD = 8.4) showed nor- tial, pressure and thermal properties of pain whereas affective
mal distributions and Cronbach’s α coefficients were 0.46 for aspects of pain refer to tension, fear and autonomic properties
the state version, and 0.67 for the trait version. of the pain [20]. To prevent the results (labor pain assessments)
from being distorted by the possibility that women (healthy
The anxiety sensitivity index (ASI) primiparas) might become more alert to different types of pain
The second measure was the ASI [18], a 16-item self-report during labor  [22–25] the short-form McGill Questionnaire
measure of anxiety sensitivity. It has excellent psychomet- was not used to assess labor pain expectancies.
ric properties in both clinical and non-clinical samples.
A Cronbach’s α between 0.84–0.90 suggests good internal Statistical analyses
reliability  [18]. The results in our sample (N = 60; M = 18.0, The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 12.0
(SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for data analyses. The
Table I.  Demographic features of the study sample. level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
M (SD) Range N
Age 26.18 (4.952) 17–40 60
Results
Marriage satisfaction 9.46 (0.816) 7–10 59
frequency % N Demographic features
Education 60 Demographic features of the study sample are presented
  Primary school 6 10.0 in Table  I. Women with and without complete data from
  High school 40 66.7 all three stages of the research did not differ in age, marital
  Some college 4 6.7 status, income, education, state and trait anxiety and anxiety
  College graduate 10 16.7 sensitivity. The excluded women and women with complete
Household income (monthly) 60 data from all three stages of the research did not differ in age,
  <$1000 20 33.3 marital status, income, education, state and trait anxiety and
  $1000–$1500 24 40.0 anxiety sensitivity.
  $1501–$3000 14 23.3
  >$3000 2 3.3 The relationship between labor pain assessments,
Marital status 60 anxiety and anxiety sensitivity
  Single 1 1.7 To establish the relationship between labor pain assessments,
  Married 54 90.0 anxiety and anxiety sensitivity in three different time periods,
  Living with partner 5 8.3 Pearson’s correlation coefficients were computed. As pre-
M, mean; N, total sample number; SD, standard deviation. sented in Table II, anxiety as state correlated significantly with

Table II.  Pearson’s correlation coefficients between anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, anxiety sensitivity physical concerns and labor pain assessments in three
­different time periods.
Expected Expected Experienced Experienced MPQ-SF MPQ-SF Recalled Recalled MPQ-SF
maximum average maximum average experienced experienced maximum average recalled
labor labor labor labor maximum max.sensory labor labor max.labor
pain pain pain pain labor pain labor pain pain pain pain
State anxiety 0.352b 0.325a 0.254 0.109 0.089 0.042 0.193 0.036 0.240
Trait anxiety 0.306a 0.244 0.146 0.164 0.047 0.035 0.055 0.079 0.000
Anxiety sensitivity 0.151 0.118 0.032 0.144 0.197 0.245 0.061 0.141 0.010
Anxiety sensitivity− 0.128 0.137 0.133 0.177 0.268 0.335a 0.151 0.211 0.022
physical concerns
Anxiety sensitivity− 0.121 0.028 0.017 0.160 0.122 0.154 −0.039 0.041 −0.026
psychological concerns
Anxiety sensitivity− 0.162 0.170 −0.292a 0.155 −0.043 −0.069 −0.050 0.010 0.052
social concerns
N1 = 60, N2 = 49, N3 = 46.
ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01.

Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology


Anxiety sensitivity and labor pain  201

Table III.  Descriptive statistics for maximum and average labor pain assessments in women divided into four groups according to high/low levels of trait
anxiety and anxiety sensitivity.
↓ASI ↓STAI ↑ASI ↑STAI ↓ASI ↑STAI ↑ASI ↓STAI
(N = 15) (N = 16) (N = 11) (N = 4)
Expected average labor pain M 5.85 5.95 6.54 4.60
SD 2.477 1.774 1.941 1.140
Expected maximum labor pain M 7.25 8.14 8.31 6.40
SD 2.359 1.621 1.702 1.342
Experienced average labor pain M 6.19 6.56 6.67 6.80
SD 3.016 2.308 2.309 2.168
Experienced maximum labor pain M 8.81 8.88 9.18 8.60
SD 1.601 0.885 1.240 1.949
MPQ-SF − experienced max. labor pain M 24.75 24.63 22.92 26.00
SD 8.933 8.702 7.798 8.246
Recalled average labor pain M 7.00 6.75 7.64 8.75
SD 2.420 2.049 1.567 0.957
Recalled maximum labor pain M 8.27 8.38 9.08 9.75
SD 1.870 1.310 0.751 0.500
MPQ-SF − recalled max. labor pain M 18.40 17.25 20.18 16.75
SD 8.576 9.567 7.250 7.042
M, mean; MPQ-SF, short-form McGill pain questionairre; ASI, anxiety sensitivity index; STAI, state/trait anxiety inventory (trait anxiety scores); N, number of women; SD,
standard deviation; ↑ASI, anxiety sensitivity index median score above 18; ↓ASI, anxiety sensitivity index median score under 18; ↑STAI, trait anxiety median score above 36;
↓STAI, trait anxiety median score under 36.

maximum (r = 0.352, p < 0.01) and average (r = 0.325, p < 0.05)


labor pain expectancies, whereas anxiety as trait correlated
significantly with maximum labor pain expectancies (r = 0.306,
p < 0.05). Anxiety shared no significant relationship with per-
ceived nor with recalled labor pain. Anxiety sensitivity shared
a significant relationship with labor pain assessments when
these measures were divided into three dimensions (physical,
psychological and social concerns) (Table II). Namely, the
physical concerns dimension of anxiety sensitivity shared a
significant relationship with experienced maximum sensory
labor pain (assessments completed during labor – before the
pushing stage) (r = 0.292, p < 0.05).

Examining differences among women grouped


according to high/low levels of trait anxiety and
anxiety sensitivity
In addition, we explored differences in expected, experienced and Figure 1.  Maximum labour pain assessments in three different time
recalled labor pain between woman of varying anxiety sensitivity periods in women divided into four groups according to high/low lev-
and trait anxiety levels. An interesting trend was observed when els of anxiety and anxiety sensitivity.
women were divided into four groups according to high and
low levels of trait anxiety and anxiety sensitivity using a median- significantly in assessments of expected, experienced or recalled
split method, a method used in previous research [26,27]. Scores labor pain. Women high in anxiety sensitivity, but low in anxiety
above a median of 36 on the STAI indicated high trait anxiety, 1 month postpartum demonstrated a trend (although not statis-
and those below the median indicated low trait anxiety. Scores tically significant) to provide higher estimates of both maximum
above a median of 18 on the ASI indicated high anxiety sen- and average labor pain postpartum than those completed dur-
sitivity, whereas those below the median indicated low anxiety ing labor. In the other three groups, a trend (although not sta-
sensitivity. These numbers are comparable with those from pre- tistically significant) towards a reduction in recalled maximum
vious research conducted by Staničić and Jokić-Begić [27] with and average labor pain 1 month postpartum was observed. To
a sample of Croatian women, where the median score for trait provide a better view of the observed trends, maximum labor
anxiety was 36 and the ASI median score was 17. Descriptive pain assessments are presented as a graph (Figure 1).
statistics for maximum and average labor pain assessments in
these four groups are presented in Table III. To establish whether
Discussion
the four groups of women differed significantly in assessments
of expected, experienced or recalled labor pain, analysis of vari- High perceived and recalled labor pain correlates with nega-
ance was performed. The four groups of women did not differ tive labor experiences that can influence the occurrence of

Copyright © 2011 Informa UK Ltd.


202  D. Curzik & N. Jokic-Begic
postpartum psychological problems [28]. In order to prevent play a pivotal role in the pathogenesis of anxiety and anxiety
the occurrence of such problems, it is important to establish disorders [30]. Arguably, it is possible that the correlation of
personality traits that correlate with high perceived labor sensory pain assessments and the physical concerns dimen-
pain intensities. Previous research showed higher pain inten- sion of anxiety sensitivity in the present research is a result of
sity in primiparous women in comparison with multiparous the interaction between interoceptive sensitivity and selective
women  [29]. In order to exclude the influence of former attention to bodily sensations. Furthermore, interoceptive
labor pain experiences on labor pain assessments, especially sensitivity might have also been a factor contributing to the
on labor pain expectancies, only primiparous women were observed trend of higher recalled labor pain in women high
included in this research. in anxiety sensitivity and low in anxiety. We hypothesized that
In our research, state anxiety correlated significantly with low levels of anxiety in this group of women had lead to low
expectancies of maximum and average labor pain. These results labor pain expectancies. During labor, actual labor pain might
were expected because these measures were completed only a have come as a surprise to these women and, due to a low
few weeks before labor and thus reflected the woman’s state anxiety level, they would not have attributed it to a state of
before her first birth. In other words, because labor presented a great anxiety. Finally, because of a greater tendency to inter-
new experience to our participants, expectancies of maximum pret bodily sensations and pain as threatening and danger-
and average labor pain escalated as levels of incertitude, anxiety ous, women in this group provided higher recalled labor pain
and fear grew. Trait anxiety correlated significantly only with intensities than those reported during and immediately after
maximum labor pain expectancies. Trait anxiety is character- labor. In other words, a form of interoceptive conditioning
ized by a tendency to experience fear, uneasiness or worry in may have occurred in this group of women. It is consistent
situations that are perceived as threatening, for example, when with the model of acute pain recall proposed by Gedney &
experiencing the highest intensities of labor pain. Thus, women Logan [31]. They postulated that over time negative emotions
higher in trait anxiety focused more on maximum labor pain mediate pain memory processing such that increasingly the
intensities. The findings of this study are consistent with those aversiveness of the experience and not the pain intensity per
of Zakka et al. [29] in that higher pain intensity before vaginal se, will be remembered. Of course, this hypothesis is a matter
delivery was associated with higher anxiety levels. Similarly, for future research to investigate.
Schnur et al. [15] found that anxiety was significantly corre- There are several limitations to the present research.
lated with postoperative pain expectancies in women sched- First, the investigator’s presence could have been a distracter
uled to undergo an excisional breast biopsy or lumpectomy. to pain perception, although pain assessments during
State and trait anxiety did not correlate significantly with labor were taken after the last part of active labor. Second,
pain assessments during labor or those made 1 month postpar- our sample was relatively small. A larger sample in future
tum. However, the physical concerns dimension of anxiety sen- research would provide a more equal distribution of partici-
sitivity correlated significantly with sensory pain during labor. pants in groups according to different levels of anxiety and
These findings support Lang et al.’s [4] hypothesis that sensory anxiety sensitivity. This would allow further examination of
labor pain may not be influenced by diffuse anxiety, as it has for the trends in recalled labor pain observed in this research.
a long time been considered, but rather by the mother’s belief Finally, for better insight into expected and perceived labor
that these experiences are threatening and potentially harmful. pain, we recommend that future research use a fear of pain
Recent research conducted by Spice et al. [5] also found that questionnaire as well as a more adequate measure of affec-
physical concerns are distinct from trait anxiety in predicting tive labor pain than the currently used short-form McGill
fear of childbirth. Similar to other fears, the results of the pres- pain questionnaire.
ent research support the possibility that anxiety sensitivity may
be a risk factor for elevated fear of childbirth.
Conclusion
The results of our research are consistent with Norton and
Asmundson’s amended fear-avoidance model of pain [6]. In The findings from this research emphasize the role of anxiety
their model, they emphasize the manner in which cognitive sensitivity in the perception of labor pain. In other words, these
components of pain-related fear impact physiological pain findings imply that, during labor, there is no reason to expect
symptoms. They argue that physiological arousal can produce greater labor pain intensity in women high in anxiety, but it can
pain, leading to bodily sensations such as muscle tension, fol- be expected in women high in anxiety sensitivity. Rather than
lowed by the misinterpretation of the origin of these bodily directing attention solely to anxiety as a psychological con-
sensations. In our research, a pregnant woman with high lev- tributor to labor pain and complications during birth, child-
els of anxiety sensitivity was more likely to be fearful of intero- birth classes should also address the role of anxiety sensitivity
ceptive sensations during labor such as pain and other bodily in labor pain perception. This is consistent with the findings
sensations and to interpret them as more threatening and of Arntz & Claassens  [32], who experimentally proved that
dangerous. The interaction between interoceptive sensitivity, the meaning people attach to a stimulus influences its expe-
misinterpretation and physiological reactivity among women rienced painfulness. The clinical implication of such findings
high in anxiety sensitivity might have lead to increased rates is that the correction of dysfunctional interpretations of pain
of sensory pain. might help to reduce the burden of the experience of pain.
Interoceptive sensitivity, defined as sensitivity in detecting Different cognitive-behavioral techniques, including inducing
signals arising from the inner organs, has been suggested to fear related bodily sensations (in order to achieve habituation

Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology


Anxiety sensitivity and labor pain  203
to a fear-eliciting stimulus) and education of highly anxiety 14. Klages U, Ulusoy O, Kianifard S, Wehrbein H. Dental trait anxiety
and pain sensitivity as predictors of expected and experienced pain in
sensitive pregnant women about how they differently process stressful dental procedures. Eur J Oral Sci 2004;112:477–483.
information on pain and other bodily sensations should help 15. Schnur JB, Hallquist MN, Bovbjerg DH, Silverstein JH, Stojceska
minimize the role of psychological factors in increased labor A, Montgomery GH. Predictors of expectancies for post-surgical
pain and fatigue in breast cancer surgical patients. Pers Indiv Differ
pain perception. The use of different cognitive pain coping 2006;42:419–429.
strategies significantly extends the duration of pain toler- 16. Terry R, Niven C, Brodie E, Jones R, Prowse M. An exploration of the
ance [33]. Research studies also suggest that regular massage relationship between anxiety, expectations and memory for postop-
erative pain. Acute Pain 2007;9:135–143.
with relaxation techniques from late pregnancy to birth can 22. Brown ST, Campbell D, Kurtz A. Characteristics of labor pain at two
result in lower labor pain perception  [34]. Combined, such stages of cervical dilation. Pain 1989;38:289–295.
interventions might have an impact on woman’s overall birth 23. Lowe NK. The nature of labor pain. Am J Obstet Gynecol
2002;186:S16–S24.
experience and can help prevent the occurrence of postpar- 24. Sheiner E, Sheiner EK, Shoham-Vardi I. The relationship between
tum psychological problems. parity and labour pain. Int J Gynaecol Obstet 1998;63:287–288.
25. Ranta P, Jouppila P, Jouppila R. The intensity of labor pain in grand
Declaration of interest: This study was performed as part multiparas. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 1996;75:250–254.
17. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait
of the larger project “Anxiety and Depression in Life-long Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970.
Perspective”, funded by the Ministry of Education, Science 18. Peterson RA, Reiss, RJ. Anxiety Sensitivity Index Manual. 2nd ed.
and Sport of the Republic of Croatia. Worthington: International Diagnostic Systems; 1992.
19. Rodriguez BF, Bruce SE, Pagano ME, Spencer MA, Keller MB. Factor
structure and stability of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index in a longitudi-
References nal study of anxiety disorder patients. Behav Res Ther 2004;42:79–91.
20. Melzack R, Katz J. The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Appraisal and
  1. Saisto T, Halmesmäki E. Fear of childbirth: A neglected dilemma. Current Status. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain
Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 2003;82:201–208. assessment. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2001. pp 35–52.
  2. Ryding EL. Investigation of 33 women who demanded a cesar- 21. Stockman AF, Altmaier EM. Relation of self-efficacy to reported
ean section for personal reasons. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand pain and pain medication usage during labour. J Clin Psychol Med S
1993;72:280–285. 2001;8:161–166.
  3. Reading AE, Cox DN. Psychosocial predictors of labor pain. Pain 26. Sigmon ST, Dorhofer DM, Rohan KJ, Boulard NE. The impact of anxi-
1985;22:309–315. ety sensitivity, bodily expectations, and cultural beliefs on menstrual
  4. Lang AJ, Sorrell JT, Rodgers CS, Lebeck MM. Anxiety sensitivity as a symptom reporting: A test of the menstrual reactivity hypothesis.
predictor of labor pain. Eur J Pain 2006;10:263–270. J Anxiety Disord 2000;14:615–633.
  5. Spice K, Jones SL, Hadjistavropoulos HD, Kowalyk K, Stewart SH. 27. Stanicic A, Jokic-Begic N. Psychophysical characteristics of the pre-
Prenatal fear of childbirth and anxiety sensitivity. J Psychosom Obstet menstrual period. Coll Antropol 2010;34:1421–1425.
Gynaecol 2009;30:168–174. 28. Goodman P, Mackey MC, Tavakoli AS. Factors related to childbirth
  6. Norton PJ, Asmundson GJG. Amending the fear-avoidance model of satisfaction. J Adv Nurs 2004;46:212–219.
chronic pain: What is the role of physiological arousal? Behav Ther 29. Zakka AB, Teixeira T., Siqueira MJ. Siqueira JTT. Evaluation of
2003;34:17–30. pain before and after vaginal delivery. Clin Exp Obstet Gynecol
  7. Asmundson GJ, Taylor S. Role of anxiety sensitivity in pain-related 2009;36:241–244.
fear and avoidance. J Behav Med 1996;19:577–586. 30. Domschke K, Stevens S, Pfleiderer B, Gerlach AL. Interoceptive sen-
  8. Flink IK, Mroczek MZ, Sullivan MJ, Linton SJ. Pain in childbirth sitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: An overview and integration
and postpartum recovery: The role of catastrophizing. Eur J Pain of neurobiological findings. Clin Psychol Rev 2010;30:1–11.
2009;13:312–316. 31. Gedney JJ, Logan H. Memory for stress-associated acute pain. J Pain
  9. Reiss S. Expectancy model of fear, anxiety and panic. Clin Psychol Rev 2004;5:83–91.
1991;11:141–153. 32. Arntz A, Claassens L. The meaning of pain influences its experienced
10. Keogh E, Cochrane M. Anxiety sensitivity, cognitive biases, and the intensity. Pain 2004;109:20–25.
experience of pain. J Pain 2002;3:320–329. 33. Jokić-Begić N, Ivanec D, Markanović D. Effects of cognitive pain-
11. Murphy D, Lindsay S, Williams AC. Chronic low back pain: coping strategies and locus of control on perception of cold pres-
Predictions of pain and relationship to anxiety and avoidance. Behav sor pain in healthy individuals: Experimental study. Acute Pain
Res Ther 1997;35:231–238. 2009;11:113–120.
12. Kent G. Memory of dental pain. Pain 1985;21:187–194. 34. Kimber L, McNabb M, Mc Court C, Haines A, Brocklehurst P. Massage
13. Kent and Warren. A study of factors associated with dental anxiety. J or music for pain relief in labour: A pilot randomised placebo con-
Dent Res 1985;64:1316. trolled trial. Eur J Pain 2008;12:961–969.

Current knowledge on this subject

• We reviewed the available literature and observed that only a single study investigated the role of anxiety sensitivity in labor
pain. The study suggested that anxiety sensitivity, rather than trait anxiety significantly predicts labor pain intensity. We also
encountered rather inconsistent data regarding the role of trait anxiety in pain expectancies. Namely, some studies found that
trait anxiety correlated significantly with pain expectancies, while other implied it as a correlate of actual pain intensities.

What this study adds

• This is the first study that investigated the role of trait and state anxiety and anxiety sensitivity in expected, experienced and
recalled labor pain. The results suggest that anxiety is related to labor pain expectancies, while anxiety sensitivity physi-
cal concerns dimension shares a significant relationship with actual sensory labor pain. The findings from this research
emphasize the role of anxiety sensitivity in the perception of labor pain and it has potential role in recalled labor pain.

Copyright © 2011 Informa UK Ltd.

You might also like