Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Active head lifting from supine in early infancy: an indicator for


non-optimal cognitive outcome in late infancy
INGRID C VAN HAASTERT 1 | FLORIS GROENENDAAL 1 | MARIA K VAN DE WAARSENBURG 2 | MARIA J C
EIJSERMANS 3 | CORINE KOOPMAN-ESSEBOOM 1 | MARIAN J JONGMANS 1 , 4 | PAUL J M HELDERS 3 | LINDA S
DE VRIES 1

1 Department of Neonatology, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht. 2 Faculty of Medicine, University Medical Centre Utrecht, Utrecht.
3 Child Development and Exercise Center, Division of Pediatrics, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht. 4 Faculty of Social Sciences,
Department of Special Education, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
Correspondence to Dr Linda S de Vries at Department of Neonatology, Room KE 04.123.1, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, Lundlaan 6, 3584 EA Utrecht,
the Netherlands. E-mail: l.s.devries@umcutrecht.nl

This article is commented on by Hadders-Algra on page 489 of this issue.

PUBLICATION DATA AIM To explore whether active head lifting from supine (AHLS) in early infancy is associated with
Accepted for publication 13th December 2011. cognitive outcome in the second year of life.
Published online 14th March 2012. METHOD The presence of AHLS was always recorded in the notes of infants admitted to our ter-
tiary neonatal intensive care unit. Random sampling was used to pair infants with AHLS with two
ABBREVIATIONS comparison infants without AHLS whose sex, gestational age, birth year (1993–2009), time of
AHLS Active head lifting from supine assessment, and developmental test (Griffiths Mental Development Scales, Mental Scale of the
CUS Cranial ultrasonography Bayley Scales of Infant Development-II, or cognitive subtest of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Tod-
GMDS Griffiths Mental Development dler Development-III) were comparable. Brain injury identified from neonatal cranial ultrasound
Scales scans was classified as no – mild or moderate – severe. Z-scores of cognitive test outcomes were
BSID-II-NL Bayley Scales of Infant Develop-
calculated for multivariable analysis.
ment (2nd edition), Dutch version
BSITD-III Bayley Scales of Infant and Tod-
RESULTS Eighty-seven preterm (34 males, 53 females) and 40 term (17 males, 23 females) infants
dler Development (3rd edition) with AHLS were identified. AHLS was documented at a mean (corrected) age of 7.0 (SD 1.7) and
8.1 (SD 2.2) months respectively. The cognitive assessments were performed at a mean corrected
age of 15.7 (SD 1.7) and 23.9 (SD 1.6) months in preterm infants, and 19.1 (SD 2.3) months in term
infants. The mean cognitive outcome of preterm and term infants with AHLS was lower than that
of infants without AHLS (p=0.002 and p=0.004 respectively). This remained after excluding infants
with cerebral palsy with matching comparison infants (p=0.001 in preterm and p=0.001 in term
infants). The mean difference was highest (1.35SD) between term male infants and comparison
infants (p=0.001).
INTERPRETATION AHLS is associated with a less favourable cognitive outcome in the second year
of life in preterm as well as in term-born infants than in comparison infants.

One of the objectives of neonatal follow-up is assessment of motor behaviour has, to our knowledge, never been described
the development of high-risk preterm and term-born infants in relation to cognitive outcome.
and children, following discharge from the neonatal intensive Over the years, we have recognized AHLS and wondered
care unit. Observation of spontaneous motor behaviour is an whether there could be a relation between this behaviour in
important part of such an outpatient assessment.1 early infancy and cognitive outcome. The possible association
One example of spontaneous motor behaviour of an infant with cognitive development was suggested by suboptimal
that can be easily observed during the first year of life, particu- behavioural performance at the time of AHLS and became
larly between 6 and 9 months of (corrected) age, is active head apparent during later follow-up.
lifting from supine (AHLS). Although AHLS may occasionally When AHLS is seen as part of the spontaneous motor rep-
be seen in healthy infants, AHLS observed in our infants ertoire of an infant, this behaviour can offer information that
occurred immediately the infant was put in a supine position may alert clinicians to follow the child more carefully.
and tended to be sustained for long periods. Postural reactions The aim of this study was, therefore, to explore whether
to hands-on manoeuvres like traction and vertical and AHLS in early infancy is associated with cognitive outcome in
horizontal suspension have been described as diagnostically the second year of life. We also wanted to explore whether
relevant;2 however, AHLS as an example of spontaneous neonatal brain lesions detected with cranial ultrasonography

538 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04259.x ª The Authors. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology ª 2012 Mac Keith Press
(CUS) might act as a confounder on the relation between What this paper adds
AHLS and cognitive development. • AHLS is mostly observed between 6 and 9 months in the first year of life.
• AHLS is associated with a less favourable cognitive outcome in the second
METHOD year of life in preterm as well as in term-born infants.
Design • Term-born male infants who show AHLS seem particularly at risk for poorer
cognitive outcome.
In this retrospective case–control study, random sampling was
used to pair each infant with AHLS with two comparison We also wanted to investigate the influence of brain injuries
infants without AHLS whose sex, gestational age, birth year, detected by sequential CUS on AHLS. CUS was performed in
time of assessment, and developmental test used were compa- all infants during their stay in the neonatal intensive care unit and
rable. For this type of study, no informed consent or permis- a repeat CUS during the first visit at our neonatal follow-up
sion from the internal review board was required in our clinic (i.e. at term-equivalent age in infants born preterm and at
hospital. 3 months of age in infants born at term). CUS findings were clas-
sified into two categories: 0, no or mild brain injury (preterm:
Participants germinal layer haemorrhage, intraventricular haemorrhage
Infants born between April 1993 and December 2009 were grade II, periventricular leukomalacia grade I; term: punctate
recruited from the neonatal follow-up programme. In this white matter lesions); and 1, moderate (preterm: intraventricular
programme, the (neuromotor) development of preterm and haemorrhage grade III or cystic periventricular leukomalacia
term infants discharged from our level three neonatal intensive grade II; term: watershed injury or focal infarct) or severe brain
care unit was assessed by experienced developmental special- injury (preterm: unilateral parenchymal haemorrhage, cystic
ists until 5 years 6 months of age. The presence of AHLS was periventricular leukomalacia grade III, or cerebral artery infarc-
always separately recorded in the notes. tion; term: basal ganglia ⁄ thalamic injury or large stroke).
In total, 127 infants were identified: 87 preterm (34 males, Motor development in the first year of life was assessed by
53 females) and 40 term (17 males, 23 females) infants; for observation and assessment of the spontaneous general move-
each study infant, two comparison infants were selected. ment repertoire, postures, and postural reactions by use of the
motor assessment of the developing infant, according to the
Measurements Alberta Infant Motor Scale, and passive and active muscle tone
AHLS is a spontaneous motor activity that infants may show and responses deliberately to elicit primitive reflexes according
in the first year of life when placed in a supine position. The to Amiel-Tison.4–6 The surface on which the infant was lying
head is raised forward with the shoulders and upper extremi- was always the same: a yellow, soft, but firm mattress of
ties off the surface, often in combination with extension and 110cm · 85cm · 5cm. This mattress was always in the same
adduction of the lower extremities off the surface, with the room at the outpatient clinic, where the temperature was very
ankles in plantar flexion and clawing of the toes. The upper stable.
extremities may show variable postures or movements. The Cognitive outcome in the second year of life was assessed
infant is able to maintain this motor behaviour for minutes or using the Griffiths Mental Development Scales (GMDS), the
repeatedly during the time that is spent in supine (Fig. 1). Mental Scale of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd
When present, AHLS is usually seen immediately when the edition, Dutch version; BSID-II-NL), or the cognitive subtest
infant is put in a supine position. of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (3rd
We wanted to examine whether infants who were small for edition; BSITD-III).7–9 Before 2003, all children up to 2 years
gestational age are more prone to AHLS behaviour. Small for of age were assessed using the GMDS, consisting of five
gestational age was defined as a birthweight centile less than subscales: locomotor, personal – social, hearing and speech,
10. Birthweight centiles were determined according to the eye–hand coordination, and performance. Information can be
data of the Perinatal Registry of the Netherlands.3 obtained by testing or direct observation of the performance
of the child. In addition to quotients derived from the sub-
scales, an overall developmental quotient and a z-score can be
calculated. The mean (SD) developmental quotient for the
general population is 100 (12).7 From 2003 onwards, the
BSID-II-NL was used for children with a gestational age
younger than 30 weeks or a birthweight less than 1000g at
24 months corrected age, and from March 2008 onwards the
BSITD-III. All others were assessed using the GMDS. Items
within the Mental Scale of the BSID-II-NL are related to
memory and object permanence, problem solving, perceptual
organization, number concepts, language, and sociability.8
The cognitive subtest of the BSITD-III ‘includes items that
Figure 1: Active head lifting from supine with simultaneous lifting of the assess sensorimotor development, exploration and manipula-
lower extremities in extension, adduction, plantar flexion of the ankles, tion, object relatedness, concept formation, memory, and
and clawing of the toes. other aspects of cognitive processing’.9 Raw scores of the

Active Head Lifting and Cognitive Outcome Ingrid C van Haastert et al. 539
BSID-II-NL were converted into a mental development (1.7) months and at a mean (SD) chronological age of 9.2
index, and raw scores of the BSITD-III into a composite (1.7) months.
score. Z-scores were also calculated. The mean (SD) in the In the second year of life, there were two follow-up appoint-
normative population for the Mental Scale of the BSID-II- ments: at 15 (T1) and 24 (T2) months corrected age. At T1
NL and for the cognitive subtest of the BSITD-III is 100 (15). (mean 15.7mo corrected age, SD 1.7, n=261), all but one infant
Normative values for the Dutch population are not yet avail- was assessed using the GMDS, and at T2 (mean 23.9mo cor-
able for the BSITD-III. rected age, SD 1.6, n=207), children were assessed using the
GMDS (n=151), the BSID-II-NL (n=37), and the BSITD-III
Statistical analysis (n=19). Z-scores of case infants differed significantly from com-
Data were analysed by the Predictive Analytic Software for parison infants for the total sample for neurodevelopmental
Windows version 17.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA): t-tests outcome (mean difference 0.40SD, p=0.004) at T1, as well as at
were performed for continuous variables, and v2 tests for cate- T2 (mean difference 0.58SD, p=0.001, see Table II). Male case
gorical variables. A p value of <0.05 was considered significant. infants differed significantly from male comparison infants
Multivariable analysis using general linear modelling was per- (mean difference 0.58SD, p=0.023) at T1, as well as at T2 (mean
formed for z-scores of cognitive test outcomes as the depen- difference 0.94SD, p=0.001; see Table II and Fig. 2). Female
dent variable, and AHLS, gestational age, birthweight (small infants differed significantly from female comparison infants
or appropriate for gestational age), and CUS abnormalities as (mean difference 0.35SD, p=0.031) only at T2.
covariates. Gross motor activities such as hand-to-foot contact, rolling
from supine to prone, and independent walking showed no
RESULTS statistically significant differences between case infants and
Characteristics of the preterm and term samples are presented comparison infants.
in Table I.
Term-born infants
Preterm infants Again, slightly more female (57.5%) than male infants were
Slightly more female infants (60.9%) than male infants were identified. The mean (SD) combined gestational age of study
identified. The mean (SD) gestational age of infants and com- and comparison infants (n=120) was 40.1 (1.6) weeks and the
parison infants combined (n=261) was 30.6 (2.4) weeks and mean (SD) birthweight 3468 (574) g. AHLS was most often
the mean (SD) birthweight 1470 (521) g. There were no sta- observed during the second follow-up visit at a mean (SD) age
tistical differences between the proportion of infants and the of 8.1 (2.2) months. For the total term sample, the mean age
comparison group for the total sample and subsamples for at follow-up of infants in whom AHLS was observed was sig-
these variables. AHLS was most often observed during the nificantly lower than of comparison infants (mean difference
second follow-up visit at a mean (SD) corrected age of 7.0 0.9mo; Table I). However, this did not apply to the subsam-

Table I: Characteristics of preterm (n=261) and term (n=120) infants with and without active head lifting from supine (AHLS)

Male Female Total sample

Infant Comparison p Infant Comparison p Infant Comparison p

Preterm, n 34 68 53 106 87 174


GA mean (SD), wk 31.0 (2.6) 30.9 (2.6) 0.972 30.2 (2.3) 30.3 (2.2) 0.775 30.5 (2.4) 30.6 (2.4) 0.855
BW mean (SD), g 1595 (594) 1670 (560) 0.534 1312 (425) 1381 (469) 0.367 1423 (514) 1494 (524) 0.297
CA AHLS mean 7.4 (1.8) 6.8 (1.1) 0.046 7.2 (2.0) 6.9 (1.7) 0.346 7.3 (2.0) 6.9 (1.5) 0.062
(SD), mo
CHR age AHLS 9.5 (2.0) 8.8 (1.1) 0.070 9.4 (2.1) 9.2 (1.8) 0.382 9.5 (2.1) 9.0 (1.6) 0.081
mean (SD), mo
SGA, n (%) 5 (14.7) 0 (0) 0.003 5 (9.4) 0 (0) 0.004 10 (11.5) 0 (0) 0.001
CUS, n (%) 5 (14.7) 1 (1.5) 0.015 10 (18.9) 4 (3.8) 0.003 15 (17.2) 5 (2.9) 0.001
CP, n (%) 3 (8.8) 0 (0) 0.035 3 (5.7) 0 (0) 0.036 6 (6.9) 0 (0) 0.001
Term, n 17 34 23 46 40 80
GA mean (SD), wk 40.1 (1.7) 40.2 (1.7) 0.799 40.0 (1.7) 40.1 (1.5) 0.856 40.1 (1.7) 40.2 (1.6) 0.758
BW mean (SD), g 3257 (837) 3556 (432) 0.181 3418 (684) 3507 (480) 0.577 3349 (747) 3528 (458) 0.171
Age AHLS 8.0 (2.2) 8.7 (1.8) 0.227 7.1 (2.1) 8.2 (2.4) 0.069 7.5 (2.2) 8.4 (2.1) 0.030
mean (SD), mo
SGA, n (%) 6 (35.3) 0 (0) 0.001 4 (17.4) 1 (2.2) 0.039 10 (25) 1 (1.3) 0.001
CUS, n (%) 10 (58.8) 1 (3)a 0.001 7 (30.4) 1 (2.2) 0.001 17 (42.5) 2 (2.5) 0.001
CP, n (%) 3 (17.6) 0 (0) 0.033 5 (21.7) 0 (0) 0.003 8 (20) 0 (0) 0.001

a
Cranial ultrasound (CUS; moderate or severe abnormalities) of one comparison infant is missing. GA, gestational age; BW, birthweight; CA,
corrected age on which AHLS was observed or not; CHR age, chronological age on which AHLS was observed or not; SGA, small for gestational
age; CP, cerebral palsy.

540 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2012, 54: 538–543


Table II: Outcome of preterm (n=261) infants at 15mo follow-up (T1) and 24mo corrected age follow-up (T2) and term (n=120) infants at 19mo follow-up with
and without active head lifting from supine (AHLS)

Male Female Total sample

Infant Comparison p Infant Comparison p Infants Comparison p

Preterm
T1, n 34 68 52a 106 86a 174
CA mean (SD), mo 15.3 (1.9) 15.4 (1.6) 0.784 16.1 (1.9) 15.7 (1.5) 0.183 15.8 (1.9) 15.6 (1.6) 0.395
DQ mean (SD) 100 (16) 107 (8) 0.023 103 (12) 106 (9) 0.056 102 (14) 106 (9) 0.004
Z-score, mean (SD) )0.03 (1.36) 0.55 (0.66) 0.023 0.25 (1.0) 0.53 (0.76) 0.056 0.14 (1.15) 0.54 (0.72) 0.004
T2, n 28 56 41 82 69 138
CA mean (SD), mo 23.5 (2.4) 23.8 (1.7) 0.500 24.1 (1.8) 24.1 (0.9) 0.959 23.8 (2.1) 24.0 (1.3) 0.585
Z-score, mean (SD) )0.54 (1.14) 0.40 (0.81) 0.001 )0.09 (0.92) 0.26 (0.76) 0.031 )0.27 (1.03) 0.31 (0.78) 0.001
Term, n 17 34 23 46 40 80
Age mean (SD), mo 20.2 (2.7) 19.4 (2.5) 0.276 18.9 (2.4) 18.7 (1.8) 0.688 19.5 (2.5) 19.0 (2.2) 0.286
DQ mean (SD) 91 (15) 107 (8) 0.001 98 (16) 110 (9) 0.001 95 (16) 109 (8) 0.001
Z-score, mean (SD) )0.77 (1.3) 0.58 (0.63) 0.001 )0.19 (1.3) 0.82 (0.73) 0.001 )0.44 (1.3) 0.72 (0.69) 0.001

a
One female infant was too ill to be tested during the follow-up visit. CA, corrected age; DQ, developmental quotient.

a b
4.00

2.00
Z-scores at 24mo CA

2.00
Z-scores at 19mo

0.00
0.00

–2.00

–2.00
–4.00
*
–4.00 –6.00
Males with Males without Females with Females without Males with Males without Females with Females without
AHLS AHLS AHLS AHLS AHLS AHLS AHLS AHLS
Preterm born infants with and without AHLS Term-born infants with and without AHLS

Figure 2: Z-scores based on scores of the Griffiths Mental Development Scales, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development second (Dutch) version, and the
Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (3rd edition) for preterm born infants (n=207) and term born infants (n=120). The circles (infants) represent
outliers (1.5–3 interquartile distance from the box), the asterisk an extreme (interquartile range >3). Preterm male infants differ significantly from male com-
parison infants (p=0.001); preterm female infants differ significantly from female comparison infants (p=0.031); term male infants differ significantly from male
comparison infants (p=0.001); and term female infants differ significantly from female comparison infants (p=0.001). AHLS, active head lifting from supine.

ples of term males and females. In the second year of life, there vs 69% of 87.5% of available data, p=0.001). Finally, infants
was one follow-up appointment at about 18 months of age. without AHLS (n=70) walked 3 months earlier on average
The GMDS was administered at a mean (SD) age of 19.1 than those with AHLS (n=30): mean (SD) 14 months (2) and
(2.3) months. Z-scores of study infants differed significantly 17 (5) months respectively (p=0.002; data available for 87.5%
from comparison infants for the total sample for neurodevel- of infants).
opmental outcome (mean difference 1.15 SD, p=0.001). The
mean difference between male infants and male comparison Multivariable analysis
group was 1.35 SD (p=0.001), and between female infants and Multivariable analysis showed that only AHLS and CUS were
female comparison group 1.01 SD (p=0.001; Table II and independently associated with cognitive test outcomes at a
Fig. 2). mean age of 24 months corrected age in infants born preterm,
Term infants with AHLS tended to have less hand-to-foot and at a mean age of 19 months in infants born at term
contact compared with those without AHLS (71% vs 80% of (Table III).
65% available data), but this did not reach statistical signifi- Because six preterm born infants were later diagnosed to
cance. However, almost all infants without AHLS rolled over have cerebral palsy (CP), the same analysis was performed
from supine to prone compared with those with AHLS (97% excluding infants with CP and their matching comparison

Active Head Lifting and Cognitive Outcome Ingrid C van Haastert et al. 541
further motor development, social interactions, and later
Table III: Predictors of cognitive outcome (based on z-scores) for preterm
cognitive development. Although there is not much evidence
infants (n=207) at 2y corrected age and term infants (n=120) at 19mo using
about the influence of positioning while awake on motor
multivariable analysis
development, AHLS may result from too much time spent in
Coefficient SEM p 95% CI
supine position or in a device like an infant seat, with fewer
possibilities to experience a variety of play positions and motor
Preterm strategies.13–15 Until now, we have had no exact information
Constant 0.34 0.07 0.001 0.20 0.48
AHLS )0.42 0.13 0.002 )0.68 )0.16
about these aspects.
CUS-2 )0.70 0.22 0.002 )1.14 )0.26 Another assumption is that AHLS is based on reduced cere-
Term bral connectivity due to diffuse or overt brain injury, and that
Constant 0.75 0.10 0.001 0.55 0.95
AHLS )0.65 0.22 0.004 )1.09 )0.22
this stereotypic behaviour is ‘the motor correlate of impaired
CUS-2 )0.92 0.27 0.001 )1.44 )0.40 cognitive function’.16,17 The present study shows that not only
did AHLS make a unique contribution to the prediction of a
SEM, standard error of the mean; AHLS, active head lifting from less favourable cognitive outcome in the second year of life in
supine (0, no; 1, yes); CUS-2, cranial ultrasound scans in two
categories (0, no or mild; 1, moderate or severe brain injury). preterm and term-born infants compared with comparison
infants, but also moderate or severe CUS abnormalities when
infants. AHLS remained associated with cognitive outcome infants with CP were included. No interaction between these
(p=0.001), whereas CUS did not reach statistical significance and the other aforementioned variables could be detected in
any more (p=0.051). Similarly, we repeated the analysis with the analyses.
the term-born sample where eight infants were later diagnosed McPhillips et al. showed ‘how the educational functioning
as having CP. Again, AHLS remained associated with cognitive of children may be linked to interference from an early neuro-
outcome (p=0.001), but CUS did not reach statistical signifi- developmental system (the primary-reflex system)’. The sym-
cance anymore (p=0.12). metrical tonic neck reflex is one of the more than 70 reflexes
known within the primary-reflex system, which can be nor-
DISCUSSION mally observed between 4 and 6 months of age.18,19 AHLS
AHLS noted during the first year of life in infant graduates partly mirrors the symmetrical tonic neck reflex at which
from neonatal intensive care units appears to be associated with infants not only show flexion of the head but also extension of
a less favourable cognitive outcome in the second year of life the legs, often combined with plantar flexion in the ankles and
than in those who do not show AHLS. This applies to preterm a tendency to adduct the feet or cross them, as we show in
as well as to term infants and supports our hypothesis. In addi- Figure 1. However, the position of the upper extremities may
tion, the term infant with AHLS is less inclined to roll over be variable.
from supine to prone, and starts to walk independently at a later The strength of this study is that it is the first to show a
age. Moreover, AHLS is often sustained and repeated by the relation between AHLS and cognitive development in pre-
infant when lying in supine. The infant seems unable to use term and term infants who were at high risk for developmen-
other movement strategies when supine or to move out of this tal difficulties. However, owing to the nature of the design of
position. It is our experience that parents interpret AHLS as this study, data cannot immediately be generalized, not even
the intention of their child to come to a sitting position and that to graduate populations from neonatal intensive care units.
they tend to be proud of the strong abdominal muscles of their For instance, in this retrospective study, we were only able to
child, or to interpret AHLS as being an expression of curiosity. use information that was available to us. As the infants were
We do not know of any explanation for the underlying seen at about 6 months corrected age (preterm) or at about
mechanism or neural substrate for this behaviour. Transitions 9 months (term), we cannot exclude the possibility that
from variable, non-adaptive behaviour to adaptive selection AHLS was more common than noted by us. Another limita-
strategies occur at specific ages for different motor functions.10 tion is that the GMDS and BSITD-III are not yet standard-
Interestingly, AHLS is most often observed between 6 and ized for Dutch children.7,9 Judging the results of the preterm
9 months in the first year of life, the same period in which comparison infants on the GMDS, BSID-II-NL, and
‘selection of the most efficient postural adjustment in which BSITD-III, it is noteworthy that although the start of life of
all direction-specific neck, trunk, and proximal leg muscles are these infants was not optimal, the mean values corrected for
activated’ occurs, according to Hadders-Algra.11 One can the degree of preterm birth were higher than expected at T1
speculate about the potential influences of factors associated as well as at T2, with the mean developmental quotients and
with the environment, parent–infant interactions, and the neu- z-scores well above the normative mean and the SD lower
ronal development related to AHLS. It is possible that an than the norm values (Tables II and III). This applied also to
infant is curious to see what is happening in the environment the term-born comparison infants. The question of whether
and in the absence of other motor strategies uses AHLS to normative values for cognitive development in the general
interact socially. De Groot12 postulated that the influence of Dutch population are different from other populations has
the environment and spontaneous motility both play an not yet been answered.7,9 However, our findings are in accor-
important role in the fine-tuning of postural control. As such, dance with those of Anderson et al.,20 who compared the
stereotypic postural control in early infancy may interfere with results on the BSITD-III of extremely preterm, low birth-

542 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2012, 54: 538–543


weight infants with those born at term with a normal birth- However, subgroup analyses revealed that motor and mental
weight, and detected higher mean values than expected for outcomes of intervention in infants with bronchopulmonary
the comparison group. dysplasia and combined biological and social risk factors
It is challenging to unravel the underlying mechanism or improved.22
neural substrate of AHLS. In the meantime, parents should be In conclusion, this study provides provisional evidence of a
provided with information on how to handle and position relation between AHLS and subsequent consequences for
their child. Children who show AHLS, especially those with cognitive outcome, an interference that may result from an
moderate to severe CUS abnormalities, need a long-lasting underlying developmental deficit. Within the present study,
follow-up to detect possible special needs in time. Whether an AHLS in the first year of life was significantly associated with
intervention programme may be beneficial to improve the a less favourable cognitive outcome in the second year of life,
cognitive development of a child who shows AHLS in early in preterm as well as term-born infants. Further research is
infancy remains to be seen. A systematic review and meta- needed about the prevalence and neurodevelopmental conse-
analysis could not detect long-term benefits in cognitive quences of infants showing this remarkable clinical sign.
outcome of developmental intervention programmes for pre-
term-born infants.21 Koldewijn et al. showed, in a multicentre, ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
randomized, controlled trial in which parents of 83 infants of We are grateful to L. F. Sinnecker who performed some of the data
very low birthweight received a post-discharge intervention collection, and I. Janssen who created the drawing (Fig. 1) of an infant
programme, that groups did not differ on the mental develop- who actively lifts the head from supine.
ment index and other variables at 24 months corrected age.

REFERENCES
1. Sauve R, Lee SK. Neonatal follow-up programs and follow- 9. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. 16. Hadders-Algra M. Reduced variability in motor behaviour:
up studies: historical and current perspectives. Paediatr Child San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, 2006. an indicator of impaired cerebral connectivity? Early Hum
Health 2006; 11: 267–70. 10. Heineman KR, Middelburg KJ, Hadders-Algra M. Develop- Dev 2008; 84: 787–9.
2. Zafeiriou DI. Primitive reflexes and postural reactions in the neuro- ment of adaptive motor behaviour in typically developing 17. Hadders-Algra M. Variation and variability: key words in
developmental examination. Pediatr Neurol 2004; 31: 1–8. infants. Acta Paediatr 2010; 99: 618–24. human motor development. Phys Ther 2010; 90: 1823–37.
3. Visser GHA, Eilers PHC, Elferink-Stinkens PM, Merkus 11. Hadders-Algra M. The neuronal group selection theory: 18. McPhillips M, Hepper PG, Mulhern G. Effects of replicating
HMWM, Wit JM. New Dutch reference curves for birth- promising principles for understanding and treating develop- primary-reflex movements on specific reading difficulties in
weight by gestational age. Early Hum Dev 2009; 85: 737–44. mental motor disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol 2000; 42: children: a randomised, double-blind, controlled trial. Lancet
4. Einspieler C, Prechtl HF. Prechtl’s assessment of general 707–15. 2000; 355: 537–41.
movements: a diagnostic tool for the functional assessment 12. De Groot L. Posture and motility in preterm infants. Dev 19. McPhillips M, Sheehy N. Prevalence of persistent primary
of the young nervous system. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Med Child Neurol 2000; 42: 65–8. reflexes and motor problems in children with reading diffi-
Rev 2005; 11: 61–7. 13. Bartlett DJ, Kneale Fanning JE. Relationships of equipment culties. Dyslexia 2004; 10: 316–38.
5. Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor Assessment of the Developing use and play positions to motor development at eight months 20. Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, Roberts G, Doyle
Infant. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders, 1994. corrected age of infants born preterm. Pediatr Phys Ther LW. Underestimation of developmental delay by the new
6. Amiel-Tison A, Grenier A. Neurological Assessment during 2003; 15: 8–15. Bayley-III Scale. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2010; 164: 352–6.
the First Year of Life. New York, NY: Oxford University 14. Fetters L, Huang HH. Motor development and sleep, play, 21. Orton J, Spittle A, Doyle L, Anderson P, Boyd R. Do early
Press, 1986. and feeding positions in very-low-birthweight infants with intervention programmes improve cognitive and motor out-
7. Griffiths R. The Abilities of Young Children. A Comprehen- and without white matter disease. Dev Med Child Neurol comes for preterm infants after discharge? A systematic
sive System of mental Measurement for the First Eight Years 2007; 49: 807–13. review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009; 51: 851–9.
of Life. London: The Test Agency, 1984. 15. Majnemer A, Barr RG. Influence of supine sleep positioning 22. Koldewijn K, van Wassenaer WA, Wolf MJ, et al. A neurobe-
8. Van der Meulen BF, Ruiter SAJ, Lutje Spelberg HC, Smr- on early motor milestone acquisition. Dev Med Child Neurol havioral intervention and assessment program in very low birth
kovsky M. BSID-II-NL, Dutch Manual. Lisse, the Nether- 2005; 47: 370–6. weight infants: outcome at 24 months. J Pediatr 2010; 156:
lands: Swets Test, 2002. 359–65.

Active Head Lifting and Cognitive Outcome Ingrid C van Haastert et al. 543

You might also like