Plasticity, Plasticity, Plasticity and The Rigid Problem of Sex

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 2

Forum: Science & Society

Plasticity, plasticity, plasticity. . . and the rigid problem


of sex
Cordelia Fine1, Rebecca Jordan-Young2, Anelis Kaiser3, and Gina Rippon4
1
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, Melbourne Business School, and Centre for Ethical Leadership, University of
Melbourne, Carlton, VIC 3053, Australia
2
Department of Women’s, Gender, and Sexuality Studies, Barnard College, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
3
Department of Social Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, 3012, Switzerland
4
Aston Brain Centre, School of Life and Health Sciences (Psychology), Aston University, Birmingham, B4 7ET, UK

Why is popular understanding of female–male differences others, and aspects of the environment to influence brain
still based on rigid models of development, even though and behavior through reciprocal modulation of the endo-
contemporary developmental sciences emphasize plas- crine system (see [4]). Steroid (and other) hormones, such
ticity? Is it because the science of sex differences still as testosterone, have come to be regarded as key mediators
works from the same rigid models? of behavioral plasticity, enabling animals to be responsive
to key environmental characteristics in both the short and
In recent months, a new book co-authored by best-selling long term [5]. In addition, although certain steroids (espe-
author John Gray hit the shelves that, like his many other cially testosterone and estrogen) continue to be character-
books, claims there are ‘hardwired’ differences in the ized as ‘sex hormones’ by both popular and scientific
brains of females and males [1]. In the news media, con- convention, the notion that these steroids affect the brain
tinuing commentary on the causes of the global financial in a way that is more or less bifurcated by sex is challenged
crisis point to a testosterone-fueled finance industry that by the complexity of the endocrine system. For example,
would benefit from more women, whose lower levels of both circulating levels of testosterone and receptor respon-
testosterone make them intrinsically more risk averse sivity are modulated by behavior and social context [4,5],
(e.g., [2]). Meanwhile, on the website of the popular maga- and women appear to get more ‘behavioral bang’ for their
zine Psychology Today, several evolutionary psychology ‘hormonal buck’ [6]. In line with a complexity that rules out
blogs draw on the familiar theme of the brains of cavemen, simple dimorphic relations between ‘sex hormones’ and
and cavewomen, to explain contemporary gender relations behavior, assumptions of causal links between high abso-
(e.g., Homo consumericus and Evolutionary Entertainment, lute levels of testosterone and masculine characteristics
accessible from http://www.psychologytoday.com/topics/ have often not been supported by the data [7]. Meanwhile,
evolutionary-psychology). subfields within human evolutionary behavioral science,
Curiously, the scientific ideas implicit in these popular as well as data from developmental neurobiology, have
understandings have long been left in the dust: assump- strongly challenged the major tenets of the evolutionary
tions that brain circuitry is largely fixed by a genetic psychology view that, as Cosmides and Tooby [8] put it,
blueprint, that there is a unidirectional, causal pathway ‘[o]ur modern skulls house a Stone Age mind’, arguing a
from genes to behavior via hormones and brains, and that more prominent role for culture in both evolution and
evolution has left us with brains and mental processes individual development, consistent with contemporary
strongly reminiscent of our Paleolithic ancestors, have accounts of neurodevelopment, and human behavioral
been widely rejected following conceptual and empirical diversity (e.g., [9]).
upheavals in the relevant scientific fields. These conceptual changes clearly complement and res-
It is now clear that the functional and even structural onate across the three fields. Humans have evolved an
organization of the human nervous system is a continuous adaptively plastic brain that is responsive to environmen-
and dynamic process that persists throughout one’s life. tal conditions and experiences, and the modulation of
‘Experience-dependent plasticity’ has been demonstrated endocrine function by those experiential factors contrib-
time and again in the acquisition of skills as wide ranging utes to that plasticity. Why, then, do popular understand-
as musical performance, basketball, dancing, taxi driving, ings of female/male behavior as rooted in a biological core
and juggling (reviewed in [3]). remain entrenched in scientific ideas characteristic of the
Behavioral neuroendocrinology has been transformed previous century? Is it, in part, because the sex/gender
by an increasingly large body of research demonstrating science within these three fields is similarly entrenched?
the power of an individual’s behavior, the behavior of The major account of sexual differentiation of the brain,
brain organization theory, still posits that prenatal hor-
Corresponding author: Kaiser, A. (anelis.kaiser@psy.unibe.ch). mones give rise to (or ‘hardwire’) permanent structural
and functional sex differences, despite considerable and
long-standing evidence that early hormonal effects are
not permanent (see [10]). In functional neuroimaging,

550
Forum: Science & Society Trends in Cognitive Sciences November 2013, Vol. 17, No. 11

investigation of experience-dependent plasticity has only important areas have a responsibility to recognize
rarely been applied to the emergence, maintenance, and how social assumptions influence their research and,
plasticity of gendered behavior (e.g., [11]). Instead, studies indeed, public understanding of it. Moreover, they
tend simply to compare the biological sexes, as though the should also recognize that there are important and
implicit aim were to identify fixed, universal female versus exciting opportunities to change these social assump-
male signatures [12]. Similarly, investigations of female/ tions through rigorous, reflective scientific inquiry and
male differences in ‘sex hormones’ and social behavior are debate.
often correlational, with analyses implying that hormonal
level is a ‘pure’ biological and causally primary variable, References
rather than taking into account the fact that biological 1 Annis, B. and Gray, J. (2013) Work with Me: The 8 Blind Spots between
Men and Women in Business, Palgrave MacMillan
factors are ‘entangled’ with the individual’s social history
2 Kolhatkar, S. (2010) New York Magazine, 29 March
and current social context (see [13]). In addition, in evolu- 3 May, A. (2011) Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult
tionary psychology investigations of female–male differ- human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 475–482
ences, it tends to be left to researchers outside the field to 4 van Anders, S. and Watson, N. (2006) Social neuroendocrinology:
identify the environmental and cultural factors that are effects of social contexts and behaviors on sex steroids in humans.
Hum. Nat. 17, 212–237
important in moderating supposedly ‘universal’ sex-related
5 Oliveira, R.F. (2009) Social behavior in context: hormonal modulation
preferences (see [9,14]). of behavioral plasticity and social competence. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49,
Feminist critics of each research domain have repeat- 423–440
edly noted that it is not only that the outputs of such 6 Sherwin, B. (1988) A comparative analysis of the role of androgen in
science contribute to unwelcome and scientifically unjusti- human male and female sexual behavior: behavioral specificity, critical
thresholds, and sensitivity. Psychobiology 16, 416–425
fied cultural understandings of female–male relations as 7 Van Anders, S. (2013) Beyond masculinity: testosterone, gender/sex,
fixed, inevitable, and ordained by nature, but also that and human social behavior in a comparative context. Front.
such science itself is flawed and unenlightening [10,12]. An Neuroendocrinol. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.07.001
understanding of gender as a complex, multilevel, hierar- 8 Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1997) Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer,
chical structure that shapes not only institutions, inter- Center for Evolutionary Psychology
9 Bolhuis, J. et al. (2011) Darwin in mind: new opportunities for
relations, cognition and perception, but also the brain, evolutionary psychology. PLoS Biol. 9, 1–8
endocrine system, and the manifestation of evolutionary 10 Jordan-Young, R. (2010) Brain Storm: The Flaws in the Science of Sex
processes, can bring about better and more informative Differences, Harvard University Press
science. Happily, there are growing examples of this. Take, 11 Wraga, M. et al. (2006) Neural basis of stereotype-induced shifts in
women’s mental rotation performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neur. 2, 12–19
for instance, a large-scale longitudinal study that found
12 Fine, C. (2012) Is there neurosexism in functional neuroimaging
that fatherhood reduced testosterone levels in men, and investigations of sex differences? Neuroethics 6, 369–409
more so in fathers who spent more time in the physical 13 Kaiser, A. (2012) Re-conceptualizing ‘sex’ and ‘gender’ in the human
caring of their young offspring [15]. This complements brain. J. Psychol. 220, 130–136
findings from a comparison of two neighboring cultural 14 Brown, G.R. et al. (2009) Bateman’s principles and human sex roles.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 297–304
groups in Tanzania, which found lower testosterone levels
15 Gettler, L. et al. (2011) Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood
among fathers from the population in which paternal care decreases testosterone in human males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
was the cultural norm compared with fathers from the U.S.A. 108, 13194–16199
group in which paternal care was typically absent [16]. 16 Muller, M. et al. (2009) Testosterone and paternal care in East African
Together, these studies show how the social construction of foragers and pastoralists. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 347–354
gender roles for fathers modulates endocrine state.
1364-6613/$ – see front matter ß 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
The relations between science and society are two- http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.010 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, November 2013,
way. Scientists who work in politically sensitive and Vol. 17, No. 11

551

You might also like