Pepin Sky 1964

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

CONVERGENCE:

A PHENOMENON IN COUNSELING AND IN PSYCHOTHERAPY:


HAROLD B. PEPINSKY AND THOMAS O. KARST
Ohio State University

lOUNSELING" and "psychotherapy" are changing the client, has an observable and measur-

"C labels for a special kind of interaction


between persons. Unless otherwise spec-
ified (e.g., with a modifier such as "group" or
able counterpart in therapist-patient "expectancies"
about the interaction; and we are simply asserting
—in a manner consistent with empirical findings—
"multiple") the process of interaction may be as- that observable interactional change during the
sumed to occur under the following conditions: course of a therapy is related, in kind and degree,
(a) It involves two persons, one member of the to these expectancies (see Goldstein, 1962). For
pair who is designated as "counselor" or "psy- this to happen as a therapeutic process, by defini-
chotherapist" and the other, as "client" or "pa- tion, the first three conditions must also be
tient"; (b) the two persons are together long satisfied.
enough for interaction to take place, which may
range from a single episode, lasting only for a CONVERGENCE DURING THERAPY
brief interval of clock time, to longer periods,
The postulated interactional change in state on
repeated on one or more weekly occasions over
which this paper focuses is one that, following the
a number of years; (c) the two persons interact
lead of Sherif (1936) and Asch (1952), we have
directly with each other (e.g., not through a third
labeled as convergence. Sherif's and Asch's pioneer
person and without externally imposed delay in
experiments on group norm-setting behavior, out of
feedback), saliently by means of spoken language
which the present concept of convergence derives,
but also by other sound and physical movement;
are prototypic of later work on what is now apt to
and (d) the verbal and nonverbal interaction of
be called "conformity" behavior. In the present
the two persons is instrumental to producing in the
context, we prefer the earlier term. As originally
client or patient member of the pair a change in
used by these authors, the term "convergence"
state (Pepinsky & Pepinsky, 1954, pp. 71-73),
referred to a lessening of discrepancy in judgments
which may vary widely, e.g., from an overt social
made by pairs (or larger groups) of subjects
"reconditioning" (Krasner, 1962) to the modifica-
exposed to the stimulus of a stationary point of
tion of an inner, existential "being" (Rogers,
light in an otherwise darkened room. Among early
1959). In this paper, the process will be referred
experimental manipulations and findings the fol-
to as "therapy," and the two protagonists in it,
lowing are pertinent. Individual subjects who
as "therapist" and "client."
watched the light reported different amounts of
If the above conditions of a therapeutic process
movement in it (the autokinetic effect); when
are warranted, by their occurrence as observable
asked to watch the light in pairs, following the
events, then one may postulate that, during the
individual session, their discrepancies in judgment
course of the therapy, a related and observable
tended to become less over successive group ses-
change in state occurs in the interaction of thera-
sions. In contrast, subjects who were first paired
pist and client. For example, Condition d, which
tended immediately to reconcile their differences in
specifies that the interaction is instrumental to
judgment; and when individually presented the
a
This paper was presented at a symposium on "Social stimulus, after the group sessions, tended to retain
Influence, Counseling, and Psychotherapy," at American their similarity in judgment. In related experi-
Psychological Association, Phildelphia, August 1963; its ments it was found that when one member of each
preparation was supported in part by the Behavioral
Sciences Section, Air Force Office of Scientific Research,
pair had been instructed in advance by the experi-
under Grant No. AF 49(538)-373. Special thanks are menter to hold to a fixed judgment, his unin-
due Karl E. Weick for helpful advice and criticism. structed partner tended to shift in his judgment
333
334 AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST

toward that of the instructed member of the pair.2 meaningful propositions about convergence in the
The last experimental condition and the kind of therapeutic interaction and other events that
convergence it tends to elicit, i.e., a shift in belief on appear to be logically related to it.3
the part of the uninstructed subject toward that of Fortunately, as implied earlier, there exists an
the instructed subject, is especially germane to what abundance of empirical evidence to support our
Frank (1959, 1961) has described as a "successful" general thesis that interactional changes in state
therapeutic interaction. As he states the case, a do occur during therapy. One of the most ex-
therapy is likely to be viewed by the two par- haustive and relevant studies that we know of was
ticipants as successful when the therapist (the made by Lennard and Bernstein (Lennard et al.,
healer) is able to persuade his client (the sufferer) 1960), who obtained verbatim recordings of eight
to alter his despair over his condition to hope for therapies, each lasting for nearly a year and
his recovery. The key to the process is the ability providing SOO treatment hours, more than 120 of
of the therapist to influence the client either to which were coded and analyzed. The analysis
change his beliefs or to modify his behavior, or to included nearly 6,000 exchanges and more than
do both things; in Frank's view this is the reason 41,000 verbal propositions, multiply coded. Al-
why therapists with widely different therapeutic though the amount of information yielded for
rationales and rituals can legitimately claim suc- analysis by the authors' application of a computer
cesses. And Frank draws an analogy between technology is almost too staggeringly large to be
psychotherapeutic belief and ritual, on the one comprehensible, Lennard (1962) has more recently
hand, and the use of placebos, religious revivalism, summarized their findings relative to "therapy . . .
Chinese thought reform, faith healing, etc., on the as a[n] ... interaction system," and reports "in-
other. His paradigm, it should be noted, is the creasing correlation over time between specific
laboratory-experimental study "of the processes by kinds of patient and therapist communications
which one person influences another [1961, p. 97]." [p. 225]." Most relevantly, this finding also
Frank also suggests that a therapist's success in provides support for our postulate of a therapist-
"indoctrinating" his client toward a change in state client convergence during a therapy.
through the process of therapy is highly related Indeed, in examining others' systematic dis-
to a previous indoctrination, which the therapist cussions of the therapeutic process and their sup-
himself has undergone and by which he is induced porting evidence, we are struck more by the
to believe in the efficacy of his own therapeutic relevance and potential utility of the convergence
ritual; the analytic training institute is used to postulate than by its spanking newness. Thus,
afford a clear illustration of how such antecedent Matarazzo (in press) and Heller (1963), among
indoctrination may take place. If for "previously others, have clearly demonstrated that the
indoctrinated therapist" we now substitute the therapist's behavior can "shape" that of his client.
phrase "previously instructed subject" and if we Frank's (1959, 1961) brilliantly lucid and scholarly
substitute for "the client" the phrase "naive sub- discussion of the therapist's indoctrinating par-
ject," the analogy drawn by Frank between the ticipation and its effects upon the patient has
process of therapy and experimental studies of already been cited; so has Goldstein's (1962)
social influence processes becomes even more com- splendidly organized discussion of therapeutic
plete. Thus, we may anticipate finding in the interaction in relation to therapist-client expect-
therapeutic interaction itself a dependent event of ancies. The latter authors have a good deal to
convergence analogous to that of convergence in say about conditions that seem to be logically and
judgment-making and other behavior found in empirically antecedent to "convergence," as we
the laboratory by Sherif, Asch, et al., wherein the 3
In fact, the social-psychology laboratory-therapy anal-
client shifts in degree toward norms and standards ogy can be extended, as suggested by R. Rosenthal (1963).
made available to him by the therapist. And, as On the one hand, we have "the mediation of E's bias via
will be shown, the analogy is useful in suggesting his research assistants [p. 279]," and, on the other, the
"experienced" therapist's "biasing" effects upon a neo-
2
Reference to Asch's 1937 experiments on subjects' shifts phyte's therapy through an antecedent "good" or "poor"
in judgment as related to manipulation of the autokinetic prognosis (see also Frank, 1961; Goldstein, 1962). This
phenomenon may be found in Sherif and Cantril (1947, antecedent biasing effect upon the therapeutic encounter
pp. 53-55). will be discussed in a forthcoming paper.
CONVERGENCE 335

conceptualize it. Moreover, a wealth of supportive must bring about a change in the client as the
evidence and implication can be found in the report therapist wants him to change although one would
of the second APA-sponsored conference on Re- expect minimal agreement among these authors
search in Psychotherapy (Strupp & Luborsky, on the "what," the "good," and the "how." Rotter
1962). Lennard (1962), for example, suggests (1962) makes a nice point in this regard by
that future studies will identify "general principles showing how Kelly (1955) and Rogers (1959)
governing the way In which one individual changes deal with similar phenomenal content but wind up
another through communication [p. 235, italics with highly diverse objectives and procedures.
added]," Krasner (1962) cites considerable evi- Using Frank's (1961) scheme, we may characterize
dence to show how therapists and (clients) can Rogers as an "evocative" therapist; Kelly as
and do exercise "behavior control" to "influence" "directive"; e.g., the latter focused in his thera-
clients' behavior. Butler, Rice, and Wagstaff peutic goals, the former more open-ended. Yet,
(1962) are able to generalize that client "respon- according to Frank, both therapists would be
siveness" to therapist "expressiveness" is directly "persuasive" in their attempts to induce change in
related to what "the same therapist calls successful the client during therapy, for any "evocative"
treatment [see Saslow's 'Final Summary'—1962, therapy has an ideology that can be made explicit
p. 302]." Interestingly enough, the more "expres- (cf. Samler, 1960).
sive" therapist is also shown to be more "experi- In light of the above systematic, empirical, and
enced." Again, Frank (1962) adds his inference hortative support for our concept, let us assume
that "the ability of the therapist's formulations to that every therapeutic interaction is characterized
directly modify the [client's] feelings" mediates by an amount of convergence, which involves a
"cognitive insights" in the client, "which can measurable shift in client behavior toward that
influence [the client's] behavior [p. 11]." Levin- of the therapist. But what is the nature of the
son's (1962) paper is representative of several convergence in which we are interested? And what
research reports that imply a relationship between research strategy might be used in eliciting in-
"social class" of therapist and client, on the one formation about it? Suppose we now postulate a
hand, and what we refer to as convergence, on the more explicit interactional process in which the
other. client acquires a psychological grammar * made
Elsewhere, D, Rosenthal's (19SS) research im- available to him by the therapist. We may define
plies that the therapist-client interaction may "psychological grammar" in this context as (a)
influence the client to shift toward a pattern of categories of belief and action and (b) rules of
"moral values" more consistent with that of the belief and action introduced by the therapist. By
therapist. Glad (1959) even postulates that definition, the client converges toward the therapist
Freudian, Sullivanian, Rankian, and Rogerian in the therapeutic interaction to the extent that
therapists differ essentially in the "values" that (a) during the course of the therapy the therapist
they "teach" the client during therapy. The impor- makes such a grammar available to the client, and
tance of inducing "desirable" value change in the (b) the client makes use of it. The postulated
client as a therapeutic goal is argued, e.g., by Glad event of convergence is assumed to have as its
(1959) and Samler (1960). In this vein, Rotter empirical counterpart a phenomenon that is, in
(1954, 1962) prescribes a "successive structuring" principle, observable in any therapeutic situation
of the therapeutic situation by the therapist. This and measurable in amount. We recognize that,
would draw upon the therapist's knowledge of empirically, convergence is not likely to be a
personality theory (what to change in the client), strictly one-way phenomenon: i.e., part of the
his value system (what the "good" changes are), therapist's grammar may be acquired from his
and a theory of learning (how to change) to deter- client.
mine how the therapist is to behave toward the
client during a therapeutic interaction. In Kelly's RESEARCH ON THE COVERGENCE PROCESS
(1955) "fixed role therapy" the client's "personal
The postulated event of convergence suggests a
construct system" is to be explicitly modified by
number of research tasks, which will be outlined
the therapist. Thus, Glad, Rotter, and Kelly are
alike in arguing that the therapeutic interaction * This term has been adapted from Johnson (1963).
336 AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST

below. One set of tasks is centered on study of another category may be conceived of as therapist
the phenomenon itself; a second set, on the postu- and client "expectancies" for the therapy and its
lation of (a) logical antecedents to convergence as outcomes. Still another, given extensive review by
well as (b) its logical consequences, and with tests Krasner (e.g., 1962), may be described as "rein-
of empirical predictions implied by statements of forcement" by the therapist of a desired client
relationship. grammar. Finally, but not exhaustively, a major
group of antecedents includes the training and
The Study of Convergence indoctrination of the therapist (e.g., see Frank,
Our proposed initial research strategy differs 1961; Levinson, 1962). In this context a scheme
from that employed by many previous students of of organizational power and member compliance,
therapy, for we assume that the content of a such as that presented by Etzioni (1961), may
therapeutic grammar may be arrived at inductively help us to inquire into the relationship between
by a careful reading of individual therapy proto- antecedent sociocultural and organizational in-
cols, supplemented by the viewing of sound films volvements of the therapist, on the one hand, and
taken of the same original therapeutic interactions. the kinds and amounts of convergence he is able
It appears likely that the latter observational to elicit from different classes of clients, on the
material will add contextual meaning to the other.
language of a typescript.5 It may be, for example,
Some Possible Consequences
that every therapist-client pairing evokes its own
idiosyncratic grammar; yet there is warrant for A number of the studies and discussions cited
believing that experienced therapists will be more above also indicate that convergence in therapy
likely than inexperienced ones to be consistent has potential importance, not only as a dependent
from client to client in the kind of grammar that event, but as a precursor of still other therapeutic
they make available to clients (cf. Hoffman, 19 59). consequences. As implied by the arguments of
The important pioneer research contributions of Glad (1959) and Frank (1961), for instance, the
Rogers and Robinson and their students (e.g., event of convergence may be followed by "success"
Robinson, 1950, 1955; Seeman & Raskin, 1953) in otherwise widely different kinds of therapy.
provide important methodological guidelines for Indeed, empirical knowledge of the grammar in-
typescript analysis, some of which are seemingly troduced by a therapist and assimilated by his
ignored in later research. Illustratively, researchers client may be valuable in pointing up more ex-
such as Lennard and Bernstein (Lennard et al., plicitly than is customary in therapy research what
1960) might have added to their harvest of findings changes the therapist is attempting to effect in
had their sampling within interviews been based on his client and what the criteria for "improvement"
meaningful context units rather than units of clock should be—in the therapist's own terms! From
time (e.g., see Danskin, 1959; Muthard, 1958). this analysis specific, as well as general, predic-
tions about therapeutic outcomes can be made and
A Search for Antecedents tested. Still other events, such as extratherapeutic
interpersonal attraction, client attitude formation
A review of previous research studies indicates
and change, and even his mannerisms and other
a number of possible antecedents to the con-
expressive movements, may be shown to vary with
vergence phenomenon; formal postulation of rela-
convergence in therapy, adding to its construct
tionships will not be attempted in this discussion.
validity.
One such category, as suggested by Frank (1959,
1961) and Heller (1963), may be labeled as Other Studies
"social persuasion"; following Goldstein (1962)
We shall conclude this introductory paper by
6
We are heartened in this view by reports from suggesting some other research implications of the
Frederick Kanfer and Robert Rosenthal, who are among convergence postulate. First, it affords meaning-
those using sound films in their systematic inquiry into
interpersonal behavior (R. Rosenthal, 1963, p. 279; also fulness and focus to the making of a social-
personal communications, May S, 1963). Also, Johnson structural analysis of the therapeutic process.
(1963) shows that contextual cues add to the meaningful- Thus, by reference to our concept R. Rosenthal's
ness of language analysis. (1963) seminal research on "experimenter bias"
CONVERGENCE 337

can be made highly relevant to a functional psychiatrists- or psychologists-in-training. It may


analysis (Merton, 1957) of the therapeutic process. be that the latter, as against the former, simply
Rosenthal, like Frank (1959) and Goldstein tend to "lose" their clients in a morass of words.7
(1962), has discussed the "biasing" effects of
antecedent expectancies upon the therapeutic proc- REFERENCES
ess. One may well ask, "What is the 'latent func- ASCH, S. E. Social psychology. New York: Prentice-
tion' (what are the unexpected consequences?) Hall, 1952.
of a therapeutic encounter, as against its 'manifest BUTLER, J. M., RICE, LAURA N., & WAGSTAJT, ALICE K.
function' (those consequences overtly intended by On the naturalistic definition of variables: An analogue
of clinical analysis. In H. H. Strupp & L. Luborsky
the therapist)?" Analysis of the kind and amount (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy. Vol. 2. Washington,
of convergence that occurs could help us to answer D. C.: American Psychological Association, 1962. Pp.
this type of question. Again, the study of con- 178-205.
vergence and events related to it may be helpful DANSKIN, D. G. Roles played by counselors in their in-
in describing the forms that a therapist's bias may terviews. (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University)
Ann Arbor, Mich.: University Microfilms, 1959, No.
take; e.g., we may be able to state more explicitly 59-2SS5.
than Gilbert and Levinson (1957) what are the ETZIONI, A. A comparative analysis of complex organiza-
denning attributes of a "custodial" versus a tions. Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1961.
"humanitarian" treatment—or to discover still FRANK, J. D. The dynamics of the psychotherapeutic
other dimensions.6 relationship. Psychiatry, 1959, 22, 17-39.
FRANK, J. D. Persuasion and healing. Baltimore: Johns
Second, research on therapeutic convergence Hopkins Press, 1961.
may help to make clear in what ways therapy, as FRANK, J. D. The role of cognition in illness and healing.
a social event, may differ from other diadic rela- In H. H. Strupp & L. Luborsky, Research in psycho-
tionships in everyday life (Pepinsky, 1961). For therapy. Vol. 2. Washington, D. C.: American Psycho-
example, scalar judgments of simple to complex logical Association, 1962. Pp. 1-12.
GILBERT, DORIS C., & LEVINSON, D. J. "Custodialism"
applied to the grammar introduced may provide an and "humanism" in mental hospital structure and staff
empirical answer to the old (and often tiresome) ideology. In M. Greenblatt, D. J. Levinson, &
question about differences between psychotherapy R. Williams (Eds.), The patient and the mental
and counseling. It is possible that counseling hospital. Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1957. Pp. 20-35.
simply involves fewer categories and less complex GLAD, D. D. Operational values in psychotherapy. New
York: Oxford Univer. Press, 1959.
as well as fewer rules than psychotherapy. Related GOLANN, S. E. Description and preliminary evaluation
scalar judgments, implied by Butler et al.'s (1962) of performance as staff members of mental health
research, could be made along a continuum of a agencies. In E. M. Bower (Chm.), Nontraditionally
well-communicated to poorly communicated gram- trained counselors-psychotherapists—their training, em-
mar. It may be that the more expressive therapist ployment progress, and perceived employability. Sym-
posium presented at American Psychological Association,
of Butler et al.'s description is able to make his
Philadelphia, August 1963.
grammar more available to his client than is the GOLDSTEIN, A. P. Therapist-patient expectancies in psy-
less expressive therapist. chotherapy. New York: Macmillan, 1962.
With these scalar judgments and others like HELLER, K. Interpersonal style in an interview analogue.
them one may be able to study empirically and In R. A. Myers (Chm.), Social influence, counseling, and
psychotherapy. Symposium presented at American
meaningfully salient differences among the psy- Psychological Association, Philadelphia, August 1963.
chological grammars of, say, lawyers, pastoral HOFFMAN, A. E. Analysis of counselor subroles. /.
counselors, and housewives. Illustratively, Mar- counsel. Psychol., 1959, 6, 61-67,
garet Rioch's (Golann, 1963; Magoon, 1963; JOHNSON, N. F. Linguistic models and the functional unit
of language behavior. Paper read at Institute in Psy-
Rioch, 1963) previously uninstructed housewives, cholinguistics, George Peabody College for Teachers,
who were trained to become psychotherapists, may June 1963.
be successful because their grammar is simpler KELLY, G. A. The psychology of personal constructs.
Vols. 1 & 2. New York: Norton, 1955.
and perhaps better communicated than that of, say, KRASNER, L. The therapist as a social reinforcement
8 machine. In H. H. Strupp & L. Luborsky (Eds.),
Lennard's (1962) data on unintentional "system-
T
maintaining" behavior manifested by therapist and client Louis A. Wienckowski has suggested this possibility
are also pertinent here. (personal communication, June 26, 1963).
338 AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST

Research in psychotherapy. Vol. 2. Washington, D. C.: traditionally trained counselors-psychotherapists—their


American Psychological Association, 1962. Pp. 61-94. training, employment progress, and perceived employ-
LENNARD, H. L. Some aspects of the psychotherapeutic ability. Symposium presented at American Psychological
system. In H. H. Strupp & L. Luborsky (Eds.), Re- Association, Philadelphia, August 1963.
search in psychotherapy. Vol. 2. Washington, D. C.: ROBINSON, F. P. Principles and procedures in student
American Psychological Association, 1962. Pp. 218-236. counseling. New York: Harper, 1950.
LENNARD, H. L., ET AL. The anatomy of psychotherapy: ROBINSON, F. P. The dynamics of communication in
Systems of communication and expectation. New York: counseling. /. counsel. Psychol., 1955, 2, 163-169.
Columbia Univer. Press, 1960. ROGERS, C. R. A tentative scale for the measurement of
LEVINSON, D. J. The psychotherapist's contribution to process in psychotherapy. In E. A. Rubinstein &
the patient's treatment career. In H. H. Strupp & M. B. Parloff (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy. Vol. 1.
L. Luborsky (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy. Vol. 2. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association,
Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association, 1959. Pp. 96-107.
1962. Pp. 13-24. ROSENTHAL, D. Changes in some moral values following
MAOOON, T. M. Attitudes toward the employability of psychotherapy. J. consult. Psychol., 1955, 19, 431-433.
mental health counselors: Surveys of reactions among
ROSENTHAL, R. On the social psychology of the psy-
directors of family service agencies, university counseling
chological experiment. Amer. Scientist, 1963, 51, 268-283.
centers, community psychiatric clinics, and chairmen of
departments of psychiatry. In E. M. Bower (Chm.), ROTTER, J. B. Social learning and clinical psychology.
Nontraditionally trained counselors-psychotherapists— New York: Prentice-Hall, 1954.
their training, employment progress, and perceived ROTTER, J. B. Some implications of a social learning
employability. Symposium presented at American Psy- theory for the practice of psychotherapy. Unpublished
chological Association, Philadelphia, August 1963. manuscript, Ohio State University, 1962.
MATARAZZO, J. D. The interview. In B. B. Wolman SAMLER, J. Change in values: A goal in counseling.
(Ed.), Handbook oj clinical psychology. New York: J. counsel. Psychol., 1960, 7, 32-39.
McGraw-Hill, in press. SASLOW, G. Final summary. In H. H. Strupp & L. Lubor-
MERTON, R. K. Social theory and social structure. sky (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy. Vol. 2. Wash-
(Rev. ed.) Glencoe, III.: Free Press, 19S7. ington, D. C.: American Psychological Association, 1962,
MUTHARD, J. E. The relative effectiveness of larger units Pp. 299-307.
used in interview analysis. (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio SEEMAN, J., & RASKIN, N. J. Research perspectives in
State University) Ann Arbor, Mich.: University Micro- client-centered therapy. In O. H. Mowrer (Ed.),
films, 1958, No. 25,459. Psychotherapy theory and research. New York: Ronald
PEPINSKY, H. B. Counseling and psychotherapy as an Press, 1953. Pp. 205-234.
instance of coalition. In A. Jacobs, J. P. Jordaan, & SHESIF, M. The psychology of social norms. New York:
S. C. DiMichael (Eds.), Counseling in the rehabilitation
Harper, 1936.
process. New York: Teachers College, Columbia Uni-
versity, Bureau of Publications, 1961. Pp. 61-76. SHERIF, M., & CANTRIL, H. The psychology oj ego-involve-
PEPINSKY, H. B., & PEPINSKY, PAULINE N. Counseling; ments. New York: Wiley, 1947.
Theory and practice. New York: Ronald Press, 1954. STRUPP, H. H., & LXJBORSKY, L. (Eds.) Research in psy-
RIOCH, MARGARET. Selection, training, and evaluation chotherapy. Vol. 2. Washington, D. C.: American Psy-
during training. In E. M. Bower (Chm.), Non- chological Association, 1962.

You might also like