Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence - Wikipedia PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

Fluid and crystallized intelligence


In psychology, fluid and crystallized intelligence (respectively abbreviated Gf and Gc) are factors of general
intelligence, originally identified by Raymond Cattell.[1] Concepts of fluid and crystallized intelligence were further
developed by Cattell's student John L. Horn.

Fluid intelligence or fluid reasoning is the capacity to reason and solve novel problems, independent of any knowledge
from the past.[2] It is the ability to analyze novel problems, identify patterns and relationships that underpin these
problems and the extrapolation of these using logic. It is necessary for all logical problem solving. Fluid reasoning
includes inductive reasoning and deductive reasoning.

Crystallized intelligence is the ability to use skills, knowledge, and experience. It does not equate to memory, but it
does rely on accessing information from long-term memory. Crystallized intelligence is one's lifetime of intellectual
achievement, as demonstrated largely through one's vocabulary and general knowledge. This improves somewhat with
age, as experiences tend to expand one's knowledge. Crystallized intelligence is indicated by a person's depth and
breadth of general knowledge, vocabulary, and the ability to reason using words and numbers. It is the product of
educational and cultural experience in interaction with fluid intelligence.

Crystallized and fluid intelligence are believed to be separate neural and mental systems. Fluid and crystallized
intelligence are thus correlated with each other, and most IQ tests attempt to measure both varieties. For example, the
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) measures fluid intelligence on the performance scale and crystallized
intelligence on the verbal scale. The overall IQ score is based on a combination of these two scales.

Contents
History
Theoretical development
Fluid versus crystallized
Factor structure
Measurement of fluid intelligence
Development and physiology
Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory
See also
References

History
Fluid and crystallized intelligence were originally identified by Raymond Cattell.[3] Concepts of fluid and
crystallized intelligence were further developed by Cattell's student, John L. Horn. Since Cattell's and Horn's
publications, the concepts of fluid and crystallized intelligence have become so ingrained in the field of intelligence
that they are no longer routinely attributed to Cattell or Horn—much as Cattell's scree plot became ingrained in the
practice of factor analysis or Freud's concept of the subconscious is ingrained in psychology and in the public's
perceptions of the mind.

Theoretical development
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 1/7
5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

Fluid and crystallized intelligence are discrete factors of general intelligence, or g.[4] Although formally recognized by
Cattell, the distinction was foreshadowed by Charles Spearman who originally developed the theory of g and made a
similar observation regarding the difference between eductive and reproductive mental ability.[5]

According to Cattell, "...it is apparent that one of these powers… has the 'fluid' quality of being directable to almost any
problem. By contrast, the other is invested in particular areas of crystallized skills which can be upset individually
without affecting the others."[4] Thus, his claim was that each type, or factor, was independent of the other, though
many authors have noted an apparent interdependence of the two.[6]

Fluid versus crystallized


Fluid intelligence includes such abilities as pattern recognition, abstract reasoning, and problem-solving. Evidence is
consistent with the view that Gf is more affected by brain injury.[7][8]

Impaired performance on some tasks measuring fluid intelligence and enhanced performance on others are found on
some measures in individuals with autism spectrum disorders including Asperger syndrome.[9][10][11]

Crystallized intelligence is possibly more amenable to change as it relies on specific, acquired knowledge. When
learning new facts, someone's fund of knowledge is expanded. Vocabulary tests and the verbal subscale of the WAIS
are considered good measures of Gc. Crystallized intelligence relates to the study of aging. Belsky claims this declines
with age. In life, knowledge that is not used can be forgotten. Belsky believes that there is at least one age of maximum
crystallized intelligence; after which forgetting exceeds the rate at which knowledge is acquired.[12]

Not surprisingly, people with a high capacity of Gf tend to acquire more Gc knowledge and at faster rates. The process
of acquiring factual knowledge is sometimes called "cognitive investment."[13]

Some researchers have linked the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence to Piaget's conception of operative
intelligence and learning (see Piaget's theory of cognitive development#Pre-operational stage).[14][15] Fluid ability and
Piaget's operative intelligence both concern logical thinking and the eduction of relations. Crystallized ability and
Piaget's treatment of everyday learning reflect the impress of experience. Like fluid ability's relation to crystallized
intelligence, Piaget's operativity is considered to be prior to, and ultimately provides the foundation for, everyday
learning.

Factor structure
Fluid intelligence generally correlates with measures of abstract reasoning and puzzle solving. Crystallized intelligence
correlates with abilities that depend on knowledge and experience, such as vocabulary, general information, and
analogies. Paul Kline identified a number of factors that shared a correlation of at least r=.60 with Gf and Gc.[16]
Factors with median loadings of greater than 0.6 on Gf included induction, visualization, quantitative reasoning, and
ideational fluency. Factors with median loadings of greater than 0.6 on Gc included verbal ability, language
development, reading comprehension, sequential reasoning, and general information. It may be suggested that tests of
intelligence may not be able to truly reflect levels of fluid intelligence. Some authors have suggested that unless an
individual was truly interested in the problem presented, the cognitive work required may not be performed because
of a lack of interest.[17] These authors contend that a low score on tests which are intended to measure fluid
intelligence may reflect more a lack of interest in the tasks rather than inability to complete the tasks successfully.

Measurement of fluid intelligence


There are various measures that assess fluid intelligence. The Cattell Culture Fair IQ test, the Raven Progressive
Matrices (RPM), and the performance subscale of the WAIS are measures of Gf. The RPM[18] is one of the most
commonly used measures of fluid abilities. It is a non-verbal multiple choice test. Participants have to complete a

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 2/7
5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

series of drawings by identifying relevant features based on the spatial organization of an array of objects, and
choosing one object that matches one or more of the identified features.[19] This task assesses the ability to consider
one or more relationships between mental representations or relational reasoning. Propositional analogies and
semantic decision tasks are also used to assess relational reasoning.[20][21]

Standardized IQ tests such as those used in psychoeducational assessment also include tests of fluid intelligence. In
the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities,[22] Gf is assessed by two tests: Concept Formation (Test 5) in the
Standard Battery and Analysis Synthesis (Test 15) in the Extended Battery. On Concept Formation tasks, the
individual has to apply concepts by inferring the underlying "rules" for solving visual puzzles that are presented in
increasing levels of difficulty. Individuals at the preschool level have to point to a shape that is different from others in
a set. As the level of difficulty increases, individuals increasingly demonstrate an understanding of what constitutes a
key difference (or the "rule") for solving puzzles involving one to one comparisons, and on later items identifying
common differences among a set of items. For more difficult items, individuals need to understand the concept of
"and" (e.g. solution must have some of this and some of that) and the concept of "or" (e.g. to be inside a box, the item
must be either this or that). The most difficult items require fluid transformations and cognitive shifting between the
various types of concept puzzles that the examinee has worked with previously.[23]

Concept Formation tasks assess inductive reasoning ability. In the Analysis-Synthesis test, the individual has to learn
and orally state the solutions to incomplete logic puzzles that mimic a miniature mathematics system. The test also
contains some of the features involved in using symbolic formulations in other fields such as chemistry and logic. The
individual is presented with a set of logic rules, a "key" that is used to solve the puzzles. The individual has to
determine the missing colors within each of the puzzles using the key. Complex items present puzzles that require two
or more sequential mental manipulations of the key to derive a final solution. Increasingly difficult items involve a mix
of puzzles that require fluid shifts in deduction, logic, and inference.[23] Analysis Synthesis tasks assess general
sequential reasoning.

In the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (WISC IV),[24] the Perceptual Reasoning Index contains two
subtests that assess Gf: Matrix Reasoning, which involves induction and deduction, and Picture Concepts, which
involves induction.[25] In the Picture Concepts task, children are presented a series of pictures on two or three rows
and asked which pictures (one from each row) belong together based on some common characteristic. This task
assesses the child's ability to discover the underlying characteristic (e.g. rule, concept, trend, class membership) that
governs a set of materials. Matrix Reasoning also tests this ability as well as the ability to start with stated rules,
premises, or conditions and to engage in one or more steps to reach a solution to a novel problem (deduction). In the
Matrix Reasoning test, children are presented a series or sequence of pictures with one picture missing. Their task is to
choose the picture that fits the series or sequence from an array of five options. Since Matrix Reasoning and Picture
Concepts involve the use of visual stimuli and do not require expressive language, they are considered to be non-verbal
tests of Gf.[25]

Within the corporate environment, fluid intelligence is a predictor of a person's capacity to work well in environments
characterised by complexity, uncertainty, and ambiguity. The Cognitive Process Profile (CPP) measures a person's
fluid intelligence and cognitive processes. It maps these against suitable work environments according to Elliott
Jacques Stratified Systems Theory.

Development and physiology


Fluid intelligence, like reaction time, typically peaks in young adulthood and then steadily declines. This decline may
be related to local atrophy of the brain in the right cerebellum.[26] Other researchers have suggested that a lack of
practice, along with age-related changes in the brain may contribute to the decline.[6] Crystallized intelligence typically
increases gradually, stays relatively stable across most of adulthood, and then begins to decline after age 65.[6] The

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 3/7
5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

exact peak age of cognitive skills remains elusive, depending on the skill measurement as well as on the survey design.
Cross-sectional data shows typically an earlier onset of cognitive decline in comparison with longitudinal data. The
former may be confounded due to cohort effects while the latter may be biased due to prior test experiences.[27]

Working memory capacity is closely related to fluid intelligence, and has been proposed to account for individual
differences in Gf.[28]

Improving fluid intelligence with training on working


memory
According to David Geary, Gf and Gc can be traced to two separate brain systems. Fluid intelligence involves both the
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, and other systems related to attention and short-term
memory. Crystallized intelligence appears to be a function of brain regions that involve the storage and usage of long-
term memories, such as the hippocampus.[29]

Some researchers question whether the results of training are long lasting and transferable, especially when these
techniques are used by healthy children and adults without cognitive deficiencies.[30] A recent meta-analytical review
conducted by researchers from the University of Oslo concluded that "memory training programs appear to produce
short-term, specific training effects that do not generalize."[31]

In a study using four individual experiments, 70 participants (36 of them female, all with a mean age of 25.6) recruited
from the University of Bern community, Susanne M. Jaeggi and her colleagues at the University of Michigan found
that healthy young adults who practiced a demanding working memory task (dual n-back, a task that has strong face
validity, has received some criticism regarding its construct validity and is in widespread use as a measure of working
memory) approximately 25 minutes per day for between 8 and 19 days had statistically significant increases in their
scores on a matrix test of fluid intelligence taken before and after the training than a control group who did not do any
training at all.[32]

A further examination of these findings was published in 2008 in the Proceedings of the Nation Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America. Summarizing findings in the study as evidence that demonstrates that "fluid
intelligence is trainable to a significant and meaningful degree."

Attention is drawn to the limitations of these results and the need for specific follow up inquiery. Robert J. Sternberg
comments that"it is unclear to what extent the results can be generalized to other working-memory tasks" and states
"it would be useful to show that the training transfers to success in meaningful behaviours that extend beyond the
realm of psychometric testing". Sternberg asserts that ability level of the test participants is not necessarily examining
a wide range of ability levels, or "address whether the training is durable over extended periods of time [and not only]
"fleeting[33]."

A second study conducted at the University of Technology in Hangzhou, China, supports Jaeggi's results
independently. After student subjects were given a 10-day training regimen based on the dual n-back working memory
theory, the students were tested on Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. Their scores were found to have increased
significantly.[34]

Subsequent studies on n-back, namely by Chooi & Thompson[35] and Redick et al.,[36] do not support the findings of
the Jaeggi study. Although participants' performance on the training task improved, these studies showed no
significant improvement in the mental abilities tested, especially fluid intelligence and working memory capacity.

See also
21st century skills
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 4/7
5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

CHC theory
Deeper learning
General intelligence factor
Intelligence
Three stratum theory
Malleability of intelligence
Nootropic
Outline of human intelligence
Raymond Cattell
Spatial intelligence (psychology)

References
1. Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. New York: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 0-395-04275-
5.
2. Jaeggi, Susanne M.; Buschkuehl, Martin; Jonides, John; Perrig, Walter J. (2008-05-13). "Improving fluid
intelligence with training on working memory" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2383929).
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105 (19): 6829–6833.
Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.6829J (http://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2008PNAS..105.6829J).
doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105 (https://doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0801268105). ISSN 0027-8424 (https://www.worl
dcat.org/issn/0027-8424). PMC 2383929 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2383929).
PMID 18443283 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18443283).
3. Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. New York: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 978-0-395-
04275-5.
4. Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action (https://books.google.com/books?id=flX770mG
2HcC&printsec=frontcover#v=onepage&q&f=false). New York: Elsevier Science. ISBN 9780080866895.
5. Spearman, Charles B. (2005). The Abilities of Man: Their Nature and Measurement. The Blackburn Press.
ISBN 978-1-932846-10-2.
6. Cavanaugh, J. C.; Blanchard-Fields, F (2006). Adult development and aging (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth
Publishing/Thomson Learning. ISBN 978-0-534-52066-3.
7. Cattell, Raymond B. (1963). "Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment". Journal of
Educational Psychology. 54: 1–22. doi:10.1037/h0046743 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fh0046743).
8. Suchy, Yana; Eastvold, Angela; Whittaker, Wilson J.; Strassberg, Donald (2007). "Validation of the Behavioral
Dyscontrol Scale-Electronic Version: Sensitivity to subtle sequelae of mild traumatic brain injury". Brain Injury. 21
(1): 69–80. doi:10.1080/02699050601149088 (https://doi.org/10.1080%2F02699050601149088). PMID 17364522
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17364522).
9. Hayashi, Mika; Kato, Motoichiro; Igarashi, Kazue; Kashima, Haruo (2008). "Superior fluid intelligence in children
with Asperger's disorder". Brain and Cognition. 66 (3): 306–10. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.09.008 (https://doi.org/1
0.1016%2Fj.bandc.2007.09.008). PMID 17980944 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17980944).
10. Soulières, Isabelle; Dawson, Michelle; Gernsbacher, Morton Ann; Mottron, Laurent (2011). Skoulakis, Efthimios
M. C (ed.). "The Level and Nature of Autistic Intelligence II: What about Asperger Syndrome?" (https://www.ncbi.n
lm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3182210). PLoS ONE. 6 (9): e25372. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...625372S (http://adsabs.
harvard.edu/abs/2011PLoSO...625372S). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025372 (https://doi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.p
one.0025372). PMC 3182210 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3182210). PMID 21991394 (https://
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21991394).
11. Dawson, M.; Soulieres, I.; Ann Gernsbacher, M.; Mottron, L. (2007). "The Level and Nature of Autistic
Intelligence" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4287210). Psychological Science. 18 (8): 657–62.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01954.x (https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-9280.2007.01954.x). PMC 4287210 (htt
ps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4287210). PMID 17680932 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17
680932).
12. Belsky, Janet (1999). The Psychology of Aging: Theory, Research, and Interventions. Pacific, CA: Brooks/Cole
Publishing.

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 5/7
5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

13. Ackerman, Phillip L. (1996). "A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and
knowledge". Intelligence. 22 (2): 227–57. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90016-1 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0160
-2896%2896%2990016-1).
14. Papalia, D.; Fitzgerald, J.; Hooper, F. H. (1971). "Piagetian Theory and the Aging Process: Extensions and
Speculations". The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2: 3–20. doi:10.2190/AG.2.1.b (http
s://doi.org/10.2190%2FAG.2.1.b).
15. Schonfeld, Irvin S. (1986). "The Genevan and Cattell-Horn conceptions of intelligence compared: Early
implementation of numerical solution aids". Developmental Psychology. 22 (2): 204–12. doi:10.1037/0012-
1649.22.2.204 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.22.2.204).
16. Kline, P. (1998). The new psychometrics: Science, psychology and measurement. London: Routledge.
17. Messick, Samuel (1989). "Meaning and Values in Test Validation: The Science and Ethics of Assessment".
Educational Researcher. 18 (2): 5–11. doi:10.3102/0013189X018002005 (https://doi.org/10.3102%2F0013189X0
18002005). JSTOR 1175249 (https://www.jstor.org/stable/1175249).
18. Raven, J.; Raven, J. C.; Court, J. H. (2003) [1998]. "Section 1: General Overview". Manual for Raven's
Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
19. Bornstein, Joel C.; Foong, Jaime Pei Pei (2009). "MGluR1 Receptors Contribute to Non-Purinergic Slow
Excitatory Transmission to Submucosal VIP Neurons of Guinea-Pig Ileum" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/arti
cles/PMC2695390). Frontiers in Neuroscience. 3: 46. doi:10.3389/neuro.21.001.2009 (https://doi.org/10.3389%2F
neuro.21.001.2009). PMC 2695390 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2695390). PMID 20582273 (h
ttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20582273).
20. Wright, Samantha B.; Matlen, Bryan J.; Baym, Carol L.; Ferrer, Emilio; Bunge, Silvia A. (2007). "Neural correlates
of fluid reasoning in children and adults" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2525981). Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience. 1: 8. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.008.2007 (https://doi.org/10.3389%2Fneuro.09.008.2007).
PMC 2525981 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2525981). PMID 18958222 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.n
ih.gov/pubmed/18958222).
21. Ferrer, Emilio; O'Hare, Elizabeth D.; Bunge, Silvia A. (2009). "Fluid reasoning and the developing brain" (https://w
ww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2858618). Frontiers in Neuroscience. 3 (1): 46–51.
doi:10.3389/neuro.01.003.2009 (https://doi.org/10.3389%2Fneuro.01.003.2009). PMC 2858618 (https://www.ncbi.
nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2858618). PMID 19753096 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19753096).
22. Woodcock, R. W.; McGrew, K. S.; Mather, N (2001). Woodcock Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
23. Schrank, F. A.; Flanagan, D. P. (2003). WJ III Clinical use and interpretation. Scientist-practitioner perspectives.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
24. Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
25. Flanagan, D. P.; Kaufman, A. S. (2004). Essentials of WISC-IV assessment. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
26. Lee, Jun-Young; Lyoo, In Kyoon; Kim, Seon-Uk; Jang, Hong-Suk; Lee, Dong-Woo; Jeon, Hong-Jin; Park, Sang-
Chul; Cho, Maeng Je (2005). "Intellect declines in healthy elderly subjects and cerebellum". Psychiatry and
Clinical Neurosciences. 59 (1): 45–51. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01330.x (https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1440-1
819.2005.01330.x). hdl:10371/27902 (https://hdl.handle.net/10371%2F27902). PMID 15679539 (https://www.ncb
i.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15679539).
27. Desjardins, Richard; Warnke, Arne Jonas (2012). "Ageing and Skills". OECD Education Working Papers.
doi:10.1787/5k9csvw87ckh-en (https://doi.org/10.1787%2F5k9csvw87ckh-en). hdl:10419/57089 (https://hdl.handl
e.net/10419%2F57089).
28. Kyllonen, Patrick C.; Christal, Raymond E. (1990). "Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory
capacity?!". Intelligence. 14 (4): 389–433. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS016
0-2896%2805%2980012-1).
29. Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence (http://www.apa.org/
pubs/books/4318015.aspx). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
30. Todd W. Thompson; et al. (2013). "Failure of Working Memory Training to Enhance Cognition or Intelligence" (http
s://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3661602). PLoS ONE. 8 (5): e63614.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063614 (https://doi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0063614). PMC 3661602 (https://ww
w.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3661602). PMID 23717453 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2371745
3).

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 6/7
5/7/2019 Fluid and crystallized intelligence - Wikipedia

31. Melby-Lervåg, Monica; Hulme, Charles (2012). "Is Working Memory Training Effective? A Meta-Analytic Review".
Developmental Psychology. 49 (2): 270–91. doi:10.1037/a0028228 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fa0028228).
PMID 22612437 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22612437).
32. Jaeggi, Susanne M.; Buschkuehl, Martin; Jonides, John; Perrig, Walter J. (2008). "Improving fluid intelligence with
training on working memory" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2383929). Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences. 105 (19): 6829–33. Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.6829J (http://adsabs.harvard.edu/ab
s/2008PNAS..105.6829J). doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105 (https://doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0801268105).
JSTOR 25461885 (https://www.jstor.org/stable/25461885). PMC 2383929 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articl
es/PMC2383929). PMID 18443283 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18443283).
33. Sternberg, R. J. (2008). "Increasing fluid intelligence is possible after all" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/article
s/PMC2383939). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105 (19):
6791–6792. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803396105 (https://doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0803396105). PMC 2383939 (http
s://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2383939). PMID 18474863 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/184
74863).
34. Qiu, Feiyue; Wei, Qinqin; Zhao, Liying; Lin, Lifang (2009). "Study on Improving Fluid Intelligence through
Cognitive Training System Based on Gabor Stimulus" (http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1727465). 2009 First
International Conference on Information Science and Engineering. pp. 3459–62. doi:10.1109/ICISE.2009.1124 (ht
tps://doi.org/10.1109%2FICISE.2009.1124). ISBN 978-1-4244-4909-5.
35. Chooi, Weng-Tink; Thompson, Lee A. (2012). "Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy
young adults". Intelligence. 40 (6): 531–42. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.intell.201
2.07.004).
36. Redick, Thomas S.; Shipstead, Zach; Harrison, Tyler L.; Hicks, Kenny L.; Fried, David E.; Hambrick, David Z.;
Kane, Michael J.; Engle, Randall W. (2012). "No Evidence of Intelligence Improvement After Working Memory
Training: A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Study". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 142 (2): 359–
379. doi:10.1037/a0029082 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fa0029082). PMID 22708717 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.go
v/pubmed/22708717).

Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence&oldid=894839687"

This page was last edited on 30 April 2019, at 10:50 (UTC).

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. By using
this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia
Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fluid_and_crystallized_intelligence 7/7

You might also like