Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

SPECIAL ARTICLE

The enigma of facial beauty: Esthetics,


proportions, deformity, and controversy
Farhad B. Naini,a James P. Moss,b and Daljit S. Gillc
London, United Kingdom

The study of the face and the ability to alter its form have fascinated mankind for thousands of years. The
clinical ability to alter dentofacial form, whether through orthodontics, facial growth modification, or surgery,
requires an understanding of facial beauty, including the evaluation of facial esthetics, proportions, and
symmetry. The purposes of this article were to give a brief contemporary overview of our concepts of facial
beauty and esthetics and to consider a long-standing and controversial debate on the treatment of patients
with dentofacial deformities. (Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2006;130:277-82)

O
rthodontists have a special interest in facial Wolfe Hungerford (1878) famously said, “Beauty is in
beauty, although, over time, the relative im- the eye of the beholder.”5
portance of esthetics has waxed and waned in Both of these quotations, and their respective phil-
relation to other considerations. In part of his ongoing osophical ideologies, assume that the “sense” is sub-
review of orthodontic history, Wahl1 wrote, “Now it jective to each person. However, the 18th century
appears that facial esthetics is again in the forefront as philosopher Francis Hutcheson said, “Esthetic judge-
we realize why patients come to us in the first place.”1 ments are perceptual and take their authority from a
Perceptions of facial beauty are multifactorial, with sense that is common to all who make them,” and “The
genetic, environmental, and cultural foundations.2 Beauty origin of our perceptions of beauty and harmony is
can be defined as a combination of qualities that give justly called a ‘sense’ because it involves no intellectual
pleasure to the senses or to the mind. Esthetics is the element, no reflection on principles and causes.”6 The
study of beauty and, to a lesser extent, its opposite, the philosopher Immanuel Kant (1790) reiterated this view,
ugly. The 18th century philosopher Alexander Baum- saying that, “The beautiful is that which pleases uni-
garten, who established esthetics as a distinct field of versally without a concept.”7
philosophy, coined the term, which is derived from the Therefore, if a beautiful face “pleases universally,”
Greek word for sensory perception (aisthesis).3 then some part of our sense perception must be com-
The first question to consider is difficult to answer: mon to all men and women. After all, when we describe
is the origin of the human perception of facial beauty a face as beautiful, we do not merely mean that it
dependent on each person’s sense perception, or is this pleases us. We are describing the face, not our judg-
sense common to all people? Is facial beauty a quality ment. We will often point to features of the face to back
of the observed face, or does the sensory enjoyment of up our statement.
the observers also depend on their own ideas, feelings, A paradox therefore emerges. Obviously one can-
and judgments, which themselves have a direct rela- not make a judgment about the beauty of a face one has
tionship to sensory enjoyment. The philosopher David never seen. Therefore, facial beauty is related to some
Hume (1741) said, “Beauty in things exists in the mind quality of the observed face that tends to be universally
which contemplates them,”4 and the writer Margaret accepted. However, each person’s ideas and feelings,
a
Consultant, Director of Research and Graduate Education, Orthodontic De-
like a conditioned response, also have a direct relation-
partment, St George’s Hospital and Medical School, Kingston Hospital, ship to his or her judgment, hence the difference in the
London, United Kingdom. extent of rating a face as beautiful depending on the
b
Professor, Orthodontic Department, Royal London Hospital, London, United
Kingdom. “eye of the beholder.”
c
Consultant, Orthodontic Department, Eastman Dental Hospital, London, This philosophical question and its related argu-
United Kingdom. ment continue to be debated. However, beauty un-
Reprint requests to: Dr Farhad B. Naini, Consultant, Orthodontic Department,
St George’s Hospital and Medical School, Blackshaw Rd, London, SW17 0QT, doubtedly has a strong influence on human life. Ac-
United Kingdom; e-mail, farhad@naini.freeserve.co.uk. cording to Shakespeare, “Beauty itself doth of itself
Submitted, April 2005; revised and accepted, September 2005. persuade the eyes of men without an orator.”8
0889-5406/$32.00
Copyright © 2006 by the American Association of Orthodontists. The philosopher Pascal commented, “Had Cleo-
doi:10.1016/j.ajodo.2005.09.027 patra’s nose been shorter, the whole face of the world
277
278 Naini, Moss, and Gill American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
September 2006

would have changed!”9 From Homer’s Helen of Troy, said, “I know not what beauty is, but I know that it
whom the poet Christopher Marlowe described as having affects many things in life.” Dürer explained that,
a “face that launched a thousand ships,”10 to Queen although the concept of facial beauty was immersed in
Nefertiti, whose name literally means the “Perfect One,” subjectivity, the assessment of facial proportions could
to modern models and actors, facial beauty has always be undertaken objectively.22 He maintained that dispro-
been the most valued aspect of human beauty. portionate human faces were unesthetic, whereas propor-
The next question therefore is how do we know that tionate features were acceptable if not always beautiful.
a face is beautiful? What guides and validates our Therefore, the appropriate goal for the orthodontist’s
judgment? clinical examination is to find facial disproportions.23 The
Our perception of facial beauty might have its inevitable question is, therefore, where did the evidence
foundation in our heredity, environment, or perhaps for ideal facial proportions originate?
both. Langlois et al11 found that infants as young as 3 In representing the human form, painters and sculp-
months of age can distinguish between attractive and tors in the past developed many canons or guidelines.
unattractive faces, showing signs of preference for the These were mainly based on the “good eye” of the
former. It is unlikely that by 3 months of age an infant respective artist, which is anecdotal evidence. How-
will have been subjected to or responded to any cultural ever, many of these guidelines are still used by clini-
or environmental influences; therefore, this is evidence cians today, albeit somewhat modified from the origi-
to support a genetic theory. The evolutionary basis is nals.
that facial beauty, including facial symmetry and sec- A major preoccupation of Greek sculptors was the
ondary sexual characteristics, is a requirement for idealization of human proportions. In the fifth century
sexual selection, leading to reproduction.12 BC, one of the most famous, Polycleitus, wrote a
A study by Martin13 found that both white and theoretical work that discussed ideal mathematical
black American men preferred black female faces with proportions for the parts of the human body. In his
white features, whereas black African men showed a Canon, he also described the importance of the concept
preference for black female faces with Negroid fea- of symmetry in the human form, called “symmetria.” In
tures. This lends support to environmental or cultural the first century BC, the Roman architect Marcus
reasons for the human perception of facial beauty. Vitruvius Pollio wrote his famous work, The Ten Books
However, Perrett et al14 found that both white and on Architecture. In Book 2, Chapter 2, Vitruvius
Japanese men and women ranked female faces as most defined symmetry as “a proper agreement between the
attractive when youthful facial features, such as large members of the work itself, and relation between the
eyes, high cheekbones, and narrow jaws, were evident. different parts and the whole general scheme, in accor-
Esthetic judgments therefore seem to be similar across dance with a certain part selected as standard. Thus in
various cultural backgrounds. A meta-analysis by Lan- the human body there is a kind of symmetrical harmony
glois et al15 seemed to confirm that there is cross- . . . and so it is with perfect buildings.”24 Although the
cultural agreement about facial attractiveness. relationship between bilateral facial symmetry and
Studies by Sir Francis Galton,16 Charles Darwin’s beauty remains to be clarified,25 evidence seems to
cousin, provide evidence to support averageness as the suggest that mild facial asymmetry is compatible with
ideal,17 with composite facial photographs of subjects facial beauty.26 Vitruvius also described the facial
gaining higher attractiveness ratings than individual trisection, emphasizing that the ideal face can be
facial photographs.18 However, Perrett et al14 showed divided vertically into 3 distinct thirds, a concept still
that, contrary to this averageness hypothesis, the mean used today in planning facial surgery.
shape of a group of attractive faces is preferred to the The great Renaissance artist and thinker Leonardo
mean shape of the sample from which the faces were da Vinci emphasized the importance of harmony be-
selected. In addition, attractive composite faces were tween art and science. Leonardo defined proportion as
made more attractive by exaggerating the shape differ- the ratio between the respective parts and the whole.27
ences from the sample mean. Therefore, an average The figure of Vitruvian man (Fig 1), which Leonardo
face shape is attractive but might not be optimally based on guidelines described by Vitruvius, shows the
attractive,19 and highly attractive facial configurations importance of proportions in the human form. He also
are not necessarily average.20 studied the proportions of the human head (Fig 2). The
distance from the hairline to the inferior aspect of the
FACIAL PROPORTIONS chin is one-tenth of a man’s height. The distance from
The assessment of facial beauty is essentially sub- the top of the head to the inferior aspect of the chin is
jective.21 In the 16th century, the artist Albrecht Dürer one-eighth of a man’s height. These have important
American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics Naini, Moss, and Gill 279
Volume 130, Number 3

Fig 2. Leonardo da Vinci’s Male head in profile with


proportions, ca 1490. Vitruvian anterior vertical facial
thirds are evident: hairline to eyebrows, eyebrows to
base of nose, base of nose to below chin. Lower facial
Fig 1. Leonardo da Vinci’s Vitruvian man, ca 1490. This third is again divided into upper third (upper lip) and
famous figure shows that proportionate human form fits lower two-thirds. Ear is one third of facial height. What
perfectly in perfect geometric shapes— circle and square, later came to be described as Frankfort plane and its
with navel at center. Vertical facial trisection is shown. perpendicular, facial vertical from soft-tissue nasion,
Vertical face height (hairline to inferior aspect of chin) is are also shown (courtesy of Gallerie dell’Accademia,
one tenth of standing height. Interestingly, this is equal Venice).
to length of hand (courtesy of Gallerie dell’Accademia,
Venice).
orthodontic specialty, however, has been at the fore-
front in the assessment of the facial soft-tissue profile,28
clinical implications. If a patient’s vertical facial pro- mainly because of the use of lateral cephalometric
portions are to be altered with surgery, the treatment radiographs that provide many analyses.29-35
plan must take into account the proportion of the Clinicians therefore can assess facial esthetics more
patient’s total face height to his or her standing height objectively by diagnosing and helping to correct facial
and stature. The use of absolute numeric values of disproportions.
facial measurements rather than facial proportions can Since the establishment of our specialty over 100
be misleading, because the vertical facial height of a years ago, orthodontic theory and practice have been
patient who is 6 feet tall is different from that of a based on the Angle paradigm.36 The goal of treatment
patient 5 feet tall. was to produce perfect occlusion of all the teeth, and
Dürer, in 1525, maintained the importance of study- facial beauty was thought to follow. Although this
ing facial proportions, criticizing artists of his day, and concept was discredited with the introduction and
saying, “They have not learnt Geometry, without which growth of cephalometrics, the basic idea that the
no one can either be or become an absolute artist.”22 dentofacial skeleton determined the goals of treatment
Therefore, the guidelines used by clinicians today are remained intact.37
mainly based on those initially described in art. The It is only recently that the concept of the soft-tissue
280 Naini, Moss, and Gill American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
September 2006

paradigm, focusing the diagnosis and treatment of proportion.44 Therefore, more research evidence is
dentofacial problems on the soft tissues of the face required to substantiate the true significance of this
rather than on dentoskeletal structures, has emerged in fascinating concept in the clinical assessment of facial
orthodontics and orthognathic surgery.23 The den- esthetics.
toskeletal structures of the face are like the scaffold
over which the soft tissues drape. However, it is the FACIAL DEFORMITY AND THE
soft-tissue proportions, not the skeletal proportions, CONTROVERSIAL DEBATE
that are the goals of treatment.38 Proffit et al23 have led The final question concerns patients with facial
the way in the emergence of this paradigm shift, anomalies of varying magnitudes and whether they
placing greater emphasis on the clinical examination of should be treated, especially when the deformity is not
the patient and our assessment of the soft-tissue part of an active disease process.
changes that occur with each dentoskeletal change and A person’s self-perception of facial appearance is of
with age, thus allowing for greater accuracy in treat- utmost importance. It has been said, “Nothing has so
ment planning. marked influence on the direction of a man’s mind as
his appearance, and not his appearance itself so much
FACIAL PROPORTIONS AND THE as his conviction that it is attractive or unattractive.”45
GOLDEN PROPORTION There is, of course, considerable individual variation in
An often-quoted but rarely substantiated concept is people’s abilities to adapt to their facial deformities,
that of the “golden proportion.”39 The mathematician whatever the severity, with some remaining compara-
Euclid (ca 325-265 BC) described this in The Elements, tively unaffected and others having significant difficul-
his treatise on mathematics. The origin of this concept ties that affect their quality of life. It has even been
is unknown, having been attributed to both Pythagoras argued that facial deformity might be a social disability,
and Plato. In his edition of Euclid’s Elements, the because it impacts not only the person affected, but also
mathematician Luca Pacioli (1509) renamed the golden contributes to the opinions other people form of them.46
proportion the “divine proportion” because he thought Although these opinions can change as interpersonal
the concept could not be rationalized. Pacioli also relationships develop, a person’s first impression on
published a treatise entitled De Divina Proportione (On others might well affect his or her self-esteem and
Divine Proportion) for which Leonardo da Vinci drew quality of life.47
figures of symmetrical and proportionate faces and Research seems to indicate that mild to moderate
bodies. Later in that century (1597), the first known facial deformities actually cause a patient greater psy-
calculation of the golden proportion as a decimal was chological distress than severe facial deformities.48
given by Maestlin in a letter to his former pupil, This could be because other people’s reactions towards
Kepler.40 The number is 0.618 for the length of the milder deformities are less predictable, whereas more
longer segment of a line of length 1 when it is divided severe deformities tend to evoke more consistent reac-
in the golden proportion. The ratio of the shorter tions, albeit negative, allowing the patient to develop
section to the longer section of the line is equal to the better coping strategies. The variability in people’s
ratio of the longer section to the whole line. The point reactions to milder facial deformities also causes con-
at which the line is divided is known as the golden siderable patient distress. Most patients seeking orth-
section and is represented by the symbol ⌽ (phi) odontic treatment or facial esthetic surgery fit into the
derived from the name of the Greek sculptor Phidias mild or moderate category in terms of facial deformi-
who incorporated it into the architectural design of the ties, as opposed to craniofacial malformation syn-
Parthenon.41 dromes or severe facial trauma or disease.
There have been attempts to correlate ideal facial There is a long-standing and controversial debate
proportions with the golden proportion.42 However, in about the ability to alter facial appearance. The debate
a 3-dimensional study analyzing the faces of profes- centers on whether a patient with a facial deformity
sional models, the authors found that they did not fit the should learn to live with his or her facial appearance.
golden proportion and, interestingly, that they had This argument states that a psychologically healthy
various malocclusions and a wide range of cephalomet- patient should be able to adjust to the social environ-
ric values.43 In another study of the esthetic improvement ment. The supporters of this view also state that
of patients having orthognathic surgery, the authors found education of the public and changes in public attitudes
that, whereas most subjects were considered more attrac- toward visible facial differences are the correct way
tive after treatment than before, the proportions were forward. The argument against this case states that the
equally likely to move away from or toward the golden negative effects of facial deformity on a patient’s
American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics Naini, Moss, and Gill 281
Volume 130, Number 3

psychosocial well being are enough to warrant clinical REFERENCES


intervention. The 2 sides seem unable to reach common 1. Wahl N. Orthodontics in 3 millenia. Chapter 1: antiquity to the
ground. mid-19th century. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2005;127:
We believe that there is merit in both sides of the 255-9.
2. Naini FB, Moss JP. Three-dimensional assessment of the relative
argument, and that they are not mutually exclusive. contribution of genetics and environment to various facial
There is no doubt that attitudes in society toward all parameters with the twin method. Am J Orthod Dentofacial
types of deformity have changed, and public education Orthop 2004;126:655-65.
is undoubtedly the most important factor, with charities 3. Baumgarten AG. Aesthetica. Paris: L’herne; 1989.
4. Hume D. ‘Of Tragedy’ in essays, literary, moral and political.
such as Changing Faces49 helping to both educate the London: Oxford University Press; 1963.
public and change public opinion regarding facial 5. Hungerford MW. Molly Bawn. London: Smith, Elder & Co;
deformities. Modern society’s media-fuelled obsession 1890.
with the so-called perfect appearance is at best unhelp- 6. Hutcheson F. An inquiry into the original of our ideas of beauty
and virtue. Indianapolis: Liberty Fund; 2004.
ful and at worst detrimental. People compare them-
7. Kant I. The critique of judgement. London: Oxford University
selves to those who are unrealistically portrayed by the Press; 1973.
media to represent cultural ideals of beauty, and, if their 8. Shakespeare W. The rape of Lucrece. London: New Penguin
own appearances do not measure up, this might result Shakespeare; 1971.
in body-image dissatisfaction, possibly leading to al- 9. Pascal B. Pensees. London: Penguin Classics; 1966.
10. Marlowe C. Doctor Faustus. New York: Dover Publications;
tered self-recognition and conditions such as eating 1995.
disorders and body dysmorphic disorder.50 Therefore, 11. Langlois JH, Roggman LA, Casey RJ, Ritter JM, Rieser-Danner
although it is unrealistic to expect the media’s obses- LA, Jenkins VY. Infant preferences for attractive faces: rudi-
sion with appearance to significantly change, greater ments of a stereotype? Dev Psychol 1987;23:363-9.
12. Jones S. Almost like a whale: the origin of species updated.
responsibility on the media’s part would be com-
London: Doubleday; 1999.
mended. In addition, television documentaries, perhaps 13. Martin JG. Racial ethnocentrism and judgment of beauty. J Soc
interviewing patients and families of patients with Psychol 1964;63:59-63.
dentofacial anomalies, and following patients through 14. Perrett DI, May KA, Yoshikawa S. Facial shape and judgements
of female attractiveness. Nature 1994;368:239-42.
their daily lives as they describe their challenges, would
15. Langlois JH, Kalanakis LE, Rubenstein AJ, Larson AD, Hallam
undoubtedly be useful for educating the general public. MJ, Smoot MT. Maxims or myths of beauty: a meta-analytic and
Such education might help lessen society’s intolerance theoretical overview. Psychol Bull 2000;126:390-423.
for what is considered an abnormal facial appearance 16. Galton F. Composite portraits, made by combining those of
and contribute to transforming the public’s perception many different persons in a single resultant figure. J Anthropol
Inst Gr Brit Ir 1879;8:132-44.
of those with facial deformities. 17. Edler RJ. Background considerations to facial aesthetics.
However, it would be simplistic and possibly un- J Orthod 2001;28:159-68.
ethical to withhold treatment on the basis that the 18. Langlois JH, Roggman LA. Attractive faces are only average.
problem is with attitudes in society alone. If treatment Psychol Sci 1990;1:115-21.
19. Arvystas M. Orthodontic management of agenesis and other
can significantly improve a patient’s quality of life,
complexities: an interdisciplinary approach to functional esthet-
then it is for the patient to make the ultimate informed ics. New York: Martin Dunitz Ltd; 2003.
decision of whether treatment is the correct way for- 20. Alley TR, Cunningham MR. Averaged faces are attractive, but
ward. very attractive faces are not average. Psychol Sci 1991;2:123-5.
21. Peck H, Peck S. A concept of facial esthetics. Angle Orthod
1970;40:284-317.
CONCLUSIONS 22. Dürer A. The art of measurement. San Francisco: Alan Wofsy
Fine Arts; 1981.
The aim of this article was to briefly describe our 23. Proffit WR, White RP, Sarver DM. Contemporary treatment of
contemporary understanding of facial beauty and to dentofacial deformity. St Louis: Mosby; 2003.
24. Howe TN. Vitruvius: the ten books on architecture. London:
debate the challenges faced by patients and clinicians in
Cambridge University Press; 1999.
the treatment of dentofacial deformities. There is no 25. Rhodes G, Sumich A, Byatt G. Are average facial configurations
doubt that the philosophical debate and the research attractive only because of their symmetry? Psychol Sci 1999;10:
will continue, because our clinical practice should 52-8.
always be based on a sound knowledge of theory. One 26. Zaidel DW, Cohen JA. The face, beauty, and symmetry: per-
ceiving asymmetry in beautiful faces. Int J Neurosci 2005;115:
may only conclude that patients requiring alterations in 1165-73.
facial appearance remain a considerable clinical chal- 27. Pedretti C. Leonardo da Vinci: notebook of a genius. Milan:
lenge. Powerhouse Publishing; 2001.
282 Naini, Moss, and Gill American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
September 2006

28. Downs WB. Analysis of the dentofacial profile. Angle Orthod 40. Herz-Fischler R. A mathematical history of the golden number.
1956;26:191-211. New York: Dover Publications; 1998.
29. Steiner CC. Cephalometrics for you and me. Am J Orthod 41. Green CD. All that glitters: a review of psychological research on
1953;39:729-55. the aesthetics of the golden section. Perception 1995;24:937-68.
30. Ricketts RM. A foundation for cephalometric communication. 42. Ricketts RM. The biologic significance of the divine proportion
Am J Orthod 1960;46:330-57. and Fibonacci series. Am J Orthod 1982;81:351-70.
31. Merrifield LL. The profile line as an aid in critically evaluating 43. Moss JP, Linney AD, Lowey MN. The use of three-dimensional
facial esthetics. Am J Orthod 1966;52:804-22. techniques in facial esthetics. Semin Orthod 1995;1:94-104.
32. Holdaway RA. A soft tissue cephalometric analysis and its 44. Baker BW, Woods MG. The role of the divine proportion in the
application in orthodontic treatment planning. Part 1. Am J esthetic improvement of patients undergoing combined orth-
Orthod 1983;84:1-28.
odontic/orthognathic surgical treatment. Int J Adult Orthod
33. Holdaway RA. A soft tissue cephalometric analysis and its
Orthognath Surg 2001;16:108-20.
application in orthodontic treatment planning. Part 2. Am J
45. Tolstoy LN. Childhood, boyhood and youth. London: Penguin
Orthod 1984;85:279-93.
Books; 1970.
34. Bass NM. The aesthetic analysis of the face. Eur J Orthod
46. Macgregor F. After plastic surgery: adaptation and adjustment.
1991;13:343-50.
35. Bergman RT. Cephalometric soft tissue facial analysis. Am J New York: Praeger; 1979.
Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 1999;116:373-89. 47. Rumsey N, Clarke A, White P, Wyn-Williams M, Garlick W.
36. Ackerman JL. Orthodontics: art, science or trans-science? Angle Altered body image: appearance-related concerns of people with
Orthod 1974;44:243-50. visible disfigurement. J Adv Nurs 2004;48:443-53.
37. Park YC, Burstone CJ. Soft tissue profile: the fallacies of hard 48. Macgregor F. Social and psychological implications of dentofa-
tissue standards in treatment planning. Am J Orthod Dentofacial cial disfigurement. Angle Orthod 1970;40:231-3.
Orthop 1986;90:52-62. 49. Coutinho W. Changing faces: facing disfigurement with confi-
38. Sarver DM. Esthetic orthodontics and orthognathic surgery. St dence. London: Changing Faces; 2001.
Louis: Mosby; 1998. 50. Sarwer DB, Wadden TA, Pertschuk MJ, Whitaker LA. Body
39. Ricketts RM. Divine proportion in facial esthetics. Clin Plast image dissatisfaction and body dysmorphic disorder in 100
Surg 1982;9:401-22. cosmetic surgery patients. Plast Reconstr Surg 1998;101:1644-9.

You might also like