Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Environmental Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/envres

Micronutrients during pregnancy and child psychomotor development: MARK


Opposite effects of Zinc and Selenium

Kinga Polanskaa, , Wojciech Hankea, Anna Krola, Jolanta Gromadzinskab, Renata Kurasb,
Beata Janasikb, Wojciech Wasowiczb, Fiorino Mirabellac, Flavia Chiarottic, Gemma Calamandreic
a
Department of Environmental Epidemiology, Nofer Institute of Occupational Medicine, Lodz, Poland
b
Department of Biological and Environmental Monitoring, Nofer Institute of Occupational Medicine, Lodz, Poland
c
Center for Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health, National Institute of Health, Rome, Italy

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Studies on the impact of micronutrient levels during different pregnancy periods on child psychomotor functions
Zinc are limited. The aim of this study was to evaluate the association between maternal plasma concentrations of
Selenium selected micronutrients, such as: copper (Cu), zinc (Zn), selenium (Se), and child neuropsychological develop-
Copper ment. The study population consisted of 539 mother-child pairs from Polish Mother and Child Cohort
Prenatal period
(REPRO_PL). The micronutrient levels were measured in each trimester of pregnancy, at delivery and in the cord
Child neuropsychological development
blood. Psychomotor development was assessed in children at the age of 1 and 2 years using the Bayley Scales of
Infant and Toddler Development. The mean plasma Zn, Cu and Se concentrations in the 1st trimester of preg-
nancy were 0.91 ± 0.27 mg/l, 1.98 ± 0.57 mg/l and 48.35 ± 10.54 μg/l, respectively. There were no statisti-
cally significant associations between Cu levels and any of the analyzed domains of child development. A po-
sitive association was observed between Se level in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and child language and motor
skills (β=0.18, p=0.03 and β=0.25, p=0.005, respectively) at one year of age. Motor score among one-year-
old children decreased along with increasing Zn levels in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and in the cord blood
(β=−12.07, p=0.003 and β=−6.51, p=0.03, respectively). A similar pattern was observed for the association
between Zn level in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and language abilities at one year of age (β=−7.37,
p=0.05). Prenatal Zn and Se status was associated with lower and higher child psychomotor abilities, respec-
tively, within the first year of life. Further epidemiological and preclinical studies are necessary to confirm the
associations between micronutrient levels and child development as well as to elucidate the underlying me-
chanisms of their effects.

1. Introduction pathways and structural components (Georgieff, 2007; Georgieff and


Rao, 2001; Dobbing, 1990; Rao and Georgieff, 2000; Kretchmer et al.,
Pregnancy is a period of increased metabolic demands mainly due to 1996; Thompson and Nelson, 2001). Zinc (Zn) is an essential element
the changes in a woman's physiology and requirements of a growing with a multitude of biological functions, which plays an important role
fetus. Nutrients and growth factors regulate brain development during in brain development and synaptic plasticity (Shah and Sachdev, 2006;
fetal and early postnatal life. The developing brain is particularly vul- Frederickson and Danscher, 1990). In particular, Zn is not only a
nerable to nutritional insults as the rapid trajectory of several neuro- component of several enzymes but it is found in synaptic vesicles in
logic processes, including synapse formulation and myelination areas of the brain endowed with high plasticity such as cortex and
(Georgieff, 2007; Georgieff and Rao, 2001; Dobbing, 1990; Rao and hippocampal mossy fibers. Zn is also involved in the metabolism of
Georgieff, 2000; Kretchmer et al., 1996; Thompson and Nelson, 2001). thyroid hormones, hormone transportation, receptor binding as well as
Thus, certain nutrients, such as: proteins, essential fatty acids, iron, in the metabolism of neurotransmitters (Shah and Sachdev, 2006;
zinc, copper, iodine, selenium, vitamin A, choline and folates, are Frederickson and Danscher, 1990; Nakashima and Dick, 2009; Morley
especially significant for the developing brain (Nyaradi et al., 2013). et al., 1980; Golub et al., 1995). Studies have shown an association
Effects of any nutrient deficiency or its excess on brain development between prenatal Zn deficiency and infant psychomotor development
is a function of its requirement for a nutrient in specific metabolic (Yang et al., 2013; Boroujeni et al., 2009; Cetin et al., 2010; Leung


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: kinga.polanska@imp.lodz.pl (K. Polanska).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2017.06.037
Received 14 January 2017; Received in revised form 4 June 2017; Accepted 29 June 2017
0013-9351/ © 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
K. Polanska et al. Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

et al., 2011; Georgieff, 2007). On the other hand, there is growing Table 1
evidence suggesting that excess of Zn can exert neurotoxic action, lead Characteristics of the exposure and outcome variables.
to cellular damage in vitro (Yang et al., 2013; Zhu et al., 2012) as well
Variables Mean SD Min Max
as in vivo (Kong et al., 1998) and can be associated with decreased
child psychomotor development (Yang et al., 2013; Hamadani et al., Zn concentrations (mg/l)
2002). in the 1st trimester of pregnancy (N=392) 0.91 0.27 0.37 2.41
in the 2nd trimester of pregnancy (N=304) 0.78 0.27 0.23 1.96
Copper (Cu) is a crucial cofactor for copper-containing enzymes that
in the 3rd trimester of pregnancy (N=299) 0.74 0.21 0.32 1.45
function in a number of important processes, including energy pro- in maternal blood at delivery (N=284) 0.77 0.30 0.26 3.20
duction, oxidant defense, extracellular matrix (ECM) protein cross- in cord blood (N=290) 1.08 0.30 0.24 2.64
linking, immune function, blood cell maturation, neuropeptide and Cu concentrations (mg/l)
catecholamine synthesis, myocardial contractility, iron mobilization in the 1st trimester of pregnancy (N= 392) 1.98 0.57 0.55 4.88
in the 2nd trimester of pregnancy (N=307) 2.36 0.59 0.81 4.52
and trafficking (Uriu-Adams et al., 2010; Turski and Thiele, 2009;
in the 3rd trimester of pregnancy (N=299) 2.55 0.53 0.37 5.00
Gambling et al., 2011). The existing studies indicate that balance be- in maternal blood at delivery (N=288) 2.39 0.65 0.66 4.59
tween Cu and iron assures proper neurocognitive and neurobehavioral in cord blood (N=290) 0.59 0.28 0.14 1.72
development (Cetin et al., 2010; Gambling et al., 2011, 2008; Georgieff, Se concentrations (µg/l)
in the 1st trimester of pregnancy (N=410) 48.35 10.54 16.12 91.40
2007; Leung et al., 2011; Uriu-Adams et al., 2010; Beard et al., 2003;
in the 2nd trimester of pregnancy (N=151) 42.25 9.08 14.82 69.68
Penland and Prohaska, 2004; Gybina and Prohaska, 2006; Beard, 2008; in the 3rd trimester of pregnancy (N=130) 37.29 9.79 14.16 61.70
Prohaska and Gybina, 2005). in maternal blood at delivery (N=310) 38.44 11.79 13.83 86.29
The well-known biological role of selenium (Se) is mainly associated in cord blood (N=311) 31.07 8.22 13.81 56.31
with selenoproteins, which are involved in the antioxidant defence Composite score for the one-year-old children
N=501
system and thyroid hormones metabolism (Rayman, 2012; Jablonska
Cognitive 106.2 10.7 80.0 145.0
et al., 2013; Roman et al., 2014; Holmgren and Lu, 2010). Studies in- Language 107.8 13.0 68.0 141.0
dicate that Se can have impact on child neuropsychological develop- Motor 104.4 13.4 73.0 151.0
ment (Yang et al., 2013; Skröder et al., 2014). In our previous analysis Composite score for the two-year-old children
N=341
prenatal Se status was associated positively with a child's psychomotor
Cognitive 110.2 16.1 80.0 145.0
abilities within the first two years of life (Polanska et al., 2016b). Language 101.7 12.7 74.0 144.0
On average, for adults, Se intake of 70 μg/day (2014), Cu intake of Motor 111.5 14.5 73.0 154.0
1.3 mg/day (2015) and Zn intake of 7.5 mg/day (2014) is re-
commended by the European Food Safety Authority, with increasing Zn-zinc, Cu-copper, Se-selenium.
requirements among pregnant and lactating women (European Food
Safety Authority). The nationally representative dietary survey data weeks of single pregnancy, no assisted conception, no pregnancy
from eight European countries, including Poland, has highlighted that complications and no chronic diseases.
there is a risk of low intakes of essential trace elements in specific po- Based on the study protocol, the women were interviewed once in
pulations and age groups (Mensink et al., 2013). That analysis has in- each trimester of pregnancy to collect and update socio-demographic
dicated that 1.1% of women in Poland had Cu intake below the lower data, medical and reproductive history as well as information about
reference nutrient intake (LRNI - intake value below which it is unlikely environmental, lifestyle and occupational exposure. During each visit
that normal health can be maintained over longer periods) and 13.1% and after delivery, biological samples (including saliva, urine, blood,
below the estimated average requirement (EAR – the intake adequate hair and cord blood) were collected.
for 50% of the population). These estimates for Zn were: 4.6% and A child's exposure, health status and psychomotor development
16.6%, respectively. Mean Se contents in the daily food rations col- were evaluated at one year of age and the assessment was repeated
lected from various public canteens and a group of students ranged when the child reached the age of 24 months (Polanska et al., 2011).
from 20 to 59 µg/day (Jablonska et al., 2013). The current analysis was restricted to 539 children who have been
The existing studies evaluating influence of Zn, Cu and Se levels on examined for their neuropsychological development. Among them 303
children's development have produced conflicting results, mainly be- children had both assessments (at one and two years of age), 198 were
cause of the lack of a valid indicator and assessment of these micro- examined only at around the 12th month of age and 38 only at the 24th
nutrients. Therefore, the aim of this study was to evaluate the impact of month of age (Table 1). The following factors were responsible for
the micronutrients: Cu, Zn, Se, measured in the blood collected in losses in the follow-up: refusal, child health problems, unknown address
pregnancy and in the cord blood, on psychomotor development of or telephone number and unknown reasons (Polanska et al., 2016a).
children enrolled in the prospective Polish Mother and Child Cohort The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Nofer
study - REPRO_PL. Institute of Occupational Medicine, Lodz, Poland and a written consent
was obtained from all the study subjects.
2. Methods
2.2. Micronutrients assessment
2.1. Study design and population
Blood samples were collected from each woman during the first
The analyses were based on the mother-child pairs from REPRO_PL (8th−12th week of pregnancy), second (20th–24th week of preg-
cohort - a multicenter prospective cohort established in 2007 that ex- nancy), third (30th–34th week of pregnancy) trimester of pregnancy, at
amines the relationship between environmental (including heavy me- delivery and from the cord, using a venoject system free from trace
tals, phthalates, polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) as well as lifestyle/ elements with lithium heparin as an anticoagulant. After centrifugation,
psychosocial determinants (including smoking, alcohol consumption, the plasma was collected and frozen at −20 °C until the analysis.
stress, family functioning, BMI, physical activity, microelements and Plasma Zn and Cu concentrations were analyzed by means of the flame
vitamins) and multiple aspects of development and health of a child atomic absorption spectrometry (FAAS) (Agarwal and Henkon, 1985).
(Polanska et al., 2009, 2011, 2016a). The detailed description of the This method had been validated using the reference material (lyophi-
cohort methodology has been published previously (Polanska et al., lized human reference serum samples of Seronorm from Nycomed
2009, 2011, 2016a). Briefly, the women were invited to participate in Pharma AS, Oslo, Norway) and through participation in the inter-
the cohort if they fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: up to 12 laboratory comparison trials. Measurements of plasma Se concentration

584
K. Polanska et al. Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

were performed using the graphite furnace atomic absorption spectro- education, child gender and maternal smoking status during pregnancy
metry (GFAAS) on a Unicam Solar 989 QZ apparatus with Zeeman ef- based on the cotinine level. The analysis focusing on the effect of mi-
fect background corrector, in accordance with the modified method of cronutrient levels during pregnancy on the change in cognitive, lan-
Nève et al. (Neve and Molle, 1986; Neve et al., 1987). Accuracy of the guage and motor scores between 1 and 2 years of age applied to all the
method for Se determination was verified using the internal quality children undergoing both evaluations. An examiner assessing devel-
control of the certified reference material BCR-637 (IRMM, Belgium), opment at one year of age and an examiner performing assessment at
where reference value and measured concentration were: 81.0 µg/l (in two years of age were included as covariates in the model. In addition,
the range 74–88 µg/l) and 82.5 ± 0.7 µg/l, respectively, and external the study population was divided in quartiles according to the micro-
quality control of the German External Quality Assessment Scheme (G- nutrient levels. Low (less than 1st quartile), medium (between 1st and
EQUAS) for analyses in biological materials. 3rd quartile) and high (more than 3rd quartile) groups were compared
in order to evaluate the effect of micronutrients on a child's develop-
2.3. Child psychomotor assessment ment (result of the analysis is presented in the Supplementary mate-
rials).
The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley 3rd The variance inflation factor (VIF) was computed for any variable in
edition) was used to assess children's neuropsychological development each model to verify the presence of multicollinearity among ex-
(including cognitive, language and motor functions) at one and two planatory variables. Regression coefficients are reported together with
years of age. Details regarding child psychomotor assessment have been their standard errors. STATA Statistical Software (Release 8.1) was used
published elsewhere (Polanska et al., 2011, 2016b). for the statistical analyses.

2.4. Covariates 3. Results

The following covariates were evaluated: parental age and educa- 3.1. Parental and child characteristics
tion; marital status; socio-economic status (SES); child gender; major
pregnancy complications which appeared after inclusion into the study; Parental and child characteristics are summarized in the
type of delivery; gestational age and birth outcomes; breastfeeding; Supplementary materials (Tables S1, S2). A high percentage of the
number of siblings; day care attendance; maternal cigarette smoking women were married (75.0%), employed (84.8%) and had a university
and alcohol consumption during pregnancy as well as child environ- degree (62.8%). Alcohol consumption during pregnancy was indicated
mental tobacco smoke (ETS) exposure within the first two years of live. by 8.5% of the women and, based on the cotinine level in saliva, 15.1%
Details regarding socio-demographic and birth outcome data collection were classified as smokers. About 7% of the children attended day care
as well as prenatal/postnatal ETS exposure have been described else- at one and 23% at two years of age. ETS exposure after birth was noted
where (Polanska et al., 2016b; Stragierowicz et al., 2013). In the cur- for 35.8% of the children.
rent analysis, the women whose cotinine level in saliva was equal to or The mean composite scores for cognitive, language and motor de-
higher than 10 ng/ml were defined as active smokers (SRNT velopment were on an average or high average level (Table 1). A po-
Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification, 2002). sitive correlation was observed for each subscale of the Bayley test
results for one year assessment (p≤0.001) and between cognitive and
2.5. Statistical analysis language as well as motor and language functions (p≤0.001) for two-
year assessment (Table 3). The correlation was weaker for comparisons
Levels of microelements in each trimester of pregnancy, at delivery performed between one and two years of age.
and in cord blood and cognitive, language and motor developmental
scores at 1 and 2 years of age are summarized by means, standard 3.2. Micronutrient concentrations during pregnancy
deviation, and minimum and maximum values in the overall group of
children. The micronutrient concentrations in the blood collected in each trime-
Correlation between microelement levels and psychomotor devel- ster of pregnancy, at delivery and in the cord blood are presented in Table 1.
opmental scores (cognitive, language and motor) was examined by the Mean Zn concentration was higher in cord blood (1.08 ± 0.3 mg/l) and in
Pearson linear correlation coefficient. The multivariate linear regres- the 1st trimester of pregnancy (0.91 ± 0.27 mg/l) than in the other mea-
sion was performed to assess the effect of Zn, Cu and Se levels measured surement periods (p < 0.001). Mean plasma Cu concentration was slightly
during the 1st trimester of pregnancy and in cord blood, on child lower in the 1st trimester of pregnancy (1.98 ± 0.57 mg/l) and remained
cognitive, language and motor scores at 1 and 2 years of age, and on the fairly constant during the two other periods of pregnancy and in maternal
developmental score variation between 1 and 2 years of age. Following blood collected at delivery (2.36 ± 0.59 mg/l in the 2nd trimester,
the study protocol, data on micronutrients was theoretically available 2.55 ± 0.53 mg/l in the 3rd trimester and 2.39 ± 0.65 mg/l at delivery).
for each visit; however, as a result of organizational (i.e., the lack of Cu concentration was significantly lower in the cord blood than in preg-
samples or usage of the samples for other purposes) and financial rea- nancy period (0.59 ± 0.28 mg/l) (p < 0.001). Mean Se concentrations de-
sons (in that case randomly selected samples were evaluated), not all creased throughout pregnancy and at delivery (from 48.35 ± 10.54 μg/l in
the samples were analyzed for micronutrient levels. In particular, the the 1st trimester to 38.44 ± 11.79 μg/l at delivery) (p < 0.001). Correla-
number of samples collected during the 2nd and the 3rd trimesters of tions between different microelements as well as their concentrations in
pregnancy for which micronutrients were available was much lower, different time periods are presented in Table 2.
thus, micronutrient levels measured at these time points were not in-
cluded in the multivariate analyses. The multivariate model included 3.3. Association between micronutrient concentrations and child
confounders significant at 0.1 level. As for education, a mother and a psychomotor development
father's data were highly correlated (r > 0.5; p < 0.05), so only ma-
ternal age and education were selected and included in the model. The Table 4 presents the association between maternal micronutrient
final multivariate models were performed on 239 subjects at 12 months levels and child psychomotor development with adjustment for con-
assessment and 168 subjects at 24 months assessment, for which there founders. There were no statistically significant associations between
were both Zn, Cu and Se in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and in the Cu levels in the blood collected during the 1st trimester of pregnancy
cord blood, and the confounding variables that were known to affect and in the cord blood and any of the analyzed domains of child de-
child's psychomotor development: examiner, mother age, mother velopment. A positive association was observed between Se level in the

585
K. Polanska et al. Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

Table 2
Pearson correlation (1st line=coefficient, 2nd line=sample size) between the micronutrient levels: zinc, copper, selenium in each trimester of pregnancy, at delivery and in the cord
blood.

Zn (A) Zn (B) Zn (C) Zn (D) Zn (E) Se (A) Se (B) Se (C) Se (D) Se (E) Cu (A) Cu (B) Cu (C) Cu (D) Cu (E)

Zn(A) 1
392
Zn(B) 0.31** 1
301 304
Zn(C) 0.46** 0.49** 1
296 288 299
Zn(D) 0.16* 0.25** 0.29** 1
283 253 251 284
Zn(E) 0.21** 0.21** 0.26** 0.29** 1
289 258 256 279 290
Se(A) 0.12* 0.04 0.12* 0.12** 0.08 1
390 301 296 283 289 410
Se(B) −0.12 0.21* 0.13 0.18* −0.02 0.35** 1
151 150 144 132 135 151 151
Se(C) −0.20* 0.17 −0.005 0.10 −0.04 0.38** 0.50** 1
130 125 129 119 120 130 125 130
Se(D) 0.23** 0.23** 0.35** 0.20** 0.03 0.32** 0.27* 0.47** 1
306 254 252 278 279 310 135 121 310
Se(E) 0.14* 0.11 0.11 0.06 0.17* 0.14* 0.08 0.15 0.35** 1
308 253 251 272 280 311 133 118 302 311
Cu(A) 0.01 −0.005 0.002 0.04 −0.07 0.04 0.09 0.05 0.06 0.08 1
302 301 296 283 390 390 151 130 306 308 392
Cu(B) 0.07 0.10 0.23** 0.19* 0.12* 0.12* 0.10 0.13 0.15* −0.04 0.34** 1
304 303 291 255 261 304 150 126 256 256 304 307
Cu(C) 0.04 0.02 0.07 0.04 −0.001 0.02 −0.004 0.13 −0.04 0.07 0.17* 0.27** 1
296 288 299 251 256 296 144 129 252 251 296 291 299
Cu(D) 0.003 −0.02 −0.10 0.05 0.01 0.008 0.01 0.03 −0.05 0.03 0.18* 0.19* 0.44* 1
287 256 255 282 284 287 133 119 277 277 287 259 255 288
Cu(E) 0.20** 0.27** 0.30** 0.26** 0.24** 0.17* −0.03 −0.04 0.14* 0.11 −0.05 0.07 0.14* 0.15* 1
289 258 256 279 290 289 135 120 279 280 289 261 256 284 290

A: blood collected between the 8th–12th weeks of pregnancy, B: blood collected between the 20th–24th weeks of pregnancy, C: blood collected between the 30th–34th weeks of
pregnancy, D: blood collected from mothers at delivery, E: cord blood; Zn-zinc, Cu-copper, Se-selenium.
Correlation coefficients significantly different from 0 are reported in bold: *, **=p < 0.05 or p < 0.01, respectively.

1st trimester of pregnancy and child language and motor skills pregnancy and the change in psychomotor development scores between
(β=0.18, p=0.03 and β=0.25; p=0.005, respectively) at one year of 1 and 2 years of age is presented in Table 5. None of the results were
age. Interestingly, motor scores among one-year-old children decreased statistically significant (> 0.05).
along with increasing Zn levels in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and in
the cord blood (β=−12.07, p=0.003 and β=−6.51, p=0.03, re- 4. Discussion
spectively). A similar pattern was observed for the association between
Zn level in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and language abilities at one Our prospective cohort study indicated a positive association be-
year of age (β=−7.37, p=0.05). The analysis with the study popula- tween Se level in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and child language and
tion divided in quartiles according to micronutrient levels indicated motor skills at one year of age. In addition, motor and language scores
similar results to these with micronutrients as continuous variables decreased along with increasing Zn levels and there were no statisti-
(opposite impact of Zn and Se on child motor abilities at the age of 1 cally significant associations between Cu levels and any aspects of child
year) (Table S4). The analysis focusing on the impact of micronutrient development.
levels on child psychomotor development at the age of 2 years did not Micronutrient concentrations are different in pregnant than in non-
give any significant results (with and without child developmental pregnant women. Plasma Zn concentration begins to decline in early
scores at 1 year of age as additional covariates) (Tables S3 and 4). pregnancy and continues to decline until delivery, when it is about 35%
An additional analysis regarding micronutrient levels during below its level in non-pregnant women (Wasowicz et al., 1993; King,

Table 3
Pearson correlation between the Bayley test results for one- and two-year assessments.

Psychomotor abilities Assessment at the age of 1 Assessment at the age of 2

Cognitive Language Motor Cognitive Language Motor

Assessments at the age of 1 Cognitive 1


Language 0,25** 1
Motor 0,31** 0,43** 1
Assessments at the age of 2 Cognitive 0,13* −0,03 0,12* 1
Language 0,04 0,15* 0,08 0,49** 1
Motor −0,04 0,22** 0,16* 0,06 0,35** 1

For correlations within the assessment at one year N=501; for correlations between the assessment at one and two years N=303; for correlations within the assessment at two years
N=341.
* p < 0.05.
** p≤0.001.

586
K. Polanska et al. Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

Table 4
Micronutrient levels during pregnancy and child psychomotor development–the multivariate model.

Microelements 1-year old children β (95% CI) (p) 2-year old children β (95% CI) (p)

Cognitive Language Motor Cognitive Language Motor

Zn (1st trimester) (mg/l) −1.74 −7.37 −12.07 1.36 3.71 −0.60


(−8.33;4.84) (−14.73;−0.01) (−20.12;−4.02) (−9.46;12.19) (−6.14;13.56) (−10.61;9.42)
(0.60) (0.05) (0.003) (0.80) (0.46) (0.91)
Zn (cord blood) (mg/l) 0.11 −3.77 −6.51 1.70 −0.19 −5.68
(−4.79;5.01) (−9.25;1.71) (−12.50;−0.52) (−6.72;10.12) (−7.85;7.48) (−13.47;2.11)
(0.97) (0.18) (0.03) (0.69) (0.96) (0.15)
Cu (1st trimester) (mg/l) −0.14 1.38 0.32 −1.15 2.40 −1.47
(−2.45;2.17) (−1.21;3.96) (−2.5–3.15) (−4.75;2.45) (−0.88;5.68) (−4.80;1.86)
(0.91) (0.30) (0.82) (0.53) (0.15) (0.39)
Cu (cord blood) (mg/l) −1.03 1.90 5.06 1.70 0.69 0.78
(−6.03;98) (−3.70;7.49) (−1.05;11.18) (−6.22;9.63) (−6.52;7.90) (−6.55;8.11)
(0.69) (0.50) (0.10) (0.67) (0.85) (0.83)
Se (1st trimester) (µg/l) 0.11 0.18 0.25 0.22 0.07 0.15
(−0.03;0.25) (0.02;0.34) (0.08;0.42) (−0.02;0.47) (−0.15;0.29) (−0.07;0.38)
(0.13) (0.03) (0.005) (0.07) (0.52) (0.18)
Se (cord blood) (µg/l) 0.005 0.10 0.17 −0.06 0.09 0.07
(−0.16;0.17) (−0.08;0.29) (−0.03;0.37) (−0.32;0.19) (−0.14;0.32) (−0.16;0.31)
(0.95) (0.28) (0.10) (0.62) (0.44) (0.55)

Adjusted for: examiner, maternal age, maternal education, child gender and maternal smoking status during pregnancy based on the cotinine level.
For the assessment at the age of 1year N=239.
For the assessment at the age of 2 years N=168.
Data is reported as regression coefficients (β), 95% Confidence Intervals (95% CI), p values (p).

Table 5 earlier study performed in Poland, cord plasma Zn level was 0.8 mg/l
Micronutrient levels during pregnancy and the change in psychomotor developmental (Wasowicz et al., 1993). The other study in Poland (although not cov-
scores between 1 and 2 years of age–the multivariate model.
ering pregnant women) has indicated the mean intake (analytical data)
Microelements Change of a child's developmental score between 1 and 2 of Zn of 8.9 mg/day (5.3 mg/day calculated from 24-h diet recalls),
years of age β (95% CI) (p) which is comparable to the mean Zn intake in Spain (7.6 mg/day) and
lower than that in Germany (10.9 mg/day) (Jablonska et al., 2013;
Cognitive Language Motor Flynn et al., 2009). Similar results have been also obtained for 18−60
Zn (1st trimester) −2.00 6.77 0.70
year-old females in a nationally representative dietary survey data from
(mg/l) (9.51;13.51) (−3.82;17.36) (−9.50;10.91) eight European countries (Mensink et al., 2013).
(0.73) (0.21) (0.89) Based on the existing data, intake of Se varies hugely worldwide
Zn (cord blood) 0.85 1.68 −1.95 from low to even toxic concentrations with the mean values of 40 μg/
(mg/l) (−8.60;10.29) (−6.84;10.20) (−10.17;6.28)
day in Europe, and 93 μg/day in the United States (Rayman, 2012). The
(0.86) (0.70) (0.64)
Cu (1st trimester) −1.99 3.26 −0.57 detailed discussion regarding Se levels observed in our cohort has been
(mg/l) (−5.90;1.91) (−0.26;6.77) (−3.94;2.80) published previously (Polanska et al., 2016b). It is worth noting that in
(0.32) (0.07) (0.74) Poland plasma Se level was 48 μg/l in the studies performed between
Cu (cord blood) 2.65 −0.93 −1.53 1981 and 1983 and above 30 μg/l in that performed between 1997 and
(mg/l) (−6.00;11.31) (−8.79;6.93) (−9.18;6.12)
(0.55) (0.82) (0.69)
1999 and between 2007 and 2009 (REPRO_PL study) (Zachara et al.,
Se (1st trimester) 0.11 0.07 0.12 1986; Jablonska et al., 2013; Wasowicz et al., 2003; Polanska et al.,
(µg/l) (−0.16;0.39) (−0.18;0.32) (−0.12;0.35) 2016b).
(0.42) (0.58) (0.33) In our study, mean plasma Cu concentration increased throughout
Se (cord blood) −0.05 0.07 −0.01
the pregnancy period (from 1.98 mg/l in the 1st trimester of pregnancy
(µg/l) (−0.33;0.22) (−0.17;0.32) (−0.25;0.23)
(0.70) (0.56) (0.94) to 2.36 ml/l in the 2nd and 2.55 mg/l in the 3rd trimester of preg-
nancy). This is consistent with the results reported in other studies on
Adjusted for: examiner at first and at second assessment, child gender, maternal age, pregnant women (Pathak, 2004). Studies have found that an increase in
maternal education, maternal smoking status during pregnancy based on the cotinine the Cu level during pregnancy is mainly in a bound form due to an
level and child developmental score (cognitive, language or motor, respectively) at 1 year
increase in the carrier proteins, ceruloplasmin in response to a stimu-
of age (N=149).
lation by maternal estrogens (Pathak et al., 2004; Martín-Lagos et al.,
Data is reported as regression coefficients (β), 95% Confidence Intervals (95% CI), p
values (p). 1998; Kalra et al., 1989; Dokumov, 1968). With regard to the mean Cu
intake in Poland, based on the female population, it was 1.1 mg/day,
2000; Cetin et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2013). Significant decrease of Zn which was similar to that observed in France (1.2 mg/day), The Neth-
concentration was also observed in our study (from 0.91 mg/l in the 1st erlands (1.1 mg/day), the UK (1.1 mg/day) and lower than that re-
trimester of pregnancy to 0.77 mg/l at delivery). This decline in Zn ported in Germany (2.2 mg/day) (Mensink et al., 2013; Flynn et al.,
levels has been attributed to hemodilution and/or different Zn affinity 2009). Jablonska et al. have indicated the mean intake of Cu to be
to plasma proteins as a consequence of hormonal changes during 1.4 mg/l (based on the analytical data) and 0.8 mg/day (based on the
pregnancy and it can result from active transportation of Zn from the data calculated from 24-h diet recalls) (Jablonska et al., 2013).
mother to the fetus (Wasowicz et al., 1993; King, 2000). The median Our findings support the existing evidence indicating a significant
level of cord plasma Zn (1.08 mg/l) in our study was similar to that role of Se in the brain and behavior development. We observed a po-
reported in Arctic Canada (1.1 mg/l) and higher than that reported in sitive association between Se level in the 1st trimester of pregnancy and
China (0.73 mg/l) (Butler Walker et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2013). In an child language and motor skills at one year of age. It is important that
the data concerning the effect of Se levels (also in cord blood) on child

587
K. Polanska et al. Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

psychomotor development at the age of two years are in the same di- maternal BMI was low, which indicates that they do not necessarily
rection, although not statistically significant. In the study by Skröder reflect the overall nutritional status. It is worth mentioning that in our
et al., similarly to our observations, an increase in the maternal Se cohort more than one-fifth of the women consumed multiple micro-
concentration has been associated with improvement in children's nutrient capsules (more than 20% of the population with Se and about
language and psychomotor development (Skröder et al., 2014). What is 30% of the population with Zn as one of their components). However,
interesting is that in the study by Yang et al. low (< 100 µg/l) and high this neither influenced micronutrients status nor was associated with
(≥100 µg/l) levels of cord serum Se had negative effects on neonatal any aspects of child psychomotor development (Polanska et al., 2016b).
neuropsychological development (Yang et al., 2013). However, in our The other limitation of the study is related to the fact that we did not
assessment, the maximum Se level in cord plasma was 56 μg/l, which is measure/or include internal exposure to neurotoxic elements such as
much lower than the Se level observed by Yang et al. More details on As, Cd, Pb, Hg. Additionally, although we measured micronutrient le-
the impact of Se on child psychomotor development can be found in our vels in different pregnancy periods (for a substantial proportion of
previous publication (Polanska et al., 2016b). children) the final sample, taking into account availability of the data,
The existing studies evaluating the impact of Zn supplementation was limited.
during pregnancy as well as Zn level in the maternal and/or cord blood
on child development are not consistent. As an example, two studies 5. Conclusion
have found no effect of Zn supplementation during pregnancy on child
psychomotor development (Tamura et al., 2003; Caulfield et al., 2010), In conclusion, our study showed that prenatal Zn and Se statuses
while in two other studies, a negative effect of Zn supplementation on were significantly associated with child psychomotor abilities within
developmental outcomes has been observed (Hamadani et al., 2002; the first years of life. It needs to be highlighted that micronutrient levels
Wehby and Murray, 2008). In the study by Yang and coworkers the in pregnant women depend on dietary sources and are influenced by
cord serum Zn level ≥ 0.794 mg/l had an adverse effect on neonatal hormonal and metabolic changes throughout the pregnancy period. Our
neurobehavioral development (Yang et al., 2013). This is in agreement results support the need of assuring a proper nutritional status of
with what we observed in our study – decreasing motor and language women to prevent detrimental nutritional unbalance (Cetin et al.,
scores along with increasing Zn levels. It is important to be aware of the 2010) and suggest that micronutrient supplementation during preg-
fact that in the present study, in about 88% of the cord blood samples nancy should be considered with more caution, especially under op-
Zn level was higher than the threshold determined by Yang et al. (Yang timal conditions. It would be of interest to explore the effects of preg-
et al., 2013). To explain the negative effects of Zn on psychomotor nant women's diet on micronutrient levels and neuropsychological
scores at one year, it is worth mentioning that Zn may have both development of their offspring. The effects which are reported here, and
neuroprotective and neurotoxic properties. Specific mechanisms un- those of Zn in particular, appear to be transient as they are no more
derlying Zn neurotoxicity have been described by linking excess Zn to evident at 2 years of age. However, a follow-up of this population may
oxidative stress and an increased rate of cell death (Morris and be useful to exclude effects on neuropsychological functions at later
Levenson, 2012; Yang et al., 2013). Furthermore, given the pivotal role ages. Altogether, further epidemiological and preclinical studies are
of Zn in glutamate neurotransmission, it can be expected that even needed to confirm the association and elucidate the underlying me-
relatively slight changes in vesicular Zn levels during early neurode- chanisms of these effects.
velopment may influence cortical/hippocampal synaptogenesis and
possibly later behavioural functions (Nakashima and Dyck, 2009). Acknowledgements
We have not found statistically significant associations between Cu
levels and any aspects of child psychomotor development; however, This study was partly funded by the National Science Centre under
some studies in this field indicate that such associations exist, and that the call JPI HDHL Nutrition and Cognitive Function (2015/17/Z/NZ7/
balance between Cu and iron assures proper child neurocognitive and 04273) and partly by the National Science Centre, Poland, under the
neurobehavioral development (Cetin et al., 2010; Gambling et al., grant DEC-2014/15/B/NZ7/00998, by FP7 HEALS Grant N° 603946,
2011, 2008; Georgieff, 2007; Leung et al., 2011; Uriu-Adams et al., the Ministry of Science and Higher Education under grant agreement
2010; Beard et al., 2003; Penland and Prohaska, 2004; Gybina and no. 3068/7.PR/2014/2 and by CROME Grant N° LIFE12 ENV/GR/
Prohaska, 2006; Beard, 2008; Prohaska and Gybina, 2005). 001040.
An advantage of the current analysis is the assessment of micro-
nutrient status based on measurement of Zn, Cu and Se in the blood Appendix A. Supplementary material
over pregnancy period and not just on the questionnaire data regarding
diet and supplements. What is important is the fact that our analysis Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the
evaluates the impact of all the three micronutrients (included in the online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2017.06.037.
multivariate model) on child psychomotor development. Additionally,
in comparison to the existing studies, the current analysis takes ad- References
vantage on inclusion of a variety of potential confounding factors and
the use of a well-standardized test evaluating neuropsychological ef- Agarwal, R.P., Henkon, R.L., 1985. A simple method for simultaneous estimation of zinc
fects in young children (Bayley 3rd edition). and copper in erythrocytes. Biol. Trace. Elem. Res. 7 (4), 199–208. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/BF02989246.
Nevertheless, limitations of the study are also worth noting. The Bayley 3rd edition. Retrieved from 〈http://www.pearsonclinical.com/childhood/
main limitation concerns the lack of information on the dietary habits products/100000123/bayley-scales-of-infant-and-toddler-development-third-
of the women in the study in order to understand whether specific edition-bayley-iii.html〉.
Beard, J., Erikson, K.M., Jones, B.C., 2003. Neonatal iron deficiency results in irreversible
dietary styles might be significantly associated with plasma levels of changes in dopamine function in rats. J. Nutr. 133 (4), 1174–1179.
specific micronutrients and with child psychomotor development. It has Beard, J.L., 2008. Why iron deficiency is important in infant development. J. Nutr. 138
to be pointed that all the nutrients are important for neuronal cell (12), 2534–2536.
Boroujeni, S.T., Naghdi, N., Shahbazi, M., Farrokhi, A., Bagherzadeh, F., Kazemnejad, A.,
growth and development (Georgieff, 2007). In our study we focused
Javadian, M., 2009. The effect of severe zinc deficiency and zinc supplement on
only on Zn, Cu and Se and did not take into account general dietary spatial learning and memory. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 130 (1), 48–61. http://dx.doi.
patterns of the pregnant women. We also did not include the intake of org/10.1007/s12011-008-8312-7.
Butler Walker, J., Houseman, J., Seddon, L., McMullen, E., Tofflemire, K., Mills, C.,
protein, iron, iodine, folate, vitamins, choline and long-chain poly-
Corriveau, A., Weber, J.P., LeBlanc, A., Walker, M., Donaldson, S.G., Van Oostdam,
unsaturated fatty acids, which are also crucial for brain development. In J., 2006. Maternal and umbilical cord blood levels of mercury, lead, cadmium, and
the current analysis correlation between micronutrients status and essential trace elements in Arctic Canada. Environ. Res. 100 (3), 295–318.

588
K. Polanska et al. Environmental Research 158 (2017) 583–589

Caulfield, L.E., Putnick, D.L., Zavaleta, N., Lazarte, F., Albornoz, C., Chen, P., Dipietro, graphite tube and Zeeman effect background correction. Acta Pharmacol. Toxicol. 59
J.A., Bornstein, M.H., 2010. Maternal gestational zinc supplementation does not in- (7), 606–609.
fluence multiple aspects of child development at 54 mo of age in Peru. Am. J. Clin. Nyaradi, A., Li, J., Hickling, S., Foster, J., Oddy, W.H., 2013. The role of nutrition in
Nutr. 92 (1), 130–136. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2010.29407. children's neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood. Front
Cetin, I., Berti, C., Calabrese, S., 2010. Role of micronutrients in the periconceptional Hum. Neurosci. 7, 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00097.
period. Hum. Reprod. Update 16 (1), 80–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/humupd/ Pathak, P., Kapil, U., Kapoor, S.K., Saxena, R., Kumar, A., Gupta, N., Dwivedi, S.N., Singh,
dmp025. R., Singh, P., 2004. Prevalence of multiple micronutrient deficiencies amongst
European Food Safety Authority. Retrieved from (〈https://www.efsa.europa.eu/en/ pregnant women in a rural area of Haryana. Indian J. Pediatr. 71 (11), 1007–1014.
topics/topic/drv〉). Penland, J.G., Prohaska, J.R., 2004. Abnormal motor function persists following recovery
Dobbing, J., 1990. Vulnerable periods in the developing brain. In: Dobbing, J. (Ed.), from perinatal copper deficiency in rats. J. Nutr. 134 (8), 1984–1988.
Brain, behavior and iron in the infant diet. Springer, London, United Kingdom, pp. Polanska, K., Hanke, W., Gromadzinska, J., Ligocka, D., Gulczynska, E., Sobala, W.,
1–25. Wasowicz, W., 2009. Polish mother and child cohort study- defining the problem, the
Dokumov, S.I., 1968. Serum copper and pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 101 (2), aim of the study and methodological assumption. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health
217–222. 22 (4), 383–391. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10001-009-0037-0.
Flynn, A., Hirvonen, T., Mensink, G.B.M., Ocke, M.C., Serra-Majem, L., Stos, K., Szponar, Polanska, K., Hanke, W., Jurewicz, J., Sobala, W., Madsen, C., Nafstad, P., Magnus, P.,
L., Tetens, I., Turrini, A., Fletcher, R., Wildemann, T., 2009. Intake of selected nu- 2011. Polish mother and child cohort study (REPRO_PL)- methodology of follow-up
trients from foods, from fortification and from supplements in various European of the children. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 24 (4), 391–398. http://dx.doi.
countries. Food Nutr. Res. 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.3402/fnr.v53i0.2038. org/10.2478/s13382-011-0026-y.
Frederickson, C.J., Danscher, G., 1990. Zinc-containing neurons in hippocampus and Polanska, K., Hanke, W., Krol, A., Hanke, A., Waszkowska, M., Jacukowicz, A.,
related CNS structures. Prog. Brain Res. 83, 71–84. Gromadzinska, J., Wasowicz, W., Jerzynska, J., Stelmach, W., Stelmach, I., 2016a.
Gambling, L., Andersen, H.S., McArdle, H.J., 2008. Iron and copper, and their interactions Polish Mother and Child Cohort Study (REPRO_PL) - Methodology of the follow-up of
during development. Biochem. Soc. Trans. 36 (6), 1258–1261. http://dx.doi.org/10. the children at the age of 7. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 29 (6), 883–893.
1042/BST0361258. http://dx.doi.org/10.13075/ijomeh.1896.00811.
Gambling, L., Kennedy, C., McArdle, H.J., 2011. Iron and copper in fetal development. Polanska, K., Krol, A., Sobala, W., Gromadzinska, J., Brodzka, R., Calamandrei, G.,
Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 22 (6), 637–644. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.semcdb.2011. Chiarotti, F., Wasowicz, W., Hanke, W., 2016b. Selenium status during pregnancy
08.011. and child psychomotor development-Polish Mother and Child Cohort study. Pediatr.
Georgieff, M.K., 2007. Nutrition and the developing brain: nutrient priorities and mea- Res. 79 (6), 863–869. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/pr.2016.32.
surement. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 85 (2), 614–620. Polish Mother and Child Cohort study RERPO_PL. Retrieved from (〈www.repropl.com〉).
Georgieff, M.K., Rao, R., 2001. The role of nutrition in cognitive development. In: Nelson, Prohaska, J.R., Gybina, A.A., 2005. Rat brain iron concentration is lower following
C.A., Luciana, M. (Eds.), Handbook in developmental cognitive neuroscience. MIT perinatal copper deficiency. J. Neurochem. 93 (3), 698–705.
Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 491–504. Rao, R., Georgieff, M.K., 2000. Early nutrition and brain development. In: Nelson, C.A.
Golub, M.S., Keen, C.L., Gershwin, M.E., Hendrickx, A.G., 1995. Developmental zinc (Ed.), The effects of early adversity on neurobehavioral development. Minnesota
deficiency and behavior. J. Nutr. 125 (8), 2263–2271. Symposium on Child Psychology 31. Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 1–30.
Gybina, A.A., Prohaska, J.R., 2006. Variable response of selected cuproproteins in rat Rayman, M.P., 2012. Selenium and human health. Lancet 379 (9822), 1256–1268.
choroid plexus and cerebellum following perinatal copper deficiency. Genes Nutr. 1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61452-9.
(1), 51–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02829936. Roman, M., Jitaru, P., Barbante, C., 2014. Selenium biochemistry and its role for human
Hamadani, J.D., Fuchs, G.J., Osendarp, S.J., Huda, S.N., Grantham-McGregor, S.M., 2002. health. Metallomics 6 (1), 25–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1039/c3mt00185g.
Zinc supplementation during pregnancy and effects on mental development and Shah, D., Sachdev, H.P., 2006. Zinc deficiency in pregnancy and fetal outcome. Nutr. Rev.
behaviour of infants: a follow-up study. Lancet 360 (9329), 290–294. http://dx.doi. 64 (1), 15–30.
org/10.1016/S0140-6736(02)09551-X. Skröder, H.M., Hamadani, J.D., Tofail, F., Persson, L.Å., Vahter, M.E., Kippler, M.J., 2014.
Holmgren, A., Lu, J., 2010. Thioredoxin and thioredoxin reductase: current research with Selenium status in pregnancy influences children's cognitive function at 1.5 years of
special reference to human disease. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 396 (1), age. Clin. Nutr. 34 (5), 923–930. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clnu.2014.09.020.
120–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2010.03.083. SRNT Subcommittee on Biochemical Verification, 2002. Biochemical verification of to-
Jablonska, E., Gromadzinska, J., Klos, A., Bertrandt, J., Skibniewska, K., Darago, A., bacco use and cessation. Nicotine Tob. Res. 4 (2), 149–159.
Wasowicz, W., 2013. Selenium, zinc and copper in the Polish diet. J. Food Compos. Stragierowicz, J., Mikołajewska, K., Zawadzka-Stolarz, M., Polańska, K., Ligocka, D.,
Anal. 31 (2), 259–265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfca.2013.05.016. 2013. Estimation of cutoff values of cotinine in urine and saliva for pregnant women
Kalra, R., Kalra, V.B., Sareen, P.M., Khandelwal, R., 1989. Serum copper and cer- in Poland. Biomed. Res. Int. 2013, 386784. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/
uloplasmin in pregnancy with anaemia. Indian J. Pathol. Microbiol. 32 (1), 28–32. 386784.
King, J.C., 2000. Determinants of maternal zinc status during pregnancy. Am. J. Clin. Tamura, T., Goldenberg, R.L., Ramey, S.L., Nelson, K.G., Chapman, V.R., 2003. Effect of
Nutr. 71 (5), 1334–1343. zinc supplementation of pregnant women on the mental and psychomotor develop-
Kong, X., Liu, L., Sheng, X., 1998. Effects of excessive zinc in fodder on brain development ment of their children at 5 y of age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 77 (6), 1512–1516.
and abilities of learning and memory and their mechanisms in young rats. Zhonghua Thompson, R.A., Nelson, C.A., 2001. Developmental science and the media: early brain
Yu Fang. Yi Xue Za Zhi. 32 (4), 225–228. development. Am. Psychol. 56 (1), 5–15.
Kretchmer, N., Beard, J.L., Carlson, S., 1996. The role of nutrition in the development of Turski, M.L., Thiele, D.J., 2009. New roles for copper metabolism in cell proliferation,
normal cognition. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 63 (6), 997–1001. signaling, and disease. J. Biol. Chem. 284 (2), 717–721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1074/
Leung, B.M.Y., Wiens, K.P., Kaplan, B.J., 2011. Does prenatal micronutrient supple- jbc.R800055200.
mentation improve children's mental development? A systematic review. BMC Uriu-Adams, J.Y., Scherr, R.E., Lanoue, L., Keen, C.L., 2010. Influence of copper on early
Pregnancy Childbirth 11, 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2393-11-12. development: prenatal and postnatal considerations. Biofactors 36 (2), 136–152.
Martín-Lagos, F., Navarro-Alarcón, M., Terrés-Martos, C., López-García de la Serrana, H., http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/biof.85.
Pérez-Valero, V., López-Martínez, M.C., 1998. Zinc and copper concentrations in Wasowicz, W., Wolkanin, P., Bednarski, M., Gromadzinska, J., Sklodowska, M.,
serum from Spanish women during pregnancy. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 61 (1), 61–70. Grzybowska, K., 1993. Plasma trace element (Se, Zn, Cu) concentrations in maternal
Mensink, G.B., Fletcher, R., Gurinovic, M., Huybrechts, I., Lafay, L., Serra-Majem, L., and umbilical cord blood in Poland. Relation with birth weight, gestational age, and
Szponar, L., Tetens, I., Verkaik-Kloosterman, J., Baka, A., Stephen, A.M., 2013. parity. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 38 (2), 205–215.
Mapping low intake of micronutrients across Europe. Br. J. Nutr. 110 (4), 755–773. Wasowicz, W., Gromadzinska, J., Rydzynski, K., Tomczak, J., 2003. Selenium status of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S000711451200565X. low-selenium area residents: Polish experience. Toxicol. Lett. 137 (1-2), 95–101.
Morley, J.E., Gordon, J., Hershman, J.M., 1980. Zinc deficiency, chronic starvation, and Wehby, G.L., Murray, J.C., 2008. The effects of prenatal use of folic acid and other dietary
hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid function. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 33 (8), 1767–1770. supplements on early child development. Matern. Child Health J. 12 (2), 180–187.
Morris, D.R., Levenson, C.W., 2012. Ion channels and zinc: mechanisms of neurotoxicity Yang, X., Yu, X., Fu, H., Li, L., Ren, T., 2013. Different levels of prenatal zinc and selenium
and neurodegeneration. J. Toxicol. 2012, 785647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/ had different effects on neonatal neurobehavioral development. Neurotoxicology 37,
785647. 35–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2013.04.001.
Nakashima, A.S., Dyck, R.H., 2009. Zinc and cortical plasticity. Brain Res. Rev. 59 (2), Zachara, B.A., Wasowicz, W., Gromadzinska, J., Sklodowska, M., Krasomski, G., 1986.
347–373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.10.003. Glutathione peroxidase activity, selenium and lipid peroxides concentrations in blood
Neve, J., Chamart, S., Molle, L., 1987. Optimization of direct procedure for the de- from a healthy Polish population. Maternal and cord blood. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 10
termination of selenium in plasma and erythrocytes using Zeeman effect atomic ab- (3), 175–187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02795616.
sorption spectroscopy. Trace Elem. Anal. Chem. Med. Biol. 4, 349–358. Zhu, L., Ji, X.J., Wang, H.D., Pan, H., Chen, M., Lu, T.J., 2012. Zinc neurotoxicity to
Neve, J., Molle, L., 1986. Direct determination of selenium in human serum by graphite hippocampal neurons in vitro induces ubiquitin conjugation that requires p38 acti-
furnace atomic absorption spectroscopy. Improvements due to oxygen ashing in vation. Brain Res. 1438, 1–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.031.

589

You might also like