Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 29, 75-81, March 1986

DEVELOPMENT OF C O N V E R S A T I O N A L REPAIR STRATEGIES


IN R E S P O N S E TO R E Q U E S T S F O R C L A R I F I C A T I O N

BONNIE BRINTON MARTIN FUJIKI


University of Nevada School of Medicine, Reno

DIANE FROME LOEB


Washoe County School District, Reno, NV

ERIKA WINKLER
Easter Seals Society, Sparks, NV

Conversational repair sequences are an important aspect of communicative interaction. These sequences may occur in the event
of communication failure ira listener requests clarification of a previous aspect of the speaker's message. The purpose of this study
was to investigate the repair strategies employed in conversation by children at four age levels.
Ten linguistically normal children were sampled from each of the following age levels: 2:7 to 3:10, 4:10 to 5:10, 6:10 to 7:10,
and 8:10 to 9:10 (years:months), resulting in a total of 40 subjects. Each subject was asked to describe a series of action pictures
for an examiner who was seated behind a screen. At regular intervals, the examiner responded to the child's description by
initiating a stacked repair sequence. Results indicated that subjects at all age levels complied with the initial request for
clarification the majority of the time. However, with increasing age, subjects became more adept at handling the stacked sequence
of requests for clarification. Older children were more responsive to the requests, and 9--year-old subjects demonstrated a wider
range of strategies in providing repairs.

Recent interest in the pragmatic aspects of language participate in conversational repair sequences is an inte-
acquisition has focused on the development of conversa- gral part of the language acquisition process.
tional skills. To function in conversation, children must Repair sequences represent an early developing mech-
develop the ability to appreciate and respond to listener anism that is vital to discourse regulation. Gallagher
feedback. This developing awareness is evident in se- (1977) observed that children in Brown's (1973) language
quences during which children provide repairs in re- stages I to III (mean length of utterance-morphemes 1.5
sponse to indications that a message has not been under- to 2.9) responded to neutral requests for clarification
stood. ("What?") from an adult by repeating or revising the
One stimulus for conversational repair is the request for original message. She also observed that the revision
clarification. The request for clarification is a type of strategies available to the children increased in variety as
unsolicited contingent query (as described by Garvey, language age increased.
1977). It may occur anywhere during discourse when a Gallagher (1981) noted that children in Brown's lan-
listener indicates that a speaker's message has not been guage stages I to III demonstrated some knowledge of at
understood or cannot be interpreted without clarification. least two types of clarification requests: requests for
Several types of clarification requests have been de- confirmation and neutral requests for repetition. All of the
scribed (Gallagher, 1981; Garvey, 1977). Some types and children seemed to appreciate the conversational obliga-
examples are as follows: tion imposed by these requests even though they were
Neutral or nonspecific requests for clarification, such as not always entirely accurate in their responses. In a
"Huh?" or "What?" For example: similar study, Langford (1981) analyzed the responses of
Speaker 1. "I went to a party last night." 4-year--old children to requests for clarification produced
Speaker 2. "Huh?" by their adult caregivers. He noted that the children were
Requests for confirmation. For example: responsive to the requests, and that they were sensitive to
Speaker 1. "I went to a party last night." indications from the adults that the requests had been
Speaker 2. "Last night?" satisfied. Garvey (1977) found that children between the
Requests for repetition of a specific constituent. For ages of 34 and 67 months responded to several types of
example: clarification requests (a subset of the contingent queries
Speaker 1. "I went to a party last night." that were described) at least 80% of the time in conver-
Speaker 2. "To a what?" sational dyads.
The ability to clarify a previous utterance in conversa- These studies have indicated that clarification request
tion might seem to be a linguistic nicety that is acquired sequences are functional in the conversation of very
after other language "basics" are mastered. However, this young children. Relatively unsophisticated language
does not appear to be the case. Rather, the ability to learners appear to be sensitive to feedback that their

© 1986, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 75 0022-4685/86/2901-0075501.00/0

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
76 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 29 75-81 March 1986

messages have not been understood, and are able to were selected from the kindergarten, first grade, and third
adjust their contributions during discourse to accommo- grade classrooms at an elementary school located in
date their conversational partners. This research in clari- Sparks, Nevada. The youngest subjects were selected
fication sequences also suggests that the strategies to from children attending a preschool program that was also
provide clarification change with age. located in Sparks. Each group was randomly selected
Although conversational repairs have been examined from a subject pool that had been screened to meet
by several researchers, the ability to respond when re- several criteria based on procedures used by Stark and
quests for clarification are produced recursively in con- Tallal (1981). Subjects had no history of:
versation has received less attention. This type of se- 1. hearing loss or middle ear infection;
quence occurs when a response to a request for clarifica- 2. speech and language problems;
tion is met by a second request for clarification. Spilton 3. mental retardation;
and Lee (1977) noted that such sequences of clarification 4. behavioral disturbance;
requests and responses illustrate certain conversational 5. neurological impairment (including oral, motor or
phenomena that are not demonstrated in a single clarifi- sensory deficits); or
cation request-response interchange. These authors stud- 6. academic difficulties requiring special remedial
ied "elaborative sequences" that consisted of an unclear services.
utterance and at least four utterances exchanged in an The case history information for each subject was ob-
attempt to clarify the first utterance. The result was the tained from classroom placement data, school records,
production of a stacked sequence of requests for clarifi- classroom teacher reports, and school speech-language
cation in which the response to the first request was met pathologist reports.
with a second request for clarification. These elaborative
sequences, or stacked sequences of requests for clarifica-
tion, provided an indication of how children were able to Procedures
adapt to the needs of their listeners.
As Spilton and Lee observed, these sequences of stacked Each child was examined individually in a quiet room.
requests for clarification and responses demonstrated both The same investigator studied all of the children. The
the mutual efforts of conversational partners to communi- seating arrangement placed the child directly across from
cate and their successive approximations to reach an under- the investigator. A 42 x 20~/~"cardboard screen was placed
standing. They reported that 4-year-old dyads demon- between the investigator and the child, thereby eliminating
strated reciprocity in the production of these sequences, visual but not auditory cues. All verbal exchanges were
inasmuch as most of the responses provided the clarification audiotaped using a Sony BM 12 portable audiocassette
requested. The 4--year--old speakers tended to adapt their recorder and an EMC external microphone. The micro-
contributions to the conversation according to the nature of phone was attached to the screen and positioned between
the listener feedback they received. the child and investigator. Additionally, a second investiga-
Stacked sequences of requests for clarification and the tor was present as an observer. The observer was seated to
repairs that they elicit appear to be a rich source of infor- view both the primary investigator and the child.
mation about children's ability to cooperate with a listener Prior to task administration, each subject was given the
during discourse. This study was designed to investigate following instructions: " I ' m going to sit here and you'll sit
the manner in which children respond to stacked requests here. We can't see each other. I ' m going to put some
for clarification during a structured conversational task (pic- envelopes through this slot. There are pictures in the
ture description). Only neutral requests were employed. envelopes. Only you can see them, I can't. Don't let me
The following questions were posed: How do children of see them. You tell me all that's happening in the pictures.
differing ages respond to stacked sequences of requests for When you tell me all about the picture, I'll put a token in
clarification of a message? What types of responses are here (in the jar). When we're all done, you can trade your
produced to each request for clarification in a stacked tokens for a prize over here (prize board)." The child was
sequence? How do these types of responses differ accord- then seated behind the screen and a trial item was
ing to the age of the subjects? How do these types of administered. After the subject successfully completed
responses differ according to the type and position of the the trial item, the 20 picture cards were presented, one at
request within the stacked sequence? a time, through the slot as the investigator said, "Tell me
about this one." A total of 20 8Y4 x 11" line-drawn action
pictures was presented to the subjects: 10 control pictures
METHOD alternating with 10 experimental pictures. All of the
pictures had pull-out tabs attached to aid the child in
Subjects removing the picture from the envelope. The pictures
were presented in the same order for all subjects.
The investigator responded to the child's descriptions
Subjects consisted of 10 children at each of the follow- of the control pictures with statements of acknowledg-
ing age levels: 2:7 to 3:10, 4:10 to 5:10, 6:10 to 7:10, and ment such as, "uh huh," or " m m m . " When each subject
8:10 to 9:10, resulting in a total sample of 40 subjects (20 described an experimental picture, the investigator elic-
boys and 20 girls). Children in the three older age groups ited a stacked sequence of repairs. The sequence was

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
BRINTON ET AL.: Conversational Repair Strategies 77

initiated when the investigator responded to the subject's Repetition. This category was designed to capture in-
initial description with "Huh?". Following the subject's stances where a subject repeated all or part of his/her
response to this request for clarification, the examiner original utterance in response to the request. In these cases,
said, "What?". The examiner waited for the subject's no information was added and the utterance was not re-
response, and then said, "I didn't understand that." If the structured. Examples of this category are provided below.
child responded to this third request for clarification by 1. Child's original utterance: "The dog ran fast."
providing a repair, the examiner indicated that the re- Request for clarification: "Huh?"
quest had been satisfied by saying, "Oh, I see." An Child's repair: "The dog ran fast."
example of this sequence is provided below. 2. Child's original utterance: "The dog ran fast."
Child's initial description of the picture. Request for clarification: " H u h ? "
Request 1 Examiner: "Huh?" (upward intonation) Child's repair: "Ran fast."
Child's response to request for clarification Revision. This category included responses in which
(repair). semantic content was held constant although the utter-
Request 2 Examiner: "What?" (upward intonation) ance form was altered. Revisions included instances in
Child's response to second request for clar- which subjects used alternate labels or different syntactic
ification (repair). structure without adding meaning. If subjects revised
Request 3 Examiner: "7 didn't understand that." aspects of form and also added new meaning elements,
Child's response to third request for clari- the response was not included in this category but was
fication (repair). considered an addition. Examples of the revision cate-
Examiner: "Oh, I see." gory are as follows:
Three different neutral requests for clarification were 1. Child's original utterance: "Some children are play-
used in an attempt to preserve the naturalness of the ing."
sequence. Although the form of these requests varied, Request for clarification: "Huh?"
their function in discourse was considered to be similar Child's repair: "Some kids are playing."
(Gallagher, 1981; Garvey, 1977). The response that fol- 2. Child's original utterance: "The boy's kicking the
lowed the examiner's request for clarification was consid- dog."
ered the repair response. In the event that the child did Request for clarification: "Huh?"
not respond at all to the request, a 5-s interval was Child's repair: "The dog's being kicked by the boy."
allowed before the examiner initiated the next request. Additions. This category included utterances in which
The subject received a token after completing the dis- the subject attempted to repair by adding specific infor-
course pertinent to each picture. The tokens were ex- mation to the original utterance. The nature of the added
changed at the completion of the task. information was described according to the semantic
A token reinforcement system employing a "reward relation(s) the addition represented with respect to the
menu" format was used for all subjects to maintain original utterance. Examples of additions are as follows.
attention to task. The reward menu consisted of various 1. Child's original utterance: "The dog is swimming."
prizes or rewards that were displayed in a menu format. Request for clarification: "Huh?"
At the completion of the task, children exchanged tokens Child's repair: "The dog is swimming in the water."
for a prize (Gelfand & Hartmann, 1983). (+ locative)
2. Child's original utterance: "A girl's playing on the
swing."
Data Analysis Request for clarification: "Huh?"
Child's repair: "A girl and boy are playing on the
swing." (+ agent)
The data consisted of 400 stacked repair sequences and
Cues. This category described instances in which sub-
a total of 1,200 individual repairs. A classification system
jects repaired by defining terms in the original utterance.
for repair type was established. The system consisted of
It also included repairs that offered background context
five main categories that were further divided into sub-
that could precede the original message. This background
categories. Following the analysis, the data were statisti-
context seemed to provide a framework in which the
cally evaluated by performing a two-way analysis of
original utterance could be interpreted. Instances when
variance on each of the major category types, using age
subjects talked about repairing were called formulations
level and clarification request type (Huh?, What?, and I
and were also included in this category. Frequently, all of
didn't understand) as independent variables.
these types of cues involved added information. If mean-
ing elements were added as well as a cue, the response
was considered in the cue category. The following are
Primary Repair Categories examples of cue strategies.
1. Defining term:
Each repair was evaluated according to its relationship Child's original utterance: "They're on a teeter-tot-
to the child's original utterance. Repairs were categorized ter."
into one of five separate categories. Each of these major Request for clarification: "'Huh?"
categories is listed and described below. Child's repair: "'They're on a teeter-totter, some-

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
78 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 29 75-81 March 1986

thing that one person sits on one side and you go ing the training period on the data from the pilot study
up and down on it.'" before the examiners began coding the experimental
2. Background Context: data. Once the utterances were coded into appropriate
Child's original utterance: "He's on a stool getting categories, the responses were summed for each major
cookies." category heading for each age group.
Request for clarification: "Huh?"
Child's repair: "He's on a stool getting cookies."
(repetition)
Request for clarification: "What?" RESULTS
Child's repair: "He's on a stool getting cookies."
(repetition) The repairs produced in response to each of the three
Request for clarification: "I didn't understand that." clarification request types (Huh?, What?, and I didn't
Child's repair: "'There's a family in a kitchen. understand) were categorized according to the five repair
There's a boy. He's on a stool getting cookies." categories. A two--factor analysis of variance with re-
(background context) peated measures (ANOVA) (Age x Request for clarifica-
3. Formulation: tion type) was performed on each of the response catego-
Child's original utterance: "A girl's swinging on a ries: repetition, revision, addition, cues, and inappropri-
swing." ate. An arc-sine square root transformation was performed
Request for clarification: "Huh?" on the raw data, but did not significantly alter the results
Child's repair: "A girl's swinging on a swing." (rep- of the analysis. Hence, the ANOVAs and subsequent
etition) Duncan Multiple Range Tests (Duncan, 1973) were per-
Request for clarification: "What?" formed using raw data.
Child's repair: "A girl's swinging on a big swing." Figure 1 illustrates the repetition responses that were
(addition) elicited by each of the three requests for clarification.
Request for clarification: "I didn't understand that." Although none of the age differences reached signifi-
Child's repair: "I don't know how to say it better." cance, there was a significant difference between ques-
(formulation) tion types, F(2, 72) = 55.24; p < .01. Analysis using the
Inappropriate. This category described instances in Duncan Multiple Range Test revealed that significantly
which subjects did not respond or comply with the (p < .01) more repetitions were produced in response to
request for clarification. Inappropriate responses in- the first request for clarification, "Huh?" than to the
cluded off topic utterances, failures to respond, and at- second request for clarification, "What?" across age
tempts to discontinue. For example: groups. Additionally, a significantly greater number of
1. Child's original utterance: "There's a girl chasing a repetitions was produced in response to each of the
boy." requests "Huh?" and "What?" than to "I didn't under-
Request for clarification: "Huh?" stand that." As can be observed in Figure 1, each age
Child's repair: "A dog ate somebody's shoe." group demonstrated this pattern.
2. Child's original utterance: "Two kids are swinging." Figure 2 illustrates subject performance in the revision
Request for clarification: "I didn't understand that." category. As can be seen, 7- and 9-year-olds used more
Child's repair: "I don't know." revisions than did younger subjects; however, differences
between age groups did not reach statistical significance.
No age group depended heavily on revision strategies.
Coding and Transcription Reliability
100-
Each of the audiotapes from the 40 subjects was tran-
9O-
scribed by one of two investigators (each investigator
transcribed 20 audiotapes). Interjudge reliability was 80: 76
p~ -
determined based on a randomly selected 13% of the 70-
o . 63 64
sample. The percentage of agreement between the tran-
scribed samples was calculated using the following for- 50- 50 iiiiTiil
mula: A/N x 100, where A = the total number of word
agreements, and N = the total number of words (calcu- iiWYi,,! ........ 4.

lated by adding word agreements and disagreements). ao- !jlii!ii!N 25


This produced a percentage of agreement of 98.94%. !iZi!Ti! -/,--,4
The repairs were coded independently by two examin-
ers. Prior to coding the experimental data, a training ......... ~/A ilriii:iiil
1 i i I
period was used to familiarize each examiner with the 3 yr. old 5 yr. old 7 yr, old 9 yr. old
categorization system and to establish interjudge reliabil-
ity. Coding reliability was determined using a formula I h°" I what I 1 ~ didn't I
similar to that used in calculating transcription reliability. FIGURE 1. Frequency of repetition responses to requests for
An interjudge reliability level of 90% was achieved dur- clarification.

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
BRINTON ET AL.: Conversational Repair Strategies 79

20- 20-
18

15
~15-
13
14
•15-

!iii!~1l
o

9 ~10-
o iiiiiiiii~I 0
8 ii!i!i!iii!l $ %
..Q
E 6 6 E
c 5- c 5-
4
2
1 1
0 o1~ o o1~ I~
0
3 yr. old 5 yr. old 7 ,r. old 9 r. old 3 yr'. old 5 yr, old 7 yr. old 9 yr. old

I I~1 hUh ] ~ what 1[7"~ didn't I I huh I what d,dnt ]


FIGURE 2. Frequency of revision responses to requests for FIGURE 4. Frequency of cue responses to requests for
clarification. clarification.

Figure 3 illustrates subject performance in the addition request for clarification, "I didn't understand that." The
category. Subjects of all ages were able to repair by Duncan Multiple Range Test indicated that the
adding information, and this strategy increased signifi- 9-year--old subjects produced significantly more cue re-
cantly with age level, F(3, 36) = 4.53, p < .01. Addition- sponses on the third request for clarification than any of
ally, significant differences were observed between re- the other age groups in response to any request type (p <
quest for clarification type, F(2,76) = 6.43, p < .01. The .01). It was notable that this type of repair was rare in the
results of the Duncan Multiple Range Tests revealed that younger age groups. Only the 9-year-olds produced cue
7- and 9--year-olds added information significantly (p < repairs in any numbers, and then only in response to the
.05) more often than did 3-year-olds, and 9-year-olds third request in the sequence. This pattern was respon-
added information significantly (p < .05) more often than sible for the significant interaction effect between age
did 5-year-olds. Regarding clarification type, it was level and request type.
found that the subjects added significantly more informa- Figure 5 illustrates subject production of inappropriate
tion on the third request ("didn't") than on the first responses to the requests for clarification. Significant
("Huh?"). differences were found in the production of inappropriate
Figure 4 illustrates subject performance on the cue responses with regard to both age level, F(3, 36) = 12.35;
category. Statistical analysis revealed significant differ- p < .01, and request for clarification type, F(2, 72) =
ences regarding age level, F(3, 36) = 3.8, p < .05, and 11.66; p < .01. In addition, the interaction of these
request for clarification type, F(2, 72) = 3.92; p < .05. variables was also significant, F(6, 72) = 4.10; p < .01.
Additionally, the interaction of these variables was also The Duncan Multiple Range Test indicated that the
significant, F(6, 72) = 3.64; p < .01. performance of the 3-year-olds on all three request for
As illustrated in Figure 4, these differences centered on clarification types and the performance of the 5-year-olds
the performance of the 9-year-old subjects on the third on the "I didn't understand that" type differed signifi-
cantly (p < .01) from the performance of all of other age

40

35

0-1 t 34
38 41

~: 35-
40-

0¢/)3 0 -
43

37

2 25-
0

..Q
20-
E=15-
10-
5 6
0 J i
5-
3 yr. old 5 yr. old 7 yr. old 9 yr. old WA
I
3 yr. old 5 yr. old 7 yr. old 9 yr. old
I "°h wha, d,dn't I
FIGURE 3. Frequency of addition responses to requests for
I huh w,at I ]
clarifieation. FIGURE 5. Frequency of inappropriate responses.

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
80 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 29 75-81 March 1986

levels on all the other requests for clarification. The revise, and add information in response to the frst and
significant interaction between age and clarification type second requests. It may be that on the third request, they
was the result of the large number of inappropriate either lacked the ability to persist in using a previous
productions in the 3 - and 5-year-old groups to the third repair a third time, or they lacked the flexibility to try a
request for clarification. Seven- and 9-year-olds pro- different repair strategy after previous attempt s had
duced a small number of inappropriate responses on all failed. It is interesting to note that 2 5-year-old subjects
three trials. and 1 3-year-old subject frequently provided repairs to
"Huh?" and "What?" yet responded to "I didn't under-
stand that" with utterances such as "I don't know," "I
DISCUSSION don't either," "I can't remember," or "Just let me forget
about it." It seems likely that these children realized that
This study investigated the manner in which children additional repair was needed, but that they were unable
of different ages responded to stacked sequences of neu- to provide it.
tral requests for clarification. The purpose of the investi- For subjects in all four age groups, repetition was the
gation was to determine what types of responses were most frequently used repair strategy. This was not sur-
produced, as well as to determine if these responses prising considering the general or neutral nature of the
differed according to subject age and the position of the requests for clarifieation that were used in this study.
request within the sequence. Garvey (1977) referred to these types of requests as
A review of the results indicated that subjects in all age nonspeeifie requests for repetition, and in the current
groups responded to the majority of the requests for study, these requests most frequently elicited repetitions.
clarification by providing some type of conversational Corsaro (!976) suggested that the first function of this
repair. In most cases, the subjects complied with these type of request was to indicate that an utterance was
requests by repeating, revising, or supplementing their unintelligible or had not been heard. If this were the
messages ~in response to listener feedback. However, case, a repetition would be an appropriate response.
instances in which subjects made no attempt to repair, or However, Langford (198D noted that these neutral re-
produced inappropriate responses, were also evident. quests for clarification were more often used to indicate
Analysis of these inappropriate responses offered some that some part of a message had not been understood,
insight into the nature of the stacked sequences of re- even though it had been heard. Thus, a repetition could
quests as well as into the differences demonstrated by provide adequate repair, but other strategies that revised
subject groups. Overall, the number of inappropriate or added to the original message would also be effective.
responses decreased with age. The 3-year-old subjects In addition, when neutral requests for clarification are
produced significantly more inappropriate responses than stacked, as in this study, the determining function of the
any of the older groups. It might be argued that the second and third requests might differ from that of the
comparatively large number of inappropriate responses first request in the sequence. In other words, if the first
produced by 3-year-olds reflected the difficulty of the request indicated that the message was not intelligible or
entire picture description task. However, it should be interpretable, and a repetition was provided, then the
noted that almost two-thirds of the responses of the second and third requests might indicate that the mes-
3-year-olds to the initial request for clarification were sage was received, but still not understood in its present
appropriate repairs. In these cases, the subjects evidently form The speaker would then be pressed to provide
recognized that clarification was needed, and provided it. some additional clarification by using different strategies.
While the 3-year-old subjects often were able to provide This appeared to be the case in this study, in which the
a single repair, a trend was obseived that suggested that greatest number of repetitions were produced in re-
they had difficulty responding to the indication that this sponse to the first request for clarification, "Huh?" As the
repair had not provided sufficient clarification. This pat- examiner continued to press for clarification, repetitions
tern was m o r e pronounced in the 5-year-old subjects. were used with less frequency. It was of note that the
These children produced a relatively small number of repetition category was the only strategy to decrease in
inappropriate responses to the first and second requests frequency as the requests were stacked. It is interesting
for clarification. However, the number of inappropriate to consider why this happened at different age levels.
responses to the third request increased significantly. Although the younger subjects were able to respond to
One explanation of this difficulty is that although the the initial request for clarification with a repetition, they
5-year-olds realized that "Huh?" and "What?" were had more difficulty responding to the second request, and
requests for additional feedback, they did not have the marked difficulty responding to the third request using
same understanding o f " I didn't understand that." How- any strategy. This was indieated by the increasing num-
ever , it seems more likely that their difficulty stemmed ber of inappropriate responses produced by younger
from the fact that this request was the third indication in subjects to the second and third requests. In contrast,
a row that the message had not been understood. It older subjects appeared more aware that if a repetition
appeared that these subjects were unable to provide did not provide elarification, other alternatives might be
further clarification. However, it was not necessarily the effective. Additionally, 9-year-old subjects had a wider
ease that these 5--year-old subjects lacked strategies to variety of repair strategies available to them.
repair, because they demonstrated the ability to repeat, The revision category included repairs that involved

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
BRINTON ET AL.: Conversational Repair Strategies 81

the revision of form while content was held constant. In tempted to identify the source of the breakdown, and
these cases, subjects chose alternate labels for items they then select repair strategies accordingly. This was illus-
had described, or altered the syntactic form of the mes- trated by the fact that some 9-year-olds provided back-
sage without providing any additional information. This ground context, defined terms, or discussed the source of
strategy was used relatively infrequently by subjects in misunderstanding in response to the third request in the
all age groups. It appeared that subjects preferred to sequence.
repair by either repeating the previous message or by These findings suggest some developmental patterns in
adding information in some way. Although revisions the way that children of different ages respond to se-
limited to form were relatively rare, repair strategies that quenced requests for clarification. This exploration of
added information were fairly common. Children in all clarification sequences taps into an important aspect of
age groups provided repairs that added information to communicative competence, the ability to cooperate with
each request type in the sequence. a listener to communicate a message. Continued investi-
Older subjects repaired by adding information more gation into the acquisition of these and other types of
often than younger subjects. Nine-year-olds and conversational repair mechanisms will contribute to the
7-year-olds added information significantly more often understanding of how children adapt to listeners in con-
than did 3-year-olds, and 9-year-olds added information versation.
more frequently than did 5-year-olds. Subjects also
added information more often as the request sequence
progressed. In fact, 9--year-old subjects produced more ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
additions than repetitions in response to the third re-
quest, "I didn't understand that." The authors would like to thank Dr. Bruce Tomblin and two
Perhaps the most interesting repair strategies were anonymous reviewers for their helpful critiques of this manu-
script. We would also like to thank Randy Rancl~ for his technical
those classified as cues. These strategies were employed assistance in the preparation of this manuscript and Renee
to define a term within the original utterance, provide Bohman for her assistance in subject selection.
contextual background, or discuss the possible source of
misunderstanding. It was notable that these strategies
were rarely used by 3-, 5--, and 7-year-old subjects. Only REFERENCES
9--year-olds provided these cues in any numbers, and
then only in response to the third reques t for clarification BROWN, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cam-
in the sequence. Thus, it appeared that 9-year-olds bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
recognized that the examiner's third request for clarifica- CORSARO, W. A. (1976). The clarification request as a feature of
adult interactive Styles with young children. Language i n
tion indicated that previous repairs had been inadequate. Society, 6, 183-207.
In addition, they were able to respond with a variety of DUNCAN, S. (1973). Toward a grammar for dyadic conversation.
sophisticated methods for dealing with the difficulty. Semiotica, 9, 29--46.
These subjects seemed to address what they perceived as GALLAGHER, T. (1977). Revision behaviors in the speech of
normal children developing language. Journal of Speech and
the source of the breakdown by defining terms they felt Hearing Research, 20, 303=318.
were unfamiliar, providing background context in which GALLAGHER , T. (1981). Contingent query sequences with
their message could be interpreted, or talking about the adult--child discourse. Journal of Child Language, 8, 51-62.
process of formulating a repair. GARVEY, C. (1977). The contingent query: A dependent act in
conversation. In M. Lewis and L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), Inter-
In summary, subjects at all four age levels seemed to action, conversation, and the development of language. (pp.
recognize the obligatory nature of neutral requests for 63-93), New York: Wiley.
clarification. The children demonstrated sensitivity to GELFAND, D. M., & HARTMANN, D. P. (1983). Child behavior
this type of listener feedback by employing strategies to analysis and therapy (2nd ed.) Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
LANGFORD, D. (1981). The clarification request sequence in
provide conversational repairs in response to the re- conversation between mothers and their children. In P.
quests. While a variety of repair strategies was elicited, French & M. Maclure (Eds.), Adult-child conversation. (pp.
the use of these strategies differed according to subject 159-184) New York: St. Martin's Press.
SPILTON,D., & LEE, L. C. (1977/. Some determinants of effective
age and the position of the request within the sequence. communication in four-year-olds. Child Development, 48,
Although it is difficult to separate the influence of the 968-977.
type of request (Huh?, What?, I didn't understand that.) STARK, R. E., & TALLAL, P. (1981). Selection of children with
from the effect of the position of the request within the specific language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 46, 114-123.
sequence, it seemed likely that the position of the request
was extremely important. Younger subjects had increas- Received January 24, 1985
ing difficulty providing repairs as the sequence pro- Accepted September 18, 1985
gressed, while older subjects usually responded appro-
priately to a!l the requests in the sequence. In addition, Requests for reprints should be sent to Bonnie Brinton,
Speech Pathology and Audiology, University of Nevada School
the 9-year-old subjects demonstrated a wider variety of of Medicine, Mackay Science Building, Room 108, Reno, NV
repair strategies. In some cases, the older subjects at- 89557.

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 03/24/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like