Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

SELF-EFFICACY who have a high sense of efficacy visualize success sce-

narios that provide positive guides for performance. Those


who judge themselves as inefficacious are more inclined
Perceived self-efficacy is concerned with people’s beliefs to visualize failure scenarios that undermine performance
in their ability to influence events that affect their lives. by dwelling on personal deficiencies and on how things
This core belief is the foundation of human motivation, will go wrong. A major function of thought is to enable
performance accomplishments, and emotional well-being people to predict the occurrence of events and to create the
(Bandura, 1997, 2006). Unless people believe they can means for exercising control over those that affect their
produce desired effects by their actions, they have little daily lives. It requires a high sense of efficacy to stick
incentive to undertake activities or to persevere in the face with the laborious cognitive activity needed to extract pre-
of difficulties. Whatever other factors may serve as guides dictive and operational knowledge from information that
and motivators, they are rooted in the core belief that one contains many ambiguities, redundancies, and uncertain-
can make a difference by one’s actions. ties. The stronger the perceived self-efficacy, the more
People’s beliefs about their efficacy are developed from effective people are in their analytic thinking and in
four principal sources of information. The most effective devising successful courses of action (Wood & Bandura,
way of instilling a strong sense of efficacy is through 1989).
mastery experiences. Successes build a robust belief in
one’s personal efficacy. Failures undermine it, especially if
Motivational Processes
frequent failures occur in early phases in the development
of competencies. Development of resilient self-efficacy Beliefs of personal efficacy play a central role in the
requires experiences in overcoming obstacles through self-regulation of motivation. Most human motivation
perseverant effort. The second source of information is is cognitively generated. In cognitive motivation, people
social modeling. Models serve as sources of competencies motivate themselves and guide their actions anticipato-
and motivation. Seeing people similar to oneself succeed rily through the exercise of forethought. They form beliefs
by perseverant effort raises observers’ beliefs in their about what they can do, they anticipate likely outcomes of
own capabilities. Social persuasion is the third type of prospective actions, and they set goals for themselves and
influence. Realistic boosts in efficacy can lead people to plan courses of action designed to realize valued futures.
exert greater effort, which increases their chances of Different theories—attribution theory, expectancy-value
success. People also rely partly on their physiological theory, and goal theory—have been built around these
and mood states in judging their capabilities. They various forms of cognitive motivators.
read their tension, anxiety, and depression as signs of Perceived self-efficacy operates as a central factor in
personal vulnerability. In activities that require strength each of these forms of cognitive motivation (Bandura,
and stamina, they read their fatigue and lack of energy 1986, 1991). Efficacy beliefs bias the extent to which people
as indicators of low physical efficacy. This fourth way attribute their successes and failures to personal capabili-
of altering self-efficacy beliefs is to enhance physical ties or to external factors. People act on their beliefs about
strength and stamina, reduce stress and depression, and what they can do, as well as on their beliefs about the
correct misinterpretations of somatic states. likely outcomes of various actions. The effects of outcome
expectancies on performance motivation are, therefore,
partly governed by self-beliefs of efficacy. There are many
Efficacy-Activated Processes activities that, if done well, guarantee valued outcomes,
Self-efficacy beliefs regulate human functioning in four but they are not pursued by people who doubt they can
major ways: cognitive, motivational, emotional, and selec- do what it takes to succeed. Perceived self-efficacy also
tion processes. contributes in several ways to motivation through goal
systems (Bandura, 1991; Locke & Latham, 1990). It is
partly on the basis of efficacy beliefs that people choose
Cognitive Processes what challenges to undertake, how much effort to expend
Much human behavior, being purposive, is regulated by in the endeavor, and how long to persevere in the face of
forethought embodying cognized goals. Personal goal set- obstacles and failures.
ting is influenced by self-appraisal of capabilities. The Human attainments and positive well-being require
stronger the perceived self-efficacy, the higher the goal an optimistic sense of personal efficacy (Bandura, 1986,
challenges people set for themselves and the firmer their 1997). This is because ordinary social realities are strewn
commitment to meeting them. with difficulties. In a world full of impediments, failures,
Most courses of behavior are initially shaped in thought. adversities, setbacks, frustrations, and inequities, people
People’s beliefs about their efficacy influences the types of must have a robust sense of personal efficacy to sustain
anticipatory scenarios they construct and rehearse. Those the perseverant effort needed to succeed.

The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology, edited by Irving B. Weiner and W. Edward Craighead.
Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
2 SELF-EFFICACY

Affective Processes Selection Processes


People’s beliefs in their coping capabilities also play a The final way in which self-beliefs of efficacy contribute
pivotal role in the self-regulation of emotional states to human adaptation and change concerns selection pro-
(Bandura, 1997). There are three principal ways in which cesses (Bandura, 1995). Beliefs of personal efficacy shape
self-efficacy beliefs affect the nature and intensity of the course of lives by influencing selection of activities
emotional experiences: Such beliefs create attentional and environments. People tend to avoid activities and
biases and influence how potentially aversive life events situations they believe exceed their coping capabilities,
are construed and cognitively represented. They affect but they readily undertake challenging activities and
the exercise of control over perturbing thought patterns. pick social environments they judge themselves capable
And they sponsor courses of action that transform dis- of handling. Any factor that influences choice behavior
tressing and threatening environments into more benign can profoundly affect the direction of personal develop-
ones (Williams, 1992). These alternative paths of influ- ment. This is because the social influences operating in
encing emotional states are well documented in the selected environments continue to promote certain com-
self-regulation of anxiety arousal and depressive mood. petencies, values, and interests long after the decisional
People who believe they can exercise control over poten- determinant has rendered its inaugurating effect. Career
tial threats do not conjure up apprehensive thoughts choice and development is but one example of the power of
and hence are not distressed by them. But those who self-efficacy beliefs to affect the course of life paths through
believe they cannot manage potential threats experience choice-related processes (Hackett, 1995; Lent, Brown, &
high levels of anxiety arousal. They dwell on their cop- Hackett, 1994; Pajares & Urdan, 2006).
ing deficiencies, view many aspects of their environment People with a low sense of efficacy in a given domain
as fraught with danger, magnify the severity of pos- of functioning shy away from difficult tasks, which they
sible threats, and worry about perils that rarely, if ever, tend to perceive as personal threats; have low aspira-
happen. Through such inefficacious thought, they distress tions and weak commitment to the goals they choose; turn
themselves and constrain and impair their functioning. It inward on their self-doubts instead of thinking about how
is not the sheer frequency of perturbing cognitions, but to perform successfully; when faced with difficulties, dwell
the perceived inefficacy to turn them off that is the major on obstacles, the consequences of failure, and their per-
source of distress (Kent & Gibbons, 1987). sonal deficiencies; attribute failures to deficient capability;
In addition, people with a high sense of efficacy to man- slacken their efforts or give up quickly in the face of difficul-
age unpleasant emotional states by palliative means can ties; are slow to recover their sense of efficacy after failures
get themselves to relax, direct their attention to favorable or setbacks; and are prone to stress and depression. People
things, calm themselves, and seek support from friends, who have a strong sense of efficacy, by contrast, approach
family, and others. For those who believe they can get difficult tasks as challenges to be mastered rather than as
relief in these ways, anxiety and sadness are easier to threats to be avoided; set challenging goals and sustain
tolerate. strong commitment to their goals; concentrate on how to
A low sense of efficacy to gain and maintain what one perform successfully rather than on disruptive personal
values highly contributes to depression in at least three concerns in the face of problems; attribute failures to
ways. One route is through unfulfilled aspiration. People insufficient effort or deficient knowledge and skills that
who impose on themselves standards of self-worth that are remediable; redouble their effort in the face of obsta-
they judge they cannot attain drive themselves to bouts of cles; quickly recover their sense of efficacy after failures
depression. A second efficacy route to depression is through or setbacks; and display low vulnerability to stress and
a low sense of social efficacy to develop social relationships depression.
that bring satisfaction to one’s life and cushion the adverse
effects of chronic stressors. A low sense of social efficacy
REFERENCES
contributes to depression both directly and by curtailing
development of socially supportive relationships (Hola- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
han & Holahan, 1987). The third route to depression is cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
through thought control efficacy. People live in a psychic Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through antic-
environment largely of their own making. Much human ipatory and self-regulatory mechanisms. In R. A. Dienstbier
depression is cognitively generated by dejecting rumina- (Ed.), Perspectives on motivation: Nebraska symposium on motiva-
tive thought. A low sense of efficacy to exercise control over tion (Vol. 38, pp. 69–164). Lincoln: University of Nebraska
ruminative thought contributes to the occurrence, dura- Press.
tion, and recurrence of depressive episodes (Kavanagh & Bandura, A. (Ed.) (1995). Self-efficacy in changing societies. New
Wilson, 1989). York: Cambridge University Press.
SELF-EFFICACY 3

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and
Freeman. task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Per- Pajares, F., & Urdan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Self-efficacy beliefs of adoles-
spectives on Psychological Science, 1, 164–180. cents. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Hackett, G. (1995). Self-efficacy in career choice and develop- Schwarzer, R. (Ed.). (1992). Self-efficacy: Thought control of action.
ment. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing societies Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
(pp. 232–258). New York: Cambridge University Press. Williams, S. L. (1992). Perceived self-efficacy and phobic disabil-
Holahan, C. K., & Holahan, C. J. (1987). Self-efficacy, social ity. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action
support, and depression in aging: A longitudinal analysis. (pp. 149–176). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Journal of Gerontology, 42, 65–68. Wood, R. E., & Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory of
Kavanagh, D. J., & Wilson, P. H. (1989). Prediction of outcome organizational management. Academy of Management Review,
with a group version of cognitive therapy for depression. 14, 361–384.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 333–347.
Kent, G., & Gibbons, R. (1987). Self-efficacy and the control of ALBERT BANDURA
anxious cognitions. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Stanford University
Psychiatry, 18, 33–40.
Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a uni- See also: Self-Control; Self-Protection; Social Learning
fying social cognitive theory of career and academic interest, Theory
choice, and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45,
79–122.

You might also like